THE INFLUENCE OF LATERAL DOMINANCE 0N ACCURACY AND DIRECTIONAL ERRORS WHEN THROWING AT A TARGET Thesis for the Degree of M. A. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY . EMMA JEAN McCONKIE 1974 II I IMIIIIQ' --g.,,.x-m..vl.4_l , — :5, Y- I H V I.. l. .‘ a I Q - K ' I P ’5’ . Z k' ’ . ‘3“‘ if:- ‘_. . 2’3 Ui‘zLV‘gmii)’ w; ‘2 __ __ __ 3". W— '- ABSTRACT THE INFLUENCE OF LATERAL DOMINANCE ON ACCURACY AND DIRECTIONAL ERRORS WHEN THROWING AT A TARGET BY Emma Jean McConkie The purpose of the present study was to determine the influence of hand dominance, eye dominance, and combinations of hand and eye dominance on the accuracy of throwing at a target, and on the direction of errors made when throwing at a target. The directions considered were the right and left halves of the target and the upper and lower halves of the target. Eleven subjects were randomly selected from each of four laterality strata (right-handed and right-eyed; right-handed and left-eyed; left-handed and right-eyed; left-handed and left-eyed) which had been obtained from a population of 238 high school girls at Eastern High School, Lansing, Michigan. They ranged in age from 15 years to 19 years. The mean age was 16.2 years. Approxi— mately 82 percent of the sample were Sophomore girls enrolled in a required physical education program. The ' remaining 18 percent were Junior or Senior girls enrolled in an elective physical education program. Eac‘ during each of each see twenty-five directiona tional err five trial analyses . A? test the dominance dominance of throw errors . central a Emma Jean McConkie Each subject was given 30 trials of dart throwing during each of two class periods. The first five throws of each session were allowed for practice and the last twenty-five throws were scored for accuracy, right-left directional error (horizontal), and above-below direc- tional error (vertical). Darts were thrown in sets of five trials each and scores by sets were used in the analyses. Analysis of variance was used to statistically test the obtained data. The results indicated hand dominance, eye dominance, and combinations of hand and eye dominance failed to significantly influence the accuracy of throwing at a target and the direction of throwing errors. §_priori comparisons tests of ipsilateral and contralateral-dominant groups also failed to detect a significant effect of dominance on throwing performance. THE INFLUENCE OF LATERAL DOMINANCE ON ACCURACY AND DIRECTIONAL ERRORS WHEN THROWING AT A TARGET BY Emma Jean McConkie A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Physical Education 1974 To my family, for their love and understanding ii of my G PIEpara Dr. Joh stages on the HeUSner invalua Worth-w of Mr . Miller I Eastern ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to express my gratitude to the members of my Guidance Committee, who greatly aided me in the preparation of this thesis. I extend Special thanks to Dr. John Haubenstricker, who guided me throughout all stages of the work; to Dr. Vern Seefeldt, who advised me on the proposal andon the final draft; and to Dr. William Heusner, who gave so willingly of his time to provide invaluable statistical assistance. Each of these men made worth-while suggestions which resulted in improvements. I would also like to acknowledge the cooperation of Mr. Don Johnson, Principal of Eastern High School; Miss Jean Robinson, Miss Candy Whitmore, and Miss Gloria Miller, Physical Education Instructors at Eastern High School; and the girl's Physical Education classes at Eastern High School for their respective roles in the collection of the data. Sincere appreciation is expressed to the women students at Michigan State University, who volunteered as subjects for the pilot study. iii LIST 0: LIST 0; Chapte II. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES O O O O O I O O O O O O 0 LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . Chapter I. II. THE PROBLEM . . . . . . . . . . . Lateral Dominance . . . . . . . Need for the Study . . . . . . . Purpose of the Study . . . . . . Research Hypotheses . . . . . . . Scope of the Study . . . . . . . Limitations of the Study . . . . . Significance of the Study . . . . . Definitions . . . . . . . . . RELATED LITERATURE . . . . . . . . Theories of Dominance Development Anatomical . . . . Hereditary . . . . Social . . . . . . Characteristics of Dominance Eye Dominance . . . Hand Dominance . . . . . Implications . . . . . . . Incidence of Dominance in the Population Measurement of Dominance . . . . . Tests of Eyedness . . . . . . Tests of Handedness . . . . . Dominance and Motor Proficiency . . . Unilateral Dominance . . . . . Contralateral Dominance . . . . Mixed-Lateral Dominance . . . Dominance and Accuracy of Direction iv Page vi vii ooooxloxmwwtu H |—' O wLouwCJBJNbohJNPHPJHPHPJHPH occa:a+aann¢>mc3annoxnwohuo Chapter III. IV. Chapter Page III. RESEARCH METHODS . . . . . . . . . . 42 SU-bjects O O O O 0 O O 0 O O O 42 Determination of Eye Dominance . . 44 Determination of Hand Dominance . . 45 Research Design . . . . . . . . . 46 Apparatus . . . . . . . . . . 46 Measures . . . . . . . . . . 49 Procedures . u. . . . . . . . . . 50 Data Collection . . . . . . . . 51 Scoring and Recording . . . . . . 52 Treatment of the Data . . . . . . . 53 Statistical Procedures . . . . . 54 Significance Level . . . . . . . 55 IV. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . 56 Relationships Among Dependent Variables . 56 Statistical Results . . . . . . . . 57 Accuracy . . . . . . . . . . 58 R-L Directional Error . . . . . . 61 A-B Directional Error . . . . . . 65 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . 68 V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS . . 72 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . 72 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . 73 Recommendations . . . . . . . . . 74 APPENDICES A. Pilot Study . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 B. Eye Dominance Test . . . . . . . . . 87 C. Raw Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 LIST OF REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95 Table Table 3.1 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 A-l A-2 C-l LIST OF TABLES Laterality of Sophomore Population . . Correlations Between Dependent Variables ANOVA of Accuracy Scores . . . . . ANOVA of R-L Directional Error . . . ANOVA of A-B Directional Error . . . Pilot Study Statistics . . . . . . Pilot Study Raw Data . . . . . . . Raw Data . . . . . . . . . . . vi Page 43 57 60 63 68 78 81 89 Figure 3.1 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 4.5 4.6 4.7 A‘1 Figure 3.1 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 4.5 4.6 4.7 A-l LIST OF FIGURES The Target Face . . . . . Overall Accuracy by Laterality Groups Ipsilateral-Contralateral Accuracy R—L Directional Error by Laterality Groups Ipsilateral-Contralateral R—L Error . Correction Scores from Means of Handedness A-B Directional Error by Laterality Groups Ipsilateral-Contralateral A—B Error . Learning Curves for Dart Throwing vii Page 47 59 60 62 64 65 66 67 79 skills Peltforn Marion identi; Source that O SubSeq CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM One important aspect of the psychometric trend in skills research is the study of individual variations in performance and the factors that contribute to skilled performances. Investigators continue to delve into the identification of differences between individuals as one source of variation. Once identified, the question becomes that of determining the effect these differences have on subsequent behavior or performance. Many of the existing differences between the sexes that affect motor ability are obvious. Other differences that exist between and within individuals of the same sex are not apparent. Some of these include: (a) domi- nance of eye, hand, or foot (Irwin, 1938); (b) movement time between preferred and non-preferred limbs, and between arms and legs (Letter, 1960; Bartee, 1971); (c) kinesthetic perceptivity between preferred and non- preferred arms (Phillips and Summers, 1954); and (d) a more perfect eye-moving apparatus in champion atheletes than in non-athletes (Graybiel, Jokl, and Trapp, 1955). domina. of mot Are th ipsila there disadx influe and t] The e targe Prese patte PletE SQve: the . In 1 Sub]- The Out! and The problem of individual differences in lateral dominance and how these differences influence performance of motor skills has not been conclusively documented. Are there activities in which it is an advantage to have ipsilateral dominance of hand and eye? Similarly, are there activities in which contralateral dominance is a disadvantage? Perhaps only certain skills are significantly influenced by dominance. If so, they should be identified and the physical education teacher made aware of them. The effect of lateral dominance on the performance of a target throwing task was selected as the focus of the present study. Lateral Dominance The relationship of various lateral dominance patterns to the performance of motor skills has received increasing attention since the mid-1950's. As revealed in the review of literature, Chapter II, results of com- pleted studies are confusing and often conflicting. Several authors have reached conclusions which indicate the unilaterally dominant individual is superior in motor ability (Poindexter, 1965; Robinson, 1965; Kisler, 1971). In 1958, Way claimed the superiority of mixed-dominant subjects on motor ability tests and in selected skills. The right-eyed, left-handed men in Roth's study (1942) outperformed all other laterality combinations of hand and eye. In a study of baseball players, Baughman (1968) found con did unila C investiga directior laterals dominant the othe: activitie I r{Wealed IEOl‘mance Frahm. 15 may not 1 a Viable found contralaterals achieved more bases on balls than did unilaterals. Only one study of gross motor skills specifically investigated the relationship of hand—eye dominance to directional errors. Shick (1971) reported that contra- laterals made more errors toward the side of their dominant eye in basketball free throw shooting than to the other side. Does this phenomenon occur in other activities where accuracy of direction is important? Although the majority of the literature has not revealed specific lateralities to be a factor in per- formance of skills (Sinclair and Smith, 1957; Fox, 1957; Frahm, 1967; Adams, 1965; Cooke, 1969), all activities may not be similarly affected. Thus, the issue remains a viable one for investigation. Need for the Study The reasons for the development of a particular dominance pattern in an individual remain a mystery. It appears to be similar to the issue of 1.0. in that heredity is a factor, but the role of environment and the changes that result from experiences affect its development. . The practical significance of dominance in eyedness differs considerably from that in handedness. Binocular vision is obviously the most common, while in many activities unimanual hand usage is more prevalent than the simultane that eye not Chang become th hand, in\ not the I performed required. 'I activity as the do. HHS may 1 Coordinat‘ Clark (19! AlthOT latere View 1 It has adVant e 85 e 91V in Wha d dISa CQrtai Sighti Whethe is not Se‘ toward a t- C (b) does h simultaneous use of both hands. It is generally agreed that eye dominance is established early in life and does not change. The non—dominant eye cannot be trained to become the dominant eye (Clark, 1957). On the other hand, invariability in handedness is the exception and not the rule. Hand usage depends on the action being performed and the skill or precision of the movement required. The hand which is chosen for use in a particular activity may or may not be on the same side of the body as the dominant eye. Little is known as to what effect this may have on hitting or missing the target in hand-eye coordination activities requiring accuracy of direction. Clark (1957) has summarized the situation well: Although it has been established that crossed laterality is common, this does not exclude the view that unilateral dominance is the ideal. It has frequently been asserted that it is an advantage to have the preferred hand and eye on the same side . . . At present no decision can be given on this issue since it is not known in what activities crossed laterality may be a disadvantage. Its effect may be limited to certain situations, for example, monocular sighting in such activities as rifle shooting. Whether it is important in binocular situations is not established. (p. 75) Several questions that need answers have been suggested. In tasks requiring accuracy of direction toward a target: (a) are left handed individuals more accurate than right handed individuals, or vice versa; (b) does hand dominance or eye dominance have the greatest influence on the direction in which errors are made; or (c) is it most inf I establis? accuracy Physical mation t result 1‘ the Eff dominan made wh Was deg (c) is it the combination of hand-eye dominance which is most influencial on the direction of misses? If a direct and measurable connection can be established between hand-eye dominance patterns and accuracy of direction when throwing at a target, the physical education teacher or coach may use this infor- mation to help students select focal points which will result in more successful performances. Purpose of the Study The purpose of the investigation was to study the effects of hand dominance, eye dominance, and hand-eye dominance combinations on accuracy and direction of errors made when throwing at a target. Specifically, the study was designed to: 1. Explore the influence of hand dominance on three dependent variables: (a) overall accuracy of throwing at a target; (b) dir- ectional errors in the right and left halves of the target; and (c) directional errors in the upper and lower halves of the target. 2. Explore the influence of eye dominance on the same three dependent variables. 3. Explore the influence of the four hand-eye dominance combinations (RH-RE, RH-LE, LH-RE, LH-LE) on each of the three dependent variables. Th hYpOtIiese s StUdy ; 4. Explore the influence of ipsilateral and contralateral dominance of hand and eye on each of the three dependent variables. W The review of literature suggested the following hypotheses would be supported by the findings of the study: 1. Hand dominance (R or L) has a significant influence on accuracy and/or directional errors in the performance of a target throwing task. 2. Eye dominance (R or L) significantly affects accuracy and/or the direction of errors made in the performance of a target throw- ing task. 3. Combinations of hand-eye dominance (RH-RE, RH-LE, LH-RE, LH—LE) significantly influ— ence accuracy and/or directional errors in the performance of a target throwing task. 4. Ipsilateral (RH—RE and LH-LE) and contra- lateral (RH-LE and LH-RE) dominance of hand and eye significantly affects accuracy and/or the direction of errors made in the performance of a target throwing task. Scope of the Study Sophomore high school girls, enrolled in a required physical education program, were potential subjects for the study. The potential subject population was stratified with respect to eye dominance and preferred hand for dart throwing. Experimental subjects were randomly selected using disproportionate sampling from each of the four strata 01 data on subject trials 0 trials 0 last 25 Measuren days. 1: categorj (lateral Statistj strata obtained. Dart throwing was used to obtain the data on the three dependent variables. Each experimental subject attended two measurement sessions in which 30 trials of dart throwing were administered. The first five trials of each session were allowed for practice with the last 25 trials being scored for the dependent variables. Measurement sessions were separated by a period of seven days. Results of the 50 scored trials were compared to categories and combinations of the independent variables (lateral dominance of hand and eye) using parametric statistics. Limitations of the Study Results of the investigation were limited in respect to the following: 1. The sample selected was a stratified random sample of female Sophomore students attending Eastern High School, Lansing, Michigan, and included three Juniors and five Seniors who were selected to fill the left-handed cate- gories when these strata did not contain sufficient Sophomore students. The resulting sample had a mean age of 16.2 years. General- izations must therefore be limited to the population from which the subjects were selected. Dance pa 0f value accuracy Results on the it Enter 5k hand, Or Vities. the mem 2. Generalizations regarding accuracy and/or directional errors and dominance patterns are limited to the skill of dart throwing. Significance of the Study Positive conclusions relating hand and eye domi- nance patterns to measurable accuracy deviations may be of value to the teacher or coach of activities where accuracy of direction toward a target is important. Results of the study may serve to guide future research on the role of lateral dominance in the performance of motor skills. Definitions Lateral Dominance.