A STUDY N THE RELIAB'TLTTY 0F HYPNOTSC AGE-REGRESSION AS A TOOL IN THE ASSESSMENT OF FUNDAMENTAL MOTOR SKiLLS Thesis for the Degree of M. A. MICHTGAN STATE UNIVERSTTY LOIS, CHARLOTTE RAPPOLT 1969 - paw-1m“ r alumna IY "ME & SNNS' BLUUK BINDERY INC. | RA? ‘ BINDERS ' "‘ " "TCII‘I' : Il;\\ Ag:- JD. .2 ABSTRACT A STUDY IN THE RELIABILITY OF‘KZTHOTIC AGE-REGRISSSIOIT AS A TOOL IN TIT?) ASSESSI-IIJITT OF MITTDAIIEITTAL IIOTOR SKILLS By Lois Charlotte Rappolt The purpose of this study was to determine if the technique of hypnotic age-regression can be used to facilitate the study of motor development. If adults under hypnosis could reproduce motor patterns of younger years, disadvantages of longitudinal motor performance studies could be avoided and there would be many new opportunities for research in motor development. The specific objects of the experiment were: 1) to determine if, through hypnotic age-regression, adults can perform at the motor ability level of an earlier age (for this study, ive years), and 2) to investigate the degree of accuracy to which an adult in a hypnotized state can reproduce motor patterns elicited under hypnosis during a previous interval of time. The investigator utilized ten subjects in each of three groups as follows: 1) a control group consisting of five-year-old girls, 2) an experimental group Lois Charlotte Rappolt consisting of hypnotized adult females, regressed to five years of age, and 3) a simulator group consisting of adult females who pretended to be hypnotically regressed to the age of five years. Basic motor developmental tests (one-hand ball catch, high jumping, and tracing skills) and motion pictures were used to determine the validity of performance. The tests were administered on two occasions by an instructor unfamiliar with the experimental design and the identity and role of the subjects. Chi square tests for k independent samples were used to determine if observed disparities among groups occurred by chance or if changes in performance might be attributed to factors other than chance. No significant differences among the three groups were revealed in the pass-fail results or reasons for missing for the high jumps, the circle trace and the maze trace. It is concluded that the hypnotized subjects, age—regressed to five years, can perform at the level of a five-year-old child for these skills. However, essentially. the same level of performance can be produced by simulators who pretend to be five years old. Lois Charlotte Rappolt There was a significant difference between the performance of the children and the adults for the one-hand catch. This condition is attributed to adult p ysica1.maturation, specifically the hand size. Skill performance of hypnotized, age-regressed subjects and of adults simulating the hypnotized, age-regressed state was consistent from trial to trial. The children, however, displayed a variety of inconsistencies in motor skills from.one testing session to the next. Subjective film analysis of the gross motor skills revealed that the performance of the experimental group contained.more characteristics of five-year-old children than the performance of the simulator group. A STUDY IN THE RELIABILITY OF HYFNOTIC AGEeREGRESSION AS A TOOL IN THE ASSESSI-IENT OF F'UT‘TDAI-TENTAL MOTOR SKILLS BY Lois Charlotte Rappolt A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of I-TASTER OF ARTS Department oleealth, Physical Education and Recreation 1969 ACKNOULEDGEHERTS Grateful acknowledgement is made to the author's adviser, Dr. Vernal Seefeldt, for his invaluable help and guidance. The writer wishes to thank Dr. Arthur H. Steinhaus and Dr. Joseph Reyher for their inspiration and encouragement in the area of hypnosis. The writer also wishes to express her gratitude and sincere appreciation to her subjects for their dedication, and to Willoughby George Sheena, Jr. for his oriticism.of the manuscript. .ii I. INTRODUCTION . . e e e 0 II. REVISE}! OF' TIL-'3 LITLLATURE III. PROCEDURES . . e . . . 0 IV. RESULTS. . . . . . . . . v. sugary, CONCLUSIONS AITD BIBLIOGRAPI‘FJO o e e e e e e e e 0 75'0"" 7“ Ai¢jimIC~JJO e e e e e e e e e e 0 Appendix 7 A. Hypnotic Induction fiethod. B. Stanford Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibili C. Post-hypnotic Suggestions for Bleep. Do IIIEh Jump. 0 o o e o e e E. One-hand Catch . . . . . F. Circle Trace . . . . . . G. Maze Trace . . . . . . . iii 10 15 33 38 Table 2. 3. LIST OF TABLES Page Chi Square Values for Pass-Fail Differences Among Groups for Gross and Fine Motor Skills. . . . . . . . . . 16 Group Comparisons in the High Jump With the Reasons for Failing Presented inPerOentaEOSeeeeeeeeeeeee 19 Group Comparisons in the OneéHand Catch With the Reasons for Failing Presented in Percentages . . . . . . . . 20 Group Comparisons in the Circle Trace Test with the Reasons for Failing Presented in Percentages . . . . . . . . 22 Chi Square Values for all Groups Indicating the Pass-Fail Changes from Trial One to Trial Two . . . . . . . . , 23 Chi Square Values for all Groups Indicating the Reasons for Failure Between Trials for Each Skill. . . . . . 2h CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Physical maturation of the child is accompanied by increased efficiency in such fundamental motor patterns' as crawling, walking, and jumping. Although the sequential development of some of these skills has been determined, little is known about effects of training, genetic development and learning situations on motor activity. To attain knowledge in these areas, researchers have made longitudinal studies of groups of children. However, this approach has several disadvantages: 1) Such studies require a long period of time to reach fruition, normally a minimum of m. years, 2) they usually involve large numbers of subjects (to offset absenteeism and attrition) and 3) the results can be affected by external factors to which subjects may be exposed during the course of a project. Also, little is known about hypnotic age-regression, a.method of recall through reliving an earlier time (19, p. 121). Hypnosis itself has had a renaissance in scientific study and many investigators are using it to study the psychological, physical and physiological implications of performance (13,21,22,26, 27,31,38ah5). However, there is little evidence pertaining to the use of hypnotic age-regression as a tool in the study of motor performance. The purpose of this study was to determine if the technique of hypnotic age-regression can be used to facilitate the study of motor development. Can age-regression, if used with a small group of subjects who execute basic skills over a short period of time, secure reliable and valid results and thus avoid the disadvantages of a longitudinal study? If adults under hypnosis can reproduce motor patterns of younger years, there would be many new opportunities for research in motor development. These Opportunities might include observation of changes in motor skills, study of the sequence of skills, evaluation of teaching techniques, and possible improvement of present skills through re-learning under simulated conditions of childhood. It is relatively easy to persuade a hypnotized adult to assume the verbal role of a child. However, hypnotists have noted that motor behavior is not entirely consistent with the chronological age assumed by a subject (9, p. 121). The validity and reliability of such an assumed role is the focus of this study. PROBLEM The purpose of this experiment was to assess the technique of age-regression as a research tool in the performance of gross and fine motor patterns commonly learned in childhood. By examining hypnotized subjects, the researcher attempted to determine: 1) if an adult can perform Specific motor patterns at the motor ability level of the age in question (five years), and 2) if a hypnotized subject can repeat such skills at the same level of performance in successive sessions. Performance of gross motor skills (high jump, ball bounce and one~hand catch) was recorded on film, while performance of fine motor skills (circle and maze traces) was evaluated on the basis of the paper and pencil record produced by the subject. Thirty-five female students from.Michigan State University physical education activity classes volunteered for the experiment. After several group induction sessions, twelve subjects qualified as somnambulists as defined by the Stanford.Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Fbrm A (which measures achieved depth characteristics portrayed by one who is in hypnotic sleep). Ten additional students from physical education activity classes volunteered to serve as simulators. Both groups were informed that the eXperiment would include the performance of gross and fine motor skills while in a state of chronological age-regression, but neither group knew'what age nor which skills would be examined. A third (control) group consisted of ten five-year-old females who performed the same skills as those who were hypnotized and those who simulated a hypnotic condition. In two identical sessions, all subjects in each group performed two gross and two fine motor skills. Individually, each five-year-old, each subject in the experimental group (regressed to her fifth birthday), and each subject in the simulator group (simulating five years of age) executed a jump, bounce and one-hand catch and two maze tracing skills. Through analysis of test results, films and test sheets, data were examined for: l) reliability, by comparing two performances of each subject, and 2) production of the regressed skill, through comparison of the performance of the skill to the established levels of performance of five-year-old children. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF 'IHE LITERATURE Motor developmental sequence. As early as the 1930's, researchers explored.many aspects of motor skill development of children. Maturation appears to be one of the predominant factors influencing the development of proficiency in gross and fine motor skills. Shirley (35, p.95) defined.maturation as "the sum total of the growth process." She concluded that progress in motor control comes from.this process. In an analysis of walking, fer example, it can be seen that the developmental control of the head and neck region, shoulders, trunk, and legs must have occurred before the final stage of walking is reached. In a study of throwing patterns, Wild (uh) described a developmental sequence in which the immature throw of the early years is integrated into a more complex pattern as one grows older. 'With the greater range of movement at the advanced age, the throw becomes more efficient. Similarly, in the hurdle jump diagnostic test, Cowan (h, p. 121) verified that "maturation is a determining factor in the height of the jump." He found that children with advanced chronological age performed better than children of younger ages irrespective of their heights or weights. In reviewing Deach's study (5) of the genetic development of motor skills of throwing, striking, kicking and bouncing, Rarick (32. p. 25) concluded that: a. Motor control passes from gross movements to fine coordinations and from total body action to individuation of parts. b. Patterns of performance increase in complexity with increased maturity, with elements of each pattern precise enough to be clearly defined. 0. There is a gradual fusion of new elements with those of old established patterns, so that there is an increased ability to perform according to adult standards. Children normally pass through the developmental sequences in the same order, with the behavioral pattern usually following the structural change. Although practice, training, and other environmental influences affect the development or acquisition of a skill, Hicks (19) noted that structural maturation was the most significant factor. Commenting on neurological research, which has revealed that some individuals may have more neurons than others, Harrison (17) suggested the possibility of more developed motor patterns at different stages of maturation. She also posed the possibility that some dendrities have the potential for optimum development at specific periods in the life span of an organism, Tests such as the Lincoln-Oseretsky I-Iotor Development Scale (37), The Denver Developmental Screening Test (11), and The Stott Tests of Motor Impairment (2h) provide standards for assessment of motor development in the early school years. In his analysis of the Stott Tests, Keogh (2h) provided current, specific standards which indicate that skill level improves as the total growth process continues. Hypnotic age-regression. After a period of quiesence, scientific interest in hypnosis was revived in the 1930's. Both the British Medical Association and the American Medical Association later supported the teaching of hypnosis (1950's) and it became recognized as a useful tool in both the experimental and clinical fields(2o), Little experimental research has been attempted- in the area of'hypnotic age-regression and motor skill. Hypnosis has been used in studies of strength (21,26,38) and other physiological phenomena (2,22,26,27,30,h5) but use of hypnotic age-regression has been limited to studies of intelligence. (lh,25,29,31,3h,h0), There are two general attitudes toward age-regression: 1) "it is a product of an altered level of ego-functioning, made possible by the subject's surrender of his control over his ego processes to the (Hf. NW)" ‘ I. - ' I— ‘M “- rm :.~.-'.A."L‘ up. .. .‘.~ I I ,’\ . "“¥[qv\3!"r m5! LANDING (‘fiff‘hllf‘ . . -. ‘Y ' W mm! 4.23.323 hypnotist, and 2) regression is not a true return to an earlier level of functioning but rather the product of the subject playing the role appropriate to the ‘ suggested age" (1h, p.112). These opposing views resulted from several studies: The writers report that a full role-enactment of earlier experience does not occur (1h,20,25,29). It appears that there is regression as tested by various psychometric measures, "but outcomes tend to correspond to those of an age somewhat more mature than that to which the subject is presumably regressed" (20, p.172). Many physiological phenomena can be produced by an unhypnctized subject who is asked to imagine or simulate a particular task (2). Barber (2) contends that hypnotic age-regression is not "real" and that characteristics portrayed by the subject are often not characteristic of most children, However, in two seperate investigations, a psycho-phsysiological reorganization in the Babinski sign (dorsiflexion or upward extension of the large toe) was produced through age-regression (13,h0). Known only to be present in infants, this phenomenon would seem.to represent authentic age-regression (33). Other tests reported that subjects who were regressed to the second-grade level changed their handwriting to print (20). In cases where the subject was judged to be in a good regressed state (hypnotist judged on the basis of answers to various questions) the correlation of handwriting with depth of regression was .78 for the fifth-grade regression and .83 for the second-grade level. ‘ Summary. Research indicates that motor patterns in children:move in sequence from.birth to maturity. Proficiency in motor skills improves with advancing age until the mature pattern is attained. At each stage of development, former patterns are performed more efficiently and new’skills emerge. The accuracy to which a.hypnotica11y age-regressed adult can reproduce the motor patterns of an earlier age has not been determined. To the knowledge of this writer no research.has been reported in this area. It is the purpose of this study, therefore, to answer two questions: 1) can the motor characteristics of a five-year-old dhild be produced in a hypnotically age-regressed adult? and 2) to what degree of accuracy can an adult in a hypnotized state reproduce the motor patterns elicited under hypnosis at an earlier point in time? CHARTER III PROCEDURES Thirty-five female students, ranging in age from eighteen to twenty years and enrolled in Michigan State University physical education activity classes, volunteered for the experiment. After one group induction session during which the students were hypnotized by the visual-fixation and verbal suggestion of sleep method (see Appendix A), twenty subjects were asked to return (on the basis of observed hypnotic susceptibility) for two additional sessions. .During the latter sessions, the items from the Stanford.Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Form A (20, p.83)‘were utilized to determine the depth of hypnotic sleep (see Appendix B). During these sessions, twelve subjects passed the depth tests, which classified them as somnambulists, and were given a post-hypnotic suggestion for rapid induction. (see Appendix C). A private meeting with each of the twelve subjects and a group session with all subjects were held to introduce the subjects to age-regression and to maintain rapport between the operator and the subjects. The volunteers were informed that the experiment would include age-regression and physical activity, but knowledge concerning the ages and types of 10 (11 activity was withheld. In the first session, involving regression to five years of age, each subject wrote her name and age, drew a picture with a crayon, experienced a smell hallucination, balanced on one foot, walked backwards, and caught a bounced ball with two hands. In the second practice session, the hypnotized subjects were regressed to three years of age and attempted to jump in place, balance on one foot, recognize three colors, and draw with crayons. At this point, two of the twelve subjects withdrew frem the experhment for personal reasons. The remaining ten subjects will hereafter be referred to as the "expertmental" group. .The experimental group was compared to a ."simulator” group and to a "control" group in terms of quantitative and qualitative characteristics of motor skills. Ten female physical education students (eighteen to twenty years of age) frem.Hichigan State University, who were not volunteers for the hypnosis experiment agreed to act as simulators. The simulators were informed that they would imitate young children and perform certain physical skills. However, as with the experimental group, the exact age and skills were not mentioned. In order to become familiar with some characteristics of hypnotic behavior (but not with the 12 experimental tasks), subjects in the simulator group were permitted to observe one session during which the subjects in the experimental group were hypnotized. The reasons for formation of the simulator group were: 1) to determine if age-appropriate behavior could be imitated by adults who had not been hypnotized and age-regressed, and 2) to eliminate possible bias on the part of the photographer and the person administering the test. The control group was composed of ten female children ranging in age from.5.0 to 6.0 years. These subjects were randomly selected from a list of participants in a longitudinal motor performance study in progress at Michigan State University. The skills of one-hand catch, jumping over various heights, and maze traces had not been introduced in the longitudinal study and there is no reason to believe that the ability level of the group differed from.that of other children within the age described above. All individuals in the experimental group (regressed to the morning of their fifth birthday), in the simulator group (imitating five years of age), and in the control group were filmed while performing two gross motor skills. The gross motor skills were modifications of the Stott high jump and the Stott one-hand catch of a bounced tennis ball (see Appendices D and E). In addition, each subject executed two fine motor skills, 13 the Stott circle and the Stott maze trace (see Appendices F and G). These tests were chosen because: 1) they permitted reliable scoring, 2) hey involved no danger to the participants, 3) they did not require elaborate test material, and b) they had significant positive correlation with age (h,2h). The age of five was chosen because it was the earliest age for which standard tests with a range of resulting data were available (2h). At the first testing session, the adult subjects were introduced to an instructor who was unaware of the purpose or the exact nature of the experiment. The instructor was under the impression that all the adults in the group were hypnotized and age-regressed. (In actuality, simulators were placed randomly among the age-regressed subjects).. The instructor proceeded to explain and/or demonstrate (as required) the tests to each subject individually, before the subject executed a skill (in front of the camera). Directions were identical for each subject. The tracing tests were also administered by the instructor. All skills were retested fourteen days later, under the same environmental conditions (instructor, cameramen, rooms, equipment, time of day). All tests were scored as directed and recorded at the time of performance. 1h he same testing procedures were used for the five-year-old children, except that they were tested on different days. In this manner, the adults were not tempted to imitate the children. Analysis of the test results included performance comparisons of the five-year-old children, age-regressed adults, and subjects simulating the performance of children at five years of age. Filmed characteristics such as preparation for take-offs, landings, and manner of catching and bouncing were noted. (Criteria for judging are in test descriptions, Appendices D,E,F5G). The researcher also compared the filmed and written results of the successive testing sessions. The chi square test was the statistical tool used to evaluate the results of this experiment. Using this method, results obtained were compared to results expected on the basis of the hypothesis that the hypnotized (experimental) group would not differ from the five-year-old children (control group). Also, a comparison was made between the expected and observed frequencies for each trial of each group. CHAPTER _IV RESULTS The purpose of this study was to determine: 1) if the motor characteristics of a five—year-old child can be produced in a hypnotically age-regressed adult, and 2) to what degree of accuracy an adult in a hypnotized state can reproduce the motor patterns elicited under hypnosis at an earlier period in time. Basic motor development tests were used as instruments to determine the validity of performance. Iovement characteristics were noted from a subjective film.analysis of skills. Chi square techniques for k independent samples (36) were employed to determine if group differences existed between test items and between the two trials. With similar methods, the possible difference between each group performance for each trial was investigated. The data from each of two trials consisted of pass-fail scores and categorized reasons for failure of the test items during the performance. Table 1 shows the chi square values for the pass-fail differences among groups for gross and fine motor skills. It should be noted that in ten of twelve trials there was no significant difference in the performance level of the 15 16 three groups. Only the one-hand catch with chi square values of 21.818 and 15.833 indicate a significant difference among groups