ADULT ROLE PLAYING RESPONSES TO VARYING CHILD COMMUNTCATIONS - o r Thesis for the Degree Of M A MECHTGAN STATE UNIVERSITY EDWARD CHARLES TEYBER ' 197 5 ‘ ' - ooooo o gussm ‘ ‘ h J. V \ A _ r‘ L r " . x ‘ "- I 5 “315358 a : 31$.» 3? “ HUAB & SBNS‘ 800K BINDERY'INB. -. LIBRARY BINDWS msmrmcum v , E a I ‘ 7 . LE...»— ADULT ROLE-PLAYING RESPONSES TO VARYING CHILD COMMUNICATIONS BY Edward Charles Teyber A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1975 ADULT ROLE-PLAYING RESPONSES TO VARYING CHILD COMMUNICATIONS By Edward C. Teyber The purpose of this study was to provide additional information relevant to how adults respond to varying types of child communications. More specifically, the present research examined the extent to which types of adult responses and their communication of acceptance and rejection are affected by child communication sequences involving loving, neutral, and/or hostile messages. In an attempt to gain information about adult responses to these types of emotion-laden child communications that would be reflective of actual social behavior, the present research had subjects role play a parent and verbalize their immediate response to two sequences of tape recorded child communications. A global rating of acceptance/rejection was used in conjunction with 26 scoring categories that assessed the specific and concrete responses used by adults in their communications to the child. The scoring categories utilized were designed to describe adult responses along affective and behavioral dimensions indicative of either theoretically sensitive adult responding to children (Stollak et a1, 1973) and insensitive behavior (Gordon, 1970). These measures Edward C. Teyber were used to test the hypotheses that 1) various child communications would elicit reciprocal or similar adult responses, and 2) initial child communications of a positive and negative nature would establish a response "set" and influence the adult's response to a second sequential child cue. Subjects for the experiment included 180 (90 male and 90 female) undergraduates who responded to the role-play tape. Adult behavior was scored by raters into 26 categories. The mean frequency of category usage was factor analyzed and six composite variables were generated. These factors described the following modes of responding: I) teaching- lecturing; II) control dominance; III) adult expression of their own experience and child's influence upon them; IV) empathy; V) intimidation-interrogation; and VI) instru- mental control. These factors, along with a global acceptance- rejection scale, served as dependent variables which were examined in relation to the independent variables by means of a multivariate analysis of variance. Results confirmed the initial hypothesis where adults were found to respond in_§ind to child messages with reciprocal acceptance and rejection. Concerning specific modes of response given, Factors I, II and V were elicited in response to negative-rejecting child cues, and Factor III was elicited in response to positive-loving child cues. A sex difference was obtained where males were both globally rated as being less accepting and demonstrated greater usage of the punitive- Edward C. Teyber rejecting Factors II and V than did females. The very low frequency of the child-centered Factor IV mode of response was also noted. These results were discussed in terms of their implica- tions for education and training in child-care-giving. The methodology used in this study was discussed as a useful instrument for studying the adult-child communication process, and finally, an explanation for the failure to confirm the second hypothesis was proposed. / 7W ./ ‘ / r f I" I w 4 " /. / 734%5 To my parents, Kate and Ed ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to "acknowledge" and thank Dr. Gary E. Stollak both for teaching me much about children and for being with me as a mutually respected friend--his guidance in this thesis being just one manifestation of these things. I want to acknowledge and thank Dr. Lawrence A. Messe' for saying things like "well; cross that bridge when we come to it" and then following through with an extra amount of very patient time and work. Also, I wish to thank him for allowing me the pleasure of sharing time with his family. My appreciation to Dr. Elaine Donnelson for stepping in at a last minute's notice. And finally, to my companion Marta who shared a lot with me. iii TEBLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . LITERATURE REVIEW. . . . . . . . . Acceptance. . . . . . . . . . . Specific and Global Measurement Child Communications . . . . Interaction Sequences . . DireCtion Of EffeCtS o o o o 0 Theoretical Formulation . . . . Sensitive Adult Responding . . Methods of Study . . . . . . . Hypotheses. . . . . . . . . . METHODoooooooooooooo Subjects and Design HeaSIlreS o o o o o o o o o o AdIHiniStI‘ation o o o o o o -o o Scoring the Dependent Variable msmTS O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O Inter Rater Reliabilities . . . Relations Between Categories . Category Usage 0 o o o o o 0 Factor Analysis . . . . . Multivariate Analysis of Varian Summary of Results . . . . . . Factor Analysis Results . . Summary of Manova Results . DISCUSSION SECTION . . . a . o . . Reciprocal Adult Responding . Empathyooooooooooo Sequence Effects . . . . . . PIGthOdOlogy o o o -E o o o o O Generalizing to Other Popula i n iv 00.000000 . ooomoooo o o o o o S 00...... O O... O OOOOOOOQJ. 