rt, .. RTE-3" PREDICTORS OF TELEVISION VIEWING AMONG JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTO TIIE$I8 FOR THE OEOBEE OF M. A. _ MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY BRENDA OEBVIN ”I 9 5 O .' . '- " .-,~‘. . 4-?" . ..‘..‘,.‘I.“= 1 AUG 0; 200“ ABSTRACT PREDICTORS OF TELEVISION VIEWING AMONG JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS by Brenda Dervin The four purposes of this study were: 1) to replicate some previous work on correlates of frequency of child television viewing; 2) to include certain variables which have been partly or wholly over- looked in past work; 3) to extend the analysis to a multivariate method; and QI‘to specifically look at the types of relationships which exist between the criterion variable and its predictors. Respondents for this study were 252 seventh and eighth grade boys ‘I K 'nd girls -- the entire junior high school class in a suburban school system outside Milwaukee, Wisconsin. The data used here were drawn from a larger survey tapping mass media behaviors and family life style patterns. Survey questionnaires were self-administered in regular classes during the 196u-65 school year. From the available data, 63 variables were selected -- 62 pre- dictors and the criterion. Among the major results and conclusions of the study were the following. 1) Of the 62 predictors, 21 were significantly related (by X2 and/or r analyses) to the criterion variable. Brenda Dervin 2) No single variable alone eXplained a great deal of variance in the criterion. Maximum variance explained by any one variable was 18% with a curvilinear model, 9% with a linear model. This result suggests the need for multivariate approaches. 3) Variables were subdivided into seven categories and multiple Rs were run within categories to determine which variables contributed significantly to variance explained, assuming a linear model. The "best" within category predictors were: a) parent media use -- amount of parent TV viewing and variety of parent radio use; b) respondent media use -- amount of respondent radio use, variety of respondent radio use,‘variety of respondent book preferences, frequency of reapcrdent movie attendance, and medium respondent would miss most; c) family cohesiveness -- none; d) community integration -- none; e) self-orientation -- re5pondent outside home employment and respondent math knowledge level; f) consumer orientation -- re3pondent attitude toward credit and variety of respondent Spending; and g) demography_-- occupational prestige. n) Assuming a linear model, the best category of predictors (in terms of variance accounted for in the criterion ) was respondent media use (18%). Second best was parent media use (1u%), followed by self-orientation and consumer orientation (8% each). Demography accounted for 6%. The multiple Rs for family cohesiveness and community integration were not significant. One of the better predictor categories -- consumer orientation -- included almost all new variables -- i.e. variables not looked at as predictors of TV viewing in prior research. Brenda Dervin 5) When the "best" within category predictors were pooled in one multiple R.equation, the resulting R.was .50, accounting fOr 25% of the variance in the criterion. When all 62 vairables, regardless of category, were included in one multiple R equation, the resulting R was .65, accounting for 42% of the variance. ' 6) A comparison of the linear r with the curvilinear Eta correlations for the relationship of each predictor to the criterion indicated that the curvilinear model fit the data better. 7) A comparison of the current results with prior research suggested that certain assumptions derived from early child-television- research need re-examination, particularly in light of today's high media saturation environment. As an example, the present results offered little support fer two often-used generalizations about television iewing -- the frustration hypothesis and the functional displacement C +‘ . hypotneSLS. PREDICTORS or TELEVISION VIEWING AMONG JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS By Brenda Dervin A THES IS Submitted to g Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Communication 1968 JI Accepted by the faculty of the Department of Commmication, College of Communication Arts, Michigan State University, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Master of Arts degree. .‘ /Z f (rs ' __ ‘ flaflél'l - Director of. lThesis ACKN OW LB DGIEN TS Special thanks go, first and foremost, to Dr. Bradley Greenberg, who has been a most patient advisor and perceptive critic. Thanks go also to Dr. Vincent Farace and Dr. Thomas Baldwin who both served on the author's thesis committee; Albert Talbott, Jeffrey Katzer, and Anita Immele who all gave considerable computer assistance; and Mrs. Shirley Sherman who typed the final manuscript. Po [-0. TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION . . . . . . Rationale . . . . . . Generalizations from Prior Research . . . . . . . . . Classification of Variables II HYPOTHBSES . . . . . . Respondent Perceptions ReSpondent Media Use . Family Cohesiveness . Self-Orientation . . . Consumer Orientation . Demography . . . . . . III METHODOLOGY . . . . . . of Parent Media Use . . . . . . Questionnaire Administration . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Respondents and Their Community . . . . . ... . . nItem Selection . . . . tem Measurement . . . Item COding O O O 0 O I v RES ULTS O O O O O O O 0 Reduction of Items by Deletion and Indexing . . . . . Description of the Criterion Variable . . . . . . . . Statistical Analyses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Re5pondent Perceptions of Parent Media USe . . . . . . Re3pondent Media Use . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Family Cohesiveness . Community Integration Self-Orientation . . . Consumer Orientation . Demography . . . . . . Comparison of Multiple Linear Correlations Withi Variable Categories Results of Multiple Linear Variable Categories v SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS BIDLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . O O O O O O O O O O O O O C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O C O O O O C O O O O 0 Correlation Analyses Across Page .17 .21 .26 .36 .39 .42 .U8 .H9 .99 .50 .51 .52 .53 .59 .67 .77 .89 .95 102 111 118 126 127 132 151 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Means for Respondent Scores on the Top 16 Television how Checklist, Reported by Questionnaire Administration Date 0 I O I O O O C O O O C O O O C O 0 O C O O O O O O O O 56 2. One-Way Analysis of Variance Table Testing the Difference Between Scores on the Top 16 Television Show Checklist by Questionnaire Administration Date . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56 3. Means for ReSpondent Scores on the 10 Miscellaneous Adult Drama Television Show Checklist, Reported by Questionnaire Administration Date 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O C O O O 5 7 4. One-Way Analysis of Variance Table Testing the Difference Between Scores on the 10 Miscellaneous Adult Drama Television Show Checklist by Questionnaire Administration Date . . . . 57 5. Summary of Results for Individual Hypotheses on the Relationship of Parent Media Use Variables to Frequency of Child Television Viewing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68 I 6. Results of Multiple Linear Correlation Analysis for the Parent Media Use Variable Category . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 7. Contingency Tables and Chi-Square Values for the Relationship of the Parent Media Use Variables to the Criterion Variable 73 8. Summary of Results for Individual Hypotheses on the Relationship of Re3pondent Media Use Variables to Frequency of Child Television Viewing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 9. Results of Multiple Linear Correlation Analysis for the . Respondent Media Use Variable Category . . . . . . . . . . . 82 10. Contingency Tables and Chi-Square Values for the Relation- ship of Respondent Media USe Variables to the Criterion variable 0 O C O O O O O O O O O O O C O O C O O O O O O C 8“ ll. Summary of Results for Individual Hypotheses on the Relationship of Family Cohesiveness Variables to Frequency of Child Television Viewing ~. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 12. Contingency Tables and Chi-Square Values for the Relation- ship of Family Cohesiveness Variables to the Criterion vari able 0 O O O O C O C O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O 9 1 iv List of Tables--continued Table Page 13. Summary of Results for Individual Hypotheses on the Relationship of Community Integration Variables to Frequency of Child Television Viewing . . . . . . . . . . 95 14. Contingency Tables and Chi-Square Values fer the Relationship of Community Integration Variables to the Crite rion Vari ab le 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 9 9 15. Summary of Results for Individual Hypotheses on the Relationship of Self-orientation Variables to Frequency of Child Television Viewing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102 16. Results of Multiple Linear Correlation Analysis for the Self-orientation Variable Category . . . . . . . . . . . 105 17. Contingency Tables and Chi—Square Values fer the Relationship of Self-orientation Variables to the crite rion variab le 0 O O I. O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 lo 7 , I 18. Summary of Results for Individual Hypotheses on the Relationship of Consumer Orientation Variables to Frequency of Child Television Viewing . . . . . . . . . 111 19. Results of Multiple Linear Correlation Analysis for the Consumer Orientation Variable Category . . . . . . . . . llu 20. Contingency Tables and Chi-Square Values for the Relationship of Consumer Orientation Variables to the Criterion Variable . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ._ 115 21. Summary of Results fer Individual Hypotheses on the Relationship of Demographic Variables to Frequency of Child Television Viewing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118 22. Results of Multiple Linear Correlation Analysis for the Demographic Variable Category . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 121 23. Contingency Tables and Chi-Square Values for the Relationship of Demographic Variables to the Criterion variable 0 O O O O O O O C C O O O O O O O O 0 l2 3 2n. Summary of Within Variable Category Multiple Linear Correlation Analyses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 126 25. Result of Multiple Linear Correlation Analysis with "Best" Predictors from Within Variable Categories . . . . 128 List of Tables--continued Table 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 33. 3“. 37. Result of Multiple Linear Correlation Analysis of All Predictor Variables Regardless of Variable Category . . . . Items, Codes, and Interjudge Coding Reliabilities Listed by Variable Category and Variable Name . . . . . . . . . . Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-ReSponse Counts for the Five Items Used to Create Two Indexes of Permissiveness’ . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Matrix of Pearson Product Moment Correlations Between the Five Items Used to Create Two Indexes of PemiSSj-veness O O O O O O O O O. O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-Response Counts for the Three Items Used to Create an Index of Child-Parent commication o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o 0 Matrix of Pearson Product Moment Correlations Between the Three Items Used to Create an Index of Child-Parent Communication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 . Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-Re3ponse Counts for the Two Items USed to Create an Index of Fm]: 1y Togethemess O O O I O O O O O O O O O O r. O O O O 0 Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non- Response Counts for the in Items Used to Create an Index of Re3pondent Home Responsibilities . . . . . . . . . . . . Matrix of Pearson Product Moment Correlations Between the 1M Items Used to Create an Index of ReSpondent Home Respons ibilities O O ' O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-Re3ponse Counts for the Feur Items Used to Create an Index of Respondent Knowledge of Family Operation . . . . . . . . . Matrix of Pearson Product Moment Correlations Between the Four Items Used to Create an Index of Respondent Knowledge of Family Operation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-ReSponse Counts fer the Two Items Used to Create an Index of ParentGregariousness .................. Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-Re5ponse Counts for the Four Items Tapping Re5pondent Gregariousness VJ. 130 157 178 178 179 180 180 182 182 18% 184 185 186 #1. 42. 1‘3. '44. .1. (.VI ”6. 47. #8. Page Matrix of Pearson Product Moment Correlations Between the Four Items Tapping Respondent Gregariousness . . . . . 186 Code Ranges, Mean , Standard Deviations, and Non-Response Counts for a “s ‘ ed to Create the Outside Home Orientation Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 187 Matrix of Pearson Product Moment Correlations for the Seven Items Used to Create the Outside Home orientation Index 0 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 188 Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-Response Counts for the Two Items Used to Create the Frequency of Church Attendance Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 188 Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-Response Counts for the Five Items Used to Create the Math KnOVIIlEdge Level Index. 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 189 «ix of Pearson Product Moment Correlations Between the Items Used to Create the Math Knowledge Level INdex 190 Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-ReSponse Counts for the Two Items Used to Create the Family Shopping Orientation Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 191 Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-Response Counts for the Four Items Used to Create the Socio- Economic Status Index of Possessions . . . . . . . . . . . 192 - Matrix of Pearson Product Moment Correlations Between the Four Items USed in Creating the Socio-Bconomic Status Index of Possessions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 192 Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-Response Counts, Listed by Variable Category and Variable Name . . 191+ APPENDIX A. LIST OF APPENDICES ITEMS, CODES, AND INTERJUDGE CODING RELIABILITIES LISTED BY VARIABLE CATEGORY. FUND VAR-IABLE NAIE O O O O O O O C O O O O O O 157 INDEXING PROCEDURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . 176 CODE RANGES, MEANS, STANDARD DEVIATIONS, AND NON-RESPONSE COUNTS LISTED BY VARIABLE CATEGORY AND VARIABLE NAME . . . . . . . . . 193 viii I ...._, . as -_.+_ A . 44 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Rationale Ever since television burst upon the U.S. scene in the 1950's, the attention of researchers, critics, and the public has been focused on possible effects of the new, imminently attractive, and pervasive medium on children. Generally, attention to the medium has been rooted in a concern for possible detrimental effects. Does viewing television ruin eyesight? Does it cause children to withdraw from real life concerns? Does it lead to increased aggression, cause juvenile delinquepcy? Does it debase tastes? While the critics and researchers have been busy looking for effects, the public has been busy accepting the new medium. In a true success story of the 20th century, the proportion of 0.8. homes with television sets rose from 7% in 1950 to 82% in 1957 (Witty 1963, Bogart 1958). By 1960, saturation had reached 90% (Bogart 1962) and,. in 1965, reports of 9A to 100% saturation were common. -(Witty 1966, Rainwater, Coleman, and Handel 1959, Huber 1965.) Television continues to be the major leisure time activity with the typical family television set operating 5 hours a day, the average man viewing for 2 1/2 hours, average woman for 3 1/2 hours, and average child for u hours (Bogart 1962). While the popularity of television rose with relative ease, the search for effects did not. Indeed, the search might well be summarized by saying that the more researchers looked for effects, the less they found. In 1957, Meyersohn noted that few of the Inmdreds of studies on television and children had "succeeded in providing much of consequence." (Meyersohn 1957) As late as 196u, Ekbramm asserted that one should not expect "too much, too soon, and too specifically" from effects research (Schramm 1964). In delineating the reasons for the lack of clarity offered by effects research, there seems to be general agreement in pointing to the failure of the "hypodermic model" of communications research. Schraum, White, Klapper and others have all pinpointed the evolution of research on the mass media from a "direct effects" model to the currently accepted phenomenalistic, functional, situational, or uses and gratifications approaches (Schramm 1962, White 196u, Klapper 1960, Katz and Foulkes 1962, Bauer 1964). While once the mass media researCher asked "what do the mass media do to people", today he more often asks "what do people do with the mass media?" While once the researcher viewed the individual child in isolation from his family, social background, and environment, today the researcher looks to these "situational and contextual" elements fer explanation of the television-child interaction. While once the researcher was concerned primarily with measuring effects almost totally in terms of source expectations, today he looks to umsequences which may be entirely independent of source intent. lerper's generalization that mass communication ordinarily does "not serve as a necessary and sufficient cause of audience effects but rather functions among and through a nexus of mediating factors and :hufluences" best sums up the result of the evolution (Klapper 1960, p. 8). A major consequence of this newer approach to media research is that the task is now immensely more complicated. The hypodermic model filits simplicity failed to yield clarity while the functional model in its complexity must be concerned with an enormous number of variables axltheir interactions in order to begin to yield clarity. In ahcepting a more functional approach to their subject, re- searchdrs have Spread their concern from analysis of aggregates or demographic characteristics (White 196%) to concern for the ability, maturity, personality, peer group relations, and family relations of the hild (Himmelweit, Oppenheim, and Vince 1958, Riley and Riley 1955, Bailyn 1959, Maccoby 196M, Schramm, Lyle, Parker 1961).1 Along with the emergence of the functional approach, there has been an increasing awareness of the conditions necessary for developing causality statements in social science research. While once the researcher was quite ready to infer effect statements from field 1Two of these references -- Himmelweit, Oppenheim, and Vince (1958) and Sohramm, Lyle, and Parker (1961) will be referred to extensively in Chapters I and II. All further citations of these two references will be made simply to Himmelweit (1958) and Schramm (1961). Iemxxth, today researchers are careful in emphasizing that their fkfid studies on the relationship of television and children are cmnelational in nature. After careful analysis of the methods of ldfldng for the effects of television, Maccoby concluded that only theleboratory experiment can come close to causality requirements. Quanoted that even precise before-after field surveys using matching tedudques do not offer true randomization, so causal connections cannot begfinned down from such research (Maccoby 196u). As a result of these two trends 1. the acceptance of the functional axuoadh to research and the awareness of the limitations of field zesearCh -- the current state of affairs in television and children research seems to include: 1) an acceptance of'non-experimental field research as a search for "correlates" of media use rather than "effects" of media use; 2) an acceptance of the need to look at a number of areas of the child's life in order to more fully understand and describe the child-television interaction; and 3) a call for increased eXperimental work from which "effect statements" may be derived. We find Schramm summarizing his intensive review of the past television-children research by saying that the classic field surveys 'have gone about as far as it is possible to go with survey methods. . . ." (Sdntmm1196u, p. 7). Yet, rejection of the "correlate" approach to researdh on television and children may be a bit too hasty. While the -u MIA correlational-type approach will not yield effect statements, the possibilities of the approach have not been tapped to their fullest extent. Most of the past work, while acknowledging a correlational approach, has been very much rooted in a search for effects. Findings on television and children have come mainly from studies concerned with the differences between matched reSpondents in television and non-television households. The two classic studies -- Himmelweit (1958) and Schramm (1961) —- were based on a concern for the effects of the introduction of television although both did look somewhat at correlates of amount of viewing. ' Whileithere is a great deal of research on television and childrea only a small subset is directly relevant to the television environment of the 1960's with its almost 100% saturation of TV households. The absence-presence of viewing is almost no longer relevant. White, Bdelstein and others have made references to the need to take a new look at audience composition and utilization of media in terms of the current media environment. (Edelstein 1966, White 1964) They suggest that once well-supported principles of mass media behavior like the "all or none principle" (first posited by Lazarsfeld and Kendall 1948) may no longer hold in today's context of media overload. One recent factor analytic study of media exposure patterns of teenagers, for example, suggests that once rather clear- cut relationships between various aspects of media usage may no longer be quite so clear. (Troldahl and Costello 1966). u--. r .. p. .. 