SOCIAL STRUCTURE, BEHAVIOR, AND THE MEANING CQMPQ MEN? OF SEW-SYMBOLS Thesis for the Degree of M. A. MlCHiGAN STA3'5 UNNERSITY- Worth Cary Summers @964 ABSTRACT SOCIAL STRUCTURE, BEHAVIOR. AND THE MEANING COMPONENT OF SELF-SYMBOLS by Worth Cary Summers The central focus of this thesis is the inter- relationship between self-conception, social structural involvement, and behavior. Symbolic interactionist theory has asserted thatindividuals describe themselves in terms of the group memberships which they feel identify them- selves, the categories according to which their reference groups identify them. and in accordance with norms associated with their reference groups. Additionally. it is asserted that these self-descriptions permit prediction of how an individual will behave by indicating how he will define situations. and the norms to which he will adhere. Empirical results from the Twenty Statements Test have attested to the validity and potential usefulness of this view. In an attempt to improve upon the kinds of self- social structural-behavioral links that were forthcoming from the Twenty Statements Test the Asset-Liability Instrument (ALI) was developed. This instrument requires respondents to list their most important assets and their most important liabilities and thereby elicits evaluated Worth Cary Summers self-descriptions. It was reasoned that by thus specifically requiring respondents to evaluate themselves, the value according to which such self-assessments were made would sharply delineate both the kind of involvement that the respondent had in the social structure and his potential behavior. A sample of 372 students from introductory sociology and social psychology classes were administered the ALI and their responses were classified into nine content categories. Hypotheses designed to determine the ability of the ALI to provide the desired links between respondents, their involvement in the social structure, and their behavior were developed. Problems encountered at a number of points in working with the ALI and in interpreting results from it led to a reconsideration and reformulation of the rationale which had been used to justify its use. This reformulation focuses upon the "relational" property of symbols including self-symbols. In this view the meaning of all symbols constitutes a class of forthcoming behaviors between classes of actors and objects. It is on the basis of the meanings of an actor's symbolic self-concept that he is related to the environment. and according to which his behavior is shaped and guided. Other symbols representing the limits of the behavioral relationships which are appro— priate to any given self-symbol are organized into normative evaluative scales. A comparative relationship between the Worth Cary Summers symbolic representation of actual behavior and these limit- ing symbols constitutes a self—evaluation. Difficulties encountered in using the ALI are attri— buted to the fact that rather than elucidating the connection between an actor, the social structure, and behavior, self-evaluations actually obscure it by virtue of their added complexity. This fact, in addition to various other technical problems associated with the ALI, are sufficient to severely limit its utility as a means of yielding the kind of information for which it was designed. Recommendations are made for developing instruments capable of exploiting the theoretical possibilities of the reformulated position. SOCIAL STRUCTURE, BEHAVIOR, AND THE MEANING COMPONENT OF SELF-SYMBOLS BY Worth Cary Summers A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Sociology and Anthropology 1964 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS There are a number of people to whom I owe a great deal in connection with this thesis. I am and remain especially indebted to Dr. Frederick B. Waisanen who served as my thesis advisor until his departure for South America. It was from him that I received a most stimulating and challenging introduction to the ideas of Symbolic Inter- actionist theory and it was he who suggested that I look into the possibilities of developing an instrument utilizing self-evaluations and who secured the sample used in the present study. After Dr. Waisanen's departure Dr. Robert L. Stewart kindly made room in a very busy schedule to assume the time-consuming task of directing the thesis to completion. The present form of the thesis owes a great deal to his relevant and often painfully penetrating criticism and suggestions. Fortunately for me, Dr. Stewart's skill as a critic is matched by his patience. Thanks, too, are due to Dr. Archibald 0. Haller for valuable suggestions and criticisms, and to Dr. Carl Couch the third member of the thesis committee. Needless to say, I alone am responsible for any shortcomings in the present work. It is unfortunate but inevitable that no expression of gratitude would be adequate to convey my appreciation to my wife Janet who has helped me in more ways than she knows. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter I. THE ASSET-LIABILITY INSTRUMENT . . . . . . . Introduction: Purpose and Organization of the Thesis . . . . . . . . . . . . The Asset-Liability Instrument . . . . . Coding the ALI Scoring the ALI Additional Measures and Indices . . . . Hypothe ses O O O I O O O O O O O O O O 0 Description of the Sample II. FINDINGS O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Method of Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . Results 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Discussion and Critique . . . . . . . . III. THEORETICAL REFORMULATION AND EVALUATION OF THE ASSET-LIABILITY INSTRUMENT . . . . . Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Theoretical Restatement . . . . . . Evaluation of the Rationale Justifying Self-Descriptions . . . . . . . . . . Evaluation of the Asset—Liability Instrument . . . . . . . . . . . . . Implications for Further Research and Development of the Theory . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . BIBLIOGMPHY O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O C O O O 0 iii LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Percentage distribution by people by number of statements made in each asset and liability category . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13 2. Percentage description of sample in terms of sex, grade in school and socioeconomic level 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 27 3. Class in school and number of males making social-skill asset and liability statements 33 4. Class in school and number of males making normative asset and liability statements . . 33 5. Number of role and job skill asset and liability statements by sex . . . . . . . . 37 6. Grade point average by number of intellectual, scholastic asset statements . . . . . . . . 39 7. Grade point average by number of intellectual scholastic liability statements . . . . . . 39 8. Number of Protestants and Catholics making 0 or 2 or more normative asset statements by importance of religion . . . . . . . . . . . 41 9. Number of Protestants and Catholics making 0 or 2 or more normative liability statements by importance of religion . . . . . . . . . 41 10. Relationship between number of social skill asset and liability statements and organizational membership . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 ll. NUmber of individuals above and below the median self-esteem score by socio-economic level 0 O O O O O O O I O I O O O O O O O O 45 12. Self—esteem and perceived agreement with significant others in self-evaluation . . . 46 iv LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. The Asset-Liability Instrument . . . . . . . . 7 2. Question for important others and agreement with important others . . . . . . . . . . . 18 3. Graphical representation of self-evaluation . . 65 CHAPTER I THE ASSET—LIABILITY INSTRUMENT Introduction Belief that the self (a) reflects involvementin the social structure, and (b) that it is an important determinant of behavior, has resulted in continued efforts to use measurements of the self as a technique for empirically linking individuals to the social structure and for predict- ing their behavior. The method that has been most often employed for this purpose is the Twenty Statements Test (TST)l in which respondents write up to twenty statements in answer to the question "Who am I?" The theoretical rationale underlying its development and justifying its use in empirical research has been stated best by Kuhn: There is no way for the individual to answer such a question except by referring to himself as a member of the groups that he feels identify him, or to social categories which his reference groups hold to be significant in identifying him, or finally, to evaluations of himself which hinge on norms held by his reference groups. Whichever of these referents he chooses, his answer consists of attitudinal statements, which are the best indexes of what he will do in any situation. They point 1 (Thomas MCPartland).Manua1 for the Twenty Statements Problem (revised, Department of Research, the Greater Kansas City Mental HBalth Foundation, 1959). to the norms that he will invoke to define a situation, to determine the role that he will play, and to supply the values that he will seek to effectuate.2 Responses on the TST have been subjected to several different scoring procedures and methods of content analysis; results have confirmed an association between certain cate- gories of content analysis and social structural involvement, such as, religious affiliation,3 social class,4 age, sex, and involvement in professional schools,5 and certain behaviors, such as, differential readiness to evaluate performance on the basis of cues from others,6 and behavior of patients in a psychiatric ward.7 The development of the Asset—Liability Instrument (ALI), which is to be discussed and evaluated in this thesis, was stimulated by this theory and by these promising results. 2C. Addison Hidkman and Manford Kuhn, Individuals, Grogps, and Economic Behavior (New York: The Dryden Press, 1956): PP. 43-44. 3Manford H. Kuhn and Thomas S. McPartland, "An Empirical Investigation of Self-Attitudes," American Sociological Review, Vol. 19, No. l (1954), pp. 68-76. 4Thomas S. McPartland and John H. Cumming, ”Self- Conception, Social Class and Mental Health," Human Organization, 5Manford Kuhn, "Self-Attitudes by Age, Sex, and Professional Training,” Sociological Quarterly, vol. 1, No. l (1960), pp. 39-55. 6Carl J. Couch, "Self-attitudes and Degree of Agreement with Immediate Others," American Journal of Sociology, V01. 63, No. 5 (1958), pp. 491-496. 7Thomas S. McPartland, John H. Cumming, and WYnona S. Garretson, "Self-Conception and Ward Behavior in Two Psychiatric Hespitals," Sociometry, Vol. 24, No. 2 (1961), pp. 111-124. The ALI will be discussed in detail at a later point in this chapter. Briefly, however, it asks respondents to indicate their "most important assets (positive factors)" and their "most important liabilities (negative factors)." Behind the selection of this particular approach to the measurement of the self was the belief that it would link respondents to the social structure and to their behavior more clearly and conclusively than had the TST. It was reasoned that as with the TST, subjects would describe themselves in terms that were associated with the reference groups with which they were involved; but in addition, by asking for assets and liabilities they would also assess and describe themselves in accordance with the values held by their reference groups. It was believed that these values would (a) closely reflect their source in the social structure and (b) would be an important influence on their behavior. Following Kuhn then, it was proposed that there was no way in which an individual could answer the question posed by the ALI--"What are your most important assets (positive factors?)" and "What are your most important liabilities (negative factors?"--except by: 1. "referring to himself as a member of the groups that he feels identify him, or to social cate- gories which his reference groups hold to be significant in identifying him, 2. [and by simultaneously evaluating himself according to the values derived from norms held by his reference groups.] 3. "Whichever of these referents he chooses, his answer consists of attitudinal statements, which are the best indexes of what he will do in any situation. They point to the norms that he will invoke to define a situation, to determine the role that he will play, and to supply the values that he will seek to effectuate." Accordingly, the original objective of this thesis was to determine whether the ALI would, in fact, live up to these expectations. Hewever, this determination has been complicated considerably by developments which occurred subsequent to the collection of the data. Problems encountered in developing the coding categories, in the selection of hypotheses, and in attempting to analyze and interpret the results led to a reconsideration and reformulation of the theoretical rationale underlying and justifying the development of the ALI. This reformulation has had two important consequences which have greatly affected the organization of this thesis. First, it has confirmed the soundness of Kuhn's assertion regarding the importance of self—descriptions. Second, it has, on the other hand, demonstrated the invalidity of the basic assumption upon which the ALI rests--that self-evaluations would yield "better" results than a self-description,—-and furthermore that this invalidity" is the source of much of the difficulty encountered in using the ALI. While these problems can be understood and explained in terms of the reformulated position, they cannot be eliminated in this thesis for they are a necessary result of the instrument itself and can only be avoided by using different techniques. As a result of these developments, the emphasis of this thesis has shifted from an evaluation of the ALI based on empirical results to an evaluation based upon a consider- ation of the theoretical foundation upon which the instrument rests. With respect to the organization of the thesis this occurs in the following ways: 1. The discussion of the ALI and an interpretation of the results of the ALI is carried out in accordance with the original line of argument upon which it was based, insofar as this is possible. 2. Difficulties encountered in attempting to do this are indicated. 