RUTH CAVAN s HYPOTHETICAL BEHAVIORAL CONTINUUM:- f j AN EMPIRICAL TEST Thesis for the Degree 0? M. A. MICHTGAN STATE UNWERSITY '- DONALD GENE WILLTAMS. 1968 A“ ’ LIBRARY Middt lSdec 'TH Egg Umvcrsny RUTH CAVAN'S HYPOTHETICAL BEHAVIORAL CONTINUUM: AN EMPIRICAL TEST By Donald Gene Williams A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Sociology 1968 ACKNOT‘TLEDGI‘IEI‘TTS This author is indebted to many persons who freely contributed their time and energies to the completion of this project and wishes to express his appreciation for their assistance and advice. Special mention is due the writer's faculty advisor, Dr. Orden Smucker, for his guidance and patience. Donald Gene Williams -11- TABLES OF CONTENTS Chapter V l: II: III: Introduction ................ An Overview of Approaches to Criminal Behavior ............. Crime as Deviant Behavior --------- Cavan's Behavioral Continuum ------- Underconformity and Overconformity - - - - Summary .................. Development of Hypotheses and Methodology- - Empirical Evidence of a Behavior Continuum ................. Hypotheses ................ Methodology ................ Characteristics of Sample --------- Technique of Gathering Data -------- Pretest .................. Definition of Terms ............ Results ................... Attitudes Toward Underconformity and Overconformity .............. Severity of Attitudes Toward Under- conformity and Overconformity ------- Significant Others Named By Sample - - --- -iii- Page 1 2 8 l2 15 2R 25 26 32 R1 R1 11,6 50 SM 58 58 66 81 Chapter Page IV: Conclusion and Summary ----------- 95 Summary ------------------ 95 limitations of Current Study ------- 96 Implication and Suggestions for Needed Research -------------- 101 APPENDIX --------------------- 108 BIBLIOGRAPHY ................... 117 -1v- Table I: II: III: VI: VII: VIII: IX: LIST OF TABLES Characteristics of stages of continuity in behavior --------- Average number of offenses reported by Texas college students among 200 men and 137 women in pre-college days, and the percentage reporting one or more of the offenses ------- Percentage of 1020 men and 678 women committing specific offenses in law York City .............. Honesty continuum.with characteristics of stages according to the traditional interpretation and to Cavan's interpretation —————— - ...... Percentage of males and females re- sponding to the following question: Which one of these things would be the hardest for you to take -- your parents' disapproval, your teacher's disapproval, or breaking with your- friends? ................ Whose advice do adolescents take?- - - - Social characteristics of college class and sample ............ Scale of juvenile delinquency as developed by Short and Eye ....... Characteristics of overt behavior represented by fictitious persons in stories used in study .......... -v- Page 21 27 30 3h RO ML R9 52 Table Page X: Number and percentage of students in college freshman sample re- porting attitudes toward various degrees of conformity ---------- 61 XI: Number and percentage of male students in college freshman sample reporting attitudes toward various degrees of conformity ------ 67 AII: NUmber and percentage of female students in college freshman sample reporting attitudes toward various degrees of conformity ------ 72 XIII: Number and percentage of students in college freshman sample reporting attitudes toward various degrees of conformity ---------------- 76 XIV: Number and percentage of students in freshman sample who named at least one person from the following categories of significant others as being concerned about "How well you obey the rules of honesty?" ------- 83 XV: Number and percentage of college freshmen who named a person from one of the following categories of significant others first in response to "Name those persons you feel are most concerned about how well you obey the rules and regulations of honesty." ---------------- 8h XVI: Number and percentage of students in freshman sample who named at least one person from the following categories of significant others as being concerned about "How well you obey the rules of honesty" -------- 86 -vi- Table XVII: XVIII: Number and percentage of students in freshman sample who named a person from one of the following categories of significant others first in response to ”Name those persons you feel are most con- cerned about how well you obey the rules and regulations of honesty." ----------------- 88 Number and percentage in pretest sample who named at least one person from each of the following categories of significant others ----- 91 -vii- LIST OF FIGURES Figure I: Hypothetical formulation of a behavior continuum as presented by Gavan ------------------ II: Traditional formulation of behavior continuum ................. III: Discrepancies between lower-class and middle-class evaluation of identical behavior ------------- IV: Graphic presentation of percentage of students in college freshman sample reporting attitudes toward various degrees of conformity -------- V: Graphic presentation of the median attitudinal responses of the students in the college freshman sample toward various degrees of conformity ----------------- -viii- Page in AZ 65 79 LIST OF APPENDICES Appendix Page I: Research Instrument -------------- 108 -1x- CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Social scientists have offered.many and varied explanations for deviant behavior. All of these expla- nations have been based on three common assumptions: first, deviant behavior is the result of a pathological condition; second, the deviant is the critical variable in the analysis of deviant behavior; and third, behavior can be described in terms of a continuum extending from disapproved deviant behavior through tolerated conform- ity to idealized conformity to idealized conformity. Chapter I will explore the plausibility of each of these assumptions. Primarily this thesis will examine the relative merit of two opposing models of the behavioral continuum. The first model conceives the behavioral continuum as a linear model; that is, approval for behavior increases prOportionately to the degree of conformity of that be- havior. On the other hand, the second.model suggests that a more appropriate scheme is a curvilinear model. That is, approval for behavior increases proportionately to the degree of conformity of that behavior; but only up to a certain point, then disapproval for behavior in- creases proportionately to the degree of overconformity represented by that behavior. -2- Before an attempt is made to systematically examine these assumptions, a brief review of the var- ious approaches to criminal behavior is in order. 5p OVERVIEW pg APPROACHES pg CRIMINAL BEHAVIOR Throughout the ages, man has sought to explain and understand social deviation. Prior to the 18th century, the explanations offered tended to be demonological in nature; that is, the individual was considered to be possessed by "other world powers." During the 18th century Cesare Beccaria founded the classical school of criminology. The major position of this school was that the individual's behavior was guided by rational judgment and free will; that is, an individual engaged in criminal activities because of the anticipation of the pleasure the activities would bring. The neo-classical school modified this position in.part as it allowed exclusions from.punishment because of mitigating circumstances, age of the offender (youth), or the psychological condition of the offender (insanity).l Since the early 19th century when the positivistic school of criminology replaced the neo-classical school, there has been a long succession of criminological the- ories, most of which have not stood the test of time. 1 For a complete discussion of the early history of criminological theory, see George Vold, Theoretical Criminology_(New York, 1958). -3- It is possible to classify these theories into three groups depending upon their theoretical orientations: biological and constitutional theories, in which devi- ancy is explained through the inherited physical and mental makeup of man; psychogenic theories, in which deviancy is explained through the formation of an anti- social character; and sociological theories, in which deviancy is explained through the pressures and pulls of the social milieu. While these classifications are arbitrary and tend to obscure the interdependency of significant variables, they do have utility for ana- lytical purposes.2 The biogenic orientation, the dominant theory in Europe today, points to the inheritance of physiological weaknesses or the inheritance of pronenesses toward crime and delinquency. The specific formulation of this hypothesis varies considerably: the attempt to prove inheritance of proneness through the method of studying criminal twins (Lange, Rosanoff, and Kraz); the attempt to identify body-mind types (Kretschmer, Sheldon, and Hooton); the attempt to identify and ex- plain habitual (serious) offenders as contrasted with occasional offenders or offenders of opportunity (Frey, vervsech, and Exner); the attempt to trace the 2 For an excellent summary of the three orientations to Criminological theo , see Halter Reckless, The Crime Problem (New Ybrk, 1961 , Chapters 12-18, pp. 233-360. -14... inheritance of bad strains through the descendants of notorious degenerate families (Dugdale, Goddard, Davenport, and Estabrook); and the specification of the mesomorphic somatotype (muscular) as the type of con- stitution which is most usually related to delinquency (Sheldon, and Gluecks).3 The psychogenic school views character and per- sonality as a function of early childhood deve10pment. August Aichhorn, one of the fountainheads of this orien- tation, maintains that faulty development in the first few years of life makes it impossible for the child to control his impulses. The child lingers on as sort of an aggrandizing infant, living in the pleasure principle and failing to deve10p the reality principle in life. Friedlander refers to this process as an antisocial character structure, and Redl calls it a faulty super- ego. Others in this tradition have attempted to show the relationship between deviant behavior and feeble- mindedness (Goddard, Kuhlman, and Zeleny). Still others view deviant behavior as an eXpression of neurotic 3 For a complete discussion of the biogenic orienta- tion, see Ibid., Chapter 1h, pp. 270-290; and Herbert Bloch and Pgank Flynn, Delinquency (New York, 1956), Chapters 5 and 6, PP. 964Ih9. -5- mechanisms (Gluecks, Hathaway, and Monachesi).LL Sociologists, ever since Ferri, have been calling attention to bad environmental conditions as causal factors of deviant behavior. This hypothesis was echoed by Bonger, who placed the blame for a dispro- portional amount of crime and delinquency among the proletariat on the pressures of the capitalistic sys- tem.5 Using the sociological framework, American criminological theory has taken divergent forms: Vbld argues that criminal behavior is the outcome of groups with opposing interests which are in conflict to main- tain their respective statuses and/or to gain new sta- tus;6 Taft argues that criminal behavior is the result of the general cultural structure of American society which provides opportunities for crime (A community has as much crime as it deserves!);7 Shaw, Mo Kay, and Thrasher explain deviancy in terms of community or social disorganization;8 Sutherland argues that criminal U For a complete discussion of the psychogenic orientation, see Reckless, pp.‘pip., Chapter 15, pp. 291-300; Bloch and Flynn, gp. cit., Chapter 7, pp. 151-175; and Marshall Clinard,_33ciology pf Deviant Behavior (New York, 1963), Chapter 5, pp. 116-IHI. 5 Ruth Shone Gavan, Readi 3 3p Juvenile Delin- quency (New York, 196h), p. I60. 6 Vbld, pp, cit., pp. 203-261. 