DOGMATISM, RELIGIOUS BELIEF, AND RELIGIOUS ETHNIZICENTRISM AND THEIR RESPONSE TO PERSUASIVE COMMUNICATION Thesis for the Degree of M. A. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY JOHN M. PHILLIPS 1958‘ ‘ S‘- ; flyig‘nax' 5.01 . ‘.‘.'\’;- t' \p’ Iii" IIHI'H‘R; ' L) 5 5 . 9‘ ,7“ JUL I 6 I994 ABSTRACT DOGMATISM, RELIGIOUS BELIEF, AND RELIGIOUS ETHNOCENTRISM.AND THEIR RESPONSE TO PERSUASIVE COMMUNICATION by John M. Phillips This study involved construction of a Religious Belief Scale (RBS) and a Religious Ethnocentrism Scale (RES) and exploration of the relationships of the scales to each other and to Rokeach's Dogmatism Scale. The effects of a derogatory communication concerning dogma— tism on the three scales were also investigated. The Dogmatism Scale, RBS, and RES were combined into one "Attitude Inventory" and administered to 132 Introductory Psychology students at Michigan State Uni— versity. Four weeks later an Experimental group consist- ing of 51 of the subjects who had taken the Attitude In- ventory listened to a tape recording of the communication and then were readministered the three scales of the study. A Control group of 22 subjects retook the scales prior to hearing the communication. 1 John M. Phillips The results of the study indicated positive rela- tionships between religious belief and dogmatism, between religious ethnocentrism and dogmatism, and between reli— gious belief and religious ethnocentrism, as defined by the scales of the study. The communication, however, had no significant effect on dogmatism, religious belief, or religious ethnocentrism scores. Also high dogmatics did not lower their scores on the three scales appreciably more than low dogmatics in response to the communication. No age or class status differences were found on the three scales, but it was found that females obtained higher RBS scores than males. Religious affiliational differences were found on both the RBS and the RES. Investigation of the data revealed the RBS to have an adequate variance, as well as high test-retest relia- bility and high internal consistency. The Shorter RES had a lower variance and a somewhat lower reliability and lower internal consistency. Possible causes for the failure of hypotheses con- cerning the effects of the communication were discussed, along with implications involving the adequacy, accuracy, John M. Phillips and validity of the religious scales as measuring devices. Possibilities for further research were also mentioned. ./ 0- f I .7 ) / /' /' /rf,. ; Approved: xQCLt¢a¢,mfl/I(Qfl¢£(xrx I 5, v , /”' Date : [{L a: (gym, (2:159, / .54 Q4 J / Thesis Committee: Dr. Norman Abeles, Chairman Dr. Terrence Allen Dr. Milton Rokeach DOGMATISM, RELIGIOUS BELIEF, AND RELIGIOUS ETHNOCENTRISM AND THEIR RESPONSE TO PERSUASIVE COMMUNICATION BY John M. Phillips A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1968 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I am deeply grateful to all who assisted in this study. I especially want to thank the chairman of my. committee, Dr. Norman Abeles, for his patience, guidance, and encouragement during the more trying moments of the project. I also want to express my appreciation to Dr. Terrence Allen for his cogent criticism concerning crucial aspects of data collection and analysis, and to Dr. Milton Rokeach, whose previous work on dogmatism helped make the present research possible. My thanks goes also to Mr. Michael Figler for his cooperation in the collection of data for this study. The insights of two of my colleagues, Bernard Silverman and Peter Hampton, were also appreciated. ii TABLE OF ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS . . . . . . LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . LIST OF APPENDICES . . . . . INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . Dogmatism. . . . . . . . Religious Belief . . . . Religious Ethnocentrism. Relationships Among Dogmatism, Religious CONTENTS Belief, and Religious Ethnocentrism. Religious Attitudes, Dogmatism, and Attitude Change . . . . . . . . Hypotheses to be Tested. METHOD . . . . . . . . . . . Subjects . . . . . . . . The Instruments. . . . . The Communication. . . . Procedure. . . . . . . . iii Page ii vii 12 l7 l9 19 21 29 3O Table of Contents (Cont.) Page RESULTS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 Intercorrelations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 Experimental Effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37 Scale Reliabilities. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 Demographic Variables. . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48 Intercorrelations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48 Experimental Effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 The Scales . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 Future Research. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70 SUMMARY. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73 REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75 APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 iv Table LIST OF TABLES Demographic Information on Total, Experi- mental, and Control Groups of Subjects . Results by Religious Affiliation of Preliminary Pilot Study on Religious Belief Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Results of Pilot Study Involving Dogmatism, Religious Belief, and Religious Ethnocen— trism Scales (N = 80). . . . . . . . . . Pearson Product-Moment Correlation Coeffi- cients among D—Scale, RBS, and RES Scores on the First Administration for 132 Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Comparison of Intercorrelations among First Administration Scores of the D-Scale, RBS and RES (N = 132). . . . . . . . . . . . Pearson Product-Moment Correlation Coeffi— cients among First and Second Administra- tion D—Scale, RBS, and RES Scores for Control Subjects (N = 22). . . . . . . . Pearson Product-Moment Correlation Coeffi- cients among First and Second Administra— tion D-Scale, RBS, and RES Scores for Experimental Subjects (N = 51) . . . . . Comparison of Differences Between First and Second Administration D-Scale, RBS, and RES Scores for Experimental and Control Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Page 20 23 25 33 34 35 36 37 List of Tables (Cont.) Table 9. Comparison of Correlation Coefficients Between First Administration D—Scale Scores and the Amount of Lowering of D—Scale, RBS, and RES Scores Between the First and Second Administration for Experimental and Control Subjects. . . 10. Results of the First Administration of the Dogmatism, Religious Belief, and Reli- gious Ethnocentrism Scales (N = 132) . . . 11. Comparison of Test-Retest Reliability Coef- ficients for D-Scale, RBS, and RES Scores Between Experimental and Control Groups. . 12. Test-Retest Reliability Coefficients for D—Scale, RBS, and RES Scores for Experi— mental and Control Groups Combined (N = 73) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13. Coefficient Alpha Correlations for First Administration D-Scale, RBS, and RES Scores (N = 132) . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14. Pearson Product-Moment Correlations Between Class Status and Age and First Administra- tion D—Scale, RBS, and RES Scores (N = 132) 15. Sex Differences on First Administration D-Scale, RBS, and RBS Scores . . . . . . . 16. Religious Affiliational Differences on First Administration D—Scale, RBS, and RES Scores . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17. One—Way Analyses of Variance Across Reli- gious Affiliations for Results of the First Administration of the D—Scale, the RBS, and the RES . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi Page 38 4O 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 LIST OF APPENDICES Appendix Page A. DOGMATISM SCALE (FORM E) . . . . . . . . . 80 B. RELIGIOUS BELIEF SCALE . . . . . . . . . . 84 C. RELIGIOUS ETHNOCENTRISM SCALE. . . . . . . 87 D. ATTITUDE INVENTORY . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 E. ATTITUDE SCALE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97 vii DOGMATISM, RELIGIOUS BELIEF, AND RELIGIOUS ETHNOCENTRISM AND THEIR RESPONSE TO PERSUASIVE COMMUNICATION By John M. Phillips INTRODUCTION The present study represents an investigation of the relationships among dogmatism, religious belief, and religious ethnocentrism, as well as the possibility of alterations in these constructs subsequent to the presen— tation of feedback information regarding the nature of and personality correlates of dogmatism. Dogmatism In his book, The Open and Closed Mind (1960), Rokeach, employing a general description of the indi- vidual's belief-disbelief continuum, delineates formal definitions of Open- and closed-mindedness and presents an analysis distinguishing the two. He actually presents l two definitions of dogmatism--one based on the defining characteristics of open-closed systems and a second, simpler definition based on a more intuitive approach to the concept. In the first definition he states that a closed system may be distinguished along three dimensions: (a) With respect to organization along the belief—disbelief continuum a closed system may be characterized by a high rejection of disbelief systems, isolation within and be— tween belief and disbelief systems, a greater degree of differentiation within the belief system than within the disbelief systems, and little differentiation within the disbelief system. (b) In regard to organization along the central—peripheral belief dimension, dogmatism is characterized by a feeling that the world is a threaten- ing one to live in, a belief that "authority is absolute and that people are to be accepted and rejected according to their agreement or disagreement with such authority" (p. 56), and the fact that beliefs and disbeliefs per- ceived to emanate from authority are in relative isolation with each other. And finally (c) with respect to organ- ization along the time-perspective dimension, the closed system involves a "relatively narrow, future—oriented time perspective" (p. 56). As a more basic definition, Rokeach offers the following: The degree to which an individual's system is open or closed may be gauged by the extent to which the person can receive, evaluate, and act on information received from the outside on its own intrinsic merits, unencumbered by irrelevant factors in the situation arising from within the person or from the outside and, in particular, those irrelevant external pressures of "reward and punishment arising from external authority" (p. 57). Thus "the more closed the belief system, the more difficult it should be to distinguish between infor— mation received about the world and information received about the source" (p. 58). In an effort to obtain an objective measure of this Open-closed mindedness variable, Rokeach has devised the Dogmatism Scale (D-Scale), em- ployed in this study. Religious Belief One important belief-disbelief subsystem is that of religion. Investigators have made numerous attempts at measuring the totality or selected aspects of the religious values and attitudes which the individual enter- tains. One such significant attempt, purporting to mea- sure what prominence among several which an individual affords religious values, is the Allport-Vernon-Lindzey Study of Values (3rd ed., 1960). Researchers have also attempted to devise measures of various aspects of the individual's total religious commitments. One of these is the "intrinsic/extrinsic" dimension (Allport, 1959), measured by a 21-item scale. Two basic limitations of this instrument, however, are that it assumes the subject has a moderately religious attitude and that its validity may be compromised by a tendency to assume a globally positive response set toward religion (Allport and Ross, 1967). Nevertheless, the scale does provide a differen— tiation on one continuum among more religious individuals. Hogge and Friedman (1967) have devised a Scriptural Liter- alism Scale, which measures the extent to which the indi- vidual accepts Biblical Scripture as literally true and which was found to differentiate between Unitarians, Methodists, and Southern Baptists, between Freshmen and Seniors, and between males and females. The scale is limited, though, in that it taps only one basic area of religious experience. Some attempts have been made to measure fundamentalism (Dynes, 1955; Stanley, 1963, 1964; Feagin, 1964), but the scales devised have also suffered from a bias toward more religious individuals. Accordingly, the present study incorporates a Religious Belief Scale (RBS) which attempts to measure the extent to which an individual accepts as true or false the basic beliefs of traditional Christianity and which is applicable to persons having few religious convictions as well as those of a more religious bent. Religious Ethnocentrism Some research has been done in connection with religious ethnocentrism (Adorno, gt 31., 1950: Rokeach and Jensen, 1956; Davis, 1958; and Maryo, l958)——the convic- tion that one's own religious group or belief system is superior to any other and that certain religious groups are inferior or should be avoided strictly on grounds of differences in religious ideology. However, the general question of measurement has heretofore been approached obliquely, as in the measurement of prejudice against specific outgroups, in the measurement of the frequency of certain prejudicial or non—prejudicial behaviors such as interfaith marriages, or in the measurement of the de- gree of rejection of specific religious disbelief systems. Prior to the present research the problem of de— termining the extent of the individual's overall religious ethnocentrism had not been approached, although Allport (1966) delineates two existent theological contexts for prejudice which are particularly applicable to religious ethnocentrism: (a) the doctrine of revelation, leading a religion to "claim exclusive possession of final truth concerning the destiny of man, as well as sole authority and means for interpreting that end" (p. 449); and (b) the doctrine of election, which divides the ins from the outs with surgical precision . . .