HTHS AN ENVESTIGATmfi GE THE CONSTRUCT VALSDETY 0F MARITAL ADJUSTMENT AND THE SiMlLARITY BETWEEN MARS‘IAL ADJUST: EN? OF SPOUSES Thesis for the Degree a. M. ii. MECHSGAN STATE UN!‘~1’L'1$3TY Kees C. Human 39.6.6 .‘-.'—0_ —--_.-.—.-‘ LIBRA R Y 1‘" Michigan State University P u (tmsrmazkwmrxrfléah A,” {"5 f: H 0 T5"? {3}” 1 :‘als’.’ (fie ‘ ., . ‘ $2? “‘3 _.l' .940? M PM?! away. ABSTRACT AN INVESTIGATION OF THE CONSTRUCT VALIDITY OF MARITAL ADJUSTMENT AND THE SIMILARITY BETWEEN MARITAL ADJUSTMENT OF SPOUSES by Kees C. Hofman Several instruments purported to measure marital adjustment are described in recent literature. High correlations among the independent instruments, to be expected if marital adjustment is a valid construct, are not consistently reported. Similar ambiguity of evidence is found regarding the degree of similarity of marital ad- Justment between spouses. The Marital Adjustment Test (MAT), the Marital Roles Inventory (MRI), the Family Con- cept Q Sort (FCQ) and a provisional, time saving variation of the FCQ, the Family Concept Scale (FCT-F), were admin- istered to twenty-five married couples under testing conditions which prevented cooperation. MAT and FCQ scores were correlated to a high degree, while MRI scores were Kees C. Hofman independent of MAT and FCQ scores, thus supporting the construct validity of marital adjustment and casting doubt on the utility of the MRI as a measure of marital adjustment. Interspouse correlations were substantially lower than those most frequently reported, suggesting the failure of previous research to control the testing conditions adequately, and that marital adjustment of spouses is relatively independent. The FCT-F was found to be internally reliable and yielded scores correlating to a high degree with FCQ and MAT scores. / Approved/ ,44ég€;(;4/Z:~7 ,7Chairman,Thef 5 Committee .1 Dat e ’l; AM [0-4223 Thesis Committee: JOhn R. Hurley, Chairman Lucy R. Ferguson Dozier W. Thornton AN INVESTIGATION OF THE CONSTRUCT VALIDITY OF MARITAL ADJUSTMENT AND THE SIMILARITY BETWEEN MARITAL ' ADJUSTMENT OF SPOUSES By Kees C. Hofman A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology May, 1966 ACKNOWLEDGMENT Acknowledgment is gratefully extended to Dr. John R. Hurley, chairman of my committee. His guid- ance and assistance in the delineation of the problem, the execution of the investigation and the preparation of this manuscript is deeply appreciated. I also wish to thank Dr. D. Thornton and Dr. L. Ferguson for their advice and their willingness to serve on my committee. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENT . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . METHOD , . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A Marital Adjustment Tests Selection of Subjects . . Testing Procedure . . . . : Statistical Analysis . . . RESULTS 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O Biographical Data Sheet . . . . . . . . Marital Adjustment Scores . Correlations among Marital Adjustment Scores Interspouse Correlations . . . . . . . DISCUSSION 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Sampling Procedure . . . Relationships among the Family Concept QLSOrt, the Family Concept Scale and the Marital Ad- justment Test . . . . . . . . Interspouse Correlations . . . . . . The Family Concept Scale . . . . . . . The Marital Roles Inventory . . . . Differences between Husband and Wife Score Implications for further Research . . . SUMMARY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iii 8 O O O O O Page ii i? TABLE 1. LIST OF TABLES Page Sample Attribute Means, Standard Deviations, and Ranges compared with Characteristics of Locke and Wallace' and Hurvitz' Samples . . . . . . . IA Mean.Marital Adjustment Scores and Standard De- viations Compared with those Obtained by Locke and Wallace, Hurvitz, and van der Veen, 33 al,. 15 Coorelations among Couples' Scores and Correla- tions among Individuals' Scores for the Separate Tests. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16 Correlations among HusbandS' Scores and among Wives' Scores for the Separate Tests . . . . . 16 Comparison of Correlations among Individuals' Scores and those Obtained by van der Veen, £2 £0 ’ and HurVitz O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 l7 Interspouse Correlations for the Marital Ad- justment Tests and those Reported by others . . 18 iv LIST OF APPENDICES APPENDIX Page A. Marital Adjustment Tests and Biographical Data Sheet . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A-l B. Letter . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A-9 C. Raw Scores on Marital Adjustment Tests for Entire Sample a o o o o o o o o o o o o o o A‘lo INTRODUCTION Attempts to assess the quality of the marital re- lationship have been made by many researchers since the late 1920's. An early impressive, and ambitious study was made by the psychiatrist Hamilton in 1929. The same year saw the publication of a book by Davis entitled: Factors in the Sex Life of Twenty-Two_Hundred Women. Marital happiness was evaluated inthis study by asking the subject: "Is your marriage happy or unhappy?" A recent rise in attempts at family-unit therapy, and a heightened awareness that the Origin and consequent development ofmaladaptive behavior of children may large- ly be determined by the family relationships, has resulted in renewed attention paid to family evaluation. Since the marital relationship appears to be of central significance it seems timely that some of the basic assumptions made regarding its measurement be re-evaluated. The earliest as well as subsequent attempts at marital evaluation were made with the assumption that the relationship between two married people has a particular quality; moreover, this quality can be measured