7 Donald Taft, Criminology (New York, 1956), p. 321. 8 Clifford Shaw, and Henry Mo Kay, Juvenile Delin- quency and Urban Areas (Chicago, l9k2 ; and Frederic Thrasher, The Gang (Chicago, 1936). -6- behavior is the result of differential association (according to this theory, delinquent or criminal behavior is learned - as are most other kinds of be- havior - learned in association with others, according to the frequency, intensity, priority, and duration of contacts);9 Glaser proposes differential identification as a substitute for differential association (one takes over the models of behavior from those (reference) groups with which one identifies);lo Cohen, employing Merton's anomie as an acute disjunction between the cultural values and goals and the socially structured capacities of members of certain groups to act in accord with those values and goals, contends that working class boys who turn their backs on middle class virtues and values find a solution for their status problems in the delinquent subculture of the gang (alternative solutions are the stable street corner boy who conforms to the working class style of life, and the college boy who strives for middle class status by adopting middle class styles);11 and Cloward and Ohlin, combining the anomie tradition and the Chicago tradition, assert that the urban slum boys gravitate to delinquent subcultures when they do 9 Edwin Sutherland and Donald Cressey, Principles pf Criminology(Chicago, 1955), pp. 6-8. 10 Daniel Glaser, "Criminality Theories and Be- havioral Images," American Journal p£_Sociology, VXI (Nay.11956), NBO-NSO. . 1 Albert Cohen, Delinquent Boys (New York, 1955). -7- not have access to legitimate avenues of success, but they do have access to illegitimate avenues (they con- tend that if both the legitimate and the illegitimate avenues to status are closed, the boys will gravitate to either a retreatist or a conflict subculture).12 Due to the inability of the previously mentioned theories to explain all delinquent acts that occur, some criminologists have turned to multiple factor expla- nations of deviant behavior. Out of their research of 500 delinquent and 500 nondelinquent boys, the G-luecks13 prOposed a five point causal law. According to this formulation, delinquents are distinguishable from non- delinquents (l) physically, in being essentially mesomorphic; (2) temperamentally, in being restless, impulsive, aggressive, destructive; (3) emotionally, in being hostile, defiant, resentful, assertive, and non- submissive; (h) socioculturally, in being reared by unfit parents; and (5) psychologically, in being direct, concrete learners. Also using the multiple factor ap- proach, Reckless proposes the containment theory to in- corporate and merge pull, pressure, and push theories 12 Richard Cloward and Lloyd Ohlin, Delinquency and Opportunity_(New York, 1960). 13 Sheldon and Eleanor Glueck, UnravelingJuVenile Delinqgengy(New York, 1950), pp. 28I?282. -8- into one theoretical framework.1hr He argues that be- havior is the result of the interaction of the push forces of the internal motivational system, the pull and pressure forces of the external social system, and the internal and external containments upon these forces; that is, delinquency occurs when the forces (push and pressure) toward delinquency are greater than the forces (inner and outer containments) against delinquency. CRIME A§_DEVIANT BEHAVIOR The assumption that deviant behavior is the result of a malfunctioning in the individual and/or in the social order and the assumption that the deviant is the critical variable in the explanation of deviant behavior have been questioned by the developing perspective of the sociology of deviancy. Historically, explanations for criminal behavior have been based on these two assumptions. Most of these explanations consider crime to be a vagrant form of human activity which has somehow broken away from the more orderly currents of social life and which needs to be controlled. The primary issue raised by most criminol— ogists has been the explanation of the comission of an offense by an individual. Since it has generally been understood that criminal behavior would occur only if l h Reckless, 3p. cit., Chapter 18, pp. 335-360. -9- something was wrong with the individual involved or if something was wrong within the social organization it- self, explanations for deviant behavior, as noted in the preceding section, were given in terms of "machinery in poor condition." In other words, deviancy is the result of biological malfunctions, psychological disorders, or social disorders and anomie. Further, most of these explanations focused on the deviant or the circumstances in which the deviant was found as the critical variable. As seen from the developing perspective of the sociology of deviancy, deviant behavior can be defined as conduct which requires the attention of social control agencies. Deviance is not a property inherent in certain forms of behavior; it is a property conferred upon these forms by the audiences which directly or indirectly witness them. Howard Becker refers to this as the proc- ess of labeling:15 Social groups create deviance by making the rules whose infraction constitutes deviance, and by applying those rules to particular people and labeling them.as outsiders. From this point of view, deviance is not a quality of the act the person commits, but rather a consequence of the application by others of rules and sanctions to an 'offender." The deviant is one to whom that label has successfully been applied; de- viant behavior is behavior that people so label. The critical variable, then, is the social audience rather 15 Howard Becker, The Other Side: Perspectives pp. Deviance (New York, 196E7} p. 3. ~10- than the individual actor since it is the audience which eventually determines whether or not any episode of be- havior or any class of episodes is labeled deviant.16 This process of labeling, according to the sociology of deviancy, also involves a process of selection. First, the community isolates only a few scattered episodes of behavior and decides that they reflect what a person is really like. After all, even the worst criminal conforms to societal norms most of the time: he wears the "proper" clothing, eats the "proper" food and in a "conventional" manner, speaks the "proper" language, and in a thousand other ways, respects the ordinary conventions of society. Secondly, society does not label all possible acts of nonconformity as deviant but selects only certain in- dividuals and episodes to so label. This screen is not as selective when dealing with extreme forms of deviance such as serious crimes; but in the day to day type of screening, the process is sensitive to such things as the individual's social class, his race, his sex, his past record as an offender, the amount of remorse, and so forth. Society usually provides a sharp rite of transition when one enters the distinctly deviant role. These rites provide a "formal conformation" between the deviant and representatives of society (as in a criminal trial); they 16 Kai Erikson, "Notes on the Sociology of Deviance," Social Problems, IX (Spring, 1962), 311. -11.. announce some "judgment" about the nature of deviancy (the verdict in a trial, for example); and they perform an act of "social placement" which redefines the in- dividual's position in society (for instance, that of prisoner).17 Because of this social replacement, meme bers of society can accord the deviant treatment con- sidered to be appropriate for such a deviant. At the same time, the deviant usually accepts this redefinition and readjusts his behavior accordingly:18 No more self-defeating device could be dis- covered than the one society has developed in dealing with.the criminal. It proclaims his career in such loud and dramatic forms that both he and the community accept the judg- ment as a fixed description. He becomes con— scious of himself as a criminal, and the community expects him.to live up to his reputation, and will not credit him if he does not live up to it. A central thesis in this kind of analysis is that "self- fulfilling prophecy mechanisms" help to explain deviance; that is, we define an individual as deviant and treat him accordingly, and because of the definition and the treat- ment, the individual behaves in a deviant manner. His "19 deviant behavior then justifies our original "prophecy. An important aspect of deviancy analysis in these 17 Ibid., p. 316. 18 Edwin Schur, Crimes Without Victims (Englewood Cliffsa 1965). p. 3. 19 Erikson, gp. cit., p. 311. -12.. terms is that crime promotes solidarity in the social group. According to this theory of deviancy, one of the main organizational components of a social system is that of boundary maintenance. A well-functioning social system should be analyzed in terms of two dif- ferent and often competing forces: those forces which promote a high over-all degree of conformity among its members, and those forces which encourage some degree of diversity so that actors can be deployed throughout social space to patrol the system's boundaries. In other words, the deviant performs a function for society by representing those forces which lie outside the group's boundaries; that is, the deviant informs us "what evil looks like," or "what shape the devil can assume."20 Each time the group censures some act of deviation, it then sharpens the authority of the vio- lated norm and declares again where the boundaries of the group are located. CAVAN'S BEHAVIORAL CONTINUUM The assumption that behavior can be described in terms of a continuum extending from disapproved deviant 20 21 For a complete discussion, see Becker, gp.‘g£§.; Erikson, gp. cit., pp. 307-3lk; Marshall Clinard, Sociology of DEViant Behavior (New Yerk, 1963); John Kitsuse, liSgcietal Reactions to Deviant Behavior," Social Problems, IX (Winter, 1962), 2k7-257; Schur, gp. cit.° and Gresham.Sykes, Crime and Sociepy (New Yerk, I936 . Ibid,, p. 15. -13- behavior through tolerated behavior to idealized be- havior is questioned by Ruth Cavan's proposal for a hypothetical formulation of a behaVior continuum.22 According to this hypothetical formulation, behavior falls into a continuum.ranging from.condemnable be- havior (area A) through decreasing degrees of dis- approved behavior to the central area (area D) and then through increasing degrees of good behavior to near per- 23 fection (area G) which is also condemned. 22 This hypothetical formulation of a behavior con- tinuum is outlined in Ruth Cavan, Juvenile Delingpenpy (New Ybrk, 1962); Ruth Cavan, Readipgs ip Juvenile Delinquency_(New York, 196k); and Ruth Gavan, "The Con- cepts of Telerance and Contraculture as Applied to Delinquency," Sociological Quarterly, II (1961), 2h3-258. 23 Cavan contends that the line above the curve represents the volume of behavior which falls into each area. She cites the following sources as evidence to support a bell-shaped curve: Floyd H. Allport, "The J-Curve Hypothesis of Conforming Behavior,‘ Journal 2; Social Ps cholo V (193h), 1h1-83; and R. T. La Piers and P. R. Farnsworth, Social Psychology (New York, 1936), p. MOO. -1);— Figure I: Hypothetical formulation of a behavior con— tinuum as presented by Gavan u B1 '11 c) A 7B C D Extreme underconformity Moderate underconformity Minor underconformity Normal conformity Minor overconformity Moderate overconformity Extreme overconformity Q’IJtIJPQtIHD Historically, criminological theorists have tended to think in terms of dichotomies: the sinner and the saint, the criminal and the law-abiding citizen, the juvenile delinquent and the modal child. They tend to think in terms of black and white; on the other hand, Gavan argues that between these two rare extremes are many shades of gray. For instance, Gavan proposes such a series as “pitch black, charcoal gray, slate gray, tattletale gray, dingy white, offwhite, and lily white."25 2” Source: Gavan, 196R, p. 17. 