-—A preference for one eye, hand, or foot over the opposite member in unimanual acti- vities. In bilateral tasks, dominance is exhibited by the member which performs the more complex maneuvers. Unilateral or Ipsilateral Dominance.-—The pattern where hand and eye, or hand, eye, and foot are dominant on the same side. Contralateral Dominance.--The pattern where two of the three members are dominant on one side, and the third is dominant on the opposite side. When only hand and eye are involved, one is dominant on one side while the other is dominant on the opposite side. It may also be referred to as crossed-dominance. domin and t. refer; exhibi cient. miss ti halves the tar and an I right Or DEIQw a 1 IH [’71 It" Mixed Dominance.--The pattern where one member is dominant on one side, one is dominant on the opposite side, and the third does not exhibit dominance. It may also be referred to as mixed-lateral. Ambidextrous or Ambilateral.-—Dominance is not exhibited by either side. Both sides are equally profi— cient. (True ambidexterity is rarely found.) Directional Errors.—-The direction of throws that miss the bull's eye. They may be in the right or left halves of the target, or in the upper or lower halves of the target. A miss may have both a rightwleft component and an up-down component. - R-L Directional Error.--A throw that is to the right or left of a vertical line drawn through target center. A-B Directional Error.--A throw that is above or below a horizontal line drawn through target center. RH:§§.--Right-handed and right-eyed. RH:L§.--Right-handed and left eyed. LH:R§.--Left-handed and right-eyed. LH-LE.--Left-handed and left—eyed. Tl effect of direction Primary (3; relations} The the”: the incide measul‘emex fOundatim CHAPTER II RELATED LITERATURE The purpose of this study was to determine the effect of lateral dominance patterns on accuracy and direction of errors when throwing at a target. The primary emphasis of this review was to investigate the relationship of lateral dominance to motor proficiency. The theories pertaining to the development of dominance, the incidence and characteristics of dominance, and the measurement of dominance were included as a necessary foundation for the present study. Theories of Dominance Development Numerous theories have been proposed to explain the observations of preferred laterality or handedness in the population. Any theory, to be tenable, must take into consideration most of the empirical evidence. The early theories were concerned with accounting for the right-hand preference of the majority and were inadequate when applied to left-handedness. Attempts to explain left“ handedness did not occur until the nineteenth century. 10 At to be a mys Theories ti anatomical Anatomical \I 11 At present, the development of dominance continues to be a mystery to physiologists and psychologists alike. Theories that have been advanced may be classified as: anatomical, hereditary, or social in nature. Anatomical Theories of structural differences date back to Aristotle and Plato. Aristotle contended that the organs were more powerful on the right side of the body and this caused a right-sidedness. Left-sidedness could not be accommodated by his theory. Many of the early anatomical theories postulated asymmetries within the fetus as the cause of dominance. Some of the ideas were: (a) the unequal development of the two sides of the brain before birth, with a greater mass on the left side (Jackson, 1905); (b) the position of the fetus in intrauterine life where its left arm lies posteriorly, thus giving the right arm greater facility of movement (Hildreth, 1949, II); (c) the arrangement of the blood supply from the heart favoring the right arm by providing better nutrition to the left cerebral hemisphere (Blau, 1946, citing H. E. Jordan, 1911); and (d) the dis- placement of the center of gravity to the right of the medial plane due to the greater weight of the liver and lungs on that side, which in time developed stronger muscles on the right side (Jackson, 1905, citing Professor Alexander Buchanan of the University of Glasgow). These theox postt tribr posit from right binoc Of vi: in the that j tWO he domina flame] be con: 1949, J 12 theories have since been disproved because: (a) any postural asymmetry in infancy tends to be equally dis— tributed in the population; and (b) persons with trans- position of heart position are not reversed in handedness. Parson (1924) advocated that handedness originates from eyedness, and that the right hand was guided by the right eye. His theory is refuted by the fact that in binocular vision it is not possible to detect the field of vision of one eye from the other. Also, handedness in the blind population does not differ appreciably from that in the normal population. An asymmetrical neurological functioning of the two hemispheres has been claimed to be the basis of dominance development. However, complete cortical domiv anance has not been proven and the left hemisphere cannot be considered dominant for all nervous functions (Hildreth, 1949, III; Dimond, 1970). All attempts to identify a structural basis which can explain lateral dominance have been unsuccessful. If handedness arises from some structural difference, then genetic factors should transmit it from generation to generation. This has not occurred. Hereditary The need to explain the consistent minority of left-handed individuals within a pepulation brought the geneticists into the field at the beginning of this centur to some left-ha that ha from a have sh known 1. the inh: Offered TraVis l ideIltica by Rife involvem a‘TLbidext and 0the; are gene‘ not read; a right—I viduals I: that the to Some d SI heredity i 13 century. Their investigations were hampered by the fact that no society permitted the normal development of the left hand. There is little reason to doubt that handedness is to some extent hereditary (Wile, 1934). Wile regarded left-handedness as a Mendelian recessive, and suggested that handedness is probably not a single gene but arises from a combination of factors. Subsequent observations have shown, however, that left-handedness does not follow known laws of heredity. Empirical evidence to support the inheritance theory of dominance development has been offered by Ojemann (1930), Brain (1945), and Tuttle and Travis (1935). Observations of the incidence of handedness in identical twins and studies of palm patterns were used by Rife (1951) to propose a hypothesis of hereditary involvement. According to his scheme, genetically ambidextrous persons (Rr) are easily shifted by training and other environmental influences, whereas persons who are genetically right-handed (RR) or left-handed (rr) are not readily susceptible to training. As our society is a right-handed society, the naturally ambidextrous indi— viduals become functionally rightnhanded. Rife concluded that the influence of environment on handedness depended to some degree on one's heredity. Several authors have expressed opposition to heredity as a theory of dominance development (Jackson, 1905 to b DOWHI socie (194E train were futile May ment i man's‘ The the Signifi develop Prefere 50“. 19 to traix acquirec IhbitUal that lef of the i; eduCatiOI EXplanatj H.957) . 14 1905; Downey, 1933; Blau, 1946). They consider the theory to be contrary to experience and unsupported by facts. Downey and Blau believed dominance to be a product of social cultivation and learning influences. Hildreth (1949, III), who believed that handedness should be trained and not left to chance, stated that if handedness were hereditary then attempts to change it would be futile. Social The social conditioning view of dominance.developu ment is currently the most widely accepted. It emphasizes man's adaptability and conformity to group pressure. The theory suggests the social demands of today are more significant than natural physical inclinations in the development of hand dominance. Most individuals, at birth, do not have a hand preference and are therefore amenable to education (Jack- son, 1905; Hildreth, 1949, I). Both hands are responsive to training. Blau (1946) advocated that laterality was acquired through training and education, and became a habitual response by social conditioning. He believed that left-handedness reflected a sociological deviation of the individual due to an inherent deficiency, faulty education, or emotional negativism. A psychological explanation of left-handedness was also offered by Clark (1957). She stated that left~handedness is a revolt of of the since develc stated acquis learn: ing f1 0f rig years, curve factor indica. (1949, little deXteri until t Burke ( enhance grade c? anOUtCOI 085611 ar ltexists 15 of the individual which becomes a distinct disadvantage since society is based on right-handedness. The best argument for a social theory of dominance development was presented by Hildreth (1949, III). She stated that handedness is learned. In other words, the acquisition of handedness follows the same laws of learning and habit formation as does all behavior result- ing from practice or performance. When the proportion of right-handed individuals was plotted versus age in years, a very typical negatively accelerated learning curve resulted. That early training and activity are important factors in the determination of dominance has been indicated by Palmer (1964) and Provins (1956). Hildreth (1949, II) stated that up to age three the child has little need in daily activities for Specialized hand dexterity, and that consistent sidedness does not occur until the child learns motor coordination. However, Burke (1969) found motor training did not significantly enhance the development of lateral preference in first grade children, but it did assist in the development of lateral awareness. The development of handedness was reported to be an outcome of ontogenetic patterning and organization by Gesell and Ames (1947). They believed ambidexterity, if it exists at all, would seem to be an abnormality. handed; rather Barsley has nor. phenome many wa: apparem Various emPhasis con tras t inflUEnc the dist; handecines hand dOmi dominance left and : third are 1349' III as right, 30perCen 16 In summary, the bulk of evidence indicates that handedness is a product of both heredity and environment, rather than being due entirely to one or the other. Barsley (1966) concluded that the question of handedness has now become one of treatment for a familiar, accepted phenomenon. Characteristics of Dominance The qualities peculiar to hand dominance differ in many ways from the characteristics of eye dominance. This apparent difference may have been confounded by the various methods used to evaluate handedness and by the emphasis placed on right-handedness in all societies. In contrast, the establishment of eye dominance has not been influenced by social conditioning. Eye Dominance Although not equally distributed in the population, the distribution of eye dominance is not as extreme as handedness. Left-eye dominance is more common than leftv hand dominance (Miles, 1929; Hildreth, 1949, III). Eye dominance in left—handed people is equally divided between left and right eyes, while in right-handed people one- third are left-eyed and two-thirds are right—eyed (Hildreth, 1949, III). Crider (1944), who classified eye dominance as right, left, or impartial—eyed, also found approximately 30 percent of right-handed individuals were left—eyed. I been idei 17 Other characteristics of ocular dominance have been identified by Hildreth (1949, III) as: 1. At birth, infants are ambi-eyed and remain so until control of hand function is needed. 2. Eye movements develop in infancy ahead of hand movements. Throughout life, the eyes control and direct the hands. 3. The eye dominance established early in infancy persists unless the dominant eye becomes incapacitated. 4. The choice of the sighting eye is purely involuntary because there is no social convention to influence the pattern of develOpment. 5. Establishment of ocular dominance does not appear to be as essential for performance of skilled tasks as is manual dominance. 6. There is a connection between dominant eyedness and early handedness habits. A definite relationship has been found between eye dominance and head tilt (Greenburg, 1960). Right—eyed subjects tend to carry their heads to the left and left“ eyed subjects tend to carry their heads to the right. The ambi-eyed do not manifest a consistent head tilt. Greenburg suggested a functional interpretation of this phenomenon; namely, that the dominant eye is unconsciously carried accurat is firm Hildret appear become better acuity the sex but the tion of as freq: eVidencg 1957), eyed-HESS Cliff, 19: the p0pu1‘ 18 carried closer to the midline of the body for a more accurate alignment of the entire individual. There is ample evidence to show that eye dominance is firmly established prior to school age (Clark, 1957; Hildreth, 1949, III). Once established, it does not appear to be possible to train the non-dominant eye to become dominant. Even though the preferred eye may have better visual acuity than the non-preferred eye, visual acuity is not the basis of eye dominance (Clark, 1957). No significant difference has been found between the sexes in distribution of eye dominance (Crider, 1944), but there is an apparent sex difference in the distribu- tion of handedness. Males exhibit left—handedness twice as frequently as males (Hildreth, 1949, II). Present evidence indicates this difference is probably due to environmental rather than hereditary factors (Clark, 1957). It is generally agreed that the correlation between eyedness and handedness is low (Gesell and Ames, 1947; Cuff, 1931; Hildreth, 1949, III). That is, eye and hand dominance are not related in a substantial proportion of the population. Hand Dominance It has been suggested that hand dominance beComeS manifest as early as the sixth month.of infancy and is' fairly well established at the age of one year (Wile, 1934). Hand pre: urges anI later so relative children thereaft at an ea directly thus be not aris tonic-me of hands (1947). to 3 str 19 Hand preference then becomes fixed, partly from inherited urges and partly as a result of parental direction and later social education. Gesell and Ames (1947) found a relatively clear—cut right dominance in the majority of children at two years of age, but that certain periods thereafter were characterized by bilaterality. There have been attempts to predict handedness at an early age. In 1924, Parson thought handedness was directly caused by a fixed type of eyedness and could thus be predicted. It is now agreed that handedness does not arise as an effect of eyedness (Cuff, 1931). The tonic-neck reflex in infants was found to be predictive of handedness in 14 out of 19 cases by Gesell and Ames (1947). They reported that left-handedness was related to a strong infantile left—tonic—neck reflex. There appears to be a close relationship between the age at which dominance appears and the degree of dominance (Clark, 1957). Right-handedness is apparent at an earlier age than left-handedness and left-handed individuals are not as strongly left dominant as right- handed individuals are right dominant (Hildreth, 1949, II). Most people show inconsistencies in hand .usage and every physically normal person is left-handed to some degree. The choice of hand used depends on the specific situation. New skills can be learned as easily with the left or the right hand (Hildreth, 1949, III). Hildreth. was a strong proponent for the training of handedness, fron left (Hil« emot: the c and h infer: ness, there . Psycho; Points reversi. Problenu Childrer and a fe 1968) , 13% hand 0r e reported instituti. epilepti greatEr I510” . 