ATTREBUSES 9F MEMGRY, RETROAC‘TWE ENHEBETEON, QUEENS MED FREE RECALL LEQREEENG “2.1222323 f3? 2222 3921222253 2 2‘ E25 2’2. 2 ECH’EGAN 3‘2 UTE UMU’ER‘EW U‘2 23:2 LOU. 3E PEWE‘EGTON 5"} it)”; ABSTRACT ATTRIBUTES OF MEMORY, RETROACTIVE INHIBITION, CUEING AND FREE RECALL LEARNING By Joyce Louise Pennington Underwood (1969) hypothesized that encoding multiple attributes of an event can facilitate memory for that event by providing more means of access to the event and that interference between two events can be decreased by encoding multiple attributes of the events and by decreasing the number of attributes the two events have in common. The present study tested these hypotheses by varying the attributes encoded during List 1 learning (semantic, acoustic, or dual) and the attributes encoded during List 2 learning (semantic, acoustic, or dual). Following List 2 learning subjects recalled List 1 once without cues and once when cued with the attributes they had used for encoding list words during original List 1 learning. The attribute encoded was manipulated by varying the organization of the lists, and all subjects learned the same List 1 and List 2 words. For subjects who encoded two attributes of the words, the organization of the list was changed from Trial 1 to Trial 2. The List 1 learning data revealed that semantic organization subjects recalled more than acoustic or dual organization subjects. Thus, multiple encoding does not appear to facilitate learning. In recalling List 1 after List 2 learning, there was no effect due to the number of attributes encoded. However, retroactive inhibition was less for those subjects who received different organizations of successive lists (i. e., List 1 Joyce Louise Pennington semantic organization-List 2 acoustic organization or List 1 acoustic- List 2 semantic organization) than for those who received the same organization of the two lists (i. e., semantic organization of both lists or acoustic organization of both lists). The implications of the data from subjects encoding multiple attributes were not clear. Finally, the cueing effect was larger for the subjects who encoded semantic attributes of List 1 than for those who encoded acoustic attributes or multiple attributes. The results were discussed in terms of the plausibility of the consistent organization versus multiple organization views of retrieval processes. ATTRIBUTES OF MEMORY, RETROACTIVE INHIBITION, CUEING AND FREE RECALL LEARNING By Joyce Louise Pennington A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1972 ; ’4' ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The author wishes to express her appreciation to the graduate committee members, Drs. Donald Johnson, Lester Hyman, and Gordon Wood, for their assistance and support in the preparation of this thesis. A special note of thanks is due Dr. Gordon Wood for his continued guidance and interest in the development of my career. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page List of Tables iv Introduction 1 Method 9 Design 9 Lists 9 Procedure 10 Results 12 List 1 Learning and Organization 12 List 2 Learning and Organization 17 Stage 3: Noncued Retroactive Inhibition 21 Stage 3: Cued Retroactive Inhibition 25 Discussion 29 References 34 Appendix 36 iii LIST OF TABLES Mean Number of List 1 Words Recalled by the Nine Groups for Trials 1 and 2 and the Mean Total Recall for the Two Trials. Mean Clustering of List 1 Words by the Nine Groups for Trials 1 and 2 and the Mean Total Clustering for the Two Trials. Mean Number of List 2 Words Recalled by the Nine Groups for Trials 1 and 2 and the Mean Total Recall for the Two Trials. Mean Clustering of List 2 Words by the Nine Groups for Trials 1 and 2 and the Mean Total Clustering for the Two Trials. Mean RI Scores for the Nine Groups (Stage 3). Mean Clustering Scores for the Nine Groups for the Stage 3 Recall of List 1 Words. Mean Number of Additional Words Recalled by the Nine Groups after Cueing during Stage 3. The Mean Cued RI Scores for the Nine Groups (Stage 3). iv Page 13 15 18 20 22 24 26 27 INTRODUCTION An important issue in memory research pertains to the character- istics of the information stored in memory. Underwood (1969) has adressed himself to this issue using the construct of attributes of memory. Underwood proposes that a memory consists of a number of attributes and that one memory is distinguished from another by its specific collection of attributes. The attributes which he identifies are temporal, spatial, frequency, modality, orthographic, associative nonverbal (acoustic, visual, affective, contextual), and associative verbal (class). Whereas certain attributes function primarily to distinguish one memory from another (e. g., frequency), others act as a means of retrieval of the memory (e. g., associative verbal). Within this framework Underwood emphasizes the importance of encoding multiple attributes of the to-be-recalled material as a means both of facilitating retrieval processes and of minimizing interference. More specifically, if multiple attributes are encoded, there are presumably more ways to "gain access" to the to-be-recalled event. Similarly, the greater the number of attributes encoded for a to-be- recalled event and the less these attributes are a part of other to-be- recalled events, the less the interference. Although Underwood implies that the attributes of a single event are "stored together," it is possible that under some conditions there is multiple, independent representation of single events. The purpose of this research is to determine if encoding multiple attributes facilitates recall and decreases the amount of retroactive inhibition. There is considerable evidence supporting the view that attributes of the to-be-recalled material are stored and not the specific content of the material (Bobrow, 1970; Bower, 1970; Light