MECHANOSENSORY PROJECTIONS FROM MOUTH PARTS j ‘ . . T0 VENTROBASAL THALAMUS m opossum. - ,SQUIRREL MONKEY, Aeoun, CAT AND moon _ .; ' . - ' - Thesis for the Degree of MS. 3 ' MICHiGAN STATE UNIVERSITY ROCCO A. BOMBARDiERI, Jr. 1971 H125.) L I B R A R Y Michigan State University ABSTRACT momosmsom PROJECTIONS mom MOUTH PARTS 'ro VENTROBASAL rmuuus IN opossum, SQUIRREL MONKEY, AGOU‘I'I, on, AND mccoon By Rocco A. Bombardieri, Jr. The mechanosensory projections from mouth parts were examined in the ventrobasal complex of the thalamus in the opossum, Didglphig gagggpigligj the squirrel monkey; §gig§£i sc‘ us: the acouti. W m: th- cat. mu m: and the raccoon, zxggygg,1312;, Microelectrode recording techniques were used in acute experiments to determine the character (;,g, ipsilateral or contralateral) and organisa- tion of projections from the intraoral surfaces primarily and secondarily from the perioral surfaces. In the five species studied the perioral representation in the thalamus is primarily contralateral. It was shown that in the opossum the projections from the intraoral surfaces are essentially completely contralateral. A small ipsilateral representation was found in the thalamus of the squirrel monkey and the agouti but the projections are primarily contralateral. Both the cat and the raccoon show a large ipsilateral represen- tation and in the case of the raccoon the contralateral component is greatly reduced. These data are compared to those of Cabral and Johnson (1971) on the sheep, Qxillggigg, They reported a large ipsilateral representation from the perioral surfaces and an essentially completely ipsilateral representation from the intraoral surfaces. Rocco A. Bombardieri, Jr. In the contralateral projections to the ventrobasal thalamus of the opossum, agouti, and squirrel monkey the dorsal or maxillary mouth parts project dorsally to the ventral or mandibular mouth parts. In the cat the intraoral ipsilateral projections are ventromedial to the contra- lateral projections. In the raccoon thalamus the teeth project dorsally to the other mouth parts. No consistent organization was demonstrated withinrthe projection pattern of the teeth themselves. Projecting below the teeth is the palate and below that either the tongue or the incisor pad, with a tendency for the tongue to project more medially and the incisor pad more laterally. In every species studied most of the intraoral projections are from the teeth. In the agouti only the incisors were seen to project to the ventrobasal complex. Every subject species also had projections to the thalamus from the dorsal surface of the tongue. Projections were seen from the gum in the opossum; from the mucous membrane surrounding the lower incisors in the agouti; from the ventral surface of the tongue in the cat; and from the check, the rostral throat area and the pad just posterior to the upper incisors in the raccoon. It is suggested that the variation in character of the projections to the ventrobasal complex reflects the major subdivisions (cohorts) of the class Mammalia as defined by Simpson (1945). An alternate hypothesis is suggested in which the variations represent specialisations of groups Rocco A. Bombardieri, Jr. lower than cohort. MECHANOSENSORY PROJECTIONS FROM MOUTH PARTS TO VENTROBASAL THALAMUS IN opossum, SQUIRREL MONKEY, AGOUTI, CAT AND RACCOON "\- ‘ By Rocco A3 Bombardieri, Jr. A THESIS Submitted to MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Department of Zoology 1971 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I gratefully extend my appreciation to: Dr. John I. Johnson, Jr. who served as chairman of the research committee and who provided assistance and encouragement throughout the course of this work; Drs. Robert Raisler and Rollin Baker who served as committee members and assisted in the prepara- tion of the manuscript; Dr. Richard V. Dukelow for assistance in the procurement and handling of squirrel monkeys; Dr. Gilberto B. Campos for assistance in analysing the agouti data; Andrew Harton, Grace Kim, Emeline Haight, and Kristi Dege for their work in histological preparation of the tissue; John R. Haight for maintenance of the electronic equipment; Paul Herron for photographic assistance; and Joan Bernstein and Dan Hoekema for assistance in the collection of data. This research was supported by NIH training grant GM 01751 and research grant NS 05982. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES Introduction Subjects and Preparation Recording Apparatus Response Criteria Mapping Procedures Tissue Preparation Results Overview Opossum Squirrel Monkey Agouti Cat Raccoon Discussion Bibliography General References iii .s-s-s-a-a "30““ 19 24 36 42 62 iv 19 55 59 LIST OF TABLES TABLE 1 Subjects and Treatments 2 Summary of Character of Responses 3 Intraoral Structures Eliciting a Response in Vb. 4 List of Abbreviations iv PAGE 12 20 22 25 FIGURE 1. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. LIST OF FIGURES Diagrammatic representation of mechanoreceptor projections in the ventrobasal thalamus of sheep (Cabral and Johnson, 1971) and raccoons (Velker and Johnson, 1965) Ma'or extant Mammalian groups as de ined by Simpson (1945). Samples of criterion responses, unit clusters, in the five species studied. Projection pattern in a coronal row of punctures in opossum 553. Projection pattern in a coronal row of punctures in opossum 556. Projection pattern in a coronal row of punctures in squirrel monkey 562. Projection pattern in a coronal row of punctures in squirrel monkey 563. Pattern of projections in a coronal row of punctures in agouti 593. Pattern of projections in a coronal row of punctures in agouti 591. Pattern of projections in a corOnal row of punctures in cat 560. Pattern of projections in a coronal row of punctures in cat 557. Pattern of projections in a rasagittal row of punctures in raccoon 65. Pattern of projections in a coronal row of punctures in raccoon 564. PAGE 10 15 26 28 32 34 38 4O 45 47 51 53 INTRODUCTION The ventrobasal complex of the mammalian thalamus (vb) represents an area of second order synapses of the medial lemniscal system and displays a somatotopic organisation of mechanosensory projections from the body surface (Rose and Mountcastle, 1959). The pattern of projections from body parts in vb forms a figure, often with areal distortions, which, in coronal section, depicts a lateral view of the body surface. Figure 1 demonstrates the representation pattern in the raccoon and in the sheep. The caudal body parts are represented laterally and the rostral parts are represented medially. The axial body parts are represented dorsally and the more distal parts ventrally in the thalamus. Note also the large forepaw in the schematic raccoon in Figure 1. This indicates the large amount of thalamic tissue devoted to projections from the forepaw in the raccoon. In the sheep there is a large face and intraoral projection area. In most animals studied the projections from the trunk and limbs are all from the contralateral (cl) side and the projections from the head and mouth are from either the cl side, or the ipsilateral (ips) side, or from both sides of the body. It should be noted that the above description is generalised and each animal displays some specialisations. One portion of vb which is the subject of much confusion in the literature is the most medial region, 1.1,, that region receiving projections from the perioral and the 1 Figure 1. Diagrammatic representation of mechanoreceptor projections in the ventrobasal thalamus of sheep (Cabral and Johnson, 1971) and raccoons (Velker and Johnson, 1965). Top: Diagram to show location of this region in the brains. Center: Outline diagram of coronal section through the thalamus showing approximate locations of projections from ipsilateral and contralateral body parts. Bottom: A pictorial representation of these projections, which for raccoons anticipates data from more caudal planes. A separate ipsilateral representation of tissues inside the mouth occurs in both animals, with tongue projections ventral- most; just dorsal to these are projections from the palate and upper teeth; .next dorsal are projections from the lower gums and teeth. In addition, in sheep there is a massive projection from the ipsilateral face, largely from the nose, ventralmost, the hairy protions of skin between the nose and lips dorsal to these glabrous pipillae of upper and lower lips next, and dorsai-most, from hairy skin between lips and chénS (Figure and caption from Johnson, Hatton, and Rubel, 19 9 . V) RACCOON ..va \ ‘ \‘5 ‘\‘_‘~“ . , ,\ \\§\‘\\\“_‘ 3‘ \ \ \ \, . \‘~ A \\ ‘:~\\\ incisors molars dental pod pclcte CONT RALAT ERAL Figure 1 4 intraoral body surfaces. Cabral and Johnson (1971) studied the mechanoreceptive projections in vb in sheep, Q: g agigg, and discovered three particularly interesting and unusual features of this somatotopic organisation: 1. There is a greatly enlarged head and face area represented in the thalamus. 2. The projections from ips body parts dominate vb in sheep to a greater extent than in any animal studied so far. 3. In the most medial areas of the thalamus, where there are projections from the ips face and intraoral regions, the somatotopic organisation is quite unusual. In the cl representation in most mammals studied, as in sheep, the projections from the maxillary lips and the surrounding hairy skin are dorsal to those from the corresponding mandibular regions. In the ips projection areas of the sheep this situation is reversed. A similar situation exists with regard to projections from the interior of the mouth. Going through the thalamus in a dorsal-ventral direction Cabral and Johnson (1971) found ips lower teeth, upper teeth, and finally, most ventrally, tongue (see Figure 1). The organization of vb was examined by Mountcastle and Henneman (1949) in the cat. They found both cl and ips responses from the face region, but the face responses were predominantly cl. The organization of the cl responses resembled that of the head itself,,1rg., the maxillary projections were dorsal to the mandibular. The ips face responses