3. The problems encountered are then explained as resulting from technical characteristics of the ALI, and from inconsistencies between the assumptions underlying the ALI and the reformulated theoretical position. In the remainder of this chapter the Asset-Liability Instrument, the procedure for coding and scoring it, and the hypotheses relating to it are discussed. In addition, some of the problems encountered in working with it are pointed out. In Chapter II, the results of the hypotheses are presented along with a brief summary and discussion of some of the technical and interpretive difficulties associated with its use. In Chapter III, the theoretical reformulation is presented, followed by an evaluation of the ALI in which it is indicated how the inconsistencies between the ALI and the reformulation led to the problems encountered in working with and in interpreting the results of the instrument. Finally, a brief discussion of the implications that the theory and the flaws uncovered in the ALI have for the development of suitable instruments and for further theoreti- cal development is presented. The Asset—Liability Instrument The ALI is an adaptation of questions initially employed as a technique for distinguishing between inner and other directedness. Riesman8 asked respondents to indi- cate their three best personality traits, and Littunen and Gaier,9 and Gaier and Wambach10 had respondents list three of their greatest personality assets, and three of their greatest personality liabilities. Although the results of these studies are irrelevant11 to the main 8David Riesman, Faces in the Crowd (New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press, 1950). I 9Yrjo Littunen and Eugene L. Gaier, "Occupational values and Mbdes of Conformity," Journal of Social Psychology, 10Eugene L. Gaier and Helen S. Wambach, "Self- Evaluations of Personality Assets and Liabilities of Southern White and Negro Students," Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 51, 1960, pp. 135-143. 11They are irrelevant largely because they are uninterpretable, Riesman‘s because of the extreme informality with which the responses were used, ani Littunen £3.éi because of inappropriate application of statistical analysis. purpose of this thesis, the kinds of answers which these questions elicited suggested that a similar question might lead to a significant differentiation of respondents according to the evaluated self—descriptions. The ALI appears below. Figure l. The Asset-Liability Instrument. 1. In the space below please indicate what you think are your most important ASSETS (positive factors). 11. In the space below, please indicate what you think are your most important LIABILITIES (negative factors) It can be seen that several characteristics were built into the ALI in order to bring it into accord with the theoretical statement with which this chapter was begun. First, as has already been indicated, evaluations are explicit— ly called for by asking for assets (positive factors) and for liabilities (negative factors). Second, the instrument was made more open-ended than the question employed by Riesman, and Littunen, Gaier, and wambach, by providing for eight, rather than three, responses in order to allow for the possibility that, if given the opportunity, the subjects might provide additional useful responses. Third, since the kinds of self-responses that are made are of central importance for the theory it was felt that by removing the term "personality,' as used by the above mentioned authors, the possibility of a systematic biasing of the self-symbols would be removed. Coding the ALI Content Categories Nine categories were developed into which the asset and liability responses were separately coded by content analysis. It is to be emphasized that the theory provided no explicit basis for deciding a priori what properties or characteristics the categories should exhibit. In a general and rather vague way it was assumed that all of the responses included within a category would represent some underlying value or valued category of self-identification, and that it would suggest, (a) a possible reference group as its source, and (b) certain kinds of behaviors. The basis for deciding on such a connection is undefined by the theory; more about this will be said under the section headed Hypotheses in Chapter I. Because of the lack of explicit criteria for grouping certain kinds of responses into categories, meanings were imputed to responses and hence also to the different categories. More will be said in Chapters II and III concerning the validity of imputing meanings to self-symbols. 1. Physical. These are statements which deal with the respondent's physical appearance, or other attributes which suggest that the respondent views him- self as a physical object. Statements dealing with physical characteristics of health are also included. Examples include: "strength," "looks," "coordination," "complexion," "too tall," etc. 2. Material or Economic. These statements reflect differential economic advantage or disadvantage or possession or lack of possession of material goods. Individuals are Q9; included in this category. Examples include: "few luxuries," "fail to make ends meet," ”need of money,” "accumulated savings," but not "my friends." Also included are statements indicating desire for economic advantage, e.g., "desire for financial gain." 3. Significant Others. These statements presumably indicate close primary relationships or an appreciation of specific individuals. For example, "my friends," "good wife," would be scored as category 3, but, ”friendly" would be scored as category 4—-social skills. Similarly, "my fiance" would fall in category 3, but "engaged" would fall under category 6--statements of Social Location or Categorical Placement. Similarly, fhusband," “father," given by a male respondent as an asset would be placed in category 6-— Social Location, but if he had listed "my wife," or "son," these would be included in the present category. 4. Social Skills. These statements mention a specific social ability or lack of ability and are regarded as indications that the respondent perceives interaction and social involvement in general to be important per se. All statements in which people are mentioned are automatically included provided they do not fall into category 3-—for example, "ability to speak before a group." Not included are some statementsjn which social relationships themselves appear in a negative light. For example, "too easily influenced by others," seems to be a statement of insuf- ficient autonomy and is classified into category 5—- Normative, Imperative, or Directive statements. 10 Examples include, "poised," "talk too much," extrovert," "interested in others," “sarcastic," "sense of humor," "personality." 5. Normative, Imperative or Directive. These statements seem to be generalized descriptions of ideal or normative behavior frequently having the characteristics of values or of moral or ethical imperatives. They are regarded as representing valued, culturally carried, generalized recipes for behavior applicable to many situations. They are abstractions of complex ways of acting or behaving. Examples include, "haven’t a set goal," "ability to adjust," "not enthusiastic enough," "rebellious to authority," "desire," "liberal outlook," "good Republican," "little prejudice." But statements such as, "feel insecure," "moody," are placed in category 8, a residual category which includes markedly subjective and affect states. 6. Social Location or Categorical Placement. These statements serve to place the respondent within some clearly defined social category. They distinguish him from others and establish his social position on the basis of certain well defined roles or other classificatory criteria. In contrast to category 5 statements, it is assumed that by making self-assessments within this category a respondent would tend to see himself in terms of categories of other actors rather than in terms of the more general and pervasive ideals or norms. Examples include, "member of the middle class," "white," "ROTC Cadet," "married," "fraternity man," "male . ll 7. Job or Role Skills or Their Lack. These statements express the respondent‘s possession of skills or abilities requisite to performance within a specific role or setting. Also included are capacities to enjoy various activities. Examples include, "manual skills," "athletic," "fast runner,“ "good musician,“ "good salesman." 8. Non—Specific or Idiosyncratic. These are state— ments of mood or affect and general statements that are neither imperative nor especially predictive or helpful in defining subsequent behavior. They may indicate a lack of facility in defining one's relationship to people or situations. Examples include, "luck," "upbringing," "early family environment," "faith in the future of America," "education," "backing of my parents and family." Also included are statements of future events which may indicate 11 a lack of direction, e.g., "no job for the summer," "armed service obligation," and abstract or general conditions which may accrue to one, e.g., "opportunity to advance," "freedom as an American," "have traveled much," ”think all people have some good." 9. Intellectual or Scholastic Characteristics. This is a category which was created for the college sample. Statements which are included would ordinarily have been classified into one of the other categories. Included are all references to characteristics which would directly pertain to scholastic or intellectual competence. Examples include, "smart," "have difficulty concen- trating on my school work," ”I catch on to things quickly," "poor grades." ' There were occasions when certain responses could have, with some justification, been placed in either of two categories. In such instances, if the statement in doubt appeared between two other statements, both of which clearly fell into one of the disputed categories, then it was included in that category: otherwise, the statement was included in that alternative category which had the greatest frequency or saliency for the respondent. Coding Reliability Two measures of the reliability of the coding procedure are available: one, a measure of the reliability over time using the same coder, and the other, a measure of inter-coder reliability. After a lapse of 2-1/2 years the original coder was able to code 94% of all of the responses of 20 subjects into the same categories into which they had originally been placed. On another sample of 20 subjects, using an independent coder, 87% of all of the 12 responses from 20 subjects were placed into the categories into which they had been placed by the original coder. The essential point to consider in connection with the coding procedure, however, is not the apparent "reasonableness" of the consistency-reliability figures but the validity of the coding procedure; that is, the correspondence between the meanings that the coded responses have for the subjects and the meanings which the various categories have to the researcher. A great deal more will be said in reference to the validity of the coding procedure in the course of the discussion of meaning of the categories which occurs at the end of Chapter II under Critique, and especially in Chapter III. The frequency distribution of the respondents' responses in each of the asset and liability categories appears in Table l. The frequency distribution of each category as it relates to the scoring and analysis of the results will be discussed in this chapter under Scoring the ALI, and in Chapter II under Method of Analysis. Scoring the ALI Three alternative procedures for scoring the ALI were considered--the Medal, Proportional, and the Absolute. In the Modal technique each individual would be assigned to the category into which the majority of his responses were coded. In the Proportional technique each individual would be given a score for each response category according to the 14 .mnommumo muflaanmea some new ummmm some now mum u 2* 00H m. m. H.H m.mH m.mh oaummaosom HmsuomaamucH OOH m. o.¢ o.ma m.mm o.mm can ImuocmmompH oamaommmlcoz ooa m. m. o.m m.om maaflxm maom so now 00H m. o.m m.mm coeumooq ameoom ooa m. m. v.m o.v H.m ¢.ma m.mm m.bm mbfiumauoz ooH m.a m.HH m.mm m.mm maaaxm Hafiuom 00H m. m. H.H w.nm mumsuo DQMUHMflcmHm 13 OOH m. m. m.m e.m O.sm oHeocoom Ho amauwumz OOH m. O.e s.OH O.Os HmOHmsem s Hmuoe O s O m e m m H O mucmEmumum muHHHQwflH mo HmQEsz OOH 0.0 e.ee O.se UHummHoeom HmsuomHHmucH . . . . . UHHmuocsmomOH OOH m m H m s sH H on onHommmuaoz OOH m.H O.e O.mH m.ms mHHme Odom so now OOH m. m. m.H O.e 0.0H m.mm coHumooH HMflUOm OOH m. m. m. m. m.e O.m m.HH m.sH 0.0~ H.Om m>Hum2uoz OOH m. m.H H.H e.O 0.0H e.Om 0.0m mHHme HMfloom OOH m. O.H O.m O.s m.HH m.ms mumeuo DCMUAMHcmfim OOH m. m. O.H N.N m.s m.sm UHsocoom Ho HMHkumE OOH m. m. m. m. m.O H.Om m.mO Hmonsem s Hmuoe OH O m s O m e m m H O mmHHommumo mucmfiwumpm ummmm mo Hmnfisz ucmusoo Ucm ummmm comm CH meme mucmfimumum mo Menage >9 mamomm mo soflusnfluumwp mmmucmoumm e.