and Carter-Sobrel, 1970). A particularly useful approach is to hold the to-be-recalled material constant and encourage subjects to encode different attributes of the same material by varying the context or organization of the material. Bower (1970) reported an unpublished study by Bobrow and Light in which ambiguous words were used. Each ambiguous word (e. g., cardinal) could be classified into two categories (e. g., bird or clergy). By varying the context of the ambiguous words subjects were encouraged to encode different attributes of the words. If the study context had encouraged the classification of the word according to one category (e. g., priest, cardinal, minister), the category label appropriate to that context (e. g., clergy) was an effective retrieval one while the other category label (e. g., bird) was not. In a later study using ambiguous noun pairs as the subject and direct object of sentences, Bobrow (1970) manipulated the attribute encoded by varying the sentence context. There were three context conditions: exact repetition of the sentence (e. g., dog bark--baseball pitcher); changed context-same semantic attribute (e. g., animal bark-- big—league pitcher); and changed context-different semantic attribute (e. g., medicinal bark--porcelain pitcher). At recall subjects were given the subject of the sentence and were asked to give as much of the rest of the sentence as possible. There was little difference in performance between the two groups who were encouraged to encode the same semantic attribute on successive trials; therefore, simply changing the sentence context does not impair performance. However, when subjects were encouraged to encode different semantic attributes on successive trials, there was almost no improvement in performance over trials. These studies strongly support the position that attributes of the to-be-recalled material are stored rather than specific content of the material. If specific content were stored, no difference in performance should have been observed between the two groups receiving changed sentence contexts. Of greater interest for the present study, however, is the evidence provided by Bobrow's study on the effect of processing more than one attribute of the material. Contrary to the hypothesis, Bobrow's data suggest that encoding more than one attribute (i. e., more than one meaning) is detrimental to learning. However, because this study was not designed as a test of Underwood's hypothesis, it is open to certain criticisms as a valid test of the hypothesis. If multiple attributes facilitate recall by providing more retrieval routes to the memory, a design in which retrieval cues are given (paired-associate task) destroys the advantage of multiple encoding. Nevertheless, this argument does not explain the low degree of learning attained by the encoding variability group. Other evidence also suggests that inconsistent encoding results in poorer learning than consistent encoding. Bower, Lesgold, and Tieman (1969) presented subjects with a twenty-four word list in six groups of four words each. The four-word groups were presented visually on cards and subjects were instructed to imagine a scene in which the objects interacted. One group received the same grouping of the words on successive trials. Presentation of the words for the second group was such that no two words belonged to the same group more than once. Performance of the first group was far superior to that of the second group, who showed little improvement over trials. The authors interpret the data in terms of the importance of a consistent organiza- tion and stable higher-order memory units. However, because context is an important determiner of the attribute encoded, it is also likely that subjects who received changed groupings of the words were encoding different attributes on successive trials. In this case, the results indicate that consistent encoding of one attribute, or set of attributes, results in better learning than variable encoding. The final study to be considered relating to multiple encoding and free recall learning provides data inconsistent with that already presented. Wood (unpublished study) used a word list which could be organized according to four criteria: one set of semantic categories, a second set of semantic categories, first letter, and number of letters in the word. Prior to recalling the words, all subjects sorted the words four times. One group sorted the words into the same set of semantic categories four times (one—half of the subjects using each set of semantic categories). The second group sorted the words twice into each of the two sets of semantic categories for the four sorts. The third group sorted the words in all four ways, in the order listed above. (There was also an incidental vs. intentional learning manipul- ation which will not be considered as the results are similar for both.) The data are somewhat ambiguous. There was no difference in the amount recalled by subjects who sorted according to one or two criteria (semantic categories), but both of these groups were superior to the group using four sorting criteria. The inferiority of the group sorting according to four different attributes may be due to the fact that the two final sorts involved attributes (first letter and number of letters) which are probably not very effective either as organizers or as retrieval cues. These final sorts may have had a detrimental effect on the processing which had already occurred. If so, the results of the first two groups indicate that encoding multiple attributes does not impair learning. The relationship between the number of attributes encoded and free recall learning is certainly not clear. The data of Bobrow (1970) and Bower, Lesgold, and Tieman (1969) indicate that consistent encoding of the same attribute is the better method