THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN FULFILLMENT 0F INTERFERSONAL NEEDS AND ORGASM IN WOMEN ' . Thesis to: the Degree of M. A. MlCHlGAN STATE UNIVERSITY ALLISON BLAKE STERN ‘ 1975 «mum; W E lfl @131 131» W n} ll‘ 4 7 n '5 ’ L I B AC 4'4 771 1 hi! -. ~11 . ”'9 Av; ”a '4‘: lo; -Ioatl L"; Cl v. ’5 ‘v'v b 1',.v L. ”9“,: .- {JET-3.“! ,‘LR-r Ly 63:3”! 3““ m.ms «fl?» Ia LeflWV-k ABSTRACT THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN FULFILLMENT 0F INTERPERSONAL NEEDS AND ORGASM IN WOMEN By Allison Blake Stern Sexual histories and descriptions of last sexual experience were collected from 899 women. The protocols of the 382 women who reported coitus as their last sexual experience were used to evaluate the hypothesis that women having an orgasm during their last coital experience demonstrate more fulfillment of interpersonal needs than women reporting no orgasm during their last coitus. Two raters scored the protocols using a scale developed for this study from Apperson's (1973) Sexual Motive Projective Test for measurement of fulfillment of interpersonal needs. The results soundly supported the hypothesis. The association of fulfillment of interpersonal needs and or- gasm during last coital experience held for women reporting ‘Allison Blake Stern 25-l00% of orgasm attainment, but the relationship was not observed in the low orgasm percentage group (0-24%). Number of months of previous coital experience was not related to interpersonal'gratification scores. Orgasm during Tast coital experience, however, was less likely for women who had engaged in intercourse less than one month. For women having had intercourse longer than one month, there was no association between orgasm and amount of previous coital experience. Orgasm during last coitus was independent of in- volvement in a relationship. ‘Relationship type (e.g., married; going steady) and duration were also unrelated to last coital orgasm. The interpersonal needs most frequently scored were Affiliation, Succorance, and Nurturance. "There was no difference in the mean word length of the sexual de- scriptions nor any difference in the mean time elapsed since last coitus of women who were orgastic and nonor- gastic during their last coital experience. The subjects in the present study were compared to previously researched groups based on national proba- bility sampling. The incidence of types of sexual Allison Blake Stern experiences, percentage of orgasms during these sexual experiences, monthly coital frequency, and number of coital partners reported by subjects in the present study was similar to previous studies. The number of subjects using contraception was similar to previously researched samples, but the type of contraceptives used differed. Qualifications and implications of the findings of this research were discussed and future research di- rections were suggested. A speculative and syncretic model of female sexual functioning was developed contain- ing hypotheses for future testing. Approved: ZM/éfiez Date: _5s/;(/475 THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN FULFILLMENT OF INTERPERSONAL NEEDS AND ORGASM IN WOMEN By Allison Blake Stern A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1975 To Don, My lover and companion I have awakened to fresh mornings of whimsy with you I have hurt and haped with you And together we have shared the meaning we both sought ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I cannot thank Dr. Joseph Reyher or Dr. Donald Grummon enough. These men have been consistent sources of knowledge for me. That would have been enough. How- ever, the gifts of warmth, compassion, honesty, and humor they gave have significantly changed my life and will never be forgotten. I wish to thank Dr. Albert Aniskiewicz. I am grateful for his participation. Marc and Beth Oleshansky and JoAnne Etkin were more than experimenters. Without their thoughtfulness, enthusiasm, and diligence this research would not have been completed. I also wish to thank David Morganstern. Because of his efforts, the computer is no longer a dan- gerous beastie. but rather, a tool of value. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES. LIST OF FIGURES . INTRODUCTION. METHOD. Subjects. Materials . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Measurement . RESULTS Reliability . Interpersonal Gratification Other Variables Age of Subjects . . . Percentage of Orgasm Attainment . . Interpersonal Gratification and Orgasm Percentages of Orgasm Attainment . Time Since First Intercourse. . Time Since First Intercourse and Interpersonal Gratification. . Time Since First Intercourse and Orgasm . Types and Duration of Relationships Interpersonal Needs . Number of Words in the Sexual Descriptions. Time Elapsed Since Last Coitus. . . . Comparability of this Sample to Others. Summary of the Data . . . . . . . iv Page vi . viii \JNNOD IT IT 13 14 14 16 16 20 21 23 24 27 28 3O 30 37 Table of Contents (cont'd.) DISCUSSION. . . . . . . . . . . . . .'. . . . . Interpersonal Gratification and Other Variables Directions for Future Research. Speculative Theory of Female Sexual Behavior. REFERENCES. . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX A. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Sexual Description Form . . . . . . Sexual History Form . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX B. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Definitions of Interpersonal Needs. APPENDIX C. . . . . . . . . . Scoring Instructions. . . Interpersonal Gratification Scale . Page 4l 47 54 55 63 67 67 68 7O 72 72 72 75 LIST OF TABLES Comparison of mean interpersonal gratification of orgastic and nonorgastic women during last coital experience. . . . Age of subjects. All women (A). Women report- ing intercourse as their last sexual experience (B). . . . . . . . Comparison of the ages of women reporting orgasm during their last coital experience. Reported percentages of orgasm attainment of women who had coitus as their last sexual experience and who have had coitus longer than six months Comparisons of the mean interpersonal gratifi- cation of orgastic and nonorgastic women who reported various percentages of orgasm attainment. Time since first intercourse of women reporting coitus as their last sexual experience. Comparison of interpersonal gratification and orgasm during last coital eXperience of women with more than two years of coital experience. . . . . . . . . . . . Analysis of variance for the effects of orgasm and experience on interpersonal gratification during last coital experience Types of relationships reported by women having coitus as their last sexual experience. vi Page 13 15 15 I7 19 20 21 22 25 List of Tables (cont'd.) Table Page l0. Comparison of the number of words used by orgastic and nonorgastic women in their descriptions of coitus. . . . . . . . . . . . 28 ll. Comparison of the time elapsed since last coitus of orgastic and nonorgastic women. . . 30 l2. Incidence of types of sexual experience of lB-Zl year old women in this sample and of previous reports of similar aged women. . . . 32 .l3. Percentages of l8-Zl year old women reporting having experienced orgasm through types of . sexual experiences. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 14. Number of partners in coitus the lB-Zl year old women in the present sample reported as com- pared to previous samples of similar aged subjects. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 15. Frequency per month of engaging in coitus of 18-21 year old women currently sexually active in the present study as compared to a previous study of similar aged subjects . . 36 l6. Contraceptive use by lB-Zl year old women in the present study who had intercourse as their last sexual experience as compared to the contraceptive use of a previous study of similar aged women. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure ‘ Page 1. Histogram of the number of individual needs scored for all subjects who reported coitus as their last sexual experience. . . . . . . 29 viii INTRODUCTION This study was an attempt to directly assess whether or not gratification of interpersonal needs is associated with orgasm in women during coitus. Affili- ation, aggression, achievement, deference, dominance, exhibition, nurturance, and succorance were assumed, on a priori basis to be a salient, though not necessarily exhaustive, subset of interpersonal needs playing a role during coitus. The gratification or lack of gratifica- tion of these needs, not the psychological significance of their presence or absence, was ascertained. The approach used in this investigation contrasts with the strategy of those studies which attempt to relate global personality characteristics, adjustment, and pathology in women to measures of sexual behavior. Numerous investigations have attempted to ferret out the personality characteristics of women with differ- ent degrees of sexual responsiveness. The personality characteristics are often condensed by using such reified labels as psychotic, neurotic, and normal. The problems I of reliability and validity associated with such nosolog- ical terms are well documented (e.g., Sarbin, 1967; Rosen- han 1973, 1975). The measures of sexual responsiveness used in the investigations vary and assumptions should not be made that these measures correlate highly with each other (Fisher, 1973). However, commonly used meas- ures of sexual responsiveness are orgasm attainment, fre- quency of intercourse, and satisfaction with intercourse. The results of these quests for the personality profiles of women with differing degrees of sexual respon- siveness are mixed. Some investigations have found an in- verse relation between degree of maladjustment and sexual responsiveness (e.g., Landis gt_al., 1940; Coppen, 1965; Winokur gt_gl., 1959; Terman, 1938, 1951; Purtell gt_gl., 1951; Swenson, 1962) O'Neil and Kempler, 1969), others have not (Winokur g3_al., 1959; McCulloch and Stewart, 1960; C00per, 1969; Freedman, 1965; Miller and Wilson, 1968). Personality variables, historical information, and demographic data have been collected about men and attempts have been made to relate these variables to the sexual responsiveness of their partners (e.g., Terman, 1938, 1951). Again, the empirical literature is mixed regarding partner attributes (Fisher, 1973). Investigations which assess personality character- istics of women with differing degrees of sexual respon- siveness elucidate little about interpersonal factors and sexual responsiveness, even allowing for the problems of reliability and validity associated with such studies. Personality variables should not be viewed as absolute entities. People cannot be shuffled from relationship to relationship with similar interpersonal effects and part- ners cannot be defined globaliy‘with such hypostatized concepts as easy-toérelate-to vs: difficult-to-relate-to. Fisher (1973), when discussing the finding that wives married to difficult-to-relate-to partners (i.e., alco- holics) do not display limited orgasmic functioning, questions the importance of the association of interper- sonal factors and sexual responsiveness (p. 32). Such ana1yses overlook the probability that interpersonal needs are relative, that it is not necessarily the psycho- logical significance of the presence or absence of speci- fic traits or needs which is important to assess, but the degree to which interpersonal needs are fulfilled in a relationship, regardless of what they are. Some investigations have examined the relationship between both participants in coitus to assess the interdependence of interpersonal factors and sexual func- tioning. However, most investigations have used reTa- tively global measures of interpersonal relationships. Dickinson and Beam (1931) and Terman (1938, 1951), report unhappy couples have intercourse just about as frequently as happy couples. Landis §t_gl, (1940) could not