25 Ibid., p. 18. -15- In this series of seven, the area of normal conformity (area D) is not white but tattletale gray. (Cavan borrowed this term from the advertisements of a few years ago in which the sheets flapping on the line were tattle- tale gray because the housewife had not used the right kind of laundry soap.) Cavan contends that observed behavior falls into similar gradations: "the child may break into a store at night and steal (black); deliberately pick up valuables during store hours; occasionally pick up things as Opportunity arises; pilfer small objects (tattletale gray); be meticulous about taking things; remonstrate with others who steal; or report other children to teachers or police for even minor pilfering (lily white)."26 UNDERCONFORMITY.AND OVERCONFORMITY Modern criminology recognizes a continuum of criminal behavior ranging from "criminal" to "law-abiding law- breaking" to "law-abiding."27 This continuum is based on the tendency to think of social norms not as workable expectations of behavior but as ideal or perfect standards. / 2° Ibid., p. 18. 27 For example, see Austin Porterfield, Youth 33; Trouble (Fort WOrth, 19h6); James Wallerstein and Clement wyle, "Our Law-abiding Law-Breakers," National Probation (April, 19h7), pp. 107—112; and James Short and Ivan Nye, "Extent of Unrecorded Juvenile Delinquency, Tentative Conclusions," Journal of Criminal Law, Criminology, and Police ScienooI'IE'CIOES), 296:302. -16... According to this information, most people fit their behavior into the middle area of tolerable "law-abiding law-breaking" behavior. Figure 11 : Traditional formlation of behavior continua-23 r“\ A B G D I I ' G Disapproved The Ion Approved Deviations Tolerance units Deviations Disapproved Deviations Behavior Patterns Approved Deviations (Institutions) 1. The Poor 1. Economic 1. The Health 2. Criminals 2. Political 2. 'Best' Citisens 3. The Sick 3. Medical 3. The Very Healthy 4. ktrenely Hal- 4. Domestic 1.. (mi-.1: adjusted 5. Bored, Ova-worked 5. Recreational 5. v.11 6e 1.013“. 6e Communal, We 6e Adjusted 7e Illiterates 7e Educational 7e Th. 11m 8. Vandals 8. Esthetio 8. Esthetes 9. Violently Anti- 9. Woo-Religions 9. The Virtuous, Religions Devont 28 Source: Adapted from George A. Lnndberg, Clarence G. Sohrag, and Otto l. Larsen, W (New Iork, 1958), p. 349. -17- T6 the left of this tolerable area is disapproved be- havior (criminal); however, to the right, this for- mulation shows approved deviations. Approved deviations, according to this continuum, exceed the standards set by the group and include at the extreme some 2 or 3 per cent of the peOple who more than conform to standards of the group and who are given public recognition for their overconformity. According to modern criminology, the ideal standards for behavior would be at the extreme right, would constitute virtual perfection, and would be attained practically by almost no one. Everyone except the 2 or 3 per cent on the right side would be "deviants", or "criminal."29 Current criminological research is often based on a deviant-ideal continuum. For example, Sheldon and Eleanor Glueck in their much discussed book, Unraveling Juvenile Delingpency, use this continuum in their comparison of delinquent children (area A) with near-perfect children (area G).3O To make this comparison, the Gluecks:matched 500 correctional school boys with 500 boys of exemplary behavior who had identical ages, intelligence, and social backgrounds. Not only were these control boys without any police, court, or correctional-school record, but 7k per cent were without any known delinquency of eVen a 29 For a complete description of this continuum, see Ibid., ppe 339'3750 3O Glueck, 9p, cit. -18.. minor nature. The Gluecks had difficulty in finding 500 such overly good boys, and eventually had to include a few boys guilty of such.misbehavior as smoking in early years, hopping trucks, once or twice swiping much desired articles from.department stores, crap shooting, dis- obeying parents, and sneaking into movies. Most of the deficiences were very trivial and had occurred when the boys were seven or eight years old. The Gluecks, then, were comparing boys from area A, the most seriously underconforming, with boys from area G, the near-perfect overconforming. Their findings report that whereas the delinquents tended to be active, aggressive, impulsive, and rebellious, the control group of "normal boys" tended to be neurotic, fearful of failure or defeat, and sub- missive to authority. Actually, it seems very doubtful whether so much admiration is really accorded the overconforming group as most criminologists state or imply. The good behavior and achievements that are rewarded by society seem much more likely to be in area D or E (tolerable limits) than in areas F or G (overconforming deviations). Gavan argues that the distribution of this admiration can be seen by a consideration of the descriptive terms and epithets used to describe the behavior of the boys falling into -19- each of the areas:31 Boys in area A are often referred to as little savages, hoodlums, punks, bums, or gangsters-- not very complimentary terms. But boys in area C also are not complimented; they are often referred to as sissies, goody-goods, teacher's pet, drips, brains, fraidy-cats, wet blankets, or squares. Adults and youth alike admire the boys in area D, who are essentially conforming but not rigidly so. The area D youth is "all boy," or the all-American boy; he can take care of himself; he is ambitious; he can hold his own with the best of them; he is a good sport. A little later, in college, me makes a "gentle- man's C." He may occasionally borrow small things that he needs and forget to return them, truant off and on but not enough to damage his school record, cheat on tests in subjects that he doesn't like, mark up the walks and walls of a rival high school, do some property damage under the stress of excitement, outwork and outsmart his rivals, lie for his own advantage, and occasionally sass his parents and neglect his home chores. But he stays within the tolerance limits; he is developing, even in misbehavior, traits that will help him.fit into the adult comp petitive D pattern of behavior; he is moving toward the social expectations for his future as an adult. Gavan contends that overconformity is fully as deviant as underconformity, but in the opposite direc- tion. Youths who fall into areas C and E are regarded as members of the social institutions and groups that control area D. They are considered to be "one of ours," erring a little, but to be brought back into the group, disciplined if need be, and forgiven. Over- conformers are urged to get into the swing of things, to enjoy themselves, and not to interfere with other people's fun. Youths who fall into areas B and F are felt to be 31 Cavan, 196h, p. 22 -20.. marginal deviants. The underconformers are warned or arrested by the police, but not referred to the juvenile courts. Overconformers are socially ostra- cized, ignored in invitations to parties, and excluded from.membership in.many groups because it is felt that they hamper activities. The predominant attitude toward these youths is one of reclamation; that is, those people in area D try to encourage those youths in areas B and F to engage in "normal" activities. Those youths in areas A and G are the "real" deviants. Undercon- formers may be expelled from.achools, or may be committed to a correctional institution. Overconformers are socially ostracized and even, at times, held to be in violation of the law (for example, certain religious zealots have been committed to correctional institutions for refusing to serve in the armed forces.) -21.. senate—"em I 0 send 95.33.33 I h 3.: war—06H I u send 309a: edu- «eogaeooe . 3:98.“:an I a seed accuse no Hoonoe .3 coupes 5533.8 I e eehq hos-ems Hoaooe 3 Houseman «codenamed... Haemo- «uuonfiss eofluom I m send Hooaoe deuoavoeunoe 3 as; goofing-nae noose- .nodaoe we: I 4 send nodal—I880 I a eehq Hebgnneedn I u send Hogans coo-31. doggone“. I n so: Hobos-Es 5.3 soda-Bench. I u send Hobos-Es wee-3.3 dogged-on. I 0 send depended-.8 I n so: eeuoo when. 3303.930 I d sand 53038.net eschews I a so: Dion-noose: ensue; I u send 33350905 .352 I u send had-Echoes dunno: I u send 3302333: song I 0 send hogan-noose? evens—vex I m seed gaseous-oceans: g I d dead «meow-.23 5 53838.. to ocean- uo 8.3. 2-38.88 .H .2: a fiOfiflOd.‘ Onflfim new—5.3.1: 0.233 38.3 -22- ooosno» anyone .3369 .enoaaefleu wee Hen-hon hoods «gen wen-«honeyed: awn—«seam hosEI n .394 , 38:98! no wagons Escapade." . wee Hashes stage Bataan «mud-ween use sung-um.- I o seu4 emu-Av no on: goaeeooo Managers“. Ken .35! «hedged-noun «season—Hep Mo honeoueAI m seu4 8333. min «answerer Nee esouhee nee-k «bananas “been?" eel—4I 4 3.3 gogaoofifltsoauog euefio 55 season I h sen4 each-So 25 e e4 I n eeh4 veesaonevnoo mad-.3830 s e4. I n eeu4 passe-”38:0: addressee? .4 I o sen4 «Second-sauna... tosses-consona- asflbo Jeep—saddens e4 I 4 seh4 sends» a no sedan-anew I o een4 eke-3o a.“ honed-3n no 5.33.30 I h eeh4 penance denounce nu eager-so on I M 894 eundaeeu ads» on an no need.» no season-.34 I n sen4 0H9» no eusaaeeu «a no needs» no 3390004 I o een4 sends» a no soapoeneu use easeoneoos see-.5!— ununebe: I m 3.: n «o sends» Mo sodas—”em I 4 e84 Avegdanoov H Quasa- .33 .vaeooooIMHee e .330 .335 et-s» coups: . .326 -23- 9350 .3 3530- uo can 3 8.34395 .hnnoooaa a. 9.333 .8309 no honouov 3. 9353.3 nofli no». anon-m I o 3A4 v.3- nmaonv n25 .335 gaudy—fiv- o: «unoffic- 3" no». .3533: nouonao mo 53.3.30 .ngauooonon ulofio 03 3. won 0.30 2.83 I h 6:4 588 on $43.on on “owing a.“ bade 303...?" No. Foam-ion 5%) Duo SEEP—om I M £4 30330.33 H Ida-n. .8 .a fig .8nuon an A333 Seal-nu .. 2 h- SUMMARY Throughout recorded history, man has sought to explain and.understand social deviation. His expla- nations for this deviation can be grouped under three orientations: biological, psychological, and socio- logical. All of these orientations are based on three common premises: first, deviancy is the result of a malfunctioning in the individual and/or in the society; second, the deviant is the critical variable for the analysis of deviant behavior; and third, human be- havior can be described in terms of a continuum.ranging from.deviant behavior through tolerated behavior to ideal behavior (most people fall between the two rare extremes). This chapter has suggested that these basic premises need not be upheld; in fact, the support of these premises might be detrimental to the complete under- standing of social deviancy. First, social deviancy need not be the result of a.malfunctioning individual or group, but merely the overt expression of a boundary maintenance function in an organized group. Second, social deviancy need not be analyzed via the social deviant, but might be analyzed via the social audience (either rule makers or rule enforcers). Third, human behavior can be described in terms of a continuum.ranging from disapproved underconformity through normal behavior to disapproved overconformity. CHAPTER II EVELOPHENT OF HYPOTHESES AND METHODOLOGY In her Presidential Address to the Midwest Sociological Society in 1961,1 Ruth Shone Cavan intro- duced her hypothetical formulation of a behavior continuum. At that time, she presented three basic hypotheses which had not yet been submitted to empirical testing. The hypotheses were as follows: 1) behavior may be placed on a continuum running from an undercon- forming contraculture through various degrees of disapproved behavior to normal conformity and then through stages of overconforming behavior to an over- conforming contraculture; 2) the reaction of the nor- mally conforming segment of the pOpulation to deviations varies in severity according to the threat posed to the social norms by either under- or overconformity; 3) minor deviants usually are drawn back into conformity, but serious deviants often are treated so severely that they are alienated and withdraw into a contraculture. The purpose of this thesis is to empirically examine Cavan's hypothetical formulations. 