>uommumo huHHHQMHH .H magma 15 ratio of the number of his responses in that category to his total response frequency. In the Absolute technique some arbitrary number of responses in a given category would be defined as distinguishing between individuals that have or have not responded to that category in a way that is meaningful for the analysis. Although the Mbdal technique is simple and has frequently been employed with the TST, responses on the ALI did not fall clearly into modal categories and so this approach was not used. A final choice between the Proportional or the Absolute technique would ultimately need to be based on theoretical and empirical considerations. Since this thesis is not intended as an exploration of various scoring techniques, and since neither the original theory as stated by Kuhn nor the reformulation of it in Chapter III provided any basis for deciding between one or another of these alternatives, the decision to use the Absolute technique was based upon the fact that of the two it raised fewer difficulties than the Proportional technique. The main difficulty in using the Absolute technique is the establishment of cutting points; the basis on which this was done in this thesis is to be found at the beginning of Chapter III. The Proportional scoring method was not used for several reasons. It is possible to have anywhere from 0 to 8 responses in any one of the nine categories for assets or for liabilities; because of the great number of 16 categories in relation to the total number of responses the maximum was around 4. Furthermore, for each category the median frequency was always either at 0 or 1 responses. Since the median number of Egggl responses was 5 for Assets and 3 for Liabilities, any score based upon a pro- portion would ultimately rest upon very small numbers in both the numerator and denominator. This would have the consequence of making the Proportional score highly sensitive to variations of one or two responses and hence, sensitive also to any unreliability or invalidity of the coding procedure. Uhder these conditions, using such proportions as a metric score would be highly misleading. One bit of evidence gives weight to the validity of using the Absolute scoring technique. In the course of the analysis of each of the hypotheses the sample was dichotomized according to those making fewer and more than the median number of total responses for assets and for liabilities. In every case there was no difference in the results for those who made more than the median and for those who made fewer than the median number of total statements. Thus, it would appear that the absolute pre- sence or absence of a response in a category was more signifi- cant than its frequency relative to the total number of statements. Assets and Liability statements were analyzed separately in this thesis because it was not known if they could be meaningfully combined. For example, it was not 17 known if a Social Skill asset statement was equivalent to or opposite from a Social Skill liability statement. Furthermore, it was not known how they would behave with respect to different independent variables. Additional Measures and Indices The purpose of the ALI is to link an actor to the social structure and to behavior by virtue of his self— assessments; therefore, several indices and measures of reference group involvement and of behavior were obtained. These are described and defined below prior to their use in the hypotheses. The extent to which the following measures are appropriate to the problem is considered in Chapters II and III. Important Others and Agreement with Important Others The following question was used to elicit the sub— ject's reference groups and his perception of the extent to which they agreed with his own self-assessment. It was originally intended that the "others" would, like the ALI responses, be classifiable into categories, e.g., primary, secondary, etc.; however, it was not possible to work out a satisfactory set of categories. Nevertheless, the number of "others" mentioned, as well as the amount of Agreement, do figure in the Mean Agreement Score. The scoring of the Agreement and Mean Agreement portion of the following question are given below. Figure 2. 18 with important others. Question for important others and agreement All of us have certain "other" (individuals and/or groups) who are particularly important to us. Please list some of your important others in the spaces at left below. For each "other" listed, indicate whether there would most likely be agreement or disagreement with the assets and liabilities you have listed above. Others Who are Important to You Would this "other" generally agree or disagree with you regarding most of the assets and liabilities you have listed appropriate Would Would (Would Would Would Would Would Would Agreement and Mean Agreement agree agree agree agree agree agree agree agree Score above? response. can't I can t I can t t can t I can t I can t I can t can't Circle the say say say say say say say say may may may may may may may may disagree disagree disagree disagree disagree disagree disagree disagree In the Important Other and Agreement with Important other question respondents were asked to indicate if each important other which they listed "would agree," "can't say,‘ liabilities statements which they made. or "may disagree," with most of the assets and Each respondent was given an agreement score by scoring "would agree" as 2, "can't say" as l, and "may disagree? as 0 and then l9 summing across all "important others." A Mean Agreement Score was then computed by dividing the Agreement Score by the total number of important others listed. This last score served as the measure of perceived agreement with significant others and was independent of variations in the number of significant others listed by the subjects. Index of Self—Esteem .This index was obtained by dividing the total number of asset statements by the total number of liability state- ments. The rationale for this is based upon the assumption that more asset than liability statements are an indication of a more favorable than unfavorable self-evaluation. In order to avoid certain metric difficulties involved in utilizing ratios as scores the index of self-esteem and the mean agreement score were divided at the median when they were used in the testing of hypotheses. Importance of Religion Respondents were asked "how important is your religion in the way in which you live your life?"; five alternatives were possible from "One of the most important influences in the way that I live my life," to "One of the most unimportant influences in the way that I live my life." Responses were dichotomized by combining "one of the most important . . ." and "a fairly important . . . into 20 an "Important" category, and "Neither important or unim— ll portant . . . "Fairly unimportant . . ." and "One of the most unimportant . . ." into a "Not important" category. Socio-economic Level Respondents were asked to indicate their fathers' occupations; five socio-economic levels were computed using the NOrth-Hatt index of occupational prestige and are based upon a breakdown employed previously by Davis g; $1.12 The Elite group contains such occupations as major pro- fessionals, presidents of medium large firms, management in large firms; for the Middle-Middle, secondary teachers, wholesale salesmen and middle managers are representative: Working Class Elite include plumbers, carpenters, owners of small retail stores, white collar supervisors, and farmers; Respectable Working Class is characterized by postmen, barbers, mechanics, busdrivers, clerks in retail stores, and machine operators; Low Status by garbage collectors, janitors, and truck drivers. Hypotheses Not all of the possible combinations of content categories of the ALI and independent measures of involve- ment with reference groups or behavior are specifically 12James A. Davis and David Gottlieb, Jan Hejda, Carolyn Huson, Joe L. Spaeth, Stipends and Spouses/The Finances of American Arts and Science Graduate Students (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1962). 21 accounted for in the following hypotheses. Eclectic as they are, these specific hypotheses were chosen because, when taken as a whole, they seemed to provide a reasonable test of whether or not the ALI constituted a valid means of linking, (a) the self, (b) reference groups, and (c) behavior. There was no intention that the hypotheses should contribute to any substantive sociological or sociaL- psychological knowledge. While all of the following hypotheses seem to be more or less "reasonable" in light of Kuhn's statement and the meanings which have been imputed to the response categories, reasonableness is a far cry from theoretical specification. The position represented by Kuhn's state- ment seems capable of asserting only that the symbols in terms of which individuals represent themselves will ig some unspecified way have their links or connection to ggggp and to behavior revealed ipso facto. This serious theoreti— cal hiatus between the self-symbol, social structural involvement, and behavior is bridged in the case of these hypotheses only by intuition, empathy, and some vague sense of appropriateness. Nor is there any help along these lines to be had from the theoretical reformulation in Chapter III. The nature of the difficulty is more clearly seen, however, and it is possible to offer some suggestions for theoretical development in this area. Hypothesis I. According to Clark the "collegiate subculture," characterized by an orientation that is 22 predominantly concerned with social activities, "has been a strong if not the dominant governor of student life ever since the 1890's. . . . Today, the fraternities and sororities set the patterns of the collegiate way of life."13 This view of one of the predominant value orientations of college students, in conjunction with the foregoing inter— pretation of the meaning of the Social Skill category, leads us to assume that over time, the effect of the "collegiate subculture" would befatjincrease the likeli- hood that subjects would come to value and to evaluate themselves according to standards that were consistent with the kinds of responses which were categorized into the Social Skills category. Hence Hypothesis Ia. Ia. The number of Social Skill asset and liability statements will increase with grade in school. At the same time that socialization is presumably occurring within the "collegiate subculture" the effects of socialization from the family and church are presumably weakened and decrease in importance. The Nbrmative category is presumed to represent values and criteria for behavior derivedirom a familial or religious context. Hence Hypothesis Ib. Ib. The number of Nermative asset and liability statements will decrease with grade in school. 13Burton R. Clark, Educating the Expert Society (San Francisco, Calif.: Chandler Publishing Company, 1962), p. 203. 23 Hypothesis II. The traditional role for the male in our society is to assume responsibility for securing a livelihood; therefore, we expect males to be more likely to see and evaluate themselves in terms of these roles. II. Males will make more Role and Job asset and liability statements than will females. Hypothesis III. Either because the definition and evaluation of one's self in terms of Scholastic or Intel- lectual attributes leads to higher grades or because higher grades lead to such self-assessments, saliency of Intellectual or Scholastic statements should be associated with objective evidence of grades.14 IIIa. Higher gradegpoint averages will be associated with those individuals who make Intellectual or Scholastic asset s 8 se 0 t ose who make none. IIIb. Lower grade point averages will be associated with those individuals who make Intellectual or Scholastic liability statements as opposed to those who make none. Hypothesis IV. If religion is perceived to be an important influence on the way in which one lives his life, then we would expect individuals to view and evaluate them— selves in terms of standards associated with that religion. Such evaluations presumably would yield responses on the l4Wilbur B. Brookover, Ann Patterson and Shailer Thomas, Self-Concept of Ability_and School Achievement, Final Report of Cooperative Research Project No. 845, Office of Research and Publications, Michigan State University, 1962. 24 ALI that would fall into the Normative category. Hence, IVa. Those who perceive their religion to be important in the way_in which they_live their lives will make more Normative asset and liability statements than will thgse er thm religign IS at little gr hQ importance. Hypothesis V. Membership in many organizations should be associated with a tendency to value interpersonal relations and the possession of certain associated skills. V. Those who belong to more organizations while in school will make more Social Skill asset and liahility statements than will those individuals who belopg to few or no organizations. Hypothesis VI. There is ample evidence15 that individuals at the lower economic levels of our society are exposed to fewer of the opportunities and possess fewer of the skills requisite for adequate performance in educational, social, and economic pursuits.. Such a condition is likely to produce experiences which cause these individuals to evaluate themselves relatively unfavorably. Furthermore, differences between socio- economic levels are likely to be more marked when they are within a context which places a premium on certain skills that tend to be distributed in favor of those at higher economic levels. 15 of Status to Experience, Perception and Value, Journal of Sociology, Vol. 66 (1960), pp. 1-31. Cf.. Alex Inkeles, "Industrial Man: The Relation “ American 25 VIa. Self—esteem as measured by_the ratio of asset to liability statements will increase with socio-economic level. Hypothesis VII. Assuming that individuals attempt to maximize relatively favorable evaluations of themselves, that these evaluations are largely based upon cues from significant others, and that such cues will more likely be forthcoming when an actor shares the values of these significant others, then, when the evaluations of signifi- cant others are perceived to correspond to one's own self- evaluations, there should be a maximizing of favorable responses from them, and correspondingly, there should be high self-esteem. VII. There will be agppsitive correlation be- tween self-esteem and mean agreement score. This list of hypotheses originally included several hypotheses which related categories of the ALI and categories of Important Others. Hewever, these had to be abandoned when the proposed classification of Important Others proved to be totally unworkable. Description of the Sample The questionnaire was administered to classes of beginning social psychology students at four institutions—- 112 were from Michigan State University, 112 from Northern Michigan University, 107 from Michigan Tech., and 40 were from Suomi College. Table 2 presents a summary description 26 of the sample in terms of sex, class in school and socio- economic level. The fact that this sample was not randomly drawn will not be of concern in this thesis since there will be no attempt to generalize any results to a population. Furthermore, the mere presence of connections between an actor, social structure, and behavior, which are expected to be revealed by the self, are assumed to obtain regard- less of what subsamples of any population might be observed, although the kinds of connections will vary from sample to sample. 