ABSTRACT EARLY DEVELOPMENT OF BEHAVIOUR PATTERNS IN GROUP AND ISOLATION REARED BOBWHITE QUAIL (COLINUS VIRGIANIUS) BY Mary Lee Nitschke Results of studies on socially reared and isolated birds in species closely related to the bobwhite quail suggested that the isolate birds develop the same patterns evidenced in the socially reared birds, but show these later in some instances and with greater variation in the pattern and sequence of development. The present study is an attempt to (1) provide a baseline of early develOpment of the behaviour of the bobwhite, a non-domestic precocial galliform, under laboratory controlled conditions with minimal human contact or intervention and under a natural lighting regime; (2) extend previous findings on the effects of isolation with the galliforme birds by observing a new species having a longer period of develOpment before reaching maturity than those previously studied; and (3) to describe components of behaviour classes not previously reported in eXperimental studies. Mary Lee Nitschke During the first 29 days post-hatch the isolate birds were housed individually under tactual and visual isolation conditions. From day 30 on the isolate birds were combined into a social living unit. The socially reared birds were housed communally from the outset. All birds were maintained on ad lib water and food on a natural diurnal light cycle, virtually without human contact. Observations were made from a darkened, adjoining room and data recorded electromechanically and electro- magnetically. The results obtained extend and support previous findings on the effect of isolation. In addition, this set of observations provided descriptions of the following behaviours which have been previously unreported for this Species reared in the laboratory: (1) distinct forms of Care of the Body Surface components, (2) components of the dusting pattern occurring on wire, (3) the age of onset of crouching and "cautious" postural patterns, (4) the Cubicle Pacing and Sustained Tactual Contacting patterns as a persistent behaviour of the isolated subjects, (5) an attempt to characterize the synchronous versus the frag- mented nature of the topography of the behaviour patterns as they differed for the two groups, and (6) the progression and development of components of the "roosting disk" Resting Group Structure, a species-Specific pattern for which the bobwhite quail is famous. Mary Lee Nitschke In conclusion, the results of the present study, taken in conjunction with those of other studies on the galliformes, indicate that generally isolation may retard but does not prevent the develOpment of social behaviours. One exception was found, the Ring Nesting Resting Group Structure never appeared in the isolate group even after having been combined and living together 45 days. EARLY DEVELOPMENT OF BEHAVIOUR PATTERNS IN GROUP AND ISOLATION REARED BOBWHITE QUAIL (COLINUS VIRGIANIUS) BY Mary Lee Nitschke A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1971 AC KNOWLEDGMEN'I‘ S The author wishes to thank Dr. Stanley C. Ratner for his patient guidance and direction of this thesis. I would also like to express my appreciation to Dr. Terrence M. Allen and Dr. Robert K. Ringer for their technical assistance and the enthusiastic encouragement they provided for my work in this area. Several of my peers assisted as extra observers during the study. Gratitude for this goes to Suzanne Power Marshall, Peter L. Borchelt, Robert E. Otis, and John Rosenketter. Finally, this thesis is dedicated to Rusty who taught me the most about Bobwhite quail. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS . . . . . . . LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . METHOD 0 O O O O O O O I C General Organization . . Subjects Apparatus Phase I . . Subjects . Apparatus . Procedure . . . . O O O O O O O O O O O O ; Phase II: Procedure Phase III: Procedure RESULTS 0 O O O I O O O O 0 Definitions of Behaviour Classes . Outline of Behaviour Classes . . Measurement . . . . . . . Classes Common to Both Groups . Classes Unique to Isolate Group . Classes Unique to Social Group . Descriptions of Behaviour . . . Behaviour of the Social Group . Behaviour of the Isolate Subjects Comparisons Between Social and Isolate Groups DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . Behaviour of the Social Group . . Behaviour of the Isolate Subjects . Summary . . . . . . . . . BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . APPENDIX C O O O O O O O O O Page ii iv 15 15 16 19 21 21 22 22 26 27 3O 31 31 32 32 37 39 40 4O 69 91 105 105 109 112 114 117 Table 1. LIST OF TABLES Page Behaviour classes with respective response patterns of social and isolate groups showing age of onset (lst occurrence), median onset age, and day the behaviour had been shown at least once by each group member (100% onset day) . . . . . . . 42 Range and median frequency of allOpecking encounters of a series of observations for each week of age in a group of socially reared bobwhite quail . . . . . . . 68 Frequency of allOpecking encounters observed in the isolate reared group (N=7) on the day of assembly into a social living unit . 90 iv LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Design sketch of basic living unit . . . . 20 2. Onset of (1) general preening; (2) oil gland preening . . . . . . . . . . . . 45 3. Mean frequency per day of general preening bouts in Social and Isolate groups over a 4 week period . . . . . . . . . 46 4. Mean frequency per day of Bill Wiping in Social and Isolate groups over a 4 week periOd O O O O O O O O O O O 0 48 5. Onset of AlIOpreening in Social and Isolate groups . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 6. Onset of Flying in Social and Isolate groups 53 7. Initial appearance of wing quills in Social and Isolate groups . . . . . . . . 54 8. Onset and duration of predominate structures of group resting patterns observed in the Social and Isolate groups throughout the StUdy O O O O O O I O O O O O O 5 7 9. Onset of freezing in social and isolate reared groups of bobwhite quail . . . . 60 10. Onset of the Cautious Alert Posture . . . 63 ll. Onset of Crouching in Social and Isolate groups . . . . . . . . . . . . 65 12. Onset of Running in Social and Isolate groups 75 13. Onset of Jumping in Social and Isolate groups 76 14. Onset of Frolic with object Locomotion pattern in Social and Isolate groups . . 80 Figure 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. Mean frequency per day of incidence of Sustained Tactual Contacting in seven Isolate subjects over 4 weeks of isolation . . . . . . . . . . . Mean frequency per day of three rates of Cubicle Pacing by Isolate group subjects over 4 weeks of isolation . . . . . . Onset of Sophisticated Preening in Social and Isolate groups . . . . . . . . Onset of Bill Wiping in Social and Isolate groups 0 O O O O O O O O O O O Onset of Dust-Bathing in Social and Isolate groups . . . . . . . . . . . . Onset of Popping in Social and Isolate groups Onset of Flit-pOpping in Social and Isolate groups I O O O O O O O O O O O Onset of Generalized Pecking in Social and Isolate groups . . . . . . . . . . Onset of Frolic Locomotion pattern in Social and Isolate groups . . . . . . . . Initial appearance of tail feather shafts in Socially reared and Isolation reared groups vi Page 82 83 93 94 95 96 97 98 100 102 INTRODUCTION Natural history and semi-technical observational reports on the bobwhite quail recommend it as an excellent preparation for the study of many of the major classes of consummatory behaviours. Stoddard's (1931) monograph stands to date as the classic source of background informa- tion on the behavior of the bobwhite quail. In addition to being one of the most pOpular of the upland game birds of widespread distribution throughout the United States, its varied and distinctive behavioural repertoire offers a challenge to our understanding of the behaviour of a non- domestic member of the galliformes. As a precocial bird it is also an excellent laboratory animal and is now beginning to be utilized as a subject in eXperimental behavioural investigations. The lack of behaviour work with this animal to date is surprising since this species has many attributes that would recommend it for behaviour research, in addition to its being a hardy and easily handled animal when reared in groups and requiring minimal maintenance and space. As a good preparation for studying several classes of behaviour in a social species the bobwhite exhibits 1 powerful examples of behaviour classes that have received little intensive attention from experimental investigators. In the present instance the classes of locomotion, care of the body surface, resting, and predator defense (behaviour of the prey animal) are primary examples. Although not specifically considered here, the bobwhite also exhibits clear and often elaborate examples of the classes of sexual behaviour, care of the young, nesting, and intra- specific communication possessing representatives of each of the classes of calls named by Collias (1960). Thus, smithin the aves, the bobwhite could profitably be investi- gerted as a species that offers an integrated picture of a nwrjority of the consummatory classes of behaviour (with thxa possible exception of fighting) as listed by Ratner (Deanny and Ratner, 1970). The bobwhite quail is a small variegated brown and budff colored precocial galliforme, native to the United Startes. The chick hatches into the company of 7-15 other tier "feathered bombshells" (Bent, 1963) each weighing on th£3 average of 6 grams and covered with natal down. At orue day of age the brood begins foraging eXpeditions under tine direction of both parents. For the next two weeks or SC) the chicks spend 75% of their time being brooded by one Of the parents until they become thermoregulatory and begin acquiring juvenile plumage. The daily activity of the family for the next several weeks over the summer consists primarily of an active early morning foraging period followed by bouts of maintenance activities such as preening, dusting and resting, until a few hours before sunset when another active feeding period begins and con- tinues until the group gathers together for the night (Stoddard, 1931). One of the striking aspects of a quail family or a quail covey (a collection of quail larger than the family) is their highly integrated group behaviour. The group appears to act in concert as a unit, particularly with regard to daily activity and predator defense patterns. A typical observation made by Stoddard (p. 18) illustrates this point: . . . every little while some imaginary alarm causes every bird to freeze and remain absolutely motionless for periods of seconds or minutes, when suddenly, as if at a given signal . . . all relax and go about their business. The information on development of behaviour patterns in the bobwhite chick is sketchy. Stoddard (1931) mentions that the first week the chicks jump and make flying leaps in pursuit of insects. At two weeks chicks will "flush; when disturbed. He describes this as: "All members freeze to the ground with feet firmly placed, only to spring into the air with a thunderous roar . . . ." This perhaps is the origin of the title "feathered bombshell." He noted a group of quail hatched in captivity and reared with a brooder, never having been in contact with adult quail, take up the circular roosting formation character- istic of bobwhite at four weeks of age. This formation is described as: ". . . heads out and bodies pressed so tightly together that the tails are forced skyward, and the sides are in close contact" (p. 45). At eight weeks the chicks are entirely in juvenile plumage and the sexually dimorphic coloration is evident. At fifteen weeks their plumage is indistinguishable from that of the parents without close visual inspection of the primaries. Another source of general information on the behaviour of the bobwhite is a more recent paper by Stokes (1967). The book by Stoddard represents a conservation- ist's orientation but contains careful description of several of the behaviour patterns observed in the field, with a focus on social and parental behaviours within the covey. Stokes (1967) utilizes Stoddard's descriptions but focuses his three year investigation on the function of agonistic, sexual and parental behaviour patterns and vocalizations of the mature animal. Although other recent investigators (Garreffa, 1969) have also studied the behaviour of this species, most have concentrated on a specific experimental problem utilizing the mature bird and minimal attention has been directed to the very young animal and the early deve10pment of its behavioural repertoire. Recently, Borchelt (1970) provided initial experi- mental information on the deve10pment of fear displays in the bobwhite. This study suggests that responses to predators appear sequentially as (1) crouching, (2) freez- ing, and (3) immobility or fear display. Borchelt found freezing first appeared in this context in 50% of his birds between 10-19 days; with immobility appearing most reliably at about the age (roughly two weeks) that the bird begins to achieve physical independence from the nest. Prior to this, the only deve10pmental data on fear responses and a basic motor coordination in precocial birds relative to other birds was provided by Nice (1962). She proposed a stages-of-development scheme with motor patterns changing throughout the following stages: (1) post-embryonic, (2) preliminary, (3) transition, (4) locomotory, and (5) socialization. However, definite age limits for these stages and precisely defined criteria for identifying the behaviours associated with the stages were not provided; nor is this information available for the bobwhite quail. An initial step in the preparatory stages of the comparative analysis of behaviour, as proposed by Ratner (Denny and Ratner, 1970), is the consideration of the question "what does this organism do?" He suggests that an analysis and understanding of consummatory behaviour requires the investigator to be familiar with the natural history and the specific behavioural repertoire of the animal being studied. Thus, identification and character- ization of behaviour as a prerequisite for a powerful functional classification system requires careful descrip- tive work. When this body of information is not available an eXploratory descriptive study is necessary to provide baseline data before an investigator can legitimately proceed to ask incisive questions with regard to choosing powerful variables for an experimental analysis of classes of behaviour. It is generally recognized that species, age and prior eXperience are powerful variables influencing the process of behavioural deve10pment in a vertebrate organism. In reviewing the comprehensive paper on social behaviour in the mature bobwhite by Stokes (1967), the question arises as to the origin of these behaviours and how the various components fit into a classification of patterns. One technique for investigating the origin of social behaviours is to restrict aspects of the social experience of an animal and discern the effect of this restriction of stimulation. An extensive literature exists on the effects of isolation as a rearing experience in a variety of social species. However, if