-—a divinely sanctioned ethnocentrism" (p. 450). Considering the paucity of cogent research con- ducted in this area, a Religious Ethnocentrism Scale (RES) was devised for the study. The instrument consists of 12 statements concerning the superiority of one's own reli- gious beliefs and/or group and the need for isolation or at least separation from "inferior" religious groups on such matters as marriage, friendship, and ideology. Relationships Among Dogmatism, Religious Belief, and Religious Ethnocentrism The relationship between religious belief and dogmatism has been approached by Rokeach in The Open and Closed Mind (1960). He discovered that, in general, Catholics were more dogmatic than Protestants, who, in turn, were more dogmatic than nonbelievers. Feather (1967) found that there was increased dogmatism among members of religious societies which require subscription to "a set of basic beliefs about which there can be little argument . . . and which depend upon the authority of the Church and/or the Scriptures" (p. 10). Stanley (1963) also found a sizable correlation (+ .58) between dogma— tism and fundamentalism--that Protestantism emphasizing as fundamental the literal inerrancy of the Scriptures, the second coming of Jesus Christ, the virgin birth, physical resurrection, and substitutionary atonement—- among a group of theological students. His measure of fundamentalism consisted of a single question concerning the inspiration and veracity of the Bible. Stanley con- cluded that fundamentalism might be considered thg_dog- matic factor in religious belief. While these researchers have tackled the problem of the relationship between dogmatism and religious belief through analysis of the content of the beliefs, they have not approached the question of whether and to what extent a general relationship exists between the two constructs. Yet, while it is undoubtedly true that many individuals hold deep religious convictions while remaining relatively open-minded, it seems probable that, in general, the greater the number of traditional religious beliefs a person agrees to, the more likely he is to accept, to one degree or another, his church and/or the Bible as being authoritative. Following this reasoning one step further, the greater is one's acceptance of an authoritarian atti- tude toward religious belief, the greater should be the extent to which his total belief-disbelief system is in- fluenced or governed by religious doctrine, and thus the higher his level of dogmatism. Hence it appears that, in general, the more religious beliefs an individual accepts, as well as the greater the degree of his acceptance of them, the higher should be his level of dogmatism. The present study is concerned with determining the extent of such a relationship, if indeed it exists. There is theoretical argument as well as some experimental evidence supporting a connection between strength of religious ethnocentrism and degree of dogma- tism. Rokeach (1960) has hypothesized that certain re- ligious groups, due to their organizational structure, tend to coerce each member to commit himself in advance to the group's ide- ology and to reject in advance alternative ideologies. Such social pressures should, over time, leave their mark on the adherent's belief system—-that is, his belief system should increasingly betray the defining characteristics of the closed mind (p. 118). Feather (1967) suggests that while high dogmatic indi- viduals are more apt to join religious societies which incorporate a firm doctrinal basis to which they can sub— scribe, commitments to the ideologies of these societies and rejection of alternative ideol- ogies may then tend to reinforce dogmatism (p. 10). He concludes that perhaps subscription to such belief systems and the concomitant dogmatism aid and abet one another. Concerning the instruments employed in the present study, implicit in the content of each of the items of the RES, although more or less apparent and stated positively in some, negatively in others, is the concept that one 10 certain religious faith or one church (namely, one's own) has more truth than any other. And to the extent that one is certain of the truth of a particular belief sub- system, he may be said to have adopted an authoritarian point of view--precise1y what the D-Scale is designed to measure. Thus the individual's degree of religious eth- nocentrism should correlate positively with his level of dogmatism. A number of investigators have found indications of such a relationship. Rokeach (1960), employing subjects from six religious denominations, including Catholic, re— ported that in 46 out of 48 comparisons the high dogmatics of each sect rejected other disbelief subsystems to a greater extent than did the low dogmatics of that sect. Maryo (1958), using subjects from the same six denomina- tions, found that high dogmatics in five of the six groups consistently rejected other denominations to a greater extent than did their low dogmatic brethren. David (1958) and Rokeach and Jensen (1956), employing subjects from two denominational groups (Catholics and Methodists), reported similar findings; the latter researchers also concluded that an increase in an individual's dogmatism had the effect of increasing his faith in his own position. 11 In view of the foregoing, one of the objectives of the present research is to substantiate, formalize, and generalize the relationship between dogmatism and religious ethnocentrism. According to Festinger (1957), it would be cog- nitively dissonant for an individual to subscribe firmly to a set of religious beliefs and yet not feel that his beliefs are superior to any others. In order to reduce this dissonance he must either give up his beliefs or adopt the position that they are indeed superior or even represent "the Truth." To carry this reasoning one step further, an individual initially subscribing to a certain religious group's beliefs will either tend to support that group as being superior or will tend to reject the group's beliefs. In either instance the result is a positive relationship between religious belief and religious ethno— centrism. This relationship is lessened somewhat, of course, by the existence of religious groups who subscribe to few traditional Christian beliefs, although these groups form a minority of religious sects in the United States. The relationship between religious belief and re- ligious ethnocentrism is further complicated by a response set noted by Allport and Ross (1967), who observed that 12 some individuals responding to religious questionnaires formed a positive response set, answering ubiquitously affirmatively toward religion, almost without regard to the specific content of the items. This phenomenon also would tend to increase the measured correlation between the two constructs. An objective of this study was to determine the existence and extent of a positive correlation between religious belief as measured by the RBS and religious ethnocentrism as measured by the RES. Religious Attitudes, Dogmatism, and Attitude Change Attitude has been defined as the manner of think- ing, feeling, and reacting to people, groups, social issues, or, in general, to any event in one's environment which one learns and develops in the process of coping with and adjusting to his social milieu (Lambert and Lam— bert, 1964). Attitude organization can become inflexible and stereotyped, especially for those persons who have been encouraged over long periods of time to react in standard or "accept— able" ways to particular events or groups. If a person's attitudes become firmly set, he is then too ready to categorize people or events into one of his emotionally toned patterns of thought (Lambert and Lambert, 1964, p. 51). 13 Perhaps these latter statements could be rephrased as follows: Attitude organization (belief system) can be— come rigid and closed for those individuals who have been encouraged (by some authority) over long periods of time to react in standard or "acceptable" ways to particular groups, events, and beliefs. While a vast and continuously eXpanding amount of work has been conducted in the area of attitude change, lasting (or even transitory) changes in some attitudes remain exotic in occurrence; and while numerous experi— mental changes in "attitudes" have been reported, Rokeach (1966) has argued that many reported changes in attitude actually represent changes only in expressed opinion. One method he proposes for assessing attitude change more adequately is to test for other behavioral changes accompanying a given opinion change. If a single expressed Opinion change truly represents a change in underlying attitude, it is reasonable to expect that such a change will be accompanied by other changes-~cognitive, affective, or behavioral-- which theoretically should be related to the change in attitude (p. 547). In line with the above discussion, one approach to the measurement of change in attitude is the following: By sensitizing the individual's awareness of and altering 14 his attitude toward dogmatism, a lowering may be induced in his level of dogmatism; and with this reduction, other attitudes and beliefs would theoretically become more accessible to change. And if, further, the attitudes in question were intrinsically associated with the individual's conception of and attitude toward dogmatism, then a lower- ing in dogmatism might lead automatically to a correspond— ing lowering in those attitudes (Cf. McGuire, 1960). More specifically, if the strength and extent of a person's religious belief and/or ethnocentrism is indeed positively correlated with his level of dogmatism, then a change in dogmatism might consequently result in an alteration of religious attitudes. If through persuasive communication a certain atti— tude or belief is presented as socially and personally undesirable and if subsequently those individuals receiving the communication are placed in a situation where normally they would respond in accordance with that undesirable attitude, then, according to Festinger (1957), those indi— viduals are in a position of cognitive dissonance. They may resolve this dilemma by 1. . . . changing one or more of the elements involved in the dissonant relations. 15 2. . . . adding new cognitive elements that are consonant with already existing cognition. 3. . . . decreasing the importance of the ele— ment involved in the dissonant relations (p. 264). In brief, when some of the cognitive elements involved in a dissonance are cognitions about one's own behavior, the dissonance can be reduced by changing the behavior, thus directly changing the cognitive elements (pp. 264, 265), as well as possibly closely related ones. In particular, a persuasive communication explaining the meaning of dog— matism, as well as portraying the concept as being soci- ally and personally undesirable, might result not only in a lowering of dogmatism but also in an altering of reli- gious attitudes. Wolfer (1967) did in fact hypothesize that lower dogmatism scores would be obtained by subjects who were informed as to what the D-Scale was designed to measure. However, in his study he found that subjects did not obtain appreciably lower scores when informed as to the purpose of the scale. Nevertheless, it may have been that the informative instructions were insufficient to arouse a socially desirable response set (as Wolfer hypothesizes), that, rather, the students retained a response set of 16 compliance to what they felt the experimenter wished of them (possibly a high reliability score with the same scale administered 10 weeks previously). In the same study, Wolfer showed that students could easily fake the scale in either an open— or closed—minded direction if explicitly instructed to do so. This study includes an attempt to induce a lower— ing of dogmatism in a group of subjects by means of a per— suasive communication on the nature of dogmatism, empha- sizing the more socially unacceptable aspects of the con— cept (Rokeach, 1960; Zagona and Zurcher, 1965: Vacchiano, §E_§l., 1968). Further, the subjects are not to be advised as to the nature and purpose of the instruments they are to be subsequently re-administered. It is thus hOped that their perception of the relationship between the communi- cation and the following inventories will be an indirect one, and hence, a decline not only in dogmatism but also in strength of religious belief and ethnocentrism might be obtained. In accordance with Rokeach's (1960) second defini- tion of dogmatism, a closed-minded individual will be more influenced by the irrelevant aspects of a situation, for example, his own ego involvement and dissonance reduction. 17 This has been substantiated in part, at least, by Fillen- baum (1964), who found a significant correlation between dogmatism and amount of dissonance reduction. Hehce more dogmatic individuals, when placed in a dissonant situation, wherein there is threat to self-esteem, are apt to respond more to the threat and to their ego involvement than to the reality of the situation. Thus following a disparag- ing communication concerning dogmatism, closed—minded subjects would be more likely to seek dissonance reduction by the various means outlined by Festinger (1957), includ- ing, in this instance, lowering their D—Scale scores, as well as lowering their RBS, and RES scores. Hypotheses to be Tested In light of the preceding discussion, five major predictions were investigated: Hypothesis 1: There is a significant positive correlation between degree of dogmatism as measured by the D-Scale and strength of religious belief as measured by the RBS. Hypothesis 2: There is a significant positive correlation between degree of dogmatism as measured by the D—Scale and amount of religious ethno- centrism as measured by the RES. 18 Hypothesis 3: There is a significant positive correlation between strength of religious belief as measured by the RBS and amount of religious ethnocentrism as measured by the RES. Hypothesis 4: Following the derogatory communication concerning dogmatism, there will be a sig- nificant lowering in levels of dogmatism, religious belief, and religious ethnocen- trism as measured by the three scales in- volved in the study. Hypothesis 5: Following the communication there will be a significant positive correlation between degree of dogmatism as measured by the first administration of the D-Scale and amount of lowering of dogmatism, religious belief, and religious ethnocentrism as measured by the differences in scores between the first and second administration of the three scales involved in the study. In addition, test—retest and internal consistency measurements of scale reliabilities were obtained. Also, sex, age, and religious affiliational differences in de— gree of dogmatism, religious belief, and religious ethno- centrism were explored. MET HOD Subjects The subjects first administered the experimental scales were 57 male and 75 female undergraduate students enrolled in Introductory Psychology classes at Michigan State University during the summer of 1968. The age range for the group was from 17 to 29 years, with a mean of 18.9 years. Ninety—seven were Freshmen, 17 were Sophomores, 9 were Juniors, and 9 were Seniors. The subjects reported their religious affiliations as follows: 64 Protestant, 40 Catholic, 11 Jewish, 6 Other, and 11 None. Selection of subjects for the experiment was not made on a volunteer basis; rather all students present at the seven recitation sessions of the psychology class were requested to participate in an experiment. None declined. Following the first administration the subjects were told that another segment of the experiment would be adminis- tered "some time in the future." The subjects were not advised as to the intent of the experiment until all data had been collected. 