V ' ‘ _ ‘— 77 v . . - . . _ ' ' .. . vu..,... K.’ . _ o -. 0.....4. '0. .. ... .' \ . . . n. .. ‘ '12.?! ,..,I .... .. ..T u ‘ . T .. .Q 0 .‘ ' ‘ - . I. . < - ‘ . | . - '.. . . run ~.,_‘4 -l. '.‘. ‘ H h . ‘ . -‘ N. . . .I - v - T . '- I . . . . "1 ' - In. In . . u» . ....T .w,’ . I . . ,l . ~ ‘ " . ' I I ‘ . . . -. V ‘ I . .1 -‘( ‘ I- - | ‘ . " ""‘ " “'.. ., A . . -. .o T . - . _. . . . . .o A . . Q“ .,. . T .. ,I .. . . . _ - I In - w. v. .. . v . - o a . .., . O . .‘ -"‘ H""" .. o- ..A . . .v... t ....... \ . A I o . o o, . ‘ . . . . ‘ l I ’ I '\ A DUMPARATNE STUDY ATTHE EFFETTWENESS OFQT I . . STRATATTAA TA CHAT‘TGT‘TG RAAATAR ATASRRAAH ‘ ’ - _3 TEACATRR ATTTTAATs TARAAA THE AAAAAAAAA = : '_-;.-.‘z ' 0F DTCEPTTTTTTAT CHTLARTR THTA THE. RFGULAR * - ‘ ' = ‘ ' ‘ ' -; " CLASSRDOM . 1110313“ The Degree cf Pb. IL ' . - .A‘TTcHTGAN STATE UNIVERSITY: . ‘ VINCENT PALACTNCT Tr; : ' .‘Tm- 'TTTTTTATTTTTTTTTT TTTTTTST“ " ' ' *5 " m R ,. R". NilChlbuIA -' .3 University ‘ This is to certify that the thesis entitled A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF THE EFFECTIVENESS OF SIMULATION IN CHANGING REGULAR CLASSROOM TEACHERS' ATTITUDES TOWARD THE INTEGRATION OF EXCEPTIONAL CHILDREN INTO THE REGULAR CLASSROOM presented by Vincent Palacino, Jr. has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph. D. Education degree in V Major professor Date ‘€//(/:3 0-7639 _— ‘— _. OF E. In recent ] exceptional chi: has been aimed a ically, and acac 9f the teacher j regular classroc It Was the m the integrati of regular flags into the rEgular one exceptionali for mOdifying the aCOUI‘Se in the ‘ The grOUp S ( TEdUCHtiOI-l 0f EXC ABSTRACT A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF THE EFFECTIVENESS OF SIMULATION IN CHANGING REGULAR CLASSROOM TEACHERS' ATTITUDES TOWARD THE INTEGRATION OF EXCEPTIONAL CHILDREN INTO THE REGULAR CLASSROOM By Vincent Palacino, Jr. In recent years, much attention has been given to the problems of exceptional children in regular class situations. ‘Most of the attention has been aimed at the specific problems the child has socially, psycholog- ically, and academically but, little attention has been given to the role of the teacher in the successful integration of exceptional into the regular classroom. It was the purpose of this study to consider the teacher as a variable in the integration process. The study attempted to change the attitudes of regular classroom teachers toward the integration of exceptional children into the regular classroom. This was done by developing a simulation of one exceptionality, hearing loss, and using the simulation as the vehicle for modifying the attitudes. The simulation was developed as a part of a course in the ”Education of Exceptional Children." The groups consisted of regular classroom teachers enrolled in "Education of Exceptional Children" during the Summer 1971 term of 1971 at Michigan State University. The subjects were pretested utilizing the Classroom Integration Inventory (as a measure of attitude) and the General Information Inventory (as a measure of cognitive information). The data 1| ([{I l II‘ I Vincent Palacino, Jr. Page 2. from the General Information Inventory was used to match the subjects in the two groups on the basis of cognitive information. Two subgroups were formed within each of the larger groups ... a high cognitive information group and a low cognitive information group. Both groups were post tested with the Classroom Integration Inventory only and scored to obtain a realism score and an acceptance score as indicators of attitude shift. The Treatment group experienced a simulation of hearing impairment as a part of a three day presentation on the acoustically impaired child. The Control group received the same information as the Treatment group but did not experience the simulation. The simulation consisted of a classroom presentation given by a "teacher” on a topic unfamiliar to the population. The presentation was videotaped and the audio track was filtered through a General Radio Multi-Filter to simulate three different types and degrees of hearing loss. The tape then permitted this investigation to impose upon the subjects a situation similar to that experienced by the hearing impaired child in the regular classroom. The gain scores from the Classroom Integration Inventory were used in the analysis. The Classroom Integration Inventory yields two sets of scores, one on ability to realistically place an exceptional child in the proper educational setting (as compared to the judgements of ”experts") and the other was acceptance of the exceptional child. An analysis of covariance was used to determine significance for the four hypotheses. Two of the hypotheses dealt with the realism score and the other two with the acceptance score. The comparisons were made between the Control and Treatment grout group . The analys acceptance of E hypotheses deal Vincent Palacino, Jr. Page 3. Treatment groups and the high and low cognitive groups within the Treatment group. The analysis show significant shifts for both hypotheses dealing with acceptance of exceptional children but no significant difference for the hypotheses dealing with realistic placement. A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF THE EFFECTIVENESS OF SIMULATION IN CHANGING REGULAR CLASSROOM TEACHERS' ATTITUDES TOWARD THE INTEGRATION OF EXCEPTIONAL CHILDREN INTO THE REGULAR CLASSROOM BY Vincent Palacino, Jr. A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR.OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Secondary Education and Curriculum 1973 ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ‘I The realization of a long-time dream requires the efforts of a mul- titude of peOple who believe in the dream and believe in you as an in- dividual. The completion of this dissertation puts me one step closer to the realization of that dream, The list of people who have contributed to this effort is almost endless and I will not burden the reader by listing even the majority of them. I must, though, thank that central group, my doctoral commit- tee, whose guidance helped me through my program and this dissertation; Dr. Charles F. Schuller, committee chairman and major advisor; Dr. Charles V. Mange, dissertation director; and Dr. Herbert Oyer, cognate advisor. I would like to add a special thanks to Dr. Mange fer the initial idea for this study, for allowing me to use his classes as my population, and for his unselfish giving of self in reading, editing, and generally directing this dissertation. Among others I would like to thank are Dr. Joseph Rosenstein and Mr.David Knight of the Office of Research and Evaluation of the Model Secondary School for the Deaf in Washington, D.C. whose assistance was invaluable in the design of the study and the analysis of the data and the interpretation of the results. Finally, I would like to apologize to the many people who helped me whom I have not mentioned. I would like to express my heartfelt thanks to thos , n ment he pe iii to those many, many people whose assistance, understanding, and encourage- nent helped me reaCh my goal. '— ~i_ A RKJGIOWII List of List of" (IIAPTTHZ Puxj lVatT Phil Defi CHAPTER I Forri Atti‘ AttiT Simul Simul Simul. GDPTER TH} Pqula Stimul Instm Ration; The C1; Scoring is f Rfiis fi 9 Gene Scoring Major Re. alYSis (“PIER FOUR - TABLE OF CONTENTS Acknowledgements List of Tables List of Figures CHAPTER.ONE - INTRODUCTION Purpose of the Study Nature of the Problem Phi1050phy of Integration Definition of Terms CHAPTER TWO - REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE Formation of Attitudes Attitudes of Teachers Toward the Handicapped .Attitudes of Teachers Toward the Integration of Exceptional Children into the Regular Classroom Simulations - Introduction Simulation - Uses in Teacher Training Simulation of Handicaps CHAPTER THREE - DESIGN OF THE STUDY Population and Sample Stimulus Material and Treatment Instrumentation Rationale for Selection of the Measuring Instruments The Classroom Integration Inventory Scoring the Classroom Integration Inventory Basis for Determining the Realism Score Basis for Determining the Acceptance Score The General Information Inventory Scoring the General Information Inventory: Determining the Knowledge Score Major Researdh Hypotheses Experimental Design and Procedures Analysis of the Data CHAPTER FOUR - ANALYSIS OF THE DATA Testing of the Hypotheses Discussion ii vii HTDNH T—I CHAPTER FIVE - SUMIARY AND CDNCLUSIONS Summary Conclusions Implications for Future Research Bibliography Appendecies 81 81 86 89 91 98 EA Table 5.) 10 Table 10 LIST OF TABLES Approximate Intensity Levels At Which Frequencies Were Passed During Filtering. Analysis of Covariance for "Acceptance" for the Treatment and Comparison Groups. Comparison of the Gain Score Mean, Range and Standard Deviation for the Treatment and Comparison Groups. An Analysis of Covariance fer "Realistic Placement (Realism)” for the Treatment and Comparison Groups. Analysis of Covariance for "Acceptance" for the High and Low Cognitive Level Groups. Comparison of the Gain Score Mean, Range and Standard Deviation for the High Cognitive and Low Cognitive Subgroups within the Treatment Group. Analysis of Covariance for "Realistic Placement (Realism)" for the High and Low Cognitive Level Subgroups. Summation of Results of Analysis of Covariance for all Research Hypotheses. Supportive Data for the Rejection of Null Hypotheses l and 3 and the Support of Directional Alternate Hypotheses IA and 3A. Pertinent Data Relating to Null Hypotheses 2 and 4. Page 48 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1 Approximate audiometric configuration of the three segments of the simulation tape. 47 2 Frequency analysis of Segment One of the simulation (Flat Hearing Loss). 49 3 Frequency analysis of Segment Two of the simulation (Gradual High Frequency Hearing Loss). 50 4 Frequency analysis of Segment Three of the simulation (Precipitous High Frequency Hearing Loss). 51 5 Frequency analysis of ambient noise in the room in which the simulation was presented. 54 6 Experimental Design of the Study 66 7 Two-by-Two Data Analysis Design 70 m _~4.——~ Pumose o The j greater 5] classroom children : about hear mat. A tr ferent lev Compared w ditional l: The $1 CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCTION Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study is to determine whether there is a greater shift, in a positive direction, in the attitudes of regular classroom teachers toward the integration of acoustically impaired children into the regular classroom as a result of simulation training about hearing loss rather than the traditional lecture-discussion for- mat. A treatment group who experienced the simulation of three dif- ferent levels of hearing loss, interms of frequency and intensity, was compared with a second group that received the information in the tra- ditional lecture-discussion manner. The study specifically tested the following hypotheses: l. A simulation experience will result in greater acceptance of acoustically impaired children by regular classroom teaChers than will a corresponding lecture-discussion pre- sentation. 2. A simulation experience will result in a greater attitudinal shift relative to realistic placement of acoustically im- paired children by regular classroom teachers than will a corresponding lecture-discus- sion presentation. The above hypotheses were tested on two separate groups of regular classroom teachers who had enrolled in Education 424, "Education of Exceptional Children," during the Summer quarter of 1971 at Michigan State University. Nature of the Problem ”The most commonly stated goal of special education programs is to meet the needs of exceptional Children Whose needs cannot be ade- quately met in the regular classroom” (Christoplos and Renz, 1969, p. 371). Over the years, special educators have taken a certain amount of pride and satisfaction in the rapid expansion of special education programs CMaCkie,§t,al,, 1963; NBA, 1967). Their pride has recently been shaken by a ground swell of criticism of such programs emanating from several educators within special education. It seems that the split was brought into the open by Dunn (1968) who, in the opening comments of an article, said that special educators should ”stop being pressured into continuing and expanding a special education pro- gram that we know to be undesirable fOr many of the children we are dedicated to serve." (Dunn, 1968). Dunn's comments were specifically aimed at the special education classes that serve the educable and mildly mentally retarded Child but, they can also be directed at all special classes for mildly and moderately handicapped children. Despite some very basic philosophical questions that have, as yet gone unanswered, Special education programs have continued to pro- liferate to the point where it is estimated that one in every fOur school age children in the United States is being served to some degree by a special education program (Kirk, 1972). ResearCh has shown us that there exists little or no difference in academic performance between those exceptional children.placed in special classes and those placed, on a full-time or part-time basis, in regular classes (Pertsch, 1936; Blatt, 1956; Mackie, 1965). The question that would logically fellow after having reviewed.the findings of these and other studies would be, why have we allowed this dual sys- tem of education to continue? Too often this second.system or "track" ends up as an inescapable dead end fer the student (Warren, 1954). In a recent decision, the United States District Court for the District of Columbia struck down the District of Columbia's "track system" of education fer the very reason that it was inescapable (NCSEA, 1968). Students were placed in one of the many "tracks" of this system and remained in that track throughout their entire school career, with no chance of moving laterally from one track to another. This same approach tends to characterize the special education ”track" in most of our public schools. Children with mild, moderate, and severe handicaps, and all areas in between, have been placed in special education classes and, as a result, tend to be stigmatized in the eyes of their peers and teadhers. In addition, groups who are seg- regated into such a "track" seem to show increased feelings of frus- tration wit 1970) . Compor among some The ine "special tra an inherent ' this fatalis on the studer (COMY, 197 Educaton am”? PI‘Otectj tentionall)’ Or that has made ‘ OUtside 0f educ These tem and certain degr term as "SPGCi aj have evolved to tration with regard to both academic and social growth (Jansen, gt, al,, 1970). Compounding this ”tracking” in special education is the feeling among some special educators that: Special education programs were not initiated in response to the needs of exceptional Child- ren, but rather as an expedient measure to re- sist a perceived threat to existing goals fer "normal" children who were being more or less adequately served.by regular sChool programs. (Christoplos and Renz, 1969, p. 373). The inability of special education pupils to move from.their own "special track" into the normal flow in the average school is seen as an inherent weakness in existing Special education programs. "It is this fatalistic approach of many educators, an attitude that rubs off on the students, and their parents, that is Objectionable...." (Cormany, 1970. p. 642). Educators have, for years, used nebulous terms and phrases as an armor protecting themselves from the outside world and, whether in- tentionally or accidentally, have created a dissonance within the field that has made communication between areas, both within education and outside of education, difficult. These terms and phrases carry with them a multiplicity of meanings and certain degrees of social and educational stigma. Such catch-all terms as "special education,” "slow learner," "mental retardation" have evolved to where they are allowed to "serve as a convenient rationale for assigning students to the proverbial scrap heap of ob- scurity" (Wasser, 1971). We, as teachers, frequently use our language concepts to blend us into a world of nonsense or no-sense....Our concepts, such as ”bright," "average," "needs," and a myriad of others, blind us not only to our problems but to any real hope of coming up with truly important and potentially lasting solutions to our con- cerns. under these conditions, students cease becoming individuals and easily become de- humanized entities, bodies to affix to our widely employed empty labels and concepts. (Wasser, 1971, p.2) Typical of this categorization is the placement of children in special classes for a wide range of reasons relating to some sort of teacher-perceived behavioral or learning problem (Lilly, 1970). A number of new developments in the areas of individualizing in- struction, curriculum design and development, and research on learning in addition to the recent debate over the need fer and the effectiveness of special classes for the mildly and moderately handicapped, have re- sulted in an increase in classroom integration of exceptional child- ren, either on a full-time or a part-time basis. Educators have re- ceived the impetus needed to increase the numbers of exceptional child- ren that are being placed in regular classrooms. A number of inter- dependent developments have contributed to make integration more viable: ii (.