A FURTHER CROSS-CULTURAL COMPAEEISON OF CHILDREN'S JUDGMENT OF PARENT-CHILD CONFLICT IN GERMANY. FINLAND, ENGLAND AND THE UNITED STATES Thesis ‘0'.th chreo c! M. A. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY Barbara Mac Phail Robinson 1955 'lthm‘, ABSTRACT This study was an analysis of Story No. 4, The Ink Spots, Series A, of the Anderson In: mplete Stories. Story No. 2, The Lost Meat,, had been analyzed in a previous study, and it was possible to make com- parisons between the two analyses and further evaluate the Anderson Incomplete Stories as a measuring instrument of intercultural differences. Samples used were from data collected in Firmingham, Eng‘Lnd; in Helsinki, Finland; in Karim-r5 and Munich, German? and in Fnoxville, Tennessee, U;S.A. Subjects were 1,227 children in the seventh school year in these cities. Children‘s stories were coded in two ways: by four defined levels I‘ of outcome (integrat£"', Ecnirrijor~resistance, ambiguous, or domination— submission); and by categories representing the psychological thenas in the stories. In Story No. 2 there vere 33 categories for thought, feeling and action by the child-character and 46 for the mother- character. In Story No. 4 there were 37 catejories for thought, feeling, and action by the child—character and 53 categories for the mother-character. Aims were to ana yze Story No. b and make sex and socio-economic compariscrs in categorr clrsters and outcome levels, and these same comparisons in or): ~cr tnrul samples, and to make C?”:?”1“CFS of Story No. b with Story No. 2. Reliability of coding showed percentages of agreement ransing from 93.5% to 100%. The Birmingh° sample was highest in the two outcomes of spontan- eity of the child, integrative outcome and dggigation—resistan£_ It was lowest in ambiguous outcomes. That is, human relations in Birmingham were more clearly defined by these children in their stories. Helsinki was significantly different from all other samples in ambiguous, unstructured outcomes of the stories, indicating a low order of communi- cation, and significantly lower in domination—submission. Hamburg, Munich and Knoxville were alike in all four outcomes and showed no significant differences among themselves. More boys showed ambiguous, unstructured outcomes than girls in all five samplings, the differences being significant in four. More children in the high socio-economic groups showed integrative outcomes in both Story No. 2 and Story Ho. H, the differences for Story No. # bein. significant in Hamburg and hunich. More children in the low socio-economic groups had ambiguous outcomes than in the high groups in all samples, the differences being significant in Helsinki, Hamburg and Munich. Analyses of the data showed that in completion of the stories children were sensitive to the st-icture of the stimulus material. There were two structural differences in the two stories that showed effects in the analysis of the data. Story No. 4 had a question after it: ”How do they both feel about it?" Calling attention to both the mother and the child seemed to account for higher numbers of integrative outcomes, higher percentages of constructive behavior by both the mother and the child and higher frequencies of anxiety than were found in Story No. 2, while themes of domination and submission were lower in frequency. Second, in Story No. 4 the mother enters Just as Susan finds herself in a predicament. There was less opportunity or necessity to tell truth or lie, and there were much lower frequencies of both tells truth and tells lie in Story No. 4. The girls wrote longer stories with wider variety of themes than boys. As Heter found in Story No. 2, girls gave higher frequencies in category clusters of constructive behavior Ly mother, submission, and anxiety. Consistently in all five samples girls were high in constructive mother category clusters; with the exception of Helsinki, constructive behavior by_the child; and, with the exception of Knoxville, child tells igg, For all five samples girls had higher frequencies of a rigty and submiggion, the differences being generally in the same direction as for Story No. 2, and six of the ten differences for these two categories being significant. Analysis of data for Socio-economic levels showed that children from the high groups generally wrote longer stories. They had consistent- 1y higher frequencies of integrative outcomes (as in Story No. 2), the differences in Hamburg and Munich being significant. The high groups had consistently higher frequencies in the category cluster of construgt— izg behavior by the mother and, with the exception of Helsinki, construct- i1; behavigr by the ghild. Children in the high group had higher fre- quencies of tells lie in Birmingham and Helsinki. Children in the low group had higher frequencies of tells lie in Hamburg and Munich, the difference in Munich being significant. Socio-economic levels in the four cities were exactly reversed for child tells truth. Children in the high socio-economic group had high percentages of tells truth in Hamburg and.Munich, while tells truth was found more frequently in Birmingham and Helsinki among children at the low socio-economic level. Children in the low groups tend to write shorter stories. Hamburg and Munich were significantly different from each other in category clusters of constructive behavior hy the mother, truth, lie, and gggiety, supporting the findings for Story Ho. 2, which leaves open the question of a German national character. Knoxville stories reveal more initiative and personal integritv in the child and more respect for the child for both Story N0. 2 and Story No. 4. Knoxville children showed the highest percentage of t§;;§_ tgggh and the lowest percentage of tgl;§_li_. They showed high attention to the child in having highest percentages of constructive behavior by §g§§g, and lower attention to the mother in having next to lowest percentages of children with themas in the constructive :ehavior by. mgthg; category cluster. Munich and Hamburg were lowest in frequency for constructive behavior Ly_§pggp, and Hamburg children, who gave little attention to constructive behavior EK.&22.Ehlli. were highest in fre- quency of constructive behavior hy_the gather. Evidence shows that according to the children's stories highest frequencies of tggth and lower frequencies of tells lie were accompanied by milder punishment and milder forms of parental domination in Knoxville. Adnonishment about future behavior ranked first in frequency and admonishment about past behavior ranked third. In all the other samples scolding ranked first among the types of domination. Admonishment did not rank high in any of the other samples and scolding did not rank high in Knoxville. It was pointed out that admonishment represents chiefly disapproval of some kind of behavior. Scolding and physical punishment represent an attack on the child himself. A JURTHHR CROSS—CULTURAL COMPARISON OF CHILDRhN'S JUDGMEFT OF RAEEHT-CHILD COKFLICT IN GERMANY, FINLAHD, EEGLAND AHD THE UNITED STATES 13y Barhsra Mathail Robinson Submitted to the College cf Science and.Arts of iichigan State University of.Agriculture and.Applied Science in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF.ARTS Department of Psychology 1955 (In says Acknowledgments The author wishes to extend her sincere appreciation for the continuous guidance and assistance she received from Dr. Harold H. Anderson and Dr. Gladys L..Anderson in preparing this thesis. The author also wishes to express her gratitude to Mr. Franz Geicrhaas for his coOperation in coding the German stories, to Mr. Geierhaas and Mr. Richard Haber for working out the problems of reliability and for other assistance, and to Mrs. Bonita Edgar and Hrs. Ruth Vogt for the typing of the thesis. ' The author deeply appreciates the financial support of the scholarship provided by Michigan State University which made it possible to continue with her work. TABLE OF CONTENTS LI ST OF TABIJ-ES . O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O I. II. INTRODU TION. . RELIABILITY OF CODING . . . . III. COMPARISON OF CODING CATEGORIES FOR STORIES KO. 4, mm INK SPOTS, AND no. 2, THE LOST MEAT IV. OUTCOMES OF STORIES. . . . . . . . . . . Results . . . . . . . . . . -. . . . Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . V. THE BIRMINGHAM, ENGLAND, SAMPLE. . . . . . . Sex Comparisons in Story No. 4 and Story No. . Discussion of Sex Comparisons . . . . . . Socio—economic Comparisons. . . . . . . . ..Story No. b - Story Ho. 2 Comparisons . . . . Discussion' . . . . . . . . . . . . VI. CROSS-CULTURAL COMPARISONS . . . . . . . . Page 16 17 19 22 22 26 29 32 32 3» Category Cluster: Constructive thinking, feeling, ”1rd action 32 mother . . . . . . . . . Category Cluster: Constructive thinking, feeling, _;nd action 3:_§u§§n . . . . . . . . . Category Cluster: Susan tells $3312. . . . . Category Cluster: Susan tells lie . . . . . Category Cluster: Resistance. . . . . . . Category Cluster: Anxiety. . . . . Category Cluster: Submission. . . . Category Cluster: Domination. . . . Discussion of Cross-cultural Comparisons Birmingham . . . . . . . . . Helsinki . . . . . . . . Hamburg and Munich. . . . . . . Knoxville. . . . . . . . . . length of Stories . . . . . . . VII. CROSS-CULTURAL SEX COMPARISTHS . . . . VIII.CRCSS—CULTURAL SOCIO—ECOHOMIC COMPARISONS IX. TYPES OF DCMINATIOH. . . . . . . . Discussion of Types of Domination . . K. SIMI-‘ARY . . . . . . . . . . . . sameness . . . . . . . . . I. . . .APPEHDIX A. . . . . . . . . . . . . APEIDIX B O O 0 O O O O O O O O O Table 1. Table 2. Table 3. Table 4. Table 5. Table 60 Table 7. Table 80 Table 9. LIST OF TABLES Distribution of Numbers of Boys and Girls by Socio-economic Level . . . . . . . . . Outcomes of Story No. 4. Frequencies and Percentages for Children in Each of Five Cities in the Outcomes of Their StorieS. . . . . . . . . . . . Birmingham, England: Numbers and Percentages of Boys and of Girls with Themas Coded in Eight Cate— gory Clusters, and According to Classification of Outcomes of Stories No. 4 and No. 2 . . . . Birmingham, England: Numbers and Percentages of Children from High and Low Socio-economic Levels with Thomas Coded in the several Category Clusters, and.According to Outcomes for Stories No. 4 and NO. 2 o o o o o o o o o o o o o o 0 Birmingham, England: Numbers and Percentages of Children.With Thenas Coded in Eight Category Clusters, and Integrative Outcomes, of Stories NO. 1+ and N0. 2 o o o o o o o o o o o 0 Numbers and Percentages of Children in.Each of the Samplings with Thomas Coded in the Category Cluster, Constructive Thinking, Feeling, and Action by Mother for Stories No. 3 and No. 2 . . Numbers and Percentages of Children in Each of the Samplings with Themas Coded in the Category Cluster, Constructive Thinking, Feeling, and Action by the Child for Stories No. 4 and No. 2 . Numbers and Percentages of Children in Each of the Samplings with Thomas Coded in the Category Cluster, Child Tells Truth, for Stories No. 4 and NO. 2 o o o o o o o o o o o o o 0 Numbers and Percentages of Children in Each of the Samplings with Themas Coded in the Category Cluster, Child Tells Lie, for Stories No. 4 and NO. 2 o o o o o o o o o o o o o o Page 6 18 25 30 35 36 Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 17. 18. 19. 20. Numbers and Percentages of Children in Each of the Samplings with Themas Coded in the Category Cluster, Resistance, for stories No. 4 and No. 2. Numbers and Percentages of Children in Each of the Samplings with Themas Coded in the Category Cluster, Anxiety, for Stories No. 4 and No. 2. . Numbers and Percentages of Children in Each of the Samplings with Themas Coded in the Category Cluster,Submission, for Stories No. 4 and No. 2 . Numbers and Percentages of Children in Each of the Samplings with Themas Coded in the Category Cluster, Domination, for Stories No. 4 and No. 2. Average Number of Thema Tallies per Story for Story No. 4 in Each of the Samples According to Sex Groups and Socio-economic Groups; for the Munich and Hamburg Samples, Sex Comparisons of tories NO. 4 and N0. 2 e e e e e e e e e Cross-cultural Sex Comparisons. Recapitulation of Differences Between Boys and Girls from Tables 3, 19, 20, 21, 22 . . . . . . . . rose-cultural Socio—ecanomic Comparisons. Recapitulation of Differences Between High, Middle and Low Socio-economic Levels from Tables 4, 23, 24, 25 . . . . . . . . . . Story No. 4. Percentages of the Total Numbers of Children in the Five Samplings Giving Themas of Domination of the Child, and Percentages of Total Numbers of Children in Each Sampling Giving Themas in Twelve Categories of Domination . . . Rank Order of Types of Domination within Each of the Five Samples, Taken from Table 17 . . . . Helsinki, Finland: Numbers and Percentages of Boys and Girls with Themas Coded in the Eight Category Clusters, and.According to Outcome . . Hamburg, Germany: Numbers and Percentages of Boys and Girls with Themas Coded in the Eight Category Clusters,and According to Outcome. . . ii Page . 38 41 42 54a 56a 60 63 72 73 Table 21. Table 22. Table 23. Table 24. Table 25. Munich, Germany: Numbers and Percentages of Boys and Girls with Themas Coded in the Eight Category Clvsters, and According to Outcome . . . . . Knoxville, Tennessee: Numbers and Percentages of Boys and Girls with Themas Coded in the Eight Category Clusters, and.According to Outcome . . Helsinki, Finland: Numbers and Percentages of Children in the High, Middle and Low Socio— economic Groups with Themas Coded in the Eight Category Clusters, and.According to Outcomesfor Stories No. 4 and No. 2 . . . . . . . . . Hamburg, Germany: Numbers and Percentages of Children in the High, Niddle and Low Socio- economic Groups with Themas Coded in the Eight Category Clusters, and According to Outcomes for Stories No. 4 and No. 2 . . . . . . . . . Munich, Germany: Numbers and Percentages of Children in the High and Low Socio-economic Groups with Themas Coded in the Eight Category Clusters, and According to Outcomes for Stories No. 4 and 1:0 0 2 O O I O O I O O O O O O O O 0 iii Page 74 75 76 77 78 I. INTRODUCTION This study is part of a research program being carried on by H. H..Anderson and G. L. Anderson (2, 3). The program is cross- cultural in nature and is designed to investigate, through analysis of children's written responses, the kinds of interaction that take place between children and their peers, their parents, and their teachers. The approach has been to present children with a projective device, the Anderson Incomplete Stories (see Appendix B), and ask them to write down their own conclusions for the stories. This pro- cedure has been carried out in classrooms of selected public schools in the German cities of Karlsruhe, Braunschweig, Hamburg, and Munich: in Helsinki, Finland; in.Drammen, Norway: in Stockholm, Sweden: in Birmingham, England; in Mexico City and some rural areas in.Mexico; and in Knoxville, Tennessee. Analyses have been made of data on Story No. 2, The Lost Meat, Series.A, from Karlsruhe, Germany, and from Mexico City by‘Anderson and.Anderson (3). Anderson (2) and Geierhaas (6, 7). Geierhaas has dealt with problems of reliability of the children, the trans- lator, and the coder. Heber (8) has also analyzed data from Story No. 2, The Lost Meat, and has made sex comparisons, socio-economic comparisons, and cross-cultural comparisons of reSponses of children from German, English, Finnish, United States, and Mexican cities. The hypotheses being investigated by.Anderson and his co-workers in the research program of which this study is a part are: 1. Children brought up in an extremely authoritarian and dom— inating culture (Germany) are different in their interper— sonal relations from children in less dominating (more democratic cultures. ~2. As compared with children in a more democratic culture, children in the more authoritarian culture will show sig- nificantly higher frequencies of responses of: anxiety, lying, cheating, deception, punishment, daydreaming (fantasy unrelated to reality), escape from reality, ambiguous un— structured relating, conformity, guilt, and submission in fantasy conflict situations. ‘9) 0 Children in the more authoritarian culture will also show significantly fewer responses of honesty, sense of fair play, social problem solving, communication, action, spontaneity, cooperation and integrative behavior (working together). 