1 Ruth Gavan, "The Concepts of Tolerance and Contraculture as applied to Delinquency," Sociological Quarterly, II (1961 2u3-258. -25- -26.. EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE CE :3 BEHAVIOR CONTINUUI-I Cavan's premise that human behavior can be placed on a continuum ranging from.strict adherence to the legal code to serious violations of that code can be supported by empirical research. Most of this research has been identified as an effort to examine the extent of un- recorded crimes and juvenile delinquencies. Porterfield demonstrated the utility of a behavioral continuum.in reference to juvenile delinquencies. Porterfield analyzed 20u9 cases brought before the Fort worth Juvenile Court and compared their records with the admitted delinquencies of a group of 337 college students, composed of 200 men and 137 women. Offenses of the college students were obtained in answer to a question- naire presented personally and returned anonymously. 100% of both the college men and women admitted pre- college offenses. 2 Austin Porterfield, Youth 3g Trouble (Fort Worth, 19,4-6 ) o . -27- .R .n add .855... n 3. 95 can «an d S c.8a 0.8” .88 e8 83a >3 0.2” mad can am am 0.03 c.8a $38 .. 803 oé «.5 2. new 5 5 c.8n 98” 8S. .. 83¢ o.» «.3 .5 3n on. R 98“ c.8H $3» .. 83. ea “.3 em as on 3 0.8” 98." $8 u 83 ed 0.3 e 3 m n c.8a c.8a 83 :38 sgxaHinuipn I... «.3 I... «.3 I an In... 0.8” 383a. #2333. o4 «.2 8 H3 3 o 0.8” 0.8a sassy: e4 93 o3 E. 8 d 98” 98” 383...» Seen «a 95 5 8. t. 3 c.8a 28” 285° .38 in flan a: «.3 mm 3 98” c.8a .3333 I... «.2 II 3 I .s II... c.8a .3333 a; “.3 an «3 on ea 0.03 c.8a 2626 e6 5a Sm o3 nu ma c.8H 0.8” 2°23. .3 .3: 8 8m «a 5 98“ 98” £28.38 «A «A; «on as on mm 98“ 903 853nm all” «in a log... .18.. «3.... 33....“ led: a Human lfiflmfl. III—flag a 238.3 its no .55- .5 no 28: no 26 agar—on nia.nzasx:§n 22:90 on» no one. .3 one wanton-u 59:00.39 one and .5 $0,309.93 5 gnu-“o: RH and :1- cow ago-u canoes». ouoHHoo uguoa_hn coauonou nonnouuo no son's: ouuugnd .HH canon -28.. As Table II shows, these students represented a cross section of all college classes and all economic groups. The offenses themselves were as serious as those com, mitted by the cases referred to the court but, appar- ently, were not as numerous. Porterfield concluded that some of the college students were as delinquent as the juvenile court cases, but society had not seen fit to bring the former into court."L The Cambridge-Somerville Study in Massachusetts gives additional credence to this point of view.5 This study covered material secured through case workers in a delinquency prevention project. The social workers had contacts with sixty-one boys who were never brought to court and forty whose offenses were registered with the court. These boys had committed 6&16 offenses, only ninety-five (1.5 percent of total) of which had ever received official court action. Approximately lhOO were infractions of city ordinances none of which re- sulted in a court complaint; thO were minor offenses and only twenty-seven or 0.6 percent were prosecuted. Of 616 serious offenses, sixty-eight (11 percent) were punished. The study concludes that most juvenile of- fenses, apparently, tend to be hidden and that most h Ibid., Chapter 2. 5 Fred Murphy, Mary Shirley, and Helen Witmer, "The Incidence of Hidden Delinquency,’ American JOurnal g£_ OrthOpsychiatry, XVI (October, l9h6), 686:696. -29- 6 boys commit some juvenile offenses. Impressed by the above findings, James wallerstein and Clement Wyle of the Randen Fbundation in New York City devised a questionnaire listing forty-nine offenses covered by the categories given below.7 The question- naire was submitted to 1020 men and 678 women representing a cross section of the pOpulation and a balanced pro- portion of social religious groups in the metropolitan area of New York City. These persons were asked to indicate whether or not they had committed any of the offenses in the following categories: malicious mischief; disorderly conduct; assault; auto misdemeanors; health law violations; indecency; gambling; larcency; burglary and possession of burglar's tools; robbery and illegal possession of firearms; bribery; falsification and fraud; election frauds; tax evasion; coercion and extortion; conspiracy and compounding a crime; and criminal libel. (Murder was not included in the categories.)8 Answers to the questionnaires indicated that 91 per- cent of the respondents admitted that they had committed offenses after they were sixteen years old. The average number of offenses committed in adult life was 18 for all 6 Ibid., pp. 695-696. 7 James Wallerstein and Clement Wyle, "Our Law- Abiding Law-Breakers," National Probation, (April, 19h7), pp. 107-112. 8 Ibid., p. 109. -30- men respondents with a range of 8.2 for ministers to 20.2 for laborers and ll for all women respondents with a range of 9.8 for laborers to lh.h for those in.military and government work. Wallerstein and Wyle conclude that a sizeable prOportion of adults are "law-abiding law- breakers."9 More recently, Short and Nye compared the confessed Table III: Percentage of 1020 son and 678 women collitting specific offenses in les’Iork City.1° nines: launsailha £hunnailsnnn Malicious mischief 8!. 81 Ifleankuiy4mMMMct 85 76 Asmmfli ‘9 5 lwuasfisduuummrs 61 39 lhdmmmmy 77 74 Gmflfling 7‘ 56 Lsrcenqy 89 83 Grmmiiareuuq'hummpttnnm) 13 11 Anhaimeft 26 8 Ihumiuur 17 4 Romnwy 11 1 Gmmumlulwmunms 35 3 Porjury 23 17 Filsificstion and fraud 46 34 Edmflfloninnmds 7 4 Tax evasion 57 ‘0 Coercion 16 6 Conspiracy 23 7 Gmuuntllibdl 36 29 9 Ibid., pp. 111-112 10 Source: Ibid., p. 110. -31- misbehavior of 2350 public high school students with that of 320 state training school students.11 Question- naires, consisting of twenty-one items of legal delin- quencies translated into language more understandable to adolescents, were anonymously filled out by the two samples. From.the initial twenty—one item.delinquency check list, nine items were selected for scaling on the criteria that (l) the items might measure a common dimension and (2) the offenses were committed by an appreciable proportion of the respondents. The nine items included: driven a car without a driver's license or permit; taken little things (worth less than $2) that did not belong to you; bought or drank beer, wine or liquor (include drinking at home); purposely damaged or destroyed public or private prOperty that did not belong to you; skipped school without a legitimate excuse; had sex relations with a person of the Opposite sex; defied parents' authority to their face; run away from.home; and taken things of medium.value (between $2 and $50).12 Analysis of the completed questionnaires revealed that every offense on the list was checked by 11 James Short and Ivan Nye, "Reported Behavior as a Criterion of Deviant Behavior," Social Problems, V (Winter, 1957—58), 208-213; "Extent-ET’Uhrecorded Juvenile Delinquency: Tentative Conclusions " Journal of Criminal Law, IL (November-December, 1958), 296-302; End—"Scaling Delinquent Behavior," American Sociological Review, XXII (June, 1957), 326-3hl. 12 Ehiég. 1957-58. pp. 208-213. -32- some high school boys and girls, although often only a few. A.much higher percentage of the training school boys and girls checked offenses, and admitted repetition of the offenses. Short and Nye conclude that twenty-two percent of the training school students are less delin- quent than ten percent of the public high school students.13 In short, the above researches hypothesize and empirically demonstrate that human behavior can be placed on a continuum ranging from.strict adherence to criminal codes to serious violations of the codes. Furthermore, most human behavior falls between these two extremes. HYPOTHESES One example of the behavior continuum.is the "honesty" continuum. This continuum would range from armed rob- bery, burglary, and auto theft at one extreme through petty theft and cheating on income tax returns through minor cheating and unauthorized borrowing through bor- rowing only with permission and critism of minor pilfering to the reporting of even minor pilfering to authorities at the other extreme. There are two interpretations of such a continuum. The first interpretation is the traditional interpre- tation: the extreme represented by armed robbery is interpreted as disapproved deviation with the resulting public condemnation; the center area is interpreted as the area of tolerable behavior; and the Opposite extreme 13 Ibid., June, 1957, p. 330. -33... is interpreted as approved deviation or idealized behavior. In short, the traditional viewpoint interprets the honesty continuum as ranging from.dis- honesty at one extreme through "tolerable dishonesty" to honesty at the other extremal"lr The second inter- 15 pretation, stemming, from the work of Gavan, asserts that this continuum would range from deviant under- conformity through normal conformity to deviant over- conformity. Consequently, the public reaction to behaviors at either extreme would be condemnation or disapproval, and the public reaction to behavior be- tween these two areas would be approval. In short, Cavan's interpretation argues that the honesty continuum ranges from dishonesty through honesty to "overhonesty." Table IV summarizes the traditional interpretation and its comparison with Cavan's interpretation of the honesty continuum. lu See Chapter I, pp. 9-17. 15 See Chapter I, pp. 11—17. named—05%” 0330..” «Bach-30.305 .35: I 0 00.3 coached".- haquHnoohecfi each-E ”— m dead doe: 003703 I o 00.4 Havana-ad I h 8.3 Hogan u send a... 3...... i ._ a. o 00.3 aerospace-B I m 00.2 scan-.5008 I d no.3 u o 00.3 3030A . 000302.. 006.394 h send a send bacon—0:03 esp-keno» a 00.2 aces-n 8583-. 0 .82 €30.33 «used-veg corona—no.3 u m 00.3 __ 4 send 033.3500 3 gonad-u hoods nope ton—cu I a 00.3 0.35.0 no 3.33:0 uenouueeeeom 01350 on: 3 «on 0.30 caravan I u send noun-Eon 5.7.3 230 mud-5.309 I u send Egon venue-3:25 «unionism H05": I n 00.3 nor-sad defines. on. eager- I 0 8.2 00.3031» no 50:00.9.“ I m 093 03-5; Expo." e25 .. a 8.: land-E8 duet-.3 .cesoeougfl .- .550 0063.394 033nm 595030 ago-Fog 30533333” 30.3.808- tE-aom £25 finoaefiafioafi .- 3950 3 es. 8338933 303893. on» 0v unfit-"0000 00 a. no ecdve «M30805 5.“: gang bacon—om . PH capo-u .30 .0 :33 .2805.” 08 80 .0 .3000 .030 .55 .3 a 3 a .550 53 0.. 03080038 a .0 00 0.3.0 00 00300000000 I a 00.3 ”03000003 I h 003 10.0.0000 0.005.? 00000300. I n 00.3 200000 035 00000300. I n 003 200000 9005.? 00000300. I o 003 33000008 I m 00.3 0030000008 I 4 003 0000303 0.0.300 a 003 aha-0000030.. . 3000000000000 08002 H h 00.3 030000000000 0.30033 “3000000000000 0000: I n 00.3 00.32. 