‘I‘I‘l‘ A .l“. illll‘ l. the refinement of diagnostic instru- ments and procedures, 2. the advancements in prosthetic de- vices for handicapped children, a. the increased training of special- ists and itinerant personnel, 4. the increased number of pre-school training programs for handicapped children, 5. the increased awareness on the part of educators of the educational, social, and emotional needs of ex- ceptional children. (Haring, 1956). It is with the fifth development noted by Haring (1956) that this study is to be concerned. ”There are more people with physical dis- abilities in the world than ever before" (Abyerson, 1963). Hicks (1970) has pointed out that the total deaf population is increasing at an alarming rate. In its Rubella Surveillance Report (1969), the Nation- al Communicable Diseases Center reported that the rubella epidemic of 1964-1965 produced 20,000 children with congenital rubella syndrome, more than 8,000 of whom are deaf. These children have now reached school age and special placement is required for those who are so se- verely handicapped that they cannot perform in or profit from placement in the regular classroom. Supportive services also are required for those who can be integrated into the regular classroom and can perform in and profit from this placement. As a result of this increasing population of what would be clas- sified as exceptional children. there have been increasing numbers of Children entering the regular classroom, on a full-time or part-time basis, and an increasing diversity in the organization of special ed- ucation programs. The array of patterns now includes full- time attendance in a special class, part- time attendance in a special class, in- struction part-time by an itinerant or resource room teaCher, instruction in the pupil's home, and instruction in a hospital sanitorium, or convalescent home.... To the extent that these are available within any one school system (or geographically convenient area) this represents opportunity for flexible place- ment of children according to their special educational needs. The large number of exceptional children spending part of their school day with a special teacher and part with a regular teacher implies, among other things, the need for careful coordination of the child's educational program and orientation of the regular classroom teacher in edu- cation of exceptional children. (Mackie, etfal., 1963). The wide diversity of programs cited by Mackie, gt; a}: (1963) indicates that there has been some movement toward the placement of exceptional children in the regular classroom and that these children are receiving supportive services from numerous forms of special ed- ucation programs and facilities. One of the most pressing problems relating to the integration of exceptional children into the regular classroom has been the attitude of the classroom teacher toward the exceptional child. Siegel (1969) points out that ”the truly neglected group of children....remains the marginally_exceptional-~those who are neither significantly 'exceptional' to warrant special placement nor sufficiently 'normal' to benefit from regular classroom placement withoutgproper orientation on the part of the teacher and other school personnel." He continues "...mildly handi- capped children in the regular classroom--particularly those with learning and/or behavior impairments--all need supportive and accepting teacher attitudes"(Siegel, 1969). Kough and DeHaan (1957) believe that "if the teachers' attitude toward the youngsters is positive, if [teachers] accept the handicap as a limitation that can be overcome, [they] will communicate... [their] feelings to the youngster and help generate a spirit of confi~ dence...." It is this acceptance that must be dealt with in both in-service and pre-service teacher training programs. Teacher training programs tend, at least superficially, to present the idea that "acceptance" and "understanding” are essential characteristics of a successful teacher, but there seems to be an almost total lack of consideration of the problems of the exceptional child outside of existing special education programs. Theory suggests that attitudes are affected by at least two major variables - knowledge and/or experience (Remmers, 1950). While there is a great deal of content and experience in the average teaCher training program dealing with the "normal” Child, little, if anything, is said about the exceptional child. Philosophy of Integration The need for the inclusion of some form of program to acquaint regular classroom teaChers with the needs of the exceptional Child and to prevent or correct misconceptions about exceptional children has been expressed by a.number of noted educators, both from within special edu- cation and from outside the field. Haring (1956) states, ”the suc- cess of any plan of integration depends largely upon how the teaCher feels toward the exceptional child.” Cruickshank and Johnson (1967) lend support to Haring's assumption when they recommend ”well devel- oped orientation programs" designed to help teachers "approach more positively all types of exceptional children." Several noted special educators have expressed the thought that, in many cases, the exceptional child can best benefit in the regular classroom. Cruickshank and Johnson (1967) expressed the thought "not all exceptional [Children] need special education. Some may simply require modifications in their regular classroom or program." Wooden (1953) feels that "not all exceptional children need special education -- and many who need it for a time do not need it indefinitely....an exceptional Child may make, under favorable conditions, a reasonably good adjustment in the regular classroom.” Kirk (1950) has said that the needs of the exceptional child may best be met ”...through the 10 medium of a special class or special school; but, in many cases, they may be provided for individual pupils in a regular class...." The feeling has been expressed by some that the crucial factor separating the special class teacher from.the regular Class teacher is the training received by the special class teacher. ”Special ed— ucation...is provision for those individual differences among Child- ren for which regular teachers are not trained and fer which they do not possess competence...." (Wooden, 1953). "An assumption under- lying the placement of educable mentally retarded children in special classes and in special preparation of the teachers is that the special teachers require instructional skills not normally required of teachers of the regular grades” (Blackman and Sparks, 1965). There is a need for special training, or at least the inclusion of coursework in a training program, for regular classroom teachers so that they can adequately deal with exceptional children who have been integrated into their Classes. It is the above concern which lead to the development of the pre- sent study for it grew out of the assessment of the need for a course in the education of exceptional children which might be included in the curriculum for regular classroom teachers at Michigan State University. 11 Definition of Terms The following are considered by this investigator to be the opera- tional definitions of the critical terms related to this study. 1. Simulation - ”Simulations...compose a more or less accurate repre- sentation or model of some external reality with which the players [participants] interact in much the same way they would interact with the actual reality” (Zuckerman and Horn, 1970, p. 14). 2. Exceptional Children - ”...the exceptional child is defined as the child who deviates from the...normal child (1) in mental characteristics, (2) in sensory abilities, (3) in neuromuscular or physical character- istics, (4) in social or emotional behavior, (5) in communication abilities, or (6) in multiple handicaps to such an extent that he re- quires a modification of school practices, or special educational ser- vices, in order to develop to his maximum capacity” (Kirk, 1972, p. 4). 3. Special Education - "consists of the modifications of, or additions to, school practices...that are unique, uncommon, of unusual quality, and in particular are in addition to the organization and instructional procedures used with the majority of children” (Kirk, 1972, p. 34). 12 4. Attitude - ”Attitudes have generally been regarded as either men- tal readiness or implicit prediSOpsitions which exert some general and consistent influence on a fairly large class of evaluative responses. These reSponses are usually directed toward some Object, person, or group. In addition, attitudes are seen as enduring predispositions, but ones which are learned rather than innate” (Zimbardo and Ebbesen, 1970, p. 4). 5. Integration - ”...educating the exceptional Child.with his normal peers to whatever extent is compatible with his fu11est potential de- velopment" (Frampton and Gall, 1955, p. 127). 6. Regular Class - Refers to the classroom which deals, primarily, with the ”normal" school population. (i.e. - that population that would not normally require the supportive services needed by the ex- ceptional child). CHAPTER TWO REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE The need for definition and identification of specific affec- tive attributes of regular classroom teachers toward the integration of exceptional Children into the regular classroom was discussed in the previous Chapter. This chapter will be concerned.with (1) re- lated researCh and previous studies on the fOrmation of and factors related to attitudes, (2) the attitudes of teachers toward exceptional children, (3) attitudes of teachers toward the integration of excep- tional children into the regular classroom, (4) simulation, (5) sim- ulation uses in teacher education and (6) the effectiveness of sim- ulations of handicaps . Formation of Attitudes The term attitude carries with it numerous meanings, varying as widely as there are Specific divisions of the behavioral sciences. Green (1954) noted that while there are differences in meaning, two common factors run through all definitions of the concept of attitude: (1) consistency or predictability of responses and (2) the idea that attitudes are learned (Green, 1954). Sherif and Cantril (1945) noted that attitudes are learned as a result of contact with the environment. 14 Allport (1935) discussed four (4) common conditions for attitude formation: 1. Integration of numerous specific re- sponses of a similar type. An infant is totally specific and fragmentary in his responses. In childhood, his attitudes become gradually pieced together. 2. Individuation, differentiation or segregation. An infant has two primordal non-specific attitudes, namely approaching and avoiding. From this matrix he must segregate action patterns and conceptual systems which will supply him with ade- quate attitudes for direction of his adaptive conduct. 3. Dramatic experience and trauma. It is well known that permanent attitudes may be fermed as the result of compulsive organization in the mental field following a single tense emotional experience-- (fears, desires, prejudices, predilecta- tions). 4. Through the imitation of parents, teaChers or playmates they are sometimes adopted, ready-made. Even before he has a background of appropriate experience, a child may form many intense and lasting attitudes toward races and professions, toward religion and marriage, toward foreigners and servants, toward morality and sin (Allport, 1935). The fourth of these conditions is of particular interest to the present study. One might consider the research which points to the problems in social development and adjustment of exceptional children in the regular classroom. A large number of studies point to the fact that exceptional children, both in special classes and in the regular classes, are considered less desirable by "normal" children than their 15 ”normal” peers (Baldwin, 1958; Blatt, 1956; Lapp, 1957; and Jehnson, 1962). Allport (1935) points out that a non—exceptional child may develop such non-accepting attitudes as a result of long term development, through imitation, or a single tense emotional experience. He also states that a Child.may adopt his attitudes "ready-made," and that the source of negative, "ready-made" attitudes toward exceptionality may be the classroom teacher. Moffitt (1932), Manske (1936), and Manson (1942) all found that there was a positive relationship between teacher attitudes toward a particular group and student attitudes toward that same group. .Attitudes can exist in two fbrmats: (1) they may be expressed by overt behavior, or (2) they "may exist emotionally without being evi- denced through overt behavior" (Boyd, 1943). An attitude...is the way an individual is disposed to act toward something as a re- sult of previous experience. It is dynamic in quality and...may vary in intensity (Choate, 1958). It may, therefore, be said that attitudes are developed as a result of interaction with the total environment. Values, positive or negative, and the resultant attitudes, which an individual attaches to an object, person, or statement would be the result of this interaction. An impor- tant ingredient in this interaction and in the resultant development of social attitudes is "the values and norms of the individual's reference 16 groups” (Sherif, 1960). Pressure from the group and the desire to be accepted by or belong to the group tends to be a most crucial part of attitude formation. In summary, it can be said that attitudes have been found to have develOped as a result of some form of interaction with the environment. The interaction may be a single-tense emotional experience where the at- titudes may be ”adepted, ready-made” (Adlport, 1935) or may be the re- sult of a long term interaction and are the result of an extensive learning experience (Sherif and Cantril, 1945). In addition, it has been found that the responses which result from these attitudes are ex— hibited in a consistent or predictable manner (Green, 1954). Attitudes of Teachers Toward the Handicapped Until recently, there have been few published research studies dealing with attitudes of teachers toward exceptional children and even fewer dealing with attitudes of teachers toward the integration of these children into the regular classroom. The question of integration was, in part, brought into the open in 1968 by Dunn. After the publication of his article, more and more papers have been published in journals and magazines about the merits and dangers of integration of exceptional children into the regular classroom. 17 Unfortunately, most of these papers were either opinion or posi- tion papers or were of the ”show and tell" variety (Jansen, EExél; 1970; Porter, 1971; Nelson and Schmidt, 1971). That is, these, and many others, merely related one man's opinion or described a program or a single instance where a sChool, or a system, or just a single class had tried integrating an exceptional child or children into regular classroom situations, either full-time or part-time. So as not to demean this kind of reporting, it must be noted that articles of this type can be used in the identification of existing programs but are of limited value in the design of research to study the effects of integration on both the exceptional child and.his ”normal” peers. PUblic attitudes toward the exceptional (or handicapped) have been shown to be much like those toward other minorities such as Blacks and Jews. Roeher (1961) reported that societal attitudes toward the physically handicapped are similar to those toward other minorities. Societal attitudes such as, social distance, segregation (particularly in schools) and vocational disadvantage were the primary ones noted. Cowen, et, a1, (1958) found "significant correlations be- tween negative attitudes toward blindness and anti-minority, anti-Negro, and pro-authoritarian attitudes....” One of the arguments that has been raised against special classes for exceptional children, particularly mentally retarded children, is that these children are stigmatized because of their segregated place- ment . 18 There is some evidence that shows that, despite integration into regular classrooms, exceptional children have little guarantee of receiving greater acceptance from their ”normal” peers. Johnson (1950) was concerned with the social position of the mentally handicapped child in the regular classroom. Using a sociometric technique for gathering his data, he found that the mentally handicapped children were rejected by their classmates significantly more frequently than their "normal" peers (Johnson, 1950). How much influence teachers' attitudes toward the handicapped child had on the attitudes of the other children in the classes is not known. There have not been any studies dealing with the influence a teacher's attitude toward the exceptional Child has on the child's normal peers. There have been some studies that have looked at various aspects of the teaCher's attitudes and self-image and how these influence the children in their classes. Notably, Cummins (1960) reported a signi- ficant relationship between teacher acceptance of self and others and their students' acceptance of self and others' attitudes. Flanders (1965) reports that the 1956-1957 Minnesota and New Zealand studies indicate that "teacher's behavior exerts more effect on pupils' at- titudes than pupil behavior exerts on teacher influence.” While there is little, if any, direct evidence that a teacher's attitudes toward exceptional children, as a class of persons, can sig- nificantly influence the attitudes of the students in her class, the above discussion tends to lend some support to the idea that teachers 19 can, indeed, influence their pupils' attitude toward Specific classes of persons. Attitudes of Teachers Toward The Integration of Exceptional Children Into the Regular Classroom The literature relating to the adjustment of exceptional children in the regular classroom is somewhat weighted against integration. Johnson and Kirk (1950) found that, while mentally retarded children are physically present in a school, they may be segregated, psychologi- cally, as compared to their "normal" peers. Johnson (1950) and Thurstone (1959) have shown that mentally retarded children have a higher proba- bility of rejection and isolation because of the attitudes exhibited by teachers and their "normal" pupils in regular classes. While these tend to support the need for special classes and re- fute the call for integration, some more recent studies seem to show favorable results from integration. \ Bennett and Pertsch (1970) found that retarded children in special classes did poorly in physical, personality, and academic areas as com- pared with retarded children in regular classes. Their findings were substantiated by Flynn and Flynn (1970), Cormany (1970) and Grosenick (1970) who found that exceptional children's performance in regular classes, whether full-time (as in the case of the Cormany (1970) and 20 GroseniCk (1970) studies) or part-time (as in the case of the Flynn and §( Flynn (1970) study), tended to be equal to or better than the perfor- mance of their exceptional peers in the special classes. All of the studies that have been mentioned thus far and a num- ber of others (Doll, 1967; Tolor, gt, E13: 1967; Fine, 1967; Mutimer and Rosemier, 1967; McGee, 1970) tend to indicate that a good part of the exceptional Child's adjustment and success, or lack of them, in the regular classroom, as well as in the special classroom, may be dependent upon the teacher's attitude toward the child and her ability to instill this same attitude in the other students. Haring (1956) found teachers to be less accepting of exceptional children than they were of non-exceptional Children. He, also, found that knowledge and experience are closely related to the acceptance- rejection attitude of the teacher toward teaching certain groups of exceptional Children, as expressed by the teachers themselves. Tolor gt, a}, (1967) asked teachers to rate certain behaviors as normal or abnormal and found that experience and knowledge were significant factors in the teaChers' ability to judge these behaviors successfully. An interesting point was brought out by Fine (1967) in a study of the attitudes of regular and special class teachers toward the educable mentally retarded Child. He feund that Special class teachers tended to be less demanding in the area of academic performance than did regu" lar classroom teachers. In addition, though special class teachers seemed to be more interested.in personal and social adjustment factors, the difference between them and regular class teachers was not Signifi- cant. This seems to support the findings of MByerowitz (1962) that a 21 group of educable mentally retarded children might increase feelings of’7( self-derogation after a one-year placement in special classes. Dickstein and Dripps (1958) attempted to ascertain whether dif- ferent childhood worker groups vary in attitude toward exceptional Children and to determine whether certain categories of exceptionality tended to be more accepted or rejected than others. Their conclusions showed that all workers tended to prefer to teach those categories about which they felt they knew the most. The findings of Dickstein and Dripps (1958) seem to bear out the conclusions made in the attitude research discussed earlier. That is, \ that there is a definite relationship between knowledge of and attitude ‘K toward a Specific Object class. In their total analysis, Dickstein and Dripps (1958) found that the'\; gifted appeared to be most generally accepted and preferred.by regular / classroom teaChers while the delinquent, visually handicapped and audi- torially handicapped tended to be the least accepted and preferred. While studies in the area of teacher attitudes toward the integra- tion of exceptional children into the regular classroom are few and sketchy, it appears that one basic conclusion that can be drawn from them is that there is a need for more and better teacher education con-'fikfi cerning the problems of the exceptional child (Wrightstone, 1957). This conclusion is, perhaps, the most important in relation to the present \ , \ . 