“*vvv— -' . SIMILARITY AS A BASIS FOR _ - INTERFERSONAL ATTRACTION AMONG GROUPS IN LONG-TERM ISOLATION Thesis for the Degree of M. A. MlCHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY ROBIN RICHARD VALLACHER ‘ I. 19] 2 a - I . . l I . - .-’. n ‘ ‘ . - . . I. . . : ' . v 'a- , . n ' . ' V . ‘ ‘ ’ ' ' ’ . a I - ... ‘ ' "v‘ ‘ ‘ .A. . . .. . .- . r o - ' . . '5 n : . - .. . ' ' - -‘ . l ' I ‘ --." ’ .1. . ‘ I " a - " I cr'. -.' . -.. , . - ,» r7 - " . 4 .a _ - - . .QO - _._ . ' - . . - I’I . - J. ’ ‘- " ‘7‘ 3' :r’ — . . ‘ ‘ . . . . ° . _ ‘ " ' ’ . . -"' ' . ‘ . . . _ .. n f} o " - ' ~ ‘ . ‘ - Ifo . " ‘ . l .., .‘\' ‘, 1"- J .’ - ' '5 . ' :r- ‘ ’ " ’ z' ‘i c’l ' 'vr- l;- ‘ ' , -' v . . . . . . > .., '. . . , (>— , . . - . ~.-l.0 h.” I’A ‘ ' - . ' . -— ’4 , . . 1’ , ' . ' - _.‘ f ’4 0 3 .. . . ' . o ' ->< . ' - , I._ 0 I - ' ' . F '. ’ ‘ I. ‘ ' ' ' ’t ' _ ‘ . . u . - .v < ' - , f" . ‘ J ‘ l t 3].. ,r. . I v 4' .2” _ . 14,- -' ‘ o I " ' ‘ ‘ . _ .‘. -. ‘ u ‘_ a ‘ ._ ovu.‘ 1' 7'1' . ,6. . r w 0 '. _r I! . ‘ _’. - r , '0 ‘ 1", V“ ”—{ - - 4 . h ‘ . . O..v> ,.-., 714-: .I'. ” . - r '- ‘ "- ‘J'o ' v I. ‘ ' " . ' ' ‘ In ' " ’ . . I-"'/ ' l‘ . ‘ . "~ -.:-‘. 9‘. . - ', - .I . -.-:'-J W" - —. 'I--'°""" """ ' -' r--Hi 'M I.’ o — on I l ' P ' C ' . ‘ v ‘ 'tl ’ 2’ ' . - T—.; "'4' l"""" "" :f’fl': '.’.‘ "I if...“ V”. ‘ t: ‘i' ’v ,. , Z“... . ‘,'.. ~ . _ y‘_.._-JIJ"IOI’JO".’ ' .', 3 , A 1’31"” 'f' ‘ . v "‘ ." ." '." Ir." I ' ' J " I‘ '5’ p E: . - J" ‘ - '~ " ' .' .-.-:’ -.. . n 1- """ " r~a.';;'!:"1¥ ' " v' ' - ,. - .t - ,r; ’ O , , t . .; ,r . 6'! O .. ... "(’3,’ ---mflw7 910 '9 , I '- ... 3-... . 1f- ‘ ) ' .." (VI"_!{' A .~ . o I . ,' ' ‘1. ’ ‘ ;V'>II.J . :14 I " ' 'V'f" ': . .,.,. ‘ . . ,..v -. -p I ' ‘ " ._ n 'v‘. n - ' ‘ " l ' You! - at.1( I? . ' ‘ _ l I 4-v I “'F’ ‘ r. . . ' 33K. ‘ " . .4, r " 24"" " ',' vylf “WC .. 4 n- v . o .a ' t c o ' . o . . ., . ' ’ . . , ' o A II.- ' ' ' ‘. I aft-1“, . - . , . ’. . .' 0 , .1 ‘l _ .c’oJ .. ’1‘;- 0" "(I . . I). :I’ 2.153.]. ',I : :'O... :14" 4’ '. A - " . ., ' ‘ ' I‘ 'I‘ .q _ .I I - q . ' , ' -‘l . ' v.hf’ '- n ‘ A ‘ A . - ‘Il ‘ - .‘. - . '! .-_‘.';':.a-‘ I’A‘ ' '1’- ABSTRACT SIMILARITY AS A BASIS FOR INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION AMONG GROUPS IN LONG-TERM ISOLATION BY Robin Richard Vallacher The present study investigated the role of simi- larity in friendship formation. Research in the area of interpersonal attraction and friendship formation is marked by conflict in theory and inconsistency in empir- ical findings. Particularly confusing is the role of sim- ilarity in attracting individuals to one another. This confused state of affairs is in part due to the limited generalizability of the settings and subject samples em— ployed in many of the studies dealing with this issue. The present study avoided such difficulties by examining natural groups in a field setting. Specifically, groups of men who lived and worked together for approximately one year in an isolated environment (Antarctica) were examined to see if any consistent relationships between 1 Robin Richard Vallacher friends could be found. Correlational analyses were used to assess these relationships. Contrary to prediction, friends in Antarctica were found to be similar on few personality dimensions, and only slightly similar in background characteristics. While in Antarctica, however, friends did tend to develop similar attitudes regarding their station group experi— ence. On the other hand, as predicted, pairs of individ— uals who avoided each other as friends were significantly different on various dimensions and tended to deve10p conflicting attitudes while in Antarctica. It appears, therefore, that in a stressful task-oriented situation it is easier to predict incompatibility than compatibility on the basis of various traits and characteristics of the individuals involved. It was also predicted that depending on how sal— ient a given dimension is for an individual, he should be attracted to a larger or smaller range of peOple on that dimension. The results of the analyses of this hypothesis were generally nonsignificant, although the results for several personality measures were in the pre- dicted direction. Thus, at least to some extent, 2 Robin Richard Vallacher individuals who were "high" on a personality dimension tended, as a group, to have as friends individuals who were relatively homogeneous on that dimension, i.e., they were similar to each other. Several aSpects of the setting involved in this study were discussed as possible reasons why more signif— icant results were not obtained. Some of these (task— orientation, prescribed membership, temporary suspension of common psychological needs) were held as more tenable than others (dissatisfaction with self, dissimilarity as stimulation in a monotonous environment). {Various theoretical and methodological problems with the published research in the area of friendship for- mation and interpersonal attraction were discussed. Some of these (single trait approach, unknown relationship be- tween test responses and behavior, use of correlational analyses) were evident in this study, while others (con— trol of temporal factors, examination of nonlinear rela- tionships) were not. SIMILARITY AS A BASIS FOR INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION AMONG GROUPS IN LONG-TERM ISOLATION BY Robin Richard Vallacher A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1972 To the members of my karass, one and all: ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would have been impossible without the invaluable assistance given me by members of the Navy Med- ical NeurOpsychiatric Research Unit in San Diego, Cali- fornia. In addition to making the data for this thesis available to me, they assisted a great deal in the analyses and provided me with relevant Unit reports and publications. I especially wish to thank Dr. E. K. Eric Gunderson, Head of the Operational Psychiatry Division, who made the data and assistance available to me; Mary Paul, who wrote the necessary computer programs; and George E. Seymour, a good friend as well as a competent research associate, who pro— vided much assistance and acted as the liaison between the Unit and myself at all stages in the deve10pment of this thesis. I would also like to thank the members of my thesis committee, Dr. Lawrence A. Messé, Dr. Gary E. Stollak, and Dr. William D. Crano, for their assistance and patience during the writing of this thesis. I would iii like to give special thanks to Dr. Messé, the committee chairman, who made helpful comments on the early drafts. This research was supported by the Bureau of Med— icine and Surgery, Navy Department, under Research Work Unit MFlZ.524.00l—9003D. Opinions eXpressed are those of the author and are not to be construed as necessarily re— flecting the official view or endorsement of the Depart- ment of the Navy. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii INTRODUCTION. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Theoretical Support for Similarity-Attraction Hypothesis. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 Theoretical Support for Complementarity- Attraction Hypothesis . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 Methodological Approaches to Similarity- Attraction Hypothesis . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 Review of Similarity-Attraction Literature. . . 9 Setting . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . l4 Hypotheses. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19 METHOD. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26 Subjects. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26 Procedure and Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . 27 Analysis. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34 RESUDTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4O Prediction Variables. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 Station variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 DISCUSSION. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 TABLE OF CONTENTS (Cont.) Page SUMMARY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80 REFERENCES. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 vi Table 10. LIST OF TABLES Page AVERAGE CORRELATIONS, STANDARD ERRORS, AND T—TESTS FOR PERSONAL HISTORY VARIABLES. . . 41 AVERAGE CORRELATIONS, STANDARD ERRORS, AND T-TESTS FOR NEED SCALES . . . . . . . . . . 43 RESULTS OF F—TESTS FOR NEED SCALES. . . . . . 44 AVERAGE CORRELATIONS, STANDARD ERRORS, AND T-TESTS FOR ATTITUDE SCALES . . . . . . . . 46 AVERAGE CORRELATIONS, STANDARD ERRORS, AND T-TESTS FOR SELF SCALES . . . . . . . . . . 47 AVERAGE CORRELATIONS, STANDARD ERRORS, AND T—TESTS FOR FIRO-B SCALES . . . . . . . . . 49 RESULTS OF F-TESTS FOR FIRO—B SCALES. . . . . 50 AVERAGE CORRELATIONS, STANDARD ERRORS, AND T-TESTS FOR EARLY AND LATE STATION SCALES . 52 AVERAGE CORRELATIONS, STANDARD ERRORS, AND T-TESTS FOR EARLY AND LATE.SYMPTOM SCALES . 55 AVERAGE CORRELATIONS, STANDARD ERRORS, AND T-TESTS FOR SUPERVISOR AND PEER SCALES. . . 56 vii The purpose of this thesis is twofold: l) to re— view theoretical approaches and research findings regarding interpersonal attraction and friendship formation; and 2) to relate these to the problem of interpersonal attrac- tion in groups in long-term isolation. More specifically, groups of men who "wintered-over" at United States Antarc- tic scientific stations were examined on a wide variety of measures to see if there are any consistent relationships between friend pairs. In addition to providing a test of several issues in interpersonal attraction (e.g., simi— larity vs. complementarity, the role of temporal factors, etc.), the present study was designed to identify factors which contribute to the prediction of compatibility under unique and often stressful circumstances. This latter aspect has increasing relevance as man more and more must adapt to new and continually changing conditions such as increasing loss of "personal space." Perhaps the most parsimonious model relevant to the prediction of interpersonal attraction involves the notion of similarity along various dimensions between 1 individuals who are attracted to one another. No one, of course, maintains that similarity is the sole determinant of interpersonal attraction. Other important factors in- clude such things as prOpinquity (e.g., Festinger, Schac- ter, and Back, 1950), the reinforcing prOperties of the situation (e.g., Lott and Lott, 1960), the temporal length of the relationship (e.g., Rosenfeld and Jackson, 1965), characteristics like boastfulness and self—depreciation (Pepitone, 1964), the respective status of each individual (e.g., Jones, 1964), and perceived liking for oneself on the part of the other person (e.g., Backman and Secord, 1959). However, these other factors are not as potentially useful in choosing the composition of groups as is the similarity principle. Those factors that are related to the situational context (e.g., reinforcing properties of the situation) give no clue as to the possible influence of personality variables, while those factors that do re- late to personality either are not of a predictive nature (e.g., perceived liking for oneself) or are contingent on the situational context (e.g., the reapective status of each individual). Theoretical Support for Similarity-Attraction Hypothesis Several theories are often advanced to support the idea that similarity is conducive to positive interper- sonal attraction. Some of these appear to be more rele- vant to the problem at hand than others. Similarity be- tween friends would seem to follow naturally from the var— ious consistency theories. Heider's balance theory (1958), for example, holds that peOple strive to make their senti— ment relationships harmonious with their perception of the unit relationship existent between objects. According to Heider, separate entities which are similar tend to be perceived as belonging together (having a unit relation— ship). Thus, positive unit formation (perceived similar- ity) shouLd induce a harmonious sentiment relationship (liking). However, this line of reasoning is not partic- ularly convincing since it is abstract and non-intuitive, that is, it doesn't consider attraction from the point of view of a person's needs, feelings, etc. A much more intuitive notion is that similarity facilitates attraction because it allows for the predic- tion of another's future behavior. If a person appears sociable and says he likes to be included in group activ— ities, we may reasonably eXpect that he will engage in group activities at every Opportunity. If we ourselves are also high in need for affiliation, we may like another who we know to be similarly high on that need—-not for reasons of cognitive consistency but simply because we anticipate many rewarding interactions, engaging in af- filiative behavior with someone who also enjoys engaging in affiliative behavior. Another possible reason, related to the one de— scribed above, involves the notion of perceived liking for oneself on the part of the other person. It has been shown that people tend to like those who like them. It may be the case that when we learn that another is similar