AVOMANCE LEAEENENS AND RELEARNING AS A FUNCTiGN OF SHWTLEEOX WHENSIONS Thesis {:or fin: Dogma of EECEEGEXN S'i'éTE Ufii’i’Ef‘ Jay 0. Thomas 1960 M. A. 531“ V .f 54. THESIS LIBRARY Michigan State University AVOILANCE LEARNIx-SG AM) RtLEARAhG AS A FUNCTION c; SHUTTLEBOX DIMENSIONS By JAY o moms ‘A THESIS Submitted to the College of Arts and Sciences of Michigan State University of Agriculture and Applied Science in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1960 ABSTRACT This study investigates the effects of varying the dimensions; namely, ceiling height and length of gridway, on the level of original and continued avoidance learning in a shuttlebox. Seven groups, six eXperimental and one control, were run under two ceiling heights (5 anj 14 inches) and three gridway distances (30,26, and 16 inches) in an original 25 avoidance trials. One-hour later these groups were given 25 additional retention trials in the same situation. It was found that tie dimensions of the shuttlebox, primarily ceiling height, were important in t‘e learning and relearning of avoidance responses. Animals in low ceiling boxes initially showed a lower level of learning but improved when learning was continued following the hour delay. §s in high ceiling boxes showed higher learning, but poorer performance when learning was continued following the hour delay. A5) Jeff Jr 7;? I ’1’/, / L4 " I Approved: / w ’"fi<_f¢¢~3l, Date: f Ltk,:t:J/799 Major §$ofessor ‘/ ‘ To Racheal 11 ACKNOflLLDGMmNT The author wishes to express his gratitude and sincere thanks for guidance and assistance in tEe planning and execution of this research, and the develOpement of this manuscript to Dr. M. Ray Denny, chairman of his committee. In addition, he wishes to convey thanks to Dr. Charles Hanley and Dr. Abraham Barch for their excellent criticism and advice, during the preparation of this thesis. TABLE CF CCh’ILLnTS LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES L‘ITRODUCTICN IvItL’I‘lICD ELSSLTS DISCUSSION SUIiE-BRY APEENDICILS REFERENCES Page vi \fi 12 15 1b 19 iv LIST OF lABLLS AND FIGURES TABLE Summary of analysis of avoidance responses for the six experimental groups Comparison of mean avoidance responses of the present and the Denny studies Summarized data FIGURE Mean number of avoidance responses in Session I and Session II page 11 16 17 18 10 vi INTRCDCCTION The shuttlebox has been used in the investigation of avoidance and escape learning. This apparatus consists of a rectangular box with a observation window and a grid floor, either half of which can be charged by the eXperimenter. When an animal is placed in the situation and given some consistent signal that temporally precedes the onset of shock on one-half of the grid, the anihal can learn to avoid the shock by running to the uncharged grid. The typical signal is a buzzer or light that comes on five to ten seconds ‘ :‘G c+ before he shook. Shock avoidance is accomplished by animal's moving to the opposite end of tTe box when the CS is sounded. Such behavior is refered to as shuttling. If the animal does not shuttle the half of the grid upon which it is situated is electrically charted and continues to be charged until the animal moves to the uncharged region of the alley. dhen an animal shuttles following the onset of the CS but belore shock, it is said to "avoid". If it shuttles "escape". The occurrence after shock begins, it is said to of an avoidance reSponse prevents both escape behavior and the eXperience of being shocked. In an escape study, the animal is shocked on each trial; the present investigation deals with avoidance behavior. Few studies have been conducted on t‘e retention of an avoidance resgonse; probably because from the available literature, it a;[eared long lastin; and