9‘ \ study which grew out of the assessment of need for a course in the edu- cation of the exceptional Child for regular classroom teachers. There have been two major studies which have dealt directly with the 22 the attitudes of teaChers toward exceptional children and their in- tegration into the regular classroom.and which are of particular in- terest and consequence to this study. Haring (1956) dealt with his population through a series of fif- teen workshop meetings concerned with exceptional children. The gen- eral purposes of his study were: ...to determine the extent to whiCh the attitudes of classroom.teachers can be \\§ modified toward greater and more realistic acceptance of exceptional Children and to utilize a workshop fer the purpose of modifying attitudes in the direction of greater amount of knowledge and under- standing of exceptional children. The study has two specific purposes: (a) to ascertain the initial status of attitudes toward, and the amount of knowledge about exceptional children, and (b) to re-test after a thirty-week workshop period, to determine the effect of the workshop in modifying attitudes and understanding of exceptional children. (Haring, 1956). Utilizing lecture-discussion groups, Haring (1956) attempted to change attitudes of the participants through greater knowledge of the \, sUbject area (exceptionality) and through actual experience. The lecture-discussion groups covered eight areas of exceptionality: Children with intellectual retardation, Children with orthopedic or neurological impairments, 3. Children with impaired hearing and/or speech, Children with academic retardation, Children with visual impairment, Children with superior talent and/or intelligence, 7. Children with emotional disturbances, NH O‘U‘I-D- 23 8. Counseling for parents of exceptional children. To determine knowledge and attitude, Haring (1956) used four measuring devices which were administered as pretests and posttests. The four were the General Information Inventory, the Classroom In- tegration Inventory, The Activities Index, and The Picture Judge- ment Test. The first two instruments were designed by Haring (1956), with the aid of Dr. George G. Stern. The General Information Inventory (611) was designed to measure the amount of infbrmation and understanding teaChers had about exceptional children. It was a measure of cognitive infermation about exceptionality. The Classroom Integration Inventory was designed to measure a teacher's acceptance of and realistic place- ment of exceptional children. The workshop proved highly effective in increasing the amount of knowledge (cognitive information) and understanding that the teachers had about children with exceptionalities. The increases were signifi- cant at the .01 level for one group and at the .001 level for the re- maining three groups. The analysis of the Classroom Integration Inventory scores re- vealed a wide disparity in modification of attitudes toward greater acceptance of exceptional children. Two of the groups Showed signifi- cant increases in several areas of exceptionality, notably: (1) hearing handicaps, (2) visual handicaps, (3) Speech handicaps, (4) seizures, and (5) orthOpedic and cardiac disorders. 24 An interesting point made by Haring (1956) is that the two groups that showed the greatest gain in acceptance, had relatively large num- bers of handicapped children enrolled; while the other two groups had only a few children enrolled who could be classified as exceptional. It was felt that this helped support the conclusion that "teachers need actual experiences with exceptional children in order to be able to ex- press increased acceptance toward these children” (Haring, 1956). While there were significant changes in acceptance of exceptional children by the teachers in the feur groups, there were no overall changes in the teachers' abilities to realistically place children with exceptionalities into appropriate learning Situations (as compared to the judgements made by a panel of "experts” in the field of exceptional— ity). This task of realistic placement is the second dimension of the Classroom.Integration Inventory. In fact, it was concluded by Haring (1956) that ”whether or not teaChers had experiences with exceptional Children in their classrooms seemed to make little difference in their ability to be more accurate in their judgement concerning the placement of these children.” This same conclusion has been substantiated in a study done by Proctor (1967). This study will be discussed later in this chapter. The implications of the Haring (1956) study indicate that inte- gration can be more effectively achieved when accompanied by sup- porting experience such as a workshop. He recommends that workshop experience should coincide with a program of integration. 25 Haring (1956) expressed some doubts as to whether broad orienta- tion courses during college training would be effective in increasing attitudes of acceptance without actual teaching experience with excep- tional Children. It is, in part, this contention at which the present study iS aimed. Proctor (1967) utilized the same instruments as Haring (1956) in her Study to determine what relationship existed.between knowledge of, kind and amount of experience with, and attitudes toward the integra- tion of exceptional children into the regular classroom. She used, as her sUbjectS, a sample Similar to that used by Haring (1956). Her sample consisted of 147 teachers, 6 Student teachers, and 10 ancillary personnel. The ancillary personnel consisted of Six Speech therapists, one school diagnostician, and one special education coordi- nator. While Haring (1956) subjected his population to a series of fif- teen workshops and.pretested and posttested them to collect his data, Proctor (1967) mailed the test instruments to her population, along with a Personal Data Questionnaire, and requested that each member of her population complete both test instruments (The Classroom Integration Inventory and The General Information Inventory) and the questionnaire and return them when completed. Her response was more than adequate to allow her to draw statistically sound conclusions from the resultant data (a total of 154 responses out of 163 or 94.5% return). Her population was drawn from ten c00perating schools in the Jackson, 26 Nfichigan area and divided into two groups: five schools with special ed- ucation programs and five schools with no established special education programs. The scoring keys for the General Infermation Inventory and the Class- room Integration Inventory had to be reestablished by Proctor (1967) since the original keys that had been developed by Haring (1956) were no longer avai lab le . The keys were reestablished in a manner similar to that used by Haring (1956) when the original keys were developed. A group of five specialists in the area of exceptional children were given items, which were appropriate to their Speciality, from both instruments. In the case of the General Infermation Inventory, the judges merely determined which foil of each of the four-foil multiple-choice items was the correct one for that item. For the Classroom Integration Inventory, they judged each item with reference to the most realistic placement of the case described in the item. The judges worked independently in their task and reached 65 per- cent agreement on the 20 severe items, 59 percent agreement on the 20 moderate items and 65 percent agreement on the 20 slightly severe items. The judges then met as a group and came to agreement on all of the re- maining items except one. Item seventeen, in the area of emotional dis- turbances, was omitted because it "lacked inter-judge reliability" (Proctor, 1967). (The scoring of the two tests will be discussed in greater detail in Chapter III). The completed Inventories, as returned by Proctor's (1967) p0pu1a- tion, were scored and eaCh subject was assigned a score for each Inventory. 27 Prior to scoring, the subjects were categorized in terms of (1) type of teaChing experience; (2) amount of teaching experience; (3) amount of academic credit pertaining to exceptionalities; and (4) consultation ex— perience (both in terms of having given consultation and having received consultation). The relation of the two dependent variables (knowledge and acceptance) to the afbrementioned independent variables was analyzed by using a one-way analysis of variance supplemented by a multiple range test modified for unequal replications. For the most part, Proctor's (1967) findings parallel those of Haring (1956). Her findings can be summarized as follows: (1) Teachers with full-time responsibil- ity for the educational program of one or more exceptional children were not found to be Significantly more realistic in their attitudes toward the educational placement of these children than teachers with part-time responsibilities of a similar nature. (2) Teachers with one or more years ex- perience in a teaching program for one or more exceptional children (full-time or part-time) were not found to have sig- nificantly more realistic attitudes to- ward educational placement of these child- ren than the no experience group. (3) Teachers with one or more years of academic credit pertaining to exceptional children were found to have significantly more realistic attitudes toward education— al placement than teachers with less than one year of credit but, not significantly more realistic in their attitudes than the group with no academic credit pertaining to exceptional children. 28 (4) TeaChers who reported experience giving consultation about one or more of the exceptionalities covered by the Classroom Integration Inventory (CII) were not found to have significantly more realistic attitudes toward educa- tional placement than teachers who re- ceived consultation or teachers who re- ceived.no consultation. (Proctor, 1967). "The finding that 'amount of experience' with exceptional child- ren did.not increase the teaCher's ability to make more realistic de- cisions concerning the educational placement fer them.is in agreement with the reported findings...." (Proctor, 1967) of Haring (1956). An interesting conclusion could.be drawn from Item (3) above. While teachers with one or more years of academic credit pertaining to exceptional children were found to have more realistic attitudes toward educational placement than teachers with less than one year experience, it Should'be noted that the difference with the "no credit" group was not significant. In fact, "...an unexpected finding was that the Classroom Integration Inventory (CII) mean score ranking of the no credit group was higher than the Classroom Integration In- ventory (CII) mean score ranking of the some credit group. Had this been a Significant difference, one might assume some degree of truth contained in the old adage 'a little learning is a dangerous thing'” (Proctor, 1967). Another significant conclusion drawn by Proctor (1967) relates to the relationship of attitude toward classroom integration of ex- ag/ ceptional children and knowledge and understanding of exceptional 29 children. It was found that teachers who earned Significantly higher scores on the General Infbrmation Inventory (GII) than other teachers also earned Significantly higher scores on the Classroom Integration Inventory (CII). This positive correlation, Proctor (1967) reports, "...1ends additional support to the findings in Hypothesis 4a in which teachers with extensive academic credit in courses pertaining to exceptionalities were found to have a Significantly greater amount of realistic judgement (higher CII scores) toward the educational place- ment of exceptional children than teachers who reported lesser amounts of academic credit.” The present study is primarily concerned with in-service educa- c; tion of regular classroom teachers. The implications of the Proctor I (1967) study on in-service education "suggest that academic courses pertaining to exceptional Children are effective in increasing the amount of accurate information teaChers have about exceptional Child- ren." The deve10pment of realistic attitudes toward the exceptional child appears to depend less upon the amount of teaching experience than upon the type of experience. Proctor (1967) concludes that a preservice teacher who anticipates _\/ teaChing an exceptional child must have sufficient background, including \\\ coursework. This coursework would be most effective if it were supple- mented by actual teaching experience with exceptional Children. Likewise, for in-service programs, coursework pertaining to excep- tional children in conjunction with actual teaching experience with these children might be the most effective approaCh to the successful 30 integration of exceptional children into the regular classroom. The Proctor (1967) conclusions on teacher preservice and in-service educational programs parallel and support the findings and conclusions of Haring (1956). Finally, in her discussions for further research, Proctor (1967) . suggests that ”academic coursework which deals with exceptionalities \7A; is the most significant variable affecting attitudes toward class- room placement." It is an existing course dealing with exception- alities at which the present study is aimed. Simulations - Introduction Simulations have been used in numerous fields of interest and in a wide variety of applications for a number of years. They had their generic beginning in the field of engineering where the term, Simula- tion, was used to describe a physical model or mock-up. This model or mock-up was then used to test the application of scientific prin- ciples into design (such as a mock-up of an airplane wing to test the aerodynamic qualities of it). During the second world war, the armed services became interested in the application of Simulations in training. The services have, also, used their famous "war games" to analyze and evaluate the abilities of their'nen.and maChines to coordinate their activities and to meet an 31 objective (Dawson, 1962). Perhaps the most famous Simulator is the Air Force's Link Trainer. This device simulates the controls in the cockpit of an aircraft and.is still used in flight training programs (Tansey and thin, 1969). A.far more sophistocated Simulator is being used by the major commercial air- lines to train their pilots to fly the jet passenger aircraft of today. The United Air Lines system, for example, is tied to a computer which can "create," in the Simulator's cockpit, conditions ranging from rough weather handling through loss of a wing (Crawfbrd, 1963). The introduction of the Link Trainer into a human training sit- uation brought about a shift in the thinking about simulations. The history of what could be termed as simulations dates back to the de- velopment of the game of chess, Which was designed originally as a simulation of medieval military strategies (Tansey and Unwin, 1969). Up to the development of the Link Trainer, the focus of simulations had been on competition between men (it might be added at this point that, prior to the second world war, there seemed to be no distinction made between simulations and games). The Link Trainer brought a new dimension into the picture. The participant was competing, as it were, against himself and a given situation. The situation was analogous to his "on the job" Situation and he made decisions based upon his background and training. When decisions were made, he had no fear of censure from others if he had made a mistake. The participant, there- fore, was given a great deal of latitude in his decision-making and could experiment to determine what he could and could.not do in a given 32 situation, without "suffering the consequences" that might result in ”real life." In real life, of course, there is the interaction of not just hu- man to maChine but human to human. To deal with these interactions in a Simulated situation, the Systems Research Laboratory of the Rand Corporation constructed a duplicate of an air defense direction cen- ter. The entire environment of the center was Simulated in this dup- licate--including physical layout, work assignments and tasks, and me- chanical and electrical failures affecting the center's operation. The effectiveness of this simulation has been reported by Chap- man (1965). "The members of each crew became an integral unit... learned to perform.more effectively” (Chapman, 1965). Business and industry have, since World war II, been among the leaders in the utilization of simulation in their product development and training activities (Greenlaw,et,al,, 1962). The American Management Association has been instrumental in the deve10pment of management Simulations for training personnel on all levels of industrial management. Its first simulation, probably the first business Simulation, was the Top Management Decision Simulation, developed in 1956. In it, the participants are allowed to act as ex- ecutives, to make decisions, and to see the effects of those decisions (Tansey and Unwin, 1969). In business and industry, there tend to be four basic categories of games: (1) general management games; (2) fUnctional games; (3) indus- try games; and (4) bureaucracy games (Stewart, 1961). One notable 33 Characteristic of Simulations as used in business and industry is that they tend to follow the ferm of games. That is, while they simulate ”real life" situations, there is the addition of competition and scoring, in some form, in addition to the aspect of winning or losing. "Games are the primary type of Simulation [used by business and.industry] and generally are designed around deterministic models containing a few chance elements" (Gustafson, 1969). Another characteristic of the Simulations of business and indus- try is the 1aCk of researCh aimed at empirical evaluation of the ef- fectiveness of the simulation. The only evaluative infermation that has been reported has been the subjective testimonials of instructors and participants. Here, too, as with much of the "evaluation" that takes place in education, the effectiveness of a particular educa- tional package or lesson or methodology is evaluated.in terms of ”gut" feelings. That is with such subjective phrases as "I feel that it's working” or "I know it's working, I can feel it in my bones." Simulation-Uses in Teacher Training Educators have, until recently, been reluctant to try simulations in ”standard courses of instruction." Greenlaw, gt, a1. (1962), for ex- ample, suggest that simulations have been viewed by educators within the 34 context of "games” and, as suCh, felt that they were inappropriate for the goals of education. Little research