of processing material. However, neither of these studies was intended as a test of the hypothesis and there are important methodological differences between these studies and Wood's data which are more supportive of the hypothesis. The two most important differences involve the amount of control exercised by the experimenter over the attributes encoded by the subjects and the orthogonality of the attributes encoded. In order to complete the sorting task, the subject must attend to the attribute intended by the experimenter and must actively process the material. Because associative verbal attributes are dominant with meaningful material, it is necessary to encourage subjects to encode other attributes in order to manipulate the number of attributes encoded. Secondly, and more importantly, the use of orthogonal attributes in Wood's study probably led to less interference between the attributes encoded on successive trials. The word list used by Wood was designed such that the attributes were probably independent of one another. These methodological considerations have been incorporated into the design of the present research. The facilitative effect of encoding multiple attributes may depend on the number of categories used. The recall of categories and the recall of items within categories have been treated as independent processes (cf. Mandler, 1967; Tulving and Pearlstone, 1966). This distinction seems especially appropriate when considering the recall of categorized word lists. Mandler (1967) has also suggested that the number of categories which subjects can "spontaneously" recall is equal to five, plus or minus two. In Wood's study only six categories for each of the attributes were used. Therefore, subjects processing only one attribute were probably able to retrieve all of the categories. However, if the number of categories is increased beyond the number which can be "spontaneously" recalled, then the importance of having multiple retrieval routes should be increased. Therefore, a greater number of categories was used in the present design. Tulving and Pearlstone (1966) have demonstrated that information in storage can be made accessible through cueing. Multiple encoding is expected to effect retrieval processes. At retrieval, however, it is hypothesized that multiple encoding subjects have more retrieval routes and so should recall more. Cueing should destroy this advantage by facilitating the retrieval processes of all subjects. In the present study all subjects will be cued only after a standard free recall trial. 0n the other hand, if encoding multiple attributes does not increase recall, cueing may have a greater effect for the multiple encoding subjects. It has already been established that a retrieval cue is only effective when it is consistent with the encoded attribute of the to-be-recalled event (cf. Bower, 1970; Thomson and Tulving, 1970). Thus, those subjects encoding a single attribute have fewer effective retrieval cues than those subjects encoding multiple attributes. While the effect of cueing is not clear, the results of its manipulation should be interesting. The second purpose of the present study involves the relationship between the encoding of multiple attributes and retroactive inhibition. To date, no research has investigated the effect of single versus multiple encoding of attributes on retroactive inhibition. The studies most closely related to attributes and retroactive inhibition have examined the effect of organizational processes on retroactive inhibition. The principal finding has been that retroactive inhibition is greater when successive lists have similar organizations than when the organi- zations are different (Shuell, 1968; Thompson and Poling, 1969; Watts and Anderson, 1969; Winograd, 1968; Wood, 1970, 1971). Since organi- zation influences the encoding of attributes, these results indicate that retroactive inhibition is greater when the same attribute is processed on successive lists. Thus, these studies offer partial support for the hypothesis: the less the attributes of a to-be-recalled event overlap with the attributes of a second to-be-recalled event, the less the interference between the two events. However, none of these studies has manipulated the number of attributes encoded for the to-be- recalled list (List 1). In summary, there is very little evidence relating to the encoding of multiple attributes and learning or retroactive inhibition. In relation to learning, the data do not clearly support or refute the position that multiple encoding facilitates learning. Two methodological considerations important for evaluating these studies are the amount of control over the attributes encoded (learning task) and the orthogonality of the attributes encoded. Also, it appears that the number of categories may be an important factor in determining the effect of multiple encoding. In addition, if the facilitory effect of multiple encoding is the result of better retrieval processes, then cueing all subjects should destroy the advantage by improving the retrieval processes of single encoding subjects. In terms of retroactive inhibition, there is no evidence concerning the effect of encoding multiple attributes. Therefore, the proposed research deals with the effect of encoding multiple attributes on free recall learning, retroactive inhibition, and cueing. METHOD Design. The design is a 3 X 3 factorial in which List 1 organization (acoustic, semantic, or dual) and List 2 organization (acoustic, semantic, or dual) were the variables manipulated. All subjects received two study and test trials on List 1 (Stage 1), two study and test trials on List 2 (Stage 2), a noncued recall trial of List 1 and a cued List I recall trial (Stage 3). The organization of the list words was used to manipulate the attribute encoded. Words belonging to