4. When outcomes of interpersonal conflict are classified according to levels of increasing authoritarianism or domp ination, the distribution of outcomes for German children will be skewed at the bottom of the following levels of human relating: a) Integrative, problem-solving b) Domination-resistance c) Ambiguous, unstructured, indecisive d) Domination-submission. (2) Heber found in.his analysis of Et>ry No. 2, The Lost Meat, that significantly more Children in Knoxville and Birmingham gave responses portraying the child in the story as telling the truth than did children in Hamburg and Helsinki. The tells truth theme also appeared significantly more often in the Hamburg and Helsinki stories than in the Karlsruhe, Mexico City and Munich stories. For the response portraying the child as telling a lie, the order of samplings was reversed, with Munich stories having significantly more of this response than the other cities. These results were consistent with the hypotheses. The greater numbwr of children giving lg; themas which Heber found in the Munich stories was thought to account for the fact that the stories from Munich contained significantly fewer responses of submissiog than any of the other samplings, since stories with themas of lie or deception did not usually contain themas of gaggiggign. Domiggtigg themas were found to be significantly more frequentv in Birmingham than in the other samplings, while Karlsruhe had the least frequent domination themas. However, the domination in ' Birmingham was accompanied by a high frequency of :gsistange themas and a low frequency of ggbmissign themas. The Karlsruhe sample con- tained a high frequency of submission and a low frequency of‘gggiptgppg themas. Resistance is a higher level of response to domination than is submission; it is still spontaneous and communicative, whereas submission is close to psychological atrophy. Thus the pattern in Birmingham was as a whole at a higher level of response than that in Karlsruhe. 0n the thema of constructivg behavior by Michael, Birmingham and Hamburg were significantly higher than Munich, Knoxville, and Helsinki, which in turn were significantly higher than.Karlsruhe and Mexico City. On the thema of gopstructive behavio; by;pgphg;,and on stories with inggggative outcomgg, Hamburg, Helsinki, and Birmingham, in that order, were significantly higher than Karlsruhe, Munich, and Mexico City. The results from these three clusters of themas are consistent with the other clusters of themas which represent high levels of spontaneous relating (tellg truth, resistance) in that Karlsruhe, 3. Munich, and Mexico City had the lowest percentages. These results and consistencies strongly support the hypotheses. The Present Study Aimg. The aims of the present study are: (l) to analyze Story No. Q, The Ink Spots, Series A, of the Anderson Incomplete Stories, from data collected in Birmingham, Helsinki, Hamburg, Munich, and Knoxville; (2) to compare outcomes of stories for these samples; ,(3) to make a comparison of results from Story'No. # with results from Story No. 2 obtained by Heber (8), using the stories from the Birmingham sample for sex comparisons and socio-economic comparisons; (u) to find the comparative reSponses of children in each sample to several category clusters of themas; and (5) to study the types of domination mentioned by children in each of the samples. §pbjects. This study is based on data obtained from 1,227 children in the seventh school year. The distribution of subjects in the five samples by sex, socio-economic level and location are given in Table l. The samples are essentially the same as those described by Heber (8) in his study with the exception that the Karlsruhe and Mexican samples ‘have been omitted and the middle socio-economic group in Hamburg is smaller. figphpds and procedure. The methods of collecting and coding the data have been reported by.Anderson and Anderson (2, 3), Geierhaas (6, 7), and Heber (8). For purposes of statistical treatment of the data, each child's story is coded according to themas having psychological significance. The thema categories for Story No. 2 have been listed by Geierhaas (6), along with the description of units used for coding and classifying of stories as to outcome. The categories used for Story No. 4 are to be described under Chapter III of this study. In addition to coding of themas into categories, each story is Judged as to general outcome and is assigned to one of six mutually exclusive outcome classifications. These are described under Chapter IV of this study. In the tabulating of the coded themas it frequently happens that in a given story there are several themas for the same category. In the treatment of data in this study multiple tallies have been dis— regarded and unless specifically stated otherwise, frequencies refer to numbers of children. Within category clusters each child is counted only once. 5. 1114133"1 1 DISTRIBUTION OF EUHBZRS OF BOYS.AHD GIRLS BY SOCIO—ECOKCEIC IEVE BOYS GIRT Socio-Economic Level Socio-Economic Level Location High Middle Low All High Middle low All Total Birmingham 156 . 64 122 53 395 Hel sinki 36 1+0 27 38 1+0 27 208 Hamburg”; 36 57 30 33 53 37 246 Munich 36 37 #8 M3 164 Knoxville 93 121 214 Total 1227 II. RELIABILITY OF CODING As reported.above, considerable effort was made to analyze Story No. 2, The Lost Meat, before analysis was made of Story Be. h, The Ink Spots. The writer has participated.with the Andersons, Geierhaas and Heber in revising the coding categories for Story Be. h. Reliability of coding Story No. h was first established between the writer and Geierhaas on a sample of Karlsruhe stories, and after a lapse of 23 days established again on a sample of English stories. The following percentages of agreement were obtained: 93.5% on cate— gories and 95.5% on outcomes for the Karlsruhe sample, and 96.6% on categories and 100% on outcomes for the Birmingham sample. Since this thesis was to involve a comparison of Story No. 2 and Story No. b for the Birmingham sample, and since Heber had coded all the Birmingham children's Stories No. 2, it was desirable to determine if the writer could code the Birmingham stories reliably with Heber. Reliability between.the two was established with 97.6% agreement on categories and 95% agreement on outcomes. III. COWARISON OF CODING CATEGORIES FOR STORIES NO. 4, mm INK srors, AND NO. 2, mm LOST mm In the design of the Anderson.Incomplete Stories it was desired to have two stories presenting a potential conflict between a child and his mother. Story No. 2, The Lost Meat, involves a boy and his mother, and Story No. 4 involves a girl and her mother. The English texts of the two stories are as follows: Story No. L The Lost Meat. Michael's mother sends him to the store to get one pound of wieners. On the way home he puts the package of meat on the edge of the sidewalk and plays for a short time with his friends. A shepherd dog darts forward quickly, claws half of the wieners out of the package and rushes away with them. Michael wraps up the remaining wieners and takes them home. What does Michael say to his mother? What does his mother do? How does Michael then feel about it? Think about these questions, then fipish this story quickly with a few sentences. S o No The Ink S ots. Susan; is sitting in the living roomnworking on her homework. Suddenly she remembers her mother's new coat. She wonders how she would look in it. When taking the coat off she notices that she had got some ink spots on her mother's new coat. Just as Susan is trying to wipe out the ink spots her mother enters the room. 1The name of the girl used in.Knoxville in 1953 was Elizabeth. The name was changed to Susan for the Birmingham stories in 1954 because it would be shorter for the children to write. What does her mother say? How do they both feel about it? What does Susan say? What does each one do? Think about these questions and finish this story quickly with a few sentences. ‘Abbreviated titles of the coding categories as revised for Story No. 4 are the following: Categories for Susan IV Tells truth spontaneously, the whole truth ID Tells truth spontaneously, in detail 2“ Tells truth after conflict, anxiety, after fantasy lie (whole truth) 2D Tells truth after conflict, anxiety, after fantasy lie (in detail) 3W Tells truth delayed, after question, scolding, punishment (whole truth) 3D Tells truth delayed, after questions, etc. (in detail) 4W Tells truth spontaneously after lie (whole truth) 4D Tells truth spontaneously after lie (in detail) 5W Tells truth after lie, extorted (whole truth) 5D Tells truth after lie, extorted (in detail) 6. Delays explanation, facing reality 8 Plans fantasy lie or deception 9 Lies after weighing the circumstances or after question, after fantasy lie 10 Lies spontaneously, deception ll Constructive action and speaking by Susan 11: Constructive action by Susan gpd the mother 12 Makes spontaneous restitution 121 Restitution by Susan and the mother 13 Constructive thinking and feeling by Susan 13x Constructive thinking and feeling by Susan and the mother 14 Hostile, aggressive action, speaking by Susan 14: Hostile, aggressive action, Speaking by Susan and mother 15 Hostile, aggressive feeling, thinking by Susan 15: Hostile, aggressive feeling, thinking by Susan and mother 17 Regrets telling truth 19 Anxiety, emotion, fear 19x.Anxiety, emotion, by both Susan and the mother 21 Tension reduction 21x Tension reduction by both Susan and the mother 22 Begs, requests, leniency, pardon, forgiveness, sympathy 23 24 24a 25 26 27 28 41a 41b 42 45 55 56a 56b 56c 58a 58b 58¢ Submission: action, obeys Submission: thinking and feeling (non-communication) Susan regrets telling lie Voluntary, spontaneous promise to mother (communication) Promises herself, vows, (non-communication) Promises after punishment, scolding, admonishing and forgiving Negative statement pertaining to Susan, concerning some socially unacceptable act Categories for the Mother Mother perceives from Susan: something is wrong, suspects Mother perceives from non—Susan: ‘something is wrong, suspects Mother questions: fact-finder, calls Susan for questioning Mother communicates to Susan her distrust, accuses her Seeks, or gets outside verification of facts a) Communication to Susan b) Non—communication c) Without seeking, "Deus ex machine" Mother behaves vaguely Feeling of disbelief Mother becomes or feels angry, excited Miscellaneous, unhappy emotion, cries, feels sad Mother accepts Susan, action (communication) Mother's positive feeling, "understands" Mother believes truth Mother believes lie or deceptive maneuver Deprivation of food (after Susan told truth) Deprivation of food (no evidence of truth or lie) Deprivation of food (after Susan told lie or deception) Deprivation of playtime (after truth) Deprivation of playtime (no evidence) Deprivation of playtime (after lie) 60a Restitution under duress (after truth) 60b 60c 62a 62b 62c 64a 64b 64c 66a 66b Restitution under duress (no evidence) Restitution under duress (after lie) Physical punishment (after truth) Physical punishment (no evidence) Physical punishment (after lie) Mother tells father (after truth) Mother tells father (no evidence) Mother tells father (after lie) Punishment by father (after truth) Punishment by father (no evidence) 66c Punishment by father (after lie) 68a Susan sent to bed (after truth) 68b Susan sent to bed (no evidence) 68c Susan sent to bed (after lie)’ 70a Miscellaneous punishment (after truth) 70b Miscellaneous punishment (no evidence) 70c Miscellaneous punishment (after lie) 72a Scolding (after truth) 72b Scolding (no evidence) 72c Scolding (after lie) 73 Susan sent to store, or to get material for repair 74 Mother makes restitution 75a Mother admonishes, re past behavior (after truth) 75b Mother admonishes, re past behavior (no evidence) 75c Mother admonishes, re past behavior (after lie) 76a.Mother admonishes, threatens, re future behavior (after truth) 76b Mother admonishes, threatens, re future behavior (no evidence) 76c Mother admonishes, threatens, re future behavior (after lie) 77 Mother shows tension reduction 78 Mother forgives Susan (power relation) _ 81 Negative statement of mother's unhappy feeling or act A.preliminary attempt was made to code Story No. 4, The Ink Spots, using the same category definitions which had been develOped for Story No. 2, The Lost Meat, simply substituting for the name Michael the name of Susan. Psychological differences in the two stories soon became apparent, however, which called for revision and addition of categories. For example, one question asked after Story No. 2 was ”How did Michael then feel about it?" In Story No. 4 there was the question, "He? do they both feel about it?" This question resulted in many statements in the children's stories which included both parent and child behavior in one tally unit. This type of statement occurred very infrequently in Story No. 2, and when it did it was coded by giving a tally in the mother category and a 11. tally in the Michael category, two tallkm for one verb unit. It was thought that the statistical results would not be invalidated by this treatment, since its occurrence was so rare. In Story No. 4, however, the problem had to be dealt with more directly, as these "both” statements were fairly frequent. If "both" was given two tallies, one in the mother category and one in the Susan category, there would be inflation of the data which would be unwarranted according to the definition of units as one tally for each verb unit. . Accordingly, separate categories for these statements were set up. Categories in which it was possible for a "both" statement to occur were supplemented by a separate category with an "1“ after it for identification. For example, after category 11, construgtizg agtion‘by Sus , category 111 was inserted to include the child's statements of constructive action by Susan gag the mother. Similarly, "1' categories were added to categories 12, l3, 14, 15, 19, and 21. In the analysis of data.gggstrugtivg ggtggp,byp§g§an included tallies in category 11, plus the tallies in category 111. Similarly, ggg: gtgggtive actiog by thg_mothgg included tallies in category 52, ‘mgthg;,agcepts §p§gg, action (communication) plus tallies in category 111. W acting hr Sag-«an era the art”. There are differences in reSponse to the given content of Story Ho. 4 which are brought about by the degree of deve10pment of the narrative, or the point at which the givens are terminated and the child must proceed. 