4... ‘ w..- ~-. - - . uh... ‘--¢ a. h. If: ‘\ ‘v Another reason the correlational approach still has merit is that many crucial variables have been completely or partly ignored in the past work. For example, we find that family consumer behaviors have been rarely included as possible correlates of child television behaviors. Yet, with one of the main family functions being its consumption role in society, this seems a lucrative area in which to extend the television-children focus. Chapter II more fully details variables which have been partly or wholly overlooked in the past work. A third reason for not yet eulogizing the correlational approach to television and children is the almost total absence in the past work of multivariate analytic approaches. Himmelweit (1958) and SchramN (1961) illustrate the common use of contingency analysis of frequencies and percentages. While researchers have developed lists of what they consider to be the most important predictor variables of television behavior these have usually been derived from analyses of one predictor variable at a time and developed into a kind of jigsaw puzzle portrait. An exception is Bailyn (1959) who used a multivariate technique to arrive at a statement of the four variables which accounted for approximately 47% of the variance in amount of exposure to the pictorial media (comics, films, television). Multivariate analysis techniques offer several interesting possibilities for the area of television and children. First is the obvious advantage of using techniques which detect spurious corre- lations and isolate interactions. Second is the ability of these techniques to handle the larger numbers of variables demanded by the more functional analysis of media behaviors. Third is the parsimony offered by techniques which reduce a large set of variables down to the set most important in terms of variance accounted for. Related to the lack of use of multivariate approaches, the past work on television and children also shows an almost total absence of concern with the exact nature of the relationship of predictor variables to the criterion media behavior. Most predictions of relationships ' have been stated in a linear fashion. Further, even contingency analysis which can tap curvilinear relationships has been essentially used with dichotomous classification of the criterion television viewing behavior. This makes detection of curvilinearity impossible. The pichotomization of the criterion variable was a necessary con- straint for those researchers who, in the past, looked at the absence and presence of television. However, it is rare to find any more than two categories even when researchers were looking at amount of television viewing. Schramm (1961), for example, used simply high and low television viewing categories. An exception is the Himmelweit (1958) study in which three categories of television viewing were used. In summary, then, the correlational approach to studying the relationship of television and children seems to still have merit mainly because it has not been utilized to its fullest. Much of the past research does not apply to today's 100 per cent television saturation. Many crucial variables have been partly or wholly over- looked. Multivariate analysis techniques have not been used, and specific attempts to look at the nature of the relationships between predictor variables and the criterion media behavior have not been made. With this background, the present study aims to look at correlates of the amount of television viewing by junior high school students. The purposes of this study are: 1) To replicate much of the past work on correlates of frequency of child television viewing; 2) To go beyond a replication by including some variables which have been partly or wholly overlooked in past works; 3) To extend the analysis to a multivariate method; and 4) To Specifically look at the types of relationships which exist between the criterion variable and its predictors. Generalizations from_prior research It seems most efficient at this point to draw together the most parsimonious generalizations from across the literature and reserve Specific citations for supporting the hypotheses formulated in Chapter II. The generalizations stated in this section come primarily from the conceptual discussions of Himmelweit (1958), Schramm (1961), Campbell (1962), Maccoby (1964), and Klapper (1960). A thread seems to tie the various recent approaches to predicting amount of television viewing together. This thread might best be called the functionalist's agreement to look at the child as an integrated human being acting in a reasonable fashion within the context of his environment. We find such opening generalizations as "the mass media exist because they are useful in meeting human needs and that TV has come into use ... because it meets some of these needs better than any other alternative." (Schramm 1961, p. 74). This unifying vieWpoint ties together the more firm generalizations derivable from past work. Most writers have phrased their generalizations in terms of effects rather than in terms of relationships. Thus, one finds reference to the direct and indirect effects possible from television in Maccoby (1964; or a list of possible effects in terms of physical, emotional, cognitive, and behavioral effects in Schramm (1961), or to "displacement of time effects". and effects of content in Himmelweit O (1959 . The generalizations stated below have rephrased "effect" nations into "correlational" notions. A good deal of the prior research from which these generalizations are derived have dealt with both the quantity and quality of a child's television viewing behavior. Since the present study deals only with amount of television viewing, findings and generalizations are derived from past work only when relevant to the quantity of viewing. We have derived the following five generalizations: 1. The parental imitation generalization: While not explicitly Stated as such in past literature, an underlying theme of the results might be framed: "children tend to do what their parents do, all other things equal." Thus, consistently researchers have feund that one of the best predictors of any child's media behavior is the media behavior of his parents. Three major researchers all concur that if parents 10 view a lot, children tend to do likewise; if parents view moderately, children tend to also. (Schramm 1961, Maccoby 1964, Himmelweit 1958). A logical consequence of the above generalization is that a number of demographic characteristics which predict parent television behavior predict the child's behavior as well. Rather consistently the findings agree that the occupational level and education of parents predicts the amount their children view (Bailyn 1959, Schramm 1961). Specific findings on social class will be offered later but the variable is offered now as an example of the class of readily tapped variables which might be termed attributes of parents which predict the child's behavior. . 2. The demographic attributes of the child generalization: Again this generalization has not been stated explicitly in the prior research. It suggests that there are a number of good predictors of television viewing which are not accounted for by parental behavior or attributes nor strictly by generalizations 3, 4, and 5 below. For example, the child's age is often found to be strong predictors of various media behaviors. In terms of television viewing, findings rather consistently concur that the peak television viewing time for children is roughly from the ages of 11 to 13 (SChramm 1961). 3. The functional displacement generalization: This _generalization was first framed by Himmelweit (1958) and has structured numerous approaches since. In essence, the generalization is that a child must choose between activities and that he will sacrifice in lieu of television those activities which satisfy the same needs as television but do so less effectively. He will not 11 sacrifice, however, those activities which serve needs different than those served by television. The term "functional" is essential in the _generalization as what is "functional" to one group of children may not necessarily be for another. As an example, it is a rather consistent finding that, movie attendance is sacrificed for television viewing (Himmelweit 1958, Schramm 1958 and others). However, when a child reaches adolescence the movie theater becomes a meeting place fer friends and the displacement phenomenon no longer holds. One problem with the di5placement generalization is that it has most often been tested with a criterion variable of absence vs presence of television in the home. It is difficult to make the leap from a O finding in that context to a prediction that would hold fer amount of teievision viewing. It seemed quite clear that when families purchased television sets movie viewing dropped off. However, after years of television ownership, it is not so clear that the person with a high desire for the kind of need fulfillment offered.by both television and movies will fulfill that need primarily through television., 4. The frustration generalization: First stated by Maccoby (1951) this generalization States that the more frustrated a Child is, the more time he will Spend in front of his television set. The usual indices of frustration have been the degree of the child's integration within both his family and his peer group social systems. In a theoretical discussion of the use of mass media as escape, Katz and Foulkes (1962) point to the impressive evidence supporting the notion that alienation, deprivation, and frustration leads to increased exposure. 5. The information void generalization: While more directly related to the quality of a child's viewing rather than the quantity, this generalization is relevant to some aspects of the present study. Posited first by Himmelweit (1958), the generalization states that the conditions under which television content is likely to have an effect on a child's values and outlooks are: a) if the values and views recur over and over again in TV content; b) if the ferm of presentation is dramatic; c) if the contentis linked with the child's needs and interests; d) if the child is uncritical and attached to the medium; and e) if the child is not presented through peers or relatives with a standard against which to assess the views offered on television. Thus? Himmelweit found that female adolescent television viewers were more concerned than matched non-viewers with the problems of growing up and marrying. From this generalization, one might reason that since television content in the United States tends to be somewhat of the same ilk, that there should be some relationship between the amount a child views television and the child's attitudes on topics which television emphasizes but parents and peers rarely talk about. The five'generalizations above form a pattern which agrees with the functionalist's basic view of predicting the child's behavior within the context of his environment. In essence, the generalizations say that when predicting the amount a child views television, the research can account for a certain amount of variance by looking at the demographic characteristics of the child's parents and at the parent's media behavior.’ Another portion of the variance can be ,- . , u. -. l .... u I... . n.- in: ‘V 13 accounted for by looking at the child's own demographic characteristics and his other media behaviors (which, not surprisingly, are often a close replica of the media behaviors of his parents). After using these rather efficient approaches (efficient in the sense that the variables involved are relatively easy to tap), the further differences between children must be accounted for by looking at the values and attitudes of the child and the child's integration within his family unit and within his peer group. Classification of variables Researchers in this area increasingly agree that the greatest explanatory results are yielded by surveys which seek information on a‘variety of dimensions of a child's life. Thus, Himmelweit, in describing television effects, had to time and again "consider the ways these effects differed according to the ability, maturity, background, and personality of the children concerned." (1958, p. xiv). Essentially, there is agreement that a sizeable number of variables are needed to predict the child's television behavior. With this in mind; the present study deals with a large number of predictor variables, 62 in all. With such a large number of predictors, some efficient method of classifying the variables is necessary. The 62 predictor variables tapped in this study were grouped into seven categories on the following criteria: 14 1) Agreement with the methods used to group variables in past research. It has been rather standard procedure in past work to group attributes of the child's media use as a separate category of variables. Other consistently used categories have included: parent media use, family integration, community integration, personal ex- pression (e.g. hobbies), and demography. These are essentially the categories used for the present study. 2) Facilitation of the formation of hypotheses. A second criterion in the classification of variables used here-is to group together variables for which predictions form a unified whole. For example, past literature suggests that predictions about the re- lationship of1elevision viewing to family integration may all essentially be derived from the frustration hypothesis. When a 00 romp of variables seemed to easily fall into such a unified category, this advantage was utilized. 3) Concern for the number of variables within each category. Since one of the purposes of the present study is to utilize a multi- variate analysis as well as a variable-by-variable prediction method, an effort was made to keep the number of variables within each category large enough so that multivariate analysis could be utilized both within variable categories and then across the entire set of 62 predictors. Based on the above criteria, the 62 predictors in this study were organized in seven categories: 1) parent media use; 2) respondent media use; 3) family cohesiveness; 1+) community integration; 5) self orientation; 6) consumer orientation; and 7) demography. The specific 15 rationale for each category is in Chapter II. Throughout this report, these seven categories are used as a means of organizing the statement of hypotheses, findings, and implications. CHAPTER II H‘IPOTHESES In review, the purposes of this study are: l) to replicate much of the past work on correlates of frequency of child television viewing; 2) to go beyond a replication by including some variables which have been partly or wholly overlooked in past work; 3) to extend the analysis to a multivariate method; and it) to Specifically look at the types of relationships which exist between the criterion variable and its predictors. Since very little of the available work in this area has attacked‘purposes 3 and 1+, formal hypotheses will be stated only for Purp‘ose l and 2. The stage for hypotheses might best be set by a brief review 01‘ the general television behavior of children who are around the 3838 of 13.-11+. Past work suggests that these respondents are at the peak of their television viewing time -- viewing some 20 or more hours a week (Schramm 1961, Witty 1963, Himmelweit 1958, Maccoby .1963). PiuJFJ< agrees that with the introduction of television, radio listening :3‘1iFiered for both adult and child television viewers (Coffin 1955, Abrams 1956, Maccoby'lQSl, Campbell 1962, Schramm 1961, Himmelweit 1958). The relationship of amount of viewing to amount of radio use iii not quite so clear. Parker (1960) reported a non-significant 'relationship as did Troldahl and Costello (1966). Bailyn (1959) however, found a small but significant negative correlation between the two variables. In view of this conflict in evidence, the pre- diction for this variable is based on a logical inference from both the functional displacement and parental imitation generalizations outlined in Chapter I. These generalizations suggest that radio use should be displaced, to some degree at least, by increasing television usage and that children should imitate their parents in these media behaviors. 'Thus hypothesis 4 is: Amount of parental radio use will be negatively an: related to frequency of child television viewing. Variety ofgparental radio use. Pew past studies have looked especifically at this variable in relation to child television usage. .Iir their comparisons of television vs non-television children, both ESCiaramm (1961) and Himmelweit (1958) found that radio became a more SPecialized medium in television households. While the leap from the CijLcehotomous television vs no television situation to actual amount C>i?' viewing is tenuous, these findings suggest hypothesis 5: H5: Variety of parental radio use will be negatively related to frequency of child television viewing. Number of newSpapers subscribed to. Again, none of the work clfiLted here has looked at the relationship of parental use of the print rneedia to child's use of television. However, considerable prior work flee looked at the relationship of adult print media use to adult ‘television use and child print media use to child television use. The findings fairly consistently agree that, with the introduction of television, some aSpects of viewer reading suffered (Belson 1959, 20 Coffin l955, Campbell 1962, Himmelweit 1958, Bailyn 1959, Schramm 196l, and Parker 1963). The reduction appeared heaviest for books and magazines but affected some aSpects of newSpaper reading. However, several trend studies indicated that the difference between television vs non-television users tended to disappear over time. Fewer studies have looked Specifically at the relationship between amount of television viewing and use of the print media. For use of newspapers, the findings conflict. Westley and Severin (1961;) found a non- ! significant relationship between adult time spent on television and time Spent on newspapers while Himmelweit (1958) found that television viewing related negatively to newSpaper reading for their children respondents. Despite some contradictions, hypothesis 6 is: I ; H6: The number of newspapers subscribed to by f parents will be negatively related to ' frequency of child television viewing. " Number of magazines parents read. Here again evidence is uncle ax. Himmelweit (1958) found a tendency for magazine reading t° suffer with increasing amounts of viewing. The television V5 “'3 talevision comparisons (as indicated in support for hypotheSiS 5) generally agreed that magazine reading decreased with the introduction 0f television. Thus hypothesis 7 is: ‘ H7: Amount of parental magazine reading will be negatively related to frequency of child television viewing. -_ A. - -.- W s . J ‘._...