we accept that species is a powerful variable, we are obligated to begin an investigation at that level of organization before proceeding to other questions of variables assumed to have a causal effect on behavioural processes. Within the galliformes, a large body of literature exists on the social behaviour of adult domestic fowl, as evidenced by the reviews of Wood-Gush (1955) and Guhl (1953). However, few studies have concentrated on the development of behaviour from hatching and the few that have were often aimed toward an analysis of a specific behavioural pattern, e.g., sexual or aggressive. Thus, there are few studies which provide the necessary data for making direct comparisons between other birds and the early deve10pment of the bobwhite. Those available for comparison purposes are studies using domestic fowl (Guhl, 1958; Hartford, 1967; Ratner, 1965), or Japanese quail (Farris, 1964). Although these researchers were primarily .interested in aggressive or sexual behaviour, they aapproached the problem from a developmental standpoint and 'their results provide some basis for looking at the present {problem as part of a larger scheme. Guhl (1958) raised various groups of chickens (White Leghorns, White Rocks and commercial hybrids) in Lbartial isolation from other chicks. His primary purpose Vvas to determine whether partial isolation would influence flhe age at which these chicks formed peck orders when €Jrouped. He described the sequence of behaviour patterns <>ccurring in his birds as: generalized pecking, escape reaction, frolicking, sparring, aggressive pecking, avoidance behaviours, and fighting. The isolation reared birds formed a peck order after being assembled into a group within short (8-48 hour) periods. Guhl concluded that the early experience of such behaviours as frolicking, pecking, and fighting were not essential to rapid formation of social organization. However, observations were not conducted on the behaviour of the chicks while they were isolated. Therefore, as Ratner (1965) suggested, the possibility exists that Guhl's isolates did perform some of the behaviours in question prior to their assembly into groups. One should also pay attention here to the type of isolation employed in this instance, as a bird is a highly visual animal. Partial isolation was defined as chicks that could see other chicks but could not make tactual contact with them. This partial isolation may have provided considerable stimulation of an important nature during the "isolate" rearing period for these chicks. In elaborating the schedule of behaviour deve10p- Inent reported by Guhl, Ratner (1965) attempted to deter- Inine: (l) the sequence of deve10pment of social behaviours in a normal group, and (2) the effects of isolation on loehaviour development during and after a period of isola- tion. He observed White Plymouth Rock and White Leghorn cockerels reared together and chicks raised in visual isolation from other chicks. His results confirmed Guhl's observations of frolicking, sparring, aggressive and generalized pecking, and fighting; the findings differed only in age of behaviour onset. In addition, he provided the initial description of frolicking with an object, food dominate and food submit, and the juvenile fight. He found that isolation led some behaviours to be retarded in age of occurrence and that the patterns of frolic and frolic with object were indeed exhibited by some isolate birds during the isolate rearing condition. When the isolated chicks were assembled at 70 days of age aggressive interaction began almost immediately. Although the tOpog- raphy of the pecking pattern appeared less organized in the isolates, they formed a stable peck order within a few days. Following assembly most birds then showed all of the behaviours shown by the normal group, and the two groups were indistinguishable in terms of social behaviour patterns within several days. Ratner defined the age of onset for a behaviour pattern as the day the first bird in the group showed this behaviour. It would perhaps be useful to extend this information to look at onset of behaviour pattern as the day at which at least 50% of the birds within a group show a behaviour pattern, and to gain an idea of the variation within a group of subjects by looking at the range between initial day of onset and the day at which a particular 10 behaviour had been shown by every member of the group at least once. Farris (1964) studied the behaviour development of young Japanese (Coturnix) quail under both social and isolated rearing conditions and reported differences in development in terms of day of onset between the two groups on dashing, bill wiping, jumping, jumping, popping, brief flight, hOpping, dusting, and cricket calling. He reports "freezing" occurring on day one post-hatch in response to "slight disturbances" and later evolving into or combining with the "alert stance" on day 12. In general, he found there were few differences between the social and isolate birds in the course of behaviour deve10pment except for sexual behaviour. The differences referred to above are attributed to differences in the size of the home environment for the two groups. This brief review of isolation studies in the galliformes indicates that the appearance of components of social behaviour patterns generally may be retarded- altered-delayed in subjects experiencing restricted or subnormal stimulation, but rarely does this treatment prevent the occurrence of the response components them- selves. 11 Present Study Methodological Considerations In the studies cited above, several procedural points should be called to the reader's attention. 1. Guhl (1953), Ratner (1965), and Hartford (1967) all studied the domestic fowl. As stated by Ratner, ". . . in general, domestic species represent poor preparations for the purpose of uncovering behaviours that relate to consummatory activities" (Denny and Ratner, 1970, p. 212). Thus, one of the objectives of the present study was to provide a descriptive baseline of the early deve10pment of the bobwhite quail, a non-domestic precocial galli- forme, under laboratory controlled conditions. 2. Other laboratory studies have employed a variety of lighting regimes. Borchelt (1970), Hartford (1967), Ratner (1965), Farris (1964) and Guhl (1953) all employed a 24 hour continuous light procedure. Of the above only the Borchelt study utilized a warm white type of lighting. Since the amount of time a bird is exposed to light is known to have significant effects as a stimulus for the pituitary to release gonadotrOphins (Sturkie, 1965), and since the lighting cycle has been shown to alter the reproductive activity and general level of behavioural activity in the bobwhite (Kirkpatrick, 1955), lighting is a significant variable in the environment of this species. Continuous lighting is thought to produce an agitated 12 state of behaviour in birds (Menaker, 1969). Other effects of continuous lighting in the domestic fowl are an increase in the size of the eye and diminished visual acuity when subjected to continuous light over long periods of time (Sturkie, 1965). The type of light to which the bird is exposed, cool white versus warm white, also has been demonstrated to have considerable effects on the repro- ductive physiology and the neuro-endocrine system of the fowl (Sturkie, 1965). Cool white light alters the repro- ductive processes and due to its effect on gonadal and thyroid hormones, Ringer suggests in Sturkie (1965) that it influences the birds weight as well as the structure of its feathers. Although data on these specific points are not available for the bobwhite, the weight of the evidence suggests that continuous lighting of a cool white type would be a less than desirable choice of living conditions to impose on a group of birds for a behavioural study. Thus, the present study specifically inCluded as one of its objectives looking at the behaviour of this galliforme under a natural diurnal cycle of warm white simulated daylight. The third objective of the present study was to provide an environment for the subjects which did not include their being continually exposed to human inter- ‘vention or contact. The studies cited immediately above all included conditions in which the bird could View the 13 eXperimenter while data was collected. One of the inten- sively studied aspects of altered stimulation in early experience studies with birds is the phenomenon of imprint- ing (Denny and Ratner, 1970). Considering the possibility that imprinting could occur if the bird is permitted visual contact with the eXperimenter, this study was designed to eliminate that particular possibility. In addition to this, handling of the subjects was kept to an absolute minimum. A fourth objective in the present study was to provide living space for the subjects that would prevent crowding, and that would allow sufficient room for the subjects to perform many of their natural behaviours. Previous studies using social and isolated groups provided only minimal living space for the subjects. Hartford (1967), Ratner (1965), Farris (1964), and Guhl (1953) all failed to provide cages large enough for the birds to fly within the cage, and often only "commercially" sufficient space was provided for the socially reared birds. These same four investigators also obtained their subjects from sources where there was no control of their experience until up to 24 hours following hatching. Since previous work has shown (Vince, 1966) that differential pre-hatching conditions, together versus separated, produces differential stimulation for the avian embryo, this variable should be controlled in a study comparing 14 group reared with isolation reared subjects. As a fifth objective, the present study attempted to impose the differential treatment of groups prior to pipping of the egg in order to more closely define the different rearing conditions for the two groups of subjects. As previously mentioned, the bird is a highly auditory animal and this consideration suggests that auditory stimulation should be treated as a variable. However, both in the studies previously discussed and the present study, this form of stimulation was not controlled. In summary, the present study had as its focus to provide an experimental ethogram and descriptive baseline of the early deve10pment of the bobwhite quail, a non- domestic precocial galliforme, under laboratory controlled conditions with minimal human contact and under an appro- priate lighting regime. Consideration is also given to extending previous findings on the effect of isolation and the development of behaviour patterns by defining the sequence in which the components of a behaviour class developed in terms of the median onset day for the group and by observing a species previously not studied in this manner. The results will be discussed in the tradition of earlier ontogeny studies (Bolles and Woods, 1964; Nelson, 1965). METHOD General Organization This study can be considered to have three phases that are defined by living conditions and sizes of groups. Phase I involved two living conditions: (1) social, with birds housed together; and (2) visually and tactually isolated, with each bird living within an isolate cubicle. Phase I ended when the birds were 29% days of age. At the beginning of Phase II visual and tactual isolation conditions were removed and the isolate group assembled into their own social living unit by removing the parti- tions that defined the isolate cubicles. This resulted in the two subject groups now having identical cages. At the initiation of Phase II the behavioural taxonomy protocols also became identical for the two groups. This phase followed Phase I immediately and continued until the birds were 33 days of age. During I and II each bird was observed five minutes at each of two daily observation periods. Phase III was marked by the removal of seven randomly selected subjects from the social group, in order to equalize the N for the two groups. This phase continued until the birds were 75 days old. The observation schedule 15 16 later in Phase III was altered to include only one observa- tion each two—three days. The procedure of Phase I for the two separate groups will be treated first. Then the procedure of Phases II and III will be considered. Subjects The subjects for this experiment, bobwhite quail (Colinus virginianus), were obtained from the seven year old Michigan State University Poultry Science quail colony. Four hundred and fifty-seven eggs were incubated. Forty eggs were randomly selected from undisturbed incubator compartments prior to pipping and divided into two groups by alternately designating each egg S or I. This selection was made at the beginning of the twelve hour peak of hatching for the total group. The eggs designated as S were then placed in the standard group hatching tray and returned to the incubator. Each egg designated as I was placed into an individual bin within an identical tray and also returned to the incubator. These latter trays were partitioned into separate compartments by fitted metal inserts and were placed into the incubator in a manner that prevented a newly hatched occupant from viewing other chicks. The thirty-two surviving chicks were hatched within twelve hours of each other; fifteen of the twenty S or social eggs hatched and seventeen of the I or isolate eggs hatched. Within two hours of hatching each quail was l7 weighed, leg banded and returned to the appropriate tray in the incubator. All subjects remained there until transferred to their respective living units. Thus, the subjects in the social group experienced the presence of other quail from prior to completed hatch- ing to the end of the study; while the isolate group sub— jects were visually and tactally separated from other quail from the pre-hatched state and maintained in this manner until the initiation of Phase II. Throughout the study all bobwhite were maintained on ad lib water and food (Michigan State University Quail Breeder 25% protein, dry mash, King Milling Company). Food and water trays were filled on alternate evenings following the scheduled observation period. Water trays were removed and cleaned twice each week. A diurnal light cycle of 14% hours of light and 9% hours of darkness was in effect continuously. The lights in each living unit were turned on a 6:00 a.m. and remained on until 8:30 p.m. Following their arrival at the Psychology Research Building at one day of age, the subjects were never per- Initted to view the eXperimenter and were maintained without .human contact with three exceptions: Phase I--all subjects ‘were individually removed from their unit, weighed, :rebanded, and returned; Phase III--at two different times all subjects were handled briefly or experienced disturbance (noncomitant with equalizing the group sizes and with l8 checking and loosening leg bands. All of the above Opera- tions and all maintenance activities (cleaning, changing water, filling food trays, and checking cage temperatures) were carried out either in darkness or under minimal illumination provided by one 15 watt, warm white, fluorescent light covered with a dark blue filter and located approximately two meters behind the living cages. All observations were made from an adjoining darkened observation room fitted with one-way vision glass. No extra eliciting stimuli were presented to any subject at any time during the course of the study. The sex composition of the two groups remained unknown until sexual dimorphic coloration made positive identification possible around 10 weeks of age. Composi- tion of the groups as determined at the close of the study was as follows: Phase I: Social N = 14; 10 females, 3 males, 1 unknown (died before positive identification made). Isolate N 3 females, 4 males II \I ‘0 Phase II: No change Phase III: Social N = 7 5 females, 1 male, 1 unknown ‘0 Isolate N ll 4 No change. 