estab- lish a correlation between orgasm consistency in women and marital happiness. In contrast, Terman (1938, 1951), Kinsey gt_al. (1953), and Chesser g1_gl. (1957), found that marital happiness and orgasm capacity in women are positively correlated. Global estimates of the quality of a relationship, such as marital happiness, may change over the duration of a relationship with her partner, and for some women, daily. "Happiness“ cannot be used as a synonym for satis- faction of interpersonal needs, or “unhappiness" equated with a lack of fulfillment of interpersonal needs. Fur- thermore, it is quite possible that the interpersonal needs salient immediately before, during, and after coi- tus may differ from those elicited at other times in the relationship or outside coital situations. The fulfill- ment or lack of fulfillment of these needs may vary in these different situations. Likewise, the interpersonal needs present in a sexual situation may differ from coi- tal experience to coital experience in the same woman and may vary considerable for different women. Clearly, a situational examination of interper- sonal factors and sexual responsiveness, without assump- tions of personality traits and the use of global mease ures, such as marital happiness, is needed to fill the lacunae of knowledge about female sexual responsiveness. The hypothesis that fulfillment of interpersonal needs and orgasm“during a particular coital experience are associated in women will be referred to as the interper- sonal gratification hypothesis. METHOD Subjects Subjects were undergraduate female volunteers who signed up to participate in an experiment on Nwomen's feelings about intercourse." The title of the experiment was chosen so that subjects knew the general content of the research and could avoid participating if they found it offensive. The title was also chosen so that by sign- ing up for the research, subjects were not indicating publicly the extent of their sexual experience. The latter was felt to be important because in earlier re— search subjects indicating coital experience received objectionable anonymous phone calls. All subjects were enrolled in psychology courses and most were freshmen. Of the 899 subjects, 382 subjects reported having intercourse during their last sexual ex— perience. Materials '1. Sexual Description Form. The sexual descrip- tion form requests a subject to describe her thoughts and feelings about her last sexual experience, whether coitus was involved, and whether she had an orgasm (see Appendix A). 2. Sexual History Form. This form covered a range of materials regarding the subjects' sexual activi- ties and personal data (See Appendix A). Procedure One male and three female experimenters admini- stered the measures used in this study to 100 groups of women. The groups ranged in size from three to sixty sub- jects. 'The Sexual Description Form was presented first and followed by the Sexual History Form. Subjects were encouraged to be frank and assured of their anonymity. Measurement Subjects described their sexual experience in a “free response" manner in that they had no clues about the interpersonal dimensions under investigation. In order to measure the fulfillment or lack of fulfillment of interpersonal needs of these protocols, a scale was constructed from Apperson's (1973) Sexual Motive Projec- tive Test (SM-PT). Apperson (1973) constructed the SM-PT from Murray's (1938) need system to measure psychological motivation for sexual behavior in males and females. On an a priori basis, she postulated the following needs as important for sexual motivation: Achievement (Ach), A Affiliation (Aff), Aggression (Agg), Deference (Def), Dominance (Dom), Exhibition (Exh), Nurturance (Nur), Succorance (Suc). The definitions.of the motive cate- gories Apperson (1973) used to construct the items of the SM-PT are contained in Appendix B- For content validity Apperson (1973) had six clinicians rate the statements she used for measurement of sexual motivation. The items she used in the SM-PT had perfect agreement by at least four out of the six judges that the statements were re~ lated to the definitions she used. Half of the sentences which compose the SM-PT are appropriate to females; these sentences were used as cri- terion sentences by raters in the present study. Two modifications of these sentences were necessary. First, additional sentences were constructed which stated the needs in an unfulfilled form. This unfulfilled form was an opposite or negated statement of the Apperson (1973) sentences. Second, the sentences were put in the first person, rather than the third, to facilitate ease of rating. Whenever the phrase in a woman's description of her last sexual experience matched a criterion sentence, the match was noted. The criterion sentences, as stated above, were written in sets of two, one of each pair re- ferring to fulfillment of an interpersonal need, the other referring to lack of fulfillment of that particular need. -When a subject's phrase matched a "fulfillment" criterion sentence, the match was scored +1. When the match was with a “lack of fulfillment" criterion sentence, the match was scored -1. The algebraic sum of each sub- ject's protocol for all the needs was a quantitative esti- mate of the degree of overall fulfillment, regardless of the nature of the needs. This sum was weighted by the number of words in the subject's protocol sum of interpersonal needs ( number of words used x 100) to minimize the chance of a subject's score being influ— enced mainly by the brevity or length of her record. The 10 overall estimate of how many needs were satisfied or un- satisfied, weighted by number of words used, will be re- ferred to as a subject's interpersonal gratification for that sexual experience. A high interpersonal gratifica- tion score means that a subject demonstrated more fulfill- ment of interpersonal needs during her last coital exper- ience than a subject with a low interpersonal gratifica- tion score. The women's descriptions of their last coital ex- perienceS‘were typed and separated from their forms. The typing was to insure that raters would only be analyzing the protocols on the basis of what the women wrote, with- out knowledge of orgasmic status during last coital ex- perience or the information contained in the sexual his- tory form.’ The raters were two psychology graduate students. RESULTS Reliability One psychology graduate student scored all the subjects‘“descriptions of their last coital experience for fulfillment or lack of fulfillment of interpersonal needs. Forty-five records, or over 10% of the protocols, were randomly chosen from the sample of women indicating they had had intercourse as their last sexual experience for a second psychology graduate student to score. The first rater was female, the second rater was male. Scor- ing independently, the range of scores they computed was from -9 to +17 (uncorrected for word length). In 25 cases the raters scored the same score; in 11 cases they were differed by 1 point; and in 9 cases they deviated from 2-4 points. The Spearman rank-order correlation (corrected for ties was compUted for the rankings of the two raters. “The degree of interjudgment agreement was .93. In order to evaluate the results, standard t- tests were employed. In addition F tests were used to 11 12 examine whether the assumption of homogeneity of vari-° ance of the t-tests was met. In these analyses, a sig- nificant F test means that the variances were not homoge- neous. Since the groups compared were often not of equal size, relying on the robustness of the t-test, if there was heterogeneity of variance, was not considered the best approach. In addition to the t and F tests, then. the Bayesian Posterior Probability, which does not assume homogeneity of variance, was computed. (For a further description of the Bayesian Posterior Probability see Footnote C, Table 5.) A high Bayesian Posterior Probabil- ity (i.e., near 1) gives strong evidence that the sample means differ. In the tables which follow, the Bayesian Posterior Probability has been omitted if the F tests in- dicated that the homogeneity of variance assumption was met (i.e., p > .05). The t-tests were then considered appropriate and sufficient. The data from the 382 subjects reporting coitus as their last sexual experience was used in the analysis of the hypothesis. These 382 subjects, were designated as coitus-last subjects. It should be noted that subjects not reporting coitus as their last sexual experience were not necessarily virgins; they simply did not have 13 intercourse as their last sexual experience. When coitus- last subjects are described as orgastic or non-orgastic, only the presence or absence of orgasm during their last- sexual experience is meant to be indicated. Interpersonal Gratification The interpersonal gratification hypothesis was soundly supported. The mean interpersonal gratification of orgastic coitus-last subjects was significantly higher than non-orgastic coituswlast subjects. Table 1. Comparison of mean interpersonal gratification of orgastic and nonorgastic women during last coital experience. v7 Groups Mean Na Variance I tb, FC Orgastic 3.498 217 21.964 6.889* 1.049 Nonorgastic .111 162 23.031 aThree subjects did not report whether or not they had an orgasm during their last coital experience. bThe significance level reported is for a nondirectional test. CThe assumption of homogeneity variance was tested with the F statistic which is apprOpriate when normality holds (Hays, 1973). * p < .00001 14 Other Variables In order to evaluate the interpersonal gratification hypothesis, the possibility that various factors were moder- ating variables was considered. The possible moderating variables examined were age, reported percentage of orgasm attainment, period of time since first coital experience, types of relationships the women reported they were involved in, the duration of these relationships, and whether the women planned to be married to their coital partners. Age of Subjects Since Kinsey £L_ll- (1953) found a correlation of age and decade of birth with orgasm, the ages of the sub- jects were examined. The results shown in Table 2 were as expected because of the college population from which this sample was drawn. Ninety-three percent of the coitus-last subjects were 18-21 years of age.’ In fact, 82.5% of the coitus-last subjects were 18 or 19 years old. Ninety-four percent of the subjects were 18-21 years of age, disregard- ing the type of their last sexual experience. Comparisons were made between the ages of orgastic and non-orgastic coitus-last women. As can be seen in Table 3, there was no significant difference in the mean 15 Table 2. Age of subjects. All women (A). Women reporting intercourse as their last sexual experience (8). N Range Mean Standard Deviation A 897a 14-44 18.48 1.69 B 382 14-44 18.81 2.25 aTwo subjects omitted age information. Table 3. Comparison of the ages of women reporting orgasm during their last coital experience. Groups Mean Na Variance t Fb Orgastic 18.91 217 6.738 .943 2.325* Nonorgastic 18.69 162 2.898 aThree subjects omitted whether or not they had an orgasm during their last coital experience. bThe assumption of homogeneity of variance was tested with the F statistic which is appropriate when normality holds (Hays, 1973). cp < .01. 