LIPREADING PERFORMANCE AS A FUNCTION OF LIGHT LEVELS Thesis {or the Degree of M. A. MICHEGAN STATE UNIVERSITY Sharon Thomas 1962 ‘91“ fifmc 1":.- * ABSTRACT LIPREADING PERFORMANCE AS A FUNCTION OF LIGHT LEVELS by Sharon Thomas The purpose of this study was to measure the differ- ence in the lipreading performance of trained subjects during varying intensities of room illumination in an effort to determine the effect of various room lighting conditions on the lipreading efficiency of these subjects. The viewer subjects, ten men and ten women, were screened for visual acuity at 20/30. One additional female subject was present at five of the six group experiments. Her scores were used for comparative purposes. One female speaker presented the test sentences at all training and experimental groups. The subjects were trained to lipread a list of thirty test sentences. The sentences were designed to contain speech sounds in an approximate ratio to sounds in their frequency of occurrence in American con- versation. Those individuals who could not correctly identify 95 per cent of the sentences after the training session, were eliminated from the study. During training, the room lighting conditions were held constant at thirty foot-candles. White noise, which measured from 84 to 86 decibels, and inactive headsets worn as muffs, were used to mask auditory clues. Sharon Thomas During the actual experiment, the speaker presented the thirty sentences during each of six varying room lighting conditions. The six light levels were thirty foot-candles, sixteen foot-candles, eight foot-candles, four foot—candles, and one-half foot—candle, as measured by a light meter at the speaker's face. Inactive head sets and white noise were, as in the training sessions, used for the purpose of masking auditory clues from the speaker. The subjects responded in written form to the test sentences. The findings.of this study indicate that lipreading efficiency of trained subjects tends to decrease, but not significantly, as the intensity of room illumination decreases. The conclusions suggest that the lipreading efficiency of individuals who are highly familiar with the Content of the message decreases only slightly, and not significantly, as the intensity of room illumination decreases. They further suggest that lipreaders, who are highly familiar with the content of a message, when sufficiently motivated will lipread with an adequate degree of efficiency under the most adverse lighting conditions. \ :." Approved 13517 LUL“ I/fiA LU“- / ‘DirectoiJV' ) a ( LIPREADING PERFORMANCE AS A FUNCTION OF LIGHT LEVELS By ! 1 Sharon Thomas A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS College of Communication Arts, Department of Speech 1962 TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . iii Chapter I. STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM. . . . . . . 1 Introduction. 1 Statement of Problem and Purpose of Study . 3 Questions Posed at Onset 3 Importance of Study 5 Definition of Terms 5 Organization of the Thesis 6 II. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE. 8 Factors Influencing Lipreading Efficiency. . . . 8 The Need for Investigations of. the Relationship Between Light and Lipreading. . . . . . . . . . 10 III. SUBJECTS, EQUIPMENT, MATERIALS, AND PROCEDURE. . . . . . . . l2 Viewer Subjects. . . . . . . . . l2 Speaker Subject. . . . . . . . . 12 Equipment. . . . . . . . . . . 12 Materials. . . . . . . . . . . 13 Procedure. . . . . . . . . . . 13 IV. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION . . . . . . . 18 Results . . . . . . . . . . . 18 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . 21 V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS . 25 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . 25 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . 27 Recommendations. . . . . . . . . 27 BIBLIOGRAPHY. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30 ii LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Mean Per Cent of Recognition of Sentences During Six Light Levels of Decreasing Illumination Intensity . . . . . l8 2. Results of t_Test for Differences Between Total Number of Sentence Identifications for Six Light Levels. . . . . . . . 22 iii CHAPTER I STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM Introduction It has been suggested that the first handicap of l Man's need for communi— deafness lies in communication. cation with his fellow man is perhaps his greatest need and the fulfillment of his other needs is greatly depen- dent upon his ability to satisfy this basic one.2 Research indicates that the average American spends 70 per cent of his active hours communicating verbally-—listening, speaking, reading, and writing, in that order.3 The individual with a severe hearing loss is at a serious disadvantage. He is unfortunately handicapped in the most frequently occurring facet of communication-—listening. Lipreading is a valuable substitute for hearing as far as spoken language is concerned, but like other lHallowell Davis and s. R. Silverman, Hearing and Deafness (New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, Inc., 