52295 feede for t table meter windc drop; preve while remai previ remai 41 rh ident for t the b enclo diame serve. 15 Wa: attacl 19 Apparatus The birds lived in cages that provided floor, feeder, and water space well above the minimum requirements for this species. These living units rested on adjacent tables in an experimental observation room 5 x 2.5 x 2.8 meters. The cage fronts faced a series of one-way vision windows, each 22 x 51 cm. with the level of the cage floor drOpped 10 cm. below the bottom edge of the glass to prevent the subjects from viewing their reflected image while walking about in the cage. The experimental room remained unilluminated throughout the study except as previously noted. The room temperature and humidity remained essentially stable around a mean of 27.5°C and 41 rh, respectively. The two living units, each 45 x 45 x 155 cm., identical except for the removable partitions described for the isolation cubicles, were constructed as follows: the back and sides were 10 mm. unpainted plywood board, the floor was 6 mm. hardware cloth, with the tOp and front enclosed with 12 mm. hardware cloth. Tin troughs, 75 mm. diameter x 153 cm. length, resting on the floor surface served as water trays at the back of each cage and food trays at the front (see Figure l). Cage illumination was provided by three 45 cm. 15 watt, warm white fluorescent lamps (GE F15 TlZ/WW) attached to a center light bar running the full length 20 .goflufipumm mDMHOmfl mco Amv .mcmsonu cw» BE mu Amy 4| so om EU mv .o003SHa as OH Irv .mnaz as NH lac .muaz as o lac .QHSQ mEmH Apv .Hmn whommsm usmfla ADV .Dflcs Hmumms Any "pas: msfl>fla Damon mo noumxm cmflmwnln.a musmflm EU mmH JL 21 of the top of the unit. Each cage contained three lamps, providing a range from 25 to 150 lux illumination within a unit with approximately equal values between the cages. Heat was available from four 38 cm. strip-type poultry brooder heaters secured in a 75 mm. tin reflector trough suspended beneath each cage. Temperature regulation was effected by raising, lowering, or altering the angle of this heater unit. The heating units were turned off when the subjects were 36 days of age; shortly after the initiation of Phase III. Recording equipment consisted of the following: an electromagnetic tape recorder (Norelco 101), one ten pen event recorder (Esterline-Angus) Operated by a manual key board, and one luminous dial clock. Phase I Subjects At one day of age all thirty—two bobwhite were transferred from the Poultry Science Building to the eXperimental room in the Psychology Research Building and placed into their respective living units. A total of fifteen birds were housed together in the social cage. One subject from this group died of an undetermined cause on day 14. Of the seventeen isolate group subjects; ten were immediately placed into the isolate living unit; with each 22 bird occupying one complete cubicle. The remaining seven subjects were maintained in an alternate isolation cage. Within four days, ten of the isolate subjects (some from each isolate cage) died of undetermined causes. Thus from day 14 through day 29 the social group contained 14 birds. From day 5 through day 29 there were a total of seven isolated quail available for observation. Apparatus For both subject groups, each living unit within the cages provided a heat gradient ranging from 30-26°C at the water tray to 44-33°C on the floor directly over the heating unit at mid-cage. Temperatures were lowered gradually over the 29 day period and the lower figures above denote the absolute degree ranges at day 29. Unique to this phase was the isolation cubicles defined by opaque partitions (.63 cm. masonite board) fitted flush between the front and back walls within the living cage housing the Isolate subject group. Thus each cubicle provided a floor space of 270 square centimeters (45 x 45 x 15 cm.); housing one bird in a manner that prevented direct visual or tactual commerce with another quail. Procedure Phase I is defined by the living conditions of the :aubjects; social versus isolate, and by the age of the 23 subjects; including data from day 1 through day 29. The social group birds were communally housed in one observa- tion cage. The isolate group subjects were housed in an identical cage, except for the partitions that defined the isolation unit. Neither group could view subjects in the other group although both groups experienced the same auditory array, including noises from the hallway and vocalizations from any bird within the eXperimental room. Each bird was observed five minutes at each of two daily observation periods. The initial and terminal stages of the light cycle were scheduled observation periods. Data from earlier investigators (Kirkpatrick, 1955), indicated that these were periods of considerable activity for young precocial birds. The order of observation of individuals within a group was randomized by following a random number table keyed to a subject identification number. The observation order for the two groups followed a counterbalanced design. For example, a three day block of six successive data collection periods would be as follows: 1. 6:00 a.m.: Begin with the isolate group, following each bird for five minutes, with the individual subject order determined by the random number table designation for that observation number; following this the social group birds would be observed similarly. 24 2. 6:30 p.m.: Begin by observing the social group and following completion of this, the isolate group data would be collected; 3. 6:00 a.m.: The order would be social, isolate; 4. 6:30 p.m.: The order would be isolate, social; 5. 6:00 a.m.: The order would be isolate, social; 6. 6:30 p.m.: The order would be social, isolate. This counterbalanced schedule was followed throughout the observations. On day 9 following the a.m. observation the cage lights were turned off in both units and the blue light condition utilized while all subjects were individually removed from their respective units, weighed, rebanded, and returned to their cages. This was the only instance of the subjects eXperiencing human contact during this phase of the study. On day 22 an all day observation period was scheduled with two other observors alternating with the regular eXperimenter and reliability data were obtained at that time. Interobservor agreement was sufficient to allow accurate deliniation of behavioural patterns. All observations were made from a darkened adjoining-room through a one-way Vision glass. Response categories were arrived at on the basis of initial observation during the first sixteen sessions and were altered as necessary to accommodate new behaviours. These 25 were recorded on tape and later transferred to protocal sheets and inspected to arrive at a data grouping that would provide a functional taxonomy of the most frequent distinctive (in most cases mutually exclusive) behaviour patterns for each of the two subject groups. Beginning with observation seventeen, a ten pen event recorder equipped with a manually Operated finger key board was used to collect data; with verbal subject identifications and additional comments being recorded simultaneously on tape by the eXperimenter. Responses were scored (by depressing the apprOpriate key) on the event recorder in two ways, either discretely or continuously. If a response occurred in discrete units (e.g., drinking), then the apprOpriate key was depressed each time an instance of this response occurred. A continuous response is one which took an indefinite amount of time (e.g., resting); the apprOpriate key was pressed throughout the duration of a continuous response. Thus, in addition to noting the frequency of a behaviour within a general behaviour class, these patterns could often be broken into their component parts and both antecedent conditions and subsequent effects identified sequentially and temporally. Behaviours were recorded in the order of occurrence with frequency, duration and time interval information available on the event recorder paper and subject identification and other information available from the tape. Specific behaviour categories are first defined and then described in the results section. 26 In addition to scoring the behaviour categories; detailed notes were kept on the feather deve10pment of the subjects. Feather deve10pment onset was characterized in terms of visibility of the feather shafts (still encased within the cuticle) to the observer, from the observation booth. There was also a data log kept of unusual or uncontrollable conditions occurring throughout the course of the study (e.g., fire alarm warning system being tested in the building), and the reactions of the subjects to these events. In instances where events of this nature provided fortuitious data collection Opportunities; these will be identified in the results section. Phase II: Procedure One hour prior to the scheduled morning observation when the subjects were 29% days of age, food and water trays were filled in both living units. The cage top retaining hooks were removed from both units and the isolate living cage was prepared for removal of the parti- tions that provided isolation cubicles. Shortly before 6:00 a.m. the tOp was removed and replaced on the Social unit cage to provide some disturbance for the subjects in this group. Following this the tOp was removed from the Isolate cage, and screen wire strips were positioned over the cubicles to prevent subjects from escaping while the partitions were pulled. The tOp was secured into place 27 and it became identical to the Social living unit. These manipulations were carried out in subdued, blue filtered light provided by one 15 watt bulb located two meters behind the cages, with minimal disturbance to the subjects, that is, no subjects were handled by the eXperimenter. Within a few minutes the cage lights came on for the regularly scheduled observation. For this observation the regular schedule was altered. The newly assembled isolate group of subjects was observed for two hours continuously. A regular observa- tion of the social group followed. Another 30 minute observation was made at noon that day, followed by another extra observation at mid-afternoon. The regularly scheduled observation was then carried out in the early evening. These data from the extra observations were kept separate from the daily observation log. Following the initial day of Phase II, the regular observation schedule used during Phase I was in effect until the initiation of Phase III. Phase III: Procedure When the birds were 33 days of age the group sizes were equalized by removing seven subjects from the Social Group; thus each group would now contain seven subjects. Ideally the birds to be removed in order to equalize the N for the two groups should be identified by choosing from a random number table keyed to colored leg bands. However, 28 since it was important to prevent the subjects from viewing the human experimenter, a compromise procedure was utilized. The experimenter reached into the cage picking up one bird at a time until seven subjects had been removed. This was carried out in total darkness without the group of birds being disturbed. The cage was divided into three sections and a bird was removed from each section alternately until the required N was complete. The extra subjects were immediately transferred to a hold- ing cage at the far end of the room and maintained there until the completion of the study. With the exception of carrying out the removal of the subjects in total darkness, the other aspects of the manipulations carried out here duplicated those used in Phase II. That is, both cages were disturbed, extra observations were carried out immediately following the institution of this phase. Following the initial day of Phase III the observa- tion schedule was altered to include one complete observa- tion each 2—3 days during the week with the counterbalanced design continued until the birds were 75 days old and the study was terminated. At this time determination of sex was easily made by visual inspection of the beak and plumage of each bird. The birds were not weighed at this time because they were scheduled to become subjects for another study; in which the other eXperimenters wished to utilize the 29 "no—human-contact" aspect of the history of these subjects. RESULTS Behaviour classes and their components were identi— fied from three general sources: (1) the taxonomy of F consummatory behaviours presented by Ratner (Denny and Ratner, 1970); (2) previous literature describing these or similar behaviours (Stokes, 1967; Ratner, 1965; Farris, 1964; Stoddard, 1931); and (3) pilot work by the present author involving observation of this species in the labora- tory and the field. An Outline of Behaviour Classes introduces the Measurement section. Following this the behaviours are defined as they were scored for the purposes of this study. The definitions of behaviours that are general to the aves are not referenced, for example, flying, but are described here as the author observed them to occur in the present study. Behaviour patterns that are essentially identical to those observed in other species by other investigators are so indicated within the definition of that pattern. Behaviour patterns that are described for the first time in an experimental study of this species are indicated by an asterisk and those first experimentally defined by the present author are also followed by the author's name. 30 31 Following the definitions of the behaviour classes, the descriptions of behaviour for the two groups is found. Data is presented in tables and figures illustrating the onset of each behaviour pattern for the individuals in the two groups: Social N = 14, Isolate N = 7. No statistical significance tests are apprOpriate for these data. 5 Definitions of Behaviour Classes Outline of Behaviour Classes I. NUTRITION A. Feeding B. Drinking II. CARE OF THE BODY SURFACE A. Preening 1. General 2. SOphisticated* 3. Oiling* 4. Allopreening* B. Bill Wiping* C. Dusting* III. LOCOMOTION . Running Jumping POpping Flying Flit-popping* Frolic* . Frolic with Object* OWL‘UUOWS’ IV. DAILY ACTIVITY A. Generalized Pecking B. Resting C. Resting Group Structure* 1. Huddle-Heap 2. Huddle-Space 3. Linear 4. Ring or 1/2 Circle D. Cubicle Pacing* 32 V. POSTURAL PATTERNS Crouching* Freezing Cautious Alert* Sustained Tactual Contacting* 0(3U1> VI. ALLOPECKING A. Body B. Head-Nares-Beak C. Toe Picking VII. FEATHER DEVELOPMENT A. Wing B. Tail Measurement Each major class of behaviour will be descriptively identified and the components of that class listed and described as they were scored in this study. As this study focuses on the deve10pment of behavioural patterns; those classes of behaviour that are common to both groups of subjects will be described first. Following this the behaviour classes that are unique to a group will be treated. Classes Common to Both Groups I. NUTRITION A. Feeding: the act of ingesting food; pecks into food tray. B. Drinking: ingesting of water by pecking into the water tray. 33 II. CARE OF BODY SURFACE A. Preening: the act of a bird's trimming, dressing, or cleaning its feathers with its beak, in one of the following patterns: 1. general preening: a soft picking or nibbling movement directed at the feathers, characterized by orienta— tion and movement of the bill and head toward a body area. The intensity and duration varies widely but the form (tOpography) of this movement is unmistakable. 