to us, we assume that he is likely to like us, and thus we in turn like him. Perhaps the theoretical approach concerning simi- larity and attraction that is the most directly relevant to the conditions in the present study is Festinger's theory of social comparison (1954). The theory states that the individual has a drive to evaluate his Opinions and abilities, and that the opinions and abilities of Others will be used as a basis of comparison when no objective criteria for evaluation exists. Persons who are similar to oneself provide more precise comparisons than do those who are dissimilar, and the similarity functions as reinforcement. Because Of the greater amount Of rein- forcement provided, similar persons are more attractive than dissimilar ones. Although this theory is more con- cerned with the evaluation Of one's Opinions and abilities through comparison with others, it has been extended to the evaluation Of one’s personality (Griffitt, 1966). Given the unique circumstances in which the Antarctic volunteer finds himself, and hence the lack of Objective criteria by which to judge his feelings, attitudes, be- havior, etc., it seems plausible that something like so- cial comparison would Operate in friendship formation. Theoretical Suppgrt for Complementarity-Attraction Hypothesis In contradiction to the folk adage that "birds Of a feather flock together" is the equally pOpular idea that “Opposites attract." A leading prOponent of this point Of view is Winch (Winch gg_§1., 1954), who developed a theory Of "complementary needs." Although his theory and research were primarily concerned with married and engaged couples, he felt that the basic idea could be extended to inter- personal attraction in general. There are two general reasons why persons who differ in need structure should be attracted to each other. First of all, each member Of the dyad finds interaction mutually or reciprocally rewarding because his needs are expressed in behavior that is rewarding to the other person. For example, a person with strong nurturance needs behaves in a protective, nurturant manner toward another person who has strong needs tO be dependent. In this way, each individual satisfies the needs Of the other person, and is in turn satisfied. A second general reason is that persons are at- tracted to others who have characteristics to which they once aspired, but were prevented by circumstances from developing. Instead, they have modeled themselves after the image of a person with the Opposite traits, but they find themselves attracted to individual who possess the once-coveted traits because of feelings of wistful admira— tion. This second idea concerning need complementarity and interpersonal attraction reflects the "need completion principle" advanced by Cattell and Nesselroade (1967). Considering the context of the present investiga- tion-~groups Of men isolated for a long period Of time in a monotonous environment--it might reasonably be expected that individuals would be attracted to dissimilar rather than similar others, at least on certain dimensions. Dissimilar others can provide varied stimulation, such as different points of View and insights with life, new in— terests, unpredictability. Hence some degree Of excitement may be generated by interacting with these persons. Con- versely, it may be that individuals would prefer to inter- act with dissimilar others, except that they fear they would not be as well liked by dissimilar others as they are by similar others. Walster and walster (1963) found that individuals who were confident that everyone they came in contact with would like them were likely to asso— ciate with dissimilar others. On the other hand, they found that when it was especially important to be liked, or when one was unsure of his likability, an individual was likely to "play it safe" and to associate with sim- ilar peOple. Methodological Approaches to Similarity-Attraction Hypothesis Just as there are several theoretical approaches relevant to the role of similarity in interpersonal attrac- tion, different methods of investigation are employed by various investigators. One approach involves comparing an individual's test responses on some measure with the test reSponses of his friends. The scores of the pairs are then correlated and often compared with similar correla— tions for random pairs within the same population or with pairs Of mutually antagonistic or mutually indifferent subjects. In another general approach, the measure or measures are Obtained, and then previously unacquainted subjects are placed in a situation requiring some degree of interaction. Thus, similar and dissimilar pairs or groups are created, and their interpersonal reSponses are assessed following the interaction. Yet another method has been employed by Byrne and his colleagues (e.g., Byrne, Griffitt, and Stefaniak, 1967). This method involves first Obtaining the test responses of a subject and then presenting him with the test responses of a "stranger" and asking the subject toevaluate the stranger on various dimensions. The test responses of the "stranger" are actually created by the experimenter to be similar to the test responses of the subject on from 0 to 100% of the items. Review of Similarity- Attraction Literature Despite the amount of research that has been con- ducted regarding the relative effects of similarity and complementarity on interpersonal attraction, the issue is far from settled. First of all, there are problems in- herent in the methods described above which confuse the interpretation of the results found in many studies (some Of these problems are discussed below). Also, whether similarity or complementarity is a better predictor of friendship is no doubt dependent in part on the situa- tional context (e.g., task versus social-emotional orien- tation) as well as on the kinds of variables being con- sidered. Generally, similarity on such characteristics as age (Glick and Landau, 1950; Hollingshead, 1951), race (Golden, 1954; Morton, 1941), religious affiliation (Hol- lingshead, 1950; Thomas, 1951), ethnic origin (Bossard, 1939), economic status (Byrne, Clore, and Worchel, 1966), 10 education (Landis, 1945), and previous marital status (Bowerman, 1953) has been shown to have a positive effect on compatibility. Attitudinal similarity has also been found to be positively associated with interpersonal attraction. New- comb (1961), for example, in a study of friendship forma- tion among male university students, found support for the general hypothesis that given adequate Opportunity for individuals to become familiar with each other's attitudes, attraction is predictable from actual attitudinal agree— ment. Other studies, Of a more experimental nature, have also supported the proposition that attitude similarity is a determinant Of interpersonal attraction. Byrne and Nelson (1965), for example, showed that attraction is a positive linear function of the prOportion of similar to dissimilar attitudes rather than the absolute number of similar attitudes. With regard to personality variables, however, the results have been less consistent. Evidence has been found to provide support for the similarity hypothesis (Banta and Hetherington, 1963; Izard, 1960a, 1960b; Maisonneuve, 1954; Mehlman, 1962; Miller, Campbell, Twedt, 11 and O'Connell, 1966; Murstein, 1961), for the complemen- tarity hypothesis (Cohen, 1956; Kerckhoff and Davis, 1962; Rychlak, 1965; Winch, Ktsanes, and Ktsanes, 1954), and for some combination of the two (Becker, 1964; Secord and Backman, 1964). Other findings give only partial support to the similarity hypothesis in that similarity is positively associated with attraction only under limited conditions, or only in Specific groups or with respect to only a few variables (Bonney, 1946; Bowerman and Day, 1956; Katz, Glucksberg, and Krauss, 1960; McLaughlin, 1971; Posovac, 1971; Rosenfeld and Jackson, 1965; Van Dyke, 1940). Izard (1963), for example, in a replication Of an earlier study, found a relationship between similarity and sociometric choice among college freshmen, but not among college seniors. Finally, a number of investigators have simply found no relationship between personality similarity and attraction (Corsini, 1956; Gordon, 1957; Hoffman, 1958; Hoffman and Maeir, 1966; Katz, Cohen, and Castiglione, 1963; Pintner, Forlano, and Freedman, 1937; Reilly, Cum- mins, and Stefic, 1960; Thorpe, 1955). 12 Since many Of the studies in this area have used as subjects married or engaged couples, the applicability Of such findings tO interpersonal attraction in general is limited. Those studies that have dealt with interpersonal attraction more generally have, for the most part, involved short-term laboratory groups or presented subjects with personality profiles Of alleged strangers. The generali- zability of such studies is also limited since they prob- ably do not make salient important characteristics which could be a potential source of conflict in many "real life" situations, particularly over an extended period Of time. Several studies have shown, for instance, that temporal factors have an influence on the relationship between sim- ilarity and attraction (Newcomb, 1961; Rosenfeld and Jack- son, 1965). In addition, subjects in laboratory studies generally represent a rather homogeneous pOpulation, i.e., college sephomores. Again, this limits the generaliza- bility of such studies. The sample and setting involved in the present study, heterogeneous groups Of men isolated for an extended period Of time, did not encounter the par— ticular limits on generalizability mentioned above. A different type of problem encountered in studies Of this nature involves the explanation for any Observed l3 relationship between similarity and attraction. That is, since most of these studies involve correlational analyses, there are several possible explanations for any Observed relationship between similarity and attraction. The pOp- ular eXplanation is that peOple select their friends on the basis of similar characteristics. Another possibility is that friends become similar due to their association. This possibility seems more likely with regard tO atti- tudes than with regard to personality characteristics. Finally, it may be that individuals run into similar peOple for reasons that have nothing to do with personal preferences. The importance of propinquity as a deter- minant of interpersonal attraction is well established (e.g., Festinger §§_§l,, 1950), and it may be that peOple tend to be thrown together with those who possess similar characteristics. This might be particularly true in cases where a particular type Of personality is drawn to a cer- tain type Of occupation. For most Of the variables used in the present study, any relationship between similarity and friendship would most likely be the result of individuals choosing their friends on the basis Of similarity. This is so be— cause scores for most variables were Obtained from the 14 subjects before their deployment to Antarctica. Scores for a few variables (attitudes, emotional symptoms, and performance criteria), however, were Obtained while sub- jects were living together at the duty station, so that for these variables there is the possibility that any Ob- served relationship between similarity and friendship is due to friends becoming similar as a result Of their asso— ciation. The third possibility (similarity as an artifact Of prOpinquity or occupational similarity) is possible for all Of the variables, but not likely, since the groups are isolated for several months and thus there is much Oppor- tunity for group members.to interact with and get to know one another intimately. Thus, for most of the variables examined in the present study, the interpretation Of any Observed relationship between similarity and friendship is not as ambiguous as it was in most studies in this area. Setting Since the International GeOphysical Year of 1957- 1958, the United States has maintained several year-round stations on the Antarctic continent to implement the Ant— arctic Research Program supported by the National Science 15 Foundation and the United States Navy. At the Antarctic stations, civilian scientists and technicians collect re- search data, while Navy personnel provide necessary logis- tical support. Groups varying in size from 15 to 40 men live and work together in close association for approxi— mately one year. For a period of approximately eight months, all stations are completely isolated from each other and the outside world, except for intermittent radio communication. There is no possible way for members to leave the station nor for help to reach them during the isolation period. In addition to the extreme isolation of the Ant— arctic stations, the physical setting is undoubtedly the most rugged environment inhabited by man. Temperatures below —100 degrees Fahrenheit have been recorded, and winds Of more than 100 miles per hour may prevail. During the Antarctic summer months (when sunlight is nearly contin- uous), construction, repair, and storage tasks must be performed at every station in addition to the collection Of scientific data. It is essential for these tasks to be