(DWEM‘MNDEDNESS AND SELF-«RB-EAL BESCREPAKCY AS THEY AFFECT ATTMCFEON 11'6" A HELPENG SSWUAIEGEQ Thesis for flu: Dogma of M. A. MECEIEGAN STATE URIVEESETY Lynne G. Tenbusch £967 11125.53: ABSTRACT OPEN-MINDEDNESS AND SELF-IDEAL DISCREPANCY AS THEY AFFECT ATTRACTION TO A HELPING SITUATION by Lynne G. Tenbusch Attitude toward becoming involved in a helping sit- uation is shaped by many factors. two of which should be receptivity to new information and the extent to which one describes his real self as differing from his ideal self. Attraction to a helping situation. receptivity to new information. and self-ideal discrepancy were investi- gated by assessing the relationship among these variables. In general it was hypothesized that: l) dogmatism is nega- tively related to attraction to therapy. 2) size of self- ideal discrepancy is positively related to attraction to therapy. and 3) size of self-ideal discrepancy is nega- tively correlated with dogmatism. Two hundred sixty undergraduate students enrolled in introductory psychology courses. who had never had any 1 Lynne G. Tenbusch psychological counseling or psychotherapy. filled out Ro- keach's Dogmatism Scale for a measure of openness to new information. Leary's Interpersonal Check List (ICL) as an index of self-ideal discrepancy. and Libo's Picture Impres- sions Test (PIT) for an attraction to therapy indicator. Males and females were analyzed separately. The hypothesis that dogmatism and attraction to therapy would be negatively related was not supported at a statistically significant level. but data tended in the ex- pected direction. The relation between ICL discrepancy score and PIT score. predicted as positive. turned out neg- ative. but not significantly. Further analysis indicated that degree of dogmatism and size of self-ideal discrepancy are positively correlated at a significant level for females but not for males. Difference in N was postulated to ac- count for the lack of significance in males. 4 If .3 . l I n" I .li "‘ / 7-. Approved: [CUlbfltx %;H€I;\) Committee Chairman Date: jg}! 3:6 W357 Thesis Committee: Norman Abeles. Chairman Charles Hanley Milton Rokeach OPEN-MINDEDNESS AND SELF-IDEAL DISCREPANCY AS THEY AFFECT ATTRACTION TO A HELPING SITUATION . BY Lynne G. Tenbusch A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1967 A ‘. ’3 .' I J 1% V. ,f‘ /n 3/ (5,7 3A7“/”‘ . ,. x 75-. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to express my deepest appreciation to Dr. Norman Abeles for acting as the chairman of this thesis committee. His guidance in designing the research and his assistance in preparing this manuscript. as well as his supportive understanding. patience. and insightful ideas were invaluable in bringing this study to fruition. To the members of the committee. I acknowledge a debt of gratitude. Dr. Hanley's stimulating criticisms and helpful advice in the statistical analyses of the data. and Dr. Rokeach's ready availability. continual support. and penetrating questions contributed to making this study personally valuable as a learning experience. I would like to extend special thanks to Gerald Gillmore. graduate student. for his help with computer pro- graming and computer laboratory routines. not to mention his unfailing patience. I am happy to express my appreciation to my friends whose continual encouragement transformed even the discourag- ing parts of this research into an enriching experience. ii To all those people who contributed to this study by making class time available or assisting with adminis- tration of the tests. I would like to indicate my gratitude. Finally. I am indebted to the National Institute of Mental Health for their generous financial support during the past two years. iii Dedicated to Tammela Lynne and Michelle Karen ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. LIST OF TABLES INTRODUCTION METHOD . . . RESULTS. . . DISCUSSION . SUMMARY. . . APPENDIX . . BIBLIOGRAPHY TABLE OF CONTENTS Page 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 ll 0 O C O O O C O O O O O O O O 0 V1 0 O O O O O O O O I O O O O 1 LIST OF TABLES Table I Page I. Scoring Criteria for the PIT. . . . . . . . . . 21 II. Means. Standard Deviations. and Correlation Coefficient for Dogmatism and ICL Discrepancy Scores from Subjects with No Scoreable Pic- tures on the PIT. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 III. Inter-Scorer Reliability for the PIT. . . . . . 30 IV. Means and Standard Deviations for Dogmatism. ICL Discrepancy. and PIT Scores . . . . . . . 30 V. T-Values from Comparison of Male and Female Scores on Dogmatism. ICL Discrepancy. and PIT 31 VI. Summary of Pearson Product Moment Correlation Coefficients for Dogmatism. ICL Discrepancy. and PIT Scores. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32 VII. Quartile Comparisons Between Criterion and Dependent Variables on Dogmatism. ICL Dis— crepancy. and PIT Scores. . . . . . . . . . . 34 VIII. T-Values for Upper Versus Lower Quartiles as Found in Table VII. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 IX. Summary Data from Analysis of Variance. . . . . 38 X. Comparison of Predicted PIT Total Score with Obtained PIT Total Score. . . . . . . . . . . 39 XI. T-Ratios from Comparison of 'Attracted' versus 'Not Attracted' Groups on Dogmatism and.ICL Discrepancy Scores. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41 vi List of Tables (continued) Table Page Appendix A. Obtained Values of Eta for all Combinations of Variables. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69 B. Male Pictures for the Picture Impressions Test. 70 C. Dogmatism Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 D. Interpersonal Check List. . . . . . . . . . . . 75 vii OPEN-MINDEDNESS AND SELF-IDEAL DISCREPANCY AS THEY AFFECT ATTRACTION TO A HELPING SITUATION INTRODUCTION What types of peOple are attracted to therapy? Are there some characteristics common to those individuals who approach a therapeutic situation? Since attitudes play an important role in behavior. it is reasonable to suspect that one's attitude toward helping situations in general will be influential in whether he will approach such situations. But. then what shapes attitudes toward helping situations? Given that therapeutic settings are oriented to reception. evaluation. and integration of new information. ideas. and experiences. it seems plausible that one's attitude toward helping situations will be in- fluenced by his degree of openness to new information (cognitive functioning). But given that an individual's cognitive functioning is flexible enough to enable him to receive and objectively evaluate new information. it does not necessarily follow that he will get involved in therapy. 1 A second relevant factor. obviously. is recognition by the individual that he is different from what he wants to be. or a feeling of dissatisfaction with himself. and the con- sequent desire to change. This lack of satisfaction should be reflected in the way he perceives himself. How are perceptions and attitudes influenced by cognitive functioning? If we define the latter in terms of openness or closedness (dogmatism). then certain percep- tions should vary systematically with degree of openness. Specifically. the manner in which one perceives himself as he really is and the ideal person that he would like to be. as well as his perception of helping situations should be related to his cognitive functioning. That is. one's per- ception of discrepancy between his real and ideal self. and his perception of helping situations should vary with in- creasing degrees of cognitive openness. Thus. receptivity to new information and self-ideal discrepancy may be deter- mining factors in whether or not an individual will seek therapy. It is assumed that one's perception of a given situ- ation or object reflects his attitude toward it. Thus. we can refer to an attitude toward. or perception of. a helping situation. In the same manner. we can speak in terms of an attitude toward oneself or in terms of perception of self- ideal discrepancy. This investigation was concerned with openness of belief systems and self-ideal discrepancy as they relate to attraction to helping situations. It was reasoned that one's receptivity to new information influences how he per- ceives himself. and that both variables then contribute to forming his attitude toward helping situations. Libo (1957. p. 35) defines attraction as ". . . the resultant of forces acting on the patient to maintain his relationship with the therapist." In the present study. that definition was extended. interpreting attraction as the resultant of all forces acting on the subject to perceive helping situ- ations in a positive manner. Self-ideal discrepancy. or the extent to which one describes his real self as differ- ing from his ideal self. is considered as one force contrib- uting to attraction. with attraction increasing as amount of discrepancy increases. On the other hand. closed or dogmatic cognitive functioning is interpreted as a force working against attraction to a helping situation. Low dogmatism may be conceived as the removal of an obstacle to attraction. becoming a positive force for attraction only when accompanied by a sufficient degree of discrepancy between real and ideal self. According to this approach. an individual's attrac- tion to a helping situation is positively related to his willingness to accept new information. to recognize and explore his subjective experiences. and to perceive and conceptualize himself and his experiences in each of pos- sible alternatives. Furthermore. self-ideal discrepancy is negatively related to degree of closed-mindedness. and posi- tively related to attraction to therapy. For example. if a person sees himself as deviating to a great extent from the ideal to which he aspires. he should perceive a helping sit- uation more favorably than should an individual unaware of any self-ideal discrepancy. If a person's cognitive func- tioning is very dogmatic. he should be less capable of per- ceiving how he differs from his ideal self. and less inter- ested in obtaining help. On the other hand. an open-minded approach should facilitate one's perception of differences in real and ideal self and. given a sizeable discrepancy. should dispose the individual favorably toward obtaining help. Thus. according to the ideas presented here. the manner in which an individual perceives himself is related to the manner in which he perceives a helping situation. and also to the Openness of his belief system or c0gnitive functioning. Rokeach (1960) has developed a scale to measure one aspect of cognitive functioning—-how open is the individual in his approach to new experiences. problems. situations. opportunities. etc.? Open—mindedness is defined as ". . . the extent to which the person can receive. evaluate. and act on relevant information received from the outside on its own intrinsic merits. unencumbered by irrelevant factors in the situation arising from within the person or from out- side." (Rokeach 1960. p. 57) Irrelevant internal pressures functioning to limit realistic reception of information in- clude unrelated habits. beliefs. and perceptual cues. irra— tional ego motives. power needs. the need for self aggran- dizement. the need to allay anxiety. etc. External pres— sures include reward and punishment from external authority as exerted by parents. peers. social and institutional group norms. reference groups. and cultural norms. The same author (Rokeach. 