has been done on the effect of simulation in the ed- ucational process. There have been a few projects that have been carried out that are worth noting. The first published study of the application of simulation in ed- ucation was the Jefferson School Township District Simulation. The pro- ject, directed by Hemphill, Griffiths, and Frederiksen (1966), closely paralelled the typical "in-basket/out-basket" type of business simula- tion. Aimed at correlating administrative performance with personality traits of the participants, the researchers did a job analysis of ele- mentary school principals, and.then developed a hypothetical system (all based on geographical and sociological studies of a real school system), including information on class size, record procedures, rules, and.policies. Once participants were familiar with all of the background infor- mation, which was deveIOped in a variety of media--film, Slide, tape, and.print--they were put into the role of principal of the Simulated school and were subsequently called upon to make decisions relevant to that position. The assuming of the role of principal is a key to this simulation for "a simulation cannot be effective if the participant is detached from the activity and is coolly academic in his outlook. It is the high degree of involvement that Simulation induces that makes it an effective instructional tool" (Tansey and Unwin, 1969). By analyzing each participant's responses to each stimulus in the 35 simulation, the researchers were able to gain normative data on the be- havior of elementary school principals thus permitting "sUbsequent clin- ical examination of 'on-the-job' behavior in Similar simulated situations, and [permitting] the putting forth of a theory that simulation could.be used...to replace unsatisfactory techniques of interview in appointment to various positions...." (Hemphill, Griffiths, and Frederiksen, 1966). In this way, it could be determined who possessed the Specific charac- teristics for a specific job. For many years, driver education has utilized driver training Simulators to teach behind-the-wheel techniques. Having grown out of the techniques used with the Link Trainer, behind-the-wheel Sim: ulators generally use a motion picture of various driving situations and the student, sitting behind the wheel in the simulator, reacts to these situations as if he were on the road. His reactions, speed, and accuracy, are recorded and used to evaluate his performance (Zaun and Schroeder, 1962). Three notable contributions on the use of simulation in teacher training all deal with the same simulation model. Kersh (1963) devel- oped a simulation of classroom Situations to observe the behavior of teachers, in-service and pre-service, in these situations. Based upon the observed responses, Kersh (1963) developed a series of Simulated consequence situations which would give the teacher feedback on the con- sequences of a response. Kersh (1965), again, used the same model in a second study and de- termined that, in this kind of situation, an actual physical response 36 along with a verbal response was more effective than Simply a verbal re- sponse. The combined response mode enabled the student teacher to bet- ter understand the consequences of his response in Specific classroom situations. Vlcek (1965) and Bond (1965) also utilized the Kersh simulation in two separate experiments. Vlcek (1965) found that the group which had been exposed to the Simulation did significantly better in coping with the problem Situations with which he was confronted. Bond (1965) used the Simulator to determine its effectiveness in changing the attitudes of education majors toward professional objectives. He used a semantic differential pretest-posttest and found no significant difference be- tween the group which had been exposed to the Simulation and the group that had not. Gustafson (1969) used a simulation model developed by Kagan, et: 31. (1967) to Simulate anxiety situations in a classroom setting. Using student teachers as his subjects, Gustafson (1969) presented a series of 31 short filmed situations portraying ”high school students acting out various emotions directed toward the viewer” (Gustafson, 1969), that is, the Student teaCher. The subjects were videotaped while viewing these situations and the tape played back at the end of eaCh situation. The subject, then, re- acted to the tape (which was played back split-screened to include both the subject's face and the stimulus material). The subjects were then Observed via audio tape samplings, while 37 they were student teaching to determinine what carryover resulted after exposure to the simulation. The recordings were analyzed using the Flanders Interaction Scale. The results Showed no significant difference between the experimental group and control group except for one area, student talk in the classroom. Gustafson (1969) suggests that the results were inconclusive for several reasons, some of which were weaknesses in the design of the study. One development that is of interest to teacher education, in gen- eral, and special education, specifically, is a report by Johnson (1967). Using a computer, variables were programmed into the memory and, upon the setting of the desired parameters, an entire 4th grade class could be generated by the computer. The system was able to gen- erate classes with specific characteristics to simulate special clas- ses, vocational classes, or other more typical classes. MicroteaChing techniques are gaining fairly wide acceptance in teacher training programs. In many cases, the fermat used is similar to the model developed by Allen and Gross (1955) in which a lesson is videotaped with the student teaching the lesson. It is immediately played back and discussed by the Student and his supervisor. After the discussion, the lesson was retaught incorporating any points which had come up during the critiquing of the first taping. Other institutions may use commercially prepared films which present Specific teaching Sit- uations in an ”open-ended” format. There have been a few other attempts at utilizing simulations in 38 education, but most have provided only subjective evaluation of their effectiveness. Cruickshank (1966) and Cruickshank, gt, gt, (1967) report that Simulations are valuable tools but, provide no supportive evidence other than testimonial and other subjective support. Sage (1967) developed a Simulation for administrators of special education programs. The simulation, known as the Special Education Administration Task Simulation (SEATS) Game, was pilot-tested on two different occasions utilizing three different methods of evaluation: 1. A category system for scoring per- formance in response to the simulated task according to dimensions of inter- personal relationships suggested by a theoretical construct concerning the role. 2. .A test, with alternate forms to be used on a pre- and post-training basis, designed to assess preference in choice of the same dimensions when pursuing the solution to problems. 3. An opinionnaire for obtaining from participants a subjective appraisal of various factors dealing with the use- fulness of the materials (Sage, 1967). The findings were not significant in several areas, notably in the performance on the simulated activities and behavioral choice tests as a function of demographic variables and no support was found for his theoretical constructs, based upon the mean scores of the pretest and posttest. Though the results were not what Sage (1967) had expected, several additional pilot tests were run, after revisions, and, as a result, the 39 SEATS game has gained fairly wide usage (Sage and Sontag, 1970). It has been used.numerous times by its author as a part of "Special Study Institutes,” sponsored by the New York State Education Department, Di- vision of Handicapped Children. It has also been used in several uni- versity and college programs, such as Syracuse university and Michigan State University. Burke and Sage (1970) used the Sage (1967) model, which was de— signed for use in training personnel to assume leadership roles in Spe- cial education, to sensitize ”general education administrators to sig- nificant issues concerning the education of handicapped children”(Burke and Sage, 1970). That is, to deal with the attitudes of regular educa- tion personnel toward the integration of handicapped children into the regular classroom. Burke and Sage (1970) found significant change in only three of the ten concepts with which they dealt. They found a significant changes in attitudes relating to: (l) placement of gtt_handicapped children in special classes; (2) placement of moderately handicapped children in regular classes; and (3) Special educators having sole responsibility fer the handicapped (Burke and Sage, 1970). While the results were not as conclusive as they had hoped, Burke and Sage (1970) felt that the results and participant reactions (sub- jective evaluation) indicate, that the SEATS game can be an effective tool, given some slight modifications. To summarize, while the results from those studies that have uti- lized simulations have been inconclusive, there is still some evidence 40 that simulation can be an effective tool in modifying teachers' atti- tudes about, among other things, the integration of exceptional child- ren into regular classrooms. Simulation of Handicaps There have been innumerable incidents, in undergraduate and grad- uate courses on exceptionality and abnormal behavior, where students have been asked to live fer one day, or a shorter or longer period of time, with a blindfold over the eyes or where the phonograph record "How We Hear," which was developed by Dr. Earl Harford (1964) at North- western University, has been played to give the students some sense of what a hearing loss might sound like to an individual with a loss. To date, though, there has been very little research reported on the effectiveness of simulating handicaps in changing attitudes toward the handicapped. In the four studies relating to attitudes toward the handicapped and experience of various types and degrees with the handicaps, there have been conflicting results reported. .All fOur studies used the Attitudes Toward Disabled Persons (ATDP) Scale to measure attitudes, as developed by Yuker, Block, and Campbell (1960). They feund a positive correlation between vicarious experience with a disability and Shifts in the ATDP scores. Bell (1962), however, 41 found no relationship between years of professional experience in reha- bilitating the disabled and the ATDP scores. Similarly, Siller and Chapman (1964) feund no relationship between experience with disabilities and ATDP scores. Wilson and Alcorn (1969) also found no relationship between the ATDP scores and.experiences with disabilities. The principle difference between the other studies and the Wilson and Alcorn (1969) study is that, in the latter, the subjects experienced the disability through a simulation of the disability. The effectiveness of simulation of disabilities to change attitudes about the disability is somewhat uncertain, based on the very small amount of research that has been done to date. ‘Wilson and Alcorn (1969) and Siller and Chapman (1964) conclude that, based on their findings, the ATDP may be too insensitive to the quick changes which might have resulted from.the experiences and to the multidimensional and complex factors underlying attitudes. It would therefore seem.safe to conclude that there is some dis- agreement on the effectiveness of disability Simulation in Changing at- titudes toward the disabled. CHAPTER TI {REE DESIGN OF THE STUDY The primary purpose of this study is to investigate the effect of a simulated handicap, in this case, hearing impairment, on the attitudes of regular classroom teachers toward the placement of exceptional child- ren in the regular classroom. This chapter will present pertinent data relating to (l) the pop- ulation and sample used in the study; (2) the nature of the stimulus material (the simulation); (3) the rationile for selection and scoring of the instruments used to determine changes in attitude toward and knowledge of exceptional children (acoustically impaired children); and (4) reliability and validity data on these instruments. This will be fellowed by a statement of the research hypotheses and a discussion of the experimental design used for this study, the method by which the collected data.were analyzed. Population and Sample Since the original impetus for this study grew out of an investi- gation of the need for a course on the education of exceptional Child~ ren fer regular classroom teachers, the population which was selected consisted of those members of two sections of Education 424, "Education of The Exceptional Child,” which was taught during the Summer Term of 43 1971 at Michigan State University. The two sections were composed of both regular education majors and Special education majors, both pre-service and in-service teachers. One student in the experimental group was an Audiology and Speech Sciences major and, because of this, was classified as a special education major. He was the only person in either group who was not an education major. Each student was asked to complete a personal data questionnaire which gave what was felt to be key information about the student's present status and past teaching experience as well as an indication of what past contact he had had with exceptional or handicapped.per- sons. .A copy of the questionnaire may be feund in Appendix A. The class rosters placed the enrollments of the two sections at 48 and 23 students. The first day of the treatment fOund only one absentee. Over the entire three days of treatment, a total of four students were eliminated. These four students were eliminated for two reasons, other than the initial absence, they either did not re- turn the posttest or were absent on the last day of the treatment and did not receive the posttest to complete. Additional data were col- lected from.the personal data questionnaire about the subjects (age, sex, years of teaching, etc.). While all of these data are not crucial to the analysis of the researCh data or the hypotheses of this study, it did give some interesting information about the make-up of the groups. Since the present study is concerned with changing regular class- room teachers' attitudes toward the integration of exceptional children 44 into the regular classroom, the treatment and comparison samples were drawn from the regular education majors in both of the above mentioned sections. Since the comparison group consisted of only 14 regular ed- ucation majors, the decision was made to match those 14 regular educa- tion majors with 14 regular education majors in the treatment group. The matching also helped overcome the variable of non-random assign- nent. Since the groups from Which the samples were drawn were not randomized (the two class sections were filled through normal regi- stration procedures) it was felt that the matching procedure would help provide two more equal groupings. The relationship between knowledge and attitude has already been discussed in Chapter 11. Further discussion is not needed here but, to summarize briefly, it was shown that there was a positive relation- ship between attitude and knowledge. That is, the more one knew about an attitude object class, the more accepting one was toward that object class. The criterion for matching was the pretest score on the General Information Inventory. In addition, the groups were matched in terms of teaching experience; every member of each group had had at least one year of classroom teaching experience. Also, while not intentional on the part of this investigator, the groups each contained 2 males and 12 females. Once the two groups were determined, each was divided into two sub- groups of seven each. The criteria for this division was, again, the 611. The range of scores in the groups was 3-9 for the comparison group 4S and 4-9 for the treatment group. The subjects were divided into high cognitive knowledge and low cognitive knowledge subgroups with the score of six being the upper limit for the low cognitive group. Therefore, the high cognitive group had a score range of 7-9 and the low cognitive group a score of 3—6 for the comparison group and 4-6 for the treatment group. Briefly, the sample for the present study consisted of 28 subjects; all were regular education majors with at least one year of teaching experience. The sample was made up of two groups, a treatment group and a comparison group. Each group was sub-divided into two sUb- groups, a high cognitive knowledge and a low cognitive sub-group. The groups and sub-groups were matched on the basis of the General In- formation Inventory pretest scores. Stimulus Material and Treatment The simulation was a videotaped presentation of a lesson on the Swislocki Acoustic Bridge, for measuring the acoustic impedance of the tympanic membrane and the middle ear reflex. This topic was chosen be- cause it was felt, by the present investigator, that the topic for the simulation should be totally unfamiliar to the subjects and it was be- lieved to be highly unlikely that the subjects would be familiar with the Zwislocki Acoustic Bridge. 46 The reason for selecting an unfamiliar topic grew from discussions with several members of the Special Education faculty of MiChigan State University. It was felt that such a choice would be best so as not to permit the teachers in the group to guess what was being said in the presentation. The feeling was that a totally unfamiliar topic would come closest to simulating how an acoustically impaired child feels at that crucial time in a lesson when the teacher is presenting new ma- terial with a new vocabulary. After the lesson was videotaped, the master tape was duplicated and the audio track was filtered to simulate three different configurations of hearing loss. With the assistance of Mr. Donald Riggs of the Mighigan State University Audiology and Speech Clinic, the audio track was fil- tered utilizing a General Radio Multi-Filter Medel 1925. The General Radio Medel 1925 is a one-third octave filter with a rejection rate of 50 dB per octave. This provided a fairly reasonable simulation of a hearing loss on the videotape sound track. Three different audiometric configurations were attempted. The approximate representation of these configurations are Shown in Figure l. The three configurations approximate (l) a flat hearing loss with a three frequency average of about 20 dB hearing level; (The three fre- quency average is a standard procedure of reporting the loss for the frequencies that are crucial to speech intelligibility - 500 hz, 1000 hz, and 2000 hz.); (2) A gradual high frequency hearing loss with a three frequency average of about 40 dB hearing level; and (3) a precipitous 90 \ '3: T» I25 250 500 I000 2000 4000 8000 3000 6000 FREQUENCY (HZ) HEARING THRESHOLD, LEVEL in dB 03 O zI" x —’< SEGMENT I e----e SEGMENT2 0- -o SEGMENT 3 FIGURE I: APPROXNAATE AUOIOMETRIO CONFIGURATION OF THE THREE SEGMENTS OF THE SIMULATION TAPE.. 48 high frequency loss with a three frequency average of about 63 dB hearing level. Table 1 gives the approximate intensities at which each of the frequencies was passed. TABLE 1. APPROXIMATE INTENSITY LEVELS AT WHICH FREQUENCIES WERE PASSED DURING FILTERING. (FREQUENCIES GIVEN ARE THE DENTER FREQUENCY OF EACH OF THE FILTER BANDS. INTENSITIES GIVEN ARE RE: AUDIOMETRIC ZERO, ISO). SECMENT SEGMENT SEGMENT FREQUENCY 1 2 3 250 hz 0 10 20 500 hz 10 20 40 1000 hz 20 40 70 2000 hz 30 60 80 4000 hz 20 60 100 8000 hz 20 70 -- Frequency analyses of each of the segments are shown in Figures 2, 3, and 4. These analyses Show that the first segment, Figure 2, which approximates a flat loss of about 20 dB hearing level, has a fre- quency spread from about 100 hz to about 10,000 hz. This is a ”normal” spread for the human voice, including overtones, and this segment is merely a loss in intensity. The analysis of the second segment, Figure 3, the gradual high frequency loss, shows that all frequencies above 2000 hz are effectively eliminated. This is now a loss of both inten- sity and frequency. The analysis of the third segment, Figure 4, the .3... e523: fir... Eta—=55 2:. E — ESE...