27 mum KOCH KOCH KOCH KOCH KOCH KCOH Hmuoa CNH mm Ow O.Nm m.Cm m.mm mm mHmEmm mmm ms gm v.5O m.mm m.hm me mHmz 3. .NA N anv wanna H Cuzv 3.53 m vuzv doing Hm anv QQKA b muzv OBIKACCHZC Hmuoe mHQmCHmuHmomC =wuHHm: zmHOUHS zmuHHm :mmMHO =m5umum xmm uoz prpHZ: mmMHU OCHMHOK 30H: OCHHHOS: mHQmuommmmM= Hm>oq UHEOCoomloHoom th KOCH KOCH KOCH KOCH KOCH Hmuoa ONH C m.ON m.mm O.bm m.O¢ mHmEmm Nmm COH m.m> m.CO m.mO m.mm mHmz HsHuzO HOOuzO lemnzv HOOHuzC Hemuzv Hmuoe mmumspmuw mHOHCmm mHOHCCC mmHOEOCmom Cmfismmum xwm Hoonom CH mpmno .Hm>wH UHEOCoom .m mHQma IOHoom pCm HOOCUm CH OUMHC :xwm mo mEHmu CH wHQEmm m0 CoHumHnommp mmmquUHmm CHAPTER II FINDINGS In this chapter the results from the hypotheses will be presented and discussed; in addition, a brief critique of the ALI will be made in terms of these results and hi terms of certain of the methodological and interpretive difficulties associated with the instrument itself. A theoretical critique of the instrument will be postponed until Chapter III. Method of Analysis Having decided to use the Absolute method of scoring for the ALI, it is necessary to determine those cutting points of the distribution that will yield groups which differ in the property represented by the response category in question. With self-esteem, mean agreement score, and gradepoint average it was possible to use the median as a point for dichotomizing the distributions--a procedure that has the double advantage of being consistent from application to application and of being the most power- ful point at which to dichotomize these distributions for the Chi Square test. Unfortunately, the task was more compli— cated for the frequency distributions of ALI categories. As can be seen from Table l in Chapter I, these distributions 28 29 are markedly skewed in the direction of many responses for each of the categories and the modal frequency was always either 0 or 1. As a result of the restricted variance and skewed- ness of these distributions and because the total number of persons at the median comprised a rather large proportion of the total number of persons in the sample, the use of the median as a cutting point posed some special problems in the analysis. Three possible ways of arriving at cutting points for the frequency distributions of the various ALI categories were considered. All respondents who fell in the group in which the median also fell could be: 1. Randomly assigned to the groups on either side of the median. This procedure was rejected because reassigning the large number of cases in the median group would tend to wash out any difference that might exist between the groups above and below the median. 2. Dropped from the analysis. In which case, a) when the median fell at 0 it would be clearly impossible to analyze the results, there no longer being any group below the median. b) When the median fell at l the analysis could proceed with a 0 or 2 or more response split. 3. Retained as a separate group. In which case, 30 a) when the median fell at O the analysis could proceed with a 0, or 1 or more, response split. b) when the median fell at l the analysis could proceed with a three-way split of 0, l and 2 or more responses. Of methods "2" and "3" the preference for the analysis was for method "2"-—dropping the median group from the analysis--for two reasons. First, the groups which remained would be "extreme" and more likely to reveal differences in relation to control variables. Second, it is believed that method "2" tends to reduce any effects of unreliable coding. Thus, it is more likely that gas of the responses of an individual might be miscoded than that twp might be and it is therefore possible that the "1 response" category has a disproportionate share of individuals who are there by virtue of coding error. Nevertheless, method "3" was used under the two following conditions: (1) if discarding the "1 response" category drastically reduced the number of individuals falling in the "2 or more" response group thus resulting in the distribution being too skewed for a reliable application of the Chi Square test or (2), if the result of retaining the "1 response" category was clearly the same as the result obtained by omitting it. Under the second condition it was retained in the analysis to emphasize the fact that the relationship was strong enough to be revealed by one response. 31 This last contingency was applied to only one case in the analysis, Hypothesis IVa. It should be noted that in every case the result of omitting the "1 response" category was such that it strengthened an already existing relationship or had no effect when a relationship was not present. In no case where this category was omitted would its inclusion have changed the direction of any obtained relationships; therefore, the procedure provided a "fair" test of the hypotheses. Results Hypotheses Ia and Ib proposed that: The number of Social Skill (SS) asset and liability statements will increase with grade in school. The number of Normative asset and liability state— ments will decrease with grade in school. It will be recalled that a complimentary increase and decrease of these two categories of evaluation was expected as a result of the assumption that they were reciprocally related modes of evaluation. That is, students would come to reflect, through their SS statements, what was assumed to be a dominant orientation of student reference groups--an emphasis upon social activities, personal attractiveness, and acceptance by others. It was also assumed that normative moralistic criteria of behavior supposed- ly derived from familial and religious reference groups, and supposedly also related to the Normative response category, 32 would become less relevant or appropriate in the new context. The results, presented in Table 3 and 4, do not support such a simple interpretation. Only males were included in this analysis since it was found that females made significantly more SS statements than did males, thus introducing the possibility that the scoring by the Absolute technique would be affected by different numbers of females at the various class levels. Although females did not differ from males in this category, for the sake of uniformity in the discussion of results females were also removed from the analysis of Hypothesis Ib, since the two hypotheses are related. When males and females were analyzed separately for Hypothesis Ia there were some slight departures from the overall pattern of relationships1 which had originally obtained with the combined group. While these were not great and the overall pattern remained the same, males and females differed sufficiently to introduce an unwanted interpretive complexity at this stage of the analysis. The tables reveal that, in general, males increase the number of Normative ahg SS evaluations which they make as grade level increases. For, whereas there are fewer than the expected number of freshmen making "many" asset or liability statements for both categories of response, seniors are overrepresented in all of these categories. It lThe departures merely consisted of differences in the relative magnitudes but not direction of the discrepancy be— tween actual and expected frequencies at the sophomore and junior grade levels for the combined group. =.mHmmHmCd mo COCHOEO CH Mm wHCH CuH3 OUCMOHOUUM CH pmumHCm mm3 uCHom OCHHHCOHH .mOHUCwCOmHm pmuommxm UCmmmHmmH mmmmCquHmm CH mmHCmHme ONO. A C A OO. O .O.O Oe.O u NH OOO. A C A HO. O .H.O OO.NH u NH OO be OOH Os OO Hapoe em HON.OHO OH HON.OHO ON OO AN0.0HO O HOO.OHO KN OCOHOOO we HOA.ONO HN HHN.NNO HN OO AOO.NNO ON HHH.HNO 5N muoHOOO Oe HOO.ONO HN HOO.HNO ON Oe HO0.0NO NN HOO.ONO ON mmuosoemom OO ANO.ONO ON HOO.NHO HH OO HOO.OHO ON ANO.OHO OH Omeemmum Hmuoe mCOZ OHOE no N Hmuoa mCoz OHOE no N Hoosum CH mmMHo mquEmumum huHHHQwHH mquEmume ummm¢ % .mquEmeum muHHHQmHH pCm ummmm m>HHmEHOC mCwaE memE m0 HmHECC pCm HOOCUm CH mmMHU .g OHQMB OO. A C A OH. O .0.0 Oe.s u NH OOO. A C A HO. O .0.0 HO.NH u NH eON OOH OO OOH NO OO Hmpoe OO HOH.ONO ON HOO.ONO ON OO ANH.ONO OH HOO.OHV NN muoHcmm Ne Ass.NeO we HON.ONO ON OO HON.eNO ON ANN.OHO AH muoHasO OO AOO.OOO em HHO.ONO em Ow HOH.ONO 5N ANO.ONO NN mmnosoemom es HOH.ONO HO HOO.OHO OH OO AOO.OHO ON «ANO.OHO N cmsemmum Hoosum Hmuoe mCoz OHOE Ho H HMDOB mCoz OHOE no N CH mmMHo *emquEmHmum muHHHQMHH mquEmumum ummmm .mqufimumum muHHHQMHH pCm Hemmm HHmeIHMHUOm mCmeE memE mo HOQECC UCm HOOCom CH.mmMHU .m mHQMB 34 is concluded that males as freshmen do not tend to evaluate themselves according to either SS or Normative values but that they do so evaluate themselves by the time they are seniors. The fact that there is a significant increase in the number of individuals evaluating themselves by Normative standards rather than the anticipated decrease requires a modification of the interpretation of the meaning of this category. One possible interpretation of this result is that standards of behavior and conduct are acquired con- currently with school attendance. Hence the category may represent abstract, general criteria for behavior that have been derived from the greater exposure to ideas and diverse kinds of people encountered in school, and not from the accumulated teachings of home and church as had been previously supposed. Evidence from Hypothesis IVa reported at a later point, which shows that the Normative category is not associated with stated importance of religion, further supports this interpretation. There is a parallel increase in self-assessments for hgth assets Egg liabilities for the Normative and SS categories and this would support the View that these evaluations are being made in terms of certain standard values, since the incorporation of values should reveal itself both in favorable Egg in unfavorable evaluations. There remains the problem of accounting for the correspondence between the theoretical and actual frequencies 35 of evaluation in these two categories for assets and liabili- ties at the sophomore and junior grade levels. While there is insufficient information to determine what sort of pro— cesses may be involved, three possibilities can be con- sidered. First, it is possible that the Chi Square test is insufficiently sensitive to changes; computing the mean proportion of statements for each class might have been more sensitive, although as indicated previously, such an approach would have raised some formidable difficulties. Second, the apparent leveling off of the rate of change in evaluations might be the result of some complex interaction between differential rates of change in self-evaluations for asset and liability statements. Thus, if self-evaluations according to some set of values would result primarily in liability assessments, then it is possible that there would be less of a tendency to evaluate oneself in terms of such values until such time as asset statements would be forthcoming as well. Finally, it is possible that a change from no or few evaluative statements in one category to many statements in that category involves a period of transition during which time the values are learned only imperfectly, and, hence, during which evaluations in terms of these values might have been improperly coded. The foregoing interpretations suffer from the fact that they assume chronological processes when the data are not longitudinal. Consequently the possibility that these findings are artifactual cannot be entirely ruled out. In 36 addition, this interpretation would not be complete until all of the other categories of self-evaluation have been analyzed in a similar manner——a task which was too extensive to be undertaken in this thesis. One source of artifactual contamination can be ruled out. On the possibility that variations in the frequency of Normative and of SS statements could be explained on the basis of the total response frequency, a "best" median split of the total number of asset and the total number of liability statements (including all categories of classifi- cation) was run against grade level in school. No signifi— cant differences were found either for total number of asset statements or for total number of liability statements. The possibility that the total number of responses might be associated with any of the asset or liability categories examined was similarly tested for all subsequent hypotheses. This possibility was rejected in all cases. In general, then, the results are interpreted as indicating an association between one‘s mode of evaluation on the ALI and a certain connection with the social structure. For males, increasing involvement in college, as represented by increasing grade level, was associated with greater tendency to evaluate oneself according to Social Skill and Normative categories. The Normative category is reinterpreted as possibly indicating the presence of abstract norms of behavior which are not necessarily 37 confined to early socialization, but which may result from contact with other reference groups (in the college perhaps) and which may increase with wider experience. Hypothesis II proposed that: Males will make more role and job asset and liability statements than will females. The data in Table 5 below do . not permit the acceptance of the hypothesis. Table 5. Number of role and job skill asset and liability statements by sex. Role and Job Skill Statements Assets Liabilities Sex 1 or more None Total 1 or more None Total Males 55 197 252 6 246 252 Females 24 96 120 7 113 120 Total 79 293 372 13 359 372 X2 n.s. X2 n.s. The most likely interpretation of these results is that the meaning of the Job or Role Skill category upon which the hypothesis was derived is incorrect. A re- examination of the responses which were coded into this category do, indeed, confirm this;2 the kind of behavioral and social structural context to which these responses refer 2Some examples include, "good at sports," "manual skills," "good musician,” etc. 38 are at once so specific and diverse that they cannot be regarded as relating specifically to sex roles as had been thought. In short, in view of the kinds of responses which were actually included into this category the original hypothesis appears to be manifestly bad. This raises, in addition, the question of just what it is that all of these responses have in common which would justify their inclusion into the same category. Knowing that they all refer to some specific skill in a specific kind of setting hardly seems a sufficient basis for inferring an additional general property which they exhibit in common. If there is in fact such a general property it is certainly not given either from the theory or from the nature of the responses. It may well be that for subjects who are more directly involved in the actual business of earning a living, or who are closer to it than is a sample comprised predominantly of lower division college students, this particular category might become more dominant and reveal the expected pattern. In any case, there is no relationship between self-assessments of job or role skill and differentiation according to sex roles. Hypotheses 111a and IIIb proposed respectively that: Higher grade point averages will be associated with those individuals who make Intellectual or Scholastic asset statements as opposed to those who make none. Lower grade point averages will be associated with those individuals who make Intellectual or Scholastic liability statements as opposed to those who make none. The data, presented in tables 6 and 7 permit us 39 to accept both of these hypotheses. Table 6. Grade point average by number of Intellectual, Scholastic asset statements. Number of intellectual Grade Point Average* scholastic asset Above Below statements median median Total 2 or more 18 10 28 (14.48) (13.52) One 91 68 159 (82.23) (76.77) None 72 91 163 (89.29) (78.71) Total 181 169 350 X2 = 7.416, df = 2,p<.025 Table 7. Grade point average by number of Intellectual Scholastic liability statements. Grade Point Average Number of intellectual scholastic liability Above Below statements median median Total One or more 25 47 72 (37.23) (34.77) None 156 122 278 (143.77) (134.23) Total 181 169 350 X2 = 10.47, df = 1, p < .005 *Because grade point average was originally compiled using grouped data it was impossible to compute the average grade point for the different number of statements made. As a result, a less powerful best median break was utilized. The median grade point average was 2.50. 