» I ~ . . AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP OF MORAL RESPONSES OF 12 YEAR OLD BOYS AND MORAL RESPONSES OF THEIR FATHERS AND MOTHERS Thai: for the Dogma of M. A. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY .10 Anne H. Lifshin I966 I11!lllllllflzblflfljilllfllljfllfllllfljll“MIMI FFFFFF University JF— LIBR A R Y Michigan State ' T ABSTRACT AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP OF MORAL RESPONSES OF 12 YEAR OLD BOYS AND MORAL RESPONSES OF THEIR FATHERS AND MOTHERS by Jo Anne H. Lifshin Investigations of children's moral responses explicitly or implicitly recognize the acquisition of moral standards through incorporation of parents' stand- ards. This modeling is assumed to assure continuation of the parent's love and may be directly reinforced. This research was designed to investigate the relationship be- tween moral responses of twelve year old boys and their parents' moral responses. The investigation focused on relationships between the boys' responses and their mothers' and fathers' reports of their own responses at age twelve, the average of these responses, perceptions of the child's responses by the father and mother, and the average of these perceptions. In addition to a total guilt score and scores for three areas of transgression, the following forms of moral responses were assessed: direct acknow- ledgment, indirect manifestations, defense mechanisms, and externalization. It was hypothesized that significant relationships would be found between boys' moral response scores and Jo Anne H. Lifshin were more consistently interrelated than children's or mothers' scores. Patterns of significant relationships were found for forms of moral response. Fathers' indirect manifestations were generally related to children's scores, and mothers' defense mechanisms were highly related to children's responses. Resistance to temptation data showed a significant relationship only between children's responses and mothers' perceptions of children's responses to disobedience. Generally greater similarity was found between children's responses and parents' reports of their own responses at 12 than between children's responses and parents' perceptions of their responses. The anaclitic model of identification, as postulated by social learning theory, was more directly supported by the data than was defensive identification, particularly in reference to death wishes and disobedience. It was concluded that the mother may provide a more nurturant object, whose continued love is dependent on conformity to her standard, and the child may respond more to this reinforcement than to motivation for global incorporation of the father's standards. The research findings suggested that the father may present a less situation- ally dependent model for moral standards than the mother and may be more influential in culturally determined situations. It was also suggested that the child may model his superego on the parent's superego as indicated by Jo Anne H. Lifshin their parents' corresponding scores. Greater similarity was predicted between boys' responses and fathers' re— sponses than between boys' responses and mothers' re— sponses. Twenty-six 12 year old, seventh grade boys from Traverse City Junior High School and their fathers and mothers participated in the research. Story completion items from Allinsmith (2), concerning death wishes, theft and disobedience, with related forced choice questions derived from possible responses to stories, were administered to the boys at school. The same items were responded to by parents for self at twelve years and perceptions of child's responses. Pearson product moment correlation coefficients and phi coefficients, where appropriate, were computed for guilt and resistance to temptation data. Findings showed limited support for hypotheses of significant relationships between children's and parents' responses for moral areas and forms of moral response. No relationship was found between total guilt of boys and total guilt scores of parents. Major findings included greater mother—child similarity than father—child similarity; this finding was non-supportive of the hypothesized relationship, which was derived from-the psychoanalytic theory of identifica- tion. Greater mother-child similarity appeared for death wishes-and disobedience than for theft. Fathers' scores Jo Anne H. Lifshin greater similarity between parents' and children's in- directly manifested responses than between their di- rectly acknowledged responses. Approved: LJJCM1I. mm. H Q Amv mocmflUmDOmHQ x cm. u n Amv chose x u n AHV nonnfiz cocoa Amv Amv va Amy sz Amv Amv AHV mapmfihm> manmflmm> .UHHQO ho mmeHOOm QHHSU MO hOCODmHmCOU HNCMODCHIF.m. *mmw. **om. **mm. **mw. **mm. **mm. H A Amv Hmuoe x **mm. **ow. *xmm. **mm. **ww. *amw. u a Auv coapmmfiamcsouxm x *xwm. *mow. **Hm. *xmm. **mm. H a Amv mEchwcooz oncomoo x *xmm. 3*05. **mm. **Nm. u & Amv mCOHOMOmOMHcmz OOOLHOCH x **mm. **~>. **mw. u L sz pcoewpoflzosxo< commas x *xmo. **mm. H L Amv mocmHOmOOmHQ N **Hm. u h Amy DMOQB x u n AHV cocoa: spoon va Amv Amv Amv sz Amv Amv AHV manwfimm> manmfipm> .OSHoze um monumm mo mmsoom OHHBU mo homepmfimcoo HOCLOOCHII. a OHOmB 2 Ho. v o** mo. v o* 61 ccos. u s Amv Hcooe mm. n s va OOHOHNHHHCLmOxm **mm. ..I.. «H Amv mEchmzomz oncomom **m>. u h Amv mCOHpmmeMHcmz pompHUsH *o:. u p Aqv psoEmOoasocxo¢ poopHQ mm. H p Amv mocmaomnomfim ma. u & Amv phone x u L AHV mmsmfiz gamma manmwhm> Adv oaocfisc> .o>ao39 um compo: mo mmhoom uafiso mo monoumfimcoo HMOQOOOHII.QH magma 62 mw u z Ho..H.Q** mo. v Q* x **mw. **om. **mw. *xam. **mm. *xmm. **mm. H & va Hmpoe x **mm. **oa. *gmc. **ca. **ma. **mm. u n ARV coflocNHHocnooxm X **mw. **N©. **ww. **Nm. **mm. H h Amv mEchmzooz oncomoa x **mm. **:m. **ww. **H©. u h Amv mQOHpmumochmz pomLHUCH x **ww. **mw. mm. H A sz pcoemooazocxom poopao x *xmm. *m:. u a Amv.oo:mHOOOOmHQ x Hm. u a Amv umoce x n a AHV momma: Samoa Awe Ago loo lav isv Ami imv AHV chocncc> maamwsm> .o>ao39 pm pconmm mwmhmxwd. (HO wmcHOOm uHHSU rHO hoflmpmHmCOO HMCVHQDCHII.QH GHQMB 63 :m u 2 Ho. VI o. 3" * m C) V o. * **Nm. **mw. *xwm. *xmw. **mw. **mm. **:w. *xmw. *amm. *xom. **>w. **mm.. *xaw. X **ow. *oz. **w©. **ww. **ow. x *m:. *xaw. **©m. **>m. x ##05. **MN. **©m. X **:>. **mw. x xxmm. H L Amv HMPOE H L ANV COHPMNHHGCHMPXM n & va mEmficmnooz oncomoa u h Amv mCOAOmpmmchmz BOOLHOOH u s sz pcoEwOOHZcho< poopfio h Amv mocmfivopOmHQ u s Amv phone A AHV moans; cocoa va ARV on Amv sz Amv Amv AHV oeomwnc> .monoom oafioo n.oHHco maanhm> mo mcofipoapmpm m.honpmm mo mOcOpmHmcoo Hmspochll.mH manme 6A mm H 2 Ho. v Q** mo. v Q* x **mm. *xmm. *xwm. **:N. **:w. *xmm. **mw. n a Amv HmOOB N am. 3*wm. *mz. **N©. **NN. *N:. n h ANV COHpmNHHMChmpxm x **©m. :m. *m:. **wm. **mm. va mEchmnooz mmcwmom x *o:. *xoo. **mw. **mw. u a Amv mOOfipmpmomacmz poosfich x **mw. *xmw. *xmm. ucoEwOoazocmOm wwwpflo N **m©. **Nm. n & Amv moCmHUODOmHD N *xmm. u A ANV pthE x n n AHV cocoa: cocoa va Amv on mmv sz Amv Amv AHV oasHapm> .monoom passe m.oHHco OHOmHhm> mo mQOHuoHOOLm m.pocpoz mo unappmfimcoo HHCLOOCHII.mH manme 65 :m u 2 Ho. v o** mo. v o* x **mm. **Hm. **mm. *xaw. xxmm. **:m. **mm. u L va pr09 x *xwm. **mn. *om. **om. **mm. *azm. u L va COHOmNHHHCLouxm x **ow. mm. **mw. **ww. *xmw. u p va mamficmzooz Oncomoo x **mm. *xmw. **mm. **om. u h Amv mcoHBMOmOMH2mE poomHUcH x *xmw. **mw. **mw. n h sz pcoempmasocxo< poopfim x **mm. **om. L Amv cocoapmpomfia x **no. u a Amv phone x n AHV moans: cocoa Amv ARV on Amv sz Amv Amy AHV OHQwHum> ; .mmpoom OHHSU macaflno mo mQOHpoHOmpm mapcopmm owmso>< no mocmpmamcoo HMOAOOCHII.UH OHQmE manmfihm> Table 2.--Internal Consistency of Resistance to Temptation Versus Guilt for Theft and Disobedience. Theft Disobedience N Phi Chi Phi Chi Square Square Child 26 .A6* 5.AA .33 2.76 Father at 12 26 .25 1.59 .09 0.82 Mother at 12 26 -.06 0.78 .00 0.00 Father's Prediction of Child 2A .26 1.61 .05 0.07 Mother's Prediction of Child 26 .28 2.00 .29 2.13 *p 1 .05 Table 2A.—-Extended Internal Comparisons of Child's Resistance to Temptation and Guilt. Resistance to Temptation Theft Disobedience Guilt Phi Chi Phi Chi Square Square Theft .A6* 5.AA .2A 1.51 Disobedience .22 1.22 .33 2.76 Total .32 2.66 .32 2.58 26 a: *0 A O U! 2 II disobedience was significantly related for children. NO significant interrelations prevailed for mother at 12, and only the disobedience score was significantly related to death wishes and theft for average parent at 12. Parents' predictions of child's responses were more consistent than parents' responses at 12. 67 Scores for forms of moral response were consistently significantly related to total guilt, but few significant interrelations were found. Only indirect manifestations and externalization covaried consistently for children. All were consistently significant for father at 12, father's prediction of child's response, average parent at 12 and average parent prediction of child's response. Mother at 12 showed significant interrelations of all forms of response except defenses as related to direct acknowledgment, indirect manifestations and externalization. The same pattern prevailed for mother's prediction of child's response with the exception that defenses were significantly related to indirect manifestations. Comparison of forms of moral response to moral areas showed significant interrelations for children, except for comparison Of death wishes and externalization. This same pattern plus absence of significance for defenses compared with theft resulted for the mother at 12. The highest predictive power of total guilt for children existed in theft and disobedience situations. All contributed approximately equally for fathers at 12 (all above r = .80). Direct acknowledgment and indirect manifestations were less related for mother at 12; death wishes less predictive for average parent at 12; direct acknowledgment less predictive for father prediction; direct acknowledgment, defenses and externalization less predictive for mother prediction; and direct acknowledgment 68 provided poor prediction for combined parent's prediction of child's response. The only significant relationship of resistance to temptation and guilt, as may be noted from reference to Table 2, resulted from comparison of child's guilt and resistance to temptation for theft. Comparison of resistance to temptation for theft and disobedience as compared with total guilt for children tended toward significance (p i .10), and guilt versus resistance to temptation for disobedience resulted in phi of .33 (p i .10). Relation of resistance to temptation and guilt for parents was not significant in any case. Resistance to Temptation Data from phi coefficient analysis of resistance to temptation was generally non—supportive of Hypothesis II, as is indicated in Table 3. A significant relationship between 12 year old boys' moral responses and moral responses of parents was predicted. Child resistance to temptation in the disobedience situation was significantly related to mother's perception of child's resistance to temptation for disobedience. This finding is contrary to Hypothesis IV, that relationships hypothesized for 12 year Old boys‘ moral responses and fathers' predictions of their moral responses would be higher than the relationships between boys' responses and mothers' predictions of their responses. Prediction of child resistance to temptation by mothers and by fathers for theft was significant. 