“ :— u_u>_«=< 55:55... I « hz.—E...— g- ...-I8 5 Maria ....0 ... a l It'll II III I II III I: _ an E I... can :3 2: :3: 2. 55:35. s.- 3. I I n I. It ..- a o 1 n i.— o .2... All: i.- o ...... p.. .72.. 52.8: .3: .235. 3:225 2: s N use?” 8 «2.2.1253»: I e E3: 3N x— w Eon» Edam 8.8 can amN 2: NEH: z. Kala—aux..— a. A T I I I ‘ I I v I I I I v I I I IQ out-g to“ «Ow-n ...-.—._.-. ‘T'T" I I I L__ . I ___-_-__._. _'._.,..-...._.__ ._ . .LrI I . — _ u.I I I I I I I. ,. .I n . I I A II III IIL. IIfI. I. .I III .. ILI’III Eu ...u d .2:- bfld to ...u d 33- 5’ iv ...a d .030 h‘otd ..33 52.8: 5:. 32332... 552.5 2: s a 22.53 S 22.2: 8.33.: I a use... a n gonm gunk 8.. :- W N .01 on; I: «we “a 400m“! ”I ‘I-11 5.- o .3.- x— .20 can :3 2: ~23: 2. 3:33....— ”‘0‘. o‘ .0“ _*-—-- -..-..— .—- -_—._-*—,_...__... 3 a... ‘ .0”... 5’4 i ‘2. . .03.. .D’.‘ {U s... d 1‘... Ew‘ .0 l!‘EII'I-I.I'o 52 precipitous high frequency loss, shows that all frequencies above 850 hz are effectively eliminated. With this loss, many of the key speech frequencies are also eliminated. The format for the treatment was a simulation of hearing loss in- tegrated into the existing structure of the curriculum for Education 424 as a part of the unit on the acoustically impaired child. The simulation was presented to the experimental group on the third day of a three day unit on the acoustically impaired child. The first two days of the unit were presented by Dr. Charles V. Mange, Professor of Special Education at Michigan State University and the regular in- structor for Education 424. During the first two days, both groups were given identical infor- mation (in a lecture-discussion format) about the nature of hearing loss and some of the educational implications of hearing loss. On the third day, this investigator took charge of the two groups. Since the simulation was to be presented on television, it was felt that some form of television presentation was necessary fer the comparison group in an attempt to equalize both groups as much as possible. The com— parison group was Shown a videotaped interview with an elementary school student who has a hearing loss, and wears a hearing aid, and attends a regular class program, in Ingham County, Michigan. This was used as a basis for discussion for the class. The experimental group was presented the simulation in its normal classroom setting. Television monitors were set-up in the room and an initial audio level for the tape was set, using a BrUel and Kjaer Model S3 2203 Sound Level Meter, fitted with a Random Incidence Corrector. This level had been previously determined as the referent level fer the in- tensity of the audio track throughout the entire tape. To assist in determining the level needed, ambient noise levels were taken in the room and it was found that the average levels in the room were at about 66 dBC. The ambient noise was recorded and analyzed using the BrUel and Kjaer 2107 Frequency Analyzer and the 2305 Power Level Recorder. The resultant analysis is shown in Figure 5. The subjects were told that there was some very important informa- tion on the tape, made by a noted guest lecturer, and that they were to pay attention to what was being said, attending both auditorally and visually to the message. The tape was played in its entirety. After the tape was shown, a Short ten item multiple choice quiz om the infermation contained in the tape was given to the subjects. This test was given merely to add some more realism to the simu- lation by asking the members of the group to fUnction on a test as a hearing impaired child might under similar circumstances. After the presentation of the videotaped simulation, the group was asked to reSpond to two questions which had been designed to elicit dis- cussion. The questions were: (1) now that you have been through a sim- ulation of this problem (hearing loss), what demands would you, as the child in this situation, make upon the educational setting of this class- room to assist you in operating more efficiently and effectively; and (2) if you were the teacher in this classroom and.were told that you were to have an acoustically impaired child in your classroom, what .3332.— »a: 5:525» 2:. fizz ... =33. 2: ... as: Ens—.2 8 «was: 8:38: I m #3.: 5: an: 5 flag I... 3 g E , 3.0 —'..‘ "cc —.~ 0.- .O: a .33 i... . ...... $. III. I. . I I‘- ... I II I! -II IIII- r. III- I a... a: 2: ...... 3. .k.._11.,& J; 2 . . Q “a: a. '01! F .... d .0?- ...8 O .0... - 55 changes would you make in your classroom behavior, classroom setting, and classroom management to accomodate this child? The discussion of these two questions took up the remainder of the one hour session. Instrumentation Rationale for Selection of the Measuring Instruments The research hypotheses mentioned previously (in Chapter 1 and to be restated later in this chapter) served as guidelines for the selec- tion of the instruments which would measure the effect of the simula- tion on the two dependent variables: attitudes of regular classroom teachers toward the integration of exceptional children into the reg- ular classroom (as measured by the Classroom Integration Inventory) and the amount of accurate information known about the cognitive Objects (exceptional children) chosen as the attitude stimulus (as measured by the General Information Inventory). rt—‘d—I‘A n—‘utq' 56 The Classroom Integration Inventoty_ The Classroom Integration Inventory, developed by Haring (1956), was selected for the present study as the most appropriate instrument fer measuring the attitudes of teachers toward the integration of ex— ceptional children in regular classrooms. The Classroom Integration Inventory, as discussed in Chapter Two, contains six items in each of ten categories of exceptionality. The ten areas are: (1) behavior dis- orders; (2) emotional disturbances; (3) impaired hearing; (4) impaired speech; (5) retarded or superior intelligence; (6) orthopedic and car- diac disorders; (7) physical attractiveness; (8) seizures; (9) bowel and bladder incontinence; and (10) visual handicaps. The Classroom Integration Inventory consists of a series of brief descriptions of behavior of school children and the subjects are asked to react to each of these items in terms of the following types of ed— ucational placement: 1. If you feel you could handle such a student in your regular classroom without any fUndamental change in your present procedures. 2. If you feel you could handle such a Student in your regular classroom provided that advice from a specialist or consultant were occasionally made' available to you whenever you felt a need for such aid in dealing with some particular problem. 57 3. If you feel you could handle such a student in your regular classroom provided that a full—time specialist were available at your school who could provide supplementary training for the student and frequent consultation with you. 4. If you feel that such a student would benefit most by being assigned to a Special class or school. 5. If you feel that suCh a child can- not be handled profitably within the context of regular or special public education (Haring, 1956). For the present study, the six items on impaired hearing were the key items. In a personal correspondence, Dr. Norris Haring indicated that he saw ”no reason why...any portion of the test [could not be used independently] to evaluate the effect of [the] teaching unit with reg- ular classroom teachers." Since there was still some doubt about ad- ministering only those items dealing with impaired.hearing, a decision was made to administer the entire test and evaluate the results for the six items on impaired hearing only. Scoring the Classroom Integration Inventory The general scoring procedures for the Classroom.Integration In- ventory Scale are described by Vinacke (1952) who described the Likert 58 (1932) system as easy to use and reliable. Haring (1956) described the Classroom Integration Inventory Scale as a ”highly cognitive and self-validating" Likert-type acceptance scale adapted to measure at- titudes toward integration. The Classroom Integration Inventory can be scored in two different ways to yield two different types of scores: (1) the Realism Score which indicates the persons attitude based upon the ability of the in- rurv__w-*1 dividual to educationally place an exceptional Child and (2) the Accep- tance Score which indicates the individual's attitude based upon his acceptance of the child in the regular classroom. Basis for Determining the Realism Score Each item of the Classroom Integration Inventory has five possible answers from which to choose (see above). By assigning numerical weights to eaCh of the answers (alternatives), varying from 5 for the most de- sirable placement to l for the least desirable placement, a score can be computed that represents the individual's realism score relative to the placement of exceptional children in educational settings. The scoring key used for the present study was the same key that was used by Proctor (1967). Proctor (1967) had re-established the key for the Classroom Integration Inventory since the original key, de- veloped by Haring (1956) for the original study, was not available. 59 Proctor (1967) re-established the scoring key fer the realism score in a manner similar to that used by Haring (1956) in the original study. A panel of five specialists in special education "...judged each item with reference to the most desirable placement of the case de- scribed in the item, The judges working independently, rated each item on 'most desirable placement'" (Proctor, 1967). A typical item which the judges had to rate would be: "Fred can feel the vibrations of loud music from a radio or phonograph, knows when a door has been Slammed, but does not hear speech unless it is shouted." The entire Classroom Integration Inventory can be found in Appendix B. The realism score for eaCh subject was Obtained by assigning a value of five if the subject's choice of placement was the same as the judge's first Choice for that item. .A value of four, if the subject's choice was the same as the judge's second choice; a value of three if the choice was the same as the judge's third choice; a value of two, if the choice was the same as the judge's fourth choice; and a value of one, if the choice was the same as the judge's last choice, that is, the least desirable placement. The totals were computed and the range of scores for the entire Classroom Integration Inventory could be from 59 to 295. It should be reiterated at this point that Proctor (1967), based on the result of her re-establishment of the scoring key, eliminated Item 17 ”because it lacked inter—judge reliability" (Proctor, 1967). For the six items on impaired hearing, the scores could range from 6 to 30. Using a Likert-type (Likert, 1932) weighting procedure, the subjects' V “V. ’2‘“)an 60 responses were scored to get an indication of their attitude toward in- tegration by getting a measure of their ability to place exceptional children into appropriate learning environments. The subjects' responses on the Classroom Integration Inventory range from regular classroomIplacement without Special assistance to complete exclusion from school. The most appropriate placement is de- pendent upon two conditions, the type and severity of the handicap. The validity and reliability of the Classroom Integration Inven- tory were reestablished by Proctor (1967): The judges' choice fer placement among the three levels of severity ranged from 53 percent inter-judge agreement in the behavior category to 73 percent agree- ment among the judges' choices for edu- cational placement fer exceptional children in the impaired speech cate- gory. It would seem reasonable to assume that a full-time special class teacher's realism score would show a greater degree of homogeneity than a regular classroom teaCher. It also seems reasonable to assume that the attitude realism score of special class teachers would approximate more closely the judges' choices of placement than the attitude realism gggtgg of regular classroom teachers. The above assumptions were borne out in both the Haring (1956) and Proctor (1967) studies and are accepted as indicators of the validity of the Classroom Integration Inventory. 61 Basis for Determining the Acceptance Score The acceptance score is a means of gauging the individual's accept- ance of exceptional children. In determining the acceptance score, the selection of number one type of educational placement (if you feel you n: - -‘.-.A41-a~- 'I fl could handle suCh a Student in your regular classroom without any funda— mental change in your present procedures), was considered the most ac- cepting response irrespective of the nature of the handicap, and number five (if you feel that such a child cannot be handled profitable within the context of regular or special public education), was considered the least accepting. The acceptance score was calculated by weighting num- ber one as 5, number two as 3, number three as 2, number four as t, and number five as 0, which gave a possible range in scores from 24_to g. The General Information Inventory The General Information Inventory was developed by Haring (1956) as a test of knowledge about exceptional children. The original test contained 100 multiple-choice items, ten in each of the categories of exceptionality covered by the test: (1) behavior disorders; (2) emo- tional disturbances; (3) impaired hearing; (4) impaired speech; 62 (5) impaired vision; (6) orthopedic disorders; (7) miscellaneous (medi- cal, attitudes, and responsibility); (8) seizures; (9) retarded; (10) superior intelligence. These categories are similar to those in the Classroom Integration Inventory. In the case of the General Information Inventory, only those ten items relating to impaired hearing were administered as part of the pre- test. It was felt that there was far too much extraneous material in the entire test, in relation to the specific material covered in the treatments used in this study. It was felt that there would be no loss in reliability and validity of the test by using only those ten items which dealt with impaired hearing. Scoring the General Information Inventopy: Determining the Knowledge Score As with the Classroom Integration Inventory, Proctor (1967) had to re-establish the scoring key for the General Information Inventory since the original key, as developed by Haring (1956), was not available. The scoring key was re-established in a manner Similar to that used for the Classroom Integration Inventory. The items were Smeitted to a panel of experts who made judgements as to which of the four foils for each of the items was the correct choice. FL I". c ...-’1‘- ant-dross! 63 One point was assigned to each correct response on the General In- formation Inventory. TherefOre, the scores on the knowledge test could range from tg_to 0, Since it has already been shown that knowledge does have a bearing on a person's attitude toward a person, object, or group, the subject's amount of knowledge about hearing impairments could give an indication of that subject's acceptance of such a child. Major ResearCh Hypotheses To evaluate the effectiveness of the simulation in changing the at- titudes of teachers toward the integration of exceptional children the following hypotheses were generated and tested: H-l. A simulation experience for in- service regular classroom teachers will not result in an attitudinal shift, toward the acceptance of acoustically impaired children in the regular class- room, as compared to a corresponding lecture—discussion presentation. H-lA. A simulation experience for in- service regular classroom teachers will result in an attitudinal shift, in a positive direction, toward the accep- tance of acoustically impaired children as compared to a corresponding lecture- discussion presentation. 64 H-2. A simulation experience for in- service regular classroom teachers will not result in an attitudinal shift, toward the realistic place- ment of acoustically impaired child- ren, as compared to a corresponding lecture-discussion presentation. H-ZA. A simulation experience for in- service regular classroom teachers will result in an attitudinal Shift, in a positive direction, toward the realistic placement of acoustically impaired children, as compared to a corresponding lecture-discussion presentation. H-3. In-service regular classroom teachers with high initial levels of cognitive information about hearing and hearing impairment will not have a greater Shift in attitude, relative to the acceptance of acoustically im- paired children, as a result of a simulation experience as compared to regular classroom teachers with low initial levels of cognitive infor- mation about hearing and hearing im- pairment. H-3A. In-service regular classroom teachers with high initial levels of c0gnitive infermation about hearing and hearing impairment will have a greater Shift in attitude, in a posi- tive direction, relative to the ac- ceptance of acoustically impaired children, as a result of a simulation experience as compared to regular classroom teachers with low initial levels of cognitive information about hearing and hearing impairment. H-4. In-service regular classroom teachers with high initial levels of cognitive information about hearing 65 and hearing impairment will not have a greater attitudinal shift, relative to the realistic placement of acous- tically impaired children, as a result of a simulation experience as compared to in-service regular classroom teachers with low initial levels of cognitive information. H-4A. In-service regular classroom teachers with high initial levels of cognitive information about hearing and hearing impairment will have a greater attitudinal shift, in a posi- tive direction, relative to the real- istic placement of acoustically im- paired children, as a result of a simulation experience as compared to in-service regular classroom teachers with low initial levels of cognitive information. Experimental Design and Procedures The specific experimental design for this study was the Campbell and Stanley (1963) Design Number 10, The Nonequivalent Control Group Dggtgp, Figure 6 illustrates the design of this study. One day prior to the beginning of the unit on acoustically handi- capped children, the pretest packet (consisting of the Classroom Inte- gration inventory and the General Information Inventory) and the per- sonal data questionnaire were given out to all of the subjects (the test packet may be found in Appendix B and the questionnaire in Appendix A). After both groups had completed the three day unit, which included the simulation on the last day fer the treatment group, the Classroom F l'. . “*IL' 66 POPULATION NON-RANDOM' ASSIGNMENT TREATMENT CONTROL GROUP GROUP PRETEST-AFFECTIVE PRETEST-AFFECTIVE AND COGNITIVE AND COGNITIVE REGULAR, IN-SERVICE REGULAR, IN-SERVICE TEACHERS MATCHED ON TEACHERS MATCHED ON BASIS OF COGNITIVE BASIS OF COGNITIVE PRETEST. PRETEST. SUB-GROUPS SUB-GROUPS FORMED FORMED SIMULATION NON-SIMULATION PRESENTATION PRESENTATION POSTI'EST-AFFECTIVE POSTTEST-AF FECT IVE ONLY ONLY I I , 1 ANALYSIS OF DATA FIGURE 6. Experimental Design of the Study. 