2. *SOphisticated preening: a general preening movement directed to a specific feather shaft and character- ized by the feather being passed between the mandibles, which "nests" the barbules. This movement may be accompanied by a greater frequency of bill Opening and closing during the time the bill is directed to the base of the feather (Nitschke). 3. *oil gland preening: a directed movement bring- ing the bill to the dorsal base of the tail, accompanied by raising of feathers in the dorsal caudal tract as the beak tugs on the nipple of the urOpygial gland or oil gland; following this the beak is directed to various body areas as the bird oils the feather surfaces, legs or feet. Another component of this pattern, frequent but variable, is a vigor- ous rubbing of the beak in the area of the gland, character— ized by rapid movements of the head from side to side (Nitschke). B. *Bill Wiping: bird's head is directed to the floor surface at various angles; beak contact with the 34 surface is evident as both sides of the mandible are brushed against the surface in any one full sequence. This may be repeated several times in succession with minimal accompany- ing body movement or locomotion, after Farris (1964). C. "Dusting": bird rests on ventral body sur- face with wings partially extended while body is "rolled" to one side as the contralateral foot executes scratching move- ments. Simultaneously feathers in the caudal, alar and capi- tal tracts are raised, as both wing and tail primary feathers spread and close several times in succession. The bird U' typically rolls from side to side repeating these movements and pecking rapidly at the sustaining surface between shifts in position. Shifts in position may or may not involve vigorous scratching with alternate feet. Head and neck extension may accompany the dusting pattern; the head is also "rolled" or brought into contact with the surface in a rub- bing motion. This resembles the bill wipe movement except that the neck is twisted in such a way that the head feathers contact the substratum. In a soft medium, e.g., dry mash in a food tray, a body sized depression may be formed as a result of this activity, after Stokes (1967). III. LOCOMOTION A. Running: terrestrial and rapid movement of the bird in an upright position; head slightly extended from a "streamlined" body, i.e., the feathers compressed against the body and tail feathers trimmed. 35 B. Jumping: bird springs vertically from floor, somewhat forward; wing movement may or may not be invclved in ascent or descent. When wing movement is in- volved it does not appear to be precisely synchronous. A shallow crouch may precede jumping but is not a reliable predictor of this behaviour. Compared to popping the tOpography of this pattern is jagged in appearance. C. *Popping: bird springs directly upward in rapid vertical ascent with body streamlined on ascent; at tOp of upward spring, wings Open, tail flares and bird is postu— rally in correct attitude to initiate forward flight. Flight may or may not follow. The smooth but "explosive" character of this movement serves to make it quite distinguishable in form from other locomotor patterns, after Farris (1964). D. Flying: an aerial, forward propelling, coordinated wing movement pattern that may be preceded by any or none of the above patterns. E. *Flit-popping: the popping pattern, but characterized by a distinctive deep crouch preceding the ascent (Nitschke). F. *Frolic, or wing-flutter-run: a short burst of running accompanied by wing movements (after Ratner, 1965). IV . DAILY ACTIVITY A. Generalized pecking: pecking movements directed at inanimate objects not within the food tray; nail heads, feces, etc. (after Ratner, 1965). 36 B. Resting: bird remains motionless with eyes closed in one of three positions: (1) lying on side with head and feet stretched out; (2) squatting with undersides touching the floor with head withdrawn; or (3) standing up- right, often on one foot, with head withdrawn (after Farris, 1964). C. Resting Group Structure: 1. *huddle-heap: three or more birds touching in a squatting posture and stacked into a heap such that at least one bird does not occupy floor space (Nitschke). 2. *huddle and spacing: a huddle group in which some birds at the edge of the group occupy floor space near the group but may or may not be touching members of the huddle group (Nitschke). Contacting the environment (Denny and Ratner, 1970) or investigatory behaviour (Scott, 1958) were classes of be- haviour that were not scored in this study because of their non-specifiable characteristics with regard to the nature of the present problem. V. POSTURAL PATTERNS A. *Crouching: bird's breast is near or touch— ing the floor; the neck and head are withdrawn, with the body posterior extended vertically and the legs in a flexed posi- tion beneath the body (Nitschke). B. Freeze: bird momentarily ceases all observ- lable movement and remains motionless for a minimum of 15 sseconds, in whatever posture characterized termination of the 37 previously ongoing behaviour at the onset of the freeze posture. This pattern may be followed by a cautious alert posture once movement is initiated. C. *Cautious alert posture: bird stands up- right with legs straight beneath the body, the tail lowered with the head and neck extended forward, giving the bird an erect and stretched-out appearance. Generally only the head makes noticeable movements; shifting of a foot position may be observed, although infrequently. On occasion, a bird may exhibit this pattern following the freeze posture, and in I-‘vc I this instance may take a few steps while maintaining this posture (Nitschke). Classes Unique to Isolate Group V. POSTURAL PATTERNS D. *Sustained tactual contacting: the general configuration of this pattern involves the bird touching a vertical surface, usually a partition, with at least one wing, and the beak or head may also be in contact with the surface. The typical intense expression of this pattern is a "pushing" against the partition such that the feathers are noticeably re-arranged during the extent of the contact and the bird appears to be "flat" on one side. This form of contacting may also be observed in the squatting or standing resting posture or pacing pattern. In "pushing" the bird's toes are hooked into the wire floor 2-5 cm. away from the partition with which it is in contact, with the legs defining an angle less than 90 degrees 38 with the bird's body. This gives the appearance of a con- siderable contact pressure being sustained between the bird and the vertical surface in the standing position (Nitschke). IV. DAILY ACTIVITY D. *Cubicle pacing: bird travels the length of the cubicle, crossing the mid-line in continuous and suc- .. cessive trips, walking rapidly with the body upright and the D head slightly extended (Nitschke). This is often accompanied by contact between the beak or breast area and a vertical ‘I .'.‘ . . cage surface throughout the traversal of the cubicle. Three movement rates were defined by the frequency with which the bird crossed the mid—line of the cage and traversed not less than one half of the possible area on any one trip: 1. Low Frequency = less than or equal to 5 trips during one observation period (5 minutes) more than 5 and less than 15 trips 2. Medium Frequency "frantic" pacing = 15 trips or more each minute for two or more minutes of the observation period. 3. High Frequency Classes Unique to Social Group Through Phase I VI. ALLOPECKING A. Body pecking: pecks delivered to body (usually dorsal-caudal tract) area of another bird of suffi— cient force to elicit at least a recoil response on the part of the recipient. B. Head or nares pecking: a peck directed at the head, nares, or eye of another bird of sufficient force 39 to elicit a recoil and usually avoidance from the bird receiving the peck. C. Foot or toe pecking: pecks directed at the toes or lower tarsus of another bird, may often involve pur- suit of the victim by the bird delivering pecks and attempted escape on the part of the victim. II. CARE OF BODY SURFACE t} A. Preening: 4. *AllOpreening: a general preening pattern directed at the head or body area of another bird; may 1" ._ involve pecking bits of food from feathers, but this cannot be determined by visual observation. The pattern is dis- tinguished from body pecking by observing the reaction of the recipient, i.e., there is no recoil or avoidance move- ment. This pattern may occur frequently during huddling. III. LOCOMOTION G. *Frolic with object: frolic while carrying some object, e.g., a feather, in the beak (after Ratner, 1965). This pattern may elicit pursuit by other birds. IV. DAILY ACTIVITY C. Resting group structure: in addition to the basic resting postures and structures previously described, the social group exhibited two patterns never observed in the isolate group quail, as follows: 40 3. *Linear: three or more birds squatting in a resting posture touching each other laterally but with every bird occupying his own floor space. The birds may or may not be facing the same direction but the configuration appears linear (to be one of a line) (Nitschke). 4. *Ring or half-circle configuration of a resting group: several birds in the linear structure form a con— tinuous line that may take the shape of a semi-circle or complete circle, with the heads of all the participating birds oriented in the same direction and the tails of these birds defining the interior of the circle (after Stoddard, 1931). Descriptions of Behaviour Behaviour of the Social Group General description of typical observations.--A typical pattern of activity observed for the birds in this group would be as follows: 1. Within a few minutes after the onset of light most birds would have arisen from their resting huddle and begun to drink, feed, or engage in brief bouts of general preening; a 2-3 minute feeding bout invariably marked th beginning of their day. - 41 2. The next prolonged activity was Care of the Body Surface. 3. Within ten minutes the synchrony of the group's activity was less evident as bouts of feeding, preening, drinking, moving about the cage, generalized pecking, and resting were interspersed with each other. The quail spent various amounts of time feeding or climbing in and out of the food tray for much of the first hour before settling down as a group for their first morning nap. As shown in Table 1, it can readily be seen that the birds were all eating and drinking well under the laboratory conditions of this study at one day of age. The evening observation differed only slightly from the above description. Often the group would be found resting or scattered in small groups throughout the cage preening and dozing. At least one and usually several group feeding bouts would be noted; these also would occur alternately or be interspersed with other general activities. Incidents of allOpecking encounters were typically uni- lateral and were observed to be interspersed throughout the other activities and appeared to be integrated into the daily pattern. These encounters were brief and never resulted in fighting. A few of the Locomotion and Postural Pattern behaviours occurred in isolated instances in a single bird; however, the more usual incidence of these 42 mm MH «H. m HOHoom om ow mm. on 3303 uomflno 5H3 oHHoum MH OH no. v HmHoom Ov NH Hp. NH oumHOmH UHHoum OH OH mv. MH HMHoom mm mm 3. S 3203 33.18-33 mm ON vH. OH HmHoow om S 3. 3 3302 9:me n m Hh. m HOHoom om H «H. HH mumHOmH mcHdmom OH m mm. m HMHoom m m OH. m oumHomH mcHQEdO m m OM. H HmHoom h n ma. m mumHOmH mchcdm ZOHBOEOOOH mm OH Om. pH HmHoom H on 3. mm .3303 mcHumso O O no. u HOHoom 3 a I. e 3202 95.33 33 OO om no. OH HmHuom mm mm mu. Hm mumHOmH mchmmumoHHd ON nH no. m HmHoom mm vm OH. OH oumHOmH OcmHo HHO SH mH OH. HH HmHoow pH mH mm. «H wumHOmH OODOOHuchmom N m OOH N HmHOOm O m OH. m oumHOmH Hmumcmw vchwmum mofimmam woom mme m0 mméu H H OOH H HwHoom H H OOH H oumHOmH mcchHuO cam mcHUomm ZOHBHMBDZ uomco wOOH. cmHOoz mm ucou Mom mucouusooo umuHm msouo mmmHU usoH>mswm .wocmu ochoc v new H mcEOHOO .Axmc ummco wOOHO HOOEOE dsonm 20mm an coco umMOH um c3onm coon cm: usoH>m£on on» amp can aroma uomco :memE .Hmocouusooo umHv uwmco no one mcH3ocm mmsoum mumHomH can HmHoom mo mcumuuMQ mmcoammu O>Huommmou :uH3 mommmHo unoH>mnmmul.H mqmde ‘43 page cH maan HmseH>HecH .wcsum mnu usonmsounu msoum chu Sn :3onm uoc HOOH>mnom .mcHuusooo Eoum chu Ooucw>mud mcoHuHOcoo HmucmEHummxm x .OEHu mco umOOH um cumuumm MOOH>mnon may :3onm Om: adoum on» NO «Hm: umOOH um :och so how on» mH 0mm ummco cmHOoz .1 ON ON OH MN MH om NH Om m om HH om Om ON om (DWI r-lt-i NH ON NH OH om om INN Fir-l Om. ON. VH. mN. no. OOH HO. OOH mv. OOH mm. OH. OH. OOH VH. OH. OH. mN. OOH HN. OOH OOH OOH OOH OOH OOH vH. vH. NH OH O NH om om VH HH HOHoom mumHOOH HOHoom wumHOmH HmHoom moumHomH HOHoom wuwHOmH HmHoom mumHOmH HOHoom mumHowH HmHoom oumHOmH HmHoom wumHOmH HmHoow oumHOmH HmHoom OHMHOmH HmHoom mumHOmH HmHoom mumHOmH HmHoom mumHOmH HmHoom oumHomH HmHoow mumHomH HMHoom mumHOmH HHOB mcHz ezmzmoam>ma mmmeamm oconHm woe mconom mmHOZIOmo: mconom zpom wszUmmOqu mcHuomucou Hmzuoma cmchumsm uuoH< mOOHuOmO onwmum . OcHnosouU mzmmfiedm Hdmsemom mcHomm OHOHOOU mHouHOuMHm: new mch musuosuum ummcHH mcHommm can OHOOOI amomumHOOsm wusuosuum QOOMU mcHumom msHummm mconmm UONHHmumcwo >9H>HBO< NHH D‘Q‘ rm pcw II... 3.32: min 338 CID OCHHHO msonm Hmumcmm 45 HQ-H A .3 .2 .mm 6m E .8 .3 .E r: .3 .mm .mm HOOH queo 18d eArqunan 46 .OOHHOQ xmmz O m Hm>o mmsoum mDmHomH Ocm HOHoom OH mason mcHnmem Hmumqmm mo MOO Hmm hocmsvmhm qm02||.m OHOmHm mxmmz OH mom m H _ uN . .O .3 w E U .3 m 9 b n . 