EFFEm. GF AUDITQR‘! FREQUENCY .3659 MAREEER {3F 9R§SE§Q¥X§EQ§€ G? SIfiNALS 6N JUDGMEN? OF 93$?ANCE 3‘? $5.53??? if‘éQi’fiDifiALS Thesis for the: 339339 of M. A. MICHJGAM Si‘ATE U’NEZ’ERSSW‘ Lymz E. Miner €963 LIBRARY Michigan State University. ABSTRACT EFFECTS OF AUDITORY FREQUENCY AND MANNER OF PRESENTATION OF SIGNALS ON JUDGMENT OF DISTANCE BY BLIND INDIVIDUALS by Lynn E. Miner This study is concerned with the auditory skills of blind individuals. The purpose of this thesis was to analyze the effect of auditory frequency and manner of presentation of signals on the judgment of distance by blind individuals. Three primary variables were investi- gated in this study; They were auditory frequency, distance from a sound source, and presentation of the auditory stimulus. Thirty-six visually handicapped students from the Michigan School for the Blind participated in this experi- ment. Each subject was presented a certain set of conditions at a distance of ten feet from the sound source in the test room. Next, he was disoriented in the test room and repositioned at another predetermined distance. He was then instructed to walk forward until he again perceived his distance from the sound source as ten feet. The varying conditions for the subjects were auditory frequency, distance from the sound source, and manner of presentation (continuous vs. intermittent) of the auditory stimulus. Lynn E. Miner The error judgment in perceived distance for each subject was measured and recorded. An analysis of variance was computed to determine the significance of differences between mean scores. No statistically significant results were obtained. None of the F ratios were significant at the .05 level of confidence. The interactions between these variables were computed and found to be nonsignificant at the .05 level of confidence. The major findings of this study do not indicate that auditory frequency, distance from the sound source, and the manner of presentation of the auditory stimulus are significant variables affecting the perception of distance from the sound source by the blind. EFFECTS OF AUDITORY FREQUENCY AND MANNER OF PRESENTATION OF SIGNALS ON JUDGMENT OF DISTANCE BY BLIND INDIVIDUALS By Lynn E. Miner A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Speech 1963 DEDICATION Dedicated -- not to a person but to a profession which is devoted to serving people. May we never lose sight of our goal. ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The writer wishes to express his profound appreciation to those who have given advice and guidance in the preparation of this thesis. Gratitude and reCOgnition are extended to Dr. James W. Hillis for his diligent direction of this thesis. Acknowledgement is extended to Dr. Herbert J. Oyer who constantly motivated and inspired the author to broaden his understanding and appreciation of our professional field. The writer is conscious of his obligation to the administration, faculty, and students at the Michigan School for the Blind who directly or indirectly have been a professional influence. The author wishes to thank Mrs. Deirdre Circle for meticulously editing and typing the trial and final editions of this thesis. Finally, the writer is thankful to his parents for providing him with a philosophy of education based on insight and wisdom. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter Page I STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM . Introduction . . . Statement of the Problem Definitions. . . . Questions Posed at Outset. II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE . . . . . . . . . . . III SUBJECTS, EQUIPMENT, AND PROCEDURES SubJeCtS O O 0 O O O O O O O O 0 Equipment. . . . . . . . . . . . Procedures . . . . . . . . . . . O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O d—L—L—L IV RESULTS AND DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . Results. 0 o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o' 0 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . —‘\O\O O\O\U‘lU1 x] Ul-P'J-‘d‘ Mud—L V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS. . . . . . summary 0 O O O O O O O 0 O O O 0 Conclusions. . . . . Implications for future research 0 O O O [UNIUIU CDCDU'IU'I BIBLIOGRAPHY 0 0 O O O O O 0 O o O O O O O O O 0 O 0 K)! O APPENDICES O O 0 O O O O o O O O O O 0 O O I O 0 O O KN DJ iv LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1 ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20 2 SOUND PRESSURE LEVELS IN TEST ROOM. . . . . . . 23 CHAPTER I STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM Introduction Our senses keep us informed of the happenings in our environment. These senses, especially the distance senses of sight and hearing, help us orient to our surroundings. Individuals with diminished visual acuity are, of necessity, forced to rely on audition in gaining information about their environment. Through hearing, a blind person can learn to evaluate personality. . . . Through changes in pitch, rhythm, and quality of tone he can note the various moods corresponding to the gamut of the emotions. How well he is able to associate the various aspects of the human voice with the emotions will depend upon his intelligence intuition and experience in contact with people.1 Blindness imposes certain basic limitations on the blind individual. Each limitation necessitates definite adjustments in the learning process for the visually handicapped. First, blindness places restrictions in the range and variety of experiences in which the individual may partake. The visual and tactual senses experience considerable limitations. Second, blindness imposes restrictions in the control of the environment and the self-concept in relation to the environment. Vision gives 1Carl Weiss, "Reality Aspects of Blindness As They Affect Case Work," The Family, (February, 1946), p. 