in terms of a happy - unhappy, satisfactory - unsatisfactory, or well-adjusted - poorly- l adjusted continuum. Another assumption frequently made regarding the construct of marital happiness is that the effects of a marital relationship are present in both partners to a nearly equal degree. It is therefore ex- pected that both partners of most marriages will be very-, moderately-, or dis-satisfied with their marriage, with relatively few instances where one partner is well-satis- fied while the other is dis-satisfied or vice versa. It is the purpose of this study to investigate the tenability of these hypotheses. Since the late 1920's many attempts at marital assessment have appeared. Most of the authors of these have entertained different theories regarding the most promising way in which to measure this construct. Kirk- patrick (1937) identified marital adjustment with the social stimulus value of a marriage. Terman (1938), how- ever, contended that marital happiness is a subjective phenomena which can therefore be measured only by evaluating the experience of the marital partners. In recent years new approaches have appeared. Examples are the Marital Roles Inventory (MRI) deve10ped by Hurvitz (1959) and the Family Concept Q Sort (FCQ) introduced by van der Veen, gt_gl. (I964). The Marital Adjustment Test (MAT) developed by Locke and Wallace (1959) is an instrument which is based on several earlier ones. The authors of the three last mentioned tests all approached the problem of evaluating marital adjustment differently. Hurvitz (1959) approached marital adjustment within the framework of role theory. Marital adjustment is assumed to exist when the partners carry out their roles in the way their spouses want or prefer them to carry out their roles. Marital strain or unhappiness is assumed, for example, when there is a discrepancy between the roles the wife would like her husband to see as important, and the roles which the husband actually considers important. Locke and wallace (1959) approach the evaluation of a marital relationship by questioning the marriage partners directly concerning their experienced satisfaction with the marriage and the degree of cooperation and similarity of attitudes between them. Van der Veen, gt 3;, (1964) used a Q-sort of statements de- Scriptive of the family relationships. Marital adjustment is postulated when a member of a family describes the family similar to an ideal family as defined by the description in terms of the same Q-sort by a group of judges. Information regarding the validity and reliability of these tests is available in the literature. Hurvitz (1959) argues, §_priori, that the MRI is a measure of mari- tal adjustment. He backs up the argument with small but significant correlations between the husband's MRI score and the marital adjustment of both the husband (a: -.22, §_= 10A) and wife (§_= -.23, N.= 104). Locke and Wallace (1959) report a split-half reliability of .90 (N_= 236), and a significant difference between the MAT scores of a high and low adjustment group as measured by an independent process. Van der Veen, 33 31. (196A) report that the FCQ scores of a high adjustment group were significantly higher than the scores of a low adjustment group (N.= 20), and that the test-retest reliability was found to be .71. Since all three tests attempt to measure the same construct, viz. marital adjustment, and if it is assumed that the tests are independent instruments, the validity of this construct would be supported by a high degree of agree- ment of the separate tests concerning the marital adjustment of individuals. The evidence available is ambiguous on this point. Van der Veen, £3 31., for example, report (196A) that the FCQ scores of a sample of 20 married couples correlated .67 with their MAT scores. Hurvitz (1959) also correlated the MRI scores of 10A couples with their MAT scores. He reports that the correlations between husband's MRI scores and the husband's and wife's MAT scores were -.22 and —.23 respectively. No significant relationships were found between wives' MRI scores and the MAT scores of either husbands or wives. ' Similar ambiguity exists concerning the equality of two partners' marital adjustment. If this relationship does exist one would expect high correlations between the marital adjustment scores of two married partners. The interspouse correlations reported differ considerably. Burgess and Cottrell (1936), Hurvitz (1959) and van der Veen, gt 3;, (196A) have reported high interspouse correlations. Terman (1938) concluded that the high interspouse correla- tions obtained by Burgess and Cottrell were due to cooperation between Spouses, but that there is a correlation between spouses' scores nonetheless (p. 81 and 82). Powell (1965) reports an interspouse correlation which is sufficiently . low (£_= .12, fl;= A7) to lead her to suggest also that the interspouse correlations reported by some researchers may have been produced by their failure to minimize communica- tion between Spouses, with the resulting performance often being a joint effort, thus creating a misleadingly high interspouse correlation. It is plain that the available evidence falls short of supporting clearly either the construct validity of marital adjustment or the hypothesis that spouses will tend to evaluate their marriage similarly. The present study was designed to yield more definitive evidence on these problems by administering a number of marital adjust- ment tests to a sample of married couples under controlled testing conditions. A secondary function of this investigation was to gain initial information regarding a possible substitute instrument for the FCQ. A substitute is desirable since the FCQ is a relatively time-consuming instrument, as