69 Table 3.—-Comparison of Child's Resistance to Temptation Versus Parents' Resistance to Temptation. Child's Resistance to Temptation Theft Disobedience Parents' Resistance Chi Chi to Temptation N Phi Square Phi Square Father at 12 Theft 26 —.0A 0.03 -.02 0.00 Disobedience 26 -.0A 0.03 -.10 0.29 Mother at 12 Theft 26 .37 3.A8 —.12 0.36* Disobedience 26 .10 0.26 .22 1.25 Father's Prediction of Child Theft 2A -.12 0.31 -.16 0.60 Disobedience 2A .0A 0.05 -.30 2.1A Mother's Prediction of Child Theft 26 .32 2.68 —.18 0.83 Disobedience 26 .16 0.65 .39* 3.90 *p 1.05 Those relationships tending toward significance (p i .10) were mother at 12 and child for theft, mother's perception of child's reSponse and child for theft, mother at 12 and father at 12 for theft, mother at 12 and father at 12 for disobedience, and mother's perception of child and father's perception of child for disobedience. Comparison of parent resistance to temptation and child guilt yielded no significant relationships. Child guilt for theft and mother at 12 resistance to temptation for theft tended toward significance (p i .10). 70 Table 3A.--Comparison of Father's Resistance to Temptation Versus Mother's Resistance to Temptation. Theft Disobedience Chi Chi N Phi Square Phi Square Father at 12 Versus Mother at 12 26 .35 3.15 .32 2.73 Father's Prediction Of Child Versus Mother's Predic- tion Of Child 2A .51* 6.17 .3A 2.7A *p< .05 Table A.—-Comparison of Child's Guilt Scores and Parents' Resistance to Temptation. Child's Guilt Scores Theft Disobedience Chi Chi Parents' Resistance N Phi Phi to Temptation Square Square Father at 12 Theft 26 .25 1.59 .13 0.A7 Disobedience 26 .18 0.83 — 22 1 27 Mother at 12 Theft 26 .35 3.15 .23 1.32 Disobedience 26 .09 0.23 .06 0.10 Father's Prediction of Child Theft 2A .17 0.70 .05 0.05 Disobedience 2A .19 0.89 .22 1.22 Mother's Prediciton of Child Theft 26 .18 0.83 .06 0.10 Disobedience 26 .12 0.36 .10 0.26 71 Child resistance to temptation for theft was signifi- cantly related to father guilt for theft (Refer to Table 5). This is the only case in resistance to temptation data that Hypothesis IV of higher relationship of child-father responses than child-mother responses was supported. Those relationships which tended toward significance (p i .10) were father at 12 total guilt and child resistance to temptation for theft and average parent at 12 guilt for disobedience and child resistance to temptation for theft. Child resistance to temptation was not related to parent prediction of child's guilt in any case. The relationship of child response to parents at 12 appears greater than that of child response to parents' prediction of child's response. The above statements represent possible trends only, however, as the number of significant relationships is no greater than would be expected by chance. Comparison of Child Guilt Versus Parent Guilt Moral Areas Pearson product moment correlation coefficients for child guilt as compared to parent guilt at 12 are shown in Table 6 and comparison of child guilt to parents' prediction of child's guilt are shown in Table 7. Correlation coefficients indicate little support for Hypothesis I that there is a significant relationship between 12 year old boys' moral responses to death wishes, theft and disobedience and fathers' and mothers' moral responses in these areas. Support for this hypothesis and for the 72 Table 5.—-Comparison of Child's Resistance to Temptation and Parents' Guilt Scores. _— Child's Resistance to Temptation Theft Disobedience Parents' Guilt N Phi Scfiére Phi SCh: Scores q qu re Father at 12 Theft 26 .A6* 5.A3 .2A 1.51 Disobedience 26 .13 0.AA -.02 0.02 Total 26 .37 3.A8 .19 0.96 Mother at 12 Theft 26 —.08 0.16 .13 0.AA Disobedience 26 .00 0.00 .00 0.00 Total 26 .00 0.00 .00 0.00 Average Parent at 12 Theft 26 .00 0.00 .00 0.00 Disobedience 26 .37 3.A8 .19 0.96 Total 26 .25 1.67 .13 0.AA Father's Prediction of Child Theft 2A —.05 0.05 .00 0.00 Disobedience 2A .08 0.1A -.11 0.26 Total 2A .03 0.02 —.07 0.12 Mother's Prediction of Child Theft 26 .09 0.21 .19 0.96 Disobedience 26 —.23 1.35 .19 1.22 Total 26 -.08 0.16 .13 0.AA Average Parents' Prediction of Child Theft 2A .18 0.82 “.07 0.1A Disobedience 2A .16 0.62 -.29 2.07 Total 2A .03 0.02 .00 0.00 *p :.-O5 73 Table 6.——Comparison of Child's Guilt Scores for Moral Areas and Parents' Guilt Scores at 12 for Moral Areas. - Child's Guilt Scores I ParegZZreguilt 3:2;28 Theft Disobedience Total Father at 12 Death Wishes .22 .26 .2A .29 Theft —.13 .29 .25 .22 Disobedience -.06 .12 .13 .10 Total .00 .26 .2A .2A Mother at 12 Death Wishes .A2* .09 .25 .26 Theft .05 -.15 .0A «.05 Disobedience .A0* .18 .22 .30 Total .39* .0A .23 .22 Average Parent at 12 Death Wishes .52** .19 .18 .31 Theft —.O7 .11 .18 .12 Disobedience .12 .16 .18 .19 Total .16 .19 .26 .25 *p 1.05 **p_<_.01 N= 26 Data expressed as correlation coefficients. 7A Table 7.-—Comparison of Child's Guilt Scores for Moral Areas and Parents' Predictions of Child's Guilt Scores for Moral Areas. ‘——v L Child's Guilt Scores Parents' Predictions of Child's Guilt Death Scores N Wishes Theft Disobedience Total Father's Prediction of Child's Guilt Death Wishes 2A .00 .0A .10 .06 Theft 2A —.15 .01 —.01 -.0A Disobedience 2A -.1A -.05 —.12 -.12 Total 2A -.ll .00 —.01 -.0A Mother's Prediction of Child's Guilt Death Wishes 26 .30 -.O6 -.05 .03 Theft 26 .13 .05 .0A .08 Disobedience 26 .A0* -.11 -.05 .03 Total 26 .31 -.03 -.Ol .06 Average Parent's Prediction of Child's Guilt Death Wishes 2A .10 -.09 -.05 -.05 Theft 2A -.06 -.Ol -.02 —.O3 Disobedience 2A .ll -.13 -.1A -.11 Total 2A .03 -.08 —.07 —.07 *p 1-05 Data expressed as correlation coefficients. 75 collateral hypothesis that the relationship would be higher for the same moral area than between moral areas appeared only in the finding that child guilt for death wishes was significantly related to mother at 12's guilt for death wishes, for disobedience and total guilt. Some support was obtained from the relationship between child guilt for death wishes and average parent guilt for death wishes. The data were non—supportive of the hypothesis of greater father-child similarity than mother—child similarity. Less support for similarity of child and parents' predictions was found for comparison of guilt in three moral areas than similarity of parents at 12 and child (Table 7). Only mother's prediction for disobedience and child's guilt for death wishes were significantly related for this condition. Although only a few correlations were significant, some suggestion of greater similarity between child and parent at 12 than between child and parents' perception of child appears in the data. This finding tended to be supported by resistance to temptation data and guilt in three moral areas. Forms of Moral Response Resistance to temptation comparison to guilt was reported in a previous section. Pearson product moment correlation coefficients for child guilt as compared to parent guilt at 12 are shown in Table 8 and comparisons 76 Table 8.—-Comparison of Child's Guilt Scores for Forms of Moral Response and Parents' Guilt Scores at Twelve for Forms of Moral Response. m Child's Guilt Scores Parents‘ Guilt D.A. I.M. D.M. Ext. Tot. Scores (1) (2) (3) (A) (5) Father at 12 Direct Acknowledgment (1) —.11 .16 .05 .09 .08 Indirect Manifestations (2) .15 .35 .20 .39* .A1* Defense Mechanisms (3) —.07 .A1* .02 .09 .1A Externalization (A) .10 .27 -.03 .07 .16 Total (5) .01 .3A .09 .18 .2A Mother at 12 Direct Acknowledgment (1) —.02 .2A -.10 -.08 .01 Indirect Manifestations (2) .01 .28 -.07 —.A .0A Defense Mechanisms (3) .20 .6u** .AA* .62** .67** Externalization .12 .19 -.22 —.01 .0A Total (5) .08 .A5* -.01 .09 .22 Average Parent at 12 Direct Acknowledgment (1) -.10_ .23 —.02 .02 .05 Indirect Manifestations (2) .09 .36 .07 .13 .25 Defense Mechanisms (3) —.0A .62** .2A .39* .AA* Externalization (A) -.1A .29 -.13 .05 .13 Total (5) .0A .A2* .06 .17 .25 *p ;_.05 **p 1 ~01 N = 26 Data expressed as correlation coefficients. 77 of child guilt to parents' predictions of child's guilt are shown in Table 9. Comparison of father's guilt at 12 to child guilt showed only father's indirect manifestations related to child externalization and total guilt and father's defense mechanisms related to child's indirect manifestations. Mother's defense mechanisms at 12 were significantly related to child's indirect manifestations, defense mechanisms, externalization and total guilt. Mother at 12 total guilt was related to child's indirect manifestations. The combined score for parent defense mechanisms at 12 was related to child's indirect mani- festations, externalization and total guilt. Parent total guilt at 12 was related to child's indirect manifestations. The above findings show some support for Hypothesis II that there is a significant relationship between the child's forms of moral response and parents' forms of moral response. Direct acknowledgment was not in any case significantly related to other forms of moral response nor in comparisons between child and parents. The col- lateral hypothesis that the relationship would be higher for each form of response than between forms was supported only for defense mechanisms for mother at 12 and child. The pattern of greater mother-child similarity than father-child similarity obtained in comparison of moral areas appeared in analysis of forms of moral response. Correlations were generally lower for child guilt and 78 Table 9.