-—— ..WWHP‘ .... ..q 21 Parental sources of news. None of the literature cited here 3155 looked at the relationship between parental sources of news and axxnmt of child television viewing. However, other work in the germral area of media credibility and media perceptions suggests that perceptions of major news sources is related positively to amount of media use (Carter and Greenberg 1965, Westley and Severin 1961+). Thus, hypothesis 8 is: H8: Children who perceive their parents' major source of news as television will be more frequent television viewers than children who. perceive their parents' major news source as a medium other than television. Number of phonographs owned. None of the television use studies cited here have looked at phonograph usage as a predictor Variab 1e. The only support available comes from consumer studies (Such as Huber 1965, Caplovitz 1963) that indicate that families that have gone in debt for television sets have very often also done so for phonOgmaphs. On the basis of this slim evidence, hypothesis 9 is: H9: Number of phonographs owned will be positively related to frequency of child television viewing. Re\Sp°\rldent media use The second category of variables treated in this study taps the re . . SpQT‘Adent'S own media behaViors. Most category schemes developed by re . . Searchers have a class of this sort (e.g. Schramm 1961, Himmelweit 22 Predictions within this category have been derived mainly from me functional displacement generalization, stating that other activities will be sacrificed in lieu of television if they serve the same needs as television but do so less well. Amount of respondent radio use. Support for a prediction here is essentially the same as that offered for hypothesis 1 stated on page 18. Hypothesis 10 is: H10: Amount of respondent radio use will be negatively related to frequency of television viewing. Variety of respondent radio use. Again, support is essentially the same as that offered for hypothesis 2 stated on page 18. Hypothesis ll is: Hll: Variety of respondent radio use will be negatively related to frequency of television Viewing. Variety of respondent newspaper use. Most of the prior work has looked at another dimension of newspaper usage, namely time SPént on neWSpaper reading. The evidence is fairly well summed up in Support of hypothesis 6 stated on page 20 on parental newspaper use -- newsp aI>er reading suffered with the introduction of television but the mlat ionship of actual newspaper use to time Of viewing is “0': clear. _Schralnm (1961) further suggests that with junior high school age childPen, at least, newspaper usage is jUSt beginning to expand 5° V““'\‘ culanee on the variable is restricted. This evidence suggests my? Q‘ihes is 12: 23 H12: Variety of reSpondent newspaper use will be negatively related to frequency of television viewing. Number of magazines respondent reads. This variable is also bunkered by lack of support and conflicting evidence as noted in the discussion preceding hypothesis 7 stated on page 20 on parental xnagazine use. DeSpite the conflict, the prediction on respondent magazine use agrees with that on parent magazine use. Hypothesis 13 is: H13: Number of magazines the respondent reads will be negatively related to frequency of television viewing. Variety of reSpondent book preferences. As with the other print media use‘variables, evidence here is unclear. However, in predicting he rgelationship of book reading to amount of television viewing, rt SEVTBIFal researchers have found.negative relationships between the Quéuaftzitative dimensions of both variables (Himmelweit 1958, Bailyn 1959) ”hiJLfie others have found non-significant relationships. Parker(l963) e”(Pl—Eiined the lack of significance in terms of the displacement gene I‘alization which suggests that television viewing should displace only certain kinds of book reading, namely fiction which fulfills the mane: :fantasy gratification function. While the bulk of evidence Sug‘gests a negative relationship bet-teen amount of book reading and amo‘nl‘t of television viewing, interestingly the only finding available for the variable -- variety of respondent book preferences -- suggests a Positive relationship. Himmelweit(1958) found (in' her ‘ absence vs pbe‘S-ence of television viewing situation) that television widened the heading tastes of viewers. While making the leap from this finding 24 to amount of television viewing is, perhaps, tenuous, hypothesis 1% is: H14: Variety of respondent book preferences will be positively related to frequency of television viewing. Respondent preference for comics. Evidence here is clearer than for the other print media. Parker(l961l Himmelweit(l959l and Bailyn<1959)all found a positive relationship between amount of comic book reading and amount of television viewing -- a relationship that is particularly strong for addict television viewers. Thus, hypothesis 15 is: H15: ReSpondent preference for comics will be positively related to frequency of television viewing. ' ReSpondent library use. This variable has received attention mainly from Parker (1963) and Bogart (1958). In his absence vs presence of television analysis, Parker reported that television displaced library fiction circulation mainly but that library use did decrease generally. Again, the leap to a prediction for amount of television viewing is tenuous. However, on the basis of the above evidence, hypothesis 16 is: H16: Respondent library use will be negatively related to frequency of television viewing. Frequency of respondent movie attendance. Most of the evidence here applies to the absence vs presence of television situation and generally agrees that movie attendance was hard hit by the introduction of television (Coffin 1955, Campbell 1962, Belson 1959, Abrams 1956, Maccoby 1951). However, Himmelweit (1958) emphasized that, for 25 teenagers, television should not functionally replace movies as the movie theater becomes an arena for social interaction at this age. While that finding might suggest a non-significant relationship between movie attendance and televisionrviewing, Bailyn (1959) found a significant positive relationship between the two variables. Hypothesis 17 is: H17: Frequency of reSpondent movie attendance will be positively related to frequency of television viewing. ‘ Variety of respondent record preferences. Support here is the same as that offered for hypothesis 8 stated on page 21 on parental ownership of phonographs. Hypothesis 18 is: H18: Variety of reSpondent record preferences will be positively related to frequency of television viewing. I Media respondent would miss most. Hypotheses for respondent media preference and media credibility ratings must be derived mainly fumistudies other than those done on children's television use. Wenfley and Severin (196R) and Carter and Greenberg (1965) suggest that gwxeeptions of the media are directly related to media use. Hypothesis 19 is: H19: ReSpondents who indicate they would miss television most will be heavier television Viewers. 3 Respondent media credibility ratings. Based on the support oflrred for hypothesis 19 above, hypothesis 20 is: 26 D n: r.) 0 Respondents who indicate that television is their most believed medium will be heavier television viewers while those who indicate television is their least believed medium will be lighter television viewers. Family cohesiveness The third category of variables tapped in this study includes respondent reports on the character of interaction within his family. An impressive number of researchers have emphasized the need to look at the child's media behavior within the context of his family life. (Freidson 1955, Freedman 1961, Meyersohn 1957, Campbell 1962, Clark 1965, Riley and Riley 1955). s DeSpite agreement on the need to look at the child's family life, regatively little work has been done in the area of family relation- ships and mass media. One recent review of child development re- search, for example, lists not a single reference in a one-hundred- plus item bibliography on the relationship of the mass media within the context of the child's development (Douvan and Gold 1966). .Most of the support cited for hypotheses below, therefbre, is quite recent and indicative of an attempt (pointed out by Clarke 1965). to study the development of media use patterns by looking at parental socialization techniques. A growing body of evidence suggests that this is a fruitful approach but one also fraught with problems. Himmelweit (1958), Schramm (1961), and other of the major children-television researchers have emphasized their collective findings that the quality of a child's home life is a predictor of his preoccupation with television. 27 1 Host of their findings may be derived from the frustration generalization which simply states that the more frustrated a child is the more he will turn to television as an escape. Freidson (1953) and Katz and Rmdkes(l962) have dampened the clarity of this gen- eralization however, with their assertion that high exposure to television may be as much a sign of lack of frustration in a closely knit family as it is a sign of frustration and resulting efferts to escape in a disruptive family. Other researchers (e.g. Clausen 1966) have also begun to Stress the need for looking at the structure of a family as a complex class of variables needing analysis in at least several dimensions in order to yield clarity. DeSpite these : admonitions which point up glaring weaknesses in existing attempts to look at the mass media within the family life context, the available work does provide a baseline fer making predictions for the present study. Parental‘permissiveness. This variable seems most closely aligned with the variable past researchers have used to test the frustration hypothesis in the context of family life. Maccoby (1954), for example, found that highly frustrated middle class children Spent more time viewing television than children who were not frustrated. A major portion of Maccoby's index of frustration was the severity of punishment and the degree of percental permissiveness. Becker (1964) 1Most of the literature has applied this prediction to middle class reSpondents only. The respondents for the present study are for the most part middle class, as will be pointed out in Chapter III. 28 also notes that restrictive socialization techniques tend to lead to fearful, dependent, submissive children. The Maccoby finding has been consistently replicated by Schramm (1961), Bailyn (1959), and Lyle (1962). Thus, hypothesis 21 is: H21: Parental permissiveness will be negatively related to frequency of child television viewing. Child-parent communication. None of the literature cited here deals Specifically with the degree to which the child feels able to talk to his parents. However, several of the major television and children studies make references to child-parent communication and child-parent conflicts. Thus, Maccoby (1954) used as one of her indices of frustration the degree of warmth in the child-parent in- teraction. Schramm (1961) found that when children saw a conflict between themselves and their parents they Spent more time with television. Fine and Maccoby (1962) and Campbell (1962) indicated in their absence vs presence of television studies that while television families spent more time together there was less inter; personal interaction during that time. The intersection of this evidence suggests hypothesis 22. H22: The degree to which the child sees himself as being able to talk to his parents will be negatively related to frequency of child television viewing. Family togetherness. A variable related to the one above is the number of activities family members share together. Findings from the absence vs presence of television studies agree that while 29 families with television tended to Spend more time in the home, the time spent on non-television activities was reduced. (Himmelweit 1958, Coffin 1955, Hamilton and Lawless 1956, and Maccoby 1951). Thus, hypothesis 23 for this study is: H23: The number of activities in which family members mutually share will be negatively related to frequency of child television viewing. Parent orientation. None of the literature cited here has —— specifically dealt with the degree to which the child sees one or both of his parents as having done the most fer them. However, this variable lOgically seems like another indice of the nature of the child-parent relationship and is cited (Douvan and.Adelson 1966) in child develoPment literature (usually labelled as emotional attachment to parents or its opposite, emotional autonomy). One study (Campbell 1962) found that television children cited their parents less frequently as "ego ideals." Since most evidence suggests that junior high school students are just beginning to broaden their social contacts to outside family members (Remmers 1957), it would seem that the child who expresses open separateness from his parents. is indicating a degree of parent-child conflict. Thus, hypothesis 2# for this study is: H2“: Those reSpondents who name neither parent as "having done the most fer them" will be most likely to be heavy television viewers; those respondents who name only one parent will be next most likely; and those who name both parents will be least likely to be heavy viewers. 30 Parent decision making. Some studies have tapped various behaviors indicating whether the mother or father dominates various household activities or whether both parents mutually Share in authority. One absence vs presence of TV study indicated that in television families, both parents tended to mutually share in the settling of program disagreements and selection of family activities (Hamilton and Lawless 1956). While sparse, this evidence provides hypothesis 25: H25: Where parents mutually share authority, their children will be heavier television viewers than where one parent holds most of the authority. Respondent's home responsibilities. A number of prior studies have tapped as another index of the character of the child's home life, the degree to which the respondent himself Shares in household responsibilities. Evidence from such child development researchers as (Hansen (1966) indicates that American children in general Share minimally in household tasks and that children who have a high involve- ment in household tasks tend to be more compliant and submissive. These latter attributes are often applied to children who are heavier tears of television (Himmelweit 1958)., Specific findings from the television and children studies indicate that fer the absence vs presence of television situation, members of television families 1rnded to spend more time on household chores and share the burden thousehold tasks (Hamilton and Lawless 1956, Belson 1960). The above 31 emdence suggests hypothesis 26: H26: The number of home reSponsibilities a child has will be positively related to frequency of child television viewing. ReSpondent knowledge of family operation. None of the prior work reported here has specifically looked at the respondent's knowledge of such everyday household operation questions as whether his family has banking accounts, what type of heat is used in the household, and so on. However, Himmelweit (1958) and others have hypothesized that television accelerates the impact of adult life, suggesting that the heavier television user may be more attuned to such questions. Thus, hypothesis 27 is: H27: Respondent knowledge of family operation . will be positively related to frequency of television viewing. Parent-child agreement on television program choices. Little of the prior work has Specifically looked at this. variable. However, the intersections of evidence provided for several prior hypotheses suggests that parent-child agreement on television choices Should relate positively to amount of child television viewing. If amount 0f child viewing is posifirely related to amount of parent viewing, then the child has, in part, at least acquired his viewing habit from Parental imitation. Further Himmelweit (1958) and others have pointed out that heavy television users tend to come from families where television is used as a child distractor and "babysitter" and where Childreri's choices, implicitly at least, tend to dominate program " 1 c . o . ~ - o o o °e-eCtionS. This then prov1des anor: “313 on which parent-child television choices should seem in agreement as amount of TV viewing Idses. Thus, hypothesis 28 is: H28: Parent-child agreement on television program choices will relate positively to frequency of child television viewing. Respondent's Perceptions of His Own and His Parents Community Integration The fourth category of variables tapped in this study include reSpondent perceptions of the degree of his own and his parents integration within the community. The same researchers who have stressed the need to study media use patterns within the context of infily life have also stressed the need to look at these behaviors ydthin the context of the individual's integration into peer and ammunigy groups. Additional support for the emphasis is offered by research on teenagers which suggests that friendship relations are a.major problem for adolescents as they begin for the first time to mkmch outside their families for their contacts (Remmers 1957). ufleman (1961) further suggests that there is increasing evidence that today actual family relationships have less influence on behavior as adolescents look more to each other and less to adults fin~their social rewards. As with the family cohesiveness class of variables, most pre- (fictions of the relationship between community integration and amount cfi television viewing are derived from the frustration generalization. The prediction fer variables which seem to be tapping any type of alienation would be for a positive relationship between degree of alienation and amount of exposure. Again, this class of variables presents a problem in terms of predictions. Much of the literature suggests that such variables relate to quality of use differences between reSpondents rather than quantity of use differences (Riley and Riley 1955). Television may be escape for a low gregarious reSpon- dent and a method of entertaining friends fer a high gregarious respondent. Parent gregariousness. None of the literature cited here has specifically looked at the relationship between parent gregariousness and frequency of child television viewing. However, the literature generally shows that the introduction of television led to a reduction in gregariousness (in terms of interaction with peers informally) I for both adults and children (Campbell 1962, Belson 1960, Hamilton and Lawless 1956). Himmelweit (1958). emphasized that one of the characteristics of ‘.her "addict" viewers was their less frequent visiting with friends and their feeling of rejection by peers. Conflicting evidence, suggesting a non-significant relationship, is offered by Maccoby (1951) in her television vs no television com- parison and Troldahl and Costello (1966). Both found no relationship between time Spent with teenage friends and time Spent viewing. When gregariousness is Operationalized in terms of participation in community activities, the bulk of the evidence applies only to the television vs no television comparison and shows no relationship (Abrams 1956, Belson 1959, and Himmelweit 1958). Despite the conflict 34 in findings, a combination of the parental imitation and frustration generalizations yields hypothesis 29: H29: Parental gregariousness will be negatively related to frequency of child television viewing. Respondent gregariousness. On the basis of the evidence cited for hypothesis 29, hypothesis 30 is: H30: Respondent gregariousness will be negatively related to frequency of television viewing. Outside home orientation. A good deal of the literature has been concerned with the relationship between television behaviors and the i family's use of community resources. Most of the evidence comes F“ I rem television vs no television comparisons and shows that television use did relate to a reduction in attendance at outside home events. Hypothesis 31 is: H31: The degree of a family's outside home orientation will be negatively related to the frequency of child television viewing. Length of time in community. Little evidence is available on this variable. However, Himmelweit (1958) noted that one of the characteristics of -her "addict" viewers was that they were newer to their neighborhoods. Based on this sparse evidence, hypothesis 32 is: H32: Length of time in community will be negatively related to frequency of child television viewing. 35 Frequency of attendance at church. Evidence here conflicts with several researchers finding that the introduction of television lead to a reduction in church attendance (Hamilton and Lawless 1956, Campbell 1962); others finding no such reduction (Himmelweit 1958); and others finding no relationship between time Spent on church activities and time Spent on television (Westley and Severin 1964). Bailyn (1959) found, interestingly, that Catholics were more exposed to her "pictorial media", including television, than non-Catholics and this variable was one of her four strong predictors of exposure. Despite the conflict in findings, if church attendance is conceptualized as a measdre of community integration, hypothesis 33 results: I H33: Frequency of attendance at church will be negatively related to frequency of child television viewing. Respondent knowledge of local and state_public figures. The one tut of evidence available for this variable comes from Schramm (1961) who found that light television viewer children were more able to, identify statemen than heavy television viewers. Ordinarily, this variable is placed in the category of variables dealing with reSpondent knowledge levels (called the "self-orientation" category :hxthe present study). However, the variable has been placed in the community integration category for this study because the respondents were not actively involved in the study of local and state affairs. This suggests that their knowledge in the area would more logically he an index of the degree to which their parents are concerned with 2 transformation test of significance (McNemar 1962), the critical ‘mlue of r at p<(.05 fer an n of 252 is .12. All correlations :flgnificant at or beyond the .05 level will be noted in the findings sections. I Eta curvilinear correlation ratio. Eta or the correlation ratio is a measure which taps degrees of relationships in general, whether they U' o linear or curvilinear. If a relationship is actually linear, the Eta auithe product moment correlation will be the same. If the relationship hsactually curvilinear, the Eta will be larger than the product moment anrelation. (McNemar 1962). On the basis of this reasoning, Etas are reported for the mflationship of each of the predictor variables to the criterion vanumle. Since there are two Etas for each relationship, the 63 specific Eta reported here is the one which taps the accuracy with which the criterion variable (Y) can be predicted by the predictor variable (X).1 One question that might be asked is why use Eta when the contingency analysis described earlier taps curvilinearity. One problem with contingency analysis and the use of the chi-square distribution, however, is the constraint of needing adequate cell us. This requires collapsing variables, often to such a degree that. there is not sufficient Spread for tapping curvilinearity. Since Eta :3 computed from an analysis of variance model, the constraint of sufficient ns is not as restricting -- i.e. for each value of a wuiable, a minimum number of reSpondents is needed in order to enable cabculating a measure of variance. By this logic, Eta is a :easure more directly comparable to the linear product moment unmelation which taps all values of each variable in its computations. The calculation of Etas for this study was done by computer. h1the process, each variable was standardized and then broken into muegories, each 1/u standard deviations in width. Purpose of this grocedrre is to enable the calculation of the one-way analysis of vafience from which Eta is derived. Ti atzent of non-reSponses for 'fie Eta analysis is like that for the product moment correlations -- amen values for each variable replaced all non-responses. Thus, the l - . The other Eta taps the accuracy of predicting in the opp031te ditxmion -- the accuracy with which X may be predicted by Y._ 64 n for each Eta was 252.1 In order to compare the predictive power of a linear versus a curvilinear model, the procedures used here was to square the r and the Eta for the relationship of each predictor to the criterion. The squaring process produces comparable figures -- percentage of variance accounted for in the criterion by the predictor. With such a comparison, the question must be asked: by how much must Eta exceed r before we reject the notion of linearity and accept the notion of curvilinearity? McNemar (1962) reports an analysis of variance method er the significance of the difference between product moment A 5‘) C) '5 d (D U) r f F: correlations and Eta correlation ratios, These tests have not been performed for the present study and comparisons are made on an intuitive I One note of caution must be added. To the extent that Eta is larger than r, we obtain an indication that a curvilinear model may better fit the data. However, Eta says nothing about the nature of the relationship. For the present study, gleanings about the nature of the relationships may be obtained from the contingency tables. Multiple correlation. One purpose is to use a multivariate 1Unlike the computation of r, the computation of Eta does require some collapsing of the variables involved. The question may be raised whether the Eta derived from slightly collapsed scores is directly comparable to the r derived from actual raw scores. (McNemar 1962) a specific check on this problem was made by comparing r's derived from raw scores with r's derived from the categories used in the computation of Eta. In all cases, the two r's were equal. 