50.0808 038.. I 0 8.: 1088 580008 0.035000 €030,003 Rm 0.309 -36- Utilizing Cavan's formulation of the honesty continuum, it is hypothesized that: HYPOTHBSIS I: The normally conforming segment of the late adolescent population re- acts unfavorably both to under- conformity (dishonesty) and to overconformity (overhonesty). Furthermore, Cavan theorizes that because of the greater severity of the potential threat to the existing social norms at the extremes on the behavioral continuum, it would be expected that the severity of the reactions to deviations would increase as one moves from area C on 17 the behavioral continuum through area A. Because over- conformity, as well as underconformity, represents a threat to the existing social norms, it would be expected that the severity of reactions to social deviancy would increase as one moves from area B to area G. Thus it is hypothesized that: HYPOTHESIS II: The reaction of the normally conform- ing segments of the late adolescent population to deviations varies in severity according to the degree of either overconformity or undercon- formity. The foregoing hypotheses rest on the contention that l7 Gavan, 22, cit., pp. 18-26. -37.. a characteristic of adolescence is the immense impor- tance of the opinions of the peer group for the adolescents.18 Most research findings suggest that the peer group dominates the adolescent's thinking and his behavior. Most social scientists argue that adolescents are essentially conservative where their own age mates are concerned; that is, adolescents conform both to the opinions and to the appearances of other adolescents re- gardless of their departure from adult standards of conduct, dress, or acceptance of values. It is argued that the motto of the peer group (adolescent) reads: ”One just doesn't do that."19 For example, if mini- skirts, and bleached straight hair are generally worn by adolescent girls, then the girl who wishes to escape the opprobrium of being "different" must wear these styles of fashion; or if long-hair is the latest fad for teenage boys, then this haircut must be adopted by any adolescent boy who wishes to be completely accepted by his peers. If by some chance the adolescent is prevented by the parents from following the ways of the agemates, the 18 See John Horrocks, Ps cholo of Adolescence (Cambridge, 1951), pp. 86-12 3 . oseph Stone, and Josepg Church, Childhood and Adolescenc%O(NewTEOrk, i957), . 2 l-29 ; James Bossard and Eleanor ll e 800 o of a? Child Divelopment (New York, 1966), pp. 363:522; Irene Tasselyn, The Adolescent And His World (New York, 1952); James Coleman, The Adolescent Society (New York, 1961); and H. H. Hammers and D. H. Radler, The American Teenager (New York, 1957). 19 Josselyn, 92, cit., p. 39. -38- adolescent is faced with.a.most embarrassing situation against which.she or he is sure to struggle more or less overtly. To an adolescent the fact that "the other kids are doing it" is the most cogent and overpowering reason for doing a thing, and parents will alienate their sons and daughters by refusing to agree. In short, these theorists argue that the adolescent peer group has iron control of its members. An opposing vieWpoint is presented by Coleman20 and Remmers and Radler.21 Coleman contends that adolescents are not oriented solely to one another; yet the pulls are extremely strong, as the responses in Table V suggest. Hammers and Radler attempted to determine the social Table V: Percentage of males and females responding to the following question: Which one of these things would be the hardest for you to take -- your parents' disapproval, your teacher's disagproval, or breaking with your friends?2 Boys Girls ParentS' Disapproval 53.8% 52.9% Teacherls Disapproval 3.5% 2.7% Breaking wi th Friend [(2.7% A3466 Number of Cases (3621) (389M) orientation of the American Adolescent. Their questions 20 0018171811, 92. Cit.) pp. 1‘57. 21 Remmers, 22, cit., pp. 178-237. 22 Source: Adapted from Coleman, g2. cit., p. 5. -39- set up a number of hypothetical situations and then asked whose opinions or feelings the students considered more important in each situation: people their own age, parents or people of their parents's age, or neither age group. An analysis of the results, summarized in Table VI, suggest that the typical adolescent is responsive to the feelings and opinions of his peers on such questions as what to wear to a party, what club to join, how to act with the gang, and personal grooming. On the other hand, he is sensitive to the feelings and opinions of his parents and other adults about his political feelings, about how to spend his money, and about his personal problems or troub- les. In other words, adolescents are responsive to the pressures of the peer group with regard to some aspects of behavior; but in other areas of behavior, they are more responsive to adult standards. -h0- Table VI: Vhoee advice do adoleecente take?” Parente or Neither One People their people their ie lore in- flnanflsaa sameunL___. .anL______. :mnduai... What to wear to a party: Malee 19$ 12% tube 75‘ 15$ 10‘ Total 72$ 17$ 11‘ How you feel about people of other raoee or nationalitiee: Melee 31$ 33$ 36% Penalee 281 325 395 Total 30$ 32$ 33‘ Your political feeling” 11.1.. 23% 51.5 23% Pelalee 191 55$ 26% tunmi 2x! aux 23: How you epend your money: u.1.. 35% 43% 2°! resales 26$ 53% 215 Total 31$ 49% 20$ 01an you Join: Melee 65$ 20$ 15% renalee 63$ 25% 12% Total 61.1 22$ 141 How to act when out with the gang: Melee 60$ 25% 151 Penal“ 54$ 33$ 13% runs. an! 291 145 Advice on pereonal problem or troublee: Halee 16$ 74$ 10$ Penalee 17$ 76% 71 Total 16$ 75$ 91 Personal crooning (how to comb your hair, how to dreee, etc.): Hales 61$ 18$ 21$ renalee 55$ 25% 20$ Total 58$ 21$ 21% 23 Source: Adapted from More, a. 913., pp. 234-235. -1131- In as much as honesty seems to be more closely related to those questions on which the adolescents where more responsive to the pressures of parents, it is hypothesized that: HYPOTHESIS III: Adolescents will tend to name parents as those most concerned with their observance of rules of honesty. METHODOLOGY This section is concerned with the characteristics of the sample, the techniques of gathering the data, and the definitions of terms. CHARACTERISTICS 9g; SAMPLE Gavan theorizes that each social class or other large subcultural group has its own definition of what be- havior falls into the area of tolerance, what behavior is mildly disapproved, and what behavior is condemned.2u EWen when these groups share a basic culture and verbally accept the social norms, their concepts of approved and disapproved behavior may differ. An example of this discrepancy at the left hand extreme of the continuum is the case of the father whose son was in a correctional school for taking a car for joy riding. The father said, "Of course, he took a few cars, but he did not strip them; he just wanted to use them. He is not a bad boy." But in the eyes of the judge, the boy had stolen the cars. 2h Gavan, g2. cit., p. 27. '1L2- An example of this discrepancy at the right hand extreme of the continuum is the case of attitudes toward petting in the lower-class and in the middle-class adolescents: some middle-class groups regard petting as an acceptable Figure III: Discrepancies between lower-class and middle-cl ss evaluations of identical behavior2 Lower Class Evaluation: A B C D E F G A B C D E F G Middle Class Ehaluation: substitute for premarital intercourse, but the lower- class would regard this replacement as prudish over- conformity. This attitudinal discrepancy would be reflected in the area of honesty. In as much as this hypothesis was not to be tested in this research, the sample was drawn to reflect a homogeneous grouping; however, the homogeneous sample limits the number of important variables which can be employed in the analysis of the data. The sample for this research consists of all single freshmen enrolled in a large section of a sociology class at Michigan State University who reported no prior conviction record and who were under twenty years of 25 Source: Adapted from Ibid., p. 28. -u3_ age.26 All the students in this class filled out the questionnaire (n = 197), but only k9 females and 21 males who fit the above characteristics were included in the final sample. The following individuals were excluded from the final sample: 10h non-freshmen, hB sophomores, 21 juniors, and 35 seniors; 18 persons older than nineteen years of age, 10 twenty years old, 6 twenty-one years old, and 2 twenty-two years or older; h,married persons; and 1 individual who confessed to all possible crimes. Table VII indicates that there are no significant differences between the final sample and the class sample for most major social variable; the only exception were college class, age, and marital status differentials. The majority of the respondents are 18-19 years old, white Single college freshmen whose parents were born in theUnited States. About 50% of the respondents are Protestant, 20% Catholic, 20% Jewish, and approxi- mately 10% report no religious affiliation; furthermore, 26 Also rejected those questionnaires which confessed to all possible crimes'and those filled out in a hap- hazard manner. See questions 16-29 on questionnaire in particular. Table VII: Claee W mm College Olaae: Ireahnan Sophomore Junior Senior Sex: Hale Finale Religion: Proteetaat Catholic Jewiah lone Church Attendance: ‘Heekly Monthly Lees than.nenthly lever Subject-Parent Religion: Sale Different 18': 17 18 19 20 21 22 or older Marital Statae: Single Harried Divorced Hidcw or widower Race: White negro 175 58 32 31 158 34 195 2 -hh, 67.2 24.6 10.7 17.8 32.5 67.5 ‘702 19.8 17.8 15.2 37.1 11.7 27.9 23.6 88.8 11.2 .5 20.9 29.6 16.2 15.7 17.3 80.2 17.3 2.0 .5 99.0 1.0 8anp1e m 70 . 65K)! 26 18 17 388.: Social characterietice of college claae and ealple Utilieed ZIIEIIEIII 100.0 30.0 70.0 48.6 20.0 18.6 12.9 37.1 12.9 25.7 24.3 88.6 11.6 1.6 57.1 61.5 100.0 100.0 ~15- Table VII (Continued) Glass Sample W m: 23mm m: Hone Colnunity: Rural 23 11.7 . 9 . Snell town 30 15.2 11 Large city 35 17.8 12 'Very large city 38 19.3 13 Suburb 64 32.5 22 Class Identification: Lower class 2 1.0 ‘Uorking class 13 6.6 5 Middle 01.... 175 88.2 62 Up”? 613.. 7 3e6 3 Father's Occupation: Professional and large business 55 27.9 19 white collar and snall business 109 55.3 39 Skilled.nanual later 29 14.7 10 Other 4 2.0 2 Father's Education: Grade school graduate 5 2.5 3 Bone high school 22 11.2 9 High school graduate 34 17.3 12 Sons college 71 36.0 22 College graduate 41 20.9 15 Post-graduate 24 12.2 9 Police Contact: lone 115 58.4 40 Minor 72 36e5 26 Serious, infernal 10 5.1 I. Serious, for-a1 Utilised W 12.9 15.7 6.3 17.1 18.6 31.4 7.1 88.6 4.3 27.1 55.7 14.3 2.9 6.3 12.9 17.1 31.6 21.6 12.9 57.1 37.1 5.7 -ue- about 90% of the sample hold the same religious faith as do their parents. h0% of the sample attend church at least once a month, while 25% of the sampe never attend church. The majority of these students are dependent upon their fathers for support, and the majority of these fathers are white collar, small busi- ness, or professional workers. 66% of the fathers has at least attended college, and 96% of the fathers had at least attended high school. About half of the respondents report no "official" contacts with law enforcement agencies, and a majority of those who report contact have had contact only because of minor infractions (traffic violations). Only four respondents had had official contact with police because of major offenses (theft, illegal use of alcohol, and disorderly conduct) but formal charges were never pressed. TECHNIQUE OE GATHERING DATA There are several methods of obtaining desired in- formation from.p0pulations. Because of the advantages associated with the use of a written questionnaire as compared to interviewing or direct observation, this method was utilized for this research. The primary advantage is the reduction of the biasviewpoint' effect;27 that is, this technique gives the respondent 27 Matilda Riley, Sociological Research (New Yerk, 1963). pp. 166-19A- -u7- a sense of privacy and thus the respondent will often report more freely. The questionnaire consists of four sections. Section I, questions 1 to 13, was utilized to gather information on the social characteristics of the respondents. This section was modeled after a questionnaire developed by Vener and Smucker28 and one utilized by Maddox and 29 Mc Call. The questionnaire was further refined accord- ing to the rules of questionnaire-deveIOpment outlined in Goods and Hatt.30 Information secured in this section includes: college class, age, sex, marital status, religious affiliation, church attendance, race, parents' birth.p1ace, residence, education and occupation of ' father, and prior criminal record. Occupational in- formation was coded according to Center's Occupational Index.31 5 Section 11, questions 1h and 15, was intended to identify significant others in relation to the honesty 28 Arthur Vener and Orden Smucker,A udy of Social Rules and Regulations, Michigan State University, East Lansing. 