accomplished if the group is to survive the savage on- slaught Of the Antarctic winter. Once the darkness of the winter sets in, station members must limit themselves 16 almost exclusively to indoor activities. For the remainder of the long winter, the men live and work together in vir- tual confinement with no possibility Of evacuation or Of additional supplies. Men are initially selected for Antarctic assign— ments on the basis of competence in an occupational Spe- cialty. However, all applicants are subjected to thorough physical and psychiatric screening examinations. Volun— teers tend to be youthful; participants range in age from 20 to 45, but most are under 35. The typical member of an Antarctic wintering group is likely to be totally in- experienced with respect to cold weather and isolation. Since each station must be a completely self- sustaining community for many months, a variety Of scien- tific, technical, and military occupations are represented. The Navy personnel work in occupational specialties in- volving support and maintenance, while scientific research is conducted by civilians in areas such as glaciology, ionospheric physics, and meterorology. Because of the wide variety Of occupational Specialties, Antarctic groups represent very heterogeneous pOpulations. Occupational group differences (largely Navy-civilian differences) have been demonstrated in areas such as motivation and personal 17 values (Gunderson and Nelson, 1966), personality traits (Gunderson and Mahan, 1966), job satisfaction and perform- ance (Doll and Gunderson, 1969), and in the predictive validity of screening measures (Doll, Gunderson, and Ryman, 1969). While the frontier-like existence at the Antarctic station lends itself to a friendly and informal atmOSphere, the close quarters and sameness of the physical and social environments tend to highlight personal and cultural idio- syncrasies during the long winter. Morale and work are affected to some degree by the interpersonal frictions that inevitably arise. Thus, psychosocial factors become important, and it is here that the greatest complexities exist, and knowledge is least adequate. In 1961 a program of psychological studies involv- ing groups wintering-over in Antarctica was instituted at the Navy Medical Neuropsychiatric Research Unit (NMNRU) in San Diego, California. This research is sponsored by the Bureau Of Medicine and Surgery and is under the direction of Dr. E. K. Eric Gunderson. These studies have been pri- marily concerned with the validity Of selection criteria, the measurement of job performance, and the determination Of related correlates Of adaptation in individual and 18 group behaviors which might influence task accomplishment. The NMNRU reports have been largely based upon data Ob- tained in absentia from questionnaires, supervisor evalu— ations, and peer ratings administered by the station Officer-in-charge two or three times during the winter. (Problems in the collection and limitations of this type of data were outlined by Gunderson and Nelson, 1965.) Gunderson (1966) has prepared a "Deep Freeze" bibliography with listings of NMNRU reports from 1962 to 1966. Wilkins (1967) reviewed Antarctic psychological studies in the context of group behavior in long-term isolation. The basis of interpersonal attraction in Antarc- tica has not been previously examined p§£_§§, although Nelson (1964) has looked at the sociometric structures that develop during the winter. These small wintering- Over Antarctic station groups seemed to provide a unique Opportunity to examine the basis Of interpersonal attrac- tion. During the isolated winter months group members have little work to occupy their time, and, consequently, they have much time available to cultivate interpersonal relationships. A long period of time is involved, so the effects of temporal factors can be studied through repeated measures Of many Of the variables. A wide variety of data 19 is available on all Of the individuals involved, including such things as personal history, personality measures, attitudes, measures Of adaptation, satisfaction, and job performance while in Antarctica, and various sociometric measures. The sample is large (N = 204) and heterogeneous, and thus somewhat more representative Of the general pOpu- lation than, say, college SOphomores. Since a field or natural laboratory setting is involved, problems with demand characteristics, evaluation apprehension, etc., are negligible. Finally, as mentioned earlier, problems with the interpretation Of any Observed relationship be- tween similarity and friendship are not involved. Hypotheses Personal history yapiablpp, Interpersonal attrac- tion has been found fairly consistently to be facilitated by similarity on various background characteristics (see p. 9). Newcomb (1961), however, found that the importance Of similarity on certain "quasi-demographic" character- istics in friendship formation diminished over time. Therefore, since friendship choices are ascertained after months in isolation, during which time no doubt other 20 traits assume prepotence, rather low level but positive correlations were expected between friends. Pppponality variables. As pointed out earlier (p. 10), the empirical findings regarding personality similarity (or complementarity) and friendship are in— consistent and conflicting. No doubt this state of af- fairs is due in part to the relative neutrality Of the settings used in such studies, i.e., certain psychological traits are not made salient. Given the setting Of the present study, however, it is unlikely that an individ- ual's psychological characteristics would not become highly manifest. As Secord and Backman (1964) have pointed out, whether similarity or complementarity is a better predic— tor Of attraction depends on the particular variable in question. Thus, with regard to certain needs examined in the present study (autonomy, achievement, and orderliness), similarity between friends seems a reasonable expectation. For instance, highly autonomous peOple should not be at— tracted to persons low on autonomy who need leadership and attention from others, but rather to individuals like themselves who are not demanding Of others. On the other 21 hand, complementarity between friends seems a more reason- able prediction with regard tO need for nurturance, which was also examined in the present study. That is, it was expected that a highly nurturant individual would not be attracted to another individual who similarly "needs" to give attention to others, but rather to an individual who has a "need" for receiving attention from others. Certain scales measuring self-perceptions were employed in the present study. Generally, research has shown that friends tend to perceive themselves in much the same way (e.g., Griffitt, 1966), although the findings are less than perfectly consistent (e.g., Miller 22.913} 1966). Thus, given the results Of most past studies, similarity between friends was expected for the scales used in the present research. Additional scales (FIRO-B; Schutz, 1958), which purportedly measure an individual's orientation to inter- personal relations, were examined in the present study. Similarity was expected between friends on those scales measuring the individual's affection and inclusion ten- dencies; that is, sociable and warm peOple should be drawn to each other. On the other hand, complementarity was predicted between friends on the scales assessing control 22 tendencies, since, for example, individuals who want to be controlled should not be attracted to someone who has similar motivations but rather to someone who will provide control. Attitudes. The theoretical orientations and em- pirical findings are fairly consistent regarding attitude or opinion similarity and interpersonal attraction, as was pointed out earlier (p. lfim Therefore, with respect to the attitudes examined in this study (some of which were measured prior to Antarctic duty and some of which were measured while at the duty station), similarity was expected between friends. Emotional symptoms. While at the Antarctic sta- tion, several stress symptoms were assessed at two time periods (early and late). Since research has shown that there is similarity between friends regarding their emo- tional states (Zimbardo and Formica, 1963), similarity was predicted for friends in their symptomatology. Further- more, this similarity was eXpected to increase from the early to the late administration, as the volunteers' reac— tions to their environment become more manifest in stress symptoms. 23 Performance evaluations. It has been shown that performance evaluations Of friends tend tO be similar (Deutsch and Solomon, 1959). Furthermore, friends tend to have reputational similarity (Miller pp 31,, 1966). Therefore, in the present study, friends were expected to be seen by both supervisors and peers as having similar standing on various task and social—emotional dimensions. In addition to assessing the degree of similarity or complementarity between friends, tests were also per— formed tO assess the influence of each of several person- ality dimensions on the tendency for subjects as a group to choose as friends persons who are similar or dissimilar to each other on the trait in question. It is an everyday observation that some people seem to relate effectively with a wide variety Of peOple, while others seem to relate only to a very narrow range of personalities. It seems plausible that these differences are related to differences in certain personality traits. For instance, it may be that individuals who are highly nurturant relate only to highly succorant individuals, while individuals for whom nurturance is less salient can relate well to individuals with widely differing degrees Of succorance. Thus, it was hypothesized that individuals who scored "high" on a given 24 personality dimension would, as a group, have as friends individuals whose scores on the same personality dimension would be relatively homogeneous, that is, the within group variance of scores would be small. On the other hand, individuals who scored "low" on a personality dimension were expected as a group to have friends whose scores on that dimension were relatively heterogeneous. Additional variables. The sample employed in this study was split into two groups: Navy and civilian per- sonnel. However, since the sample was split primarily for methodological reasons no hypotheses are presented to account for possible Navy-civilian differences in observed relationships. Also, as a basis Of comparison for rela- tionships Observed between friends, the analyses were per- formed on a group Of "mutual isolates," that is, pairs Of individuals who did not choose each other as friends. Although no hypotheses are presented for this group, it should be noted that it is not a control group but rather a group composed of pairs Of individuals who chose to avoid each other rather than to become friends. In many situations, ignoring or not choosing a person could merely mean a lack of sufficient contact for the development Of a 25 crystallized attitude. Given the circumstances Of the present study, however, it is unlikely that the individ- uals at a station had not formed definite attitudes about every other station member. Furthermore, Borgatta (1968), in a discussion of sociometric literature, concluded that persons who are unchosen when the positive choice form of the sociometric question is used tend to be the ones who are rejected in the negative form of the question. Thus, relationships Opposite to those hypothesized to occur between friends were eXpected to occur between "mutual isolates." MET HOD The subjects for the present investigation were 204 male volunteers who were assigned to U.S. Antarctic stations for a period Of one year during 1964—1969. Sixty- One percent of the subjects were Navy personnel; the re— maining 39%.were civilian scientists, technicians, and engineers. Only subjects from the two largest stations (Byrd and South Pole) were included in the analysis. The size of these stations ranged from 18 to 30 men. The median age for both the Navy and civilian personnel was 27 years, with a range for the total sample Of 18 to 43 years. Approximately half of the men were married. In terms of personal background, the Navy and civilian personnel represented different populations. The civilian personnel had higher levels of formal education (66% were college graduates) than did the Navy personnel (59%.were high school graduates). The civilians more fre— quently came from urban communities and were raised in 26 27 smaller families Of higher socio—economic status than were the Navy personnel. In addition to background differences, the Navy and civilian personnel have been shown to differ along various other dimensions. As mentioned in the Introduc- tion (p. 16), there are Navy-civilian differences in the areas Of motivation and personal values, personality traits, job satisfaction and performance, and in the pre— dictive validity Of their scores on the screening measures. Because of these cultural and psychological dif- ferences between the Navy and civilian personnel, the sample was split into these two sub—groups