65 technique to determine how much variance the 62 predictors account for in the criterion variable. The multivariate method used here is the multiple linear correlation or multiple R. The reason fOr the choice of this particular method is, first, that it is one of few multivariate techniques available. Secondly, it is the most efficient in terms of ease of prediction for it assumes a linear model. While not all predictor variables will be related in a linear fashion to the criterion variable, the multiple R analysis delineates which variables predict ”best" assuming linearity. The loss, of course, is that some variables with a non-linear but high relationship to the criterion variable are necessarily deleted in the multiple R analysis. (McNemar 1962) . Several multiple R analyses were done for the present study. First, multiple correlations were run within each variable category (as outlined in Chapter I and II). This operation answered the question: which of the variables within each variable category accounts fbr most of the variance in amount of child television viewing? In the second phase, the best predictors from each category were analyzed- to determine which of the category predictors were "best" overall. The multiple correlation analyses were done by computer. The procedure involved first computing a multiple R using all the variables for a particular analysis as predictors of the criterion variable. The question of which variables are the "best" predictors was then answered by a least squares deletion routine. In this routine, predictor variables are deleted one by one according to which of the variables contributes CHAPTER IV RESULTS In presenting results, the following format is used. Results are presented first within variable categories. For each variable category, the discussion begins with a table presenting the relation- ships of all variables to the criterion variable. Results are then discussed in terms of those variables for which hypotheses were confirmed and those which were not. The report for eadh variable catesory ends with a comparison of the linear correlations. model g? to the curvilinear model and a report on the multiple correlation .I . .. . analySis within that category. The chapter ends With a report of the multivariate analysis between variable categories. ReSpondent_perceptions of parent media use Table 5 summarizes the results fer individual hypotheses on the relationship of parent media use variables to frequency of child. television viewing. The results indicate that of ten variables, four m re significantly related to frequency of child television viewing -- three in the direction predicted by hypotheses in Chapter II and one in the opposite direction., The variables which are not significantly related to child television viewing are: number of television sets, amount of parental radio listening, number of newspapers: dailies, number of newspapers: weeklies, number of magazines parents read, 67 66 the least to eXplaining variance in the criterion variable. The process allows variables to remain only if they account for a significant proportion of the variance in the criterion variable over that accounted for by the other predictor variables and the mean of the criterion variable. Every variable which does not contribute to explanation at p cowmfl>caop Hmucopma mo hpowmm> 73 mmm Hm mm mm mm ma coumo onaonOm mo.uv m ma.m Had “seam am am saw .maaa: m an mono .em>mz mcmsow> COMmm>maou Hepconma mo pesoe< a he ax : as a: a memsonuwzo mewsow> :Qwa>oaow Udaeo mo >UCmsvmnh mabmwnm> mouowomnm cownowwnu may 0% moanmwem> on: wanes unseen 029 MO mangprmHmn use new wonam> osssvmlwco ocm.moanm#_ho:oucflpcoo .b adamh 7w E a: on mm as a cane am am am am ea awn; sambmpmeoz mo ea mm mm .mw 20H sampmnmeoz 89V m 3.3 am "a 2 mm mm ram :3 mm: oflomn Hmpcmemm mo zvowmm> «mm oma nu ma am am embmo as an on Hm ea washbasom .m.: a am.e a: u: as am am sad manage an mono .eosmz meadowmwd Owens Howsmmmm mo pcsoe< men and mm mm mm om pew one can» 20: .m.: m mm. add": mm mm as an“ new coawa>mamb mco oaocomso: Qfi meow coflmw>mamp mo nonesz a we ax : mm a: a one: mlwco measow> cofluw>oaop tawzo mo %o:oswonm OHQW ..nrHfluxr ..HOPU MU gnu Umicwuazoo I... N. Gang h:m me mm mm as ea names on o>wd a: ma on ma mm 950m we Hm mm hm ma omega .w.: m m>.mH mm u: mm mm om rm“ 039 ow ocoz one,» we comma mocwwmmme mo Lobesz mmm mma mm mm mm mm 990% ow 0:0 .m.: m mm.m man: am. am Hm saw meoz .3 7. moflaxoos "0p cobweombsm whomwmmzo: mo nonesz mmw med um mm mm mm m>ww op 039 .m.c m mm. m:au: mm mm hm wan oco no 0:02 wowawme “ow cobwbombsm mememawsoc we nmbEsz a an «x z :m z a onmsomzwco mcflsow> coflmw>oao+ pawzo we homosdoem oaemfimm> mowofioenm Unscwwcoolxh vague .moabmp one mmonom wcwomom waoa pew mm coospmn Hmpop hoe .onomononp .ocm mmo omecsom coon o>mc wommpcoonom .30 .m ”we .mz was .42 mam .q "mam maagmw.mmmau an wao>ma wcwzow> mew mom m.: Enamcwz .mm .: mum .32 Maw .42 mom .4 one wcwzow> mo mao>oa mcwmnm> any mom m: .Aouwm ransom Eseflxmev .mmm u : Hmuoy cons .omyoamo ones moabmwmm> nouowooem new so momcoemmnncon omsmoon moanm> means“ one mom a Houoe .cmwn u : .zmfln mampmnmeoe u m: msoa maopmnmeoe u a: amnesmw> 30H u A .HHH ampameo cw ombflpomop mm meaflunmsw Opr oomamaaoo mm: oabmwem> cowmovwmo mean A 0mm am am mm am an names on wages mm :m :m an :N 039 .w.c w mh.w hm u: mm mm mm wnm . mco ow ocoz % @056 mcmmnwocozm mo Lonesz Hmw hb mm mm mm pH >9 Aoa.vv m :o.w :bau: Hm mm mm ram >9 cosy mozwo O m 0 c wSGG mo moonsom Hopcwpmm a we ax ii use as a oemsvmufico mewsow> newmw>®H0p UHHLO we mocmsvohm manmwpm> powowomgm UUZCflaccoilh mfladh 77 -espondent Media ‘se Table 8 summarizes the resu ts for individual hypotheses on the relationship of respondent media use variables to frequency of child television viewing. Table 8. Summary of results for individual hypotheses on the relation- ship of reSpondent media use variables to frequency of child television viewing. Variance Variable Pre- 2 acgounted fpr» diction X D r Eta r‘ Eta[_ Anemit of a radio use - an: .1251 .35 .01 .12 Variety of. R radio use - <.01 .25“1 .uo .06 .16 Etriety of R newspaper use - n.s. .ou .32 .00 .10 Number of magazines R reads - n.s. .05 .31 .00 .10 lariety of R book preferences + (305 .18a .41 .03 .17 Eipreference for comics + <,02 .213 .3” .04 .12 R library use , - n.s. -.08 .32 .01 .10 Frequency of R mov1e attendance + n.s. .173 .36 .03 .13 Variety of R record preferences + n.s. .ou .31 .00 .10 redia R would miss most + n.s. .15a .28 .02 .08 2V3 most believed media + n.s. .09 .33 .01 .ll Rls least believed media - n.s. -.08 .3“ .01 .12 5Significant at least at p.<.05. Critical values for r's, by r to z transformation test of significance, are: p.<.05 = .12; p‘<.01 = .15; p< .001 = .19. DWhile results for this variable are significant, they are in a direction opposite to that predicted. 78 km results indicate that of 12 variables, six are significantly related n>frequency of child television viewing. Four of these are significant mzboth the X2 and r analyses; two have non-significant X25 but significant rs. The variables which are not significantly related to we criterion variable by either statistical analysis are: variety of impendent newspaper use, number of magazines reSpondent reads, nepondent library use, variety of respondent record preferences, and :espondent media credibility ratings (most believe and least believed India). Table 10 (starting on page an) presents the contingency cross- meaks and chi-square values for each of the predictor variables in ads category. Details on the variables which are significantly related tithe criterion variable follow. amount of respondent radio use. The X2 for this variable is 25.;fnificant at p4 .01. The r of .12 is also significant (pC .05). Any lationship is positive with frequency of child television a ’3 (b fiewing rising significantly as amount of reSpondent radio use rises. Rule the results are significant, the hypothesis for this variable medicted a negative relationship between amount of reSpondent radio we and frequency of television viewing. This is the second radio use uniable (variety of parental radio use in the preceding section is the onmx0 which has related to frequency of child television viewing in adirection opposite to that predicted. A possible rationale for its counter finding is offered below in the discussion on variety of remxmdent radio use which also relates significantly to the criterion lfifi‘ble in a direction Opposite to that hypothesized. 79 Variety of respondent radio use. The X2 for this variable is significant at p*<.0l with the distribution of reSponses again showing a positive relationship. The r Of .25 is significant at pv(.001. In _general, then, as variety of reSpondent radio use rises there is a significant tendency for television usage to rise also. As with amount of reSpondent radio usage, the hypothesis for variety of radio usage suggested a negative relationship. In fact, the hypotheses fOr all radio usage predictor variables suggested a negative relationship between them and child viewing. Yet, three of the fOur radio usage variables in this study show a positive relationship -- variety of parental radio use, amount of reSpondent radio use, and variety Of reSpondent radio use. Amount of parental radio use was non-significantly related.to the criterion. An explanation of these counter findings is difficult to draw. As the review of past research in Chapter II indicated, early television studies consistently showed that television displaced radio usage -- i.e. consistently negative relationships were fOund between- the variables, absence vs presence of television and amount of radio usage. Variety of radio usage was, itself, seldom tested but the few available findings also suggested that variety became more restricted with the introduction of television. These findings, however, pertain mainly to the early introduction of television and the old- style dramatic radio. In the intervening years, radio has changed as a medium to the currently accepted music-news fOrmat. The most recently reported correlations Of children's radio use and television __——_. __~_..4 4~_—. 4 80 viewing have been non—significant (Parker 1960 and Troldahl and Costello 1966). One study (Bailyn 1959) found a small but significant negative correlation between amount of usage for both variables. In the context of these most recent findings, the results for the present study seem incongruous. Analysis of the contingency crossbreaks for the three radio use variables shows that very high and very low levels Of radio usage and radio variety seem to generally go with very high and very low levels Of television usage, reSpectively. This distribution of reSponses for the three variables may suggest a new kind of "all or none" principle applying only to electronic media usage. Variety of respondent book preferences. The X? for this variable is significant at p(.05. The distribution of responses indicates a generally linear trend with increased variety in book pre‘erences going with increased television usage. The significant (p<<.01) r of .18 supports this positive relationship. The hypothesis predicting a positive relationship between variety of reSpondent book preferences and frequency of child television viewing is confirmed. Respondent preference for comics. The X2 for this variable is also significant (p (.02) as is the r of .21 (p(.001). The hypothesis predicting a positive relationship between preference for comics and frequency Of Child television viewing is confirmed. 2 Frequency of respondent movie attendance. While the X for this relationship is not significant, the r of .17 is (p<:.01). The discrepancy between the two statistics is probably accounted for by the extensive collapsing required for the contingency analysis. While the crossbreak is not significant, the distribution of reSponses does show the hypothesized positive trend supported by the r -- i.e. higher levels of child viewing go with higher frequency of movie attendance. Nedia respondent would miss most.' Again, this is a variable for which the X2 is not significant but the r of .15 is (p 4,01). The distribution of reSponses is the crossbreak suggests the source of the positive correlation. While reSpondents indicating television as their most missed medium are slightly more likely to be heavier television viewers, respondents who indicate another medium as their most missed are slightly less likely to be heavier viewers. Given the significant r, the hypothesis predicting a positive relationship between reSpondent reports of television as their most missed medium nd H I”) f —requency of television viewing is considered confirmed. Comparison of linear and curvilinear models. The summary table 2 for this category of variables (Table 8) reports the r2 and Eta or variance accounted for in the criterion by the predictor with a linear versus curvilinear model. As with the parent media use category of variables, respondent media use variables account for more variance in the criterion with the curvilinear model. The discrepancy in variance accounted for ranges from a low of 6% to a high of 14%. While the linear r's account for from O to 6% of the variance in the criterion variable, the curvilinear Etas account for from 8 to 17% of the variance. 82 Eultiple linear correlation analysis. DeSpite indications that a curvilinear model better fit the present data, the multiple linear correlation analysis was applied to reSpondent media use variables. r1“ 5' ‘ ' Tile 1111(111". s below, then, apply to a linear prediction. Table 9 reports the results of the multiple R analysis within this variable category. Table 9. Results of multiple linear correlation analysis for the reSpondent media use variable category. Value of Multiple Correlation (R) Variables retained after_ ' least squares deletion With all 12 With variables variables retained after Variable r to DV included in least squares equation deletion .423 .39a Freougncv R movie .17b atten an'ce Amount R radio listening .12b Variety R radio b use .25 Media missed most .le Variety R book b preferences .18 aSignificant at p ( .0005. b The partial r's for these variables all approximately equal the r's. The largest discrepancy between an r and a partial r is .03. 83 Assuming a linear model, the 12 respondent_media use variables in combination, account for approximately 18% of the variance in frequency of child television viewing. At the end of the least squares deletion process, the resulting Multiple R equalled .39, accounting for 15% of the v riance in the dependent variable. The variables retained after the {U deletion process included: frequency of respondent movie attendance, amount of reSpondent radio use, variety of reSpondent radio use, media missed most, and variety of respondent book preferences. 84 ozm mm a: mm mm Ha awn: mm mm mm mm on mpmnaeoz .8.v a «as: man. a 3 . mm am new 33 mm: oflomn pcwocoamon mo mpowbm> mam - on ma a: ma ma see a mason m away one: om am mm on ad saw a mason m on a: on mm :H . m: we see m neon H on a: HotV m mm.mm on u s :m mm Hm wnom xmms m mason : cmcu wwoa .9o>mz a pcoocoamop mo pesos< a we ax : . as a: a onmsownflco mwcflsow> CmeH>oHep pawco mo homosvosm wannanm> novowoonm ma .oagmwam> sewnowwfio one ow modnmwho> om: oHUoE.uc0pcoameA mo chmcoflymaoh mzw hoe woSHo> m#ad@caw£o pcm moanew kocoucwwcoo .OH oanna :zm an em mm mm ma bgmnm op anon we mm mm mm ma means mm mm pm me am 029 .m.a m mo.s om u a mm mm em emu one on ocoz momma Homocommmfi mocmumwme mo amossz Hmm .3 8 am am om mm on awn: m: as am am me new; sambwnmeoz as am am mm mm god sambmnoeoz .m.: m mm.m me u : am mm mm mam zoo om: nomwamsm: pcoocommmn we muownw> a me «x : :2 a2 a oemzwmuflco measofl> cofimw>oaop paflzo mo zocosvonm ‘ essences paponemnm I 0::H#5001\OH OHde 51m no mm on ma mm copwo med bu om om . mm moefivmeom .w.: m ma.m mm u : mm :m 5H wow nm>oz om: mnmnnwa pcoocoomom 86 .HMMI :HH 3m mm Hm 5H moweoo mnemonm mo.v. m no.0H swan : ma mm mm whw mowsoo nomona p0: moon womsoo now monogamone pceocommom Hmw “mm. mm om mm ma awn: a: mm am am as awn; snobmnoeoz mm mm mm :H mm 20H sambmnoeoz mo.v. m m>.ma :m u c :H mm mm wmm . 304 woocobomond xoon pomocoamon mo xpownm> a me. «x x :2 oz 4 onmsvunflco msflsofl> CmeH>oHoe oaazo to monograph wannabe> nobowoosm C 0: C we swam! : OH O -... n1» .H. 87 owe mm mm :N am >9 .m.c m m:.m so a ma om om em“ >9 amen amnpo pmos mmwm UHDOZ pcoocoameh mace: Hmm as mm mm mm ma swam He mm Hm mm as awn: sambmnmeoz as am am Hm :a 30H sambmnmeoz .m.c m :m.m a: a as an an sea son moocosommnm onooon pceoCOmen mo meownm> .omm em on om an we genes m 00:0 swap who: as mm mm mm ad space m mono psog< .m.: a :s.o add u a ma mm mm new eoeamm .nm>mz mocwocopum mw>OE vcooeoomon mo >ocosomhm a ea mx 2 m: a: a obmsomafico mzwsom> commw>oaow SAHzo k0 hocozvonm oaoewnm> pepofloonm lli- C $35.. u. :00 I I OH OH. on; 88 .mmaamp ecu mmonom mcwomon .waoa one mm coozpmn kuou has .mnommnmcp .ocm mmo raccoon coon m>mn mmmmpeoonomb .mm .2 mom .:2 ”no .az mam .4 "ohm madame mama» an mam>ma wcfizmfi> on“ now we EDEHCw: .mo .2 who .:2 mew .42 mom .4 "ohm weflsmw> mo mam>ma wew>nm> may now we Amman mamemm ESEmeEv .mmm u a Hobo“ cos: .oopmamo one: mmHQMan> pouowponm ecu co newcommop use: enamomn wofinm> madam“ may now : Hmu09 .zmmn u : “swag zaopmnmooe u z: msoa mampmnmooa u A: mmnmzmw> 30H u a .HHH ampmwcu cw ownapowmp mm mmafiunmsc Opr roommaaoo was oanmwnm> :oflnmpflno mn9m - 0mm 9m rm ma :w mm >9 .m.: m mm.m mam" : mm mm mm wow >9 away nmcvo wwomE oo>omaon unwed ”mucwpwa >pflawnflomno warms pemocommmm 0mm mzd mm mm mm ma . >9 .m.c m 9:.H med u a mm mm on row >9 swap nonuo . mHomE om>ofiaon umoE “mmcwpmn >uflamnwpmno warms pcmocoamwm a he ax lms, rel a: a ommncmuflco msfl30w> cownfl>oaob peace we mocozwenh odomflbm> nowowoonm I‘lli‘l UUZCH#COOIXCH CHASE 89 Family Cohesiveness Table ll summarizes the results for individual hypotheses on the relationship of family cohesiveness variables to frequency of child television viewing. The results indicate that of 10 variables, none Table 11. Summary of results for individual hypotheses on the relation- ship of family cohesiveness variables to frequency of child television viewing. Variable Pre- é Variance diction X p r Eta accounted for 2 P Eta Parental permissiveness: Knowledge R whereabouts - n.s. .02 .30 .00 .09 Parental permissiveness: Restriction on hours - n.s. .06 .32 .00 .10 Child-parent communication - n.s. .ll .34 .01 .12 Family togetherness - n.s. .07 .26 .00 .07 Parent orientation - (.10 .11 .29 .01 .08 Parent decision making: Who pays bills + n.s. .06 .32 .00 .10 Parent decision making: Who pays allowance + n.s. .06 .26 .00 .07 R home responsibilities + n.s. .09 .32 .Ol .10 R knowledge family Operation + n.s. -.07 .31 .00 -.10 P-R agreement on TV programs + n.s. -.O2 .24 .00 .06 are significantly related to frequency of child television viewing. Table 12 (starting on pagesu.) presents the contingency crossbreaks and chi-square values for each of the 10 predictor variables in this category. 