12. In Story No. 4, unlike Story No. 2, the mother has entered the picture when the child is in the midst of his misdeed so that some of the categories in Story No. 2 providing for activity before the child faces the mother are unnecessary. This is the case with category 18, Aggiety, gmotion, before mgggipg_3hg_ggthezg this category has, therefore, been drapped from the manual for Story No. 4. In those rare cases where the storywriter alters the givens to create anxiety before the child faces the mother, it is coded as 19, Anxiety, emotigg, fggg. ‘Another difference in reSponses occurs because in Story No. 4 the storywriter might easily presume that the mother can see the facts about the ink Spots there before her. The significance of 53333 and 11:3,in this story is therefore somewhat altered from that in The Lost Meat story. There was at first some confusion in coding when Susan was represented as telling §_part—truth ouly(i.e., telling her mother that she Just wanted to try the coat on but not saying anything about the ink spots), for it often seemed that the author may have been presuming that the mother already knew the truth. It was decided, then, that except in those cases where it was evident from the context that Susan was trying to deceive, the child should be given the benefit of the doubt, and part-truth would be coded the same as truth. Category 7, part-truth‘gnly,has been omitted from the manual for Story'No. 4. Category 16, Disnlaced emotion_was used in Story No. 2 when Michael became angry at the dog which had taken the meat. There was no similar agent in Story No. 4, and this thema was not found in any of the samples. Category 16 was therefore deleted from the manual for Story No. 4. Confusion and unreliability in the Mother categories was encountered in coding 43a, Mothe; scolds, pgeS'verbal domination or abuse before .tzuthngg.11g, and 43b, Mgthgz,usgs physiga; dgmination before truth qulig. One coder might infer that the author of the story perceived the truth as self-evident, and code verbal or physical domination at the beginning of the author's story in one of the categories for verbal or physical domination after truth or lie (categories 56 through 72, and categories 75 and 76); while another coder might say that Elizabeth had not actually said either the truth or a lie and code it as 43a and b, W mm mm m; Hg. The confusion was resolved by omitting categories 43a and b and adding a third choice, to each of the verbal and physical domination categories, namely, M 1m m gvidmce as, 19, m 9;; L3. The numbering and ' lettering of the domination categories was changed for greater conven- ience. In Story No. 2, for example, categories 56 and 57 were 56, Deprivation of food (after Michael told t ruth) 57, Deprivation of food (after Michael told lie). In Story No. 4 this became 56a, Deprivation of food (after Susan told tinth) 56b, Deprivation of food (no evidence of truth or lie) 56c, Deprivation of food (after Susan told lie or deception). A.source of unreliability in Story No. 2 had been the difficulty in differentiating between category 49, Miscellaneous, gghgnnx emgtign (communicatiog) and category 51, flgthgr'g negativg feeling, feel; sad (gog—communicatiog). These were supposed to separate, among other 14. things, communicated from uncommunicated behavior. But to draw a ‘ line between communicated and uncommunicated behavior was often very difficult. For instance, "The mother looked sad" can mean that those around.her know how she looks and she is communicating her feelings, ~or it can be meant as purely subjective feelings of the mother. It requires an inference to code the statement as either #9 or 51. Category 50, fiotheg.ggig§, is closely related to 49 and.5l, and it seemed that there was little to gain from coding this one particular thema separately. Hence #9, 50, and 51 were combined into 49, gisgellaneous, unhappy emotion, which includes both communicated and uncommunicated behavior. IV. OUTCOMES OE‘STORIES Each child's story was classified according to the outcome of the story as a whole. The outcome classifications have been defined by Anderson and.Anderson (3) and abstracted by Geierhaas (6) as follows: HI. nte ative° P oblem-so vi Outcom The solving of the problem. There must be evidence of high spontaneity in both the mother and her child; also of harmony and working together, active communication and evidence of mutual confidence. For harmonious cooPeration there must be active communication in the place of mere thinking and fantasy. ”D-R, ngination-Resistance Outcome IA, .A process of hostility and aggressiveness. There must be evidence of mutual hostility, active tension, signs of working against each other; the mother's hostility can be eXpressed in the form of domination (punishment, scolding, exacting promises), that she uses, and the child's resistance need not be expressed orally or by action but simply on the think- ing and feeling level. flbiguous , Indecigive Ogtcome There is lack of closure, low level of communication, story is essentially unstructured. There is little or no inter— action between the mother and the child. Problem has not been faced, and no attempt has been made at its positive or negative solution. Relation between parent and child little changed at end from that at beginning. The experience did not serve as a basis for social learning. 'D-S, Domination-Submission Outcome ID, NS, Authoritarian person makes the decisions, has the answers. Child merely submits, conforms, obeys. These stories show mother's domination and Michael's subsequent submission. Child accepts mother‘s verdict, submits to mother's ideas, punishment, and often eXpresses his submissive acceptance in the form of a promise, vow, or submissive action. 29mination, with no statement of resistance or submission Submission, with no statement of domination.“ Subdivisions of submissiqn_and domination were used by Geierhaas and, although tabulations have been made the data are not treated by these subdivisions. In this study the outcome defined as domination- submission will include domination outcomes with no evidence in the story of resistance or submission, and submission outcomes with no evidence in the story of domination. Results The results of these classifications are shown in Table 2 as total numbers and percentages for the samples from each city. The mass of the percentages of outcomes fell in the two lower levels, ambiggous and domination-submission outcomes, where communica— tion is generally one-way or altogether lacking. For four of the I five samples, the largest percentages-of outcomes were in the domination- submission classification. By contrast the Helsinki sample showed significantly fewer figmination-submission outcomes than any of the other samplings. The highest percentage of children in Helsinki wrote stories with ambiguous. unstructured outcomes. Furthermore, the percentages of ambiguous outcomes for the Helsinki sample were significantly higher than percentages of ambiguous outcome for any of the other samples. There were significantly fewer ambiguous outcomes in the sample from.Birmingham.than if any of the other samples, Without exception, the percentages of gggygtion-resistance out- comes were lower than the percentages of integrative outcomes. The Birmingham sample, in addition to having fewer ambiggous outcomes than the other samples, had significantly more gpmination-resistange_outcomes and significantly more integratixg outcomes than the other samples. TABLE 2 OUTCOMES OF STORY #4. RDCUBHCIES 11:11) Pmcmfrgcns FOR 0511133321 111 EACH OF F1113 CITIES 1:1 TEL: OUTCOBES 37‘ 151;; n 3110121113 CLASS 1112:) ACCORDING TO OUTCOMES OF I (Emma-arms, PROBIEI—L—SOLVING), D-R (DQ-th’fl‘lON—RfilSISTANGIE), A (gs-BIGUOUS, Il-TDIJCISIVE), mm Tm; SUM or D (1301-; memos), S SUBEIISSIO‘II), .1111) D-S (DOE-1117.13IOl~T—SUISI~-1ISSIOH) Bi mingmam H els ink'i Hamburg Iviuni ch Knoxvi lle Out come 11 3% N ft N -;a II 5!. N p’ I 90 22.8* 32 15.1} 30 12.2 21 12.8 29 13.6 D-R f 25 16.3* 12 5.8 13 5.3 12 7.3 7 3.3 A 73 18.5* 93 “it-7* 84 34-1 55 33-5 59 27.6 D. S, and 13-3 207 52A 71 3L».1* 119 118.1» 76 116.3 119 55.6 ¥ —. Total 395 208 2% 161+ 211+ *Sié’nificzmtl-y different from the percentages of other population samplings at the same level of outcomes, by chi-square test, at the .05 level or better, 18. Discussion Birmingham outcomes were significantly different from the out— comes of the other samples in three of the four outcome classifications. Though the amount 0f.Q2EiBé£lQE.and submissiun shown in Birmingham was similar to that of Munich, Hamburg, and Knoxville, the amount of ambiguity was significantly less than that of the other samples, while the amount of domination-resistance and Luggggggigu shown were significantly higher than the other samples. As mentioned in the introduction, Heber also found that Birmingham stories contained sig- nificantly more resistance than the other samples. In a culture tending more toward a democratic than an authoritar- ian atmosphere one would expect more spontaneity, more social problem- solving in conflict, more initiative and individual particiption even in the form of resistance in conflict. The Birmingham children have put these characteristics into their stories. Inluuuination—submiguion outcomes the Birmingham children were second only to the children in Knoxville in percentages, which is not consistent with the hypotheses concerning findings in a democratic culture. The percentage of children in Birmingham who wrote stories with dumination-submission outcomes was not significantly different, however, from the percentages of Children in.Hamburg and Munich. Further understanding of this apparent inconsistency is given in the discussion of cross-cultural comparisons and of types of domination. Helsinki differed from the other samples in having significantly lower percentages of domination—submission and significantly higher 19 percentages of ambiguous outcomes, while the percentages of domination— zesistange and of integration in Helsinki were not very different from percentages in Munich, Hamburg and Knoxville. Heber, too, found the Helsinki sample in Story No. 2 lower than the other samples in the category cluster of domination themas. The history and traditions of Finland indicate that it is a land of a freedom—loving people, who have a high sense of personal reaponsibility, honesty, integrity and respect for the individual person. Their respect for the indiv- idual may even be confused with a cautiousness in communication which in this study is expressed in the significantly higher outcomes of ambiguity and which is furthermore inconsistent with the hypotheses about high participation in a democracy. In the outcomes of their stories the Finnish children have revealed human relations between the mother and child which, though low in domiuution and low in submission, are also low in communication. The Finnish children are second only to Birmingham in iuteggativg or problempsolying outcomes and, though higher in frequency than Knoxville, Munich and Hamburg, in that order, are not significantly different from the children in these other samples. The relative frequencies of outcomes for Munich and Hamburg were quite similar to each other. Heber, in his cross-cultural comparisons of clusters of themas, found that in many clusters Munich and Hamburg were significantly different from each other, leading to the con- clusion that the concept of a German national character is not neces— sarily valid. It will be found in a later section of this thesis that the analysis of categories for Story No. 4 yielded results for Munich and Hamburg similar to Heber's results with Story No. 2. The coding by categories is a finer, more detailed analysis and brings out differences in Specific themas. Coding by outcomes is a more gross measure and is not sensitive to some of the differences revealed in the analysis of categories. On the other hand, the coding of categories is a more static analysis of the process of relating, with little or no regard for time, sequence, or evolution of human relations as the story unfolds. The outcome when coded as domination-submission is probably static in the manner revealed by coding separate categories. If the story moves toward a constructive, integrative end, it can be filled with themas of domination, lying, evasion, or resistance up to the point of release in the story into an integrative relating. Knoxville had the greatest number of domination—submission out- comes, quite contrary to the hypotheses and eXpectations, V. THE BIRMINGHAM, ENGLAND SAMPLE Sex Comparisons in Story No. 4 and Story No. 2 Categories by which Story No. # was coded have been grouped into clusters of themas as described below. Constructive behavior by mother 11: Constructive action by Susan and the mother 121 Restitution by Susan and the mother 13: Constructive thinking and feeling by Susan and the mother 52 Mother accepts Susan, action (communication) 53 Mother's positive feeling, "understands“ 54 Mother believes truth 74 Mother makes restitution Constructive behavior by Susan ll Constructive action by Susan 11: Constructive action by Susan and the mother 12 Makes spontaneous restitution 12x Restitution by Susan and the mother 13 Constructive thinking and feeling by Elizabeth 131 Constructive thinking and feeling by Susan and the mother Susuu tells truth 1W Tells truth spontaneously, the whole truth 1D Tells truth spontaneously, in detail 2W Tells truth after conflict, anxiety, after fantasy lie (whole truth) 2D Tells truth after conflict, anxiety, after fantasy lie (in detail) 3W Tells truth delayed, after question, scolding, punishment (whole truth) 3D Tells truth delayed, after questions, etc. (in detail) Resistancg 1% Hostile, aggressive action, Speaking by Susan 1&1 Hostile, aggressive action, speaking by Susan and mother 15 Hostile, aggressive feeling, thinking by Susan 15x Hostile, aggressive feeling, thinking by Susan and mother 17 Regrets telling truth 22. Anxiety 19 Anxiety, emotion, fear 19:.Anxiety, emotion, by both Susan and the mother 21 Tension reduction 21: Tension reduction by both Susan and the mother 22 Begs, requests, leniency, pardon, forgiveness, sympathy Submission 23 Submission: action, obeys 2“ Submission: thinking and feeling (non-communication) 24a Susan regrets telling lie 25 Voluntary, spontaneous promise to mother (communication) 26 Promises herself, vows, (non-communication) ‘ 27 Promises after punishment, scolding, admonishing and forgiving 28 Negative statement pertaining to Susan, concerning some socially unacceptable act Domination 56a Deprivation of food (after Susan told truth) 56b Deprivation of food (no evidence of truth or lie) 56c Deprivation of food (after Susan told lie or deception) 58a Deprivation of playtime (after truth) 58b Deprivation of playtime (no evidence) 58c Deprivation of playtime (after lie) .60a Restitution under duress (after truth) 60b Restitution under duress (no evidence) 60c Restitution under duress (after lie) 62a Physical punishment (after truth) 62b Physical punishment (no evidence) 62c Physical punishment (after lie) 66a Punishment by father (after truth) 66b Punishment by father (no evidence) 66c Punishment by father (after lie) 68a Susan sent to bed (after truth) 68b Susan sent to bed (no evidence) 68c Susan sent to bed (after lie) 70a Miscellaneous punishment (after “truth) 70b Miscellaneous punishment (no evidence) 70c Miscellaneous punishment (after lie) 72a Scolding (after truth) 72b Scolding (no evidence) 72c Scolding (after lie 73 Susan sent to store, or to get material for repair 75a Mother admonishes, re past behavior (after truth) 23. 