were few and it is unclear whether they are 5 organised as are the cl responses or are inverted as in the sheep ips area. The responses from the interior of the mouth were primarily ips and here again it is unclear as to how they are organised. Rose and Mountcastle (1952) again studied the cat thalamus but their data do not define the organisation of the ips projections from the face or mouth. The thalamus of the macaque monkey was examined by Mountcastle and Henneman (1952). Again both ips and cl face and mouth representation was located. The somatotopic organisation in the regions receiving ips projections was not examined in detail fine enough to determine exactly the pattern of organisation. In the rabbit thalamus the representation of the face and mouth is quite large (Rose and Mountcastle, 1952). The most striking information that may be inferred from the study on the rabbit is that the projections from the maxillary face are much greater than those from the mandibular face. It seems that most of the face is represented from the cl side. There is apparently considerable representa- tion from the perioral and intraoral surfaces but the exact nature of the somatotopic organization was not described. Emmers (1965) examined the albino rat thalamus and reported an organisational pattern which has been found in no other mammalian thalamus. He found two complete series 6 of projections which he calls SI and $11 to correspond to the SI and SII receiving areas in the cortex which have been seen in many mammals (Noolsey, 1958). Emmers found that the projections in the area he termed SI were cl and those in the area he termed 811 were bilateral. His results are very unusual for two reasons: 1. The discovery of an SI and an $11 thalamus is a unique finding and 2. In his SI area, which includes the most medial area of vb, he found no ips representation. In every other animal studied so far there has been at least some ips representation reported in the most medial part of the complex. The raccoon, Egggygn,lgtgg, thalamus was examined by Welker and Johnson (1965) and they found some ips representation of the face, mouth, and tongue but they did not examine in detail the organisation of these ips responses. Pubols and Pubbls (1966) examined the thalamus of the opossum, nigglphig gagggpiglig, and found some ips head representation. They did not, however, describe the organisa- tion of these projections. They did not find any responses from the interior of the mouth because, apparently, they didn't look for them. The reported face and mouth representation in the spider monkey, Atglgg, thalamus (Pubols, 1968) is consider- ably less extensive than in either the macaque, nagggg, (Mountcastle and Henneman, 1952) or the squirrel monkey, m m, (Blomquist, Benjamin, and Emmers, 1962). Only a few ips responses were found but the author admits 7 that the mapping procedures used may have made the localisation of ips responses unlikely. Thus, researchers have electrophysiologically mapped the thalami of the sheep, 9:11. amiss: the cat. 22m. seam: the albino rat. mm W: the reccoon. 2mm lens; the ope-ewe. W W: the neceque monkey. m end the spider monkey,_Aiglgg. The ips projection area in the thalamus of sheep is large and its organisation is inverted when compared to the organisation of the cl projection areas seen in other animals studied. Cats seem to have an ips component in vb but its organisation is unclear. Rabbits have a large face and mouth representation but the somato- topic organisation has not been described. The data from the rat vb are very unusual and therefore of little help in the elucidation of any general organisational pattern which may exist in the mammalian vb. In both the raccoon and the opossum, ips responses were located by previous researchers but their organisation was not described. Two monkeys, the macaque and the spider monkey, have been studied but in both instances the ips response area was not mapped. From the above literature review it should be clear that little attention has been paid to the mechanosensory projections from the mouth parts to vb. It should also be clear that the relative proportions of ips vs cl projections from the mouth parts have not been adequately elucidated for the mammals which have been studied with the exception of 8 the work on sheep (Cabral and Johnson, 1971). Again with that exception, the detailed organisation of the mouth parts' projections has not been described. In this study I have analysed by means of electrophysiological recording, and described the mechanosensory projections from the mouth parts to vb in five mammals, the common opossum, Didslnhis W: the squirrel monkey. my. sums: the agouti. pagypgggta,aguti; the domestic cat,.zglig.ggtng; and the raccoon, 2:33y33,;012;. The rationale for choosing the above animals deserves some mention. The opossum was used because it was available in the Laboratory of Comparative Neurology where this research was conducted and because it represents the infra- class Metatheria, the pouched mammals (Simpson, 