H | 1 l‘ 1 i 1 i i $ H11! Ii ‘ ! WIN 1‘ l I TH _ This is to certify that the thesis entitled THE INFLUENCE OF FEMALE WING COLOR VARIATION ON MALE MATE CHOICE IN TIGER SWALLOWTAIL BUTTERFLIES (PAPILIO SPP.) presented by Matthew L. Aardema has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the degree in Entomology lfl/M %% ’ Majormofessor’sSi , ature 7 glee; 5% gm) Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Employer LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5/08 KilProlecc8-Pres/ClRC/DateDueiindd THE INFLUENCE OF FEMALE WING COLOR VARIATION ON MALE MATE CHOICE IN TIGER SWALLOWTAIL BUTTERFLIES (PAPILIO SPP.) By Matthew L. Aardema A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Entomology Ecology, Evolutionary Biology and Behavior 2010 ABSTRACT THE INFLUENCE OF FEMALE WING COLOR VARIATION ON MALE MATE CHOICE IN TIGER SWALLOWTAIL BUTI'ERFLIES (PAP/LIO SPP.) By Matthew L. Aardema Variation both within and between populations is an important component of the speciation process. This variation can be found in all biological arenas and includes ecological variation, developmental variation and variation in sexual characteristics. In Lepidoptera, this last source of variation has had an important role in producing divergence between populations. It has been shown that males of the eastern tiger swallowtail, Papilio glaucus, and the Canadian tiger swallowtail, F’. canadensis, both prefer to mate with female P. glaucus in two- choice field experiments. This suggests that there is a species difference between females of the two species that is recognizable to males. One possible difference is the amount of blue scaling on the dorsal wing surface of females. P. glaucus females have relatively more area of their wing surface covered by this blue coloration than do P. canadensis females. However, my experiments to determine the importance this blue coloration has for males selecting a mate suggests that its influence is minimal and hence the difference in blue coloration seen between P. glaucus and P. canadensis is unlikely to explain differences in male mate choice. Other explanations for asymmetrical male mate choice may be differences in body size and/or yellow coloration in females of these two species. Although quantified, these were not experimentally tested to determine their importance in male mate selection. To G-ma ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank Rodrigo Mercader and Gabe Ording for discussions and overall good advice, Shane Empie and John Acorn for advice on UV photography, Jay Sobel for assistance with spectrophotometery, Ange Kosnick and Brittany Sonke for assistance rearing Ohio females for tethering experiments, Howard Romack and Bob Howtz for rearing Vermont and Pennsylvania females for tethering experiments, Sean Ryan for field assistance, Matthew Douglas for original mentorship, and Amanda Lorenz for being there. I would also like to thank my guidance committee, Anthony Cognato, Doug Schemske, and Jeff Conner. Lastly, and most importantly, I would like to thank my advisor Mark Scriber. This work was funded by MAES project #01644, NSF DEB# 0716683 and NSF DEB# 0918879, a Scriber Scholar award, a Hutson research grant, the Jeffery Boettcher Travel Fund, and MSU College of Natural Science undergraduate research support. TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Tables ......................................................................................... vi List of Figures ..................................................................................... viii Chapter 1: Introduction ......................................................................................... 01 The Tiger Swallowtail System .................................................................. 02 Male Mate Choice in Tiger Swallowtails ..................................................... 11 Research Objectives ............................................................................. 13 Chapter 2: Variation in wing coloration between Papilio canadensis and P. glaucus populations Introduction ......................................................................................... 14 Materials and Methods .......................................................................... 19 Results ............................................................................................... 24 Discussion .......................................................................................... 29 CHAPTER 3: Courtship and mating response of male Papilio spp. to variation in female-limited wing coloration Introduction ......................................................................................... 39 Materials and Methods .......................................................................... 40 Resutts ............................................................................................... 47 Discussion .......................................................................................... 51 CHAPTER 4, Possible alternative explanations for asymmetrical male mating preferences in Papilio spp. Introduction ......................................................................................... 56 Materials and Methods .......................................................................... 59 Results ............................................................................................... 61 Discussion .......................................................................................... 69 CHAPTER 5; Summary and Conclusions ..................................................................... 71 Appendix 1: Record of Deposition of Voucher Specimens ............................. 74 Appendix 1.1: Voucher Specimen Data ................................................... 75 LITERATURE CITED ............................................................................ 76 LIST OF TABLES Table 1.1. Summary of morphological, ecological and genetic differences between P. glaucus and P. canadensis .................................................... 05 Table 2.1. Results from analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) for a comparison between female P. canadensis and yellow morph P. glaucus for the total area of the wings and the area of blue coloration with species as a cofactor. The data was |0910 transformed to homogenize variance between the two species. The additive model appears to have the greatest predictive value and suggests that the two species increase in blue area in similar fashion with increased overall size (statistically equivalent slopes), but that P. glaucus females have more blue on an absolute scale. Bolded numbers represent the values for the preferred model in the three different analyses (Adj. R2, AIC and BIC). See text for more details ................................................................................................ 31 Table 3.1. Number of two—choice pairings for each population examined. When male preferences were examined over muftiple years for the same population, results are combined. No difference was significant (see text for values) ......... 49 Table 3.2. Number of one-choice pairings for each population examined. When male preferences were examined over multiple years for the same population, results are combined. No difference was significant (see text for values) ......... 51 Table 4.1. Summary statistics for the relative relationship between forewing length (mm) and abdomen weight (g) in female P. glaucus and P. canadensis...62 Table 4.2. Results from analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) for a comparison between P. canadensis and P glaucus females for wing length and abdomen weight with species as a cofactor. The simple model which does not take into account species appears to have the greatest predictive value and suggests that the two butterflies do not differ significantly in their basic allometric relationship between wing length and abdomen weight. Bolded numbers represent the values for the preferred model in the three different analyses (Adj. R2, AIC and BIC). See text for more details ................................................................ 63 vi Table 4.3a. Summary statistics for hue value in female P. glaucus and P. canadensis ......................................................................................... 66 Table 4.3b. Summary statistics for chroma value in female P glaucus and P canadensis ......................................................................................... 66 Table 4.4a. Summary statistics for hue value in populations of female P canadensis (Alaska and Michigan) and P. glaucus (Ohio and Florida) .............. 67 Table 4.4b. Summary statistics for chroma value in populations of female P. canadensis (Alaska and Michigan) and P. glaucus (Ohio and Florida) .............. 67 Table 4.5. Significance (p) values from Tukey’s HSD for all pairwise comparisons between populations in the yellow color analysis between P. canadensis (Alaska and Michigan), and P. glaucus (Ohio and Florida). Values on the lower left side of the figure represent chroma comparisons and values on the upper right side of the figure represent hue comparisons. Bolded values in the dark gray boxes are significant (p50.05). See text for more details ............................................ 68 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1.1. Distribution map of P. canadensis, P glaucus, and areas of hybridization between them in North America ............................................. 03 Figure 1.2. a.) Dark morph, P. glaucus female; believed to mimic the distasteful pipe-vine swallowtail, Battus philenor b.) Battus philenor male 0.) Yellow morph, P. glaucus female d.) P. glaucus male ....................................................... 06 Figure 2.1. Locations of sample populations for blue color analysis. Gray circles represent P. canadensis populations and black circles represent P. glaucus populations. a.) Fairbanks-North Star Co., Alaska b.) Charlevoix 00., Michigan c.) Bennigton Co., Vermont d.) Jackson and Gallia 005., Ohio e.) Habersham and Rabun Cos., Georgia f.) Levy Co., Florida ........................................... 21 Figure 2.2. Process of wing analysis to determine total wing area and area of blue coloration. a.) First, one forewing and one hindwing were photographed. This was done for both dorsal and ventral sides b.) The image was made binary and the total area was determined. A ruler in the photograph was used to determine scale. c.) Lastly, all coloration except the blue was removed and the area of this remaining coloration was determined. See text for more detail. . . ...22 Figure 2.3. Mean relative blue area 2!: SD for all species/sex/color morph combinations. Pc : P. canadensis, Pg : P. glaucus, YW : yellow morph females and DK : dark morph females. For each combination, the light grey bar shows the mean blue coloration on the dorsal wing surface and the dark grey bar shows the mean blue coloration on the ventral wing surface. Bars with the same letter are not significantly different at the p<0.05 level (pairwise t test with Holm’s correction) .......................................................................................... 26 Figure 2.4. Mean relative blue area 1 SD for all female P. canadensis (Po) and yellow morph P glaucus (Pg). For each combination, the light grey bar shows the mean blue coloration on the dorsal wing surface and the dark grey bar shows the mean blue coloration on the ventral wing surface. Bars with the same letter are not significantly different at the p<0.05 level (painrvise t test with Holm’s correction) .......................................................................................... 27 Figure 2.5. Mean relative blue area 1: SD for all male P. canadensis (Pc) and P. glaucus (Pg). For each combination, the light grey bar shows the mean blue coloration on the dorsal wing surface and the dark grey bar shows the mean blue coloration on the ventral wing surface. Bars with the same letter are not significantly different at the p<0.05 level (pairwise t test with Holm’s correction). ......................................................................................................... 28 viii Figure 2.6. Log transformed comparison of absolute total wing area and area of blue coloration for P. canadensis and yellow morph, P. glaucus females. Area values are in cmz. Regression lines are drawn using the additive model (See table 2.10). The slopes are not significantly different. See text for more details. ........................... 32 Figure 2.7. An example of my photographic examination of ultraviolet coloration in tiger swallowtail butterflies. a.) The visual image of the dorsal side of a yellow morph (left) and dark morph (right), female P. glaucus. b.) This image was taking using a UV bypass filter. The wings displayed are the same as those in ‘a.’ Areas that reflected UV light are white and areas that absorbed UV light are black. See text for more details .............................................................................. 34 Figure 2.8. An example of the reflectance spectrum patterns of blue, yellow and black coloration from one yellow morph, P glaucus female. The other three individuals assessed in this manner had similar spectral patterns. This figure shows a clear UV signature (300-400 nm) for blue coloration, and virtually no UV reflectance for yellow and black coloration. See text for more details .............. 35 Figure 3.1. Three examples of female P. glaucus dorsal colors showing the ”spectral reflectance signature of blue coloration, black coloration and blue coloration covered by black ink from a sharpie brand marker. UV coloration is that which occurs between 300 and 400 nm. Black ink effectively conceals both the UV and visual elements of the blue coloration ....................................... 45 Figure 4.1. Comparison of wing length (mm) and abdominal weight (g) in P. canadensis and yellow morph, P. glaucus females. Regression lines are drawn using the simple model (See table 4.2). The slopes are not significantly different. See text for more details ........................................................................ 