19 20 Four weeks following the first administration, 73 of the original subjects retook the three scales. At that time the students were separated into Experimental and Con- trol groups. The EXperimental group, consisting of 51 stu- dents, listened to a communication concerning dogmatism prior to retaking the three scales, whereas the Control group responded to the inventories the second time before listening to the communication. Table 1 summarizes by groups the demographic information concerning the subjects. TABLE 1 Demographic Information on Total, Experimental, and Control Groups of Subjects Factor Subject Total Experimental Control (N=132) (N:51) (N222) Sex Male 57 28 10 Female 75 23 12 A e Range 17-29 17—29 17—26 9 Mean 18.9 19.1 18.1 Frosh. 97 41 19 Soph. l7 7 1 Class Status Junior 9 2 1 Senior 9 l 1 Prot. 64 29 10 Reli ious Cath. 40 14 9 Afgiliat ion JeWiSh ll 3 2 Other 6 l 1 None 11 4 0 21 In general, the sample can be considered represen- tative of the principally lower biennium students enrolled in Introductory Psychology at Michigan State University. The Instruments As mentioned previously, three scales were employed in the study——the Dogmatism Scale, a Religious Belief Scale, and a Religious Ethnocentrism Scale. Dggmatism Scale.——Form E of the D—Scale* is a 40- item inventory of statements asserting beliefs concerning personal and social issues. The statements are to be rated by the subject according to a six point Likert scale, rang— ing from -3 (strongly disagree) to +3 (strongly agree). All items of the scale are positively (non-democratically) worded, and thus the higher an individual's total score is, the greater his degree of dogmatism is said to be. In scoring, four points are added to each item response, in— suring a positive total score for each individual. Theo- retically, scores may range from 40 to 280. Reported test— retest and split-half reliabilities of the scale have ranged from .68 to .93 (Rokeach, 1960; Ehrlich, 1961; *See Appendix A. 22 Zagona and Zurcher, 1965). Rokeach (1960) failed to find any significant correlation between dogmatism and intel- ligence, although Ehrlich (1961) reported a significant negative correlation (-.28) between dogmatism and scores on the Ohio State Psychological Examination, and Zagona and Zurcher (1965) found a correlation of -.18 between the D-Scale and the Verbal subtest of the College Quali— fication Test. Alter and White (1966), using a very large student sample, found statistically significant but very small differences between male and female mean scores. Research has shown that the D-Scale appears to reliably gauge the extent to which an individual orders his belief- disbelief continuum according to the dictates of an au— thority, i.e. the extent to which his belief-disbelief system may be said to be closed. Religious Belief Scale.--Initia11y a means was sought for determining the degree of an individual's per- sonal investment in religious beliefs; and the inventory presented by Allport, Gillespe, and Young (1948) seemed to provide a means of tapping some of the variables in religious thinking which were to be investigated in the present study. However, the inventory was somewhat dated and was arranged in a cumbersome format, not readily 23 translatable into a quantitative measure of overall reli- gious belief. Accordingly, the inventory was reworked: Items were discarded, others added; some questions were expanded into two or three items; and all items were couched in a language which all persons, regardless of their orien- tation, could answer. Finally, the format of the scale was altered so that the subject responded to the items by use of a six point Likert scale. In a preliminary pilot study, the 30-item scale was administered to 19 Introductory Psychology students and 5 graduate students. Analysis revealed a sample range of 62 to 195 (possible range was 30 to 210), a mean of 130, and a standard deviation of 30.8. An analysis of the data according to religious affiliation is shown in Table 2. TABLE 2 Results by Religious Affiliation of Preliminary Pilot Study on Religious Belief Scale Religion N Mean S.D. Protestant 13 130 Catholic 5 158 Jewish 3 118 Agnostic 3 93 TOTALS 24 130 30.8 24 Subsequently, the scale was analyzed and revised: a few items were added; those items which did not correlate highly with the scale as a whole were either revised or discarded, and other items which were judged to be measures of religious ethnocentricity rather than simply religious belief were included as part of a separate Religious Ethno— centrism Scale, to be discussed below. The RBS was thus reduced to a 25-item instrument and administered as part of a pilot study to a group of 40 male and 40 female Seventh—day Adventist students en— rolled in Introductory Psychology classes at Andrews Uni- versity, a religiously affiliated college with an under— graduate enrollment of approximately 1500. The age range for the group was from 17 to 24 years, with a mean of 19.0 years. Of the 80 students, 59 were Freshmen, 18 were Soph— omores, 3 were Juniors, and none were Seniors. Results of the study, summarized in Table 3, show that scores on the RBS ranged from 91 to 173, with a mean of 152.5. Inasmuch as the subjects tested were all from a conservative reli— gious background, the skewed distribution found was ex- pected and could be interpreted as providing some further validation for the scale. 25 TABLE 3 Results of Pilot Study Involving Dogmatism, Religious Belief, and Religious Ethnocentrism Scales (N = 80) Possible Obtained Scales Mean S.D. Range Range Dogmatism 40—280 129—217 170.2 18.15 Religious Belief 25—175 91-173 152.5 14.43 Religious Ethnocentrism 8—56 20-55 43.7 8.70 Following the pilot study, additional minor revi— sions of the scale were made. In its final form, the RBS* consists of 24 statements covering such issues as the ex- istence of God, the veracity of the scriptures, the divinity of Christ, and the relevance and importance of religious sentiments, rites, and organizations. The scale has been balanced with 12 positively and 12 negatively worded state- ments in an effort to eliminate any corresponding biasing factors arising from response style, although Rorer (1965) and Rokeach (1967) contend that there is no real evidence for a major effect due to response style. It was also felt *See Appendix B. 26 that reversal of 12 of the items did not result in any major alteration in the content of those items (Cf. Rok- each, 1963; Rorer, 1965). Finally, it was anticipated that the items were not so specific as to favor or dis— criminate against any particular sect, nor so general as to preclude understanding or identification by the sub- ject. As with the D—scale, each item on the RBS was rated by the subject according to a Likert scale, ranging from —3 (disagree very much) to +3 (agree very much). In scoring, the signs (+ or -) of the values assigned to reversed items by the subject were changed; thus, the final score for any item was always in terms of increasing values for more "religious" responses. Following these conversions, four points were added to each of the items, insuring that all total scores would be positive. Religious Ethnocentrism Scale.—-As mentioned pre— viously, certain items in the version of the RBS employed in the preliminary pilot study were felt to be measures of religious ethnocentrism rather than religious belief. Subsequently, these items formed the nucleus of an 8—item RES used in the Andrews University pilot study, the re- sults of which are summarized in Table 3. The findings 27 show that, as with the RBS, the distribution was skewed toward the upper end of the range continuum. This also was expected, as the Seventh—day Adventist Church is a proselytization-oriented sect which instills a firm con— viction in its membership that the church is in possession of a major portion of the "Truth" (ggestions on Doctrine, 1957). Analysis of the eight items resulted in revision and expansion of the scale. The final version of the RES* consists of 7 positively and 5 negatively worded statements concerning the superiority of one's own religious beliefs and group and the need for separation from other religious groups on such matters as marriage, friendship, and ide- ology. Thus as with the RBS, the RES is relatively bal- anced with positively and negatively worded statements purportedly based on the same construct of religious eth- nocentricity. The individual items present a relatively large number of situations involving religious ethnocen- trism, as well as a variety of intensities of the construct. Also the prejudicial content of the statements is not directed in favor of or against any particular religious *See Appendix C. 28 group; yet the statements are not so vague as to eliminate all notion of discrimination. It was thereby anticipated that the scale would prove applicable to all individuals, regardless of their orientation toward religion. Each item on the RES, as with the D-Scale and the RBS, was rated by the subject according to a Likert scale ranging from +3 (strongly agree) to -3 (strongly disagree). A similar scoring method to that employed with the RBS was utilized with the RES; i.e., the signs of the responses to negatively worded items were reversed, and then four points were added to each of the responses on the scale to insure all total scores would be positive. For purposes of the study and because they are similar in design the three instruments, D—Scale, RBS, and RES, were combined into one large, 76-item inventory, the items from each scale being randomly mixed in the combined questionnaire. Two forms of the combined inventory were constructed*, the "Attitude Inventory," employed in the first administration, and the "Attitude Scale," adminis— tered four weeks later. The only differences were that demographic information was requested only on the *See Appendices D and E. 29 Attitude Inventory, and in the Attitude Scale the items were presented in reverse order to that of the Attitude Inventory. The Communication Prior to the second administration of the three scales, the EXperimental group was presented a communi- cation which concerned the meaning of dogmatism as well as some of its socially undesirable correlates. The communication was adapted from statements and findings in The Open and Closed Mind, from the findings of Vacchiano, Strauss, and Schiffman (1968), and from a communication formulated by Wolfer for use in his 1967 study (Personal communication from the author, 1968). The communication was presented as follows: Dr. Rokeach, a professor in Social Psychology here at Michigan State University has devised some good definitions of dogmatism. He defines it in terms of belief systems. A person with an open belief system is open-minded and non— dogmatic. He holds many of his beliefs tenta- tively and often has doubts and questions about them. Such a person is receptive to and tolerant of opinions and ideas that differ from his own. He evaluates ideas on the basis of their own merits rather than on the basis of authority. A person with a closed belief system, on the other hand, is considered closed-minded or dog— matic. He seldom has doubts and questions about 30 his beliefs. Such an individual is unreceptive to and intolerant of Opinions and ideas that differ from his own. He evaluates ideas on the basis of their own merits. Thus he tends to accept or reject ideas solely on the basis of authority. Rokeach as well as others have found that communists, fascists, and racial bigots are highly dogmatic on the whole. And recently others have found that highly dogmatic people in general also lack self-esteem, are doubtful about their own self—worth, are anxious, lack confidence in themselves, are frustrated by changeable conditions, are submissive and con— forming, and are restrained, diffident, timid, tense, impatient, and conservative. They con— clude that personality maladjustment and insta— bility appear to underlie dogmatism, thus sub- stantiating Rokeach's earlier hypothesis that a dogmatic attitude is sometimes a defense mechan— ism employed as sort of a shield by a basically maladjusted or neurotic individual. Procedure 1. The D-Scale, the RBS, and the RES were randomly mixed in a combined inventory designated the Atti— tude Inventory. The order of the items on this inventory was reversed to produce the Attitude Scale. 2. The Attitude Inventory was administered to 132 stu- dents enrolled in Introductory Psychology at Mich- igan State University during the summer of 1968. 31 Of the seven recitation sections of Introductory Psychology, four were designated Experimental and three Control. Four weeks after the administration of the Attitude Inventory the Control sections were administered the Attitude Scale. Twenty—two of the subjects who took the first inventory were present in the Control sections on the date the second inventory was given. At the same time, the instructor of the Experimental sections recorded on tape the communication con~ cerning dogmatism and used the tape as an integral part of a lecture on the psychology of conformity. (This instructor frequently taped sections of his lectures, and hence, this procedure did not appear artificial to the students.) The instructor told them they would be required to write and hand in a statement on the content of the recording immed- iately following their listening to it. These procedures thus insured uniformity in the presenta- tion of the communication, attentiveness to its content by the students, yet also their naivete concerning its purpose. 