16 ages. However, the variances were significantly differ- ent. The orgastic coitus-last group included the oldest and youngest participants. Percentage of Orgasm Attainment Table 4 presents the reported percentage of orgasm attainment of coitus-last subjects. Only the coitus-last subjects who reported having had coitus over six months are shown, since the reported percentage of orgasm attain- ment of women with minimal experience is difficult to in- terpret. Namely, women experiencing coitus for the first time have a percentage of orgasm attainment of either 0% or 100%. Six months coital experience was arbitrarily chosen as providing sufficient time for a subject to have had enough coitus to base an estimate of her percentage of orgasm attainment. Interpersonal Gratification and Percentages of Orgasm Attainment In order to evaluate the interpersonal gratifica- tion hypothesis in terms of reported percentages of orgasm attainment, comparisons were drawn between interpersonal 17 Table 4. Reported percentages of orgasm attainment of women who had coitus as their last sexual ex- perience and who have had coitus longer than six months. Orgasm . N % of Mean Standard Percentages Subjects Deviation 0-252 62 33.0 i .472 .369 25-50% 25 10.0 50-75% 46 18.5 75-10074a 95 38.3 Missingb 24 272 99.3“d aInclusion is at the lower end point of the interval. bMissing data is from subjects who did not give their orgasm percentages. c . . Percentages are based on nonm1ss1ng data. dInexact percentage totals are due to round-off error. 18 gratification of orgastic and non-orgastic coitus-last subjects falling within various ranges of percentage of orgasm attainment. Only subjects reporting having had coitus over six months were used in the analyses because of the aforementioned difficulty in interpreting an or- gasm percentage of a woman with minimal coital exper- ience. As can be seen in Table 5, the mean interpersonal gratification of orgastic coitus-last women was signifi- cantly higher than the mean-interpersonal gratification of nonorgastic coitus-last women, except the 0-24% range of attained orgasm. The trend in this group is the same as that for the other groups, though not significantly so (p < .07). A puzzling finding is the significant dif- ference in the variances of the interpersonal gratifica- tion scores of the women on the extreme ends of reported range of orgasm attainment. Consistently, the non- orgastic coitus-last subjects had greater dispersion of scores than orgastic coitus-last women. 19 Table 5. Comparisons of the mean interpersonal gratification of or- gastic and nonorgastic women who reported various percen- tages of orgasm attainment. Probabilityc a . b that the Groups Mean N Variance t F first mean > the 2nd mean 0—24% Orgastic 2.095 21 9.491 1.872 3.562** .991 Nonorgastic -.400 60 33.80 25-74% Orgastic 3.810 42 20.792 4.013*** 1.103 .999 Nonorgastic -.571 28 18.847 _ 75-lOO% Orgastic 4.151 66 23.146 2.392* 1.888 .967 N onorgastic .688 16 43.696 a b C U * All subjects reported having had intercourse longer than six months. Thirty-seven subjects having coitus as their last sexual experience and having intercourse over six months, did not give their orgasm percentages. The assumption of homogeneity of variance was tested with the F sta— tistic which is appropriate when normality holds (Hays, 1973). The Bayesian posterior probability computed was the probability that the true strata means are ordered in the same way as the sample means. It is derived from an approximation to the Behrens-Fisher distribution. A high probability (i.e., near 1) gives strong evi- dence that the sample means differ. It is derived assuming normal sampling but not equal variances (Fox and Guire, 1973). The Behrens distribution approaches normality as the degrees of freedom tend to infinity. For a further description of the posterior probability computed and the approximation to the Behrens-Fisher distribution sed, see Lindley Part 2 p. 91-95. ** *** p < .05; p < .01; and p < .001. 20 Time Since First Intercourse Table 6 presents the duration of time coitus- 1ast subjects reported having engaged in intercourse since their first coitus. The number of women who had experi- enced intercourse for a prolonged period of time was small in this population studied. Table 6. The time since first intercourse of women report- ing coitus as their last sexual experience. Length of Time Na % of Subjects First Time 11 3.0 < 1 month (but not 1st time) 13 3.5 > 1 month - 6 months 72 19.6 > 6 months - 1 year 83 22.6 > 1 year - 2 years 107 29.1 > 2 years 82 22.3 . 3;;- 100—3." aFourteen subjects omitted data on length of having had coitus. The percentages are based on nonmissing data. bInexact totals due to round-off error. 21 Time Since First Intercourse and Interpersonal Gratification The possible moderating influence of time since first coitus, referred to here as experience, was consid- ered in two ways. The first comparison utilized women with more coital experience. As Table 7 indicates, there was a significant difference in the mean interpersonal gratification scores of orgastic and non-orgastic coitus- 1ast subjects who reported having had coitus over two years. If no significant difference had occurred, it would have been suggestive of a decline of importance in interpersonal factors with more experience. Table 7. Comparison of interpersonal gratification and orgasm during last coital experience of women with more than two years of coital experience. Group Mean Na Variance tb Fc Orgastic 3.520 48 20.808 4.4398* 1.4765 Nonorgastic -.706 34 14.093 aFourteen subjects omitted data on length of having had coitus. bThe significance level is for a nondirectional test. CThe homogeneity of variance assumption was tested with the F statistic which is appropriate when normality holds (Hays, 1973). 'k p < .00001. 22 To obtain further information on the variable of experience, a 2x6 Analysis of Variance was performed. The factors were orgasm (0) or not during last coital experi- ence and duration of time (E) the women reported having engaged in intercourse. The dependent measure was inter- personal gratification with subjects nested in levels of factors 0 and E and the factors completely crossed. Table 8. Analysis of Variance for the Effects of Orgasma and Experienceb on Interpersonal Gratification during Last Coital Experience. Source df SS MS F Orgasm (O) 1 287.475 287.475 13.637* Experience (E) 5 133.091 26.618 1.263 0 x E 5 144.367 28.873 1.369 Error (within)c 353 7441.423 21.031 Total 364 8006.356 aThree subjects did not report whether they had orgasm during their last coital experience. bFourteen subjects omitted data on length of having had intercourse. CThe error term used throughout was the within-cell variance (mean square). * p < .01. 23 As can be seen in Table 8, the effect of orgasm and interpersonal gratification was significant for both age groups considered, irrespective of experience. The results indicate that interpersonal factors do not decline or increase in importance as a woman's sexual experience increases. Time Since First Intercourse and Orgasm The association of the variables experience and orgasm, disregarding interpersonal gratification, was tested with coitus-last subjects. The Pearson chi square test for independence was used to ascertain if there was some dependence of these variables. Cramer's phi, w' (Hays, 1973; Blalock, 1960), was also determined to pro- vide a measure of the apparent strength of association of the variables. Experience and orgasm during last coital experience were not independent for the women (x2=ll.743, df=5, n=365, p < .03). However, the association of the variables was not strong (x7 =.l8). When extreme ne0phytes (women reporting having en- gaged in intercourse less than one month) were dropped from the analyses, independence of orgasm during last 24 coitus and experience was found (x2=2.l3, df=3, n=34l, p > .05, ¢'=.08). Likewise, when the percentages of or- gasm attainment of women reporting they had had inter- course over one month were compute ,‘a hypothesis of inde- pendence of the variables could not be rejected (x2=8.48, df=9, n=288, p > .05, 0' = .10). The results indicate that with less than one month of sexual experience, women may have difficulty attaining an orgasm, since 82% of the women having intercourse for the first time did not experience an orgasm and 62% of the women with less than one month of sexual experience did not have an orgasm during their last coitus. However, the effect of experience and orgasm quickly diminishes with time. Types and Duration of Relationships The generalizability of the interpersonal gratifi- cation and orgasm effect might be influenced by whether the women in this sample were involved in relationships, and if so, what kind (See Sexual History Forms). Since most of the coitus-last women were "going steady," the effect of interpersonal gratification and orgasm can be 25 most safely generalized to women in that type of relation- ship. Table 9. Types of relationships_reported by women having coitus as their last sexual experience. 14-44 Year Old Women Type of Relationship 3 N % of Subjects No relationship 41 10.7 Married 19 5.0 Living together (but not married) 9 224 Steady boyfriend 256 67.0 Had relationship before with coital partner (but not currently) 18 4.7 Other kind of relationship 39 10.2a 382 99.9 aInexact total due to round-off error. Whether or not the women reported being in some sort of relationship, and if they were, the kind of rela- tionship they reported, and orgasm during last coital ex- perience were independent (x2=5.692, df=5, n=379, p > .05, p'=.12). (See Table 9 for the relationship types.) 0f 26 the women reporting being in a relationship and having had intercourse with the person described in the relationship, orgasm during last coital experience and the relationship types were dependent (x2=.65, df=4, n=288, p > .05, p' = .05). Only 19 subjects reporting coitus as their last sexual experience had intercourse with someone other than the partner they described in the relationship section. These subjects were between 18-21 years of age and the re- lationships were “going steady" and "other." Orgasm dur- ing last coital experience was found to be independent of whether the subjects had intercourse with the person de- scribed in the relationship section or a person different from the person described in the relationship section (x2=.26, df=l, n=286, p > .05, p'=.06). An analysis of relationship types and reported percentages of orgasm attainment yielded too few subjects per cell to compare the variables. The duration of time women were involved in the relationships they reported should be considered, but too few subjects were involved in relationships other than "going steady." However, the relationship between dura- tion of time being involved in a "steady" relationship was examined with respect to orgasm during the last coitus as 27 well as to percent of orgasm attainment. The time ranges examined were between one month and four years. The length of time the women reported having gone "steady" and orgasm during their last coital experience was found to be independent (x2=1.7, df=3, n=219, p > .05, p'=.09). Only women having intercourse with their steady were used in these analyses. Likewise, the length of time the women stated they were "going steady" and their reported percen- tages of orgasm attainment were found to be independent (x2=8.8, df=9, n=127, p > .05, w' .15). The women used in these analyses had to have been having intercourse over six months to make the percentages of orgasm attainment more meaningful, and they had to have had their last coi- tal experience with the person with whom they were “going steady." Interpersonal Needs As can be seen in Figure l, the most frequently scored need was fulfillment or gratification of Affilia- tion. Lack of fulfillment of aggression was the least scored need. Considering each of the eight interpersonal needs scored, gratification was scored more than lack of gratification, except for the need for achievement. Lack 28 of fulfillment of the need for achievement was scored more frequently than fulfillment of the need for achievement. Number of Words Used in the Sexual Descriptions Although the interpersonal gratification scores in- cluded a correction for word length, there was no signifi- cant difference in the mean word length of the protocols of orgastic and non-orgastic coitus-last women. Table 10. Comparison of the number of words used by orgas- tic and nonorgastic women in their descriptions of coitus. Group Mean Na Variance tb FC Orgastic 104.89 216 3878.1 -l.521l 1.0808 Nonorgastic 114.91 162 4191.5 aThree subjects did not report whether they had an orgasm during their last coital experience. Word length was not computed for one subject. bp > .05; nondirectional test. cThe assumption of homogeneity of variance was tested with the F statistic which is appropriate when normality holds (Hays, 1973). 