12. us a great deal of control over our environment. Finally, blindness imposes restrictions on mobility and orientation. This is probably the most serious of all restrictions and the one with which this thesis is most directly concerned. The blind child cannot change his immediate environment; he is dependent on the assistance of others. This depend- ence could affect his social relationships and lead to self debasement.1 The role of auditory skills in mobility and orien- tation is immensely important. Unfortunately, however, as Rusalem suggests, In the area of mobility, we have largely been playing by ear. We have been aware that certain procedures work better than others and that blind persons differ in their ability to use various travel techniques. However, we have lacked the basic orientation which might make our work scientific and which would hasten the day when most blind persons could travel independ- ently. It would be argued that even our present knowledge is not being effectively dissemenated, and that is probably true.2 Rusalem adds that the . . . loss of the ability to get about has been a crucial problem in the rehabilitation of the blind. Frequently, the success of a variety of the social and vocational services depends upon preliminary success in teaching independent travel. Lack of mobility can isolate a blind person from his environ- ment. 1Paul A. Zahl, Blindness, (Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press, 1950), p. 92. 2Herbert Rusalem, "Research in Review," New Outlook for the Blind, LII, No. 8 (1958), p.-313, . 3Ibid. He goes on to limit the problem as follows: "The nub of the problem is that there is sufficient auditory information in the environment if the crucial properties of the sound field could be perceived and recognized."1 Lowenfeld described the problem similarly when he stated: The most important factor in increasing the blind child's ability to gain experiences is his own ability to get about and secure stimulation by himself. How to learn his way in familiar and unfamiliar surround- ings is a never-ending task that begins with the blind child's first steps.2 One auditory skill essential to effective mobility and orientation is mastery of sound localization. Sound localization is the process of determining the distance and direction of an acoustic stimulus. Three coordinates are fundamental to this ability: a judgment of distance and the interaction of two angles. A majority of existing studies have concentrated on the determination of the direction of the acoustic stimulus. Other studies relative to sound localization are comparative studies which deal with differences among the blind and sighted. The orien- tation of this thesis is toward the role of distance, auditory frequency and pulsation in sound localization. 1Ibid. 2Berthold Lowenfeld Our Blind Children (Springfield Ill.: Charles C. Thomas Pub., 195677Ip. 1 . , ’ Statement of the Problem For the blind individual to become mobile, he must learn to take advantage of available sensory clues. This includes mastery of certain basic auditory skills. The purpose of this research is to investigate the effects of auditory frequency and manner of presentation of signals on judgment of distance by blind individuals. The scope of this thesis is restricted to the above mentioned auditory signals. Definitions The following terms are defined for clarification in this study: Auditory frequency.--The three frequencies explored are 500, 2000, and 8000 cycles per second. Manner of presentation.--Both continuous and inter- mittent tones were utilized. Judgment of distance.--The individuals conception of the space between himself and the source of the auditory stimulus. In this instance each person was asked to make a judgment of ten feet between himself and the source of the stimulus. Blind individuals.--Thirty-six braille students at the Michigan School for the Blind. Most of the participants were totally blind. Students with light perception utilized blindfolds. Two other terms, while not directly involved in this study, need definition. The terms are frequently employed when referring to the rehabilitation of the blind. Mobility.--The term used to denote the ability to navigate from one's present fixed position to one's desired position in another part of the environment.1 Orientation.--The process of utilizing the remaining senses in establishing one's position and relationship to all other significant objects in one's environment.2 Questions Posed at Outset Questions posed at the outset of this study were: 1. Do certain auditory frequencies allow for a more accurate judgment of distance than other auditory frequencies? 2. How does initial distance from a sound source affect the blind individual's perception of another predeter- mined distance? 3. Which type of presentation allows for a more accurate judgment of distance: a continuous or intermittent tone? 4. Is there a dependence of presentation (continuous vs. 1"Orientation and Mobility Terms" (American Association of Instructors of the Blind, St. Louis, Mo., March, 1962), p. l. (Mimeographed.) 