5:3 oooooooI—lo d” O O... O 0...... Page Vi C ONC LUS I OTIS O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O BIBLIOGRJQPEEY O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O APPENDICES Appendix A. scoring; Guide to PCIPT . O o o o o o o o o o B. Instructions to Subject . . . . . . . . . . Rehearsal Dialogues. . . . . . . . . . . . . PCIPT script 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O C. Rater Instructions for PCIPT Equality . . . D. Table I: Inter Rater Reliability for Scoring Categories and Acceptance- Rejection Measure . . . . . . . . E. Table II: Mean Category Usage and Standard Deviation for the 25 Scoring Categories Utilized . . . . . . . F. Summary of Factor Loading . . . . . . . . . G. Tests of Simple Effects . . . . . . . . . . 69 72 76 82 82 83 87 9O 91 93 Table l. 2. 3. 9. 10. ll. 12. LIST OF TABLES Inter Rater Reliabilities . . . . . . . . . . Mean Category Usage and Standard Deviation for the 25 Scoring Categories Utilized . . Summary of Significant Multivariate Results Reflecting Significant Univariate Comparisons Not Qualified by Higher Order EffeCtS o o o o o o o o o o o o o 0 Mean Factor IV Responses Emitted in Situation I as a Function of Treatment I . . . . . . Mean Factor II Responses by Males and Females in Situation II as a Function of Treatment Mean Factor III Responses Within Conditions of Situation Order as a Function of Tre atment I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Mean Factor V Responses Within Conditions of Sex as a Function of Treatment I . . . . . Mean Factor VII Responses Within Situation I as a Function of Treatment I . . . . . . . Mean Factor VII Responses Within Situation II as a Function of Treatment I . . . . . . . Mean Factor II Responses Within Response I as a Function of Treatment I . . . . . . . Mean Factor III Responses Within Response I as a Function of Treatment I . . . . . . . Mean Factor VII Responses Within Response I as a Function of Treatment I . . . . . . vi Page 28 30 38 39 4o 41 42 43 43 44 45 45 Table Page 13. Mean Factor I Responses for Response I in Situation I Within Conditions of Situation Order as a Function of Treatment I . . . . . 46 14. Mean Factor I Responses for Response I in Situation II Within Conditions of Situation Order as a Function of Treatmentl0000000000000... 47 15. Mean Factor IV Responses for Response I Within Conditions of Situation Order for Treatment I . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48 16. Mean Factor V Responses for Response I Within Conditions of Situation Order for Treatment I . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 17. Mean Factor I Responses for Response 2 IEli'I'.;h..‘I.1'l Treatment 2 o o o o o o o o o o o o o 50 18. Mean Factor II Responses for Response 2 Within Treatment 2 o o o o o o o o o o o o 0 50 19. Mean Factor III Responses Within Response 2 for Treatment 2 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 20. Mean Factor I Responses in Situation I Within Conditions of Situation Order for Treatment 2 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 21. Mean Factor I Responses in Situation II Within Conditions of Situation Order forTreatmentZ00000000000000 52 vii INTRODUCTION In each moment, followed by another, we experience our own existence through our consciousness. From the base of ‘this constant, individuals acknowledge the behavior and (experience that constitutes their lives to different degrees and with varying intensities. Some find their moments more differentiated and wholly integrated into their point of experiencing than others, and each finds more or less of happiness-sadness, meaningfulness, and companionship. Early learning and childhood experiences no doubt play an important part in understanding the determinants of these differently labeled intrapersonal experiences and interpersonal behaviors. These critical and significant learning experiences of child- hood are most likely to occur in parent-child encounters. Acknowledging this important role early parent- C=hild interaction plays in psychological development, 62 ducation in child-care—giving