for both trials. Chi Square Values for Pass-Pail Differences Among Groups For Gross and Fine Hotor Skills A‘ ww— , ----~ Skill Test df Chi Square Values Chi Square Values for Trial One for Trial Two Knee High Jump h .375 3.286 12" High Jump h 2.hh0 1.292 16" high Jump h 5.369 5.695 One-Hand Catch 2 21.818 15.833 Circle Trace 2 5.700 2.857 Haze Trace 2 2.222 1.920 When df'l 2, Chi square value at .05 must equal or exceed 5.99 for significance. when df‘B h, hi square value at .05 must equal or exceed 9.h9 for significance. The table of critical values of chi square (36) indicates that a value of 5.99 or greater is required to be significant at the .05 level dfll2, or 9.h9 or greater to be significant at the .05 level with df=h. Similar t: 17 Keogh's earlier study (2h), results of the one—hand catch recorded for the five-year-old children (control group) show that 80% failed trial one while 90% failed trial two. Results of the experimental and simulator groups reveal that no one failed trial one while in trial two, 10:3 of the simulator and 2073 of the hypnotized (experimental) groups failed. There were no significant differences in the pass-fail scores among all groups for each of the three jumps or the tracing skills tests. Less than one-half of the five-year-olds passed the circle trace (in both trials) and although more than 80;: of the simulator and 70% of the hypnotized groups passed both trials, this level of achievement was not sufficiently high to obtain significant differences for this test. Chi square values of 2.222 and 1.92 indicate no significance at the .05 level for differences among groups in the maze tracing test. More than 70% of all three groups failed both trials. Reasons for misses on all of the tests were examined to determine if true motor patterns were produced. Fbr the high jump, misses were recorded for: l) a one-foot take-off, 2) a one-foot landing, 3) knocking the bar off the standard on the way up, h) knocking the bar off the standard on the way down, and 5) miscellaneous Instructional Resources Center 133 ERICKSON HALL MlCHKSAN STATE UNIVERSITY m LANSING. MICHIGAN 48823 18 reasons. Ball catch misses were noted for: l) catching the ball with two hands, 2) dropping the ball, and 3) using the body to assist in securing the ball. Uistahes on the tracing skills included: 1) pencil lift-off, 2) drawing the line beyond the bounds of the maze, 3) not following the maze correctly. A category of "no misses" was included for each test. Fbr six degrees of freedom, the table of critical values of chi square (36) shows that a value of 12.59 or greater is significant at the 5% level. For both trials of the three high jump tests, the chi square value for differences between groups did not reach significance. Table 2 indicates categories of misses and percentages of failure patterns for each group, as well as chi square values. More than 50% of the simulator and hypnotized groups had no failures on any of the three jumps. The 16 inch jumps indicate that the categories of "up, down, and miscellaneous" accounted for the greatest percentage of failures for all groups. There was a significant difference among the groups in reasons for not catching the ball in the one-hand catch test. Chi square values of lh.090 and 11.892 were reported for trials one and two respectively. Then dfhh a value of 9.h9 or greater is significant at the .05 level (36). Table 3 shows that the reason for the l9 torn new 0m.mH woooxe so Manse nmfix He>mH no we msawb meadow who e.ma c.0m m.eH m.:H eeaeflmro 0.0m m.em 0.0 m.ma seenrecahn H.pm e.wm m.:H 0.0 aepeHsaam sea 0mm.m w 9.0m H.sm m.sa 0.0 nonsense 0.mw m.ma m.ma 0.0 emaauoeaau . H.eu e.mm m.aH 0.0 aeaasasam sea mem.m H m.mm m.ma 0.mm 0.0 eeaeaaho 0.0. 0.0m 0.0m 0.0m eeaapoaarm e.mm m.mm H.HH 0.0 aepaassam zma mea.m m 0.00H 0.0 0.0 0.0 ceaeaaao m.~0 0.0 m.wH 0.0 eeaaaocaam :.Hs 0.0 e.0m 0.0 acreassam ama 0a:.m H 0.0a 0.0 0.0m 0.0m ceasefire 0.0m 0.0a 0.0a 0.0a eeearoeoam 0.0a 0.0 0.0 0.0m aepaasram tees eme.r m 0.0m 0.0 0.0a 0.0: reseaare 0.0a 0.0 0.0m 0.0H seeareaarm - r 0.0m 0.0 0.0: 0.0a aeeaasaam ewes use.; H momma: .eaaa 02 a czomuab msfiocwq “Honoxwa m m a m areas .am eaesem are Hausa memwpcooaom CH popcomosm wcwawem pom meomeem esp aaa:.0ape seam or» an moonwawdfioo asoaw 20 significant difference appears to be that more than 70% of the children failed to catch the ball because of dropping it or pulling it to their chests. It is also noted that more than 70% of the simulators had only one or no misses, signifying no reasons for failure. TABLE 3 Group Comparisons in the One-Hand Catch with the Reasons for Failing Presented in Percentages V wmfi" Trial Chi Group Drops ball Two hands One or Square or pulls it to catch or No to chest several Misses reasons l 1h.090 Simulator 30.0% 0.05 70.05 Hypnotized 30.0% 50.0% 20.0p Children 70.0% 20.05 10.0fi 2 11.892 Simulator 10.0% 10.0; 80.03 Hypnotized I0.053 0.0% 60.0% Children 30.0% 10.0% 10.05 When de I, Chi square value at .05 must equal or exceed 9.h9 for significance. Results for the first circle trace trial reported a significant difference in reasons for missing among the groups. The chi square table of critical values (36) indicates that when dfll6 a value of 12.59 or greater is 21 required for significant differences between groups at the 5% level. Fbr trial one, the chi square value was 18.700, but for trial two, a non-significant value of 6.986 was noted. Percentages indicating the differences among groups in circle trace difficulties are noted in Table h. The children failed 60% of the time because of a pencil lift-off in trial one, but the record of trial two indicates that they missed for this reason only hog of the time. The simulator and hypnotized groups scored their higher percentage of misses because of going out of bounds. However, it should be noted that more than 705 of these two groups passed the test in general and thus would not have a high percentage in reasons for missing. 22 TABLE h Group Comparisons in the Circle Trace Test with the Reasons for Failing Presented in Percentages ===========_ ====____...—_ __...___._.._===='==== _____ Trial Chi Group Pencil Out of Pass Pass Square Lift-Off Bounds Trial Trial One Two 1 18.700 Simulator 0.0 10.0 70.0 20.0 Hypnotized 0.0 30.0 60.0 10.0 Children 60.0 0.0 20.0 20.0 2 6.986 Simulator 10.0 10.0 50.0 30.0 Hypnotized 0.0 20.0 50.0 30.0 Children no.0 10.0 h0.0 10.0 when df= 6, Chi Square value must equal or exceed 12.59 for significance. Neither trial of the maze trace test obtained significant differences among groups and their reasons for failure. The non-significant chi square values (36) were 10.562 for trial one and 7.800 for trial two when dfl- 6. In order for the obtained chi square value to be significantly different from.chance at the .05 level the value must reach 12.59. There was no significant difference noted between the two pass-fail performances of each skill test for either the simulator or hypnotized groups. Table 5 indicates the chi square values for all groups. 23 It should be noted that only three of the fifteen values reached the pro-determined level of significance. TABLE Chi Square Values for all Groups Indicating the Pass-Fail Changes from Trial One to Trial Two = .