8 OH w A HOHoom .l; .NN ON OHMHomH I 47 simultaneously with sophisticated preening and tended to occur at the same times; the two were often engaged in alternately by an individual bird. These patterns were more separated in time from other activities such as feeding or generalized pecking than the pattern of general preening, that is, they were not as likely to be inter- spersed with concurrent activities. Bill wiping also develOped within the first week and as shown in Figure 4 was algo a fairly pOpular form of activity. It was observed to occur at any time during an observation and was typically quite brief and inter- spersed with a variety of other behaviours. Bill wiping always occurred while the bird was in an upright position and the surface contacted was the cage floor. It appeared to follow generalized pecking most frequently when a bird had pecked at a fresh feces. It also followed drinking, feeding, pecking another bird, and receiving a peck from another bird, as well as occurring in instances where no previous pattern appeared to be related to the event, such as running up and down the cage, popping, and preening activities. Its ubiquitious and general occurrence suggests that it is more properly classes as a form of Care of the Body Surface than as a "greeting gesture to other chicks" as suggested by Spalding (1873). Dusting or dust bathing was not an activity I had anticipated observing in this study. However, the birds 48 .OOHHOQ Mmm3 O m Hm>o masonm OHOHOOH can HOHoom CH OQHOHB HHHm mo MOO Hmm mocmsvam cmeII.O OHOOHM mxmma CH OOH v m N H J [I‘ll—Ulla vfi w E vm u ONH "M w ill. fimH % rON W .ON [u .3 .m _ _ X... a MHUO H . w . - [I Hmm k OHOHOmH I 49 found the food troughs suitable dusting sites; and only complete bouts in this location were scored as a dust bath. The scratching and shuffling components of this behaviour also occurred on the wire cage floor directly over the heating unit strips. This suggests that the condition eliciting this pattern needs further investigation, as Schein (1968) implies that this pattern will not occur on wire. The dusting pattern was infrequent compared to the other Care of the Body Surface activities. AllOpreening occurred most frequently when the birds were in a huddle or when an individual approached an already formed huddle in which some of the birds were engaged in preening. No obvious appetitive display compon- ents were observed that appeared to function as a solici- tation to initiate allOpreening on the part of the bird receiving the preening. Proximity seemed to be a suffi- cient stimulus for the behaviour to occur. However, once initiated, the recipient would often extend the neck and turn the head so that the feathers between the mandibles and the ear were well exposed and readily available to the other bird. Allopreening bouts were generally briefer in duration than either of the more elaborate forms of auto preening; SOphisticated or oil gland preening. The possi- bility exists that hits of dry mash in the feathers of another bird would elicit this behaviour; however, it was not possible to determine this by visual inspection under 50 the conditions of this study. The form of allOpreening was overwhelmingly unilateral in occurrence, with mutual preening being observed only five times throughout the course of the study. This might be taken as supporting the suggestion that food bits served to elicit this behaviour. However, the relatively late onset of this behaviour pattern as shown in Figure 5 would suggest that the food bits were probably not the critical factor. In addition to this, feeding off the back of a bird (that had been covered with food as a result of a quail taking a dust bath in the food tray, or scratching in the food) was observed to occur within the first week and had a tOpography that distinguished it as clearly different from an allOpreening behaviour. Farris (1964) observed mutual preening frequently in the Japanese quail, but no comparable data exist for the bobwhite. The sequence of deve10pment of the basic Locomotion patterns in the group using the median onset day was as follows: Running (3), POpping (3), Jumping (9), Frolic or wing-flutter-run (10), Frolic-with-object (l3), Flit- pOp (l4), and Flying (20). These patterns were evident at various times during an observation and, once they were well develOped in all of the subjects, appeared to occur most frequently as a result of social facilitation. This was particularly true of the patterns of pOpping, flit- pOpping, flying, and frolic-with-object. Once flying was 51 .mmsoum OHOHomH cam HOHoom QH OQHQOOHQOHHH mo ummco:l.m OHOOHM Omm mo mmma OHOHOOH . HOHoom AU .hm 21.1180 18d 8111:7191:an 52 initiated by an individual, often within seconds, the entire cage would be a flurry of birds flit-pOpping and flying from one end of the cage to the other. It was impossible to discern what had initially stimulated the first bird when this occurred. However, the daily fre- quency of popping appeared to be related to the arousal state of the group. This inference was suggested by the fact that when workmen were repairing heating ducts in the ceiling of the building during a series of observations the birds appeared to startle easily and often this was followed by the cage suddenly exploding into a group of "popping" birds. The frolic-with-Object pattern often developed into a sequence of activity involving various numbers of birds over a continuous 3-4 minute period. This pattern tended to occur most frequently late in the morning observation period or early in the evening period and usually stOpped as abruptly as it began. Shortly after the wing primaries were achieving some length (see Figures 6 and 7) the quail began flying the length of the cage and on several occasions a bird flying overhead elicited crouching in a bird on the cage floor. Unfortunately due to the flurry of activity within the cage at these times it was impossible to obtain a consistent measure of this relationship, but it should prove a fruitful area for further investigation. 53 .mmsoum OHOHomH OOO HOHoom OH OOHNHM mo uOmOOII.O OHOOHO mom mo mhma HN ON OH OH OH OH OH .O .h .OH .HN .mN OOOHomH . . HOHoom O . .2 .9... .S .3 .HH 0 .2. .OO .8 53 queo 18d eArqetnmna 54 .mmsonm mpmHomH OOO HOHoom OH mHHHOv OOHB mo OOOmHmmmmm HOHHHOHII.> OHOOHM OOO mo mama ON NN ON OH OH OH NH OH O O n P D v D p r P L .o .5 .OH OHOHomH . .HN HOHoom 0 .ON .Om .mO .om .hm .OO .Hh .mh queo 18d eArqunan .mm OOH '1 55 Generalized pecking had been observed in each of the birds in this group by the third day of age. This was a very frequent behaviour and was likely to occur any time a bird was up and moving about the cage. Included in this category of behaviour was a component of the "competitive pecking" pattern described by Farris (1964) as "two or more birds pecking at the same object." The most fre- quently pecked "object" in this instance was a fresh feces on the cage floor. Individual birds pecking the cage wall was by contrast an infrequent occurrence. Resting as a class of behaviour occurred several times within any one observation, particularly during the first two weeks of age. The postures of lying on one side or standing upright were less typical of the resting behaviour of these subjects than was the squatting with the underside touching the floor. A group of birds resting appeared to have considerable eliciting strength as a stimulus to approach. If an individual bird had been part of a resting group and the others in the group moved to a new location in the cage, the bird left alone, upon awaking, would arise and run rapidly to join another resting group. Since shifts in position of the individuals in a resting group were frequent, particularly during the first 10-14 days, the composition of the resting group was continually changing concommitant with a change in 56 location within the cage. Thus, a sleeping bird might often go to sleep in a group and awaken to find himself alone. As noted in Figure 8 the Resting Group Structure went through several major changes in predominant forma- tion patterns. The first few days the group of birds most resembled a pile of bodies stacked into a corner, with individuals continually changing position within the heap. Toward the latter part of the first week, this heap of birds began to flatten out somewhat as fewer birds attempted to crawl onto the top of the pile; instead they vwould more frequently join a resting group by approaching tflie edge and nesting down near a bird on the periphery or airtempt to crawl in between their mates and eventually sxmcceed in inserting their body into the group and gaining scnne floor space. The onset of this Huddle-Spacing struc- tixre also marked a change in the location of resting groups axvay from the ends of the cage next to the plyboard walls, toward the middle or Open areas of the cage, but still Ipxrimarily positioned over the heating units. Depending <311 the general level of activity within the cage, there Unjught be one or more of either of these groups formed at Eirry time during an observation as the quail interspersed Short resting bouts with most other activities. The Idirlear Resting Group Structure became evident toward the 57 .O>OOO OOHOO OEHH OOH HOHMO HOOmOO NHHOHHOOmmO OHOB NOOH OOHHO mo QOOOO OOH mo OHOHOOHHO OOHHOOH HOOOHEOOOHQ OOH mo OOHHONHHOHOOHOOO O OO .HO>O30O .OOHHO OOH mo OEOO OH OEHH OH OEHH ECHO OO>HOmOo OO OH OOOOHHOOO mOHOHOOHHm HOOOHH UOO .OOHOOOOIOHOOOO .mOOmlOHOOOm OOH .MOOHO OOH HOOOOOOHOH mmOOOO OHOHomH MOO HOHoom OOH OH OO>HOOOO mOHOHHOm OOHHOOH OOOHO mo mOHOHoOHHm OHOOHEOOOHQ mo OOHHOHOU OOO HOmOO .O OHOmHm OOO mo thn mm NH. mOOOMO OO $333. 3 NOmmOmmmoth ONHNOHmH NHO O m ....-.....----..l-%3§3 mDUUO BOZ GHQ w mfifim ODOOO Roz OHO HOOOHH .om HMO OOOMOO OOOOHHOOOO OOHOOmm — 1m OOHOO>OOO mOOHHHOOoo O O O OO HOHOOEHHOme H OmOOm H U m» meOOomH OOOO . OHOOOO H mHouHo N\H H F __ A O OOHm EOVTOSI r 1|. HOOOHH OOHOOQO w. _ a OHOOsm D @505 7 mHfifidm S O 3 I w 58 end of the second week and was also typically observed to occur over the heating units. One of the distinguishing aspects of this pattern was that the birds less frequently attempted to crawl in between other resting birds; rather a bird would approach a resting group and settle next to the side of a bird bringing his wing area into contact with the bird laterally and sink into the squatting posture. Although the above patterns were observed at various times throughout an observation, the Ring or Nesting Circle Resting Group Structure was most frequently noted at the beginning of either a morning or evening observation and sometimes during the evening observation. This pattern was also somewhat different than the others in that it appeared to be more stable or, once formed involved fewer position shifts among the constituents, and at full intensity involved all of the animals within the cage when it was observed. Once the nesting circle broke up at the beginning of a morning observation it rarely reformed during that period at full intensity, that is with all the group participating. However, there were several instances of smaller groups forming the Half- Circle configuration for brief periods during both scheduled observations. When smaller groups formed the pattern it most frequently took the 1/2 circle form, which 59 concurs with Rosene's (1969) statement that due to the positioning of individuals within a "roosting disk" the minimum number required to form the complete circle is six birds. Also lending support to this suggestion is the fact that one of the few changes in behaviour observed to occur directly in relation to the initiation of Phase III of this study, the removal of seven subjects from this group, is that the full circle resting structure was observed less frequently immediately following the Phase III manipulation than previously. However, it did not cease to occur among the remaining seven subjects. It is impossible at this point to rule out the possibility that removal of birds from a group is a dis- ruptive or interfering event with regard to a social or group behaviour pattern. The question of number of animals certainly suggests itself as a variable to be investigated in further research of this nature. The sequence of deve10pment of the behaviours referred to as Postural Patterns was: Freezing, Cautious Alert, and Crouching. As shown in Figure 9 the freezing pattern occurred in all the birds in the group on day 7. Since there was no attempt at a systematic manipulation of conditions that might elicit freezing in the bobwhite, the day of onset shown here may only be incidental developmental information. Other investigators (Stokes, .OOm OH HOOOH HO How mmOHOOHHOE mOHOEOO OOO HOOEO>OE OHOO>OOmOo HHO mOmOOo OHHOOHOOEOE OOHO "OOHNOOOO .HHOOW OHHOBOOO mo mmsoum UOOOOH OHOHOOH OOO HOHoom OH OOHNOOHO mo HOmOo .O OHOOHm O04 mo OOOQ ON ON ON mN HN OH. OH OH MH Hm O b -O -O ' ' I ’ ' 60 Aw .o .O .3 .Hm .mm .3 .2 SO .3 3 .2 .2. .3 .MO .OOH OHOHomH @ HOHoom O queo zed eArqunmna 61 1967; Bent, 1963; Stoddard, 1931) report freezing to occur at one day of age in response to auditory stimulation. The occasion for freezing on day 7 in the present data was a metal flashlight accidentally dropped on a metal stool in the recording booth. The striking aspect of this pattern for the social group was the synchrony of the behaviour; every subject in the social cage froze for 90 seconds and all the subjects appeared to terminate the pattern at the same instant. That is, the birds appeared to begin move- ment as abruptly as they had ceased movement. Prior to the onset of the freeze, there was a momentary flurry of activity within the cage that was so rapid and brief that it was impossible to describe or attempt to measure. This very likely could have been the escape-fleeing behaviour (Guhl, 1958) or dash-to-cover (Stokes, 1967) described by other investigators as the initial response to a "strange sound." Freezing was observed to occur in this synchronous pattern at various times throughout the study and in most instances it could be identified as a response to unusually loud noises occurring in the hallway outside the experi- mental room, such as a soft-drink bottle kicked down the hall. The most striking occurrence of this pattern was a freeze of four minutes in duration that was initiated by the janitor opening the door to the experimental room rapidly and banging it into the table on which the living units rested. This occurred during an evening observation in which I was recording this group. 