--Comparison of Child's Guilt Scores for Forms of Moral Response and Parents' Predictions of Child's Guilt Scores for Forms of Moral Response. - ‘-‘——‘ r ‘ 4 Child's Guilt Scores Parent's Predictions of N D.A. I.M. D.M. Ext. Tot. Child's Guilt Scores (1) (2) (3) (A) (5) Father's Prediction of Child's Guilt Direct Acknowledgment (1) 2A —.09 .02 .05 -.21 -.06 Indirect Manifestations (2) 2A —.20 .22, —.15 .16 .00 Defense Mechanisms (3) 2A -.17 .23 .00 .05 .03 Externalization (A) 2A .0A -.01 -.10 -.25 —.10 Total (5) 2A —.1A .15 -.05 —.07 -.ou Mother's Prediction of Child's Guilt Direct Acknowledgment (1) 26 -.01 .31 —.13 -.09 -.02 Indirect Manifestations (2) 26 -.07 .2A —.l3 -.19 —.06 Defense Mechanisms (3) 26 —.0A .AA* .09 .15 21. Externalization (A) 26 .12 .22 —.20 .02 .06 Total (5) 26 .00 .A0* —.12 —.06 .06 Average Parent's Prediction of Child's Guilt Direct Acknowledgment (1) 2A —.0A .09 -.02 -.30 -.11 Indirect Manifestations (2) 2A -.20 .23 —.18 -.08 -.10 Defense Mechanisms (3) 2A -.20 .32 .0A .09 .06 Externalization (A) 2A .10 .07 —.1A —.25 -.06 Total (5) 2A —.1A .2A -.09 -.17 -.07 *p 1,.05 Data expressed as correlation coefficients. 79 parents' predictions of child's forms of moral response than for child guilt and parents at 12. The only correlations which achieved significance among child- parent perception comparisons were mother's perception of child's defenses and mother's perception of child's total guilt as related to child's indirect manifestations. Thus, Hypothesis II was not supported for comparison of child and parents' perception of child's response. Hypothesis III that there is a significant relation- ship between total guilt response of 12 year Old boys and total guilt response of parents received no support. Moral Areas Versus Forms of Moral Response Comparison of forms Of parent moral response to child guilt for three moral areas, as may be noted in Table 10, showed father's indirect manifestations (related to child externalization and total guilt) related significantly to theft and disobedience for child. Mother's use of defense mechanisms (related to indirect manifestations, defense mechanisms, externalization and total guilt of child) was related to child death wishes, theft and disobedience. Mother's total guilt at 12, as noted previously, was related to child's death wishes. No correlations of child subtotals for moral areas and parents' predictions of child's forms of moral responses achieved significance. 80 Table 10.--Comparison of Child's Guilt Scores for Moral Areas and Parents' Guilt Scores for Forms of Moral Response. _- _— I;— Child's Guilt Scores for Moral Areas Parents' Guilt Scores for Death Forms of Moral Response N Wishes Theft Disobedience Father at 12 Direct Acknowledgment 26 -.13 .10 .15 Indirect Manifestations 26 .02 .AA* .Al* Defense Mechanisms 26 .13 .12 .12 Externalization 26 -.03 .23 .11 Total 26 .OO .26 .2A Mother at 12 Direct Acknowledgment 26 .2A -.16 .05 Indirect Manifestations 26 .22 -.10 .10 Defense Mechanisms 26 .A5* .56** .59** Externalization 26 .27 .08 .00 Total 26 .39* .0A .23 Average Parent at 12 Direct Acknowledgment 26 .02 .00 .12 Indirect Manifestations 26 .1A .18 .29 Defense Mechanisms 26 .32 .37 .38- Externalization 26 .12 .13 .08 Total 26 .16 .19 .26 Father's Prediction of Child's Guilt Direct Acknowledgment 2A -.1A -.09 .05 Indirect Manifestations 2A -.07 .00 .0A Defense Mechanisms 2A —.05 .09 .00 Externalization 2A -.08 .00 —.18 Total 2A —.11 .00 -.01 Mother's Prediction of Child's Guilt Direct Acknowledgment 26 .32 —.16 -.0A Indirect Manifestations 26 .1A —.11 -.08 Defense Mechanisms 26 .26 .19 .10 Externalization 26 .16 .06 -.03 Total 26 .31 -.03 -.01 Average Parent's Predic- tion of Child's Guilt Direct Acknowledgment 2A .03 —.16 —.07 Indirect Manifestations 2A —.02 —.10 —.07 Defense Mechanisms 2A .02 .11 .00 Externalization 2A .02 .00 -.16 Total 2A .03 —.08 —.O7 *p i .05 **p i .01 Data expressed as correlation coefficients. 81 Comparison of child's forms of moral response and parents' subtotals for three moral areas, as may be noted in Table 11, showed child's indirect manifestations (related to father at 12's defense mechanisms, mother at 12's defense mechanisms and total guilt, average parent at 12's defense mechanisms and total guilt, and mother's perception of child's defenses and total guilt) was related significantly to mother at 12's response to death wishes and disobedience and mother's perception of child's response to disobedience, and average parent's death wishes at 12. Mother Versus Father Comparisons A generally high positive relationship was obtained between mothers at 12 and fathers at 12 and mother's predictions of child's guilt and father's predictions of child's guilt (Refer to Table 12). Exceptions appeared in mother's response to death wishes both for response at 12 and prediction of sons' response which was in no case related to father's responses and for mother's perception of direct acknowledgment by child. Total guilt of mother at 12 was related to all scores for father at 12 and all father's predictions of child subtotals. Total guilt of father at 12 was not related to mother at 12 for death wishes, defense mechanisms and externalization although all correlations were above p i .10. Father's total guilt for perception of son was not related to mother's death wishes or direct 82 Table ll.--Comparison of Child's Guilt Scores for Forms of Moral Response and Parents' Guilt Scores for Moral Areas. ! — ___- _ I-V— ___‘_f of Moral Response Parents' Guilt Scores D.A. I.M. D.M. Ext. Tot. # for Moral Areas (1) (2) (3) (A) (5) Child's Guilt Scores for Forms Father at 12 Death Wishes 26 .07 .35 .17 .25 .29 Theft 26 .05 .26 .l0 .l3 .23 Disobedience 26 —.07 .28 -.03 .11 .10 Mother at 12 Death Wishes 26 .05 .A9* .08 .17 .26 Theft 26 .03 .10 -.26 —.O6 —.05 Disobedience 26 .13 .A0* .25 .08 .30 Average Parent at 12 Death Wishes 26 .16 .59** .05 .1A .31 Theft 26 .0A .22 -.07 .06 .12 Disobedience 26 .00 .36 .08 .ll .19 Father's Prediction of Child's Guilt Death Wishes 2A -.15 .21 .02 .13 .06 Theft 2A —.19 .13 -.0A -.0A —.0A Disobedience 2A .01 .0A —.12 -.30 -.12 Mother's Prediction of Child's Guilt Death Wishes 26 .02 .33 —.09 -.1A .03 Theft 26 -.O9 .28 ~.O2 .09 .08 Disobedience 26 .10 .A6* —.27 -.16 .03 Average Parent's Pre- diction Of Child's Guilt Death Wishes 2A -.18 .29 -.O7 -.lO -.05 Theft 2A —.18 .17 —.02 —.05 —.03 Disobedience 2A .0A .2A —.20 —.38 -.11 *p :_.05 **p i .01 Data expressed as correlation coefficients. #D.A. (1)—Direct Acknowledgment I.M. (2)—Indirect Manifestations D.M. (3)vDefense Mechanisms Ext. (A)-Externalization Tot. (5)—Total 83 .COfipoopfio O>HpHmOQ CH Ham .mpCOHOHmuooo COHumHopcoo mm commopdxo mama mm H 2 Ho. W 9;. mo. [VI of. seem. mm. mm. *ms. *mm. *som. *cs. em. Amv sosoe *ss. *mm. ms. *ms. mm. *cs. em. sm. Ass cosooussccaooxm ssmm. ems. am. *As. ems. ssom. *ms. mm. Acv nsnscccooz oncocom **mm. mm. *ss. *ss. mm. mm. mm. mm. Amv moose swamOMHcmz OOOLHOCH *mz. mm. :N. #0:. mm. mm. *m:. HH. pCOEMUOHBOCXOd. MNWLHD *cs. sm. sm. mm. mm. ssmm. cm. ss. Amv oocosoooomsa ssmm. mm. sm. *ms. *ms. was. *ss. em. Amv chose *ms. em. *ms. *mm. mm. mm. *os. mm. isv mocmsz cocoa Ass “as Ass Ame Ass Ame Amv Ass osocstas osoossc> ms so schema ms at cacao: .O>HOBB pm mmpoom passw m.pm£pmm msmho> o>ao39 um mOLoOm OHHSG m.pohpoz mo comHLmQEOOII.¢mH manna 8A .COHOOOAHO O>HOHmOQ CH Ham .mpcmwoammooo cowpmaophoo mm commopqxo memo sm 2 so. .w ass mo..w as **mm. asam. *om. **mm. ,zm. ,xam. axmm. 2m. / flwv Hmpoe **mm. **on. mm. *xmm. mm. *om. *smm. ma. Nev COHOHNHHHCAOOxm sssm. ssmm. ssmm. ems. ms. *ms. *sss. ms. “av msascccooz cantata *xwm. *ms. *Hm. *ms. Hm. *mq. *smm. ma. Amv mcofipmpmomflcmz OOOAHUCH *om. *os. um. *ms. *ms. mm. **mm. mm. pcmemomazocxo< MWWLHQ seem. **mm. mm. seem. sm. ssmw. *.so. sm. Amv oocosoooomso *xmw. **m©. #0:. **:m. mm. *m:. *szm. mm. Amv phone *m:. :m. *Hm. *3:. :N. *mm. **mm. ma. Adv memHZ Epwmfl Amv Anv Amv Amv ., Asv mmv Amv AHV manmfipm> mahmfipm> OHHSU m.OHH£o pHHSU w.UHH£o co cossoscosa n.socooz ho soscosoosm m.tocoom / .notoom sssso n.csaso no cosooscosm m.sospmm mzmpo> mocoom OHHBU msOHHno mo COHOOHOOLm mspospoz mo comHstEoosl.mmH mapme 85 acknowledgment. Closer agreement existed for perception of child's responses than for parents at 12. All comparisons of parents' perceptions of child‘s responses were significant except mother's response to death wishes and direct acknowledgment and some comparisons with defense mechanisms. These findings are important in relation to findings of motherAS defenses as related to child guilt.. CHAPTER VI DISCUSSION General Considerations Results of comparison of twelve year old boys' moral responses and those of their parents consistently pro— vided greater evidence of mother-son similarity than of father—son similarity. In only one case, child resistance to temptation and father guilt for theft, was this trend reversed. The consistently greater mother-son similarity contradicts expectation of greater father-child similarity based on the psychoanalytic theory of identification. Re— sults also emphasized greater similarity of children's moral responses and their parents' reports of their own responses at twelve than of children's responses and parents' predictions of their responses. Generally high internal consistency indicated presence of some common factor measured by the total score. Intercorrelations of guilt scores for fathers showed consistently signifi- cant correlations; this consistency contrasted with less internal consistency for children and mothers. The impli- cations of the differential consistency Of subtotals will be discussed in relation to greater mother-child similarity than father-child similarity. The hypothesis that children's moral responses in three situations would be significantly related to parents' 86 87 similar areas of moral response gained some support of a limited nature. Although trends appeared, the generality of conclusions based upon these trends must be limited due to the large number of nonvsignificant findings. Support of the child—parent similarity emerged particularly for the motherschild comparison of death wishes. In no case was total guilt related for children and parents. Selective patterning of response rather than general support was obtained for the hypothesis regarding significant relationships between forms of moral response. FathersA indirect manifestations and mothers' use of defense mechanisms appeared most closely related to child guilt. A marked lack of significance of child—parent relationship for direct acknowledgment resulted. Resistance to temptation data showed one interesting comparison of father guilt and child resistance to temptation for theft at the same level Of significance as child guilt related to resistance to temptation for this situation. Only child's resistance to temptation for disobedience and mother's prediction of the child‘s resistance to temptation for disobedience were significantly related among comparisons of resistance to temptation for children and parents. 