21bid. O\ intermittent) on distance from the sound source in the auditory perception of a distance of ten feet? Is there a dependence of auditory frequency on initial distance from a sound source in the auditory perception of a distance of ten feet? Is there a dependence of auditory frequency on presen- tation (continuous vs. intermittent) in the auditory perception of a distance of ten feet? CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE In the area of mobility and orientation of the blind, little systematic information is available regarding the function of auditory sensations. The bulk of available information consists of comparative studies contrasting the blind and sighted on auditory performance tasks. Most of these studies are concerned with sound localization. One of the few studies dealing with the role of pitch was conducted by Cotzin and Dallenbach. They investigated the function of pitch in the perception of obstacles by the blind. This phenomenon is sometimes referred to as "facial vision." As a result of their investigation, they indicated that "aural stimulation by reflections from the obstacles is both a necessary and sufficient condition for object perception." They concluded further that "changes in pitch are the basic clues of the perception of obstacles by the blind, and that they do not occur unless the higher partials, approximately 10,000 cps and above, are present in the stimulus sounds."1 Supa joined forces with the above authors in an 1M. Cotzin and K. M. Dallenbach, "Facial Vision: The Role of Pitch and Loudness in the Perception of Obstacles b the Blind," Amegican Journalgof Psychology, LXIII (1950 , p. 512. earlier study of obstacle perception by the blind. Their study was exhaustive in nature and represented an important contribution to the final solution of this problem. Seven experiments were conducted, three preliminary experiments and four main experiments. The preliminary experiments were exploratory and normative. Blind and sighted subjects, all with blindfolds over their eyes, were placed at various distances in front of either a wall or a portable masonite board. They were asked to walk toward the obstacle and (1) indicate their first perception of it and (2) to approach it as closely as possible without touching it. In each of the preliminary experiments, twenty-five trials were obtained from each subject. The average distance and mean variation of the subject's first perception and final appraisal were computed for the statistical data of tables. In the first experiment the end wall of a large hall was the obstacle and the starting point of the subject varied. In the second experiment the screen was the obstacle and its position was varied; the subjects started from a fixed point. In the third experiment the position of both the subject and the screen was varied. The authors found that while the blind in this study possessed a high degree of ability to perceive obstacles at a distance, the sighted could easily acquire this ability with practice. The purpose of the four main experiments by Supa, Cotzin, and Dallenbach was to control certain sensory cues. The first main experiment was conducted in order to determine whether pressure sensations aroused by reflected air waves were necessary to the perception of obstacles. The second main experiment was designed to ascertain whether or not aural stimulation by reflected sound waves was a necessary condition for the perception of obstacles. In the third main experiment the exposed areas of the skin were left open to air and sound waves but all auditory stimuli were drowned out by a masking sound conducted to a set of head- phones worn over the ears of the subject. In the fourth main experiment each subject was placed in a sound proof room with high-fidelity headphones over his ears through which he could listen to the sounds of the experimenter who, carrying a microphone, walked in another room towards the obstacle. All subjects were able to report first perceptions and final appraisals in this experiment where only ear stimulation could play a role. Their most general, relevant conclusion to be noted here was that "aural stimulation is both a necessary and sufficient condition for the perception of obstacles."1 Fletcher investigated the accuracy with which blind- folded subjects could localize the distance of sound sources. Three main variables were studied. They included the dis- tance of the sound source from the subjects, the frequency 1M. Supa, M. Cotzin, and K. M. Dallenbach, "Facial Vision: The Perception of Obstacles by the Blind,".Amer;can Journal of Psychology, LVII (April, 1944), p. 183. 10 of the sound source, and the audible angle at the subjects. His findings indicated that the subjects could localize the distance of sound sources with more than chance accuracy. Distance was the only significant variable.1 A study by McCarty and Worchel makes reference to the possibility of a mechanism similar to the one used by bats in their environmental orientation. It is known that bats use a type of vocal radar to guide them in utter darkness, and thus it would seem that a similar mechanism is at work in humans; but if an object is closer than fifty feet the echo and original sound blend to such an extent that they can- not be heard separately by the human ear. Work by Cotzin and Dallenbach, however, indicated that rather than being the pure echo, it was a dif- ference in tone between the original sound and the echo. This is known as the Doppler Shift, and it is the same as the often observed difference in the sound of a car as it approaches and then recedes." In other words, the blind perceive objects when they hear a rise in pitch of the echo (Doppler Shift). This difference in frequency with its relative intensity and manner of presentation tells them where the obstacle is located. Griffin has investigated some of the variables in sound localization. He stated that a thermal noise (100 through 12,000 cps) was more effective in detecting an obstacle than were pure tones. He also noted that pure 1John L. Fletcher, "Localization of Sounds in Depth," U. S. A. Medical Research Laboratory Report, MXMLVII, . NO. 302 ll (1957), P. 1E. 2M. McCarty and P. Worchel, "Rate of Motion and Object Perception in the Blind," New Outlook for the Blind, XLVIII (November, 1954), p. 315. 