for purposes Of analysis. That is, since they come from different backgrounds and have different values, etc., they may have different criteria for friend selection, which, when con— sidering the two groups together, could mask or wash out strong relationships that might exist in each group sep- arately. Procedupe and Variablgp As mentioned earlier, all applicants undergo a rigorous screening program several months prior to their 28 deployment to Antarctica. In addition to a physical exam- ination and separate interviews with a psychiatrist and a psychologist, each applicant is administered several ques- tionnaires and inventories. Of these, the following were included in the present investigation. (1) Personal History Questionnaire This questionnaire Obtained basic background data pertaining to the applicant's developmental history. The particular items included in the analysis were level Of education, church attendance, number Of siblings, boyhood town size, father's education, and mother's education. (2) Opinion Survey This survey was developed eSpecially for the Ant- arctic screening program. Factor analysis was employed to identify highly inter—correlated clusters of items which appeared to represent meaningful psychological concepts. Four of the test scales measured common psychological needs (Need Scales): Achievement, Autonomy, Nurturance, and ngerliness. The content Of these four scales are gener- ally similar tO those of the corresponding Edwards Per- sonal Preference Schedule scales (1958), although the format of the items was entirely different. Statements 29 were presented as single items to be evaluated on a six— pOint scale ("strongly agree" to "strongly disagree") and response values were summed for items composing each scale. Also derived through factor analysis were five scales which measured attitudes toward the Antarctic as- signment (Attitude Scales): Motivation, Usefulness, Boredom, Confidence in organization, and Confidence in medical care. As with the Need scales, statements were presented as single items to be evaluated on a six-point scale Of agree-disagree, and response values were summed for items composing each scale. Finally, four scales represented personality self— descriptions based upon clusters Of intercorrelated traits (Self Scales). The scales and the traits composing them are as follows: Decisive (Decisive, Obedient, Handy, Alert, Orderly, and Self—improving); Excitable (Excitable, Argumentative, Hostile, Suspicious, and Impulsive); Blppp (Blunt, Stubborn, Rough in manner, and Hard); and Absent- minded (Absentminded, Awkward, and Slow). Each of these traits was presented as an adjective to be evaluated by the volunteer as being descriptive of himself along a six- point scale. Response values were summed for all traits composing a scale. 30 (3) FIRO—B (Fundamental Interpersonal Relations Orientation-Behavior) The only published test used consistently in Ant- arctic research has been the FIRO-B questionnaire (Schutz, 1958). According to Schutz's "Postulate of Interpersonal Needs": "(a) Every individual has three interpersonal needs: inclusion, control, and affection. (b) Inclusion, control, and affection constitute a sufficient set Of areas Of interpersonal behavior for the prediction and explanation of interpersonal phenomena [1958, p. 13]." He defined these three needs as follows: Inclusion: ". . . the need to establish and maintain a satisfactory relation with peOple with respect to interac- tion and association." (1958, p. 18.) Control: ". . . the need to establish and maintain a satisfactory relation with respect to control and power." (1958, p. 18.) Affection: ". . . the need to establish and maintain a satisfactory relation with people with respect to love and affection." (1958, p. 20.) 31 The FIRO-B measures two complementary aspects (expressing and wanting) of these three needs. There are thus six scales, each of which consists of nine items, and the scales are cumulative (Guttman) in type: Ipplpsion Exppessed (IE): participating in group activ— ities; Inclusion Wanted (IE): wanting to be included in group activities; Control Expressed (QE): controlling others, exercising dominance or leadership; Control Wanted (CW): wanting direction or regulation from others; Affpption Exprepsed (pg): being affectionate with others; Affection Wanted (AW): wanting affection from others. Scores on each of the six scales can range from 0 (low need) to 9 (high need). In addition to the above measures (ppediction variables), which were Obtained prior to Antarctic duty, various measures were Obtained on each subject while he was at his duty station (station variablgp). Of these measures, the following were examined in the present study. All of the following measures except the Supervisor 32 evaluations (6) and Peer evaluations (7) had two adminis— trations: one month after the beginning of the winter isolation period (March) and one month before the winter isolation period ended (September). (4) Station Opinion Survey This questionnaire tapped the station member‘s perceptions of his Antarctic eXperience and his impres- sions Of how well the station functioned. Each station member rated 34 statements along a six—point scale of agree-disagree. Factor analysis was used to derive six scales (Station Scales): Egalitarian atmosphere, Compat— ibility, Motivation, Accomplishment, Usefulness, and Job dissatisfaction. Response values were summed for all items composing a scale. (5) Common Complaints Each of ten "stress" symptoms was rated by the station member along a four-point scale according to how much he was troubled by it while at the station. Four scales were derived by factor analysis (Symptom Scalgp): Depression, Insomnia, Hostility, and Anxiety. ReSponse values were summed for all items composing a scale. 33 (6) Supervisor Evaluations Both the military supervisor and the civilian supervisor at a station independently rated each station member on ten dimensions, using an eight—point scale for each dimension. The military and civilian evaluations on each dimension were then averaged for each subject. On the basis Of content, four scales were derived and used in the present study (Supervisor Scales). The scales and the items of which they are composed are as follows: Tapkfpgrformance (Industriousness, Motivation, Proficiency); Emotional ptabilipy (Emotional control, Acceptance of authority); Social compatibility (Likability, Cheerfulness, Considerate Of others); and Leadership (Leadership abil- ity). Response values were summed for items composing a scale. Individuals at each station were ranked and as- signed T-scores (§'= 50, S.D. = 10) in terms Of average supervisor ratings on each of the four behavior dimensions. T-scores were utilized in order to control for station size and differences in supervisor's rating distributions. (7) Peer Evaluations On each Of ten items, each station member nominated from one to five men at the station whom he felt stood out 34 on the dimension tapped by the particular item. Four Of these items correspond to the four Supervisor scales de- scribed above and were examined in the analysis. Thus, there were four scales (Peer Scales): Task performance, Emotional stability, Social compatibility, and Leadership. As with the Supervisor scales, the subjects at each sta- tion were ranked and assigned T-scores in terms Of the number of peer nominations on each of the four behavior dimensions in order to control for station size. As men- tioned earlier, peer nominations were Obtained only once during the Antarctic assignment: one month before the end of the winter isolation period (September). One of the items from Peer Evaluations was con- sidered separately in the present analysis. On this item, each station member nominated those men at the station whom he had found to be his closest friends while at the station (Closest friend). Analysis For each of the variables described above, Product— Moment correlations were computed for each station between "closest friends" (as determined by the Closest friend 35 item). That is, for each variable there were two columns Of figures: in one column were the scores on that vari- able Of all individuals at a station; in the other column were the correSponding scores of the "closest friend" (first choice on the Closest friend item) of each indi- vidual in the first column. Correlations were then com- puted between these columns of scores separately for each station on each variable. The Fischer Z transformation was then used to transform each of the station correlations on each var— iable. These z—scores were weighted by Ni (the number of pairs of "closest friends" at station i) and an average z-score was Obtained across stations on each variable, which was then transformed back into a correlation and the mean correlations were tested against the null hypoth- esis that the population correlation was zero by means of t-tests. As a basis Of comparison, a similar analysis was performed for "mutual isolates," i.e., pairs Of individ— uals at a station who did not choose one another on the Closest friend item. This comparison group was derived in the following way. First, all individuals at a station who were not chosen at all by individual A on the Closest 36 friend item were looked at to see if they in turn did not choose individual A on the Closest friend item. Since there were Often several such pairings between individual A and his mutual non-choices, only one such pairing was chosen randomly for individual A. This procedure was carried out for every individual at a station, so that every individual was paired with a "mutual isolate." Correlations were then computed between these pairs of "mutual isolates" and tested against the null hypothesis that the pOpulation correlation was zero in the manner described above for "closest friends." In addition to the correlations obtained for each variable individually, correlations were computed between the following pairs of FIRO-B scales for both the "closest friends" and "mutual isolates" groups: Inclusion EXpressed (IE) vs. Inclusion Wanted (IW); Inclusion Wanted (IW) vs. Inclusion EXpressed (IE); Control Expressed (CE) vs. Con- trol Wanted (CW); Control Wanted (CW) vs. Control Ex— pressed (CE); Affection Expressed (AE) vs. Affection wanted (AW); Affection Wanted (AW) vs. Affection EXpressed (AE). According to Schutz (1958), ideal reciprocal com— patibility can be achieved by a dyad only if for each need area the expressed behavior of each member is equal to the 37 wanted behavior of the other. This reciprocal condition parallels that which Winch pp_al. (1954) refers to as "type II complementariness," where a positive correlation exists between the need of one member and the complemen- tary need of the other. Thus, these correlations tested the notion of need complementarity between "closest friends" and "mutual isolates." The correlational analysis described above for the total sample was also performed separately for the Navy sample. The rationale for splitting the sample was given earlier (see Subjects, pp. 26-27). Thus, both "closest friends" and "mutual isolates” groups were derived for the Navy personnel. In a few cases, Navy personnel chose civilian scientists as "closest friends"; such cases were included in the analysis. However, the "mutual isolates" group was composed entirely of Navy subjects. Originally it was planned to perform this same analysis on the civil- ian sample, but because of the low number of civilians at each station (average N = 5) it was felt that the result— ing correlations from separate stations would be tOO un- stable. In addition to the correlational analyses, further analyses were employed for certain Of the personality 38 variables (Need scales and FIRO-B scales). The assumption underlying these analyses (see pp. 23-24) was that if an individual is ”high" on some characteristic (e.g., need for Nurturance), he should be highly sensitive to that characteristic in others and hence should choose friends on that basis. On the other hand, a person "low" on that characteristic should be less discriminate in his choice of friends along that dimension since it has less salience for him. Thus, F-tests were employed to test the general hypothesis that for a group of individuals "high" on some characteristic there should be greater homogeneity on that characteristic in their "closest friend" choices than there should be in the "closest friend" choices Of a group Of individuals "low" on that characteristic. These tests were performed in the following way. First, a frequency distribution Of scores was Obtained for each variable in question (e.g., need for Nurturance). Those persons whose Nurturance scores were in the upper one- third of the distribution were classified as "high." Sim— ilarly, those persons whose Nurturance scores were in the lower one-third of the distribution were classified as "lows." Next, the "closest friends" chosen by the "highs" were determined (high chosen), as were the "closest friends" 39 chosen by the "lows" (19w chosen). The variance Of Nur- turance scores in the high chosen group was then compared to the variance of Nurturance scores in the lgw chosen group by means of an F-test that was the ratio of the respective mean squares Of the two groups of scores. The F-tests were performed separately for the Navy "closest friends" group and the civilian "closest friends" group on each of the Need scales and the FIRO-B scales. For both the correlational and the F-test analyses the .05 level (two-tailed) was used to define statistical significance. RESULTS Prediction variables Table 1 presents the results of the correlational analyses for the Personal History variables. It appears that "closest friends" in the total sample were signifi- cantly similar on church attendance, father's education, and mother's education. None Of the variables were signif- icant for "closest friends" in the Navy sample. "Mutual isolates" in the total sample differed significantly on church attendance and boyhood town size, while "mutual isolates" in the Navy sample differed significantly on church attendance. Thus, "closest friends" tended to be similar on a few aspects of their personal history, while “mutual iso- lates" tended to Be different on certain aspects Of their personal history. An exception to this pattern, however, is the significant similarity between "mutual isolates" in the total sample on amount Of formal education. There was no relationship between "closest friends" on this dimen- sion. Apparently, individuals who are somewhat similar 40 41 ~66. v m*.