88 Child Similarity to Mother as Opposed to Similarity to Father Psychoanalytic theory predicts that following the resolution of the Oedipus situation, the boy adopts the characteristics of the father in a global manner to alleviate castration anxiety. Anaclitic identification, emphasizing emulation of the controlling authority to postpone or avoid loss of love, is conceived as a forerunner of superego development in psychoanalytic theory but receives greater emphasis in social learning theory as the product of an important constellation of parent—child interaction. The limited number of significant results prevents a broad generalization of findings. Trends show greater similarity of child responses to mother's responses at twelve, and to her prediction of the child's responses, than similarity to the father's responses in both instructional conditions. These suggest greater support for a model of personality emphasizing identification with a controller of resources whose love the child wishes to maintain. The alternate explanation would necessitate assumption that the boys in the sample had, in the areas tapped, failed to achieve adequate resolution of the Oedipal situation and father identification. Previous research (15) found greater cross—sex identification as boys progressed from prepubescence (10 years) to adolescence (1A years). The particular age of the subjects in the sample may thus 89 influence the relative amount of identification. Of importance to the finding of greater mother—child similarity than father—child similarity are the parallel findings of greater consistency of father's responses to the three moral areas and forms of response to moral situations than children's or mother's. Becker (11) reports that a large number of investigations show that both sexes perceive the mother as more nurturant and as using more psychological control, and the father as stricter and exhibiting more physical punishment, especially for boys. Boys perceive fathers as more frustrating and fear—arousing and as granting less autonomy. The results suggest a greater unity and less situational dependence of fathers' standards for moral behavior. Lower intercorrelations for mothers' responses to moral areas particularly may indicate support for Bronfenbrenner's (16) Observation from psychoanalytic theory that the male superego is always more punitive, the female superego more emotionally involved. The data suggest that the boys in the sample have not yet developed a unified superego based on global incorporation of the father's standards as proposed by psychoanalytic theory. The possibility of regression at the emergence of puberty may influence identification. The more probable explanation, however, appears to 90 involve emulation of the mother's standards as the nurturant, caretaking model for differential response to various situations incurring moral judgment. Identification with the Parent's Superego The pattern of significant correlations between child guilt and parent guilt provides some support for the suggestion (16) that the child identifies with the superego of the parent. Direct acknowledgment was in no instance significantly related to any other form of moral response nor to total guilt. Father's indirect manifestations, mother's defense mechanisms, and combined parent defense mechanisms were more frequently highly related to child guilt than other forms of moral response. This may indicate that the child models his response to moral situations on subtle cues of internalized guilt rather than direct expression of remorse. Allinsmith (2) suggests that the parent provides a model of inhibition of aggression when psychological discipline techniques are used, and Bandura and his associates' (7, 8, 9, 10) research on modeling responses indicate that a child will learn to inhibit, to aggress, or to punish himself from Observation without direct mediation of instrumental learning. The child may be sensitive to the parents' indirect and defensive maneuvers and so incorporate this framework of reaction to anticipated transgression as readily or more so than direct training in moral areas. 91 Absence of significant correlations for direct acknowledg- ment does not preclude the possibility that the parent may reinforce various expressions of guilt although he does not manifest these himself nor predict the child's use of these manifestations. Greater similarity of the child's responses to parents' reports of their own responses at twelve than to parents' perceptions of the child's responses suggest that the parents may not be aware of the child's actual responses and of similarity of their manifestations of guilt and the child's guilt responses. The parent may not be aware of the child's guilt responses expressed through defense mechanisms and indirect acknowledgment which he manifests himself without conscious awareness. The findings appear to support identification of the child's superego with the parent's superego as the parent does not seem to show awareness of the manner in which the child shows similarity to him. Helper, according to Bronfenbrenner (16), found children's self-perceptions as closely related to the parent's ideal for the child as to the parent's self- perceptions. Perceptions of the child's responses in this research were not necessarily the parents' ideal for the child. Parents' predictions varied from extreme overestimation to extreme underestimation, but some responses, particularly resistance to temptation and direct acknowledgment of guilt, may have been 92 influenced by cultural expectations and family values. Some influence of sex-role standards may have affected mothers' perceptions of children's responses. These data suggest that similarity of moral responses of parents and children predominantly appears in comparisons of indirect forms of expression of guilt, and that the parent's prediction of the child's responses may be less a function of accurate Observation of the forms of expression the child acquires which are manifested by the parent as other factors. Indirect forms of expression, like Helper's parent ideal for the child, appear to reflect the internalized standards of the parent which are later represented in the child's moral responses. Differential Roles of Parents in Transmission of Standards Differential roles of the parents in transmission of culture may be indicated in findings of similarity of father guilt to child resistance to temptation for theft and child resistance to temptation and child guilt for theft, as opposed to a high relationship between child and mother for disobedience. The father may provide a standard for culturally approved behavior and the mother be more influential in the home environment. Findings of previous research (25, A3) suggest that the.boy may identify with appropriate sex typing while the girl identifies with characteristics of the mother which 93 differentiate her as an adult from the girl. Boys, according to Emmerich (25), tend to perceive the father as more controlling. The mother, however, may be the controller of resources in the home. Some difference may be observed with stages of development in moral judgment. Movement into the community and concern for community standards may be the product of later development than concern about disobedience of the mother. The findings of greater father—child similarity for theft and mother- child similarity for disobedience are particularly interesting in the light of the higher mother—father correlations for theft and disobedience situations than for death wishes. These are both observable situations where parent agreement may be greater, as Opposed to the death wish situation where parent correlations were near chance but mother-child correlations were generally significant. The child may identify more with the mother in affective and home-oriented situations. The differentiation of the mother's and father's areas of discipline may be important in these respects. Peer Factors The failure to find consistently high relationships between parent moral responses and child moral responses, particularly for resistance to temptation, suggests that some factor other than parental standards may be operative. Kohlberg (39) emphasizes the importance of peer group judgment as a factor in the development of 9A moral judgment. It is possible that a peer factor may influence self—reports of resistance to temptation, and to some extent guilt, when the collection of data in the school situation is considered. Hoffman (35) reports findings that parental antecedents are related to pressure to deviate but not where it is opposed to the _ child's values and standards. These may shift in differing situations. Bias of the Sample Another factor which may contribute to low relationships of parent moral responses to child moral responses and in some cases the narrow range of guilt scores is the composition