may offer us leverage in enhancing mental health on a national scope. In the past Our society has reflected the attitude that innate guidance Provided by "maternal instinct" and such self-satisfying adult statements as "That's the way I was raised and it dlL dn't hurt me," would provide the necessary guidelines for effective parenting. Indeed, that "professionals" perpetuated this attitude is exemplified by Baldwin, Kalhorn, and Breece (1945) who concluded that "...emotionally mature parents will be able to derive for themselves satisfactory methods for handling children." A softening of this attitude of lay responsibility and expertise for parenting is demonstrated in the growing child rearing literature. Clinical child psychologists such as Axline, Moustakas, Ginott, and Gordon, have presented to therapists and parents attitudes and "modes of encountering" from a Rogerian theoretical base. The need exists, however, for behavioral science to validate and refine the directives put forth and provide data furthering our understanding of the paramenters of sensitive and effective adult child-care-giving. Among the many researchers responding to this need, Stollak, Scholom, Kallman, and Saturansky (1973) investigated the responses of undergraduates to problem situations with children. The authors found that written adult responses to the projective problem situation demonstrated a general lack of sensitivity to child needs, conflicts, and impulses as determined by scoring categories assumed indicative of effective responding designed by the authors, and usage of ineffective/destructive categories taken from Gordon (1970). Focusing on "problem ownership" (Gordon, 1970) when the adult's needs were the ones primarily being thwarted in the problem situation, the Ss' responses were both more ineffective and destructive than when the confrontation centered around primarily the child's needs. In this latter case §s did focus their communications significantly more often on the child's feelings and how he or she can express them. In a further study, Kallman and Stollak (1974) presented similar hypothetical need arousing situations to both mothers and their first, third or fifth grade children. The written responses of the parents and verbal responses of their children were coded along categories similar to the previous research. The average mean usage for the summation of the "ineffective" and "effective" responses over the projective situations was virtually identical for college adults and mothers. For both groups there was noticeably less usage of "effective" than "ineffective" responses overall, and significantly more "ineffective" responses were again found for parental need arousing situations. Thus, college students and mothers rarely focused their messages to children on their own needs or the needs of the children in situations that strongly involved feelings, needs, and wishes. The most frequently reported "ineffective" responses for both college adults and mothers were the use of logical persuasion, questioning, providing answers, and ordering. Following from this format and the study of "problem ownership," the purpose of the present study was to provide additional information relevant to how adults respond to varying types of child communications. More specifically, the present research examined the extent to which types of adult responses and their communication of acceptance and rejection are affected by child communication sequences involving loving, neutral, and/or hostile messages. Little information is available in the important area of hgw_adults respond to children in intimate encounters. I believe, and a number of theorists (cited below) assert, that it is in these especially critical moments of adult-child interaction where adult messages that significantly affect the child's feelings toward himself and others occur. Rather than obtaining thought-out written responses to hypothetical situations, data were more spontaneous Spoken responses to tape recorded child scenarios which simulated real life encounters. Becker (1964) states, "In many instances it is possible that making the parent more aware of how his or her behavior is having an impact on the child, can motivate a change in the parent's handling of the child (p 208)." In this regard, it was hOped that the results obtained would provide informa- tion concerning the nature of adult responses to children that can be utilized by parents and child rearing educators in deve10ping more effective and sensitive interaction with children. LITERATURE REVIEW Accgptance One of the most basic aims of this study was to provide information concerning the nature of adult acceptance. Al- though it has been theorized to be a fundamental and necessary parent-child