29 295 230 265 250 N=379a 235 220 205 190 175 160 115 . { 1:: N N N 1 1 M M AFF AFF SUC SUC NUR NUR DEF DEF DOM DOM ASH ACH EXH EXH AGG AGG A B A B A B A B A B A B ~A B A B 294 165 211 167 177 127 140 127 133 126 133 141 136 126 133 125 FIG. l.--Histogram of the number of individual needs scored for all subjects who reported coitus as their last sexual experi— ence. Fulfillment of the interpersonal need (A). Lack of fulfillment of the interpersonal need (8). aThree subjects did not report whether or not they had an orgasm during their last coital experience and these protocols were not scored. 30 Time Elapsed Since Last Coitus As can be seen from Table 11, there was no signif- icant difference in the mean time since last coitus of orgastic and non-orgastic coitus-last subjects. Table 11. Comparison of the time elapsed since last coitus of orgastic and nonorgastic women. Groups Mean Nb Variance tc Fd Orgastica 2.523 214 1.0955 -.3636 1.054 Nonorgastic 2.568 155 1.0392 aTime based on categorical coding rather than actual time. bThree subjects didn't put whether or not they had an or- gasm during their last coital experience. Ten subjects did not put length of time since their last coital ex- perience. cp > .05; nondirectional test. dThe homogeneity of variance assumption was tested with the F statistic which is appropriate when normality holds (Hays, 1973). “ Comparability of This Sample to Others This sample was compared to previously researched samples for incidence of types of sexual experience, or- gasm, frequency of coitus, number of coital partners, and 31 contraceptive use. Previously researched samples of roughly similarly aged subjects were used for comparison, and subjects in the present study falling outside the age range of 18-21 years were excluded. The previously researched samples cited in the following Tables were more comprehensive in that they were based on national probability samples (Sorenson, 1973; Zelnick and Kantner, 1972), or approximated them (Hunt, 1974). Kinsey's giggl, (1953) data was collected through personal interviews. The data of the other studies cited in the Tables was collected by questionnaires. Sorenson's (1973) research included married adolescents. The figures cited from the other studies were based on unmarried sub- jects. The ten married women between 18-21 years in the present study were included in the percentages which follow. As can be seen from Table 12, the present sample was very similar to previously researched samples in the incidence of types of sexual experience the women had had. Sorenson (1973) found an association between masturbation and intercourse. He found that girls having intercourse reported masturbating twice as often as girls who were not currently having intercourse (Sorenson, 1973, p. 133). The present study found that having ever had intercourse and 32 Table 12. Incidence of types of sexual experience of 18-21 year old women in this sample and of previous reports of similar aged women. Percentages of Present Sample Percentages Reported Age Range 18-21 by Previous Studies N=845-61 Kissinga ................. 100 99.5 Stimulation of breast.. 98 85.7 Manual stimulation of genitalsc’d ............ 33-91 77.5 Cunnilinguse ............. 69 53.3 Coitusfig ................. 45-46 52.4 Masturbation. ............ 39 40.4 aKinsey et a1. (1953), p. 280. Percentage based upon figures reported for "simple" kissing of women with 13-16 years of schooling. bSorenson (1973), p. 171. The percentage based on what he called "sex- ual beginners" (women without coital experience). Sorenson did not specify whether the stimulation was with or without clothing. This study specified without clothing. His percentage based on women aged 13-19. CKinsey et a1. (1953), p. 234. Of women aged 16-20, 88% "actively" petting. ' dHunt (1974), p. 136. Of women aged 18-24, 90% "actively" petting. eHunt (1974), p. 166. Percentage based on white males, aged 18-24, reports of having engaged in cunnilingus. fSorenson (1973), p. 187. Percentage based on women ages 13-19. gZelnick and Kantner (1972), p. 360. Percentage based on women ages 15-19. ' hSorenson (1973), p. 129. Percentage based on women ages 13-19. 1Two subjects omitted data on all types of sexual experience except coitus. One subject omitted data on coitus. The percentages are based on nonmissing data. 33 having ever masturbated were highly dependent (18-21 year old women x=23.64, df=l, n=845, p.< .00001, w'=.l67). However, Cramer's phi did not show a strong association. Hunt (1974) found that between the ages of 18-24, substantially more than 50% of the unmarried women he studied had "experienced orgasm through petting techniques in the past half year (p. 136)." Hunt (1974) also found that three-quarters of his young unmarried females had ex- perienced orgasms in their premarital coital relationships. Table 13. Percentages of 18e21.year old women reporting having experienced orgasm thro.ugh.'types of sexual experiences. Type of Experience . Percentage ........ NR Kissing 3.52 818 Stimulation of breast (without clothing) 10.74 701 Manual stimulation of genitals (without clothing) 56.38 632 Cunnilingus “ 54.03 427 Coitus 77.29 420 Masturbation . 57.68 318 aTwenty-three 18-21 year old subjects omitted data on or- gasm through types of sexual experience. The percentages are based on nonmissing data. 34 As can be seen from Table 13, these percentages are similar to the percentages found in the present study. The percen- tage of women reporting having experienced orgasm through masturbation is different from previous studies. Sorenson (1973) found that 71% of the 13-19 year old female adoles- cents in his sample had experienced an orgasm through mas- turbation sometime during the preceding month (p. 134). However, only 57.7% of the 18-21 year old women in the present study had ever experienced an orgasm through mas- turbation. As can be seen in Table 14, the number of sexual partners reported by the women in their sexual histories is roughly comparable to previously researched samples. The vast majority had had few partners and are probably what Sorenson (1973) terms "serial monagamists (p. 219)." Fewer subjects in the present sample reported having had 7-15 partners than in the Sorens0n (1973) study. But the number of subjects having had 16 or more partners was similar. Likewise, as can be seen in Table 15, the subjects in this sample currently having intercourse reported hav- ing coitus about as often as found in a previous study. 35 Table 14. Number of partners in coitus the 18-21 year old women in the present sample reported as compared to previous samples of similar aged subjects. Number Reported Reported Present Sample of by a by Zelnick 18-21 Years Partners Sorenson and Kantner N=353C l 50% 59.9% 48.2% 2-3 22 24.1 30.3 4-6 14 16.0e 13.4 7-15 12 6.6 16 or more 2 1.8d 100% .lO0.0% 100.3% aSorenson (1973), p. 433. Percentage based on women ages 13-19. b Zelnick and Kantner (1972), p. 365. Percentage based on women ages 15-19. cNinety subjects did not report how many partners they'd had. Percentage based on nonmissing data. dInexact total due to round-off error. eZelnick and Kantner (1972) data based on his percentages for whites only. He found 9.2% had had 4-5 partners and 6.8% had had 6 or more partners. These percentages were summed for ease of visual presentation. 36 Table 15. Frequency per month of engaging in coitus of 18-21 year old women currently sexually active in the present study as compared to a previous study of similar aged subjects. Frequency Percentage Reported Percentage in per Month by Sorensona Present Studyb (18-21) N=297 l 9 11.1 2 11 11.4 3 11 7.7 4 13 8.1 5-8 15 21.6 9-12 15 16.8 13-19 10 , 12.7 20 or more 16 ' 10.2c 1003‘ 99.6% A aSorenson (1973), p. 271. Percentages based on monogamist data since it most closely approximates relationship type data of the present study (i.e., the number of subjects reporting "going steady"). .Sorenson data based on 13-19 year old subjects. bFifty-seven subjects omitted data on frequency per month. Percentages based on nonmissing data. CInexact total due to round-off error. 37 As can be seen in Table 16, about as many subjects in the present study stated they use contraceptives as in a previous study. However, the types of contraceptives used differs drastically. ‘Many fewer subjects in the present study reported they rely on "less safe" methods of birth control, such as withdrawal and rhythm. More subjects reported taking birth control pills than in any previous study. Summary of the Data The hypothesis that women who report having an orgasm during their last coital experience have more ful- fillment of interpersonal needs than women reporting no orgasm was soundly supported. The effect held for women with most ranges of percentage of orgasm attainment (25- 100%). The women not experiencing an orgasm during their last coital experience, and reporting very low or very high percentages of orgasm attainment (0-24% or 75-100%), had greater dispersion of interpersonal gratification scores. 4 The amount of time women reported having engaged in intercourse was not related to their interpersonal gratification scores; however, women who had engaged in T 38 able 16. Contraceptive use by 18-21 year old women in the present study who had intercourse as their last sexual experience a 5 compared to the contracep- tive use of a previous study of similar aged women . Percentage Percentage Contraceptive Reported in the by Zelnick Present Study and Kantnera (18-21) n-354 N B F I D C D W one irth Control Pills oam UD iaphragm ondom ouche ithdrawal Rhythm C ombination (e . g . , condom and foam) 16.0 20.5 21.0 57.3 2.3 1.3 .5 3.2 1.4 .6 13.3 10.3 2.4 0.0 27.7 .3 2.2 .6 10.73 5.0 102.5% 100.1% a b Zelnick and Kantner (1972), p. whites only, women aged 15-19. their subjects whether or not "never,” "sometimes," "always. ported for most recently used subjects using contraceptives 369. Percentages for Zelnick and Kantner asked they used contraceptives " The percentages they re- contraceptive were based on and excluded the 16% never using contraception. The percentages they reported for most recently used contraception were modified here to include the 16% never using c ity. The 102.5% total was ov metic Operations performed. Twelve subjects did not repor contraceptives. Percentages ontraceptives for comparabil- er 100% because of the arith- t whether or not they used based on nonmissing data. 39 intercourse less than one month were found less likely to experience orgasm. There was no association between ex- perience and orgasm for women having had intercourse longer than one month. Involvement in a relationship (See Sexual History Form) and orgasm during last'coitus were independent. In addition, the types of relationships the women reported having and orgasm during last coitus were not associated. The duration of time women who were "going steady" reported they had been involved in the "steady“ relationship was not associated with orgasm during last coital experience or the women's reported percentages of orgasm attainment during coitus. The interpersonal needs most frequently scored were Affiliation, Succorance, and Nurturance. There was no difference in the mean number of words used in the coi- tal descriptions of orgastic and non-orgastic women, nor was there any difference in the mean time elapsed since last coitus of orgastic and non-orgastic women. The re- sults are most appropriately