20 and stated that young children can be easily trained away from left-hand usage. The occasional advantages of being left-handed are far out-weighed by the disadvantages (Hildreth, 1950, V). Left-hand persistence is due to an emotional resistance, according to Hildreth, and unless the child who persists in left-hand usage is encouraged and helped to become proficient, he may always be inferior. Experts differ on the question of whether handed- ness, once established, should be changed. Some believe there are no deleterious effects if it is done in a psychologically sound manner (Blau, 1946). Blau's study points out that training to a degree sufficient for reversing most children may be conducted without emotional problems. In the past, efforts to switch handedness of children resulted in many problems (real and imaginary) and a fear of changing handedness developed (Oxendine, 1968). Implications Several attempts to link abnormalities to specific hand or eye dominances have been made. Barsley (1966) reported that 16-30 percent of the inmates in mental institutions were left-handed, and that 15 percent of epileptics were left-handed. This is a significantly greater proportion than found in the normal population (5-10%). A good point has been made by Hildreth.(1949, III) 21 to explain this phenomenon. She stated that the increased number of defectives who are left-handed is due to their being less trainable and unable to benefit from instruc- tion than are other individuals. This would agree with her theory that handedness is trainable and follows a normal learning curve. In another instance, Trembly (1968) found polio victims who had contralateral dominance of hand and eye were more frequently paralyzed than unilateral-dominant individuals. He suggested increased nervous tension in these individuals made the neural system more susceptible to the polio virus. Stuttering and left-handedness are correlated. Stuttering is most noticeable in children who lack con- sistent handedness or who are delayed in establishing dominance (Hildreth, 1950, IV). The stuttering subsides when a higher degree of consistency in handedness is established. Various opinions regarding the role of lateral dominance in motor performance have been recorded. In the domain of sports, games, and recreation, Jackson (1905) advocated ambidexterity to be an advantage. He lamented the one-handed man as deficient and inferior. Parson (1924) and Clark (1957) both proposed that coordi- nation is hampered unless the dominant hand and eye are on the same side of the body. Hildreth (1949, I) stated that individuals who have not achieved manual dominance are less effective or may even be handicapped in motor perforni leads t4 perform evenly primari a minor althOUg handedn. the mos- foot and tion is ing On 1 and the reported (gradeS fOund 83 handed, handed. tiohs in consider in the [I ”931) re I 22 performance, and that delay in establishing dominance leads to uncertainty, confusion, and awkwardness in performing motor skills. Incidence of Dominance in the Population It is general knowledge that laterality is not evenly distributed in the population. Individuals who primarily use the left hand in unimanual activities are a minority. Left-eyed individuals are also a minority, although the percentage is not as extreme as that of handedness. On the other hand, footedness appears to be the most evenly distributed due to a high percentage of foot ambidexterity (Irwin, 1938). The incidence of right-handedness in the popula- tion is estimated to be between 70 and 98 percent, depend- ing on the population investigated, the author reporting, and the tests of handedness employed. Cuff (1931) reported that 93 percent of college students and children (grades one to eight) were right-handed. Uhrbrock (1970) found 83 percent of male college students to be right- handed, while 76 percent of champion athletes were right- handed. No difference was found between the two pOpula- tions in left-handedness (13%» but the athletes were considerably more ambilateral. The prevalence of left—handedness has increased in the United States during the past three decades. Cuff (1931) reported the existing condition to be from 2-6 perce 6.4 1 left- twice anoti 1947, to be hands Gross servi (1949 cent ; tion a believ histor WOuld ] Pressul 23 percent left-handed. A Detroit study in 1941, showed 6.4 percent of 13,438 elementary school children were left-handed with the proportion of boys being nearly twice as large as that of girls (Barsley, 1966). In another Michigan study of 225,000 students conducted in 1947, 10.1 percent of students in first gradewere.found to be left-handed, while only 6.6 percent were left- handed in twelth grade (Barsley, 1966). Karpinos and Grossman (1953) disclosed that 8.67 percent of selective service registrants (males) were left-handed. Hildreth (1949, II) reported left-handedness to vary from 7 per- cent in children to 4 or 5 percent in the adult p0pula- tion and to be the lowest among college students. She believed it may have been as high as 33 percent in pre— historic man. Cuff (1931) indicated native left-handedness would be between 20—30 percent today if it were not for pressures of society affecting the ratio. Clark (1957) disagreed with Cuff. She stated that an enforced change of handedness by a society cannot account for the propor- tionate difference in right-left dominance, because the incidence of left-handedness among the congenitally blind is as high as it is in the normal p0pu1ation. Clark also reported left—handedness to be more common among twins, with 18-20 percent of pairs of twins having one left— handed member. The incidence of eyedness is more clear-cut than that reported for handedness. Left-eyedness appears to occur i (Cride: 62 per centag (1970) while mixed intere. Percen' (Way, 1957; j mately and eye made th Contral III; Tyfl Adams, Varies 1 and Tyle 24 occur in approximately 30 percent of the population (Crider, 1944; Cuff, 1931), while the same authors report 62 percent and 70 percent of right—eyedness. Lower per- centages of right-eyedness were published by Uhrbrock (1970) who found 56 percent of athletes to be right-eyed, while 47 percent of college males were right—eyed. The occurrence of unilateral, contralateral, and mixed dominance of hand and eye in the population was of interest to the present study. After reviewing the percentages of unilaterality reported in the literature (Way, 1958; Adams, 1965; Clark, 1957; Sinclair and Smith, 1957; Tyler, 1971; Horine, 1968), it appears that approxi- mately 57 percent of the population have unilateral hand and eye dominance. Similarly, a generalization may be made that about one-third (33%) of all individuals are contralateral in hand and eye dominance (Hildreth, 1949, III; Tyler, 1971; Sinclair and Smith, 1957; Cuff, 1931; Adams, 1965; Clark, 1957; Way, 1958). Mixed dominance varies from 10-13 percent as reported by Horine (1968) and Tyler (1971). Measurement of Dominance The literature reveals that dominance has been determined by various methods depending on the individual researcher. There is no established, standardized procedure that is universally adopted and accepted. Available tests appear to have originated in the first half 0: group Zener are me. ones. more d. Tests < centere eyes. Crider t0 eyQ to be u dominanc Open. fi 9Y9- (the of this both QYe: include 5 or align: to Duran in the CI. was bold 25 half of the twentieth century. Recent tests, such as the group test for eye dominance deve10ped by Friedheim and Zener at Duke University and reported by Greenburg (1960), are merely a modification or reapplication of the older ones. Handedness, with its variability between tasks, is more difficult to access than eyedness. Tests of Eyedness The early studies in the measurement of dominance centered around the comparative visual acuity of the two eyes. It has since been determined by Downey (1933), Crider (1944), and others that visual acuity is unrelated to eye dominance. Sighting preference during binocular vision appears to be the most satisfactory method of determining eye dominance. When an object is sighted with both eyes open, fixation is actually due to the functioning of one eye (the dominant eye), although subjects are not conscious of this fact and have the impression that they are using both eyes. Binocular vision tests that have been used include sighting through a cone, a hole in a card, a ring, or aligning a pencil with some object. The origin of the Hole-in-Card Test is traced back to Durand and Gould (1910). They used a card with a hole in the center, a piece of tubing pushed through the hole, and an attached handle. The disadvantage of this method was holding the handle with only one hand. This fault u a I ‘ M‘ ‘m, a : rare eucamm ‘ . _ not appe was corrI centimet center, each han length w meters d dominanc test is the A-B- Utilized Sighted. differen the subj. the Card larger a] Card Wit] of the m the Cone readily c trials We between 1 0.95 fOr 0.97 f0]: 26 was corrected by Dolman (1919). He used a 13 by 20 centimeter card with a 3 centimeter round hole in the center, and had the subject grasp one end of the card with each hand. The subject raised the card slowly at arm's length while looking intently at a spot of light six meters distant. The necessity for unconscious sighting during eye dominance tests was first stressed by Miles (1929). His test is called the A-B—C Test of Ocular Dominance with the A-B-C meaning Area—Blackness-Comparison. The method utilized a tapered cone through which test cards were sighted. Each test card contained two round spots, each different in size and shade of color. The attention of the subject was controlled by giving a short exposure of the card and having the subject indicate which spot was larger and which was darker. The subject viewed the card with both eyes Open, looked through the large end of the cone toward the small end and unconsciously lined the cone up with the dominant eye. The dominant eye was readily observable by the examiner. A series of ten such trials were suggested. There was extremely high consistency between trials using this method, with a reliability of 0.95 for the test. Cuff (1931) used a similar eye dominance test and reported a reliability coefficient of 0.97 for measurement consistency; The use of a battery of eye dominance tests does not appear to have an advantage over a simple test that is repe eyednes They re (a smal hole-in sightir which i vidual highest concluc When re a batte finding reporte sex, he influen failed 0r grea 27 is repeated a sufficient number of times. Four tests of eyedness were studied by Buxton and Crosland in 1937. They reported reliability coefficients for the manuscope (a small darkened box develOped by Parson, 1924); for the hole—in-card; for ring sighting; and for a monocular sighting test. The tests were pooled into a battery which had an overall reliability of 0.85. Of the indi- vidual tests, the hole-in-card was found to have the highest reliability (r=0.97) using ten trials. The authors concluded that typical simple tests of eye preference, when repeated, prove to be statistically reliable. Using a battery of seven tests, Crider (1944) supported the findings of Buxton and Crosland. His battery had a reported reliability of 0.98. Crider also reported that sex, handedness, intelligence, or visual acuity did not influence test results. Further investigation into tests of eyedness failed to discover other methods with higher reliability or greater ease of administration. The Phi Test of domi- nance described by Jasper and Raney (1937) was very elaborate and its correlation with other tests was low. Retinal-rivalry tests also have low correlation with other tests of eye dominance (Washburn, Faison, and Scott, 1934). The group test reported by Crovitz and Zener (1965) involving the pencil alignment procedure, is suggested only as a screening device to use with large groups. Tests . effort fied b motor miners true p used e Of imp employ. indica of the 1946), are the CUtting ing a ( Writing holding Dominar or Peri reSPOn. hand r FOX Il- Streng tive Oblec 28 Tests of Handedness Numerous techniques have been employed in an effort to assess handedness. Most of these can be classi- fied by their utilization of preference, strength, speed, motor tests, or simultaneous bimanual drawing as deter- miners of the dominant hand. None of the tests give a true picture of handedness with its extreme variability. Preference tests to determine handedness have used either the questionnaire method or the performance of important unimanual activities. Although widely employed, questionnaires have not proven to be reliable indicators of the hand actually used in the performance of the activity (Barnsley and Rabinovitch, 1970; Blau, 1946). Questionnaire items that have proven reliable are those easily tested by performance, namely, hammering, cutting with scissors, card dealing, spinning a top, wind- ing a watch, holding a toothbrush, sharpening a pencil, writing, cutting when eating, drawing, throwing, and holding a tennis racquet (Hull, 1936). An Index-of- Dominance is usually employed in either the questionnaire or performance methods using the formula of %$%-to score responses (Hildreth, 1949, I). (R is the number of right- hand responses, and L is the number of left-hand responses.) Fox (1957) used a battery of 29 items and concluded that strength and dexterity tests were probably more indica- tive of hand dominance than a test of reaching for some object. Cuff (1931) found the throwing test indicates the d tests targe speed hands seems abili does L (1961 domini Prece< a Squ main ( tice g transi Reactj does I 29 the dominant hand in nine out of ten cases, and reaching tests in eight out of ten cases. The accuracy test of target throwing was found to be superior to strength and speed for diagnosis of hand dominance by Burge (1952). Relative strength tests of the right and left hands may be a doubtful measure of dominance, as there seems to be little connection between power and handedness