1960) has shown that the Doodle Bug Problem can be solved more easily by open-minded 6 than closed—minded individuals. Solution of the problem re- quires the individual to discard old beliefs. or sets. and replace them with new ones. which must then be organized into a new system by some process of integration. From his experimental findings. Rokeach concluded that open-minded persons are superior in solving this and other problems re— quiring transfer of learning. Similarly. Kemp (1960) found the performance of open-minded subjects superior to that of closed-minded subjects in handling critical thinking problems. From the demonstration of difference in approach to experience between the Open and closed-minded individual. and given the nature of the counseling situation as one re- quiring analysis and evaluation of ideas. replacement of old beliefs with new. the making of inferences. and transfer of learning. it follows that the closed-minded. or highly dog- matic person. is less likely to profit from counseling. Kemp (1961) investigated this idea. using'the Specific hy- pothesis that an open-minded person. as indicated by a low score on Rokeach's Dogmatism Scale. will have fewer personal problems and will respond more favorably to counseling than will the closed-minded person. Results indicated that low dogmatism subjects. during the counseling sessions. were described as more relaxed. having greater tolerance of am- biguity (less need for closure). attempting a more complete exploration of problems and issues. and having greater awareness of multiple contributors to a single effect. High dogmatism subjects spoke mainly about topics with no personal relevance. showed difficulty in tolerating certain ideas or areas of discussion. and tended to label these prior to ex- ploration. Kemp concluded that dogmatism influences the number of an individual's personal problems. and that highly dogmatic persons have a greater number of such problems. which are reduced to a lesser degree through counseling than for low dogmatic subjects. It should be noted that Kemp does not indicate whether his subjects were obtained from a population of students re- questing counseling. but it seems as though they were not. The 25 highest and the 25 lowest scorers on the Dogmatism Scale became the experimental group. each member of which then participated in four individual counseling interviews over the course of ten weeks. Subjects agreed to attend counseling sessions. but from all indications they did not seek such help themselves prior to the experiment. Thus. attitude toward the counseling situation is a variable overlooked by Kemp. That is. whether subjects sought help on their own initiative has important implications for their attitude toward helping situations. which could have been an influencing factor in Kemp's results. To phrase it differ— ently. we suspect that dogmatism is related to one's atti- tude toward a helping situation. which in turn affects one's behavior with respect to such situations. Concerning Kemp's study. whether or not a subject's problems were reduced during counseling may have been due to his attitude toward helping situations. which was influenced not only by the openness or closedness of his cognitive func- tioning. but also by the subject's perception of his real and ideal self. It is proposed that awareness of discrepan- cies between perception of real self and perception of ideal self is a crucial factor in one's attitude toward helping situations. Self-ideal perception. however. is also influ- \/ enced by one's receptivity to new information. But does the openness of one's cognitive functioning affect one's percep- tion of self-ideal discrepancy. or does the latter influence the former. or are both manifestations of a third factor which we have not even mentioned? In general. the present study intended to show that the relationship between dogmatism and the other variables discussed here is not as simple as Kemp's study would lead one to believe. Finally. in contrast to Kemp's demonstration that high dogmatics have more personal problems. we suspect that a highly dOgmatic person may. in fact. admit to fewer prob- lems because he is less aware of discrepancies between his real and ideal self. That is. the narrow approach of highly dogmatic persons may limit their ability to recognize their problems. in which case dogmatic persons will have fewer problems than more open—minded persons. We could not di- rectly test this last speculation. however. because our study did not include a measure of personal problems as such. Where Kemp used the Mooney Problem Check List as an index of personal problems. we used an interpersonal check list as a measure of self-ideal perception. through which we attempted to demonstrate a lower awareness of self—ideal dis- crepancy on the part of high dogmatics. Kaplan and Singer (1963) proposed that pathology is primarily characterized by various forms of closed-mindedness and rigidity. all of which detract from self-awareness. Their experimental results warranted the conclusion that the degree of dogmatism Operant within an individual reduces. to that 10 extent. his self-awareness. This limited self-awareness is reflected by decreased emphasis on personal experience and subjective reactions. and increased emphasis on “. . . objec- tification. or grasping the nature of the object . . ." (Kaplan and Singer 1963. p. 487) In addition. Kaplan and Singer found that Open and closed-mindedness are not ap- proaches specific to some areas of a person's life. Rather. they represent pervasive attitudes. An open—minded person approaches personal and_objective data openly. while a closed-minded person approaches both narrowly. An individual enters a therapy situation (or a help- ing situation) for the purpose of learning something. whether such learning refers to altered behavior patterns. increased self-knowledge. revision of a value system. or a change in stimulus response connections. Ehrlich (1961) explored the relationship between dogmatism and learning. He also pro- posed to investigate certain aspects of the validity of the Dogmatism Scale. i.e. the defining of closed-mindedness as ". . . a relatively closed cognitive organization of beliefs and disbeliefs about reality." (Ehrlich 1961. p. 148) He reasoned that if the above defining characteristic of a dog- matic person is accurate. then a highly dogmatic person 11 should exhibit less learning in the classroom situation than should the person low in dogmatism. Data indicated that low dogmatism subjects had already achieved a higher level of learning before they enrolled for the course. learned more in the course. and retained the information to a signifi— cantly greater degree than highly dogmatic subjects. Time lapse was 5 to 6 months. Split—half reliability for the Dogmatism Scale was .75. and correlation between the two administrations of the Scale was .73. In a follow-up study. Ehrlich (1961) restudied the same population after five years. and his findings replicate those of the original study. These results render further validity to Rokeach's theoretical formulations underlying the Dogmatism Scale. Restle. Andrews. and Rokeach (1964) and Moore (1961) have demonstrated the differences in types of learning which open versus closed-minded persons can achieve. Restle et a1. (1964) showed that open-minded subjects perform better at a task requiring them to seek a general principle which would predict the Experimenter's reinforcements. than do closed- minded subjects. The dogmatic subject's tendency to rely passively on authority was cited to account for the results. Moore (1961) has demonstrated the greater influence of verbal 12 reinforcement for low dogmatics than high dogmatics in a dis- crimination conditioning situation. In this situation. the only source of information available to the gs were rein- forcements from the Experimenter. thus making all reinforce- ments. initially. relevant information. A significant point illustrated in this study is the fact that the limited range of responses available to the §s put a premium on the abil- ity to differentiate the crucial responses or relevant infor- mation. Viewed in this way. the open—minded person. who is theoretically more capable of making cognitive discrimina- tions and separating relevant from irrelevant information. would be expected to respond more easily in this type of con- ditioning situation. Results confirmed such expectations and suggest that low dogmatics are more attuned to accepting all incoming information and more capable of discriminating the greater relevancy of some. Additional data pertinent to the present study were \\ obtained by Hollenbeck (1965) in her investigation of the ‘ effects of dogmatism on aspects of adjustment to severe dis- ability. Depression and mourning were used as signs of ac- ceptance of loss of sight. while denial indicated non— acceptance of the loss. Analysis. or taking in new 13 information. was equated with learning facts about the dis- ability; synthesis. or organizing new beliefs into a working system. was equated with acceptance of the disability. Re- sults showed that the reactions of denial and depression are inversely related. In addition. the less dogmatic a blind person. the more depression he showed in relation to his dis- ability. and the more dogmatic a blind person. the more he denied his disability and its effects. If a large discrepancy between self and ideal is thought of as undesirable. or as a defect or imperfection. and if it is assumed that recognition of a defect is similar to acceptance of it. then. given (from the above results) that highly dogmatic persons are less likely to accept their defects. it would follow that highly dogmatic persons will be less willing to recognize or become aware of discrepancies between real and ideal self. Then. if such recognition (not necessarily termed as self-ideal discrepancy. but generally that the person desires to become different in some way from his present self) is a necessary prerequisite to seeking therapy. it would follow that highly dogmatic persons are less likely to seek therapy or to perceive a helping situa- tion positively. 14 Rokeach (1960. p. 70) has written “. . . in the ex- treme. the closed system is nothing more than the total net- work of psychoanalytic defense mechanisms organized together to form a cognitive system and designed to shield a vulner- able mind." This. too. points in the direction of predict- ing that a closed—minded person will be less willing to recognize how and where he deviates from his ideal. and less likely to approach or have a positive attitude toward a help- ing situation. Rogers' (1961) findings that the client entering therapy exhibits considerable discrepancy between his real self and the ideal person he would like to become. contrib- utes to the vast array of literature on the subject. Typi- cal of Rogers' general findings is a decrease in such dis- crepancy during and after therapy. thus facilitating a con- gruence between self and ideal. Butler and Haigh (1954) demonstrated a significant decrease in self-ideal discrepancy as a function of therapy. The self-ideal correlation moved from a -.47 at the beginning of therapy. to a .59 at the termination of therapy. Important to the present study is the fact that the control group--a non-therapy group matched for age. sex. and socio-economic status-~had initial self-ideal 15 correlations of .58 which did not change significantly over time. measuring .59 at the follow-up. In other words. the control group had self-ideal correlations equivalent to the post—therapy correlations of the therapy group. These re- sults strongly suggest the function of self-ideal discrepancy as a positive force contributing to an individual's attrac- tion to a helping situation. Hartley (1950) has also presented evidence showing that before therapy the self as perceived had little rela- tionship to the ideal self. while after therapy. the discrep- ancy had decreased significantly. Raimy (1948) has provided testimony that successful counseling involves a change in the client's self-concept. manifested as a shift from a pre- ponderance of self-disapproval and ambivalence at the begin- ning of therapy to a preponderance of self-approval at the end of therapy. Self-approval can be defined as a signifiv cant positive correlation between real and ideal self. Braaten (1961) showed significant movement in therapy when the self-ideal correlation was used as a criterion. Similar evidence has been obtained by Sheerer (1949). From the above research. it seems justified to assume that the manifest awareness of self-ideal discrepancy is 16 related in some manner to an individual's perception of a helping situation. Libo (1953) designed the Picture Impressions Test (PIT) in an attempt to capitalize on the sensitivity of pro- jective techniques to the social situation in which they are administered. Validity of the PIT as a measure of patient- therapist attraction has been investigated (Libo. 1957; Mullin. 