generalized to women 18-21 years of age who consider themselves "going steady." The subjects in the present study were compared to previously researched samples based on national probability 40 sampling. The incidence of types of sexual experiences, orgasms through these sexual experiences, frequencies per month, and number of coital partners subjects in the present study reported was‘similar to previous studies. The number of subjects using contraceptions was similar to previously researched samples, but the type of contra- ceptives used differed. DISCUSSION The results of this study clearly indicate some association of fulfillment of interpersonal needs and or- gasm. The direction of causality cannot be empirically ascertained at this point. Women having orgasms may per- ceive that their interpersonal needs have been met or ful- fillment of interpersonal needs may be necessary for or- gasm. Comparisons can be drawn between the results of the present study and Fisher's (1973) study. Fisher (1973) found that low orgasmic women were concerned about object loss, chronically preoccupied with the possibility of being separated from persons loved and valued. One ex- planatory framework he proposed he derived from psychoana- lytic concepts about separation anxiety. Separation anxi- ety, he says, may be a fear of the recurrence of the same build-up of tension which orginally occurred when the per- son responsible for gratification of needs (e.g., mother) left. He points out that sexual excitement is a build-up 41 42 of tension, and postulates that the low orgasmic woman may fear sexual excitement because it represents the loss of love objects. Fisher's (1973) explanation focuses on the build-up of tension feared by the low orgasmic woman. However, if the emphasis is changed; to the fear of absence of a "love object," his explanation of his results and the results of the present study are parallel. Apropos to this point is the following: Certain neurotics who are still afraid of the experience of their'own excitement, at least beyond a certain intensity of this excitement, are so not because of a primary 'anxiety of the intensity of their own excitement' but because other types of anxiety have caused them to block the natural course of their excitement, turning pleasure secondarily into intense dis- pleasure.‘ This leads sooner or later to the fear that external means of satisfaction might possibly fail to arrive. It is the 'fear over loss of love' or rather loss of help and pro- tection. This fear is more intense than it would be if it represented only a rational judgment about real danger because the early self-esteem is regulated by means of external supplies so that a loss of help and protection means also a loss of self-esteem (Fenichel, 1945, p. 44). “" Rather than a fear of build-up of uncontrollable tension, the individual with separation anxiety primarily fears a lack of satisfaction of needs, a fear of loss of love and help and protection. In other words, the low orgasmic 43 woman fears a loss of interpersonal supplies--a lack of gratification of interpersonal needs. Fisher (1973) derived another explanation of his re- sults from the observation that the normal state of con— sciousness is affected by a build-up of sexual excitement (Kinsey, gt_al., 1953; Masters anleohnson, 1966). He proposed that arousal diminishes awareness of external stimuli and that "objects" may become psychologically more distant. Hence, he speculated that the woman fearing "ob- ject loss" would fear sexual excitement which, psycholog- ically blunts “objects."' The second explanation can also be used to explain the results of the present study. The only assumption neces- sary to link the results of the present study and Fisher's (1973) explanations of his results is the meaning of "loss of objects” feared by the woman with separation anxiety. The fear of "loss of objects“ can easily be viewed as fear of “loss of objects" which satisfy interpersonal needs. Both of Fisher's (1973) explanations of his results neatly fit the present study with only a slight modifica- tion. The modification is the significance of fear of "object loss" in low orgasmic women. If the significance of fear of "object loss" assumed here is accepted, the 44 meaning of the results of both studies are parallel. In- terpersonal factors and orgasm are associated. As Fisher (1973) points out, a fear of "object loss" does not necessarily exemplify a personality type, and in the present study, the interpersonal needs of a woman in her sexual experiences are not necessarily an indication of her personality type. Interpersonal needs may vary from sexual experience to sexual experience and the needs salient in a woman's sexual experiences may not typify her non-sexual interpersonal interactions. Hence, Fisher's (1973) findings about personality traits can be put into context. Fisher (1973) found that anger, coping with anger, dominance, sociability, cheer- fulness, desire to help others, achievement, and activity- passivity dimensions of personality were not significantly related to high- or low-orgasm capacity. In addition, the fulfillment of interpersonal needs (not just the presence of the needs) which this study found, seems to be an im- portant factor in linking interpersonal interactions and orgasm. Fisher (1973) had the women in his study write about their sexual experiences. He characterizes them: 1. Some women present intercourse primarily as a great adventure in which there are new 45 experiences and sensations to be sampled. Emphasis is placed upon the range of positions used, the novelty of some of the sensations aroused, and the potential for unlimited ex- ploration by each spouse of the other spouse's body. Related to this first theme is the perception of intercourse as a means of attaining ecstasy or going on a 'trip' in which there are strange sensations and loss of identity. .Included in this category are those who wish to ascribe a religious or mystical quality to sexual arousal. For some women intercourse is an opportunity to prove their superior prowess in an activity that is highly valued. They dwell on their re- sponsiveness, their ability to attain multiple orgasms, and the fact that they exceed their husbands in sexual capacity. Another theme found in the intercourse descrip- tions portrays intercourse primarily as an opportunity for intimacy and blending oneself with a sex partner. Much is said about close- ness, skin contact, cuddling, and feeling un- believably united to another human being. The process of joining with someone is depicted as comforting and anxiety allaying. A troubled martyred stance appears in some. The women in this category see intercourse as either painful or unpleasant. They picture themselves as victims of an inability to re- spond adequately to sexual stimulation. They dwell on their irritability during intercourse and their anticipation of ultimate frustration. One interesting group characterizes sexual excitement as almost too intense to tolerate. They refer to their state of sexual arousal as painfully intense and suggest that they can barely control the tension that builds up . . . . Reference is also made to excited body areas feeling 'sore' and oversensitive. Sexual excitement seems to be regarded as a force greater than self, which has potential overwhelming and disorganizing properties. There are also those who adopt a no-nonsense 'businesslike' attitude. They describe 46 intercourse as a routine, well-practiced procedure that provides adequate gratifi- cation, but which is simply one of a number of other important like activities. Several of the women who conveyed this idea indi- cated that shortly after intercourse they often resume some household task or work that needs to be done. 8. Those who experience intercourse as a reaf- firmation of feminine identity should also be mentioned. They speak of how 'womanly' they feel during and after sexual arousal. They experience sexual arousal as a demon- stration that a man has accepted them in the role of 'real' woman (Fisher, 1973, p. 214-215). The themes were similar in the present study. Al- though actual frequency counts were not performed, even a cursory analysis of the data shows that the perception of the intercourse as an opportunity for intimacy and blend- ing oneself with a sex partner was the most common theme. Sex as a force greater than oneself with potential disor- ganizing properties was the least common description. However, the range of themes Fisher (1973) found was rep- resented in the present study. In addition, other themes were present which were not captured by Fisher's (1973) descriptions. Several women viewed intercourse as a means by which they could be taken care of in a tender fashion. Several other women perceived intercourse as a gift to their partners. 47 It is important to remember the measure of arousal used (i.e., orgasm), and the method of deriving fulfill- ment of interpersonal needs. The results of this study, cannot be validly compared to studies using other measures. Orgasm, as Fisher (1973) has demonstrated, is not equiva- lent to satisfaction with a sexual experience. The inter- personal gratification scores at times were discrepant from what would have been expected from the women's state- ments about whether they liked or disliked their experi- ences . Interpersonal Gratification and Other Variables The lack of significant difference of the ages of the women in the coitus-last orgasmic and non-orgasmic groups suggests that no biases were introduced by inadver- tently having the older subjects in the orgasmic group and the younger ones in the non-orgasmic group. .Kinsey's £3 31. (1953) finding of a correlation of age and decade of birth with orgasm would have made the interpersonal grati- fication effect difficult to interpret if the subjects had been stratified in this manner. The difference in variance 48 of the ages of the orgastic and non-orgastic coitus-last women seems due to a lack of random sampling.- The distribution of reported percentage of orgasm attainment in this sample was bimodal. Only 28.5% of the women reported experiencing orgasms 25-74% of the time; whereas, thirty-three percent of the women reported exper- iencing orgasms 0-24% of the time and 38.3% reported ex- periencing orgasms 75-100% of the time. Hence 71% of the subjects were on the upper and lower ends of reported per- centage of orgasm attainment. Kinsey et_al, (1953) found that 36% of the married women he sampled reported exper- iencing orgasm 0-29% of the time during their 1st year of marriage and 39% reported experiencing orgasm 90-100% of the time. Though the populations are not strictly compar- able, the similarity in distributions is apparent,, Orgasms may be consistently experienced or not, with less variabil- ity than is usually considered. The finding that the mean interpersonal gratifica- tion of orgasmic coitus—last subjects reporting low per- centages of orgasm attainment (O-24%) was not significantly different than nonorgasmic coitus-last subjects is inter- esting. 49 The orgasmic coitus-last women in this range of orgasm attainment could have relatively low gratification of interpersonal needs and still experience an orgasm; the nonorgasmic coitus-last women could have relatively high gratification of interpersonal needs and not report an orgasm. Situational interpersonal factors would appear to make less difference in this low orgasm attainment group. Unfortunately, the design of this investigation prohibits evaluating such possible explanation of female hyposexual- ity as oedipal ties, penis envy, separation anxiety, nega- tive social learning and hormonal factors. Further re- search is necessary to clarify the result. One puzzling result was the dispersion in inter- personal gratification scores of the women reporting very high (75-100%) or very low (O-24%) percentages of orgasm attainment. The variances of the orgasmic and nonorgasmic coitus-last women were significantly different in these groups. The women not experiencing orgasms had more ex- treme scores. No explanation is immediately apparent which accounts for the significant differences in variance of bgth the high and low percentage of orgasm attainment groups. 