ability in intricate movements. However, one hand usually does have greater strength than the other hand and Henry (1961) found strength to be 54 percent specific to the dominant hand. Speed tests have involved speed of tapping and precedence of movement of the two forearms in response to a sequence of three light stimuli (Vogel, 1935). The main criticism of these tests is that the effect of prac- tice gained by one hand in other activities probably transfers to the skills employed in the speed tests. Reaction time and movement time between the two limbs does not differentiate preferred from non-preferred hands (Barnsley and Rabinovitch, 1970). Motor tests give a relative comparison of perform- ance with the right and left hands. Barnsley and Rabino- vitch (1970) state that although the same skills are to be found in either hand, the preferred hand is characterized by superior performance. Dimond (1971) suggests that the cerebral hemispheres are not isolated units, but work through each other to control the function of the limbs, and t1 respec that 6 does I hemis; Riper instr1 field be Con Cuff, shOul or ta for a favor 30 and that control may be switched as the tasks demand the respective specialization of each hemisphere. The fact that each limb is directly connected with one hemisphere does not ensure that its function is controlled by that hemisphere alone. The simultaneous bimanual drawing tests of Van Riper (1934) have been used in many clinics as a diagnostic instrument. It is considered to be too elaborate for field testing. Most authorities agree that handedness is far too complex to be described as simply right or left. It can best be viewed as a continuum from left to right with most individuals located somewhere between the two extremes. Clark (1957) has said: It is impossible to measure native left-handedness since society's attitude distorts it at an early age, and the actual amount uncovered depends on the tests employed. Even the use of a battery of tests does not necessarily produce a completely reliable measure of handedness . . . as the pressure towards right-handedness varies markedly from community to community, district to district, and even family to family, even within any one generation, one cannot estimate strength of_ preference from the number of activities for which the left hand is used . . . (p. 12-13). Current thinking indicates hand preference should be considered task Specific (Collins, 1961; Lotter, 1960; Cuff, 1931; Norrie, 1967). Therefore, the preferred hand should be determined from actual performance of the task or tasks of concern. When determining the preferred hand for a task, it is important to take the precaution of not favoring either hand through the arrangement of objects for the test. the r perfo realm the w. Irwin of do: ment < Tests and re in the and We into t I‘EIati has be 31 Dominance and Motor Proficiency Numerous investigators have attempted to discover the relationship of different dominance patterns to the performance of motor skills. The early studies in the realm of physical education appeared in the 1930's with the work of VOgel (1935), Tuttle and Travis (1935), and Irwin (1938). They were characterized by the measurement of dominance using tests involving a Simple flexion move- ment of the forearm from a key in response to a stimulus. Tests of dominance using other large muscle activities and related to gross motor skills are found in the 1950's in the studies of Fox (1957), Sinclair and Smith (1957), and Way (1958). Beginning in the 1960's and continuing into the 1970's, research pertaining to dominance and its relationship to physical education and athletic activities has become more abundant. The literature is replete with studies that Show various conflicting conclusions about the degree of association between laterality and motor performance. A clear-cut relationship between dominance and Skill has yet to be established. The studies reporting that laterality is not a factor in motor skill acquisition or performance will be reviewed first, followed by those claiming superiority of particular lateralities. When comparing groups that displayed unilateral dominance, contralateral dominance, and a lack of dominance in one or more of the three body parts, Tyler (1971) found ResulI hot a: or 1e: the d< Some 6 whethe Skill it mac learni Was tC study. 32 no significant differences between groups in the rate of learning two fine motor and one gross motor skill. He concluded that individuals with different dominance patterns are equally capable of learning new motor skills, or "that lateral dominance . . . is not a limiting factor in motor Skill performance and skill acquisition." This view differs from that expressed by Clark (1957) and Hildreth (1949, I) who regarded unilateral dominance as advantageous. Tyler suggested that if dominance is a factor in acquiring motor skill, it is an empirical phenomenon which is modifiable through practice, and not a neurologically predetermined phenomenon subject to little change. Putman (1971) studied the learning ability of right-handed college women on the pursuit rotor. They were given 5, 10, or 20 trials of 20 seconds duration. Results indicated the degree of first hand practice did not affect the total amount learned with either the right or left hand. The efficiency of teaching methods in relation to the dominant and non-dominant musculature has received some attention in the literature. Lambert (1951) studied whether it would be more effective to teach a two-handed skill to the non-dominant musculature first. He concluded it made no difference which hand was trained first in learning a two-handed skill, and the most effective method was to train both musculatures together. In a similar study, the basketball skills of dribbling, shooting, and passing One grc other g weeks c minute differq foot w tion w differ nance, HoweVe betwee 33 passing were taught to fifth grade children (Thomas, 1969). One group was taught to use the dominant hand only and the other group taught to use both hands equally during 12 weeks of daily instruction. With the exception of a half- minute shooting test using the left hand, no significant differences were found between the two groups of children. The relationship between dominant eye, hand, and foot with the take-off foot in selected skills of locomo- tion was investigated by Garrison (1969). No Significant differences were found between eye, hand, and foot domi— nance, singularly or in combination, and the take-off foot. However, the findings revealed that relationships do exist between eye and hand laterality, hand and foot laterality, and eye and foot laterality. The performance of ten year old boys on tests involving dynamic balance, motor educability, speed of arm movements, and shuttle run were compared to their exhibited lateral dominances (Horine, 1968). There.were no differences in level of performance between the various laterality groups. Handedness does not appear to be a factor in the timing of limb movement patterns. Norrie (1967) studied the effects of unequal distances and handedness on timing patterns of the limbs. She reported that the limb which was required to travel the greater distance started first and finished last. The distances involved seemed more important than handedness in determining the pattern of mover when may have usin had sinc domi tend fica throw 34 movement used. One hand was not favored over the other when the movement distance was equal. An intriguing aspect of laterality is the role it may have on accuracy in motor performances. Several authors have reported negative results. Cratty and Williams (1968), using line drawing without vision, found hand dominance had no effect upon accuracy of performance. However, since the sample contained a much larger number of right- dominant individuals (46) than left (11), this may have tended to mask the true role of hand dominance. No signi— ficant difference was found in success of basketball free throw shooting between unilateral and contralateral groups in a study by Shick (1971). Staples (1971) compared unilaterally-dominant male basketball players on Shooting accuracy under three visual conditions, each in combina- tion with four shooting positions. Results indicated shooting accuracy was best under the conditions of bino- cular vision and better under monocular vision with the dominant eye than.with.the non-dominant eye. Shooting accuracy decreased as the distance from the baSket increased and was more accurate when the shots were attempted directly in front of the basket than from equi~ distant positions at the side. Several investigators have reported no relationship between lateral dominance and motor ability in specific sports. Baseball hitting and dominance were studied by Adams (1965) and Baughman (1968). Adams found the central unilatI the pi' latera hit, a favor domina did no groups and Sn and fc and SW that s crawl Vice \ Pertir eye‘he learnj Su99es in re. b0Wle bOwl hands in p more estat impo. 35 contralateral batters did not perform better than the unilateral batters, even though their dominant eyes faced the pitcher. Baughman found no difference between uni- lateral and contralateral batters in ability to place hit, although a significant difference was reported in favor of using both eyes as compared to using only the dominant or non-dominant eye. Lacrosse throwing ability did not differ between the unilateral and contralateral groups studied by Cooke (1969). In swimming, Sinclair and Smith (1957) determined that dominance of eye, hand, and foot was not related to side preference for breathing and swimming the sidestroke. They did discover, however, that swimmers who breathe to the right side in the front crawl tend to swim the sidestroke on the left side and vice versa. Fox (1957) and Frahm (1967) investigated the pertinence of dominance to bowling. Frahm concluded that eye-hand dominance did not appear to be a factor in learning the hook delivery by the spotting method. Fox suggested that many individuals using the right hand were in reality native left-handers. Twenty such beginning bowlers were identified and four subjects were taught to bowl left-handed while the other sixteen bowled right- handed. The left-handed bowlers were significantly poorer in performance. The conclusion was that bowling involves more than the use of the arms, and it is possible the long established coordinations of eye, hand, and foot are more important than hand dominance in learning a new skill. Theref: rather Unilat indica in not studie observ studie Signif motor Vision intEre abilit vision FOllo repor imPOr that able 5 WhEn 2 bette PUr5u{ unila 36 Therefore, the preferred hand should be used to bowl rather than the natively dominant hand. Unilateral Dominance Several investigators have obtained results which indicate unilaterally-dominant individuals are superior in motor ability. It is intriguing that all of these studies, except one, were published since.1965. This observation may or may not have importance, as several studies since 1965 have also reported the lack of a significant relationship between lateral dominance and motor proficiency. One of the most common applications of monocular vision is in rifle shooting. Banister (1935) became interested in eye dominance as a factor in shooting ability when he discovered that infantrymen with the best vision did not always score highest on shooting tests. Following the collection of data on 1,000 infantrymen, he reported, "the results show that the dominant eye is an important factor affecting ability with the rifle, and that the man whose right eye is dominant has a consider— able advantage, other things being equal, over other men when required to shoot from the right shoulder." Unilateral groups have performed consistently better than corresponding contralateral groups on the pursuit rotor. Poindexter (1965) tested male and female unilateral and contralateral groups on 30 trials of the pursu indiv the c right subje group gener femal trial remai; indiv, domin‘ 50 un. high 1 hand~. HIOVem, COnt-re aCCur, equal: to Re] Contra The mi also eYe~fc RErfo: 37 pursuit rotor. The unilateral group was composed of individuals who were right-eyed and right-handed, and the contralateral group of those who were left-eyed and right-handed. No left-handed individuals served as subjects in the study. Results indicated the unilateral groups were superior in performance. Males were, in general, superior to females; however, the unilateral females exceeded the contralateral males after the tenth trial and maintained a slight superiority throughout the remaining trials. It has been proposed that unilaterallyedominant individuals have better coordination than contralaterally- dominant individuals (Robinson, 1965). Robinson compared 50 unilateral junior high boys and 50 contralateral junior high boys on 25 tests of reaction time, 15 large muscle hand-eye movement tests, and 8 small muscle hand-eye movement tests. The unilateral group out-performed the contralateral group on all tests except those requiring accuracy in large muscle movements. The groups performed equally well in large muscle accuracy tests. Unilaterally-dominant individuals have been found to perform highest on the Scott Motor Ability test, while contralateral individuals performed lowest (Kisler, 1971). The mixed group (dominance not clearly decided in a member) also out-performed the contralateral group. Results showed eye-foot laterality combinations have some influence on performance of the basketball throw for distance. Contrai latera The re colleg of the were: ambide right- subjec showed Banghn season higher 9rOUp. 38 Contralateral Dominance Two studies obtained results indicating contra- lateral individuals out-performed other dominance types. The role of eye and hand dominance on motor ability of college males was investigated by Roth (1942). Results of the various laterality groups in order of performance were: (a) RE,LH; (b) RE,RH; (c) LE,RH; (d) ambiocular, ambidextrous; (e) ambiocular, RH; and (f) LE,LH. The right-eyed, left-handed men out-performed all groups, and subjects who were ambilateral in hand and eye preference showed no definite extremes of motor ability. In 1968, Baughman recorded baseball batting averages for an entire season and found contralaterals achieved a significantly higher number of bases on balls than did the unilateral group. Mixed-Lateral Dominance Only one study claimed the superiority of mixed- dominant individuals in motor ability. In 1958, Way used the Scott Motor Ability tests and Skills of archery, bowling, badminton, and tennis to study the effect of lateral dominance on motor ability of college women. There was no indication that unilateral individuals have higher skill than contralateral persons. Among Way's conclusions were: (a) that women with mixed~dominance are superior in motor ability to those with unilateral or contralateral preference; and (b) that motor ability seemed to be related to foo that i abilit conten of mor Domina accura Judges had a Stones of thi Perfor Signif where and th 39 to foot ambidexterity since combinations of preference that included foot ambidexterity resulted in higher motor ability scores. This provides support for Clark's (1957) contention that the ability to use either foot is possibly of more value in sports than in other motor activities. Dominance and Accuracy of Direction The earliest reference to dominance as a factor in accuracy was found in the Bible Old Testament book of Judges, Chapter XX, verses 15-16. The tribe of Benjamin had a group of left-handed individuals who could sling stones with unusual accuracy. Since