1966). Operationally. Libo's study tested the abil- ity of the PIT to predict whether or not a patient will re- turn for his next interview. The test was administered to 40 patients at a psychosomatic clinic after the first inter— view. Strength of attraction to therapy was derived from both total score and number of stories with a score. Two approaches were used to measure reliability of scoring: 1) agreement ranging between 92% and 100% was obtained with one scorer who repeated scoring after six months: 2) agreement between 87% and 100% resulted from the use of two independ- ent scorers. The study included observation of whether the patients actually attended therapy the following week. Re- sults indicated a significant relationship between actual and predicted behavior. Six of the nine patients who did not return. and 24 of the 31 patients who did return. were l7 correctly predicted. In other words. 89% of the predicted returns. and only 53% of the predicted 'not returns' did. in fact. return for the second interview. Mullin (1966) explored the concurrent validity of the PIT as an index of the client's projected attraction to the therapeutic situation. More specifically. he investi- gated the ability of the PIT to reflect information about the client-therapist relationship. Results showed that the PIT does elicit information relevant to attraction between client and therapist. Students attending one counseling interview had higher attraction scores than students with no counseling contact. In addition. he found that after eliminating gs whose behavior (i.e. returning or not return- ing for counseling) was influenced by factors other than attraction. students returning for the second interview had higher attraction scores than those not returning. The present study was designed to investigate the following hypotheses: 1) The higher the dogmatism score. the less will the indi- vidual be attracted to therapy or to a helping situation. 2) The greater the perceived discrepancy between real self 3) 4) 18 and ideal self. the more will the individual be at- tracted to therapy. The higher the dogmatism score. the less will be the perceived discrepancy between real and ideal self. Dogmatism and self-ideal discrepancy will interact in such a manner that: a) The highly dogmatic person with high self-ideal dis- crepancy will show a moderate attraction to therapy. b) The person low in dogmatism with high self—ideal discrepancy will be highly attracted to therapy. c) The highly dOgmatic person with low self-ideal dis- crepancy will show a low attraction to therapy. d) The person low in dogmatism with low self-ideal dis— crepancy will show a low attraction to therapy. METHOD Sample. The subjects consisted of 260 students en— rolled in undergraduate psychology courses at Michigan State University. Ages ranged from 17 to 22 years old. Students who had in the past. or were at the time of the study. re— ceiving counseling or therapy were eliminated. Instruments. The Picture Impressions Test (PIT) is a series of four pictures. similar to TAT cards. designed to elicit stories about patient-therapist interactions. They can be used for white and Negro subjects and for adolescents and adults of both sexes (Libo. 1956). Separate cards are given to males and females (see Appendix. Table B). After the first therapy session. the therapist administers the pictures to the client. requesting that he write or tell a story about each one. In each story he is asked to include answers to the following questions: 1) What is happening? Who is involved? 2) What led up to this? What happened before? 3) What is being thought or felt? What is wanted? 4) What will happen? What will be done? About 20 to 25 minutes are required for completion of the test. 19 20 Coding renders an attraction score. with attraction being defined as ". . . the resultant of forces acting on the patient to maintain his relationship with the therapist.“ (Libo 1957. p. 35) The coding scheme is derived from the same general principles used in Libo's first study (Libo. 1953). Accordingly. to be scoreable a story must include a doctor. therapist. or examiner (i.e. medical man. dentist. psychiatrist. psychologist. etc. as opposed to a friend. lawyer. employee. etc.). and a client seeking diagnosis or therapy for himself rather than for a friend. child. etc. If either of these elements is not present. the story is marked Not Scoreable (NS). If both requirements are satis- fied. a +1 is assigned to each mention of actual or desired movement of a patient toward a therapist and of expected or derived satisfaction from their relationship. A -l is as- signed to each mention of actual or desired movement away from the therapist and of loss of satisfaction caused or expected from contact with him. A score for each story is the algebraic sum of all the plus and minus values assigned to it. while the total score is the sum of scores obtained on all four stories. Results are reported in two parts: the algebraic sum explained above. and the number of stories 21 with a score. To be considered as having a score. a story must receive at least one value other than a zero. even if the algebraic sum for the stories is zero. The number of stories receiving a score is interpreted as the Subject's degree of involvement in his story—writing. It is assumed that the higher the positive total score and the greater the number of stories from which scores are obtained. the stronger is the client's attraction to the therapist and the thera- peutic situation. Criteria for interpretation of 'attracted' or 'not attracted' are summarized in Table I. Table I Scoring Criteria for PIT (See Manual; Libo. 1956) — *— E m No. of stories . Total score Interpretation Wlth a score 2. 3. or 4 +1 or higher Attracted Any 0 or lower Not attracted 0 or 1 Any Not attracted Additional scoring criteria include the following. The scoreable unit is any word. phrase. clause. or sentence which contains an indication of the movement or satisfaction 22 described earlier. More than one scoreable part may be con- tained in a sentence. clause. or phrase. but each scoreable part can only receive one score. The shortest part necessary should always be chosen for scoring. thus facilitating the scoring of other parts of the story. A series of words which. if treated separately. would receive the same sign. is treated as one unit. but if a descriptive word is preceded by a verb. it is scoreable as a separate unit even if it ap- pears in a series. A sequence of descriptive words qualify- ing for both positive and negative scores receives one plus and one minus regardless of the number of positives and nega- tives in the sequence. Each scoreable unit in an 'either-or' statement is scored. Conditional statements. moralistic statements, and involuntary or chance actions are not scored. As mentioned earlier. Libo has conducted two studies to determine the reliability of scoring the PIT (Libo. 1956; Libo. 1957). Emphasis was on determining the reliability of scoring procedures which influence the final interpretation of a Subject as 'attracted' or 'not attracted.' Results. from two independent scorers using 30 SS showed 87% agree- ment on interpretation of 'attracted' or 'not attracted.' 100% agreement on 'number of stories receiving a score.‘ 23 and 87% agreement on 'total score.‘ Results from one coder's repeated scoring indicated 95% agreement on interpretation of 'attracted' or 'not attracted.' 98% agreement on 'number of stories receiving a score.‘ and 92% agreement on 'total score.‘ Rokeach's Dogmatism Scale was used to obtain a mea- sure of open-mindedness or openness to new information (see Table C in Appendix). The scale contains a total of 40 statements on a number of important questions. Subjects were instructed as follows: The following is a study of what the general public thinks and feels about a number of important social and personal questions. The best answer to each statement below is yourpersonalfiopinion. We have tried to cover many different and opposing points of view; you may find yourself agreeing strongly with some of the statements. disagreeing just as strongly with others. and perhaps uncertain about others. Whether you agree or disagree with any statement. you can be sure that many other people feel the same as you do. 'Mark each statement in the left margin accord- ing to how much you agree or disagree with it. Please mark every one. Write +1. +2. +3. or -1. -2. -3. depending on how you feel in each case. Scores are added algebraically and yield an index of dogmatism. Reliabilities of the final form of the DOgmatism Scale range from .68 to .93 (Rokeach. 1960). A shorter meth- od of scoring can be accomplished by simply adding the number 24 of positives without regard to the numerical value of each. This method has a reliability of .89 and renders an average score of about 17. an upper bound of 32. and a lower bound of zero (Rokeach and Norrell. 1966). Timothy Leary (1957) has constructed an interpersonal adjective check list (ICL). to be used as a tool in assessing various aspects of personal adjustment. It contains 134 words and phrases descriptive of the way peOple feel and be- have. One purpose of the ICL is to measure the discrepancy between real and ideal self. thus providing an index of self- acceptance. This level of the ICL deals with the individ- ual's perception of himself and his world as he reports them. Directions to the g were as follows: This is a list of words and phrases which des- cribe the way people behave. Circle the number of each item that is descriptive of you. Your first impression is generally the best. Therefore. go through the list as-quickly as you can. However. please be certain to consider each item. but circle only those that describe you. There are 134 items in all. To describe his ideal self. the §_was requested to circle the number of each item that is descriptive of himself as he would like to be (see Table D in Appendix). 25 The discrepant items are added algebraically. and used as a measure of self-ideal discrepancy. The entire pro- cedure requires about 10 to 15 minutes. Test-retest correla- tions average from .73 to .78. These correlations suggest that the ICL discrepancy scores have sufficient stability to be useful in personality research and clinical evaluation. Procedure. All three tests were given in the class- room situation. To avoid contamination of the data by order effects. the PIT was always administered first. and the Dog- matism Scale and ICL were each administered as the second test 50%»of the time and as the third test 50% of the time. with sex controlled (i.e. the ICL was given immediately after the PIT to 50% of the SS. half being male and half female). Because the PIT was originally designed to be admin- istered in a therapeutic situation. the directions were amended to adjust the test to a classroom setting. Pictures were shown one at a time by projecting them on to a screen at the front of the room. Before presentation of the pic- tures. gs were told: For the first part of the study. I am going to show you some pictures. Each one will be flashed on the screen for a short time. Look at it. and imagine that you or a close friend has gone to see someone about some problems you or he may have. Then make up a story about the 26 picture. answering the four questions on the an— swer sheet. By answering these questions. all the parts of your story's plot will be covered. Write a definite. interesting story. rather than a simple description of the picture. Use your imagination. There is no right or wrong way. Your story should be about what you see in the picture. There are separate pictures for males and females. I will always show the male (female) picture first. As soon as it goes off the screen. males (females) begin working on your story. Do not wait for the female (male) picture to be shown. There are four pictures in all and each has a separate answer sheet. Please work fast. Each story should take you about five minutes to write. Are there any questions? O.K. Here is the first picture for the males (females). Each picture was shown for 25 seconds. after which the SS were given 5 minutes to write a story. Male and fe- male pictures were each shown first 50% of the time. On completion of the PIT. gs were given further directions. Now you may go on to the other tests. You should each have a data sheet. two check lists. and a psychology questionnaire. but not necessar- ily in that order. Please complete them in the order in which they appear. and replace them in the envelope in the same order. Directions for each one appears at the top of the page. Please begin. 