the average subject needs approximately 25-35 minutes to com- plete the task. Another drawback of the FCQ is that if a permanent record of a performance is needed it must be made separately by the tester. A more efficient instru- ment suggested is a true-false checklist (FCT-F) containing the same descriptive items as the FCQ. To sum up, the aims of this study were: 1. to verify the construct validity of marital adjustment by investigating the relationships among the performances of a group of married couples on three independent tests of marital adjustment; 2. to verify the interspouse correla- tions of marital adjustment scores under controlled testing conditions; 3. to determine the correlation between the performances on the FCQ and the FCT-F. METHOD Marital Adjustment Tests Family Concept Q‘Sort (FCQ): The FCQ consists of 80 items which are descriptive Of a family. The subject is instructed to place these statements, which are presented to him in random order on cards, on a nine point scale on which the extremes are defined as "most like my family" and "least like my family", and to place only a predetermined number of cards in eaCh category. A list of the statements ap- pears in Appendix A. The Family Adjustment Score is determined from the A7 items which are marked. A group of judges were in high agreement thatthe items marked + should appear on the "like" side, and those marked - should appear on the "unlikefi side of the ideal family. A high score indicates high marital adjustment. Family Concept Scale (PCT-F): The PCT-F uses the same Statements as those of theFCQ.‘ However, the subject must now judge each statement as true Or false as applied to his or her family. The subject records the answers on a form.which can be maChine scored. The score is determined by summing the judgments made in agreement with those in- dicative of the ideal family. The list of items is the same as those of the FCQ in Appendix A. 7 Marital Roles Inventory (MRI): The MRI consists of two sets of roles: a set for both the husband and the wife. The husband's set is made up of ten roles and the wife's set of eight roles. The husband ranks his roles in the order of importance in which he actually carries out his roles at the present time. He then ranks his wife's roles in the order of importance in which he prefers his wife to carry out her roles. Similarly, the wife ranks her roles in the order of importance in which she actually carries them out, and then her husband's roles in the order of im- portance in which she would prefer him to carry out his roles. Thus each set of roles is ranked twice: once by the partner to which they apply and once by his or her spouse. The numerical difference in the rank order assigned to each role is cubed and summed for all roles in each set. The cube root taken from this sum of the cubed differences for the husband's role set is the husband's Index of Strain. The wife's Index of Strain is computed in an identical manner for her role set. A low score indicates high marital adjustment. The role sets appear in Appendix A. Marital Adjustment Test (MAT): The MAT is reproduced in Appendix A. It consists of Sixteen questions with multiple- option answers. The answers to these questions had differ- entiated between high and low adjustment groups in previous research. Each Option has been assigned a weight by the authors. The sum of the weights of the answers is the Marital Adjustment Score. A high score indicates high marital adjustment. Biographical Data Sheet (EDS): All subjects completed the BDS to produce information considered.pertinent for com- parison of the results from the present study with other studies performed in the same area. The BDS is reproduced in Appendix A. Selection of Subjects All 25 participating couples, except two, were associated with Michigan State University. Letters asking for the cooperation of both husband and wife were mailed to the occupants of three married housing apartment units at the university. A COpy of the letter is in Appendix B. The letters were followed by a telephone call a few days after the letters could have been expected to arrive. Of the 33 couples contacted in thhimanner, all except one indicated a willingness to participate in the study. How- ever only seventeen couples ultimately completed the instruments. A lack of free time was indicated in all instances as the reason for being unable to complete the instruments. Six couples were volunteers from the intro- ductory course in psychology at the university. The remaining two couples were part of a group therapy project for the parents of patients in a psychiatric day hospital. 10 Testing Procedure All couples completed the instruments either at their home in the examiner's presence or at a group re- search room on the campus. The couples tested at their homes completed the tests at different tables in all instances, and there was no verbal communication between partners. At the group research room the partners com- pleted the tests in different rooms so that communication was impossible. The BDS was completed first. After the BDS the subjects completed either the FCQ or the FCT-F. The second test was either the MAT or the MRI. The third test was again either the MAT or MRI depending on which one had been completed as the second test. The last test was Similarly either the FCQ or the FCT-F. To sum up: the FCQ, the FCT-F, the MAT and the MRI were completed in a random order after all subjects had completed the BDS, except that the FCQ and the FCT-F were completed either first or last. The reason for this stipulation was to minimize the reciprocal influence of these two tests which was assumed to exist since both contain the same Statements. Statistical Analysis Means were computed