90 Comnarison of linear and curvilinear models. The summary table 2 2 or or this category of variables (Table ll) reports the r and Eta HI variance accounted for in the criterion by each predictor, using a 'near versus curvilinear model. As with the two preceding categories of variables, again we find that a curvilinear model seems to have a better fit with the data. The discrepancy in variance accounted for ranges from a low of 6% to a high of 11%. While the linear r's account for from 0 to 1% of the variance in the criterion, the curvilinear Etas account for from 6 to 12%. Hultiple linear correlation analysis. As would be expected fsom the non—significant results of the individual hypothesis tests for J (J Iables in this category, the multiple R within the category is not significant (R = .22). Assuming a linear model, the 10 family cohesiveness variables either alone in in combination show no pre- dictive power for the criterion variable. as am mm mm . an seem as om an Ha Ha emae sampmnmeoz an an mm an ad god sambmgmeoz .m.a m mm.ma 90 u a ma 9m Hm ram 304 sewpmowcseeoo pampmmlpawco mam mo 9m ma 9m om mmoco>fimwwenmm swam am on om as ea ewes sambmnmeoz om om mm om ma- 20H snobmnmeoz .m.: m mo.oa m: n a :H mm mm wmm mmmcm>wmmwenmm 30a meson co coauoflnumon mu Homeow>flmwwenmmtamucmnmm Hmm mm mm mm ma :m wmocm>wmmwssom mem .m. m as.a mod n a mm am am has“ mmmco>nmmweaoa so; musonmmnmcs m emooasonx "mwmcm>flmmwemma Hopcmnmm a we «x x :2 a: a oncsvauflcu mwcw30w> :onfl>mfioy pawzo mo mononwonu maowwnm> nobowoonm eanuHLm> COwLopflbo 02w ow medotwnm> neoco>flmozoo xdflSom mo awznc0wpodoh 0:9 new cases» eaczouawzo new comedy xoceuzwpcoo .mH oan9 92 arm am am nu on pH mbcmema zoom .m.: m no.“ mean a mm am am an“ panama mco waawo m>mo 0:3 mewxmfi sewwwomo pcosmm mam am am mm am am mecmnma seem 8a mm mm S cm £83 25 ea.v m mm.aa a: u a m an om mom panama nmebnmz 0 w 0 a COMHwMuFHmWHO chfimm mmm mm on mm on ma sea: on ma mm mm Hm awn: sampmhmeoz m: mm mm Hm ma assume :3 mm mm mm on sea >Hmpmnmooz .m.: ma mm.@ mm u a an mm Hm wan 30A mmmcnmnwomov >Hwemm a he ax : -2: a: a ontzwmxfizo mcwzmw> COwwfl>oaow ramco wo xcsosvohm ofinmwam> Loyowwmhm I‘ll Unscwncoozlmd Odao9 93 m:m mm am an Ad ad new: a: mm an em 5H new: saobmnoooz mm mm am am ma esnooz mm on mm as :m 30H sHobmnoeoz .m.a «a mm.~a o: u a mm mm mm mom monpnannnmcoomon zoo mowuwawnflmcoamop oEoc m.pcoocommmm cam .mmm om mm mm ma wbcoooa abom Add mm on am am econmo oco .m.c o no.0 :m u c mm Hm 9H wow bamboo socpwmz mocmsoaam m>oa 0:3 . mcwxms cofimflooo reopen a me x : .m: a: a Gnoscwxw;o (\J m:930w> coflmw>oace peace wo >0Coswonu canowbo> nobofloona C 03 Z ......w ECO! INH wuflnflwa. .mmaomp ocp mmObom mcflommn .waoa one mm cmospmo Hmpou mos .mnomonocv .ocm mmo raccoon coon o>w: mowopcoULOAA A .sm .x mam .mz mmm .az mam .4 "ohm moaomu omega aw mao>oa mcflsmw> one new we ESEHsz .mo .: m9m .mz mam .42 mom .q "one mcfisow> mo mam>md mafi>nm> ecu pom m.c .Aonww oaasmm EDEmeEV «mm H : Hmpov cmc:.omuoaop one; moabmfl9m> bouowooae co mowcommopuaoc omsmoon moflnw> moanmw may now : Hmp09 .cwmc u : ”cw“: >Hovmnoooe n ma ”30H >Howmnoooe u a: meoSmM> 30H u A .HHH nopnmzo cw omoflnommo wmmvafiunwsd oucw powdmaaoo mm: wanmwpm> cownmuwso 0:98 arm >3 mm am on ma new: o9 om mm an :m opmnooo: .m.: o m9.m mma u : mm an em WAN 30A moofiono Emnwona comwm>oaow co vcmEmonmw pHHLonpconcm u. 9 mmm em om mm an am can: a: am am om Hm some sambmnoooz as am am am an 20a saopmnoooz .m.c m mm.: m9 u a an :m mm wow so; cofipmnmeo >Hflsem mo ompmazocx ucoococcox a me x : :2 a: a ‘l “-CHRZCDI H "-1 L) I. mewzofl> coflmw>olob cawzo mo >ocoswonh oaomflnm> powofioong 'llll'li ooSprcooncma oenfi9 CO 553-11133 ITY IN TEGRAT ION 95 Table 13 summarizes the results for individual hypotheses on the relationship of community integration variables to frequency of czhild television viewing. The results indicate that of seven variables, Table 13. Summary of results for individual hypotheses on the relation- ship of community integration variables to frequency of child television viewing. Variance Variable 2 accounted for diction X p r Eta r2 Eta2 gregariousness n.s. -.Ol .31 .00 .10 R gregariousness: organizations 4 .02b .11 . 30 .01 .09 R gre ga.r iousness: peers n.s. .04 .30 .00 .09 Jute id e home orientation n.s. .03 .30 .00 .09 Len t1 of time in community n.s. -.07 .23 .00 .05 Fiequency of attendance at church (.10b .123 .34 .01 .12 R ..c lac e local 8 state figures (.10 -.04 .22 .00 .05 aSignificant at paoumnmpoz .m.: m NH.HH mm u a ma ma mm wxm 304 whoom "mmocmsofibmmobm peoUCOQmom new mm mm mm on oa swam mm mm mm ma mm opmaopoz mo.V o m9.0~ mm H a nu mm Hm won 304 m:0wpmxflcmmso “mmchSOHsmuonm poocCOQmom - mew 9m 9N Hm om mm gmw: :9 om on ma mm mumbooox .w.: o Ho.m «9 n a ma mm mm Awma 30a meccmsowbomenm Happen o to «x z . :2 a: a monasomuwco emcwsow> conw>ony camxo mo >oco5donh I vabmwnm> nepowwmnm magmwam> COMLOPMbo 0:9 09 noanmahm> :cHquwOPCw kHHCSESOU 9o awszOHHtaon 3:; boy me:am> Qacnwwnw;o Use woascu 9023::awcov .:H cand9 mmm mad mm am am am new: a: mm on am am oregano: Aoa.vv o Hm.aa me u a «a am am emu so; 0 m. a SUQDLU Pm. QOCmGEQPPm mo >ocosvmnh mam mm am am mm am whoa no NH mo am am am mm memos Ha . a mm mm mm am am _ mnmos o u a o .m.c o :o.m om u a mm mm om row memos m - o Mm >wfiCdEEoo ow oaww mo cpmcmq mam am am am am am swam as am am mm Hm awn; maoumnoeoz as am am om an god adobmnoooz .a.: m Hm.m om u a mu ea mm ram eon conmucoflpo . mac: crampso a no ax ;m| . :2 a: a obmscnxflco mewsofl> coaww>eaow peace we mocobvoam odomwnm> nobowoonm U mozcfiwnouuisd 0.93:. .moanmp esp mmonom wcfiommn waoa one am coozpmb HmPOp mos .obowmnocp .ocm mmo coocnon coon e>mc mommpcoonomn .oo .: Mme .22 new .az mam .a "one madame omens an mam>oa wowsow> esp new we ESEwcH: .mo .2 m9m .mz mew .42 mom .4 ”one wsflsmw> mo mao>oa mcwmnm> may new we .AmNflw ransom ESEmeEV «mm u a Hero“ crazy .oopmamp one: woaomfibm> bosowoona may no womcoamop Icon omsmomn weanm> mebMp may new we Hep09 .£m«; u I mama: >Hopmnooos n :2 mSOH maopmbocoe u a: mmnmzow> 30H u a .HHH nopamco ow coownomoo mm meaflunmnw Opcw oomamHHoo mos oabwwnm> sewsopwno mc9m omm .i mmm. an ma mm mm awn: m“ Hm Hm mm am ma cane snobmnoeoz m: mm ma ea mm assume a: an mm mm an god snobwnoeo: leauvv NH om.om mm u a mm mm ma and . zoo m c memmwmw owansa women one Hmooa mo earmasocx pcmocommmm o me x z :2 a: a ohmsoonwzo wowsow> cowhepwaop pawco mo mucosvobm mabmwbw> nOHUfioonm toncflpcoonzsd GHAM9 102 “-1-: h,“'f“"\'r‘\fil"lT.e-\\ “ v- ‘ c ,_ _I 0.414- UL\~AA.\‘AAJL..\/e\ Table 15 summarizes the results for individual hypotheses on the relation of self orientation variables to frequency of child television viewing. The results indicate that of eight variables, Table 15. Summary of results for individual hypotheses on the relation- ship of self orientation variables to frequency of child television viewing. Variance Variable Pre- 2 accounted for diction X p r Eta r2 Eta2 hunber R hobbies - n.s. -.05 .36 .00 .13 Number P hobbies - n.s. -.10 .25 .Ol .06 R.knowledge: ad slogans + n.s. -.08 .30 .Ol .09 Eiknowledge: TV characters + n.s. .10 .30 .01 .09 Eiknowledge: math - n.s. -.12a .23 .01 .05 Zloutside home employment - (.01b .19a .37 .04 .14 Either hours R studies - n.s. .01 .27 .00 .07 Frequency R studies in library - n.s. -.02 .30 .00 .09 hfignificant at least at p<.05, by r to z transformation test of significance. Critical values for r's are: p (.05 = .12; p<.Ol = .15;. p<.001 = .19. DWhile results of this variable are significant, they are in a direction opposite to that predicted. six do not reach the criterion level of significance of p.(.05 in .,_ , 2 . . Either tne X or r analyses. These SlX variables are: number of respondent hobbies, number of parent hobbies, respondent knowledge of 103 ad slogans, reSpondent knowledge of TV Characters, number of hours respondent studies, and frequency respondent studies in library. Table 17 (starting on page 107) presents the contingency crossbreaks and chi—square values for each of the 8 predictor variables in this category. Details on the two variables which reach significance follow. Respondent knowledge levels: math. While the X2 for this variable is not significant, the r of -.12 is at p (.05. If the con— tingency table for the variable is collapsed to a 4 x 2 (four levels of math knowledge x two levels -- light and heavy -- of viewing), the resulting X2 is significant at p< .05 (X2 = 9.34, df = 3). The distribution of reSponses in the crossbreak as well as the negative correlation concur that generally lower math knowledge levels go with higher television usage. The hypothesis predicting a negative relation- Lip between these two variables is confirmed. Respondent's outside home emplpyment. Both the X2 and the r of .19 for this variable are significant -- the X2 at P<-Ol: the r at p<§001. The direction of the relationship indicates that more frequent employment generally goes with more frequent television viewing. This finding runs counter to the hypothesis which suggested a negative relationship. Since none of the past television work cited here has looked Specifically at this variable it is difficult to place this counter- finding within a context. Child development literature (as noted in Chapter II) has suggested that the child who works outside his home 104 prediction was made. It is possible to conjecture, however, that outside home employment for a child goes with such attributes only up to a point. At some high level of outside work, the child may be too lausy to make peer contacts and is burdened with reSponsibilities so that television then becomes a solace. The distribution of reSponses in the crossbreak for this variable (Table 17) supports this kind of curvilinear relationship. Respondents who don't work at all seem to be higher vie*ers than respondents who work "when work is available" or occasionally. The reSpondent who has a steady job, however, is b “he most likely to be a heavy viewer. Comparison of linear and curvilinear models. The summary table for this category of variables (Table 15) reports both the r2 and Eta2 or variance accounted for in the criterion by the predictor, using a linear versus curvilinear model. Again we find that a urvilinear model fits the data better. The discrepancy in variance accounted for ranges from a low of 4% to a high of 12%. While the linear r's account for from O to 4% of the variance in the criterion, the curvilinear Etas account for from 5 to 13%. Multiple linear correlation analysis. Table 16 on-the next page reports the results of the multiple R analysis within this variable category. Tasle l5. Results of rultiple linear correlation analysis for the self orientation variable category Value of Kultiple Correlation (R) Variables retained after least squares deletion With all 8 With variables variables retained after included in least squares equation deletion Variable r to DV 29a a ' t ' . v .23 Child 8 outSide home employment .19 . Knowledge level: b aThe R of .28 is significant at p =(.009; the R of .23 is significant at p = :ofimfi>eaop passe mo zocosvoah manmwnm> nowowpopm I :owumbcowpc wave m0 .Qagmw9m> coflgmwwhu onv ow moancfigw> afizmcowpcacp 0:? new ecumw> vgmzcwlwzo was mudflmp zozom:M#:Oo .bd omaah 1mm 5 mm 3 so an :3: mm mm mm mm om zmwg mamumnmpox on mm mm mm mm 30H hampmhwpoz 6.: m 3.: a: u c 0: mm ma wad 33 gyms ”mam>md mmpoazocx envenommwm 0mm a: mm mm 8 S :3: .m.c m mo.: and" : mm mm mm wen 304 W mampomamco >e "mam>0H a. omnmazocx unoccommom 0mm mm mm om mm mm :3: oo mm om um Hm :wwn hampmnmvoz Hm mm ma on an 30H mamuwnowoz .w.: m Ho.m on H mm mm :m wsa 304 mammoam p< umHm>mH emphasocx pampcoammm - a we wx : E ..E a mcflzow> coflufl>oawp UHH;Q we >;:;:co&h wabmwnm> nouowomam opmscmumzu poscwwcoouuna oases :mm mm mm mm mm mm see m meson : . m\a N mm am mm am as see m meson m . N\H a o mm.o as u e an am am sad see m 9:0: a u o weepsum pampcoawwp mnsoz mo embasz 109 0mm H: :: mm ma NH AOm zemwum mmm mod mm ma mm mm magmawm>m ma x903 cog: mxno: Ho..V Aw mo.ha moa u a ma mm :m wmm x903 #0: when pcmE>oagEo @Eoc opwmwso m.pcmpcoawmm me x : :2 42 a MCH395> ceaaa>meow paazo %o mocozdoam canmAhw> nowadfimhm ohmsomxflzo . . . . . . It casemwzooxxsa magma llO .meAMp one weepom mempmmn waoa new mm cemsumb Hmpou >mE amnommnmzp .Uem mmo pmpcson amen w>mn mommpcwonmmn .sm .: law a: mom .az mm: .q "mam mmfinmu amaze an .mam>mfi mewzww> may now we ESEHCHE .mm “2 who .32 mxm .42 mom .4 "mam mcwzmfl> wo wam>ma mammnw> mnw pm we .Amnww mamemm ESwamEV mmm n : ampov cons .pwpoamp mum: moanmflnm> QOPOflpmnm map so newcommma 1:0: mmsmown wwwpm> mmabmp exp 90% : Hmpoe .zmfl: u I mnww: mamumnmpos u :2 mzoa hamuwhmpoe u a: mmnozmw> 30H u A .HHH ampmmzo ca omnwnomwp mm mmawwnmsv ova“ pummmaaoo mm: manmwnm> cownmvwno mesa 0mm «2 mm mm :H :m xmmz m coco cozy @902 mm :m mm Hm 5H xooz m moco mm :m mm mm mm xem: m mono cmzp wmoq .m.: m mm.m me u a mm mm mm mam . no>mz >am9bfia a“ mewpzpw ecopcomwmh >ocmswonm a me «x z . :2 a: a mhuzvmchv mcflzefl> :oflmfl>waop pafizo mo xososvonu mabmfipm> mouowwwnm poszwwcoozasd vanes lll . \?‘flv-‘fh*x p‘v‘Q-H-um. . ‘T . _ .g: .' a. n n H!" be. -‘uC'V.;lJ-\ L-\.L._a.\lfl ..Oh Table 18 summarizes the results for individual hypotheses on the relation of consumer orientation variables to frequency of child television viewing. The results indicate that of eight variables, four Table 18. Summary of results for individual hypotheses on the relation- ship of consumer orientation variables to frequency of ch 1d teleVision viewing. Variance Variable Pre- accounted for diction X2 p r Eta r2 Etaz L hutner of P money worries + (.05 .lua .33 .02 .11 Hurler oi R money worries + n.s. .ll .32 .Ol .10 P Spend-save orientation - n.s.-.13a .33 .02 .11 I spend-save orientation - n.s.-.04 .27 .00 .07 Fanily use of credit + n.s. .08 .26 .Ol .07 R attitude toward credit + n.s. .15a .27 .02 .07 Variety of R Spending + .<.05 .16a .32 .03 .10 Family shopping orientation — n.s.-.09 .25 .Ol .06 aSignificant at least at p<.05, by r to z transbrmation test of significance. Critical values for r's are: p(.05 = .12, p4.Ol = .15, p<.OOl = .19. do not reach the criterion level of significance of p(.05 in either the X2 or r analyses. These four variables are: number of reSpondent money worries, reSpondent Spend—save orientation, family use of credit, and family shopping orientation. Table 20 (starting on page 115) presents 112 4.1‘ 3 the contingency crossbreaks and chi-square values for each of the 8 redictor variables in this category. Details on the four variables ' {“1 which reach significance follow. . . 2 . . Number of parent money worries. Both the X for this variable and the r of .14 are significant at p(.05. The direction of the re- lationship is positive with more parental money worries being associated with more frequent child television viewing. The hypothesis predicting a positive relationship is confirmed. Parent spend-save orientation. While the X2 for this variable is not significant, the r of -.13 is significant at p<.05. The dis- crepancy between the two statistics is best accounted for by the extensive collapsihg necessary fer the contingency analysis. DeSpite the discrepancy, the contingency crossbreak does show a distribution of reSponses indicative of a negative relationship. There is a tendency for reSpondents who indicate their parents would "Spend" or "Spend and save” a windfall of money to be heavier television viewers. On the basis of the significant r, the hypothesis predicting a negative correlation between parental spend-save orientation and frequency of child television viewing is considered confirmed. Attitude toward credit. 'Again, the X2 for this variable is not significant but the r of .15 is at p(.Ol. Despite the lack of 2 significance for the X , the contingency table of responses does show a trend toward a positive relationship -- a more favorable attitude toward 113 credit goes with heavier use of television by reSpondents. Given the significant correlation, the hypothesis predicting a positive relation- ship between attitude toward credit and frequency of child television viewing is considered confirmed. Variety of reSpondent Spending. The X2 for this variable is significant at p<.05 and the r of .16 is significant at p(.Ol. The direction of the relationship is positive, as predicted, with greater variety of respondent Spending being associated with more frequent television viewing. The hypothesis for this variable is, therefore, confirmed. ' Comparison of linear and curvilinear models. The summary table for this tategory of variables (Table 18) reports both the r2 and 31a‘4 or 'ariance accounted for in the criterion by the predictor, using a linear versus curvilinear model. Again we find a curvilinear model fits better. The discrepancy in variance accounted for by the linear versus curvilinear models ranges from a low of 5% to a high of 9%. While the linear r's accounted for from 0 to 3% of the variance in the criterion, the curvilinear Etas account for from 6 to 11%. Multiple linear correlation analysis. Table 19 reports the results of the multiple R analysis within this variable category. Assuming a linear model, the 8 consumer orientation variables in combination account for about 8% of the variance in the criterion 114 Table 19. Results of multiple linear correlation analysis for the consumer orientation variable category. Value of Eultiple Correlation (R) Variables retained after least ' squares deletion with all 8 With variables varinbles retained after includes in least squares Variable r to DV equation deletion .28a .21a Variety of b respondent spending .16 Attitude toward credit .le I The p. of .23 is Significant at p =(.009; the R of .21 is significant at p =(.003. b ... :ne -art'al rs for he two vari '(J 1 ables are .16 (variety of respondent Spending) and .l# (attitude on credit). variable. Variables were then deleted from the multiple R equation by the least squares deletion criterion. At the end of the deletion gnecess, the resulting multiple R accounted for H% of the variance in 'ne dependent variable. The variables retained in the final R equation were variety of nxmondent Spending and reSpondent attitude on credit. 115 mrm ms mm mm mm mm m>sm mos am mm mm ma ssmm was ssmam .m.: o om.m mm u a mm mm am sad esmsm coflpmpoommo o>mmaocoem Hambwm mam as am mm mm ma Hmsm>om mod ma em Hm mm mso .m.: e ms.o as u : om mm ma new . msoz . moannoz zmcoe Homecoamob mo bones: mam was as am mu ma shoe so see mo.v m eo.m me. u a ma mm om new“ ssoz moflanos wocoe pcmnmm Mo ambasz s we mx 2 I: a: a onmswmnwzo mmCHSow> nommw>waow mawzo wo monoscmph cowwmpcoflbo noenmnoo manmfinm> Loyofiombm .oaboflbm> cownopwno one On moabeabm> m0 Lazmcoflpmaob egg Low ueDHm> obnzvnlwfio new moans“ %o:om:wpnoo .ow oases mrm am am mm mm ma saassosmn smH am am am sm mesossa m mm.s an n s an am am sea . seamsosmme: pwpmbo onmzou opswwpu< 116 ::m son mm mm mm om mmebmeom .saamsm: Hm mm mm mm :H no>oc meEa< o mo.m mm u c an :N 5w wmm no>oz “moose %0 mm: hamamu Nisw mmH mm mm mm mm . o>mm m: mm mm ma NH e>mm pom ocomm s os.m os u s as ma mm as“ esssm ooflpmpcmwno o>mw . :pcwow pcopcommom es ax ii inn as - a mewzow> newefi>0mop cameo mo xccosombu manownm> QOpUMpobm onmscmlwcq — Concepcoousom oabtfi .mmanmu may mmonom mnncmmn «waoa new mm embanmb annoy zma nonomonesu .pcm wwo popcson comb o>mn momensoonomn .mw .m Mme .:2 was .42 mam .4 "was moanmp women an mam>oa wnwzmw> any new m: ESEHcHz .mo .: who .:2 m:m .4: mom «4 “mnm mewzow> mo mao>ma mcwmnm> esp now me .Amwnm oaaemm Esenxmev mmw u a Hmvov nos: .pwnmamp one: moabmfinm> nononomnm esp co momcoammnuco: mmsmomn monnm> moanmp may now : amnoe .cmnz u : mama: maopmnoooe m2 msoa maonwnopos u a: mmnmzmw> 30H u a .HHH nmvmwzo an pmbnnomop mm moaflnnmzw oven pmmamaaoo mm: manwwnw> cownwnflno scam 33m hm ma mm mm mm ooflz mm mm - on an mm sens sampmnoeoz 7 so . mm mm as am senses snapssoeo: .1 ii .m.s a as.m as u c am mm :m sea smash sonssz cowpmvcowno wammonm mwnsmm he ms nu am as ma sen: mo.v m mo.m . ms u n :m mm :m wow . 204 mcwocomw . “concoamon mo >nownm> s we ax 11 gel as a mewson> cowmw>caon mango Lo xocosvonm sabofinm> nOHUHponm onwnonlwco pozcwnc001|cm canoe 118 D1:“'-’\~"" ‘ATZ‘LTTC “J.i‘l.\,‘t\n’|L $3.}. The final category of predictor variables in this study include the demographic characteristics of the child and his family. Table 21 s‘mmarizes the results of individual hypotheses on the relation of chamcgraphic variables to frequency of child television viewing. Table 21. Summary of results for individual hypotheses on the relation- ship of demographic variables to frequency of child television viewing. Variable Pre- Variance diction X2 p r Eta Accounted for P2 Eta2 3:: status: occupation - (.01 -.21a . 34 .ou .12 83 st: ts: possessions - (.05 -.O2 .28 .OO .08 Family size + n.s. -.09 .36 .01 .13 Fother's employment + n.s. .03 .35 .00 .12 Family type - n.s. -.06 .34 .00 .12 Birth order - n.s. -.07 .26 .OO .07 Sex none n.s. .14a .27 .02 .07 aSignificant at least at p{.05, by r to z transfermation test of . significance. Critical values of r's are: p(.05 = .12, p(,01 = .15; p{.001 = .19 The results indicate that of seven variables, four do not reach the criterion level of significance of p(.OS in either the X2 or correlation analyses. These four variables are: family size, mother's employment, family type, and birth order. Table 23 (starting on page 123) presents the contingency crossbreaks and chi-square values for each 119 of the seven predictor variables in this category. Details on the three variables which reach significance follow. . . . . 