11 tones of 10,000 cps and higher provided for more accurate detection of obstacles than the frequencies 125-8000.1 One of Griffins' studies dealt with the acoustical orientation of the oil bird, steatornis. He made an acoustic analysis of the sound emitted by this bird while flying about in a totally dark cave. By emitting sounds the bird was able to avoid obstacles while in flight. The most appreciable components of the emitted sounds were between 6000 and 10,000 cps. The average frequency was 7000 cps; the duration of each sound was about one msec. with intervals between sounds averaging 2.6 msec. When the ears of the bird were plugged, they lost theirwebholocation ability.2 An investigation into the function of auditory frequencies in sound localization was authored by Fedderson, Jeffres, Sandel and Teas. They stated: We may conclude that the localization of high frequency pure tones, where there is no cue provided by the onset of the tone, demands a difference of level at the two ears which can be provided only by tones above 5000 cps. At the lower frequencies where diffraction around the head is less and the differences of level, therefore, smaller, the subject consistently underestimates the azimuth angle. This underestimation decreases with increasing frequency and increases with increasing A__A._ 1% 1Donald R. Griffin, Listenin in the Dark, New Haven: Yale University Press, 1958), p. 308. 2Donald R. Griffin, "Acoustic Orientation in the 011 Bird, Steatornis," National Academy of Sciences, XXXIX, No. 8 (1953), PD. 88 ~93. 12 azimuth angle.1 One of the parameters of the present study is concerned with the presentation of the stimulus. Both continuous and interrupted tones are utilized. Zwislocki, Hellman, and Verillo conducted a study in this regard. It was their impression that Neural responses to pulses seem to be more easily detected and quantified than responses to other sound stimuli. Consequently, in the search for physio- logical correlates of certain characteristics of psychophysical responsesé pulse stimuli appear particularly attractive. von Bakesy has written of the spatial attributes of sounds. He stated: Perception of the distance of a sound depends upon characteristics of the sound field that still are not well understood, but in the experimental situation the apparent distance was determined simply by loudness. The less the loudness, the farther away from the head the sound image seemed to be, and when the loudness remained constant this distance was always the same.3 The ratio of the direct sound to the reverberant sound intensity has been suggested by Fletcher as a factor influencing depth localization. He specified that . . . either a reduction in loudness or a decrease in ratio of direct to reverberant sound intensity, or both, cause the sound to appear to move away from the observer. 1W. E. Fedderson et al., "Localization of High Frequency Tones," Journal of the Acoustical Societ of America, XXIX (September, 1957), pp. 399-91. 2J. Zwislocki, R. P. Hellman, and R. T. Verillo, "Threshold of Audibility for Short Pulses," Journal of the AcousticalfSociet of America, XXIV, No. 10 (1962), pp. 162+8‘520 3Georg von Bakesy, Ex eriments in Hearin , (New York: NcGraw-Hill Co., Inc., 1960), p. 280. 13 . . .It has not been found possible to put these relationships on a quantitative basis. Probably a given loudness change, or a given change in ratio of direct to reverberant sound intensity, causes dif- ferent sensations of depth depending upon the character of the reproduced sound and upon the observer's familiarity with the acoustic conditions surrounding the reproduction. Since the depth localization is inaccurate even when listening directly, it is difficult to obtain sufficiently accurate data to be of much use in a quantitative way. Because of this inaccuracy, good auditory perspective may be obtained with reproduced sounds even though the properties controlling depth localization depart materially from those of the original sound.1 The auditory skills of blinded individuals training with pilot dogs were investigated by O'Neill, Oyer, and Baker. The purpose of their study was to determine whether a significant relationship existed between the ability of blinded individuals to use a pilot dog successfully and their hearing acuity, hearing discrimination, and sound localization ability. Fifty-three subjects were admin- istered a battery of tests which included the following: (1) pure tone threshold tests, (2) sound discrimination tests, and (3) sound localization tests. The trainees under study were evaluated for each subject's relative skill in the use of a pilot dog by trainers employed in the Pilot Dog training program. The results suggested that auditory acuity and the ability to localize sound may determine 1Harvey Fletcher, Speech and Hearing in Communi- cation, (Princeton, New Jersey: D. Van Nostrand Co., Inc., 1953), PP. 221-22. 14 proficiency in the use of a pilot dog by a blinded person.1 In summary, the literature contains several studies which are related to this thesis. It suggests that the higher auditory frequencies should allow for the greatest accuracy in the judgment of distance from a sound source. The main variables affecting the auditory perception of the distance of a sound source include auditory frequency, the distance from the sound source, the manner of presentation of the auditory stimulus, the intensity of the auditory stimulus, and the acoustical characteristics of the environ- ment. 1John O'Neill, Herbert Oyer, and Donald Baker, "Auditory Skills of Blinded Individuals Training With Pilot Dogs," Journal of S eech and Hearin Research, I, No. 3 (September, 1958), pp. 262-67. CHAPTER III SUBJECTS, EQUIPMENT AND PROCEDURES Subjects The subjects for this study were thirty-six high school students at the Michigan School for the Blind. Nineteen boys and seventeen girls participated in the study. All subjects were required to pass a pure tone hearing test at a ten-decibel level for each ear. The following frequencies were employed: 500, 1000, 2000, 4000, and 8000 cps. All students had been examined by