64 CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION It has been argued that variation within and between populations is one of the most interesting, yet overlooked aspects of biological diversity (Mayr, 2005). Subtle differences among individuals within and between populations may lead to remarkable divergences and biological radiations (Schluter, 2000). For example, it has been suggested that differences in beak width and depth helped to generate the diversity of Darwin’s finches on the Galapagos Islands (Grant, 1986), morphological variation and the influence this has on pollinator complexes lead to the diversification of orchids (T remblay et al., 2005), and variation in teeth shape lead to ungulate mammalian diversification (Jemvall et al., 1996). Among most lepidopteran groups important ecological (Ayres and Scriber, 1994; Bossart and Scriber, 1995, Mallet and Joron, 1999), developmental (Beldade and Brakefield, 2002), and sexual variation (Ellers and Boggs, 2003) is common within and between extant populations and such variation likely played a large role in the historical diversification of this clade (Boggs et al., 2003 and chapters within). Several factors contribute to the high degree of population variation observed in butterflies including a complex life history (Beck and Fiedler, 2009) and rapid generation times (Thomas et al., 2010). Variation in traits related to mating and reproduction has been especially important in butterfly speciation (Chamberlin et al., 2009). In Lepidoptera, the male is the homogametic sex and the sex chromosomes are designated 2 [=X] and W [=Y] respectively. This sex determination system allows for the generation of a relatively large amount of sexual dimorphism in many species, possibly because genes that generate ‘preference’ for particular sexual traits can be maintained on the W chromosome in females, without the disadvantages of also being found in males that have the potentially costly trait (Hastings, 1994). The presence of such sexual dimorphism (specifically variation in secondary sexual characteristics and preferences for these characteristics) can lead to differential selection and assortative mating (Naisbit et al., 2001) when subsets of males and females preferentially mate with one another at higher frequencies than they mate with other individuals of the population. This process can lead to the formation of new species. The Tiger Swallowtail System Within the swallowtail family, Papilionidae, there are between 475 to 563 species worldwide (Scriber, 1995). Of these, the tiger swallowtail clade comprises between 5 and 8 species in North America. In the eastern United States and Canada, two species occur with a parapatric distribution (Scriber, 1996a). The Canadian tiger swallowtail, Papilio canadensis, is found from central Alaska, throughout much of Canada, south into northern Minnesota, VVrsconsin and Michigan, and east into the New England States (Fig. 1; Scriber, 1996a). The range of F’. canadensis overlaps with that of the eastern tiger swallowtail, P. glaucus, in a narrow zone of hybridization through central Wisconsin, Michigan and New York, and along the northern Appalachian mountain range extending I P. canadensis I Hybrid Zone P. glaucus Figure 1.1. Distribution map of P. canadensis, P. glaucus, and areas of hybridization between them in North America. south. From this hybrid zone, the range of P. glaucus reaches south to central Florida and west to Texas, Arkansas, Missouri and Illinois. Despite continued gene flow between these two species, they exhibit many important morphological, ecological and genetic differences (summarized in Table 1). P. glaucus individuals are larger and have a relatively narrower black band on the anal cell of their hind wings (Luebke et al., 1988). Females have more blue swling on the dorsal side of their hindwings (see Ch. 2) which extends slightly onto the forewing. Additionally, female P. glaucus occur as two color morphs, a yellow morph which is similar in coloration to males, or a dark morph which is believed to mimic the black and blue coloration of the male pipevine swallowtail, Battus philenor (Fig. 1.2; Brower, 1958). The frequency of these two morphs within a population appears to be correlated with the density of B. philenor occurring congruently (Brower and Brower, 1962). In contrast to P. glaucus, individual P. canadensis are generally smaller and have a relatively wider black band. Females have less overall dorsal blue coloration which does not extend onto the upper wing. Female dark morphs do not exist in pure P. canadensis populations due to the absence of a melanism gene (b+, found on the W- chromosome and maternally inherited) and the additional presence of a suppresser gene (found on the Z- chromosome and paternally inherited in females; Scriber et al. 1996). The two most important ecological differences occur in diapause induction (and corresponding voltinism patterns; Rockey et al., 1987), and larval host-use Table 1.1. Summary of morphological, ecological and genetic differences between P. glaucus and P. canadensis [ TRAIT | P. CANADENSIS I P. caucus | REFERENCE Morphological Smaller (35 - 50 Larger (50 - 70 Luebke et al., Size mm uppenrving mm uppenrving 1988; Aardema length) length) Pers. Obs. Wider (50 - 90% Narrower (10 - Luebke et al., Blackband Width of anal cell 50% of anal cell 1988; Aardema width) width) Pers. Obs. Female Blue Coloration Extends No Yes Aardema, Pers. - Obs. onto Upperwrngs Hagen and Female Coloration OngoYell‘ow Yellilir‘voothark Scriber, 1989; 'p ’9 Scriber et al., 1996 Ecological Diapause Induction Obligate Facultative $5);er et al., Very Very Larval Host Use polyphagous, but 332323983156: :1: Scriber, 1988 tulip tree IS toxrc toxic Genetic . . Andolfatto et al., hgzfifggfinaelnigl 19 Fixed Differences 2003; Putnam et 9 al, 2007 Absence of Dark Presence of Morph enabler Dark Morph gene on W enabler gene on . . chromosome W chromosome Hagen and Mrmrcry Genes and Presence of and absence of Scriber; 1989 Dark Morph Dark Morph Suppresor on Z Suppresor on 2 chromosome chromosome Z—Iinked Allozyme _ _ _ _ Hagen and Pgd -125/ 80/ 150 100 or 50 Scriber, 1991 Z-linked Allozyme Hagen and tan ‘80 °' 4° 40° Scriber; 1991 Autosomal Allozyme Hagen and Hk '110 '90 °' ‘100 Scriber, 1991 AFLP Analysis Many Species Level Differences \zllggtger and Porter, Figure 1.2. a.) Dark morph, P glaucus female; believed to mimic the distasteful pipe-vine swallowtail, Battus philenor b.) Battus philenor male c.) Yellow morph, P. glaucUs female d.) P glaucus male abilities (Scriber, 1988). P. canadensis are obligatory diapausing and only complete only one generation per year (univoltine). They ovenrvinter in the pupal stage and must experience a prolonged period of cold temperatures before they will break diapause. Under natural conditions, diapause is entered regardless of the photoperiod experienced by the larvae (Rockey et al., 1987). In contrast, P. glaucus is facultatively diapausing and can either enter a diapause stage like P. canadensis, or else directly develop into an adult within the same year that larval development is completed. Which of these two strategies is initiated in the pupal stage is based primarily on light cues (day length or ‘photoperiod’), although temperature and host quality may also play a role (Rockey at al., 1987). All populations of P. glaucus complete at least two generations per season (bivoltine), and in some locations (e.g. southern Georgia and Florida), three or more generations may be possible (multivoltine; Rockey et al., 1987, Scriber at al., 2008). Tiger swallowtail butterflies are among the most polyphagous of all butterflies with regards to larval host use abilities. Larvae can utilize a large number of deciduous tree species from 10 disparate families (Scriber et al., 1982; Scriber, 1988). These include black cherry (Pmnus semtina), ash (Fraxinus spp.) and hop tree (Ptelea tn'foliata). However, adult female ovipositional preferences tend to be narrower. One of the most preferred ovipositional hosts of P glaucus is tulip tree (Lin'odendnon tulipifera). Larvae of P glaucus can utilize tulip tree effectively and grow and survive on it similar to other good host species (Mercader et al., 2009). In contrast, tulip tree is toxic to P. canadensis larvae and causes complete mortality in larval families fed this species (Scriber et al., 1982). Conversely, one of the preferred ovipositional hosts of P. canadensis is quaking aspen (Populus tremuloides) which is toxic to larvae of P. glaucus (Scriber et al., 1989). Interestingly, in ovipositional assays P. canadensis females will often lay a high proportion of their eggs on tulip tree (Mercader and Scriber, 2007; 2008a). This maladaptive tendency is unlikely to have any negative impact on natural populations as the range of tulip tree does not overlap with P. canadensis (Mercader and Scriber, 2007). In ovipositional assays, P. glaucus females lay very few eggs on quaking aspen (Mercader and Scriber, 2007; 2008a). Lastly, there are many genetic differences between the two species. These include fixed nucleotide differences in the COI and COII mitochondrial genes (Andoffatto et al., 2003) and in several genes found on the Z—chromosome (Putnam et al., 2007). Additionally, AF LP markers exhibit clear species diagnostic differences (Winter and Porter, 2009). There are fixed species differences in the Z-linked allozymes Pgd and th, as well as the autosomal Hk (Hagen and Scriber, 1989; 1991). Finally, the aforementioned enabler/suppressor complex of genes which facilitates the presence of dark morph females in P. glaucus populations may be considered an important genetic difference (Scriber et al., 1996a). Local adaptation between populations of each species has been observed (Ayres and Scriber, 1994; Bossart and Scriber, 1995; Scriber 1996b; 2002), indicating that in some circumstances selection pressures can be strong enough to overcome the homogenizing tendencies of migration and gene flow. However, between P glaucus and P. canadensis, historical hybridization has clearly occurred, facilitating the exchange of genomic regions and producing hybrid populations (Luebke and Scriber, 1988; Scriber, 1990a; Stump et al., 2003; Scriber and Ording, 2005, Kunte et al., In Prep). This is an interesting observation as hybridization between P glaucus and P. canadensis results in both direct fitness reductions (in part due to a Haldane effect; Hagen and Scriber, 1995) and extrinsic fitness reductions that include failure to enter diapause in areas at northern latitudes where it is impossible to complete two generations in one season (Ritland and Scriber, 1985; Hagen and Scriber, 1989), and the initiation of diapause after one generation (obligate diapause) in more southerly regions where the time spent in this stage during the warmest parts of the year incurs metabolic costs which reduce survival and fitness (e.g. fecundity, etc.; Scriber et al., 2002; Mercader and Scriber, 2008b). There are several explanations to account for incomplete reproductive isolation and continued gene flow between P. glaucus and P. canadensis. First, hybridization occurring directly between pure parental genotypes is likely rare (Donovan and Scriber, 2003). Gene flow more likely occurs along a gradient of backcrossed individuals throughout the hybrid zone from pure P glaucus south of the zone to pure P. canadensis north of the zone (Hagen, 1990). This means that direct costs to hybridization (e.g. reduced viability) are diminished and may not be strong enough to generate significant reinforcement to prevent hybridization. Furthermore, extrinsic costs to hybridization are further reduced as specific genetic regions that cause strong fitness reductions are quickly purged from populations (e.g. facultative diapause in the northern parts of the hybrid zone is ‘suicidal’; Martinsen et al., 2001; Scriber et al, 2008). Another possible explanation for the presence of continued gene flow is an asymmetrical mating preference observed in male tiger swallowtails. In two- choice mate preference assays, Deering and Scriber (2002) observed that males of both P. glaucus and P. canadensis strongly preferred to mate with P. glaucus females. This suggests that within and close to the hybrid zone, P. glaucus males may exhibit reduced inclinations to mate with P. canadensis and possibly hybrid females. However, it is also probable that male P. canadensis would readily mate with any P. glaucus females they encounter and likely hybrid females as well. There are two mveats to this suggestion. First, Deering and Scriber did not examine male mating preferences near or within the hybrid zone and as seen in other hybrid zone systems (eg. Peterson et al., 2005) mating preferences may be weaker or stronger depending on proximity to areas where hybridization occurs. Secondly, as the cues that resulted in males of both species preferring P. glaucus females are unknown (see below), we have no knowledge of how these cues may be inherited and thus how males of either species would respond to hybrids of various genetic backgrounds. Differential preference for specific phenotypes could manifest itself as complete refusal to engage a female or relatively less time/energy spent in pursuit of a female who initially rejects a male’s advances (Krebs, 1988; Aardema, pers. obs.). The interspecific dynamic in male mate preference may, in 10 part, explain continued hybridization between the two species, as well as the specific ecological and genetic characteristics of several unique hybrid populations that have been observed (Scriber and Ording, 2005, Mercader et al., 2008; Kunte et al, In Prep.). Male Mate Choice in Tiger Swallowtails Male mate choice is defined as a differential response of individual males to conspecific, reproductively mature females which vary in some quality recognizable to the male (Bonduriansky, 2001). Male mate choice is expected to arise in systems where the male contributes resources to the formation of offspring aside from sperm (Bonduriansky, 2001). Additionally, when there is a large amount of variance in female quality, males are also predicted to exhibit some degree of discrimination. When male tiger swallowtails mate, they pass a large packet of nutrients and sperm (known as a sperrnatophore) to the female. To produce this packet, males engage in a behavior called puddling, which involves the uptake of moisture and nutrients from wet substrate, dung, carrion, and other similar resources (Arms et al., 1974). Male tiger swallowtail butterflies are frequent puddlers, regularly accumulating in large numbers at high quality puddling locations (Arms et al., 1974; Scriber and Ayres, 1988; Otis et al., 2006). Male Papilio are severely limited in their reproductive ability if they are unable to intake nutrients (Lederhouse et al., 1990). The need of males to puddle in order to acquire sufficient resources to form a sperrnatophore likely limits the number of 11 successful copulations males can engage in during their lifetime (Lederhouse et al., 1990; Bissoondath and Wiklund, 1996; Lewis and Wedell, 2007). Furthermore, puddling puts males in danger from predation (Morris, 1953; Rawson and Bellinger, 1953). Finally, the first sperrnatophore of a male is significantly larger than subsequent sperrnatophores (Lederhouse et al., 1990). The sperrnatophore represents a significant contribution of time, energy, and resources (Arms of al., 1974). As encountered females will vary in the number of eggs they have available (both absolutely and due to previous oviposition), it is perhaps not surprising that males exhibit mating preferences. Examinations of spennatophore number have revealed that in most circumstances a single mating is likely sufficient to fertilize the entirety of a female’s eggs (Levin, 1973; Lederhouse and Scriber, 1987). However, when initial spennatophores are small, subsequent matings may be necessary to ensure all eggs are fertilized (Lederhouse and Scriber, 1987). Additional matings may also provide important nutrients for female somatic maintenance. Accordingly, females frequently solicit male matings (Krebs, 1988). . The high cost of a spennatophore creates a strong impetus for males to exhibit some degree of female mate choice. Furthermore, mating studies also lend credence to the assertion that males exhibit mate choice and preferences (Brower, 1959; Deering and Scriber, 2002). The female characteristics that Papilio males use to select conspecific mates are presently unknown. Nor is it known whether traits being assessed in intraspecific encounters are the same responsible for the interspecific mating preferences exhibited by P. canadensis 12 males for P. glaucus females. However, it is reasonable to suspect that the trait (or traits) used by males to judge females in intraspecific encounters are the same that generate asymmetrical, interspecies preferences and that this trait generally exists in a more ‘preferred’ state in P. glaucus females. Research Objectives Given the asymmetrical mating preferences exhibited by male tiger swallowtail butterflies, l postulate that a single morphological trait or small number of traits differs between P. glaucus and P. canadensis and is recognizable and distinguishable by male tiger swallowtails. This trait is likely female limited. I also predict that variation in some quality of this trait will result in a greater or less likelihood of males attempting to copulate. This trait may either be directly correlated with fitness, or may simply be a mate recognition cue that allows males to recognize females of their own species (or a species that is very similar in appearance). In this case, interspecific preferences may be the result of this trait eliciting a stronger mate recognition response. 