2 SOCio-economic status: occupational prestige. Both the X for this variable and the 4 of -.21 are significant at p<.Ol. As predicted, the direction of the relationship is negative with lower levels of status being associated with more frequent child viewing. The hypothesis predicting a negative relationship is confirmed. O I O O 2 . Boeio-economic status; posseSSions index. The X for this variable is significant at p(.05 but the r of -.02 is insignificant. Analyses of the contingency crossbreak for the variable suggests the reason for'the discrepancy between the two statistics. The dis- tribution of reSponses shows marked curvilinearity. Low levels of status seem to go with higher levels of viewing; moderate levels of status go with lower viewing; and high levels of status go with higher viewing. The hypothesis predicting a negative relationship between this measure of status and frequency of child television viewing is not confirmed. The review of literature in Chapter II provides some basis for explaining the discrepancy in relationship to the criterion variable between this possession index of socio—economic status and occupational prestige which shows a significant negative relationship. A possession index of status lOgically seems more like an income or ecological measure which past work has shown does not tap the same kind of status as occupational prestige. While the discrepancy in results between the two socio-economic status measures can be partially 120 explained, the reason for the curvilinear relationship of the possession index to frequency of child viewing is not clear at all. One might conjecture that reSpondents at the high possession level are more likely to have their own television sets and therefOre have more opportunity to view TV. Or, in line with the rationale offered for the consumer orientation category of variables, one might conjecture that a possession index of socio-economic status is a measure which confounds both a family's purely economic buying ability and a family's tendency toward satisfaction of immediate reward needs. This rationale would eXplain the higher viewing at higher possession levels. 63x: While the X2 is not significant, the distribution of responses in the contingency crossbreak supports the positive relationship suggested by the r of .14 (p nonowpmnm .mmm mm ma mm ma mm swam on ma om N: ha openepoz -S.v s 3.8 mm uc mm en am at: 33 omnnmonm HmCOflnmdbooo ”msnmum unaccoomnOHoom s we ax : ii: a: a mmcwsom> newwn>smon mango no >onosvonm nil mabmflnm> nononponm GHCCflLHQr :OHPHmva/HU 0:“. Cu. sombmflnm> UnzamnuOEot we enzxcofinmaon 0:9 non mozmo> onesonlflno t2o “canon kocmunwnceo .mm cane mom mm as mm mm snnamn ncmnms can mo.a as u c mm mm mm sen snnesn bashes mco waxy hawEmh 124 .mmm as an mm as manpunnsn misc: mm mm am am an . sann-nnms expo: sm.s can" c as am an as“ ago: no: mass msvmnm pcoemoamem m.no;noz .mmm Hm mm me an em meownos an . s as om mm mm mm meomnms m mn.m so u c mm mm mm awn . mcomnmc : u m mmnm zawemm ax : lei . a: a mcnzon> conen>o~in mango mo zocosdonm mabmwnm> novowwonm pmzcwnnoollmm vdbmh .mmabmn may mmonom monommn “waoa one am coosnob HmnOp zme wonomononp .pcm mmo popcoon comb o>m£ momencoonmmn .mm .: Moo .:2 mmo .az mam .a "one monoou women cw mao>od wcnzmn> may now we Ezeficwz .mm .m who .:2 mzm «a: mmm .4 "one mcwzow> mo mam>oa wnnmnm> one now we .Aownw wamemm ESEnmeV mmm u c annoy con: .oonoawo one: woaemnnm> nouonoonm can no momcoamon Icon omsmoon wowno> weapon may now a Hmnoe .nmn: u z mnmfln mamnmnopoe u :2 mzoa xaonmnmpoe u a: Munozow> 30H u A .HHH nonemzo on wobnnomop mm moannnmSW oncw pomnmaaoo mos oabmnnm> connopfino echo mom «on no as so on msom .m.: m os.m and u c on om mm . sow manna No. II 1i xom an“ as am am cm as shop been as om om an em anon onoenz .m.: o ma.s as u c mm mm mm no“ egos pmnnn nmeno npnnm a me x : z: a: a onzsomxwgo msnzuw> :Ownw>cflon Udfizo no >occsomnh sabeflno> nouowemnm Ilv Iii ll“ Tebcw930011mm Udbfla COX ARISON OF EULTIPLE LINEAR CORRELATIONS WITHIN CATEGORIES 'U Table 24 summarizes the results of the within category multiple linear correlation analyses in terms of variance accounted for by each category of variables. Table 24. Summary of within variable category multiple linear correlation analyses Variable category Variance accounted for By R with all variables By R.with variables within a category retained after least included in equation squares deletion Parent media use .14 .12 Respondent media use .18 .15 Family cohesiveness .05 .00 Community integration .03 .00 Self orientation .08 .05 Consumer orientation .08 .04 Demographic .06 .04 From the table above, we find that the two strong classes of pre- dictors are respondent media use and parent media use. Not as strong but still contributing some predictive power are variables in the self orientation, consumer orientation, and demographic categories. Two categories -- family cohesiveness and community integration -- contribute very little to variance accounted for. F.) l‘x.) \1 RESULTS OF MULTIPLE LIICE.";R CORRELATION ANALYSIS ACROSS VARIABLE CATEGORIES Two multiple R analyses were done across variable categories. The first took the ”best” predictors from each variable category and included them in one multiple R equation. The second multiple R was computed on all 62 predictor varuables regardless of category. Hultiple R of "best" within category'predictors. Each of the 12 variables retained from the within category multiple correlations was included in one multiple R equation. The resulting multiple R equals .50, accounting for 25% of the variance in the criterion variable. ITable 25 reports the results of this "best" predictor anllysis. Variables were then deleted from the equation by the Suares deletion criterion fully explained in Chapter III. At the end of the deletion process, the resulting multiple R equalled .48, accounting for 23% of the variance in the criterion variable. Of the original 12 "best" predictors, 8 remained. The feur which were deleted include: frequency of reSpondent radio listening, variety of respondent book preferences, reSpondent knowledge level: math, and variety of respondent Spending. The variables retained in the deletion process are listed in Table 25. .mooo.V a pm nnmonmwcwflmb .momzamcm mnowonmo :wcpns ecu cw pocwmnon moabmflnm> men Mo mnmna now mm pom .ma .wa .m .o modem“ mom .COMHmSGo oawcwm m oncw momzawom mnomwnwo oabmnnm> canvas men now mmmoona cemwoaoo monmsom nwmoa esp nonmo poowmnon moabmnnm> can Ham pom mnmxamcm m mamwuHsE weeks 128 ma.o Hm.n omwnmonm COnnmmsooo "wonmvm om .ma. ma. . unpono co opzpwpn< 2H. ma. pcoemoameo 050; opnwnso m.pango ma. ma. pmoe commas mace: ma. mm. om: owpmn m >pownm> on. ma. oocopconno ow>0E m mocoswonu om. am. we: enemn agenda anownm> ma. ma. mcfisow> >9 neonmm ucooe< mm. Ame. mm._ pom. mm m . . mm m cownoaoo connmsuo monsoon nmmoa nonmm on oopsaonm moanmwnm> n- >2 0n n one: .uscnmson moabmflnm> new: nonowoona :nmobz Ham new; one anon o eonescm newoa nonno cocnonon woabmnno> s Amv :0nnoaonnoo oaannasz mo osam> mownomondo dancers; STEM: .2an unOQUfioon; :Hmon: .37.... .....Cir.n.i.t..: :Cmumaonnoo nosing. oaawwabz uno baboon .nm muddled. 11 62 predictor variables. When all 62 v- r) y.) 0 7.) O "h Cu predictor var A bles, regardless of category, were included in one F J (J Multiple R equation, the resulting R equalled .65, accounting for 42% of the variance in the criterion variable. (See Table 26 on the next page). When variables were deleted by the least squares criterion described in Chapter III, 11 variables remained. The resulting multiple R equalled .54, accounting for 29% of the variance in the criterion variable. A comparison of this overall multiple R with the analysis of the ”best" within category predictors shows a great similarity between them. The differences between the two analyses are most apparent in terms of the size of the multiple R before the least squares deletion I fprocess. The 12 ”best" predictors from the within category analyses account ) '1 for 25% of the variance in the criterion while the entire set of 62 predictors regardless of category account for 42% of the variance. A-ter the deletion process, however, the "best" predictor R accounts of the variance while the "overall" R accounts for 23%. So, in terms of the least squares criterion, the predictive power resulting from either analysis is somewhat comparable. The other difference between the two analyses is the larger number of variables retained after the least Squares deletion in the overall analysis. For the I‘best" predictors analysis, 8,of the original 12 variables were retained. For the "overall" analysis, 11 i'.he original 62 variables were retained. Seven of the variables retained in both analysis are identical. These variables are 1 Sunscripted in Table 26 with a small "a." pom .moHnnos memos peonmm mo nonesn .QOHvoncoHno mnHamosw :: omonn mo oonns .mHmmHmcm nonunponm :nmob: onp CH poononon Hon ono: :w: pmHnombsm m Ann: poxnme poo moHannm> one .mHmmHocm mng cH non nob mHthoom pony oH oonHmuon mm; 1: wcHecoaw m.oHHno mo knoHno> In pooHno> one Amm oHeoe oomv .moHnowonoo pooHno> :« thaw: mnOHoHponm :pmob: one no mammHmoo m onHnHSE one now ooCHwnon onomHnm> m one mo 5 one moHoonno> omoasm mH.a HN.|0 . ocHnmona HmcoHnoooooo “msnmnm omo mH.u mo.u eoHyonooHnomtmmmonw mH. mH. unpono no opoannocoswonh mH. mH. nmoE oomeE oHoozm mH. mm. om: OHpmn m xnoHnm>o mH. NH. oms oHpon m posos< mw mH. SH. oocmooonpo oH>OE m mooosoonhm 1 pH. Hm. om: Onoon “condo monno>m :H. mH. mcHon> >9 peonom ncsoe< . . . . m mm 03m me One we x mm m cOHpoHov ooprovo n HmHnnoe >2 on n pooHnm> monsoon unmoH nopmm :H popsHocH mpomHnm> oosnoson moaooneo> sun: sononoono mo Ham can: coHnoHow monmzoo unmoH nopmm UoCHonon mpozflnm> . Amv cowumHonnoo oHaflstz mo ode> ll 1 mnemonoo oHnoHno> mo commenomcn epoeHno> nononeone nHe we mHthocm :oHnoHonnoo noocHH onHPHSE no pstom .mm pooa Mi- - he fact that both analyses retain seven ”common” variables suggests that the logical categorization of variables utilized for this study has some empirical merit. frequency of church attendance -- were not among the "best" predictors as determined by the within category multiple Rs. The other -- frequency of reSpondent radio use -- was among the "best" predictors but was deleted from the final "best predictors" R. bThis multiple R put all 62 predictor variables into a multiple R equation regardless of their variable categories. CSignificant at p (.0005. CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Summary Of the 62 predictor variables in this study, 21 were significantly related to the criterion variable -- frequency of child television viewing. These 21 variables, listed by variable category, were: Parent media use: The best1 predictors were amount of parental television viewing and variety of parental radio use. Other significant correlates were variety of parental television viewing and parental News sources. All four variables were positively related to the s . l . “asunncent media use: The best predictors were amount of respondent radio use, variety of respondent radio use, variety of reSpondent book preferences, frequency of respondent movie attendance, and media reSpondent would miss most. One additional variable -- reSpondent preference for comics -- was a significant correlate. All six variables were positively related to the criterion. Family cohesiveness: None of the variables in this category were significantly related to the criterion. Community integration: Significant correlates were respondent gregariousness: organizations and frequency of family church attendance. 1These variables are "best” predictors within categories as de— termined by the multiple R analyses. See later section of this summary for explanation. 132 Both variables were positively related to the criterion. This a. I" ”2 ' category had no ‘best predictors. . . . 2 . Self-orientation: The nest predictors were reSpondent knowledge level: math and reSpondent outside home employment. These two variables were also the only significant correlates in this variable category. The knowledge level variable was negatively related to the criterion; respondent outside home employment was positively related. 0 O 2 O 0 Consumer orientation: The best predictors were respondent attitude on credit and variety of respondent Spending. Other significant correlates were number of parental money worries and parent Spend-save orientation. Parent spend-save orientation was negatively related to ne criterion variable; the other three variables were positively 1 2 O I . emography: Tne best predictor was SOCio-economic status: occupational prestige. Other significant correlates were socio-economic status: possessions index and sex. The best predictor was negatively related to the criterion and the possessions index was curvilinearly related. On the third variable (sex), boys were significantly heavier viewers than girls. Two statistical measures were used to tap the relationship of each predictor to the criterion variable -- chi-squares and Pearson product moment correlations. Reasons for using both measures were detailed in Chapter II. The variables listed above are those that 2These variables are ”best” predictors within categories as de- termined by the multiple R analyses. See later section of this summary for €Xample . 134 were significant on one or both of the statistical analyses. The two rt analyses agreed that ll of he variables were significantly related to the criterion. Of the remaining 10 variables, 3 were significant only in he chi—square analyses and 7 were significant only in the r analyses. Discrepancies between the two analyses were for the most part small. Thus, if a variable was significantly related in one analysis, the trend was clearly evident in the other analysis. The reasons for the discrepancies between the two analyses were: 1) extensive collapsing, particularly on the criterion variable, for the chi-square lyse , depressing the chi-squares; 2) restriction of range and OJ {J in 0) skewed distributions on predictor variables, depressing the rs; and 3) curvilinear relationships between predictors and the criterion, depressing the rs. In Chapter II, 51 different hypotheses were formulated. The results confirmed 13 of these and showed significant findings in a direction Opposite to that predicted for 6 hypotheses. The variables involved in the 6 counter-findings were: 1) variety of parental radio use; 2) amount of reSpondent radio use; 3) variety of reSpondent radio use; u) reSpondent gregariousness: organizations; 5) frequency of family attendance at church; and 6) respondent outside home employment. For all six, hypotheses predicted negative relationships to the criterion variable while findings indicated positive relationships. The hy- pothesis for one additional variable -- socio-economic status: possessions index -- predicted a negative relationship while findings indicated a :significant curvilinear relationship. O :J C) O rt :3 (L3 '3 'U a O (I) O O l”h r f .7 H 0') U] tudy was to analyze the nature of the relationships existing between the predictor variables and the criterion. For this, correlation ratios or Etas were computed. Variance accounted ‘ or in the criterion by tne ‘ta curvilinear correlation was compared to ['1 ccounted for by the linear r. While significance tests 4 m ’5 it , g, 0 (D m were not computed to test the significance of the difference in variance accounted for, inSpection of the results shows clearly the Etas account for more variance than the rs. While the linear rs accounted for from O to 9% of the variance in the criterion, the curvilinear Etas accounted for from 5 to 18% of the variance. The discrepancy in variance accounted for by the two meaSures ranged from H% to 14%. As tcrfiative conclusion, then, the predictor variables in this study (I) explain more of the variance in the criterion variable with a curvilinear model than a linear model. Despite the indications of curvilinearity noted above, one purpose of the present study was to do a multivariate prediction of frequency of child television viewing. Since the easiest multivariate method available is the linear multiple regression, multiple Rs were run . both within and.between variable categories. Results showed that the respondent media use category of variables accounted for the most variance in the criterion (18% with an R of .42). Parent media use variables accounted for the second greatest amount of variance (14% with an R of .37), followed by self-orientation variables and consumer for 8% of the variance with Rs of firs:r:a"*-fb: '5 r-av-n.-"*‘v q) -r~."‘ - ”5+!“ \y-—VL‘£C—;Coa ‘a--C.-J-eu ecbdd QCCOL‘L-‘& UQ .28). Demographic variables accounted for 6% of the variance (with an AA (JO R of .25). The multiple Rs for both family cohesiveness and communitv orientation variables were not significant. When the least squares deletion criterion was applied to each of the multiple Rs within categories, the variance accounted for was reduced a maximum of 49. Of the 21 variables significantly related to the criterion by the X2 and r analyses, 12 remained as "best" predictors within categories after the least squares deletion process. These 12 variables were then included in one multiple R equation. The resulting multiple R equalled .50, accounting for 25% of the variance in the criterion. The least squares criterion was also applied to this multiple R. After the deletion process, the resulting multiple R I equalled .us, accounting for 23% of the variance in the criterion. Of the 12 ”best" predictor variables included in the equation, 8 remained after the deletion process. These variables were: 1) amount of parental television viewing; 2) variety of parental radio use; 3) variety of reSpondent radio use; u) frequency of respondent movie attendance; - 5) media respondent misses most; 6) respondent outside home employment; 7) reSpondent attitude toward credit; and 8) socio-economic status: occupational prestige. Conclusions The four purposes of this study were: 1) to replicate much of the past work on correlates of frequency of child television viewing; 2) to go beyond a replication by including some variables which have been partly or wholly overlooked in past work; 3) to extend the analysis to a multivariate method; and 4) to Specifically look at the types of 137 ’2 J p: 0» H ,4 O S 0 "Lips which exist between the criterion variable and its this study will be discussed in terms of these four ptxrpcses. First, in terms of the replication function, results here semen much like results from the host of prior studies done in the area. at the collective results of the many television—children 'es, several researchers (e.g. Meyersohn 1957, Schramm 1961, Himmelweit =958) termed the overall picture somewhat confusing and inconclusive. Considering the impact that television is popularly thought to have, researcn re sults generally show relatively few significant relationships V Jeen predictor variables and television use. The present study I .3ea:s to be no exception. Of 62 predictor variables, 21 are significantly And, as has been the case in past work, the vari”nce explained by any one predictor is small. Given the best offered by the curvilinear correlation ratio, Eta) the most variance any one predictor variable accounts for in the criterion is 18%. Since the size of an Eta only indicates variance potentially explainable and says nothing of the nature of the relation- ship, knowing that 18% of the variance in the criterion may be accounted for by a predictor is not a great deal of information. Some complex , of curve-fitting operation would be needed to fully use this pre- dictive power. With 82% of the variance still unaccounted for, a complex curve fitting procedure doesn't seem to have merit. Given a linear prediction model, the situation is even worse. The best single predictor accounts for only 9% of the variance in the criterion by the ranalysis. 138 Such results really are logical, if not encouraging statistically. Television viewing is obviously a complex behavior and one that can only be viewed within the context of the respondent's total life situation. As researcher after researcher has noted, complex behaviors do not have simple or single causes (e.g. Schramm 1961). One should not expect that any one variable will account for an overwhelming amount of variance in television behavior. If the problem were just one of low predictive power, the :ntuation might be clearer than it is. But, as one looks at past :findings, several confounding trends emerge. First, there are a great 0 many contradictory findings in the literature. “A A review of the ~ ‘ O O hypotheses stated in Chapter II shows that only one-third of the 51 otheses were based on clear-cut evidence -- e.g. evidence (J) -~ 13 ‘* v“ tatc \A .11“. k) agreed on the direction a variable would relate to frequency - of child television viewing. For another third of the variables, evidence was contradictory with past work showing a complex of signif'cant and non-significant findings and, in some cases, significant indings in two different directions. For another third of the H1 hypotheses, evidence from past research was Sparse or not available. he present study does little to clarify the total picture. For example, only Six of the 21 significant results in the present study agree completely with past work. Thus, this study found these variables related to child television viewing in the same direction as ts from prior studies: amount of parental television viewing, ”1’3 _ ‘93 D (a U) 'd O I) (A. 0 :3 d ’O '5 (I) "h (D t a :3 0 0 F1») 0 *i O O 53 p. O 0') *3 G) 0) ’U 0 :3 Li. (D :3 d X. :3 O 2 [..J (D Q; m (D ...: 1:" (D l-‘ (n 5 w d- {3" U eaconomic status: occupational prestige. .1 .) ile past work agreed that such variables as parental per- :nissiveness, family togetherness, respondent library use, reSpondent tiobbies, and reSpondent knowledge levels Should be significantly related ‘to amount of child television viewing, the present results were all Iion—significant. Clouding the picture even more are the contradictory ‘results obtained in the present study. Most notable of these are the tiesults for radio uSe variables. The present results Show amount and variety cf respondent radio use and variety of parent radio use v related to frequency of child viewing. Yet, past work points to either negative or (according to the most recent findings) non-significant relationships. The current study also finds reSpondent _ gregariousness: organizations, and family church attendance positively related to the criterion while past work suggests either negative or non-significant relationships. Another trend from the past research is the emergence in recent studies of non-Significant relationships for variables which in earlier studies were reasonably good correlates of television usage.