1945). The other mammals studied are members of the infraclass Eutheria, the placental mammals. The squirrel monkey was chosen on the basis of its availability and represents cohort Uhguiculata and order Primates. Also, it is known to have at least some ipsilateral tongue projections (Blomquist, Benjamin, and Emmers, 1962). The agouti was chosen to represent the cohort Glires, order Rodentia. Compared to common rodents it is a large animal with a large thalamus which permits more detailed mapping. The cat and the raccoon were chosen because they are common animals used in electrophysiological research and the data collected would therefore be especially valuable because they add to a growing fund of knowledge on the sensory systems of these animals. Both these animals 9 are members of the cohort Ferungulata, superorder Ferea, and order Carnivora. It should be noted here that the sheep is a member of cohort Ferungulata, superorder Paraxonia, and order Artiodactyla. In my final comparisons in which I include the data of Cabral and Johnson (1971) on sheep, I will compare the two infraclasses, Metatheria and Eutheria, within the subclass Theria. Within the infraclass Eutheria I will compare "representatives" of three cohorts, Unguiculata as represen- ted by the squirrel monkey, Glires, as represented by the agouti, and Ferungulata as represented by the cat, raccoon, and sheep. It should be noted here that comparison of animals related as distantly as even my most closely related subjects is a somewhat hasardous task. This study describes the pro- portions of ips versus cl projections from the mouth parts to vb in various mammals who are members of the major groups (cohorts) of mammals as defined by Simpson (1945) (see Figure 2) and presents detailed projection patterns of the mouth parts for each animal. These are compared and discussed in light of what is known of mammalian evolution. In other words this study is designed to learn how much of the mouth projections are to the ips vb versus the cl thalamus and to learn if the ips somatotopic organisation seen in sheep is a specialisation of sheep of is representative of a general mammalian pattern which is particularly obvious in sheep due to the large amount of thalamic tissue involved. 1O anachuac< noooosn mod ace mouobmnnso maoo new woman»: monsnmeam ammo» onaau ammo: nahveadonmwuom oaaeeo need veom mesa: nameoaeonau< uvmamwdfluom anoaoo .Ameo—v noamlwm ha dedwuem as mason» newaalas: assume none: easeneem masnosuumu momnaoo unwnomqam oaawdsnus naoan measvmowm shamaoe aoaasaum anon) ammuoiomsa swam nuseoeveo sameuom mauohwvoeanm comes: nemwac cvsauomsunb euonoo phonoo anonoo .N shaman auoaaoodd nswuuvmem «Masseuse: maenosuuqH ooudmndx muaaomo answmdansz cwuenvoaoum nueaonsm unmhvcam advance aeaeuaonoz 11 METHODS mmw Table 1 describes the source of each animal, number of animals used, and drugs administered to each subject species. On the day preceding each experiment the animal was deprived of food. The anesthetised subject was shaved on the top of the head and in the throat region. The trachea was exposed, opened, and a cannula inserted to permit immediate mechanical respiratory assistance as required. The skull was exposed, cleaned of all overlying skin and muscle, and secured to a head holder by means of screws fastened to each sygomatic arch. The screws were secured to the head holder bars by means of dental acrylic. Another screw was fastened to the occipital ridge and secured to the headholder by means of another bar and dental acrylic. The brain was then exPosed by removing the skull and a dam of dental acrylic was built on the skull around the brain. The dura was cut and reflected and the exposed brain protected with warmed mineral oil for the duration of the experiment. The exposed cortical surface was photographed and an enlarged print made on which the position of the electrode was noted during the experiment. Frequently, throughout the experiment, a warmed solution of normal saline (0.9%NaCl) was applied to the buccal cavity in order to prevent the tissue from drying. ”mouoos was .huoHvoham .aowahdmow .aomaeo oauemawc .an use awaemv an» «o moavmo am. an once one) seed emu «o awahnsus use assume one no nowvsmwsoue one .aneo em ocean-om .un .Hsmnso ownslwcm .nn .oucAoa: onequ .hn one) unawawama oaud .dowaadnsom one moan uses» a ha ueauommna .Hwasum .eaaoawuom oHem .uweuou scam: em acheuom wouwauerwsb one «o hue .owehdm use huoaousez no .aamen one no aewmoasnomsa env aw non-so ovuenuww .un news mowasuopcaaoo aw .D.m._ nausea .me .omdfivsoo ueaam one .emseaoo moan neaueuexoox o .monm .Hh.uonumoh .H.h .un ha vesuomuemLeuer maneswuomue «enema e n Henmmwuo w\p capo Inomwno: onm oodcmevnwsl Impound .uweueo men.z .awsw.scn¢eam anomon av asuevom assess“ us\asoe~ .sas .eaeuuuosnnp us\asm an: ”snows“ .eaoe sundown on.:H .oaua mmm .zH .ecoawvuoo mm\msm~.o Insnlnhaaswm wm\mam~ won vaauohwsb mmuqumflma. ms\msev..mH .aeaoa «on oaoaaoo aasom. .ssnveub1aswn Iacaoum Pom .hmonoo p eons .bwnb .vm Hewawnw ms m.