62 The Cautious Alert Posture appears to be similar to the "Alert" stance described by Farris (1964) in the Japanese Quail. The conditions eliciting this Cautious Alert pattern appeared to be sudden changes in the general level of stimulation or sudden alterations in the environ- ment; it was frequently seen when a drop of water was splashed onto a heater covering, producing a brief "sizzle." As illustrated in Figure 10 this pattern appeared in a few birds before the Freezing pattern. It appears that it may be related to this general class of behaviour. Once the Cautious posture had developed in all of the birds in the group; it exhibited a topography of synchrony analogous to that described for the freezing pattern. On day 20 one of the cage lights went out in the social living unit; there was an immediate flurry of activity as the quail scattered to the end of the cage and went into the Cautious Posture as a group. All of the subjects oriented toward the other end of the cage while maintaining this posture for 75 seconds and making only small twisting movements of the head. This was followed by an abrupt transition to a more "relaxed" posture and the birds began to move about the cage. The other link to the freezing pattern was that the cautious posture was observed to follow an incidence of group freezing behaviour on three occasions. However, this was not observed at every incidence of freezing. The present 63 .OOOOHOOmmO =HOo OOOOHOHHO= H5O HOOHO OO UHHO OOH OOH>HO .OHOBHOH OOOOOHxO OOOO OOO OOOO OHH3 .OOHOSOH HHOH OOO HOOHOHHO mOOH OHHB HOOHHQO mOOOHm OHHm .OOOHmom HOOHO mOOHHOOU OOH mo HOmOOII.OH OHOOHm OOO mo OMOQ om ON ON ON NN ON OH OH OH OH O O O o O u o . . b b b x P p p a .O OHOHOOH Av .O HOHoom Av .OH .HN .ON .Om .mO Hom .Om .OO .HO OO OO .OO 53 31180 18a SATQEIHUIHD I 64 observations do suggest that it might be profitable to approach these patterns as one class of consummatory behaviour and consider the possibility that the cautious posture may be a post-consummatory component. Although previous investigators have suggested that Crouching is a response related to fear in the bob- white (Borchelt, 1970; Stokes, 1967) no direct data on this question is available. It is included here, Figure 11, as a Postural Pattern because of the distinctive postural topography which characterizes it and because no firm data are available to suggest what class of events might consistently elicit this pattern in the immature quail. It was not observed to be necessarily a social or group behaviour in the manner described for the previous postural patterns. This is not to say that it never appeared in several birds in any one instance. It was most frequently observed during a period when the cage was very active or the birds appeared to be aroused and there were a variety of Locomotion patterns occurring such as pOpping and flying. Since these behaviours appeared to be socially facilitated and often involved several birds in rapid-fire sequences of behaviours, it was impossible to isolate for certain the pattern or class of events that might be eliciting the crouching pattern. In several instances it was observed to occur when a cage mate flew over the head of a quail standing on the floor, but this 65 .mmOOHO OHOHOOH OOO HOHoom OH OOHOOOOHU mo HOmOOII.HH OHOOHO OOO mo OOOQ om LN ON ON ON ON ON ON NN HN ON OH OH OH OH OH OH OH NH - s b p p n p n n p p b In 5 n + p P p OHOHomH o - OH 388 O . Hm O HO . mm O03 queo Jed eAraeInmna 66 was not found to be a consistent elicitor of the crouching pattern. Allopecking was observed within the first few days of age in a few birds and had been observed in each of the subjects by day 12. This class of behaviour was divided into three distinct types: body pecks, head or nares pecks, and foot or toe pecks. All three forms were pri- marily unilateral and only rarely involved pursuit of the pecked bird. Reciprocal or mutual pecking was so infre- quent (from 0-4) that it will not be considered further except to note that fighting with pecks (Ratner, 1965) and bill-fighting (Stokes, 1967) was essentially absent from the consistent behaviour patterns of these birds. The frequency of allOpecking was measured by counting the number of occurrences as pecking encounters. This defines each entry or each pecking event as an encounter between two birds in which pecks were delivered in one instance of pecking not separated by an intervening behaviour on the part of either bird: (1) the bird delivering the pecks walking away, going to drink, pecking the floor, or (2) the bird receiving the pecks avoiding the pecker successfully for a minimum of four seconds (one space on the event recorder paper). This means that the measure representing frequency of allOpecking shows not number of individual pecks delivered, but number of pecking encounters between subjects. 67 AllOpecking tended to occur sporadically through- out either of the daily observations and in all locations within the living unit. The condition which led one bird to begin pecking another could not be determined with certainty, there appeared to be no consistent postures, cage location, or measured behaviours that were reliably related to the initiation of a pecking encounter. The r3 data notes suggest that it might prove fruitful to investi- gate the relationship between bill wiping and drinking and 9 subsequent receipt of pecks. On several occasions various numbers of the quail would engage in what can only be . described as "playing" in the water trough; jumping in and out of it, sticking a foot in and kicking, or swishing their beaks back and forth in the water. This usually led others to engage in similar behaviour. Pecking encounters of the types observed at other times frequently followed these water games. It is quite likely that in this situation water drOplets were the elicitors of pecking. The form and descriptive criteria of allOpecking would necessarily lead one to identify these behaviours as aggressive pecks as described by Ratner (1965). However, even though it appeared to be this type of pecking that was observed, it is curious that there was never an instance of a bird being visibly wounded as a result of a pecking encounter and raw or bloody spots were never observed to occur in this group of quail. Pilot work 68 with this species and observation of the quail in the Borchelt (1970) study highlight this as a somewhat unusual set of data. However, Stoddard (1931) and Stokes (1967) suggest that "peaceful relations" between bobwhites in a group are the rule rather than the exception at all times of the year except for the mating season when mature males engage in fighting. As can be seen in Table 2 the frequency of allOpecking in this group was never very high. Body allOpecking was so infrequent that it is not shown (0-3 encounters per observation). TABLE 2.--Range and median frequency of allOpecking encounters of a series of observations for each week of age in a group of socially reared bobwhite quail. Head-Nares-Eye Pecking Toe-Foot Pecking Range Median Range Median Week 1 0-20 6 0-13 4 Week 2 7-26 13 3-25 21 Week 3 3-38 9 2-44 12 Week 4 5-31 16 2-42 13 Week 5 3-13 13 2- 7 6 Week 6 2-10 6 l- 5 1 Week 7 2- 3 3 1- 8 8 69 BehaviOur of the Isolate Subjects Behaviour during isolation.-- (1) General description of a typical observation: The first two weeks the behaviour of the isolate subjects could be characterized as showing two distinctively pre- dominant patterns, either long periods of sleeping or continual movement such as running the length of the cubicle. The running pattern was so striking that a separate measurement category was develOped to differentiate a distinct form of this behaviour, namely that of Cubicle Pacing. Following light onset in the mornings some sub- jects would remain in their resting posture and others would begin immediately to pace the length of their cubicle, from the food tray to the water tray. Feeding and drinking bouts were quite brief in duration, as were general preening episodes. When the birds were resting they slept directly over the heating units and maintained contact with a cubicle partition. The topography of this contacting behaviour and its ubiquitous occurrence in each of the subjects led to the deve10pment of another separate measurement category in an attempt to further «characterize the general pattern of behaviour in these :isolated birds. This pattern, Sustained Tactual Contacting, \vas observed to occur throughout both observation periods Enid was not restricted to periods when the birds were 70 sleeping. In fact it appeared to be a behaviour pattern that was engaged in by a bird concomitant with all other activities except those whose form directly prevented its occurrence, such as feeding or flying. The most pOpular location for this behaviour, as with resting, was also directly over the heating unit. Although Cubicle Pacing and Sustained Tactual Contacting continued to be predominant behaviours during the third and fourth weeks, more time was spent now engaging in feeding, drinking, preening, and other forms of Locomotion. In addition to the contacting and pacing patterns, all the behaviour patterns observed in the social birds (excepting those that isolation conditions prevented from occurring) were seen in at least some of the isolate subjects during isolation with one exception. The frolic-with-object Locomotion pattern was never observed in an isolate subject during the Phase I condi- tion of isolation. (2) Onset of behaviours in the different measure- ment classes: Following the outling in Table 1 it can be seen that the isolates were feeding and drinking at one ciay of age. Feeding bouts of more than a few seconds at zany one episode were unusual in these subjects. It zippeared that most of their ingestion of mash from the :fiood tray took place a few pecks at a time, in between fmacing trips. The second week three of the isolates 71 developed a pattern of vigorous scratching in the food tray. For several days this behaviour was so frequent that their food troughs had to be refilled daily instead of every other day, in contrast to the typical procedure. At times this scratching behaviour was accompanied by feeding though not consistently. Even during these scratching-feeding bouts, the feeding behaviour was a pattern of isolated individual pecks interspersed with relatively longer scratching episodes. Two of these three birds were in adjacent cubicles but the third quail that consistently scratched his food tray empty was at the other end of the living unit. Eventually all of the Ibirds were observed to scratch in their food but only 'these three evidenced the behaviour at the intensity ciescribed above and the pattern all but disappeared in tflnese birds the latter part of the third week. It was difficult to make precise notes on all (irrinking behaviour because the subjects would often aiztempt to stick their beaks through the wire between the <311bicle partitions in the water trough. However, distinct cirinking behaviour was also observed in each of the birds, lit) addition to the above pattern. Care of the Body Surface patterns began showing 11E> the first week in some birds. As illustrated in Table 1- comparing columns 1 and 4, there was considerable I 72 variation in the day of onset for the individuals in this group. Although general preening appeared in all of the birds within the first week, it was not a predominant aspect of their daily activity and was typically brief in duration, on the order of a few seconds at any one instance, when it was shown. The second to third weeks it increased considerably both in duration of individual bouts and as shown in Figure 3 in frequency of occurrence. Bill wiping was the next behaviour to develop in this class and its tOpography with respect to both duration and frequency xuas quite similar to that described for general preening. In addition to this, abortive or fragmented bill wipes xwere observed in these subjects, that is, a brief down- xward swipe in only one direction and failing to contact 1:he surface of the cage floor, was a pattern observed in 1:hese subjects. During the first 10 days this pattern “Has almost as frequent as the complete bill wipe pattern sczored as an incidence of bill wiping, as defined in the Inezasurement section. It appeared that bill wiping most ‘txrpically followed drinking in the isolate subjects but trlis was definitely not the only time at which it occurred. Early in the third week the sophisticated preening Pattern appeared to be well developed in these birds. C=O mHOOOOOm OHOHOOH OO>Om OH OOHHOOHOOU HOOHOOB OOOHOHmOm mo OOOOOHOOH mo MOO MOO OOOOOOOHH OOOz||.mH OOOOHm mOOOE OH Omfi O m N H HN W O .O u a O HO .9 n e O .O .A d O .OH O E K .NH 83 .cowumaomw mo mxme v Hm>o muomflnsm msoum mpmaowH an mnflumm mHOflQSU mo mmpmu mwusu m0 mam Ham mocwsvmnm cwmzln.wH musmflm mxmmz cw mmfi w m N H Keg Jed fiauanbexg upaw 84 useful to consider it as a competing response system, when looking at the relative infrequent occurrence of Care of the Body Surface behaviour patterns. Another aspect of the pacing pattern was the per- vasive and apparent uninterruptable nature of this activity. On day 3 the fire alarm bell was tested in the experimental building during an observation. On the first ring the social subjects huddled into a corner and remained quiet during the following five rings. In con- trast to this the isolate subjects appeared to exhibit the "scatter" pattern very briefly on the first ring and immediately returned to pacing in the cubicles. On day 7 which was the first recorded freeze for the social birds, when a flashlight was dropped in the recording booth, the isolate jubjects--apparently exposed to the same stimula- tion--only continued to pace. On day 9 in response to a soft-drink bottle being kicked in the hallway, the social subjects all froze as a group for a 25 second period. Three of the isolate subjects, one on each end and one near the middle of the living unit, never stOpped pacing; the other four isolates paused momentarily but began moving before any subject in the social group made a movement. The first clear incidence of freezing in the isolate group was a 30 second freeze in subject #10 on day 17. On day 19 an incident previously described for the social group, that of a light bulb going out, to which the social group 85 "exploded" to one end of their cage, was apparently the stimulus for freezing in five of the isolates. Subjects 3 and 4 froze for 30 seconds; subjects 7, 8, and 10 froze for 45 seconds and the other two subjects only became inactive for a short period. It is interesting to note that freezing as a synchronous behaviour of the group does not appear in the isolate subjects during isolation. The Postural Pattern of crouching in the isolate birds very clearly shows up during isolation as seen in Figure 11. On day 16 the first crouch was observed to occur in this group and appeared to be in response to another isolate subject flying in his cubicle although these were not adjacent birds. Within a few days three of the isolate birds began a pattern of crouching prior to initiating flight. The more usual pattern after this time appeared to be one of a subject crouching in response to noises in the hallway or those made by other quail in the experimental room. The Cautious Alert posture appeared under conditions similar to those described for the social birds but in contrast was only observed to occur in single subjects at any one incidence of the behaviour while the birds were isolated. Behaviour after being assembled.