19505, p. xi. 2Ibid. 3David Berlo, The Process of Communication (New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, Inc., l9EO), p. l. 1 2 substitutes, it has its limitations.1 The rapid and fre- quently obscure movements of speech itself presents a major problem. Often unfavorable conditions of extraneous nature—- uncertainties of light, position, movement, and distance of the speaker-wmake lipreading unpredictable.2 The importance of one of these variables, light, and the effect that it has on lipreading efficiency, cannot be overestimated. The lipreader is perhaps more aware of the effect of, and more dependent on the existing light condi- tions, than any other environmental factors. The individ- ual with normal hearing, when present in a situation where his vision is obscured by shadows or darkness, can function and communicate effectively by depending on auditory clues. The hard of hearing person who must, in fact, ”hear with his eyes" has, under the same circumstances, lost or limited his only means for successful interaction. This perplexing situation has been but generally commented on in the literature. There is, however, no available research on the effect that various conditions of light have on lip- reading efficiency. Knowledge in this area is important in" in the complete assessment of the handicap which a hearing loss imposes and in the measurement of the extent to which lipreading can substitute for hearing. lElizabeth Nitchie, New Lessons in Lip Reading (New York: J. B. Lippencott Co., 1950), pp. 25-26. 2Irving Fusfeld, ”Factors in Lipreading as Determined by the Lipreader," American Annals of the Deaf, 102 (March, 1958), p. 240. 3 Statement of Problem and Purpose of Study The problem from which this study arose is that of determining the effect of various room lighting conditions on the lipreading performance of trained subjects. The pur- pose of this study is to measure the differences, if any, in the lipreading performance of trained subjects during varying intensities of room illumination. Questions Posed at Onset Questions posed at the onset of this study were: 1. Is there a decrease in over—all lipreading performance as a function of decreased light level? 2. Is there a difference obtained at the first in performance between the score light level (criterion score) and the score obtained at the second light level? 3. Is there a difference score obtained at the obtained at the third 4. Is there a difference score obtained at the in performance between the first light level and the score light level? in performance between the first light level and the score Obtained at the fourth light level? 5. Is there a difference score obtained at the obtained at the fifth 6. Is there a difference obtained at the first in performance between the first light level and the score light level? in performance between the score light level and the score ob- tained at the sixth light level? 10. ll. 12. 13. 14. 15. Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained 4 a difference in performance between the at the second light level and the score at the third light level? a difference in performance between the at the second light level and the score at the fourth light level? a difference in performance between the at the second light level and the score at the fifth light level? a difference in performance between the at the second light level and the score at the sixth light level? a difference in performance between the at the third light level and the score at the fourth light level? a difference in performance between the at the third light level and the score at the fifth light level? a difference in performance between the at the third light level and the score at the sixth light level? a difference in performance between the at the fourth light level and the score at the fifth light level? a difference in performance between the at the fourth light level and the score at the sixth light level? SCOPE SCOPE SCOTS SCOPE score SCOPG score SCOPE 5 16. Is there a difference in performance between the score obtained at the fifth light level and the score obtained at the sixth light level? Importance of Study This study is considered basic research and the writer does not seek immediate practical application either in the training or the counseling of lipreaders. It is hoped that this study will provide some basis for future investigations of the effect of various environmental factors on lipreading performance, and thus enable a more complete assessment of the total limitations imposed by a hearing loss. Definition of Terms For the purpose of this study, the terms used are defined in the following manner: Snellen Vision Test.--A chart consisting of selected letters of the alphabet that are pictured in varying sizes. The size of the letter that the individual is able to read at a certain distance from the chart enables the examiner to obtain a measure of the individuals' visual acuity for distance only. Illumination.--The amount of luminous flux falling on 1 an object emitting no light of its own. 1H. A. E. Keitz, Light Calculations and Measurements (London: Cleaver Hume Press, Ltd., 1955), p. 72. 