75b Mother admonishes, re past behavior (no evidence) 75c Mother admonishes, re past behavior (after lie) 76a Mother admonishes, threatens, re future behavior (after truth) 76b Mother admonishes, threatens, re future behavior (no evidence) 76c Mother admonishes, threatens, re future behavior (after lie) The number of children in each sample who gave one or more reSponses in each of these clusters was counted; that is, no child was counted twice in any one cluster. The number of children in each cluster is then expressed as a percentage of number of children in the total sample. Table 3 shows the numbers and percentages of boys as compred to girls who gave responses in each cluster for Story No. 4 and for Story No. 2 (adapted from Heber's study). Numbers and percentages of children's stories coded at the four levels of outcomes are also shown in Table 4. Only integrative outcomes were reported for Heber's study of Story No. 2. As seen in Table 3, of the eight category clusters for Story No. 4, about Susan and her mother, the percentages of girls were greater in seven clusters and significantly greater in five. For Story No. 2, The Lost Meat, which concerned a boy and his mother, of the same eight category clusters, percentages of girls were higher in seven clusters and significantly higher in one cluster. In Story No. 4, the thema cluster which showed the greatest difference was submission, where the girls had significantly more reSponses than boys at better than the .001 level by chi-Square test. Other themas in which girls gave significantly more reSponses than boys, at the .01 level, were resistance, anxiety, constructive behavig: by Susan, and gpnstructive behavior by mother. Two of the significant differences between boys and girls in Story Do. 4, in TABLE 3 BIRMINGHAM, EHGLLND: DUMBERS AND PERCENTAGES OF BOYS ADD OF GIRLS WITH THEEAS CODED IN EIGHT CATLGORY CLUSTHRS, AND ACCORDING TO CLASSIFICRTION OF OUTCOMES OF STORIES HO. 4.AND NO. 2 Story Ho. 4 Story Ho. 21 Boys Girls Boys Girls Category Cluster H s N s N s N 3 Susan tells truth 113 51.4 82 46.9 127 57.4 103 58.2 Susan tells lie 51 23.2 46 26.3 61 27.6 62 35.0 Resistance 28 12.7 43 24.6* 46 20.8 33 18.6 Anxiety 180 77.3 156 89.1* 120 54.3 108 .61.0 Submission 76 34.6 113 58.9* 87 39.4 118 66.7* Domination 146 66.4 130 74.3 171 77.4 141 79.7 Constructive (child) 64 29.1 76 43.4* 15 6.8 28 15.8‘ Constructive (mother) 120 54.6 122 69.7* 31 14.0 33 18.6 Outcome Integrative 41 18.6 49 28.0* 10 4.5 8 4.5 Domination-resistance 17 7.7 10 4.6 (Not reported by Heber) Ambiguous 51 23.2* 2 12.6 " " " “ Domination-submission 111 50.5 94 53.7 " " " " Total N 220 175 221 177 *Significantly higher than the freq. for the other sex by chi-square test at the .01 level or better. 1 Data for Story Do. 2 adapted from Heber (8) 25. the category clusters for submissigp and constructive behavior by moths; were more frequent in Story No. 2 for girls, though not sig— nificantly so. The thema cluster of resistance was different in Story No. 2, where more boys gave resistance reSponses than girls. The distribution of tgpth themas in the two stories was in Opposite directions for boys and girls, the boys being higher in Story No. 4, and the girls higher in Story No. 2. The distribution of 1;; themas and domination themas was in the same direction for otb stories, the girls being higher in both, though these differ— ences were not significant. In Story No. 4, the girls gave significantly more integrative outcomes than boys, while in Story No. 2 there was no difference between boys and girls. Ambigugus outcomes in Story No. 4 were significantly more fre- quent for boys than for girls. For ggminotion-rosistancg and domination- submission outcomes boys and girls were not significantly different. Discussion of Sex Comparisons 2 seven of the eight clusters for Story No. 4, and in seven of the eight clusters for Story No. 2, frequencies for girls were higher. This is consistent with well-known observations that girls deVFlOp verbal facility earlier and are generally superior to boys in verbal fluency. It will be reported later that girls tended to write longer stories than boys. In writing more, the chances are greater that girls will supply a greater varie ty of themas, which seems to have been the case in Table 3. In Story No. 4, the five category clusters in which girls were 0‘ significantly higher than boys were: constructive ehavior by mgthgz, gongtructivc behavior by_§u§gn,_rcsistance, anxiety, gag submission. The fact of five significant differences related to the story about a girl and her mother in contrast with only one significant difference in the story about a boy suggests that girls may have been more involved in Story No. 4 than in Story No. 2. There were large differences in several of the category clusters between the level of p roentages of reaponse for Story No. 4 and Story'No. 2. Both boys and girls gave much higher percentages of reaponse for Story No. 4 to the category clusters constructive behavio; by mgther, gonstrugtivg behavior bz,the ghild, and anxiety than for Story No. 2. The differences in structure of the two stories accounts in part for the differences in these category clusters. These are the category clusters in which many "both" themas were given in the children's stories. For erample, if the child wrote "The mother and Susan both felt sorry about the ink spots," tallies were counted for Susan and for the mother. p1; and,tgu£h,category clusters were lower in percentages in Story No. 4 than in Story No. 2. This was probably because the children who completed the stories often assumed that the mother in Story No. 4 could see Susan wiping the ink spots out of the coat and at once knew the truth, so there was not as much need for Susan to tell either the truth or a lie. In Story No. 2, however, there was a time interval between the loss of the meat and meeting the mother. The children who wrote the stories had to make up some way for the mother to find out about the lost meat, and it was usually by having Michael tell her, either truthfully, or with a lie. Submission and domination category clusters had lower percentages in Story No. 4 than in Story No. 2. This, too, may be because the structure of Story No. 4 and the nature of the questions asked after it were conducive to more themas of constructive behavior and to fewer of submissive and dominative behavior. Integrative outcomes were more frequent for Story No. 4 than for Story No. 2. This is probably because working together of mother and child is called forth by the questions, and working together often results in problem-solving. a part of the definition for integrative behavior. In all these changes in nercentages except in the tguth,and resistance clusters, the relationship of reaponses between boys and girls remained constant; where the girls were higher in response than boys in the clusters for Story No. 2, they were also higher in Story No. 4. This consistency attests to the reliability of the Stories as a measuring instrument. In the tggth cluster, boys were higher in.percentage of resPonse in Story No. 4, and girls were higher in percentage of reSponse in Story No. 2, but differences were small and not significant. There was an inconsistency in the resistance cluster, where boys' responses were higher in Story No. 2 and girls' responses were signif— icantly higher in Story No. 4. Boys tended to show more resistance [é I “ € ‘ v v l I . I 1 C Q 0 v .( O ' S . c O a h ' ' . . o I V f C . 1 ‘ r . g I t - ’ ‘ V I - . — v I o g I in a story about a boy and girls showed more resistance in a story about a girl. It is noted, however, in Table No. 3, that in the out- come of dominationpresistance in this story about a girl the frequencies were higher for boys, though the difference was not significant. Socio-economic Comparisons Table 4 gives the numbers and percentages of children from high and low socio-economic levels in Birmingham with themas coded in the several category clusters, and according to outcomes for Stories No. 4 and No. 2. As seen in Table 4, the high socio-economic group gave higher percentages of response to all he clusters of themas except the jggfih,cluster. For Story No. 4 in one category cluster, assistance, the high socio-economic group was significantly higher at the .001 level, by chi-square test. In another category cluster, ghild tells 'lig, the high socio—economic group was significantly higher at the .10 level. Stories of children in the high socio-economic group were more frequently given integrative, domination-resistance, and dgminatign- submission outcomes. The low secio—economic group was higher in percentage of response only in the 1;; cluster. More ambigpgus outcomes were given in the low socio-economic group. The differences between high and low socio-economic levels found by Heber in Story No. 2 can be said to be consistent with the differ- ences reported in Table 4 for Story No. 4. The three significant differences reported by Heber are confirmation of differences in the same direction for Story No. 4. The inconsistencies in socio-economic 29. TABLE 4 BIRMINGHH’Z , HIGH-JD: NUMBERS All D PERCY“1 {PAGES OF CHILDREN FROM HIGH AND L03 SOCIO—ECOHOMIC LEVELS WITH THEEAS CODED IN THE SEVERAL CATEGORY CLUSTEP‘, 53D ACCORDIHG TO OUTCOKES FOR STORIES NO. 4vAED NO. 2 Story no. 4 Story No. 21 High Low High Low Category Cluster H % N t N 6 N l I o'fl Constructive (mother) 173 62.2 69 59.0 54 19.3* 10 8.5 Constructive (child) 104 37.4 36 30.8 36 12.9* 7 5.9 Child tells truth 130 46.8 65 55.6 162 57.9 68 57.6 Child tells lie 75 27.0 22 18.8 84 30.0 39 33.1 Resistance 62 22.3* 9 7.7 67 23.9* 12 10.2 Anxiety 233 83.8 93 79.5 162 ~57.9 66 55.9 Submission 128 46.0 51 43.6 144 51.4 61. 51.7 Domination 196 70.5 80 68.4 217 77.5 95 80.5 Outcome Integrative 65 23.4 25 21.4 15 5.4 3 2.5 Domination-resistance 21 7.6 4 3.4 (Not reported by Heber) Ambiguous 45 16.2 28 23.9 ” " " " Submission, Domination, 14? 52.9 60 51.3 " " " " and Domination—sub— mission Total 278 117 ‘ 280 118 *Significantly different from the other socio-economic level at the .05 level or better, by chi-square test. 1Data for Story No. 2 adapted from Heber (8). 30. BHEDEHGIHG ED: TABLE 5 IIJLBIRS RID PERCBUTnGQS CODED IN EIGHT CATQGORY CLUSTERS, ADD INTLSRhTIVE OUTCOELS, 3 0211133131: WITH T5335 0F STORIES no. 4 ill-Tel) NO. 2 Story No. 4 C Story No. 2 ,J Category Cluster N p N p Constructive (mother) 186 47.1 64 16.1 Constructive (child) 14° 35.9 ‘43 [0.8 Susan tells truth 194 49.1 230 57.8 Susan tells lie 97 24.6 123 30.9 Resistance 71 18.0 79 19.8 Anxiety 326 82.5 228 57.3 Submission 179 45.3 205 51.5 Domination 276 69.9 312 78.4 Integrative outcome 90 22.8 18 4.5 Total N 395 , 398 31. comparisons between the category clusters for Story No. 4 and Story No. 2 are small and represent non-significant differences. In general, there were more children in the high socio-economic level than.were found.among the children in the low socio—economic level giving responses in the several category clusters. Exceptions represented only small and non-significant differences. The differences between Story No. 4 and Story No. 2 in levels of percentages with clusters which were noted.and discussed under sex comparisons are also apparent in the socio-economic comparisons. Story No. 4 — Story No. 2 Comparisons Table 5 gives the total frequencies and percentages of responses in all the clusters of themas and in integrative outcomes for Story Be. 4 and for Story No. 2 for the Birmingham sampling. Percentages for Story No. 4 were significantly higher at the .001 level, by chi- square test, for the thema clusters gonstructive behavior by gggggg, constructiv; behavio; 1m;jhg,ghild, and for integrative outcomes. Percentages of response for Story No. 4 were significantly higher at the .01 level for the thema cluster anxiety.. Thema clusters inwhich percentages of response were leer for Story No. 4 than for Story Be. 2 were: 5211; taugh (.02 level), 3511; ii; (.05 level), snbmission (.10 level), and domination (.01 level). Results of only one thema cluster, resistanc , were not different at any level of significance. Discussion There were significant differences between Story No. 4 and Story No. 2 in frequency of response in all clusters of themas except 32. one. Some of the reasons for the differences have been pointed out in the discussion of sex differences. It is to be shown later that for the most part the differences occur consistently in all the samples, so that the relationship of each sample to the other samples is fairly stable from one story to the other, within each cluster of themas. The stories are structured differently and they evoke different responses from the children. In Story No. 4 the mother is already present, the truth is practically self-evident. A: long as there is no chance of evading, the situation demands that something be done to rectify it. This is not the case in Story No. 2, where Michael is alone when he discovers his dilemma -- there is greater opportunity to take devious routes to a solution. This difference in the nature of the Incomplete Stories accounts in part for the fact that so many children reaponded differently to Story No. 4 than they did to Story No. 2. 1‘2- VI. CROSS-CULTURAL COMPARISONS Category C uster: anstructive thinking, e-lir , and agtign by gather. Table 6 presents the numbers and percentages of children who wrote themas of constructive bghgzig; by th§_mgthgz in each of the five samples for Story No. 4 and Story No. 2. Hamburg children gave the highest percentage of reaponse, and it was significantly higher than the Knoxville and Munich samples at the .05 level or better. The order of the samples from highest to lowest percentages of response is exactly the same as that found by Heber for Story No. 2. Heber found the percentages of response in Hamburg, Helsinki, and Birmingham samples to be significantly higher than the Knoxville and Munich samples, at a .05 level or better. TABLE 6 nunssas.snn Psacsxiscss or CHILDREN IN’EACH or THE SAMPLINGS WITH THEMAS conan IN THE CATEGORY CLUSTER, CQNSTRUCTIEE zglrxing, gastlnc,.igg_gggigg_§1 Morass r03 STORIES no. 4vAND NO. 2 STORI Ho. 4 STORY N0. 