29 Geor e Maddox and Bevode McCall, Drinkinngmong Teen-Agers New Brunswich, 196A). 30 William.Goode and Paul Hatt, Methods ;3 Social Research (New York, 1952). 31 Richard Centers, The Psyphology of Social Class, (Princeton, l9h9), pp. h3-5l; and Bernard Barber, Social Stratification (New York, 1957), 173-175. -u8_ of the respondents; that is, this part was used to identify those peOple the respondents feel are most concerned about the respondents! honesty and whose concern is important to the respondent; this section was also used to identify the relationship existing between those named as significant others and the reSpondents. The following two questions were ems ployed to accomplish this task: Research has found that everyone is concerned about the way others feel toward him. Some peOples' opinions about you are very import- ant to you, while other peOples' opinions are not as important. Below you are asked to list the names of those people MOST IMPORTANT to you. 1h) Name those people most important in your life. Name How is this person related to you? 15) Name those persons you feel are most concerned about how well you obey rules and regulations of honesty. Name How is this person related to you? This technique is a modified form of one utilized by_ Brookover and others in the study of significant others in the learning situation of high school students.32 Section III, questions 16 to 29, was adopted from 32 Wilbur Brookover, Ann Paterson, and Shailer Thomas, "Self-Concept of Ability and School Achievement," U.S. Office of Education, Cooperative Researgh Pro'ect #8 5, (East Lansing, 1962); Wilbur Brookover, JeanlIEPere, n Hamachek, Shailer Thomas, Edsel Erickson, "Improving Academic Achievement Through Students' Self-Concept Enhancement," U.S. Office of Education COOperative Research Pro set #1636, (East Lansing 1965)} andIWilbur Brookover, on amacHek, and Edsel Erickson, "Relation- ship of Self-Concept to Achievement in High School." U.S. Office of Education COOperative Research Project #2831, (East Lansing, 1966). -ua- of the respondents; that is, this part was used to identify those peOple the respondents feel are most concerned about the respondents! honesty and whose concern is important to the respondent; this section was also used to identify the relationship existing between those named as significant others and the reSpondents. The following two questions were em— ployed to accomplish this task: Research has found that everyone is concerned about the way others feel toward him, Some peoples! opinions about you are very import- ant to you, while other peoples! opinions are not as important. Below you are asked to list the names of those people MOST IMPORTANT to you. 1h) Name those people most important in your life. Name How is this person related to you? 15) Name those persons you feel are most concerned about how well you obey rules and regulations of honesty. Name How is this person related to you? This technique is a modified form of one utilized by. Brookover and others in the study of significant others in the learning situation of high school students.32 Section III, questions 16 to 29, was adOpted from 32 Wilbur Brookover, Ann Paterson, and Shailer Thomas, "Self-Concept of Ability and School Achievement," U.S. Office of Education, Cooperative Researgh Pro'ect #8 5, (East Lansing, 1962);2Wilbur Brodkover, Jean 9 ere, n Hamachek, Shailer Thomas, Edsel Erickson, "Improving Academic Achievement Through Students' Self-Concept Enhancement," U.S. Office of Education COOperative Research Project #1636, (East Lansing 1965); and“Wi1bur Brookover,‘Don’HamaChek, and Edsel Erickson, "Relation- ship of Self-Concept to Achievement in High School." U.S. Office of Education C00perative Research Project #2831, (East Lansing, 19667, -ug- a juvenile delinquency scale formulated by Short and Ny6.33 This scale distributes the respondents on a continuum ranging from.1ow delinquency to high delin- quency. Table VIII outlines the scale of delinquency, In addition to the scale of delinquency, three questions Table VIII: Scale of juvenile delinquency as developed by Short and Nye3 Scale Code Offensea 16 17 18 19 2O 21 22 23 2h 25 00 O 0 O O O O 0 O O O 0 01 O l O 0 O O O 0 0 0 0 O2 0 l 1 O 0 O 0 O O 0 0 03 O l 1 l O O 0 O O O 0 0h 0 1 1 1 1 O O O O O O 05 1 2 1 1 1 O O O O O 0 O6 1 2 1 1 1 1 O 0 0 O 0 07 1 2 1 l 1 1 1 O 0 O 0 08 1 2 1 2 l 1 l O O O O 09 1 2 1 2 2 l 1 0 O O O 10 2 2 l 2 2 2 l O O O O 11 2 2 1 2 2 2 l 1 O O O 12 2 2 1 2 2 2 1 1 1 O 0 13 2 2 2 2 2 2 1 1 1 O 0 1h 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 l 1 O O 15 3 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 1 O 0 l6 3 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 1 1 O 17 3 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 1 1 1 18 3 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 l 1 l9 3 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 1 20 3 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 *Offense numbers refer to the reSpective questions on the study questionnaire. On the Short and Nye scale, the offenses are numbered from 1 to 10. questions were included to ascertain the degree of 33 Short and Nye, 22 cit. (Winter, 1957-58), p. 208. 34 Source: Ibid. -50- overconformity on the part of the respondents. In section IV, respondents were asked to indicate their reaction to fictitious persons in various stages of conformity to rules and laws pertaining to honesty as presented in five short stories. Question 30 was concerned with cheating: those who cheat, those who do not cheat, and those who report cheating. Question 31 was concerned with minor trespassing: those who would trespass, those who would not trespass, and those who would report minor trespassing. Questions 32 was concerned with petty theft: those who would steal small items, those who would not steal small items, and those who would report the theft of even small items to the authorities. Question 33 was concerned with burglary: those who break and enter with intent to commit theft, those who would not, and those who would report this activity to the police. Question 3h was concerned.with auto theft: those who would steal a car, those who would not steal a car, and those who would report auto theft to the police. Table IX.summarizes the character- istics of the actors in these stories. PRETEST An initial questionnaire was constructed, and administered as a pretest to high school senior classes in the Lansing area. This original questionnaire was similar to the one utilized in this research except for -51- the last section; that is, the original questionnaire utilized a revised version of the Bogardus Ethnic Distance Scale.35 In this social distance scale, the respondent is presented a list of questions which give him an opportunity to declare his desired social distance toward his sterotype of a category of individuals. The types of statements he is to respond to either negatively or positively with respect to each category of individ- uals are as follows: 1. WOuld exclude from our town, if possible. 2. Would allow to live in.my town, but would prefer in another section of town. 3. Would allow in my school, but prefer in other classes. h. Would accept as classmates in my room at school. 5. Would allow in.my social club. 6. Neuld accept in my home as a dinner guest. 7. would like as a date, or a "special" personal friend. It was apparent from.the pretest that the social dis- tance question needed to be made less ambiguous. 35 Emory Bogardus, "Race Reactions by Sexes, Sociology and Social Research, XLIII (July-AuguSt: I959),H39:HF1; and Distance Changes in the United States During the Past Thirty Years," Sociolo and Social Research, XLIII (November-December, 1958), m-IBSe -52- Table 1!: Characteristics of overt behavior represented by fictitious persons in stories used in stuw m Story 30: Ed Bill Glen Joe Story 31: Dick George San Story 32: Tom and Jin Carl Story 33: Pete Earl Jack Bruce Story 31.: John Bob W Would not cheat, even though in trouble Would initiate cheating to help himself Would cheat to help friend Would report cheating to teacher Would initiate ninor trespassing Would not engage in minor trespassing, and would not report these who would Would not engage in minor trespassing, but would report those who would Would initiate ”petty theft' Would not engage in petty theft, but would utilise the stolen goods Would not engage in petty theft, but would report petty theft to authorities Would not engage in burglary, and would report burglary to authorities Would initiate burglary Would not engage in burglary, and would not report those who did Would assist friend in burglary Would initiate auto theft Would not assist in auto theft, and would report these who did to authorities Would not assist in auto theft, and would not report these who did -53- Consequently, the social distance scale was replaced by the story-reaction technique. After the questionnaire was revised, it was again administered to another sample of high school seniors at Pewamo-Westpalphia High School. Two versions of the story-reactions were used in this testing: one using open ended responses, and the other using structured responses. The analysis of this pretest indicated that not enough additional information was obtained from the open ended responses to warrant their use. On the basis of the results of the pretests, a final draft of the questionnaire was developed. When the scheduled administration of the ques- tionnaire in Muskegon High School was cancelled,36 the decision to utilize a sample of high school seniors was re-evaluated. It was decided that, since college fresh- men are in the final stages of gaining their independence from their parents and are most susceptible to the in- fluence of peers, a sample of college freshmen would be less likely than a sample of high school seniors to demonstrate the pervasive influence of their parents. Thus by utilizing a sample of college freshmen, the re- sults would be the least likely to show the validity of Hypothesis III. In as much as the utilization of 36 Scheduled testing in a high school in the Huskegon area cancelled due to possible effect on mill- age vote. -5u_ a college freshman sample rather than a high school senior sample would not drastically influence the results of Hypotheses I and II, the decision was made to use a sample of college freshmen. As the samples used in the pro-tests of the ques- tionnaires were high school seniors and the sample to be used in the final administration was college fresh- men, the researcher retested the instrument using a small sample of college freshmen. The shortcomings re- vealed by this informal probing were taken into consid- eration in the final development of the instrument. On the basis of the results of the pretests, a final draft of the questionnaire was developed and ad- ministered. DEFINITION 03 TERMS Adolescence: Adolescence is that period of life be- ginning with the pubescent growth spurt and ending with full social maturity. Adolescence is a social-cultural phenomenon (that stage of social life between childhood and adulthood); while pubescence (that physical period of about two years preceding puberty, and the physical changes occuring during that time) and puberty (that physical point of development at which the biological changes of pubescence reach a climax.marked by the in- dicators of