50 The data on presence or absence of relationships, types of relationships, and duration of relationships clearly augments the need to study sexual behavior situa- tionally. Since adequate interpersonal gratification is the best predictor of orgasm, assumptions which devalue the interpersonal gratification possible with “one night stands'I should be abandoned as well as the assumptions about the interpersonal fulfillment of women with rela- tionships of long duration. Gross categorizations of relationships seems unfruitful. This investigation only evaluated a subset of the domain of interactions possible between people. However, every category of interpersonal needs received some scor- ings. While more women were concerned with affiliative, succorant, and nurturant themes, consistent with sex role learning (Apperson, 1973), to assume that certain needs (e.g., aggression) do not belong in bed certainly tidies up analysis of interpersonal interactions but does not elucidate them. In addition, as$umptions about which in- terpersonal needs are appropriate in a sexual context seem unwarranted and moralistic. The data on number of words used in the coital descriptions indicates that the interpersonal gratification 51 scores were not subject to biases by orgasmic status. In particular, the women who were orgasmic did not tend to write long reports and the inorgasmic women did not tend to write short ones. In addition, the mean time elapsed since coitus indicated that biases which were a result of hazy or accurate recollection due to time were not intro- duced by orgasmic status. Methodological Considerations It can be questioned whether because subjects volunteered for a study dealing with sexual matters, the subjects were a biased sample. However, according to Fisher (1973), Diamant, Martin, Marcuse, and Siegman (1956) have not found any substantial personality differences be- tween persons who volunteer for a "sex study" and those who do not. The women in this study reported whether or not theerxperienced an orgasm during their last sexual exper- ience and their percentages of orgasmic attainment. It is not known whether or not the women Werelying. The women could have felt the need to lie for a variety of reasons. They might have lied to boost their self-esteem if consis- tently inorgasmic. They may have felt that they were 52 devaluing their interpersonal relationship if they didn't experience an orgasm and lied to I'prove their love." They may have been orgasmic and reported being inorgasmic feel- it immoral to “enjoy" coitus. However, it should be noted that criticisms of studies on the grounds of subject lying are appropriate to all sex studies not monitoring subject reports with electrophysiological recordings. Some subjects may not have lied, but their records could have been distorted because of ignorance. They may not have known what the experience of orgasm feels like and reported an orgasm while being inorgasmic simply be- cause of their lack of knowledge. Not knowing what an orgasm feels like was probably the case for the three coitus-last subjects who did not report whether or not they experienced an orgasm. The orgasmic status of two subjects in the present study is dubious. These women indicated they experienced an orgasm but wrote comments about the nature of the experience which casts doubt on the probability of their having experienced an orgasm. One of these women wrote, “orgasm equals being elated and happy because he's the one you're with.“ The other woman wrote, "I enjoyed it, therefore I assume I had an orgasm." 53 If the present investigation had included a re- quest of the women to describe what their orgasms felt like (if they experienced them) the quality of this data could have been improved. Lying could not have been con- trolled for, but mistakes due to ignorance about the na- ture of an orgasm could have been estimated. This investigation could also have been improved if the Sexual History Form had been modified. The nature of the women's sexual experience should have been random- ized. In other words, rather than starting with kissing and ending with masturbation, the order of these exper- iences should have been randomly selected. The women may have interpreted an order and felt reluctant to mark that they had experienced what they perceived the investigator as considering progressively more "loose“ sexual behavior. The differences in sizes of the groups when the data was collected may have influenced the candor of the subjects in unknown ways. Some subjects may have felt “more anonymous" in a large group and have given the sex- ual information requested more freely. Other subjects may have felt more comfortable with less women around them. The sex of the experimenters administering the forms may have influenced the subject's responses. Some women might 54 have felt more comfortable with a female experimenter and others with a male. In addition, there may have been an interaction of size of the groups tested and sex of the experimenter. Since no estimate of the candor of the women was available, these effects cannot be ascertained. It has been repeatedly pointed out that the mea- sure of orgasm as one aspect of sexuality is not appropri- ately generalized to all other aspects of sexuality. In addition, trait analysis of personality and orgasm has been shown to be largely unfruitful. However, it should also be noted that orgasm as a dependent measure simply assumes a phenotypic identity between individuals. No investigations have found a genotypic identity of orgasm. The factors leading up to the same result (i.e., orgasm) may be entirely different for individuals (e.g., genetic, physiological, psychological). Directions for Future Research More research needs to be done on the validity of the measure used in this study. Research should also be done on different samples of subjects, particularly sub- jects married, older, and of different socioeconomic statuses and educational levels. 55 The consistency of interpersonal needs from sexual experience to sexual experience would also be interesting to ascertain as well as the relationship, if any, between personality traits of women and their interpersonal needs expressed in coital relationships. Additionally, the measure of interpersonal grati- fication used here could be fruitfully related to other measures of sexual behavior such as degree of strength of orgasm, satisfaction with coital experiences, frequency of coitus, etc. Of particular interest would be a situational approach to the relationship between the fulfillment of interpersonal needs of men and measures of their sexual behavior (e.g., erection, orgasm, premature ejaculation). Also the type of interpersonal needs salient for men dur- ing any particular coital experience would be useful to establish for comparison with women. Speculative Theory of Female Sexual BehaVior Drawing many concepts from a psychodynamic approach a model concerning female sexual functioning is developed here. The model is highly speculative and syncretic. It 56 is presented, not because it explains the data in the pres- ent study in an empirical sense, but as a model with hypo- theses to be used in further testing. Freud laid the groundwork for combining interper- sonal and sexual behavior. He recognized "that human sexu- ality is by no means limited to the impulses and actions that lead more or less directly to sexual intercourse (Fenichel, 1945, p. 55).“ In addition, Freud explained many phenomena as sexual which had not previously been recognized as having any connection with sexuality. Early in his theorizing Freud postulated the “ego instincts" and sexual instincts. The "ego instincts" were countersexual forces such as anxiety, guilt feelings, and ethical ideals of the personality which served the purpose of preserving the individual. Later, Freud viewed the repressing and repressed (the ego and id) as having evolved from the same source-~the‘sexual instincts. Freud felt that the sexual instincts had to be tamed in order for society to develop. He postulated that sexual needs, in- stead of uniting men, separated them. The instincts were viewed as "a borderline concept between the mental and physical; "phenomena of instincts can be considered from the physical aspect by examining 57 the source of the instinct, or they can be considered from the mental aspect by examining the drive and the resultant psychological phenomena (Fenichel, 1945, p. 54)." He recognized that affects can effect bodily changes (e.g., affect equivalence, conversion symptoms). He posthlated that, in general, “excitement and satisfaction are pheno- mena that express themselves in a physical way (Fenichel, 1945, p. 216)." It is proposed that any particular coital experi- ence reflects an individual's physiological and emotional state at that time. There may or may not be consistency in these states from one coital experience-to another. The physiological basis of coital behavior-is presumed to be mediated by hormones, particularly androgen (Whalen, 1966; Money, 1961; Waxenberg gt+31,, 1959; Foss, 1951; Salmon and Geist, 1943; Carter gt_al., 1947). Following Whalen (1966), arousal is distinguished from arousabilityt The former refers to the momentary level of sex- ual excitation of an individual,.maximal arousal characterized by the occurrence of orgasm. Arousability is the characteristic rate of approach to orgasm as a result of sexual stimu- lation of an individual. Arousability is de- termined not only by the amount-of increment in arousal produced by sexual stimuli, but also 58 by the absolute number of erotic stimuli which simultaneously arouse the individual (p. 84). Hormones are hypothesized to affect arousability by altering the threshold for responsiveness to erotic stimuli. According to Whalen (1966), current data does not allow precise formulation of physiological relation- ships between hormones and arousability.