40 In contrast to the preceding result in Hypothesis II the Intellectual or Scholastic statements do seem to share a common property which is relevant to a context from which such statements might have been drawn or to a certain kind of behavior. Thus, they all refer to mental ability and all respondents are involved in a context in which evidence to this effect is forthcoming. Although the hypotheses are born out, it is a moot point as to whether performance (and, more generally, whether behavior) is the result of self-conception or whether the self-conception is the result of performance. Symbolic interaction theory would accept both as true together, and in the final chapter a rationale for this view is elaborated. Hypothesis IV proposed that, Those who perceive their religion to be important in the way in which they live their lives will make more Normative asset and liability statements than will those for whom religion is of little or no importance. The data are presented in Tables 8 and 9 and do not permit the hypothesis to be accepted. Catholics and Protestants were analyzed separately on the chance that parochial instruction might have final to more marked effects for Catholics, although there were no measures of such separate instruction. Neither for Catholics nor for Protestants is there a significant departure from expected frequencies on either assets or liabilities. This finding, in addition to that presented in the previous discussion of Hypothesis Ib, 41 Table 8. Number of Protestants and Catholics making 0 and 2 or more NOrmative asset statements by importance of religion. Number of Normative Asset Statements Importance of Catholics Protestants Religion 0 2 or more Total 0 2 or more Total Important 29 38 67 37 65 102 Not Important 5 5 10 21 29 50 Total 34 43 77 58 94 X2 n.s. X2 n.s. Table 9. Number of Protestants and Catholics making 0 and 2 or more Normative liability statements by importance of religion. Number of Normative Liability Statements Catholics Protestants Importance of Religion 0 2 or more Total 0 2 or more Total Important 33 36 69 61 47 108 Net Important 7 7 14 25 33 58 Total 40 43 83 86 80 166 X2 .n.s. X2 n.s. requires a modification in our understanding of the Normative category of evaluation. Responses which fell into this category were believed to be the result of self-evaluations made in terms of abstract, moralistic values. It was felt that such values would most likely to imparted within a context providing for religious instruction, and that those who conceived of themselves as having their lives 42 governed by their religion would reveal the internalization of such precepts as Normative, Imperative, or Directive statements on a self—assessment. It now appears that such evaluative statements are independent of religious commitment (as we have measured it) and are more readily interpretable as the product of such experiences as college, which may foster the formation of some sort of personal credo or ideology of behavior according to which they act and evaluate themselves. HOwever, the data do not permit us to test the extent to which such values actually do influence behavior. Hypothesis V proposed that, Those who belong to more organizations in school will make more Social Skill asset and liability statements than those individuals who belong to few or no organizations. Respondents at all schools except Michigan State,3 were asked "How many organizations, clubs, or groups have you belonged to while attending college?" Since answers varied with the year in school, respondents were divided into "many" and "few" membership groups separately for each class in school so that these groups came closest to the best median break for that class. All classes were then combined for the analysis. From Table 10 it is evident that the hypothesis is only partially supported. Only Social Skill asset evaluative statements are associated with membership in more organizations. 3This question was added to the questionnaire after its initial administrat1on to Michigan State subjects. 43 Table 10. Relationship between number of Social Skill asset and liability statements and organizational membership.* Skill in Social Relationship Statements Member- ship in Assets Liabilities Organi- zations 2 or more None Total 2 or more None Total Many 47 38 85 19 66 85 (39.19) (45.81) (21.25) (63.75) Few 30 52 82 23 60 82 (37.81) (44.19) (21.25) (62.25) Total 77 90 167 42 126 167 X2 = 5.88 d.f. 1 p (.02 X2 n.s. *Graduate students were excluded and data for MSU were unavailable. The present evidence might suggest that individuals may only evaluate themselves in a manner appropriate to their involvement within the social structure when such an evaluation results in a relatively favorable self- conception. Yet, such an interpretation would be contrary to other findings already reported, e.g., Hypotheses I and III, that if individuals are evaluating themselves according to certain values they will evaluate themselves by these values 29th favorably apg unfavorably. An interpretation more consistent with the preceding remarks might be that individuals may be inhibited from becoming involved in the kinds of situations where experiences with reference groups may confirm or reinforce an unfavorable evaluation of themselves. A sufficient number of such cases would tend to wash out any trend for favorable and unfavorable evaluations 44 to be made simultaneously in a given category. Hypothesis V then gives only partial support to the view that the ALI links the self to reference groups and to possible behavior by virtue of SS evaluative statements. A more meaningful test of the category would have been to ask respondents to list the groups to which they belonged, the amount and nature of their participation in each, and the importance to them of the views of these groups. The establishment of such clear cut reference groups and degree of behavioral involvement would at least have clarified the interpretation of the results of the above findings. What ats_the effects of a disproportionate amount of liability statements over asset statements with respect to the seeking out or the avoidance of interaction, etc.? Hypothesis VI proposed that, Self-esteem as measured by the ratio of asset to liability statements will increase with socio— economic level. The assumption lying behind this hypothesis was that the higher the socioeconomic level the greater would be the opportunities to acquire the skills and to be exposed to experiences that would lead to successes and hence to favorable evaluations in a school situation. Table 11 indicates that the hypothesis cannot be accepted. Since evidence will subsequently be presented in connection with hypothesis VII that supports the validity of the index of self-esteem we can tentatively rule that 45 out as a factor which might have yielded these results. Table 11. Number of individuals above and below the median self-esteem score by socio—economic level. Self—esteem Above Below median median Total Elite 27 34 61 Middle-Middle 19 22 41 Working Class Elite 87 63 150 Respectable Working Class 17 20 37 Low Status 35 32 67 Total 185 171 356 X2 n.s. One explanation for these findings is that individuals with low self-esteem and low socioeconomic levels do not go to college, but if this were generally the case then we would expect the low status group to be relatively higher in self—esteem than the other groups-—which it is not. It may be that such socioeconomic level has been about equally able to find a context in which suitable levels of self-esteem could be maintained; research on school sub— cultures suggests that this could very well be the case.4 4David Gottlieb and Benjamin Hodgkins, "College Student Subcultures: Their Structure and Characteristics in Relation to Student Attitude Change," The School Review, Vol. 71: PP. 266-289. 46 Hypothesis VII proposed that, There will be a positive correlation between self- esteem and perceived agreement with significant others on self-evaluations. Results are presented in Table 12 and permit acceptance of the hypothesis. Table 12. Self-esteem and perceived agreement with signifi- cant others on self-evaluation. Mean Agreement with Significant Self-esteem Others High Low Total High 94 73 167 Low 69 94 163 Total 163 167 330 X2 = 6.42, df. 1, p < .01 Acceptance of the reasoning underlying the hypothesis is not such a simple matter. On the one hand, there are indications that individuals evaluate themselves in accordance with the actual and perceived evaluations of others5 while, on the other hand, there is evidence that there is distortion or selective perception involved in estimates of the evaluations of one's self by others particularly for those with high self-esteem.6 Thus, the data not only support 5See for example, S. Frank NUyamoto and Sanford Dornbusch, "A Test of Interactionist Hypotheses of Self- Conception," American Journal of Sociology, Vol. 61 (1956), 6Ezra Statland, Stanley Thorely, Edwin Thomas, Arthur R. Cohen, and Alvin Zander, "The Effects of Group Expectations and Self—Esteem upon Self-Evaluations," J. of 47 the assumptions of the hypothesis, but also support a hypothesis which has some very different implications for the interpretation of human behavior. For example, the implications of an assumption that people perceive them— selves favorably because they perceive nearly everything about themselves favorably, including the reactions of others, are quite different from those leading from the assumption that people, as a result of behaving in accordance with the expectations of others, receive cues confirming self—evaluations. Although there is no way to resolve this with the present data, it is possible to suggest a rapprochement. There seems to be no reason why the two processes could not be operating simultaneously. Thus, we can con- ceive of individuals as sharing the values of others with whom they are in contact so that their behavior is favorably confirmed by these others. Such individuals would be likely to assume that most other individuals in groups with whom they were in contact would also evaluate them as they evaluated themselves--leading to distortions of the imputed evaluation of others toward oneself. Those who had a rather low self-esteem would, on the other hand, be con— cerned with, and hence more vulnerable and sensitive to, the Abnormal and Social Psychology, Vol. 54 (1957), pp. 55-63; and Arthur R. Cohen, "Some Implications of Self-Esteem for Social Influence," in Hovland and Janis (eds.), Personality and Persuasibility (New Haven: Yale University Press, 1959), pp. 102-109. 48 objective clues from others with whom they happened to be in contact. Discussion and Critique On the basis of the results and the discussion so far, six major criticisms may be leveled against the ALI. We will first consider three which are directly related to an interpretation of the foregoing results, and then three more which are more directly relevant to some of the technical characteristics of the ALI as a research device. It will be noted that some of these criticisms have been explicitly anticipated in preceding remarks while others have not. Of the nine hypotheses and sub-hypotheses discussed above Hypotheses IIIa and b, and VII were completely con- firmed, Hypotheses Ia, Ib, and V were partially confirmed if the reinterpretation and speculation with respect to them are accepted, and Hypotheses II, IV, and VI were disconfirmed. In Chapter I it was pointed out that the hypotheses which had been selected were varied enough so that the results, when taken as a whole, should indicate whether or not the ALI did link self-conceptions to involvements in the social structure and to behavior. However, in view of the mixed support that the hypotheses have received it is difficult to give an unequivocal overall assessment of the effective— ness and validity of the instrument. If one accepts as valid those reinterpretations which were given to the meanings of the content categories and the other speculative 49 explanations offered in the course of the analysis, then, in a very general way, it does appear that the instrument reflects certain kinds of involvement in the social structure and is related to certain kinds of behavior. Just what kinds of involvement and just how this is reflected is difficult if not impossible to specify for four interrelated reasons. 1. The extent and nature of the involvement within the social structure is measured quite crudely by control variables such as, socio—economic level, number of organi- zational memberships, grade level in school, etc. This is true especially in view of the fact that this is a crucially important point in the link which we are endeavoring to establish between actors, social—structure, and behavior via the symbols of self-representation. From the standpoint of Kuhn‘s statement, such involvement is the context trgm whigh_derive the symbols used in the self-assessment, and to which the symbols are to link the actor, and in which behavior occurs. 2. The basic assumption behind the ALI was that by eliciting self-representations that were evaluations, various connections between actors, social—structural involve- ment and behavior would be clearly revealed. That they are not clearly revealed is apparent from the two preceding chapters; the basic reason for this is that evaluations by their very nature obscure rather than clarify these connections. The theoretical formulation developed in the following chapter will supply the reasoning behind this apparent contradiction. 50 3. Related to "2" is the fact that meanings have been imputed to responses on the ALI with virtually no knowledge of the accuracy with which this has been done. Presumably, of course, the validity of the coding procedure was to be determined from the results of the hypotheses, the hypotheses being derived in part from the imputed meanings of responses in the various categories. The circularity which follows from trying to establish the validity of the categories at the same time that the instrument is being validated is readily apparent. 