6 Foot—candle.--One foot-candle is the illumination pro— duced by one lumen falling on a surface one foot distance away from the source. Light meter.--The light meter is a direct measure of luminance. It is used to determine the intensity of light which it measures in foot-candles. Light levels.—-Certain measures of illumination intensity. Lipreading.--The art of reading speech when the reader is deprived of the acoustic component of the oral symbol. Organization of the Thesis Chapter I has contained the statement of the problem which led to this study and the purpose for which it has been conducted. It has included an introduction to the topic, and stated the questions to be discussed. The im- portance of the study has been discussed and the terms which will be used have been defined. ‘Chapter II will contain a review of the literature which pertains to this topic. Chapter III will consist of a discussion of the subjects, equipment, materials, and testing procedure employed in this study. Chapter IV will consist of a discussion of the results of the study. 7 Chapter V will contain a summary and the conclusions of the study. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE , Factors Influencing Lipreading Efficiency No one factor fully accounts for lipreading efficiency, but rather a combination of them—-natural aptitude, easy command of the English language, acquaintance with the vagaries present in speech, and a large functioning vocab— ulary.l These factors are variables relative to the individ— ual lipreader and, in part, determine the degree of success he may expect to achieve in oral communication. The specific intellectual assets and the personality of the lipreader are sometimes considered to be variables. The research, however, reveals that there appears to be no significant relationship between lipreading skills and general intelligence.2 The relationship between lipreading ability and certain behavioral or personality factors has been investigated and supported by research.3 The sociable person who takes an active interest in the people and lFusfeld, op. cit., p. 236. 2John J. O'Neill and Herbert J. Oyer, Visual Communi— cation for the Hard of Hearing: History, Research, and Methods (Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc., lgEI): p' 39. E ' 31bid., p. do. 9 events around him usually makes more rapid progress in lip- reading than does the shy, reserved, unimaginative individ- ual.1 The alertness of the lipreader, his flexibility, and his attitude, are likely factors that affect lipreading ability. The influence of the speaker and his message are im- portant determinents of lipreading efficiency. The lip- reader‘s acquaintance with the speaker, his knowledge of the subject materials on which the conversation is based, and the manner of address of the speaker contribute to the over-all effectiveness of the verbal exchange.2 Other contributing factors include the amount of animation with which the talker is speaking, his speech mannerisms, the manner in which he articulates, and the degree of under— standing and consideration he possesses. That the amount of the speaker's face which is visible in addition to his lips is related to how efficiently and individual can lipread is evidenced in the results of a recent investigation. The results showed that there were definite improvements in lipreading scores as more of the face was visible.3 Certain factors in the environment 1Davis, 0 . cit., p. 365. 2Ibid. 3Edgar Lowell, "New Insight Into Lipreading,” Rehabili- tation Record, II (July—-August, 1961), p. 4. 10 are assumed to be partly or wholly responsible for limiting the visibility of the speaker's face. More specifically, the unsatisfactory conditions of a poorly lighted room that throws deep shadows on the speaker's face, or a seating arrangement that forces the lipreader to look into a bright light.1 Dr. Max Nelson of Michigan State University, in reference to these specific environmental factors, states that "the literature has constantly and continually advocated that lipreading be done under very clear lighting conditions. The reasons are the assumed negative influ- ences of poor lighting conditions on the ability to lip— read.”2 The Need for Investigations of the Relationship Between Light and Lipreading Although the literature does assume that the intensity and direction of the light in the immediate environment has its effects on lipreading efficiency, there is no published research in this area.3 Dr. Herbert Oyer of Michigan State University states that "for all practical purposes one would want a well lighted room as it would seem that shadows and dim light would have a deleterious effect on lipreading, 1Davis, op. cit., p. 364. 2Interview with Dr. Max Nelson, associate professor, Michigan State University, July 10, 1962. 3O'Neill and Oyer, op. cit., p. 44. ll but the importance of a well lighted room is open to ques- tion and experimentation."1 In one instance in the liter- ature it has been urged that controlled studies should be made of the effects of lighting on lipreading performance.2 1Interview with Dr. Herbert J. Oyer, Director of Hearing and Speech Clinic, Michigan State University, July 10, 1962. 