27 Instructions for the Dogmatism Scale and ICL were not changed from those outlined in the section on instru- ments. Since gs were taking three tests. each §_was given an envelope containing all test materials at the beginning of the hour. In this way. it was hoped to save time. A data sheet requesting the following information was also in the envelope: age. sex. and whether the § had in the past or was presently obtaining psycholOgical counseling or psy- chotherapy. Possible answers to this last question included; none. one session. 2-5 sessions. 6-10 sessions. more than 10 sessions. The purpose of such detail was to eliminate the possibility of a yes-no dichotomy. Finally. gs were asked not to sign their name. RESULTS Of the 260 gs who met the requirements of the study. 24 had no scoreable stories on the PIT. Their data were not included in any of the analyses. Means. standard deviations. and a correlation coefficient for dogmatism and ICL discrep- ancy score from these 24 gs appear in Table II. No signifi— cant differences were found between the mean scores of these gs as compared to the mean scores of the remaining 236 S5. The t—value obtained from a comparison of the two groups on dogmatism was .826 (df = 258) and the tfvalue from a compar- ison of mean ICL discrepancy scores was .239 (df = 258). The correlation cOefficient between dogmatism and ICL discrepancy score for the 24 §s with no scoreable stories (r = .596). however. was markedly higher than that for the group with at least one scoreable picture (r = .178 for males and .221 for females). Inter-scorer reliability for the PIT was obtained by having 25% of the PITs scored independently by a second scorer using PITs from every fourth S. The criteria for com- parison were: 1) Correlation coefficient for total score. 28 29 Table II Means. Standard Deviations. and Correlation Coefficient for Dogmatism and ICL Discrepancy Scores from Subjects with No Scoreable Pictures on the PIT (N = 24) I Measure _ Dogmatism ICL Mean 17.458 34.50 Standard deviation 5.192 13.76 Pearson r .596* *Significant at .01 level using the Pearson product moment correlation coefficient; 2) Agreement on the:'number of stories scored.' using the.Pear- son product moment correlation coefficient; 3) Agreement on interpretation of 'attracted' or 'not attracted.' using the phi coefficient. which is the Pearson r for dichotomized var- iables; 4) Agreement on interpretation of 'attracted' or 'nOt attracted.' measured in percentage. Table III summarizes the results for all four criteria. These results indicate a con- sistency in the scoring system when used by independent scorers. All test data were first analyzed with the sexes com- bined. and then identical analyses were performed on data for each of the sexes separately. Means and standard deviations 30 of dogmatism. ICL discrepancy. and PIT score for both types of analysis appear in Table IV. Table I II Inter-Scorer Reliability for the PIT L W Correlation .Corrélation Interpretation of Coefficient Coefficient 'Attracted' or 'Not for Total for Number of Attracted~I Score Stories Scored Phi Coef. % Agreement 54 .85* .89* .775 .889 *Pearson Product Moment Correlation Coefficient Table IV Means and Standard Deviations for Dogmatism. ICL Discrepancy. and PIT Scores E m Males & Females Males Females Test ‘ “* Mean St. Dev. Mean St. Dev. Mean St. Dev. Dogmatism 18.28 4.576 18.44 4.824 18.18 4.407 ICL 33.84 12.798 34.55 13.129 33.38 '12.560 # Pictures 2.114 .952 1.946 .913 2.22 .961 PIT Total Score 1.191 3.545 .630 3.071 1.549 3.775 N 236 92 144 31 To compare the mean score of males and females on dogmatism, ICL discrepancy. and PIT score (both 'number of stories scored' and 'total score'). t—tests were performed. A significant difference between males and females was ob— tained for the 'number of stories scored' on the PIT (t = 2.16. df = 234). with females having higher mean scores. The difference in means for the 'total PIT score' was very close to statistical significance. The obtained value for E_in this case was 1.949 (df = 234). which is significant at the .06 level. whereas the value necessary for significance at the .05 level is between 1.98 and 1.96. Table V presents these results. Table V .T-Values from Comparison of Male and Female Scores on DOgmatism. ICL Discrepancy. and PIT Variable Obtained t—value Df Dogmatism .426 234 ICL .686 234 # Stories Scored 2.160* 234 Total PIT Score 1.949 234 *Significant at .05 level. 32 In exploring the hypotheses of this study. several methods of analysis were used: ‘Pearson product moment cor- relation coefficient to test for linear relationships: £33 to reveal non—linear relationships; analysis of variance to test for interaction effects: and t—tests to detect differ— ences among mean scores. Results will be presented in terms of specific hypotheses. Hypothesis 1 was not supported. Analysis of dogma- tism and PIT score yielded a Pearson product moment correla- tion coefficient of -.068 for males and -.064 for females. neither of which is significant. Table VI summarizes the correlation data relating to linearity. It should be noted that though neither relationship is statistically signifi- cant.'both are in the predicted direction. Inspection of Table VI Summary of Pearson Product Moment Correlation Coefficients for Dogmatism. ICL Discrepancy. and PIT Scores W Pearson Product Moment Correlation Coefficients Sex N Dog and PIT ICL and PIT ICL and D09 Males -.068 -.075 .178 92 Females -.064 -.139 .221* 144 *Significant at .01 level. 33 the mean PIT score for upper and lower quartile dogmatism scorers provides further indication of this direction among females. For example. the mean PIT score for high dogmatism scorers (N = 34) is .62. while the mean PIT score for §s scoring low on dogmatism (N = 41) is 2.15. The relation- ship is as predicted. i.e. highly dogmatic gs as a group (disregarding ICL discrepancy scores) have a lower PIT mean score than do gs low on dogmatism. However. close scrutiny of data from male gs reveals that. though there is an over- all negative relation between dOgmatism and total PIT score. high dogmatism scorers as a group. in fact. have the highest mean PIT score (see Table VII.a). The 3535 Obtained from male data were .445 for PIT/ Dog and .527 for Dog/PIT. and from female data were .435 for PIT/Dog and .299 for Dog/PIT. None of these is statisti- aflly significant. Table A summarizes results from the non- linear analyses (see Appendix). For the Eatests. dogmatism. ICL discrepancy. and PIT scores were divided into quartiles. as in Table VII (see Cri- terion Variable). Then. SS in the high and low quartiles of a given variable were looked at more closely in terms of means and standard deviations on the other two variables. 34 .Amanmflum> cofluwufluov OHQMHHM> OHHLB m co mmnoom mo mammm on» so mmaauumso oucfl UOOH>HQ mum mm.:m£3 AmOHQMHHm> admocmmmnv moaflmfium> 039 no m203umfl>mn enmecmum ecu names; 3.3 3.3 moé 3.3 *3 mos ow m3.3 3.3 33v 00.3 33 mom. 3 3.3 ooém 3.... 3.3 x3 63 3. 33.3 3.3 63...... 3.3 *3 3m 3 3.3 3.3. 36 3.3 x3 63 am 333 3.3 33+ 3.3 $3 63 3 3.3 3.3 $6 3.3 .33 33 3 333 5.3 Sod 3.3 *3 33 mm 30H .mon aHm .o 30H .moo eHm .u 36 3.0 34V 3.3 .33 mos mm 3.3m 3m. 33¢ 3.3 .33 mos 3 $3 3.3 004. 3.3 *3 3m 3 33m 3.... ~34. 3.3 *3 3m 3 36 N33 3% 3.3 $3 63 3V 3.3m omo. 33¢ 00.3 .33 6cm 3 36 3.3 36 3.3 .33 33 3 83m 03.. 83¢ 3.: x3 33 3 .3 m . mon 33 . a . mon .33 . n 3.... 3.0 3.3 3.3 .33 mom. em 33m «3. 33.3 3.3 $3 mom. 3 :3 33 $6 3.3 .33 e3 3 833. 03.- 333 3.3 .33 3m 3 33 3.3 3.3 3.3 x3 63 o... mmo.m 36. 33m 3.3. .33 63 3 03¢ 33 3.3 3.0... .33 33 3 333 So. 63.3 3.3 $3.33 3. .>on.um cmmz .>wa.um cam: .m .>on .um cam: .>mn.um COOS .m .mon .moa aHm 30H aHm AOH OHQmHnm> magmanm> m . . mmHQMHHm> ucwecm on COAHODHHU z mmHQmHHm> unmocmmma GOHHmuHHU z mmq¢2mm mmqmz *BHm cam .AOH .EmaumEmon so mmHQMHHm> unmocmmwn cam GOHHODHHU :mm3umm mGOmHHmmEOU mHHuumno HH> magma 35 In reference to hypothesis 1. no significant differences were found between the PIT scores of the upper and lower quartile of dogmatism scorers for either males or females. Efratios of -.0462 (df = 48) and 1.586 (df = 73) were obtained for males and females respectively. Table VIII presents these results. Table VIII ‘T—Values for Upper Versus Lower Quartiles As Found in Table VII (N = 236) A4 MALES FEMALES Var. Criterion Df .T-Value Var. Criterion Df TfValue ICL D09 48 -l.854 ICL Dog 73 -2.017* PIT Dog 48 —0.046 PIT Dog 73 1.586 Dog ICL 49 -l.773 Dog ICL 63 -2.380* PIT ICL 49 0.306 PIT ICL 63 1.434 Dog PIT 43 -O.422 Dog PIT 77 0.767 ICL PIT 43 1.523 ICL PIT 77 1.172 *Significant at .05 level. Hypothesis 2 was not supported. The Pearson r for males was -.075 and that for females was -.139. neither of which is statistically significant. For both males and 36 females. however. the relationship tends in a direction Oppo- site to that which was predicted. Comparison of the mean PIT scores for high and low ICL discrepancy scorers also in- dicates a negative relationship. That is. among both males and females. the group with the highest ICL discrepancy scores has a lower mean PIT score than does the group with the lowest ICL discrepancy scores. For example. the mean PIT score for high ICL male scorers (N = 26) is .538. while that for low ICL scorers (N = 25) is .96. Regarding females. it is true that the upper quartile ICL scorers have the high- est mean PIT score. However. among males the increase in mean PIT score with decrease in mean ICL discrepancy score is not so consistent. Here the 3rd quartile of ICL scorers has the lowest mean PIT (.333). while the upper quartile has the second lowest mean PIT (.538). (See Table VII.b) It was predicted that the greater the discrepancy between the real and the ideal self. the higher would be the attraction to therapy. but results. though not significant. indicate the reverse. Statistical analysis of ICL discrepancy score and PIT score using eta_yielded no significant findings. Among the males. an eta of .323 was obtained for PIT/ICL and .398 37 for ICL/PIT. Etas for females were .294 for PIT/ICL and .335 for ICL/PIT. .E-tests revealed no significant differences between the mean PIT scores of the upper and lower quartile of ICL scorers. The obtained values of Erwere .3058 (df = 49) and 1.4342 (df = 63) for males and females respectively (see Table VIII) . Hypothesis 3 was not supported. The Pearson r for ICL discrepancy and dogmatism was .178 for males (N = 92. p < .10). and .221 for females (N = 144. p < .01). These results indicate a relationship opposite to the original prediction. §£3§_of .447 for Dog/ICL and .479 for ICL/Dog were found from male data. while eggs from female data were .387 for Dog/ICL and .50 for ICL/Dog. The only value which reached statistical significance was the e£g_for ICL/Dog from female data (F = 2.03; df = 19. 122; p < .01). A test for linearity of regression indicated that ICL/Dog for fe- males is significantly non-linear (F = 1.739; df = 19. 122; p < .05). These tests were computed according to Walker and Lev (1953. p. 278). 