32 Immediately after the presentation of the communi- cation to the Experimental subjects (and after they had handed in their statements concerning its con- tent) they were administered the Attitude Scale. Fifty-one of the subjects who took the first inven— tory were present in the Experimental sections on the date the second inventory was given. RESULTS Intercorrelations Table 4, showing the Pearson product—moment corre— lation coefficients among D—Scale, RBS, and RES scores on the first administration among the total group of 132 sub— jects, reveals that there is a small, positive correlation between D-Scale scores and RBS scores and that the RES correlates moderately and in a positive direction with both the D—Scale and the RBS. All three correlations were significant at the .05 level or beyond. Thus Hypotheses 1, 2, and 3 were confirmed. TABLE 4 Pearson Product-Moment Correlation Coefficients among D—Scale, RBS, and RES Scores on the First Administration for 132 Subjects Scale RBS RES D-Scale .17a .31C RBS .37C ap < .05, one-tailed test. Cp < .001, one-tailed test. 33 34 Tests of significance for r differences of depen- dent samples (Peatman, 1963) were computed for the corre- lations among the three scales of the study. Results of those tests, summarized in Table 5, reveal that the corre- lation between the D—Scale scores and the RES scores were significantly higher than the correlation between the D- Scale and the RBS. Further, the correlation between the RES and the RBS was significantly higher than the correla— tion between the D—Scale and the RBS. TABLE 5 Comparison of Intercorrelations among First Administration Scores of the D-Scale, RBS, and RES (N = 132)* Correlation r Correlation r Correlation r D—Scale D—Scale D—Scale vs. RBS .17 vs. RBS .17 vs. RES .31 D-Scale RBS RBS vs. RES .31 vs. RES .37 vs. RES .37 t 3.23b t 4.70c t 1.44ns *two-tailed directional test of significance for r differ— ences of dependent samples. nsp > .05 bp < .01 Cp < .001 35 Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients were also computed for correlations among the three scales for Control and Experimental groups separately. The inter— correlations of the three scales for Control subjects on the first and second administrations are presented in Table 6. The table reveals that all correlations were positive. TABLE 6 Pearson Product-Moment Correlation Coefficients among First and Second Administration D-Scale, RBS, and RES Scores for Control Subjects (N = 22) Scale RBS RES First Administration D—Scale .19 .67c RBS .25 . . . a b Second Administration D-Scale .37 .54 RBS .34 ap < .05, one—tailed test. bp < .01, one—tailed test. Cp < .001, one-tailed test. The first and second administration correlations between D-Scale and RBS scores and the second administration cor— relation between D—Scale and RBS scores were significant 36 at the .05 level or beyond. The intercorrelation of the three scales for Experimental subjects on the first and second administrations are presented in Table 7 showing all correlations to be in a positive direction, and all were significantly so, except the second administration correlation between D-Scale and RBS scores. TABLE 7 Pearson Product-Moment Correlation Coefficients among First and Second Administration D—Scale, RBS, and RES Scores for Experimental Subjects (N = 51) Scale RBS RES First Administration D-Scale .42C .32 RBS .56 Second Administration D—Scale .13 .37 RBS .71 ap < .05, one-tailed test. bp < .01, one-tailed test. Cp < .001, one-tailed test. 37 Experimental Effects The mean amount of change between the first and second administration scores on the D-Scale, RBS, and RES were determined for both Experimental and Control Subjects. Table 8 reveals that differences in amount of change be— tween the two groups were not significant at the .05 level for any of the scales. Thus Hypothesis 4 was not confirmed. TABLE 8 Comparison of Differences Between First and Second Administration D—Scale, RBS, and RES Scores for Experimental and Control Subjects Experi- Control t mental N 51 22 First Administration 149.57 150.00 D—Scale Second Administration 146.71 149.36 ns Mean Change —2.86 -0.64 0.54 SD of change 16.12 15.54 First Administration 114.53 121.45 RBS Second Administration 108.25 113.59 ns Mean Change —6.28 -7.86 —0.52 SD of change 12.98 8.63 First Administration 41.53 41.27 RES Second Administration 41.59 42.18 ns Mean Change 0.06 0.91 0.50 SD of change 6.00 7.55 n Sp > .10, one-tailed test. 38 Correlations between the first administration D- Scale scores and the amount of lowering of D—Scale, RBS, and RES scores between the first and second administrations were derived for both Experimental and Control subjects. Table 9 reveals that differences in the correlation coef- ficients between the two groups were not significant at the .05 level; Hypothesis 5 was not confirmed. TABLE 9 Comparison of Correlation Coefficients Between First Administration D—Scale Scores and the Amount of Lowering of D-Scale, RBS, and RES Scores Between the First and Second Administration for Experimental and Control Subjects Correlation Coefficients Grou N D-Scale D-Scale D—Scale p vs. Change vs. Change vs. Change in D-Scale in RBS in RES Experimental 51 .45 .17 .00 Control 22 .39 -.10 .23 z 0.25ns 1.02nS -o.85nS nsTest of Significance for r Differences of Independent Samples, p > .10. 39 Scale Reliabilities Because the study includes two entirely new scales, in addition to the D—Scale, it was felt that measures of the scales' reliabilities should be obtained. Accordingly, test-retest reliabilities and internal consistency scores for each of the scales were obtained. Table 10 compares the first administration means, standard deviations, and ranges of the Dogmatism, Religious Belief, and Religious Ethnocentrism Scales on a total of 132 subjects. The mean on the D-Scale was 150.95 and the range was 92 to 212; whereas the possible range on the scale is 40 to 280, with a theoretical mean of 160. Like- wise, the mean on the RBS was 113.20, with scores ranging from 42 to 158, in comparison to a possible range of 24 to 168 and a theoretical mean of 96. Finally, the mean and range on the RES were 40.36 and 19 to 61; whereas the pos- sible range is 12 to 84 and the theoretical mean is 48. In general, then, scores on the D-Scale and the RES were lower than might be predicted from inspection of the scales' construction, while RBS scores were above the predicted level. The standard deviation for the RBS was higher than that of the D—Scale, even though the latter possessed a 40 TABLE 10 Results of the First Administration of the Dogmatism, Religious Belief, and Religious Ethnocentrism Scales (N = 132) Scale M SD Range Dogmatism 150.95 22.04 92 - 212 Religious Belief 113.20 25.07 42 - 158 Religious Ethnocentrism 40.36 9.41 19 - 61 greater possible range. The standard deviation of the RBS was lower than that of the other two scales, as was to be expected in light of the scale's length. Three sets of test-retest reliability coefficients were obtained for the three scales. Table 11 compares test- retest reliabilities of the D-Scale, RBS, and RES for Ex- perimental and Control subjects separately. All reliability coefficients were significant at the .001 level with the exception of the RES scores among control subjects. A test of significance for r differences of independent samples revealed that none of the differences in reliability be— tween the Experimental and Control groups was significant at the .05 level. 41 TABLE 11 Comparison of Test-Retest Reliability Coefficients for D-Scale, RBS, and RES Scores Between EXperimental and Control Groups Group N D—Scale RBS RES Experimental 51 .68C .84C .81c Control 22 .76c .91C .57b 2 0.59nS 1.16nS 1.74nS nSp > .05, two-tailed test of significance for r differ— ences of independent samples. b . p < .01, one—tailed test. Cp < .001, one-tailed test. Since the experimental communication concerning dogmatism had no significant effect on the scores of the Experimental subjects, the scores of the Control and Ex- perimental groups were combined to obtain overall test— retest reliability coefficients on the three scales. Table 12 reveals that the overall reliabilities of the D—Scale, the RBS, and the RES for a total of 73 subjects were .71, .86, and .74, respectively. All three relia- bilities were significant well beyond the .001 level. 42 TABLE 12 Test-Retest Reliability Coefficients for D—Scale, RBS, and RES Scores for EXperimental and Control Groups Combined (N = 73) Scale 52:22221231’ Dogmatism .71C Religious Belief .86C Religious Ethnocentrism .74C Cp < .001, one—tailed test. It was felt that since the study included two newly devised scales, a measure of the scales' internal consistencies should be obtained as an additional check on the scales' reliabilities. Because of the short length of the RBS and particularly the RES, simple split-half reliability ratings were deemed inappropriate and likely unreliable; and since the subjects responded to the scales through use of a six-point Lickert scale rather than simply dichotomously, the Kuder—Richardson test was also inappro- priate. The Coefficient Alpha test, equivalent to the Kuder—Richardson, but applicable to scales in which more than two responses to the items are available, does provide 43 an average of all split-half correlations of a scale, thus yielding an index of common factor concentration (Cronbach, 1967). As such, the Coefficient Alpha represents an esti- mate of a scale's internal consistency. Table 13 shows that the Coefficient Alpha correlations for the D—Scale, the RBS, and the RES were .75, .91, and .57, respectively. TABLE 13 Coefficient Alpha Correlations for First Administration D-Scale, RBS, and RES Scores (N = 132) Scale Coefficient Alpha Dogmatism .75 Religious Belief .91 Religious Ethnocentrism .57 Demographic Variables At the first administration of the three scales, the subjects were requested information regarding their sex, age, class status, and religious affiliation. The numbers of students in each of the demographic categories were summarized in Table l. 44 Pearson product-moment correlations between the variables of class status and age and scores on the first administration of the D—Scale, RBS, and RES are presented in Table 14. Age and class status correlated negatively with D—Scale and RBS scores and positively with RES scores. However, all of the correlations were small and statis- tically non—significant. Thus neither age nor class status was a significant determinant of D—Scale, RBS, or RES scores among individuals in the preponderantly lower bien- nial college population involved in the study. TABLE 14 Pearson Product—Moment Correlations Between Class Status and Age and First Administration D—Scale, RBS, and RES Scores (N = 132) D-Scale RBS RES Age -.06ns -.06nS .03ns Class Status —.09nS —.10nS .02nS nsp > .05 Comparisons of mean scores obtained by males and females on the three scales are presented in Table 15. No significant differences were found between scores of males 45 and females on the D-Scale and the RES. On the RBS, how- ever, the difference between mean male and mean female scores was significant at the .05 level, females scoring higher. TABLE 1 5 Sex Differences on First Administration D-Scale, RBS, and RES Scores D-Scale RBS RES N M SD M SD M SD Males 57 149.54 20.42 108.00 24.31 40.32 8.11 Females 75 162.03 23.09 117.16 24.88 40.40 10.26 t 0.64ns 2.08a 0.05nS s . n p > .05, two-tailed test. ap < .05, two-tailed test. An analysis of the results of the first administra— tion of the D-Scale, RBS, and RES by religious affiliational categories is summarized in Table 16 showing that Protes- tants scored highest on the D-Scale and the RES, whereas Catholics were highest on the RBS. Simple one-way analyses of variance were carried out across religious affiliations 46 TABLE 16 Religious Affiliational Differences on First Administration D-Scale, RBS, and RES Scores . . D-Scale RBS RES Religious Affiliation M SD M SD M SD Protestant 64 151.89 23.7 111.52 37.8 42.05 10.7 Catholic 40 141.75 19.7 127.45 21.9 40.98 10.1 Jewish 11 149.64 21.6 97.09 24.0 39.09 7.3 Other 6 146.67 20.7 102.33 29.6 33.17 12.4 None 11 146.27 19.9 93.27 16.4 33.55 6.8 for the three scales. Table 1? reveals that the F value for the D-Scale analysis of variance was 0.23, not signif— icant at the .05 level. The values of F for the RBS and RES analyses of variance were 7.74 (p < .01) and 3.10 (p < .05), respectively. Since the latter two F values were statistically significant, post—hoc individual t—tests for differences between affiliational groups on the RBS and the RES were made. For the RBS significant differences were found be— tween the means for Protestants vs. Catholics (t = 2.60, p < .01), Protestants vs. None (t = 2.59, p < .05), 47 Catholics vs. Jews (t = 3.63, p < .01), and Catholics vs. None (t = 5.45, p < .001). For the RES, significant dif— ferences were found between the means for Protestants vs. None (t = 3.35, p < .01) and Catholics vs. None (t = 2.76, p < .01). TABLE 17 One-Way Analyses of Variance across Religious Affiliations for Results of the First Administration of the D-Scale, the RBS, and the RES Scale Source of Variation df MS F . . . . . ns Religious Affiliation 4 112.98 0.23 D-Scale Error 127 500.12 Total 131 Religious Affiliation 4 4058.79 7.74b RBS Error 127 524.46 Total 131 Religious Affiliation 4 259.06 3.10a RES Error 127 83.61 Total 131 nSp > .05 ap < .05 b p < .01 DISCUSS ION Intercorrelations Table 4 reveals that Hypothesis 1 was confirmed; dogmatism was significantly positively related to reli- gious belief for the population sampled. Nevertheless, the correlation was small (.17) and indicates that less than three percent of the variance in religious belief might be attributable to dogmatism, and vice—versa. In regard to the extent of the relationship, Rokeach (1960) has emphasized that beliefs are not necessarily dogmatic in and of themselves; only when they dictate how the be- liever will perceive other information do they contribute to an overall closed-minded attitude. For example, consider the two statements, "Man has the possibility of eternal life," and "A11 Scripture is given by inspiration of God." Affirmation of the first item says little about the respondent's degree of adoption of an authoritarian attitude, but simply indicates a basic— ally religious orientation. However, a person who strongly 48 49 endorses the statement that all Scripture is God—inspired is more liable to accept those same Scriptures as guide- lines for his behavior, thereby adopting a dogmatic atti- tude. In point of fact, the correlation between the former item and the D-Scale was found to be —.02, whereas the latter items correlated .31 with dogmatism. As mentioned previously, Stanley (1963) found a positive correlation of .58 between scores on the D—Scale and response to the single question, "All the Bible is divinely inspired and literally true," among a group of theology students. Ap- parently, then, some aspects of religious belief incorporate a dogmatic element, while others do not. Hypothesis 2 was also confirmed; dogmatism was positively related to degree of religious ethnocentrism. In this case the correlation was moderate (.31); indicat- ing that about 10 percent of the variance in RES scores could be attributed to degree of dogmatism. The results seem to confirm the notion, espoused by Allport (1966), that certain aspects of religious feeling, namely the doc— trines of election and revelation, provide their adherents with a justification for an exclusive, and at the same time, closed—minded stance toward their beliefs. 