- However, he hypothesizes: hormones act centrally be determining the amount of change in arousal produced by a given stimulus, and act peripherally by de- termining the amount of-receptor response to a stimulus, and/or.the.rate of adapta- tion of the receptor (in Barnhardt and Lie- berman, 1971, p. 84). It is this author's premise.that sexual.stimu1a- tion is physical stimulation, occurring within the context of what the individual perceives (unconsciously, precon- sciously, or consciously) to be.a sexual situation, which satisfies interpersonal needs. Stimulation may be of one or more of the tactile, auditory, and visual receptors. In this view, both physical stimulation of erogenous zones and fulfillment of interpersonal needs is postulated to have arousing physiological effects, similar to Freud's view of the changes which can be effected by "mental phe- nomena." This interpersonal "stimulation" is considered 59 usually insufficient for maximal arousal, physical stimu- lation also being considered necessary. Physical stimu- lation is deemed intrinsically rewarding. If it is assumed that fulfillment of interpersonal needs is intrinsically rewarding, similar to physical stim- ulation, a difficulty is encountered.: Stated baldly, why aren't people continually aroused in-their interpersonal interactions? One could speculate that they are and are not attending to it. This arousal would be similar to that derived from tactile stimulation occurring outside the context of a sexual situation.. For instance, in this braless age, soft blouses.rubbing.against nipples may elicit nipple erection, but few women automatically rush off to copulate when this occurs.. However,aimore feasible approach is the postulation of ego mediation and a taming of the sexual instincts, as Freud suggested. The major mode of this taming of sexual instincts is postulated to be ego defenses which ward off drive; the situations in which the appropriateness of sexual activities are learned is postulated to derive from the individual's developmental learning background. This model assumes that individuals vary in their arousability. However, a certain amount-of physical and 6O interpersonal stimulation is necessary for maximal arousal (i.e., orgasm). It is also assumed that not all interper- sonal needs are consciously or preconsciously perceived. Therefore, women will perceive the fulfillment or lack of fulfillment of interpersonal needs solely in terms of the needs they are aware of. A.woman’5 perception of her partner is therefore neither strictly dependent on her partner's personality characteristics in his interaction with her, or on the "objective“ interaction between them. Because of the above assumption, a woman may have had certain interpersonal needs met which were outside of her awareness, had an orgasm, and dislike her partner be- cause he did not fulfill consciously perceived interper- sonal needs. An example is the wifeeof an alcoholic. She may perceive that her husband is not nurturant or caring with her and be dissatisfied with him, but at the same time, get sufficient gratification in her need to be dom- inant or behave in a punitively aggressive fashion, berat- ing him for his drunken, nonnurturant~behavior, to have an orgasm. Likewise, a woman not having an-orgasm because of insufficient fulfillment of unconscious needs, may perceive that her partner has satisfied all the interpersonal needs 61 she is aware of. For instance, a woman who consciously desires a relationship with strong affiliative, equali- tarian features may unconsciously need a relationship which satisfies strong passive dependent.strivings or' may wish to be dominated. Interpersonal needs.are assumed to derive from a woman's developmental background. The needs a woman is aware of are dependent upon her defensive and character- olotical structure. It should be stressed that in this model all women have needs or drives the preferred aims and objects of which they are-not aware of because aware- ness would promote too much anxiety.. Itris also assumed that the interpersonal needs salient for an individual may vary from sexual situation to sexual situation. Interpersonal needs are postulated to be expressed and fulfilled in both direct and indirect ways. In partic- ular, identical forms of techniques of foreplay may symbo- lize for women a variety of interpersonal needs, and the same forms of stimulation may vary in terms of their sym- bolic interpersonal significance from sexual experience to sexual experience for an individual woman. The emotional and physical state of the indiVTHual upon which both physical and interpersonal stimulation 62 operate, is assumed to be mediated by anxiety or lack of anxiety about sexual behavior, and anxiety or lack of anxiety about the expression of interpersonal needs, both of which are presumed to be learned within the context of the individual's cultural mileu. -According to Masters and Johnson (1966), "Anxiety inhibits sexual arousability in that it involves the sympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system, whereas, sexual arousal involves the para- sympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system before orgasm, and the sympathetic branch only later (in Paul, 1973, p. 2)." If anxiety occurs, it is assumed that de- fenses Operate in the service of reducing that anxiety, and that these defenSes may or may not be effective. The standard Freudian defenses: repression, projection, reac- tion formation, negation, iSolation, undoing, reversal, regression, and sublimation (Mahl, 1971) are those de- fenses are those defenses assumed to operate in the con- trol of anxiety. REFERENCES REFERENCES Apperson, Jean. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Mich- igan State University, 1973. Blalock, H. M., Jr. Social statistics. New York: McGraw- Hill Book Company, 1960. Carter, A. C., Cohen, E. J., & Shorr, E. The use of andro- gen in women. In Harris, R. S. and Thimann, K. V. (Eds.) Vitamin and hormones. New York: Academic Press, 1947, 317-391. Chesser, Eustace. The sexual, marital and-family relation- ships of the English woman. New York: Roy Pub- lishers, 1957. Cooper, A. J. Some personality factors-in frigidity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 1969, 13, 149-155. Coppen, A. The prevalence of menstrual disorders in psy- chiatric patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1965, 111, 155-167. Dickinson, R. L. and Beam, Lura. A thousand marrigges. A medical study of sex adjustment. Baltimore, Md.: The Williams and Wilkins Co., 1931. Fenichel, O. The psychoanalytic theory of neurosis. New York: N. W. Norton & Company, Inc., 1945. Fisher, 5. The female orgasm. Psychology,-physiology, fantasy. New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1973. Foss, G. L. The influence of androgens on sexuality in women. Lancet, 1951, 1, 667-669. 63 64 Fox, 0. J. and Guire, K. E. Documentation for MIDAS. Michigan Interactive data analyais system. (2nd ed.) Developed at The Statistical Research Labor- atory of the University of Michigan, 1973. Freedman, M. B. The sexual behavior of American college women: An empirical study and an historical sur- vey. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of-BehaVior and De- velopment. 1965, 11, 33-48. Gebhard, P. H. Factors in marital orgasm. Journal of Social Issues, 1966, 22, 88—95. Hays, W. L. Statistics for the social sciencess (2nd ed.) New York: Holt, Rinehart-anerinston,-Inc., 1973. Hunt, M. Sexual behavior in the-19705. «Chicago: Playboy Press, 1974. Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W., Martin, C., & Gebhard, P. Sexual behavior in the human female. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders Company, 1953. Landis, C., Landis, Agnes T., & Bolles, M. M. Sex in de- velopment. New York: 'Paul B. Hoeber, 1940. Lindley, D. V. Introduction to probability and statistics from a Bayesian viewpoint. Part 2. Inference. Cambridge: The syndics of the Cambridge Univer- sity Press, 1965. Mahl, G. F. Psychological conflict and defense. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc., 1971. Masters, W. H. and Johnson, Virginia E. Human sexual re- sponse. Boston: Little, Brown, and Company, 1966. Masters, W. H. and Johnson, Virginia E. Human sexual in- adeguacy. Boston: Little, Brown, and Company, 1970. McCulloch, D. J. and Stewart, Joan C. Sexual norms in a psychiatric population. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1960, l 1, 70—73. 65 Miller, H. and Wilson, W. Relation of sexual behaviors, values, and conflict to avowed happiness and per- sonal adjustment. Paychological Reports, 1968, 233 1075-1086. Money, J. Components of eroticism in man: 1. The hor- mones in relation to sexual morphology and sexual morphology and sexual desire. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders. 1961, 1 2, 239-248. Murray, H. A. Explorations inypersonality. New York: Oxford University Press, 1938. O'Neill, Marion and Kempler, 8. Approach and avoidance responses of the hysterical personality to sexual stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1969, 11, 300-305. Paul, G. L. and Bernstein, D. A. Anxiety and clinical problems: systematic desensitization and related techniques. Morristown: General Learning Press, 1973. Purtell, J. J., Robins, E., & Cohen, M.E. Observations on clinical aspects of hysteria. Journal of American Medical Association, 1951, 14 , 902-909. Rosenhan, D. L. On being sane in insane places. Science, 1973, 180, 250-258. Rosenhan, D. L. The contextual nature of psychiatric diag- nosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1975 (in press). Salmon, U. J. and Geist, S. H. Effect of androgens upon libido in women. Journal of Clinical Endocrinol- ogy, 1943, a, 235-238. Sarbin, T. H. Studies in behavior pathology. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1967. Schofield, M. The sexual behavior of young people. Bos- ton: Little, Brown, and Company, 1965. 66 Siegman, A. Responses to a personality questionnaire by volunteers and nonvolunteers to a Kinsey interview. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1956, sa, 280-281. ‘ Sorenson, R. C. Adolescent sexuality in contemporary Amer- iga. Personal values and sexual behavior ages 13- 19, New York: World Publishing, 1973. Swensen, C. H., Jr. Sexual behavior and psychopathology: A test of Mowrer's hypothesis. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1962, 18, 406-409. Terman, L. M. Psychological factors in marital happiness. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1938. Terman, L. M. Correlates of orgasm adequacy in a group of 556 wives. Journal of Psyphology. 1951, 32, 115- 172. Waxenberg, S. E., Drellich, M. C., & Sutherland, A. M. The role of hormones in human behavior: 1. Changes in female sexuality after adrenalectomy. Journal of Clinical Endrocrinology and Metabolism, 1959. 123 193-202. ,Whalen, R. E. In Bernhardt and Lieberman (Eds.) Human sexual behavior: A book of readings. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1971, 78-89. Winokur, G., Guze, S. B., & Pfeiffer, E. Developmental and sexual factors in women: A comparison between control, neurotic and psychotic groups. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1958-59, 115, 1097-1100. Zelnick, M. and Kantner, J. Sexuality, contraception and pregnancy among young unwed females in the United States. In U.S. Commission on Population Growth and the American Future, Demographic and Social Aspects of Population Growth (1972), q. v. APPENDICES APPENDIX A (Sexual description form) In the space below please describe your last sexual exper- ience. Please include your thoughts and feelings about this experience. Was intercourse involved (yes/no). Was this experience with a male or female. Approximate time elapsed since the experience you are describing ( ). Your partner's initials . Did you experience an orgasm (yes/no). 