13 CHAPTER 2: VARIATION IN WING COLORATION BETWEEN PAPILIO CANADENSIS AND P. GLAUCUS POPULATIONS Introduction In the context of evolutionary processes, large amounts of variation in morphological characteristics exist and persist for several different reasons. One explanation for the maintenance of such variation is an absence of consistent, strong stabilizing selection on a trait (Heegaard, 1997; Kotanen and Bergelson, 2000). Selection that acts in a relatively regular fashion will generally narrow a trait’s phenotypic variance by reducing genetic variation contained within individuals that are less fit than population conspecifics (Kaufman et al., 1977). Conversely, an absence of some regular selection permits a trait to vary to the extent genetic variability allows (Hunt at al., 2007). Environmental influences, polygenic effects, and differences in gene regulation can all increase phenotypic variation in a trait (T urelli and Barton, 2004). Another cause of extensive morphological variation is the presence of multiple, divergent selection pressures that act on a trait, preventing stabilization and the reduction of variance (Grant, 1986). This may happen within a population, causing the variance of a trait to remain broad and preventing the narrowing of allelic diversity (Saint-Laurent et al., 2003, Yeaman and Jarvis, 2006), or it may be that specific pressures differ in nature and intensity between populations, creating local adaptation with limited variation within specific areas, but wide variation within the species as a whole (Linhart and Grant, 1996). 14 Depending on their extent, migration and gene flow will impact tendencies for local adaptation substantially (Yeaman and Jarvis, 2006). Tiger swallowtails in the eastern USA are known to vary in many ecological and morphological traits within populations (Luebke et al., 1988; Hagen, 1990; Mercader and Scriber, 2007; Mercader et al., 2009), between difl‘erent populations of the same species (Hagen, 1990; Scriber, 1996b; 2002; Kunte et al., In Prep.), and between different species (Scriber at al., 1989; Stump et al., 2003; Kunte et al., In Prep.). Much of this variation is linked to specific climatic conditions in the respective geographical ranges of these species and populations. Climate and temperature influences variation either directly due to selection created by dilferent thermal tolerances (Mercader and Scriber, 2008b), or else indirectly as mediated through host-plant ranges and phenologies (Scriber and Ording, 2005). This selection is particularly important for maintaining the species boundary between P. canadensis and P. glaucus, mostly through a post-zygotic reduction in survival of hybrid offspring outside the hybrid zone. Male and female mating preferences may also have importance for maintaining these species’ identities in addition to influencing hybrid zone introgression patterns (Stump et al., 2003; Scriber and Ording, 2005; Kunte et al., In Prep). Variable traits that are important for either mate recognition and/or mate preference either within or between these species remain unknown, although evidence suggests this may be another important and possibly non- thennally linked area of variation (Deering and Scriber, 2002). Male mating 15 preferences are particularly intriguing as tiger swallowtail males have been shown to exhibit choice for both conspecifics (Brower, 1959; Deering and Scriber, 2002) and heterospecifics (Deering and Scriber, 2002), and also for females that appear ‘young’ versus those that appear ‘old’ (as indicated by scale loss; Scriber and Aardema, unpublished data). Furthermore, males will reject potential conspecific mates outright (Krebs, 1988; Aardema, pers. obs.), possible because they appear older and are likely to have less eggs or possibly because of some perception of genetic inferiority. This further indicates that male mate preferences may be a fruitful area of investigation for understanding potential pre-zygotic isolating mechanisms between tiger swallowtail butterflies. The color vision of butterflies in the genus Papilio has a wide spectral range that extends down into the UV (300 - 400 nm) and up into the infrared (700 — 800 nm) (Briscoe, 2000). Additionally, these butterflies may be among the best of any organism at discriminating between similar shades of color (Koshitaka et al., 2008). Visual assessment of females by males is common in many Lepidopteran species (Bonduriansky, 2001). These facts suggest that a visual cue or cues may be important in tiger swallowtail mate recognition and/or mate assessment. In particular, a consistent difference in some component of coloration between P. canadensis and P. glaucus females may explain previously observed asymmetrical male mating preferences (Deering and Scriber, 2002). The extent of dorsal blue coloration has been noted to appear qualitatively different in the two species, with P. glaucus females seeming to have substantially more of their wing surface covered by blue scaling (Deering and 16 Scriber, 2002). Additionally, although both males and females have some dorsal blue scaling, extensive blue coloration appears limited to females (Scriber, 1990b; Aardema, pers. obs.). Both female color morphs of P. glaucus have this coloration, making it a potential 'common recognition characteristic' that may aid males in recognizing both color morphs as conspecific females (Platt et al., 1984; Hereau and Scriber, 2002). In populations where both color morphs are present, individual males do recognize females of each morph as potential mates and will attempt to copulate with individuals of both colors (Aardema, pers. obs.). To locate females, males generally patrol forest edges along streams, roadways, power cuts and other, similar linear paths. They fly above nectar sources along these paths looking for conspecifics. It is believed that yellow coloration and movement provide initial recognition cues, as males will engage both males and females when first encountered, generally approaching from above and behind (Aardema, pers. obs.). After initial contact, males usually recognize other males and break off contact quickly. Conversely, if a female is encountered the male will pursue her until she either accepts his attempts to copulate or else loses him via rapid flight through the undergrowth (Deering and Scriber, 2002). Although some subtly differences in this behavior between P. canadensis and P. glaucus males have been noted (Deering and Scriber, 2002), general patterns of mate location are similar between the two species. This type of mate lomtion strategy suggests that a visually-based, dorsally located, gender-specific cue such as blue coloration is highly likely to aid males in locating and recognizing females. Afemale with reduced blue coloration may 17 appear ‘male-like’ and receive reduced copulation attempts (and a potentially corresponding reduced fitness) relative to females with more extensive blue. Additionally, as the sperrnatophore is a valuable resource and males seemingly exhibit some degree of mate selection, it is possible that this blue coloration may be linked to female fitness in some presently unknown way. To determine whether the relative amount of blue coloration on the dorsal wing surface differs between female P. glaucus and P. canadensis (independent of overall size), I quantified the relative extent of blue coloration in females and males of both species on both the ventral and dorsal side of the wings. Based on my qualitative observations, I predicted that the difference between males and females of both species would be significant on the dorsal side of the wings, but not the ventral side. I also postulated that female P. glaucus and P canadensis will be significantly different in dorsal coloration. I predicted that in no possible comparison (between sexes or species), would ventral blue coloration differ significantly. Additionally, because ultraviolet color is important in many lepidopteran systems for mate recognition and selection (Brunton and Majerus, 1995), I measured wing regions of blue, yellow and black coloration for the presence or absence of UV reflectance. Given that these Papilio butterflies can see well into the UV spectrum, I hypothesized that the blue coloration would reflect a relatively large amount of W light, the yellow would reflect a relatively small amount of UV light and the black would reflect no UV light. A finding of UV reflectance would suggest potentially fruitful avenues of future inquiry. 18 Materials and Methods Samples To analyze differences in blue coloration between populations and species of tiger swallowtail butterflies I collected pristine or near pristine (little or no wing damage and scale loss) wild females from populations in Fairbanks-North Star Co., Alaska (n=11), Charlevoix Co., Michigan (n=12), Jackson and Gallia Cos., Ohio (yellow morph: n=8, dark morph: n=9) and Levy Co., Florida (yellow morph: n=15, dark morph: n=10). I also obtained females from two additional populations, one in Bennigton 00., Vermont (n=7) and one in Habersham and Rabun Cos., Georgia (yellow morph: n=4, dark morphs: n=2). Collectively these six populations span the latitudinal range of both P glaucus and P. canadensis (Fig. 2.1). Males from each of these populations were also collected for female/male comparisons (Alaska= 10; Michigan=12; Ohio= 12; Florida= 11; Vennont= 9; Georgia= 3). Wing Preparation and Photography Using a fine bladed scissors, l carefully removed all four wings from each specimen at the wing base where it attaches to the thorax. After removal, wings were kept in glassine envelopes until photographed. I used an Olympus FE-310 (8.0 megapixels) digital camera to photograph both sides of minimally one hind wing and one forewing from each specimen (Fig. 2.2a). In some instances all four wings were in pristine condition and l was able to take two photographs, one of each wing set. I generally tried to match wings from the same side of the 19 body, but occasionally this was not possible because of damage or scale loss to two separate wings. Analysis of Relative Blue Area To analyze these photographs, I used the imaging program ImageJ (Abramoff et al., 2004) with standard tools to measure the total area of the two wings (Figure 2.2b). I then used the Threshold_Colour plugin (Landini, 2010) to remove all coloration in the image except blue and then the standard ImageJ tools to measure the area of this coloration (Figure 2.2c). For both measurements, area was calculated as the number of pixels per unit measurement. With the measurements of total wing area and blue area, I was able to calculate a relative proportion of blue coloration. This allowed me to make comparisons of blue area that were independent of the general size differences between P. glaucus and P. canadensis. When two images were taken for the same butterfly, the mean of their relative blues areas was used in analysis. Ultraviolet Examination I first assessed the presence or absence of ultraviolet coloration qualitatively by using a UV bypass filter attached to a Nikon Coolpix 990 (3.34 megapixels) digital camera. With this combination of filer and camera, I photographed six fore- and hind wing pairs from P. glaucus females in a darkened room (to reduce full spectrum light) with a circular black light for illumination (Acorn, 2002). Using this technique I could visualize areas that were W reflective and those that were UV absorbent. 20 PM?“ Figure 2.1. Locations of sample populations for blue color analysis. Gray circles represent P canadensis populations and black circles represent P glaucus populations. a.) Fairbanks-North Star Co., Alaska b.) Charlevoix Co., Michigan c.) Bennigton Co., Vermont d.) Jackson and Gallia 003., Ohio e.) Habersham and Rabun Cos., Georgia f.) Levy Co., Florida 21 Figure 2.2. Process of wing analysis to determine total wing area and area of blue coloration. a.) First, one forewing and one hindwing were photographed. This was done for both dorsal and ventral sides b.) The image was made binary and the total area was determined. A ruler in the photograph was used to determine scale. c.) Lastly, all coloration except the blue was removed and the area of this remaining coloration was determined. See text for more details. 22 I also measured the spectral reflectance signature of blue, yellow and black wing areas using an Ocean Optics $2000 miniature fiber optic spectrophotometer. I out three roughly 1 cm2 squares of wing that had a single solid color (yellow, black or blue) from the pristine wings of four Levy Co., Florida yellow females. Reflectance of color was measured in comparison to a white standard. Data Analysis All statistical analysis was performed using the statistical package R (Abramoff et al., 2004). I made overall mean comparisons between male P canadensis, female P canadensis, male P glaucus, and yellow- and dark morph female P glaucus for the relative blue area of both the dorsal and ventral sides of the wing. To do this I used either a standard one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) when variances were homogenous (as determined by Levene’s test; Levene, 1960), or, when variances were unequal, I used Welch’s ANOVA (Welch, 1951). I also performed ANOVAs comparing males and females of individual populations, again for both dorsal and ventral relative blue wing area. I performed comparisons for each of the populations from the above analyses using pairwise t tests with adjusted p values using the Bonferroni step-down correction method develop by Holm (Holm, 1979). This correction gives control over the family wise error, but is less conservative than the simple Bonferroni correction. Finding a statistical difference for relative blue area in species comparisons, I performed a one-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) 23 comparing the total wing area and the area of blue with species as a co-factor to determine if observed differences in blue were an artifact of size difl‘erences between P canadensis and P glaucus or rather were indicative of a fundamental allometric difference between the two species. This would be indicated by statistically significant differences in either the slope and/or y-intercept. I compared three models: a simple model without species as a cofactor, an additive model with species as a cofactor and a multiplicative model, again with F species as a cofactor. Models were compared using adjusted coefficient of I determination (Adj. R2), the Akaike information criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1978) and the Schwarz Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Schwarz, 1978). For all statistical tests, an alpha value of 0.05 was considered significant. No statistical tests were performed for ultraviolet coloration, but rather a qualitative assessment was made. Results For all comparisons, variances were homogenous except for the dorsal comparison of relative blue area between species/sex results. There was a significant difference in dorsal blue area [F(4,57.11)=32.05, p <0.001]. Specifically, both yellow-morph and dark-morph P glaucus females differed significantly from female P canadensis, male P canadensis and male P glaucus (Fig. 2.3). No other sex or species comparison differed significantly. There were no significant differences in ventral relative blue area [F(4,128)=1.71, p=0.151]; (Fig. 2.3). 