4. The weaknesses discussed in "2" and "3" are made more serious and make more serious the following criticism. As noted in Chapter I no precise theoretical rationale is available for linking particular categories of self- assessment to either involvement in the social structure, to reference groups, or to behavior. That is, while the theory is explicit in asserting a connection between self— and social structural involvement, and behavior at an abstract level, it is notably vague with respect to particular instances of these connections. This weakness is largely responsible for the difficulty that was had in showing that the hypotheses logically followed from the meaning attributed to a category. When these four factors are viewed in combination, there remains little of an established standpoint from which interpretive judgments can be made. We are placed in the position of saying with some conviction that 51 "something is going on," but are at a loss to define and evaluate precisely what that may be. This is best seen by contrasting the results forthcoming when, (a) these four criticisms are minimized, i.e., when the content category from the ALI and the control variable were relatively con- crete and clearly defined, as in the case of Hypothesis II which related grade point averages to mental or scholastic evaluative statements: and (b) when these same criticisms are most applicable--that is, when the meanings of the content category and the control variable are vague, as in Hypothesis V which related Social Skill statements to organi- zational membership. But what is the likelihood that these criticisms could be eliminated by any or all of the following measures: 1. A more precise specification and measurement of involvement in the social structure (or of reference groups), or of behavior? 2. Supplementary questions which would clarify the basis upon which the self-evaluations were made? 3. Supplementary questions which would clarify the meanings of the responses for the subjects? 4. An elaboration of the theory which would permit more precise links between ALI responses, reference groups, and behavior? Without elaborating at this point, let us only suggest that for reasons to be presented in Chapter III only the first of these remedial measures could be successful. 52 Three additional criticisms of the ALI are concerned mainly with the "mechanical" difficulties associated with it as a research technique. 5. There are many possible ways of constructing categories for the ALI responses as is suggested by "2," "3," and "4" above, and unless alternative methods are developed and tested there is no way of knowing which would yield the best results. There is a possibility that results vary with the obviousness with which a given response can be associated with some category on the ALI and the obviousness with which such a category can be associated with behavior or social structural involvements. Thus, Intellectual or Scholastic statements are rather clear cut as to what they mean and their extension to a context of scholastic achievement, e.g., grades, obvious. This seems to occur at just that point where the category is also most narrowly defined and of least general applicability. Disturbingly, this may indicate that the ALI works best where the links between actor, social-structure, and behavior are most obvious and trivial; that is, at just that point where it is needed least. For surely the results obtained in Hypothesis III could have been gotten far more easily by a straight-forward question to the effect of "wa much scholastic ability do you have?" There is more than just a strong hint of vacuousness in finding obvious connections between obvious measures. 53 6. The present system of classification yields 18 separate categories for assets and liabilities and unless this number could be drastically cut the system is an extremely cumbersome and unwieldly one with which to work. It is not yet clear whether one category is properly inter- preted as remaining the same for assets and for liability statements. No attempt has yet been made to determine whether treating these separately or in combination would be the best research startegy. Results from Hypothesis V suggest that until this kind of an investigation would be undertaken any combining of asset and liability categories would be premature. 7. As previously noted in Chapter I it is somewhat problematical whether the scoring procedure should be of the Proportional or Absolute method. No attempt has been made to test the effectiveness of one approach over another, let alone explore the consequences of the previously noted troublesome characteristics of each. In view of the fact that from the standpoint of the position to be developed in Chapter III criticisms "2" through "4" will be shown to be attributable to a judgmental flaw in the rationale behind the development of the ALI, the very great effort that would be needed to resolve "5," "6," and "7" would be best spent developing techniques more consistent with the theoretical positions presented in the following chapter. CHAPTER III Introduction It was noted in Chapter I that attention has been directed toward studying and measuring the self in the belief that it provides a uniquely important link between an actor's social-structural involvement and his behavior. It was proposed that a measure of the self which included an explicit self—evaluation would yield results that would be particularly useful in this respect. Hewever, a number of significant problems (listed at the end of Chapter II) were found to be associated with the ALI and particularly with interpretations of the results from it. These dif- ficulties precipitated a re-examination of the rationale behind the assumption that the self, as measured by techniques like the TST and the ALI, was as important as originally believed. The theoretical position arising from this re-examination is developed in the first part of the present chapter. It attempts to reassert the social and behavioral importance of the symbolically represented self by focusing on what is here called the relationality or the relational prpperty of symbols. This position is then used as a basis for reaffirming the theoretical and empirical importance of the symbolic self, and for an analysis and 54 55 explanation of the problems encountered in using the ALI. Some suggestions for further development of the theoretical position, and for the construction of instruments that are consistent with this line of thinking are also given. The Theoretical Restatement In Mead’s terminology symbols have the capacity to arouse attitudes—-tendencies to behave or imagery of contact-- toward the object symbolized:l"an organization of attitudes with reference to what we term objects is what constitutes for us the meaning of things."2 Things are meaningful, then, in terms of (a) an actor's behavioral tendencies, or (b) imagery of behavior with respect to some aspect of the environment. Relationality simply refers to the existence of such behavioral relationships between a class of actors and a class of objects.3 These behavioral relationships are thought of as being a complex seggence of actor-object relationships. Consequently, the full meaning of a symbol includes all of the potential behavioral relationships between an actor and objects which might occur in a sequence 1George H. Mead, The Philosophy of the Act (Chicago, Ill.: The University of Chicago Press, 1938), p. 222. 2George H. Mead, MindL Self and Society (Chicago, Ill.: The University of Chicago Press, 1934): P. 125. 3Throughout this paper it will be convenient to understand "object" as referring to any person, behavior, thing, or situation, or to any combination of these which have been isolated as units and given names. 56 or in different situations, either as actual behavior or as this behavior can be known through reflective thought. The relationships inherent in symbols serve as "recipes" or "formulae" accordingyto which behavior is governed, guided, and shaped. We are not here asserting a doctrine of of symbolic determinism, for "unpacking" the entflgE\meaning inherent in a symbol would reveal that behavior could materialize in a variety of alternative ways contingent upon who an actor might be and on what might transpire during the course of behavior, and yet the behavior would remain consistent with the relationships inherent in the meaning of that symbol.4 A formidable difficulty arises in explicating the concept of relationality: the thought patterns and the language in which we are necessarily forced to communicate implies a unique existence to physical objects. "Object" and "actor" are somehow felt to be isolated things, indepen- dent of and more substantial than any relationship which they might assume. waever, it is against precisely this 4It might be argued that a symbol which included many contingencies, which permitted numerous behaviors in a variety of circumstances, all of which were consistent with the meaning of the symbol, is prima faci evidence that the symbol is so vague as to be without meaning. That this is not necessarily or even generally the case can best be illustrated by an analogy to chess. In chess the meaning of the various pieces can be conveyed precisely and exhaustively in terms of rules which specify the relationships which can obtain between the chessmen and the squares of the board. The game itself, however, like all human behavior, is infinitely varied and complex. 57 notion that we are arguing, for "object" and "actor" are mutually implied by one another in terms of behavioral relationships. In conventional speech it is apparently a grammatical or conceptual convenience to focus upon one portion of actual relationships and give names to actors, objects and relationships as if there were no connection between them.5 The fact that symbols are used in this manner should not be allowed to obscure the fact that in order to understand the meaning of any symbol, regardless of whether it mamas an actor, an object, or a relationship, it is necessary to refer symbolically to each of the others. For example, it is clear that the meaning of symbols which name relationships, e.g., "trip," "coctail party," "conference," "fight," or "work," can only be conveyed to others by reference to other symbols, thereby indicating what some class or sub— class of actors do in relationship to some class or sub- class of objects. We can only partially cope with these 5This may only be a handicap of Indo-European language. For example, contrast the usual mode of speaking of behavior in English with the Navaho. "The Navaho speaks of 'actors' and 'goals' (the terms are inappropriate to Navaho), not as performers of actions or as ones upon whom actions are performed, as in English, but as entities linked to actions already defined in part as pertaining especially to classes of beings. . . [In speaking the Navaho links] individuals to actions and movements distinguished, not only as actions and movements, but as well in terms of the entities in action or movement. This division of nature into classes of entity in action or movement is the universe that is given; the behavior of human beings or of any being indivi— duated from the mass is customarily reported by assignment to one or other of these given divisions." Harry Hoijer, "The Sapir—Whorf Hypothesis," in Readings in Anthropology, Vol. I, (New YOrk: Thomas Y. Crowell, 1959), pp. 219-231. 58 semantic limitations by stressing that when "object," "actor," or "relationship" are used in this essay they should be understood to be an incomplete part of the tri- polar relationship--they are only grammatically, and hgt analytically, separate. The shared meanings of a group's symbols both reflect and lead to patterned behavior on the part of the members of that group. When the behavioral relationships inherent within the meaning of a symbol are thus shared and when they constitute a common organizing principal of behavior toward the environment for a number of individuals, then that symbol can be said to be a hgrm. Frequently, relatively stable sets of behavioral relationships between classes of actors and classes of objects acquire names; when this occurs they can be regarded as rglgs, e.g., father, student, president, or as situations, e.g., dinner, examination, conference. Inasmuch as the extensions6 of a symbol are to classes or relationships, actors, or objects, it is possible to conceive of symbols, including norms and roles and situations, as differing in the specificity or generality of the classes of which they are true. For example, the extension of the symbol "house" is more inclusive with 6The concept of extensions is borrowed from logic where it is used to indicate the universe of which terms or predicates are true. 59 respect to kinds of actors and relationships than is the symbol "dog house." And, in general, symbols differ in the degree to which the behavior which they imply is specified or uniquely determined for a given universe of actors. In this thesis the theoretical importance of the concept of relationality lies in its application to particular kinds of symbols designated as self-concepts. If an actor can be said to have meaning for himself in the same way that any other symbol has meaning for him then all that has been said with respect to the relationality of a symbol applies as well when it is oneself that is symbolized. Once again the vagaries of our language force the impression that the self must be a concrete object; however, response protocols from instruments like the TST suggest that the self is more appropriately regarded as a cluster of symbols each of which defines complex relationships between a class of actors and a class of objects in particular societal contexts. From the foregoing discussion it should now be clear that very different empirical implications follow from theoretical commitments to self-concept conceived in terms of meanings and relationships than follow from a self—concept which is seen as an entity with the attendant connotations of isolation and uniqueness. In the evaluation of the ALI which appears at a later point in this chapter these implicatiOns become directly relevant. Some examples of responses on the Twenty Statements Test make the relational property of the self more apparent. 