an ophthalmologist and declared legally blind prior to their admission to the school. All of the students were totally blind. The few who had light perception were blindfolded before being exposed to the testing situation. All except three of the thirty-six subjects had lost their visual acuity before the age of two. Most of the subjects had been blind since birth; retrolental fibroplasia was the major cause for the visual losses as indicated by the ophthalmological reports. The three remaining subjects had lost their vision at age three, five and sixteen respec- tively. Only students with average or above average intel- ligence as determined by school achievement were utilized. All subjects were randomly selected within the visual, 15 16 auditory and intellectual restrictions mentioned. Equipment The experiment was conducted in a room which had been partially treated with acoustical tile. A table indicating the acoustical specifications of the room is found in Appendix A. These specifications were computed by the Building Division of the State of Michigan. Pure-tone signals were generated for the experiment by a Maico Audiometer, model H1-B. The pure tones were directed from the audiometer through a six inch speaker, type #6 CM-47 manufactured by the Oxford Electrical Cor- poration. The three pure tones presented were 500 cps, 2000 cps, and 8000 cps. Procedures Prior to the experiment each student had satis- factorily passed a pure-tone screening test at ten decibels in each ear for the frequencies previously mentioned. The following instructions were read to the subjects before the experiment began: In just a moment you will be asked to listen closely to a particular tone. You will be exactly ten feet from that tone. Next you will be taken back a greater distance from that tone and asked to walk forward until you again think you are ten feet from that tone. A guide rope is provided as an aid in walking a straight line. Remember: walk forward until you are ten feet from the tone. Once you have estimated the correct distance, remain standing still until further instructions are given. Do you have any questions? The speaker was positioned one foot from a wall and 17 five feet above the floor. A fifty-five foot guide rope was fastened between the speaker and a pole. The speaker was positioned at the approximate head level of the subjects and pointed toward the head of each subject. Afterthe subject had the opportunity to hear the stimulus at exactly ten feet from the sound, an irregular path was followed, thus attempting to disorient the indi- vidual. One of three different starting distances was employed for each subject. Subjects were positioned at either 26 feet, 38 feet, or 50 feet from the auditory signal. One of two types of auditory signals (continuous vs. intermittent) was presented to each subject. The intermittent tones were manually pulsed every .5 second for a .5 second duration. The table in Appendix B indicates the intensity of the stimuli presented. The intensity was presented at 50 decibels above audiometric zero. Audiometric zero for each frequency was ascertained by determining the threshold tested at a distance of three feet from the sound source. This procedure was conducted in the sound field of the actual test room on a pair of normal ears. Once the subject had completed his estimate of ten feet, his judgment error was measured. The judgment error was the distance between the exact ten foot mark and the heels of the subject. The error was recorded in inches and employed as the basic measure or score in the experiment. 18 Three fundamental conditions were explored in this experiment. There were several levels for each condition. Each combination of conditions, or events, was presented to two randomly selected subjects from the sample of thirty- six. The table in Appendix C indicates the specific combi- nations of events presented to each subject. The first condition examined was auditory frequency. Three different auditory frequencies were presented. They were 500 cps, 2000 cps, and 8000 cps. One of the purposes of this study was to determine if certain auditory frequen- cies allow for a more accurate judgment of distance than other auditory frequencies. The second condition investigated in this study concerned the type of presentation of the auditory signal. The two types of presentation were continuous tones and intermittent tones. Another purpose of this thesis was to determine which type of presentation allows for more accuracy in the judgment of distance from a sound. The third condition of this study dealt with the manner in which the blind individual's initial distance from a sound source affected his perception of a distance of ten feet. The subjects were randomly placed at three different starting distances from the sound source. Those distances were 26 feet, 38 feet, and 50 feet. CHAPTER IV RESULTS AND DISCUSSION maria A 3 x 3 x 2 factorial analysis of variance was employed to determine the significance of differences between mean scores.1 Six basic questions were posed at the outset of this study, namely: 1. Do certain auditory frequencies allow for a more accurate judgment of distance than other auditory frequencies? 2. How does initial distance from a sound source affect the blind individual's perception of another pre- determined distance? 3. Which type of presentation allows for a more accurate judgment of distance: a continuous or intermittent tone? 4. Is there a dependence of presentation (continuous vs. intermittent) on distance from the sound source in the auditory perception of a distance of ten feet? 5. Is there a dependence of auditory frequency on initial distance from a sound source in the auditory perception 1Allen L. Edwards, Experimental Design_in Psycho- lo ical Research, (rev. ed.; New York: Rinehart Co., Inc., 1950), p. 201-07. 