« Ho. v mac mo. v.@« o o o o o o e o o o o o HMO-Humoflflwm NNH HI Nmo Nmo I mHm H NHH HBH whN HI NmO mOH I «NHm N N50 mmH m.H0£UQz o o o o o O o o. e e o . o. HHOflgmoavm th I mmo who I Ofim H mmo NwH mom I mmo mNO I «fitmNN fi #50 NHM m.H0£¥Mh o o e o o o o I o o o o o ”NI-Hm com H- mmo cos . mum H Hes mew «mew m moo mma . mmm H mas ems asap eooasom 0 II o o ' o I o a II o I o e o o o mmfiflfl m *va N mOH mmm ®m¢ NNH mmo mflm H hmo HvH th hmo hNO HO Hmnfiflz . . . occupsmuus asOmn MI Mbo NBN I mHm. mmo. Nmo. «hmv.NI Nmo. mmH.I smfih.N omo. mmH. stages om@.I mOH. Mbo.l mmv.HI GMH. fiON.I amhm.N Vmo. MGN. mvm. mOH.. mmo. GOHHMUDVH .H .H .H .H u m up u m m. u m up u m .m ; fiend; =mmumaomH assusa= gmpsmfium umomOHO: smoumHOmw Hs5u58= gmpcmfinm ummmoau: . HQfiZflm H>¢z mam2¢m A<909 .mflflMflHm¢> NmOEMHm AdzOmmmm mom mBmNBIB 92E .mmommm DMflQZfiHm .mZOHBGAmmmOO m04mm>¢ H mnmdfi 42 in educational background tend to avoid one another in this type of environment, at least to a certain extent. In Table 2 are shown the results of the correla- tional analyses for the Need scales. Within the total sample there were no significant correlations, positive or negative, on any of these scales between either "closest friends" or "mutual isolates.” Within the Navy sample, however, ”closest friends" were significantly different (complementary) on need for Achievement. There were no significant correlations on any of the scales between "mu— tual isolates" in the Navy sample, although there was a marginally significant trend toward complementarity on need for Orderliness. The results of the F—tests on the Need scales for the Navy and civilian "closest friends" group are shown in Table 3. Although only three statistically significant F- ratios were Obtained, in all cases the variance (MS) Of the low phosen group was greater than the variance (MS) of the high chosen group. Thus, the hypothesis was generally con- firmed; that is, a group composed Of individuals with high need tended to choose friends who were more homogeneous than were the choices of persons with low need on that di- mension. The significant results were Obtained on (a) need 43 .mo. v ma mom.au moo. mma.n moo.n mod. vao.u moo. mso. ooo. me~.n ems. omo.u mmmaaaumonouc msm.au mod. mvH.u mom.- was. moo.u oo~.au moo. sea.» Hoo.n ono. ooo.u mocmusuuszuc oom.n sad. AHH.I mm».- was. NHH.- moo. Has. moo. oso. mad. moo. ssoaousanc oom.u was. moa.n «vo¢.Nu ooa. mom.u qmm.au moo. sma.u smo.u moo. omo.n usmsmpmasuanc .H .H H .H u m m. u m .m u m .m u m .m :mmusaomw Hmsusfi: =monmnum umwmoaog =mmumaomw assume: Hum =mpsmwnm pmomOHO= mans. > mamZflm M>¢Z WAfiZGm AS808 mmA¢Um Qmfiz.m0fl mBmmBIB 92¢ .mmommm Qm¢dz<fim .mZOHB¢ANmm00 m9¢mm2fl N mflmda 44 for Autonomy for civilians, (b) need for Orderliness for Navy personnel, and (c) need for Orderliness for civilian personnel.1 Thus, of the four needs measured, Autonomy proved to be salient in friend selection for the civilians, while Orderliness was salient in friend selection for both Navy and civilian personnel. TABLE 3 RESULTS OF F-TESTS FOR NEED SCALES High Low Variable Sample Chosen Chosen F N MS N MS n A hi V e t Navy 22 17.87 22 24.95 1.40 C e em “ Civilian 12 11.52 12 10.27 1.12 n_Autonom Navy 22 17.19 22 18.50 1.08 y Civilian 12 10.27 12 30.39 2.96* n_Nurturance Navy 22 12.46 22 18.81 1.52 Civilian 12 11.27 12 16.15 1.43 n_0rderliness Navy 22 8.86 22 19.47 2.20* Civilian 12 8.88 12 33.12 3.73** *p < .05 **p < .025 1It is interesting to note that in none of these three cases did the means Of the high chosen group appear to be significantly different from the correSponding means Of the low chosen group: Autonomy (civilian)--high chosen 2': 31.42, low chosen 2': 30.25; Orderliness (Navy)--high chosen §'= 17.00, low chosen 2': 15.05; Orderliness 45 Table 4 shows the results of the correlational analyses for the Attitude scales. In both the total sample and the Navy sample there were no significant cor— relations between "closest friends." There were signifi- cant correlations, however, between "mutual isolates" in both the total and Navy samples. In the total sample, "mutual isolates" tended to be significantly different on Confidence in organization and Confidence in medical care. "Mutual isolates" in the Navy sample tended to be significantly different on Boredom, as well as on Confi- dence in organization. Thus, it appears as though it is easier to predict mutual avoidance than it is friendship formation on the basis of certain attitudes related to anticipated group experience. The results of the correlational analyses for the Self scales are shown in Table 5. There were no signifi- cant correlations, either positive Or negative, for any of the scales. Apparently, self-descriptions bear no relationship to either friendship formation or mutual avoidance in this type of environment. (civilian)--high chosen 2': 16.17, low chosen §'= 16.67. However, since the F—tests were significant the assumption Of homogeneity of variance was clearly violated, thereby making t-tests inapprOpriate. Thus, statistical compari- sons between the means were not made. 46 .mo. v m». omm.| ONH. mvo.n mvm.n and. «no.1 mvo.l HOH. moo.| Nev. omo. mvo. soHum>Huoz G 6 O O O O 0 ' O 0' O O 0 ”H60 Hmolflcmz moo an who NNH I How HOH and «How N ovo mma vma I moa vao 1 ca .msoo sowusNHsmmHo «hom.mn Ono. ooa.a oom.a wed. mmH. avao.mn who. OOH.1 mme.a mmo. mma. as . . msoo «mov.mu moo. oma.| mvo.| oma. $00.: mam.u moo. 060.1 oma. maa. mHO. Sopmuom mmo.n «me. 600.: oom.u oma. ovo.l mmm.a Hmo. OOH. wom.H moo. Oma. mmosasmomo H H H H u m H u m H u m .m u m .m mHOOHHm> =mmumaomw Hooves: =monoanm unmmoflo= =mmumaomfl Hmsussa =nocwaum unmaoao. mqgm gflz mgm WQDBHFHAN mom mammalfi mflgm 93.08 O24 .9856 9335 52035580 mommEE v mflmfir 47 mmH.I vvm. NOH. mmo. mHH. mHo.I hNH.H Ono. mho. OMH.I mOH. wHo.I vowaflfilucmmnm vow.l @mo. mmo.l hHw.I NVH. mmo.l mhv.l vvo. HNO.I NNN. vHH. mNo. uGDHm mum.HI mmo. VHH.I 0mm.HI vOH. mMH.I NMh.I omo. moo.l mvm.l omo. mNo.I anmuHOxm mmn.l Hmo. mmo.l mVM.HI 00H. QHN.I vmh.l vmo. moo.l Nmo.I mmH. moo.l mbflmfloma H H H H u m H u n .W H w .W p m .m 387.83, :mmumHomH assassa :mUGGHHM DammOHU: amwumaOmH Heapssa :MUGQHHM “mTMOHU: WAQZQm w>4z mqmzdm A4808 mmAdUm mnmm mom mBmMBIB 02¢ ammommm GM¢DZ¢Hm .mZOHBflAmmmOU WUflMWPG m mam¢9 48 The results of the correlational analyses on and between the FIRO—B scales are similarly disappoint- ing (see Table 6). In both the total and Navy samples there were no significant correlations between "closest friends" on any of the scales or between complementary scales. There were, however, significant negative cor- relations between ”mutual isolates" On Inclusion Ex- pressed (IE) in both the total sample and in the Navy sample. Thus it appears that peOple who avoided each other in this situation differed in their affiliation tendencies. Like the correlational analyses, the F—tests for the FIRO—B scales yielded statistically nonsignifi- cant results. As can be seen in Table 7, which summar- izes these comparisons, the only significant F-ratio was on Inclusion Wanted (IW) for the Navy sample. This F- ratio, contrary to the general hypothesis, indicates that as a group Navy personnel who scored high on IW tended to choose as "closest friends" a group of indi— viduals who were less homogeneous on that dimension than 49 =mODMaOma amausfia =noawanw ummmoaog :WOUMHOmH HMHHDE: :mosoaHm HmomOaO: HAAZGm M>¢z madam mIOmHm mom menses oza .mmOMmm Omaozsem .szHefimmmOO moamman m HAM¢H NAQZflm AQBQE .HG. v mas .mo. v at omm. moo. oao. moo.II, «oak moa.I oom.a «co. moo. omm. maa. omo. mt .m> 3m ooo.aI amo. omo.I ova. aoa. omo. oov.I moo. vmo.I aov.I oaa. omo.I 3m .m> m4 omm.I amo. «oo.I Nam.al oma. Noa.I vmm.I ooo. ooo.I woe. moo. mmo. m0 .m> so ova. omo. mao. vom.I Naa. omo.I aoo.a moo. oaa. aoo.aI moo. amo.I 30 .m> mu emo.I ooo. ooo.I omo.a aaa. mma. mo~.I moo. omo.I ama.a moo. omo. Ma .m> 3H mmo. moo. ovo. mao. voa. moo. avm.I omo. amo.I mmo.I Nmo. moo.I 3H .m> NH o m.aI Nmo. Nva.I omo. m a. ao. mmo.I moo. mmo.I mum. «ma. mvo. Azmv pounds a a m 8368.? l O O I- O O O O O O O 0' Aav cmmmwug oom mmo amo ooo mma moo «mo ooo omo mmo I moa voo soauoommc . . .- .- . .. .- . . . . .- one .4353 mmm I Oaa moa mam oaa omo mmm omo omo I amo I omo ooo aOHusou m. o o. a. o . oa. o o. ma.a ooo. amo. a . oo. «0. Amuv Commmmem mm a o mv vv m v N «v v o aoHusoo O O O O O C O O O O O O AgHv @853 ooo Noa moo moo oaa ooo mom ooa omo aoo I omo ooo I scamsaosa . . . . . . _ . . . . . . :5 Banana «amo NI moo mma I mam ama aaa «aomm MI mmo ooa I aom mmo mmo seamsaosa H H H H u. m m. u m m u m m u m m wanvaum> iiiillllllllll TABLE 7 50 RESULTS OF F-TESTS FOR FIRO-B SCALES High Low Variable Sample Chosen Chosen F N ms N ms Inclusion Navy 24 4.26 24 3.99 1.07 EXpressed (IE) Civilian 12 4.63 12 4.45 1.04 . Navy 20 14.75 20 .79 18.74* Inclu51on wanted (IW) Civilian 12 15.30 12 8.99 1.70 Control Navy 23 3.80 23 3.42 1.11 Expressed (CE) Civilian 12 10.39 12 4.81 2.16 Control Navy 24 4.39 24 4.51 1.03 wanted (CW) Civilian 12 3.66 12 6.45 1.76 Affection Navy 24 2.34 24 3.39 1.45 ExPressed (AB) Civilian 12 2.45 12 2.64 1.08 Affection Navy 24 5.52 24 5.56 1.01 wanted (AW) Civilian 12 5.30 12 3.79 1.40 *p < .005. 51 did persons who scored low on IW.2 The most Obvious in- terpretation of this finding is that individuals who have a high need for affiliation (i.e., high on IW) want to be accepted by everyone, even by those for whom affiliation or inclusion is less important. However, it is not imme- diately clear why this should be so for only the Navy per- sonnel, and not for the civilians as well. Perhaps the greater autonomy for civilians in their work roles made inclusion a less salient dimension than it was for the Navy personnel, whose work roles required much interper- sonal contact. Station Variables In Table 8 are shown the results of the correla- tional analyses for the Station scales. For "closest friends" in the total sample, significant similarity was demonstrated on early Motivation, late Accomplishment, It is interesting to note that the mean of the high chosen group (3.7) appeared to be significantly greater than the mean Of the low chosen group (.55), al- though significance tests were not performed (see foot- note l). Apparently a similarity effect was Operating in friend choice for individuals low on IW; the very large variance in the scores Of friends chosen by "high" indi- viduals masked this effect, however, so that a nonsignifi- cant correlation was Obtained on this variable (see Table 6). 52 .aoo. v m««« .Ho. v on. .mo. v m« . . . u I . . . . . . . . soauoom moa HI ooo ooa I amo a moa Hon ooo ooo omo IaIeaa m ooo ovm Inaumnoao nos mv~.I ooo. moo.I oom.a «ma. Hon. ooo.I ooo. ooo. oma.a aoa. oaa. mnmcasmomo «mo~.~I moo. ooa.I aoo~.~ ooa. ooo. ooo. ooo. aoo. «Iaom~.v ooo. omo. ucmsaoaaosoooa omm.I ooo. aoo.I aoo.aI oaa. aoo.I ooo.a moa. ooa. ~oo.a ooa. oaa. coaum>auos aoo.aI ooo. ooo.I omv.a ooa. mma. mam.I ooo. omo. omo.a moa. ooa. Huaaanaumasoo . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.8.8.... «ooo «I omo ooa I ooo I flea aaa I amm I ooo omo com a oaa ova anaumuaamom 1388 . . . . . . . . . . . . coauomm mos I «ma aoo I omo moo moo mam oaa ooa ooo a ooo moa Imaumomao ooo omo. moo. ooo. amm. aoo. omo. aoa. ooo. ooo. Hom.I ooo. omo.I mmoaasmmno oom.I «ma. mao.I «ma. aaa. mao. ooo. aoa. ooa. mam.a oaa. ova. unassmaaasoooa «Hom.oI ooo. oaa.I omv.aI ooo. oaH.I ooa.a ooa. oaa. «Ioao.m aoo. oom. coaum>auoz aoo.aI ooa. ova.I omo. moa. oma. «aom.oI aoo. ova. ova.a ooo. oaa. muaaanaumasoo . . . . . . . . . . . . mnoamnosum ooa a ooo aoo «as ooo omo Ivoo o ooo ooa ooo I moo ooo I amanmuaamoo Amaummo H .H .H .H u m H u m H u m H u m H mannaum> :moumaoma amauss: .moamanm ummnoao= amoumaOma assuza: zmoamanm uowmoao= mam24z m mnmfla mnmz amoumaOma amsusE: amosoaHm HommOaO: :monaoma aMOHOS: .moamana unmnoau= mam2¢z mamZGm 44809 mma¢0m Edamzwm afida Qz< NQMdfl MOE mfimmfila n24 ammommm Qmmflzmfim .mZOHadammmOO m0¢mm>d m wands 56 .ao. v 944 .mo. v 9* ooo.l woo.al «amNH.MI «aHmo.MI mmo.l «www.mI Nam.l moH.NI amo. oaa. ooo. aoo. Qua. moa. mma. Naa. Noo.l oaa.l NVN.I MNN.I ooo.l mmN.I moo.l m¢N.I mom.al mmm.al oaa. mmm.aI amm>.Nl moo.I omv.l omv.l aaa. oaa. wNa. omo. moa. moa. Nva. oma. ooa.l mma.l mac. mmH.I omN.I voa.l Noo.l Noo.l oom.I ava.l omm.l mom.l Nwm. mmm.al ooN.I voo.I ooo. moo. ooo. moo. Naa. mmo. mmo. mmo. mNo.I aao.I omo.l ooo.l mmo. .moa.l mmo.l voo.l amm.l 0mm.a «omo.m oom.a NmN.al mma.l mmm.. mum.l NHa. mmo. Noa. mwo. omo. oma. aoa. maa. mvo.l mva. mmN. mma. vma.l omo.l mmo. moo.l maanoomoa Huaaanaumosoo amaoom suaaanmuo amsoauosm oosmEHouHom Home Amoamom Hommv masmHmomoq muaaaoaummaoo amaoom Huaaanmuo amsOauOSm mosmaHOMHom xmma Ammamom Homa>Homdmv u H m H u H m H u H m H u H m H amoumaOma amsusfi= =MGC6HHM HOQMOHO: amoumaoma assusaa =noamaum uoomoaoa 9992€m w>¢z 9992dm 99909 oanmanm> m99¢0m 9999 92¢.mOmH>Mflmbm 909 mammBIB 92¢ .mMQmmm 9m¢9z¢8m .mZOHBm9mmmOO mwdePG OH 99999 57 for Dominance and "closest friends" are complementary on that dimension. For the "mutual isolates" in the total sample, there were no significant correlations on any of the Super- visor or Peer scales. Within the Navy sample, however, "mutual isolates" were significantly different on Super- visor Social compatibility, Peer Task performance, and Peer Emotional stability. Within the Navy sample, there- fore, peOple who avoided each other as friends had a stronger relation on these dimensions (negative) than did peOple who chose one another as friends. Apparently, then, in this type Of environment individuals tend to avoid those who are different on task and socio—emotional dimensions, but do not necessarily form friendships with those who are similar. On balance, the results Of the correlational anal- yses are disappointing in that few significant correlations were found between "closest friends," particularly on the prediction variables. In fact, the greater number of correlations (mostly negative) on the prediction variables for "mutual isolates" suggests that perhaps avoidance is easier to predict in this type of environment than is 58 positive attraction. On the other hand, the results of the F-tests indicate that personality measures (Need and FIRO-B scales) do have some predictive power for friend- ship formation but not in the usual linear sense. DISCUSSION Generally speaking, the results Of the present study were disappointing. While similarity was demon- strated between "closest friends" on certain attitudes, personality similarity (and complementarity) proved to be a poor predictor of friendship choice. Within the total sample, similarity was demonstrated between "closest friends" on church attendance, father's education, mother's education, and in their early feelings Of motivation and late feelings Of accomplishment and job dissatisfaction while in Antarctica. In addition, "closest friends" were perceived by their peers in Antarctica as being similar in their emotional stability. The pattern Of results for "closest friends" in the Navy sample generally resembled that Of the total sample, although fewer results reached statistical significance. In fact, the only significant similarity demonstrated between "closest friends" in the Navy sample was in their late feelings of accomplishment while in Antarctica. Moreover, unlike the results for the I 59 60 total sample, complementarity was demonstrated between "closest friends" on three variables for the Navy sample: need for Achievement, feelings Of depression while in Antarctica, and supervisor perceptions of leadership ability. Since no complementary relationships were Obtained for "closest friends" in the total sample, the complemen- tary relationships demonstrated between "closest friends" in the Navy sample suggest certain Navy—civilian differ- ences in the criteria used in the selection of friends. It seems plausible that these differences are in large part related to differences between these two samples in their prescribed roles while in Antarctica. The civilians at these stations are there primarily for purposes of in- dividual research, so a high degree of autonomy is pos— sible and, if desired, these persons can avoid interper- sonal contact. The Navy personnel, on the other hand, are responsible for station maintenance, which requires much coordinated effort on their part, and hence a great deal Of interpersonal contact. In addition, the Navy personnel perform jobs that are more demanding and less intrinsic- ally rewarding than the relatively "soft" duties performed by the civilians. Thus, different factors are likely to 61 become manifest and assume importance in friend selection for these two groups. In addition, established Navy- civilian differences in background and personality char- acteristics (see p. 16) may also contribute to differences between the two groups with respect to the criteria used in friend selection. Unfortunately, Navy-civilian dif- ferences in the criteria for friend selection can only be inferred since, as pointed out earlier (p. 37), the small number of civilians at each station made separate corre- lational analysis for the civilian sample impractical. Thus, differences between the total and Navy samples are seen as merely suggestive of possible differences between the Navy and civilian samples. It is interesting to note the relatively large number Of significant relationships that were Obtained between "mutual isolates." As expected, these relation— ships were generally Opposite tO those Observed between "closest friends," that is, relationships tended to re- flect complementarity rather than similarity. Within the total sample, "mutual isolates" were found to be comple- mentary with regard to church attendance, boyhood town size, their confidence in the organization and medical care prior to their Antarctic deployment, their reported 62 participation in group activities (Inclusion EXpressed-- FIRO-B), and in their early perceptions of group compati- bility while in Antarctica. On the other hand, similarity was obtained for level Of education, and in their early perceptions of egalitarian atmosphere in Antarctica. It is not clear why similarity should be obtained on these two variables, although the relatively low level of sig- nificance (p < .05) suggests that these results may have been due to chance. As with the results for "closest friends," the pattern Of results were generally similar for both the total and Navy samples Of "mutual isolates." Complemen- tarity was demonstrated between "mutual isolates" in the Navy sample with regard to frequency of church attendance, number Of siblings, their anticipated boredom and confi- dence in the organization prior to Antarctic deployment, their reported participation in group activities (Inclu- sion EXpressed——FIRO-B), and their early motivation and late perceptions Of egalitarian atmOSphere and accomplish- ment in Antarctica. In addition, in Antarctica they were seen by their supervisors as different in their social compatibility, and by their peers as different in their tas performance and emotional stability. Unlike "mutual 63 isolates" in the total sample, they did not demonstrate significant similarity on any Of the variables. Thus, the hypothesis of complementarity between mutually indifferent or antagonistic individuals in this setting was in general supported. In fact, more signifi— cant relationships were Obtained for "mutual isolates" (19) than for "closest friends" (11). It appears, there— fore, that mutual indifference or antagonism bears an equal or somewhat stronger relationship to the variables in this study than does friendship. Furthermore, only four of the prediction variables yielded a significant relationship between "closest friends," while for the "mutual isolates" eight of the prediction variables yielded significant relationships. This suggests that, at least in the setting of this study, mutual antagonism or indif— ference is easier to predict than attraction. Perhaps even stronger relationships on many of the other variables would have been Obtained if the analysis had been done on pairs of individuals who were more clearly antagonistic. Unfortunately, an unambiguous measure of antagonism (e.g., "who don't you like?") was not Obtained from the station members. 64 In addition to the hypothesized relationships between similarity or complementarity and friendship, it was eXpected that the salience of certain personality traits would relate to the homogeneity or heterogeneity of chosen friends on the trait in question. However, the F—tests which were employed to test this hypothesis yielded only four significant relationships of this nature. Al— though the F—ratios were generally nonsignificant, it was noted earlier (p. 42) that for the Need scales, the dif- ferences in variances were all in the predicted direction. Thus, with regard to need for Autonomy and need for Order— liness, and to some extent with regard to the other needs (Achievement and Nurturance), "high" individuals as a group tended to choose as friends individuals who were relatively homogeneous on the same need. In addition to theoretical interest, a finding of this nature has Obvious practical application to the selection Of individuals for group composition, particularly in the type Of setting investigated in this study. That is, in a situation such as Antarctica where individuals must live and work to- gether in virtual isolation from the remainder of society, it is important that individuals are chosen who can 65 establish a compatible relationship with most, if not all, of the group members. Perhaps the main conclusion that can be drawn on the basis Of the results of the present investigation is that similarity (or complementarity) is not as strong a _predictor Of friendship formation in a situation of this type as originally thought. Certain questions, however, remain unanswered. Why were generally "nonsignificant" results Obtained? Would a different analysis have been more apprOpriate? Can these results be generalized to interpersonal attraction in general? Considering the published research in this area, can any firm conclusions be made about the determinants of friendship? What meth— odological changes are needed in future research in this area? First of all, certain aSpects of the situation may have accounted for the relative unimportance Of similarity and complementarity. Life at the Antarctic station is very task-oriented; physical survival is dependent upon the performance Of various jobs. Thus, group members may have been drawn to those who were most competent at per- forming their work role, and it has been shown that simi— larity affects interpersonal judgments along 66 social-emotional dimensions, but not along task-oriented dimensions (McLaughlin, 1971). Similarly, Hoffman and Maeir (1966) found that task-oriented groups composed of individuals with either similar or dissimilar personality profiles did not differ in their attractiveness for their members. Most Of the positive correlations between simi— larity and attraction have been found in groups which were principally social in nature, such as rooming-house residents (Newcomb, 1961), or work associates who social- ized together outside Of the work setting, but who were not necessarily members of the same work group (Rosenfeld and Jackson, 1959). In these situations the principal demand placed on the relationship is merely one of getting along with one another and, hOpefully, enjoying the rela- tionship. Under such circumstances, similarity of person- ality could provide the basis for enjoying the same kinds of activities, sharing similar feelings about various events, and offering sympathy when things go wrong. The extreme salience of task performance in the present study, however, may have overwhelmed the importance Of person- ality similarity in friendship. Another possible factor specific to the Antarctic setting that may have influenced the Obtained findings 67 involves the motivation of the typical Antarctic volunteer. Law (1960) concluded that "escape" is a motivating factor for these individuals. Certainly this type of situation is a haven for the technically competent individual who is deficient in social skills. Thus, to the extent that these individuals volunteer for Antarctic duty in order to "escape" their personalities, they should not be attracted to other station members who are similar to them in per- sonality. This interpretation would be more plausible if similarity had been demonstrated between "mutual iso— lates." However, the results suggest that similar indi— viduals did not necessarily avoid each other, so the above interpretation is not well supported. Perhaps the most important factor involving the setting of the present study was the prescribed membership of the station groups. Although there is considerable Opportunity for interaction between all individuals at the Antarctic stations, interaction between Navy and civilian personnel is relatively infrequent, presumably because they have vastly different roles to perform (al— though personality and cultural differences