--Removal of the partitions that provided the isolate cubicles was the occasion for continual "conversational cheeping" (Stoddard, 86 1931) among the isolate subjects but no distress calling was heard as this procedure was carried out by the experimenter. Following light onset in the newly assembled group the first noticeable behaviour was a Cautious Alert posture in the subjects. Most subjects maintained this posture while slowly swinging their body or head or both through an arc of about 180 degrees. Subsequently some of the subjects began walking about the cage while main- taining the extended neck component of the posture. This went on for several minutes until a subject popped, which resulted in the group of birds eXploding into a flurry of pOpping and flying quail. This would abruptly cease and all the subjects run to a cage wall and pile into the Huddle-Heap. Very soon one or two birds would venture out from the huddle and start walking across the cage, again in the cautious type posture, and soon the other birds would leave the huddle and traverse the cage until one bird would pOp and the cage would again eXplode and return to the Huddle-Heap Structure. One of the dramatic aspects of these huddles was the evident Sustained Tactual Contact posture observed here; the pushing component of that pattern was so forceful that birds on the interior of the huddle were continually being squeezed to the top of the pile. These birds would then begin again at the floor, also pushing against their cage mates in the huddle. This 87 resulted in a continually revolving group of birds and was the characteristic form of the Huddle-Heap seen in this group of subjects for the next few days. The carry over of this pushing component into the huddle structure was not the only incidence of the contact pattern observed in the newly assembled birds. Another pattern observed in these birds that was quite similar was what I began calling a "safe" position and involved a bird or two or three birds exhibiting the contact type pressing against a surface but with their head stuck into the corner between the cage wall and one of the troughs. Within the first week the Huddle and Spacing Group Resting Structure also began to show up in this group. It should be pointed out here that these were the only two Group Resting Structure patterns observed in this group. The birds had the same environ- mental living conditions as the other, socially reared, group and remained together until they were 75 days old and yet were never observed to form either the Linear, Ring of 1/2 Circle formations that were a regular aspect of the other group's resting behaviour throughout this period under these conditions. Within the class Locomotion, the frolic-with- object made its first appearance in this group of subjects on the combination day, as illustrated in Figure 14. Several of the other behaviour patterns that had been observed in some of the isolates during Phase I, 88 but had not reached 100% onset during that time, were now observed in the remainder of the group of birds after their assembly. These patterns were: Dusting, POpping, Frolicking, Crouching, Freezing, and the Cautious Alert Posture. The regularly scheduled observation procedure was altered on the first day of Phase II to include a con- tinuous two hour observation following assembly and two other short observations the same day, one at noon and the other at 3:00 p.m. followed by the regular evening observation. Throughout all of these observations the level of activity within the isolate cage remained high. Feeding, drinking, general preening, running, flit-pOpping, pOpping, flying, crouching, and generalized pecking were observed in all of the subjects during each of the observa— tion units mentioned above. Sleeping was not observed in any of the birds until the noon observation and sophisti- cated preening, oiling, frolicking, and bill wiping remained infrequent until the 3:00 observation. Approximately 30 minutes after the initial light onset and the beginning of these birds' eXperience at living in tactual and visual contact with conspecifics, the cautious exploratory and huddling behaviour decreased considerably and the birds began to move about the cage independently and allOpecking immediately became the predominate behaviour pattern for the group as they 89 started running wildly all over the cage pecking each other. A pecking encounter was typically unilateral and although the pecks appeared to be delivered with con- siderable force there were no wounded animals at the end of the day. It was often difficult to distinguish if a bird that was being pecked avoided the pecker or if another bird nearby provided a stimulus to approach and the bird moved off to peck some other bird. Adding to this confusion was the fact that very few pursuits were involved in these pecking encounters. The name of the game appeared to be "if a quail is anywhere near--peck it." The pecking organization was equally multilateral with one exception. There was one bird that was nearly always alone and just generally didn't appear to be involved in any of the group's activities. This bird seldom pecked other birds and was seldom pecked by other birds but didn't appear to avoid a bird that approached. A new form of allOpecking that had not previously been observed in the other group showed up toward the end of the morning observation and although it was never very frequent (a maximum of five encounters in one observation unit) its tOpography was impressive. This was a pattern called tail feather pulling and several times involved the recipient being pulled off balance when it occurred. This pattern occurred only sporadically in encounters 90 (from 0-3 in one observation) after day 30 to the end of the study. Another allOpecking behaviour that exhibited this same sporadic pattern of occurrence was the body peck. When observed in these birds it also appeared to be delivered with considerable force and often would knock the recipient off his feet. However, this allOpecking pattern did not appear until day 31. Table 3 below lists the raw frequency of encounters data for the allOpecking behaviour observed in these sub- jects on the initial day of Phase II. By comparing this with the data presented in Table 2 for the fifth week of allOpecking behaviour in the socially reared group, the constrast is readily apparent. TABLE 3.--Frequency of allOpecking encounters observed in the isolate reared group (N=7) on the day of assembly into a social living unit. Time of Observation Toe-Foot Head-Eye-Nares 6:00 - 7:00 a.m. ll 84 7:00 - 8:00 a.m. 16 106 12:00 -12:35 5 70 3:00 - 3:35 12 72 7:25 - 8:00 p.m. O 13 91 Given the generally high frequency of head pecking throughout the day on day 30, the figure for the evening observation is so low that one is tempted to surmise that the birds were too exhausted to continue pecking, particu- larly since the next morning's observation of this group resulted in a frequency tally of head pecks of 91. This high frequency of head pecking then began to flucutate through the remainder of Phase II, ranging from 31 to 91 with a median frequency for the series of observations of 73. Feet pecking remained low throughout the remainder of the study with a range of frequencies from 0-12. From the beginning of Phase III to the end of the study, days 36-75, the incidence of head pecking encounters fluctuated even more extremely than before, having a range from 8-78, with a median frequency over this series of observations of 39. Comparisons Between Social and’Isolate Groups Comparisons between the two groups can be made generally along three broad divisions: (l) Behaviour patterns shown by subjects in both groups during the period of differential rearing conditions, Phase I; (2) Group or Social interaction patterns, or patterns that appear immediately upon subjects being assembled into a group; and (3) Behaviour patterns unique to a group of 92 subjects eXperiencing the same rearing conditions for the first 30 days of age. These will be discussed in order. 1. Behaviours shown by subjects in both groups during Phase I (not including those patterns prevented by the imposed living conditions) included all the measured classes with the exception of one locomotion pattern that will be discussed later. The general patterns that are evident upon examination of Table l are: (a) the later median onset day for the Isolate group subjects in given behaviour category with (b) a correspondingly greater variation of range values (compare column 1 to column 4) for that behaviour category. In the Care of the Body Surface class of consummatory behaviours the Sophisticated Preening is the only exception to this pattern (see Figure 17). The class of Postural Patterns, Generalized Pecking behaviour, and the appearance of the cuticle shafts for both wing and tail feathers follows this pattern. In the Locomotion class, the pattern of a greater variation in range is evident throughout; however, the median onset day is earlier for the isolates than the social group in the jumping and flying categories but is identical for the running category. Figures 2, 3, 5, 6, 9, ll, 14, and 18-22 illustrate these two patterns visually for the above mentioned classes of behaviours shown by both groups. 93 .mmcoum oumaomH vac Hmwoom ca mcflcowum pmpmowpmfismom Mo pomco|l.ha wuomwm mom mo whoa ma ma va NH 0H m p F P p P p .h .VH .HN .mm .mm .mv .om chm .vw .HN .mb .mm .mm rOOH mamaomH I o queo lad eAIqunmna 94 .mmcoum mpMHOmH pom Hmwoom cH mchHz HHHm mo ummcoll.mH onsmwm cod no mama mH «H NH OH m w v m a b D b b I D P 4 I; .2 .mm .mm r2 .3 .3 -8 .d. .3 .mm 1mm is mumHomH . HMHoom O 1113:) 18d: GA'FQEIHUITID 95 .mmsoum prHomH cam HmHoom CH mcHnummlumsa mo pwmcoll.mH whomHm omfi mo mama om mm vm mm om mm mm vm ..l|\. . . , . . \ opMHomH . HMHoom O -\ .o .b .vH .HN .mm .m¢ .om .bm .wm .Hh awn .mm .mm r00H queo zed eArqunmno 96 cm mm .mmsonm ouMHomH cam Hmwoom CH mnHmmom mo uwmcoll.om musmHm mod mo whoa on «N mm om mH oH «H NH 0H m w v m n p n p p n P F a n - p - 3303 . HMHoom O . mm mm .om 11180 18d GATQQIHUIHD .mh .mm .mm .OOH 97 .mmcoum oymHOmH cam Hmwoom QH mQHmmomluHHm mo pomcoul.Hm mucmHm cod no mama mm mm Hm om mH mH 5H P p p p m p b I‘ quHomH A. HMHoom AV mH b I mH vH b ‘ MH h. NH - ‘ was 7H5 rm» row 1mm .IOOH queo 19a eArqunmna 98 .mmcoum oumHomH can HMHoom CH mQonmm ooNHHmuosmw mo pom:0||.mm_onsmwm mam mo mama .o .h oOMHOmH . .vH 380m 0 .HN .mm .mm .mv Tom .um .vm queo 19a aArqunmno .mh .mm .mm .OOH 99 2. Group or social interaction behaviours, or, patterns that appear immediately upon assembly into a group included the following: the frolic-with-object pattern of Locomotion behaviour. As defined in the measurement section, the frolic pattern is an obviously related com- ponent of the frolic-with—object pattern and as shown in Figure 23 the frolic pattern did occur in the isolate sub- jects during isolation. This makes it curious that the frolic-with-object pattern does not show up in the isolate group, see Figure 14, until after assembly. This would suggest that while isolation may not prevent this pattern neither does it encourage its eXpression. Since there was not a control group of birds that remained isolated during Phases II and III it is impossible to rule out the possibility that age can be a factor. However, the appearance of this pattern immediately following assembly would seem to support the suggestion that this is a social behaviour and thus requires the presence of at least one other animal to initiate the pattern, or to function as an elicitor to signal the appetitive components of the inter- action. Previous work (Ratner, 1965) showed that the pattern would occur in chickens isolated for 70 days, but only occurred in four out of fourteen birds. Figures 5 and 17 show the comparative onset of sophisticated preening and of allOpreening in the two groups of quail. The close correspondence of development of this 100 .mmconm mymHomH cam HMHoom SH cumupmm :OHHOEOUOH OHHoum mo ummcoI|.m~ onsmHm $04 mo mama 0v N mH wH VH NH OH m w v N b p p p p n p - n ? .o .h .VH fiHN .mm m mpMHOmH . .mm m... Hmwoom O .mv m .3. % .mm d a .@w I .2 m u .3 : .mw Tmm .OOH 101 pattern within the groups would suggest that a maturational view of this data might be a useful interpretation of these results although the data on feather deve10pment (see Figures 7 and 24), would not necessarily support that interpretation. The Resting Group Structure patterns, shown in Figure 8, illustrate that apparently the first formation E_“ of resting postures in a newly assembled group are the i more or less undifferentiated huddle type. As pointed out previously, it is interesting that while the isolate group had the same environmental conditions, in terms of light E- and heat, they did not develOp the typical bobwhite resting group formations. The data from the social group in this case point out that it is apparently not necessary to be raised in a nest with the parent quail present, for the Species-specific nesting or roosting formation to develop in a group of bobwhite young. The differences in allopecking behaviours, as shown in Tables 2 and 3, illustrate one of the more striking differences in the occurrence of the same pattern in the two groups. However, considering the natural history of this species and the age of these subjects, it is not necessarily surprising that a definite dominance order was not established in either of the groups under these living conditions. 102 .mdsoum conmmu QOHumHOmH pcm convoy AHHMHoom CH mummnm Hngmmm HHmu mo moqmummmmm HMHHHGHII.HN mhsmHm mom yo mama mN mN ¢N NN ON mH mH vH NH 0H .o .h mumHOmH . .VH .HN Hmfloom 0 .mm .mm .mv .om .hm .vm .Hh .wb .mm .mm .OOH dues 18d aArqetnan 103 3. Behaviour patterns unique to one or the other of the two groups included the following: (a) those unique to the isolate subjects included Cubicle Pacing as a predominant form of daily activity during isolation (see Figures 15 and 16), and the Sustained Tactual Contacting that contained components that carried over into the other phase of the study, adding a related component during Phase II, that of the "safe" position; the deve10pment of two forms of allOpecking that differed vastly from the forms observed in the social group; the ritual-like stereotopy patterns that were observed during isolation; and the fragmented nature of the tOpography of several of the maintenance patterns, specifically those of bill wiping, oiling and the correspondence of oiling with the sophisticated preening pattern. (b) The patterns unique to the social group included the previously mentioned Resting Group Structures of Linear, Ring, and 1/2 Circle formations; the smoother and more coordinated patterns of the Care of the Body Surface activities, and the corre- sponding higher frequency of occurrence of these behaviours as shown in Figures 3 and 4; and perhaps most striking is the apparent synchrony of the individual subjects within the group when engaged in various of the measured behaviour patterns but particularly the Postural Patterns. To conclude, further work should attempt to delineate the parameters of isolation for a social species 104 such as the bobwhite. Because it is a non-domestic species it should be a good preparation for probing what variables are involved in signaling group behaviours and perhaps skied some light on whether or not visual and tactual isolation without removal of conspecific auditory stimula- tion is a stressful condition that serves to facilitate the appearance of competing response patterns in isolated f conspecifics that normally live in a group. C“ D I SCUSSION ' The present study, in contrast to much of the previous laboratory work on behaviour deve10pment in precocial galliformes, had as one of its explicit objectives to provide a laboratory controlled environment for the subjects which more closely approximated natural living conditions. Specifically, observations involved minimal human contact between the subjects and the experimenter and regular diurnal light cycles were provided by simulated daylight or warm white illumination. In accord with the focus of the study an emphasis was placed on identifying and describing behaviour patterns as they appeared in these subjects in an attempt to provide baseline data on the behaviour of this species living under these laboratory conditions. Behaviour of the Social Group Six classes of behaviour patterns were identified and described for the group of birds reared together. The sequence in which the components of a behaviour class develOped was defined in terms of the median onset day for the group. For a summary