2 O'Neill and Oyer, op. cit. CHAPTER III SUBJECTS, EQUIPMENT, MATERIALS, AND PROCEDURE Viewer Subjects Ten male and ten female adults were subjects in this study. One additional female subject was used for compar- ative purposes. The subjects' educational background varied from one to six years of college. No previous lip- reading experience was required of these subjects, although the potential ability to lipread was necessary. Speaker Subject One female adult presented the test sentences to all training and experimental groups. Equipment Amplifier (Ampex Amplifier—Speaker, Model 620) Light fixture (Eight mounted, lOO-watt bulbs) Light meter (Spectra Gold Seal Exposure Meter, Model 8-500) Miscellaneous head sets Reostat (Powerstat, Model 116) Sound pressure level meter (M. S. A. Soundscope, Model B) White noise generator (Grason-Stadler White Noise Generator, Model A55b) 12 13 Vision test (Green Test Cabinet, Model 20) Tape recorders (Telectro-Tape, Model 5953, Wollensack, Model T1500) Materials Thirty test sentences were devised by the examiner for use in this study. Each sentence contained six syllables. A total of A84 speech sounds was contained in these thirty sentences. The examiner intentionally attempted to create a comparable ratio of the frequency of individual speech sounds occurring in these sentences with the relative frequency of occurrence of speech sounds com- iled by Dewey.l These sentences may be found in Appendix B, page 33. The comparison of the frequency of occurrence of the speech sounds is reported in Appendix C, page 34- Procedure 1. Vision screening.--Each subject was given a Snellen Vision Test for both eyes in order to test their visual acuity as far as distance was concerned. This test ~was given to each subject individually prior to training. Any individual who was unable to pass the vision test at 20/30 was eliminated from the study. 2. Orientation to the test situation.--Each subject was presented with a list of the thirty test sentences several days prior to the initial training sessions. They lHarvey Fletcher, Speech and Hearing in Communication (Princeton: D. Van Nostrund’Inc., 1961), p. 9II 14 were instructed to familiarize themselves with the content of each sentence and, if possible, practice lip reading them. 3. Training of subjects.--The training sessions took place in a room in which no exterior light was present. The room lighting conditions for training were held con- stant at the face of the speaker at thirty foot-candles as measured by the light meter. White noise was present at an intensity deemed sufficient to mask auditory clues from the speaker. The intensity of the white noise measured from 84 to 86 decibels at the subjects' seats. Unconnected headsets were worn by the subjects as further insurance against auditory clues. During the training session, the sentences were presented to the subjects in random order by the speaker. The subjects were instructed to repeat the sentences orally in a soft voice. The speaker was thus able to quickly identify the subject or subjects who had had difficulty with a particular sentence. The sentence was then repeated until the subject or subjects felt familiar with the visual clues that the sentence afforded. The speaker kept a record of the number of incorrect identifications made by each subject. The presentation of the complete list of test sentences was the final exercise in the training session. These sentences were written by the subjects and immediately scored by the speaker. Those subjects who successfully identified 15 95 per cent (28) of the sentences, were deemed eligible to participate in the experiment. The training sessions never exceeded one hour and fifteen minutes. 4. Testing procedure.--The experiment was conducted in the same room in which the subjects had been trained within 48 hours after the training session. There was no source of light present in the room other than that provided by the light fixture designed for the experiment. This light fixture was suspended 26 inches above the speaker's head. The subjects were seated in a straight row in front of the speaker. Since the experiment was conducted on six separate occasions, there were never more than five subjects, including the female control subject, present in any one group. The distance between the speaker and the subjects varied from seven feet eight inches to eight feet two inches. The speaker stood behind a rostrum during the ex— periment. The subjects were seated in the same seating arrangement in which they had been trained. White noise was present in the room at the same inten- sity as was used for the training session (84 to 86 decibels). The inactive headsets were worn by all subjects. The subjects were presented the thirty test sentences for review purposes preceeding the actual test situation. Immediately preceeding the actual experiment, the subjects were furnished with paper and pencils and were instructed to record every sentence that they were able to lipread during the succeeding light levels. 