— “—===========_—_=:==== . Chi Square Chi Square Chi Square Skill df Simulator Hypnotized Children {nee High Jump h 3.500 1.270 10.000 12: High Jump h 2.625 3.u29 0.000 16 High Jmmp b 5.556 2.u00 8.000 One-Hand Catch 1 0.000 0.000 h.hh0 Circle Trace 1 0,278 o, 76 0.000 Maze Trace 1 0.000 2,?93 h,hu0 When deh, Chi square value at .05 must equal or exceed 9.h9 for significance. When df=l, Chi square value at .05 must equal or exceed 3.8h for significance. There was a significant difference at the .05 level in the results for three of the control group performances. A significant chi square value of h.hh was noted for the one-hand catch and the maze trace. Also, a significant chi square value of 10.00 was noted for the knee high Jump. Neither the twelve nor the sixteen inch high Jumps nor the circle trace test showed significant 2h difference in pass-fail performance from.trial one to trial two. A comparison of reasons for missing between the two trials for each of the tests was made for each group. There was no significant difference between trials for any group except in the one-hand catch. As in the maze trace and catch performances noted previously, only the children differed sufficiently between trials. They missed because of dropping the ball 70% of the time in trial one and 80% of the time in trial two. Table 6 designates the chi square values for each group, with the results categorized according to the reasons for failure. TABLE 6 Chi Square Values for all Groups Indicating the Reasons for Failure Between Trials for Each Skill ”WWW”: Chi Square Chi Square Chi Square Skill df Simulators Hypnotized Children Knee High Jump 9 0.312 3.810 10.000 12" High Jump 9 0.375 8.000 0.000 16" High Junp 9 2.222 2.h00 2.167 One-Hand Catch t 2.859 2.222 10.357 Circle;Trace 9 12.381 1.500 6.667 Haze Trace 9 6.111 16.667 6.607 When df = h, Chi square value at .05 level must equal or exceed 9.h9 for significance When df‘a 9, Chi square value at .05 level must equal or exceed 16.92 for significance 25 DISCUSSION OF .FTLI-I AND TEST FJJ‘SIETS ggglygis of the one-hand qatch. A child is normally proficient in reaching and grasping at an early age (two to five years), but orientation of his hands and arms to a moving object involves more complex time-space relationships (32, p.60). The five-year-old child uses his arms and hands as one unit. Consequently, he attempts to use both hands when catching a ball which he has bounced, and he tries to secure the ball against his body if the placement of the ball on the rebound is not in a position to which he is accustomed. This inclination to use other body parts to aid in the catch ‘was evident throughout the performance of the control group. In addition, due to the uncontrolled force exerted on the ball, the direction of the bounce was often unpredictable. In such instances the ball was often contacted but not secured, or the attempt at contact failed entirely. Insufficient eye focus, control of direction, and/or understanding of the time-space relationship appeared to be common characteristics of the five-year-old children. Ball size, relative to the size of the child's hand.may also have contributed to the difficulty in catching. If the catch was not perfect (in the palm.of the hand), 26 finger control was not exhibited and further attanpts to hold the ball were abandoned. This lack of finger control appeared to be the reason for more than SQ? of the failures to catch the ball in each trial. A.mature catching pattern proceeds in the following sequence: 1) a concentrated effort to watch the ball, 2) a catch on the upward portion of the bounce, and 3) a follow-through in which the hand follows in the direction of the bounce. Host of the children bent their knees and attempted to come up under the ball, but few followed the ball in its upward path. When the ball hit the hand, they either pulled it to the body or stopped the arm movement as the fingers grasped the ball in an attempt to maintain control. 'When the ball was missed completely, the children executed a chasing motion with the palm.of the hand in a prone position, following the ball on its downward path. However, this motion was not always spontaneous. Typical of childhood indecision, it appeared to be an after-thought. Observation of the one-hand catch in the hypnotized adults (experimental group) disclosed marked similarities to the pattern of the five-year-old children. The subjects did a deep knee bend to come up under the ball and tended to catch with two hands. The arm pull to the body, lack of follow-through, and lack of controlled force on the ball were also evident. However, some of the adults (both in the experimental and simulator groups) demonstrated mature characteristics which few of the children displayed. For example, they caught the ball "palm.down" on the first bounce, used only a waist bend to reach for the rebound, and reacted quickly to a miss. Whenever a child attempted to catch the ball with the palm down she missed the ball completely. The adults? larger hand size appeared to be a definite advantage in securing the ball. In the opinion of the researcher, it is for this reason that the experimental and simulator groups performed significantly better than the children. The simulator group displayed additional characteristics which are indicative of a mature catching pattern. They waited for the bounce, made the catch at the last moment and turned the hand over after the follow-through. These actions are typical of a well-executed bounce and catch. In addition, 70% of this group had only one or no:misses for the total of twenty attempts at catching the ball. It is concluded therefore, that although the members of the experimental group exhibited.many movements 28 indicative of a five-year-old.motor pattern, their physical maturation (specifically, hand size) prevented exact duplication of the motor patterns characteristic of younger performers. There was significant inconsistency between the two trials in pass-fail performance of the one-hand catch for the children. It is the writer's opinion that the performance of motor skills in children is less consistent than in adults, regardless of the skills involved. During several years of experience in teaching swimming and other sports, the researcher has observed hat instances are common when a child can execute a skill on one day and fail to execute the same pattern on a successive day. A significant difference between trials one and two was also noted in the reasons which caused the children to fail in the one-hand catch. Again, this inconsistency appears to be characteristic of young performers. Analysis of the high jumps. The most typical element of the control group pattern in the high jump was the one-foot take-off and landing. The children had obvious difficulty in jumping over the bar with two feet, and this difficulty increased as the bar was raised. The height of the bar affected the children in several other *ways. Some hesitated to jump, took a small preparation jump in place, or took no jump at all. 29 while jumping, the arm.movement of the children was erratic. Although.most of the five-year-olds realized that use of the arms was necessary, few knew exactly what motion to use. Consequently, balance upon landing was poor and several children fell forward. It was noted, however, that each child who had difficulty tended to be consistent in her error pattern. Some of the same instances of low-level performance were noted in the simulator group, but unlike the members of the other groups, many of these participants looked back immediately to check the results of their efforts. Unlike the children, the simulators were inconsistent in the reasons for failing to clear the bar. For example, on the first jump a simulator erred because of a one—foot take-off, and on the second attempt she knocked the bar off the standard on the way down. Although there was not a significant difference between groups, a review of the films revealed that their performance characteristics were varied. Arm movement for most of the simulators was well coordinated or not used at all. In each case observed to be atypical of five-year-old performance, the result was a successful, balanced jump, even when the bar was at its greatest height (16 inches). Hembers of the experimental group (hypnotized adults) exhibited the unskilled characteristics