38 A trtest on the ICL means of the upper and lower quartile dogmatism scorers produced a significant E-ratio for females. but not for males (see Table VIII). Hypothesis 4 was not supported. A one-way analysis of variance was performed on the means of the four groups created by assigning §s according to whether they scored high or low on dogmatism and ICL discrepancy. For this anal- ysis. dogmatism and ICL diScrepancy scores were dichoto- mized into high and low groups by dividing at the value closest to the median. Table IX summarizes the results. Table IX Summary Data from Analysis of Variance m MALES FEMALES N Dog ICL Mean PIT o N Dog ICL Mean PIT O 26 Low Low 0.8077 2.8145 47 Low Low 1.8936 4.0552 19 High Low 0.8421 3.8911 30 High Low 2.0000 3.8327 20 Low High 0.7500 3.0929 34 Low High 1.5588 3.1641 27 High High 0.2222 2.8330 33 High High 0.6364 3.9514 Source SS DF F Source SS DF: F Between 6.453 3 0.3199 Between 39.173 3 0.908 Within 860.981 88 Within 2012.487 140 Total '867.435 91 Total 2051.660 143 39 Although the overall F test showed a non-significant difference. comparison of group means with the hypothesized order relation reveals some interesting trends. For example. if the dichotomized groups were arranged in descending order for mean PIT score. results would appear as in Table X. Table X Comparison of Predicted PIT Total Score with Obtained PIT Total Score Predicted Obtained PIT Dichotomized N Hyp. PIT PIT Mean Score Groups Score Score Male Female ' Dog. ICL Male Female Moderate Low .2222 .6364 High High 27 33 4.a. High Low .7500 1.5588 Low High 20 34 4.b. Low High .8077 1.8936 Low Low 26 47 4.d. Low High .8421 2.0000 High Low 19 30 4.c. Regarding the PIT scores. if 'moderate' were considered 'mod- erately high' or 'high.‘ the results become just the reverse of the original predictions. Let us consider the findings in terms of each particular hypothesis. Hypothesis 4.a: The combination of high dogmatism and high self-ideal discrepancy. predicted to result in a 4O moderate attraction to therapy. was associated with the low- est mean PIT for both males and females. Hypothesis 4.b: The group low in dogmatism and high in self—ideal discrepancy. predicted to indicate high attrac- tion to therapy. showed the second lowest mean PIT score for both males and females. Hypothesis 4.c: High dogmatism and low self-ideal discrepancy. hypothesized to interact in such a manner as to produce low attraction to therapy. was associated with the highest mean PIT score for both males and females. Hypothesis 4.d: The group low in dogmatism and low in self-ideal discrepancy. predicted to show a high attrac- tion to therapy. had the second highest mean PIT score among both males and females. Finally. tftests on mean dogmatism and ICL discrep- ancy scores comparing 'attracted' versus 'not attracted' groups yielded insignificant.£-ratios for both males and females. Table XI presents the findings of this analysis. In summary. no significant relationships were found between PIT scores and dogmatism scores or between PIT scores and ICL discrepancy scores. Analysis of variance revealed no interaction effects between dogmatism. ICL discrepancy. 41 and PIT. Results did show that ICL discrepancy and dog- matism are positively related. Table XI ‘TeRatios from Comparison of 'Attracted' Versus 'Not Attracted' Groups on Dogmatism and ICL Discrepancy Scores E MALES (N - 92) Attracted Not Attracted (N = 33) (N = 59) Variable Df t St. St. Mean Dev. Mean Dev. Dog 18.06 4.636 18.64 4.992 90 -0.551 ICL 34.21 13.946 34.75 12.885 90 -0.266 FEMALES (N - 144) Attracted Not Attracted (N = 70) (N - 74) Variable Df t St. St. Mean Dev. Mean Dev. Dog 17.68 3.752 18.65 4.956 142 -l.309 ICL 32.83 12.970 33.91 12.312 142 -0.665 DISCUSSION While none of the hypotheses of this study were sup- ported. data from one were in the predicted direction. and data from six were in a direction opposite to the prediction. Results from only one of the seven were statistically sig- nificant. None of the hypotheses involving the PIT were statis- tically significant. which we must consider a generic issue in evaluating these results. It is suspected that the lack of significant findings here is due to the particular sample used. In past research. the PIT was always used with clin- ical populations. i.e. with gs having had experience in therapy. The one exception was Mullin's study (1966). in which case the SS. being enrolled in 400-1eve1 psychology courses. can be considered more psychologically sophisti— cated than the Se in this study. the majority of whom were 18 or 19 years old. Thus. possibly the PIT is not sensitive to this age group. However. Libo claims that his projective measure can be used with adolescents or adults. The issue. then. may be that the sensitivity of the PIT is restricted 42 43 by the combination of group administration and a non-client population which is relatively unSOphisticated. Support for this suggestion is provided by Webster's studies (Sanford. 1964) of college girls. Results indicated that in terms of attitudes and amount of personality disturb- ance experienced. students entering therapy are more like seniors than freshmen. The latter Sanford (1964. p. 836) described as ". . . idealistic. sociable. well—organized. well-behaved . . . " individuals whose participation in family values and community is comfortable and uncritical. In terms of the present study. it follows that a freshman would be much less likely to project a helping situation into the four pictures of the PIT than would the senior who has had several years to develOp an awareness of personal feelings and become sensitized to value conflicts. differ- ent points of view. etc. In conjunction with this. it is important to note that the proportion of clients seeking help with personal problems at the MSU Counseling Center is greater for upperclassmen than for freshmen and sophomores. (Annual Report. MSU Counseling Center). Now let us consider the results of the present study in light of the underlying theory and explore possible in- ferences. 44 The first hypothesis predicted that as dogmatism in- creases. the individual will become less likely to project attraction to a helping situation. Results. though not sig- nificant. were in the predicted direction. If we assume that the negative trend is not due to chance. the following is a plausible explanation. The closed—minded or dogmatic person perceives things. as well as himself. differently than does the open—minded person. The former is not willing to discuss subjective experiences. to seek and/or accept new information. or to see things as changeable. Consequently. if he perceives the therapeutic setting as one facilitating the exploration of new concepts and ideas. the discussion of his feelings and experiences. and the learning of different vantage points from which to view information. issues. and experiences. then he will not be drawn to it. In short. if the dogmatic individual sees therapy as a situation conducive to change. he will not approach it. It is interesting to speculate that the relationship between dogmatism and attraction to therapy could just as easily be positive. That is. given a certain conception of the therapeutic situation. the dogmatic person could be more. rather than less. attracted to it than the open-minded person. 45 If a dogmatic individual views therapy as "a way to produce a fixed or rigid 'healthy state of mind.'" or as “a virtuous act which will be rewarded by relief of difficulties" (Gold- stein. 1962). or as a setting which he can enter. thereafter relying on the therapist to tell him What to do and solve his problems for him. he will be more likely to approach therapy than will the open—minded person with the same con- ception of therapy. Such an expectation of the therapeutic process is referred to by Apfelbaum (1958) as 'nurturant.‘ Thus. it would seem that an essential factor in relation- ships between manner of thinking and attitude toward helping situations is one's qualitative conception of the therapeutic process. The second hypothesis stated that the greater the discrepancy between real self and ideal self. the greater would be the projected attraction toward a helping situation. However. results indicated a negative trend between ICL dis- crepancy and attraction to therapy. though not at a statis- tically significant level. If there really is a negative relation between these two variables. how can it be explained? According to Leary (1957. p. 205). ". . . discrepancy between ideal and real self provides an index of self- 46 acceptance. This variable then plays a most crucial role in arousing motivation for therapy.” Are we to conclude. then. that the less a person accepts himself (as indicated by high ICL discrepancy score). the less likely he will be to seek help? Those gs who. on the basis of self-ideal discrepancy. would seem to be the most highly motivated for therapy are not really the ones who approach it. If this is true. then those who do. in fact. seek and obtain therapeutic help are those that need it the least: But recall that Rogers has demonstrated a considerable discrepancy between real and ideal self in clients entering therapy. In addition. Butler and Haigh (1954) not only have shown that such discrepancy decreases as a function of therapy. but also that a non- therapy control group had consistent self-ideal correlations similar to the post-therapy correlations of the experimental group. So we cannot conclude that in general those who really need help. as indicated by self-ideal discrepancy. do not enter therapy. or that discrepancy scores of those enter- ing therapy are not really different from discrepancy scores of those not entering therapy. Perhaps the discrepancy among freshmen students is situationally bound rather than psychically or dynamically 47 related. Thus. the star of the debate team in high school finds himself surrounded by stars of greater magnitude in college. setting up a conflict in self-concept and related self—ideal which may or may not be dynamically based. Dy- namic is used here to refer to psychological functioning. Accordingly. perhaps the discrepancy indicated by freshmen is simply a result of what they see about them (i.e. talent equaling and surpassing their own) rather than an index of how they really feel about themselves or how much they really accept themselves. An evaluation of Rogers' discrep- ancy measures suggests that his Q-sort items are more re- lated to feelings. attitudes. and values. The crucial fac- tor could be the instrument itself or the age and experience of the clients used in his studies. At any rate. it seems reasonable to suspect that his measures are a more accurate reflection of how the gs think and feel than are the ones used in the present study. In viewing the negative trend between-ICL discrepancy and attraction to therapy. we may ask what high ICL discrep- ancy reveals about an individual. Why would the person with many characteristics that he would like to change actually be unwilling to approach therapy? Perhaps we must ask What 48 the ICL is really measuring. or what is anoflner level at which the discrepancy score can be interpreted. Then. why would the person scoring high in this 'something' be less willing to enter a helping situation than the person scoring low? Perhaps the therapeutic situation would be too threat- ening for the person who is so much different than he would like to be. If this is the case..it would seem that the ICL is measuring defensiveness. At any rate. it cannot be as- sumed that ICL is necessarily an indication of an individ- ual's dissatisfaction with himself. It is reasonable to suppose that experiences at.a large university have a great deal of effect on the typical freshman. The extent to which this impact affects him in his first or second term may be quite variable. Thus. the freshman may be admitting a difference between self and ideal without feeling that he would need to do much about change (attraction to therapy) at that time. There is so much novel stimuli impinging upon him that he has not yet had a chance to evaluate and integrate it so that his whole person is influenced. To speculate as Rokeach has done (1966). perhaps the ICL measures attitude toward the object. i.e. the self. 49 whereas the PIT measures attitude toward the situation in which the self is functioning as the major focus. i.e. a helping situation. High ICL discrepancy would indicate a negative attitude toward oneself. while low discrepancy would reveal a positive attitude toward oneself. High PIT can be interpreted as a testimony that the individual feels capable of change. that he has a positive attitude toward undergoing change. or toward functioning in a situation de- signed primarily to help him change. Finally. low PIT would imply a more static concept of the self. in that it indicates a negative attitude toward being in a change-inducing situa- tion. One could also speculate using ICL discrepancy as a measure of attitude toward change in the abstract. with PIT measuring attitude toward the process of changing. At this point. it should be interesting to consider some of the responses to the PIT made by S5 with high and low ICL discrepancy scores. In answer to the question "What will happen? What will be done?" in Picture I. a S with a high ICL discrepancy score (e.g. 47) wrote the following: I will leave the counselor (in my imagination) feeling disgusted. I didn‘t find any answers. I will be depressed. I probably won't go for coun- seling. 