50 It would be interesting and worthwhile to determine whether the preponderance of the relationship between dog- matism and religious ethnocentrism is due to the influence of certain religious groups on their followers or to the gravitating of dogmatic individuals toward religious organ— izations which condone a religiously ethnocentric attitude. Rokeach (1960) has posited that there is a compounding interaction between the two mechanisms. The design of the present study, unfortunately, did not allow for investiga— tion of this interactional effect. Table 5 reveals that the extent of the relation— ship between dogmatism and religious ethnocentrism is greater than that between dogmatism and religious belief, the difference between the two correlations being signif— icant at the .01 level. Apparently, while both the RBS and the RES measure varieties of religious attitudes, the RES includes more elements of dogmatic thinking than does the RBS. In fact, inspection of the RES reveals that sev— eral of the items parallel in intent, if not in wording, the content of corresponding D—Scale items. It might even be proposed that the RES represents an approach to the measurement of religious dogmatism. Ideally, then, the RBS would be interpreted as a relatively pure measure of 51 the strength of an individual's religious beliefs, and the RES a measure of how dogmatically he holds those beliefs. Such an interpretation would require that the RBS correlate little if at all with the D-Scale, whereas there would need to be a sizable correlation between the RES and the D-Scale. The foregoing situation, however, is not necessarily ad- visable, nor is it feasible, as certain fundamental reli- gious doctrines require an authoritarian (dogmatic) stance of their advocates. The finding that the correlation between religious belief and religious ethnocentrism was moderately positive (thus confirming Hypothesis 3) is not surprising. Both the RBS and the RES are ostensibly religious scales; indeed, both were originally included in one scale of religious belief. Yet the correlation between the two was only .37. (It may appear inconsistent to emphasize the correlation between the D-Scale and the RBS, while de-emphasizing the slightly higher correlation between the RBS and the RES; but it must be pointed out that while the D—Scale and the RES are generically different scales, the RES and RBS both are scales concerned with religious attitudes and thus would be expected to be related.) One may thus conclude - that while there is a positive relationship between 52 religious belief and religious ethnocentrism, the RBS and RES measure primarily different variables. Table 5 shows that there was no significant difference between the cor— relation of dogmatism with religious ethnocentrism and the correlation of religious belief with religious ethnocen- trism. It might be implied from this that dogmatism and religious belief provide fairly equal proportions of the variance in religious ethnocentrism. Tables 6 and 7 present intercorrelations among the three scales for Control and Experimental Subjects separ- ately for both the first and second administrations. While all correlations were positive andlxnmmly comparable to the correlations among the scales on the first administration for the total group of 132 subjects, some differences be- tween EXperimental and Control groups are evident. Never- theless, no theoretical notions may be evoked to explain these differences, as the Experimental-Control designation was basically a random procedure. It may be observed, though, that whereas the correlation between dogmatism and religious belief declined between the first and second administration among Experimental subjects, it rose among Control subjects. It could be deduced from this that the derrogatory communication regarding dogmatism led to a 53 weakening of the relationship between dogmatism and reli- gious belief; but a more plausible explanation is that the correlational variations are due primarily to chance ef- fects. This interpretation is the more reasonable in light of the small size of the Control group (N = 22), as well as the otherwise negligible effects of the experimental communication, discussed below. Experimental Effects It is apparent from Table 8, that the derogatory communication regarding dogmatism did not produce signif- icant effects on the D-Scale, RBS, or RES scores. In fact, while scores on the RBS dropped among Experimental subjects at the second administration, the RBS scores of the Control group drOpped even more. It may be interesting to note, though, that in both scales which contain significant ele— ments of dogmatism, i.e. the D—Scale and RES, the Experi- mental group "lowered" their scores more than the Control. Neither of the differences, however, approached signifi- cance. Several precautions were taken to insure and pre- serve the naivete of the subjects concerning the purposes 54 of the experiment. Prior to completion of data collection, the subjects were told only that the questionnaire was "part of an experiment." As part of the instructions on the Attitude Inventory and the Attitude Scale, the subjects were advised that "the following is a study of what the general public thinks and feels about a number of important religious, social, and personal questions." Items from the three scales were presented in a mixed order in the ques— tionnaires, giving the subject the impression that just one conglomerate scale was being employed and preventing him from realizing that scores for the three scales would be compared. Also, by changing the title from "Attitude In— ventory" to "Attitude Scale," by omitting the demographic items, and by reversing the order of the scale items, it was hoped the subjects would not perceive the second ad- ministration as a reliability check on their first admin— istration performance. Even so, several subjects revealed an experimental sophistication through knowing glances at the instructor. In regard to the experimental communication, the instructor presented a lecture on "Conformity" during the first half of the class period and, in the case of the Experimental group, finished it by presenting a tape 55 recording of the communication on dogmatism. (As mentioned previously, it was not uncommon for this instructor to tape portions of his lecture.) To sustain their attention while listening to the communication, the students were advised that immediately after hearing the tape, they were to write for class credit a short paragraph concerning the message on the tape. No mention was made of any connection between the tape recording and the ensuing administration of the three scales. As a final check, when a majority of the students had completed the Attitude Scale, the instructor told them that if they had an idea as to the purpose of the experiment, they could write it on the back of the questionnaire. None of the handful of responses mentioned anything about "dogmatism" or "closed—mindedness." In light of the above, the failure to obtain lowered D—Scale, RBS, and RES scores as a result of the presentation of the communication regarding dogmatism apparently cannot be directly attributed to either knowledge of the intent of the communication or inattention to it on the part of the Experimental subjects; nor can the failure be attributed to inconsistencies in the presentation of the communication. There are three other general factors which may have con- tributed to the negative findings: (a) the relative 56 imperviousness of dogmatism, religious belief, and reli- gious ethnocentrism, (b) the subjects' perception of their task on the second administration, and (c) the subjects' perception of the communication. It is a well—established fact that attitudes and patterns of behavior formulated early in life and strengthened and elaborated over a period of years are rarely altered in a short length of time. It thus seems highly doubtful whether lasting changes in dogmatism, religious belief, or religious ethnocentrism could be effected by presenting a five minute tape—recorded speech. The most that could reasonably be expected by such a pro- cedure would be a transitory lowering of dogmatism and possibly religious belief and ethnocentrism. That such was not the case is evident from the results presented in Table 8. Despite the precautions taken to prevent the sub- jects' perception of the second administration as simply a re—presentation of the first inventory, the instructor re- ported that many of the subjects revealed their awareness of the equivalence of the two questionnaires through know- ing glances and smiles. Further, since a number of the items on the scales are somewhat striking in content, it 57 is likely that individuals taking the scales would recog— nize many of these items after four weeks. The subjects then might have perceived their task as one of providing the eXperimenter with a high test—retest reliability for his instrument. Such a perception would dampen the effect of a communication on the responses to the items of the scales. The subjects may have perceived the communication simply as a piece of factual information to be momentarily stored and subsequently regurgitated; that is, they may not have identified with the content of the message on a personal level. Perhaps the communication should have been longer and more comprehensive. Or perhaps the instructor should have placed the communication on a more personal basis by requesting the subjects to ask themselves how Open or closed-minded they were by noting how closely their own attitudes corresponded to the defining characteristics of the dogmatism construct. A short discussion following the presentation of the communication might have assisted this process (although uniformity of experimental condi— tions would have been lost, of course). In such a manner the Experimental subjects might have more readily recog- nized the extent to which their belief systems were 58 closed—minded, and then the dissonance effect delineated by Festinger (1957) might have led to a lowering of dogma— tism and possibly religious belief and religious ethnocen- trism scores. Hites (1965) as well as others have shown that religiosity among college students drOps appreciably be- tween their Freshman and Senior years. Plant (1965) has reported a similar situation in the case of dogmatism. Inspection of Table 8 reveals that for both Experimental and Control subjects RBS scores dropped appreciably between the first and second administrations. The mean drop in RBS scores for both groups combined was 6.85 points. A t—test of significance revealed the difference to be sig- nificant at the .05 level for a one-tailed test (t = 1.82, df = 72). Thus an individual's religious attitudes and degree of dogmatism ggg susceptible to change. The prob- lem becomes one of determining the causal mechanism at work in such alterations. Table 9 shows that high dogmatics did not lower their D-Scale, RBS, or RES scores significantly more than did low dogmatics; Hypothesis 5 was not confirmed. Accord— ing to Festinger's dissonance theory (1957), when in a condition of dissonance, an individual has several 59 alternatives, two of which are: (a) he may alter his behavior so as to eliminate the dissonant situation, or (b) he may discount the dissonant information or its source. If a high dogmatic accepts the source of a dis- sonant cognition as an authority, he may alter his be- havior in what he feels is a more socially approved way. If such an individual's belief system is too closed-minded, however, he may discredit the source of the dissonant cog- nition rather than abandon his beliefs and ultimately his behaviors. This latter hypothesized mechanism could have masked the effects of the former, the end-product being a lack of confirmation of Hypothesis 5. However, consider- ing the negligible effect the communication had on the EXperimental group as a whole, a more plausible explana- tion for the lack of confirmation of the fifth hypothesis is that none (or few) of the subjects (either high or low in dogmatism) considered the communication as one to re- late to themselves in a personal manner. The Scales The present study incorporated two newly devised scales in addition to Rokeach's Dogmatism Scale. Thus, in 60 part, the study represents the formulation, construction, and administration, as well as the determination of the reliability, consistency, and validity of two scales of religious attitudes. Possibly the most significant finding presented in Table 10 is the tabulation of the standard deviations of the scales. In devising a psychological scale, it seems advisable to construct one which, while maintaining adequate reliability, will provide as large a differentia— tion between individuals as is commensurate with the scale's length. That is, it is desirable to have as large a pro- portion between scale's standard deviation and its total length as is possible. Table 10 shows that despite its shorter length, the RBS had a larger standard deviation than the D-Scale. Also, the standard deviation for the RES was proportionally greater than that for the D-Scale. (While it may be deemed misleading or inappropriate to employ the D—Scale as a standard in this situation, it has been found to be an adequate measuring device and was the only scale available for comparison with the two religious scales for the student group tested.) It may thus be de— duced that, in general, for the sample there was more dis- agreement concerning the religious items than concerning 61 the items of the D-Scale. Perhaps the difference is due to the degree of controversy in the content of the reli- gious notions as presented in the RBS and RES Specifically, or perhaps religious attitudes are simply inherently more controversial than the sorts of "social and personal ques- tions" contained in the D—Scale. In any case, one may conclude from Table 10 that the RBS and RBS provide a relatively adequate differentiation between individuals on the variables which the scales in fact measure. As shown in Table 11, the test—retest reliability for the D—Scale among Experimental subjects was lower than among Control subjects. This difference may be an indica- tion that the communication on dogmatism did tend to lower the subjects' certainty of their attitudes in regard to the Open closed-minded continuum. Nevertheless, the dif- ference was slight, did not approach statistical signifi— cance, and thus was probably caused by other more random factors. The fact that for the RBS the test—rest relia— bility was higher for the Control than for the Experimental group, whereas for the RES the reliability of the Experi- mental group was higher, and the fact that neither of the differences between reliability coefficients was signifi- cant is further evidence that the Experimental—Control 62 D-Scale reliability difference as well as differences on the RBS and RBS were due to random effects. Because of the absence of any significant effect brought about by the communication, reliabilities for the Experimental and Control groups were combined to provide overall test-retest reliabilities for the scales (Table 12). Whereas the reliability coefficient for the D-Scale (.71) appeared adequate and was comparable to those reported previously (Rokeach, 1960; Ehrlich, 1961; Zagona and Zurcher, 1965), the reliabilities on the religious scales were even greater. The RBS reliability (.86) was parti- cularly high, while the lower reliability for the RES may be accounted for by the scale's shorter length. From the comparatively high reliabilities, coupled with relatively large variances for the religious scales it may be con— cluded that, whereas there is considerable disagreement between individuals on religious issues, each individual is fairly certain of and quite consistent in his own reli- gious views. This conclusion is not surprising. Many religious sects maintain and propagate definite and rigid, although frequently differing, stands on religious issues. These doctrines are transmitted to the child and subsequently 63 assimilated into his belief system at an early age. The individual is thus provided with numerous ready-made opinions on religious topics-—opinions which are fre— quently not attacked prior to college. Such is not the case with the sorts of attitudes tapped by the D—Scale. In those areas, the individual's primary sources of opin- ion are the not infrequently ambiguous and contradictory directives of his parents and revelations of his own ex- perience. Thus it might be theorized that most individuals come to college with religious beliefs which are definite and consistent, yet at the same time rigid, in comparison to their attitudes toward the "social and personal ques- tions" involved in dogmatism. If such be the case, the college Freshman's religious beliefs should undergo a more rapid change (i.e. lowering) than his authoritarian atti- tudes. The fact that in the present study there was a significant drop in religious belief among the predominantly Freshman subjects during the four weeks between the first and second administrations of the scales, whereas the drop in dogmatism was insignificant statistically, substantiates the above contentions. The Coefficient Alpha value obtained for the RBS was exceptionally high (.91) indicating the scale has good 64 internal consistency. With one exception, all of the scale's items were moderately (.33) to highly (.72) cor— related with the scale as a whole, most of the items cor- relating better than .50. The one exception was, "The foremost reason for church attendance should be the fel- lowship of friends." Upon close scrutiny it may be seen that this reversed and admittedly subtle item is not actually applicable to those more anti-religious indi- viduals who see no value in church attendance whatsoever. In fact, the item more closely represents an approach to the measurement of Allport's (1959) intrinsic/extrinsic factor, rather than the measurement of religious belief, pg; £3. The remainder of the scale's items, however, appear to be adequate devices for measuring the various facets of the same general factor. The internal consistency rating for the D-Scale was nominal (.75) and individual item correlations with the scale as a whole ranged from .12 to .51. One might deduce that the D-Scale involves several factors, all con- tributing to the dogmatism construct. Rokeach in The Open and Closed Mind (1960) has indicated that, in fact, the D-Scale was so constructed to tap several contributory variables. 