hi hlv resistent to extinction. Recently, however, Kamin (1957) has investirat=d the retention of an inconpletely learned avoidhnce resgonse. If original learning were interrupted scsn enouyh, he reasoned, only partial retention would occur, and a retention curve could be plotted. Kanin ran hooded rats for 25 trials in a typical shuttlebox and measured retention in 25 additionsl trials following celars of O, 1/2, 6, and 24 hours and 19 days. The results were un xpected; instead of a monotonic decreasing relation of retention to time, he found a V-shaped curve which declined from C to one hour and then rose from one- hour to 24 hours. Differences in retention at C, 24 hours, and 19 days were not statistically significant. Kamin interpretated his data in terms of two independent processes; one for each sebnent of the curve. The first segment of the curve, 0 to one hour agrees with tie vernacular concept of forgetting. The rising segmentv of the curve represents an incubation effect, a jelling of the avoidance habit following the initial decrement in retention. Denny (1958) reinterpreted Kamin's V-shaped curve (the "Kanin Effect") in terms of the incubation of anxiety rather than the incubation of an avoidance habit. according to this interpretation, anxiety initially builds up in the interval immediately following the original learninL trials to a point where it interferes with the act of shuttling. As observed by Denny, animals when tested one hour later, typically freeze in a second session, and this behavior is P.) incompatable with shuttling. Following a delay of approximately one hour the anxiety begins to dissipate and retention of the avoidance reSpcnse is clearly apparent after 2 hours. From this point of view, it was predicted that if the anxiety could be kept from building up, the § would no longer show a decrement in performance following an hour delay. USinL delays of O, 1, and 24 Fours Denny employed the following methods to inhibit the prowth of anxiety during t“e one-hour delay; 1) gs were on a reduced feeding regimen for two weeks and under 24—hours food deprivation when trained. They were allowed to eat alone in the home cage during the hour interval (Counter conditioning), 2) §s were left in the shuttlebox during the hour interval, without shock or buzzer present (Desensitization). It was also predicted that if the same amount of shock as the § received in original learning, were readministered in a different situation on the follow- ing day, the incubation of anxiety would be reinstated. Thus, if a second set of 2S learning trials followed one-hour later, the “Kamin Effect" would appear, even though it was now 24 hours since original learning. All these manipulations were carried out, and all yielded results which supported the incubation-of-anxiety hypothesis. However, there were certain differences in the results between the two studies; namely, considerably better original learning and slightly better 'Kamin Effect” after a one-hour delay in Denny's study as compared with Kanin's (a mean of 10.7 vs. 5.7 avoidances in Session I and 10.1 vs. 6.6 avoidances in Session II, respectively). Concomitantly there were certain apparatus difyerences: Denny's shuttlebox had a higher ceiling (14 vs. 4 3/4 inches) and a s orter Lridway (26 vs. 36 inches). Although other possible method- olOgical differences may have obtained between the two studies-- the level of shock, for instance, or the location of the observation window—- the apparatus dimensions appeared most relevant. Thus the present study is an investigation of the effect of length and 1eight of shuttlebox, both on oriLinal learning and relearning one-hour later. s I The §s were seventy experimentally-naive nocded, black, and albino rats from the