for the variables for which information was gained from the BDS. Simple product- moment correlations were computed for all pairs of scores 11 on the separate tests. A score for each test was assigned to each couple by adding the raw scores of the partners. Simple product-moment correlations were computed between all pairs of couples' scores. An interspouse correlation was computed for each test by correlating the scores of husband and wife for that test. RESULTS Biographical Data Sheet Table 1 lists the descriptive means, standard deviations and ranges of the present sample as well as those reported by Locke and Wallace (1959) and Hurvitz (1959). Van der Veen, £2.2i' (196A) did not describe their-sample in detail. Sixty percent of the couples were married less than three years, none were married less than Six months. Forty-eight percent of the couples had children, of these 75 percent had one child. The annual income was reported only to the nearest $1,000. The large majority of the sample were of the caucasian race and of middle class background. Four percent of the couples were Jewish. 0f the couples reporting religious affiliation 68 percent were Protestant, 12 percent Catho— lic, and A percent Jewish. Marital Adjustment Scores The mean scores for the separate tests and their standard deviations obtained for the individuals and the couples are shown in Table 2. 12 13 Correlations amonggMarital Adjustment Scores Table 3 presents the simple correlations among couples' and individuals' scores on the different tests. Table A Shows the correlations among the husband's and wives' scores. From these tables it is clear that the marital adjustment scores of an individual or a couple as measured by the FCQ, the MAT, and the FCT-F are highly related. The correlations between these scores are all Significantly different from zero at beyond the .005 level. It is also plain from tables 3 and A that an individual's Or couple's MRI score is independent of his or their score on any of the other tests. The correlations obtained in the present study are compared with those obtained elsewhere in Table 5. Interspouse Correlations The interSpouse correlations obtained are presented in Table 6. All, except the MRI interspouse correlation, do not differ Significantly from zero at the .05 level. Since the MRI does not seem to measure marital satisfaction, the present data do not support the hypothesis that the marital satisfaction of husband and wife tend to be nearly equal. Interspouse correlations obtained by others are also presented in Table 6. 14 mm mm em soweefiafieea usoflmflaem weapaomom owmpceonom $0m Rom Row macho amazow cmwmwosmo mmwmmosmo ammomm pcmcwaoemnm ammowmmemohm name use seaaoo these oooe . coon awe ooom easesH assess eeueafieU m eaaso the Sam "use: seeeaaso oz use a u a oa.a he. seaeaflsp es henssz «.ma m.m n.0H . m. oo.s o.m smashes: no nude» mo nopasz haemateto gem SH as . oa mm.a ea heaters emeaaoo See me we . Na me.a m.ma meats nowpmosum mo whee» no nonasz mm cm mm . ma ma.m mm ueHSSem os em os . om mm.s SN meat: ow< mom a 2 0mm u 2 cm «.2 meme: memos owdmm am. memo: lemma. sesshse leeway taasem sauteed paneepss oomHHmziwcm oxooq moadfidm .npaensm use .oomaamz use oxooq mo moanmfinopomnmso apas_venmaaoo momnmm use .MSOHumaeon unevempm .memoz epsnanpp< oamsmm .H edema 15 .OonSom Hmcwwflno SH vopnogos poz I he. .2 one mopSOHUSH mpoxomnn SH eomoaone hopes: one “6902 as e.ooa as e.em Aoav eases .hua Sea a: N.H~H as «.mm . Aoav awmtm1.hea em“: “dead. .Hm no .Soo> new cm> n: m.m . Reedy mo>a3 a: H.o Asoav.mecmnmsm . Ammmav upw>asm ts e.ae less haste .nea See .us m.mma “Sea duets .nea swam “ommav oomaamz use oxooq m.e e.om m.m o.oa o.am s.mm~ m.m m.os Ammv usagdoo m.¢ «.me o.~ 0.: ¢.oa S.SHH H.m H.mm “Omv mamseH>HUSH m.s m.m¢ S.H ~.s m.ma m.maa S.m s.mm a 5mm. mo>H3 m.m o.m¢ ¢.N 0.0 b.m~ ¢.maa H.© o.mm “may meemnms: oaaamm anemone mm. msmmfi mm mcmoz om names pm meets sawem knopno>eH pmoe aces pnomld. wanedm paoocoo hHaamm moamm Hmpfinmzupmsnn< Havana: paeoSMDTAHHamm awm.mm .Soo> new no» one .upfi>nsm .eoeaamz use oxooq he eoqawppo omonp and: uenmaaoo mzoapmueon vamvampm use menoom pcoapmsfic< Hmpwnmz new: .N magma 16 Table 3. Correlations among Couples' Scores and correlations among Individuals' Scores for the Separate Tests FCQ MAT FCT-F MRI Family Concept A Q Sort .68 .78 .09 Marital Adjustment Test .66 .76 .03 Family Concept Scale .72 .77 -.02 Marital Roles Inventory .03 -.06 .01 Note: The correlations above the diagonal indicate correla- tions among couples' scores, those below indicate correlations among individuals' scores. Table A. Correlations among Husbands' Scores and among Wives' Scores for the Separate Tests FCQ MAT FCT-F MRI Family Concept Marital Adjustment Test .51 .7A -.10 Family Concept Marital Roles Inventory .22 .17 .09 Note: The correlations above the diagonal indicate correla- tions among husbands' scores, those below indicate correlations among wives' scores. 