19 20 of a distance of ten feet? 6. Is there a dependence of auditory frequency on presen- tation (continuous vs. intermittent) in the auditory perception of a distance of ten feet? The results of the analysis of variance are indicated in Table 1. TABLE 1 ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE Source of Sum of d. f. Mean F variation Squares , Square Frequency (F) 1927.72 2 963.86 .... Distance (D) 264.05 2 132.03 .... Presentation (P) 7921.79 1 7921.46 3.24* F x D 21233.79 4 5308.45 2.33* F x P 169.73 2 84.87 .... D X P ~. 5401.06 2 2700.53 1.66* F x D x P 4907.75 4 1226.94 .... Within Treatments.~ 41825.56 18 2323.64 .... TOTAL 83651.12 35 *The resulting F was not statistically significant at the .05 level of confidence Inspection of Table 1 reveals that no significant F ratios were obtained at the .05 level of confidence. The triple interaction variance (F x D x P) was combined with 21 the Within Treatments variance in an effort to further evaluate for significant results. No significant F scores, however, resulted from this procedure at the .05 level of confidence. Discussion There was no statistically significant difference in the three frequencies relative to judgments of distance. No one frequency yielded more accurate judgments over another. Auditory frequency was not demonstrated to be a significant variable as it pertained to a judgment of dis- tance from a sound. There was no statistically significant difference in the three starting distances relative to the judgment of a sound source. There was no statistically significant difference in manner of presentation of the stimulus relative to a judgment of distance. Neither the continuous nor the inter- mittent tone was demonstrated as allowing a more accurate judgment. After assessing the statistical significance of the three parameters of this study, the interaction among these variables was computed. The interaction of presentation by distance was not statistically significant as it pertains to accuracy in the judgment of distance. The interaction of frequency by distance was not statistically significant relative to accuracy in the judgment of distance. The 22 interaction of frequency by presentation was not a statis- tically significant variable influencing the accuracy in judgment of distance. The physical dimension of the auditory stimulus in the present investigation was held constant for each subject. Due to the acoustical properties of the room, however, the subjects may have perceived variations in intensity.for the various distances. These variations in intensity were measured by a Mine Safety Appliance sound pressure level meter. The ambient noise in the test room at ten feet from the sound source was 46 db. The auditory frequencies pre- sented were analyzed by a narrow band filter with a range of 200 cps above and below each frequency. The measurements are indicated in Table 2. In this regard Békésy notes that "an increase in loudness produces a clear reduction in the distance of the diffuse image. Thus it seems that loudness has an effect upon the perceived distance only in the absence of the other more determinant physical cues."1 There are several factors which might account for the negative results of this study. One factor is the size of the sample utilized. A larger sample might have yielded significant results, especially with regard to the frequency by distance interaction. The frequency by distance F ratio obtained was 2.33; an F ratio of significance at the .05 / 1Bekésy, op, cit., p. 304. 23 level of confidence is 2.93. As previously suggested, a larger sample might have indicated significant results. TABLE 2 SOUND PRESSURE LEVELS IN TEST ROOM Frequency Distance from Sound Pressure Analyzed Sound Source Level 500 cps. 10 feet 55 db. 500 cps. 26 feet 56 db. 500 cps. 38 feet 51 db. 500 cps. 50 feet 52 db. 2000 cps. 10 feet 41 db. 2000 cps. 26 feet 38 db. 2000 cps. 38 feet 36 db. 2000 cps. 50 feet 39 db. 8000 cps. 10 feet 56 db. 8000 cps. 26 feet 40 db. 8000 cps. 38 feet 33 db. 8000 cps. 50 feet 32 db. Another factor which might account for the negative results of this study concerns the acoustic characteristics of the test room. Although the reverberation time for the room was relatively low (1.24 seconds), it is conceivable that the time lag and accompanying distortions might have influenced the subjects perception of distance from a sound 24 source. Conducting this same experiment in an anechoic chamber might reveal different findings. It is particularly interesting to note that frequency did not play a significant role in the perception of dis- tance from a sound source by the blind. This finding stands in contradiction to some of the findings in the review of the literature. Griffin, for example, stated that the higher pure tones (around 10,000 cps) provide for more accurate perception of obstacles than the lower frequencies (125-8000 cps). Again, the acoustical characteristics of the room might account for this apparent discrepancy. In summary, the findings of this study do not support the hypothesis that auditory frequency, method of presen- tation, and distance as defined in this thesis are signif- icant variables relative to the blind individual's judgment of distance from a sound source. Both the size of the sample and the acoustical characteristics of the test room are factors which might account for the negative results. CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Summary This study is concerned with the auditory skills of blind individuals. It investigates one aspect of the ways blind individuals utilize sound in mobility and orientation. The purpose of this thesis was to analyze the effects of auditory frequency and manner of presentation of signals on judgment of distance by blind individuals. Six basic questions were posed at the outset of the research. These questions were initially posed in Chapter I and elaborated on in Chapter IV. A review of the literature revealed some studies which related to this thesis but none which had a similar experimental design. Earlier research by Cotzin and Dallen- bach1 and more recent investigations by Griffin2 infer that the higher auditory frequencies might allow for the greatest accuracy in the judgment of distance from a sound source. Several authors3:4 have indicated some of the variables affecting the perception of the distance of a 1Cotzin and Dallenbach, op, cit., p. 512. 