colony of the Department of Psychology at Michigan State University. The 35 m“le and 35 female rats ranged in age from 90 to 150 days. During the study they were maintained on an ad lib feeding schedule and housed five gs of the sane sex per cage. Animals were randomly assigned to seven groups, except for balancing the nunber :h l of males and enales in each group. Running order of experimental and control 'rcups was also randomized. A C.J. Applegate stinulator was used to provide a continuous shock of 1.7 millianps directly to the Grid. The full distance of the gridway or either half could be charged, but only one half was charged at any one time. The §s completed the circuk by making contact wit“ any two of the adjacent COpper rods which were set approximately 1/4 inch apart in the grid floor. The shuttlebox, painted flat blacx with a glass front, was desiLned with removable partitions so that it could be any of three lengths: 36, 26, or 16 inches. The fixed wood ceiling was 14 inches high. To ef ectively lower ceiling height a sheet of clear glass was inserted five inches from tEe floor, thus maintaining visual similarity for the two hei¢hts. These modifications of the shuttlebox provided six combinations of apparatus conditions: 1) 36 inch long gridway with a 14 inch ceilin; 2) 2c inch long gridway with a 14 inch ceiling 3) 16 inch long gridway with a 14 inch ceiling 4) 36 inch long gridway with a 5 inch ceiling 5 26 inch long gridway with a 5 inch ceiling 6 16 inch long gridway with a 5 inch ceiling cl! «I111 urn] One experimental Croup of S5 was run under each set of conditions. Thus the present study includes the Kanin (36 inch long bridway with a 14 inch ceiling) and the Denny (26 inch long Lridway with a 5 inch ceiling) conditions. The width of all three shuttleboxes was four inches. The only difference between Kamin's shuttlebox and the conprable conditions of the present study is that the former had a glass tOp wfiile the present apparatus had a glass front. The conditioned stimulus was a buzzer of seventy decibels activated by six dry cells. The study was divided into two sessions: an inconplete learning phase of 25 avoidance learning trials (Session I) and a block of 25 additional trials (Session II), which came one-hour after the conpletion of Session I. In Session I S was placed in the shuttlebox for one minute before the trials began. The buzzer (CS) was sounded for five seconds prior to shock (US) and terminated when S crossed the midline of the box, either by avoiding or escaping. When § failed to avoid, the CS and US overlapped and both were response terminated. The inter—trial interval was one minute. Occasionally on the first trial, an S would shuttle to the CS alone; in such cases this trial was not counted as one of the 25, and the buzzer was again sounded following the inter—trial interval. In ot er words, t‘e initial trial was the first trial on which tbe animal did not respond to the CS alone and took shock. At the termination of Session I, S was returned to an outer room to be placed in its livinc cage with its ([1 cage mates present. One hour later S was returned to th apparatus for 25 more trials (Session II). For any one 1m 5 the conditions 01 See ion II we e identical wi h t‘e conditions (‘1 of Session I. A control proud was run in a shuttlebox 26 inches with a 14 incL ceiling with Session II innediately followinb Session I. In other words, this group had 50 continuous avoidance trials. The control was included primarily to deternine whether t”e present sanple of rats would perform in the same manner as in tTe earlier Denny study. For the main purpose of the present study the control group was unessential. RESULTS The eXperimental desipn, excluding the control group, lent itself to a 