65 Two factors may be posited for the lower internal consistency rating obtained for the RES. First, although the Coefficient Alpha test includes a correction factor for scale size, shorter scales still tend to have lower internal consistencies. Secondly, the range of correla- tions was .23 to .56, and there is some indication the scale may be tapping more than one factor. Perhaps, as was hinted at earlier, and as further perusal of the scale items suggests, the RES is measuring both religious ethno— centrism and religious dogmatism. As defined earlier, religious ethnocentrism is the belief that one's own reli— gious group or beliefs are superior and that other persons or groups may be considered inferior because of their religious beliefs. Religious dogmatism, on the other hand, may be conceived as an intolerance for religious views Opposed to one's own, as well as a reliance on religious leaders or doctrines as authoritative. Further research would be required to accurately and more fully differen- tiate between these two constructs. The adequacy of the D-Scale's validity has been shown by Rokeach (1960), Zagona and Zurcher (1965), as well as many others; nor was it a major objective of this study to further directly substantiate that validity. It 66 might be observed in passing, however, that the D—Scale did not correlate significantly with age, class status, sex, or religious affiliation, although previous work has shown significant relationships between dogmatism and each of these variables. Such statistically significant rela- tionships, however, were generally not great, and were ob- tained with large samples. In the present study D-Scale scores tended to be related negatively with both age and class status, as would be expected among college students. Also females tended to score higher than males. Finally, there was a tendency for Catholics and Protestants to obtain higher D—Scale scores than individuals who marked Jewish, Other, or None as their religious affiliation. The present study does involve the construction of two new scales, and thus some argument should be advanced for their validity. First in regard to the RBS, a simple inspection of the scale's items easily verifies that the scale is dealing with religious variables; the scale does have some construct validity. Also the preliminary pilot study conducted with the first form of the RBS revealed (in Table 2) the order of increasing religiosity to be Agnostic, Jewish, Protestant, and Catholic, as would be expected. Further, results of the pilot study (Table 3) 67 conducted among Seventh—Day Adventist students at Andrews University show that the distribution of RBS scores was very definitely negatively skewed in comparison to the minor negative skewness obtained with M.S.U. students em— ploying an only slightly differing scale. This result also was expected in light of the strong, conservative, religious stance of the Seventh-Day Adventist Church. Finally, Table 16 reveals that for the M.S.U; subjects, the order of increasing religious belief among different religious affiliations was None, Jewish, Protestant, and Catholic, as would be predicted. (Because of its "catch— all" designation and because only six subjects described themselves as such, the placing of the category of Other on a continuum with the other affiliations did not appear warranted.) Individual t-tests revealed that Catholics scored significantly higher on the RBS than any other af- filiational group (excluding Other), while those designat- ing their affiliation as None scored significantly lower than either Protestants or Catholics. It might also be pointed out that while no significant relationships were found between RBS scores and age or class status (due probably to the skewness in the distributions of the latter variables), females did score significantly higher on the 68 scale than did males, as would be expected if the RBS were, in fact, measuring strength of religious belief (Cf. Hogge and Friedman, 1967). One may conclude from the above, then, that the RBS has substantial concurrent validity in addi— tion to its face validity. The RES does not present as clear a picture. Though, after some thought, items of the RES may be quite easily differentiated from those of the RBS, one cannot be so certain that the individual items are indeed tapping the various aspects of religious ethnocentrism, as the construct has not been previously extensively delineated. Nevertheless, in accordance with the definition of reli- gious ethnocentrism stated in the Introduction, the scale does appear to have some construct validity. In addition, the results of the pilot study show that among the Adven- tist subjects, the distribution of scores on the original eight—item RES was, like the RBS, substantially negatively skewed. Some feel for the difference between the Andrews University and Michigan State University samples may be gained by a comparison of the average response to the items of the two versions of the scale. The mean response among Adventist subjects was 5.4 (or between +1 and +2 on the six—point Likert scale), while the mean response for M.S.U. 69 subjects was 3.4 (between -1 and neutral on the Likert scale). This difference is in keeping with the strong belief in special revelation and need for separateness from the world advocated by the Seventh—Day Adventist Church (Questions on Doctrine, 1957). Although no sig- nificant age, class status, or sex differences were found among M.S.U. students, some religious affiliational dif- ferences were detected by the scale. Individuals who marked None as their religious affiliation scored signif- icantly lower than either Protestants or Catholics. And it does seem reasonable that individuals who have no commitments to any particular church would be more accept- ing of and receptive to the religious beliefs of others than would persons who are committed to defending a cer— tain church's creeds. The RES, then, appears to possess some concurrent validity also. Some conclusions can be drawn concerning the two new scales. The large variance, combined with a high test- retest reliability and a high internal consistency reveals the RBS to be an adequate measuring device, particularly for its size. Some concurrent validity, in addition to the scale's construct validity was shown. However, further research would be required to further substantiate that 70 validity. In comparison, the RES did not fare so well. Its variance, test—retest reliability, and internal con- sistency were all lower, and the scale's validity was not so evident. Nevertheless, the RBS did prove to be basic— ally separate from the RBS, and it did correlate more highly with dogmatism, as would be eXpected for a scale measuring ethnocentric attitudes. The concept of reli— gious ethnocentrism seems worthy of measurement. The present study represents a first attempt at quantifying the construct, and the task of more precisely defining religious ethnocentrism remains for future work. Future Research Hites (1965) and Plant (1965), as well as many others, have shown that authoritarian and religious atti- tudes do indeed change among college students. This study represented one attempt at evoking that change. The attempt failed; the mechanisms of change remain unrevealed. It would appear worthwhile, however, to determine what some of those factors are. The design of the present study might have proved more productive if a more extended, authoritative, and personally oriented communiqué had been 71 presented. Or perhaps retesting after a period of weeks following the communication might have revealed the in- volvement of a time-delay factor. The present research also involved the construc- tion of two new religious scales. The RBS proved to be a precise, reliable tool, and by obtaining normative data for the scale with different populations (different age, educational, socio—economic, and religious groups), and by employing it in conjunction with various other behav- ioral as well as verba1-—paper and pencil and interview- measures of religious belief, further substantiation of the scale's validity could be obtained. The shorter RES, in defining a relatively novel concept, failed to achieve as adequate internal consistency or validity. Many more religious ethnocentrism items should be devised. The expanded scale might then be administered to a heterogeneous population. Those items which would subsequently be found to correlate most highly with the scale as a whole and which provided an adequate variance for the scale would be retained, while those items which were found to be ambiguous, which did not correlate highly with the RES as a whole, or which did not, upon closer scrutiny, appear to be measures of religious ethnocentrism 72 would be discarded. Factor analysis might then reveal which and how many variables are involved in the con- struct. Subsequently, the revised RES could be admin- istered to various population samples and in conjunc— tion with other appropriate measuring devices to better ascertain the extent of the instrument's validity. SUMMARY This study involved construction of a Religious Belief Scale (RBS) and a Religious Ethnocentrism Scale (RES) and exploration of the relationships of the scales to each other and to Rokeach's Dogmatism Scale. The ef— fects of a derogatory communication concerning dogmatism on the three scales were also investigated. The Dogmatism Scale, RBS, and RES were combined into one "Attitude Inventory" and administered to 132 Introductory Psychology students at Michigan State Uni- versity. Four weeks later an Experimental group consist- ing of 51 of the subjects who had taken the Attitude In- ventory listened to a tape recording of the communication and then were readministered the three scales of the study. A Control group of 22 subjects retook the scales prior to hearing the communication. The results of the study indicated positive rela- tionships between religious belief and dogmatism, between religious ethnocentrism and dogmatism, and between reli— gious belief and religious ethnocentrism, as defined by 73 74 the scales of the study. The communication, however, had no significant effect on dogmatism, religious belief, or religious ethnocentrism scores. Also high dogmatics did not lower their scores on the three scales appreciably more than low dogmatics in response to the communication. No age or class status differences were found on the three scales, but it was found that females obtained higher RBS scores than males. Religious affiliational differences were found on both the RBS and the RES. Investigation of the data revealed the RBS to have an adequate variance, as well as high test-retest reliability and high internal consistency. The shorter RES had a lower variance and a somewhat lower reliability and lower internal consistency. Possible causes for the failure of hypotheses concerning the effects of the communication were discussed, along with implications involving the adequacy, accuracy, and validity of the religious scales as measuring devices. Possibilities for further research were also mentioned. REFERENCES Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., and Sanford, R. N. The Authoritarian Personality. New York: Harper and Row, 1950. Allport, G. W. Religion and prejudice. The Crane Review, 1959: _2_I 8‘9. Allport, G. W. The religious context of prejudice. Jour— nal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 1966, 5, 447-457. Allport, G. W., GilleSpe, J. M., and Young, J. The reli- gion of the post—war college student. Journal of Psychology, 1948, 25, 3-33. Allport, G. W. and Ross, J. M. Personal religious orien- tation and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychologxp 1967, 5, 432—443. Allport, G. W., Vernon, P. E., and Lindzey, G. A Study of Values: A Scale for Measuringgthe Dominant Interests in Personality. (3rd ed.) Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1960. Alter, R. D. and White, B. J. Some norms for the Dogma— tism Scale. Psychological Reports, 1966, 12, 967-969. Cronbach, L. J. Coefficient Alpha and the internal struc— ture of tests. In Mehrens, W. A., and Ebel, R. L. (eds.), Principles of Educational and Psycholgge ical Measurements. Chicago: Rand McNally and Co., 1967. Davis, J. H. The disbelief gradient and frame of refer- ence. Unpublished M.A. thesis. Michigan State University, 1958. 75 76 Dynes, R. R. Church-sect typology and socio-economic status. American Sociological Review, 1955, 29, 555—560. Ehrlich, H. J. Dogmatism and learning. Journal of Ab- normal and Social Psychology, 1961, 62, 148-149. Feagin, J. E. Prejudice and religious types: A focused study of Southern fundamentalists. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 1964, 4, 3-13. Feather, N. T. Evaluation of religious and neutral argu- ments in religious and atheist students groups. Australian Journal of Psychology, 1967, 19, 3—12. Festinger, L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Evans— ton, 111.: Row, Peterson, 1957. Fillenbaum, S. Dogmatism and individual differences in reduction of dissonance. Psychological Repgrts, 1964: 1_4! 