~ Imecnwea man confine“: anennw .zH ..oe¢.oa ..un Heathen .Heaausa mam. .asoaoo Mensa anode-«adeum olm\Hmh mm\map.o .mH.snfiuom III new nouseuem head acumnusnoanam mom oawueouam Home“: .uauo e\. eonsnovnwss ..eanu .ssnaous 0mm canon vnm as\ua o+~ emm .<.m.= mammmmmmmmw 1:: has». Heaaflaa .esoe cannons In: «mm .eaaaauax mmmmmmmmm Hac— ws\mam..o nuanuaaaaese «mm ..ouoasoo season“ ms\ms oo.aH .nsavoublaeMPI heave om oswnsa mH saunas» .mpaous .stwnsue eye as: causes some? defined aw odeomvc masonsonm veo.nnu ononeuou an F mgmI (125mm Agouti 59: 42 In the dorsal-ventral direction the organization of the projections is as would be expected in any c1 thalamic projection pattern. The dorsal or maxillary face or tooth project dorsally to tho corresponding ventral or mandibular surfaces (Figure 9). In those punctures which included tongue the tongue projected most ventrally (Figure 9). These experiments have demonstrated that the thalmaic projections in the agouti are primarily cl and organised as other cl projection patterns are; 1.1. dorsal body parts projecting dorsally, ventral body parts projecting ventrally, and rostral parts projecting medially. One unusual feature of the representation is that the only teeth which were found to project to vb were the incisors. 931 In the five cats on which experiments were performed 7 thalami were explored. In all experiments except one the electrode was moved down to the thalamus in vertical plane. In animal 561 the subject's head was tilted in the headholder and therefore the electrode was introduced lateral to vb and moved at an angle toward the midline. One hundred and forty punctures were made and 51 (36.4%) of these yielded data. Twenty-two punctures included data from the body surfaces excluding face, 22 yielded data from the face, jaw, and perioral regions, and 19 yielded data from the intraoral surfaces. The intraoral surfaces from which responses were elicited included teeth, palate, dorsal surface of the tongue, and the ventral surface of the tongue and its 43 attachment to the floor of the mouth. In the 51 punctures which included data 125 response points were localised. Forty-three included responses from the body surface, 57 included responses from the face, jaw, and perioral surfaces, and 39 included responses from the intraoral surfaces. Of the 39 intraoral response loci 36 (95%) responded to stimulation of the teeth, 7 (19%) responded to stimulation of the palate, 4 (11%) responded to stimulation of the dorsal surface of the tongue and 2(5%) responded to stimulation of the tissues on the ventral surface of the tongue and the floor of the mouth. Of the 36 loci responding to stimulation of the teeth 10 (27%) responded to stimulation of the upper teeth to the exclusion of the lower: 13(36%) responded to stimulation of both upper and lower teeth. In the cat both cl and ips projections are seen in vb. All 43 data points responding to stimulation of the general body surface were, as expected, cl, while of the 57 data points responding to stimulation of the face, jaw, and perioral surfaces 53 (93%) included responses from the cl side and 5 (9%) included responses from the ips side. Of the 39 loci responding to stimulation of the intraoral surfaces 27 (69%) included responses from the cl side and 20 (51%) included responses from the ips side. Because of the small size of vb receiving projections from inside the mouth it is impossible to state, with one 44 exception, exactly what the somatotopic organisation is. In some instances the lower teeth project ventrally in the thalamus, while in other instances the upper teeth are seen to lie ventral to the lower teeth. One pattern which does appear is for the ips responses to be located ventro-medially to the c1 responses (Figures 10 and 11). These experiments demonstrate that the cat has both cl and ips projections from the face and mouth to vb and that the ips projections are ventromedial to the cl projections. These experiments have not, however, demonstrated the somatotopic organisation of the projections to the medial areas of vb. m Three thalami were explored in three raccoons. In all experiments the electrode was moved in a vertical plane, _i_.;. not tilted. Sixty-eight punctures were made and 48 (71%) of these yielded data. Two punctures included data from the body surface excluding the face, 21 included data from the face, jaw, and perioral surfaces and 40 included data from the interior of the mouth. The intraoral surfaces from which responses were elicited were the teeth, the dorsal surface of the tongue, the pad just posterior to the upper incisors, the palate, the cheek, and the rostral throat area. 4,... . ’\ 77--J._ . 1‘ - 41/34; a“ r. r) A. M”.$“"‘ "‘ -'_...-3 1b., -o.c1..,:.". "_ .1433 pvt/31V _;T1 ., o-o W1. Io V .L U ( V69 (Tbs-t / . r‘ m 1 w - ‘ ' rd n -V A . v? ‘ A“(_‘ M,“ ..447 q fuf no go ;u._ouo. I‘ #1.. ‘ ’ ’ - -4 5 ~ J... m A _f_‘ 4.1- __O \‘Q ' 4. A.” J.‘ : m ., T - 712;“ k.- ICI' CI‘CI)_LLLZ'11-o.'-vTI Ci one iuifis.u v- 2-. u 71-4.. m, .- rvr - . -- J. ,1 “'14-. 49, 4.4- ., w ~~o,w. suction 11a 13 'oLLCUCI. U .c owns all 1o f") r- j J.