2x2x3x2 analysis of variance with repeated measures on §S. This design was taken from Lindquist (1956) and is a type III design with one added factor. The analysis is summarized in Table I. Two main effects-~ceiling height and sessions-~and three two-way interactions were significant. Ceiling height was significant at the .05 level J) an sessions at the .001 level. None of the third order or the fourth order interactions were statistiCally significant. A plot of the mean number of avoidances for Session I and Session II of the six eXperimental and the control Lroup is presented in Figure I. The neans of the echrinental proups were compared by a Studentized Distribution and yielded the following significant differences: 1) SS in high ceilinL boxes made more avoidances in original learning (Session I) than did SS in low ceiling boxes, 2) SS in low ceiling boxes sVowed improvement (i.e. the nean number of avoidances in Session II was greater than in Session I), w*ereas §s in high ceiling boxes did not s?ow inprovenent, 3) performance in Session II in a low ceiling short (16 inch) box was better t‘an performance in Session II in a high ceiling short (16 inch) box. The significant interaction between height and length (distance) tells us that with a high ceiling learning tends to be better the longer the alley and t”at with a low 4‘5. ceiling just the reverse is true The sipnilicant interaction T ABLE 1 Analysis of Avoidance ReSQOnses Source of Variation Between Sex Height Distance Sex x Height Height x Distance Sex x Distance Sex x Dist x Height Error Within Sessions Sessions x Sex Sessions x Height Sessions x Distance Sess x Sex x Height Sess x Sex x Distance Sess x Height x Dist Sess x Sex x Dist x H Error Total * 4 .05 M 4 .01 DF 48 60 Mean Square \0 4.66* 7.52** c.71* 187.5** 450.0*« IL/ MEAN NUMBER OF AVOIDANCES N04. D XIIIIX mem_OZ _ OIIIIO Mm mw_OZ N a D nozamorcaxmmv _ T O X / O .F / o .o 0 X1 )X\ D O O Ui- X/ XIX o _ + a a L. w .||_ mx_m mxmm mxwm 3X5 .Axmm £wa Exmm CORT ERR between sessions and hei;ht indicates, at a minimum, that the "Kamin Effect" occurs to a greater extent with a hi¢h ceiling than with a low ceiling. Finally the significant interaction between sex and ceiling height occurs because females made considerably more avoidances in the hith than in the low ceiling box and males made a few more avoidances in the low Ceiling box than in the high. The control group performed in both sessions at levels comparable to Denny's controls, and perfortance during relearning was excellent. 11 From Session I to Session II, the control group showed significant improvement, both when conpared with itself and with the 26 inch x 14 inch eXperimental group (see Table 2). TABLE 2 Comparison of Mean Avoidances of Present and Denny Studies EXperimental conditions Session I Session II Improvement Present 14 x 26 inches 9.6 18.8 +9.2 Denny 14 x 26 inches 10.2 17.8 +7.6 Present 14 x 26 inches 9.5 8.2 -1.3 Denny 14 x 26 inches 10.7 10.1 —0.6 DISCUSSION The present investigation appears to resolve the major discrepancies between the Kamin and Denny studies. Under comparable conditions conparable results were obtained. The only possible exception was the finding or less "Kamin Effect“ under conditions similar to Kamin's. Almost dramatically, avoidance learning and relearning was affected by the dimensions of the shuttlebox used. The superiority in original learning of high ceiling vs. low ceiling sugwests that the low ceiling limits the number of avoidance-type responses in the animals repertoire, and this in turn promotes the non-instrumental act of freezing. The effect is to decrease the probability of making the final shuttling reSponse. The experimenters observation of animals in low ceiling groups frequently freezing, with