16 During the experiment, the thirty test sentences were presented randomly at each light level. Six light levels were employed: 30 foot-candles, l6 foot-candles, 8 foot- candles, 4 foot-candles, 2 foot—candles, and 1/2 foot- candle. There was a five minute break between light levels during which the subjects experienced the next light level at which they were to be tested. The succeeding light levels gradually decreased the amount of room illumination. The same speaker presented the test sentences to all training and experimental groups. She wore identical clothing and makeup for each group. An undecorated medium grey blouse was worn by the speaker in order to prevent distraction or possible glare that might arise from cloth of a brighter color. The speaker attempted to control, to the best of her ability, unnecessary ”mouthing” of words and/or unnecessary facial expressions. In order to offer some insurance that the speaker's behavior did not vary significantly from group to group, one female subject voluntarily participated in five of the six experimental groups. Her scores are reported in Appen- dix, page 32. 5. Recording and scoring.—-Each subject's list of sentences for each light level was graded and recorded separately. A sentence was considered correct if four of the six syllables corresponded with the original sentence. 17 This two-syllable error allowance was designed to give some leeway for common errors such as confusion of articles or word endings. CHAPTER IV RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Results The number of correct sentences identified by each viewer subject at each light level was tabulated and recorded. The per cent of correct identifications at each of the six light levels was analyzed and tabulated. Table 1 presents the data obtained in response to question one posed in Chapter I, namely: 1. Is there a decrease in over-all lipreading performance as a function of decreased light level? TABLE 1 MEAN PER CENT OF RECOGNITION OF SENTENCES DURING SIX LIGHT LEVELS OF DECREASING ILLUMINATION INTENSITY I — —.—.— .—‘ Light level 1 2 3 4 5 6 Illumination intensity 30fc* l6fc 8fc 4fc 2fc 1/2fc Per cent of recognition 96.83 96.16 94.66 94.66 94.50 88.83 *fc = foot-candles. 18 19 A comparison was made of the differences between the total number of identifications made at each of the several light levels in response to questions two through sixteen posed in Chapter I. 2. The questions were as follows: Is there a difference in performance between the score obtained at the first light level (criterion score) and the score obtained at the second light level? Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained a difference in performance between the at the first light level and the score at the third light level? a difference in performance between the at the first light level and the score at the fourth light level? a difference in performance between the at the first light level and the score at the fifth light level? a difference in performance between the at the first light level and the score at the sixth light level? a difference in performance between the at the second light level and the score at the third light level? a difference in performance between the at the second light level and the score at the fourth light level? SCOPE SCOPE SCOPE SCOPE SCOPE SCOPE lo. 11. 12. l3. 14. 15. 16. Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained Is there obtained obtained 20 a difference in performance between the score at the second light level and the score at the fifth light level? a difference in performance between the score at the second light level and the score at the sixth light level? a difference in performance between the score at the third light level and the score at the fourth light level? a difference in performance between the score at the third light level and the score at the fifth light level? a difference in performance between the score at the third light level and the score at the sixth light level? a difference in performance between the score at the fourth light level and the score at the fifth light level? a difference in performance between the score at the fourth light level and the score at the sixth light level? a difference in performance between the score at the fifth light level and the score at the sixth light level? To analyze the data obtained in response to questions 21 two through sixteen the following formula was employed:1 Ml - M2 t = '— y/r .zld2 N (N-l) The results of this analysis are presented in Table 2. Discussion The per cent of recognition does not differ greatly}\ among the six light levels; however, it does appear to de- crease as a function of decreased light level, thus indi- cating a tendency for lipreading efficiency to decrease as illumination intensity decreaSes. Table 1 shows the mean per cent of correctly identified sentences for each light level. The subjects were able to correctly identify 96.83 per cent of the sentences during the first light level and 96.16 per cent of the sentences during the second light level. The comparison of the differences between the scores for these two light levels indicated no significant differ- ence. The intensity of illumination had been decreased from thirty foot—candles (first light level) to 16 foot-candles (second light level). A comparison of the difference between the first and third light levels revealed no significant difference. The per cent of recognition for the light level was 96.83 per 1E. F. Lindquist, Statistical Analysis in Educational Research (New York: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1940), p. 59. 