17 Table 5. Comparison of Correlations among Individuals' Scores and those Obtained by van der Veen, et a1. and Hurvitz Correlated Present Author Scores Sample Van der Veen, et a1. (All N's =10) Family Adjustment Scores and . Marital Adjustment Scores of: High Adjustment Group .AA Low Adjustment Group .65 . Combined Sample (N = 20) .67 .66 Hurvitz (All N's = 10A) HusbandsT Index of Strain and Marital Adjustment Score of: Husbands -.22 -.10 Wives -.23 -.29 Wives' Index of Strain and Marital Adjustment Scores of: Husbands -.O8 .38 Wives -.O7 .18 l8 0 a ma mo. sehaaseseta e empaoetm p02 a eoa u.m Remedy uufieusm mm u.m Ameoav .HHoSOS mamamm uosfinaoo OH TLW anono pcoapmsfiv< 304 as u z eases emwewmsfie< swam Aeomav .Hm no .Soo> new sm> Antsy oamom pmoosoo hawamm anoSSO>SH moaom Humane: Shea peeepmene< Hepfitez whom a paeonoo mawamm Ed. was. om. mm. m2 0m. NH. om. amz we. ob. mm u_m paemonm whonuo mqofiumHOMLoo nopsoawnoaxm Shae untapuSnea amused: mnonpo he voppoaom chomp use memos pmeapmanvd Havana: one pom mmoaumaonnou ammoamnech .o oases DISCUSSION SamplingiProcedure The response rate from the couples contacted by mail was 52 percent. The remainder of the sample con- sisted of six volunteers from an introductory class in psychology and two couples drawn from a group therapy project for parents of patients at a psychiatric day hospital. Even though all couples, except one, who were contacted and did not complete the instruments indicated a lack of free time as the reason for their non-partici- pation, it is probable that other selective factors resulted in a sample biased toward being well-adjusted. When considering the magnitude of the correlations among the scores on the separate tests, it seems quite clear that the most probable effect of the non-randomness of the sample is a restriction of the scores to the more highly adjusted range, and an associated attenuation of the correlations. Had the sample been more representative; i.e. if it had also included couples with low marital ad- justment, the correlation coefficients obtained might have been higher. A similar consideration is appropriate for 19 20 the impact of the non-randomness of the sample on the correlation between the FCQ and the FCT-F scores. A different picture emerges when considering the impact of the non-randomness with regard to the compara- bility of the present results with those obtained by other researchers. Here a difference in degree of representa- tiveness would certainly have a detrimental effect on the value of any conclusion drawn. Review of the sampling procedures used by Locke and wallace (1959), Hurvitz (1959), and van der Veen, gt Elm (196A) makes it plain that their samples are also far from truly randomly selected Samples. The best evidence for the comparability of results is the distribution of marital adjustment scores obtained by the separate researchers as well as the distributions of bio- graphical data. A comparison of the sample characteristics and marital adjustment scores is presented in Tables 1 and 2. Table 1 Shows that the average subject of the present sample was considerably younger, and had been married for a shorter period of time than that of the other studies. The subjects of the Hurvitz sample were considerably older, had been married longer, and had a greater number of children per family. The samples seem to be very similar interms of education, religious affiliation and racial predominance with the notableexception that 50 percent of the Hurvitz sample was Jewish. The average annual income of the present 21 sample was considerably lower than that of the other samples but it must be remembered that at least one, and often two of the Spouses of twenty-three of the twenty-five couples were students. Van der Veen, 32.31, (196A) do not report any of the sample characteristics in detail. Review of the selection procedure used suggests that their sample is com- parable to that used by Hurvitz. The present scores are also quite similar to those reported of the other samples. From Table 2 it can be seen that the mean scores for the present sample of the FCQ and MAT are slightly below those reported for high-adjustment sub-groups of van der Veen, gt_§l, (196A) and Locke and Wallace (1959), while the current scores are well above those reported for their low-adjustment sub-groups. Trends in the mean Index of strain for husbands and wives are not as clear. The mean Index of Strain of the husbands is higher than that reported by Hurvitz (1959) while the reverse is true for the Index of Strain of the wives. These comparisons suggest that the non-random selection procedure of the present sample does not produce any gross non-comparability between the present findings and the results of the pertinent prior studies. Relationships among the FCQ, the FCT-F, and the MAT The correlations presented in Tables 3 and A indi- cate clearly that the construct of marital adjustment has 22 some validity as indicated by the high correlations be- tween the FCQ and the MAT scores across these independent instruments. The correlations obtained were likely limited by the non-random selection procedure as discussed above, but they were also attenuated by the limited reliability of the separate tests. Van der Veen, gt_§l. (196A), for example, report a test-retest reliability coefficient of .71 for the FCQ. If it is assumed that the FCQ is a per- fectly valid instrument, the maximum possible correlation between the FCT-F and the FCQ as well as between the MAT and the FCQ is .8A instead of 1.00. No test-retest reliability coefficients are available for the MAT and FCT-F. Interspouse Correlations From Table 6 it is apparent that the interspouse correlation under the controlled test situation which mini- mized communication between the spouses is considerably lower than that reported by most other researchers. The conclusion drawn is that the marriage relationship is not one which is evaluated by the partners in an identical manner. It is rather common for one partner to describe the marriage as a happy or well-adjusted one while the other considers it to be an unhappy and unsatisfactory relation- ship. This finding underlines the need to view the marriage relationship not as an entity in itself, but rather as two 23 attitudes or evaluations from two different people which tend to be related only in the sense that it is their interaction which is evaluated. The Family Concept Scale In view of the high correlation obtained between the FCT-F and the FCQ, and the greater efficiency of the former, it is suggested that the FCT-F be considered as a sbustitute or improved version of the FCQ. Initial re- sults indicate that the FCT-F is internally reliable. (g = .8h, estimated by Kuder Richardson formula.) 