with 30 greater consistency than the simulator yroup. Their hesitations, one-foot take-offs and landings, inaccurate use of arms, and unbalanced jumps approximated the unskilled patterns of the five-year-old children. However, like the simulators, more than SOS of the group passed the highest jump. Although this level of achievement was not significantly different from that of the control group, it appears that physical maturation (specifically, the mature growth in height) had an influence on performance. Due to the difference in height between the children and adults, the levels chosen for the high jump may be open to question. Changing this factor, however, would entail regulating all test variables according to the relative physical size of each person. The significant difference noted in the pass-fail results from trial one to trial two in the knee high jump for the children again points out the variation in a child's actions and performance. The scores for the twelve and sixteen inch high jumps, as well as their reasons for missing in all jumps, were consistent from trial to trial. Results differed from previous findings (h), however, in that some of the children performed the sixteen inch jump. Analysis of the Tracing Skills. The single difference noted between groups in a visual comparison of the tracing skills was that, for the children, the 31 non-dominant hand produced a lighter pencil mark on the paper. Although the number of line crossings and pencil lift-offs increased with the non-dominant hand in all groups, a heavy line was noted for both hands on the experimental and simulator group tests. Thus, one might suppose that these two groups exerted more control and greater force when executing the tracing skills with the non-dominant hand. A significant difference between trial performances of the circle trace for the children was noted. It is the researcher's opinion that this phenomenon could have been caused by the children's short attention span when listening to directions or to lack of concentration during the tracing test itself. Likewise, the significantly greater number of circle trace lift-offs in the control group might have been caused by the inability to concentrate. Similar findings have been reported for both tracing tests (2h). Since the maze test was intended for seven-year-old children (and designed to be difficult for five-year—olds) better performance by the simulators had been expected, but more than 70¢ of all groups failed the teat. 32 Scoring of the tracing tests might have revealed greater differences between groups if the test had been administered to each participant on an individual basis. It is possible that one investigator was unable to note all pencil lift-offs, when testing groups of ten or twenty individuals. CHAPTER V. SUITIIARY, COITCLUSIOIIS AND RECOIIILIDATIOIE SUHNARY' The purpose of the current study was twofold: l) to determine if, through hypnotic age-regression, adults can perform at the motor ability level of an age in question (for this study, five years), and 2) to investigate the reliability of producing gross and fine motor skills over an interval of time. The investigation utilized ten subjects in each of three groups as follows: 1) a control group consisting of five-year-old girls, 2) an experimental group consisting of hypnotized adult females age-regressed to five years of age, and 3) a simulator group consisting of adult females who pretended to be hypnotically regressed to age of five years. Basic motor developmental tests and motion pictures were used to determine the validity of performance. The tests were administered on two occasions by an instructor unfamiliar with the experimental design and the identity and role of the subjects. Chi square tests for k independent samples were used to determine if observed disparities among groups differed by 33 314 chance or if changes in performance might be attributed to factors other than chance. The research findings were: 1) There was no significant difference in pass-fail performances among the three groups for the high jump tests, circle trace test and the maze trace test. 2) The children differed significantly from the experimental and simulator groups in their pass-fail performances on the one-hand catch test. In both trials, 80% failed the test, while 20% or less of the simulator and hypnotized groups failed either of the trials. The children also differed significantly in their reasons for missing the ball in this test. In both trials, over 70% of their failures were caused by dropping the ball and/or pulling it to the chest. 3) There were no significant differences in reasons for failure among the groups for the high jump tests and the maze trace test. h) Trial one of the circle trace test showed a significant difference among the groups in reasons for failing the test. Pencil lift-offs appeared to be the primary problem for the children while the eXperimental and simulator groups failed because of going out of bounds. In trial two, there was no significant difference among the groups. 5) When examining each group for pass-fail performance changes from trial one to trial two, no difference was noted in any of the tests for either the experimental or 35 simulator groups. The children had significant between trial differences in results for the knee height jump, the one—hand catch, and the maze trace tests. 6) An investigation of reasons for failing the tests indicated significant differences between trials for the children in the one-hand catch. No differences between trials were obtained for either the simulator or the hypnotized groups. 7) An investigation of test performance through a subjective film analysis indicated that the hypnotized subjects eXhibited many of the characteristics noted in the five-year-old children. These characteristics were not as evident in the group which simulated the motor performance of five-year-old children. CONCLUSIONS 1) Because no significant differences were reported in pass-fail results or reasons for missing for the high jumps, the circle trace and the maze trace, it is concluded that the hypnotized subjects, age-regressed to five years, can execute skills at the level of a five-year-old child. However, this same level of performance can be produced by simulators who pretend to be five years of age. 2) The difference in performance between the children and the adult groups in the one-hand catch is attributei to adult physical maturation, specifically the hand size. 3) Skill performance of hypnotized subjects and of those simulating the role of hypnotized persons is consistent from trial to trial. Children at five years of age diaplay a variety of inconsistencies in motor skills from.one testing situation to the next. h) The subjective film analysis of the gross motor skills revealed that the performance of the experimental group contained.more characteristics of five—year-old children than the performance of the simulator group. REC 013 IFEIIDATIO MB Additional investigation of hypnotic age-regression in the study of’motor development should be undertaken to answer some of the questions which resulted from this experiment. Also, due to the lack of experimentation in this field, further exploration is warranted. Some of the following specific recommendations might be considered: 37 l) The criterion tests of motor skill should contain definable sequences of development rather than rely on the qualitative "pass-fail" scoring of the current tests. 2) Examination of specific movement characteristics peculiar to each age group and skill should be pursued. These characteristics could be quantified in on-going longitudinal studies. 