67 (sexual history form) Age Are you married (yes/no). If yes, how long have you been married . What are your husbands initials Are you currently unmarried but living with someone (yes/ no). If yes, how long have you been living together 00 you plan to be married (yes/no). If yes, do you have a date of marriage tentatively planned (yes/no). When . Your partner's initials Do you currently have a "steady" boyfriend (yes/no). If yes, how long have you been involved with each other 00 you plan to be married (yes/no). If yes, do you have a tentative date of marriage planned (yes/no). When . Your partner's initials Are you involved in a relationship that has not been de- scribed above (yes/no). If yes, please briefly describe it: Check which of the following you have experienced: A. kissing 8. being stimulated on the breast by someone else (without clothing) C. being stimulated around and in your vagina manu- ally by someone else (without clothing) 0. oral stimulation of your vagina E intercourse (penetration of vagina by penis) F. masturbation Did you experience orgasm during any of the activities (yes/no). If yes, which: A B C D E F If you have been engaging in intercourse, how long would you estimate you have been so doing . How many 68 69 partners in intercourse would you estimate you've had How many times per month would you estimate you have intercourse now . If you have ex— perienced orgasm during intercourse, what percentage of the time would you estimate you have orgasm during inter- course now . Do you use contraceptives (yes/ no). If yes, what kind? APPENDIX B (definitions of interpersonal needs) Succorance To have others provide help when in trouble, to seek encouragement from others, to have others be kindly, to have others by sympathetic and understanding about per- sonal problems, to receive a great deal of affection from others, to have others do favors cheerfully, to be helped by others when depressed, to have others feel sorry when one is sick or hurt; to solicit sympathy, affection, or emotional support from others. Aggression To engage in behaviors which attack or hurt others; to tell others off when disagreeing with them, to get re- venge for insults, to become angry, to blame others when things go wrong. Dominance To argue for one's point of view, to be regarded by others as a leader, to make decisions, to persuade and influence others to do what one wants, to supervise and direct the actions of others, to be influential and con- trolling in individual relationships. Achievement To do one's best, to be successful, to accomplish tasks requiring skill and effort, to be a recognized authority, to do a difficult job well, to solve difficult problems and puzzles, to be able to do things better than others. Nurturance To engage in behaviors which extend material or emo- tional benefits to others; to help friends when they are 70 71 in trouble, to treat others with kindness and sympathy, to forgive others, to do small favors for others, to be gen- erous with others, to sympathize with others who are hurt or sick, to show a great deal of affection toward others, to have others confide in one about personal problems. Affiliation-Intimacy To be loyal to friends, to do things for friends, to share things with friends, to do things with another per— son rather than alone, to form strong attachments. Exhibition To behave in such a way as to elicit the immediate attention of others; to talk about personal adventures and experiences, to have others notice and comment upon one's appearance, to be the center of attention. Deference To seek and sustain subordinate roles in relationship with others; to get suggestions from others, to find out what others think, to follow instructions and do what is expected, to praise others, to tell others that they have done a good job, to accept leadership by others, to con- form to custom and avoid the unconventional, to let others make decisions. From Apperson (1973), p. 136-137. APPENDIX C Scoring Instructions Divide the protocol into meaningful phrases. Compare each phrase of the subject's protocol to the sentences in the scale. If any of the sentences in the scale are equi- valent to what the subject is saying in her phrase, write above the phrase the category name, and the sentence num- ber which is matched. For instance, if the subject says, “I told Joe all my thoughts and feelings," write above the sentence Aff la. Context in the protocol is utilized. For example, if the subjects say, "It was good," find out what "it" refers to from the context of her record and score if appropriate. If the subject is speaking about a general situation or a generalized other, score that phrase as if the subject were speaking about herself. If the subject uses serial adjectives, nouns, or verbs with the same theme, score each adjective, noun or verb. For instance, if the sub- ject says, "he made me feel wanted, needed, important, whole," write above the subject's sentence Suc 4a x 4. Score each phrase for one and only one category, except the examples provided under "conventions of scoring." 72 73 Conventions of scoring 1. The distinction between Nur 7a and Aff 23 and 6a is that Nur 7a is used when intercourse is a gift to the subject's partner. Score, "We plan to be married," and "We are going steady," Aff 6a. However, ganlt score, "My fiance and I," or ”My steady and I." The latter are usually used in place of the subjects partner's name. Score, "I feel sex is okay if you love each other," Aff 6a. 00 not score talking or doing activities together (e.g., taking a shower, lying together) unless the interpersonal needs involved are clear and strong. Score context sparingly. For example, if no inter- personal needs were scored throughout the subject's protocol, and then the subject says, "It was beauti- ful," at the end, "It was beautiful," might be scored depending on the context, but you should 333 then score other phrases not previously scored. Score, "I love him and he loves me," Aff 6a and Sue 2a. Score "We love each other," Aff 6a. Don't usually score sexual techniques, unless it's very, very clear what those techniques mean to the 74 subject interpersonally. 8. Score, "We both like trying to please each other," Suc 3a and Nur 2a. "A" sentences are equal to +1 and indicate satisfac- tion of a particular need. "B" sentences are equal to -l and contain both unfulfilled needs (e.g., I didn't feel taken care of by my partner) and negated needs (e.g., I don't like talking about my sexual experiences). The assumption is that a negated need is unacceptable to the subject and was not fulfilled in the experience the sub- ject is describing. Algebraically sum the interpersonal needs. Count the number of words and divide into the summed needs. In this corrected total, a negative overall score indicates that the subject's interpersonal needs were unfulfilled. A positive score indicates that, overall, the subject's interpersonal needs were fulfilled. The corrected total is a correction in order that the magnitude of the sub- ject's score is not simply a function of brevity or length of her record. Interpersonal Gratification Scale AFFILIATION l. a. I shared my feelings and thoughts with my sexual partner. b. I didn't share my feelings and thoughts with my partner. 2. a. I wanted sexual relations because I was experienc- ing warm and friendly feelings toward my partner. b. I wanted sexual relations, but not because I was experiencing warm and friendly feelings toward my partner. 3. a. We shared an (intense), intimate experience through our sexual relations. b. We didn't share an (intense), intimate experience through our sexual relations. 4. a. I am loyal to my partner. b. I am not loyal to my partner. 5. a. I prefer intercourse to masturbation as an outlet for my sexual tensions. b. I prefer masturbation to intercourse as an outlet for my sexual tensions. 6. a. I feel a strong emotional attachment for my sexual partner. 75 6. I don't feel a strong emotional attachment for my sexual partner. 7. We share (many) other activities and this lessens my enjoyment of our sexual activities. DEFERENCE l. I went along with my partner's desire for sex though I was not (originally) in the mood for it. I couldn't go along with my partner's desire for sex because I was not in the mood for it. 2. He initiated the sexual relations. I wanted him to initiate the sexual relations in- stead of me. 3. I like following his lead in sexual relations. I wanted to follow his lead in sexual relations but couldn't. 4. My main interest was in pleasing him. I couldn't please him. 5. I like to have sex the conventional way. I don't like sex when it's in the conventional way. 6. I like following the guidelines for sexual tech- 76 nique. 77 6 b. I don't like following guidelines for sexual technique. 7. a I feel he is superior to me in (many) ways. b. I don't feel he is superior to me. AGGRESSION 1. a. I felt a close link between sex and aggressive feelings. b. I don't feel sexual when I am angry. 2. a I got even with my partner for insulting or hurt- ing me by either refusing sex when he wanted to or pushing sexual relations on him when he didn't want to. b. I didn't get even with my partner by pushing sex on him or refusing to have sex with him even though he had hurt and/or insulted me. 3. a Things went wrong. It was his fault. b. Things went wrong. It was not his fault. 4. a I felt sexually aroused when I was angry. b. I don't feel sexual when I am angry (l b). 5. a I had fantasies of attacking or hurting him. b. I didn't have fantasies of attacking or hurting him. 78 My aggressive urges were expressed in a satisfying way through our sexual relations. My aggressive urges were unsatisfied in our sexual relations. We tusseled around, kind of fighting each other in our sexual relations. He didn't want to “play fight" with me. NURTURANCE l. I showed him a great deal of affection. I couldn't show him much affection. I helped him feel cared for. I couldn't help him feel cared for. I did small favors for him. I didn't do any small favors for him. I helped him feel at ease in our sexual relations. I couldn't help him feel at ease in our sexual relations. My partner was feeling down. I comforted him. My partner was feeling down. I couldn't comfort him. He confided in me and told me his troubles. He doesn't confide in me or tell me his troubles. 79 7. a. I tried to fulfill his needs (in every way I could). b. I couldn't/didn't fulfill his needs. DOMINANCE l. a. I experienced a feeling of power in possessing my partner sexually. b. I felt powerless in my sexual relations with my partner. 2. a. Our sexual relations involve a pleasurable element of a "contest of wills." b. The pleasurable element of a "contest of wills'I was lacking in our sexual relations. 3. a. Our sexual relations are a contest in which there is a winner and a loser. b. I feel lost in the "contest" of our sexual rela- tions. 4. a. I felt in charge of directing my sexual interaction With my partner. b. I wanted to direct my sexual interaction with my partner but didn't. 5. a. I took the lead in initiating sexual relations. b. I wanted to lead in initiating sexual relations but he did. 8O 6. a. I persuaded him to go to bed with me. b. I wanted to persuade him to go to bed but he per- suaded me. 7. a. We had sexual relations because I wanted to have them. b. We didn't have sexual relations though I wanted to very badly. SUCCORANCE 1. a. I felt secure in the close contact of our (sexual) relations. b. I didn't feel secure in our (sexual) relations. 2. a. My partner showed a great deal of affection toward me. b. My partner didn't show much affection toward me. 3. a. I felt taken care of by my partner. b. I didn't feel taken care of by my partner. 4. a. My partner was sympathetic and understanding toward me. b. My partner did not understand or feel sympathetic toward me. 5. a. My partner does (little) things just to please me. b. My partner doesn't do the little things I need to please me. 6. a I like the warm comfortable feeling of being in my partner's arms. b. My partner didn't want to hold me in his arms. 7. a My partner is someone I can count on in rough times. b. I can't count on my partner during times of trouble. EXHIBITION l. a. I like him looking at my body. b. I didn't want him looking at my body. 2. a He noticed and commented upon my appearance. b. He didn't notice or comment upon my appearance. 3. a I like talking about my sexual encounters and experiences. b. I don't like talking about my encounters or experiences. 4. a I am the center of attention with my partner. b. I am not the center of attention with my partner. 5. a I felt sexy and sophisticated. b. I didn't feel sexy or sophisticated. 6. a I undressed before him. 81 I didn't want to undress before him. 1 82 7. a. During sexual relations I am aware of the way I look and sound. b. I don't want to think about the way I look and sound. ACHIEVEMENT l. a. I feel I am a very good lover. b. I don't feel I am a good lover. 2. a. I like to puzzle out or anticipate what will be most successful with my partner. b. I am not very good in figuring out what will be successful with my partner. 3. a. I think I know a lot about sex. b. I don't know very much about sex. 4. a. I feel I performed well sexually. b. I don't feel I performed well sexually. 5. a. I feel I am more able to be a skillful sex partner than other people. b. I don't feel I am more able to be a skillful lover than other people.‘ 6. a. I feel I have the skill and make the effort for successful lovemaking. b. I don't feel I have the skill or make enough effort for successful lovemaking. HICHIGRN STRTE UNIV. LIBRARIES l "Ill! H 111 ll 1 31293 02064791