construct, the conditions under which it is expressed have not been investigated through the study of adult responses to varying types of child communication. Data from the present study provides insight into whgg and hgw acceptance and rejection is elicited and withheld. In particular, information was gathered concerning where in the communication process do rejecting adult responses occur that may lead to anxiety, guilt, escape-avoidance mechanisms, and constricted expression and awareness for the child. Carl Rogers (1961) defined unconditional positive regard as valuing the person irrespective of the differential values which one might place on his specific behaviors. This is the concept of accepting the whole person. Rogers theorized that when the self experiences of the young child are discriminated by significant others as being more or less worthy of positive regard, then self-regard becomes selective - that is, you have conditions of worth. For Rogers, this is the basic estrangement of man, "...for the 5 sake of preserving the positive regard of others the child has now come to falsify some of the values he experiences and to perceive them only in terms based upon their value to others." The child's concept of self is based on a distorted symbolization where the child learns "I perceive this behavior as unsatisfying" rather than as parental attributes. His/her feelings get mixed up and values that the child attaches to his/her experience become divorced from his/her own organismic functioning and, in this manner, the capacity for self-differentiation is impaired. There is a history of empirical support for this funda- mental weighting of an acceptance dimension. Parental acceptance has been correlated with a wide set of child attributes such as self-acceptance, adjustment, peer accept- ance, positive affective orientation, and cooperation (Medinus & Curtis, 1963; Symonds, 1939; Sommers, 1952; Hoffman, 1963; Newirth, 1971). Acceptance has also been studied as a global process occuring in interaction with other dimensions. Coopersmith (1967) concluded that unconditional parental acceptance is a necessary but not sufficient condition for high self esteem. Similarly, Baumrind (1967) found high parental warmth and acceptance to be one of several dimensions necessary for the pre-school aged child's display of behaviors indicative of instrumental competence. Becker (1964) noted in his review of parent discipline that the same behavior has different effects on the child depending on the context of warmth-hostility in which it occurred. The present study included a global measure of acceptance/rejection that complemented the specific behavioral and content categories that were utilized. Specific and Global Measurement of Adult-Child Communication One purpose of this study was to provide descriptive :information about how adults respond to various types of (sommunications that children commonly express in their day -1:o day lives. Information needs to be gathered about what (floss occur in adult-child encounters before we can teach ‘nlhat should occur. Eschewing specificity, most parent-child ssstudies utilize global dimensions of interaction variables ssruch as warmth-hostility, restrictive-permissive, and eagxxious emotional involvement vs. calm detachment (Becker, 21.964). Observational studies such as Baumrind (1967) cziluster discrete behaviors into broad dimensions such as "'self control" or "maturity demands." Thus, general factors ‘tleat bear some empirical relationship to each other have been <5L<3veloped but they tend to obscure the specificity of the C: ontent of exchange. Syntonic with the focus on measurement of specific a—<3L1m1t responding in this study psychological speculation has 'bHHH.I *mmdm.| Hemo. dmmo.l mHOH.I ome.l NSHO. mmmo. m moeo¢mH.I bmmo.l Omoo. *ebom. mmoa. omma. mbma.l M¢NN. *mmflr. *mmmw. ommm. vmma.l HHOm. H moeoxm Or-INM'd' Hr-ir-ir-ir-i r-INMd'LOKOL‘COm .m<> .m¢> .m<> .m<> .m¢> .m<> .m¢> .m¢> .m¢> .m¢> .m«> .m<> .m4> HNrfid'mKOPCDm mGZHQm. mmmw. mwbm. have. mHNm. when. mmmm. Hmwm. . 220 0 HOOm. tho. Hmdb. #mm0.I *Hm¢r. mH¢H.I mOHN.I *mmmm.l NmH0.I ommo. *mmmm. abHH.I HmHo.I HHmO. m m080<@ mmmv. mbho. boon. ¢MHH. ammo.l mmNH.I wmhl. bmmo.l m>m0.I HM0H.I mmmo.n HmN0.I vao. hmm0.I m MOBUm¢0.l mwwo. mNMH.I mmmm.l 0m¢0.l mbmo.l mmmo.l mmmH.I mMH0.I bemo. m moaogm NHOH. NHOH. mme. mmaH. owOH.I ammo.l *mOmm.I mmmm.| bwwH. NmOO. mmmo. Hmmo.u ¢Hm0. ¢¢m0.I H m090<@ .>.m .200 .m¢> .momm .040H .Hm mm ¢N mm mm HN 0N mH 0H NH 0H mH .m<> .m¢> .m<> .m¢> .m<> .m¢> .m¢> .m<> .m¢> .m¢> .m<> mm hm mm mm em mm mm Hm 0N mH mH PH 0H mH A.e.psoov a aHazmaaa APPENDIX G TESTS OF SIMPLE EFFECTS APPENDIX G TESTS OF SIMPLE EFFECTS Sex X Treatment 1 X Situation. The multivariate analysis of variance reflected a significant (p.