of the sample. The sample tended toward a middle class, Protestant group who might be expected from previous research (A7, 58) to show more internalization of guilt than a more heterogeneous sample. The one family whose income was under $3,000 showed all child and parent situations reflecting deviation and guilt above 70%. The desirability of a more heterogeneous sample is emphasized, and it may be speculated that selectivity of parents who agreed to cooperate in the investigation biased results. Other factors to be considered are family differences in values. Some families indicated that the disobedience situation would be less severely evaluated in their families than other situations. Findings of significant 95 relationships of boys' resistance to temptation and guilt for theft may reflect a less ambivalent, culturally determined situation than disobedience or death wishes. Control for motivation and need for achievement has been stressed by Hoffman (35), and may introduce an extraneous variable in situational and child-parent comparisons. Allinsmith (2) notes that peOple who disown moral needs and deceive themselves about remorse are most likely to yield in resistance to temptation situations. The importance of Allinsmith's finding may appear in the limited relationship of children's and parents' resistance to temptation; some children who showed high guilt and deviated had fathers who received zero scores on guilt. Comments on the Instrument The need for revision of the guilt scale may be indicated by the narrow range of guilt scores for some subtests. A means of control for denial and suppression of guilt is needed. Mosher (50) found high guilt subjects relatively insensitive to situational cues while low guilt subjects were highly situationally dependent. Further research could incorporate this difference in the research design. The two resistance to temptation situations may have cued low guilt scores among some subjects. The limited number of significant correlations between resistance to temptation and guilt severity, 96 however, suggests that, for these subjects, guilt scores were relatively independent of resistance or deviation. Trends toward a positive relationship between resistance to temptation and guilt severity indicate that the anticipation of transgression may result in a guilt response which is as severe as that following deviation. These results perhaps support the assertion that the anticipation of guilt serves to prevent deviation. Subjects who obtained very low guilt scores were consistent across the three moral areas which further suggests that guilt responses were not dependent on resistance or deviation. Attempts at Integration The most cogent explanation of the varied and somewhat confounded findings of this research appears to be that the father presents a more unified, less situationally determined standard of moral behavior which the child may model in acceptance of cultural expectations. The mother appears to respond in a more situationally determined manner, providing a more love— oriented model of moral standards. She appears to be dominant in determination of affectively oriented situations and in standards involving the home. Both parents serve to inculcate their standards in the child through subtle communication, reflected in modeling of the superego of the child on the superego of the parent 97 rather than the parent's overt verbalization. The parents' indirect responses to moral areas, indirect manifestations for fathers and defense mechanisms for mothers, assert more influence on moral standards of twelve year old boys than direct acknowledgment or resistance to temptation. The many non-significant relationships suggest that other sources of moral response are important. The anaclitic model of identification is more directly supported by these data than is defensive identification, particularly in reference to death wishes and disobedience. The mother may provide a nurturant Object whose love is dependent on conformity to her standard rather than the father's standard. Child externalization was highly related to father's use of indirect manifestations of guilt. This may indicate relation to the father as the greater authority, with the child more likely to expect physical punishment from the father. Further Considerations The investigator plans to make further modifications of the objective items concerning death wishes, theft and disobedience. A reliability study, which is in the process of completion, should provide further information about discriminatory potential of items and stability of subjects' scores over time. Modification of wording of 98 some items and expansion of items for some forms of moral response would probably increase the effectiveness of the instrument. The possibility of more age—relevant items for parents is being considered. Methodology may be refined further by the inclusion of other parent variables, more accurate evaluation of the social desirability of certain responses and control for denial of guilt. Family interviews, utilizing a structured interview schedule or role playing technique, might be utilized. Data from instructions for children, involving perceptions of father and mother, would be desirable. Some measure of rigidity appears relevant in View Of differential consistency of moral responses. As noted previously, a more heterogeneous sample is needed for effective evaluation of the parent—child comparisons of moral responses. The findings of this study must be restricted to the sample which participated as a more heterogeneous sample might yield differing conclusions. Data from children of differing ages could enable evaluation of developmental influence on parent—child agreement and changes in congruence of children's responses and parents' predictions of their responses. Situations in which the items were administered could be varied to evaluate influence of home and school. 99 Differing areas of moral response in relation to high and low guilt subjects provide a promising area for further research. CHAPTER VII SUMMARY The major purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship of moral responses of twelve year old boys in the areas of death wishes, theft and disobedience and their parents' moral responses in these areas as they per- ceive themselves at twelve and as they perceive their sons' responses. Hypotheses relevant to these comparisons pre- dicted generally high relationships between children's and parents' responses for guilt and resistance to temptation in three areas and in use of forms of moral response. Forms of moral response included direct acknowledgment, indirect manifestations, defense mechanisms and externalization. Hypotheses generated initially from psychoanalytic con- ceptions of identification were viewed from the alternate approaches of psychoanalytic theory and social learning theory. Pearson product moment correlations were computed to obtain comparisons of child, father at twelve, mother at twelve, father's perception of child's guilt, mother's perception of child's guilt, average of mother-father at twelve, and average of parents' prediction of child's responses. Hypotheses tested for significance at the .05 level by Pearson product moment correlations for guilt scores and phi coefficients for resistance to temptation comparisons were: 100 101 Hypothesis I: There is a significant relationship between moral responses of twelve year old boys in moral areas of death wishes, theft and disobedience and moral responses of their parents in these areas. Hypothesis II: There is a significant relationship between twelve year Old boys' forms of moral response, defined as resistance to temptation, direct acknowledg- ment, indirect manifestations, defense mechanisms, and externalization, and parents' forms of moral response. Hypothesis III: There is a significant relationship between total moral response of twelve year old boys and total moral response of parents. Hypothesis IV: It is hypothesized that similarity between child and father will be greater than similarity between child and mother. Twenty—six 12 year old boys from Traverse City Junior High School and their fathers and mothers participated in the study, this number representing the number of completed responses from an initial contact of 175 families. The instrument included story completion items from Allinsmith (2) and forced response objective questions from Allinsmith's levels of guilt severity, Adams' (1) levels of guilt severity, and additional items checked by graduate students and the investigator's research director. Guilt items were scored and weighted for severity. Correlations were computed for all relationships of guilt scores. Phi coefficients (66) were used for comparisons of resistance 102 to temptation for children and parents and of resistance to temptation and guilt for theft and disobedience. All interrelations for child, father at twelve, mother at twelve, father's perception Of child's guilt, mother's perception of child's guilt, average parent at twelve, and average parent's perception of child's guilt were found to have subtotals significantly related to total guilt. Interrelations of guilt scores for fathers were considerably more consistent than interrelations for children and mothers. Resistance to temptation comparisons with guilt did not show such high correlations. The limited number of significant correlations between resistance to temptation and guilt severity suggested that, for these subjects, guilt scores were relatively independent of resistance or deviation. Some support was gained for Hypotheses I and II, especially for child compared with parents' reports of their own responses at twelve. Resistance to temptation comparison showed only the child's response and mother's perception of child's response for disobedience to be significantly related. Parent resistance to temptation was not significantly related to child's guilt, and children's resistance to temptation was related to parents' guilt only for child resistance to temptation for theft and father's guilt for theft. This was the only instance in which father-child similarity was greater than mother—child similarity. 