the onset of buzzer, would tend to support such an interpretation. If the § in a low ceiling box is to learn the avoidance reSponse, freezing behavior must extinguish. This may account for the relatively better performance after one- hour delay for the low ceiling §S. The one-hour delay, according to the incubation-of-anxiety hypothesis would, in general, increase the tendency to freeze. SS in high ceiling boxes, who have not been freezing, have not had an Opportunity to extingu sh this type of reSponse and consequently are at a disadvantage, i.e. show more "Kamin Effect". Conversely, §s under high ceiling conditions can make more reSponses to the stimulus situation: jumping, standing, and leaping. These responses can readily chain in with the correct response of shuttling. At first ;lanee, these results pose a paradox, for by lowerin; the ceiling one might expect an increase in the Specification of the correct response of shuttling, instead, it appears that we are promoting a reSponse which is inconpatible with shuttling and inconpatible with responses allied with shuttling. Such an interpretation.is consistent with new theoretical Behavioristic approaches that emphasize the importance of each reSponse elicited at every step of the analysis (Denny and Adelman, and Logan). The less important variable of length may Operate as follows. Under low ceiling conditions, the escaping rat can only run, which means it runs through shock to escape shock, being consistently punished during the early stages of making the correct response. Thus learning tends to be poorer the longer the box. In high ceiling boxes, where escape is possible in a number of ways, this length factor is not as critical. The interaction between sex and ceiling height indicates that females made more avoidance responses under high ceiling conditions than under low ceiling conditions. Males, it was found, avoided slightly less under low ceiling _conditiens. In order to shed some lith on this interaction, it should be mentioned that Kamin, with a low ceiling box and 1.1 millianp shook level, found no differences between males and females (personel comnunication to M. Ray Denny), whereas Denny found it necessary to use a higher shock 14 level with females in order to Let the "Kamin Effect? (1.7 milliamps for females vs. 1.1 milliamps for males) with a high ceiling. From this fact, it can be inferred that females are particularly prone to freeze in a low ceiling box but will freeze in a high ceiling box only if the shock level is sufficiently high. Therefore, it is not sumrising with the shock level of the present study that females froze more and thus avoided less under low ceiling conditions than was the case under high ceiling conditions. 15 SUMMARY This eXperiment studied the effects of shuttlebox dimensions upon the amount of avoidance learning and relearning. Seventy hooded, black, and albino rats--35 male and 35 females-- were given 25 shuttlebox trials and then rerun one-hour later for an additional 25 trials. It was found that tie dimensions of the shuttlebox, primarily ceiling height, were important in the learning and relearning of avoidance responses. Animals in low ceiling boxes initially showed a lower level of learning but improved when learning was continued following the hour delay. gs in high ceiling boxes showed higher learning, but poorer performance when learning was continued following the hour delay. A tenative interpretation of the results was included, which seemed consistent with new behavioristic positions. APPENDICIES SUMMARIZED gain 16 fiiofg F Session I. Session II Subjects 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total Blocks of 5 trials 1 C 2 C 1 1 O O 1 O 1 6 O 1 O 3 1 2 1 1 2 O 11 2 1 1 1 1 O 1 O 1 1 O 7 3 1 1 O 