24 Between populations of female P canadensis and female yellow-morph P glaucus there were significant differences between the mean relative dorsal blue area [F(4,48)=23.235, p<0.001]. In concordance with the species/sex result, both P glaucus populations differed from the three P canadensis populations (Fig. 2.4). No female P canadensis populations differed significantly from one another, nor did the three P glaucus populations differ from one another. There were no significant differences in ventral blue coloration between any population of female P canadensis and female yellow-morph P glaucus [F (4,48)=1 .724, p=0.160]; (Fig. 2.4) Variance between the three populations of dark-morph female P glaucus was homogenous for dorsal blue, and the means did not differ significantly between the populations [F(2,18)=0.735,p=0.494]. Variance between the two populations for ventral blue was homogeneous and an ANOVA revealed no difference in means as well [F(2,18)=0.276,p=0.762). Surprisingly, there were differences in mean male dorsal blue between populations [F(4,49)=7.590, p<0.001]. However, unlike in females this variation did not occur in relation to species (Fig. 2.5). Males from the Vermont population (P canadensis) differed from the Alaska and Michigan populations (also P canadensis), and Ohio (P glaucus) likewise differed from Alaska and Michigan. Also surprisingly, male ventral blue coloration differed significantly between some populations 25 9.0 - g e C 8.0 1 r .9 E 7.0 a % 6.0 — O 3 5.0 - .0 ‘8 4.0 ‘ b b d “ b b b E 3.0 _ a. a. ,C i a.c a. ,0 ac a, ,C C re 2 0 l o . - - .5, l j 19 1.0 « I 1 0 " "' I m 0.0 I'I' n" n n Pc Female Pg Female Pg Female Pc Male Pg Male (YW) (DK) Figure 2.3. Mean relative blue area i SD for all species/sex/color morph combinations. Pc : P canadensis, Pg : P glaucus, YW : yellow morph females and DK : dark morph females. For each combination, the light grey bar shows the mean blue coloration on the dorsal wing surface and the dark grey bar shows the mean blue coloration on the ventral wing surface. Bars with the same letter are not significantly different at the p<0.05 level (pairwise t test with Holm’s correction). 26 4.0 SQ? c.e e v c,e c: s I l E 3'0 ' a.c a.e.g l a.e,g '3 b,c,d.g,f a,f 0 . 3 d _ , a. a 2.0 ab a'd , “5 ‘ a as I I 9 I I to 0 1.0 g . .9 0 CE 00 n= n= n3 Alaska Vermont Michigan Ohio Georgia Florida PC PC Pc Pg Pg Pg Figure 2.4. Mean relative blue area :I: SD for all female P canadensis (Pc) and yellow morph P glaucus (Pg). For each combination, the light grey bar shows the mean blue coloration on the dorsal wing surface and the dark grey bar shows the mean blue coloration on the ventral wing surface. Bars with the same letter are not significantly different at the p<0.05 level (painrvise t test with Holm’s correction). 27 3.0 ‘ (1.9 b,e.f,g.h b,e,f,g.i b.c.d a,c,g a,d,h,i 2.0 ' a.f 1.0 ‘ Relative area of blue coloration (%) ‘0 I'Ii D II- 0.0 Alaska Vermont Michigan Ohio Georgia Florida Pc P0 P0 Pg Pg Pg Figure 2.5. Mean relative blue area 1 SD for all male P canadensis (Pc) and P glaucus (Pg). For each combination, the light grey bar shows the mean blue coloration on the dorsal wing surface and the dark grey bar shows the mean blue coloration on the ventral wing surface. Bars with the same letter are not significantly different at the p<0.05 level (pairwise t test with Holm’s correction). 28 [F(4,49)=3.840, p=0.009]. Again, this cannot be generalized to species differences (Fig. 2.5). I performed an analysis of covariance between total wing area and area of blue coloration for female P canadensis and yellow-morph P glaucus dorsal wing blue as species-level differences were significantly different (Table 2.7). An F test showed that variance within the two species was not homogenous, so both blue and total area were log transformed to normalize the data. I compared the regression slopes for a simple model without species as a cofactor, an additive model with species as a cofactor and a multiplicative model, again with species as a cofactor using R2, AIC, and BIC (Table 2.10). The model with the greatest predictive value appears to be the additive model that incorporated population adjustments to the intercept. This suggests that within both female P glaucus and P canadensis, absolute blue area increases in a similar fashion (statistically equivalent slopes) from smaller to larger individuals, but that between females of the same size, P glaucus females have fundamentally more absolute dorsal blue coloration (Fig. 2.6). Ultraviolet photography of dorsal wing coloration revealed clear areas that reflect UV and areas that appear to be entirely UV absorbent (Fig 2.7). Reflectance appeared to occur primarily in the blue regions of the wing. The spectral analysis revealed a strong UV signature for blue coloration (Fig 2.8), but virtually no UV reflectance for yellow or black coloration. Discussion 29 Yarn _ V .. . I examined differences in blue coloration between male and female tiger swallowtail butterflies and between the sister species P glaucus and P canadensis. As predicted, I found that males and females of P glaucus differed significantly in the relative amount of blue on their dorsal wing surface, but not their ventral. However, I also predicted that female and male P canadensis would differ significantly in their dorsal wing coloration, but this proved to be incorrect. Neither the dorsal nor the ventral amount of relative blue coloration differed significantly between the sexes in P canadensis. Females of each species differed from one another in their relative amount of dorsal blue coloration, but not their ventral. P glaucus females had significantly more relative blue coloration than did P canadensis females. Furthermore, regression analysis suggests that the area of blue coloration increases in a similar manner between P canadensis and P glaucus, but that P glaucus has more blue on an absolute scale, irrespective of overall wing size. Unexpectedly, there were significant differences between male populations for both dorsal and ventral relative blue coloration, but these were not species specific. Finally, I found that blue coloration in general is UV reflective, whereas other coloration is not. These observations lend support to my hypothesis that female-limited, dorsal coloration has importance for male mate recognition and possibly mate preferences. If males use the extent of blue coloration as a cue to differentiate between males and females than more blue coloration may result in a greater ‘preference’ for a particular female when multiple females are present. With 30 Table 2.1. Results from analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) for a comparison between female P canadensis and yellow morph P glaucus for the total area of the wings and the area of blue coloration with species as a cofactor. The data was l091o transformed to homogenize variance between the two species. The additive model appears to have the greatest predictive value and suggests that the two species increase in blue area in similar fashion with increased overall size (statistically equivalent slopes), but that P glaucus females have more blue on an absolute scale. Bolded numbers represent the values for the preferred model in the three different analyses (Adj. R2, AIC and BIC). See text for more details. . . Ad'. . Model Specres Model Equation R! AIC BIC . (log1oblue area) = 2.104 * S I NA - .7 8 -210.9 -211.4 Impe (log1ototal_area) - 3.123 O 6 P. (Iog1oblue_area) = 1.043 * canadensis (10910 total_area) - 1.951 Additive 0.836 -229.2 -182.6 log1oblue area) = 1.043 * P. I I — g aucus (log1o total_area) - 1 .636 P. (log1oblue_area) = 1.289 * canadensis (10910 total_area) - 2.231 Multiplicitive 0.835 -228.0 -159.9 P. glaucus (Iog1oblue_area) = 0.842 (log1o total_area) - 1.362 31 A P glaucus Log10 of blue area e P. canadensis 10 1'5 20 25 30 Log10 of total wing area Figure 2.6. Log transformed comparison of absolute total wing area and area of blue coloration for P canadensis and yellow morph, P glaucus females. Area values are in cmz. Regression lines are drawn using the additive model (See table 2.10). The slopes are not significantly different. See text for more details. 32 regard to previous interspec'rfic preference comparisons (Deering and Scriber, 2002), my results suggest that more relative blue coloration may explain why males of both P glaucus and P canadensis preferred to mate with female P glaucus over similar sized P canadensis females. Although it is common to find multiple females feeding in close proximity to one another (Aardema, pers. obs.), most encounters likely involve single males and females. In these instances, the amount of dorsal blue may influence a male's initial inclinations to engage the female or the amount of effort given to pursuit if it occurs. Differences in blue coloration may also influence the amount of nutrients and sperm a male passes to the female in his sperrnatophore (Bonduriansky, 2001; Xu and Wang, 2009). It is not presently known whether male tiger swallowtails vary the size of their spennatophore based on a 'quality' evaluation of their mate. Although some unusual and extreme patterns in male blue coloration have been observed (Scriber, 1990b), more general variation in male blue coloration was unexpected. Perhaps even more surprisingly, this variation does not correspond to the two different species. It seems that deviation in blue coloration within the Michigan population explains most observations of difference. Michigan had a relatively small amount of blue both dorsally and ventrally compared to the majority of the other populations. It is presently unknown what factors might result in this population having less blue (or conversely, other populations having more blue). Given the observed patterns of difference in blue coloration between the species and the sexes, mate recognition and/or preferences remain very likely candidates for selection pressures influencing the amount of blue on the wings, 33 Figure 2.7. An example of my photographic examination of ultraviolet coloration in tiger swallowtail butterflies. a.) The visual image of the dorsal side of a yellow morph (left) and dark morph (right), female P glaucus. b.) This image was taking using a UV bypass filter. The wings displayed are the same as those in ‘a.’ Areas that reflected UV light are white and areas that absorbed UV light are black. See text for more details. 35% 30% 8 o : 25/o g 20% .43 15% (D a: 10% °\° 5% 0% 30 400 500 600 Yellow .5... Blue — Black q Wavelength (nm) Figure 2.8. An example of the reflectance spectrum patterns of blue, yellow and black coloration from one yellow morph, P glaucus female. The other three individuals assessed in this manner had similar spectral patterns. This figure shows a clear UV signature (300-400 nm) for blue coloration, and virtually no UV reflectance for yellow and black coloration. See text for more details. 35 especially for females. However, it is easy to speculate that amount of blue coloration may have multiple selection factors acting on it, some of which may be antagonistic to one another. One pressure that may work to reduce blue coloration is avian predation. Birds have visual capabilities that extend into the UV spectrum and it has been shown that Lepidoptera with extensive ultraviolet coloration may be at a higher risk of attack relative to Lepidoptera that have little UV coloration (Church at al., 1998; Majerus et al., 2000, Lyytinen at al., 2004). It is presently unknown how predation pressure would act to reduce blue coloration in males and females, and given differences in daily behavioral activities (eg. puddling in males versus ovipositing in females), it is possible that selection on the two sexes by avian predators differs. Another influence on blue coloration may be temperature and a need for therrnoregulation. Most ultraviolet coloration in Iepidopterans is structurally based (Ghiradella, 1991; Prum at al., 2006) and such structural color has been shown to significantly increase absorbance of solar radiation (Bosi et al., 2008). However, two lines of evidence suggest this is unlikely to be a large selective force on blue coloration. First, the populations that would be predicted to have the greatest need for increased solar absorbance would be those located in more northerly localities. However, these populations have the least amount of blue coloration. Secondly, it is only the basal one-third of the wing that strongly influences thoracic and abdominal temperature (Douglas, 1981). This means 36 that most blue coloration in both males and females is unlikely to strongly affect an individual's body temperature. Yet another influence on the extent of blue coloration may be sexual harassment by other butterflies either of the same species or different species. Although polyandrous, female tiger swallowtails will in some circumstances refuse male mating attempts, likely based on whether they have recently mated and possibly due to perceived quality of the male. While females feed, they are often subjected to harassment by males (Lederhouse, 1995; Deering and Scriber, 2002; Aardema, pers. obs.). This type of intraspecific interaction has been shown to have negative fitness consequences for other insect species (Svensson et al., 2005; Takahashi and Watanabe, 2010). If males utilize blue as a mate recognition cue, than male harassment would likely oppose pressures acting to increase blue coloration due to a disproportionally greater amount of harassment directed towards females that have more extensive blue. Additionally, in some populations of tiger swallowtail, interspecific harassment occurs from males of other butterfly species (Deering and Scriber, 1998; Aardema, pers. obs.). Like intraspecific harassment, interspecific harassment likely has negative fitness consequences on females (McLain and Pratt, 1999; Nielsen and Watt, 2000). However, this pressure could act to either increase or decrease blue coloration depending on what recognition cues the males of other species react to. Furthermore, interspecific sexual harassment could have negative impacts on males in addition to females (Groning and Hochkirch, 2008). 37 Although I have shown that dorsal blue coloration is significantly different between P glaucus males and females, and between female P canadensis and P glaucus, its utility for mate recognition and/or mate selection is by no means a certainty. Only further examinations will elucidate whether blue coloration is actually utilized by males for the purpose of recognizing potential mates and/or judging these mate's reproductive quality. An investigation of these potential influences on tiger swallowtail blue, combined with studies of other selection factors, will help to reveal why variation in blue coloration exists and possibly what influences this variation could have on the dynamics of gene flow in the hybrid zone. Of most interest may be how pre-zygotic mating barriers are influenced by this seeming sexual characteristic, and how it contributes to divergence, reproductive isolation and adaptive radiation in the tiger swallowtail species complex. 