103 Correlations between child guilt for three moral areas and parent guilt for these areas indicated that child's guilt response for death wishes was significantly related to mother's guilt for death wishes, disobedience and total guilt. Child's guilt for death wishes correlated significantly with average parent guilt at twelve for death wishes. Parents' prediction of child's guilt was significantly related only for mother's prediction of child's guilt for disobedience and child's guilt for death wishes. Forms of moral response showed selective patterning of significant relations, but little general support for Hypothesis II. Fathers' indirect manifestations of guilt at twelve was related to child's externalization of guilt and total guilt, and father's defense mechanisms at twelve was related to child's indirect manifestations. Mother's defense mechanisms at twelve was related to child's indirect manifestations, defense mechanisms, externalization and total guilt. Mother's total guilt at twelve showed a significant relationship to child's indirect manifestations. Combined parents' defense mechanisms at twelve showed a significant relationship to child's indirect manifestations, externalization and total guilt. Parents' total guilt at twelve was significantly related to child's indirect manifestations. Comparisons of direct acknowledgment showed no significant relationships. Parents' perceptions of child's responses 10A yielded only the significant relationship of mother's perception of child's defense mechanisms and perception of child's total guilt to child's indirect manifestations. Thus, Hypothesis 11 received little support from comparison of child's guilt with parents' perceptions Of child's guilt. Comparison of child's total guilt with parents' total guilt showed no significant comparisons; therefore, Hypothesis III received no support. Father's indirect manifestations were related to child's theft and disobedience scores. Mother's defense mechanisms at twelve were related to child's death wishes, theft and disobedience. Mother's total guilt showed a significant relationship to child's death wishes. No significant relationships resulted among comparisons of child's responses to moral areas and parents' predictions of forms of child's moral response. Child's indirect manifestations were related to mother's death wishes at twelve, disobedience at twelve, mother's perception of child's disobedience response, and average parent's death wishes at twelve. Generally high positive relationships were found in comparisons of mother's and father's responses. Mothers' response to death wishes at twelve and prediction of child's death wishes failed to show a relationship to fathers' responses although the mother—child similarity for death wishes was high. Mother's perception of 105 child's direct acknowledgment was generally not related to father's perception of child's guilt. Total guilt for mother at twelve was related to all Of father's subtotals. Total guilt of father at twelve was related to all of mother's subtotals except mother's death wishes, defense mechanisms and externalization. Closer agreement existed between father—mother responses for perception of child's guilt than for parents at twelve. Major findings were greater child—mother similarity than father-child similarity, non—supportive of Hypothesis IV, and more supportive of acquisition of standards through anaclitic identification as viewed by social learning theory or imitation of a controlling parental model than the psychoanalytic concept of identification with the aggressor following resolution of the Oedipus situation. Fathers' guilt scores were more consistent than those of mothers' or children, which suggested that the father may serve as a model for cultural standards, especially in view Of.a significant relationship between father's guilt for theft and child's resistance to temptation for theft. The mother may function as the model for home and affective behavior. Parents' responses at twelve showed greater similarity to children's responses than parentks per- ception of children's responses.‘ Data may reflect the identification of the child's superego with the parents' superego; the parents may not be aware of guilt manifestations in the child if they defend against guilt 106 or express it indirectly themselves. Total guilt comparisons showed no significant relationships, suggesting that guilt may be expressed in different forms, the summation of which may not be comparable to the summation of the separate forms of response assessed. It was suggested that the father may present a more unified, less situationally determined standard Of moral behavior which the child may model in acceptance of cultural expectations. The mother appears to respond to more situation cues, and provides a more love-oriented model of moral standards. She appears to be dominant in affective and home situations. Subtle communication and parental forms of resonse to moral situations may be more influential than direct acknowledgment. This communication seems to be reflected in modeling of the superego of the child on the superego of the parent. The anaclitic model of identification was more directly supported by data than defensive identification, particularly in reference to death wishes and disobedience. CHM; mother may provide a nurturant object whose continued love is dependent on conformity to her standard, and the child may respond more to this reinforcement than to motivation for global incorporation of the father's standards. Child externalization was highly related to father's indirect manifestations of guilt, which may indicate the boys' relationship to the father as the more punitive, especially physically punitive, authority. 107 One may conclude that some relationship exists between moral responses of twelve year old boys and their fathers' and mothers' moral responses, although this research may not fully describe this relationship. The large number of non—significant relationships suggests that other sources of moral responses than those considered are important. Greater mother—child similarity than father-child similarity and greater similarity of child's responses to parents' reports of their own responses at twelve than parents' perceptions of child's moral responses raised interesting questions for further research. The use of objective questions in relation to story completion items appears promising. BIBLIOGRAPHY 108 BIBLIOGRAPHY Adams, Charlene. Manifest Anxiety, Severity of Guilt and Scholastic Achievement in Children. M. A. TheSls, Michigan State University, 196A. pp, 67. Allinsmith, Wesley. "Moral Standards: II. The Learning of Moral Standards," in Daniel R. Miller and Guy E. Swanson, Inner Conflict and Defense. New York: Henry Holt and Co., 1960, pp. 1Al-l76. Allinsmith, Wesley, and Thomas C. Greening. "Guilt Over Anger as Predicted from Parental Discipline: A Study of Superego Development," American Psychologist, 10 (1955), p. 320. Abstract. Aronfreed, Justin. "The Nature, Variety and Social Patterning of Moral Responses to Transgression," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63 (1961), pp. 223s2fic. Aronfreed, Justin. ”The Effects of Experimental Socialization Paradigms Upon Two Moral Responses to Transgression," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66 (1963), pp. A37—A38. Bach, George R. "Father—Fantasies and Father-Typing in Father-Separated Children," Child Development, 17 (19A6), pp. 63—80. Bandura, Albert,and Aletha C. Huston. "Identification as a Process of Incidental Learning," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63 (1961), pp. 311—318. Bandura, Albert, Ross, Dorthea, and Sheild A. Ross. "A Comparative Test of Status Envy, Social Power and Secondary Reinforcement Theories of Identification Learning," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67 (1963). pp. 527—53A. Bandura, Albert, Ross, Dorthea, and Sheild A. Ross. "Transmission of Aggression Through Imitation of Aggressive Models," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63 (19617, pp. 575-582. Bandura, Albert, and Richard H. Walters. Social Learping and Personality Development. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 196A. Pp. 329. 109 ll. 12. 13. 1A. 15. l6. l7. 18. 19. 20. 21. 110 Becker, Wesley C. "Consequences of Different Kinds of Parental Discipline," in Martin L. Hoffman and Lois W. Hoffman (eds.), Review of Child Development Research. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 196A, pp. 169—208. Beitner, Marvin S. "Word Meaning and Sexual Identifica- tion in Paranoid Schizophrenics and Anxiety Neurotics," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63 (1961), pp. 289—993. Bieri, James, and Robin Lobeck. "Self-Concept Differences in Relation to Identification, Religion and Social Class," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62 (1961), pp. 9A-98: Bloomer, Richard H. "Identification Figures of Boys and Girls Under Varying Degrees of Implied Stress," Psychological Reports, 15 (196A), pp. 635-6A2. Brodbeck, Arthur J. "Learning Theory and Identification: IV. Oedipal Motivation as a Determinant of Conscience Development," Journal of Genetic Psychology, 8A (195A). pp. 219—227. Bronfenbrenner, Urie. "Freudian Theories of Identification and Their Derivatives," Child Development, 31 (1960), pp. l5—A0. Burnstein, Eugene, Stotland, Ezra, and Alvin Zander. ”Similarity to a Model and Self-Evaluation," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62 (1961), pp. 257-26A. Burton, Roger V., Maccoby, Eleanor, and Wesley Allinsmith. "Antecedents of Resistance to Temptation in Four Year Old Children," Child Development, 32 (1961), pp. 689—710. Carlson, Rae. "Identification and Personality Structure of Preadolescents," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67 (1963), pp. 566—573. DeLucia, Lenore A. "The Toy Preference Test; A Measure of Sex Role Identification,“ Child Development, 3“ (1963), pp. 107—117. Dembo, Tamara. "Recent Studies in Moral Conflict at the Iowa Child Welfare Research Station," Psychological Bulletin, 38 (19Al), pp. 733. 22. 23. 25. 29. 30. 31. 32. 