sexual maturity) are biological phenomenon; -55- and juvenile (that age-level during which.a youngster can be tried as a juvenile offendor: usually between the ages of ten and seventeen in America, but reaching as low as seven and as high as nineteen at times) is a legal concept. For the general purposes of this thesis, we need not concern ourselves with the specific delinea- tion of particular boundaries for each of the above categories. Juvenile delinquency: Delinquency is both a legal con- cept and a sociological concept. Delinquency as a legal concept refers to that behavior which the people of a state and their leaders believe to a threat to public safety or a hindrance to the best development of the child, and whose prohibition they have incorporated into law. Delinquency as a sociological concept refers to that behavior which peeple identify as delinquency and react to asdelinquency. The usual confusion surrounding this concept in criminological research and literature concerns the demarcation of behavioral acts as delinquent. This confusion is often the result of the failure to dis- tinquish delinquency as a legal concept from delinquency as a sociological concept. The positions of this debate can be summarized as follows: some argue that the commission of an illegal act constitutes delinquency, others contend that an illegal act is delinquent only when it is brought to the attention of official agents, -56- other argue that delinquency occurs only when an offender is brought to the attention of the court, others argue that delinquency occurs only when an individual is adjudicated as a delinquent, and others contend that delinquency occurs only when an individual is labeled as a delinquent by society regardless of the individual's interaction with the legal system. This debate is of little concern for this particular thesis and will not be settled herein. However, this author maintains that the last position is the most fruitful sociologically. Deviant Behavior: Deviant behavior is that behavior labelled by the social audience as an infraction of societal rules and regulations, and to which resulting sanctions and consequences are applied. In essence, the deviant is one to whom the deviant label has successfully been applied; deviant behavior is be— havior that people so label. Underconformity: Underconformity is an exaggeration of the tolerance allowed by the modal norms. Overconformity: Overconformity is an exaggeration of the strict observance of formal social norms.37p 37 Values are ideals or ultimate goals. Social norms are the specific formulations to implement values. Modal norms are the practical, attainable formulations which tolerate some flexibility and minor deviations. For adequate functioning of society, a balance must be maintained between the rigid social norms and the more flexible modal behavior. -57.. Attitude: Attitude refers to relatively enduring system.of affective, evaluative reactions based up- on and reflecting the evaluative concepts or beliefs which have been learned about the characteristics of a social object or class of social objects.38 That is, an attitude is the positive, neutral, or negative feelings toward an object or class of objects. The attitude should always be distinquished from the overt behavior presumably related to it. Significant others: Significant other is a person defined as important by an individual to that individ- ual and whose expectations for behavior influence the behavior of that individual. Significant others oper- ationally defined are those listed in answer to the following question: Name those persons you feel are most concerned about how well you obey rules and regulations of honesty. Honesty: The character or quality of being honest; that is, not cheating, stealing, or lying. 38 Marvin Shaw and Jack Wright, Scales for-the Measurement 9£_Attitudes (New York, 1967). CHAPTER III RESULTS This chapter will focus on the analysis of the findings of the research conducted for this study. Generally, the discussion will examine the reactions of the normally conforming adolescents to various degrees of conformity (underconformity - conformity - overconformity). In addition, the discussion will also examine those who influence the adolescent's conformity to the rules and regulations of honesty. ATTITUDES TOWARD UNDERCONFORMITY AND OVERCONFORMITY Hypothesis I asserts that the normally conforming segment of the late adolescent population reacts neg— atively both to underconformity (dishonesty) and to overconformity (overhonesty). Underconformity is an exaggeration of the tolerance allowed by modal norms. In terms of honesty, underconformity consists of those behavioral acts labeled as theft, cheating, or tres- passing. Overconformity is also an exaggeration of the tolerance allowed by modal norms. In terms of honesty, overconformity consists of those behavioral acts such as strict adherence to the rules of honesty and "tattle-taling" on others. In short, hypothesis -53- -59- I suggests that the normally conforming segment of the late adolescent population disapproves both of underconformity (not strict enough adherence to the rules of honesty) and of overconformity (too strict adherence to the rules of honesty). If hypothesis I is valid, one would expect that the college freshman sample would express disapproval both of those fictitious persons representing dis- honesty and of those persons representing overhon- esty. The results of this study support this conten- tion. Table X reports a summary of the data pertaining to this hypothesis; that is, Table X shows the per- centage of students in the college freshman sample who report approval or disapproval of behaviors reflecting various degrees of conformity to the modal norms of honesty. The table indicates that a majority of the students in the sample disapprove of those who engage in cheating, petty larcency, burglary, and auto theft. The table also shows that a majority of the students disapprove of those who report minor tres- passing (swimming in a private lake), who report minor theft (stealing watermelons), or who report cheating. On the other hand, a majority of the students express approval of those who would report auto theft or burglary, and of those who would not engage in auto -60... theft, cheating, or trespassing. In short, the students in the college freshman sample expressed disapproval of underconforming behaviors and of overconforming behaviors; but they expressed approval of the conforming behaviors between these two extremes. By arbitrarily assigning a numerical value to each attitudinal response (approval: 0; no opinion: 1; and disapproval: 2) and determining the median attitu- dinal values toward various degrees of conforming behaviors, these behaviors can then be ranked according to the magnitude of their median attitudinal values. The following behaviors received a median value of 2 (disapproval): auto theft, burglary, cheating, petty larcency, reporting minor trespassing, reporting minor theft, and reporting cheating. The following behaviors received a median attitudinal value of O (approval): reporting burglary, refusing to commit auto theft, neither committing nor reporting cheating, and neither committing nor reporting minor trespassing. As shown in Table X, one can identify some of the behaviors receiving a median score of 2 as underconformity and others as overconformity. Thus the placement of the underconforming behavior at the bottom of the table and the overconforming behavior at the top of the table re- sults in a behavioral continuum similar to Cavan's hypothesized continuum. By converting the data from tabular form (Table X) aecpe we unseen eon venoenaene on assesses us coupons masseuse sweeps nanosepun eeanecnee octane «sauces seawee- on confidanpnu wt é o hwmrIIupIIIDRV InImupmpnmIIhrw mununurIIhpIhmp. werequowe.weupcodpnn denunc- uenweea on aommwmmmmu neuron menmnme n mneoene unseen .mwmmmml .mwmmmw ”amend Ipuow aneuueaa: pun Amen. w >.u o HM.o um m~.o m woven» space are»& wevoud eweeawun xenon» acne arena Amoco as “0.0 He up.» no ~m.a .u mavens cewmueuw cheev uh qq.u u 4.H HM Hm.e o zepdweu oownpé new Havana drone Arenaqv mm mm.o o H~.o u b.w o ueneuew ooanun non woven» . -62- add...” o.ww on n..nm He n.oH o 0.0 o o.o o o.o o AHA-av .5nuHuunm 3 R E 3 8 no 3&5 Bot.» xanvgzw Huronnwi-Hn Hupounwunan aoHano ngbn d. Huron m4 undue-Haas ouoabunvm mass.» on 28a» 8% Aooaudanoov HHN QHAQH -75- underconformity (dishonesty). Another measure of the relationship between the degree of conformity and the severity of the response to that conformity is a median value calculated for the attitudinal responses. This median attitudinal value was calculated by arbitrarily assigning a numerical value to the various degrees of approval-disapproval: strong approval - O; approval - 1; no Opinion - 2; disapproval - 3; and strong disapproval - n. Thus the lower the median attitudinal value, the greater the degree of approval toward that behavioral category. Conversely, a high.median attitudinal value indicates high disapproval of the behavior. The median attitudinal values for the male college sample are reported in Table XI; the values for the female students are reported in Table XII; and the values for the total college sample are reported in Table XIII. Statistical analysis of the differences between the median values for the males and females indicates that the differences are not statistically significant.1 The only two exceptions were the following: the male students were more disapproving of those who would report minor trespassing than were the females, l The median test, involving the use of Chi square, was utilized to test for statistical significance. For a detailed description of this test, see Sidney Siegel, Nonparametric Statistics For The Behavioral Sciences (New York, 1956). -76- H 0.0 o m.< n 0.0 o H.bm oq 0.0m Hm o 0.0 o v.H H o.» o o.on Hm coco «v H «.H H QN u «v.3 a 98 an Qua mm H n.¢ n <.HH o H.b m romo we w.HH a n.H w.HH n H.bH NH <.H~ «H b.n¢ an n.< n n H.bH NH n.6 q Hfim om QS on QNH m. «A H 335 uaHouunuoua nonH: «4 a 3M. 8 3h 3 0.8 «H «.H H 30o: 9.? ahoncu no: «HI-60 nonaHoz a m u 3w n mm m J... a ‘ Illuflflgéqlll 3 5 .3 3 2: no .8.»on 33.3» uuchx. ngounaaan Hupoumauan aoHano Hupbunnd Hu>onmn< mnHvooHuom nuoupuavm uaouvm on vacuum Ho I a av Anagpaoov HHHN 0.3.08 -73- although both sexes disapproved of this behavior;2 and the females were more disapproving of petty larcency (stealing watermelon from a farmer) than were their counterparts, however both the males and the females disapproved of this behavior.3 The median scores reported in Tables XI, XII, and XIII support Hypothesis II. The median attitudinal values increase as the degree of the nonconformity in- creases. Those behaviors identifiable as conformity to the modal norms received relatively low median attitu- dinal values indicating approval: the median value for those who would neither commit nor report cheating was 0 (strong approval); the median value for those who would neither commit nor report minor trespassing or auto theft and for those who would report burglary was 1; and the median value for those who would report auto theft was 1.5. As behavior departs from the modal norms, the extent of disapproval increases: minor trespassing and failure to report burglary received median attitudinal scores of 2 the reporting of minor trespassing, minor theft, or ‘0. cheating, as well as cheating and petty larcency received scores of 3; and burglary and auto theft received scores of u. However, the median attitudinal values for over- conformity (overhonesty) did not increase to the same 2 This difference was statistically significant at the .10 level. 