0n the basis of feedback to the experimenter re- ceived from the subjects it is also suggested that a further improvement of the FCT-F might be the inclusion of a third, "does not apply" category in addition to the "true“ and “false" choices. This would make the alternatives more similar to those made in the FCQ and might also increase the score variance, thus increasing the correlation between the FCQ and the FCT-F. The Marital Roles Inventory The MRI used in this study is now an outdated form. The experimenter became aware, after most of the data had been collected, that the MRI had been expanded to include eleven roles for both the husband's and wife's set of roles 24 (Hurvitz, 1960). This test has been published and is available from the publisher with a manual. The empirical evidence cited in support of the expanded version is based on correlations with the original test scores. Consequently, the validity of the expanded version stands or falls with the original version used in this study. Even though the present sample differed substan- tially in several respects from that used by Hurvitz, it is concluded that the MRI does not measure the quality of a marital relationship as perceived by the partners. The only empirical evidence cited by Hurvitz in support of the validity of the instrument is the correlation between the husband's Index of Strain and the husband's and wife's MAT score. This correlation was not replicated. Differences between Husband and Wife Scores From the standard deviations in Table 2 it can be concluded that the wives are a more homogeneous group in terms of marital adjustment scores than the husbands. The average marital adjustment scores for the wives seem to in- dicate a greater marital adjustment for all tests. This may however be due to a greater conformity to social desir- ability on the part of the wives, or it may be that the tests are more insensitive to the important factors in the wife's evaluation of a marriage relationship. 25 Hurvitz' conclusion (1959) that wives are better adjusted than husbands seems puzzling. He based his con- clusion on the fact that the mean Index of Strain for the wives was 5.26 and that of the husbands 6.09. Although this is a highly statistically significant difference, it can be accounted for without inferring any variation in marital adjustment by the fact that the wife's Index of Strain is based on the cube root of the cubed differences in rank order for a set of eight roles, while the husband's Index of Strain is based on a set of ten roles. To illus- trate this, random samples of 25 paired role rankings for eight and ten role sets were drawn from a random digit table. Indices of Strain were computed for both of these samples. The average Index of Strain for the eight randomly matched roles for a sample of 25 was 7.17. This average score was 9.9h for the sets of ten roles. This difference is also highly statistically significant. Thus, the differ- ence in Indices of Strain obtained by Hurvitz can be anticipated solely on the basis of the different number of roles included in each set. Implications for Further Research While the results supported the marital adjustment construct's validity, Hurvitz' claim that the MRI measures marital adjustment (1959) was not supported. It does not 26 necessarily follow that the MRI does not evaluate a useful and valid attribute of a marriage. Whether it does or not is beyond the scope of this study, but should be investi- gated. Similarly, other constructs may prove useful for a better understanding of a marriage. Particularly when pre- diction of marital relation effects on children is of interest more information is needed than the marital adjust- ment scores of the parents. Further research should take into consideration that evaluation of a marital relationship may be independent across Spouses. Further investigation of the reliability and validity of the FCT-F is necessary before it can be recommended as a substitute of the FCQ. SUMMARY Several instruments purported to measure marital adjustment are described in recent literature. High correlations among the independent instruments, to be expected if marital adjustment is a valid construct, are not consistently reported. Similar ambiguity of evidence is found regarding the degree of similarity of marital ad- justment between spouses. The Marital Adjustment Test (MAT), the Marital Roles Inventory (MRI), the Family Con- cept Q Sort (FCQ) and a provisional, time saving variation of the FCQ, the Family Concept Scale (FCT-F), were admin- istered to twenty-five married couples under testing conditions which prevented cooperation. MAT and FCQ scores were correlated to a high degree, while MRI scores were independent of MAT and FCQ scores, thus supporting the construct validity of marital adjustment and casting doubt on the utility of the MRI as a measure of marital adjustment. Interspouse correlations were substantially lower than those most frequently reported, suggesting the failure of previous research to control the testing 27 28 conditions adequately, and that marital adjustment of spouses is relatively independent. The FCT-F was found to be internally reliable and yielded scores correlating to a high degree with FCQ and MAT scores. REFERENCES Burgess, E. W. and Cottrell, L. S. The prediction of ad- justment in marriage. Amer. Soc. Rev. 1936, 1, 737-751. Davis, Katharine B. Factors in the sex life of twentyrtwo hundred women. New York: Harper and Brothers, 1929. Hamilton, C. V. A research in marriage. New York: A & C Boni, Inc., 1929i7 Hurvitz,N. The index of strain as a measure of marital . sagisfaction. Sociol. & Social Res. 1959, pg, Hurvitz, N. The marital roles inventory and the measure- . ment of marital adjustment. J. Clin. Psychol. 