67 Integration Inventory was handed out again and the subjects asked to re- take the test. They were permitted to take the test home with them for both the pretest and posttest. This was done for two reasons, it was felt that the tests would require about an hour to an hour and a half to complete and that class time should not be taken up for this task. __1 Also, it was felt that, if the tests were administered during class F124? .... “was" time, there might be undo pressure on the subjects to complete the test creating unwanted test anxieties and raising the possibility of erroneous responses, particularly on the Classroom Integration Inventory. The second group did not experience the simulation but, instead was given further lecture-discussion presentation utilizing television (keeping the modes of presentation fer the two groups as equal as pos— sible). Because the second group was not left untreated, as with most control groups, the second group is being referred to as a comparison group. Since the present investigator could not exert any control over registration for the two sections of the course, it became necessary to use a design for unmatched grOUps, or nonequivalency. In an effort to overcome some of the nonequivalencies between the groups, a matChing technique was used to construct the groups and sub- groups. The matching was done on the basis of the test scores on the ten items of the General Information Inventory relating to knowledge about hearing impairment. Several studies (dickstein and Dripps, 1958; Haring, 1956; and Proctor, 1967) have shown the existence of a positive 68 correlation between attitudes toward and knowledge about a particular object class. For this reason, it was felt that cognitive infermation was an appropriate matChing tool. Since the Treatment Group had only 14 regular classroom teachers with past teaChing experience, all 14 subjects were used and.were matched.with 14 from the Comparison Group. In addition to using cognitive information levels for matching, the groups were matched as closely as possible by: (1) sex (12 females and 2 males in eaCh group); (2) age (treatment group mean age was 27.0 and the comparison group mean age was 26.86); and (3) teaching experience (all sUbjects had at least one year of actual teaching experience). One additional factor which.nay have helped minimize intergroup and intragroup differences is the fact that all of the subjects had elected to take the course, voluntarily. There was, aparently, some interest in the problems of exceptional children and possibly a positive attitude toward integration had been developed prior to enrollment in the course. 69 Analysis of the Data The test of Significance of each of the research hypotheses is based upon an analysis of covariance, as described by MCNemar (1962). Because of the non-random groups used as the sample in the present study, the analysis of covariance was selected in order to make allow- ances for uncontrolled variables and for setting ferth the sampling er- ror adjustment which is needed in testing the statistical significance of the diference between "corrected" means (MtNemar, 1962). The methods for scoring the Classroom Integration Inventory and com- puting the realism score and the acceptance score have been discussed previously in this chapter. The tests were scored by hand by this in- vestigator and rechecked by two other persons. The scores were then recorded to be prepared for analysis. Since the General Information Inventory responses were on the same machine-scorable answer Sheets, a key was punched and these responses were also scored and checked by hand. The same check-recheck procedures were used with these items as was used with the Classroom Integration Inventory. The scores were computed and recorded to be prepared for analysis. The data gathered were analyzed in relation to the kind of treat- nent given each group. That is, to determine whether the simulation did cause a shift in teacher acceptance and realistic placement among 70 the subjects in the experimental group and if that shift was positive and significant as compared to the comparison group. The data were analyzed utilizing an analysis of covariance in a 2 X 2 factorial design as illustrated in Figure 7. "'1 i. rh (IDMPARISON TREATMENT HIG-I (DGNITIVE HIG‘I (DGNITIVE CDMPARISON TREATMENT LOW (DG‘IITIVE DOW OJGINITIVE Figure 7. Two-by-T‘wo Data Analysis Design. For the purposes of this study and in consideration of the research hypotheses of this study, acceptance of a hypothesis will be contingent upon reaching an alpha level of at least .05. CHAPTER FOUR ANALYSIS OF THE DATA This chapter will deal with the testing of the hypotheses listed in Chapter III and will include a brief discussion of the findings re- lated to each of the hypotheses. While the findings will be discussed in this chapter, conclusions and recommendations based on these findings will be presented in Chapter V. Testing of the_Hypotheses In an effort to determine the effectiveness of Simulation in changing teaChers' attitudes toward the integration of exceptional children into the regular classroom, a simulation of hearing loss was developed and its effectiveness was tested in terms of the hypotheses listed in Chap- ter III. The first hypothesis to be tested in the present study was: H-l. A simulation experience for in- service regular classroom teachers will not result in an attitudinal shift, toward the acceptance of acoustically impaired children in the regular classroom as compared to a corresponding lecture-discus- sion presentation. Table 2 reports the sums of squares, adjusted sums of squares, and 72 degrees of freedom which are required for an analysis of covariance and the F test for significance. The resultant F value of 7.82 is greater than the critical F value of 7.77 which is needed for significance at an alpha level of .01. Therefore, Null Hypothesis 1 may be rejected and support fer Alternate Hypothesis 1A is implied. The Comparison and.Treatment Group means, standard deviations, and ranges are reported in Table 3. Table 2. -- Analysis of Covariance for “Acceptance" for the Treatment and Comparison Groups. Total ‘Within Between F Sum of Squares 182.0 160.28 21.72 -- Adjusted Sum of Squares 181.04 137.89 43.15 7.82 Degrees of Freedom -- 25 l --- F= 7.77 at Alpha .01 The analysis of covariance and the F test value do not indicate the direction of the shifts for the groups. Examination of the means of the pretest-posttest gain scores indicates additional support fer Alternate Hypothesis 1A. The pretest-posttest gain score for the Treatment Group was + 1.50 and for the Comparison Group was - 1.00 (indicating a negative Shift on the part of the Comparison Group). The shift fer the Treatment Group was positive and away from the Comparison Group. This, then, adds additional support fer the Directional Alternate Hypothesis 1A, which was presented in Chapter III. 73 Table 3. -- Comparison of the Gain Score Mean, Range and Standard Devi- ation fer the Treatment and Comparison Groups. Acceptance Scores Realism Scores Group Mean S.D. Range Mean S.D. Range Treatment +1.50 2.98 -5 to +5 +1.43 2.41 -3 to +8 Comparison -l.00 3.37 -5 to +5 +.57 2.79 -4 to +5 The second hypothesis to be tested was: H-2 —.A Simulation experience for in- service regular classroom teachers will not result in an attitudinal Shift, toward the realistic place- ment of acoustically impaired Children, as compared to a corresponding lecture- discussion presentation. As with Hypotheses l-lA, an effort was made to determine the effec- tiveness of the simulation in changing teachers' attitudes toward the in- tegration of acoustically impaired Children into the regular classroom. This hypothesis relates to the teacher's ability to realistically deter- mine appropriate educational placement of an acoustically impaired Child. Table 4 reports the sum of squares, adjusted sum of squares, and de- grees of freedom which are required for an analysis of covariance and the F test of significance. The resultant F value of 2.65 is well below the critical F value of 4.24 which would be necessary to claim significance for rejecting the null hypothesis at an Alpha level of .05. The Null Hypothesis 2 cannot, therefore, be rejected. *1 V...“ -1!“- A k"! 0‘qu \ 74 Table 4. -- An Analysis of Covariance for "Realistic Placement (Realism)'l for the Treatment and Comparison Groups. Total Within Between F Sum of Squares 291.0 263.0 28.0 -- Adjusted Sum of Squares 288.27 260.67 27.6 2.65 Degrees of Freedom -- 25 l -- F= 4.24 at Alpha .05 The third hypothesis to be tested was: H-3 - In-service regular classroom teaChers with high initial levels of cognitive information about hearing and.hearing impairment will not have a greater shift in attitude, relative to the accept- ance of acoustically impaired children, as a result of a simula- tion experience as compared to regu- lar classroom teachers with low initial levels of cognitive informa- tion about hearing and hearing impair- ment. In the testing of this hypothesis, knowledge about hearing and hearing impairment are looked at as variables affecting the teachers' attitudes about the acoustically impaired child. The relation of the initial level of knowledge, as measured by the General Information Inventory, to the experience resulting from the simulation was considered. The measure used to look at attitude was the teacher's acceptance of the acoustically impaired child. 75 Table 5 reports the necessary sum of squares, adjusted sum of squares, and degrees of freedom.for conducting an analysis of covariance and the resultant P value. Table 6 reports the means, standard deviations, and ranges for the high cognitive and low cognitive subgroups within the Treatment Group. The F value of 10.15 is well above the critical F value of 7.77 needed for Significance at an Alpha level of .01. The null hypothesis is, therefore, rejected and support established for the Al- !I:. ' ' - “\f.‘“ffinifi‘ ternate Hypothesis 3A. Table 5. -- Analysis of Covariance for "Acceptance" fer the High and Low Cognitive Level Groups. Total Mflthin Between F Sum of Squares 182.0 176.86 5.14 ~- Adjusted Sum of Squares 181.04 137.89 43.15 10.15 Degrees of Freedom -- 25 l -- F= 7.77 at Alpha .01 While the P value is sufficient for rejection of the null hypothesis, it does not indicate the direction of the shift for the groups. An ex- amination of the means of the pretest-posttest gain scores shows that the high cognitive level subgroup had a mean gain of + 1.86 and the low cog- nitive level group had a mean gain of + 1.14. This, then, would indicate a shift in a positive direction and give additional support to the direc- tional Alternate Hypothesis 3A. 76 Table 6. -- Comparison of the Gain Score Mean, Range and Standard Devi- ation for the High Cognitive and Low Cognitive subgroups Within the Treatment Group. Realism Scores Acceptance Scores Group Mean S.D. Range Mean S.D. Range High +1.00 1.29 0 to +5 +1.86 1.77 [-1 to +2 Low +1.50 3.50 -5 to +6 +1.14 3.98 -3 to +8 The fourth hypothesis to be tested was: H-4 -Inservice regular classroom teachers with high initial levels of cognitive information about hearing and hearing impairment will not have a greater attitudinal shift, relative to the realistic placement of acoustically impaired children, as a result of a simulation experience as compared to in-service regular classroom.teaChers with low initial levels of cognitive infor- mation. As with Hypotheses 3—3A, Hypotheses 4-4A look at the relation of knowledge to the experience resulting from the simulation. This hypoth- esis relates to the teacher's ability to realistically place an acous- tically impaired child into an ”appropriate" educational setting. Table 7 reports the necessary sums of squares, adjusted sums of squares, and degrees of freedom for conducting an analysis of covariance and the resultant F value. The F value of .012 is well below the critical F value of 4.24 necessary for significance at an Alpha level of .05. As a result, the Null Hypothesis 4 cannot be rejected. ruin—"ICI— x7 77 Table 7. -- Analysis of Covariance for ”Realistic Placement (Realism)" fer the High and Low Cognitive Level Groups. Total Within Between F Sum of Squares 291.0 288.72 2.28 -- Adjusted Sum of Squares 288.27 288.13 .14 .012 Degrees of Freedom -- 25 l -- F= 4.24 at Alpha .05 Basically, this study set out to test four hypotheses which dealt with the attitudes of regular classroom teachers toward the integration of exceptional children into the regular classroom. The feur hypotheses relate to the two specific dimensions of attitude toward integration that are measured by the Classroom Integration Inventory: (1) acceptance of exceptional children by regular classroom teachers and (2) the real- istic placement of exceptional children into educational settings (as compared to similar judgements made by a panel of experts). Hypotheses l-lA and 3-3A deal with the former dimension (acceptance) while Hypoth- eses 2-2A and 4-4A deal with the latter (realism). To summarize, the results of the study, given in terms of the above dimensions, are shown in Table 8. The pertinent data are listed along ‘with the indication and level of significance or non-significance. 7! “final—wax n .‘A ' .‘ n vw n E'. 78 Table 8. -- Summation of results of analysis of covariance for all re— searCh hypotheses. Actual F Critical F Significance Reject Null Hl-lA 7.82 7.77 .01 Yes HZ-ZA 2.65 4.24 -- (.05) No 5-1 H3-3A 10.15 7.77 .01 Yes a I H4—4A .012 4.24 -- (.05) No I ‘I Discussion As has been mentioned above, Hypotheses l-lA and 3-3A deal with the acceptance of exceptional children (Specifically, acoustically impaired Children) by regular classroom teachers. An examination of the results of the analyses shows that, in both cases, the null hypotheses were re- jected and support for the directional alternate hypotheses was implied. .Additional examination of the pretest-posttest gain scores Show that the shifts were in a positive direction for the appropriate groups. Table 9 summarizes the information relating to the rejection of Null Hypothe- ses l and 3 and the support of the Directional Alternate Hypotheses 1A and.3A. 79 Table 9. -- Supportive data for the rejection of Null Hypotheses l and 3 and the support of the Directional Alternate Hypotheses 1A and 3A. Actual Cfitibal Alpha Pretest- Direction F F Level Pos ttest Gain H1 7.82 7.77 .01 + 1.50 Positive H3 10.15 7.77 .01 + 1.86 Positive As has already been mentioned, Hypotheses Z-ZA and 4-4A deal with the teacher's ability to place acoustically impaired children in appro- priate educational settings, when compared with the judgements of a panel of experts in the field of Special education. An examination of the results of the analyses shows that, in neither case, did the F value reach the critical F levels for significance at at least the .05 level and, therefbre, neither null hypothesis could be re- jected. While neither null hypothesis was rejected, an examination of the pretest-posttest gain score means shows that the shifts for the appro- priate groups, while not significant, did move in a positive direction. Table 10 summarizes the information relating to the Null Hypotheses 2 and 4. 8O Table 10. -- Pertinent data relating to Null Hypotheses 2 and 4. Actual Critical Alpha Pretest- Direction F F Level Posttest Gain H2 2.65 4.24 .05 + 1.43 Positive H4 .012 4.24 .05 + 1.00 Positive In final summary, it should be noted that, after having analyzed the gathered data, the simulation experience seems to have been most ef« fective in modifying teachers' attitudes toward greater acceptance of acoustically impaired children. While the Simulation experience seems to have had some effect on a teacher's ability to place acoustically impaired children in appropriate settings, no conclusions can be drawn because of the lack of significance in either case. CHAPTER FIVE SUIVIMARY AND CDNCLUSIONS SWWMUOI The purpose of the present study was to determine whether the at- titude of regular classroom.teaChers toward the integration of acous- tically impaired children could be modified through having experienced a simulation of hearing loss. The simulation consisted of a videotaped presentation about the Zwislocki Acoustic Bridge. Tha audio track was passed through a Gen- eral Radio Corporation Multi-Filter and, as a result, certain pre-de- termined frequencies in the voice of the teacher were eliminated. Three different types of hearing loss were thus Simulated 0n the video- tape--a flat loss of about 22 dB; a gradual high frequency loss with frequencies above 2000 hertz effectively eliminated; and a precipitous high frequency loss with frequencies above 800 hertz effectively elimi- nated. The Simulation videotape was presented to a class of regular and special education majors enrolled in an introductory course on the ”Ed- ucation of Exceptional Children." Its purpose was to simulate the con- dition of the acoustically impaired child in the regular classroom. It was this experience which was being considered as a strategy for modi- fying, in a more accepting direction, the attitudes of regular 82 classroom teachers toward the integration of exceptional Children, in general, and acoustically impaired children, specifically, into the regular classroom. The pOpulation consisted of the members of two sections of "Edu- cation of Exceptional Children" during the 1971 Summer term in the College of Education at Michigan State University. From this popula- tion, twenty-eight in-service, regular classroom teachers were chosen as the research sample. There were feurteen subjects chosen from each section and the subjects were matched on the basis of cognitive pre- test scores from the General Information Inventory, a cognitive test of knowledge about exceptionality. With the Treatment group, the f0urteen subjects were divided into two subgroups, again based upon the General Infermation Inventory pre- test scores. The subgroups were a High Initial Cognitive Infermation Level Subgroup and a Low Initial Cognitive Information Level Subgroup. The median score for the total group was used as the dividing line for setting up these sUbgroups. To measure attitude change, the Classroom Integration Inventory was selected as the test instrument. The Classroom Integration Inven- tory has two dimensions to it which makes it a useful tool for the needs of this study. Utilizing two separate scoring techniques, an acceptance score and a realism.score can be obtained. The acceptance score is an indicator of how accepting a teacher is toward exceptional children, in general, and toward exceptional Children in the regular 83 classroom, Specifically. The realism.score indicates a teaCher's ability to place an exceptional Child in an ”appropriate" educational setting, as compared with similar judgements made by a panel of experts in the field of Special education. Both test instruments, the