¢=.0260) Sex X Treatment, X Situation interaction which was associ- ated with a significant univariate effect for Factor II (p<.0003). Tests of simple effects explored this inter- action further. The data were initially divided into each condition of Situation and a two-way simple anova for Sex X Treatment 1 was performed. Looking within Situation 1 first, a Treatment 1 main effect was established (F = 10.96*). The reader is referred to the main body of the text where individual comparisons between levels of the Treatment 1 effect are completed. Looking within Situation II next, a significant Treatment 1 X Sex interaction was obtained (3 = ll.88*). This interaction was further broken down by dividing the data into each condition of Sex. The results of this anova revealed a nonsignificant Factor II usage for females (F = 2.78); but a significant usage for mates (F = 17.28*). *significant at .01 level 93 94 No further analyses were then appropriate for female responding, and the reader is referred to the text where individual comparisons between Treatment 1 conditions for males are completed. Sex X Treatment 1 X Situation Order Interactiop. The multivariate analysis of variance revealed a Sex X Treatment 1 X Situation Order interaction (p 4:.0345) which was associated with a significant univariate effect for Factor III (p<: .0005.) This complex interaction was explored further via simple effects tests. The data were first divided into each condition of Situation Order and a two-way simple effects anova for Sex X Treatment 1 was performed. Looking within Situation I Given First, a Treatment 1 main effect was obtained (F = 51.75*). A follow-up explanation of individual differences within levels of Treatment 1 is presented in the text. Within Situation II Given First, a Treatment 1 main effect was again established (F = 52.05*). This significant effect is further investigated in the main body of the results. Sex X Treatment 1 X Response. The multivariate analy- sis reflected a significant Sex X Treatment 1 X Response interaction (p4:,.0194) which was associated with a signifi- cant univariate effect for Factor V (p<:1.0002). This complex interaction was investigated further via simple effects analysis. 95 The data were first divided into each level of Response and a two-way simple effects anova for Sex X Treatment 1 was performed. Within response 1, a significant Sex X Treatment 1 interaction was obtained (2 = 9.64*). The data from this interaction were then divided into each condition of Sex. The two-way anovas for sex revealed a significant Treatment 1 effect for males (3 = 4.19). No further analyses were then appropriate for females, and individual comparisons between Treatment 1 conditions for males are completed in the text. No significant results were obtained within Response 2 as noted in the text. Sax_X Treatment 1 X Treatment 2 X Situation X Situation Qpaap. The multivariate analyses of variance revealed a significant Sex X Treatment 1 X Treatment 2 X Situation X Situation Order interaction (pa<1.0237) which was associated with a univariate effect for Factor VII (p< .01). This complex interaction was explored further via simple effects analysis. The data were first divided into each condition of Situation and a four-way simple effects anova was then computed. Looking within Situation I, the only significant result was a Treatment 1 X Situation Order interaction (E = 5.41*). This result was investigated further by performing a two-way anova within each condition of Situation Order. There tests reflected a significant Treatment I effect for both Situation I Given First (34 10.79) and for Situation 96 II Given First (F = 29.33*). The reader is referred to the main body of the text where further investigation of within Treatment 1 differences are given. Looking within Situation II, no significant effects are obtained as reported in the text. Treatment 1 X Response. The multivariate analysis of varience reflected a Treatment 1 X Response interaction (p¢:..OOOl) which was associated with three unqualified uni- variate effects: Factor II (p .0001), Factor III (p4 .0001*) and Factor VII (pg: .0001). These three interactions were each explored further via simple effects analysis. For all three univariate effects, the data were divided into each condition of Response and a two-way anova was performed. Within Response 1, a Treatment 1 main effect was found for Factor 11 (F 47.15*), Factor III (2.: 185.82*) and Factor VII (F l52.58*). The reader is referred to the main body of the text where direct comparisons between Treatment 1 levels are presented. No significant results were obtained within Response 2 as noted in the text. Tpeatment l X Sitpation Order X Situation X Response. The multivariate analysis of variance revealed a significant Treatment 1 X Situation Order X Situation X Response inter- action (p4c..0001) which was associated with three unqualified univariate effects: Factor I (p4: .0001*), Factor IV (pp; .0080), and Factor v (p4 .0048). These three univariate effects were explored further via tests of simple effects. 