-- .r‘ ‘ ,. -:‘ ~~ Imw— 7 f‘ s \‘P +1f- q e /u .3“ Mao o_ tgosJ I1 pnnocaro ,7 aic on a”; i0" . . ‘ ... w 'vr-v 4.1 ‘ .t ' , .'-‘ -r\ w 7 -. -. ~. . ' .- D 'A f‘ '1“ 1“” "V‘ ' - V v-\'r vytlz‘i l‘usjjcrbcs’ /\_) Ln.--“ / {J Lyle ..Ciu L). (I .. s . o 1 r ~o . I '1 ‘ ' ‘ _ _“ ..f‘ r 'v [\‘fi qq (‘J""~ I f .- . n o.osor;o:c;:;on oi ooot.oh .i o “incl a - .‘J " -“" “v- 4‘. " .. q - ' n ’ t1~q ‘ 1N \r‘1 9'- 3* vw— ""tCLLd' u.‘ C .LO v0.1. (108031: OK: 0:". .-L, Got .9- b0 +-r . ~ ~ nw ' ” ’ tb' ~ t' ‘ 1 ' ‘ ’ r~Ar I P a V H‘ ‘l l f‘ r) ’1 .-*L~ ’ V J- 9/3, L"-~l / 0' 2.4. ‘Jj_s).-~ G ~11 4-1—0 ch 10“. ‘ . "V‘ W. 'n 4 n ‘- J— -; 'f‘ -: N 1 2 L: .n' . .. ------..\.«\.vu-.C._' ..-“ a 2’1‘. - v L a.) *U of ounot““os in Figure. p nsos in puncture /n typo. The s tongue. r cell bodies at are. Electrode WI I“. I ... . _ _ ..4 .sv J . a ‘. 14 .d _a n a ‘5— ._n a) u i..-_ Cf, .. a w» .. .4\ . ma __ a . ._ ..., ow. .I , .— . . J n . 4 ._ O, XIV 1.4“. ‘d a. .- r t I , 1 Q . ‘1‘ . lfi ; . g ..p m, .H .. a «p W.“ . M , s.) k.’ L J,\ \ rd 4 .\ . e , ‘ J 4 . .I at n. Q ... .. d ., _. J. .1 m u .1 I .4 1‘ U r ... Illll-lll'li’sllll 7 5 5 m C 49 In the 48 punctures which included data, 119 data points were localised. Only 2 of those included responses from the body excluding the face. Thirty-four included responses from the face, jaw, and perioral surfaces and 99 included responses from the intraoral surfaces. Fifty-nine (60%) of the 99 intraoral data points responded to stimulation of the teeth. Thirty (30%) responded to stimulation of the tongue, nineteen (19%) responded to stimulation of the pad just posterior to the upper incosors. Eleven (11%) responded to stimulation of the palate. In addition two data points responded to stimulation of the cheek and one responded to stimulation of the rostral throat area. Of the 59 data points responding to stimulation of the teeth 30 (51%) responded to stimulation of the upper teeth to the exclusion of the lower teeth, 20 (34%) responded to stimulation of the lower teeth to the exclusion of the upper teeth and 9 (1%%) responded to stimulation of both upper and lower teeth. Again in the raccoon both cl and ips projections are seen innfb. Both data points which responded to stimulation of the body were from the cl side. Thirty-three (97%) of the 34 face data points included responses from the cl side and 2 (6%) included responses from the ips side. Thirty (30%) of the 99 mouth data points included responses from the cl side and 88 (89%) responded to stimulation of the ips side. 50 - The organisation of the projections from the interior of the mouth in the raccoon is so complex that only the few tendencies which are most obvious will be noted. The cl _projections are scattered among the ips projections and consist mainly of teeth projections. They are often associated with ips projections and in those cases the ips responses were generally stronger. In several instances all lower teeth, both ips and cl, responded at one data locus (see locus 12B Figure 12). The cl teeth responses were too scattered to demonstrate a clear organisational pattern. The ips responses from the teeth did not demon- strate a consistent dorsal-ventral organisational pattern within themselves but did consistently project dorsally to the other mouth components which were primarily tongue, palate, and incisor pad (Figure 12). There were too few punctures in which upper and lower teeth were represented sequentially, and these punctures showed too much varia- bility to make a conclusion about dorsal-ventral organisation. Represented below the tooth was the palate and below that either the tongue or the incisor pad. There is a tendency for the tongue to project more medially and the incisor pad more laterally. (see Figures 12 and 13). The raccoon vb receives both cl and ips projections from the body parts. The body and face are represented almost entirely on the cl side and the mouth is represented on both the cl and ips sides with the ips projections dominant. The organisation of the intraoral projections is complex and unclear except for the tendencies mentioned earlier. _ r. . . . . .1 n O A “. 41- -,'-) ~131 r.x - ~1f r1 ‘ . -,- r‘ .a_r~‘rj ,-..' J 4-: >w‘ ‘I ‘ ' T“‘Y\ ”\J-“fi‘lr‘ ~ 1 . .. k 1 u ._,... V- ‘/-\,.h~, _ ..__'\_/_ LJ _.4 \ ,.,._ «.a_1_.~. . ., .1 .... LVel b.. l /\-.\'Uo._ -... y‘ ['7‘ - n I" . r ' ~' ' __ .4 ' , V L. ‘ ~ ‘ ..L. 1 V -. 4— d A_) . —- . n '1 ' . S ‘ ‘t v fl ~ - A - ‘».. ,» a» - , l ’ ‘ 'I ," ‘7 . I 1" n ‘ - "1 l, 1' j ‘1 l < eV - 1 ‘ » "l I ~ ‘ , ' 1 ‘1 .- 4.x ‘f __ _, i ’-L-‘ . L4 - V‘ ~ V’#— x..- .1 _. U; i .L... I V‘ U .u - . --_‘ K, . ‘J. “ .4 -.LJ -'Y.\x' --. ~‘.\/ ‘li ..--.-. v \4- u. ...-U _L... \i a no‘o e W n- O o I ' ' J r1 —e- 1 nw 1.J 'v‘ ',‘| A ..A1 ra ( p / ‘PA, ‘5 'VI‘ 1‘ ' --\ ,_. 4 J n<.u/-‘ 1 I / .1 1 'V _‘._.,.x... ...vn-..x_U, -. -... - k - \4' . ---4M\... v.- -_-\«... '- ‘a- — L. K i ‘ V 'l J‘ / I I - ~ ‘ .. . , ~ 7" "\ ~"/\(\” ‘\ , V ' 1 ' ~ . ,w ‘ mud/up“ ’_ «,V--K..‘_ _‘J., ,J\, L.) ...- i u. l .. . xdv o -.i-_.._ V .v.._ .u.‘ .4. ..zk.’ ‘-3‘.U k.) . U \ \ l ' "T . . . f1 1" ‘ . "“’\ ‘r ' ' , “m L 1 7~r'-" " H “ n” .2 _ p L, '1- . +4 I 4..-”, ..~..~ .-. v (i . .-.x.’ . UkaU , - - .... x n ‘ ‘ ‘ ' j \J i) .....L a . .L‘a J.-‘~/ _‘J . \J . .\.L ..--V 3 64.\. _ . . - a ’1 v - < - - —. —. . 1 . i K‘s “, ,. 3 « .' r, c-~ .,~ 1 ‘ . ~A ’ --‘ 1"‘.‘ w A.) I‘ 7‘ . ‘~_ .__v J -J- v. *1.” ...- U- kJV‘I .1 V-- -/ \3 no. , . _V . a K1.l_~._k¢~\. Jun. _ > - 7 ‘ I — ‘ 4' J- , , , 1' I, ‘ , . f5 3 ,4 r ’1 ‘1‘ ..a— . 1 1 - ' .1 ~ . -\ n L ‘ h , I s 1 I . \ . N, i V . g __ \ L w. 1 3 - .- . J. K/ a - \4 L1 4 J\ .1 L, ‘ u~ , m 4.1.1...V \. b J. K, u - .A _V o e- ‘ . e I ~ I q. n . I ' ‘A fi 1 "I" _ ’ ‘\ J‘ " *‘ v’w‘n - - . a-~~ xn- *- "w ’1" . l r . .— ‘J . .-. _ L_ LI... k, . - i.) , -. , .- I. 1, 5-, . .4 I-‘ K. . o . .x - .-u- n is V sa- .4' V_. —- g.“ 0 . . 901a . n O .1 u e a .riJo s in out . . ..a . .‘.- . as. . o . . . . u . an . ‘1 § e a . .u ... cu.» . ... - .. u‘f .. T“. I I 0 e .v | ... z c . i........,n........._ . _ Q . . ..S ._.. .... .5. o a. . .... _'a.‘e l na‘ nu . . ...s ..r.. .s ...3. tars... «3...: . .r \. . I... .I\ V ..s.¢.-..¥' I. . . s. .\.-I‘ .1 In. ... (.ta ,0... ‘ a...“.. . ‘ \ 1 V at" Vv » .t (at) .... L‘.‘_.‘ "A -4. Vet.) ..‘* "'4. J. ( *Ls \I' u... \.-.U .n». , o L.. s Kv~~ . ,_l. a V V e 0 {a . a r. ..J ( -_ u I .~ 55 4'5 2’ 6 1.67654328 —)M 0.25m m Raccoon 564 Ill] I'llllll' 1"" I II! I'lll‘ '1’]! ii '1!) ll! i l DISCUSSION The differences in character of intraoral projections (see Table 2) are systematic when examined in light of Simpson's (1945) classification of mammals (Figure 2). The opossum, a member of infraclass Metatheria, is at one end of Simpson's (1945) "evolutionary scale". It displays a cl projection pattern from the mouth parts to vb. The cat, raccoon, and sheep are all eutherians and members of the cohort Ferungulata which ranks at the other end of Simpson's (1945) classification. They all display a very large and, in the case of the raccoon and sheep, predominantly ips projection pattern from the mouth parts. The squirrel monkey and agouti who represent respectively cohort Unguiculata and Glires show an intermediate condition. The mouth parts project primarily to the cl side with some ips projections. Both cohort Unguiculata and Glires occupy an intermediate position in Simpson's (1945) classification. Moving "up the evolutionary ladder" or out the "evolutionary limb", therefore, there is a tendency toward increasing ips projections to vb. Further evidence for the above hypothesis comes from a series of electrophysiological studies on the first somatic sensory area (SI) of the cortex. Efferent project- ions from vb have been shown to project to 81 (Clark and Powell, 1953, Pubols, 1968, and Walker and Johnson, 1965). It is,thorefore, reasonable to infer information about the 55 II! II 56 projections to vb by examining the projections upon the cortex. Electrophysiological mapping studies of the cerebral cortex support the hypothesis that variation in the character of projections to vb is systematic. Lende (1964) studied the echidna (Igghyglglgng 2&3125131) and found considerable head and tongue project- ions to the cortex which were all cl. The echidna is a member of subclass Protheria, order Honotremata (Figure 2). Lende and Sadler (1967) studied the hedgehog (fixingggng) and reported a completely cl cortical representation pattern The hedgehog is a member of infraclass Eutheria, cohort Unguiculata, order Insectivora. Lende (1970) examined the tree shrew (ggpaig) and again found only cl projections. The tree shrew is an insectivore-like primate, cohort Unguiculata, order Primates. In their study of the spider monkey (51:13:) cortex Pubols and Pubols (1971) found only two loci with ips intraoral data in 420 responding punctures. The spider monkey is a member of cohort Unguiculata, order Primates. The porcupine (EI£IEIIEB.§RZIIIHI) was studied by Lende and Woolsey (1956) and again only c1 projections were seen in SI. The porcupine is a member of cohortCEIires, order Rodentia. Voolsey and Wang (1945) studied the rabbit and reported cl and ips face projections but did not report the relative proportions of each. The rabbit is a member of cohort Glires, order Lagomorpha. Adrian (1943) reported completely ips projections from the perioral surfaces in the sheep and the goat and completely cl projections in the 57 pig and the horse. Woolsey and Fairman (1946) found both ips and cl projections in the sheep and pig. They also found ips and cl representation from the face in the dog and the cat. Pinto-Hamuy, Bromiley, and Woolsey (1956) studied the dog and reported that the palate, teeth, and lips are represented on the ips side. The sheep, goat, and pig are all members of cohort Ferungulata, order Artiodactyla. The horse is a member of cohort Ferungulata, order Perissodactyla and the cat and dog are members of cohort Ferungulata, order Carnivora. These studies are almost completely consistent with the hypothesis put forward; $.2, more ”advanced" mammalian groups show a tendency toward a higher degree of ips repre- sentation in the thalamus