111 Dien, Dora 8., and W. Edgar Vinacke. "Self—Concept and Parental Identification of Young Adults with Mixed Caucasian-Japanese Parentage," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69 (196AT, pp. A63—A66. Dollard, John, and Neal E. Miller. Personality and Psychotherapy. New York: McGraw—Hill Book Co., Inc., (1950), Pp.A88. Durkin, Delores. ”Children's Concepts of Justice: A Comparison with the Piaget Data," Child Development, 30 (1959), pp. 59-67. Emmerich, Walter. "Parental Identification in Young Children," Genetic Psychology Monographs, 60 (1959), pp. 257—308. Epstein, Ralph, and Shephard Liverant. "Verbal Conditioning and Sex—Role Identification in Children," Child Development, 3A (1963), pp. 99-106. Fenichel, Otto. The Psychoanalytic Theory of Neurosis. New York: W. W. Norton and Co., 19A5. Pp. 703. Feshbach, Seymour. "The Stimulating Versus Cathartic Effects of a Vicarious Aggressive Activity," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63 (1961), pp. 381—335. Greening, Thomas C. "Moral Standards and Defense Against Aggression," Dissertation Abstracts, 19, Part 2 (1959), pp. 336A—3365. Grinder, Robert E., and Robert E. McMichael. "Cultural Influence on Conscience Development: Resistance to Temptation and Guilt Among Samoans and American Caucasians," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66 (1963), pp. 503-507. Gump, Paul V., and Jacob S. Kounin. "Milieu Influences in Children's Concepts of Misconduct," Child Development, 32 (1961), pp. 711—720. Heilbrun, Alfred B. Jr. "Parental Identification and College Adjustment," Psychological Reports, 10 (1962), pp. 853—85A. Heilbrun, Alfred B. "Sex Differences in Identification Learning," Journal of Genetic Psychology, 106 (1965), pp. 185—193. 3A. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. A0. A1. A2. “3. AA. A5. 112 Heilbrun, Alfred B., and Donald K. Fromme. "Parental Identification of Late Adolescents and Level of Adjustment: The Importance of Parent-Model Attributes, Ordinal Position, and Sex of the Child," Journal of Genetic Psychology, 107 (1965), pp. A9—59. Hoffman, Martin L. ”Childrearing Practices and Moral Development: Generalizations from Empirical Research," Child Development, 3A (1963), pp. 295-318. Kagan, Jerome. "Acquisition and Significance of Sex Typing and Sex-Role Identity," in Martin L. Hoffman and Lois W. Hoffman (eds.), Review of Child Development Research. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 196A, pp. 137-167. Kagan, Jerome. "The Concept of Identification," Psychological Review, 65 (1958), pp. 296—305. Kagan, Jerome, and William Phillips. "Measurement of Identification, A Methodological Note," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69 (196A), pp. AA2—AAA. Kohlberg, Lawrence. "Moral DevelOpment and Identifica- tion," in Harold W. Stevenson (ed.), Child Psychology, The 62nd Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1963, pp. 277-332. Leavitt, Harry C. ”Relations Between Conditioned Patterns and Superego Development," Psychoanalytic Review, AA (1957), pp. 73—80. Lefkowitz, Monroe M., Walder, Leopold O., and Leonard E. Eron. "Punishment, Identification and Aggression," Merrill—Palmer Quarterly, 9 (1963), pp. 159—17A. Lockwood, Doris H., and Bernard Guerney. "Identification and Empathy in Relation to Self—Dissatisfaction and Adjustment," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65 (1962), ppo 353—3A7. 7‘ Lynn, David B. "Sex Role and Parental Identification," Child Development, 33 (1962), pp. SSS-56A. Lynn, David B., and William L. Sawrey. "The Effects of Father-Absence on Norwegian Boys and Girls," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59 (1959), pp. 258s262. Maccoby, Eleanor E. "Role Taking in Children and Its Consequences for Social Learning," Child Development, 30 (1959), pp. 239—252. A6. A7. A8. A9. 50. 52. 5A. 55. 56. 57. 113 MacRae, Duncan, Jr. "A Test of Piaget's Theories of Moral Development,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, A9 (195A7, pp. lA—l8. Miller, Daniel R., and Guy E. Swanson. Inner Conflict and Defense. New York: Henry Holt and Co., 1960. Pp.A52. Miller, Neal E., and John Dollard. Social Learning and Imitation. New Haven and London: Yale University Press, 19Al. (Yale Paperbound, 1962, Pp. 3A1). Mills, Judson. "Changes in Moral Attitudes Following Cheating," Journal of Personality, 26 (1958), pp. 517-531- Mosher, Donald L. "Interaction of Fear and Guilt in Inhibiting Unacceptable Behavior," Journal of Consulting Psychology. 29 (1965), pp. 161—167. Mowrer, O. Hobart. "Identification: A Link Between Learning Theory and Psychotherapy," in Learning Theory and Personality_Dynamics, Selected Papers. New York: The Ronald Press Co., 1950. pp. 573—616. Mussen, Paul, and Luther Distler. "Masculinity, Identification and Father—Son Relationships," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59 (1959), pp. 350-350. Peck, Robert F., and Robert J. Havighurst, et. al. The Psychology of Character Development. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1960. Pp.267. Rosenbaum, Milton E., and Richard de Charms. "Direct and Vicarious Reduction of Hostility," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60 (19607; pp. 105- 111. Rosenblith, Judy F. "Learning by Imitation in Kindergarten Children," Child Development, 30 (1959), pp. 69—80. Saltz, George, and Seymour Epstein. "Thematic Hostility and Guilt Responses as Related to Self—Reported Hostility, Guilt and Conflict," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67 (1963), pp. A69-A79. Sears, Robert R. Survey of Objective Studies of Psychoanalytic Concepts. New York: Social Science Research Council Bulletin #51, l9A3. Pp.156. 58. 59. 60. 61. 6A. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 11A Sears, Robert R., Maccoby, Eleanor E., and Harry Levin. Patterns of Child Rearing. Evanston, Illinois: How Peterson, 1957. Pp.5A9. Sears, Robert, Rau, Lucy, and Richard Alpert. Identification and Child Rearing. Stanford University Press, 1965. Pp.383. Shaw, Merville C., and Donald L. White. "The Relation— ship Between Child—Parent Identification and Academic Underachievement," Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21 (1965), pp. 10—13. Shore, Milton F., Massimo, Joseph L., and Ronald Mack. "The Relationship Between Levels of Guilt in Thematic Stories and Unsocialized Behavior," Journal of Projective Techniques and Personality Assessment, 28 (196A), pp. 3A6—3A9. Smith, Thomas L., and Richard M. Suinn. "A Note on Identification, Anxiety and Conformity," Journal of Clinical Psychologi, 21 (1965), p. 286. Stein, Arthur. "Guilt as a Composite Emotion: The Relationship of Child—Rearing Variables to Superego Response,” Dissertation Abstracts, 7 (1958-1959), pp. 873—87A. Stewart, Lawrence H. ”Manifest Anxiety and Mother- Son Identification," Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1A (1958), pp. 382—38A. Sutton -Smith, B., and B. G. Rosenberg. "Age Changes in the Effects of Ordinal Position on Sex—Role Identification," Journal of Genetic Psychology, 107 (1965), pp. 61—73. Walker, Helen M., and Joseph Lev. Statistical Inference. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1953 Ikh 510. Weinstein, Eugene A., and Paul Deutschberger. "Some Distinctions of Altercasting," Sociometry, 26 (1963), pp. A5A—A66. Whiting, John W. M. "Sin, Sorcery and the Superego," in Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1959 pp. l7A—l95. Whiting, John W. M., and Irvin L. Child. Child Training and Personality. New Haven and London: Yale University Press, 1953. (Yale Paperbound, 1962, Pp. 353). 115 70. Wright, John C., Hill, John P., and Richard Alpert. "The Development and Expression of Conscience in Fantasy by School Children," Paper read at 1961 meeting of the Society for Research in Child Develop— ment, Pennsylvania State University. APPENDICES 116 APPENDIX A Description of Sample 117 Table l.--Distribution of Children's Intelligence. Range N Superior (IQ = 120 or greater) 9 Bright Normal (IQ = 110—119) 6 Average (IQ = 90-109) (IQ = 100-109) 8 (IQ = 90-99) 3 Table 2.—-Distribution of Parents' Ages. Age Range Father N Mother N 50 or greater A 2 145—149 2 0 A0-AA l2 9 35—39 6 10 30-3A 2 5 Table 3.--Distribution of Parents' Education. i__, ’— J :—_— Education Range Father N Mother N Ph. D. or equivalent Master of Arts or Master of Science Bachelor of Arts Registered Nurse Some College High School Grade 8 to 11 Grade 8 Incomplete OMHNKOOI—JUWH 1...: I—‘OOOU‘ISU'II—‘O 118 119 Table A.——Income and Occupation of Head of Household. Income . Above $5,000— $3,000— Below occupation $10,000 $10,000 $5,000 $3,000 Professional A 2 0 0 Owner of Business 0 1 0 0 Managerial A l 0 0 Other White Collar 2 2 0 0 Skilled 0 A 0 1 Unskilled 0 2 l 0 Table 5.——Religious Preference. Religious Preference N Protestant 2A Catholic 2 Table 6.-—Number of Siblings. Number of Siblings 0 l 2 3 A 5 6 Frequency 1 2 8 9 A 0 2 Table 7.——Ordinal Position. Ordinal Position 1 2 3 A 5 6 7 Frequency 9 7 3 5 0 0 l Illliillill’lll‘ll APPENDIX B Instrument Instructions, Story Completion Items and Annotated Objective Questions 120 Directions for Group Administration of Instrument to Children I am Mrs. Lifshin and I am studying at Michigan State University. There I am learning about people of all ages. I am especially interested in knowing more about fellows your age, how people your age think and feel and what they do in different situations. So I am asking you to finish some stories which have no endings about young people doing different things, and to answer some questions about the stories. After you are finished, I will take the booklets back to the university so your answers will not be given to parents or the school. I am just interested in what you think. Now I'm going to read the instructions on the front of the booklet and you can read along with me. If you have any questions, ask them now or raise your hand while you are working * *Modified from Allinsmith (2). 