38 CHAPTER 3: COURTSHIP AND MATING RESPONSES OF MALE PAPILIO SPP. TO VARIATION IN FEMALE-LIMITED WING COLORATION Introduction Morphological traits that are limited to one sex and appear to have no direct utility for either basic survival or fostering offspring may be suspected to have some function in attracting and obtaining a mate (Darwin, 1879). These “secondary sexual characteristics’ may function either through intra-sexual conflict or intersexual mating preferences. General and consistent differences in these traits between closely related species may be further suspected of having importance in conspecific mate recognition and preference (Panhuis et al., 2001), which may facilitate continued reproductive isolation. I have shown that the relative amount of blue coloration on the dorsal side of the wings in female P glaucus butterflies is significantly greater than in males (Chap. 2). Additionally, this blue coloration is relatively more extensive in female P glaucus than in the sister species P canadensis. Dorsal color between male and female P canadensis does not differ significantly, nor does ventral blue color between males and females of either species. Lastly, females of the two species do not differ from one another in the relative amount of ventral coloration. Males of both P canadensis and P glaucus prefer to mate with female P glaucus in two-choice assays (Deering and Scriber, 2002). Given the observed interspecific variation in relative blue coloration, it stands to reason that this trait may be important for male mate recognition. Furthermore, although it is 39 presently unknown how the extent of blue correlates with fitness, it is possible that this trait may be used in intraspecific mate selection. This would be especially true if there is a high degree of variation in the genetic quality of females (something presently unknown). In addition to the attributes of blue coloration listed above (interspecific and male/female variation), this morphological feature is W reflective (Figs. 3.1&2) and found in both color morphs of female P glaucus. It may thus act as a shared recognition characteristic between the color morphs that enables individual males to identify both colors as potential mates. - If the presence of blue coloration acts as an important mate recognition cue, and/or a potential mate selection cue, then I postulate that male tiger swallowtail butterflies of both P canadensis and P glaucus will strongly prefer to mate with females that exhibit a greater extent of blue coloration relative to females that exhibit less blue coloration. In an extreme case, I predict that when presented with females that either retain or lack dorsal blue coloration, males will predominantly choose females that have their blue preserved. Materials and Methods Butterflies My studies were conducted during the summer from 2008 to 2010. For all years, I utilized adult, virgin females, captive reared from eggs laid by wild-caught mothers collected from Bennigton Co. Vermont (VT; a P canadensis population) and Lancaster Co. Pennsylvania (PA; a P glaucus population). Although ideally I 40 would have used females reared from the same location that the studies were conducted in, the large number of females needed made this impractical, and hence it was necessary to obtain females from sources that had the capacity to rear large numbers of individuals. As my comparisons were between females reared from the same populations, I feel that the origin of my butterflies did not influence the outcome of my results. This notwithstanding, in 2008 l was able to rear virgin females from wild mothers that I collected in Jackson Co. Ohio (OH; a P glaucus population). For VT and PA females, l utilized individuals in the year following their rearing (i.e. they overwintered in diapause). The OH females were utilized in the same year in which I originally collected their mothers. Experimental Regions I conducted two-choice mate preference experiments in two P glaucus populations, one during the first seasonal flight in Levy Co. Florida (in 2009 and 2010) and during both the first (2010) and second (2008) seasonal flights in Jackson Co. Ohio. For the Florida experiments I used all females reared from PA mothers. For the 2008 Ohio experiments (conducted during the second flight), I used females reared from mothers collected during the first flight of this population. In 2010, I used females reared from PA mothers. I also conducted two-choice experiments in a single P canadensis population in Charlevoix Co., Michigan (in 2008). For this trial, I used all females reared from VT collected mothers. All three regions were the same as those where females were collected for my blue coloration analysis (Chap. 2). In some cases, collection of females 41 and the male mate—choice experiments occurred concurrently, but there is no reason to believe that this affected my results in either case. Single choice experiments were conducted in 2009 in a P canadensis population in Charlevoix Co., Michigan using VT P canadensis and in 2010 in a P glaucus population in Jackson Co, Ohio using PA P glaucus. These were the same populations used in my two-choice studies. Based on the outcome of my two-choice assays and the single-choice experiments (see results), I deemed it unnecessary to conduct single-choice assays in Florida. General Mate Choice Experimental Methods (Used in both 2- & single—choice assays) To examine how male mating preferences were influenced by the extent of blue coloration on females, I conducted choice experiments in wild populations with known high concentrations of males. To do this, I manipulated females so that they either retained their dorsal blue coloration or had it entirely concealed. I choose to do these extremes, as it was my hope that this would result in the strongest differences and I could then look at more subtle variation in future work. In the areas described above, I presented these manipulated females to wild males. My methods are modified from Brower (1959) and Deering and Scriber (2002). I first tied a fine nylon thread carefully around the thorax of fresh (emerged from the pupal stage within 10 days prior), virgin females between the front and hind wings. The other ends of these threads were tied to metal alligator clips which allowed me to attach the females to dowels that I inserted through holes in 42 wooden stakes driven into the ground. My stakes were each approximately 2 meters long. The length of the thread varied between 40 and 60 cm. I attempted to have the females hang at a height similar to that of the nectaring plants that were being utilized by the study population. Typically, this was 1 — 1.5 m above the ground. To prevent excessive tangling, a small lead weight was attached to the thread halfway between the butterfly and the alligator clip. This prevented most tangling, although females would still occasionally fly on the thread in a manner which caused the line to kink. When this occurred, I manually untangled these females. All females in this study received one of two treatments. For the experimental treatment (blue-concealed), l masked all dorsal blue coloration using ink from a fine tipped black sharpie brand marker. For the control treatment (blue-retained) I covered up black areas of the wing with this same black ink, but left all dorsal blue coloration visible. The area of coverage in both treatments was approximately equal. This black ink effectively concealed both the visual and UV spectral components of the blue coloration and appeared very similar spectrally to natural areas of black coloration (Fig. 3.1). l assigned females to one of these two treatments haphazardly without first looking at the extent of her dorsal blue coloration. In most cases, I engaged in two tethering sessions each day, one in the morning and one in late afternoon. My previous observations indicated that these were the times when males would be most likely to initiate copulation. Tethering sessions typically lasted two hours. No female was ever left out for 43 longer than three hours. In between tethering sessions, females were kept in a cooler with ice. Two—choice Assays Two—choice male mate preference trials consisted of preparing pairs of , females in the manner described above. Within each pairing, the females were of approximately the same size as measured by forewing length from base to tip (1 1 mm). For the two—choice assay the wooden dowel was placed through the top of the stake so that equal halves stuck out horizontally in opposite directions. This allowed me to tether females approximately one meter apart. The stakes were placed a minimum of eight meters from one another either along roads that were being used as flyways (Ohio and Florida) or else scattered about a meadow where there were abundant nectar resources (Michigan). Males flying by these females would engage them if interested. Typically this involved repeatedly circling of the female on her tether. Upon contact with a male the female would generally become enlivened and flutter rapidly at the end of the string, spiraling in tight circles. At this point the male would either land on the female and initiate copulation or in some cases fly off. In many instances the male would engage both females of a pair before copulation was initiated. While tethered, I checked each pair at least once every five minutes. After I observed a mating being initiated or else found a copulation in progress, I would gently remove the male from the female. In most cases, I retained males for future analysis as part of a separate study. Othenrvise, I released males back into Blue Coloration 20% 8 __ Blue Coloration Covered g by Black Ink 3 10% — Black Coloration 0) c: °\° ‘KIALIWNJVWM #1! 0% 2. ~ +95 .2 - - ~—~ 300 350 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 750 800 180/ Wavelength (nm) 0 0) U c (0 1'3 g 9% (U o: °\° 0%. :‘ , 2,- Mm, ___r 300 350 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 750 800 Wavelength (nm) 25% 0) U c to 4.: u .3). H.— 0) r: o\° “WW 0% . ~ 300 350 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 750 800 Wavelength (nm) Figure 3.1. Three examples of female P glaucus dorsal colors showing the spectral reflectance signature of blue coloration, black coloration and blue coloration covered by black ink from a sharpie brand marker. UV coloration is that which occurs between 300 and 400 nm. Black ink effectively conceals both the UV and visual elements of the blue coloration. 45 the population after they had been marked with a unique number using a red sharpie and the 1,2,4,7 numbering system developed by Ehrlich and Davidson (1960). Most marked males were not seen again, but in a few cases the marked males would return and attempt to mate with a different female in another pair or occasionally the same female. I did not include any pairing with the same male and female twice. I did, however, include multiple mate choices by the same male for different females. These second choices account for four of my total number of pairings. After each copulation, I switched the positions of the two females on the tethering pole. There were no more than three total copulations with any collective pair during a single tethering session. Single-choice Assays It has been suggested that choice experiments such as my two-choice assay may lead to erroneous conclusions as an element of one of the females may elicit courtship behavior in males, and after this‘occurs no further preference is exhibited (Trnbergen, 1948). It is possible that the female that retained her blue coloration compelled males to engage in mating behaviors and the female he inevitable ended up with was not indicative of a choice. To account for this possibility, I also carried out single-choice trials where treated females would be available to males without a female exhibiting the opposite treatment in close proximity. For the single-choice assay, l tethered females singularly to dowels in a fashion similar to that used in the two-choice assay except that only one female 46 was present rather than two. At all times during the trials there were equal numbers of blue-concealed and blue-retained females available in the study area. These females were not sized matched between the treatments. The total number of individuals I tethered out during a given session varied from 6 to 14, depending on availability. I kept single females at a minimum distance of eight meters apart. Treated females were assigned to poles haphazardly and when a male initiated copulation with a female, I exchanged her for a female of the opposite treatment. Statistical Analysis I performed all analysis with the statistical package R (Abramoff et al., 2004). To assess whether the number of males that preferred females with blue retained was significantly greater than the number of males which preferred females with blue concealed, I used chi-square tests to compare the number of matings for each treatment to the expected null-distribution of 0.5, which would indicate random mating. I assessed each population in this manner individually, with dark and yellow morphs separate and combined. Additionally, I combined all observations for each treatment together. When the same experiment was conducted in multiple years, I combined observations for my analysis. I did not analyze years separately as samples sizes were generally small. Finally, in cases where my sample sizes were too low (expected values <5.0), I did not assess the data statistically. Results 47 Two-Choice Trials When all two-choice pairings were totaled, there were 34 male copulations with females of both species and color morphs that had their blue-retained, and 44 copulations with blue-concealed (T able 3.1). This overall result is not significant, x2(1. N = 78) = 0.26, p =0.61. In the Florida population (P glaucus), I observed 5 copulations with blue- retained, yellow-morph females, and 7 pairings with blue-concealed, yellow- morph females. This difference was not significant, x2(1, N = 12) = 0.56, p =0.45. For dark-morph females, I observed 5 pairings with blue-retained and 8 with blue- reduced. This difference was also not significant, x2(1. N = 13) = 0.41, p =0.52. When I compare all the pairings of both color-morphs (10 blue-retained, 15 blue- concealed), the result was not significant, x2(1, N = 25) = 0.32, p =0.57. Within the Ohio population (P glaucus), there were 6 mating observations with yellow-morph females with blue-retained and 5 with blue-reduced. This difference was not significant, x2(1, N = 11) = 0.76, p =0.38. For dark-morph females I observed 7 pairings for blue-retained, and 8 for blue-reduced. Again, this difference was not significant, x2(1, N = 15) = 0.80, p =0.37. A statistical comparison of all the pairings of both color-morphs (13 blue-retained, 13 blue- concealed) was unnecessary as the number of pairings for each treatment was identical. In the Michigan population (P canadensis), I obtained 11 pairings with females that retained their blue and 16 pairings with females that had their blue 48 Table 3.1. Number of two-choice pairings for each population examined. When male preferences were examined over multiple years for the same population, results are combined. No difference was significant (see text for values) TOTAL BOTH ALL 78 Female Total Number of Number of Color Number Pairings Pairings Population Species Morph of with Blue- with Blue- (P glaucus Pairin s Retained Concealed only) 9 Female Female . Yellow 12 Florida P glaucus Dark 13 Yellow 11 6 5 Ohio P glaucus Dark 15 7 8 Michigan R NA 27 11 16 canadensis 49 concealed. This was not a significant difference, x2(1, N = 27) = 0.34, p =0.56. Within the two P glaucus populations, I compared the observations for all dark- morph and yellow-morph females. For the yellow, there were 11 observations of pairing with blue-retained females, and 12 observations with blue-reduced females. This was not a significant difference, x2(1, N = 23) = 0.83, p =0.36. I obtained 12 total pairings with blue-retained, dark-morph females, and 16 pairings with blue-reduced, dark-morph females. Again, this was not a significant result, x20. N = 28) = 0.45, p =0.50. One-Choice Trials Combining all one-choice pairing observations from both Michigan and Ohio, I observed 16 pairings with females that had their blue retained and 21 pairings with females that had their blue reduced (Table 3.2). This difference was not significant, x2(1, N = 37) = 0.41, p =0.52. My Ohio samples for both yellow and dark morph females were too small to compare using the chi-square test, but for yellow-morphs, there were 2 pairings with blue-retained females, and 2 pairings with blue-reduced females. For dark-morph females, there were again 2 pairings with blue-retained, and 3 with blue reduced. Examining just the Michigan population, I observed 11 pairings with females that retained their blue and 17 pairings with females that had their blue reduced. This result was not significant, x20. N = 28) = 0.26, p =0.61. 