special note was made of this feat, it was evidently considered to be a superior performance. Investigators in several studies identified a significant relationship between laterality and activities where accuracy of direction was important. Whiting (1969) measured the accuracy of throwing at a target by six, nine, and thirteen year old males and females using their preferred and non-preferred hands. Results showed that at six years, the right-handed child appears to have a distinct advantage in throwing for accuracy, but that those who were left-handed tended to be more accurate than those who were right-handed at the ages of nine and thirteen years. One of Way's conclusions in 1958, was that laterality seemed to be of greater importance in activities that stress accuracy of direction toward a fixed target (as in archery and bowling directf free t] differr groups contra toward side 0 and no The vi studie Shiple rePort whErea the ri (1968) Visior right drawn hand ¢ 4O bowling) than in activities that do not. In studying the direction of missed shots by college women in basketball free throw shooting, Shick (1971) found no significant difference between the unilateral and contralateral groups in errors toward the dominant hand. However, the contralateral group did make significantly more errors toward the non-dominant hand (toward the dominant eye's side of the body). The relationship of lateral dominance to visual and non-visual directionality has received some attention. The visual directionality of autokinetic movement was studied in right and left-handed subjects by Wishner and Shipley (1954). They found that right—handed persons reported movement more often to the right than to the left, whereas left-handed persons reported movement equally to the right and left. In the study of Cratty and Williams (1968), which involved the task of line drawing without vision, the results indicated that lines drawn with the right hand tended to veer slightly to the left and lines drawn with the left hand veered to the right. However, hand dominance was judged not to be a factor in this "crossing-over" phenomenon. In summary, the literature on lateral dominance and motor proficiency reveals that conflicting conclusions have been reached by different investigators using dif« ferent skills. A significant portion of the older evidence would seem to indicate that dominance has an insignificant role in motor skills. However, many studies cor fi< the im} Chi fUJ 41 conducted within the past twenty years have found signi- ficant differences between laterality groups. Whether these recent significant findings may be due in part to improved research designs, improved statistical techniques, changing cultural patterns, or other variables requires further investigation. CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHODS The purpose of the study was to investigate the effects of individual differences in hand and eye domi— nance on the performance of a selected motor task. More specifically, the study attempted to determine if hand and/or eye dominance influenced: (a) accuracy of throwing at a target; (b) directional errors made in the right or left halves of the target; and (c) directional errors made in the tOp and bottom halves of the target. Subjects A population of 230 Sophomore high school girls, currently enrolled in the required physical education program at Eastern High School, Lansing, Michigan, was tested for hand and eye dominance and classified into strata based on exhibited lateralities. The four possible strata were RH-RE, RH-LE, LH-RE, and LH-LE. The results (Table 3.1) showed that the population of Sophomore girls was approximately 94 percent right— handed, 6 percent left-handed, 60 percent right-eyed, and 42 43 40 percent leftneyed. Fifty-seven percent were unilateral in hand-eye dominance and 42 percent were contralateral. These figures correspond with the incidence reported in populations by Crider (1944), Cuff (1931), Barsley (1966), Uhrbrock (1970), and Way (1958). TABLE 3.1.--Laterality of Sophomore Population. Mean age of selected Strata No. Percent sample (n=ll) RH-RE 127 55.2 15.9 RH-LE 89 38.7 16.0 LH-RE 8 3.5 16.3* LH-LE 6 2.6 16.5** Totals 230 100.0 16.2 *Includes three upper class girls. **Includes five upper class girls. Based on the results of a preliminary pilot study (Appendix A), n=ll was estimated to be a necessary and sufficient sample size from each strata. Due to the paucity of left-handed Sophomore girls, both left-handed groups were filled by selecting left—handed Juniors and Seniors from elective physical education classes. A dis- proportionate, random sampling technique was used in order to produce estimates for the left-handed groups with greater precision than a proportionate sample would yield, and to provide equal allocation of stratified subjects. A 44 table of random numbers was employed to select the experimental subjects. The mean age of the sample selected was 16.2 years. Determination of Eye Dominance Each subject was tested to determine their dominant eye using the Hole-in—Card method (Appendix B). It is a simple test to administer, is frequently used, and when repeated ten times has an established reliability of r=.97 (Buxton and Crosland, 1937; Crider, 1944). Ten trials of the test were given with the subject sighting through the hole in the card (held in both hands) at different figures or letters displayed on cards held by the examiner 10 feet (3.0 meters) away. Each_card was displayed for approximately one second. The examiner looked past the card and noted which eye the subject used unconsciously to sight the object. The eye that was lined up with the hole in the card was recorded for each trial. The dominant eye was determined as the one used for the majority of sightings. Of the 238 subjects tested, 233 used the same eye for all 10 trials, three used the same eye for 9 out of 10 trials, and two used the same eye for 8 out of the 10 trials. To obtain unconscious sighting by the subject, as suggested by Miles (1929), subjects were not told the true purpose of the test. They were told their quickness and sharpness of vision was being tested. 45 Several points regarding the administration of eye dominance tests, as cited by Clark (1957), were followed. Care was taken to eliminate the effects of the dominant hand by having the subject hold the card with both hands. When such precautions was taken, the correlation between eyedness and handedness is low. Similarly, care was taken to insure that the subject kept both eyes open and looked directly through the hole in the card. The advan- tage of using the Hole-in—Card method was that the inves— tigator could determine the dominant eye and did not have to rely on the subject's report. Determination of Hand Dominance Hand preference for the study was determined by asking the subject, when commanded, to pick up a dart (which had been placed at the subject's midline on a table directly in front of her) and to throw it as quickly as possible at a dartboard 10 feet (3.0 meters) away. The subject was cautioned not to take time to aim, to ensure the choice of hand would be their natural hand for throw- ing. Five darts were thrown in this manner and the hand used was recorded. All subjects used the same hand for all five trials. To mask the true purpose of the test, each subject was told it was a test of their ability to react quickly. 46 Research Design The study was essentially an Ex Post Facto design because the independent variables, hand and eye dominance, had already been established in the subjects and thus pre-assigned them to comparison groups. The four compari- son groups obtained from the hand and eye dominance testing were: Group I--right-handed, right-eyed. subjects; Group II--right-handed, left-eyed subjects; Group III-- left-handed, right-eyed subjects; and Group IV--left- handed, left-eyed subjects. Weighted dart throwing was used to obtain measures for the three dependent variables; overall accuracy, directional errors in the right and left halves of the target, and directional errors in the top and bottom halves of the target. It was reasoned that weighted darts would reduce the chance for bias due to previous partici- pation in dart throwing with regular darts. Apparatus A 36 x 36 inch (91.5 x 91.5 centimeter) target (Figure 3.1) made of a compressed material was mounted in a vertical position between two uprights. The uprights were constructed so the target face could be adjusted to the height of each subject. The target face consisted of six concentric circles of equal width (4.5 centimeters) drawn on an off-white board in black ink, with each +6 +5 +4 \\ \ *3 \\ \\\ 47 -2 -l o +| +2 +3 +4 +5 +6 //l Jes- -3 -4 / // "'5‘ -5 -6 + +5 *3 / // I cm. = 4.5 cm. Scale: -2 -I O +I +2 +3 +4 +5 +6 -3 -4 -5 -6 Figure 3.! The Target Face -2\\\ 48 demarcation line being 2/16 of an inch (3 millimeters) wide. The mid—point radii of the intervals thus formed were 4.5, 9.0, 13.5, 18.0, 22.5, and 27.0 centimeters, respectively. The total scoreable diameter of the target face was 58.5 centimeters (approximately 23 inches). The bull's eye in the center was a circle 4.5 centimeters in diameter and solid black in color. A grid was superimposed on the target face using fine, red lines so that it was visible to the experimenter, but not the subjects. Each square of the grid measured 4.5 x 4.5 centimeters, which corresponded to the width of the concentric circles. There were six grid intervals to the right and six to the left of the bull's eye. The inter- vals to the right were numbered from +1 to +6 and the intervals to the left from -1 to -6, starting beside the bull's eye and numbering outward. Similarly, there were six grid intervals above the bull's eye and six below. The intervals above were numbered from +1 to +6 and the intervals below from -1 to -6, starting beside the bull's eye. The plus and minus signs were used to indicate dire ection only. Each interval or directional error score (one to six) could be converted to distance from the bull's eye by multiplying by 4.5 centimeters. The darts used were manufactured by the Unicorn Dart Company in England (code 03285), had an overall length of 6.5 inches (16.5 centimeters) and a metal point of .87 inch (2.3 centimeters). The weighted metallic grip was 49 2.62 inches (6.7 centimeters) long, and the upper end of the shaft was wood and had a length of 1.75 inches (4.5 centimeters) on the end feathered. The average weight of the six darts used during the study was 29.36 grams with a range of 29.24-29.44 grams. (One of the original five darts was broken during the study and replaced by one of comparable weight.) Measures Darts were thrown at the target from a distance of 10 feet (3.0 meters). The 10 foot subject-target distance was adopted because it was found to be the best distance from the board for adults (Johnson, 1919), and is close to the nine feet used in the English."20—Point" Dart Game. Each dart thrown received three scores. One for accuracy, one for right-left directional error, and one for direc- tional error above or below the bull's eye. Accuracy (X1) was measured using the six concentric circles. A zero was recorded for a bull's eye, one point for a hit in the innermost circle, two points for a hit in the second circle, and so forth out to a score of six in the sixth concentric circle. An accuracy score of seven was given for any hits beyond the sixth circle. Thus, the lower the score, the more accurate the throw. Right-left directional error scores (X2) indicated the magnitude of the error in the horizontal plane and its direction from the bull's eye. A plus was used to 50 show the throw was to the right and a minus to the left of the bull's eye. Scores ranged from zero in the inter- val containing the bull's eye to +6 at the far right and -6 at the far left. A score of seven was given for any hits beyond these boundaries. Lower scores indicated less deviation from the target center (greater accuracy) in the horizontal plane. Above-below directional error scores (X3) indicated the magnitude of the error in the vertical plane and its direction from the bull's eye. A plus was used to show the throw was above and a minus below the bull's eye. Scores ranged from zero in the interval containing the bull's eye to +6 at the tOp edge of the target and -6 at the bottom edge. A score of seven was given for any hits beyond these boundaries. Lower scores indicated less deviation from the target center (greater accuracy) in the vertical plane. Procedures Data was secured for the study during April, 1974. Each subject attended two measurement sessions of dart throwing within a period of seven days. Two sessions were used to avoid boredom of the subject and to obtain the desired number of trials. During each of the two measure- ment sessions, each subject was_given five successive practice throws (determined from the Pilot Study, Appendix A) followed by a brief interval while the darts were 51 retrieved by the investigator. Five additional sets of five trials each were given and the 25 trials were scored for accuracy, R-L (right—left) directional error, and A-B (above-below) directional error. The interval between sets was only long enough to score and retrieve the darts. To remove the possibility of external invalidity due to subjects' knowledge of research, efforts were made to keep the subjects uninformed as to the true purpose of the study. In order to accomplish this objective and also to increase motivation, the subject was given 100 points for each "bull's eye" and 50 points for a hit in the circle adjacent to the bull's eye. No points were awarded outside these two areas. This scoring system also provided immediate feedback for the subject. All tests and measurements were performed by the investigator during the regularly scheduled physical education class of the subject in the same unoccupied room at the school. Only the investigator and subject were present for each session. Data Collection The top edge of the target face was adjusted to match each subject's head height. This placed the bull's eye of the target at approximately shoulder height, which was about the level of dart release. Using a carpenter's right-angle square and tape measure, a restraining line was marked 10 feet (3.0 meters) from the target with a 52 two foot piece of tape. The exact center of the target was located on the restraining line and indicated by placing a piece of tape perpendicular to the restraining line. A "T" was thus formed. The subject was instructed to stand with one foot on the line indicating the center of the target. (The foot to be used was not specified to avoid biasing the data in favor of ipsilateral or contra- lateral—dominant individuals.) Other than this, no special position of the feet was required except that one foot had to touch the 10 foot restraining line and could not go over it. A subject could stand with.feet side by side or in stride position. The method of throwing the darts was not controlled except that a posterior-anterior overhand throw was required. The dart was held between the thumb and first two fingers with the hand above the subject's throwing shoulder. The throw was made by extension of the flexed forearm and wrist. Scoring and Recording During the first measurement session, trials 1-5 were allowed for practice and were not scored. Each trial 6-30 received three scores; one for each of the dependent variables X1, X2, and X3. The Xl (accuracy) score was recorded as an integer from zero to seven, representing hits in the concentric circles of the target. The X (R-L directional error) score was recorded as an 2 53 integer from zero to