50 The same S. describing what is being thought or felt in Pic- ture II. wrote: I again have this feeling of not letting my- self express my innermost feelings. I want to yet I don't feel secure enough to think he'd understand. Similarly. answering all four questions about Picture III. this S wrote: Some woman is leaving a doctor's office (pre- sumably a counselor). She isn't happy - She is confused. She can't take advice from this man - it reminds her of her father. She got mad at him. He asked her questions she didn't want to answer. He seemed to confuse her. She is frustrated — she can't feel innocent - it's always guilty. guilty. guilty. Nothing because she can trust no one or love no one. She is alone. A negative attitude toward the therapist is quite evident. On the other hand. a positive feeling is conveyed by the stories of a‘g who scored low in ICL discrepancy (e.g. 21). Describing what will happen in Picture II. she wrote: The psychiatrist will ask her many questions about her association with males before and after and during this instance. He will have her come for numerous visits and try to help her overcome her fear of getting involved and hurt again. The same S composed the following in response to Picture III: 51 Janice is leaving the office of her trusted analyst for the last time. Janice had an abnormal fear of the dark. Her husband had sent her to the analyst in the first place. because even when she went to bed she had to have a light on. Janice is feeling proud now. Proud. because she had enough courage to go to an analyst and together the two of them conquered her problem. All that is wanted is a chance. Janice will go home with more confidence in herself. She will become a much better person — and a much happier one. These stories are not representative. in that the client-therapist relationship is directly projected and des- cribed. A more typical protocol. from aig scoring low in ICL discrepancy (e.g. 19) is the following story written in response to Picture I. His A man is thinking of seeing his doctor. He had a violent argument with a friend who called him mentally insane. He wants to find out from his doctor if he needs treatment. The doctor says no - his friend is the one who needs help. story for Picture III was: This man is leaving his doctor's office. He has just had a physical. 52 He had an accident on his job. he sprained his back. The doctor has just given him the O.K. to go back to work. This is what the man wanted. The man will resume work beginning Monday. Finally. here is the story he composed about Picture IV. A man is waiting to see his doctor. He has had a chest X-ray — is waiting to find out the results. He is concerned for fear he might have can- cer. He hopes the X-ray will be negative. When he sees the doctor he will be told he has not got cancer. A representative story from a §_scoring high in ICL discrep— ancy (e.g. 63) is the following. written in response to Pic- ture III: A woman is just leaving a doctor's office. after having her yearly physical. She does this regularly. once a year. so this is no abnormal activity. The woman thinks nothing. She has had her physical. and commonly thinks little of it. She is healthy and well adjusted. Should she be thankful? Might this be lacking? The woman will go on living life as usual. She will be oblivious to those around her. per- haps even to those persons who are physically handicapped and unhealthy. Nothing will be done. because most people are like the woman. and are not concerned. 53 The same S composed the following for Picture IV: The woman is waiting for the dentist to return from lunch. because she has an appointment with him. She has a toothache which suddenly deVeloped. She called the dentist an hour ago and he said that he would see her during his lunch hour. (The toothache is. after all. quite painful.) She is waiting. but he is not there. The woman is disgusted and discouraged. be- cause the dentist has not shown up. and because her toothache is very painful. The woman will leave before the dentist re- turns from lunch. forgetting about her pain be- cause she is too mad at the dentist for failing to show up. The next day she will call up an- other dentist. and stop seeing her old dentist. Finally. another § scoring high in ICL discrepancy (e.g. 56) and low on the PIT responded to Picture III as follows: A woman is leaving the office of her family physician. (Who is involved?) She. Dr.. husband. an unborn child. She suspected that she was pregnant. She and her husband wanted a child so they had sexual in- tercourse. The woman is pleased with herself and her hus- band. and yet she is terribly afraid of having a child. She wants her husband to get very thrilled over the aspect that she is expecting. She will tell her husband. He will hug and kiss her. Eventually she will have feelings that she is less appealing and will continually need reassurance from her husband. They will have the child. but may argue a lot before it is born. 54 Her story about Picture IV was: The woman is waiting to see the doctor about her regular pregnancy checkup. (Who is involved?) Woman. Dr.. baby. She has to make these visits. (What happened before?) No complication. She is nervous for it is nearing the time of delivery. (What is wanted?) No complications. plus not having to have the baby. She will suffer and then have it. She will receive an anesthetic as soon as she can. Many of the stories written by female gs described a visit to the doctor regarding pregnancy. While physical examinations for the army constituted the theme for many of the males' stories. It is plausible that for this age group. a positive attitude toward a medical doctor or dentist has no relation to his attitude toward a therapist. This may pro- vide further indication of the PIT's lack of sensitivity to freshmen. In addition. recall that in all but one previous use (Mullin. 1966) the PIT was administered individually (in a testing room or by a therapist in his office) in a one to one relationship. and always after a counseling session. rather than in the classroom. In designing the present study. it was hoped that this factor was compensated for 55 in part by the directions to ". . . imagine that you or a close friend has gone to see someone about some problems. .“ Perhaps. with more psychologically sophisticated gs or with §s having had some experience in therapy or counseling. the classroom situation would not stifle the projection of more experiential. personal. psychological types of problems. However. in this study. gs having had therapeutic experiences were discarded for this very reason. That is. it was sus- pected that §s who had at one time obtained psychotherapy would make different responses in general than gs without such experience. We wanted to avoid confusing the 'gestalt' by contamination due to the experience factor. In light of results it is reasonable to conclude that. given the lack of certain types of experiences. the classroom situation may. indeed. elicit more medical or objective types of problems than would the clinical setting. The third hypothesis predicted that a high dogmatism score would be correlated with a low ICL discrepancy score. Results. however. showed that these two variables are posi— tively related at a significant level for females but not for males. It is suspected that the difference in N accounts for the lack of significance among males. 56 Through this study. we had hOped to show that a highly dogmatic person would be less aware of discrepancy between real and ideal self. It was reasoned that his narrow approach would prevent him from recognizing charac- teristics in himself that deviate from his ideal. Results. however. demonstrated that the highly dogmatic person ac- tually sees more about himself that he would like to change. or more that differs from his ideal. than does the less dog- matic person. Apparently then. a closed outlook does not prevent the individual from identifying desired changes in himself. But can we assume that self-ideal discrepancy in- dicates desired change? Again. what does ICL discrepancy score indicate? If we considered ICL discrepancy an index of personal problems. present results would be consistent with flaose of Kemp (1961) who demonstrated a positive rela— tion between dogmatism and number of personal problems. However. we are not completely satisfied with this inter- pretation of the discrepancy measure. The positive correlation between dogmatism and self- ideal discrepancy. and the tendency for both to be negatively related to attraction score leads to the suggestion that ICL discrepancy score is a measure of defensiveness. Given that 57 dogmatism has been referred to as a “. . . network of . . . defense mechanisms" (Rokeach 1960. p. 70» it seems plausible that recognition of large differences between real and ideal self is also a defense. Identifying a great many desired changes or less than ideal characteristics in oneself could be very threatening. But such identification could also function to reduce the threat of the possible accuracy of large discrepancies. In this case. the recognition is not integrated with the self-concept or psychological function- ing. It is. rather. kept at a safe distance. and compart- mentalized from the functioning self. In short. indicating a manifest awareness of very large discrepancies between ideal and real self can shield an individual from the threat of genuinely believing any of the less than ideal traits to which he subscribes. Within this interpretative framework. the low attraction score of highly dogmatic and discrepant l§s is easily explained as the avoidance of exposing or threatening situations. If an individual is defensive about certain areas. it follows that he will not approach situations which will focus on the vulnerable material. Let us for a moment discuss the positive correlation between ICL discrepancy score and dogmatism in terms of the 58 latter. Recall that a 'closed-minded' outlook refers to a tendency to receive. evaluate. and act upon information on the basis of irrelevant factors. Highly dogmatic persons perceive things in categories. in terms of blacks and whites with no allowance for greys. They tend to label new exper- iences prior to exploration. and they approach objective as well as personal data very narrowly. Given the tendency to categorize. it is reasonable to speculate that a dogmatic person will see himself as 'always' acting a certain way or 'never' acting that way. He will picture himself as 'all' of something or as 'none' of it because he cannot deal with shadings of a trait or with subtle mixtures of good and bad or worthy and unworthy. In terms of the ICL. he will tend to conceive of himself as 'always' or 'never' bitter or as 'all' forgiving or completely incapable of forgiving. Con- sequently. he will have a higher discrepancy score than the individual who can discern an overlap between degrees of 'always' and 'never' or between 'all' and 'none.