121 122 Background InfOrmation and Initial Instructions for Children Background Information Age: Grade in School: Ages of brothers: of sisters: Instructions We have some stories here about young people doing different things but the stories have no endings. So we are asking you and many other boys to finish the stories for us. We want to know what different young people would do in situations like these. Tell what the people in the stories are thinking and feeling. There are no right or wrong answers; we just want to know what you think might happen. It is important that you complete every story and answer all questions on the pages following the stories. Instructions for Children Concerning Perception of Father's Responses You have told us what you thought might happen in three different situations. Now we are asking you to answer the questions about the same stories as you think your father would have answered them when he was your age. You may wish to turn back to pages 2, A and 6 to reread the stories. 123 Instructions for Children Concerning Perception of Mother's Responses Now please answer the questions about the stories as you think your mother would have answered them when she was your age. Background Information and Initial Instructions for Parents Background Information Age: Education: High School College: Number of Years Occupation of head of household: Approximate annual income: Under $3,000 $3,000 to $5,000 $5,000 to $10,000 Over $10,000 Religious preference: Instructions The following stories about twelve year Old boys are incomplete. We are interested in how parents of twelve year old boys think they would have responded to the stories at twelve years. We are asking you to complete the stories as you feel you would have at that age. Complete all three stories, telling what you think the people in them are thinking and feeling. There are no right or wrong answers; we only want to know what you think might happen. It is important that you complete every story and answer all questions on the pages following the stories. 12A Instructions for Fathers and Mothers Concerning Perceptions of Child's Responses You have told us what you thought might happen in three different situations as you would have responded when you were twelve years old. Now we are asking you to answer the questions about the same stories as you think your child would answer them. You may wish to refer to pages 2, A, and 6 to reread the stories. Story Stem for Death Wishes* 1. Dave likes his baseball coach. The other day the coach promised him privately that Dave could pitch in the big game on Saturday. When the team meets for final practice, the coach doesn't say anything to Dave about pitching. Dave is afraid he has forgotten or changed his mind. He keeps thinking to himself over and over again: "The coach isn't going to keep his promise. I hope he doesn't even make the game. I wish he'd drop dead!" When Dave arrives at the game on Saturday afternoon, he sees from the scoreboard that he is scheduled to pitch. Story Stem for Theft* II. Bill's friends have formed a baseball team. They promised Bill he could be their catcher if he could get a catcher's mitt. His father told he could not buy him a catcher's mitt. Bill saved all the money he could. *Story stem from Allinsmith (2). 125 At last he had $10.00 saved up to get a really good glove. When he arrives at the sports store, he sees the sales- clerk going down the stairs to the cellar of the store. The clerk does not see Bill, Bill decides to look at the cather's gloves himself before calling the clerk. He finds just the one he wants. Then he reaches for his money. It is gone. He realizes that he has lost it on the way downtown. Bill feels awful. He looks around. There is nobody in the store or near it outside. The clerk is still in the cellar. It occurs to Bill that the mitt would just fit under the bulge in his jacket. Story Stem for Disobedience* III. One day Ted's mother goes visiting a friend Of hers in another town. At noon just after his lunch Ted phones his mother and talks with her. She tells him to be a good boy and says she will be home at suppertime. Now Ted is all alone with nothing to do. He thinks of the boxes in the top of his mother's closet. She has told him never to take down the boxes. He knows his mother won't be home until suppertime. *Story stem from Allinsmith (2). 126 Objective Questions Following Death Wishes Story You have told us in your own words what you think might happen when Dave arrives at the game. Now we would like you to check the following items. Mark Yes if the item represents what you think Dave felt or did. Mark No if you think he did not feel or act as the item suggests. When Dave saw that he was scheduled to pitch . . . . . . . . malls he blamed the coach when he made an error. (defense mechanism-projection) he felt very upset about his thoughts about the coach. (direct acknowledgment—level III) he pitched poorly. (indirect manifestations—level II) he didn't feel upset during the game but became upset without knowing why later in the day while things like eating, riding his bicycle, or finishing his homework. (defense mechanism—isolation) he was unable to pitch. (indirect manifestations—level III) he pitched more like he did when he was first learning than as he usually did. (defense mechanisms—regression) he pitched with his usual skill. (neutral item—level 0) he felt panic when he began to pitch. (indirect manifestations—level III) he was hurt in the game. (indirect manifestations—level III) he felt he was to blame when the team fell behind. (direct acknowledgment—level II) he felt nervous around the coach. (indirect manifestations—level I) he was sorry he had thought about the coach as he had. (direct acknowledgment—level I) he told the fellows on the team about the coach's promise and tried to get them to agree that the coach should have told him he would pitch at final practice. (defense mechanism—projection) he forgot about his thoughts about the coach. (defense mechanism—repression) he pitched well for the coach's sake. (indirect manifestations—level II) he acted silly and acted like a "little kid.“ (defense mechanism-regression) 127 he felt more carefree and happy during the game than he usually did. (defense mechanism-reaction formation) he led the team to victory in a tight spot. (indirect manifestations-level I) he made a special point of praising the coach that day. (defense mechanism-undoing) 128 Objective Questions Following Theft Story Do you think Bill: a. . took the glove. b did not take the glove. Check each of the following items which represent what you think Bill felt or did loo. Check the items which do not represent what you think he felt or did No. Answer every question whether or not you think Bill took the glove. Yes is Bill was afraid of being seen. (externalization—level 1) Bill left the glove where his parents would find it. (desire to be caught—level II—for total score only) He went on to play without thinking about the glove. (defense mechanism-repression) Bill felt extremely upset. (direct acknowledgment-level III) Bill was sent to jail for taking the glove. (externalization-level III) The clerk came upstairs before he could leave. (externalization-level II) Bill felt a little uncomfortable and sorry he had taken the glove. (direct acknowledgment-level 1) Bill returned the glove and confessed. (direct acknowledgment-level III) He blamed the clerk for being in the basement. (defense mechanism-projection) Bill knew it was necessary for him to have the glove. (defense mechanism—rationalization) His parents asked him how he bought the glove when he had left his money at home. (externalization-level II) Bill pitched poorly when he wore the glove. (indirect manifestations—level II) He felt more happy and carefree than he usually did. (defense mechanism-reaction formation) He left and then provoked a friend into helping him take the glove. (defense mechanism-projection) Bill began to act like a little kid; he was silly and babyish. (defense mechanism-regression) 129 He didn't feel upset walking out of the store but suddenly became upset later in the day while doing something like riding his bicycle. (defense mechanism-isolation or displacement) Bill had to work after school to pay for the glove. (externalization-level II) Bill couldn't move from the counter. (indirect manifestations—level III) He said someone else had taken the glove. (defense mechanism—projection) The fact that he had taken the glove didn't bother Bill then or later. (neutral item-level 0) He gave someone else a present afterward. (defense mechanism—reaction formation) 130 Objective Questions Following Disobedience Story Do you think Ted: a. took the boxes from the shelf. b. did not take the boxes from the shelf. Check each of the following items which represent what you think Ted felt or did Yog. Check the items which do not represent what you think he felt or did No; Answer every question whether or not you think Ted took the boxes from the shelf. Tasha Ted was sorry he opened the boxes and saw what was in them. (direct acknowledgment-level I) Ted blamed someone else for Opening the boxes. (defense mechanism—projection) He was spanked and sent to bed. (externalization-level II) Ted wasn't upset at the time but later in the day he became upset without knowing why when doing things like riding his bicycle or finishing his chores at home. (defense mechanism-isolation) A gift for someone else was in the boxes. (not categorized-level I-for total score only) He was afraid someone would see him. (externalization—level I) Ted was in a gay mood when his mother came home. (defense mechanism-denial) His mother returned home early. (externalization-level II) Ted was very upset about opening the boxes. (direct acknowledgment-level III) He replaced the boxes and forgot about Opening them. (defense mechanism-repression) He thought about how much he had gained by opening the boxes. (defense mechanism-rationalization) He acted more like a little "kid" than someone his age. (defense mechanism-regression) Ted boasted about opening the boxes. (defense mechanism-denial) He called someone else to look at what was in the boxes. (defense mechanism—projection) 131 Items in the boxes fell on him and hurt him seriously. (indirect manifestations—level III) He forgot to replace the boxes. (desire to be caught-level II-for total score only) He performed a good deed for someone else after- ward. (defense mechanism-reaction formation)