may also be a factor). Thus, ”functional prOpinquity" (Festinger pp al., 1950) may have put restrictions on friendship to a 68 considerable degree. In most studies finding a positive association between similarity and attraction, the groups were more able to regulate the extent of their relations with others or to associate more with certain members than others (e.g., Rosenfeld and Jackson, 1959). In unstruc— tured social situations, people with similar personalities may become attracted to each other as they find their attitudes, interests, and reactions to shared experiences to be compatible. Friendship formation in a group whose membership is prescribed by the organization, may, on the other hand, be a function of factors more directly related to the goals of the group. In addition to the specific factors related to the Antarctic setting, there are factors related to conditions of isolation in general that may in part account for the lack Of a consistent relationship between similarity and attraction. As pointed out in the Introduction to this study (p. 7), it could be argued that individuals re- stricted to a monotonous unchanging environment should be attracted to others who are dissimilar rather than similar, since they would provide a source of varied stimulation. However, the results indicate that while "closest friends" were not similar on many dimensions, 69 neither were they significantly dissimilar on many dimen- sions. This interpretation, therefore, does not appear to be well supported. Another, more plausible, interpretation is that this type of environmental deprivation amounts to the temporary suSpension Of basic human needs such as affec- tion, security, and significance. Thus, to the extent that the various personality measures tapped such needs, similarity (or dissimilarity) on these dimensions would not necessarily be related to friendship. The results support this interpretation. In fact, the only person- ality measure that had a relationship (negative) with friendship choice was need for Achievement among the Navy sample. This particular relationship on need for Achieve— ment probably reflects the increased salience of job per— formance in Antarctica, although it is not intuitively clear why friends should be dissimilar on this dimension. Perhaps need for Achievement also reflects need for Domi- nance, at least within the Navy sample, in which case complementarity would be expected. Besides the factors related to isolation that may in part account for the Obtained results, there are more general reasons why a consistent relationship between 70 similarity and friendship was not demonstrated. For one thing, there probably are large and consistent individual differences in the extent to which similarity or comple- mentarity affects attraction. For example, a person with high self—esteem may be attracted to similar others, while a person with low self—esteem may shun similar others and be attracted instead to those who have traits which he lacks and/or to which he aspires. Support for this idea is given by Posavac (1971), who found that similarity was a prime factor in interpersonal attraction for certain personality types, while complementarity or some other factor was a major determinant of attraction for other personality types. Furthermore, temporal factors have been shown to influence attraction. Newcomb (1961), for example, found that the effect of attitudinal similarity on attraction increased over time. At the same time, he also found that similarity in certain "quasi—demographic" variables re— lated to interpersonal attraction early in acquaintance, but not later. He reasoned that early eXpectations about interpersonal agreement may have been formed on the basis of similarity Of initially observable prOperties of the persons, but that these eXpectations subsequently were 71 disconfirmed when previously covert attitudes became ex— pressed. This interpretation is consistent with the pro— posal that the behaviors exhibited by persons in the ini— tial stages of interaction tend to be facades (Goffman, 1959) or at least nonrepresentative samples Of the be— haviors they display under more familiar circumstances (Thibaut and Kelley, 1959). In addition to the possibility that overt behavior changes over time, changes in social motivation may also occur. It is likely that increased acceptance by one's peer grOUp leads to a reduction in insecurity, thereby changing the criteria by which social choices are made. Research on early acquaintance has demonstrated that ex— pectations and fears of rejection are positively related to the choice Of similar persons (Rosenfled, 1964). Fur— thermore, even though highly similar persons are chosen because of eXpected reciprocation, less similar persons tend to be viewed as more attractive (Walster and Walster, 1963). Thus, social choices in later acquaintance may be based more upon the potential for pleasurable interaction than upon anxiety reduction. Thus, it may be that early in acquaintance station members were attracted to one another, in part, on the 72 basis of similarity in personality or social behavior. But as individuals got to know one another better, facades were drOpped and social motivation changed, so that friendships were formed on other bases, such as task performance or attitudinal agreement on topics of mutual concern. Unfortunately, there are no data regarding friendship choice early in the winter with which to test this interpretation. However, the results suggest that there was greater attitudinal agreement between "closest friends" late in the winter than early in the winter. In addition to the situational and theoretical reasons outlined above, there are methodological reasons why more "significant" findings were not Obtained. These problems are not limited to the analysis employed in the present study, but prevail throughout the literature in this area. First Of all, there is the general inadequacy of the single trait approach. In studies of this type an unknown number of unidentified variables are potentially Operative, but are not consistently controlled. Further, personality similarity itself is defined in terms Of one or at best a small subgroup Of personality variables so that similarity along all other dimensions is also not 73 controlled. The situation, therefore, is one in which the effect of a very limited number of independent variables on attraction is determined in a context where a large number Of uncontrolled independent variables are Operating. Furthermore, even when studies examine a large num— ber of independent variables (as in this study), they are usually examined in isolation, that is, separate correla- tions are computed for each variable with the criterion. This implies the rather naive assumption that traits are additive. A much more reasonable position is that traits are interactive. Only if the variable under investigation were of sufficient strength to override all other indepen- dent variables, Or if a sufficient number of the other in- dependent variables happened to convary with it, or if the other variables were accidentally controlled through ran— domization, would the hypothesized relationship be ob— served. Since the Obtained findings in this area lack consistency, these special circumstances do not appear to occur regularly. For example, a person high in need for affiliation and low in anxiety is likely to be attracted to entirely different people than is a person high on both affiliation need and anxiety. Or, as another example, a person high in need for dominance and low in need for 74 affiliation may interact with very few people and for limited selfish purposes. On the other hand, a person similarly high in dominance but also high in affiliation may be attracted to many different peOple and act as a pOpular leader rather than a ruthless manipulator. The above examples show how complex the situation can be con- sidering only two variables at two defined levels; in the real world literally hundreds of variables may be inter— acting in a much more complex fashion. Therefore, rather than looking at traits individ- ually, the patterning of traits should be the independent variables in research of this nature. Many existing tests (e.g., MMPI, Edwards Personal Preference Schedule) yield personality profiles which could be employed in such re- search. Hoffman and Maeir (1966), for instance, used personality profiles based on responses to the Guilford— Zimmerman Temperament Survey tO form homogeneous and heterogeneous groups. Another methodological problem involves the rela- tionship between scores on a personality measure and be— havior in an interpersonal situation. Personality dimen— sions influence attraction only to the extent that such dimensions have an effect on behavior in a given situation; 75 it is the behavioral stimuli to which subjects respond and not to the hypothesized personality dimension. But the relationship between personality test scores and be— havior in various everyday life encounters or even behav- ior in a controlled laboratory situation is for the most part unknown. However, it seems reasonable that a one- to—one relationship does not hold. It may be that test responses, because of social desirability, idealized self— image, etc., either do not correSpond to behavior at all, or correSpond to the behaviors exhibited early in an in- teraction, or between individuals who are less well—. acquainted than are friends. If that is the case, it is not surprising that friends do not have "similar" person- alities as measured by test responses. Friends may indeed be very similar in their behavior, motivations, and per— Spective toward life, but analyses that are based on tests that tap superficial behavior or no behavior at all will not demonstrate this. One way to overcome this problem is to have actual behavioral measures of the subjects' personalities. For instance, separate judges could rate an individual on various dimensions as he is Observed in interaction with others. However, in addition to being highly impractical—- 76 even for a small number of subjects--this method is less than satisfactory, since the behavior exhibited by a sub- ject in that situation may not reflect his behavior in different settings or with other interactants. Another possibility is to employ projective type tests to assess the subject's personality. Such tests supposedly reveal unconscious determinants of personality and hence are un- likely to be distorted by the subject's desire to appear "normal." Another methodological weakness of studies in this area is the pervasive use of correlational analyses. It seems rather naive to assume that the relationship between similarity and attraction should be a linear one. There is no reason to suppose, for example, that a person slightly below average in need for dominance should choose as friends those slightly below (or above) average in need for dominance. He has other needs that are more salient and which should determine friendship choice. The F-tests employed in this study were an attempt to examine possible relationships that were free of the assumption of linear- ity in correlational analyses. It is an everyday Observa— tion that certain individuals can effectively (or super— ficially) relate only to a narrow range of peOple, while 77 others seem capable and/Or willing to build relationships with practically everyone. This aSpect of friendship formation seems just as important as asking in what ways are all friends similar, but has never been examined em- pirically. Another possible alternative to correlational analyses would be to compute eta coefficients for friend pairs. Eta does not yield insight into the Specific na- ture of a relationship, however, and therefore has less predictive value than the product-moment correlation. Yet another possibility is to compute correlations separately for different points on the distribution Of subject scores on the variable in question. Thus, it may be that indi- viduals low on some dimension choose similar people as friends, while individuals high on the same dimension choose dissimilar (complementary) people as friends. The total correlation, meanwhile, would show nO relationship. The above possibilities are not by any means exhaustive. Certainly other analyses are also possible; the point is that results Obtained from traditional correlational anal— yses can be misleading since only linear relationships are examined. 