‘ In addi- tion. the dogmatic person is more likely to have an unreal- istic self-ideal in the sense that his ideal is based more on external sources of what is desirable than on his poten- tial and desires or on his tendency to self-actualize. 59 Consequently. the dogmatic S will be more stringent in eval- uating himself. He perceives himself one way—-he either is or is not described by given characteristics. and the des- cription he wants to fit is unrealistic. so he has little chance of qualifying for 'all of it. all of the time.‘ In light of the age group used in this study. it is interesting to consider that perhaps highly dogmatic §s per- ceive themselves as falling so far short of their ideal be— cause they are different from their parents whom they tend to glorify (Rokeach. 1960: Adorno et al.. 1950). Since dOg- matic persons tend to rely on external authority. perhaps the §s in this study were actually comparing themselves to their parents who have become their ideal. The fourth hypothesis speculated that high ICL dis- crepancy and high dogmatism Would be opposing forces in re- lation to attraction to therapy. the former pushing an indi— ‘vidual toward therapy and the latter pulling him away from therapy. The results were not significant. but it is inter- esting to infer from the obtained trends. It was found that high dogmatism and high self-ideal discrepancy both function as resisting forces. serving to inhibit an individual from being attracted to therapy. On 60 approaching this study. it was assumed that ICL discrepancy is a measure of how much the individual is hurting--i.e.. how much mental conflict he is experiencing as indicated by his dissatisfaction with himself. However. as discussed earlier. it appears that self-ideal discrepancy is another index of Openness. Further. there is reason to suspect that ICL discrepancy score is a more subtle measure of openness than dogmatism score. Indication of this possibility is provided by those gs whose scores on dogmatism.and ICL dis- crepancy were in opposite directions--i.e. high on one and low on the other. For these gs. PIT scores tended in the direction predicted by the relation between ICL and PIT. For example. §s with low ICL discrepancy scores (which would predict high PIT scores) and high dogmatism scores (which would predict low attraction) actually have the high- est mean PIT score as a group. Again. §s with high ICL dis- crepancy scores (which alone would predict low attraction score) and low dogmatism.score (which would predict high PIT score) actually have the second lowest mean PIT score as a group. There is another possibility to account for the above trends. Perhaps the ICL is a less threatening instru- ment than the Dogmatism Scale. For a college student it may 61 be more socially acceptable to admit to differences in real and ideal self than it is to be closed-minded. Several results remain to be discussed. The high correlation between dogmatism and ICL discrepancy score for the §s with no scoreable pictures warrants some comment. In light of previous discussion. it seems reasonable to conclude that projection of helping situations was too threatening for these 24 gs. Their defenses were tight enough to pre- vent projection of therapeutic settings into any of the four pictures. That is. given their high dogmatism and ICL dis- crepancy scores. they would be too threatened to see in any of the pictures. someone seeking help. To ward off the po- tential threat. these gs kept their stories far enough re: moved from anything personal as to render them completely devoid of help seeking settings or indications of attitude toward seeking help. Two minor issues seem worthy of mention. The signif- icant difference between males and females on the number of pictures scored seems explainable by the fact that girls tend to be more verbal than boys. Finally. it should be noted that the lack of sex differences in dogmatism is not consistent with past research. Rokeach and Norrell (1966) found a significantly higher mean dogmatism score for males. 62 In general the results of this study did not support the original theory. Individuals with a closed-minded ap- proach tended to be less willing than open-minded persons to become involved in a helping situation. but the differences were not statistically significant. Persons who described themselves as very different from their ideal tended to be less desirous of obtaining help than those admitting to lesser degrees of self—ideal discrepancy. but again the dif- ferences were not significant. Closed-minded gs admitted large discrepancies between real and ideal self. while the more open-minded individuals described their real selves as much closer to their ideal selves. The positive correlation was statistically significant. (For future research. it would be interesting to con- vert the Check List scores of these gs into placements on Leary's interpersonal diagnostic grid to obtain personality profiles. Also. this study might be replicated with a more sophisticated non—client population and a clearly clinical population. including individuals continuing and discontin- uing treatment. SUMMARY Since the therapeutic setting is one in which new in— formation and ideas are evaluated and personal experiences are explored. one's attitude toward such a situation should be related to his willingness to receive and integrate new information. Another important motivational factor in atti- tude toward therapy should be the extent to which one is dif- ferent from what he would like to be. It has been shown that closed-minded persons have a greater number of personal problems and are less likely to succeed in therapy than open-minded persons (Kemp. 1961). Experimental results also testify that self-ideal.correla- tions are significantly lower for §§ entering therapy than for non-therapy control §s (Butler and Haigh. 1954). and that self—ideal correlations increase as a function of ther- apy (Rogers. 1961). This study investigated the relationship between re- ceptivity to new information (dogmatism). discrepancy between description of real self and description of ideal self. and projected attraction toward a helping situation. The following 63 64 hypotheses were tested: 1) The higher the dogmatism score. the less will an individual project attraction to a helping situation. 2) The greater the discrepancy between self and ideal. the greater will be the projected attraction toward a helping situation. 3) The higher the dogmatism score. the lower will be the discrepancy between perception of real and ideal self. 4) Dogmatism and self—ideal discrepancy will interact with projected attraction to a helping situa- tion in such a way that (a) high dogmatism combined with high self-ideal discrepancy will result in moderate attrac- tion to a helping situation and (b) low dogmatism combined with high self-ideal discrepancy will result in high attrac- tion to a helping situation while (c) high dogmatism combined with a low self-ideal discrepancy will result in low attrac- tion to a helping situation and (d) low dogmatism combined with low self-ideal discrepancy will result in low attraction to a helping situation. Subjects consisted of 260 male and female undergrad- uates (who had never sought psychological help) enrolled in introductory psychology courses at Michigan State University. Separate analyses were made for males and females. Rokeach's ngmatism Scale was used to obtain an in- dex of open-mindedness or receptivity to new information. 65 Leary's Interpersonal Check List (ICL) rendered a measure of discrepancy between self and ideal. and Libo's Picture Im- pressions Test (PIT) yielded a measure of projected attrac- tion to a helping situation. Hypothesis 1 was not supported. though results indi- cated a trend in the expected direction. Analysis of data for hypothesis 2 suggested a strong trend in the direction opposite to prediction. Hypothesis 3 was supported in the direction Opposite to the prediction (p < .10 for males. p < .01 for females). Hypothesis 4 was not supported though data tended in a direction opposite to the prediction. It was suggested that the lack of significant results with regard to the PIT may be accounted for by the fact that the test was administered in the classroom situation to a ncni-clinical population of (freshman) undergraduate students. It is suspected that the combination of these two factors rendered the PIT less sensitive in eliciting personal. psy- chological types of problems of sufficient intensity to pro- vide statistically significant results. The positive correlation between dogmatism and ICL discrepancy score is plausible if both are interpreted as measures of openness. The trend toward low attraction 66 scores for high dOgmatism and high ICL discrepancy scorers was explained as the defensive avoidance of a situation that may arouse threatening material. Within the framework of Rokeach's theory on the na- ture of attitudes. it was argued that attitude toward an object (e.g. myself or therapy as an object) is quite dif— ferent from attitude toward a situation (e.g. myself in ther- apy). Thus. while highly dogmatic individuals will not be attracted to helping situations in general. individuals whose high dogmatism is accompanied by low self-ideal dis- crepancy may perceive the helping situation positively when considered for its advice-giving potential or seen in terms of someone other than themselves receiving help. On the other hand. dogmatic individuals with high self-ideal dis— crepancy. having a greater amount of personal defensiveness. fail to show attraction to therapy because they see it as an ongoing situation in which they are personally involved. Irather than something more objective and removed from them- selves. That is. their higher self-ideal discrepancy. sug— gesting more personal defensiveness and hence greater fear. facilitates their perception of therapy as a situation in which they are involved and prevents them from projecting themselves into the helping situation. 67 It was also suggested that the self-ideal discrepancy to which freshmen subscribe may be more situationally bound than dynamically related. Thus. they are willing to admit discrepancies between what they are and what they would like to be. while not necessarily wanting to do anything about it at the time because they are struggling with the impact of a new environment. APPENDIX 69 APPENDIX Table A Obtained Values of Eta for all Combinations of Variables MALES FEMALES Variables Eta Df F Eta Df F PIT/Dog .445 17.74 1.04 .435 20.123 1.43 Dog/PIT .527 17.74 1.6 .299 19.124 .633 PIT/ICL .323 16.75. .544 .294 19.124 .614 ICL/PIT .398 17.74 .813 .335 19.124 .823 Dog/ICL .447 16.75 1.187 .387 19.124 1.14 ICL/DOg .479 17.74 1.3 .500 20.123 2.04* *Significantly different from 0 at .01 level. 1.739; df cantly non-linear at .05 level (F These tests were computed according to Walker and Lev (1953. p. 278). Also signifi- = 19.123). 70 APPENDIX Table B Male Pictures for the Picture Impression Test* PICTURE! PICTURE“ l 113.7(6)}... . >3 1 4 (ELI/$8 L..._J A PICTURE m *Female cards have the same setting. but a woman appears in the picture rather than a man. 71 APPENDIX Table C Dogmatism Scale PSYCHOLOGY QUESTIONNAIRE The following is a study of what the general public thinks and feels about a number of important social and personal questions. The best answer to each statement below is your .personal opinion. We have tried to cover many different and Opposing points of view: you may find yourself agreeing strongly with some of the statements. disagreeing just as strongly with others. and perhaps uncertain about others. Whether you agree or disagree with any statement. you can be sure that many other people feel the same as you do. Mark each statement in the left margin according to how much you agree or disagree with it.) Please mark every one. Write +1. +2. +3. or —1. -2. -3. depending on how you feel in each case. +1 I agree a little. -1 I disagree a little. +2 I agree on the whole. -2 I disagree on the whole. +3 I agree very much. -3 I disagree very much. 1. A person who thinks primarily of his own happiness is beneath contempt. 