2 Size of Size of git: . Sammde ~‘;§ 1% City Sample gi_ gfi Hamburg 246 139 56.5 Hamburg 281 5n 19.2 Helsinki 208 102 49.0 Helsinki 209 35 16.7 Birmingham 395 186 n7.1 Birmingham 398 64 16.1 Knoxville 214 99 46.3 Knoxville 217 21 9.7 Munich 164 72 43.9 Munich 165 10 6.1 Catfegogz Cluster: Constructive thinking, feeling, gag ggtign by M. The numbers and percentages of children who wrote theme of constructive behavior by the child in Story No. 4 and Story No. 2 are shown in Table 7. The absolute level of percentages is much higher for Story No. 4 than for Story No. 2.. None of the differences between samples was significant in either story. The rank order of samples from highest to lowest percentages was different in the two stories. TABLE? NUMBERS AND ERCELMGES OF CHILDREN IN EACH 03‘ THE SAMPLINGS WITH Twas COD’ED IN THE CATEGORY CLUSTER, CONSTRUCTIVE W ELLEZQ, mm ACTION LY. SUCn‘xN IOR STORIES 130.14 AND NO. 2 STOBI NO. 4 STORY NO. 2 Size of Size of City Sample 11 5% City Samb_1__e L 56 Knoxville 214 79 36.9 Birmingham 398 43 10.8 Birmingham 395 140 35.4 Hamburg 281 27 9.6 Helsinki 208 71 34.1 Munich 165 13 7.9 Hamburg 246 78 31.7 Knoxville 217 14 6.5 ‘ Munich 164 49 29.9 Helsinki 209 9 4.3 ' Categog Cluster: M gills. £13113. Table 8 shows the numbers and percentages of children in each sample who wrote themas of Sugar; 1911; m, for Story No. 4 and Story No. 2. All the samples except Munich showed for this category cluster fewer responses in Story No. 4 than in Story No. 2, but the differences were not large nor significant. The percentage of reSponss in the Knoxville sample was significenitly higher than the percentage for any of the other samples at the .05 35 . level of significance by chi-square test. The percentage of reSponse in the Munich sample was significantly lower than the percentages in the Birmingham, Hamburg and Knoxville samples. In Story No. 2, the rank ordering of samples by percentage of response, from highest to lowest, was Knoxville, Birmingham, Hamburg, Helsinki and Munich. TABLE 8 mmsas m PHHCHrTACHS or CHILDREN IN EACH or THE SAMPLINGS WITH THELHS 00133.11 IN TH E CATEGORY CLUSTER, SUSAN TELLS TEETH, FOP. STCHIHS NO. 4 AND 110. 2 STORY N0. 4 STORY N0. 2 Size of Size of City Sample H 7‘6 City Sample 11 % Knoxville 214 130 60.7 Knoxville 217 140 64.5 Hamburg 246 127 51.6 Birmingham 398 230 57.8 Birmingham 395 194 49 .1 Hamburg 281 148 52.? Helsinki 208 89 42.8 Helsinki 209 104 49.8 Munich 164 6+ 39 .0 Munich 165 58 35 .2 m Cluster: _S_1,3_s__a,n tellg lie. The percentages of children in each sample who wrote themas of £9533 tell; m are shown in Table 9. In all samples there were lower frequencies of response in this thema for Story No. 4 than for Story No. 2. In both stories Munich was significantly higher than all the other samples in frequency of mm 3.2.11.3. Li; themas. In Story No. 2 Hamburg was found to be significantly higher than~ Helsinki and Birmingham and these in turn were significantly higher than Knoxville. In Story No. 4 Hamburg and 36. “to. ,\ -_-.- -. -I n -. ‘ v . p..- . 4 - fl“ — _.-_ -_.- .- V I O a" .. ' O Birmingham were significantly higher than Helsinki and Knoxville in frequency of reaponse of child tells lie. Knoxville was lowest in both samples. TABLE 9 NUMBERS.AND PERCENTAGES OF CHILDREN IN EACH OF THE SAMPLINGS WITH THEMAS CODED IN THE CATEGORY CLUSTER, SE§AE_TELLS LIE, FOR STORIES N0. 4vAND NO. 2 STOHY'NC. 4 STOBI NO. 2 Size of Size of City Sample N 5 City Sample H % Munich 164 61 37.2 Munich 165 106 64.2 Hamburg. 246 68 27.6 Hamburg 281 123 43.8 Birmingham 395 9 24.6 Helsinki 209 68 32.5 Helsinki 208 35 16.8 Birmingham 398 123 30.9 Knoxville ‘214 30 14.0 Knoxville 217 42 19.3 Qgfiggggy'Clustez: Resistancg. In Table 10 are shown the frequencies and percentages of children in eadh sampling who wrote a thema of zgsistance for Story No. 4 and for Story Ho. 2. For Story Ho. 2 the Birmingham sample was significantly higher in frequency than the other samples. For Story No. 4-Birmingham had the highest frequency, but it was not significantly higher than any of the other samples except the lowest one, Knoxville. It was significantly different from Knoxville at the .05 level by chi-square test. The order of ranking of samples was different for Story No. 4 than for Story No. 2 but did not contra- dict the one significant difference found in Story No. 2. 87. . I p . . .. . ‘ ‘ ., .ga . Q - 7' . 4 _,.., - ' . . 4. - . V “\ . o c A k I i A ‘ o -._.. ... , . . . _- a 7'. . . ,~ . e. - . Y ‘ C . e . . . . k . . .. . . _— . - fl ‘ . P-.. - -.- A ..-_l. - ~___ ' e 0 o ' o ‘ o .4 ., ~ . . . k i ,, .. _ F”-__..- 7-. - . . I ‘. : ‘ . . .- - ' ‘ ‘ .u . ‘ . C - O ‘ H . v ' ’ '.- q .. _»_ . .- ’r~ . . . C -s- .0 . . . .‘ t ‘ n . ~ ‘1: - v' ~- . . _. . . . . 4 . , . - n _ ‘ . e o .. '~ ' ‘ u ‘7‘ ' D .. - . - .- ~.. _. ‘ ' ‘ _ - - . . _ , . i- - V . . TABLE 10 NUMBERS AND PERCL AGES OF CHILDREN IN EACH OF THE SAMPLINGS UITH.THEMAS CODED IN THE CATEGORY CLUSTER, RESISTANCE, FOR STORIES N0. 4vAND N0. 2 STORY HO. 4 STORY N0. 2 Size of Size of City Sample I! % City Saniple N )4: ~‘ Birmingham 395 71 18.0 Birmingham 398 79 19.8 Helsinki 208 34 16.4 Hamburg 281 29 10.3 Hamburg 246 28 11.4 Knoxville 217 20 9.2 Munich 164 18 11.0 Helsinki 209 18 8.6 Knoxville 208 15 7.0 Munich 165 12 7.3 Qateggzz Clustez: Angigty. The frequency and percentages of response in each sample for the category cluster of gpgigtz are shown in Table 11 for Story No. 4 and Story No. 2. The absolute level of the percentages was much higher for Story No. 4 than for Story No. 2. In Story No. 4 the Birmingham sampling had a higher percentage of reSponse than the other samples, significant at the .10 level by chi-square test. The percentage of the Helsinki sample was higher than the Hamburg and Knoxville samples, significant at the .02 level, and the Hamburg and Knoxville samples in turn were higher than the Munich sample, significant at the .01 level. For Story No. 2 the Birmingham sample had the highest percentage, but it was not signif- icantly higher than the Hamburg and Helsinki samples. Birmingham, Hamburg and Helsinki percentages were significantly higher than the Munich percentage for Story No. 2, and the Munich.percentage was 38. -._ -0— . ..g a. pew ‘a .v a“. .. ~ - . . -..- in turn significantly higher than the Knoxville sample. The significant differences found in Story'No. 2 for this cluster, where Munich was significantly higher than.Knoxville in percentage of response, was contradicted in Story Ho. 4, where Knoxville was significantly higher than Munich in percentage of reaponse. TABLE 11 NUMBERS ANDPERCENTAGES OF CHILDREN IN EACH OF THE SAMPLINGS WITH THEMAS CODED IN THE CATEGORY CLUSTER. NXI T , FOR STORIES N0. 4-AND NO. 2 STORE'NO. u srosx NO. 2 Size of Size of City Sample N at City Sample N % Birmingham 395 326 82.5 Birmingham 398 228 57.3 Helsinki 208 160 76.9 Hamburg 281 152 54.1 Hamburg 246 16L» 66.7 Helsinki 209 110 - 52.6 Knoxville 214 140 65.4v Munich ' 165 63 38.2 Munich 164 81 49. Knoxville 217 28 12.9 Categggy Clustegx Submission. The frequencies and percentages of children who gave a response of gghmiggigg in Story No. 4 and in Story No. 2 are shown in.Table 12. The frequencies were all much higher for Story Ho. 2 than they were for Story No. 4. The Birmingham sample gave a higher frequency of response than the Knoxville and Helsinki samples, which in turn gave higher frequencies than Hamburg and Munich. Differences were significant at the .01 level by chi- square test. For both stories, the Knoxville and Helsinki samples were significantly higher than the Munich sample in frequency of response to the submission thema. The significant difference found in Story Ho. 2 was therefore not contradicted in Story No. 4. TABLE 12 mamas mnmscmmsss or CHILDREN IN EACH or THE smmmss WITH rssmas consn IN THE carsoonr CLUSTER, SUBMISSION, FOR STORIES no. 4 Am) N0. 2 STORY HO. 4 STOHI NO. 2 Size of Size of City Sample N % City Sample 3 f Birmingham 395 179 45.3 Knoxville 217 124 57.1 Knoxville 214 72 33.6 Helsinki 2209 115 55 .o Helsinki 208 68 32.7 Hamburg 281 148 52.7 Hamburg 246 53 21.5 Birmingham 398 205 51.5 Munich 164 32 19.5 Munich 165 74 44.8 Categogz Clustgr: Dominatiog. Fre quencies and percentages of reSponse of the children in the five samples with a thema of domination are shown in Table 13. The frequencies of the Birmingham, Munich and Hamburg samples, in that order, were significantly higher at the .05 level than the frequencies of the Knoxville sample, which was in turn higher than the frequency of the Helsinki sample, significant at the .001 level. For Story No.l+, also,Birmingham was highest and significantly higher than all other samples in frequency of reaponse; there were no other significant differences. In this cluster, too, the significant difference found in Story No. 2 was not contradicted by results of Story No. 4. TABLE 13 NUMBERS AND PERCERTAGRS OF CHILDREN IR EACH OF THE SAMPLINGS WITH THEMAS CODED IN THE CATEGORY CLUSTHR, DOMIIATION. FOR STORIES NO. 4.KND NO. 2 STOBI Ho. 4 STORY Ho. 2 Size of Size of City Sample H 2 City Sample H % Birmingham. 395 276 69.9 Birmingham 398 312 78.4 Munich 164 108 65.9 Knoxville 217 141 65.0 Hamburg 246 159 64.6 Hamburg 281 171 60.9 Knoxville 214 116 54.2 Helsinki 209 121 57.9 Helsinki 208 78 37.5 Munich 165 93 56.4 Table 14 gives for Story No. 4 the average number of thema tallies per story in each of the samples, according to sex groups, socio- economic groups, and, for the Munich and Hambung samples, sex compar- isons of Stories No. 4 and No. 2. In all of the samples, girls wrote longer stories than boys. In all of the samples except Hamburg, the high socio—economic group wrote longer stories. In Hamburg and Munich, girls wrote longer stories for Story No. 4 than for Story No. 2. In Munich, boys wrote longer stories for Story No. 2 than they did for Story No. 4. In Hamburg, boys wrote slightly Shorter stories for Story No. 2 than they did for Story No. 4. Ln - when a 1'4 .AVERAGE NUMBER.OF THEMA TALLIES P33 STORY 303 SHORT HO. 4 TN EACH OF THE SAMPLES ACCORDING TO SEX GROUPS AND SOCIO—ECONOMIC GROUPS: FOR THE MUNICH AND HAMBURG SAMPLES. SEX COMPARISONS OF STORIES N0. 4'AND NO. 2 Sex Comparisons Boys Girls Birmingham 9.8 155.5 Helsinki 5.6 8.1 Hambung 7.4 10.1 Munich 4.7 8.8 KnoxVille 6.5 706 Socio—economic Comparisons High 110w Birmingham 13.0 10.7 HB1 8111121 8.? 5.8 Hamburg 8.9 6.8 Munich 7.1 6.9 Boys Girls Story 4 7.4 10.1 Hambung Story 2 700 903 Stnry L" be? 808 Munich Story 2 5.6 6.6 Discussion of cross—cultural Comparisons One question which might come to mind about the Anderson Incomplete Stories, and about any projective device, for that matter, is, Do the children give consistent responses from story to story in the series? .Another question: Do children make up a plot for one story, and then just repeat the same plot in the rest of the stories? A partial check on these questions was made by comparing the out- come level of Story No. 2 for each child with the outcome level for his Story No. 4. In the total sampling of 1,227 children, there were 292 children whose outcomes for Stories No. 2 and No. 4 were coded at the same level. This means that the preponderance of the children did not repeat the same story for Story No. 4 that they had written for Story No. 2. Now the question arises, If children write different stories from one to another of the.Anderson Incomplete Stories, can there be con- sistency in the results? The data in Tables 7 through 14 indicate that there was consistency. Tables 7 through 14 have presented numbers and percentages of children in each of the hamplings‘with themas coded in the eight category clusters, for Stories No. 4 and.No. 2, respectively. In comparing the percentages of the samples relative to each other, it was found that the results for Story No. 4 were in general agreement with the significant results found for Story No. 2, with only one exception. The following statements may be made for both stories: (a) In ggngtnlgtive behavio; p1 _t__h_e_ m, Hamburg, Helsinki, and Birmingham had higher percentages than Knoxville and Munich: 43. (b) (e) (d) (a) (f) (s) In my}: _t_;l_1_s_ m, Knoxville had the highest percentage, and Munich had the lowest percentage; In ghild_jgllg_ ie, Munich, then Hamburg, had the highest percent- ages, and Knoxville had the lowest: In zgsistancg, Birmingham had the highest percentage: In anxigty, Birmingham had the highest percentage; In submission, Knoxville had the highest percentages for Story No. 2 and second highest in Story No. 4; Munich had the lowest in both stories; In dominatigg, Birmingham had the highest percentages, and Helsinki had the lowest percentage for Story No. 4 and the second lowest for Story No. 2. The one contradiction occurred in the agxietx category cluster, where Munich percentages were significantly lower than Knoxville for Story'No. 4, and Knoxville percentages were significantly lower than Munich for Story No. 2. Consistencies as numerous as these support the proposition that the children in a sample may change their stories individually, but when the results of the whole sample are taken at once, those charb acteristics which differentiate the sample from other samples are maintained from story to story. Further support of this proposition was found in the analysis of sex differences in the Birmingham sample in Chapter V of this study, where the differences between boys and girls were for the most part maintained from Story No. 2 to Story No. 4. The changes in the levels of the percentages for many of the category clusters from Story No. 2 to Story No. 4 appeared here in all the samples as it did in the analyses of Birmingham sex comparisons and socio—economic comparisons. To review the discussion of reasons for the changes in levels, the cw W by the ot er, constructive behavigr :91 3h; g_h_i_1_d_ and w category clusters were thought to be raised in The Ink Spots story by the many statements in the children's stories involving behavior of both mother and child. The my; and 1.12 clusters were thonght to be at a lower level of percentages in The Ink Spots story because the storywriter begins at a point where the mother can be construed to know the truth already, whereas in The Lost Meat story the mother has to be informed in some manner about what happened. The domiggtiog and gubmiggign clusters were lower in level of percentages because the structure of Story No. 4 and the questions asked after it are conducive to more themas of working together of mother and child and consequently less domination and submission. It may be noted that the zggigtgngg category cluster remained at the same level in both stories. The factors which Operated to change the levels of the other clusters did not affect this cluster. Just as the differences between boys and girls for the Birmingham sample were maintained for the most part in spite of the shift in levels of percentages for the category clusters, so also the relative positions of the cultural samples were well maintained. Birmingham. The analysis of category clusters for Birmingham has shown that for both stories the Birmingham sampling falls near 45. the middle of the range of percentages with respect to the other samplings in mm M Q}; Ml. m, and 1;; clusters. In Story No. 4 Birmingham came second to the highest in ggpgtgugtigg behavio; pygphg,ghild, but differences in this cluster were not signif- icant. Themas of g2n§t§2g3113_3§hg1123, of 1gpth, and of 1;; are not differentiating factors for the children in the Birmingham sample. This neither confirms nor denies the hypotheses about behavior in a democratic culture. The Birmingham children gave the highest frequencies of response for category clusters for resistance and domination in both stories. This is consistent with the results for outcomes discussed in Chapter IV. The Birmingham sample had the highest frequency of dominatiog- sugmission outcomes and was second only to Knoxville in domination- submissiog and was not significantly different from Munich, Hamburg, and Knoxville in domination-submisgigg outcomes. The resistance shown with highest frequencies by the Birmingham children is a higher level of relating to domination than submissive behavior shown by children who do not resist. In the category cluster of anxiet , the Birmingham sample had the highest frequency, contrary to the hypotheses. Included in the anxiety cluster