47-50. Hites, R. Change in religious attitudes during four years of college. Journal of Social Psychology, 1965, _§§, 51—63. Hogge, J. H. and Friedman, S. T. The Scriptural Literal- ism Scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychology, 1967, 66, 275—279. Lambert, W. W. and Lambert, W. E. Social Psychology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice—Hall, 1964. Maryo, J. S. Factors related to similarity, rejection and religious affiliations. Unpublished M.A. Thesis. Michigan State University, 1958. McGuire, W. J. Cognitive consistency and attitude change. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1960, .QQ, 345-353. Peatman, J. G. Introduction to Applied Statistics. New York: Harper and Row, 1963. 77 Plant, W. T. Longitudinal changes in intolerance and au- thoritarianism for subjects differing in amount of college education over four years. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 1965, 13, 247-287. Rokeach, M. The Open and Closed Mind. New York: Basic Books, 1960. Rokeach, M. The double agreement phenomenon: Three hy- potheses. Psychological Review, 1963, 29, 304-309. Rokeach, M. Attitude and behavioral change. The Public Qpinion Quarterly, 1966, l9, 529—550. Rokeach, M. Authoritarianism scales and response bias: Comment on Peabody's paper. Psychological Bulle— tin, 1967, p], 349-355. Rokeach, M. and Jensen, P. K. The disbelief gradients of religious conviction measured by rejection: A comparison of high and low dogmatics. Unpublished manuscript, 1956. Rorer, L. G. The great response-style myth. Psycholog- ical Bulletin, 1965, p}, 129-156. Seventh—day Adventists Answer Questions on Doctrine: An Explanation of Certain ngor Aspects of Seventh- day Adventist Belief. A representative group of Seventh-day Adventist leaders, Bible teachers, and editors (ed.). Washington: Review and Herald Publishing Association, 1957. Stanley, G. Personality and attitude characteristics of fundamentalist theological students. Australian Journal of Psychology, 1963, lg, 121-123. Stanley, G. Personality and attitude correlates of reli- gious conversion. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 1964, 4, 60—63. Vacchiano, R. B., Strauss, P. S., and Schiffman, D. C. Personality correlates of dogmatism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1968, ll, 83—85. U— 78 Wolfer, J. A. Changes in dogmatism scores of high and low dogmatics as a function of instructions. Psycho- logical Repprts, 1967, pp, 947-950. Wolfer, J. A. Personal communication to the author, 1968. Zagona, S. V. and Zurcher, L. A., Jr. Notes on the reli— ability and validity of the Dogmatism Scale. Ps - chological Reports, 1965, lp, 1234-1236. APPENDICES APPENDIX A DOGMATISM SCALE (FORM E) The following is a study of what the general public thinks and feels about a number of important social and personal questions. The best answer to each statement below is your personal gpinion. We have tried to cover many different and opposing points of view; you may find yourself agreeing strongly with some of the statements, disagreeing just as strongly with others, and perhaps un- certain about others; whether you agree or disagree with any statement, you can be sure that many people feel the same as you do. Mark each statement in the left margin according to how much you agree or disagree with it. Please mark every one. Write +1, +2, +3, or -1, —2, —3, depending on how you feel in each case: +1 : I AGREE A LITTLE -1 : I DISAGREE A LITTLE +2 : I AGREE ON THE WHOLE -2 : I DISAGREE ON THE WHOLE +3 : I AGREE VERY MUCH -3 : I DISAGREE VERY MUCH l. The main thing in life is for a person to want to do something important. 2. Fundamentally, the world we live in is a pretty lone- some place. 3. Even though freedom of speech for all groups is a worthwhile goal, it is unfortunately necessary to re- strict the freedom of certain political groups. 4. It is only when a person devotes himself to an ideal or cause that life becomes meaningful. 80 81 Appendix A (Cont.) 5. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. Most people just don't know what's good for them. When it comes to differences of opinion in religion we must be careful not to compromise with those who believe differently from the way we do. It is often desirable to reserve judgment about what's going on until one has had a chance to hear the Opinion of those one reSpects. In the history of mankind there have probably been just a handful of really great thinkers. In a heated discussion I generally become so absorbed in what I am going to say that I forget to listen to what the others are saying. I'd like it if I could find someone who would tell me how to solve my personal problems. If given the chance I would do something of great benefit to the world. The present is all too often full of unhappiness. It is only the future that counts. A person who thinks primarily of his own happiness is beneath contempt. The worst crime a person could commit is to attack publicly the people who believe in the same thing he does. Unfortunately, a good many people with whom I have discussed important social and moral problems don't really understand what is going on. The highest form of government is a democracy.and the highest form of democracy is a government run by those who are most intelligent. 'dmeo bursoddo aqg ur esoqg Kq uqu dmeo umo s.euo u: sdnoxb IO etdoed Kq gno gnd seep; gsurebe p1en6 uo axom eq 01 Alessaoeu ueggo S; 3: esqu SHIT semi: u: '68 'uo pagurxd age Kaqg Jaded egg quom g.ue1e sAepemou pegurid gab qorqm seep: qu go gsow ’82 '10; puegs Aaqg sburqg egg go asneo -aq ageq og emoo aAeq I etdoed go Jeqmnu e age SJGQL 'LZ °aprs umo 100 go Iefieigaq egg on speet Attensn gr esneoeq snOJabuep s; squeuoddo Ieorgrtod ino qng asrmoxdmoo om '93 'peAII Atteag gou seq esneo geeib emos ur SASIISq gou seop oqm uem v '93 ‘uommoo ur burqgou gnoqe gsnf eAeq erssnu pue segegs pegrun aqi °vz °uos19d go gios “AqseM-Aqsrmu Aggegd e aq on 513311 sr sesneo Kuem oog gnoqe orgseranque 5435 oqm UOSJed V '82 °sgquo Jog uumepu e enrb g.u0p gsnf etdoed gsow °zz °sseurddeq umo srq Ktrxemrxd sxeprsuoo sq gr qsrgtes Aggegd eq gsnm uosgad e 'esqu exit semrg uI ’TZ °dogs g.ueo gsnf I uorssnosrp pageeq e U: dn punom gab I eouo 'oz 'poogsxepun bureq me I sins sxem on semrg IEJSASS gIesAm needs: on Alessaoeu gr purg ueggo I uorssnosrp e uI '6T '1331103 st qorqm euo Atuo AtqerJd sr agaqg pIJOM srqg u: gsrxe qorqm sarqdosotrqd guaiaggrp aqg TIE go ’81 'umo s.euo se ames egg axe sgarteq pue sagseg esoqm sageroosse pue spuergg xord og sr eArI on Ken gseq qu uni 6uoI aqg uI 'LT ('guoa) v xrpueddv 28 83 Appendix A (Cont.) 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. It is better to be a dead hero than to be a live coward. There is so much to be done and so little time to do it in. Man on his own is a helpless and miserable creature. If man is to accomplish his mission in life it is some- times necessary to gamble "all or nothing at all." It is only natural for a person to be rather fearful of the future. There are two kinds of people in this world: those who are for the truth and those who are against the truth. My blood boils whenever a person stubbornly refuses to admit he's wrong. While I don't like to admit this even to myself, my secret ambition is to become a great man, like Ein— stein, or Beethoven, or Shakespeare. In this complicated world of ours the only way we can know what's going on is to rely on leaders or experts who can be trusted. A group which tolerates too much differences of opinion among its own members cannot exist for long. It is only natural that a person would have a much better acquaintance with ideas he believes in than with ideas he opposes. APPENDIX B RELIGIOUS BELIEF SCALE The following is a study of what the general public thinks and feels about a number of important religious questions. The best answer to each statement below is your personal opinion. We have tried to cover many different and opposing points of view; you may find yourself agreeing strongly with some of the statements, disagreeing just as strongly with others, and perhaps uncertain about others; whether you agree or disagree with any statement, you can be sure that many people feel the same as you do. Mark each statement in the left margin according to how much you agree or disagree with it. Please mark every one. Write +1, +2, +3, or -1, —2, -3, depending on how you feel in each case: +1 : I AGREE A LITTLE -1 : I DISAGREE A LITTLE +2 : I AGREE ON THE WHOLE -2 : I DISAGREE ON THE WHOLE +3 : I AGREE VERY MUCH -3 : I DISAGREE VERY MUCH 1. The foremost reason for church attendance should be the fellowship of friends.* 2. All Scripture is given by inspiration of God. 3. A truly self-fulfilling life includes a distinct reli- gious experience resulting in a definite deepening of religious feelings. 4. Man's history and present situation demonstrate that reliance on any divine Being for guidance is folly.* *Reversed items. 84 85 Appendix B (Cont.) 5. Personal immortality is an outmoded concept having no basis in reality.* 6. Some form of religious orientation or belief is vital in order to achieve a fully mature philosophy of life. 7. The Bible is not inspired by any Supreme Being and cannot be accepted as entirely true in any literal sense.* 8. Such concepts as miracles and divinely inspired visions have little or no value.* 9. Religion is little more than an opiate which drugs and deludes the masses.* 10. Religion should play a key role in one's unbringing. 11. Considering the present condition of the world, it would be foolish to doubt the existence of God. 12. Christ should be regarded solely as a great teacher, not as being divine.* 13. There is neither a personal Creator nor an infinite intelligent being.* 14. My religious beliefs are as firm as or firmer than the beliefs of my parents. 15. One of the greatest powers available to man is the gift of prayer. 16. Man has the possibility of eternal life. 17. The conflict between science and religion is in all likelihood irreconcilable.* 18. Man's primary goal in life should be his and others' personal salvation. *Reversed items. 86 Appendix B (Cont.) 19. Such rituals as baptism or special annual religious celebrations have little or no real value.* 20. There is an infinitely wise, omnipotent Creator of the universe and of natural laws whose protection and favor may be obtained through worship and prayer. 21. True psychological health definitely includes certain religious sentiments and needs. 22. On the whole the influence of religion is probably harmful.* 23. Christ as the gospels state, should be regarded as divine--as the human incarnation of God. 24. Beyond the personal peace of mind an individual might obtain through it, prayer has little real value.* *Reversed items. APPENDIX C RELIGIOUS ETHNOCENTRISM SCALE The following is a study of what the general public thinks and feels about a number of important religious -,4 questions. The best answer to each statement below is your personal opinion. We have tried to cover many different and opposing points of view; you may find yourself agreeing strongly with some of the statements, disagreeing just as strongly with others, and perhaps uncertain about others; whether you agree or disagree with any statement, you can be sure that many people feel the same as you do. Mark each statement in the left margin according to how much you agree or disagree with it. Please mark every one. Write +1, +2, +3, or -1, -2, -3, depending on how you feel in each case: +1 : I AGREE A LITTLE -1 : I DISAGREE A LITTLE +2 : I AGREE ON THE WHOLE -2 : I DISAGREE ON THE WHOLE +3 : I AGREE VERY MUCH -3 : I DISAGREE VERY MUCH 1. In general, it is wiser to form friends among those with similar beliefs concerning religious issues. 2. Within a certain religion are to be found the founda- tions for the most rewarding life. 3. Religious orientation should play little if any role in one's choice of a marriage partner.* 4. If all men could be converted to God's Truth, the world would certainly be a happier place in which to live. *Reversed items. 87 88 Appendix C (Cont.) 5. At the present time I am not in agreement with the beliefs of any one particular church.* 6. Distinctions between religious denominations are out— dated and should be eliminated.* 7. The true intentions of certain religious groups are definitely not to be trusted. 8. In these troublous times, a truly conscientious person, because of his religious beliefs, should consider him- self separate from the rest of society in some respects. 9. Although there are minor variations among churches, any talk about one religion having substantially more truth than another is simply not true.* 10. Since one's knowledge will at best remain imperfect, extensive studying of other religious beliefs would probably result in greater confusion. 11. I must admit I feel a greater bond with those of my own religious orientation than with those having dif- ferent beliefs. 12. A person's religious background in no way affects my basic attitude toward him as an individual.* *Reversed items. APPENDIX D ATTITUDE INVENTORY Number: 1. Age: 2. Sex: 3. Class Status: Fr.____; So.____, Jr.____, Sr. _____ 4. Religious Affiliation: (check one) a. Protestant b. Catholic c. Jewish d. Other: Specify e. None The following is a study of what the general public thinks and feels about a number of important religious, social, and personal questions. The best answer to each statement below is your personal opinion. We have tried to cover many different and opposing points of view; you may find yourself agreeing strongly with some of the statements, disagreeing just as strongly with others, and perhaps un- certain about others; whether you agree or disagree with any statement, you can be sure that many people feel the same as you do. Mark each statement in the left margin according to how much you agree or disagree with it. Please mark every 89 90 Appendix D (Cont.) one. Write +1, +2, +3, or —1, —2, -3, depending on how you feel in each case: +1 +2 . +3 . I AGREE A LITTLE -1 : I DISAGREE A LITTLE I AGREE ON THE WHOLE -2 : I DISAGREE ON THE WHOLE I AGREE VERY MUCH -3 : I DISAGREE VERY MUCH Thank you for your cooperation. Most people just don't know what's good for them. When it comes to differences of opinion in reli- gion we must be careful not to compromise with those who believe differently from the way we do. It is often desirable to reserve judgment about what's going on until one has had a chance to hear the opinion of those one respects. All Scripture is given by inspiration of God. In general, it is wiser to form friends among those with similar beliefs concerning religious issues. There are two kinds of people in this world: those who are for the truth and those who are against the truth. A truly self-fulfilling life includes a distinct religious experience resulting in a definite deep- ening of religious feelings. Man's history and present situation demonstrate that reliance on any divine Being for guidance is folly. 91 Appendix D (Cont.) 