2. The main thing in life is for a person to want to do something important. 3. In a discussion I often find it necessary to repeat myself several times to make sure I am being under- stood. 4. Most people just don't know what's good for them.; 5. In times like these. a person must be pretty selfish if he considers primarily his own happiness. 6. A man who does not believe in some great cause has not really lived. 72 Appendix Table C (continued) +1 I agree a little. -1 I disagree a little. +2 I agree on the whole. -2 I disagree on the whole. +3 I agree very much. -3 I disagree very much. 7. I'd like it if I could find someone who would tell 10. ll. 12. 13. 14. 15. l6. 17. me how to solve my personal problems. Of all the different philosophies which exist in this world there is probably only one which is correct. It is only when a person devotes himself to an ideal or cause that life becomes meaningful. In this complicated world of ours the only way we know what is going on is to rely on leaders or ex- perts who can be trusted. There are a number of persons I have come to hate because of the things they stand for. There is so much to be done and so little time to do it in. It is better to be a dead hero than a live coward. A group which tolerates too much difference of opin- ion among its own members cannot exist for long. It is only natural that a person should have a much better acquaintance with ideas he believes in than with ideas he Opposes. While I don't like to admit this even to myself. my secret ambition is to become a great man. like Ein- stein. Or Beethoven. or Shakespeare. Even though freedom of speech for all groups is a worthwhile goal. it is unfortunately necessary to restrict the freedom Of certain political groups. 73 Appendix Table D (contined) +1 I agree a little. -1 I disagree a little. +2 I agree on the whole. -2 I disagree on the whole. +3 I agree very much. -3 I disagree very much. 18. If a man is to accomplish his mission in life it is sometimes necessary to gamble “all or nothing at all.“ 19. Most people just don't give a "damn" for others. 20. A person who gets enthusiastic about too many causes is likely to be a pretty "wishy-washy" sort of per- son. 21. To compromise with our political opponents is danger- ous because it usually leads to the betrayal of our own side. 22. If given the chance I would do something of great benefit to the world. 23. In times like these it is often necessary to be more on guard against ideas put out by peOple or groups in one's own camp than by those in the Opposing camp. 24. In a heated discussion I generally become so absorbed in what I am going to say that I forget to listen to what the others are saying. 25. Once I get wound up in a heated discussion I just can't stop. 26. There are two kinds of people in the world: those who are for the truth and those who are against the truth. 27. Man on his own is a helpless and miserable creature. 28. The United States and Russia have just about nothing in common. 74 Appendix Table D (continued) +1 I agree a little. -1 I disagree a little. +2 I agree on the whole. -2 I disagree on the whole. +3 I agree very much. —3 I disagree very much. 29. In the history of mankind there have probably been just a handful of really great thinkers. 30. The highest form of government is a democracy and the highest form of democracy is a government run by those who are most intelligent. 31. The present is all too often full of unhappiness. It is only the future that counts. 32. Unfortunately. a good many people with whom I have discussed important social and moral problems don't really understand what's going on. 33. Fundamentally. the world we live in is a pretty lonesome place. 34. It is Often desirable to reserve judgment about what's going on until one has had a chance to hear the opinions of those one respects. 35. The worst crime a person could commit is to attack publicly the people who believe in the same thing he does. 36. In the long run the best way to live is to pick friends and associates whose tastes and beliefs are the same as one's own. 37. Most of the ideas which get printed nowadays aren't worth the paper they are printed on. 38. It is only natural for a person to be rather fearful of the future. 39. My blood boils whenever a person stubbornly refuses to admit he's wrong. 40. When it comes to differences of Opinion in religion we must be careful not to compromise with those who believe differently from the way we do. 75 APPENDIX Table D Interpersonal Check List DIRECTIONS FOR IDEAL-SELF This is the same list of words and phrases that you just used to describe yourself. This time. please circle the number of each item that is descriptive of yourself as you would like to be. Remember that your first impression is generally the best. so please go through the list as quickly as you can. Be sure to consider each item. but circle only those that describe yourself as you would like to be. 1. Able to give orders 26. Cooperative 2. Appreciative 27. Complaining 3. Apologetic 28. Can be indifferent to 4. Able to take care of self others 5. Accepts advice readily 29. Critical of others 6. Able to doubt others 30. Can be obedient 7. Affectionate and under- 31. Cruel and unkind standing 32. Dependent 8. Acts important 33. .Dictatorial 9. Able to criticize self 34. Distrusts everybody 10. Admires and imitates others 35. Dominating ll. Agrees with everyone - 36. Easily embarrassed 12. Always ashamed of self 37. Eager to get along with 13. Very anxious to be others approved of 38. Easily fooled 14. Always giving advice 39. Egotistical and conceited 15. Bitter 40. Easily led 16. Bighearted and unselfish 41. Encouraging to others 17. Boastful 42. Enjoys taking care of 18. Businesslike others 19. Bossy 43. Expects everyone to ad- 20. Can be frank and honest mire him 21. Clinging vine 44. Faithful follower 22. Can be strict if necessary 45. Frequently disappointed 23. Considerate 46. Firm but just 24. Cold and unfeeling 47. Fond of everyone 25. Can complain if necessary 48. Forceful 76 Appendix Table D (continued) 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. 76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 81. 82. 83. 84. 85. 86. 87. 88. Friendly Forgives anything Frequently angry Friendly all the time Generous to a fault Gives freely of self Good leader Grateful Hard-boiled when necessary Helpful Hard-hearted Hard to convince Hot-tempered Hard to impress Impatient with others' mistakes Independent Irritable Jealous Kind and reassuring Likes responsibility Lacks self-confidence Likes to compete with others Lets others make decisions Likes everybody Likes to be taken care of Loves everybody Makes a good impression Manages others‘ Meek Modest Hardly ever talks back Often admired Obeys too willingly Often gloomy Outspoken Overprotective of others Often unfriendly Oversympathetic Often helped by others Passive and unaggressive 89. 90. 91. 92. 93. 94. 95. 96. 97. 98. 99. 100. 101. 102. 103. 104. 105. 106. 107. 108. 109. 110. 111. 112. 113. 114. 115. 116. 117. 118. 119. 120. 121. 122. Proud and self-satisfied Always pleasant and agreeable Resentful Respected by others Rebels against every- thing Resents being bossed Self-reliant and assertive Sarcastic Self-punishing Self-confident Self-seeking Shrewd and calculating Self-respecting Shy Sincere and devoted to friends Selfish Skeptical Sociable and neighborly Slow to forgive a wrong Somewhat snobbish Spineless Stern but fair Spoils people with kindness Straightforward and direct Stubborn Suspicious Too easily influenced by friends Thinks only of self Tender and soft-hearted Timid Too lenient with others Touchy and easily hurt Too willing to give to others Tries to be too success- ful Appendix Table D (continued) 123. Trusting and eager to please 124. Tries to comfort everyone 125. Usually gives in 126. very respectful to authority 127. Wants everyone's love 128. Well thought of 77 129. 130. 131. 132.- 133. 134. Wants to be led Will confide in anyone Warm Wants everyone to like him Will believe anyone Well-behaved BIBLIOGRAPHY Adorno et al. (The Authoritarian Personality. New York: Harper. 1950. Apfelbaum. D. Dimensions of Transference in Psychotherapy. Berkeley: Univ. of Calif. Press. 1958. Braaten. L. J. The movement from self to nOn-self in client- centered psychotherapy. Jrn. Counseling Psy.. 1961. 8. 20—24. Butler. J. M. and Haigh. G. V. Changes in the relation be— tween self-concepts and ideal concepts consequent upon client-centered counseling. In C. R. ROgers and Rosalind Dymond (eds.) Psychotherapy and Person- .alitinhanqe. Univ. of Chicago Press. 1954. pp. 55- 75. Ehrlich. H. J. Dogmatism and learning. Jrn. Abn. and Soc. Psy.. 1961. 62. 148-149. Ehrlich. H. J. Dogmatism and learning: a S-year followup. Psy. Reports. 1961. 9. 283-286. Goldstein. A. P. Therapist-Patient Expectations in Psycho- therapy. New YOrk: Macmillan CO.. 1962. Hartley. M. Q technique—-its methodology and application. In Blake. R. R. and Ramsey. G. V. Perception--An Approach to Personality. New YOrk: Ronald Press. 1951. Hollenbeck. P. N. A study of the effects of dogmatism on aspects of adjustment to severe disability. Ph.D. dissertation. Western Reserve University. 1964. (Dissertation Abstracts. 1965. 25(4). 11. 6759-6760). Kaplan. Martin F.. and Singer. E. Dogmatism and sensory alienation: an empirical investigation. Jrn. Con- sult. Psy.. 1963. 27 (4). 314-319. 78 79 Kemp. C. G. Effect of dogmatism on critical thinking. Sch. Sci. Math.. April 1960. 40 (4). 314-319. Kemp. C. G. Influence of dogmatism on counseling. Personal Guidance Journal. 1961. 39. 662-665. LaForge. R.. and Suczek. R. The interpersonal dimension of personality: III. An interpersonal check list. Jrn. Personality. 1955. 24. 94-112. Leary. T.. and Coffey. H. S. Interpersonal diagnosis: some problems of methodology and validation. Jrn. Abn. Soc. Psy.. 1955. 50. 110-124. Leary. T. The Interpersonal Diagnosis o£_Persona1ity. New YOrk: Ronald Press. 1957. Libo. L. M. Measuring Group Cohesiveness. Ann Arbor: Univ. of Mich. Press. Institute for Social Research. 1953. Libo. L. M. The projective expression of patient-therapist attraction. Jrn. Clinical Psy.. 1957. 13 (1). 33-36. Libo. L. M. Picture Impressions: A Projective Technique for Investigating the Patient-Therapist Relationship. Baltimore. Univ. of Maryland Medical School. Dept. Of Psychiatry Publication Series. 1956. Moore. Dorothy. Dogmatism and anxiety in the conditioning of verbal behavior. Ph.D. dissertation. Louisiana' State University. 1961. Mullen. J. Client-therapist attraction as perceived on the Picture Impressions Test. M.A$ thesis, Michigan State University. 1966. Raimy. V. C. Self-reference in counseling interviews. Jrn. Consulp. Psy.. 1948. 12. 153-183. Restle. F.. Andrews. M.. and Rokeach. M. Differences between open- and closed-minded subjects on learning-set and oddity problems. Jrn. Apn. Soc. Psy.. 1964. 68 (6). 648-654. 80 Rogers. C. R. Personality change in psychotherapy. In_ Rogers. C. R. On Becomipg a Person. Boston: Houg- ton Mifflin. 1961. Rogers. C. R. Client-centered therapy in its context of research. In Rogers. C. R. On Becoming a Person. Houghton Mifflin. 1961. Rokeach. M. The Open and Closed Ming. New York: Basic Books. 1960. Rokeach. M.. and Norrell. G. Part II: Analysis. synthesis. and academic performance. Final report of research submitted in March 1966. Rokeach. M. The nature of attitudes. International Encyclo- pedia of the Social Sciences. Prepublication copy. Sanford. Nevitt (ed.). The American Cpllege. New York: J. Wiley. 1964. Sheerer. E. T. An analysis of the relationship between ac- ceptance of and respect:for.3elf and accecptance of and respect for others in ten counseling cases. Jpn. Consult. Psy.. 1949. 13. 169-175. Walker. H. M. and Lev.. J. Statistical Inference. New York: Holt. Reinhart. and Winston. 1953.