is category 22, bggg‘ re uests, lepiency, forgiveness. Statements in which Susan said she was sorry or that she did not mean to do it were coded in this category. This type of statement was very frequent in the Birmingham sample and is probably the reason Birmingham was highest of the samples in this cluster. In Story No. 2 the irmingham children gave second to the lowest frequencies of response for the category cluster for suhmigsign. In Story No. 4, contrary to this finding and to the expectations set up by the hypotheses, the Birmingham sample was the highest in frequency of submission themas. Included in the submissign_cluster in category 24, submissign: thinking and feelin , in which were coded statements that Susan was sorry that she her3 Spoiled her mother's new coat. One section of Hypothesis No. 3 in Chapter I states, ”Children in the more authoritarian culture will...show significantly fewer responses of...sense of fair play...,” which statement implies that children in a democratic cultzre will show more reSponses of fair play. Story No. 4 is different from Story No. 2, because the new coat in Story No. 4 is a person 1 property of the mother's, something which she doubtlessly values. If the child writing the story possesses a "sense of fair play," he will probably portray Susan as showing con- cern over having harmed someone else's prOperty. If the storywriter showed no concern over what was done, it would be construed as a pathological attitude, It is not really surprising, therefore, that children in the democratic cultures wrote stories containing themas of Susan saying she was sorry or feeling sorry about getting ink Spots on the mother's coat. It is part of the tradition of democracy of sensitivity to the feelings of others and reapcnsibility for one's own acts, or ”sense of fair play." “'7e Categories 22 and 24 each need to be subdivided into two dif- ferent categories because two different kinds of psychological values are included in 22 and 24. One value is an independent, reaponsible recognition of error on the part of the children combined with a motive of not repeating it; Spontaneity is high and fear is low.. In the other, which is also coded in 22 and 24, fear is high and Spontaneity is low, and therefore the attitude does not represent re8ponsible behavior, but conformity and avoidance of punishment. Helsinki. The Helsinki sample, like the Birmingham sample, was for the most part near the middle in rank with respect to the other samples, in percentages of reaponse for categories of constructiyg behavio; 31 13h; m, gopstmcjm behavig; by 313; ppm, c_:hi_l_d_ Bile. .tggph, and child_jgllgmlig. These themas do not differentiate the Helsinki sample, which again neither confirms nor denies the hypotheses about a democratic culture. Further, the category cluster of ocictanc did not differentiate the Helsinki sample. In the category cluster of anxiety the Helsinki sample was second only to Birmingham and was significantly higher than Hamburg, Knoxville and Munich in Story No. 4. This is consistent with the large number of ambiguous outcomes found in the Helsinki sample, as pointed out in Chapter IV. The traditionally low-communicative nature of human relations in the Finnish culture is thought to be related to the ambiguity. In the submission category cluster, the Helsinki percentage was in the middle of the samples, being significantly higher than Hamburg 48. and Munich. In the dominatiog cluster Helsinki was significantly lower than all the other samples at the .001 level. It was found in Chapter IV that Helsinki was significantly lower than the other samples in frequency of domination-submission outcomes. The analysis of categories suggests that among the Helsinki children, though parental domination is low, there is again the ”sense of fair play“ or respons- ibility felt by the child for Spoiling the mother's new coat. Responsibility should not be confused with submission. There were many tallies in category 24 for the Helsinki stories, which resulted in a fairly high percentage in the submission category cluster. Hamburg and Munigh. In the analysis of categories for Story No. 4, Hamburg and Munich were found to be similar to each other in some respects and quite different in others. The percentages of response in the ggngtzugtizg behavig; by;thg,ghild, resista e, 333? missign, and dgminatign clusters were almost id mtical, which is consistent with the findings for outcomes, where these two samples were very much alike at all levels of relating. In the constructive W )1 j_h_e m, m, 11.3, and anxiety clusters Hamburg and Munich were significantly different from each other. T;g¢h,and_lig,category clusters did not bear very much relation— ship to the outcomes of the Stories No. 4 and No. 2. This is not to say, however, that amounts of truth and lie were unrelated to the patterns of human relationships within a culture. Where there is more permissiveness of parents in a culture and more spontaneity, there is likely to be more telling of t ruth by the children, since 49. they need not fear retaliation.when they have made a mistake. But the structures of Stories No. 2 and 4 are such that the truth—telling or lying usually comes near the beginning of the story, and it is what develops after that in the story that determines the outcome. The fact that Munich and Hamburg were alike for the cluster of 993structivg bgh§1195_by_§hg,§hild in Story No. 4, ranking as the lowest two of all the samples, while Hamburg was highest and Munich was lowest in rank for the cluster of constructive behavior 31 Egg pgthgg, with significant differences between them, suggest differences in parent-child relationships in the two cultures. In Hamburg, it would seem to the child-authors that the parents were prone to solve the problems for their childrenwhen things went wrong more frequently than in Munich where the children did not take initiative in making amends for damage they may have caused. In Munich, like Hamburg, the children did not seem to take initiative, but neither did the parents initiate constructive behawor. In the domination cluster the Munich and Hamburg percentages were significantly higher than the Knoxville and Helsinki samples, and in ' the gubmissign cluster they were significantly lower than the Birmingham, Knoxville and Helsinki percentages. The categories composing the submisgign cluster are child categories. When a sample is found to give themas of domination in large numbers of its stories, it means that the children who wrote the stories have given these themas to the mother character in the story. When few of the stories in a.sample contain themas of submission, it is the children who wrote the stories 50. who projected little such behavior onto the child character in the story. In Munich and Hamburg, which are assumed to be authoritarian cultures, the children reflected the greater activity of the authority, the parent-character, by giving the mother more themas in their stories and by giving the subordinate, the child—character, fewer themas. Knoxvillg. The pattern of constructive behavior in Knoxville for Story No. 4 is in contrast to that for Munich and Hamburg, and in fact is directly opposed to the pattern in Hamburg. The Knoxville per- centages rank highest in cogsgggctive behaviog Ex the.ghi;d,and second to lowest in ggnstructive behavigg,py,ghg mgther, being signif- icantly lower than.Hamburg in the latter category. Apparently in Knoxville the children took relatively more initiative in constructive problempsolving and the parents took less initiative, in this story. This is consistent with hypotheses about individual reSponsibility and spontaneity in a democratic atmoSphere. At the dgmination-submission level, too, the pattern was apposed to that in Hamburg and Munich. KnOXVille was significantly lower than Hamburg and Munich in percentages for the domination cluster and signif- icantly higher than Hamburg and Munich in the submission cluster. The children in the Knoxville sample showed that the activity of children assumes more importance to them, because more of them gave themas in a child category cluster and fewer of them gave themas in the corre3pond- ing parent category cluster. As has been found consistently in analyses of Story No. 2 (2, 8), so in Story No. 4, the Knoxville sample had the highest percentages of truth and the lowest percentages of ii; clusters in its stories. This finding adds to the evidence that in the Knoxville culture, as compared 5,1- with the other cultures represented, there may be more telling of truth and less telling of lies. The amount of gnaig§z_shown by the percentage for the Knoxville sample was significantly more than that shown by the Munich percentage in Story Ho. 4. In Story No. 2, Knoxville was significantly lower than all the other samples, including Munich. This was thought to occur because Knoxville was the only sample (except Mexico City) which is not still laboring under the economic dislocation caused by'World War II. This interpretation seems to be denied by Story No. 4 results. However, as was pointed out above, the categories in the anxiety cluster included themas which should not be called anxiety. The anxiety percent- ages in Story No. 4 are therefore not really comparable to those in Story No. 2. The categories need re-defining and recasting in order for the argiety cluster to be comparable for the two stories. Length 9; Stories. Table 14 gives the average number of tallies per story for boys as compared to girls in each sample, for high, middle, and low socio-economic groups in all samples except Knoxville (which could not be subdivided by socio-economic groups), and for boys as compared to girls for Story No. 4 and Story No. 2 in Hamburg and Munich. It is seen that the Birmingham children averaged more tallies per story than any of the other samples. The stories from Birmingham were long and contained greater variety in their plots than the other samples. They more often had good closure than stories from other samples. The significantly higher percentages of integration and domination— resistaneg_outoores for the Birmingham sample may be related to the length of their stories. In general, stories which were short more often had ambiguous outcomes. The processes of democracy require that through two—way communica- tion problems be worked over until a solution is reached, and this takes time. It would be expected, then, that in a culture high in integrative problem—solving there would be more verbalization and communication. 53. VII. .CROSS-CULTUBAL SEX.COMPAEISONS Table 3 in Chapter V and Tables 19 through 22 of.Appendix.A give the data of which Table 15 is a composite. Table 15 shows whether girls or boys are higher in frequency of reSponse in each category cluster and outcome level for each of the five samples for Story No. 4. The letters B and G are used to indicate boys or girls. If the same sex was found to be higher in Story No. 2 as it was here, the letter B or G is underlined. Girls were higher than boys in 30 of the 40 category clusters. In the Birmingham sample girls were higher in seven of the eight clusters; in Hamburg they were higher in all the clusters. Differences were in the same direction for Story to. 2 as for Story Ho. 4 in 28 out of 45 category clusters plus integrative outcomes. The category clusters showing a definite trend toward sex consistency were: Constructive behavior by mother, where girls were higher in all samples and in three of the five samples sigr ficantly so; angtgugtigg behavior gy;§§§;n, where girls were higher than boys in four of the five samples and significantly so in the Birmingham sample; §3§§g,3gllg_lig, where girls were significantly higher than boys in four of the five samples; gesistangg, where girls were higher than boys in four of the five samples and significantly so in the Birmingham sample: TABLE 15 caosamnm sax conmmsoxs. RBCAPITUIATION or DIFFERENCES sen-mm: BOYS mm GIRLS mom names 3, 19, 20, 21, 22 . "B'I designates a higher percentage of boys, ”G' designates a higher percentage of girls. '*“ indicates that the difference was significant at the .05 level or better, by chi-square test. A line under "B“ or ”G” indicates that the same sex was also found to be higher in Story No. 2. Birming- Helsinki Hamburg Munich Knox- Category luster ham . ville Constructive (mother) (2" Q _G_ G“ G Constructive (child) ,fif B G Q, Q! Child tells truth B G _G_ G B Child tells lie _G_ G G '_'..'_ B Resistance G"I B_ G G G Arxiety Q“ 9* Q G" 5‘; Submission Q“ Q“ _G_ (_‘xj" Q“ Domination Q; _G_ G B Q m: Integrative G“ B _G_ G B Domination-resistance B ‘B G G B Ambiguous B‘ B* B B* B* Domination-submission G G‘ B G G‘ 54a. Anxiet , where girls were higher than boys in all of the samples and significantly so in three 0f the five; and Sub issi , where girls were significantly higher than boys in all of the five samples and significantly so in four of the five. As explained previously under the discussion of sex differences in Birmingham, the higher frequency of responses by girls to this story may be due to the greater ease for girls of identifying with the character in a story about a girl. The trends in Story No. 2, The Lost Meat, which most strongly supported the findings in the Ink Spots Story were in the category clusters of submission and anxiet , where girls were higher than boys and differences in some samples were Significant. For the rest of the category clusters, boys responded with higher frequencies more often than in Story No. 4, which could be predicted if it is easier for boys to identify with the boy in Story Mo. 2 than it is for girls. The boys were higher than girls in number of ambiggoug outcomes to stories in all five samples and significantly so in four of them. The girls were higher than boys in four of the five samples in number of domipgtigppsubmissign outcomes and three of the differences were significant. The boys' stories more often had less closure than the girls' stories. The closure which the girls gave their stories was more often in the domination-submission level. Integrative and domination—resistance outcomes showed no consistent trends of one sex having more stories in these outcome levels. 55. VIII. CROSS-CULTURAL SOCIO—ECOHOMIC COMPARISONS Table 16 is a recapitulation of Table # in Chapter 5 and Tables 23 through 25 in the Appendix and indicates the socio—economic group which gave the highest frequency of reaponse to each thema cluster and outcome level for Story No. 4. “H" means the high group had the highest frequency, "M“ means the middle group gave the highest frequency, and “L“ means the low group gave the highest frequency. Where both high and middle groups were significantly higher than the low group, but not significantly different from each other, it is indicated by H-MT. If all three groups were significantly different from each other, it is indicated by an asterisk beside both of the two highest ones in order, as H’ L'. If the same group was found to be higher in Story No. 2 as it was here, the letter is underlined. The high or high-middle socio-economic group was higher in frequency of response or in outcome level in 32 of the 48 compar- isons. Sixteen of the differences were significant. The consistent trends were in the following category clusters: _Qonstructive behavior by_mother, where the high socio-economic group was higher in all four samples, one of the differences being signif— icant; assistance, where the high or high and middle socio-economic group was higher in three out of four samples, and the middle socio- economic group was higher in the fourth, two of the differences being significant; and dominatiog, where the high and middle socio-economic groups were higher in all four samples, one difference being significant. 