seven, representing the distance from the bull's eye, and was preceded by a plus or minus to indicate the direction of the error. The X3 (A—B directional error) score was recorded as an integer from zero to seven, representing the distance from the bull's eye, and was preceded by a plus or minus to indicate the direction of the error. The second measurement session was recorded in a similar manner with trials 31-35 used for practice and 1, X2, and X3. All scores can be converted to their equivalent distances from the trials 36-60 scored for X center of the target; i.e., an accuracy score (X1) of four would indicate the throw was 18.0 centimeters (4 x 4.5 cm.) from the center of the target, and a R-L directional error score of minus three would indicate the throw was 13.5 centimeters to the left of the bull's eye. Treatment of the Data The data was treated as a fixed-effects, 2 x 2 design having right and left categories of hand dominance, and right and left categories of eye dominance. Main effects were obtained for hand dominance and for eye dominance on each of the three dependent variables. Inter- action effects of hand-eye dominance (RH-RE, RH—LE, LH-RE, LH-LE) were obtained for the three dependent variables. The influence of ipsilateral dominance and contralateral dominance on each dependent variable was obtained by 54 grouping the RH-RE and LH—LE groups together (ipsilateral), and the RH-LE and LH-RE groups together (contralateral). Sets of five trials each were used to organize the data into more representative scores than were yielded by individual trials. (Darts were thrown in sets of five.) That is, accuracy was represented by summing the scores for five trials, and R-L and A-B directional errors were represented by algebraic summation of the scores for five trials. Data was submitted to the CDC 6500 computer at Michigan State University for analysis. Statistical Procedures Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to evaluate the influence of hand and eye dominance on overall accuracy, R-L directional errors, and A-B direc- tional errors. A_priori comparison tests were used to evaluate the influence of ipsilateral and contralateral dominance on overall accuracy, R-L directional errors, and A-B directional errors. The use of multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was investigated based on the correlation shown between dependent variables on the pilot study. R—L direc- tional error had little relationship to accuracy (r=.03) or to A-B directional error (r=-.12). Accuracy and A-B directional error also were not highly related (r=—.4l). MANOVA, therefore, was not deemed necessary for analyzing the study. 55 Significance Level Alpha was set at the .05 level for the study. However, the ANOVA's for accuracy and A-B directional error were tested at the .025 level due to the additive effect caused by the dependency existing between the two variables (revealed by the pilot study, Appendix A). The .05 level of significance was selected for the study because it agrees with that conventionally used in education and the behavioral sciences. The investigator believed the risk of committing a Type I error could be tolerated in view of the possible importance that signi- ficant findings might have for future research. CHAPTER IV RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The study was undertaken to determine the influence of lateral dominance on throwing accuracy, on the direction of errors made in the right and left halves of the target, and on the direction of errors made in the top and bottom halves of the target. The chapter will begin with a brief description of the relationships found between the three dependent variables. Results will then be presented and analyzed separately for each variable in this order: accuracy (X1), R-L directional error (X2), and A—B direCn tional error (X3). Finally, a discussion offering some rationale for the observed data will be presented. Relationships Among Dependent Variables The BASTAT program was utilized to compute simple correlations between pairs of the dependent variables (Table 4.1). The results indicated that R-L directional error was not significantly related to either of the other two variables. RrL directional error had a correlation of r=-.03 with accuracy. The same correlation coefficient 56 57 TABLE 4.l.--Correlations Between Dependent Variables. X1 x2 X 3 X1 (Accuracy) 1.000 — .033 - .385 X2 (R-L error) 1.000 - .240 X3 (A-B error) 1.000 had been obtained between these two variables on the pilot study, except the relationship was positive (r=+.03). The correlation of R-L directional error with A-B direc— tional error was r=-.24, which was slightly higher than that obtained on the pilot study (r=-.12). Accuracy and A—B directional error exhibited the highest correlation, r=-.39. This was slightly lower than the pilot study correlation between the two variables (r=-.4l). The relationship between accuracy and A-B directional error was not high enough to permit prediction (R2=.16). Statistical Results The AOV program for the CDC 6500 computer at Michigan State University was used for the analyses. Line graphs have been used to present pictures of the perform- ances of the four laterality groups and of the ipsilateral- contralateral groupings. 58 Accuracy The LH-RE group was least accurate during both sessions of dart throwing as indicated by the upper curve in Figure 4.1. Although not as noticeable in Trials 6-30 as in Trials 36-60, the LH—LE and RH-RE groups occupied a middle position, and the RH-LE group was the most accurate. This phenomena was reflected in the mean accuracy scores for the groups. Means for the LH-RE, LH-LE, RH-RE, and RH-LE groups were, respectively: 3.03, 3.20, 3.20, and 3.59. Each mean, when multiplied by 4.5 centimeters, gives the average distance from the bull's eye for the group. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was not signifi- cant for the main effects of hand dominance and eye dominance or for their interaction (Table 4.2). The power of the tests were found to be less than .20, which was too low to detect significant differences between groups. The §_priori comparisons test between the ipsilateral (RH-RE and LH-LE) and contralateral (RH-LE and LH-RE) groups failed to show a significant difference (F=.35). In the graph of Trials 6-30 (Figure 4.2), the ipsilateral group appeared to be more accurate than the contralateral group. This pattern was not repeated during Trials 36-60, however. 59 .mmsouo wuflamuoumq ha momusoom Hamuo>OIl.H.v musmflm manage om mm om m: 0: on mm om we as -om -Hm -oa -Hs. -om -o~ -Hm. -oH -HH -o W alum“ eoooeeoo \\ male -.-.-.-.- mum-5 llll 3-5 Ill 03H one 00H 05H owH omH com OHN eaoos Kaeanaav Iago; aura-midsugmfinflfiufnni 5.... . 60 TABLE 4.2.—-ANOVA of Accuracy Scores. Source SS df MS F P A (Hand) .885 l .885 2.39 .130 B (Eye) .862 l .862 2.33 .135 AB .130 1 .130 .35 .556 Error 14.828 40 .371 Total 16.706 43 Ipsilateral a) 400 \\ "'—"' Contralateral u 5'; \ >‘ 380 \ ——--K\ o m *5 360 o 0 <1 1- 340 m 4.) o 9* 320 300 6- 11- 16- 21- 26— 36- 41- 46- 51- 56- 10 15 20 25 30 40 45 50 55 60 Trials Figure 4.2.—-Ipsilateral-Contralateral Accuracy. 61 R-L Directional Error The graph of the results of Trials 6—30 (Figure 4.3) indicates that all left-handed subjects threw to the right of target center for the majority of their trials, as shown by their curves in the positive half of the graph. The RH—RE group varied from side to side, but tended to be more positive than negative. The RH—LE group consistently threw to the left of target center, and became more deviant with increasing trials. The pattern changed somewhat during Trials 36-60 (Figure 4.3). The contralateral groups (RH-LE and LH—RE) continued to throw to the side of the target opposite their throwing hand. That is, the LH-RE group threw to the right half of the target and the RH-LE group threw to the left half of the target. The ipsilateral groups (RH-RE and LH-LE) varied from side to side with an average very close to center. The two ipsilateral groups had curves that were very similar during Trials 46—60. The ANOVA for R-L directional error (Table 4.3) did not demonstrate the main effects of hand dominance and of eye dominance to be significant factors in the direction in which errors were made. The interaction effect of hand and eye was also not significant. Power for the test was less than .30, which was not sufficient to detect significant differences between laterality groups. .mmsouu huflamnwumq ha Houum Hmcofiuomnflo Alm||.m.v musmflm manage ow mm on m: on on mm om ma oa Ihmh Am 1.5 Ha Lon -mm Am 1: -S -o om- flu om- My 1. HI m o oa+ “H T! on w o~+ ,- om+ QMUIM 00000000 m—H'é olo'olo'. mm-E Illl GJOOS Joaag q-g Iago; 63 TABLE 4.3.--ANOVA of R-L Directional Error. w—v Source SS df MS F P A (Hand) 1.146 1 1.146 2.16 .149 B (Eye) 1.107 1 1.107 2.09 .156 AB .260 1 .260 .49 .488 Error 21.216 40 .530 Total 23.728 43 A plot of ipsilateral versus contralateral groups (Figure 4.4) showed the ipsilateral group threw to the right of center and the contralateral group threw pre- dominantly to the left of center in Trials 6~30. The pattern was similar for both groups. This effect, so evident during Trials 6-30, did not recur during Trials 36-60. The tendency of both groups to reverse sides acted to negate the effects of Trials 6-30 in the overall effects for 50 trials. The means obtained for the four groups confirmed the observed "crossing-over" phenomena of contralateral individuals (LH-RE = -.25; RH-LE = -.39), and the tendency of ipsilateral individuals to deviate equally to the right and left of center (RH-RE = +.08; LH-LE = +.09). The negative mean for the RH-LE group created a problem for the §_priori comparisons test due to the algebraic cancellation of contralateral means which acted to obliterate the true situation. Reversion to the use of Total R-L Error Score 64 Ipsilateral T40 —_- Contralateral '8- «4 5420 \ I\ n \\ y. l \ ,” \\ '7 \\ —2o \’ \ / \ 33 \ m 5—40 -60 6- 11- 16- 21- 26- 36- 41- 46- 51- 56- 10 15 20 25 30 40 45 50 55 60 Trials Figure 4.4.--Ipsilateral-Contralateral R-L Error. absolute scores would result in simply another measure of accuracy and not of directional error. The problem was solved by changing the reference point for deviations from that of the bull's eye to that of the means for handedness (Scale A, Figure 4.5). That is, the new points of refer- ence were the means obtained for all leftwhanded subjects (+.17) and for all right-handed subjects (-.15). Devia- tions from these points reflected the influence of eye dominance on the direction of the throw. If the eye tended to correct the error by moving the mean toward zero, the deviation was considered positive. If the influence of eye dominance moved the mean in the other direction (away from zero), the deviation was considered negative. The 65 correction scores thus obtained for each group were: RH-LE = -.24; LH-RE = —.08; RH—RE = +.23; LH-LE = +.O8 (Scale E, Figure 4.5). — + + .— deviations *- RH(--15) —->- «<— LH(.17) —>— A 1 I l : 1 l 1 : J I LA -4 -3 —2 t -l 0 +1 :+2 -+3 +4 0 R S :O L . LH- ; LII—RE RH-LE ‘5 RH-RE LE : (.25) (:.39) Z (.08) ”(.09)E B l I I : l I a. l ‘ i B S ifs I -4 ‘\ :2” “x 0 ’x \‘ ’,‘\\+2 ’I +4 ‘\ I s ,’ ‘ I \ I ~ I ~ , \ ~ ” ‘~ I \ ’ ‘ I ~’I \"’ ‘l’ "I Y V v v x=-.24 x=+.23 x=+.08 x=-.08 (RH-LE) (RH-RE) (LH-LE) (LH-RE) x = correction scores Figure 4.5.--Correction Scores from Means of Handedness. The a priori comparisons test utilized the correc- tion scores to compare ipsilateral and contralateral dominance of hand and eye on the right—left direction of errors. The obtained F = 2.06 was not significant at the .05 level. A-B Directional Error All groups threw consistently to the bottom half of the target, as shown by the curves in Figures 4.6 and 4.7. The right—handed groups were least deviant. The obtained means were: RH—LE = ~.247; RH—RE = -.315; 66 .mmnouo Muflamumumq ma uounm Hmcoflpomnflo mlmll.m.¢ musmflm manage om mm om me 0: on mm om me as -on Am 6? A: 6E 6H E -o E.- I" 00..- / met m.- om- m. 0 m- we- ... a. 10 o o o. a . ... ma... ) aim-Hm 0.00.... can o\o ms almm elelelolo o‘e‘ on... a“ me-E IIII ( eaoos aoaag q-v Iago; 67 Lh-LE = -.396; LH-RE = -.709. The LH—LE and RH-RE groups tended to have similar patterns during Trials 6—30 (Figure 4.6), but not during Trials 36-60. The ANOVA (Table 4.4) indicated hand dominance, eye dominance, and the interaction of hand and eye dominance were not significant factors in the direction of errors made in the top and bottom halves of the target. Power of the tests was low, being less than .20 in each case. The a priori comparisons test between ipsilateral and contralateral groups failed to show a significant difference, with F = .36. +20 """""""" Ipsilateral Contralateral ’03 > o g ——_ Total A-B Error Score 6— 11- 16— 21- 26- 36- 41' 46- 51- 56- 10 15 20 25 30 4O 45 50 55 60 Trials Figure 4.7.-—Ipsilateral—Contralateral A-B Error. 68 TABLE 4.4.--ANOVA of A-B Directional Error. Source SS df MS . F P A (Hand) .813 1 .813 1.78 .189 B (Eye) .397 1 .397 .87 .356 AB .165 l .165 .36 .550 Error 18.237 40 .456 Total 19.613 43 DiScussion The moderate correlation found between accuracy of throwing (as measured using concentric circles) and errors above or below a horizontal line drawn through the target center can be logically explained. Accuracy is essentially a function of the power of the throw, the angle of release, or some combination of these factors. These same factors are involved in errors made above or below target center. A weak throw will drop to the bottom half of the target, especially when weighted darts are used. Likewise, the angle of release is crucial to the A-B direction of the error. Releasing the dart too soon results in.a hit above the center of the target, and holding the dart too long results in a downward trajectory and a hit in the bottom half of the target. The negative correlation between accuracy and A-B directional error should be readily apparent as all groups consistently threw below the bull's eye. As accuracy increased, indicated by 69 a throw close to the bull's eye, the A-B directional error decreased, and vice versa. The distance of the throw is related to both the A-B direction of errors and the R-L direction of errors. It is related to the A-B direction due to the power factor discussed previously. Distance is, however, deemed more important in the direction of R-L errors than in A-B errors (Moffett, 1942). Any error in the R-L direction of the throw is magnified as the distance traversed by the object increases. Thus, the "crossing—over" phenomena exhibited by the contralateral subjects should become more evident with an increase in throwing distance. Some rationale for the observed effects of hand and eye dominance on the R—L direction of errors is offered. As noted in scale A of Figure 4.5, left-handed subjects tended to throw to the right of center and right- handed subjects to the left of center. The pectoralis muscles contract to bring the arm and shoulder toward the mid-line of the body. The movement continues until the throwing arm is in line with the dominant eye. If the dominant eye is on the same side of the body as the throwing arm, the mid-line movement.