3 This difference was statistically significant at the .05 level. Figure V: Categories of Con-- mum. San Carl Joe Bob Pete Larry Ed George Jack Dick Ton-Jin Glen Dave Bill Bruce Earl John -79- Graphic presentation of the nedian attitudinal responses of the students in the college freshnen sample toward various degrees of conformity M tt tn 7 s Strong lo Strong Approval roval Opinion Disapproval Disa prevail to App? 2L 3) 3L fines are used in this graph rather than the complete description of each category. For a complete description of each category, see Tables II, II, III, and XIII. -80... prOportion as the median attitudinal values for under- conformity (dishonest). Another technique applicable for showing the relation- ship of degree of conformity-nonconformity and of the response to that conformity-nonconformity is the con- version of the tabular data (Table XIII) into graphic data (Figure V). If Hypothesis II is valid, Figure IV should show a curvilinear relationship. That is, the median attitudinal values for extreme underconformity should be high; the median attitudinal values should decrease as conformity is approached; and then the values should increase as the degree of overconformity increases. The chart in Figure V supports this contention. The be- haviors at the top of the chart represent overconformity; the behaviors in the center represent conforming behavior; the behaviors at the bottom.of the chart represent under- conforming behavior. The median values at the extreme right of the chart represent strong disapproval of the behavior by the college sample; the median values at the extreme left represent strong approval; and those median values between these two extremes represent the various degrees of approval-disapproval between the extremes. Those behaviors at the top of the chart (overconformity or overhonesty) receive relatively high.median attitudinal values (disapproval); those in the middle of the chart (various degrees of overconformity) receive progressively smaller values until the minimal median value (strong -81.. approval) is reached; and those behaviors in the bottom half of the chart (various degrees of underconformity) receive progressively higher values. However, the graph shows that the median attitudinal values for undercon- formity (dishonesty) are greater than the median at- titudinal values for overconformity (overhonesty). In summary, Hypothesis II suggests that the re- action to deviations by the normally conforming segment of the late adolescent population varies in severity according to the degree of either overconformity or underconformity. The data of this study suggest that the severity of the reactions to deviations do vary with the degree of either overconformity or underconformity; however, the data suggest the reaction to overconformity is not as severe as to underconformity.“ SIGNIFICANT OTHERS NAMED §X_5AMPLE In Chapter II, it was hypothesized that, for problems of honesty, adolescents are most responsive to the influence of parents. As college freshmen are in the final stages of gaining their independence from their parents and are most susceptible to the influence of peers, this group would be the least likely to demonstrate the n One explanation for the lack of complete Support for this hypothesis by the data is that those behaviors designated as overconformity were not deviant to the same degree as those behaviors designated as underconformity. That is, the data focused on a limited range of the be- havioral continuum, and had a broader range been used, the data might have supported the hypothesis. -62.. pervasive influence of their parents. Thus by utilizing a sample of college freshmen, the results would be the least likely to show the validity of Hypothesis III. One measure of this influence is a listing of those persons Wthh adolescents feel are most concerned with their observance of the rules of honesty. Table XIV summarizes the number and percentage of the students in the freshman sample who named at least one person from the following categories of significant others as being concerned about how well the students follow the rules of honesty; parents or people their parents' age, peeple their own age, unclassifiables, or self.5 Almost all of the respondents named their parents or peOple their parents' age (100% of the males, 9h% of the females, and 96% of the total sample). In comparison to this, only half of the males (52%), three fourths of the females (71%), and two third of the total sample (67%) named people their own age. 5 Categories utilized in Tables XIV and XV are similar to those employed by Remmers. See Chapter II, pp. 28-30; and H. H. Hammers and D. H. Radler, Th2 American Teenager (New Yerk, 1957). -83- Table XIV: Number and percentage of students in freshman sample who named at least one person from.the follow- ing categories of significant others as being concerned about "How well you obey the rules of honest?" Males Females Total (n=21) (n=b,9) (n=70) Significant Others No. Percent No. Percent No. Percent Parents or People Their age 21 100.0 as 93.9 67 95.7 People Their Own A86 11 52.h 36 73.5 h? 67.1 Unclassifiable (Society, God, Pet, etc.) 3 lh.3 11 22.u 1a 20.0 Ego (self) 2 9.5 2 u.1 h 5.7 Table XV is based on the assumption that the order of listing is relevant; that is, those persons listed first are more important to the adolescent than those listed subsequently.6 A.majority of the students in the college freshman sample (51% of the males, 66% of the females, and 86% of the total sample) named their parents or people their parents' age first in response to the following item; Name those persons you feel are most concerned about how well you obey the rules and regula- tions of honesty. Only a small percentage named_pe0ple 6 The validity of this assumption is based on interviews with a group of ten of the respondents. Al. though the generality of these findings is limited, the researcher feels that this assumption is warranted. -su- Table XV: Number and percentage of college fresh- men who named a person from one of the following categories of significant others first in response to "Name those persons you feel are most concerned about how well you obey the rules and regulations of honesty." Males Pbmales_ TQ§§1__ (n=21) (n=u9) (n-7o) Significant Others No. Percent No. Percent No. Percent Parents or People Their Age 17 81.0 M3 87.8 60 65.7 People Their Own Age 3 1h.3 S 10.2 o 11.h Ego (Self) l h.d O 0.0 1 l.h Unclassifiables (society, God, Pet, etc.) 0 0.0 l 2.0 1 l.h Total 21 100.0 A9 100.0 70 100.0 their own age (1h% of the males, 10% of the females, and 11% of the total sample). Thus the data in Tables XIV and XV support Hypothesis III; that is, the tables in— dicate that this college freshman sample is more reapon- sive to parents or to people their parentsI age than to people the respondents' own age. ' The existing relationship can better be described by replacing the categories utilized in Tables XIV and XV with a set of more delineated categories. Table XVI summarizes the number and percentage of students in the freshman sample who named at least one person from the -85- following categories of significant others as being concerned about how well they obey the rules of hon- esty: parents, adult relatives, unrelated adults, age level relatives, peers (unrelated), ego (self), and unclassifiables. The data in this table indicate that about 90% of the college freshman sample named both parents when asked to name those persons most concerned about how well they followed the rules and regulations of honesty; approximately 97% named at least one parent. Peers were the next most frequently named group with 57% of the sample mentioning at least one peer. Age level relatives (30% of the sample named at least one) and adults, other than parents (27% of the sample named at least one adult relative, and 23% named at least one unrelated adult) were named half as frequently as were peers. In short, a rank ordering of the signi- ficant others by frequency of listing would be as follows: parents, peers, age level relatives, adult relatives, and unrelated adults. In addition, Table XVI indicates that females are mentioned as significant others more often than are males; this is especially true for non-relatives. As a group, males tended to name fewer persons than did females (see Table XVI). There seems to be no difference in the percentage of females and in the per- centage of males who named at least one parent (98% of -86- o.on Hm «.HN an o.mH nu $.NN 0H H.bH NH «.HH 0 Hora @a down on «.HN «H H.b¢ mo ooo¢ no b.n¢ be «.H@ «o 3 ate Jan! bowm bu «.NN AH comm AH n.0N an «own ¢ n.¢d b b.qn he as... .: m.eu ma o.wo we m.H® av aomo >< ¢.mo we a 3.73 [Haiti o.¢H o.¢d m.o mo¢H n.1H w.< «.0 n.o moo Nona bomm ~.n¢ homo g suns ‘ shy-oaoa.uo uoflsu can swap so» Hdon.sbnu ononu vengeance undo: no sheave vquouuunuu- no uoaaouovuo uuaaodapu onv_-afiu cannon one pound as noun: on:.oagauu unannonu a“ caucus». no oudpaoonon van moans: N~t~t HMM dual .8 :33 3 can...» .3. . 83.3“ H23 .3 .8 :8.” 3 cad-3h .3. 5.93 c3355 and uCUOH ad cad-3h 0H1: unbavddom vdncd one vudon ed uvnludm Avon Odd-3h CHI: «vacuum gassed—Ea “ku oanua -87— OoOOH Ob 98 3 a.“ q 3.“ 3 a.b< mm H.bn em 34% A215 Ila-flu 063 o... 98“ a tan d «.3 n H.< m u.a a «.3 on 3.4 2 o.o¢ «u ¢.N< a o.e« cm o.o~ o a g 83: Sun: a 131ml. Ages—03 E 0.3.9 Haven. 13. .3" 538m .33 outfit-£25 €73 own oac cocoa ed cud-3h 0H1: . Age-Hgv zoom g -88.. «.H OoN H H o.o o H38. o6 o o.o m... o.o .ol , .18.... «.H H o.~ H 3. o .3. 8:338 .75 a: 3.. u H4 a o.o o . H38 mam « H.» u o6 o oils o.o o 3. o 0.0 o .1: 53: 35.8: 92 w. u.» a «2.: n H38. 5...“ a Hé a 2. u .Haflu n4 m H4 a a; H .1: “Avoaunounpv .uoom 93 mm 5.8 3 98 pH Hats «3 u o.« H as H 4H8 .33 «.H H o.~ H o6 o 35 .3. e65 mm 92. an «.2. 0H 38.3 5.8 . ”vacuum mug glam 3 Hanna-«Hug AObuav Aasnav Aaunnv macaw an. landfill nonrandom no uneavufismou on. quash one Arno so» Has:.:on anon. conuoouoo puon_ouu Hoou no» uncanon enema glaze on uncommon ca aukuu sheave vauogudsmuu no ooauomoaao muaaoaapu on» no one loan noauoa u mess: on: addsuu nuanuohu nu nus-esp. no oudvnoonom and non-:2 .HHbN CHAIR OoOOH 0% OoOOH HN OoN H 0.0 o 0.0 0 w.# H 3 34m 5.3 :95 ‘ Jada. aHfll Acosndvnouv HHbN oaadh H.909 A.opo .aom .eoc .huoHoomv nonncnuaouuaosp can uuummmummqmduamudm -90- the females, and 95% of the males did so). However, the male respondents named a smaller percentage of peers (61% of the females named at least one peer, while only h8% of the males did so), age level rela- tives (35% of the females named at least one age level relative, and 19% of the males did so), unrelated adults (at least one unrelated adult was named by 27% of the females and lu% of the males), and adult relatives (35% of the female respondents named at least one adult relative, but only 10% of the males did so). Further- more, male respondents tended to name members of their own sex less often than did the female respondents. This was especially true for unrelated peers: M9% of the females named a female peer, and hl% named a male peer; conversely, A3% of the males named a female peer, while only 29% named a male peer. Table XVII summarizes the number and percentage of students in the college freshman sample who named a person from one of the following categories of signi- ficant others first when asked question fifteenz7 parents, peers, unrelated adults, age level relatives, ego, and unclassifiables. 7 Question fifteen is as follows: 15) Name those persons you feel are most concerned how well you obey rules and regulations of honesty. Name How is this person related to you? -91- a.» n e.“ N o.m N m.bh Hm m.e~ HH c.0b mw n.bH b n.>H b m.~H n o.mo on m.mo on e.me mm n.~m an are lanai one o org 0 or. o o.ne “H H.~e 0 «.mo nH H.H~ q e.Hm o n.oH « o.8H eH HueoH eH o.ooH eH o.8H eH grandam 8W3 a sheave peeeHuHemHe Me eeHuewevee unfiaeafleu can we see. menu necked one ended as venue eh: edqseu vueaonn ea emeaeeenem use noses: .HHHbu capia n.