22 TABLE 2 RESULTS OF £_TEST FOR DIFFERENCES BETWEEN TOTAL NUMBER OF SENTENCE IDENTIFICATIONS FOR SIX LIGHT LEVELS Light Light Level of Level Mean Level Mean p score Confidence LLl 29.05 LL2 28.85 .222 Non sig. at .05 LLl 29.05 LL3 28.40 .221 Non sig. at .05 LLl 29.05 LLn 28.40 .211 Non sig. at .05 LLl 29.05 LL5 28.35 .227 Non Sig. at .05 LLl 29.05 LL6 26.65 .229 Non sig. at .05 LL2 28.85 LL3 28.40 .227 Non sig. at .05 LL2 28.85 LLu 28.40 .227 Non sig. at .05 LL2 28.85 LL5 28.35 .226 Non sig. at .05 LL2 28.85 LL6 26.65 .296 Non sig. at .05 LL3 28.40 LL“ 28.40 .000 Non sig. at .05 LL3 28.40 LL5 28.35 .398 Non Sig. at .05 LL3 28.40 LL6 26.65 .229 Non sig. at .05 LL“ 28.40 LL5 28.35 .398 Non sig. at .05 LL” 28.40 LL6 26.65 .229 Non sig. at .05 LL5 28.35 LL6 26.65 .229 Non sig. at .05 23 cent and the per cent of recognition for the third light level was 94.66 per cent. The illumination intensity was decreased from thirty foot-candles to eight foot-candles. These results are applicable to the difference between the first light level and the fourth light as the per cent of recognition was identical for both the third and fourth light levels. The intensity of illumination had been de- creased to four foot-candles at the fourth light level. At the fifth light level, 94.5 per cent of the sentences were correctly identified as compared to 96.83 per cent recognition at the first light level. The p_test revealed no significant difference between these two light levels. The intensity of illumination during the fifth light level was two foot-candles. No significant difference was found between light levels one and six. Illumination was one—half foot-candle at the sixth light level; 88.83 per cent of the sentences were identified by the subjects at this light level. It was reported by the subjects that they experienced difficulty- in writing the sentences at this light level as it was ”too dark to see the paper.” The speaker had difficulty locating and identifying the sentences to be read. No significant differences were found when any other combination of light levels were analyzed. One must remember in studying the above data that trained subjects were used. They were highly familiar with 24 the content of each sentence and with the specific visual clues that each sentence afforded. The subjects remarked that they felt highly motivated to identify the sentences presented at the two light levels of lowest illumination intensity. It was observed that the subjects made a more active, conscious effort to attend to the visual clues during these last two light levels. The results of this study indicate that there is some question as to whether or not lipreading efficiency notice- ably decreases as a function of decreased room illumination intensity when the lipreader is highly familiar with the content of the message. Questions concerning reasons for these results can only be answered hypothetically at this time. The manner in which this study was conducted seemed to present a challenge to the subjects. Some subjects explained that they were motivated by a desire to ”beat the test." Some inter—group competition was observed. CHAPTER V SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS Summary Lipreading, as a substitute for hearing spoken lang- uage, has its limitations. The evaluation of the effects that various personal and environmental factors have on lipreading efficiency is important in the measurement of the extent to which lipreading can substitute for hearing. An important variable affecting the lipreader's performance is the direction and intensity of the environmental lighting conditions. Although it has been conceded that lighting conditions are an ever-present variable and can cause the lipreader much difficulty, the actual effects of lighting conditions has not been previously investigated. The purpose of this study has been to measure the dif- ferences in the lipreading performance of trained subjects during varying intensities of room illumination in an effort to determine the effect of various room lighting conditions on the lipreading efficiency of these subjects. This study does not have immediate practical appli- cation, but may provide some basis for future investigations of the effects of various environmental factors on lipreading performance. 