(R-L direction) is halted short of the mid—line. This effectively acts to correct the natural cross-body movement for ipsilateral individuals. If the dominant eye is not on the same side of the body as the throwing arm, the mid-line movement (R-L direction) continues across the mid~line to line up 70 with the contralaterally dominant eye. This action accentuates the natural R-L movement of the throwing arm. These results were depicted in scale B of Figure 4.5. The distances involved between the mid—line and the dominant eye are indeed small, but these small R—L directional errors are magnified as distance from the body increases. The mean R-L directional error for the total sample used in this study was found to be +.01 using the BASTAT program. This was less deviant than the horizontal- directional scores reported by Malina (1968). The tendency in the present study for a mean score to the right of center on the first session and a mean score to the left of center on the second session can also be observed in the data presented by Malina. No explanation for this reversal is attempted at this time. The investigator was curious concerning the fre— quency of errors made to the right or left of the bull's eye by groups differing in hand and eye dominance. A 2 x 4 Chi—Square contingency test yielded no significant difference between the four laterality groups in the frequency of errors right and left of target center. How- ever, a comparison of ipsilateral and contralateral groups on frequency of errors to the dominant and non-dominant eye sides of the target was statistically significant at the .05 level. The 2 x 2 Chi-Square contingency test revealed the contralateral group made significantly more errors 71 toward the dominant eye side of the target than toward the non-dominant eye side. The ipsilateral group did not differentiate significantly between dominant and non- dominant eye sides of the target in frequency of errors made. This discovery would tend to support the findings of Shick (1971) who found contralateral subjects made a higher percentage of errors toward the dominant eye side of their body. CHAPTER V SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS Summary The purpose of the present investigation was to determine the influence of hand dominance, eye dominance, and combinations of hand and eye dominance on the accuracy of throwing at a target , and on the direction of errors made when throwing at a target. The directions considered were the right and left halves of the target and the upper and lower halves of the target. Eleven subjects were randomly selected from each of four laterality strata (right-handed and right-eyed; right-handed and left-eyed; left-handed and right-eyed; left-handed and left-eyed) which_had been obtained from a population of 238 high school girls at Eastern High School, Lansing, Michigan. They ranged in age from 15 years to 19 years. The mean age was 16.2 years. Approxi— mately 82 percent of the sample were Sophomore girls enrolled in a required physical education program. The remaining 18 percent were Junior or Senior girls enrolled in an elective physical education program. 72 73 Each subject was given 30 trials of dart throwing during each of two class periods. The first five throws of each session were allowed for practice and the last 25 throws were scored for accuracy, R~L directional error, and AvB directional error. Darts were thrown in sets of five trials each and scores by sets were used to analyze the data. Graphs of scores by sets of trials were constructed for each laterality group on accuracy, R—L directional error, and A-B directional error. Patterns were not con- sistent between the two sessions of dart throwing. Analysis of variance using scores by sets of trials failed to detect significant differences between laterality groups on accuracy of throwing, R—L direction of errors, or A-B direction of errors. A_priori comparison tests of ipsilateral and contralateral-dominant groups on the three dependent variables failed to show signifi- cant differences between the groups. Conclusions The following conclusions were drawn from the data within the limitations of this study: 1. Hand dominance, eye dominance, and combina- tions of hand and eye dominance failed to be significant influences on the accuracy of throwing at a target. 74 Hand dominance, eye dominance, and combina- tions of hand and eye dominance failed to significantly influence the direction of throwing errors when errors were measured by magnitude and direction of deviation. Ipsilateral and contralateral dominance of hand and eye failed to be significant factors on the direction in which errors were made or on the accuracy attained when throwing at a target. Recommendations Some suggestions for future research on the problem investigated in this study are: 1. Since the power of the test used to analyze the data was low, the study should be repeated using larger sample sizes. A study of errors made following attainment of a higher degree of skill in dart throwing, and of the results obtained when an uninter- rupted session of dart throwing is used, would be of interest. The frequency of errors made toward the dominant and non—dominant eye sides of the body by ipsilateral and contralateral 75 individuals should be investigated. A parallel study might be used to determine the magnitude of the deviations. APPENDICES APPENDIX A PILOT STUDY -s'! *W' PILOT STUDY The pilot study was conducted in February, 1974, to determine the learning curve of dart throwing and to investigate the correlation between the three dependent variables. A knowledge of the dependency between vari- ables was necessary to make decisions related to the statistical analyses of the main study. Specifically, a determination of which trials should be used to avoid a learning factor was sought; an estimate of the variability of each dependent variable was required to calculate a necessary and suficient sample size for each comparison group to be used in the main study; and an estimate of the dependency existing between the three variables was necessary to evaluate the feasibility of using MANOVA to analyze the main study and to adjust significance levels to accomodate dependence if separate ANOVA's were employed instead of MANOVA. Twenty available college women were used as sub— jects (mean age 20.5 years) with five subjects in each of the four laterality groups (RH-RE, RH-LE, LH-RE, LH—LE). Subjects were asked to throw 30 darts in sets of five each during each of two sessions separated by a one week period of time. All 60 throws were scored for X1 76 77 (accuracy), X2 (R-L directional error), and X3 (A-B directional error) in the same manner to be used in the main study. Data was analyzed by the BASTAT program of the CDC 6500 computer at the Michigan State University Computer Laboratory. Results The obtained means by trials (Table A-1) and the graphing of learning curves (Figure A-l) for each depen- dent variable both indicated a large improvement in performance occurred between the first and second sets of five trials in each session, with a relative leveling off thereafter. It was decided to allow the first five throws of each session for practice and to score the final 25 throws for the dependent variables. The standard deviation of each variable for 50 trials (Table A-1) indicated a variability of approximately one-half interval (about 2.25 cm.). Using this information, an estimate of a necessary and sufficient sample size for each group was determined to be n=ll. MANOVA was deemed unfeasible based on the correla- X tion between X and X for 50 trials (Table A—l). In 1’ 2’ 3 fact, the correlations of X2 with X1 and X3 were so trivial that R-L directional error could be considered independent of accuracy and A—B directional error. TABLE A-1.--Pilot Study Statistics. 78 Standard Sign. of Trials Mean Deviation the Mean 1-5 19.20 5.53 .005 6-30 2.80 .57 .005 p 1—30 2.97 .59 .005 3 31-35 15.90 4.97 .005 a 36-60 2.67 .64 .005 “('8 31-60 2.75 .66 .005 g 1—5 + 31—35 3.51 .89 .005 6-30 + 36-60 2.73 .56 .005 1—30 + 31-60 2.86 .58 .005 1—5 4.15 8.92 .051 6-30 .27 .74 .121 3 1-30 .36 .79 .054 3 31-35 .20 7.27 .903 03m 36-60 .13 .53 .281 X ,4 31-60 .10 .60 .448 é 1-5 + 31—35 .44 1.43 .190 6-30 + 36-60 .07 .54 .574 1—30 + 31-60 .13 .60 .343 1-5 3.15 12.15 .260 6-30 .25 .88 .205 3 1-30 .11 1.05 .644 a 31-35 .30 8.19 .872 «,3 36-60 .44 .66 .008 .>< m 31-60 .38 .66 .020 - 1-5 + 31-35 .29 1.66 .452 4 6-30 + 36-60 .35 .68 .034 1-30 + 31-60 .24 .78 .180 Correlations Significance 1 X2 X3 of Regression xl 1.000 .033 .412 xl---x2 P—.892 x2 1.000 .115 x1 x3 p—.071 x3 1.000 x2 x3 P-.628 Xl Accuracy (n=20) X2 R-L Error (n=20) X3 A-B Error (n=20) 375 350 325 300 275 250 +90 +60 +30 -30 -60 79 Session I Session II _-——————_— l- 6- 11- 16- 21- 26- 31- 36- 41- “6— 51- 56- 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 6O Trials Session 11 Session I 1- 6- ll- 16- 21- 26- 31- 36- 41- 46- 51- 56- 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 #0 45 50 55 60 Trials Session I Session II _——_—#_——— 1- 6- 11- 16- 21- 26- 31- 36- 41- 46- 51- 56- 5 1o 15 20 25 3o 35 40 45 50 55 6o Trials Figure A-l.--Learning Curves for Dart Throwing. Conclusions 1. 80 Trials 6-30 of the first measurement session and trials 36—60 of the second session would be scored in the main study. An estimated necessary and sufficient sample size for each comparison group would be n=ll. Two-way ANOVA would be used to analyze the main study. The significance level for the ANOVA of R-L directional error would be held at the .05 level originally set for the study. The significance level for the ANOVA's on accuracy and A—B directional error would be set at .025 due to the additive effect of the dependency between the two variables, and the desire to hold the overall significance level to .05. S m OH NH oH mH NH oH m m 6H mH oHo «H mH vH 0H NH HN HH vH mH NN SH SN moo mH m m «H NH m mH «H mH «H mH mH moo Hm HH me 4m 5H 6H 5H mm. NH Hm mH om boo oH oH mH NN mH SH oH mH oH HN oH mH moo pomnnam Amqlmmv HH msouo How Amomncoodex a HH 5 HH NH HN mH oH mH HH NH NH SN moo OH m oH oH NH oH HH HH MH oH HN oH woo HH m SH o OH NH oH mH HH MH HH HH moo m MH HH mH oH oH mH m h oH 0H NH Noo n oH oH oH n m m o mH m a HH Hoo pommnsm Ammlmmv H moono Mom Amomusoodex oolom mmuHm omuov mquo ovlom mmuHm omloN mNIHN oNIoH mHnHH oHIo mIH umHMHHB .6066 36m mocha uoHHm-.N-< mamas 82 OH OH O OH ON ON HH SH OH OH OH OH ONO NN OH OH SH mH ON NH OH OH HH SH HN mHO SH MN OH OH HN OH HN mN OH OH OH ON OHO O HH OH HH OH S OH O OH OH OH NN SHO OH S O OH S O HH HH OH OH OH O OHO pomnnsm AmHImHV >H msonw How Amumusoodex OH NH ON ON mH HN OH mN HN OH ON ON .1 mHo O OH OH SH NH SH m HH mH HH OH ON OHO HH OH OH OH OH SH OH OH O OH OH ON mHo O OH HH OH OH OH OH OH OH OH SH HN NHo NH OH OH ON O OH O OH SH HH OH OH HHo aomnnsm AmmlmHv HHH QDOHU How Amomudoodex OOIOO mmIHm OOIOO mOaHO OOsOm Omle omnON mNIHN ONIOH OHIHH OHIO mlH umHMHHB .UODGNUGOUII-Ntd MHmdfi 83 No+ HO- No: OOI So+ So: oo Ho+ No+ mo: Oo+ Oo+ OHO Non Oo+ OOI Non OOI mo+ No+ mo: mo: Oo+ Ho+ mo+ OOO OOI mOI Ho+ OOI Oo+ Hon Oo+ So: OO HOI mo: Oo+ OOO mo: Ho+ So+ HHI OOI OO So: mo: mo: mo: Ho: Oo+ SOO HH+ No: No+ OOI Sou moi OO Oo+ No: SOI Ho+ Oo+ OOO pomflnsm AmHlmmv HH msoum Mom Anonum HMQOHOUOHHO Hlmv Nx Ho+ No+ mo: oo Ho+ HH+ OH+ HOI Oo+ No+ Oo+ MN+ moo No: HOI No+ HOI Non Oo+ Hon No+ OOI OO Oo+ NH+ OOO Oo+ So+ OH+ OO Oo+ OH+ Oo+ OOI Oo+ OH+ Hon Oo+ moo mo: Hon OOI OOI mo: Son mo: OOI mo: mo: Hon Ho+ NOO Oo+ oo HOI OOI Non Ho: Ho: No+ OO OOI OO Hon HOO pommnsm Ammlmmv H msouo How Auouum HcGOHpomHHp Hlmv Nx OOIOm mmuHm OOIOO OOIHO OOIOm mmnHm OmION ONIHN ONIOH mHuHH OHIO OIH "mHMHHB .UOSCHHGOUII-Nld mqmde 84 NOI SoI OO NOI OO SOI NOI OO No+ NOI No+ No+ ONO HOI Hon Oo+ NOI mo+ OH+ HOI OH+ Oo+ Ho+ No+ Oo+ OHO mo+ OO NH+ OOI OHI No+ OH+ HOI No+ NH+ Oo+ OH+ OHO OO No+ HOI OO Oo+ Ho+ HoI Oo+ Ho+ No+ No+ Oo+ SHO OOI NOI NOI HOI NOI moI NOI OO No+ Ho+ Ho+ NOI OHO pomflnsm Amqnmqv >H msoum How Auouum HMGOHHUOHHO HImVNx No+ OOI OOI NOI OO Oo+ OH+ NH+ OH+ Oo+ HH+ OH+ OHO No+ HOI NOI NOI oo Ool No+ HOI NOI No+ NOI Sol OHO Non SOI OOI oo No+ NOI Oo+ Ool Ho+ HOI OOI OHI NHO No+ Oo+ NOI OH+ HOI NOI Ho+ So+ No+ OH+ OOI SOI NHO No+ Oo+ No+ Oo+ NOI SOI HOI No+ No+ Oo+ Oo+ Ool pommwmm Ame-OHO HHH macho now lacuna HacOHuomuHo q-mva OOIOm mmIHm OOIOO OOIHO OOION ONIHN ONION ONIHN ONIOH mHIHH OHIO mIH umHMHHB .UOSGHDGOUII.NIm mqmme 85 Ho+ HOI HOI SOI So+ NOI OOI mo: Oo+ Son Ho+ HH+ OHO OOI OH+ HH+ OO Oo+ OH+ HH+ No+ mo+ OH+ NH+ OH+ OOO OOI HOI Non NOI Oo+ NOI HOI No+ Oo+ HOI OOI SH+ OOO NHI No: NoI mHI OOI NHI OOI OHI OOI OOI NHI OOI SOO HOI OOI No+ NOI Nol OO NOI OOI OHI OOI HOI HHI OOO pomflnam AMHImmV HH moono H0O Anonum HmcoHuomuHO mIflva Oo+ HOI OOI Oo+ OHI NHI OO HOI No+ Non Oo+ NN+ moo Oo+ Ho+ Oo+ No+ NH+ OOI OO No+ SOI No+ SHI Oo+ OOO Hol NOI HOI OOI OOI Oo+ Oo+ mo: NOI HOI SOI Oo+ NOO OOI mOI NOI No+ OHI NOI No+ HOI NOI HOI No+ NOI NOO NOI NOI HOI OOI HOI NOI OOI No+ OOI No+ No+ Oo+ HOO hommnsm Rum-mmv H macho now lacuna HmaoHuomuHo m-mvmx OOIOm mmIHm OOIOO OOIHO OOION ONIHN ONION ONIHN ONIOH OHIHH OHIO OIH HmHMHHB . UOSGAHCOUII . Nld mam-«H. 86 HHI HHI Oo+ OOI OoI OH+ No+ NOI Oo+ OOI HOI NH+ ONO OOI oo OOI Oo+ NOI OHI OHI OoI So+ No+ Oo+ SOI OHO HOI mHI OOI Ho+ moI Ho+ OOI NHI OOI OOI HHI ONI OHO OOI Ho+ OOI HoI Oo+ No+ No+ NOI HOI Oo+ Oo+ OH+ SHO NoI NOI OOI NoI NOI oo OOI HoI NoI OoI OOI No+ OHO pomnnsm AqumHv >H macaw How “Houum HmcoHuomuHO mIava NHI moI NHI NOI OO NOI Ho+ OoI OOI NoI SOI OHI OHO OO OoI Oo+ OHI SOI NoI Ho+ NoI OOI Oo+ Oo+ HHI OHO Ho+ No+ Ho+ No+ NOI NH+ HH+ Ho+ NOI No+ Oo+ HN+ NHO Ho+ Oo+ OoI Ho+ Oo+ No+ SoI HOI OOI Ho+ Oo+ NoI NHO OOI HHI SOI No+ NOI Ho+ OOI NOI Ho+ OOI OOI NOI HHO poohnsm AmmImHv HHH msouo How Anonnm HmcoHuomuHU mINONx OOIOO OOIHO OOIOO OOIHO OOION mNIHN ONION ONIHN ONIOH OHIHH OHIO OIH umHMHHB .UOSGH#COUII.NI¢ mHm¢B APPENDIX B EYE DOMINANCE TEST THE HOLE-IN-—CARD EYE DOMINANCE TEST A card 10.1 cm. by 23.0 cm. with a hole in the center of 6 mm. diameter was used to sight objects pre- sented by the investigator at a distance of 10 feet (3.0 meters). Each card presented had a different figure or letter displayed which was large and clear. The object cards measured 12.8 cm. by 20.3 cm. Instructions to the subject were: I am going to test your quickness and sharpness of vision by showing you cards on which there is a letter or figure. I will sit at that desk, and you will sit at this desk. Leave the card with the hole in it on the desk until I tell you to raise it. When I say "ready, up," pick up the card using both hands and hold it about 6 inches (15 cm.) from your face. The investigator demonstrated, but turned sideways so the subject could not see her face directly, but could see the distance involved. Be sure to keep both eyes open. Look through the hole in the center of your card at the card which I will hold up just beneath my chin. I will hold it for one second.‘ When I put my card down, put yours down, and then tell me what you saw on my card, and what color it was. Do you have any questions about what I want you to do? The investigator recorded the eye seen through the hole for each of the ten trials. To score eyedness, a +1 87 gnu-J. ”15!".