of this schedule of develop- ment see Appendix A. The present findings suggest that 105 106' certain behaviour patterns are a useful index to the age of the bird. As the young quail matures more complex and elaborate forms of activities become a part of his behavioural repertoire. The schedule of deve10pment of the behaviour classes follow generally the report given by Stoddard (1931). It is not surprising that several differences were f“ found in the behaviour patterns of this species compared . to the domestic fowl and the Japanese quail. In contrast 1 to domestic fowl, the bobwhite is a natural gallinaceous bird; in contrast to the Japanese quail, it has a longer rum. i \‘JU period of deve10pment before reaching maturity, and it is a species that is non-territorial and is not known to fight in the wild except during the mating season (Errington, 1968; Stokes, 1967; Stoddard, 1931). Where comparisons of the data between the group and isolated birds is possible, both groups of subjects in the present study showed onset of Care of the Body Surface activities earlier than that reported for the domestic fowl by Ratner (1965) but evidenced a later onset for the comparable Locomotion categories of frolic and frolic- with-object. Both Guhl (1958) and Ratner (1965) report sparring and fighting occurring within four weeks of age in the domestic chicken with corresponding developments of dominance hierarchies. In addition to this Ratner 107 reports that bilateral pushing at the food tray develOped into food-dominate and food-submit patterns of interactions between birds and that threats and juvenile fights were observed within this period. Although aggressive allo- pecking developed early in the bobwhites and bilateral pushing was observed; threats, fights, dominate and submit patterns as described by Ratner were conspicuously absent from their patterns of behaviour. It is also of interest that the bobwhite in this study were never observed to suffer wounds as a result of the allOpecking that did occur, whereas in another study of bobwhites (Borchelt, 1970) several subjects were severely wounded from pecking encounters. However, differences between the two studies in the lighting cycle, access to food, space available per bird, and handling of the subjects may well make direct comparisons impossible to interpret in this instance. In a deve10pmental study of the behaviour patterns of social and isolate Japanese quail Farris (1964) described many of the same behaviour patterns observed in the present study. ‘Behaviour patterns included in his descriptions that were apparently similar in tOpography for these two species were the following: Resting postures, Generalized pecking, Competitive pecking, AllOpecking, Freezing, Alert, Escape scattering or fleeing, General preening, Bill wiping, Dusting, Feeding, Drinking, Walking, Running, Jumping, Popping, Flying, Frolic, and 108 Frolic-with-object. With the exceptions of Resting and Nutrition which were the same, and Popping and the Alert posture which were earlier for the bobwhite; the socially reared Coturnix showed an earlier median onset of the patterns listed above. The isolated Coturnix showed earlier median onsets for all of the patterns in comparison to the isolate group of bobwhites in the present study. Farris F‘= reports that in general there were few differences between E his two groups in age of onset in the above behaviour categories and the differences found are attributed to differences in the available living space for the two E groups. In contrast to this the birds in the present study were not subject to comparable living Space restrictions, for example, each isolate subject had suf- ficient space to fly and did make brief flights within the isolate cubicle. Therefore, available living space is not felt to be of primary importance relating to the differences observed in these two groups of bobwhite. The Coturnix is a species whose natural history differs considerably from that of the bobwhite; they are highly territorial, do not live in coveys, show minimal care of the young, and are sexually mature in 28 days. Since there are not only these species differences but also methodological differences between Farris' study and the present study direct comparison of the data is difficult to interpret. 109 The synchrony of the behaviour of individuals in the social group, the general level of activity, and the daily routine of the social group appeared to be basically similar to that described by Stoddard (1931) for wild groups of bobwhite. Specifically there were early and late feeding bouts, no fighting, and several sessions of Resting and Care of the Body Surface activities observed m1 between these early morning and late afternoon-early evening feeds. The present results suggest that laboratory conditions do permit the controlled observation of the behaviour of this species without severe disruption of natural and species-specific behaviour patterns. Behaviour of the Isolate Subjects During isolation the general behavioural deve10p- ment of the birds in this group paralleled that observed in the socially reared quail in that they showed the same behaviour patterns (with a few exceptions). However, the median onset was typically at a later age for the isolates. These results are generally in agreement with other investigations of the effects of isolation in the avian galliformes. Ratner (1965), similarly found that the isolated birds did exhibit the same individual behaviour patterns in isolation that socially reared fowl exhibited but showed them at a later age. 110 However, the birds in this group also exhibited some patterns unique to the isolated subjects. In particular, the Cubicle Pacing Pattern and the Sustained Tactual Contacting pattern were conspicuous and carried over into their behaviour after they were assembled. The pervasive nature of these patterns and the consistency with which they were seen as an integral part of the daily routine of these subjects, and their absence from the behaviour of the social group, suggest that tactual and visual contact during the first month of age is of major importance in the behaviour of the bobwhite quail. In the present study the relative frequency with which these patterns were engaged in by the isolate subjects, compared to other behaviour patterns, such as Care of the Body Surface behaviours suggests that it might be useful to consider how the pacing and contact patterns may have interferred or competed with the other classes of behaviour. That haptic stimulation from conspecifics is important to the normal deve10pment of the bobwhite is further suggested in these data by the absence of the linear, ring or half— circle group resting structure in the isolate birds even after having been assembled. The behaviour of the isolate birds immediately following their assembly into a social living environment was very similar to that described by Ratner (1965) for the parallel condition in the domestic fowl and lends 111 further support to the idea that tactual and visual isola- tion for a contact species of the galliformes is a powerful behavioural variable. In addition to this, the striking synchrony of group behaviour patterns observed in the socially reared subjects and its continual absence in the isolate group is a dimension of organization that should prove useful as a variable with which to assess the effects 7‘5 of isolation. f To conclude, the results of the present study, taken in conjunction with those of other studies on the galliformes, indicates that isolation may retard but does t not prevent the deve10pment of maintainance and social behaviour patterns in the isolated subjects in general, with two exceptions: (l) the species-Specific Resting Group Structure or roosting formation of the linear, ring or half-circle pattern, and (2) the group synchrony of Postural patterns such as freezing and the cautious alert. These patterns never appeared in the isolate group subjects in the laboratory even after having been combined into a social group and living together 45 days. However, before further studies undertake a comparison of social versus isolate rearing conditions as a determinant of later behavioural development, the author feels that a parametric study of isolation in this Species is necessary to provide the basis for making adequate interpretations that rest on a functional classification system. As indicated in the 112 literature (Stokes, 1967) the bobwhite is a highly auditory animal. in addition, the present results indicate that haptic stimulation may prove to be a critical compo- nent in the deve10pment of this bird. Further, it is generally accepted that avians are also very dependent upon their visual systems. Thus future research is planned to attempt to separate these three forms of fax stimulation in an attempt to deliniate what are the 1 actual variables being manipulated, in terms of the sensory systems of the animal, in an isolation study. 1w Summary The present study provided an initial description of several of the behaviour patterns of the very young bobwhite quail raised in the laboratory under the condi- tions described previously. The data collected confirm the reports offered by Stoddard (1931) of behaviour pat- terns of this species in a natural environment. The results obtained extend and support previous findings on the effect of visual and tactual isolation in a member of the gallinaceous birds in that these isolation conditions may retard but do not prevent the development of those behaviour patterns typically measured in this group of animals. In addition to the above, this set of observations provided descriptions of several behaviours previously unreported for this species reared in the laboratory and 113 attempted to characterize their patterns. These include the following: (1) distinct forms of preening, general, SOphisticated, and oiling; (2) components of the dusting pattern occurring on a bare wire floor and its possible relation to the intensity of thermal stimulation; (3) the age of onset of crouching as a Postural pattern in this Species; (4) the Cubicle Pacing and Sustained Tactual Con- ran tacting patterns as a persistent behaviour pattern of the f isolated subjects (Farris, personal communication) pre- viously observed but not described, measured or reported; (5) an attempt to characterize the synchronous versus the [I l fragmented nature of the tOpography of the group behaviour patterns as they differed for the two groups; and (6) the progression and deve10pment of components of the "roosting disk" Resting Group Structure for which the bobwhite is famous for to sportsmen and naturalists throughout the United States. Throughout this report an emphasis is placed on describing the behavioural repertoire of this species maintained under these laboratory conditions in an attempt to add to our understanding of the behaviour of a non- domestic animal and one which offers considerable promise as an excellent laboratory preparation for studying several of the classes of consummatory behaviours and the variables determining the form in which they are likely to occur. BIBLIOGRAPHY 114 25.32.! 31.7 BIBLIOGRAPHY Bent, A. C. Life Histories of North American Gallinaceous Birds. New York: Dover Publications, 1963. Bolles, R. C., and Woods, P. S. The ontogeny of behaviour in the albino rat. Animal Behaviour, 1964, EEJ 427-441. Borchelt, P. L. DevelOpment of fear displays (immobility) in the bobwhite quail. Unpublished masters thesis, Michigan State University, 1970. IA} 1.1:: Collias, N. E. An ecological and functional classification of animal sounds. In W. E. Lanyon and W. N. Tavolga (Eds.), Animal Sounds and Communication. t Washington, D.C.: American Institute Biological Science, 1960. Denny, M. R., and Ratner, S. C. Comparative Psychology. Homewood, Ill.: Dorsey Press, 1970. Errington, P. L. Of Predation and Life. Ames, Iowa: Iowa State University Press, 1967. Farris, H. E. Behavioural development, social organiza- tion, and conditioning of courtship behaviour in the Japanese quail, Coturnix coturnix Japonica. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, MIChigan State University, 1964. Garreffa, L. F. Group membership and group size as determinants of aggressive behaviour between pairs of male bobwhite quail (Colinus virginianus). Comm. Behav. Biol., 1969, Ali, 69-72. I Guhl, A. M. The deve10pment of social organization in the domestic chick. Animal Behaviour, 1958, 6, 92-111. Guhl, A. M. Social behaviour of the domestic fowl. Tech. Bull., 1953, 13, Kansas Agricultural Exp. Station, Manhattan, Kansas. 115 116 Hartford, A. The behaviour development of group-raised and isolation-raised female domestic fowl. Unpublished masters thesis, Michigan State University, 1967. Kirkpatrick, C. M. Factors in photoperiodism of bobwhite quail. Physiological Zoology, 1955, 28, 255-264. Menaker, M. Biological clocks. BioScience, 1969, 19, no. 18. Nelson, J. E. Behaviour of Australian PterOpodidae MegachirOptera. Animal Behaviour, 1965, 13, 544-557. Nice, M. M. DevelOpment of behaviour in precocial birds. Trans. Linnaean Soc. of New York, 1962, 8, 158-169. Ratner, S. C. Comparisons between behaviour deve10pment of normal and isolated domestic fowl. Animal Behaviour, 1965, 13, 497-503. Rosene, W. The Bobwhite Quail, its Life and Management. New BrunswiCk, N.J.: Rutgers University Press, 1969. Schein, M. W. Dust bathing in birds. In A. W. Stokes (Ed.), Animal Behaviour in Laboratory and Field. San Francisco: Freeman, 1968. Scott, J. P. Animal Behaviour. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1958. Spalding, D. Instinct. Macmillan's‘Magazine, 1873, 21, 282-293. Reprinted British J. Animal Behaviour, 1954, 2, 2-11. Stoddard, H. L. The Bobwhite Quail. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons, 1931. Stokes, A. W. Behaviour of the bobwhite quail, Colinus Virginianus. Auk, 1967, 84(1), 1-33. Sturkie, P. D. Avian Physiology. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press, 1965. Vince, M. A. Potential stimulation produced by avian embryos. Animal Behaviour, 1966, 14, 34-41. WOod-Gush, D. G. M. The behaviour of the domestic chicken: a review of the literature. British J. Animal Behaviour, 1955, 3, 81-110. APPENDIX 117 11 SEQUENTIAL APPEARANCE OF COMPONENTS OF THE MEASURED BEHAVIOUR CLASSES SHOWN BY MEDIAN ONSET DAY FOR TWO GROUPS OF BOBWHITE QUAIL Social NUTRITION CARE OF BODY SURFACE General Preening Bill Wiping Sophisticated Preening Oiling Dusting AllOpreening LOCOMOTION Running Popping Jumping Frolic Frolic-object Flit-popping Flying ACTIVITY Resting Huddle-Heap General Pecking Huddle-Space Linear Ring and Half Circle POSTURAL Freeze Cautious Alert Crouching ALLOPECKING Body Head-Nares Toe-Tarsus FEATHER SHAFT APPEARANCE Wing Tail 15 17 19 36 NH U1U'lrb-Ni-‘l-4 \ICDQ 12 12 Isolate NUTRITION CARE OF BODY SURFACE General Preening Bill Wiping Sophisticated Preening Oil Gland Preening AllOpreening Dusting LOCOMOTION Running Jumping Frolic Popping Flying Flit-popping Frolic-object ACTIVITY Resting Cubicle Pacing General Pecking Huddle-Heap Huddle-Space Linear, Ring POSTURAL Sustained Contact Freeze Crouching Cautious Alert ALLOPECKING Body, Head, Toe FEATHER SHAFT APPEARANCE Wing Tail 15 24 32 36 16 20 lllll{HillMM)"lgllfllljlIIIIIIIHIIIIIHIIIIHIllil 69 0120