1960,16, 377-380. . Kirkpatrick, C. Community of interest and the measurement of marriage adjustment. The Family, 1937, l§J 133- 137. Locke, H. J., and wallace, K. M. Short marital-adjustment and prediction tests: their reliability and vali- dity. Marriage fam. Liv., 1959. 21, 251-255. Powell, Diane, M. A personality inventory approach to the study of marital adjustment. Michigan State Uni- versity. East Lansing, Michigan, 1965. Unpublished Master's Thesis. Terman, L. M. Psychological factors in marital happiness. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1938. Thorndike,6R. L. Personnel Selection. New York: John Wiley, 19 2. van der Veen, F., Huebner, B., and Neja, P. Relationships between the parents' concept of the family and family adjustment. Amer. J. of 0rthopsychiat_y, 196A, 2& 4'5-550 . 29 APPENDICES APPENDIX A MARITAL ADJUSTMENT TESTS Marital Roles Inventory: Instructions: Number the following statements in the order of importance in which you actually carry out your roles or functions in your family at the pre- sent time. (1 being most im ortant; 10, the least important. No ties allowed. ....... I do my (He does his) jobs around the house. ....... I am (He is) a companion to my (his) wife. ....... I help (He helps) the children grow by being their friend, teacher and guide. ....... I earn (He earns) the living and support (supports) the family. ....... I do my (He does his) wife's work around the house if my (his) help is needed. ....... I practice (He practices) the family religion or philosophy... , ....... I am (He is) a sexual partner to my (his) wife. ....... I decide (He decides) when the family is still divided after discussing something. ....... I serve (He serves) as the model of men for my (his) children. - ....... I represent and advance (He represents and advances) my (his) family in the community. ....... I help (She helps) earn the living when my (her) husband needs my (her) help or when the family needs more money. ....... I practice (She practices) the family religion or philoso hy. . ....... I care (She cares) for the children's everyday needs. ....... I am (She is) a companion to my (her) husband. ....... I am (She is) the homemaker. . ....... I am (She is) a sexual partner to my (her) husband. A-l 517 .0“ v.1“ P -. .9 A ‘. . I 7 «0'7; ....... I represent and advance (She represents and ad- vances) my (her) family socially and in the community. . ....... I help (She helps) the children grow by being their friend, teacher and guide. A-3 MARITAL ADJUSTMENT TEST Instructions: Encircle the dot on the scale below which best describes the degree of happiness, everything considered, of your present marriage. The middle point, "happy" re- presents the degree of happiness which most people get from marriage, and the scale gradually ranges on one side to those few who are very unhappy in marriage, and on the other, to those few who experience extreme joy or felicity in marriage. 0 2 7 15 20 25 35 Very Happy Perfectly Unhappy - Happy State the approximate extent of agreement between you and your mate on the following items. Please encircle the ap- propriate dots. Almost Almost Occa- Fre- Always Always Always Always sionally quently Dis- Dis- Agree Agree Disagree Disagree agree agree Handling family finances: 5 4 3 2 1 0 Matters of Recrea- tion: 5 4 3 2 l 0 Demonstrations of affection: 8 6 A 2 1 0 Friends: 5 4 3 2 1 0 Sex Relations: 15 12 9 4 l 0 Conventionality (right, good or proper conduct): 5 4 3 2 l 0 Philosophy of life: 5 A 3 2 1 0 ways of dealing ‘ with in-laws: 5 4 3 2 l 0 When disagreements arise, they usually result in: hus- band's giving in 0 , wife giving in 2 , agreement by mutual give and take 10 . A-# Do you and your mate engage in outside interests to- gether? All of them 10 , some of them 8 , very few of them 3', none of them 0 ? In leisure time do you generally prefer: to be "on the go" - , to stay at home + ?* Does your mate generalIy prefer: to be "on the go" - , to stay at home + ?* Do you ever wish you had not married? Frequently 0 , occasionally 3 , rarely 8 , never 15 , If you had your life to live over, do you think you would: marry the same person-l§ , marry a different person 0 , not marry at all 1 ? Do you confide in your mate: almost never 0 , rarely 2 , in most things 10 , in everything 10 ? *+-=2;++=10;--=3 (Locke and Wallace, 1959. p. 252) A-s Family Concept Q-Sort: Instructions: Here is a set of cards, with a statement on each card. Please put each card on one of the spaces of this card, according to how much it is like your family. Zero means most unlike and eight means most like, and the points between are varying degrees of these. By family we mean the persons who live at your home and whom you consider to be part of your family group. We want you to describe this family group as honestly and accurately as you can. Put each card on the pile that indicates how much this statement is true for your family as it really is. It is important that you be completely accurate in describing your family. You can take as much time as you wish. It seems to go best when you work rapidly and arrange all the items and then go over the items again to arrange them more exactly. Only a certain number of items should go in each pile as is marked on the card. Please proceed care- fully, and ask me if you have any questions, or if there is anything you are not sure of. 1. We like to do new and different things. * 2. We usually can depend on each other. * 3. We have a number of close friends. A. We often do not agree on important matters. 