50 Table 3.2. Number of one-choice pairings for each population examined. When male preferences were examined over multiple years for the same population, results are combined. No difference was significant (see text for values). Number of Number of :30; "I???" Pairings Pairings Population Species (P Iariicus of with Blue- with Blue- ' g . . Retained Concealed only) Pairings Female Female , Yellow 2 Ohio P glaucus Dark 5 Michigan R NA 28 11 17 canadensis TOTAL BOTH ALL 37 16 21 51 Discussion I examined male mate recognition and preference for females with either their dorsal blue coloration retained or concealed in one- and two-choice trails. My results suggest that an absence or reduction of dorsal blue coloration on a female does not reduce a male’s willingness to engage in copulation with her. Males readily mated with females that had their entire dorsal blue coloration concealed. This result was surprising as several lines of indirect evidence suggest that blue coloration may be important for male mate recognition and possibly mate preference. Although not significant, there may actually have been a slight trend for males to prefer females that had their blue concealed. In all but two comparisons (both in Ohio, 'all pairings' and 'yellow-morph, P glaucus females'), the number of pairings was always greater for females with blue-concealed than for females with blue-retained. I attribute this to my low sample sizes and believe that an increase in the number of pairings would eliminate this slight trend. Nonetheless, it is possible that a significant preference for blue-concealed females would be seen with additional pairings. However, given the original secondary evidence that initially lead to this investigation, it is unlikely that this would be the case as this would predict selection pressure for a reduction in the extent of blue coloration. The continued presence of a large amount of female-limited blue would, in turn, suggest a strong counter agent selecting for the retention of blue coloration. Given my results, no such agent is presently known. 52 One problem with focusing on a single trait (such as blue coloration) is that in most systems male mate recognition and preferences are likely to be based on several concurrent and possibly interacting traits (Bonduriansky, 2001). The most important factor influencing insect male mate choice is generally the fecundity of the female or the number of eggs she has available. Genetic quality is almost certainly second in terms of selection importance (Bonduriansky, 2001). If blue were an important characteristic used in mate preference, than it would likely advertise this second component of fitness, and thus be less'important than indicators of fecundity such as body size. In the context of my experiments, the absence of blue coloration (if it had any influence), may have been overshadowed by the unmated state of my treated females and their correspondingly large abdomens. Likewise, pheromonal cues could also have played a role in male mate selection. Olfactory cues have an important role in a vast number of Lepidopteran systems where male mate selection is observed (Bonduriansky, 2001), and may be the most common form of mate assessment. Although previous pheromonal analysis conducted within the tiger swallowtail system suggests that pheromones have limited influence on males (Deering, 1998), this research did not rule out close-range or contact olfactory influences on mate selection. In general, sexual characteristics which would advertise fecundity are likely to be redundant and the resources spent on them better applied to increasing the number or success of offspring (Fitzpatrick et al., 1995). This 53 notwithstanding, when the number of matings a male can achieve is limited (independent of access to mates) such as is the case with tiger swallowtails, than female genetic quality can significantly impact a male's lifetime fitness (Bonduriansky, 2001). For instance, individuals may differ in their ability to utilize locally common hosts, and this may be reflected in morphological characteristics. If a male can recognize this, than it becomes beneficial to mate with females that exhibit traits which indicate they use hosts well. Most likely, a combination of visual, olfactory, and tactile cues combine in some sequence to create an overall impression of a female to males (Bonduriansky, 2001). If one of these elements is removed, the high favorability of others may be enough to overcome this deficiency. In the end, the presence of such multiple traits may have lead to an apparent lack of importance for blue coloration in this system. Male ‘choice’ may also be expressed cryptically as variation in the amount of sperm and/or nutrients a male passes on to a particular female (Bonduriansky, 2001). It is possible that male tiger swallowtails may have exhibited this type of mate choice in response to variation in the presence of blue coloration. Rather than classification as a secondary-sexual characteristic, female limited blue coloration may be better explained in the context of the Battus philenor mimicry complex, of which dark-morph females are considered a part of (Brower, 1958). B. philenor males have extensive blue coloration on their dorsal hind wings, and are a model for several mimetic species. Mimicking this coloration likely provides some level of protection from predators, and accordingly many edible species whose range is congruent have very similar coloration (Brower and Brower, 1962; Mullen et al., 2008). The underlying genetics which result in differential dorsal blue coloration are unknown although it would seem that the genetic regions that contribute to the generation of extensive female blue coloration are separate from the regions that confer dark or yellow morph coloration respectively, as both morphs clearly have extensive blue independent of their base coloration. It is possible that there is strong selection on dark-morph females to have large amounts of blue coloration and that this carries over into yellow-morph females. Likewise, this trait may also either be retained in P canadensis populations from its progenitor or else maintained by contemporary or historically recent introgression. Evidence from microsatellites and other traits that do not have strong divergent selection on them indicate that gene flow on such regions across the hybrid zone is high (Nowak et al., 2009). Selection for a large amount of blue coloration in dark- morph females may hence cany over into both yellow morph females and also into P canadensis populations. This alternative hypothesis is not without problems. The blue coloration in tiger swallowtails is strongly W reflective whereas the blue coloration of B. philenor is UV absorbent (Figure 3.2). Most birds have visual acuity which extends well into the ultraviolet range (Chen et al., 1984). This suggests that differences in UV reflectance between B. philenor and dark-morph, female P glaucus may be distinguishable to birds and reduce the effectiveness of this mimicry coloration. Furthermore, ultraviolet coloration in diurnal lepidoptera has 55 been shown to increase predation risk (Cuthill and Bennett, 1993; Majerus et al., 2000; Lyytinen et al., 2004). Hence this UV coloration may make yellow-morph females more conspicuous to predators relative to the blue-reduced coloration of males. It is clear that additional experimentation is needed to determine both why female tiger swallowtails retain a greater amount of dorsal blue coloration than males, as well as what traits males use in mate selection (other than blue) that resulted in the asymmetrical male matings preferences for P glaucus females. 56 CHAPTER 4: POSSIBLE ALTERNATIVE EXPLANATIONS FOR ASYMMETRICAL MALE MATING PREFERENCES IN PAPILIO SPP. Introduction In tiger swallowtail butterflies, a large amount of secondary evidence suggests that dorsal blue coloration is an important cue for mate recognition and possibly mate selection. This evidence includes three observations: 1) dorsal blue coloration is significantly most extensive in female P glaucus relative to both male P glaucus and all P canadensis (see chapter 2), 2) blue is likely highly visible to these butterflies due to substantial ultraviolet reflectance (Briscoe, 2000; chapter 2) and 3) this blue coloration may be a shared characteristic between the otherwise visually very divergent dark and yellow morph females that occur in some P glaucus populations (see chapter 2). Combined with repeated demonstrations that males exhibit mate preferences (Bower, 1959; Deering and Scriber, 2002, Aardema and Scriber, unpublished data), this evidence suggests that blue coloration may be important in mate recognition and possibly mate choice in these butterflies. However, as I have shown (Chapter 3), blue coloration in females is unlikely to have a significant on male mate choice in these tiger swallowtail populations. Experimental alteration of this trait did not elicit any differential mating response from wild male populations of P canadensis in Michigan or P glaucus in Ohio and Florida. 57 Two questions arise from this outcome. First, if blue coloration is not important in mating preferences, does it have other biological significance? l addressed this question in chapter three, postulating that the one explanation for patterns of variation in blue coloration may be its utility to dark morph P glaucus females for mimicking the toxic, pipevine swallowtail, Battus philenor. However, this use may be conflicted by the UV component of the blue scaling and the affect this has on avian predation frequencies. Needless to say, uncertainties remain. The second question concerns the traits males are using to choose mates. Put another way, what else varies between P canadensis and P glaucus females that may explain why P canadensis males strongly prefer P glaucus females? (Deering and Scriber, 2002). The list of possible traits may be long, but the most likely candidates are differences in abdomen size and variation in yellow coloration. Larger abdomens are a very common trait used by males to select females in other insect systems due to their direct correlation with fecundity (Bonduriansky, 2001 ). Female yellow variation is an important part of mate recognition and preferences in other butterfly systems (Lederhouse and Scriber, 1996; Ellers and Boggs, 2003), and would likely be a very conspicuous cue if utilized by males for the purpose of recognizing and selecting a potential mate. I postulate that P glaucus females have a fatter (heavier) abdomen compared to P canadensis females relative to overall size as indicated by wing length. With regards to yellow coloration, it is hard to conjecture what characteristics male Papilio spp. prefer, but I hypothesized that some 58 measureable value of yellow coloration will significantly differ between P glaucus and P canadensis females, and that this difference may have influence on male preferences. Materials and Methods Abdomen Size To investigate differences in abdomen size I made use of 24 captive- reared Vermont (Bennington Co.) P canadensis and 24 captive reared Pennsylvania (Lancaster 00., n=21) or Georgia (Oglethorphe Co., n=3) P glaucus females. These butterflies were all initially reared in 2009. I kept them in diapause in 4°C over winter (2009/2010) for approximately eight months and then placed them in 23°C to emerge as adults. After emergence, these butterflies were kept refrigerated until I measured and weighed their abdomens. I first measured the length of the forewings to the nearest 1 mm from base to tip with a standard metric ruler. This is a common approximate measurement for butterfly size (see chapter 1 for more details). Next, I removed the head followed by the abdomen at the ‘waist’ where it connects to the thorax. After removal the abdomen was quickly weighed to the nearest 0.001 gram. Yellow Coloration I examined yellow coloration by measuring the spectral reflectance signature of yellow dorsal areas using an Ocean Optics s2000 miniature fiber optic spectrophotometer. I took spectral reflectance measurements from 2 -3 separate yellow areas of the forewing from 17 P canadensis females and 17 P 59 glaucus females. The minimum area for spectral measurements is approximately 0.75 cm, and hence the areas selected for measurement came from wing regions that had a solid yellow patch at least this size without any other interfering colors. Females were collected from Fairbanks-North Star 00., Alaska (n=9, P canadensis), Charlevoix Co., Michigan (n=8, P canadensis), Jackson and Gallia Cos., Ohio (n=6, P glaucus) and Levy Co., Florida (n=11, P glaucus). Reflectance of color was measured in comparison to a white standard. Using the reflectance values, I was able to calculate hue and chroma for each observation. Hue is essentially the color we recognize and consider as distinctive (e.g. “yellow”, "blue", etc.), and chroma is a measure of the saturation or amount of diffusion of this color. These values were calculated based on Endler (1990), with modifications from Sobel et al., (In Prep.). The mean of multiple measurements from single individuals was used in species and population comparisons. Statistical Analysis Abdomen size (as determined by weight), was divided by wing length to get a relative measurement of the relationship between the two values for P canadensis and P glaucus females. Using these measurements, I compared the variances using an F test (to determine homogeneity) and then Students t-test to determine it the means were statistically different for the two species. I also analyzed the covariance between wing length and abdomen weight using species as a cofactor. I compared three models: a simple model that did not take species into account, an additive model with species as a cofactor and a 60 multiplicative model, again with species as a cofactor. Models were compared using adjusted coefficient of determination (Adj. R2), the Akaike information criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1978) and the Schwarz Bayesian lnfonnation Criterion (BIC; Schwarz, 1978). Yellow coloration was compared for both hue and chroma between the two species using either Student’s t test when variances were statistically equal or the Welch two sample t test when variances were unequal. Variance was examined using an F test. Hue and chroma were also compared between populations using an analysis of variance (ANOVA), followed by Tukey’s honest significant difference (HSD) for painivise comparisons. For all analysis an alpha value < 0.05 was considered significant Results Abdomen Size Summary statistics for the relative comparison between abdomen weight and wing length are displayed in table 4.1. Variances between P canadensis and P glaucus were homogeneous. There was a statistically significant difference between these two species ['I’(46)=-4.433,p<0.001]. In contrast to the relative abdomen size comparison described above, my analysis of covariance revealed that there was not a significant effect of population for my regression models (table 4.2, fig. 4.1). The slopes and y- intercepts are statistically equivalent, and the simple model which does not take 61 Table 4.1. Summary statistics for the relative relationship between forewing length (mm) and abdomen weight (g) in female P glaucus and P canadensis. Species Observations Mean Standard DeVIation P canadensis 24 0.450 0. 