25 26 A review of the literature concerning the variables affecting lipreading performance indicates that, while poor lighting conditions are generally acknowledged as having a negative influence on lipreading efficiency, there has been no reported research on this topic. The viewer subjects for this study were ten male and ten female adults. One additional female viewer subject was present at five of the six group experiments. Her scores were used for comparative purposes. One female speaker sub- ject presented the test sentences at all training and experi- mental groups. The Snellen Vision Test was employed as a screening device for subject selection. The subjects were trained to lipread a list of thirty test sentences. The sentences used were designed to contain speech sounds in an approximate ratio to sounds in their frequency of occurrence in the American language. During training the room lighting conditions were held constant at thirty foot-candles. White noise which measured from 84 to 86 decibels and inactive head sets worn as muffs were used to mask auditory clues. Those individuals who could not correctly identify 95 per cent of the sentences after the training session, were eliminated from study. During the actual experiment the speaker presented the thirty sentences during each of six varying room lighting conditions. The six light levels were thirty foot- candles, sixteen foot-candles, eight foot-candles, four 27 foot-candles, two foot-candles, and one—half foot-candle, as measured by the light meter at the speaker's face. In- active head sets were again worn by the subjects, and white noise was present for the purpose of masking auditory clues from the speaker. The subjects responded in written form to the test sentences. The findings of this study indicate that lipreading I: efficiency of trained subjects tends to decrease, but not significantly, as the intensity of room illumination decreases. Conclusions l. The lipreading efficiency of individuals who are highly familiar with the content of the message decreases only slightly, and not significantly, as the intensity of room illumination decreases. 2. Lipreaders, who are highly familiar with the con- tent of a message, when sufficiently motivated will lipread with an adequate degree of efficiency under the most adverse lighting conditions. Recommendations 1. An investigation of the recognizability of word lists by trained and/or untrained subjects under varying room lighting conditions. 2. An investigation of the lipreading efficiency of subjects involved in conversation during varying intensities of room illumination. 28 3. An investigation of the lipreading performance of untrained subjects identifying sentences at various room light levels. 4. An investigation or the recognizability of speech 'sounds during varying intensities of room illumination. 5. An investigation of the lipreading performance of trained or untrained subjects identifying sentences or words under light levels measuring from one foot-candle to zero foot-candles. APPENDICES 3O RAW SCORES OF CORRECTLY IDENTIFIED SENTENCES DURING SIX LIGHT LEVELS APPENDIX A Subject Number LLl (30 fc) LL2 (16 fc) LL3 (8 fc) l 29 29 26 2 30 28 29 3 28 29 28 4 29 30 27 5 28 26 24 6 28 27 25 7 3O 30 30 8 30 30 30 9 29 30 30 10 30 29 29 ll 28 27 26 12 28 28 28 13 28 29 29 14 30 29 30 15 30 30 30 16 30 30 30 17 3O 30 29 18 28 29 29 19 30 28 30 20 28 29 29 LL4 (4 fc) LL5 (2 fc) LL6 (1/2 fc) 1 2g 30 28 2 2 30 29 3 29 30 28 4 28 28 22 5 25 24 23 6 26 24 20 7 30 3O 3O 8 29 28 25 9 29 29 25 10 29 29 26 ll 28 26 26 12 26 26 26 13 28 28 29 ’14 29 29 29 15 30 30 30 16 30 30 30 17 29 26 25 18 29 30 26 19 30 30 30 20 29 30 26 31 32 RAW SCORES OF SUBJECT ATTENDING FIVE EXPERIMENTAL GROUPS LL1 LL LL LL 4 LL LL 29 29 22 26 24 25 30 3O 30 30 29 28 29 30 29 30 3O 29 3O 3O 30 3O 30 29 30 30 30 30 3O 30 "__T'E !:_ O O O O O O \OCD\H%U1#XUR)H APPENDIX B TEST SENTENCES His last look was costly. The log is very hot. We baked no bread today. Our room was filled with noise. I played a game of gin. Your vision is not good. The day was much too short. The card fell from the pack. He has a cute grandson. In spring we play outside. Is this the road around? Your voice seemed very loud. Have you seen this movie? A cat must have nine lives. This is a thin penny. They just ate red jello. Do get some mint candies. I just sat on a tack. It is found in that nook. This has been a pleasure. Let's go for a quick swim. The price is one nickle. A cough broke the stillness. She put on both the lights. Fred's mom dropped an armchair. Their lunch is being served. Fran's glad she went to Flint. Yes I just saw your dog. I will stop very soon. Start every gong ringing. 33 A COMPARISON OF THE FREQUENCY OF OCCURRENCE OF APPENDIX C SPEECH SOUNDS OCCURRING IN TEST SENTENCES WITH THEIR RELATIVE FREQUENCY OF OCCURRENCE IN AMERICAN CONVERSATION SpeechYSound Test Sentences Conversational u (tool) 6' tone; 0 talk a top) a' tape) 2' eat) u took) 9 ton a tap e ten i tip 1' dike) ou our) oi oil) w y h l r m n ne v 2 th (then) zh f s th (thin) sh b d J S p t ch k \1 HmmHNOmmmmwmrwmHommmwrommmppwwmmmw H H UL) ewe {$30mel—‘I—J-P’F—‘OOU'IHOWWMHOWMQWHHMOOHCDUUWQMHH i—' O O O O O O O O H O O O I O O LOOKOHl—‘O-F-‘OOO-P‘h)OMMHHCDWO-PMMWOOMCNMMOHMI—‘mI—‘l—‘ID ONUJCDNIUTUOONCBNNOOOUWmml—‘Hml—JONMH‘QIUCh-P'UONI—‘OOHQHUUU'IO 'fi LU :- p- 29 (”"9511“)! ”It 9 “ “TC 5" “Cd, Hr) j...‘2r'f}k'~/ __. . l Ci}. _.I, '__.