5. Each of us tries to be the kind of person the others will like. 6. Good manners and proper behavior are very im- portant to us. * 7. We feel secure when we are with each other. - 8. we want help with our problems. * 9. We do many things together. - 10. Each of us wants to tell the others what to do. - 11. There are serious differences in our standards and values. * 12. We feel free to express any thought or feeling to each other. 13. Our home is the center of our activities. 14. We are an affectionate family. 15. It is not our fault that we are having difficulties. 16. Little problems often become big ones for us. We do not understand each other. 18. We get along very well in the community. 19. We often praise or compliment each other. 20. We do not talk about sex. 21. We get along much better with persons outside the family than with each other. Ilfl'fl’lllfl’fl'v g...» \1 *Il-n' «fl» . 22. 23. 21*. 250 26. 28. 29. 30. 31. 320 33. 3h. 35. 36. 37- 38. 39. i to. Al. A2. 43. Ah. ' #5. *fi-I *- #6. 47. A8. A9. 500 51. . 520 530 54. . 55 56: 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 62. 63. A-6 If we had more money most of our present problems would be gone. We are proud of our family. We do not like each other's friends. There are many conflicts in our family. We are usually calm and relaxed when we are together. We are not a talkative family. We respect each other's privacy. Accomplishing what we want to do seems to be diffi- cult for us. We tend to worry about many things. Weoften upset each other without intending it. Nothing exciting ever seems to happen to us. We are a deeply religious family. We are continually getting to know each other better. We need each other. We do not spend enough time together. We do not understand what is causing our difficulties. Success and prestige are very important to us. We encourage each other to develop in his or her own individual way. We are ashamed of some things about our family. We have warm, close relationships with each other. There are some topics which we avoid talking about. Together we can overcome almost any difficulty. We really do trust and confide in each other. We make many demands on each other. We take care of each other. Our activities together are usually planned and organized. The family has always been very important to us. We get more than our share of illness. We are considerate of each other. We can stand up for our rights if necessary. We are all responsible for our family problems. There is not enough discipline in our family. We have very good times together. We depend on each other too much. We often become angry at each other. We live largely by other people's standards and values. We are not as happy as we might be. We are critical of each other. We are satisfied with the way in which we now live. Usually each of us goes his own separate way. We resent each other's outside activities. We have respect for each other's feelings and opinions even when we differ strongly. * 650 - 66. 67. 69. 70. 71. 720 730 - 71+. 75. 76. * 77. r 78. . 79. so. '0')?! fl- A-7 We sometimes wish we could be an entirely differ- ent family. We are sociable and really enjoy being with people. We are a disorganized family. It is important to us to know how we appear to others. Our decisions are not our own, but are forced upon us by circumstances. We are not really fond of one another. We are a strong, competent family. We just cannot tell each other our real feelings. We are not satisfied with anything short of per- fection. We forgive each other easily. We are usually somewhat reserved with each other. We rarely hurt each other's feelings. We like the same things. We usually reach decisions by discussions and com- promise. We can adjust well to new situations. We are liked by most people who know us. We are full of life and good spirits. (van der Veen, gt al., 196A) * indicates items indicative of the perfectly adjusted family. - indicates items indicative of the poorly adjusted family. A-8 Family Concept Scale: Instructions: Indicate whether each of the following items is mostly true or mostly false as it applies to your immed- iate family, (Husband or wife and children) and fill in the appropriate response on the accompanying answer form. First impressions are satisfactory, and most people are able to complete the scale in less than 15 minutes. It is quite important that you give a reSponse to each item, even though it may sometimes be difficult to make a de- cision. The rest of the instrument consists of the same items as those of Family Concept Q-Sort. Biographical Data Sheet: Date: Number Age: Date of Marriage: . month year Occupation List the age and sex of your children, oldest first. Is this your first marriage? Highest grade attained in school Your approximate income annually Your religious affiliation if any Couple OQ\I (TWA-PW» NH FHJFJ IOFJCNO O O O O H \n NNNNNNHHI—JH PH \np-wmwoxoooxim kw APPENDIX C A-lO RAW MARITAL ADJUSTMENT SCORES FOR ALL COUPLES FCQ 13 31 31 32 29 35 30 38 34 37 41 30 31 38 A0 35 36 29 21 38 29 MAT AA 118 1A0 124 104 151 KB 107 151 100 132 108 117 111 lfi 117 112 77 127 n6 n8 115 125 MRI {>me ownoct\.r1\1~c0to:—-Imom-4w;~ mmorNHngomqrrmwmommwupqw- 4‘0 N00 010% NF‘W our O\\O OV‘: O\\}\J\O\n\1\»\n \} C‘OQQ O OM) N5te: Columns headed by capital letters indicate male FCT-F fcq 21 37 A6 35 46 30 46 34 AA 3h 47 37 43 32 A7 31 44 36 45 3h 43 39 4h 36 Al 29 Al 32 A6 38 A6 37 48 36 Al 31 37 23 48 37 38 31 A5 33 #0 27 #3 32 A6 34 mat 132 129 127 121 77 122 123 125 141 Kb 123 130 111 121 118 119 1U 112 93 D8 110 111 96 1% UA 000.005 HH-PHOO‘QOQUINmWOmw \nHHGNmew-PHWQHCD mmqmomrwrwarrqrw .52 scores, columns headed by lower case letters indi- cate female scores. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERS TY Ll l 0 3 1293 3015 405 BRARIES 7