56. TABLE 16. CRCSS— “Tm-AL soc: .scoromc cost-meszscrs. FEWITCLATION or DIFFEBIXICES Bartram HI c: , I~iIDDLE mm Low SOCIO-ECONOMIC LEVELS FROM TABLES u, 23, 24, 25 ”H“ designates a higher percentage for the high socio- economic group, "N“ for the middle socio—economic group, and "L" for the low socio—economi: group. “H—M*" indicates that high and middle socio-ecoromic groups were both significantly higher than the low group. "3* L*" indicates that the high socio-econonie group was higher than the low socio-economic group, which in turn was higher than the middle socio—economic group. "*" indicatos that the difference was significant at the .05 level or better. A line under “H", “M“ or ”L" indicates that the same socio-economic group was also found to be higher in Story No. 2. Birming- Category Cluster ham Helsinki Hamburg Munich Constructive (mother) 2 g 11* H Constructive (child) 3 M g :1 Child tells truth L L =rL‘ H ' Child tells lie :1 g L. IN Resistance “* E, §}d* H Anxiety g, L M H Submission 3 ELM" M3. g Domination E_ ' HaM* X ‘3 Outcome Integra ive g _._._ L 3* Domination-resistance H d L H Ambiguous L L* H* ’* L* Domination-submission H H-d* M* L‘ H 56a. The high socio—economic group was higher in ipggggggizg outcomes in three of the four samples- one difference was significant. The low socio-economic group was higher in all four ggpiggggg outcomes, and three of the four differences were significant. The socio-economic groups did not show consistent trends in domination-resistancg_and gpggpgtion_subnissign outcomes. Twenty out of 36 of the category clusters and integrative out- comes showed the same group to be highest in frequency in Story No. 2 as in Story No. 4. It is interesting to note in Table 16 that in the truth and lie category clusters, where one socio-economic group was highest in truth, I the other group was highest in ;i_. The cluster of child tells lie is not necessarily a reciprocal of child tells truth. It has been found in the other studies (2, 8), however, that frequently these clusters reciprccate each other. The consistency of the responses made to the measuring instrument is supported by these findings. Table 16 shows that in domination and gpppiggigg category clusters the same socio-economic groups gave the highest percentages in all samples. That is, where the high socio-eccnomic group was highest in submission in Birmingham, Helsinki and Munich, it was also highest in domiggtion. In Hamburg the middle socio-economic group was highest in these two category clusters. It would'be eXpected that submission would be associated.with domination when the gpgipgglgg is very strong: and severe and the child's Spontaneity is relatively low. Heber (8) found that the middle socio-economic groups in.Belsinki and Hamburg did not give percentages of reSponse to Story No. 2 that 57. were significantly different from the other socio-economic groups. In Story No. 4 the middle socio-economic groups were in some cases different from both the high and low socio-economic groups and did not fall in between the high and low groups in percentages of response. In Helsinki the middle socio-economic group was highest in dominetiqge resistance outcomes. In the Hamburg sample the middle socio—economic group was highest in category clusters of gpgigtz and domination. In outcomes of ambiguity and dominationesuhmission the Hamburg middle socio—economic group was significantly higher than either the high or low socio~economic groups. :0 IX. TYPES or Dommrlom When the coding system for analysis was designed. provision was made for finding what types of punishment were most frenuent in the samplings being studied. Categories were provided for several Specific types of domination. and tallies were given in the appro- hriate categories whenever a child's story mentioned a particular type of punishment or domination. The results are given in Table 17 as the “ercentages of chi‘dren in each sample giving reaponses in each of the domination categories. The percentages in each category are not necessarily mutually exclusive, because a child may have mentioned two or more types of domination and would then be counted in each type he mentioned. Category 64, Jother ggllg fathgr, is not included in the cluster of themas of domigatign, but it is included in Table l? as a matter of interest in the frequency with which children in the different samples brought the father into the story. From Table 17 several observations can be made: Esprivatigg‘gfi‘iggdiwas mentioned as punishment more frequently in Birmingham than in the other samples. Dgprivatignigfi g;aytimg was more frequent in the Hamburg and Munich :samples than in the others. Hgstitutioglugdggfldugg§g_was more frequent in the Birmingham, Hamburg and Knonille samples than in the Helsinki and Munich samples. Ehygigglipggighmggtgwas more frequent in the Munich and Helsinki samples than in the others. 59. TIBLE l7 STOPY YIO. 4. PITCIIT 0.37 OF THE TOT L IUKBIRS 0F CHIIDII'I'I1 THE FIVI’D EIELI ES GIVIl} TEE": 1‘3 03‘ DOI- I." .1.“ 1011 03' THE CHILD, AID PHRCIL ;333 OF TOT IL LJIE HS 0‘ T'TLDIII II E10H SAMPLING GIVIIG THIF1S II T NLVI n1nu0 D0111 111101I In the percentages of total domination each child is counted only once even though he may have given themes for two or more categories of punishment or other domination. In the percentages for each category a child is counted only once, though the same child may be counted in more than one category. 17‘ :JS .1. Percenta 3es Type of Domination Birmingham Helsinki Hamburg Munich Knoxville Total domination 69.9 37.5 64.6 65.9 54.2 Deprivation of food 2., 0 O .6 .9 Deprivation of pl% 7time 2.5 1.9 4.1 8.5 1.4 Restitution under duress 21.0 3.4 16.3 7.3 14.0 Physics 11 punishment 7.6 11.1 4.1 14.0 6.1 Mother tells father 4.6 1.4 2.4 1.8 1.4 Father punishes 3.8 .5 .8 .6 .5 Susan sent to bed 15.7 .5 5.7 .6 2.8 Miscellaneous punishment 8.6 7.2 4.9 3.7 7.5 Scolding 27.6 18.3 48.8 34.8 6.1 Susan sent to cleaners 6.8 2.4 8.1 3.0 1.9 AdmoniShment (past behavior)15.2 5.8 4.1 1.8 10.7 Admonishment (future 9.9 2.4 4.1 1.8 17.8 behavior) 60. Mother ggllg father was most frequent in Birmingham and second most frequent in the Hamburg sample. Egnishmentlpyufgghgg_was more frequent in the Birmingham sample than in the others. ‘§p£§n‘§ggt.tg pg; was most frequent in Birmingham and second most frequent in Hamburg. Misgellaggqgg‘punighmgnt‘was more frequent in Birmingham, Knoxville and Helsinki than in Hamburg and Munich. Statclents like "The mother punished Susan” were coded here. §gglging showed a wide range, with large differences between the frequen- cies of all samples. Che Hamburg frequency was highest, then Munich, Birmingham. Helsinki, and Knoxville. in that order. Sgsan.gent to §§g£§_(or cleaners) was most freouent in Hamburg and next highest in Birmingham. Admonishment ganggrning‘pggt behavior was most frequent in Birmingham and next most frequent in Knoxville. Admonishment gpqut.§g§ggg behavior was more frequent in the Knoxville sample than in any of the others, and in the Birmingham sample was second. Table 17 shows the amount of each type of domination in a sample relative to the other samples, but it is difficult to tell from this table which types of domination were most prevalent within a given sample with respect to those most prevalent in the other samples. The types of domination were therefore ranked according to their frequency in each sample. The rankings are shown in Table 18. In Table 18 it can be seen that: Deprivation,g§.£ggg was not a relatively prevalent form of punishment in any of the samples. Deprivation 91 playtimg ranked high only in Munich. Restitution.gn§g; duress was a relatively prevalent form of domination in all the samples, eSpecially in Birmingham, Hamburg, and Knoxville. Physical pgpishment ranked high in Helsinki and Munich. The ranking of the father as an agent was quite low in all the samples. §p§gptggp§_tp,bgg was a prevalent form of punishment in Birmingham and Hamburg. Miscellaneous punishment ranked fairly high in Helsinki and Knoxville. chlding was the most prevalent form of domination in Birmingham, Helsinki, Hamburg, and Munich. It was not very prevalent in Knoxville. §p§§pL§§ntltg.thg‘§tggg was fairly prevalent in Hamburg. Admonishment,a§gp&_p§§t penivigz ranked fairly high in Knoxville, Birmingham and Helsinki. Admopighment‘gpgg§,£u§ugg behavior ranked highest as a form of domina- tion in Knoxville, and it was not very high in the other samples. It should be borne in mind that the figures in Table 18 are independent of the total domination in each sample; that is, the rank of one for scolding in Helsinki and Birmingham does not mean that there is as much scolding mentioned in the Helsinki sample as there is in the Birmingham sample. It only means that 2:.ihé.&2£§l kinds of domination in the Helsinki and Birmingham samples, scolding was the most prevalent in both. ‘As a reminder, the total percentage of domination which was found in each.sample is given at the top TnBLE 18 3113K ORDER OF TYPES OF DUI-11173101? WITHIN ELCH OF THE F115.) 5.3139138 Till-£11 330M TILBIE. 17 ..-.— .5 Rank Orders Type of Domination Birmingham Helsinki Hamburg Munich Knoxville Total domination (per cent) 69.9 37.5 6L.6 65.9 5n.2 Deprivation of food 11.5 12 12 11 11 Deprivation of playtime 11.5 8 7.5 3 10 Restitution under duress 2 5 2 4 2 Physical punishment 7 2 7.5 2 5.5 Mother tells father 9 9 10 8 10 Father punishes 10 10.5 11 11 12 Susan sent to bed 3 10.5 4 11 7 Miscellaneous punishment 6 3‘ 5 5 U Scolding l l 1 1 5.5 Susan sent to cleaners 8 6.5 3 6 8 Admonishment (past behavior) 4 4 7.5 8 3 Admonishment (future behavior) 5 6.5 7.5 8 1 of each column in Tables 17 and 18. Discussion of Types of Domination In Chapter IV, in the discussion on outcomes of stories, it was found that Hamburg, Munich, and Knoxville were very much alike in outcomes. The question was raised as to whether or not the domination-submission in a more democratic culture is the same as the domination-submission in a more authoritarian culture. Some insight into the answer to this question may be gained by comparing figures for Hamburg, Munich and Knoxville in Table 18. In the Knoxville sample, admonishment about 1353;; behavior ranked first and admonishmept apggtlpggt pghgvigz ranked third. In all the other samples, scolding ranked first. Furthermore, admgnishment did not rank high in any of the other samples, and scgldigg did not rank high in Knoxville. Admonishing is a milder form of domination than scolding, and this is one clear difference between domination in the Knoxville sample and in the other samples. From the evidence of several of the findings it is proposed that the domination of parents in Knoxville is of a milder form than the domination of parents in Hamburg and Munich. .Admonishing usually has reference to the act of the child: scolding has reference to the child. Physical punishment may have reference to the behavior but by definition is addressed to the child as a person. The data suggest further that the submission of the children in Knoxville is different from the submission of children in Hamburg and Munich. It is marked by high acceptance of the ethical code including telling the truth, 64. accepting the blame for one's own mistakes, and taking initiative in making amends for those mistakes. The Specific findings which supported this interpretation include (1) the high ranking of admonishmgnt and of restitution m m as two of the most prevalent kinds of domination in the Knoxville sample; (2) the significantly high percentage of response for the Knoxville sample in the j§;;§_tzgth cluster of themas, and the low percentage of reSponse in the tell§_lig cluster of themas; (3) the fact that the Knoxville sample showed the highest percentage of reSponse in the gonstzuctive behavior Ly §g§an category cluster but next to the lowest percentage of children with themas in the construgtivg behaviozlhx mother category cluster. Munich and Hamburg were lowest in frequency of constructivg behavigruhx Sgsag themas, and Hamburg was highest in frequency of constructive pehavior.hzumgthg; themas. 65. X. SUEEARY This study was an analysis of Story No. 4, The Ink Spots, Series A, of the Anderson Incomplete Stories. Story No. 2, The Lost Meat, had been analyzed in a previous study, and it was possible to make com- parisons between the two analyses and further evaluate the Anderson Incomplete Stories as a measuring instrument of intercultural differences. Samples used were from data collected in Birmingham, England; in Helsinki, Finland; in Hamburg and Munich, Germany; and in Knoxville, Tennessee, U.S.A. Subjects were 1,227 children in the seventh school year in these cities. Children's stories were coded in two ways: ‘by four defined levels of outcome (integrative, domination-resistance, ambiguous, or domination- submission); and by categories representing the psychological themas in the stories. In Story No. 2 there were 33 categories for thought, feeling and action by the child-character and 46 for the mother- character. In Story No. 4 there were 37 categories for thought, feeling, and action by the child-character and 53 categories for the mother-character. Aims were to analyze Story No. 4 and make sex and socio-economic comparisons in category clusters and outcome levels, and these same comparisons in cross-cultural samples, and to make comparisons of Story No. h with Story'Ho. 2. Reliability of coding showed percentages of agreement ranging, 66. from 93.57% to 1007?. The Birmingham sample was highest in the two outcomes of spontan- eity of the child, intczrative outcome and dom nation—resistanc . It was lowest in ambiguous outcomes. That is, human relations in Birmingham were more clearly defined by these children in their stories. Helsinki was significantly different from all other samples in ambiggous, unstructured outcomes of the stories, indicating a low order of communi- cation, and significantly lower in ggmination-submissign. Hamburg, Munich and Knoxville were alike in all four outcomes and showed no significant differences among themselves. More boys showed ambiguous, unstructured outcomes than girls in all five samplings, the differences being significant in four. More children in the high socio-economic groups showed integrative outcomes in both Story No. 2 and Story No. 4, the differences for Story No. 4 being significant in Hamburg and Munich. More children in the low socio-economic groups had ambiguous outcomes than in the high groups in all samples, the differences being significant in Helsinki, Hamburg and Munich. Analyses of the data showed that in completion of the stories children were sensitive to the structure of the stimulus material. There were two structural differences in the two stories that showed effects in the analysis of the data. Story Ho. 4 had a question after it: ”How do they both feel about it?" Calling attention to both the mother and the child seemed to account for higher numbers of integrative S outcomes, higher percentages of constructive_behavigr by both the mother and the child and higher frequencies of anxiety than were found in Story No. 2, while themas of domination and submission were lower . *in frequency. Second, in Story No. 4 the mother enters Just as Susan finds herself in a predicament. There was less opportunity or necessity to tell truth or lie, and there were much lower frequencies of both teng truth_and.1gll§.lig in Story Ho. 4. The girls wrote longer stories with wider variety of themas than boys. As Heber found in Story Ho. 2, girls gave higher frequencies in category clusters of constructive behavior Ly mothe , submission, and naggiggz. Consistently in all five samples girls were high in constructive mother category clusters; with the exception of Helsinki, gonstgugtivg bghayig; by 33g.ghild; and, with the exception of Knoxville, child tgllg lie. For all five samples girls had higher frequencies of anxiety and submission, the differences being generally in the same direction as for Story No. 2, and six of tle ten differences for these two categories being significant. Analysis of data for socio-economic levels showed that children from the high groups generally wrote longer stories. They had consistent- ly higher frequencies of integrative outcomes (as in Story No. 2), the differences in Hamburg and Munich being significant. The high groups had consistently higher frequencies in the category cluster of gonstruct— .11; behavior by tgg_mother and, with the exception of Helsinki, construct- izg behavig; by_th§ child. Children in the high group had higher fre- quencies of.tg;;§.lig i111 Birmingham and Helsinki. Children in the low group had higher frequencies of tells lie in Hamburg and Munich, 68- the difference in Munich being significant. Socio—economic levels in the four cities were exactly reversed for child tells truth. Children in the high socio-economic group had high percentages of 3g;;§,tguth in Hamburg and Munich, while tgllg taggh was found more frequently in Birmingham and Helsinki among children at the low socio-economic level. Children in the low groups tend to write shorter stories. 