the same category were presented consecutively along with the category name. For those subjects receiving both the acoustic and the semantic organizations of the words, half received the acoustic organization on the first trial and the semantic organization for the second trial while for the other half of the subjects this order was reversed. ‘Ligtg. List 1 consisted of 48 words selected so that they could be organized both according to twelve rhyming categories (e. g., sleigh, bay; rain, plane) and according to twelve conceptual categories (e. g., transportation, weather) with from three to five words in each category. The 48 List 2 words were selected so that they could be organized into the same rhyming and conceptual categories used for List 1, again with from three to five words in each category. The words in both lists were concrete nouns, and the two lists contained no words in common. In as far as was possible, no two words belonging 10 to the same conceptual category also belonged to the same rhyming category. The two lists appear in Appendix 1, organized both into the rhyming and conceptual categories. Procedure. A total of 144 subjects were randomly assigned to the nine experimental conditions such that 16 subjects were in each condition. All subjects were tested in groups composed of from four to twenty individuals. All subjects were enrolled in introductory psychology classes at Michigan State University and received class credit for their participation. Upon arrival at the experimental session, subjects were randomly assigned to the conditions. At the beginning of the session each subject received a booklet containing a cover page, instruction page, List 1 study page, test page, filler sheet, List 1 study page, test page, instruction page, List 2 study page, test page, filler sheet, List 2 study page, test page, instruction page, recall sheet, filler page, and a cued recall sheet with instructions. Prior to the presentation of each list, subjects were informed of the organization of the list (i. e., the type of categories to be presented) and were told that although the category names might help them learn the words, they would not be asked to recall the category names. Subjects receiving both organizations of a list were informed that the organization of the list would change from Trial 1 to Trial 2. For subjects studying a single organization of the list, the input order was constant. All subjects were told that there would be two trials and that on each test trial they were to recall as many words as possible in any order. After subjects had read the instructions and the experimenter had answered any questions, the experiment was begun. 11 There were two alternating study and test trials for List 1 learning. Subjects were allowed 150 seconds to study the words and four minutes to recall the words on each trial. Instructions prior to List 2 learning were almost identical to those for List 1 learning. Those subjects who received the same organization of the two lists were told that the same categories would be used but that there would be new words in the categories. Those subjects receiving a different organization were informed of the change. The List 2 study and recall times were the same as those used for List 1 learning. Immediately following List 2 learning, subjects read the instructions requesting them to recall the List 1 words again. In addition, the experimenter verbally emphasized the fact that List 1 was to be recalled. Four minutes was allowed for List I recall. For the cued recall of List 1 subjects were provided with the category labels which had been present when they had studied List 1. Thus, subjects who received both organizations of List 1 received 24 cues while all other subjects received either 12 conceptual cues or 12 rhyming cues. Since Thomson and Tulving (1970) have demonstrated that cues, to be effective, must be encoded with the to-be-recalled material, there was no reason to provide all subjects with all possible cues. Subjects were informed that the cues might help them recall additional words. Subjects were allowed to refer back to their last recall sheet to check on which words they had already recalled, but they were instructed to write any additional words which they recalled on the page with the cues. Following the experiment subjects were informed of the purpose of the experiment. RESULTS List 1 Learning and Organization Recall protocols were scored for the number of words correctly recalled on Trials 1 and 2 of Stage 1. The mean number of words correctly recalled by the nine groups on Trials 1 and 2 and the total mean recall for the two trials are presented in Table 1. An analysis of variance revealed a List 1 Organization effect, §_(2, 135) = 61.39, p-£.001; a Trials effect, E (l, 135) = 349.91, p (.001; and a Trials by List 1 Organization interaction, §_(2, 135) = 11.33, p‘<.001. It is clear from inspection of Table 1 that the List 1 Organization effect is due to the higher recall of the semantic organization subjects compared to both the acoustic and the dual organization subjects. The Trials by List 1 Organization interaction reflects the fact that semantic subjects show the greatest improvement over trials while the dual organization subjects show the least improvement. The fact that the List 2 Organization effect was nonsignificant indicates that the three groups comprising each of the List 1 organization conditions did not differ prior to the introduction of the List 2 organization manipulation. Recall protocols were also scored for the amount of clustering on each trial. The clustering measure is the ratio of the obtained to maximum possible clustering, which can vary from 0.0 to 1.0. A cluster is counted every time two words belonging to the same category are 12 13 No.5m NH.~N om.mH oo.me mo.o~ mm.wH em.om No.HN Hm.mH H