3) The age range of the control group should be reduced to plus or minus three months of the chronological age of five years. h) Two different ages should be examined in the course of one experiment. This procedure would mean that the hypnotized and simulator groups would portray, for instance, five and ten years of age. The control groups would consist of five-and-ten-year old children. Additional group differences might be noted if the simulator group were asked to differentiate between two age patterns when performing the same skill. S) A cinematographic study of additional motor skills such as the standing long jump or a throw is suggested. This analysis would permit the quantification of angles, velocities and forces. It would also provide for the possibility of defining differences in movement which may not be evident as the result of a qualitative approach. BI BLI OGRA PHY Instructional Resources Center 133 ERICKSON HALL UCHIGAN STATE umvaasm m wsma. MICHIGAN 48823 10. 11. 12. BI BL IOGRA PHY Ames, L.B. "The Sequential Patterning of Prone Progression In the Human Infant, Genetic Psychological Honogram, l9:hO9-h60, 1937, Barber, T.X. "Hypnotic Age Regression: A Critical Review," Psychosomatic Ledicine, 2h: 286-299, 1962. Barber, T.X. and Calverley, D.S. "Toward a Theory of HVpnotic Behavior: Effects of Suggestibility of Task‘motivating Instructions and Attitudes Toward Hypnosis," J. Abnormal Soc. Psychol., 67:557-565. 1963. Cowan, E. and Pratt, B.H. "The Hurdle Jump as a Developmental and Diagnostic Test of meter Coordination for Children Three to Twelve'Years of Age," Child Development, 5:107-121, JUne, 193h. Deach, D.F. "Genetic Development of Hotor Skills in Children Two Through Six'Years of Age," Micro-film Abstracts, 11: 287, 1951. Dusenberry, Lois "A Study of the Effects of Training in Ball Throwing by Children.Ages Three to Seven," Research Quarterly 23:9-1h, 1952. Espenschade, A.S. "Development of Motor Coordination inlBoys and Girls," Research Quarterly 18:30-h3, 1947. Sspenschade, A.S. and Ekert, Helen M. Hotor Develogment. Columbus, Ohio: Charles E. Kerrill,fifnc.,‘l967. Estabrooks, G.H. Hypnotism. (rev. ed.) flew York: Dutton, 1957. Fisher, Seymour. "Problems of Interpretation and Controls in Hypnotic Research," In G.H. Estabrooks (ed.) Hypnosis: Current Problems. Hew'York: Harper and Row, 1962. Frankenburg, um. C. and Dodds, J.B. "The Denver Developmental Screeninr Test," The J. of Pediatrics, 71: no. 2, 181-191, 19 7. A Gesell, A. and Ames, L.B. "The Development of Handedness," J. of Genet. Psychol., 70:155-175, 19h7. 39 13. 1h. 15. l6. 19. 20. 21. 22. 25. 26. no Gidro-Frank, L. and Bowerbuch,‘N.C. "A Study of the Plantar Response in Hypnotic Age Regression " h J. Nervous and Mental Disorders, 107:1;173-156, 191w. Gordon, J.E. and FTeston,H. "Role Playing and Age Regression in Hypnotized and Hon Hypnotized Subjects," . Personal, 32: hll—h19, 196M, Guttridge, H.V} "A Study of Motor Achievements of Children," Arch. Psychol., 2hh: l-178, 1939, Halverson, Lolas E. "Development of'Hotor Patterns in Young Children " Quest VI A Symposium on Hotor Learning, hfi-53, flay, 1966. Harrison, Virginia F. "Review of Heuromuscular Bases F g Motor Learning," Research Quarterly, 33: 59-69, 19 2. Hartman, Doris H. "The Hurdle Jump as a Heasure of Motor Proficiency of Young Children," Child Develoflaent, 11;: 206-215, 19143. """""' Hicks, J. Allan. "The Acquisition of’Hotor Skill in Young Children," Univ. of Iowa Studies in Child Welfare, 1; No. F, U. “61‘ Iowa Press, 1929. Hilgard, Ernest R. The Experience of’Hypnosis. New'York: Harcourt, Brace and Horld,Inc., 1965. Ikai, Hichio and Steinhaus, A.H. "Some ictors Modifying the EXpression of Human Strength," J. of Applied Physiology, 16: 157-163, 1961. Johnson, U.R. "Body Hovement.Awareness in the Hon Hypnotic and.Hypnotic States," Research Quarterly, 32: 263-26h, 1961. Kane, R.J. and Meredith, H.V. "Ability in the Standing Broad Jump of School Children 7, 9, and 11 Years of Age," Research Quarterly, 23: 198-208, 1952. Keogh, Jack F. .Analysis of Individual Tasks in the Stott Test offfibtor Tm airment. Technical Report 2-68 {USERS Grant HD UEUS9) Department of Physical Education, U.C.L.A., October, 1968. Kline, L. "Hypnotic Age Regression and Intelligence," J. Genet. Psych., 77: 129-132, 1950. London, P. and Furhrer, H. "Hypnosis, motivation and Performance," J. Personal, 29: 321-333, 1961. hl Head, S. and Roush, C. "A Study of the Effect of Hypnotic Suggestion on Physiologic Performance," Arch. Phys. Hed‘i, 302 700-705, l9h9. ITcCaskill, C.L., and T'Iellman, B.L. "A Study in Common Hotor Achievements at the Preschool Ages," Child Development, 9: 1hl-150, 1933. Orne, Iartin T. "Explorations in Hypnosis,” Research Project Summaries of the National Institute of fichtal Health, Supt. cf;DocumentsfiU}S. Govermnent Printing Office, Washington, D.C., 52—59, 1963. Pattie, 33A. "A Report of Attempts to Produce Uniocular Blindness by Hypnotic Suggestion," In T.U. Hitchell (ed.) The British Journal of Eedical Psychology, Cambridge: The University Press,‘1936. Platonov, H. The Uord as a Physiological and Therapeutic Factor. hoscow: Foreign‘Languages’Publishingghouse, 7 a. Rarick, G. Lawrence. Hotor Development During_Infanoy and Childhood. hadison,’7isconsih: College Printing and”Typing Co., 1961. Reyher, Joseph. H* nosis. Dubuque, Iowa: Hm. C. Brown Company éuBIishers, 1968. Sarbin, T. "Mental Age Changes in Experimental Regression," J. Personal, 19:221—228, 1950. Shirley, TarV'K. "A Longitudinal Study of the Notor Sequence," Reprinted from pages b-C, 3h-h6 of Volume 2 of The First Two Years. Pinnecpolis: U. oflfiinnesota Press,_1933. Siegel, Sidney. Honparametric Statistics, Hew'Iork: NcGrawwEill Book Cbmpany,71956. Sloan, U. "Lincoln-Osaretsky Hotor Development Scale," Genetic Psychology'tonographs, 51: 183-252, 1955. Slotnick, R.S. "Enhancement ofifiuscular Performance in Hypnosis through Exhortation and Involving Instructions," J. Personal, 33: 37—h5, 1965. Stott. ".3 .77., Itoyes, RA. and Headridge, Sheila :1. Test of Hotor Impairment Revision II (derived fromfthe‘Gollnitz revision of the Oseretsky Test of Voter Ability). University of Guelph, Guelph, O ntario, 19650 '43- e2 True, R.I. ”Experimental Control n T"pnoth A e Regression States," Science, 110: - 1911.9. Uaterland, Joan C. "The Effect of Force Cradation on Rotor Patterning: Standing Broad Jump," Quest Collage, 15-25, Hay, 1967. Heitzenhoffer, A.Y. General Techniques of Hypnotism. New York: Grune and"Stratton, Inc.,71937. Hellman, B.L. "Hotor Achievements in Preschool Children," Child.Bducation, 13: 311-316, 1937. 'Tild, Honica R. ”The Behavior Pattern of Throwing and Some Observations Concerning its Course evelopment in Ch ildren," Research Quarterly, 98 (3). 20- 2b. 1938 Young, P.C. "An Experimental Study of Rental and Physical Functions in the Hormel and'Hypnotic States," Amer. J. Psychol., 36: 2lh—232, 1925. Young, P.C. "Hypnotic Age Regression - Fact or Artifact?" J..Abnorma1 Soc. Psychol., 35: 273- N7}, 1940. Ziegenfuss, U. B. "Hypnosis - A Tool for 1Sducation, ” Education, 82: 505-507, 1962. A PPSITDI CBS API’im'DI I: A Hypnotic Induction 'fiethod The method of induction used was a variation of Ueizenhoffer's general technique (h2, p.206-207) and Kline's visual imagery (h2, p. 2M6). The operator learned this technique from Dr. Arthur H. Steinhaus in a seminar on Mind and Body, Autumn, 1968 at Kichigan State University. The subjects were seated around a table and the operator spoke in a conventional manner: I want you to look upward at a spot on the ceiling or wall and fix your eyes on it. Any spot will do, just as long as it is not in a corner or between the ceiling and wall. Do not worry if your eyes begin to blink or water. That is all right. Don't be tense. Just relax and listen closely to my voice, to what I say. (pause) Keep watching the Spot. It is beginning to move farther away, but it is still clear. You are very relaxed and the Spot is moving farther and f-a-r-t-h-e-r away. (pause) In fact, it is beginning to become blurry. The Spot is v-e-r-y far away but you can still see it. Your eyelids are starting to feel heavy and you're beginning to feel sleepy. (pause) Your eyelids are very heavy, v-e-r-y h-e-a-vy. They want to close. Ybu are drowsyo Your eyes are getting heavier and h-e-a-v-i-e-r and you are so sleepy, s-o s-l-e-e-p-y. Your eyes are closing and the spot is gone. 11h Appendix A (continued) It is so restful and peaceful to close and relax your eyes. Ybu are s—o t-i-r—e-d, and it feels s-o good to be sleeping. Ybu are in a deep sleep, going d-e-e-p-e-r, and d-e-e-p-e-r. It is so relaxing and peaceful. Ybu will pay attention to nothing but the sould of my voice. Ybu will not awaken until I tell you to. Ybu are in a d-e-e-p, d-e~e—p sleep. AFPJITDI I! B Stanford Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility (20) Stanford Scale Fbrm A. 1. 