General Information Inventory and the Classroom Integration Inventory, were administered prior to the three- day experience and following the simulation, which was presented on the last day of the three-day experience, the Classroom Integration Inven- tory was readministered. The pretest-posttest gain scores on the Classroom Integration Inventory were used in the analysis and to de- termine significance. An analysis of covariance and F test were used to analyze the gathered data and to test fer significance of results. One analysis compared the gain scores of the Treatment Group (which experienced the Simulation) to the gain scores of the Comparison Group. A.Sec0nd analysis compared the High Initial Cognitive Infermation Sub-Group (in the Treatment Group) gain scores to the Low Initial Cognitive Infer- mation Sub-Group (also in the Treatment Group). A.minimum alpha level of .05 was used as the critical level of significance fer rejecting the null hypotheses. The hypotheses which were tested in this study were: 84 p5 H-l. A simulation experience fer in- service regular classroom teachers will not result in an attitudinal shift, toward the acceptance of acou- stically impaired children in the regular classroom as compared to a corresponding lecture-discussion presentation. H-Z. A simulation experience for in- service regular classroom.teaChers will not result in an attitudinal Shift, toward the realistic place- ment of acoustically impaired child- ren, as compared to a corresponding lecture-discussion presentation. H-3. In-service regular classroom teachers with high levels of cognitive information about hearing and hearing impairment will not have a greater shift in attitude, relative to the ac- ceptance of acoustically impaired children, as a result of a simulation experience as compared to regular classroom teachers with low initial levels of cognitive information about hearing and hearing impairment. w H-4. In-service regular classroom teachers with high initial levels of cognitive information about hearing and hearing impairment will not have a greater attitudinal Shift, relative to the realistic placement of acoustically impaired children, as a result of a simu- lation experience as compared to in-service regular classroom teachers with low initial levels of cognitive information. The analysis of the data supported the rejection of Null Hypotheses 1 and 3 and support was implied for Alternate Hypotheses 1A and 3A. 85 Alternate Hypotheses 1A and 3A were directional hypotheses which indi— cated that the shifts would be in a positive direction. In addition to implying support for the Alternate Hypotheses, further support was gained upon analysis of the direction of movement of the scores. The scores for both grOups and for all of the individuals within each of the groups moved in a positive direction. Several difficulties were encountered during the progress of this study. First, the sample size was limited to 14 per group because of the limited number of subjects in the Treatment Group. With only 14 regular, in-service classroom teachers in the Treatment Group, it was necessary to limit the Comparison Group to the same number. A second difficulty which was encountered was the problem of standardizing the output of the sound track of the Simulation. A sound level meter was used in an attempt to maintain a constant output level. Despite this, the acoustics of the classroom in which the simulation was presented, presented numerous problems, e.g. - dead spots. Also, at least once during the presentation, the University grounds crew passed by the win- dow of the classroom with a power mower that tended to drown out the audio. 3 I 86 Conclusions Based on the analysis of the data, the following conclusions can be supported: 1. The simulation experience was effective in modifying the at- titudes of in-service, regular classroom teachers toward the acous- tically impaired child. 2. The Simulation experience was most effective in creating a more accepting attitude toward the acoustically impaired child, on the part of in-service, regular classroom teachers. 3. There was no change noted in the teachers' ability to realis- tically place acoustically impaired children into the most appropriate educational setting. 4. In-service, regular classroom teachers with high initial levels of cognitive information about hearing and hearing impairment had Sig- nificantly greater changes in attitude, in terms of acceptance of the acoustically impaired child, than in-service, regular classroom teachers with low initial levels of cognitive information. While the above conclusions are self-explanatory, some further dis- cussion is warranted to support these conclusions. The analysis of the data Showed that the two null hypotheses which were concerned with the teacher's acceptance of the acoustically impaired child (Hypotheses l and 3), had been rejected. Support was gained, therefore, for the 87 Alternate Hypotheses (1A and 3A) dealing with acceptance. While the analysis showed a change in the attitude (acceptance), it was necessary to examine the mean gain scores to determine the direction of the Shift. This subsequent examination showed that the Shifts for the appropriate groups (the Treatment Group in Hypothesis 1 and the High Initial Cog- nitive Information Level Sub-Group in Hypothesis 3) were in a positive direction. Thus, it may be said that the simulation was effective in creating a more accepting attitude on the part of in-service, regular classroom teachers toward the acoustically impaired child. The data analysis relating to the two hypotheses which were con- cerned with the teaCher's ability to realistically place an acousti- cally impaired child into an appropriate educational setting (Hypotheses 2 and 4) showed that the shifts which occured as a result of the simu- lation were not sufficient to be Significant and, therefore, the null hypotheses could not be rejected. The findings of this study, related to realistic placement, sub- stantiate those of Haring (1956) and Proctor (1967). Both of these studies used different methods of presentation of the information on exceptionality and neither found that, on the basis of experience or information, there was a change (improvement) in the classroom teacher's ability to realistically determine the most advantageous placement for the exceptional children described in the Classroom Integration Inventory. Both Haring (1956) and Proctor (1967) concluded that: 88 whether or not teachers had experience with exceptional children in their classroom seemed to make little dif- ference in their ability to be more accurate in their judgment con- cerning the placement of these child- ren (Haring, 1956). A review of the results of the three studies which used the Classroom Integration Inventory shows that there is apparently little in or no relationship between experience and/or knowledge and a teacher's ability to realistically place exceptional children in appropriate educational settings. A reason for this lack of evidence might be the inability of the test instrument (the Classroom Integration Inventory) to measure what it was designed to measure. When Proctor (1967) reestablished the scoring key, she reported a large disparity in inter-judge agreement on the most appropriate educational placement. She shows a range of agreement from 73 percent to 53 percent (Proctor, 1967). With a panel of experts having such difficulty determining appro- priate placement, it seems that the task might be far too difficult for a classroom teacher. In summary, the Classroom Integration Inventory may not be a valid measure of a teacher's ability to realistically place exceptional children in appropriate educational settings. It can also be concluded, on the basis of the data, that teacher 89 attitudes toward exceptional children can be changed as a result of experiencing a simulation of the handicap involved. There are, though, some words of caution.which must be voiced with regard to the changing of teachers' attitudes through simulation or by any other means. The instruments used for measuring attitudes and,/v attitude change was a paper and pencil test. Such an instrument re- quires only one form of response on the part of the teacher; that is, a low level written response in the form of selecting one choice of five givens. ‘Caution must be observed in drawing conclusions on ,,/' attitude change solely on the basis of verbal response. verbal behavior is only one indicator of attitude (Zimbardo and Ebbesen, 1970). There is need for more and better indicators of attitude in order to make some wide-ranging conclusions about the effectiveness of one method or another in changing an individual's attitude. Implications for Future Research As mentioned above, there are other dimensions of behavior related to attitude than verbal behavior. In order to determine the effective- ness of one method or another in changing attitudes, it is necessary to consider several of the dimensions of attitude-related behavior. 90 One such possibility would be in the form of a longitudinal study observing teacher behavior in the classroom with an exceptional child. Procedures similar to those used in this study (simulation) or by Haring (1956) (workshop--i.e.-in-service training) could be used but with the addition of several Observational evaluations of the teacher's behavior in the classroom. r —- -— 1‘7ng 1' _,—.i Another alternative would be to develop a new simulated situation into which a teach would be placed. This Situation would require the teacher to perform for a given period of time in a classroom with an exceptional Child integrated as a part of the class. While this type of evaluation may not provide information on behavior in the same way ‘Vf that actual experience "on-the-job" would, it would still provide a wider range of response to the Situation than does a written test. BIBLIOGRAPHY (N 10. ll. 12. 13. 14. BIBLIOGRAPHY Allen, D. and R. Gross. ”Microteaching.” National Education Asso- ciation Journal, 55:25-26 (1955). Allport, G.W. "Attitudes." In Murchison, C. (ed.), Handbook of Social Psychology, 1935, Chapter 17, 798-812. Baldwin, Willie Kate. ”The Educable Mentally Retarded Child in the Regular Grades.” Exceptional Children, 25:106-108 (1958). Bell, A.H. "Attitudes of Selected Rehabilitation WOrkers and Other Hospital Employees Toward the Physically Disabled." Psychological Report, 10:183-186 (1962). Bennett and Perstch as cited by Stephen Lilly, "Special Education: A Teapot in a Tempest.” Exceptional Children, September 1970. Blackman, Leonard S. and Howard L. Sparks. "What is Special About Special Education Revisited: The Mentally Retarded." Exceptional Children, 31:242-247 (1965). Blatt, B. "Characteristics of Children Who Are Mentally Retarded in Various Classroom Situations.” Pennsylvania State Review of Educa- tional Research, 8:1 (1956). Bond, J. Using Simulation Techniques to Change Attitudes of Educa- tion Majors TowardiProfeSSional Course Objectives. Monmouth, Oregon: Oregon State System Of Higher Education, 1965. Boyd, G.R. "C1assroom.AdjuStment of the Underchosen Child Through Changes in Teachers' Attitudes and Behavior.” Troy, Alabama: Cg; gperative Research Program 672, Troy State College, 1943. Burke, Philip J. and Daniel D. Sage. "The Unorthodox Use of A Sim- ulation Instrument.” Simulation and Games, 2:155-171 (1970). Campbell, Donald T. and Julian C. Stanley. ”Experimental and Quasi- Experimental Designs for Research on Teaching.” Handbook of Research on Teaching, Edited by N.L. Gage. Chicago: Rand MCNally and Co., 1963. Chapman, J. ”Simulation in the Military." Simulation Models for Education. Edited by N. Fattu and S. Elam. Bloomington, Indiana: P51 DEIta Kappa, 1965. Choate, Mary A. "Children's Aspirations, Attitudes, and Concepts.” Oregon University Curriculum Bulletin, 14:184 (1958). Christoplos, Florence and Paul Renz. ”A Critical Examination of Special Education Programs.” The Journal of Special Education, 3: 371-379 (1969). 15. 16. 17. l8. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 92 Cormany, Robert B. "Returning Special Education Students to Regular Classes.” Personnel and Guidance Journal, 48:641-646 (1970). Cowen, L. , Rita Underberg, and R.T. Verrille. "The Development and Testing of an Attitude to Blindness Scale." Journal of Social Psy- chology, 48:297-304 (1958). Crawford, James R. Simulation.hethods and Medel Design. Santa Monica, Calif.: Systems DeveIOpment C0rp., 1963. Cruickshank, D. ”Simulation: New Direction in Teacher Preparation.” Phi Delta Kappan, 48:23-24 (1966). Cruickshank, D., gt, a1. TeachingProblemS Laboratoryt_Simulation Director's Guide. ChIEago: SCience ResearChTAssoc., 1967. Cruickshank, W.M. and 6.0. Johnson (eds.) Education of Exceptional Children and Youth. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.:EPrentice-Hall, 1967. Cummins, R.E. ”Research Insights into the Relationship Between Teachers' Acceptance Attitudes, Their Role Concepts, and Students' Acceptance Attitudes.” Journal of Educational Research, 53:197-198 (1960). Dawson, R. ”Simulation in the Social Sciences." In Simulation in Social Sciences. H. Guetzkow (ed.). Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: PTentice-Hall, 1962. Dickstein, Jean and Elaine Dripps. ”An Analysis of Attitudes of Acceptance-Rejection Towards Exceptional Children.” Unpublished Masters Thesis, Boston University, 1958. D011, Geraldine L. "Principles and Practices of Special Education.” Cerebral Palsy Journal, 28:3-6 (1967). Dunn, Lloyd, ”Special Education for Mildly Retarded-Is Much of it Justifiable?” Exceptional Children, 35:5-24 (1968). Fine, Marvin J. "Attitudes of Regular and Special Class Teachers Toward the Educable Mentally Retarded Child.” Exceptional Children, 33:429-430 (1967). Flanders, N.A. ”Teacher Influence, Pupils' Attitudes, and Achieve- ment." U.S. Dept. of Health, Education, and.welfare, Cooperative Research Monogrgph, 12:49-65 (1965). Flynn, Tim and Lynda Flynn. "The Effects of a Parttime Special Education Program on the Adjustment of EMR Students.” Excgptional Children, 36:680-81 (1970). 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 93 Frampton, M.E. and Elena D. Gall (eds.) Special Education for the Exceptiongl Children. Boston: Porter Sargent, 1955. Green, B.F. ”Attitude Measurement." In G.Lindzey (ed.) Handbook of Social PS cholo , V01. 1. Reading, Mass.: Addison-wesley PfiBIisfiing Co., 195%. Greenlaw, P. et. gt, Business Simulation In Industrial and Uni- versity_EducaEIbn. Englewood.Cliffs, N13}: PrenficeTHall, 1962. Grosenick, Judith K. ”Assessing the Reintegration 0f Exceptional Children into Regular Classes.” Teachinngxceptional Children, 2:113-119 (1970). ‘ GustafSon, Kent L. "Simulation 0f.Anxiety Situations and Its Re- sultant Effect on Anxiety and Classroom Interaction of Student TeaChers.” Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Michigan State Uni- versity, 1969. Harford, Earl. "How we Hear." Gordon N. Stowe and Associates, 1964. Haring, Norris. ”A Study of the Attitudes of Classroom TeaChers Toward Exceptional Children." Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Syracuse university, 1956. Henphill, John K. , Daniel E Griffiths, and Norman Frederiksen. Administrative PerfOrmance and Personality. New York: Columbia Uhiversiiy Press, 1962. “ ' ' Hicks, Doin E. "Conparison Profiles of Rubella and Non-Rubella Deaf Children." American Annals of thetDeaf, 115:86-92 (1970). Jansen, Megens, et. gtt_ "Is Special Education Necessary? - Can This Program POSSIEly Be Reduced?" Journal of Learning Disabil- ities, 3:11-16 (1970). ' Johnson, 6.0. "A Study of the Social Position of Mentally Handi- capped Children in the Regular Grades." American Journal of Men- tal Deficiency, 55:60-89 (1950). ., "Special Education fer the Mentally Retarded-A Para- doxT“' Exceptional Children, 29:62-69 (1962). and S.A. Kirk. "Are Mentally Handicapped Children Segregatedin the Regular Grades?" Exceptional Children, 17:65-68 1950). ‘ ‘ I 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 94 Johnson, M. "Simulation of Classroom variables By Computer." American Educational ResearCh Journal, 4:289-293 (1967). Kagan, N., et. al. Studies In Human Interaction. East Lansing, Mich.: Michigan—State—Uhiversity, 1967. Kersh, Bert. Classroom Simulation: A New Dimension in Teacher Edu- cation. Mbnmouth, Oregon: Oregon State System of Higher Educafion, 1963 . Classroom Simulation: Further Studies On Dimensions of Realism. Monmouth, Oregon: Oregon State System or Higher Edu- cation, 1965. Kirk, Samuel A. "Basic Facts and.Principles Uhderiying Special Ed- ucation." In The Education of Exceptional Children, NSSE, Forty- ninth Yearbook, Part II. Chicago: UhiverSity of Chicago Press, 1950. . Educating Exceptional Children. Boston: Houghton- Mifflin Co. , 1972. Kough, Jack and Robert F. DeHaan. Helping_Students with Special Needs. Chicago: Science Research Associates, 1957. I Lapp, E.R. "A Study of the Social Adjustment of Slow Learning Child- ren Who were.Assigned Part-Time Regular Classes." American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 62:254-262 (1957). If I " Likert, REA. "A.Technique fer the Measurement of Attitudes." At; chives 9f Psychology, 22:5-43 (1932). Lilly, Stephen M. "Special Education: A.Teapot in a Tempest." Exceptionaerhildren, 37:43-49 (1970). Mackie, R.P. "Spotlighting Advances in Special Education." Excep- tional Children, 32:77-81 (1965). ., HgM. Williams, and Patricia P. Hunter. "Statistics of Special Education for Exceptional Children and Youth, 1957-1958." In Biennial Survey of Education in the united States,_l956-1958, Chapter 5, OE-35048} U.S. Department of Health, EdUcation, and wai- fare, 1963. Manske, A.J. "The Reflections of Teacher Attitudes in the Attitudes of Their Pupils." Contributions to Education, No. 702. New York: Columbia University Pféss, 1936. I “T SS. 56. 57. 58. S9. 60. 61. 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. 95 Mason, A.H. Effects of Attitudes of High School Teachers of Social Studies Upon Attitudes of TheiiiPupils. Studies in Higher Education XLV, Further StudiesiinlAttitudES, Series V. H.H. Remmers (ed.). Lafayette, Indiana: Purdue University Press, 1942. McGee, Donald I. "The Benefits of Educating Deaf Children with Hearing Children." Teaching Exceptional Children, 2:133-137 (1970). MCNemar, Quinn. Psychological Statistics. New York: John Wiley 8 Sons, Inc. 1962. Meyerson, L. "SomatOpsychology of Physical Disability." In PS - chology of Exceptional Children and Youth, WIM. Cruickshank (ed.I. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: PrentiEe-Hall, 1963. MOffitt, J.C. "An Analysis of Race Prejudice." Educational Admini- stration and Supervising, 18:641-648 (1932). .Mutimer, Dorothy D. and RObert A. Rosemier. "Behavior PrOblems of Children as Viewed by Teachers and the Children Themselves." Journal of Consulting Psychology, 31:583-587 (1967). NEA. Programs for Handicapped Children. Washington, D.C.: NEA ResearCh Division, 1967. Nelson, Calvin C. and Leo J. Schmidt. "The Question of the Efficacy of Special Classes." Exceptional Children, 37:381-384 (1971). NCSEA. The Skelly Wright Decision and Its Implications for Education. washington, D.C.:TNational Council of StateiEducation Associations, 1968. Perstch, C.F. A Comparative Study of the Progress of SUb-Normal Pppils in the Grades and in SpeCial Classes. New York? TeaChers College of’Columbia, Bureau of delications, 1936. Cited by S.A. Kirk, "Research in Education." In H.A. Stevens and R. Heber (eds), Mental Retardation. Syracuse, N.Y.: Syracuse University Press, 1964. Porter, Rutherford. "The Significance of Similarities Between Education and Special Education." Training School Bulletin, 68: 109-114 (1971). Proctor, Doris I. "An Investigation of the Relationships Between Knowledge of Exceptional Children, Kind.and Amount of Experience, and Attitudes Toward Their Classroom Integration." Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Michigan State Univ., 1967. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. 76. 77. 78. 79. 96 Remmers, H.H. "Social Attitudes." In D.H. Fryer and E.R. Henry (eds.). Handbook of.Applied Psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1950. Roeher, G.A. "Significance of Public Attitudes in Rehabilitation of the Disabled." Rehabilitation Literature, Reprint DR-28, National Society of Crippled Children and Adults, 22:66-72 (1961). Rubella Surveillance Report. Atlanta, Georgia: National Communi- cable DiSeases Cénter, 1969. Sage, Daniel D. The Development of Simulation Materials for Re- search and Training in.Administration of’Special Education. Final Report, ProjectiNo. 6-2466. MESEington,‘DZC.: U.S. Office of Edu— cation, 1967. Sage, Daniel D. and Edward W. Sontag. The Development and Evalua- tion of Reality-Based Simulation Materials for the Training of SpeCial EdfiCatiOn Administrators in State Departments of Education. Final Report, SpeCial Pioject Grant OEG-0-70-129OTT603). Washing- ton, D.C.: U.S. Office of Education, 1970. Sherif, M. "Some Needed Concepts in the Study of Social Attitudes." In J.C. Peatman and E.L. Hartley (eds.), FestsChrift for Gardner Mutphy. New York: Harper-Row, 1960. and Cantril, H. "The Psychology of.Attitudes." Psy- chological Review, 52:295-319 (1945). Siegel, Ernest. Special Education in the Regular Classroom. New York: John Day Co., Inc., 1969. Siller, J. and A. Chapman. "Factorial Structures and Correlates of the Attitude Toward Disabled Persons Scale." Educational Psy- chological Measurement, 24:831-839 (1964). Stewart, L. "A Survey of Business Games." Simulation and Gaming: A Symposium. A. Newgarden (ed.). New York: AmeriCan Management Association Report #55, 1961. Tansey, P.J. and Derick unwin. Simulation and Gamingtin Education. London: Methuen Educational Ltd., 1969. Tolor, Alexander, et. at, "Teacher's Attitudes Toward Children's Behavior Revisited:"' Journal of Educational Psychology, 58:175-180 (1967). Thurstone, Thelma G. An Evaluation of Education of Mentally Handi- capped Children in Special Classes and in Regular Classes. Cooperative ResearCh Project, OE-SAE 6456. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Office of 80. 81. 82. 83. 84. 85. 86. 87. 88. 89. 90. 97 Education, Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1959. Vinacke, W.E. The Psychology of Thinking, New York: McGraw-Hill Co., 1952. Vlcek, C. "Assessing the Effect and Transfer Value of 3 Classroom Simulator Technique." unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Michigan State University, 1965. warren, Earl. Brown vs. Board of Education. washington, D.C.: U.S. Supreme Court, 1954. wasser, Laurence. The Deucational Science of Cognitive Style: An Introduction. Bloomfield Hills, MiChigan: Oakland Community Col- lege Press, 1971. Wilson, Earl D. and Dewaine Alcorn. "Disability Simulation and Development of Attitudes Toward the Exceptional." Journal of Special Education, 3:303-307 (1969). WOOden, Harley 2. "What is Special About Special Education: The Child Who IS Deaf." Exceptional Children, 19:179-182 (1953). Wrightstone, J.W. "Studies of Orthopedically Handicapped Pupils." Exceptional Children, 23:4 (1957). Yuker, H.E., J.R. Block, and W;J. Campbell. "A.Scale to Measure Attitudes Toward Disabled Persons." Albertson, New York: Human Resources Foundation, 1960. Zaun, C. and M. Schroeder. "The Driver Trainer: A Teaching Machine." Journal of Secondary Education, 37:112-116 (1962). Zimbardo, Philip and Ebbe B. Ebbesen. Influencing Attitudes and Changing Behavior. Reading, Mass.: Addison-weSIey Publishing Co., 1970. Zuckerman, David W. and Robert E. Horn. The Guide to Simulation Games for Education and Training. Cambridge, Mass.: InfOrmatibn Resources, Inc., 1970. APPENDIX A 99 PERSONAL QUESTIONNAIRE The following questionnaire is designed to give us certain information about you and your background as a teacher. We request that you answer all applicable questions as thoroughly as possible. 1. NAME 2. SEX: M F 3. STUDENT NUMBER: 4. AGE___ 5. MAJOR: (i.e. - secondary ed}, special ed., etc.) 6. Are you presently employed as a teacher? _____YES NO \1 If yes, how long have you been teaching? (check appropriate box): 0-1 years; 2-5 years; more than 5 years. 8. If no, have you student taught yet? YES NO. 9. On what level do you now teach (or, if you have only student taught, on what level did you student teach)? Pre-School Primary Middle School ______ Junior High (7-9) Senior High (10-12) Post-Secondary (This would include any area outside senior high school - such as voc-ed, community college, etc.) 10. Have you ever had a handicapped or exceptional child in one of your classes? YES NO 11. If yes, what was the nature of the handicap? (i.e. - Mentally re- tarded, crippled, hearing impaired, etc.). 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 100 If you have had contact with an exceptional or handicapped child in your classroom, how many years have you had such contact? 1-2 years; 3-5 years; more than 5 years. Do you have anyone in your family who might be considered handi- capped or exceptional? YES NO What is the nature of the handicap? A descriptive phrase such as hearing impaired or visually impaired will be sufficient here. Have you.had any other form of contact with a handicapped or ex- ceptional child or adult? (such as a neighbor, an associate at work, a friend, etc.). YES NO If yes, what was the nature of the contact? If yes, how long have you had this contact? 1-2 years; 3-5 years; more than 5 years. Why are you enrolled in Education 424? Finally, if you were teaching in the public schools, and were told by your principal that this coming Fall your class would include a Child who was exceptional or handicapped and would require some special accomodations for the class, what would your reaction be? APPENDIX B TEST PACKAGE Teachers are ordinarily faced with a wide variety of problems, arising from the many different kinds of students they work with each cay. On the following pages you will find brief descriptions of the behavior of a number of exceptional Children. In each case, you are to indicate how you would prefer to handle the situation if the decision were entirely up to you. INSTRUCTIONS: Read each item and mark the correspondingly number space on the answer sheet as follows: 1A If you feel that you could handle such a student in your regular classroom without any fUndamental change in your present procedures. B If you feel that you could handle such a student in your regular classroom provided advice from a specialist or con- sultant was occasionally made available to you whenever you felt a need for such aid in dealing with some particular problem, C If you feel that you could handle such a student in your regular classroom provided there was a full-tine specialist available at your school who could provide supplementary training for the student and frequent consultation with you. D If you feel that suCh a student would benefit most by being assigned to a Special class or school. B If you feel that such a child cannot be handled.profitably within the context of regular or special public education. Mark each item clearly, filling the space between the dotted lines on the special answer sheet. Please do not make any marks in this booklet. IN REGULAR CLASSROOM WITH PART-TIME AID WITH FULL-TIME AID IN SPECIAL CLASS OR SCHOOL NOT FOR PUBLIC EDUCATION WP??? 1. Alfred is defiant and stubborn, likely to argue with the teacher, be willfully disobedient, and otherwise interfere with normal classroom discipline. 2. Barbara weras thick glasses, and her eye-balls jerk spasmodically from side to side; she can't see the blackboard very well, and reads poorly. 3. Chuck can get about only in a.wheel chair; someone must move it for him, or carry him.in their arms, because he is unable to control his limbs. mane? 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 103 IN REGULAR CLASSROOM WITH PART-TIME HELP RUTH FULL-TIME HELP IN SPECIAL CLASS OR SCHOOL NOT FOR PUBLIC EDUCATION Donald is six years old and does not speak very much; what he does say is indistinct and childish, with.namy missing or incorrect sounds. Earl is eight and wears coWboy boots to class because he hasn't learned to tie his own Shoelaces; he is generally cheerful and.well- behaved, but talks very little and is incapable of following any but the most simple directions. Florence is immature and oversensitive, likely to burst into tears at the slightest provocation. When Alice wears her hearing aid She hears as any other youngster; her voice sounds flat and hollow, and is somewhat unpleasant to hear. Suzy frequently gets so excited She loses control of herself and.wets the floor. Ruth is very muCh like other eleven-year-olds in most respects but, occasionally, during the day, a rhythmical quiver will pass over her face and she becomes totally oblivious fer a few seconds. Roger's face was severely disfigured in an auto accident; although he is completely recovered physically, the surgeons do not expect to be able to make his appearance more acceptable for mahy years. Alan wears a leg brace and walks with the aid of crutches; he gets along quite well by himself though and ordinarily needs no help fromIanyone. Bernard is a bully, given to teasing other children and provoking fights with them. Cora is supposed to have a hearing loss, but She seems to hear all right when she sits at the right end of the front row of seats. Debby cannot use bathroom facilities unless someone is there to help her; She is perfectly capable of making her needs known in ample time to avoid accidents. Clara has a noticeable scar on her upper lip; her speech seems to be coming through her nose and she is hard to understand. Dotty is eight; she has difficulty following the class and doesn't seem to learn to read at all. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 104 IN REGULAR CLASSROOM WITH PART-TIME AID RUTH FULL-TIME AID IN SPECIAL CLASS OR SCHOOL NOT FOR PUBLIC EDUCATION Eight-year-old Edward sucks his thumb all the time, apparently in- different to reactions of parents, teachers, or other children. Every few weeks, without any warning, Stella will have a violent physical convulsion during which She may bite her tongue or lose control of her sphincters; after several minutes She returns to consciousness. Sylvia's height is grotesque; she towers over every other child in elementary school and.wears adult-Size clothes. Flora has neither bladder nor bowel control and must be taken to the bathroom at frequent intervals. David squints through his eyeglasses, even when he sits at the front of the room, and cannot read the blackboard or his book quite as rapidly as many of the other children. Occasionally Edward will repear a sound two or three times before he seems able to go on; he speaks when called upon, but does not volunteer much. Chuck doesn't seem to catch on to things as quickly as most, and needs to have things explained over and over afain; eventually, though, he appears to learn everything the others do even though it has taken longer. Doris is Slow, absent-minded, and a daydreamer; she seems usually quiet and withdrawn, avoids others, and is inhibited and restrained in her behavior. Every hour or so Henry stares upwards at the ceiling for several seconds and loses consciousness; he has been like this for several years but is otherwise developing normally. Fred can feel the vibrations of loud music from a radio or phono- graph, knows when a door has been slammed, but does not hear Speech unless it is shouted. Greg tires easily and needs frequent opportunities to rest; excessive stimulation or excitement must also be avoided. Harold is a capable student but has a physical defect which appears to evoke laughter, ridicule, avoidance, and rejection from the other children. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 105 IN REGULAR CLASSROOM WITH PART-TIME AID WITH FULL-TIME AID IN SPECIAL CLASS OR SCHOOL NOT FOR.PUBLIC EDUCATION Irv is sexually precocious; masturbates in class, uses Obscene lan- guage, and has made advances to several girls in his class. Jane can tell the direction from which the sunshine enters her classroom; She cannot read the letters in an ordinary book. Albert does not pronounce all of his speech sounds correctly, but can be understood. Betty is only a little over seven but she can read the fifth grade reader very well; however, her handwriting is poor and She is about average in most other things. Chester is deceitful, tells lies, and cheats in school and at play; he has been involved in several thefts, and is a persistent truant. Generally Speaking, Everett can control his bladder and bowel, al- though he is likely to have an occasional accident. Jerry does perfectly good work as long as he is left alone; he be- comes extremely tense and anxious, however, whenever an adult speaks to him. Virginia rubs and blinks her eyes occasionally when reading, and seems to find it difficult to distinguish between certain letters of the alphabet. Andy hears most, but not everything, that is said in class even though he wears a hearing aid. Stan's walk is a slow Shuffle; he gets along on level surfaces or moderate inclines quite well, but is unable to manage stairs at all. Roy has a bright purple birthmark which covers one cheek and the Side of his neck. Several times a day Lester says he smells bananas; usually this means that he will soon fall to the floor in a convulsion which may last several minutes. Carla is a persistent talker, whisperer and notepasser. Bert could play songs with one finger on the piano when he was four; now, in the first grade, he has begun composing little melodies to which he gives names like "Rainy Day," "Bert's Bike," or "Juice-Time." monoo> 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 106 IN REGULAR CLASSROOM WITH PART-TIME AID WITH FULL-TIME AID IN SPECIAL CLASS OR SCHOOL NOT FOR.PUBLIC EDUCATION June's eyes are crossed but She has adequate vision in either eye despite the muscle imbalance. Laura's speech is laboriously slow, tortured, jerky and indistinct; her voice is monotonous in pitch and she cannot control its intensity. Harry sulks, and sometimes gets quite noisy, whenever he loses the direct attention of the teacher. William.can't hear anything with his left ear, but he gets along fairly well if he can Sit in one row by the window, in a room on the quiet Side of the building, with the class to his right. Ben is unable to walk and has been confined to a wheelchair; he manages this very skillfully and needs very little help. Les was born with a malfOrmed left hand which is withered and mis- shapen up to the elbow. When Terry was five he was run over, losing both his legs and genitals; he gets around quite well now but his bladder discharges into a.bag which must be emptied several times a day. Once or twice during the year Peter has complained of a peculiar feeling in his Stomach; about a minute afterwards he has lost con- sciousness and his body has been first ridgid and then convulsed for several minutes. John has no difficulty on the playground or at the blackboard but he gets quite uncomfortable when he has to use his eyes at close range for any length of time. Hugh eventually mutilates and destroys everything that gets into his hands; his books are marked and torn, his desk ink-stained and scarred, and he has even managed to crack a blackboard panel. When anything happens to John the whole school knows it. A bump on the playground produces tears and wailing, an "A" for an exam brings on unrestrained shrieks of delight. Sam.moves about somewhat awkwardly and his limbs are in a slight but eontinual tremor that becomes pronounced when he is nervous or excited. ITIUOWED 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 107 IN REGULAR CLASSROOM WITH PART-TIME AID MOTH FULL-TIME AID IN SPECIAL CLASS OR SCHOOL NOT FOR.PUBLIC EDUCATION Arnold is an extremely bright nine-year-old who is far ahead of the rest of the class in most subjects; he spends a good deal of his time working on a mathematical system he calls "kinestatics." Bill has difficulty in starting to talk, grimaces and strains, and repeats sounds on about half the words he says in class. Kate weighs enough for two children her age; it is almost impossible for her to aqueeze into a standard desk. Although Melvin does not really soil himself, as the day draws on he begins to smell more and more of feces. A.hearing aid provides no help for Harriet; she lipreads fairly well and can hear when she is not facing the speaker if shouted at. Helen's right hand may sometimes begin to tremble uncontrollably; during the next few minutes the spasmodic movement spreads along her arm, Shoulder, and.hear before it finally stops. IL 108 INSTRUCTIONS: Read eaCh item.and mark the correspondingly numbered Space on the answer sheet. Mark each item clearly, filling the space between the dotted lines on the Special answer Sheet. 101. 102. 103, 104. 105. 106. 107. 108. 109. 110. The congenital deaf child.will probable display (1) articulatory disorders; (2) voice abnormalities; (3) retarded language growth; (4) all of the above. The deaf, deafened, and hard-of—hearing are different categories based mainly on (1) degree of hearing loss; (2) Speech development; (3) lip reading ability; (4) amount of hearing loss and age of onset. Hard-of—hearing children usually have a decibel loss of (1) 0-15 dB; (2) 20-60 dB; (3) 70-90 dB; (4) 90-100 dB. The criticism of the manual method of theching the deaf is that (1) it is too difficult to learn; (2) it is difficult for these pupils to communicate with hearing people; (3) few teachers know the method; (4) it is too symbolic. Educating and rehabilitating the hard-of hearing is primarily (1) developing language; (2) fitting hearing aids; (3) giving audiometric tests; (4) teaching lip reading and Speech correction and auditory training. The oral method of teaChing the deaf refers to (1) teaching by means of speech and lip reading; (2) only by auditory training; (3) de- velOping speech and language; (4) teaching arithmetic and reading. The criterion used for placement of a child in a class for the deaf is (1) speech deve10pment, intelligence, and hearing loss; (2) disease causing the loss and intelligence; (3) Speech development alone; (4) hearing loss alone. The attitudes of teaChers toward handicapped children is (l) ver- balized acceptance but somewhat rejecting; (2) completely accepting; (3) the same as toward their normal children; (4) more understanding. The attitudes of parents toward their disabled children tend to be (1) oversolicitous, rejecting; (2) accepting, understanding; (3) the same as toward their normal Children; (4) more positive than toward their normal children. The attitudes of disabled persons toward themselves tend to be (1) not significantly different from normal children; (2) negative; (3) accepting; (4) more positive than normal children. HICHIGQN STQTE UNIV. LIBRRRIES IIIIIIIIIIIIII III 7 312931019 2135