60 As noted above, symbols will vary with respect to the generality of the classes of actors, objects, and relationships which are their extension. This factor may be observed in the self-definitions of the TST protocols. For example, responses such as "one with red hair," "21 years old," seem to be so general with respect to the classes of actors or objects of which they might be true that the behavioral relationships which would be implied by these symbols would be largely indeterminate, that is, they do not have specific or fixed meanings. On the other hand, "friend," "wife," "doctor,' and "night club performer,‘ seem to be increasingly specific with respect to classes of actors and classes of objects and consequently, increasingly specific with respect to the behavioral relationships which could exist between them. Such responses suggest shared meanings and relation— ships; that is, they imply norms and roles. Thus, in these responses there is an implied social context in which the meanings of these symbols are shared, in which they emerge, and in which they govern behavior. Such self-designations, besides specifying, i.e., naming, a class of actors alsg imply socially defined ways of behaving toward specific categories of individuals, objects, or groups, in certain specific kinds of situations. In short, an individual's potential behavior is glyph in the meanings of the symbols by which he chooses to designate himself.7 7This position on the self appears to be quite close to that advanced by Sherif and Sherif. These authors conceive of the self as a subsystem of interrelated attitudes 61 With the foregoing in mind, the self can be defined as the symbolic representation of a set of relatively stable relationships to the environment which an actor experiences as "being" himself-—the self is the sum of the meanings which he has for himself. One reason for believing that the self-concept is an important area of concern for social-psychology is simply that it is, in the present view, the locus of the meanings whereby an actor establishes behavioral relationships with his environment. These symbols should reveal, then, the different ways in which individuals are engaged with other persons, groups or events. Furthermore, a knowledge of the symbols by which an individual represents himself to himself should yield important information about the context in which he has established these behavioral relationships. From the properties of the social-structural context additional inferences about the actor and his behavioral relationships might be drawn. For, if the objects, i.e., the people, situations, and things, in relation to which the actor de- rives his meanings can in turn be seen to change or to be organized in certain ways, then much more of the likely behavior of the actor could be known and explained. For which an actor has toward himself, others, groups and institutions, and values and "which define and regulate his relatedness to them in concrete situations." Muzafer Sherif and Carolyn W. Sherif, An Outline of Social Psychology (New York: Harper and Brothers, 1956), p. 581. 62 example, it is quite possible that self-conceptions of the kind yielded by the TST can be arranged according to the degree to which the relationships which they suggest are components of formal or informal structures; knowing this would be to know much of the structure and organization of the objects to which an actor would or would not be addressing himself. The meanings attached to self-symbols differ then in the degree to which they are fixed in formally constituted group processes, in the degree to which they imply particular settings or contexts, and in the degree to which they permit the establishment of behavioral relationships in different kinds of situations. It is assumed that the extent and form of the be— havioral relationships which may constitute the meaning of any particular self-concept for an actor are derived from the norms, values, and beliefs of groups which are important to him. These may vary in their lack of contradiction, the accuracy with which they are perceived, the degree of consensus with which they are held, and the strength of any sanctions involved in violation or compliance with them, as well as in numerous other ways. Important as these factors might be in determining the nature of the relation— ship between a self-conception of some particular sort and actual behavior, they cannot be dealt with in this essay. Also to be excluded is any discussion of the precise mechanisms involved in the acquisition of self-conceptions. For this thesis it suffices to make the following three 63 related assumptions relevant to this point: First, self- concepts are formed from the store of symbols available in the groups with which actors are in contact. These symbols represent any of the behavioral relationships which have acquired meanings in these groups. Second, individuals behave toward an actor according to the meanings which he has for them, and, ultimately, according to the relationships which he affects with them or according to the identity they have assigned to him. Third, individuals acquire meaning for themselves as "selves" from the nature of the relation- ships which they establish with their environment, parti- cularly from cues in the actions and behavior of other individuals around them. Fourth, the greater the amount of time spent in some location within the social structure the greater will be the likelihood of forming a self-concept from the symbols at that location in accordance with 'assumption three, in particular, the greater is the likeli- hood that an aspect of the self-concept will be linked to a normative reference group associated with that location. A fifth assumption summarizes and condenses much of what has already been said with respect to the self and actual behavior: to some important extent the meanings which the world has for an actor derive from experienced relationships to it by virtue of the relational property of his self—symbols--since behavior will be consistent with the meanings it will also tend to be consistent with the self. 64 This last assumption suggests the existence of an evaluative dimension to the self. Judgments as to the consistenoy of actual behavior with behavior implied by the meanings of a self—concept implicitly involve a comparison by the actor of his actual behavior against certain criteria or standard relationships—-in short, it entails an evaluation according to relevant normative standards by means of symbolic behavior. It would not be inconsistent with the view that the experience of social reality is largely a process of experienced relationships to assert that for social groups certain actor-object relationships serve as standards and represent different degrees of, for example, goodness, value, sacred- ness, or appropriateness. These patterns of ordered relation— ships may be thought of as shared internalized evaluative scales, while values, as distinct from an evaluation, may then be regarded as relatively favorable locations on such a scale. The symbols "good," "immoral,” "funny,” ”honest," or "shocking" which result from an evaluation convey meaning to others; that is, they are significant symbols and rgprg- sent the relativeppositions between whatever relationships that have been evaluated and those behavioral relationships that may comprise comparativetpoints on the shared scale of reference. The actor's experience of the outcomes of these evaluative acts with respect to his self-concept constitutes his self-esteem. Just as the self was regarded as a set of 65 relationships so also can self-esteem be regarded as a set of evaluations of these relationships. Consequently, it is possible to have as many specific self-esteems as there are somewhat distinct relationships comprising the self. It is possible, too, that there is a generalized self-esteem as the composite of all such evaluations. Self-esteem at any one time, then, might consist of any number of experienced discrepancies or convergencies between actual and ideal relationships. A graphical but over-simplified representation of self-evaluations and self-esteem may be helpful in conveying these concpets and their connection to self-symbols. Figure 3. Graphical representation of self—evaluation. Ideal Relationship ‘(/'~‘\b Normative Evaluative Scale Actor IIHIHH Actual or Experienced Relationship In Figure 3, three relationships relevant to the meaning of a given self-symbol, S, are represented by straight lines. Ri represents an ideal behavioral relationship as given by the meaning of S, Ra is an experienced behavioral relationship relevant to S, and R.e is the symbolically experienced discrepancy between the ideal and the actual relationships. This discrepancy is experienced as a level of self-esteem, and the comparison of the two relationships Ri and Ra is 66 an evaluation. The meaning of this and other possible discrepancies consist of relationships between the ordered relationships which comprise points on the normative evalua- tive scale appropriate to S. From this example it can be seen that self—evaluations have a more complex meaning structure than do non-evaluative self-symbols; a self- evaluation is in fact a relationship of relationships. The significance of this distinction between Self-symbols and self—evaluations will be pointed out later in this chapter in connection with the critique of the ALI. One theoretical convenience of thinking of the self in terms of itsesteem—dimension is that it suggests that changes in self-concept can possibly be interpreted in terms of fairly specific propositions of level of aspiration theory. Should this be true then it would have implications for the interpretation of the self as a meaning. For, changes in the relative positions of an ideal and an actual self, by shifts of either one along some scale, would also, by definition, result in changed relationships between the actor and his symbolic environment. In speaking ofnormative comparative scales we are simply indicating that such scales can be reconstructed from 8 the behavior of individuals. It does not necessarily imply 8The recent work of Clyde Coombs describes theoretical and methodological techniques of great promise for investi- gating the existence and the characteristics of such scales. See Clyde H. Coombs, A Theory of Data (New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1964). 67 any awareness of scales as such on the part of those so behaving. That individuals can choose between alternatives with some consistency is sufficient justification to attempt to use scales as an explanatory device. The concept of reference group offers one possibility for tying the relational property of the self, along with the interpretation of norms as scales, back into the social structure. Groups to which an individual compares himself—- comparative reference groups~qnuigroups from Which an indi- vidual has adopted norms or values—-normative reference groups--have been elaborated by Merton and Rossi.9 Within the framework which is being employed, normative reference groups can be regarded as those groups which provide the scales governing the appropriateness of various relation— ships which actors affect with their environments, and which are a major factor in the development and maintenance of the self-concept. Comparative reference groups can be viewed as occupying important points of comparison along one kind of normative evaluative scale. In summary then, location and involvement in the social structure affect an actor's self-concept through reference groups, which in turn provide the behavioral or 9Robert K. Merton and Alice S. Rossi, "Contributions to the Theory of Reference Group Behavior,” in Robert K. Merton, Social Theory and Social Structure (revised and enlarged; Glencoe, Illinois: The Free Press, 1957), Chapter VII. 68 symbolic settings and conditions under which an actor establishes stable relationships with his environment, which then become symbolized as a self-concept. They provide normative depth to the behavior associated with any self, and they may provide important points of reference in relation to which the self is evaluated. Second, self- concepts influence behavior since, to a large extent, they select and give meaning to certain portions of the environ- ment in relation to which behavior occurs. Evaluation of the Rationale Justifying,Self-Descriptions It can now be seen how this theory reconfirms Kuhn's justification of the assertion that self-descriptions provide lihhs_between an actor, the social structure, and behavior. The particular symbols in terms of which an actor conceives of himself have the virtue of (a) being a portion of the shared symbols of some group, (b) designating a class of actors, (c) designating classes of objects, and (d) designat- ing a class of relationships between "(b)" and "(c)" which define both “(b)" and "(c)" in terms of one another in a context of on-going behavior. Implicitly then, given the symbols in terms of which an actor defines himself and the meanings which these symbols have for the actor, we should also have (1) a significant portion of the behavior in which that actor is likely to engage, and (2) some understanding of the nature and scope of his social—psychological location 69 in the social structure both inferred from the fact that these symbols will reflect their source in the unique symbolic universe shared by some groups in the social- structure. The links arise then from the context from which the symbols were derived and are "given" by relationality comprising their meaning. Evaluation of the Asset-Liability Instrument It is possible also to show that some of the funda- mental difficulties associated with the ALI are explainable from the standpoint of this theoretical position. Since the significant criticisms were most relevant to this issue, and because in what follows we will attempt to show that they are sufficiently damaging to warrant rejecting the ALI as a technique for linking self to reference groups and to behavior, only these three criticisms will be con- sidered here. It will be recalled that in essence the criticisms labeled "2," "3," and "4" centered around the following points: "2." Self-evaluations obscured the very links between actors, social-structural involvement, and be- havior that they were supposed to reveal, "3." Lack of knowledge of the meanings to be attri- buted to the responses which would justify their categorization, "4." Absence of explicit theoretical basis for linking a response on the ALI to social structure or to behavior. 