1 O 2 O 1 3 9 3 2 3 3 C 2 4 2 2 3 2 23 C 3 3 1 3 2 1 2 3 1 19 4 3311222244 24 4111142221 19 5 3 3 5 5 3 2 4 3 2 1 31 3 5 3 C 1 O 2 4 2 4 24 Group 14 x 26 Subjects 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total Blocks of 5 trials 1 C O O 1 O O O O O O 1 O 1 1 1 O 1 5 O 4 O 13 2 1 1 3 O 2 O 3 O 2 1 13 C 1 2 C 1 2 4 1 4 3 18 3 O 5 3 1 1 4 5 1 5 2 27 O 4 3 1 3 3 2 O 3 O 19 4 1 3 5 1 1 4 5 O 3 1 24 4 O 5 O O 2 2 1 O 1 15 5 1 4 4 O 2 4 5 5 4 1 26 1 2 4 1 1 3 3 O 1 2 18 Group 14 x 36 Subjects 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total Blocks of 5 trials 1 1 O 1 O 1 1 1 O 1 1 7 2 O O 3 4 2 1 1 3 O 16 2 1 3 2 C 3 4 2 4 1 3 23 1 2 1 3 1 2 2 2 2 4 2C 3 2 2 1 1 2 2 3 4 3 A 24 2 2 3 1 O 1 2 2 3 3 19 4 3 2 3 5 4 3 5 4 5 1 35 3 2 1 2 3 2 4 5 4 1 27 17 groapé Session_£ Session II Subjects 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total Blocks of 5 trials 1 O 1 O 1 C O O 1 O O 3 1 1 3 1 2 O O 2 2 2 14 2 1 O 1 1 1 1 O O O 1 b 2 1 1 3 1 2 O 2 1 4 17 3 3 2 1 1 3 1 O O 1 1 13 4 4 2 2 4 4 2 3 2 3 3C 4 2 O 2 2 3 3 O 1 O 1 14 4 4 4 5 5 3 1 4 3 3 36 5 3 1 3 2 4 2 1 3 O 1 40 5 2 3 4 5 3 3 3 3 3 34 Group 5 X 26 Subjects 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total Blocks of 5 trials 1 1 C O C O 1 O O O O 2 1 O 1 O 2 O O 4 3 O 11 2 C C O 1 O O 1 1 1 1 5 3 O 2 3 1 1 2 O 1 1 14 3 2 1 1 3 2 O 1.3 2 1 16 O 4 2 1 3 2 1 3 5 4 25 4 1 2 1 1 O O 2 O 2 O 9 4 5 4 3 4 2 4 3 3 4 36 5 2 4 3 1 3 1 3 1 1 1 2O 5 4 5 3 2 1 5 4 4 2 35 Group 5 x 36 Subjects 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total BlocKs of 5 trials 1 C O O O O 1 O 1 O O 2 O O 1 2 1 2 O 1 O O 7 2 O 1 1 1 O O 1 1 1 O 6 O 3 2 3 3 4 2 1 2 0 2O 3 1 1 O 2 1 2 O 1 2 O 10 3 O 1 2 2 1 2 3 1 2 17 4 2 1 2 1 1 3 1 1 O O '12 4 2 2 1 1 5 3 3 O 1 22 5 O 1 2 2 2 3 1 3 1 2 17 2 2 4 5 5 3 2 2 3 3 31 17 Group Session I Session II 5 x 16 Subjects 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total Blocks of 5 trials 1 O 1 O 1 O O O 1 O O 3 1 1 3 1 2 O O 2 2 2 14 2 1 O 1 1 1 1 O O O 1 6 2 1 1 3 1 2 O 2 1 4 17 3 3 2 1 1 3 1 O O 1 1 13 4 4 2 2 4 4 2 3 2 3 3C 4 2 O 2 2 3 3 O 1 O 1 14 4 4 4 5 5 3 1 4 3 3 36 5 3 1 3 2 4 2 1 3 0 1 20 5 2 3 4 5 3 3 3 3 3 34 Group 5 x 26 Subjects 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total Blocks of 5 trials 1 1 C 0 0'0 1 O O O O 2 1 O 1 O 2 O O 4 3 O 11 2 C C O 1 O O 1 1 1 1 5 3 O 2 3 1 1 2 O 1 1 14 3 2 1 1 3 2 O 1 3 2 1 16 O 4 2 1 3 2 1 3 5 4 25 4 1 2 1 1 O O 2 O 2 O 9 4 5 4 3 4 2 4 3 3 4 36 5 2 4 3 1 3 1 3 1 1 1 2O 5 4 5 3 2 1 5 4 4 2 35 Group 5 x 36 Subjects 1 2 3 4 5 6 7.8 9 10 Total 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total BlocKs of 5 trials 1 C O O C O 1 O 1 O O 2 O O 1 2 1 2 O 1 O O 7 2 O 1 1 1 O O 1 1 1 O 6 O 3 2 3 3 4 2 1 2 0 2O 3 1 1 O 2 1 2 O 1 2 O 10 3 O 1 2 2 1 2 3 1 2 17 4 2 1 2 1 1 3 1 1 O O 12 4 2 2 1 1 5 3 3 O 1 22 5 O 1 2 2 2 3 1 3 1 2 17 2 2 4 5 5 3 2 2 3 3 31 Group Session I Session II Control Subjects 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total Blocxs of 5 trials 1 1 O O O 1 C C 1 O 0 3 4 5 4 5 5 3 2 4 4 O 36 2 1 3 2 2 1 O O O 1 2 12 2 2 3 4 5 4 5 5 2 1 33 3 5 2 5 3 4 1 O 1 2 O 23 5 4 5 2 5 5 4 4 5.2 41 4 3235341240 27 5444345231 35 5 4 3 3 4 4 5 1 2 4 1 31 4 5 4 5 4 5 5 4 5 2 43 19 REEERENCpS Denny, M.R., and Adelman, H.M. Elicitation theory: An analysis of two typical learning situation. Psychol. Rev., 1955, 02, 290-296. Denny, M.R. The "Kamin effect" in avoidance conditioning. Amer. Psychol., 1958, 13, 419. Kamin, L.J. The retention of an Incompletely learned Avoidance Response. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1957, 50, 457-460. Logan, F.A. .A Micromolar Approach to Behavior Theory. Psychol. Rev., 1956. 63. 03-73. Lindquist, E.F. Design and analysis of EXperiments in Psychology and Education. Boston Houghton Mifflin, 1956.