‘ As an example, we find that recent studies agree that the relationship among various aspects of media usage are tending to be non—significant (Edelstein 1966). Results from the present study generally agree. In addition, the present study showed nothing but non-Significant relationships for two major classes of variables -- family cohesiveness 1. community integration -— which had, in the past, been somewhat fruit- .9.) '1'] ’4 ’1'! predictors 0 television viewing. Of course, confounding all predictions is the problem of the leap frxxn he many television versus no television studies to correlates of anxount of television viewing. Relatively few findings are available trust eXplicitly looked at correlates of amount of viewing. Even firmer findings are available from data collected in the mid-sixties witji'television at almost 100 percent saturation. The probable impact of the changing media environment is seen znost clearly in terms of a review of the five generalizations derived frcn past reseirch (outlined in Chapter I). As a brief review, these rive generalizations were: l. :he parental imitation generalization: children tend to do rt‘ what heir parents do, all other things being equal. 2. The demographic attributes of the child generalization: certain demographic attributes of the reSpondent predict his television usage. 3. The functional displacement generalization: the child will sacrifice in lieu of television those activities which satisfy the same needs as television but do so less effectively. 4. The frustration generalization: the more frustrated a child is, the more time he will Spend in front of his television set. 5. The information void generalization: television is likely to have an effect on a child's values and outlooks if a) the values and views recur over and over again in TV content; b) the content is linked to the child's needs and interests; c) the form of presentation is 11+]. tiranatic; and d) the child is not presented through peers or relatives Iiith a standard against which to assess the views offered on television. nesults from this study generally support the parental imitation ggeneralization with amount of parental television viewing and parental :socio-economic being two of th ”best” predictors. In this reSpect, the present findings are in agreement with the major studies of the Ipast (e.g. Schramm 1961, Bailyn 1959, Himmelweit 1958). While generalization #2 —- the demographic attributes of the child generalization -- applies more to television usage changes through- che childhood years, the present study offers some support. The V one variable closest to a measure of intelligence —- reSpondent knowledge‘level: math -- is one of the strong predictors, in agreement witn past work (Himmelweit 1958, Bailyn 1959). In addition, sex was significantly related to viewing in the current study while all past work found non-significant relationships. The two generalizations which are least supported by the current study are the functional displacement and frustration generalizations.' While the functional displacement generalization should predict results for most of the reSpondent media use variables, as well as some of the H) anily cohesiveness, community integration, and self orientation variables, we find little support for the generalization in the present study. Past work would suggest that use of such media as radio would be displaced by increased television usage. The present study finds the Opposite -- a positive relationship. Past work would suggest that family activity levels and family use of outside home entertainment Should.be displaced by increased television usage. The present study inds non-significant relationships for these variables. These results bring the whole question of what is "functional” to “fine fore. It appears as if activities which once were "functional" equivalents to television usage may not be any longer. This is most clear with radio usage since radio itself has undergone major changes as a medium since the introduction of tievision. Within the teenage culture in which pepular music is a major focus of concern, there seems no reason to expect that amount of radio use would be diSplaced by increasing television q ularly since popular music is the only radio content for t u 'L‘} L: 9 rt ’4 O "Lny stations. various researchers (e.g. Himmelweit 1958, Katz and have warned that inferences about "functional equivalents" can not be safely made without some empirical support. What may be functionally equivalent for one group of reSpondents may not be for another. The frustration generalization receives even less support from the present study. As a predictive tool, it should apply to most of the variables measuring family cohesiveness and community integration in this study. Yet, the results show that not a single family cohesiveness variable is significantly related to the criterion. Further, the two significant community integration variables -- respondent gregariousness: O rganizations, and frequency of family church attendance -- are related positively to frequency of child television viewing while the frustration generalization predicts a negative relationship. The present results for these two classes of variables are not geatly different from those in past research. In general, variables nithese two classes have not yielded a great deal of n..- - I u .1 ..‘ '- a ... [.1 .r: (0 predictive power and have resulted in conflicting findings.l ticularly in the most recent work, we find warnings that the very behaviors may be indicative of both frustration and lack of fmstration. Katz and Foulkes (l962) emphasize the point. The child up is in conflict with his parents may escape to television; the child zip has a harmonious relationship with his parents may watch more nflevision in order to be with his parents. The child with few friends my escape to tievision; the child with many friends may use television {u ssa means of relating to them. As with the diSplacement generalization, itseems erroneous to make inferences about underlying reasons for vaious television'behaviors just from the behaviors themselves. Interestingly, the relationships to the criterion of some of the uniables in the consumer orientation category may also be predicted inaxtne =rustration hypothesis. For example, to the extent that the rapcndent sees his parents as worried about money problems, the fnmtration hypothesis would suggest television viewing would increase. he problem of whether level of parent money worries is an indicant of fmstration still remains. Consumer variables, in general, turned out tdbe among the better predictors in the present study. An equally gum.rationale based on deferred vs immediate gratification patterns may Jacrrived to explain results. Again, some sort of explicit check of ‘h— 1 LvVO nimmelweit (1962) noted the lack of order in results from studies taxing the relationship of various family variables to frequency of dfildtelevision viewing. She suggested that television may act as a anahst showing up the characteristic mode of relationships within the Ififily. She concluded, however, that the unclear results in the area Stanin part from lack of adequate measures of family variables. Currently mednmasures seem too superficial to be related to the "core of family life.“ inferences about underlying behaviors is called for. Tne final ”information void" generalization is really tangentially related to the present study. The one variable which seems most clearly related to it -- reSpondent attitudes on credit -- was significantly -elated to the criterion variable in the positive direction predicted. Again, however, any inference that the reSpondent's attitude toward credit is related to an "information void" on credit in his family or peer group might be erroneous. Attitudes toward credit may simply be another indicant of a basic immediate vs deferred gratification pattern. In sum, the 'iscussion above suggests a major theoretical obstacle nets to explain and predict frequency of child usage. Support for the :encralibations seems hampered by lack of clarity on the "meaning" cf television viewing to the reSpondent. More will be said about the question of the ”meaning" of television at the end of this chapter. Despite the confusion indicated by the individual hypothesis tests, one encouraging consequent of the present study was the results on some of the "new" variables which were added to it in addition to the variables included mainly for replication purposes. All the xeriables in the "consumer orientation" category were "new". Four of them were among the 21 variables significantly related to the criterion and one was among the "best" predictors from the overall multiple regression analysis. Another "new" variable which turned up among the strong predictors in this study was reSpondent outside home employment. These results suggest fruitful areas of eXpansion. The consumer dimension of the family’s behavior, in particular, seems a lucrative area of focus because 1145 1. t H) 0 *h .e o sible relationship 0 immediate vs deferred gratification ' C} m 'U atterns to both consumption behaviors and media behaviors. Another encouraging result of this study was the fact that the :nultivariate analysis isolated in its group of "strong predictors" the two predictor variables most often found in past work -- amount of parent television viewing and family socio-economic status (occupational prestige). To this extent, then, the present analysis agrees with past work. 3-1 n general, of course, the "strong" predictors in the multiple R analyses a:e those predictors which were most highly related to the ion variable in the individual hypothesis tests. Thus, comments I made earlier on the results to the individual hypothesis tests apply Another encouraging aspect of the multiple R analyses is the amount of variance that can be accounted for in the criterion given the restrictions of a linear model. The eight "best" predictors account for 23% of the variance. This cerainly is not overwhelming but is a sizable increase over the 9% accounted for by the one single variable with the highest correlation (r) to the criterion. In terms of full explanation of television viewing, however, the restriction of a linear model appears to be a serious one. The analyses of the Eta curvilinear correlation ratios showed that the relationships between the predictors and the criterion are accounted for more fully by a curvilinear than a linear function. As suggested earlier, however, the amount of variance accounted for even with a curvilinear function is; at no point remarkable. The best variable accounts fer 18% of the *vardence in terms of Eta. Thus, any extended foray into plotting curves for~the relationships of each predictor to the criterion is certainly not suggested. However, these results do suggest potential for the use of a uniltivariate technique that makes no assumptions about the nature of the functional relationship between predictors and the criterion. The size of the Etas obtained suggests that no one variableoffers great 3predictive power but that some complex combination of predictors might. ssentially, this is the kind of question which the multiple R analysis aspts to answer. But, the multiple R assumes linear functions and what is needed is a multivariate technique that makes no such assumption. Tne possible technique would be a configurational approach, such as the Automatic Interaction Detection (AID) method developed.by Sonquist and Morgan (1964). All the discussion above has suggested different analytic techniques and inclusion of new variables in the attempt to explain and predict the frequency of a child's television viewing. Throughout the discussion an underlying difficulty has been the problem of what ’generalizations or inferences may be made about the results of a correlational analysis of frequency of child viewing. The results of this study as well as the more recent discussions by researchers in the area suggest that inferences about the "meaning" of television viewing need to be explicitly checked.‘ As noted earlier, two of the more often cited generalizations on television behavior 147 require the researcher to make inferences about frustration states in respondents and about functionally equivalent activities. These 'nferences require that assumptions be made about the "meaning" of }_l (’1’ elevision as an activity to reSpondents. Is television an escape from real world frustration, such as having too few friends? Is television a means of entertaining friends? Is television a substitute for missing communication with parents? Or, is television a means of sharing an activity with parents? A major difficulty seems to be that, in today's ubiquitous television environment, the very same "end" behavior -- frequency of television viewing -- may have very different meanings for different respondents. This author does not intend to suggest that frequency is not an important or relevant variable. Rather, looking at television viewing as an end unto itself seems incongruous when one considers that television is one artifact in an environment filled with artifacts. Television usage might more lOgically be seen as a type of intervening variable in the reSpondent's life. This idea is certainly not new as such researchers as Meyersohn (1957), Himmelweit (1962), Bauer (196u), Schramm (1961), and Troldahl (1965) have all called for an explicit attempt to look at the "meaning" of television exPosure. This study seems to make the need even more pressing. The question would become more complex than what are the correlates of amount of television viewing. Rather, the question would become what are the correlates of various needs of the reSpondent and how does television (as well as other environmental artifacts) intervene in these relationships. 148 This approach would require that previously made assumptions about th (I) nderlying reasons for behaviors be checked. For example, it would not be assumed that reduced child-parent communication leads to frustration on the part of the child. Either some measure of anxiety would be used or reSpondent introspections would be collected. These data would allow the develOpment of various typologies of the relation- ship of child-parent communication to anxiety. Television usage would then be analyzed within the context of these typologies. As another example: varying levels of child gregariousness could be related to varying levels of reSpondent reports of need for affiliation. Television e could then be seen within the context of the relationship of actual ariouénass to need for affiliation. Once these relationships have been established between various reSpondent and family attributes and resulting needs or states, television usage may be analyzed as it "intervenes" in these relation- ships. Frequency of television viewing would still be a meaningful measure. However, given that the very same end behavior -- frequency of viewing - may serve different functions-for different respondents, an attempt to tap the "meaning" of television in the child's life would need to go beyond pure quantity measures. Other possible measures include: 1) Respondent intrOSpections on why television is wanted. This ap_roadh -- simply asking reSpondents why they like television in general and why they like certain shows —- has been suggested by Schramm (1961). Troldahl (1965), and others. 149 2) Quality of espondent viewing. Some work has been done andlyzin*r the content of child viewing. Such a content orientation would be crucial to understanding how the same amount of exposure may serve different purposes for different respondents. 3) Respondent perceptions of TV content. Not only might the same amount of exposure have different meaning for reSpondents but the same content of exposure might have different meaning. Various researchers (e.g. Schramm 1961, Bailyn 1959) have suggested that two different children viewing the same program will select content which suits their own particular needs. important aspect of the search for the "meaning" of television E; would be 118 emphasis on the medium as one of many artifacts in the environment. As Katz and Foulkes (1962) have pointed out, if a child has a need to escape a certain kind of frustration, there are many alternative routes for doing so. Analyzing television as one of many intervening artifacts or activities in the childs life seems like one way in which the problem of "functional equivalency" can be attacked. To the extent that different media continually serve as "intervening variables" in the same way, they may well be "functionally equivalent." Before concluding this chapter, a few cautions must be applied. Conclusions based on the present study face several serious limitations. The most constraining are: l) the uncertainty of just how the questionnaire was administered to the reSpondents; 2) the form of measurement of *mndables in the original questionnaire; and 3) the fact that reSpondents mere all students in one rather homogeneous suburban school system. “‘1'! *U In conclusion, the major directions suggested for future research ave included: :34 discussed in this chapter 1. The need to include classes of variables (e.g. consumer behavior) previously ignored in attempts to predict child television viewing; 2. The need to use multivariate techniques to predict television 3. The need to use statistical techniques which make no assumptions about the nature of the functional relationships between predictors and the criterion; Q a. [he need to look at television viewing as an "intervening behavi r” whibh mediates various respondent and family attributes and -eSpondent needs; and 5. The need to look at aspects of television usage otherithan quantity -- content of exPosure, reSpondent perceptions of content, and respondent introspections on why they watch television. BIBLIOGRAPHY Abrams, Hark. ”Child Audiences for Television in Great Britain," Journalism Quarterly, 33, 1 (Winter l956), pp. 35-u1, Bailyn, Lotte. "Kass Media and Children: A Study of Exposure Habits and COgnitive Effects," Psychological MonOgraphs, 73, l (1959), Whole No. 471. "P“ Bauer, Raymond A. The Obstinate Audience: The Influence Process from the Point of View of Social Communication," American PsychOIOgist, 'lliam S. "The Mass Media and Young PeOple," Journal of .adcasting, 51, 1 (Winter 1960-61), pp. #9-58. Foundation, 196%, I, pp. 169-208. Belson, William A. "The Effects of Television on the Reading and the Buying of Newspapers and Magazines," Public Opinion Quarterly, 25, 3 (Fall 1961), pp. 366—381. "Television and Family Life," Advancement of Science, XVI, 54 (1960), pp. ans-353. . ”The Effects of Television upon Family Life," Discovery, XXI, l0 (1960), reprinted by the Research Techniques Division of London School of Economics and Political Sciences, Reprint #127. . ”Effects of Television on the Interests and Initiative of Adult Viewers in Greater London," British Journal of Psychology, 50 (May 1959). pp. 145—158. Bogart, Leo. "American Television: A Brief Survey of Findings," Journal of Social Issues, 18, 2 (March 1962), pp. 36-42. . The Age of Television. New York: Frederick Ungar, 1958. _ Cambell, W.J. Television and the Australian Adolescent. Sydney, A'stralia: Angus and Robertson, 1962. Caplovitz, David. The Poor Pay More. New York: The Free Press, 1963. Quter,1tichard F. and Bradley S. Greenberg. "Newspapers or Television: [dutch do you Believe?" Journalism Quarterly, 42 (Winter 1965), lSl 152 Clarke, Peter. ”Parental Socialization vehes and Children's Newspaper Reading,” Journalism Quarterly, 42, H (Autumn 1965), pp. 539—546. Clausen, J hn A. ”Family Struct are, Socialization, and Personality, " o n Lois liladis .offman and Ifartin L. Hoffman, ed., Review of Develonm ent .-seuroh New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 966, II, pp. 1-53. ‘ Coffin, Thomas E. ”Television's Impact on Society," American Psychologist, 10 (1955), pp. 630-6ul. Coleman, James S. The Adolescent Society. New York: The Free Press, l96l. Douvan, E. and Adelson, J. The Adolescent Experience.' New York: Wiley, 1966. and} Iartin Gold. "Modal Patterns in American Adolescence," i. Lois Eladis Hoffman and Martin L. Hoffman, ed., Review of Child Development Research. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1966, II, pp. #69- 528. n, Alex. PerSpectives in Mass Communication. Kobenhavn: Liner har cks Forlag, 1966. Eine, B.J. and N. Maccoby. Television and Family Life (mimeo). Boston: Boston University School of Public Relations and Communication, 1952. Freedman, Lawrence Zelic. "Daydream in a Vacuum Tube: A Psychiatrist's Comment on the Effects of Television," in Wilbur Schramm, Jack Lyle, and Edwin B. Parker, Television in the Lives of Our Children. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1961, pp. 189-194. Eteiw‘ on, Eliot. "The Relation of the Social Situation of Contact to the Media in Mass Communication," Public Opinion Quarterly, 17 (1953), pp. 230-238. . "Communications Research and the Concept of the Mass," in Wilbur Schramm, ed., The Process and Effects of Mass Communication. erana: University of Illinois Press, 1955, pp. 380-388. (biger, Kent and Robert Sokol. "Social Norms in Television Watching," American Journal of Sociology, 65 (1959), pp. 174-181. Hafilton, Robert V. and Richard H. Lawless. "Television within the Social Matrix," Public Opinion Quarterly, XX, 2 (Summer 1956), pp. 393-404. 