9. 10. ll. 12. Postural sway Eye closure Hand lowering Arm inmobili zatio (1 Finger lock Arm rigidity Hoving hands together Verbal inhibition (nmne) Fly hallucination Eye catalepsy Post-hypnotic suggestion (change chairs) Amnesia h6 Scoring Criterion subject sways forward or backward upon suggestion eyelids closed before told to close them deliberately lowered at least six inches before told to let hand down deliberately hand not lifted more than one inch when told to stop trying fingers incompletely separated when told to stop trying arm bent less than two inches before told to stop trying hands not more than six inches apart when told to return them to resting position did not shake head before told to stop trying outward acknowledgement of effect eyes remain closed makes at least observable partial movement to change three or fewer items listed in three minutes, before amnesia lifted l. 2. 3. APPENDIX Pest-hypnotic Suggestions Kathy: Diane: Peg: Barb: Janice: Rickie: Eartha: Pam: Lee: Sally: "green grass" "green goblin" (operator's hand "It's time to go "green grass“ "funny clown" (operator's hand "It's time to go (operator's hand "It's time to go "yellow banana" "green bean" "yellow banana" on to on to on to C for Sleep right shoulder) sleep, Peg." right shoulder) sleep, Nickie." right shoulder) sleep, hartha" In the case of the two-word suggestions, the post-hypnotic instructions included the facts that sleep would occur only if the operator said the words and only if the words were spoken separately, and not in a sentence. Fbr the sentence suggestions, the operator directed that only she could put the subject to sleep in this manner. 1:7 . PPJEIITDIIC D High Jump (2i, p. 75) Hodified Stott: Subject jumps over a bar with feet together, the bar is at knee height. Failure is recorded if the bar is touched or knocked down, or if, in jumping or landing, the feet are not together. Three attempts. a. Knee-height is taken as the height from the floor to the lower edge of tie knee-cap. b. A timid subject may be helped over the bar a few times to overcome fear of jumping. c. The jumping stands are three feet apart. d. Fbr he experimental and simulator groups, initial height of the bar is approximated at 102 inches, based on measurements of twenty five-year-old children. e. Fbr special experimental purposes, if the subjects in any group passes the initial test, the bar is raised to twelve inches for three attempts. If success is achieved at this height, the bar is raised to sixteen inches and three additional attempts are allowed. Instructions: 1. E demonstrates: "Jump over the bar the way I do." Give instructions about use of feet only if child has diffiCUltL-o 2. The feet must be together for take-off and landing, but landing position does not need to be held at the end or the Jmpe 3. The subject jumps with shoes on. l. Pass ~ Fail: three trials 2 . Note error as: a. Bar: touches or displaces bar on way up. touches or displaces bar on way down. b. Feet: not a two - foot take-off not a two — foot landing MB PPZIUDIIC E One - hand Catch (2h, p. 79) Modified Stott: Subject bounces a standard tennis ball on the floor and catches the rebound, using the same hand. When bouncing and catching, the subject makes five attempts with each hand. A successful trial is a total of five successful catches out of ten. a. The floor surface must be hard and even so that rebounds are predictable. b. The ball must be caught cleanly in the hand, not against the chest. Instructions: 1. The subject is tested while standing. 2. 2 demonstrates three catches stressing use of one hand to bounce and the same hand to catch. 3. The catch must be clean and not trapped against bocy part or clothing. Scoring: 1. Score the number of successful catches for each hand. 2. Five attempts with each hand. 3. Hots reason for miss as: two hands trapped against body drops it 119 APPENDIX F Circle Trace (2h, p.76) Stott: A piece of paper with a circular track is placed on the table in front of the subject. Using the preferred hand, the subject draws a continuous line inside the track. Failure is recorded if the line is broken or the subject makes more than two crossings of the boundary. Two attempts. a. A careful eXplanation and demonstration is useful. b. A no. 2 pencil should be used. Instructions: 1. The subject must keep the pencil in contact with the paper until finished (continuous, unbroken line). 2. E demonstrates: "Draw a line between these two lines until you have gone all the way around the race track. Keep you pencil on the paper until you finish." 3. The subject may pause or stop but'the pencil must stay in contact with the paper. b. Circular track is two concentric circles of 1 2/8" and 2 1/8" diameter which provides a 1/h" drawing track. Scoring: 1. Two trials: preferred hand only. Second trial administered only if subject breaks the line (lifts-off) or crosses a boundary. 2. Score number of boundary crossings and note if line is not continuous. 50 APPENDIX G haze Trace (2h, p. 81) Stott: A piece of paper with the mazes is placed on the table in front of the subject. The upper maze is followed with a pencil as far as possible, keeping an unbroken line; the subject then goes on to the lower maze without a pause. Failure is recorded if the dominant hand makes more than two crossings of the boundary, or the other hand more than three. Two attempts for each hand. a. Before starting, subject should be shown the entrance to the lower maze. He should go on to the second.maze as soon as he has finished the first. b. If necessary, the route through the maze may be demonstrated by the tester. c. The line must be clearly over the boundary to count as a failure. Pauses may occur but the line through each maze must be continuous. d. For the right hand the mazes should be inverted so that the START is at the bottom right hand corner. This minimizes visual difficulty. Instructions: 1. The subject uses a no. 2 pencil. hazes approximate Oseretsky mazes as used by Sloan (1955). 2. Demonstrate, indicating entrance and exits and the need to keep pencil on the paper. The pencil may be lifted when.moving from.maze one to maze two. 3. For the left hand begin at the top of the upper maze (no wiggly lanes) and exit to enter the'top-left of the lower maze. For the right hand, invert the mazes, enter the lower-right of the bottom maze and exit to the lower-right of the upper maze. Scoring: 1. Two trials for each hand. The second trial administered only if child breaks the line (lifts off) or crosses boundaries. » 2. Score the number of boundary crossings and note if line is not continuous. 51 Degree 0 f M.A. 1969 Rappolt, Lois Charlotte lT/j’Uxuctional RBSOUHJBS tame: ‘ ' CKSON HA‘-L mfiA‘fli UNweasnv_ EAST LANSING. Icwgfigzs 7 MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY LIBRARIES 3 ”93 03175 8091