'Knoxville stories reveal more initiative and personal integrity in the child and more resPect for the child for both Story No. 2 and Story No. 4. Knoxville children showed the highest percentage of tells tgpgh and the lowest percentage of tells lie. They showed high attention to the child in having highest percentages of constructive behgvigr Ly Sgggn, and lower attention to the mother in having next to lowest percentages of children with themas in the gonstructivg fighgxigz;mz mother category cluster. Munich and Hamburg were lowest in frequency for constructive behavior hy_§p§gg, and Hamburg children, who gave little attention to constructive behavior §y_tre child, were highest in fre— quency of constructive behavior L; the mother. Evidence shows that according to the children's stories highest frequencies of truth and lower frequencies of tells lie were accompanied by milder punishment and milder forms of parental domination.in Knoxville.x Admonishment about future behavior ranked first in frequency and admonishment about past behavior ranked third. In all the other samples scolding ranked first among the types of domination. Adnonishment did not rank high in any of the other samples and scolding did not rank high in Knoxville. It was pointed out that admonishment represents chiefly disapproval of some kind of behavior. Scolding and physical punishment represent an attack on the child himself. 69. Hamburg and Munich were significantly different from each other in category clusters of constructive behavior by the mother, truth, i , and anxiet*, supporting the findings for Story Ho. 2, which leaves Open the question of a Cerman national character. Anderson, H. H. Domination and Integration in the Social Behavior of Young Children in an Experimental Play Situation, G net. Psychol. Mopqgg., 19: 341-409, 1937 Anderson, H. H. Children's Judgment of Social Conflict: An Intercultural and cross-national Study. (A we er presented to the Mich. Acad. Sci., Arts, and Letters. 19553 Anderson, H. H. and Anderson. G. L. Children's Perceptions of Social Conflict Situation: A Study of Adolescent Children in Germa.‘. Apggjpfp.gggrn;l pf Qrtho— psychiatry, Vol. XXIV, No. 2, 195D, 246-257 Anderson, H. H., and Brewer, H. M. Studies of Teachers' Classroom Personalities, I: Dominative and Socially Integrative behavior of Kindergarten Children. Anpl. Psvgthgl. Mop. 0:31., No. 6, 1945 Anderson, H. H., and Brewer, J. E., and Reed, M. F. Studies of Teachers' Classroom Personalities, III: Follow-up Studies of the Effects of Dominative and Integra— ‘ive Contacts on Children's Behavior. Appl. Psychgl. Monogr., Stanford University Press, 1946 Geierhaas, F. G. Problems of Reliability in Evaluating Story Completions about Social Conflicts by German Adolescent Children. Unpublished M. A. thesis, Michig’n State College, 1955 Geierhaas, F. G. Problems of reliability in Evaluating Story Completions about Social Conflicts by German Adolescent Children. (A paper presented to the Mich. Acad. Sci., Arts, and Letters, 1955 Heber, H. F. A Cross—cultural Comparison of Children's judgment of Parent-Child Conflict in Germany, England, Finland, United States and Mexico. Unpublished M. A. thesis, Michigan State College, 1955 HELSINKI, FINLAND: " ‘ I APPLH DIX A TABLE 19 ITUlZBLHES "TD PEPE} . HT G..38 OF BCYS.RHD GIRLS 31TH THJMAS CODED IH THE E GHT CA? CORY CLUSTBRS, nzn ACCORDIHG TO OUTCOME Story Ho. 4 Story No. 2 Boys Girls Boys, Girls Category Cluster N % N fl N 3 H fl Constructive (mother) 48 46.6 54. 51.4 17 16.5 18 17.0 Constructive (child) 39 37.9 32 30.5 4 3.9 5 4.7 Child tells truth 42 40.8 47 44.8 49 47.6 55 51.9 Child tells lie 15- 14.6 20 19.0 32 31.1 36 34.0 Resistance 17 16.5 17 16.2 11 10.7 7 6.6 Anxiety 71 68.9 89 84.8* 51 49.5 59 55.7 Submission 25 24.3 43 40.9* 48 46.6 67 63.2* Domination 37 35.9 41 39.0 58 56.3 63 59.4 9232222. Integrative 18 17.5 14 13.3 7 6.8 7 6.6 Domination-Resistance 10 9.7 2 1.9 (Not reported by Heber) Ambiguous 55 53.4* 38 36.2 " " " " Domination-Submission 20 19.4 51 48.64l " " " " Total 103 105 103 106 *Sifinificantly higher than the other sex at the .05 level or better, by chi-square test. \J f\) th3033, 9374;; : x'v firm 1“ r3111.) TABLE 20 AED PERCENT.GES OF BOYS AND GIRLS WITH THINKS CODED IN TIE EIG’T CaTEGORY CLUSTERS, AND ACCORDIKG T0 OUTCOME Story no. 4 Story No. 2 Boys Girls Boys Girls Category Cluster h % H % N s N % Constructive (mother) 66 53.7 73 59.3 20 14.1 34 24.55 Constructive (child) 34 27.6 44 35.8 14 9.9 13 9.4 Child tells truth 62 50.4 65 52.8 65 45.8 83 59.7* Child tells lie 32 26.0 36 29.3 72 50.7* 51 36.7 Resistance 11 8.9 17 13.8 16 11.3 13 9.4 AflXiety 76 61.8 88 71.5 72 50.7 80 5706 Submission 21 17.1 32 26.0 70 49.3 78 56.1 Domination 76 61.8 83 67.5 88 62.0 83 59.7 Outcome Integrative 13 10.6 17 13.8 14 9.9 14 10.1 Domination-resistance 6 4.9 7 5.7 (Eot reported by Heber) Ambiguous 44 35.8 40 32.5 " " " " Domination—Submission 60 48.8 58 47.2 " " " " Total 123 123 142 139 *Significantly higher than the other sex at the .05 level or better, by chi-square test. 73- TABLE 21 MUNICH, GERMANY: NUNBERS AL PER 33 AGES 0? BOYS AND GIRLS WITH THEMAS CODED IN THE EIGHT CATEGORY CLUSTERS, AND ACCORDING TO OUTCOME Story No. 4 Story Ho. 2 Boys . Girls Boys Girls Category Cluster N 923 N 55 1‘1 36 N ~35 Constructive (mother) 24 32.9 48 52.7‘ 5 6,8 5 5,4 Constructive (child) 20 27.4 29 31.9 5 6.8 8 8.7 Child tells truth 25 34.2 39 42.9 26 35.6 32 34.8 Child tells lie 27 37.0 34 37.4 45 61.6 61 66.3 Resistance 6 8.2 12 13.2 8 11.0 4 4.3 Anxiety 29 39.7 52 57.1"l 28 38.4 35 38.0 Submission 7 9.6 25 27.5“ 26 35.6 48 52.2* Domination 49 67.1 59 64.8 42 57.5 51 55.4 Outcome Integrative 8 11.0 13 14.3 3 4.1 2 2.2 Domination-Resistance 1 1.4 11 12.1 (Not reported by Heber) Ambiguous 34 46.6"I 21 23.1 " " ' " Domination-Submission 30 41.1 46 50.6 " u " " Total 73 91 73 92 *Significnntly higher than the other sex at the .05 level or better, by chi—square test. 74. TABLE 22 KKOXVILIE, TEDNESSEE: I'IRL ETD PSRC "TrG OF BOY} "133D G II—lLS 1' I23 TESL-.8 CODED IN '1'.) EIGHT C.TI~3GORY CLUST'1 .3113. 113118, 211.1) 210 C 0131:) 1 11 rG- T 0 OUT C 01-23 Story No. 4 Story Ho. 2 Boys Girls Boys' Girls Category Cluster N ,3 I: ,1; R 7‘6 N ,5 Constructive (mother) 2 45.2 57 47.1 2 12.2 9 7.6 Constructive (child) 27 29.0 52 43.0* 4 4.1 10 8.4 Child tells truth 58 62.4 72 59.5 60 01.2 80 67.2 Child tells lie 16 17.2 141.6 23 23.5 19 16.0 Resistance 6 6.5 9 7. 4 14 14.3 6 5.0 Anxiety 58 62.4 82 67. 8 12 12.2 16 13.4 Submission 24 25.8 48 39.’7 ., 41 41.8 83 69.7* Domination 46 49.5 70 57. 9* 56 57.1 85 71.4* Outcome Integrative 15 16.1 14 11.6 4 4.1 5 4.2 Domination—Resistance 5 5.4 2 1.7 (Not reported by Heber) Ambiguous 2 34.4* 27 22.3 " " " " Domination-Submission 41 44.1 78 64.5* " " " " Total 93 121 98 119 *Significantly higher than the other sex at the .05 level or better, by chi—square test. 75. TABLE 23 HELSIIII, FIULAHD: NUMBERS AND PBHGHITAGES OF CHILDREN IN E 8 HIGH, MIDDLB.AND LOJ SOCIO—ECOHOMIC GROUPS U TH THEMnS CODDD IN THJ EIGHT CATEGORY CLUSTERS, ADD ACCORDIHG TO OUTCOI”S FOR STORIES HO. 4 AND NO. 2 Story To. 4 Story 10. 2 Category High hiddle Low High kiddie Low Cluster n 3 N s N t n 9 N 5 n 9 Constructive 39 52.7 37 46.3 26 48.1 22 29.7* 8 10.0 5 9.1 (mother) . Constructive 21 28.4 30 37.5 20 37.0 3 4.1 5 6.3 1 1.8 (child) Child tells truth 29 39.2 34 42.5 26 48.] 40 54.1 37 46.3 27 49.1 Child tells lie 17 23.0 10 12.5 8 14.8 32 43.2* 27 33.8* 9 16.4 Resistance 13 17.8 14 17.5 '7 13.0 10 13.5 5 6.3 3 5.5 Anxiety 58 78.4 59 73.8 43 79.6 49 66.2* 35 43.8 26 47.3 Submission 26 35.1 31 38.8 11 20.4 36 48. 47 58.8 32 58.2 Domination 33 44.6* 35 43.8* 10 18.5 38 51.4 49 61.3 34 61.8 Outcome Integrative 15 20.3 12 15.0 5 9.3 8 10.8 3 3.8 3 5.5 Domination- 4 5.4 5 6.3 3 5.6 (Not reported by Heber) resistnnce Ambiguous 27 36.5 34 42.5 32 59.3* " " " " Domination- 28 37.8 29 36.°* 14 25.9 " " " " submission Total 74 80 54 74 80 55 *Significnntly higher than .01 level. the other 76. socio-economic group or groups at we immune, 03m; MIDDLE ADD TICUT 113..» L05 SOCIO- CiTZGORY CIU -1" .lJU TABLE 24 .ITY: NIH-332338 -‘ITD PTT~€CITT£GLS OF CHILDREN III THE HIGH, ONCE-.10 GROUPS '9’ TH TEX-lifts CODHD III THE STDRS, ADD ACCORDIHG TO OUTCOM18 FOR STORIQS NO. 4 AND NO. 2 Story No. 4 Story Ho. 2 Category High Middle Low High. hiddle Low Cluster n s N 8 h 8 n s n 9 N 3 ConstruCtive 51 73.9* 55 50.0 33 49.3 16 23.5* 33 22.6* 5 7.5 (mother) Constructive 30 43.5 27 24.5 21 31.3 8 11.8 16 11.0 3 4.5 (child) Child tells 44 63.8* 45 40.9 38 56.7* 40 58.8 73 50.0 35 52.2 truth Child tells 18 26.1 30 27.3 20 29.9 29 42.6 63 43.2 31 46.3 lie Resistance 10 14.5* 15 13.6* ‘3 4.5 8 11.8 15 10.3 6 9.0 Anxiety 45 65.2 83 75.5 36 53.7 40 55.8* 90 61.6 22 32.8* Submission 12 17.4 34 30.9* 7 10.4 38 55.9 79 54.1 31 46.3 Domination 37 53.6 80 72.7 42 62.7 45 66.2 83 56.8 43 64.2 Outcome Integrative 9 13.04 12 10.9 9 13.4 8 11.8 18 12.3 2 3.0 Domination-~ '3 4.4 4 3.6 6 9.0 (Hot reported by Heber) resistance Ambiguous 38 55.1* 24 21.8 22 32.8* " " " " Domination— 19 27,5 70 63.64 30 44.8* n n n u submission Total 69 110 67 68 146 67 *Significantly higher than the other socio-economic group or groups at the .05 level or better, by chi-square test or t—test. 77. TABLE 25 MUNICH, Gnhhnnr: I. TLBLLRS rdTD PERCEITTAG .38 OF CHILDREI.T IL? T1133 EIG: HUD LO'. SOCIO-ECOKOI-LIC GROUPS 1'." TH TEX-.115 CODED 1LT T1113 EIGHT .LTFBORY CLUSTERS , ALD JiCCORDIl-TG TO OUTCOI‘LJJS FOR STORIES ITO. 4 1112') ITO. 2 Story Ho. 4 Story Ho. 2 High Low High low Category Cluster N % N 9 N % N % Constructive (mother) 37 4410 35 43.8 5 5.8 5 6.3 Constructive (child) 29 34.5 20 25.0 7 8.1 6 7.6 Child tells truth 37 44.0 27 33.8 . 26 30.2 32 40.5 Child tells lie 24 28.6 37 46.3* 59 68.6 47 59.5 Resistance 10 11.9 8 10.0 4 4.7 8 10.1 Anxiety 42 50.0 39 48.8 31 36.0 39 40.5 Submission 17 20.2 15 18.8 39 45.3 35 44.3 Domination 57 67.9 51 63.8 49 57.0 44 55-7 misses Integrative 14 16.7 7 8.8 3 3.5 2 2.5 Domination-resistance 7 8.3 5 6.3 (Not reported by Heber) Ambiguous 21 25.0 34 42 .5‘ " " " " Domination-submission 42 50.0 '34 42.5 " ” " " Total 84 80 86 79 *Significantly higher than the other socio-economic group at the .02 level, by chi-square test. 78- “P3111712: B Anderson Incomplete Stories: Form 2. Age at last Birthdats: birthday! 30y: Girl Birthplace: Church attendst HolongY onghavo you fved in School: ' Grado:_____ th 3 community. ciiy. town?_________ Occupation of father: - _1 Deception of mother: 1. Frank and Peter were walking to school. Suddenly Frank grabs Peter's cap and throws it high up into the nearest tree so that Peter could not reach it with his hand. Frank had never done anything like this before. Neither had Frank and Peter quarrolod with each other the day before. - Why did Frank do this? What does Peter do? What does Frank think? How does Peter feel about it? Think about those questions. then finish the story quickly with a few sentences. 2. Michael's mother sends him to the store to get one pound of wieners. On tho wry home he puts the package of meat on the edge of the sidewalk and plays for a. short time with his friends. A shepherd dog darts forward quickly. claws half the wither:- out of the package and rushes away with them. Michael wraps up the remaining wieue: and takes them home. What does Michael say to his mother? What does his mother do? How does Michael then feel about it? Think about those questions. then finish this story quickly with a few sentences. 79. ~2— 3. The teacher suddenly discovers that fifty cents has disappeared from her desk. She looks up and sees that all the class are working on their arithmetic assignment. She wonders what happened to the money and what she should do. What does the teacher do? Finish this story also with a few sentences. Tell what happened to the money and also exactly how the teacher feels and what she does. 1}. Elizabeth is sitting in the living room working on her homework. Suddenly she remembers her mother's new coat. She wonders how she would look in it. When taking the coat off she notices that she had got some ink spots on her mother's new coat. Just as Elizabeth is trying to wipe out the ink spots her mother enters the room. What does her mother say? How do they both feel about it? What does Elisabeth say? What does each one do? Think about these questions and finish this story quickly with a few sentences. ~3— 5. George and Karl are playing with a football. They know that they should not play football on the narrow space in front of the house. George gives a strong kick and the ball hits a window and makes a big crack in it. Karl thought that someone had appeared behind the window. No one could have seen who lad kicked the ball against thO “Nous Finish this story with a few sentences. Tell how the two boys feel about it and what they do. 6. Else. often handed in her home composition late to the teacher. This time it was an especially important composition and she had. moreover. written it on time. On the way to school she lost her composition notebook and could not find it anywhere. What does Elsa say to her teacher? What does the teacher say? Think about these questions and may this story with a few sentences. 495 7-31—53 an 81. . k 71‘11 .._ .- ‘-th§~‘ ' -‘ ' ““I...,I...'._".?- 'u W0 W35 A; "V 18 WW}; Wa- MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY LIBRARIE LILIL LILILLIL IIILLLIII