— MESH-\quflluli 88 was given for each right-eye response and a -l for each left-eye response. A final positive score indicated a right eye dominance or tendency, and a final negative score indicated a left eye dominance or tendency. APPENDIX C RAW DATA 89 O O OH OH OH SH NH OH NN OH NNO OH NH O OH OH OH NH OH ON SH HNO OH HH OH OH OH OH OH OH NH HH ONO OH NN SH OH OH SH OH NH OH HN OHO O O NH OH NH HH NH OH OH HN OHO NH OH OH OH NN SH NN NN HH SH SHO OH OH ON HN OH OH ON OH OH NN OHO NH NH OH NH NH OH OH OH O OH OHO NH NH SH S NN OH OH SH OH NH OHO OH OH O NH ON SH NH NH SH HN NHO HH SH OH OH NH SH OH ON NH OH NHO pomflnsm AMHImmV HH msonw mom AwomucoomvHx NH ON OH OH ON OH NN SH OH OH HHO NN SH SN OH SH OH HN NH ON OH OHO HN OH HN OH ON HH OH OH O NH OOO O OH NH OH OH ON NN ON NN OH OOO O OH OH OH NH OH OH OH OH SH SOO NH OH ON OH SH OH HH NH SH HN OOO OH ON OH OH SN OH OH HN SH SH OOO OH NH OH NH OH OH OH NH NH NH OOO OH HH OH OH O O OH NH OH O NOO NH HN OH OH OH OH OH OH NH NN NOO OH HH OH OH OH OH NH SH OH O HOO nomnnsm Ammlmmv H muono How Amomusoodex OOIOO OOIHO OOIOO OOIHO OOION ONION ONIHN ONIOH OHIHH OHIO umHMHHB .MHMQ SmmII.HIU MHmH msouw HomAmomusoodex OH OH HN SH OH OH HN SH HN NN NNO OH NN HN SN ON OH ON ON ON ON NNO HN SN OH OH OH OH OH OH OH OH HNO ON ON OH ON OH OH SN ON OH ON ONO ON OH OH OH OH NH OH OH NH SN ONO O O OH OH NH OH O NH OH O ONO SH OH OH ON NN OH ON OH NH NH SNO SH OH NH SH OH ON HN ON OH NN ONO ON OH HN HN S OH SH OH NH ON ONO OH OH SH ON O ON HH ON OH HH ONO SH OH OH OH OH NH NH OH OH ON NNO pomnnam AmmImHO HHH msono How ASOMHDOUNOHN OOIOO OOIHO OOIOO OOIHO OOION ONION ONIHN ONIOH OHIHH OHIO umHMHHB .UOSGHfiGOUIl-HIU HHMQB No+ OO OOI Oo+ OHI OOI OHI OOI NHI OOI NNO 50- N0- O0- O0- mH- HHI m0- 0HI OHI NHI HNO 00- N0- MH+ O0- m0+ 00 Oo+ m0+ 00+ No+ 0N0 Ho+ 50- Ho+ 00+ N0I Oo+ No+ m0: m0+ Oo+ OH0 m0- H0- Oo+ H0+ 00 Oo+ 00 00 00 m0+ OH0 50- 50- NHI 00- O0- O0I O0: m0- O0- m0+ 5H0 O0- H0- 40- Oo+ O0- O0I m0I O0- O0- HHI OH0 00 No+ 00+ No+ Oo+ Oo+ 50- 50+ Ho+ 00- mHo H0+ 00 O0- No+ O0I 00- 00- 50- 00 Oo+ OH0 H0- m0- m0- m0- m0- O0- 00+ H0- NH+ 50+ NH0 00- O0- Oo+ m0- Oo+ N0- 50- Ho+ mo- Oo+ NHO pomnnsm Ame-OOO HH @5606 you 146006 HmaoHuomuHO a-momx 91 00 HH- 00- N0+ Ho+ H0I 50- O0- m0- m0+ HHO No+ N0- 00+ 40- No- O0- HHI Oo+ m0- 00 0H0 Ho+ 00 50+ O0- 00- H0- Ho+ Oo+ mo- 40+ O00 Ho+ H0- 00 m0+ 00+ m0- Oo+ OH+ HH+ OH+ O00 H0- 00- mo- 00- Ho- mo- m0+ 00- m0- HHI 500 00 m0- No+ Ho+ 00 H0- 00+ m0+ Oo+ 00 O00 40- m0- 00+ Ho+ O0+ 00+ m0+ No+ m0- H0- O00 00+ 00+ 00 m0+ 00+ 00 50- N0- ~0- N0- O00 50+ 00- O0; No+ 00 O0- Ho+ No+ m0- N0- N00 00 No+ Oo+ 00- 00 Oo+ O0+ Ho+ Oo+ 0H+ N00 m0+ m0- H0- 0H+ «0+ Oo+ 00+ Oo+ Ho- 00+ H00 pomflnsm AmmImmv H muono How AHOHHO HMGOHOUOHHO HImVNN OOIOO OOIHO OOIOO OOIHO OOION ONION ONIHN ONIOH OHIHH OHIO umeHHB .UOSCHunooll-HIU MHm¢B 92 O0+ «0+ «0+ Oo+ «0+ 00+ 00+ m0+ Oo+ 00+ OO0 OH+ No+ Oo+ No+ OOI Oo+ Oo+ So+ Oo+ So+ NOO 00 «0+ Oo+ Ho+ 00 «0I «0+ 50- Oo+ 00+ «O0 NoI OHI OOI OOI SoI OOI NOI OOI OHI OHI HOO Oo+ Ho+ Oo+ Oo+ NOI No+ OO OH+ Oo+ Oo+ OOO Oo+ OHI OoI OHI NoI So+ OOI OOI Oo+ Oo+ ONO No+ OoI NOI NOI HOI NoI Ho+ NOI SoI NOI ONO Ho+ Oo+ HOI Oo+ oo Ho+ HOI NOI Oo+ NOI SNO HoI SOI Oo+ NOI OoI Ho+ So+ OOI SOI HOI ONO NOI NOI Oo+ NOI HoI So+ Oo+ ON+ HH+ No+ ONO No+ Ho+ OO OOI OO SOI Ho+ SOI HOI OO ONO unmanam AmHImHO >H macho How “House HMQOHuomHHO HImVNx So+ NH+ Oo+ Oo+ No+ oo OOI Oo+ Ho+ Ho+ NNO NoI Oo+ HH+ So+ HH+ HOI Oo+ NH+ Oo+ OH+ NNO OO Oo+ NOI OHI HOI Ho+ No+ Ho+ Ho+ OH+ HNO OoI OHI Oo+ No+ Ho+ HH+ SOI OH+ HOI No+ ONO No+ No+ Oo+ So+ No+ Oo+ Oo+ Oo+ OOI No+ ONO Oo+ No+ OOI No+ No+ OO No+ Ho+ No+ HOI ONO OO Ho+ OOI No+ OHI OHI OH+ OHI NOI OOI SNO OoI SOI OOI OoI OOI OOI NOI OOI OOI OHI ONO OoI So+ NOI Ho+ No+ NOI No+ NOI HOI OHI ONO Oo+ No+ Oo+ OH+ OO HH+ Oo+ Oo+ HH+ Oo+ ONO Ho+ No+ HOI OO No+ Ho+ So+ NOI SOI Oo+ NNO uomflnam AmmImHv HHH muonw How Anouum HMGOHHUOHHO Hlmva OOIOO OOIHO OOIOO OOIHO OOION ONION ONIHN ONIOH OHIHH OHIO umHmHHB .GODQHPGOUII.HIU NHm¢B 93 «0- «0- m0- Ho+ 00- «0+ 00- 00- 00+ «0I ««0 00 00+ «0- 00- 00- «0- 00- «0- «0+ Oo- HNO 50- 00- m0I O0- N0I 00 «0I 00- 00+ 00+ 0N0 Oo+ 00- NH+ Oo+ 00- HH+ OH+ 00 Oo+ HH+ 0H0 O0I 50- «0- 0HI O0- 00- 50- «0I Ho+ O0- 0H0 «0+ «0I H0- 50: Oo+ 00- O0- H0- H0+ 00+ 5H0 «HI 00- O0- «HI Ho+ Ho+ 0H- 00: 50: 00- OH0 00- «0+ NH+ O0- 50+ 00- 00+ 00 00+ O0- 0H0 OOI No+ OOI Ho+ OoI Oo+ Oo+ OOI No+ Oo+ OHO 00- 00- H0- «0+ 00- 00- 00- 00- Ho+ H0I 0H0 00- 00 00- Oo+ 00+ 0HI Ho+ Ho+ 50+ 0H+ «H0 pomwnam Amqlmmv HH anHOHU HON ARCH-H0 HMSOHflOOH-wmu mldv mum 00+ Oo+ H0- 50+ HH+ 00- «H+ 00- H0I OoI HHO «0- 0H- OHI O0I 50- 00- 0H- O0- OH- «0- 0H0 OHI 0H- OOI 0H- «0I 50I OHI Ho+ 00- H0I 000 H0- «HI 00 «0I H0- H0- O0I Ho+ 00+ 00+ O00 00 «0+ «HI Ho+ 00- 00+ «0I Ho+ 00+ «0+ 500 «0- 00- 00- 00 Oo+ m0I 00- «0I 00- «0+ 000 «0+ 0HI H0- O0+ 50+ Oo+ 50I Oo+ 00- 50+ 000 HHI Ho+ O0- O0- 00- O0+ «0+ mo- O0+ 00 000 0H+ 00 m0- «0- O0- 00 50+ «0- 00+ Ho+ 000 00+ O0- 50- 50+ H0I «0+ Ho+ 00 00- «0+ «00 00- 00+ 00+ «0+ O0I H0- 00+ 00+ 00- H0: H00 uoannam LOO-OOO H @5000 How “00000 H020H0000Ho m-Ovmx 00-0m mm-Hm 0m-OO OOIHO 0O-00 00-O« ONIHN 0«-OH OHIHH 0H-O umHmHHB .UOSGHMOOUII.HIO mHmfla gamma-Erf; .13“ mi 94 OoI OOI NOI OOI OOI SOI OO SoI NOI Ho+ OOO Ho+ HOI NOI HOI OoI OO NOI OO OOI HOI NOO NOI NHI OHI NOI OH+ OOI SOI No+ OHI OOI NOO OOI OOI No+ Oo+ Oo+ OO OoI No+ No+ Oo+ HOO OHI OO OHI OOI OO Oo+ SOI OOI SOI NHI OOO OO OHI Ho+ No+ OOI ONI HH+ OHI Ho+ NHI ONO OO Oo+ So+ No+ Ho+ No+ Oo+ Oo+ Oo+ Oo+ ONO NOI Oo+ OHI OHI Oo+ Oo+ OOI Ho+ NHI OH+ SNO SOI NOI OOI OOI NH+ OO NOI So+ HOI OO. ONO OOI OOI NOI HOI OoI OOI NOI NOI SOI So+ ONO OOI No+ OOI No+ OHI Oo+ OOI Oo+ OO Oo+ ONO pomhnam AqumHV >H maouw How Auouuw HmcoHuomHHO mIvax OoI NHI No+ No+ NOI OHI OoI HOI OOI NHI NNO SOI ONI NHI NOI OOI OHI HNI SOI OHI OO NNO OHI NHI No+ Oo+ NOI NOI No+ HOI OH+ OOI HNO OOI SOI OOI Ho+ OOI OOI OOI SOI OOI NNI ONO OH+ No+ So+ No+ Oo+ Oo+ NOI No+ No+ OOI ONO OO OO OO NOI OO NOI NOI NOI Ho+ HOI ONO OOI HHI So+ OHI NoI Oo+ NHI No+ NoI HOI SNO No+ Oo+ OO Oo+ OOI OHI SOI Oo+ OO OH+ ONO OHI OO SOI Oo+ No+ OHI SOI No+ Oo+ OOI ONO NHI OOI OOI HoI Oo+ OOI OO OHI OOI OO ONO OOI OOI OOI OHI HOI OOI HOI NOI OoI NOI NNO nomnnam AmmImHv HHH msouw now Auouuo HmnoHuomHHp mIvax OOIOO OOIHO OOIOO OOIHO OOION ONION ONIHN ONIOH OHIHH OHIO uchHHB .omsaHucoo-I.H-o sends LIS T OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES Adams, Gary L. Effects of eye dominance on baseball hitting. Research Quarterly. 36:3-9, 1965. Banister, H. A study of eye dominance. British Journal of Psychology. 26:34-42, 1935. Barnsley, Roger H. and Rabinovitch, M. Sam. Handedness: Proficiency versus stated preference. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 30:3430362, 1970. Barsley, Michael. The Left—Handed Book. London: Souvenir Press, Ltd., 1966. Bartee, Horace H. An investigation of simple reaction time and movement time of the dominant and non- dominant arm of elementary school children. Unpublished Master's thesis, North Texas State University, Denton, 1971. Baughman, Larry G. Two methods of determining the effects of sighting dominance on baseball hitting. Unpub- lished Master's thesis, University of Maryland, College Park, 1968. Blau, Abraham. The Master Hand. American OrthOpsychiatric Association, Research Monograph No. 5, 1946. Brain, W. R. Speech and handedness. Lancet. 249:837- 841, 1945. Burge, Ivor E. Some aspects of handedness in primary school children. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 22:45—51, 1952. Burke, Nelda J. Selected motor activities and the develop— ment of laterality in first graders. Unpublished Master's thesis, University of Texas, Austin, 1969. Buxton, E. E. and Crosland, H. R. The concept of eye preference. American Journal of Psychology. 49:458-461, 1937. 95 96 Clark, Margaret M. Lgft Handedness. London: University of London Press, 1957. Collins, E. H. The concept of relative limb dominance. Human Biology. 33:293—318, 1961. Cooke, John P., Jr. The relationship of lateral dominance to the learning of a complex motor skill. Unpub- 1ished Master's thesis, Michigan State University, East Lansing, 1969. Cratty, Bryant J. and Williams, Harriett G. The veering tendency of the arm, when drawing a straight line without vision. . Research Quarterly. 39:932-936, 1968. Crider, Blake. A battery of tests for the dominant eye. Journal of General Psychology. 31:179-190, 1944. Crovitz, Herbert F. and Zener, Karl. A group test for assessing hand and eye dominance. American Journal of ngchology. 75:270—276, 1965. Cuff, Noel B. A study of eyedness and handedness. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 14:164-175, 1931. Dimond, Stuart J. A reappraisal of the concept of cerebral dominance. Journal of Motor Behavior. 3:57—61, 1971. Dolman, P. Tests for determining the sighting eye. American Journal of Ophthalmology. 2:867—868, 1919. Downey, J. E. Laterality of function. Psychological Bulletin. 30:109-142, 1933. Durand, A. C. and Gould, G. M. A method of determining ocular dominance. Journal of the American Medical Association. 55:369—370, 1910. Fox, Margaret G. Lateral dominance in the teaching of bowling. Research Quarterly. 28:327-331, 1957. Frahm, Arlene M. Comparison between lateral dominance and cross dominance in learning the hook delivery in bowling. Unpublished Master's thesis, Washing“ ton State University, Pullman, 1967. Garrison, Roberta A. The study of the relationship between certain traits of laterality and the take—off foot in selected skills of locomotion. Unpublished Master's thesis, Texas State University for Women, Denton, 1969. Gesell, 97 A. and Ames, L. B. The develOpment of handed— ness. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 70:155—175, 1947. Graybiel, A., Jokl, E., and Trapp- C. Russian studies of vision in relation to physical activity and sports. Research Quarterly. 26:480-487, 1955. Greenburg, George. Eye dominance and head tilt. American Journal of Psychology. 73:149-151, 1960. Henry, Franklin M. and Smith, Leon E. Simultaneous versus separate bilateral muscular contractions in rela- tion to neural overflow theory and neuromotor specificity. Research Quarterly. 32:42-49, 1961. Hildreth, Gertrude. The development and training of hand dominance; I. Characteristics of handedness. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 75:197-220, 1949. . The development and training of hand dominance: II. Developmental tendencies in handedness. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 75:221-254, 1949. . The development and training of hand dominance: III. Origin of handedness and lateral dominance. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 75:255—275, 1949. . The development and training of hand dominance: IV. Developmental problems associated with handedo ness. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 76:39—100, 1950. . The development and training of hand dominance: Horine, V. Training of handedness. ‘Journal of Genetic Psychology. 76:101—144, 1950. Lawrence E. An investigation of the relationship of laterality groups to performance on selected motor ability tests. Research Quarterly. 39:90~ 95, 1968. Hull, C. J. A study of laterality test items. Journal Irwin, of Experimental Education. 4:287-290, 1936. L. W. A study of the relationship of dominance to the performance of physical education activi- ties. Research Quarterly. 9:96-1l9, 1938. Jackson, John. Ambidexterity. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner, and Co., Ltd., 1905. 98 Jasper, J. H. and Raney, E. T. The phi test of lateral dominance. AmeriCan Journal of Psychology. 49: 450-475, 1937. Johnson, B. J. Practice effects in a target test and a comparative study of groups varying in intelli- gence. Psychology Review. 26:300-316, 1919. Karpinos, B. D. and Grossman, H. A. Prevalence of left- handedness among selective service registrants. Human Biology. 25:36-49, 1953. Kisler, Geraldine L. The determining laterality factor for differences in performance on a gross motor ability task. Unpublished Master's thesis, University of Iowa, Iowa City, 1971. Lambert, P. Practice effect of non-dominant versus domi- nant musculature in acquiring two-handed skill. Research Quarterly. 22:50-57, 1951. Lotter, W. S. Interrelationships among reaction times and speeds of movement in different limbs. Research Quarterly. 31:147-155, 1960. Malina, Robert M. Reliability of different methods of scoring throwing accuracy. Research Quarterly. 39:149-160, 1968. Miles, W. L. Ocular dominance demonstrated by unconscious sighting. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 12:113-126, 1929. Moffett, Donovan C. A study of accuracy of direction in motor skills at different distances as determined by the relative size of the angle of error. Research Quarterly. 13:466-480, 1942. Norrie, Mary L. Effects of unequal distances and handed~ ness on timing patterns for simultaneous movements of arms and legs. Research ggarterly. 38:241v 245, 1967. Ojemann, R. A. Studies in handedness; I. A technique for testing unimanual handedness. ‘Journal of Educa— tional Psychology. 21:497-611, 1930. Oxendine, Joseph B. Psycholggy of Motor Learning. New York: AppletonJCentury-CrOIts, 1968. Palmer, Robert D. Development of a differentiated handed- ness. Psycholggical Bulletin. 62:257-272, 1964. 99 Parson, B. S. Lefthandedness. New York: The MacMillan Company, 1924. Phillips, Marjorie and Summers, Dean. Relation of kinesthetic perception to motor learning. Research Quarterly. 25:456-469, 1954. Poindexter, M. C. Unilateral and crossed—lateral eye“ hand dominance and performance on a perceptual motor task. Unpublished Master's thesis, University of California, Berkley, 1965. Provins, K. A. Handedness and skill. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 8:79S95, 1956. Putman, Mary L. Bilateral transfer as a function of the degree of first-hand practice and the direction of transfer. Unpublished Master's thesis, Univer- sity of Maryland, College Park, 1971. Rife, D. C. Heredity and handedness. Scientific Monthly. 73:188-191, 1951. Robinson, Edwin N. A comparison of laterally dominant to crossed dominant individuals in tests of reaction time and hand-eye coordination. Unpublished Master's thesis, University of California, Santa Barbara, 1965. Roth, Charles. Hand-eye dominance as a factor in motor ability. Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, New York University, 1942. Shick, Jacqueline. Relationship between depth perception and hand-eye dominance and free—throw shooting in college women. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 33: 539—542, 1971. Sinclair, Caroline B. and Smith, Inez M. Laterality in swimming and its relationship to dominance of hand, eye, and foot. Research Quarterly. 28:395-400, 1957. Staples, Lionel. A comparison of the effects of binocular vision and monocular vision on accuracy in basket- ball shooting. Unpublished Master's thesis, Springfield College, Springfield, 1971. Thomas, Earnestine. Teaching for ambidexterity and uni— 1aterality in basketball skills for fifth.grade students. Unpublished Master's thesis, Texas Women's University, Denton, 1969. 100 Trembly, Dean. Crossed dominance of hand and eye in rela- tion to poliomyelitis. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 26:231-234, 1968. Tuttle, W. W. and Travis, L. E. The relation of precedence of movement in homologous structures to handedness. (Iowa Supp.) Research Quarterly. 6:3-14, 1935. Tyler, Robert W. Lateral dominance as a factor in learning selected motor skills. Journal of Motor Behavior. 3:253-258, 1971. Uhrbrock, R. S. Laterality of champion athletes. Journal of Motor Behavior. 2:285—291- 1970. Van Riper. A new test of laterality. Journal of Experi- mental Psyghology. 17:305-313, 1934. Vogel, O. H. The relationship of dominance to acts of skill. (Iowa Supp.) Research Quarterly. 6:15- 18, 1935. Washburn, M. F., Faison, C., and Scott, R. A comparison between the Miles A-B—C method and retinal-rivalry tests of ocular dominance. American Journal of Psychology. 46:636, 1934. Way, Eunice. Relationships of lateral dominance to scores of motor ability and selected skill tests. Research Quarterly. 29:360-369, 1958. Whiting, H. T. A. and Eastwood, P. Handedness and accuracy of throwing with age. Physical Education Associa- tion of Great Britain and Northern Ireland. Research in Physical Education. Vol. 1, No. 4, 1969. Wile, I. S. Handedness. Right and Left. Boston: Lothrop, Lee and Shepard Co., 1934. Wishner, J. and Shipley, T., Jr. Direction of autoninetic movement as a test of the "sensory-tonic—field" theory of perception. Journal of Personality. 23:99-106, 1954. ‘ MICHIGAN STATE UNIVE SITY LIBRARIES 3145 3586 R I III III 3 1293