97 For Factor I, the data were initially divided into levels of Response. A three-way Treatment 1 X Situation Order X Situation anova was performed and found significant (F = 21.98*). This result was then divided within each level of Situation Order. This two-way anova found a significant Treatment 1 effect for both Situationl (F = 6.15*) and Situation II (E = 72.56*) Given First. The reader is referred to the main text, where further investigation of the Treatment I effects are presented. Within Situation II, a two-way anova was again performed for the Treatment 1 X Situation Order interaction. This interaction was found significant (F = ll.46*) and the data were further investigated within each condition of Situation Order. This two-way anova again reflected a significant Treatment 1 effect for both Situation I (F = 48.59*) and Situation II (F = 6.9*) Given First. There Treatment 1 effects are further investigated in the main body of the text. No significant results are obtained for Response 2, as noted in the text. For Factor IV, the data were divided into each level of Response and a three-way anova was performed. Within Response 1, a significant Treatment 1 X Situation Order X Situation interaction (F = 7.67*) was obtained. This inter- action was further broken down by looking within each Situation condition. No significant results were obtained 98 for the Treatment 1 X Situation Order interaction for either Situation I (g = 3.04) a Situation II (a = 1.73). No further analyses are appropriate and any further statements about Treatment 1 cannot be made appropriately, as noted in the text. Within response 2, again, no significant results were obtained. For Factor V, the data again were initially divided into each level of Response, and a three-way anova was performed. Within Response 1, the Treatment 1 X Situation Order X Situation interaction was significant (F = 5.55*). This result was further broken down by looking within each condition of Situation. Looking within Situation I, a Treatment 1 main effect was found (3 = 8.16*). Within Situation II, a Treatment 1 main effect was also found (F = 10.78*). The reader is referred to the text where individual comparisons between levels of Treatment 1 are presented for both Situations Conditions. No significant results were obtained for Response 2 as noted in the text. Treatment 2 X Response. The multivariate analysis of variance reflected a Treatment 2X Response interaction (p4. .0001) which was associated with three univariate effects: Factor I (p4 .0001), Factor II (p4 .0001), and Factor III (p<: .0001). Each of these results was investigated further via simple effects analysis. 99 For all three Factors, the data initially were divided into each Response condition and a two-way anova was per- formed. Looking within Response 1, no significant Treatment 2 effects were found, as noted in the text. Within Response 2, a Treatment 2 main effect was found for Factor I (F_= 20.92*), Factor II (F = 66.0*) and Factor III (F= l82.6*). The reader is referred to the main body of the text, where individual comparisons between each Treatment 2 level for all three Factors is presented. Treatment 2 X Situation Order X Situation. The multi- variate analysis of variance reflected a Treatment 2 X Situation Order it Situation interaction (2‘: .0001) which was associated with a univariate effect for Factor I (p4 .0001). This interaction effect was explored further via simple effects analysis. The data initially were divided into each condition of Situation and a two-way anova computed. Within Situation I, this test found the Treatment 2 X Situation Order inter- action significant (F = 6.99*). This result was further broken down by looking at the data within each level of Situation Order. No Treatment 2 effect was found for Situation I Given First (F41), but a significant effect for Situation II Given First was found (F = l3.97*). The reader is referred to the text where further investigation of this Treatment 2 effect is presented. lOO Within Situation II, the two-way anova again found the Treatment 2 X Situation Order interaction significant (F = 4.86*). This result was further investigated by dividing the data into each condition of Situation Order. A Treatment 2 effect was established for Situation I Given First (F = ll.72*), but not for Situation II Given First CF41 l). The reader is referred to the text where the significant interactions are explored further. 31293 31 mum/Hum;nmummy!)111114131144u