and cortex. Among non-eutherian mammals studied ips projections seem to be insignificant. In cohorts Unguiculata and Glires ips representation has been reported but in all cases the ips projections from the face and mouth seem to be small. Large amounts of ips representation have been reported in all ferungulates studied except the horse. In other words the variation in character of projections to the higher centers of the medial lemniscal system is consistent and systematic in light of Simpson's (1945) classification of mammals.. A second hypothesis to account for the variation seen in this and other studies is that these variations represent specializations (which are of course evolutionary changes) at levels lower than the major mammalian groups which 58 Simpson (1945) called cohorts. In other words if more animals from various groups were examined variations within groups may prove to be as great as variations between groups. Only further systematic studies which pay particular atten- tion to the projections from the face and mouth will resolve this question. BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Adrian, E. D. 1943. Afferent areas in the brain of ungulates. B1312. 66: 89-103. Baldwin, H. A., S. Frenk, and J. I. Lettvin. 1965. Glass-coated tungsten micro-electrodes. figlgggg, 148: 1462-1464. Blomquist, A. J., R. M. Benjamin and R. Emmers. 1962. Thalamic localisations of afferents from the tongue in squirrel monkey ( ). Igg,gg§;nal 2!. mm W ~88 Cabral, R. J. and J. I.Johnson. 1971. The organization of mechanoreceptive rejections in the ventrobasal thalamu- of sheep. Jamal 91 W W. 141: 17- . Clark, W. E. LeGros and Powell, T. P. S. 1953. On the thalamo-cortical connexions of the general sensory cfiortexaof_Macaccafl.‘1 :WW 3; m min Emmers, R. 1965. Organisation of the first and second somesthetic regions (81 and 811) in the rat thalamus. 1b.: ml 2.1? W W. 124: 215-229 Hubel, D. H. 1957. Tungsten microelectrode for recording from single units. figigggg, 125: 549-550. Johnson, J. 1., G. I. Batten, and E. V. Rubel. 1969. Related specializations of brains and behaviours in sheep and raccoons. Presented at Second Annual Winter Conference on Brain Research. Snowmass-as- Aspen, Colorado. Lende, R. A. 1964. Representation in the cerebral cortex of a primitive mammal. Sensorimotor, visual, and auditory fields in the echidna ( ac cglegtug). mo na 23W. 27: *4- Lende R. A. 1970. Cortical localization in the tree shrew (Tupaia). Brain w. 18: 61—75 Lende, R. A. and K. M. Sadler, 1967. Sensory) and motor areas in neocortex of hedgehog (3:11:2211.) 21212 gigggggh, 5: 390—405. 59 60 Lende, R. A. and C. N. Voolsey. 1956. Sensory and motor localization in cerebral cortex of porcupine ( 0 marine) muW. 19: . Mountcastle, V. B. and E. Henneman. 1949. Pattern of tactile representation in the thalamus of the cat. mmW.1zx 35-100. . 1952. The representation of tactile sensibil1ty 1n the thalamus of the monkey. II: m}. at. W W 97: 409-440 Pinto-Hamuy, T. P., R. B. Bromiley, and C. N. Voolsey. 1956. Somatic afferent areas I and II of dogs cerebral cortex. isms]. at Wm 19. 485-499. Pubols, B. H. 1968. Retrograde degeneration study of somatic sensory thalamocortical connections in brain of Virginia opossum. ngig,figgggzgh, 7: 232-251. Pubols, L. M. 1968. Somatic sensory representation in thalamic ventrobasal complex of spider monkey (Atglgg). Brain. W- 1' 305-323- Pubols, B. H. Jr., and L. M. Pubols, 1966. Somatic sensory representation in thalamic ventorobasal complex of the Virginia ope-Inl- m M at W We 127: 19-34 Pubols, B. B. Jr., and L. M. Pubols. 1971. Somatotopic organization of spider monkey somatic sensory cerebral 23:31:}. 111.: Meal 91 W W- 141: Rose, J. E. and V. B. Mountcastle. 1952. The thalamic tactile region in the rabbit and cat. Ihg,£gn;n§1,gz, WW- 97: 441-490. . 1959. Touch and Kinesthetics. In Field 3. (Ed. ) W¥§ngbgok Section I W.V ' e I G‘sfifigton. American ' ysio og1ca eciety. 387-429. Simpson, G. G. 1945. iiiiiiplga f; Siiggiféfigtion 22d snug fifi‘ . L '85 O ' - O Welker, W. I. and J. 1. Johnson. 1965. Correlation between nuclear morphology and somatotopic organization in ventrobasal complex of the raccoon thalamus. gournal 21.‘B£1£I¥. (London and Cambridge). 99: 761- 61 Woolsey, C. M. 1958. Organization of somatic sensory and motor areas of the cerebral cortex. In H. F. Harlow and C. M. Woolsey (Eds.). W m B' Bases 2;,nggzig;_ Madison, Wisconsin. Un versity o 1scons1n Press. 63-81. Woolsey, C. N. and D. Fairman. 1946. Contralateral, ipsilateral, and bilateral representation of cutaneous receptors in somatic areas I and II of the cerebral cortex of pit, sheep, and other mammals. §nzggzx, Woolsey, D. M. and G. H. vang. 1945. Somatic sensory areas I and II of the cerebral cortex of the rabbit. Federation 9; America? §ggigtns f9; W nglogy. Federation oceedings. 4: 9. 62 General References Oswaldo-Cruz, E. and C. E. Rocha-Miranda. 1968. 2h; . . A g: 21 $ we (11411211: oar hi 9 t 1 Atlas ;¥,St s i uto e B10 is ca, n vers1 de Janeior. M'TlTilfilfll711flflfijfliTll l1 (Infill 0711111111111?”