2Griffin, Listeningfin the Dark, 100. cit. 3Harvey Fletcher, op. cit., pp. 221-22. 4Békésy, op, cit., p. 208. 25 26 sound source. These variables include auditory frequency, distance from the sound source, manner of presentation of the auditory stimulus, intensity of the auditory stimulus, and acoustical characteristics of the environment. The experimental procedure utilized thirty-six visually handicapped students at the Michigan School for the Blind. Each of them had normal hearing and average intelligence or above average intelligence. Nearly all of the subjects were blind at birth. Three primary variables were investigated in this study. The first variable was auditory frequency. The three frequencies utilized were 500 cps, 2000 cps, and 8000 cps. Another variable was the manner of presentation of the sound stimulus. Both a continuous tone and an intermittent tone were employed in the experimental pro- cedure. The pure tone auditory signals in this study were generated from an audiometer through a speaker into the test room. The third variable was the distance from the sound stimulus. The subjects were positioned at three dif- ferent distances from the sound source; those distances were 26 feet, 38 feet, and 50 feet. The subjects were led into the test room and given the instructions outlined in Chapter III. Next, they were taken to a location exactly ten feet from the sound stimulus. Each subject was randomly presented one stimulus. It was one of the three frequencies mentioned above and was 27 presented as a continuous tone or an intermittent tone. The table in Appendix C indicates the exact conditions for each subject. After the subjects had the opportunity to hear their stimulus at a distance of ten feet from the sound source, they were disoriented in the room and repositioned at either 26 feet, 38 feet, or 50 feet from the sound stimulus. Their instructions were to walk forward until they again perceived the distance of ten feet from the sound source. Their only clue was the auditory stimulus presented to them. Each subject participated in just one condition. The error judgment for each Subject was measured and recorded. An analysis of variance was computed to determine the significance of differences among mean scores. The results of the statistical computation revealed nonsignif- icant findings. None of the F scores were statistically significant at the .05 level of confidence. This study indicated that auditory frequency, distance from the sound source, and manner of presentation of the auditory stimulus were nonsignificant variables affecting the perception of distance from a sound source by the blind. The interactions between these variables were also nonsignificant at the .05 level of confidence. It is conceivable, however, that a larger sample and a change in the acoustical specifications of the environment might find one or more of these variables more significant than indicated in this study. 28 W The following conclusions are noted on the basis of the results obtained in this investigation: 1. It cannot be said that any of the auditory frequencies investigated allows for a more accurate judgment of distance from a sound source than the others. 2. It cannot be said that the initial distance from a sound source affects blind individuals' perception of another predetermined distance. 3. It cannot be said that either type of presentation (continuous vs. intermittent) allows for a more accurate judgment of distance than the other. 4. It cannot be said that there is a dependency of pre- sentation (continuous vs. intermittent) on distance from the sound source in the auditory perception of a distance of ten feet. 5. It cannot be said that there is a dependency of auditory frequency on initial distance from a sound source in the auditory perception of a distance of ten feet. 6. It cannot be said that there is a dependency of auditory frequency on presentation (continuous vs. intermittent) in the auditory perception of a distance of ten feet. Implications for Future Research It would be interesting to repeat this experiment With a larger number of subjects. Since the F for frequency by distance interaction was almost statistically significant 29 at the .05 level of confidence, a larger sample might yield significant results. Additional research relative to the function of auditory signals in the perception of distances is needed. It might be found that some type of mechanical guidance device emitting an auditory signal could be utilized by the blind in negotiating the obstacles of their environment. Also, such a device might be employed by the military in detecting obstacles at distances which cannot now be located by conventional means. B IBLIOGRAPHY 31 Eagles Davis, Hallowell, and Silverman, Richard. Hearing and Deaf- ness. New York: Holt-Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1960. Edwards, Allen L. Experimental Design in Psychological Research. rev. ed. New York: Rinehart and Co., Inc., 19 0. Fletcher, Harvey. Speech and Hearing in Communication. Princeton, New Jersey: D. Van Nostrand Co., Inc., 1958. Griffin, Donald R. Listenin in the Dark. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1958. Hirsh, Ira J. The Measurement of Hearing. McGraw-Hill Co., Inc., 195 . Lowenfeld, Berthold. Our Blind Children. Springfield, Illinois: Charles C. Thomas Pub., 1956. Newby, Hayes A. Audiology. New York: Appleton-Century- Crofts, Inc., 1958. von Bakesy, Georg. Experiments in Hearing. New York: McGraw- Hill Co., Inc., 19 O. Zahl, Paul. Blindness. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press, 1950. Articles Cotzin, M., and Dallenbach, K. M. "Facial Vision: The Role of Pitch and Loudness in the Perception of Obstacles by the Blind," American Journal of Ps cholo , LXIII (1950), 512. , Fedderson, W. E. et al. "Localization of High Frequency Tones," Journal of the Acoustical Societ of America, XXXIX (September, 1957), 98 -91. Fletcher, John L. "Localization of Sounds in Depth," U. S, A, Medical Research Labopatory Report, MCMLVII, No. 302 11 (1957). 14: 32 Griffin, Donald R. "Acoustical Orientation in the Oil Bird, Steatornis," National Academ of Sciences, XXIX, McCarty, B., and Worchel, P. "Rate of Motion and Object Perception in the Blind," New Outlook for the Blind, XLVII (November, 1954), 316 O'Neill, John, Oyer, Herbert, and Baker, Donald. "Auditory Skills of Blinded Individuals Training With Pilot Dogs," Journal of S eech and Hearing Research, I, No. 3 (September, 1953). 262-67. ' Supa, M., Cotzin, M., and Dallenbach, L. M. "Facial Vision: The Perception of Obstacles by the Blind " American Journal of Psychology, LVII (April, 1944 .183. Weiss, Carl. "Reality Aspects of Blindness As They Affect Case Work," The Famil , (February, 1954) 12. Zwislocki, J., Hellman, R. P., and Verillo, R. T. "Threshold of Audibility for Short Pulses," Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, XXIV, No. 10 (1962), 1 -52 o Unpublished Materia; American Association of Instructors of the Blind, "Orien- tation and Mobility Terms," St. Louis, Missouri, March, 1962. (Mimeographed.) Appendix A 34 Appendix A ACOUSTICAL SPECIFICATIONS OF ROOM WHERE STUDY WAS CONDUCTED* r fi Surface Material Area Coef. Absorb. Ceiling Tectum 2" 5800 .65 3700 Floor Asphalt Tile 5700 .03 171 Wainscott Plastic Paint 2250 .06 135 Walls Painted Block 5260 .06 314 ‘Walls Tectum 2" 2037 .65 1324 Air 143,000 éu. ft. 143 TOTAL 5787 Room dimensions: 62 feet by 92 feet 5,700 square feet 143,000 cubic feet Reverberation time: Key: Formula: T = ,05V T = reverberation a time T : .05 (143.000) V = room volume 5857 a = total T = 1.24 absorbtion *Source: Building Division, State of Michigan Appendix B 36 Appendix B INTENSITY OF THE STIMULI Cycles Minimum Audible Intensity Level Per Sound Field In Of Presented Second Reference to Atten- Stimuli in Refer- uator Setting* ence to Atten- uator Setting 500 35 db. 85 db. 2000 30 db. 80 db. 8000 30 db. 80 db. *The intensity of the stimuli utilized in this emperical study was presented at 50 decibels above the minimum audible field. The minimum audible field was ascertained by determining the threshold for each of the three frequencies tested at a distance of three feet from the source. This procedure was conducted sound field in the actual test room on a pair of normal ears. Appendix C PRESENTATION OF STIMULI 38 Appendix C Subject Starting Frequency Tone Number Distance Tested Presentation 1 26 feet 500 cps. Intermittent 2 38 feet 500 cps. I 3 50 feet 500 cps. I 4 26 feet 2000 cps. I 5 38 feet 2000 cps. I 6 50 feet 2000 cps. I 7 26 feet 8000 cps. I 8 38 feet 8000 cps. I 9 50 feet 8000 cps. I 10 26 feet 500 cps. Continuous 11 - 38 feet 500 cps. C 12 50 feet 500 cps. C 13 26 feet 2000 cps. C 14 38 feet 2000 cps. C 15 50 feet 2000 cps. C 16 26 feet 8000 cps. C 17 38 feet 8000 cps. C 18 50 feet 8000 cps. C 19 26 feet 500 cps. I 20 38 feet 500 cps. I 21 50.feet 500 cps. I 22 26 feet 2000 CPS. I 23 38 feet 2000 cps. I 24 50 feet 2000 cps. I 25 26 feet 8000 cps. I 26 38 feet 8000 cps. I 27 50 feet 8000 cps. I 28 26 feet 500 cps. C 29 38 feet 500 cps. C 30 50 feet 500 cps. C 31 26 feet 2000 cps. C 32 38 feet 2000 cps. C 33 50 feet 2000 cps. C 34 26 feet 8000 cps. C 35 38 feet 8000 cps. C 36 50 feet 8000 cps. C Appendix D 40 Appendix D ERROR JUDGMENT FOR EACH SUBJECT IN INCHES Subject Error Judgment 1 54 Inches 2 7 Inches 3 54 Inches 4 10 Inches 5 111 Inches 6 216 Inches 7 28 Inches 8 51 Inches 9 85 Inches 10 72 Inches * 11 6 Inches 12 39 Inches 13 7 Inches * 14 34 Inches 15 55 Inches 16 9 Inches 17 21 Inches 18 22 Inches 19 192 Inches 20 10 Inches 21 7 Inches 22 46 Inches 23 5 Inches * 24 31 Inches 25 120 Inches 26 72 Inches 27 11 Inches 28 8 Inches * 29 31 Inches 30 2 Inches 31 3 Inches * 32 120 Inches * 33 47 Inches * 34 8 Inches 35 58 Inches 36 35 Inches * *Nearly all of the subjects tended to overestimate the judgment of ten feet. The asterisk indicates those subjects who underestimated the judgment of ten feet. MEAN SCORES IN INCHES 41 0F ERROR JUDGMENTS variable Conditions of Mean Score Variable Frequency 500 cps. 40.27 Inches 2000 cps. 57.08 Inches 8000 cps. 43.33 Inches Presentation Continuous 32.06 Inches Intermittent 61.72 Inches Distance 26 feet 46.42 Inches 38 feet 43.83 Inches 50 feet 50.42 Inches F x P 500 cps. - Continuous 26.33 Inches 2000 cps. - Continuous 44.33 Inches 8000 cps. - Continuous 25.50 Inches 500 cps. - Intermittent 54.17 Inches 2000 cps. - Intermittent 69.83 Inches 8000 cps. - Intermittent 61.17 Inches F x D 500 cps. - 26 feet 81.50 Inches 500 cps. - 38 feet 13.50 Inches 500 cps. - 50 feet 25.75 Inches 2000 cps. - 26 feet 16.50 Inches 2000 cps. - 38 feet 67.50 Inches 2000 cps. - 50 feet 87.25 Inches 8000 cps. - 26 feet 41.25 Inches 8000 cps. - 38 feet 50450 Inches 8000 cps. - 50 feet 38.25 Inches F x D Continuous - 26 feet 17.83 Inches Continuous - 38 feet 45.00 Inches Continuous - 50 feet 33.33 Inches Intermittent - 26 feet 75.00 Inches Intermittent - 38 feet 42.67 Inches Intermittent - 50 feet 67.50 Inches ‘11. ft ‘ _ 3‘ ¥ o . . 1‘) 0'. 1 1i. 9 ‘ a a. $1357) )5 ,-, s? 1" 14. '-‘ a .. 3" I4 '1' 9’ n I “n: 41. 2's v . a .- 11.4w- . 7. “it." 9 t - 31.11:"21‘85 ~- ._ J