11 5 P glaucus 24 0.628 0.159 62 Table 4.2. Results from analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) for a comparison between P canadensis and P glaucus females for wing length and abdomen weight with species as a cofactor. The simple model which does not take into account species appears to have the greatest predictive value and suggests that the two butterflies do not differ significantly in their basic allometric relationship between wing length and abdomen weight. Bolded numbers represent the values for the preferred model in the three different analyses (Adj. R2, AIC and BIC). See text for more details. Model Species Model Equation Ag! AIC BIC . weight = 0.016 * _ _ Simple NA wi n g_l e n gth _ 0.536 0.597 263.01 119.17 P weight = 0.015 * d ' ' I h - .4 Additive cane 9"” w'"9- engt 0 69 0.591 -261.39 -115.69 weight = 0.015 * P' g’aucus wing_length - 0.451 P weight = 0.012 * . . _ canadensis wing_length - 0.337 Multiplicative _ ht 0 017 0.588 -260.12 -112.55 weig = . * P’ glaucus wing_length - 0.580 63 A 0.45— A P. glaucus . A o P. canadensrs ‘ A t A 040- , 39 A A 4—11 ‘ A g) 0 35 ~ ‘ a .5 o A g 0 30 1 0 A C O G) O o O 0 _. 0 g 025 o A ‘C _ O O .o 020 o o < o 8 8 o ‘ A o 15 — o o o o O A 0-10 T l i i i 45 50 SS 60 Wing Length (mm) Figure 4.1. Comparison of wing length (mm) and abdominal weight (g) in P canadensis and yellow morph, P glaucus females. Regression lines are drawn using the simple model (See table 4.2). The slopes are not significantly different. See text for more details. species into account was determined to be the best based on adjusted R2, AIC and BIC values. Yellow Coloration Summary statistics for species’ hue and chroma comparisons are presented in tables 4.3 a&b, and for population comparisons in tables 4.4 a&b. The variance between P glaucus and P canadensis for hue was homogeneous and the difference was significant [T(32)=4.463,p<0.001]. For chroma however, the variances were not homogonous, and the means were not significantly different [T(19.654)=-1.217,p=0.238]. In line with the results found in my species analysis, analysis of variance showed that there was a significant difference between populations for hue [F(3,30)=9.998,p<0.001], but not for chroma [F(3,30)=0.998,p=0.407]. For hue, observed significance was the result of differences between the Florida P glaucus samples and samples from the two P canadensis populations (Alaska and Michigan). The P canadensis samples were not significantly different from the Ohio P glaucus samples (P values from Tukey’s HSD are presented in table 4.5). Discussion I examined two possible morphological differences between P canadensis and P glaucus that may explain asymmetrical male preferences for P glaucus females relative to P canadensis females. These were abdomen weight (as a proxy for ‘size’) and yellow coloration. I found that P glaucus females have 65 Table 4.3a. Summary statistics for hue value in female P glaucus and P canadensis . . Standard Species Observations Mean Deviation P canadensis 17 0.245 0.022 P glaucus 17 0.265 0.066 Table 4.3b. Summary statistics for chroma value in female P glaucus and P canadensis . . Standard Species Observations Mean Deviation P canadensis 17 1.276 0.082 Pglaucus 17 1.123 0.115 66 Table 4.4a. Summary statistics for hue value in populations of female P canadensis (Alaska and Michigan) and P glaucus (Ohio and Florida) . . Standard Population Observations Mean Deviation Alaska 9 1 .266 0.084 Michigan 8 1.287 0.085 Ohio 6 1.202 0.109 Florida 11 1 .08 0.096 Table 4.4b. Summary statistics for chroma value in populations of female P canadensis (Alaska and Michigan) and P glaucus (Ohio and Florida). . . Standard Population Observations Mean Deviation Alaska 9 0.246 0.026 Michigan 8 0.243 0.019 Ohio 6 0.245 0.028 Florida 11 0.276 0.078 67 Table 4.5. Significance (p) values from Tukey’s HSD for all painivise comparisons between populations in the yellow color analysis between P canadensis (Alaska and Michigan), and P glaucus (Ohio and Florida). Values on the lower left side of the figure represent chroma comparisons and values on the upper right side of the figure represent hue comparisons. Bolded values in the dark gray boxes are significant (p50.05). See text for more details. Florida 5‘ .9" It,~.'\'.v“.~.“'. ’3 .‘l - ' _ ‘-t'i‘ “37‘s.," .')}I. ' 7 ' 1.1: . .7 .. enH Alaska Michigan Ohio Alaska 0.965 0.571 Michigan 0.999 0.346 Ohio 1.000 1.000 Florida 0.545 0.483 0.613 Chroma 68 relatively heavier abdomens (in relation to wing length), but an absolute comparison of the two traits revealed a similar allometric relationship between wing length and abdomen weight for both species. This suggests that similar sized individuals (as indicated by wing length) should have similar abdomen sizes regardless of species. Why then the relative differences were significant is unclear. One explanation may be that the observations for each species were not spread out evenly across the complete distribution, but rather most of the P r. glaucus observations occurred at longer wing lengths and heavier abdomens weights, whereas most of the P canadensis observations were for shorter wing lengths and correspondingly lighter abdomen weights. For yellow, the mean hue value was significantly different between P canadensis and P glaucus females, but not chroma. This was mostly due to Florida P glaucus observations deviating from the other three populations. This suggests that the fundamental ‘color’ is different between the two species, or at least between Florida and the other populations. Color has immense importance for many other components of fitness (e.g. thennoregulation; Ellers and Boggs, 2003), and how males respond to females of different colors is unknown at this time. To that end, both of these examinations are extremely preliminary and should be considered mostly in the context of future areas for study. Nonetheless, in both cases results appear promising for potential differences between the two species that may help to explain male mating preferences. Of the two, abdomen size is arguably the more likely candidate for a mate 69 preference cue, even if it is not strongly different between the two species. A large abdomen likely would suggest more eggs and correspondingly more potential offspring to a male, or else larger eggs that may result in offspring more likely to survive the eariy stages of their life (Fischer et al. 2002). It may be possible that a female with a smaller abdomen would be less pursued by males or even rejected outright. Careful study of how these two traits vary between Papilio species and populations as well as how they influence male mate preferences will likely reveal a great deal of useful information regarding both the tiger swallowtail butterflies specifically and the evolution of mating systems in general. 70 CHAPTER 5: SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The divergence of tiger swallowtail butterflies is a fascinating example of how many evolutionary influences can interact to generate new species. Vicariance, sexual selection, adaptation to divergent climates, host use divergence, mimicry and hybridization have likely all played important roles in the diversification of this small group. I examined one component of sexual selection in tige‘r swallowtails in the hope of better understanding the role of male mate recognition and preferences in their diversification. Specifically, I examined variation in coloration between two common species, the Canadian tiger swallowtail, Papilio canadensis and the eastern tiger swallowtail, P glaucus. I found that the amount of dorsal blue coloration in females of P glaucus was significantly more extensive then this same coloration on male P glaucus and both male and female P canadensis. I also found that this coloration was strongly UV reflective; making it likely that it is highly visible to other tiger swallowtail individuals. Previous research found that males of both species preferred to mate with P glaucus females when given the choice between these and similarly sized P canadensis females. This observation, combined with my finding that P glaucus females have a large amount of blue coloration, suggested that this blue was a good candidate for a trait males may use to distinguish and select females. I investigated this possibility using one- and two-choice mate preference 71 experiments and found to my surprise that males do not appear to select mates based on the extent of their dorsal blue coloration Perhaps the most important lesson that can be drawn from this work is the necessity of conducting rigorous experimentation before assigning utility to a morphological characteristic. Certainly, in most cases the hypothesized function of a trait will be found to be correct, but there may also be other, ancillary functions or, as in the case of blue coloration on tiger swallowtail females, the postulated purpose will turn out to be completely incorrect. Another interesting finding of this work is in the illumination of areas where there is clear morphological divergence between P canadensis and P glaucus. Although it was qualitatively recognized that blue coloration appeared to be more extensive in P glaucus females, this observation has now been quantified and verified. Additionally, it was previously unknown how these butterflies appeared in the ultraviolet spectrum, something which has now been assessed. Although I ultimately disproved the importance of dorsal blue coloration in male mate selection, I have opened up many paths for future research on the influence of blue in other areas such as mimicry, thennoregulation and sexual conflict with conspecifics or other species. It may be particularly illuminating to investigate the apparent inconsistency between the visual blue coloration and the UV coloration of dark morph P glaucus females. Furthermore, the problem of what traits males were cuing in on that generated overwhelming preference for P glaucus females remains. I have shown that variation in yellow coloration and 72 abdomen size may be good candidates to explain this, but both remain to be tested. Overall, tiger swallowtails compose a diverse, complicated and fascinating clade that have the potential to maintain scientific inquiry for many years to come. There is little doubt that this inquiry will lead to a better understanding of how variation in organic beings influences and is influenced by evolutionary processes. Likewise, it will generate much general information that will contribute to our knowledge of the natural world. 73 Appendix 1 Record of Deposition of Voucher Specimens“ The specimens listed on the following sheet(s) have been deposited in the named museum(s) as samples of those species or other taxa, which were used in this research. Voucher recognition labels bearing the Voucher No. have been attached or included in fluid-preserved specimens. Voucher No.: 2010-06 Title of thesis or dissertation (or other research projects): THE INFLUENCE OF FEMALE WING COLOR VARIATION ON MALE MATE CHOICE IN TIGER SWALLOWTAIL BUTTERFLIES (PAPILIO SPP.) Museum(s) where deposited and abbreviations for table on following sheets: Entomology Museum, Michigan State University (MSU) Other Museums: lnvestigator’s Name(s) (typed) Matthew L. Aardema Date AUG. 3. 2010 *Reference: Yoshimoto, C. M. 1978. Voucher Specimens for Entomology in North America. Bull. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 24: 141-42. Deposit as follows: Original: Include as Appendix 1 in ribbon copy of thesis or dissertation. Copies: Include as Appendix 1 in copies of thesis or dissertation. Museum(s) files. Research project files. This form is available from and the Voucher No. is assigned by the Curator, Michigan State University Entomology Museum. 74 Pages of1 Appendix 1.1 Voucher Specimen Data Page 1 75 \v - mumm— .5 QSQXSSAV N Somme: 98 B. Em E2025 99m $9222 65 c_ .883 Le mcmEeeam 02m: 06% 85 emzcomm mEmEm< 32:22 8-28 .62 6898 @6862 8969882. Locoso> $6380: : mucosa 6:03:33 32 Lentom .52.. 38-57.. 9.3 5:22 =N§>o=m>>m .69... cmfimcmo Ems. m .00 cmmxonwco Z>o__m>>mcloq_._. cmfimcmo 392 m .00 coHdEEmm h2021m> $959250 0:..le Lentom .52.. moomicm—zém 823cm 9me seem >3. ammo .530:mb LcmHEEmmm 392 m .00 >>m._ 9. .muco €50:mb .03. 59me :ms. a co 56.. 3&9“. 635% Siam m w 10 0+ 8 e u are ..u are s s m “hp a 5 “32.836 new new: e e s h u n P w 9 . coxmtcfio Lo 36on sh t u u u _ m 9 Lo moooomcmEGcamB m cm . me o M M P Wu E a... . EBA: so .8252 LITERATURE CITED Abramoff MD, PJ Magelhaes, SJ Ram (2004) Image processing with ImageJ. Biophotonics lntemational 11:36-42. Acorn J (2002) Assessing ultraviolet reflections of Iepidopterans with video and digital cameras. News of the Lepidopterists’ Society 44:60. Akaike H (1978) A Bayesian analysis of the minimum AIC procedure. Annals of the Institute of Statistical Mathematics 3029-14. Andolfatto P, JM Scriber and B Charlesworth (2003) No association between mitochondrial DNA haplotypes and a female-limited mimicry phenotype in Papilio glaucus. Evolution 57:305-316. Arms K, P Feeny, and RC Lederhouse (1974) Sodium: stimulus for puddling behavior by tiger swallowtail butterflies, Papilio glaucus. Science 185:372- 374. Ayres MP and JM Scriber (1994) Local adaptation to regional climates in Papilio canadensis (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). Ecological Monographs 64:465- 482. Beck J and K F iedler (2009) Adult life spans of butterflies (Lepidoptera: Papilionoidea + Hesperioidea): broadscale contingencies with adult and larval traits in multi-species comparisons. Biological journal of the Linnean Society 96:166-184. Beldade P and PM Brakefield (2002) The genetics and evo—devo of butterfly wing patterns. Nature Reviews Genetics 3:442-452 Bissoondath CJ and C Wiklund (1996) male butterfly investment in successive ejaculates in relation to mating system. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 39:285-292. Boggs CL, WB Watt, and PR Ehrlich (2003) Butterflies: Ecology and Evolution Taking Flight. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, USA. Bonduriansky R (2001) The evolution of male mate choice in insects: a synthesis of ideas and evidence. Biological Review 76:305-309. Bosi SG, J Hayes, MCJ Large and L Poladian (2008) Color, iridescence and thennoregulation in Lepidoptera. Applied Optics 47:5235-5241. Bossart JL and JM Scriber (1995) Maintenance of ecologically significant genetic 76 variation in the tiger swallowtail butterfly through differential selection and gene flow. Evolution 49: 1163-1 171 . Briscoe AD (2000) Six opsins from the butterfly Papilio glaucus: molecular phylogenetic evidence for paralogous origins of red-sensitive visual pigments in insects. Journal of Molecular Evolution 51 :110-121. Brower JVZ (1958) Experimental studies of mimicry in some North American butterflies: part II. Battus philenor and Papilio Troilus, P polyxenes, and P glaucus. Evolution 12:123-136. Brower LP (1959) Speciation in butterflies of the Papilio glaucus group. II. Ecological relationships and interspecific sexual behavior. Evolution 1 3:21 2-228. Brower LP and JVZ Brower (1962) Relative abundance of model and mimic butterflies in natural populations of Battus philenor mimicry complex. Ecology 43: 154-158. Brunton CFA and MEN Majerus (1995) Ultraviolet colours in butterflies: intra- or inter-specific communication? Proceedings of the Royal Society B 260:199-204. Chamberlin NL, RI Hill, DD Kapan, LE Gilbert, and MR Kronforst (2009) Polymorphic butterfly reveals the missing link in ecological speciation. Science 326:847-850. Chen D, JS Collins, and TH Goldsmith (1984) The UV receptor of bird retinas. Science 225:337-339. Church SC, ATD Bennett, IC Cuthill and JC Partridge (1998) Ultraviolet cues affect the foraging behaviour of blue tits. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 265:1509-1514. Cuthill IC and ATD Bennett (1993) Mimicry and the eye of the beholder. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 243:203-204. Deering MD (1998) Preferentail mate selection by males as a reproductive isolating mechanism between swallowtail species; Papilio glaucus and P canadensis (Lepidoptera, Papilionidae). MS Thesis, Michigan State University. Deering and Scriber (1998) Heterospecific mating behavior of Papilio palamedes in Florida (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). Holarctic Lepidoptera 5:49-51. Deering MD and JM Scriber (2002) Field bioassays show heterospecific mating 77 preference asymmetry between hybridizing North American Papilio butterfly species (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae) Journal of Ethology 20:25-33. Douglas MM (1981) Therrnoregulatory significance of thoracic lobes in the evolution of insect wings. Science 211 :84-86. Ehrlich PR and SE Davidson (1960) Techniques for capture-recapture studies of Lepidoptera populations. Journal of the Lepidopterists Society 14:227- 229. Ellers J and CL Boggs (2003) The evolution of wing color: male mate choice opposes adaptive wing color divergence in Colias butterflies. Evolution 57:1100—1106. Endler JA (1990) On the measurement and classification of colour in studies of animal colour patterns. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 41 :315— 352. Fischer K, BJ Zwaan and PM Brakefield (2002) How does egg size relate to body size in butterflies? Oecologia 131:375-379. Fitzpatrick S, Berglund A, and G Rosenqvist (1995) Ornaments or offspring: costs to reproductive success restrict sexual selection processes. Biological Journal of the Linnaean Society 55:251-260. Ghiradella H (1991) Light and color on the wing- structural colors in butterflies and moths. Applied Optics 30:3492-3500. Grant PR (1986) Ecology and evolution of Danivin’s finches. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, USA. Grbning J and A Hochkirch (2008) Reproductive Interference Between Animal Species. The Quarterly Review of Biology 83:257-282. Hagen RH (1990) Population structure and host use in hybridizing subspecies of Papilio glaucus (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). Evolution 44:1914-1930. Hagen RH and JM Scriber (1989) Sex-linked diapause, color, and allozyme loci in Papilio glaucus glaucus- linkage analysis and significance in a hybrid zone. Journal of Heredity 802179-185 Hagen RH and JM Scriber (1991) Systematics of the Papilio glaucus and P Troilus species groups (Lepidoptera, Papilionidae)- inferences from allozymes. Annals of the Entomological Society of America 84:380-395. Hagen RH and JM Scriber (1995) Sex chromosomes and specition in the Papilio 78 glaucus group. In: Scriber JM, Y Tsubaki, and RC Leaderhouse (eds) The Swallowtail Butterflies: Their Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Scientific Publishers, Gainesville, FL, USA, pp 211-228. Hastings IM (1994) Manifestations of sexual selection may depend on the genetic basis of sex determination. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 258:83—87. Heegaard E (1997) Morphological variation within Andreaea blyttii in relation to the environment on Hardangervidda, Western Nonivay: a quantitative analysis. The Bryologist 100:308-323. Hereau H and JM Scriber (2003) Interspecific copulation of a dark morph Papilio glaucus female and a male P polyxenes (Papilionidae): observation and significance. Journal of the Lepidopterists Society 57:71-74. Holm S (1979) A Simple Sequentially Rejective Bonferroni Test Procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6:65 -70. Hunt J, MW Blows, F Zajitschek, MD Jennions and R Brooks (2007) Reconciling strong stabilizing selection with the maintenance of genetic variation in a natural population of black field crickets (Teleogryllus commodus). Genetics 177:875-880.21 Jemvall J, JP Hunter and M Fortelius (1996) Molar tooth diversity, disparity, and ecology in Cenozoic ungulate radiations. Science 274:1489-1492. Kaufman PK, FD Enfield and RE Comstock (1977) Stabilizing selection for pupa weight in Tribolium castaneum. Genetics 87:327-341. Kotanen PM and J Bergelson (2000) Effects of simulated grazing on different genotypes of Bouteloua gracilis: how important is morphology? Oecologica 123:66-74. Krebs RA (1988) The mating behavior of Papilio glaucus (Papilionidae). Journal of Research on the Lepidoptera 26:27-31. Kunte K, C Shea, ML Aardema, JM Scriber, PAndolfatto, T Juenger, LE Gilbert, and MR Kronforst (In Prep.) Sex chromosome mosaicism and hybrid speciation among tiger swallowtail butterflies. Science Landini G Threshold_Colour, Version 1.12 for ImageJ. Available from http:llwww.dentistiy.bham.ac.ukllandiniglsoftware. Accessed on 10 May, 2010. Lederhouse RC (1995) Comparative mating behavior and sexual selection 79 In North American swallowtail butterflies. In: Scriber JM, Y Tsubaki and RC Lederhouse (eds) Swallowtail Butterflies: Their Ecology and Evolutionary Biology. Scientific Publishers, Gainesville, FL, USA. Lederhouse RC and JM Scriber (1996) Intrasexual selection constrains the dorsal color pattern of male black swallowtail butterflies, Papilio polyxenes. Evolution 50:717-722. Lederhouse RC, MP Ayres, and JM Scriber (1989) Evaluation of spennatophore counts in studying mating systems of Lepidoptera. Journal of the Lepidopterists Society 43293-101. Lederhouse RC and JM Scriber (1987) Ecological significance of a postrnating decline in egg viability in the tiger swallowtail. Journal of the Lepidopterists Society 41:83-93. Levin MP (1973) Preferential mating and the maintenance of the sex-limited dimorphism in Papilio glaucus: evidence from laboratory matings. Evolution 27:257-264. Lewis 2 and N Wedell (2007) Effect of adult feeding on male mating behaviour in the butterfly, Bicyclus anynana (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae). Journal of Insect Behavior 202201-213. Linhart YB and MC Grant (1996) Evolutionary significance of local genetic differentiation in plants. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 27:237-277. Luebke HJ, JM Scriber, and BS Yandell. (1988) Use of multivariate discriminant analysis of male wing morphometrics to delineate a hybrid zone for Papilio glaucus glaucus and P g. canadensis in Wisconsin. American Midland Naturalist 119:366-379. Lyytinen A, L Lindstrom, and J Mappes (2004) Ultraviolet reflection and predation in diurnal and nocturnal Lepidoptera. Behavioral Ecology 15:982-987. McLain DK and AE Pratt (1999) The cost of sexual coercion and heterospecific sexual harassment on the fecundity of a host-specific, seed-eating insect (Neacoryphus bicrucius) Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 46: 164- 1 70. Majerus MEN, CFA Brunton, and J Stalker (2000) A bird’s eye view of the peppered moth. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 13:155-159. Mallet J and M Joron (1999) Evolution of diversity in warning color and mimicry: 80 polymrophisms, shifting balance and speciation. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 30:201-233. Martinsen GD, TG Whitham, RJ Turek, and P Keim (2001) Hybrid populations selectively filter gene introgression between species. Evolution 5521325- 1335. Mayr E (2005) Fonrirard. In: Hallgn’msson B and BK Hall (eds) Variation: a central concept in biology. Elsevier Academic Press, Burlington, MA, USA, p xvii. Mercader RJ, ML Aardema, and JM Scriber (2009) Hybridization leads to host use divergence in a polyphagous butterfly sibling species pair. Oecologia 1 58:651-662. Mercader RJ and JM Scriber (2007) Diversification of host use in two polyphagous butterflies: differences in oviposition specificity or host rank hierarchy? Mercader RJ and JM Scriber (2008a) Divergence in the ovipositional behavior of the Papilio glaucus group. Insect Science 15:361-367. Mercader RJ and JM Scriber (2008b) Asymmetrical thermal constraints on the parapatric species boundaries of two widespread generalist butterflies. Ecological Entomology 33:537-545. Morris WA (1952) The chipmunk as a predator of the adult yellow swallowtail butterfly. Journal of Mammalogy 34:510-511. Mullen SP, EB Dopman, and RG Harrison (2008) Hybrid zone origins, species boundaries, and the evolution of wing-pattem diversity in a polytypic species complex of North American admiral butterflies (Nymphalidae: Limenitis). Evolution 62: 1400-141 7. Naisbit RE, CD Jiggins, and J Mallet (20010 Disruptive selection against hybrids contributes to speciation between Heliconius cydno and Heliconius melpomenei. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 268:1849-1854. Nielsen MG and WB Watt (2000) Interference competition and sexual selection promote polymorphism in Colias (Lepidoptera, Pieridae). Functional Ecology 14:718-730. Nowak C, CM Brown, and JJ Hellmann (2009) Microsatellite markers for the hybridizing tiger swallowtails, Papilio glaucus and P canadensis, and their applicability to historic and congeneric species. Molecular Ecology Resources 9:800-803. 81 Otis GW, B Locke, NG McKenzie, D Cheung, E MacLeod, P Careless, and A Kwoon (2006) Local enhancement in mud-puddling swallowtail butterflies (Battus philenor and Papilio glaucus). Journal of Insect Behavior 19:685- 698. Panhuis TM, R Butlin, M Zuk, and T Tregenza (2001) Sexual selection and speciation. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 16:364-371. Peterson MA, BM Honchak, SE Locke, TE Beeman, J Mendoza, J Green, KJ Buckingham, MA White, and KJ Monsen (2005) Relative abundance and species-specific reinforcement of male mating preference in the Chrysochus (Coleoptera: Chrysomelidae) hybrid zone. Evolution 5922639- 2655. Platt AP, SJ Harrison, and TF \Mlliams (1984) Absence of differential mate selection in the North American Tiger Swallowtail Papilio glaucus. In: Vane-Wright, RI and PW Ackery (eds) The Biology of Butterflies. Academic Press, London. pp. 245-250. Prum RO, T Quinn and RH Torres (2006) Anatomically diverse butterfly scales all produce structural colours by coherent scattering. The Journal of Experimental Biology 209:748-765. Putnam AS, JM Scriber, and P Andolfatto (2007) Discordant divergence times among z-chromosome regions between two ecologically distinct swallowtail butterfly species. Evolution 61 :912-927. Rawson GW and PF Bellinger (1953) Sparrows feeding on congregating Papilio. Lepidopterists’ News 7:27. Ritland DB and JM Scriber (1985) Larval development rates of 3 putative subspecies of tiger swallowtail butterflies, Papilio glaucus and their hybrids in relation to temperature. Oecologia 65:185-193. Saint-Laurent R, M Legault and L Bematchez (2003) Divergent selection maintains adaptive differentiation despite high gene flow between sympatric rainbow smelt ecotypes (Osmerus mordax Mitchill). Molecular Ecology 12:315-330. Schulter D (2000) The ecology of adaptive radiation. Oxford University Press Inc., New York, USA. Schwarz G (1978) Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics 6;461-464. Scriber JM (1988) Tale of the tiger: beringial biogeography, binomial 82 classification, and breakfast choices in the Papilio glaucus complex of butterflies. In: Spencer K (ed) Chemical Mediation of Coevolution. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, USA, pp 241-301. Scriber JM (1990a) Interaction of introgression from Papilio glaucus canadensis and diapause in producing spring form eastern tiger swallowtail butterflies, P glaucus (Lepidoptera, Papilionidae). Great Lakes Entomologist 23:127- 1 38. Scriber JM (1990b) Two new aberrant forms of tiger swallowtail butterfly from the great lakes hybrid/transition zone (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). The Great Lakes Entomologist 23:121-126. Scriber (1995) Overview of swallowtail butterflies: taxonomic and distributional latitude. In: Scriber JM, Y Tsubaki and RC Lederhouse (eds) Swallowtail Butterflies: Their Ecology and Evolutionary Biology. Scientific Publishers, Gainesville, FL, USA. Scriber JM (1996a) Tiger tales: natural history of native North American swallowtails. American Entomologist 42:19-32. Scriber JM (1996b) A new ‘cold pocket’ hypothesis to explain local host preference shifts in Papilio canadensis. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 80:315-319. Scriber JM (2002) Latitudinal and local geographic mosaics in host preferences as shaped by thermal units and voltinism in Papilio spp. (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). European Journal of Entomology 99:225-239. Scriber JM and M Ayres (1988) Observations on the puddling behavior of the Canadian tiger swallowtail butterfly Papilio glaucus canadensis R&J in northern Michigan. Great Lakes Entomologist 21 :141-142. Scriber JM, RH Hagen and RC Lederhouse (1996) Genetics of mimicry in the tiger swallowtail butterflies, Papilio glaucus and P canadensis (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). Evolution 50:222-236. Scriber JM, K Keefover, and S Nelson (2002) Hot summer temperatures may stop movement of Papilio canadensis butterflies and genetic introgression south of the hybrid zone in the North American Great Lakes Region. Ecography 20225-33. Scriber JM, RL Lindroth, and J Nitao (1989) Differential toxicity of a phenolic glycoside from quaking aspen to Papilio glaucus butterfly subspecies, hybrids, and backcrosses. Oecologia 81 :186-191. 83 Scriber JM, GL Lintereur and MH Evans (1982) Foodplant suitabilities and a new oviposition record for Papilio glaucus canadensis (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae) in northern Wisconsin and Michigan. The Great Lakes Entomologist 1 5: 39-46. Scriber JM and GJ Ording (2005) Ecological speciation without host plant specialization; possible origins of a recently described cryptic Papilio species. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 115:247-263. Scriber JM, GJ Ording and RJ Mercader (2008) lntrogression and parapatric speciation in a hybrid zone. In: Tilmon KJ (ed) Specialization Speciation and Radiation: The Evolutionary Biology Herbivorous Insects. University of California Stump AD, FAH Sperling, A Crim, and JM Scriber (2003) Gene flow between great lakes region populations of the Canadian tiger swallowtail butterfly, Papilio canadensis, near the hybrid zone with P glaucus (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). Great Lakes Entomologist 36: 41-53. Svensson El, J Abbott and R Hardling (2005) Female polymorphism, frequency dependence and rapid evolutionary dynamics in natural populations. The American Naturalist 165:567-576. Takahashi Y and M Watanabe (2010) Female reproductive success is affected by selective male harassment in the damselfly Ischnura senegalensis. Animal Behaviour 79:211-216. Thomas JA, JJ Welch, R Lanfear and L Bromham (2010) A generation time effect on the rate of molecular evolution in invertebrates. Molecular Biology and Evolution 27:1173-1180. Tinbergen N (1948) Social releasers and the experimental method required for their study. The Wilson Bulletin 60:6-51. Tremblay RL, JD Ackerrnan, JK Zimmerman, and RN Calvo (2005) Variation in sexual reproduction in orchids and its evolutionary consequences: a spasmodic journey to diversification. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 84:1-54. Turelli M and NH Barton (2004) Polygenic variation maintained by balancing selection: pleiotropy, sex-dependent allelic effects and G x E interactions. Genetics 166:1053-1079. Winter CB and AH Porter (2010) AF LP linkage map of hybridizing swallowtail butterflies, Papilio glaucus and Papilio canadensis. Journal of Heredity 1 01 :83-90. Xu J and Q Wang (2009) Male moths undertake both pre- and in-copulation mate choice based on female age and weight. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63:801-808. Yeaman S and A Jarvis (2006) Regional heterogeneity and gene flow maintain variance in a quantitative trait within populations of lodgepole pine. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 273:1587—1593. 85 "'ilillljlllij @11lein ljillljlijljijlllies