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. A man who does not believe in some great cause has not really lived. Within a certain religion are to be found the foundations for the most rewarding life. Personal immortality is an outmoded concept having no basis in reality. While I don't like to admit this even to myself, my secret ambition is to become a great man, like Einstein, or Beethoven, or Shakespeare. Some form of religious orientation or belief is vital in order to achieve a fully mature philosophy of life. It is only natural for a person to be rather fear- ful of the future. Religious orientation should play little if any role in one's choice of a marriage partner. The Bible is not inspired by any Supreme Being and cannot be accepted as entirely true in any literal sense. Man on his own is a helpless and miserable crea— ture. The foremost reason for church attendance should be the fellowship of friends. Unfortunately, a good many people with whom I have discussed important social and moral problems don't really understand what is going on. Such concepts as miracles and divinely inspired visions have little or no value. In this complicated world of ours the only way we can know what's going on is to realy on leaders or experts who can be trusted. 92 Appendix D (Cont.) 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. If all men could be converted to God's Truth, the world would certainly be a happier place in which to live. A group which tolerates too much differences of opinion among its own members cannot exist for long. Religion is little more than an opiate which drugs and deludes the masses. To compromise with our political opponents is dangerous because it usually leads to the betrayal of our own side. There are a number of people I have come to hate because of the things they stand for. Religion should play a key role in one's unbring- ing. Considering the present condition of the world, it would be foolish to doubt the existence of God. It is better to be a dead hero than to be a live coward. There is so much to be done and so little time to do it in. At the present time I am not in agreement with the beliefs of any one particular church. It is only natural that a person would have a much better acquaintance with ideas he believes in than with ideas he opposes. Christ should be regarded solely as a great teacher, not as being divine. If man is to accomplish his mission in life it is sometimes necessary to gamble "all or nothing at all." 93 Appendix D (Cont.) 35. Distinctions between religious denominations are out—dated and should be eliminated. 36. Most of the ideas which get printed nowadays aren't worth the paper they are printed on. 37. In times like these it is often necessary to be more on guard against ideas put out by people or groups in one's own camp than by those in the opposing camp. 38. There is neither a personal Creator nor an in— finite intelligent being. 39. A person who gets enthusiastic about too many causes is likely to be a pretty "wishy-washy" sort of person. 40. My religious beliefs are as firm as or firmer than the beliefs of my parents. 41. In the history of mankind there have probably been just a handful of really great thinkers. 42. In a heated discussion I generally become so ab- sorbed in what I am going to say that I forget to listen to what the others are saying. 43. I'd like it if I could find someone who would tell me how to solve my personal problems. 44. One of the greatest powers available to man is the gift of prayer. 45. Man has the possibility of eternal life. 46. Even though freedom of speech for all groups is a worthwhile goal, it is unfortunately necessary to restrict the freedom of certain political groups. 47. The true intentions of certain religious groups are definitely not to be trusted. 94 Appendix D (Cont.) 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. The conflict between science and religion is in all likelihood irreconcilable. The present is all too often full of unhappiness. It is only the future that counts. A person who thinks primarily of his own happiness is beneath contempt. The worst crime a person could commit is to attack publicly the people who believe in the same thing he does. In these troublous times, a truly conscientious person, because of his religious beliefs, should consider himself separate from the rest of so- ciety in some respects. Of all the different philosophies which exist in this world there is probably only one which is correct. Man's primary goal in life should be his and others' personal salvation. If given the chance I would do something of great benefit to the world. Although there are minor variations among churches, any talk about one religion having substantially more truth than another is simply not true. In a discussion I often find it necessary to re- peat myself several times to make sure I am being understood. Such rituals as baptism or special annual reli- gious celebrations have little or no real value. Most people just don't give a "damn" for others. 95 Appendix D (Cont.) 60. 61. 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. Since one's knowledge will at best remain imper- fect, extensive studying of other religious be- liefs would probably result in greater confusion. The highest form of government is a democracy and the highest form of democracy is a government run by those who are most intelligent. In the long run the best way to live is to pick friends and associates whose tastes and beliefs are the same as one's own. Christ, as the gOSpels state, should be regarded as divine--as the human incarnation of God. My blood boils whenever a person stubbornly re— fuses to admit he's wrong. There is an infinitely wise, omnipotent Creator of the universe and of natural laws whose protec- tion and favor may be obtained through worship and prayer. In times like these, a person must be pretty self- ish if he considers primarily his own happiness. I must admit I feel a greater bond with those of my own religious orientation than with those hav- ing different beliefs. It is only when a person devotes himself to an ideal or cause that life becomes meaningful. True psychological health definitely includes certain religious sentiments and needs. Once I get wound up in a heated discussion I just can't stop. A person's religious background in no way affects my basic attitude toward him as an individual. 96 Appendix D (Cont.) m 72. 73. 74. 75. 76. Fundamentally, the world we live in is a pretty lonesome place. On the whole the influence of religion is prob- ably harmful. The United States and Russia have just about noth— ing in common. Beyond the personal peace of mind an individual might obtain through it, prayer has little real value. The main thing in life is for a person to what to do something important. APPENDIX E ATTITUDE SCALE Student Number: The following is a study of what the general public thinks and feels about a number of important religious, social and personal questions. The best answer to each statement below is your_pgrsona1 opinion. We have tried to cover many different and opposing points of view; you may find yourself agreeing strongly with some of the statements, disagreeing just as strongly with others, and perhaps un- certain about others; whether you agree or disagree with any statement, you can be sure that many people feel the same as you do. Mark each statement in the left margin according to how much you agree or disagree with it. Please mark every one. Write +1, +2, +3, or -1, -2, -3, depending on how you feel in each case: +1 : I AGREE A LITTLE -1 : I DISAGREE A LITTLE +2 : I AGREE ON THE WHOLE -2 : I DISAGREE ON THE WHOLE +3 : I AGREE VERY MUCH -3 : I DISAGREE VERY MUCH Thank you for your cooperation. l. The main thing in life is for a person to want to do something important. 2. Beyond the personal peace of mind an individual might obtain through it, prayer has little real value. 97 98 Appendix E (Cont.) 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. The United States and Russia have just about nothing in common. On the whole the influence of religion is prob- ably harmful. Fundamentally, the world we live in is a pretty lonesome place. A person's religious background in no way affects my basic attitude toward him as an individual. Once I get wound up in a heated discussion I just can't stop. True psychological health definitely includes certain religious sentiments and needs. It is only when a person devotes himself to an ideal or cause that life becomes meaningful. I must admit I feel a greater bond with those of my own religious orientation than with those having different beliefs. In times like these, a person must be pretty self- ish if he considers primarily his own happiness. There is an infinitely wise, omnipotent Creator of the universe and of natural laws whose protec- tion and favor may be obtained through worship and prayer. My blood boils whenever a person stubbornly re- fuses to admit he's wrong. Christ, as the gospels state, should be regarded as divine--as the human incarnation of God. In the long run the best way to live is to pick friends and associates whose tastes and beliefs are the same as one's own. 99 Appendix E (Cont.) l6. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. The highest form of government is a democracy and the highest form of democracy is a government run by those who are most intelligent. Since one's knowledge will at best remain imper- fect, extensive studying of other religious beliefs would probably result in greater confusion. Most people just don't give a "damn" for others. Such rituals as baptism or special annual reli— gious celebrations have little or no real value. In a discussion I often find it necessary to re- peat myself several times to make sure I am being understood. Although there are minor variations among churches, any talk about one religion having substantially more truth than another is simply not true. If given the chance I would do something of great benefit to the world. Man's primary goal in life should be his and others' personal salvation. Of all the different philosophies which exist in this world there is probably only one which is correct. In these troublous times, a truly conscientious person, because of his religious beliefs, should consider himself separate from the rest of so— ciety in some respects. The worst crime a person could commit is to attack publicly the people who believe in the same thing he does. A person who thinks primarily of his own happiness is beneath contempt. 100 Appendix E (Cont.) 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. The present is all too often full of unhappiness. It is only the future that counts. The conflict between science and religion is in all likelihood irreconcilable. The true intentions of certain religious groups are definitely not to be trusted. Even though freedom of speech for all groups is a worthwhile goal, it is unfortunately necessary to restrict the freedom of certain political groups. Man has the possibility of eternal life. One of the greatest powers available to man is the gift of prayer. I'd like it if I could find someone who would tell me how to solve my personal problems. In a heated discussion I generally become so ab- sorbed in what I am going to say that I forget to listen to what the others are saying. In the history of mankind there have probably been just a handful of really great thinkers. My religious beliefs are as firm as or firmer than the beliefs of my parents. A person who gets enthusiastic about too many causes is likely to be a pretty "wishy-washy" sort of person. There is neither a personal Creator nor an infi- nite intelligent being. In times like these it is often necessary to be more on guard against ideas put out by people or groups in one's own camp than by those in the opposing camp. 101 Appendix E (Cont.) 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. Most of the ideas which get printed nowadays aren't worth the paper they are printed on. Distinctions between religious denominations are out-dated and should be eliminated. If man is to accomplish his mission in life it is sometimes necessary to gamble "all or nothing at all." Christ should be regarded solely as a great teacher, not as being divine. It is only natural that a person would have a much better acquaintance with ideas he believes in than with ideas he opposes. At the present time I am not in agreement with the beliefs of any one particular church. There is so much to be done and so little time to do it in. It is better to be a dead hero than to be a live coward. Considering the present condition of the world, it would be foolish to doubt the existence of God. Religion should play a key role in one's unbring- ing. There are a number of peOple I have come to hate because of the things they stand for. To compromise with our political opponents is dangerous because it usually leads to the be- trayal of our own side. Religion is little more than an opiate which drugs and deludes the masses. 102 Appendix E (Cont.) 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 62. 63. 64. 65. A group which tolerates too much differences of opinion among its own members cannot exist for long. If all men could be converted to God's Truth, the world would certainly be a happier place in which to live. In this complicated world of ours the only way we can know what's going on is to rely on leaders or experts who can be trusted. Such concepts as miracles and divinely inspired visions have little or no value. Unfortunately, a good many people with whom I have discussed important social and moral problems don't really understand what is going on. The foremost reason for church attendance should be the fellowship of friends. Man on his own is a helpless and miserable crea- ture. The Bible is not inspired by any Supreme Being and cannot be accepted as entirely true in any literal sense. Religious orientation should play little if any role in one's choice of a marriage partner. It is only natural for a person to be rather fear- ful of the future. Some form of religious orientation or belief is vital in order to achieve a fully mature philosophy of life. While I don't like to admit this even to myself, my secret ambition is to become a great man, like Einstein, or Beethoven, or Shakespeare. 103 Appendix E (Cont.) 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. 76. Personal immortality is an out moded concept hav- ing no basis in reality. Within a certain religion are to be found the foundations for the most rewarding life. A man who does not believe in some great cause has not really lived. Man's history and present situation demonstrate that reliance on any divine Being for guidance is folly. A truly self-fulfilling life includes a distinct religious eXperience resulting in a definite deep- ening of religious feelings. There are two kinds of people in this world; those who are for the truth and those who are against the truth. In general, it is wiser to form friends among those with similar beliefs concerning religious issues. All Scripture is given by inspiration of God. It is often desirable to reserve judgment about what's going on until one has had a chance to hear the opinion of those one respects. When it come to differences of opinion in religion we must be careful not to compromise with those who believe differently from the way we do. Most people just don't know what's good for them. MICHIGAN STATE UNIV. LIBRARIES I III III 0 III III I|||||l|||II||| 312931 1 38034