78 Still another problem with studies in this area is the confusion between "interpersonal attraction" and "friendship." It is one thing to view another person as attractive and quite another thing to build a friendship with that person. While many peOple may be seen by an individual as attractive, most peOple have a relatively small number of close friends. The qualities we admire in a person may not be the qualities we would desire in a person with whom we confide, confess our weaknesses, etc. As pointed out earlier (pp. 70—71), at different steps in the acquaintance process we may exhibit different behaviors or have different social motivations. Suppo— sedly, friends are at a fairly advanced point in the acquaintance process, so that facades to some extent are drOpped and persons at this stage interact for different reasons than do those who are less well acquainted. Lab- oratory studies of interpersonal attraction, however, look at individuals with no prior acquaintance (e.g., Walster and Walster, 1963), or ask subjects to evaluate supposed "strangers" whom they never will meet (e.g., Byrne pp al., 1967). Thus, conclusions regarding similarity and attrac- tion from such studies may not be generalizable to 79 real-life interaction past the first encounter, let alone to something as unique as friendship. This particular weakness was not evident in this study since the subjects were isolated together for sev- eral months. However, it may be that really close rela— tionships were not formed and that "closest friends" were individuals who were merely more compatible with each other than with anyone else at thestation. This seems plausible since there were relatively few mutual or re- ciprocated friend choices; only 40 percent of the indi- viduals chosen first on the Closest friend item recipro- cated in their first choice (although almost all individ— uals chosen first reciprocated on later choices). Thus, the results of the present study may reflect interpersonal attraction in this type Of situation rather than friend- ship per pp, SUMMARY Contrary to prediction, "closest friends" in Ant- arctica were found to be similar on few personality dimen— sions, and only slightly similar in background character— istics. While in Antarctica, however, "closest friends" did tend to develOp similar attitudes regarding their experience at the station. On the other hand, as pre- dicted, "mutual isolates" (pairs of individuals who were not attracted to one another) were significantly different on various dimensions and tended to develOpIconflicting attitudes while in Antarctica. It appears, therefore, that in a stressful task-oriented situation it is easier to predict incompatibility than compatibility on the basis Of various traits and characteristics of the individuals involved. It was also predicted that depending on how sal— ient a given dimension is for an individual, he should be attracted to a larger or smaller range of peOple on that dimension. The results of the analyses of this hypothesis 8O 81 were generally nonsignificant, although the Obtained F- ratios for several personality measures (Need scales) were in the predicted direction. Several aSpects of the setting involved in this study were discussed as possible reasons why more signif- icant results were not obtained. Some Of these (task- orientation, prescribed membership, temporary suspension of common psychOlOgical needs) were held as more tenable than others (dissatisfaction with self, dissimilarity as stimulation in a monotonous environment). Various theoretical and methodological problems with the published research in the area of friendship for- mation and interpersonal attraction were discussed. Some Of these (single trait approach, unknown relationship be- tween test responses and behavior, use of correlational analyses) were evident in this study, while others (con— trOl of temporal factors, examination of nonlinear rela- tionships) were not. REFERENCES REFERENCES Backman, C. W., and Secord, P. F. The effect of perceived liking on interpersonal attraction. Human Rela— tions, 1959, ng 379-384. Banta, T. J., and Hetherington, M. Relations between needs Of friends and fiances. Journal of Abnormal and Becker, G. The complementary—needs hypothesis, authori- tarianism, dominance, and other Edwards Personal Preference Schedule scores. Journal Of Person— ality, 1964, 32, 45—46. Bonney, M. E. A sociometric study of the relationship Of some factors to mutual friendships on the elemen- tary, secondary, and college levels. Sociometpy, 1946, 2, 21-47. Borgatta, E. F. Sociometry. In Sills, D. (Ed.), Interna— tional EncyclOpedia of the Social Sciences, Vol. 15. The MacMillan Company and The Free Press: 1968, 53-57. Bossard, J. H. S. Nationality and nativity as factors in marriage. American Sociological Review, 1939, 4, 792—798. Bowerman, C. E. Assortive mating by previous marital status: Seattle, 1939—1946. American Sociolog— ical Review, 1953, 18, 170-177. Bowerman, C. E., and Day, B. R. A test of the theory Of complementary needs as applied to couples during courtship. American Sociological Review, 1956, 21, 602—605. 82 83 Byrne, D., Clore, G. L., Jr., and Worchel, P. Effect of economic similarity-dissimilarity on interpersonal attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 4, 220-224. Byrne, D., Griffitt, W., and Stefaniak, D. Attraction and similarity of personality characteristics. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 5, 82-90. Byrne, D., and Nelson, D. Attraction as a linear function of prOportion Of positive reinforcements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 1, 659-663. Cattell, R. B., and Nesselroade, J. R. Likeness and com- pleteness theories examined by 16 personality factor measures on stably and unstably married couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 1967, 1, 351-361. Coates, T., and Mazur, S. Personality characteristics and interpersonal attraction. Psychology, 1969, p, 2—9. Cohen, A. R. EXperimental effects of ego-defense prefer— ence on interpersonal relations. Journal of Ab— normal and Social Psychology, 1956, p2, l9-27. Corsini, R. J. Understanding and similarity in marriage. Journal Of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1956, .52. 327-332. Deutsch, M., and Solomon, L. Reactions to evaluations by others as influenced by self-evaluations. Socio- metry, 1959, 22, 93-112. Doll, R. E., and Gunderson, E. K. E. Occupational group as a moderator Of the job satisfaction—job per- formance relationship. Journal of Applied Psye chology, 1969, 5;, 359—361. 84 Doll, R. E., Gunderson, E. K. E., and Ryman, D. H. Rela- tive predictability of occupational groups and performance criteria in an extreme environment. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1969, 4;, 399-402. Edwards, A. L. Manual for the Edwards Personal Preference. Schedule (revised edition). New York: Psycholog— ical Corp., 1959. Festinger, L. A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 1954, 1, 117—140. Festinger, L., Schachter, S., and Back, K. Socialgpres- sures in informal groups: A study of human factors in housing. New York: Harper, 1950. Glick, P. C., and Landau, E. Age as a factor in marriage. American Sociological Review, 1950, 4;, 517-529. Goffman, E. The presentation Of self in everyday life. Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday, 1959. Golden, J. Patterns of Negro-white intermarriage. Amer- ican Sociological Review, 1954, 12, 144-147. Gordon, J. E. Interpersonal predictions of repressors and sensitizers. Journal of Personality, 1957, 2;, 686-698. Griffitt, W. B. Interpersonal attraction as a function Of self-concept and personality similarity- dissimilarity. Journal of Personalityyand Social Psychology, 1966, 4, 581-584. Gunderson, E. K. E. Adaptation to extreme environments: Prediction of performance. U.S. Navy Medical NeurOpsychiatric Research Unit Report NO. 66—17, April, 1966. Gunderson, E. K. E., and Mahan, J. L. Cultural and psy- chological differences among occupational groups. Journal of Psychology, 1966, 4;, 287-304. 85 Gunderson, E. K. E., and Nelson, P. D. Personality differ— ences among Navy occupational groups. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1966, 44, 956-961. Gunderson, E. K. E., and Nelson, P. D. Individual perform- ance measures for extremely isolated groups. U.S. Navy Medical NeurOpsychiatric Research Unit Report NO. 65-6, February, 1965. Heider, F. The psychology of interpersonal relations. Wiley, 1958. Hoffman, L. R. Similarity of personality: A basis for interpersonal attraction? Sociometry, 1958, 4;, 300-308. HOffman, L. R., and Maeir, N. R. F. An eXperimental re— examination Of the similarity-attraction hypoth- esis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol— pgy, 1966, 4, 145-152. Hollingshead, A. B. Cultural factors in the selection of marriage mates. American Sociological Review, 1950, lg, 619-627. Hollingshead, A. B. Age relationships and marriage. American Sociological Review, 1951, 44, 492-499. Izard, C. E. Personality similarity and friendship. Journal Of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1960, 91. 47-51 (a). Izard, C. E. Personality similarity, positive affect, and interpersonal attraction. Journal Of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1960, pi, 484-485 (b). Izard, C. E. Personality similarity and friendship: A follow-up study. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 44, 598-600. Jones, E. E. Ingratiation: Aysocial psychological anal- ysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1964. 86 Katz, I., Cohen, M., and Castiglione, L. Effect Of one type of need complementarity on marriage partners' conformity to one another's judgments. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, pg, 8-14. Katz, I., Glucksberg, S., and Krauss, R. Need-satisfaction and Edwards PPS scores in married couples. Journal of Consultipg Psychology, 1960, 44, 203-208. Kerckhoff, A. C., and Davis, K. E. Value consensus and need complementarity in mate selection. American Sociological Review, 1962, 21, 295—303. Landis, P. H., and Day, K. H. Education as a factor in mate selection. American Sociological Review, 1945. $9; 558-560. Law, P. Personality problems in Antarctica. Medical Journal Of Australia, 1960, 41, 273-282. Lott, B. E., and Lott, A. J. The formation of positive attitudes toward group members. Journal of Ab- normal and Social Psychology, 1960, pl, 297-300. Maisonneuve, J. A contribution to the sociometry Of mu- tual choices. Sociometry, 1954, 41, 33-46. McLaughlin, B. Effects Of similarity and likableness on attraction and recall. Journal Of Personality and Social Psychology, 1971, 49, 65-69. Mehlman, B. Similarity in friendships. Journal of Social Psychology, 1962, p1, 195-202. Merton, R. K. Intermarriage and the social structure. Psychiatry, 1941, 4, 371-374. Miller, N., Campbell, D. T., Twedt, H., and O'Connell, E. J. Similarity, contrast, and complementarity in friendship choice. Journal of Personality and Social Psyghology, 1966, ;, 3-12. 87 Murstein, B. I. The complementary need hypothesis in newlyweds and middle-aged couples. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psycholggy, 1961, 4;, 94—197. Nelson, P. D. Structural change in small isolated groups. U.S. Navy Medical NeurOpsychiatric Research Unit Report NO. 64-24, 1964. Newcomb, T. M. The acqpaintanceyprocess. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1961. Pepitone, A. Attraction and hostility. New York: Ather- ton, 1964. Pintner, R., Forlano, G., and Freedman, H. Personality and attitudinal similarity among classroom friends. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1937, 4;, 48-65. Posavac, E. J. Dimensions of trait preferences and per- sonality type. Journal of Personalipy and Social Psychology, 1971, 42, 274-281. Reilly, M. S. A., Commins, W. D., and Stefic, E. C. The complementarity Of personality needs in friendship choice. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1960, pl, 292-294. ' Rosenfeld, H. M. Social choice conceived as a level of aSpiration. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy- chology, 1964, pg, 491-499. Rosenfeld, H. M., and Jackson, J. Effect of similarity of personality on interpersonal attraction. American Psychologist, 1959, i4, 366-367. Rosenfeld, H. M., and Jackson, J. Temporal mediation Of the similarity-attraction hypothesis. Journal of Personality, 1964, 4;, 649-656. Rychlak, J. F. The similarity, compatibility, or incom- patibility Of needs in interpersonal selection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 4, 334-340. ' 88 Secord, P. F., and Backman, C. W. Interpersonal congru- ency, perceived similarity, and friendship. Sociometpy, 1964, 41, 115—127. Schutz, W. C. FIRO-B: A three—dimensional theory of interpersonal behavior. New York: Rinehart and CO., 1958. Thibaut, J. W., and Kelley, H. H. The social psychology Of groups. New York: Wiley, 1959. Thomas, J. L. The factor Of religion in the selection of marriage mates. American Sociological Review, 1951, lg, 487-491. Thorpe, J. G. A study Of some factors in friendship forma- tion. Sociometry, 1955, 48, 207-214. Van Dyke, V. E. Personality traits and friendship forma- tion in adolescent girls. Journal Of Social Psy- chology, 1940, 4;, 291—303. Walster, E., and Walster, G. W. Effect Of eXpecting to be liked on choice of associates. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 91, 402-404. Wilkins, W. L. Group behavior in.long-term isolation. In M. H. Appley and R. Trumbull (Eds.), Psycho— logical Stress. New York: Appleton—Century- Crofts, 1967. Winch, R. F., Ktsanes, J., and Ktsanes, V. The theory of complementary needs in mate selection: An anal- ytic and descriptive study. American Sociological Review, 1954, 49, 241—249. Zimbardo, P., and Formica, R. Emotional comparison and self-esteem as determinants Of affiliation. Journal of Personality, 1963, 4;, 141-162. MICHIGAN STATE UNIV. LIBRARIES I III II II lllllH ll 31293100287121