70 It will also be recalled that the fundamental assumptions justifying the ALI were that by merging self-description and self-evaluation (1) the values according to which the self-assessments would be made would closely reflect their source in the social structure (as had the categories of self-description in the TST) and (2) the values would be an important influence on behavior. It was assumed that on the basis of these two considerations the ALI would be an effective means of linking the self to reference groups and to behavior. Now the difficulties noted in criticisms "2" through "4" can be seen to directly negate assumption "(1)" above; that is, on just those points where the ALI was expected to be the most effective it has been found to be the most defective. An explanation of this curious result from the standpoint of the present perspective follows.» Criticism "2": In Chapter I, no clear distinction was drawn between categories of self—description and self- evaluations as to the possible consequences that each might have in establishing links between actors, the social- structure, and behavior. It was felt that the symbols would somehow reveal both values and categories and would, there- fore, yield more obvious relationships, and that the responses themselves would somehow reveal their connection to the social structure and demonstrate their implications for behavior. waever, it can now be seen that the symbols on the ALI are far more complex than non-evaluative self— descriptions, for self—evaluations are relationships gt 71 relationships. For, whereas symbols of self-description are relationships of classes of actors, and objects, an evaulation of such symbols introduce an additional relation- ship--the one between the symbol and another symbol on an evaluative scale. From the standpoint of the present theory the symbols elicited by the ALI have, by virtue of their complexity, so obscured the kinds of links which were supposed to be forth- 10 coming from them that the ALI is rendered virtually useless for accomplishing the task for which it was designed. ‘ Criticism "3": Closely related to "2," and partly resulting from it, is the absence of certainty associated with the meanings which were imputed to various responses on the ALI. waever, in accordance with the present chapter, it should be apparent that it is just these meanings that are essential to the task of relating the selftp reference groups and behavior--to which end the ALI was constructed. Without these meanings there are no relation— ships and, consequently, no links except those that can be derived solely on the basis of an uncertain and unreliable empathy. 10This is true with the possible exception of some obvious and trivial instances such as the previously noted connection between grades and Intellectual or Scholastic asset and liability statements. 72 Criticism "4": Absence of a theoretical basis for relating specific categories of response on the ALI to specific instances of reference group involvement or behavior did not entirely frustrate all attempts at deriving testable hypotheses. But, if it is true that the self is important because it links modes of symbolic representation to specific contexts of social processes and to activity, then it is particularly serious that it is at just this point that there is no theoretical rationale according to which these links are predictable. It is at this point too, that the present theoretical statement is silent and in greatest need of development; more will be said of this later. Since the difficulties giving rise to these three criticisms of the ALI appear to reinforce the obscurity of the very relationship which the ALI was designed to reveal, and since criticism "2" appears to be a sufficient condition preventing the elimination of "3" and "4" and is by the very nature of the ALI not capable of being eliminated, we can only conclude that, from the standpoint of the theoretical statement in this chapter, the Asset-Liability Instrument does not accomplish what was intended for it. Implications for Further Research and Development of the Theory Two of the most significant problems for empirical research posed by the position developed in this chapter are (a) finding suitable techniques for gathering these self— symbols, and (b) finding techniques for getting at the 73 meanings inherent within these symbols. If the ALI is inadequate for doing this, what sort of techniques would be acceptable and empirically feasible? First, in any such technique the fundamental error of the ALI, which gave rise to criticism "2", must be avoided. Specifically this means that self-descriptions and self-evaluation should be independently measured. These two components of the self can prdbably be much more pre- cisely and fruitfully studied by instruments designed to mesh with the theoretical properties of evaluations and self- representation. Combining these into one instrument only obscures both.ll’ 12 Second, the gathering of self-symbols could best be done by an instrument with a minimum amount of structure in order to avoid leading subjects to represent themselves in terms of symbols which they otherwise might not use. This requirement seems to be admirably met by the TST, consequently our attention will be directed to the problem of discovering the meanings of symbols derived from this or some similar technique. 11This depends, of course, on the presence of a theory which will give sudh information. 12For an example of promising research which combines such separate measures see, F. B. Waisanen, "Self- Attitudes and Performance Expectations," Sociological Quarterly, V01. 3 (1962), pp. 208-219. 74 Third, it would be as unnecessary as it would be difficult to completely catalog all of the meanings of all of the symbols gathered from some population of respondents. From the standpoint of research which would utilize these meanings, the problem would reduce to getting just enough material to fit the requirements of a particular problem.13 Any number of techniques of questionnaire construction currently employed in social-psychology would probably be adaptable to this end. Hewever, it would be premature to suggest specific techniques until one had acquired a ”feel" for the ways in which respondents indicate the nature of the relationships entailed by their self-symbols. This might best be done in an interview situation. Specific techniques should follow only after a close look has been had at the relational structure of self-symbols as they appear to the respondent. Fourth, the measurement of these relationships (at such time as suitable techniques are developed), should proceed concurrently with independent efforts to assess the respondent's involvement in and commitment to reference groups and the context of his behavior, i.e., the objects toward which relationships are established. It is at this point that a wedding of the theoretical position presented 13This would be true provided, of course, that the state of knowledge and of theoretical development were sufficiently advanced to permit this. 75 in this chapter with some form of organizational theory might be fruitfully consummated. Finally, the lack of an adequate theory whereby responses on the ALI can be linked to the social structure and to behavior seems to be partly a result of a lack of information concerning the meanings which the symbols possess. That is, no adequate theory is possible until we know what the self-statements mean and we won't know that until we let the respondents tell us. There are, however, some likely avenues of approach to the development of sudh a theory which have been suggested already in this chapter. The one that has some empirical confirmation behind it is the rationale employed by McPartland and Cumming and McPartland, Cumming and Garretson, in which it is suggested (in different terminology, of course) that there is a certain isomorphism between the structure of responses on the TST and the structure of behavior associated with it, e.g., bizarre self—responses and bizarre behavior, nonsocial self— 14 Theoretical responses and non-social behavior, etc. developments could be forthcoming too from the observation that self-symbols may vary in the extent to which the relation- ships implied by them were to formally or to informally l4In criticism "5" of Chapter II it was noted that the obviousness and narrowness of the categories on the ALI might be associated with strong but theoretically trivial relationships. This example of McPartland et al. is an instance of manifestly hghrtrivial and hgh-obvious relation- ships yielded by a rather specific and mph-obvious theoretical proposition. 76 constituted groups. Still other theoretical propositions might follow from a knowledge of the specificity or generality¢1fclasses of objects and relationships which might be associated with the degree to which any self- symbol implies a narrow or broad range of behavior. Similarly, the degree of specificity or generality of the class of actors to which a self-symbol referred could be related to the degree of similarity or uniqueness of behaviors and social-structural involvements of a number of individuals. Summary The central focus of this thesis has been an assess— ment of the Asset—Liability Instrument. This instrument was developed as a technique for eliciting self-symbols that would enable respondents to be linked in important Ways to their location in the social structure and which would enable predictions to be made regarding their behavior. This was to be accomplished in the ALI by combining self- descriptions and self-evaluations into one instrument. This combination would, it was reasoned, accomplish two things. First, it would yield categories according to which indivi- duals thought of themselves and which had already been proven useful in research with the TST. Second, the most salient values held by the respondents would also be forth- coming. It was argued that these values would reflect the groups in which the respondent was significantly involved, and would also be of consequence for his behavior. 77 A number of problems were encountered in using the ALI and in interpreting results from it. This in turn led to a reconsideration of the theoretical justification behind the basic assumption that self-symbols were an adequate way of linking an actor to the social structure and to his behavior by means of self-symbols. This reconsideratiOn had two major consequences for the thesis: (1) it reconfirmed the rationale behind the assumption of the theoretical and empirical value of self-representations, and (2) it also confirmed the fact that the ALI is incapable of eliciting such useful self—descriptions. From this new perspective the links between self- symbols, social—structure, and behavior are seen to be a consequence of two characteristics of the symbolic self. First, they are derived from a unique context which is significant to the actor and in which symbols and their meanings are socially shared and developed. Second, the meanings of self—symbols establish behavioral rslationships between certain socially defined categories of actors and objects. For a number of reasons the ALI fails as a method of empirically linking an actor to his social-structural involvement and to his behavior. Hewever, one reason in particular is sufficientto condemn it: the combination of self-descriptive and self-evaluative statements into a single instrument which was to give the ALI its unique value 78 as a method of revealing these links, is just that feature which obscures them. Self—description and self-evaluations are theoretically and empirically distinct; as a consequence of combining them the meaning of the responses on the ALI, and hence the relationships which they were to reveal, are hopelessly confounded. Finally, some very general suggestions for further development of the theory and for developing instruments which avoid the major flaws exhibited by the ALI are presented and briefly discussed. BIBLIOGRAPHY Brookover, Wilbur B., Ann Patterson and Shailer Thomas. Self-Concept of Ability and School Achievement. Final Report of Cooperative Research Project No. 845. Office of Research and Publications, Michigan State University, 1962. Clark, Burton R. Educating the Expert Society. San Francisco: Chandler Publishing Company, 1962. Cohen, Arthur R. "Some Implications of Self-Esteem for Social Influence," in H0vland and Janis (eds.). Personality and Persuasibility. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1959. .Coombs, Clyde H. A Theory of Data. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1964. Davis, James A. with David Gottlib, Jan Hajda, Carolyn Huson, Joe L. Spaeth, Stipends and SpousesZThe Finances of American Arts and Science Graduate Students. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1962. Gaier, Eugene L. and Helen S. Wambach. "Self-Evaluations of Personality Assets and Liabilities of Southern White and Negro Students," Journal of_§ocial Psychology, Vol. 51 (1960), 135-143. Gottlieb, David and Benjamin Hodgkins. "College Student Subcultures: Their Structure and Characteristics in Relation to Student Attitude Change,“ The School Review, Vol. 71 (1963), 266-289. Hickman, C. Addison and Manford Kuhn. Individuals, Groups, and Economic Behavior. New York: The Dryden Press, 1956. Hoijer, Harry. "The Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis," in Readings in Anthropology, Vol. I. New York: Thomas Y. Crowell, 1959. Inkeles, Alex. "Industrial Man: The Relation of Status to Experience, Perception and Value," American Journal of Sociology, V01. 66 (1960), 1-31. 79 80 Littunen, Yrjo and Eugene L. Gaier. “Occupational Values and Modes of Conformity," Journal of Social Psychology, V01. 51 (1960), 123-133. McPartland, Thomas. Manual for the Twenty Statements Problem, (Revised, Department of Reserach, the Greater Kansas City Mental Health Foundation, 1959). Mead, George H. Mind, Self and Society. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1934. . The Philosophy of the Act. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1938. Merton, Robert K., and Alice S. Rossi. ”Contributions to the Theory of Reference Group Behavior,” in Robert K. Merton, Social Theory and Social Structure. Revised and enlarged; Glencoe, Ill.: The Free Press, 1957. Miyamoto, S. Frank, and Sanford Dornbusch. "A Test of Interactionist Hypotheses of Self-Conception,” American Journal of Sociology, Vol. 61 (1956). Riesman, David. Faces in the Crowd. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press, 1950. Sherif, Muzafer and Carolyn W. Sherif. An Outline of Social Psychology. New York: Harper and Brothers, 1956. Stotland, Ezra, Stanley Thorely, Edwin Thomas, Arthur R. Cohen, and Alvin Zander. “The Effects of Group Expectations and Self-Esteem Upon Self-Evaluations," J. of Abnormal and Social Psychology, V01. 54 (1957), 55-63. Waisanen, F. B. "Self-Attitudes and Performance Expectations," Sociological Quarterly, V01. 3, No. 3 (1962), 208-219. ‘Aiz‘iHIGAH ATE UNIVERSITY LIBPARIPS I !g‘ ”i“ V 1‘ I!) III . :1 l 1“ {'1’ \I '2‘ [:(W ”I 1‘ #1) i 3 1 1! 93 03146 1969