153 innolweit, Hilde T. “A The retical Framework for the Consideration of the Effects of Television: A British Report," Journal of Social Issues, 18, 2 (IMa ch 1962). pp. 16- 28. , A.N. Oppenheim, and Pamela Vince. Television and the Child. London: Oxford University Press, 1958. Huber, Milton J. A Study of Financially Over-extended Families (mimeo). Detroit: Merrill-Palmer Institute, 1965. Kahl, Joseph A. and James A. Davis. "A Comparison of Indexes of Socio- Economic Status," American Sociological Review, 20, 3 (June 1955), pp. 317- 325. Eatz, Elihu and David Foulkes. "On the Use of the Mass Media as 'Escape': Clarification of a Concept," Public Opinion Quarterly, XXVI, 3 (Fall 1962), pp. 377-388. d oper, Joseph T. The Effects of Mass Communication. New York: The Free Press, 1960. :;.:arofeld, Laul F. "Some Reflections on Past and Future Research on Broadcasting," in Gary Steiner, The People Look at Television. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1963, pp. MO9-422. , and Patricia Kendall. "The Communication Behavior of the Average American," in Paul F. Lazarsfeld and Patricia Kendall, Radio Listening in America. New York: Prentice-Hall, 19u8. Lyle, Jack. "Immediate vs Delayed Reward Use of Newspapers by Adolescents," Journalism Quarterly, 39, 1 (Winter 1962), Pp. 83-850 inccoby, Eleanor E. "Effects of the Mass Media," in Martin L. Hoffman and Lois Wladis Hoffman, ed., Review of Child Development Research. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1964, I, pp. 323-348. . "The Effects of Television on Children," in Wilbur Schramm, ed., The Science of Human Communication. New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1963, pp. 116-127. "Why Do Children watch Television,"-Public Opinion Quarterly, 13, 3 (Fall 195u), pp. 239-244. ________3 "Television, its Impact on School Children," Public Opinion Quarterly, 15 (1951), pp. u21-4uu. Kdmmar, Quinn. Psychological Statistics. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1962. Heycnssohn. Rolf B. ”Social Research in Television," in Bernard Rosenberg and David Manning White, Mass Culture. New York: The Free Press, 1957, pp. 345-357. , .win B. "The Effects of Television on Public Library Circulation,” Public Opinion Quarterly, 27, 4 (Winter 1963), pp. 578—589. ”’1' . Changes in the Function of Radio with the Adoption of Television," Journal of Broadcasting, 5, 1 (Winter 1960-61), pp. 39-48. Beirmuater, Lee, Richard P. Coleman, and Gerald Handel. Workingman's Wife. New York: MacFadden Books, Inc., 1959. Remmexms, H.H. and D. H. Radler, The American Teenager. New York: Bobbs-Merrill, 1957. Riley, iflJJ. and S. Flowerman. "Group Relations as a Variable in Communication Research," American Sociological Review, 16, 2 (1851), pp. 178-179. and'John W. Riley Jr. "A Sociological Approach to Communications Research," Public Opinion Quarterly, 15 (1951), pp. 445-460. Samuelson, Merrill, Richard P. Carter, and Lee Ruggels. "Education, Available Time, and Use of Mass Media," Journalism Quarterly, 90 (Fall 1963), pp. #91-u96. Stanley. The Psychology of Affiliation: Experimental Studies 0 the Sources of Gregariousness. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1959. Schneider, Louis and Sverre Lysgaard, "The Deferred Gratification Pattern: A Preliminary Study," American Sociological Review, 18, Sfuemm, Wilbur, ed., The Effects of Television on Children and Adolescents: An Annotated Bibliography with an Introductory Overview of Research Results. Paris: UNESCO Mass Communication Techniques Division, 1964. . "Mass Communication," Annual Review of Psychology, 13 (1962), , Jack Lyle, and Edwin Parker. Television in the Lives of Our Children. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1961. , Jack Lyle, and Edwin Parker. "Children's Learning from Te evision," Studies in Public Communication, 3 (Summer 1961), DD. 86-98. fScott, Lloyd. ”Relationships between Elementary School Children and Television," Journal of Educational Research, 52, 8 (December 1958), pp. 13%-1370 ' Siegel, Sidney. Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: McGraw-hill Book Co., 1956. Sonquist, John A. and James N. Morgan. The Detection of Interaction Effects. Ann Arbor: Survey Research Center, Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan, Monograph No. 35, 1964. Steiner, Gary A. The People Look at Television. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1963. Troldahl, Verling C. and Daniel E. Costello. Mass Media Exposure Patterns and Interpersonal Community Behavior of Teenagers. Paper read at the Theory and Methodology Division Sessions of the Aspociation for Education in Journalism Convention, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa, August 1966. r7“ " .roidahl, Verling C. "Studies of Consumption of Mass Media Content," Journalism Quarterly, #2 (Autumn 1965), pp. 596-606. . ”Occupational Prestige Scale," in Verling C. Troldahl and Robert Van Dam, Public Affairs Information-Seeking from Expert Institutionalized Sources. East Lansing: Department of Communication, Michigan State University (mimeo), 1965, Appendix E. U.S. Bureau of Census. U.S. Censuses of Population and Housing, Census Tracts, Final Report PHC(l)-92. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1961. Westley, Bruce H. and Werner J. Severin. "Some Correlates of Media Credibility," Journalism Quarterly, 41 (Summer 1964), pp. 325-335. I White, David Manning. "Mass Communication Research: A View in PerSpective," in Lewis Anthony Dexter and David Manning White, People, Society, and Mass Communication. New York: The Free Press, 1964, pp. 521-5H6. Witty, Paul A., Paul Kinsella, and Anne Coomer. "A Summary of Yearly Studies of Televiewing, 1989-1963," Elementary English, October 1963, pp. 590-597. :itty, Paul. ”Studies of the Mass Media, 1949-1965," Science Education, March 1966, pp. 119-126. Yarexng {eon J. “Separation from Parents d'ring Early Childhood," in Martin L. Hoffman and Lois Wladis Hoffman, Review of Child DSVGlOpTQMt Research. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 196%, I, pp. 89-136. ' APPENDIX A ITEMS, CODES, AND INTERJUDGE CODING RELIABILITIES LISTED BY VARIABLE CATEGORY AND VARIABLE NAME 158 econ mo xmeaa ca .coprcmmeo mom WHH nopdmno mom .xmocH pooEmoemm owmpcmono .eOHumcmmeo now HHH novmmso new .momcommon ooocoueoao peopeommon mo mHmmHmem peoweoo poanvmn Heep omonp one mebmem> Aev poeemwm .meoooo wooem HomEmmewm mponEoo mopmodoCH mcv mm; com: hHHHHAmHHon mcHooo oprHLoHCH mo enammme ones A mmsonHHmnlwmzoc‘wmoHooeoo‘dwmemno .meomo .Hmonse .m.coeoz .mmmodo meow om: oHomn mm nmvoHnm> cmH: .zoxOOH omHo .munomm "ow comeH myconma Hmuaonma ow BOHV mushy m o 930% or memnmona oHomn mo peHx “we: mo huoHnm> no>oc u o meHeoumHH mm oHst :H mono u H «hope: no BoHHns oHomn moEHHmeom n m m CH mono mem>o .moEHuoEom .coumo Hmueonmm coumo u m oHemn one o» coumHH wwconmd each on mo pesoe< oHocomzo: pom >e o: u o moHnmupom mew meme so; .oco emcu oboe cH mpow am you H u H o>mn :0» MH moHnmunom w you any mH :onH>onu meow onoe no N u m meow :OHmH>oHov m :30 mHHemm poo» moon mo nonesz mmmemev .menopm>E . .msoa .onomEoo .eoHpon ooemHom wcHsmH> .muco>o HmHooam .munoam .menovmos conH>oHou mm AHHoHec> ech .mmnodo meow ”Hume mxHH mesonma Hmuewnmm ow :oHv m:oznm u o pro» on mzonw mo oeHx was: mo >pmHem> nope: u o weHon> rem oHst :H mono u H meo>oe conH>oHow moEHHmuo u N no .oHHcs.m cH mono revue .m)a_H«m com Hmpcwnmm om: MHooE coumo u m .copmo conH>oHow nouns mezohme poem on we pesoe< Humane mnoHHHHHroHH oooo EoHH pomHem> know.owmo sci ec..ooo poaHem> oucsH; ozH m_i:efibeuaL > new xwoemmwwluwmmflmmmndflWMHWflHmfiiflflfiflflrmmfiwwfim tempos ocpsHeopsH pom motoo .wHVHH .nm m:;mmN 159 um AapoHpm> :an maumHonmnov eonpo .memnwoem meme .msoem mpoeoo .mHmHAom no mmeomo doom .wunOQm .mzozm moxUOH opr "been om: oHomn peoocommee on soHv moose m s o oxHH so» on memewoem oHomn we ocHx “we: we xpoHem> no>me n o xooz m 2 deep mmoH u H mm amp m 9302 H u N ano>oe .xoms m moo m mason mam u m meson : emzv won mmmp m 990: H “roam mm: OHpma zoo m . whee m meson mnm when m meson mu: HomeGOQmom om: mHeme meson @905 no m u : "oHomn one ow copmHH 50% or oowmo so: mo pcsoe< pceocoamom posse mm mpmmemoeonm m z o mono meamnwoeona hHHEmm poo» meow mnamewoeonm home 30: mo noberz «90:90 no .woeHumwme some .mnodmdmsoc Eonm .:0HmH>oHov Eoem mm cOHmH>oHow #0: u H .oHdooa ov meHmev Eonm :1 pHeoz may meme conH>oHou n m eH so mcHom m.pmn3 prone use: eHonp we no mooeoom pmoe pow mucoema prom xcHnw so» on when: Hmpcopmm mmoeo pegs "mom mH .wooHwame mom or omen mueoemm em mocHnommE m I o oanomnom upcoema each on «mHQMHzmme mmeHwamE omen mueonme 9:02 on mocHnmme pus: mo embesz mm unease : u o needed szomz peonowHHo.noAE:z wmm wedded m a o needed mmocsw paw shame pampmemae pmaesz mmoHonoz cam ow woanomnsm .meoemd seesaw .meoema HHHmo In mHHEmm mnemmamzoc Leo» CH you :o% on whoammmso: Hun; mo nonesz maHHHHHLmHH owou EouH pomHnm> anomoymo gem mcHroo oHannm> ouoszUHCH Ii tr;:Hrzooxism pomb ..Hmurog n O EOUHmm u H abooe m mono u m mem>oc .EOpHow .bpeoe w some p50bm mm cpeoe m mOHsv u m .zpcoe m ooHsp usonm .xoms Agape xmoz >Ho>o n : "moH>OE may 09 ow so» or emvmo 30m oocmocmupm .oH>oe ueoocoamme zoomsoonm eo>oe u 0 mm moEHpoeom n H «copmo so: .mo» mH «semenHH oHHbsm ems memebHH :oymo u m on“ Eoem pro meOA zooso 50% om Homecoamome OOH we: oHo u H m>oem mm 05mm moHEoo mom moHeoo poxooeo u m mononomoea HemoGOQmom expand uxoonov nonpo .mxoon oHeoo .wxoon Ho>mny .moHpopmme .mmHnomnwOHn .moHeovm HmEHcm moocopomonm .moHnoum am: .eonon ooeoHom .COHHonnco: goon O AHHoHem> ean ow .mHo>oc HMUHLOPmH: .onQOHm o>0H .oesuco>om Homecomwon ”m cm ROHV mommy H n o "Hmon oxHH so» on mxoon mo mweHx pee: mo hpoHem> momma unopeommoh mocHNmmme mm mocHnmmoe a u o mmHansmon omen pom op mocHnmmme wee: mo embesz mapmHonoeov moHeoo .poonm comma .msm: huwHoow .aoHuoow >Bn0Homa .momma w.cmEoz .mwmm HMHocmch .mHchesHoo arm HHHOHem> .mom Hem: .HmHQOpHom .mzm: eonuo .owma om: nommmwsoa an; ow sOHV peonm .ocHwamE .munOQm "omen so» Homecommon moomwoom m I o op nonmawzom mew mo mopooom ems: mo upwHem> eoHpHHHLoHH ooou 1 1;:ltla _aouH memHnm> Anomopmo toe acfleoo oHanpm> omosmnowcH .epoempcooa-sm passe pro ow v.emo .E.m m u m poéieua .sdmu: .ed a ... m and 0H ... m am .ad .2 u m uancoHE u b meson Hons .mo> HH mmzmexmoz no 950: ocHHomop o: u m :Hmpnmo m pm cH on ou o>m£ so» on Am memo: co II Homoeo>HmmHEeom nww: 204mm mzth weOHuoHeuwmn ow zOHV mH I Ho 039 mom mmmoom wszzzm rm ammezH "H xmazH mmmeo>meHenom mmocopHmonoo awn-age... H353 wo>mHHon ow hHoxHH oo>oHHob ummma wm o>0bm mm msmm HumoH on 50% oHsos eOHweo> gone... "m eonnm> mm conH>oHoy Hoe u H mo>oHHob ow hHoxHH popoHHoA “mo: conH>oHow u w umoe on so» oHsoz cOHunm> :och... “H conne> 11 ...mocHNmmmE . um no mnodmmmso: :H .conH>mHow no OHpma mwerme may no HHOPm meow on“ mo meonno> szHHnHomno HomeommHo omen no oemon :0» MH memE uaooeommom mmomx so» oHsoz nOHQS mmoeHNmmmE no .wemdmmmzoe .oHomn pmoe mmHE oHsos mm conH>oHov no: u H w .pom QOHwH>oHoH m "MCHsoHHom mew Homoeoamee conH>oHoH u m mo one cho moox pHsoo 30> omomazm . MHpoz mApmHonmnov peeve .NhMm .momeoo Hmonze u m .mpOHmHmon .mHmoo> .mHepeoespwm woodenomonm mom AmHoHnm> :mH; ICH .HHos c noon .chmE XHom pnoooe Homoaoawoa op sOHV moemw s u o "oxHH so» or mfinooo_ we oeHx Hob: mo honnm> co ..m 3.3 an H .H 5me0 ii- -. I..-I----..II. JimmpIH 82... ES Hg Iom aCHwoo eHanbe> oaemmnobcH pochHcoozlbm QHQUH «on no mdb ow MCHow ohm menu wcHevaOm mm o>0bm mm oemm gnome :0HnHmo prom 90m 50% xmm eo>o mHHEmH poo» CH unannonw any on HA OGHH mm onHpoeom u m mepoonm 950% psonm me» u m unnamed prom op mev 50% emu Am Amesz I- Acoaumoaaseeoo emu: 204mm memeH memes coapmoaesaeoo ow onv p I H mze mom mmmoom oszzsm rm ammeZH peonmmIoHHnoe so o>onm mm mEmm mmpeoxmoz co pro on.so» con: on HH.soz opens mweonmd 950% HHoH 50% on no m“ so o>onm mm meow wmmcho>o :1 may cH woo $9.50% cos: we HH.:om when: mucoemm 9:0» HHop 50% on HA «Hoonom no» u H popwm poo on.so> dogs on HH.:O% we eoeHbmeow no 0: n m memes mpemnmm poo» HHow so» on Am masonmoeonz m.veoocommoa mo 304mm memeH enema mwepazoex heaped mom mmmoom wszzam wm memQZH "a xmmzH meson Hogs amo%_mH wwocoxoos so n50: msm mpore no meow :Hpueoo m Hm :H on OH one; so» on An -wamwiHbeHH coco IIII-:I:III!I EoHH pomHem> mnemoumo In; demoeo GHQmem> nwmsmhcwcH IIIIII. ncesHHCQOIIbm pooe 30an £00 cme: u N #00900 H 00%: n H om #00900 «50% pom pmoe cOvapcoHpo oocmec @080: n o mzw 0000 mm: xcH:# 50% 00 common awn: vcmpmma Qm>wc u N mm :mpmo mpozpoWOH muoowoaa was moEHpmfiom u H CO xaoz pm>m hHHEmm poo» moon AA wfimeononov ooHo mum>Hpm ow 0m «mm>HpmHoa no 0am mocoHpm yHmH> .Amo 0H 000090 00H“ Hm .roch OH #50 cm .mmew mmHa .wcHaEmo .050: um chwmmpam nwchaonw .on>oe .moEmw HHmn "ponummoy mwcng wcHBOHHOM mm moHpH>Hvom OH I Ho may m0 how on 90>0 hHHEmm 930% moon Am Axmasz -- Aommcpmspomoo smog zoqmm mzmeH mmmzmmzemmoe 0w 30HV HH 1 Ho 02H are mom mm¢oom oszmDm wm nmxmazH quz mgoaopmo oflcsfigoch oHanLm> ooscwwcou::wm 04009 mm mm mm mm OCH OCH OCH mm mm hm mm mm o>oom mm o>onm mm 9500 mm m>oom mm m>oom mm o>onm mm m>oom mm m>onm mm m>oom mm m>oom mm 9000 mm 9m>ma moEHumeom cmumo 05mm mm30©5H3 may :mwz AH 050m «900 may 2mm: Ax memo mmHmmE x000 Am 05mm mx903 090% 00 AH 0500 wmwnpOHo :30 co9H A: 05mm «mongHo w.%HHEmm 009H Am memo mmmngHo :30 cwwz Am memo mmonpoHo m.%HHEmm 5mm: A0 memo mwm5on mHosz :mmHo mHms Au mama «6009 530 cmoHo A0 0500 mcm9wHHno 9owc50% Mo 09mo 0x0» an n o u H u a mooom 90% monm Am 0 o o oOHv 50% CC. PM£3 «0.0.. “H @050; pm X9030m5oc 0:9 59H: #50 5H0; 50% on Axmasz M moHyHHHnHmcoam¢9 x. --Am09999909mcogmm9 nmflg ”040m mEmHH zmmemaom mac: 09 2090 mm - 00 9:9 moo mmmoom oszzam 9m omxmazH m.u:mccoammm w5H%mm mm 0>opm mm 050m m50% ow pH mm>Hw on: .50% 09 mocmonHm co>Hw mH %050E wchcomm 950% mo 0500 MH "N onmmm> u wc99mm 9995 wmm 05000 00 000m m%HHEmm 950% CH mHHHA on“ m%mm on: ”H onmmm> . :0HpmcH80o 9509mm« muflfl So I. 208 vii :. 58,9 3 0m 28> fiom 38 to: mammou mHQmH9m> cm©5m9owcH UQSCszoozzbm oHan 50H90>0 05 u o H 50 u009w0 u H m 50 0009w0 u N 030:0 m 50 0009w0 u m 0%H90H5wm9 50903 050909050 90 50909>0H09 50 H0 050990055 3900 90% 0950905 950% 00 030:0 9509 9053 003050 9505009w0 0050: 030:0 : 0500 n : 50909>0H09 09990>0m 9509 950% 090 90:: 0H9£0|9509050 mm 0>000 00 0500 000509505H 59H00: 0>0c %H950m 950% 5H 000%50 0005 A0 00 0>000 00 0500 wmcwonHom 0:9 mo %:0 90m %0505 030 05090905m 90 0950905 950% #5959 50% 05 no 5 ”m 00599 50 00H905 0 90>o 09505%05 %H:9505 m0 009900 0 :9 0%05 50:9 050 000509H550 90 095995959 mm 0>090 00 0500 0%59 %H950w 950% MH zocx 50% 05 AA mm 903050 50 0>0m u o 050H0095 %0505 95000 :20:x 9.500: 0900 n H %9903 0950905 950% x5959 50% 05 A0 Axmosz 509909050 %H950m 5000092000 30000 2099 0000 me 000092009 -- :09 09 50050 3 u 0 0:9 000 000000 0292000 90 0090029 90005000000 am 0:090 0 0500 0:050 H0>0£0 A: $00 0>on0 00 0500 . 5000090m 050 0:09 A5 memewnuwH ., 0000 x 509H 0H00H90> %9om0900 1.3; 552000 waflmwflm> ow70m9095H ’lli'i 50:599mooszm 0H205 mm 05 u H «5993 055090 w505 00% u m 50% 0050999 90 50555 0 0>05 50% 05 A0 9x00290 :3 90005050990w09m 309mm 02099 09005 0090 09 3090 0 - 0 00009 009 000 000000 0292200 90 0090029 “0 209000> 00050 900: .00% 99 909505 H00500 90H5m09 0090950 59 09059099905 050 09 m5OH05 50% 50953 0509900950w9o 9o 055Ho H00500 09059 09< 59909Hx0050v 90590 .0H99o 3055905 .090950505 0.500 .05509w 5950% 0509m9H0m .55Ho .0%0m 90005050990009m 5095 .095000 %om n ho 0005050990w09w ”m 9500505005 90>05 u H «0050: 99059 59 00>990H09 mm 005990500 u m 050 0050999 9909>. . .m5930HHom 50990 n m 059 90 %50 00 90>0 %H9509 950% 0005 AA QCOS u H mm 309 %90> u N 000999>9900 0500 u 0 %99555500 90 90H 0 59 09059099905 90H 0 u 0 050909050 90 0950905 950% 05 90 9000290 90005050990w09m 205mm mzmeH 0005050990W09w 509909w0959 .. 0090 09 2090 9 - 9 009 029 000 000000 0290200 90 0000029 900900 299005000 00999995099 '09 059000 0000 : 5099 0950990> %9000900 000509095H 090a9am> ’tl‘! III-"l 0055995001105 0H909 o 050 0 0%99555500 0959 59 00>99 50% 0>05 0509 305 05905 50% 0903 0905: 00095095 50 cm Aw 000959009 099555 09 0m 99 0099050 909090050 09 0m 90 0905590 09 950 om 90 009900500 09055 09 0m 90 00%095 00090 09 OM A5 0059>990 950 cm A0 . . .m59309909 059 mo %50 00 90>0 %99509 950% 0005 "305mm mEMBH zm>mm mmb mom mmmoom UZHEEDm %m memmzH 000505 .0050999 950% 90 9509590>0 %090 90>0 50% 05 90 000505 .0050999 950% 90 9909> 90>0 50% 05 A5 .5. 900% 9 5059 0009 0 oo .0900% m 5059 0009 u 90 mm 09 5059 0009 u 09 .09% 09 5059 0905 u 09 0905 5905 u :9 0m 05 n 9 wmh u N 00 0>050 00 0500 mm 0>050 00 0500 mm 0>050 00 0500 mm 0>050 00 0500 mm 0>050 00 0500 fim>0fi u H W 00 005990500 u N 10 fimPMO u m 950990950990 :1 0505 0090950 5&95 09 2090 09 1 00 am 0>050 00 0500 90>05 u 9 who 005990500 u m 309%0 u m .09999990099 0000 :05 559000 0000950909 !l| 3', '. .| .v 0 ‘1'! %99555500 59 0599 90 090000 AxmazH 50990950990 0505 0090950 0099 0950990> %9000900 0950990> 0055995oozl0m 09500 «Anemmmm Mo macaw coymy>mawy may my vagmyompmzo 0H Mo ymwav "mymmyoyypmm maoyow no mpwyowamzo mayzoyaom mLy guys: mamyowpmno >9 mm yomanoo wmma no m u a Cy msocm >e mzy mo wmemc any 09m ymn: “H onmmm> yoonaoo Ham u m mam>ma mmwmaxoax ycmwcomwom mm vmem: wmwnaoz o I o wymsz .mmm mH wwmynnos mam mmynno: ycmpmm m>mn mcmwmmmsw mo mycmamm 950% on mo hwnssz .. mmwaaoc ycmwcommma coyymycmyno mm cmEmc mmflnno; w a o wymnz .wmm mH wwmwppos %cw m>mz 50% on we LmnEyz mamm «AyamEcam>ow uymyw go a n a ‘ ycmEc9m>om mywo Emymzm Hoosow may . .% nywz vmyowccoo wcwmn mm >MHycmow oy mmpsmym owansm 1. wmxmw mum: mycwvcommmn soaps wmaswyw mymym cam Hmooa :m mayomaaoo wowwwycwvw HmUOA wcm mymym mafia mo ymwaxomno mo mwvmazocx amaswmm m I o mv “onmQ vamya mamomm msy 09m 0:: ycmvcommmm am on u a wagonm m mm %Hamaswmm mamey no mum u m cognac ncoyym mawemm 9:0» mmom an «8.5: u H mcogno who mmeflymaom u m 0y om...a@£ymmoy mcmzoyaoy may ccymo u m Mo mam ow Qo>o mawamm 930% mwoa Aw Axmasz . 509330 :. Amocmocmppm noqum mzmeH pm moqmucoupm guy; ow zeav m I m 03y m:y mop mmmoow wgflmusm ym amxmmzH mo mocmsvmpm woymmwygwfifi mvoo -:;a--:ll.r::l-a EoyH mynmypa> mnomaymo as; mcflvoo maamflaw> mmcswgong C 0384.8; mou-unm magma ages yon mmov u y «x90: 50% ow yamemoymsm mm myamyym>m cmaz u m cmyyo 30: wow 50% ow x90: yo wayx ymaz man mvymyso zyywmmym mayo: u m .mmm my maOm mEyyuyamQ m m>ma 30% on m.y:mvcommmm« .mco you mm mammm may now ywmgmycy aose mm mmEyy mmm oy m>ma cane: mam .mammh 03y yo vcm may yyyab ooy m>oam mm mEmm omm may xmma mmm yon mysoo mama My Am mm m>oam mm mewm . my mwmmao mommny may Aw mm .m>oam mm mEmm w my mymg ymmamycy may Am a my ooy m>oam mm mamm mymyQnym may .m>oam COyymmsw may CH Aa 9 m . my 0mm mo w: .zmcoE may mo mms may now 9mm» 9mm w: myoa: Ama mmm omym mam .pmmm mco mo cum may ym 0mm may %mmmn pmzmcw ymeLOUCy u o oy wmmywm mam .myons pma 509m 0mm mm Amncam ymmymom u y zopgoa oy Uma mama .myomoya m man 09 Am Axmazyv i - 3.30m 5mg . 23mm m5: gyms amya oy :oyv m u o m>ym mzy mom mmxoom ozyzzsm wm amxmazH "m onmxm> mamEmyy acmya may cynyyyy zmyv ycmmmpmmn mcmuoym mcyzoyyoy may ow myoswopm mcmwoym cm was yomgyoo oy I oo yma: wygmmxm maymyygm>wm cm 50% ma< ”m ZOHmmm> gmumflwmmemmwsttvst mwoo Ii!:t:i2s- ,, Emyy myamypm> myommywo 1: v3.2.4.5 3%..22, vw;:am3nrtaH vmscyyrooz:>m myamb mnymyyxmmaov wycmeyymymcy %amE ooy .mmcoe amss ooy mmymm3.nmayoe .hmcoe amas ooy mmymm: hmaymm «mmayoym mom hmcoe awsonm yo: .awya ooy m.ycm& may .myyya poymow same ooy .ymymcmw Cy myyya zsme ooy .vooy mom mmcos awsocm yo: "mmyapoz mmcoE ymmmwya nymay mam xcyay 30> om ymaz memyaonm hmcoe myamamm 3m mmxmmao mmyagoz m n o ysoam mane: myampmm 950% anyay so» on "y onmmm> mmyaaoz hmcoE coyymycmyno “yo pmaezz amasmcoo figwfi ..u o xmm: m mEyy y sway mmmy u y mnmaayy nu xmm: m mEyy y u m :y mmycsym mu xmms m mmfiyy mymsom u m ycmwcommmfi mm mmmmum>m u : «mummayy may :y zosym pm>m so» on mmamswmam m\a oy a: u o y my a: u y N\H a op a: u m N oy as u m N\H m op a: u a m my a: u m m\a m op as u m mmymsym 9 my as n b mmvsym yamwaommmn a>a .mga : away mmoE u m 90% om %mw m mmsoa mums 30a ysoa< magma mo nmasnz amyyyyyamyy mmoo Emyy myamypm> muowmymm um; mcycoo myamypw> muwzmmm.:H mossyyammtamm myazb meyy mo coynma cma u y m pm>o mycmemmm %yaycos yo mmyamm M 5m mommamo u w my wwcyay pom maymmm :1 myomEyymym yyompm ooow u m .:y :y mwcyay msa oy mmoy cma wamsoy m no mmmy ooow m m.yy xcyay 90% on mosyyyy< nm>mc u o om>mc ywoeym u y mmEyy mo ooymmm m mm mmEyymEow u m 9m>o wycmsmmm hyaycoe :y mmo cmay mam yywmhm %yymSm: u m mcyayoyo man 9m>m zyyemy 990% mmon yo man hyyemm - :m m>oam mm memm myy ayy: ow oy yam: ymos so» wysoz ymaz ycmwcommma .30: yawyy omm 50> m>mm maOmEom My "m onmmm> mm Ham mamam u a wyy gpyz om m.>mny , mm mpom 0cm macaw n m xcyay no» om ymaz .oooym mxyy .hmaoe mycmpma yam m>mm u m mo yoy m mycmpmm 990m m>mw maomeom My "y onmmm> mcoyymyamyao m>mmuoommmm mfiymyyxmmamv mmyaaoa amopomma .mxmmcm ammyymmzm yooamm .yzo mayom ammamazy .wcywcmmm .mmayoyo .myaogm oom mmnofi amwocm yo: ycmvcommmfi mwm wmxmmao mmyypo: m I o ”mEmyaoym %mCOE ywmp g:0% mam ymaz "N onmmm> mayyyyaimyy mwoo EmyH myawygm> anowmymo ‘.w hzanvou ‘.;ynomay QHQUHnHC> silt lvl‘Ii'I-‘OO- «mamyomo mcymaoam :y no .mooaaoaawyma 930% my .czoyczow mcyayoyo mzsa hyymzm: mm 96%. mm memm mnymny 990m mamas mmgoym may 92 Aa mmpmycmo mm mmpm y vmyym u y wcymmoam :y yo wmpoyw czoycsoo mya 1* mm mmmpm m vmyyo u a may :y no mmpoym wooapoaawymc :y mmmpm m omyyo u m 00am 0y ammmom hyyemm Loom mmoa Am Axmmzyv coyymycmyao 2033520 2&3 $6.5m mzmS mcynaonm .. 3 20220 a I m 2; 5:. mom mmmomm mzHEsm E nmxmmzy >35.“ mfiymyyxmmaOV pmayo .wyyyw .wymomm .mmyaaoa .myygmam .mmyyomzw yooamw «msy>mm ayamECymymmycm nmxmmcm ammyymanom ,moaomm .mmCmewma mayoammm Ahymygm> amya 0y zoyv .mmypca nmmayoyo nmmaocsy .mymymamo yamwcommmh wom vmxmmao m:myy my I oo "Loy >3c0£ pom» woman 30% 0o yma: mo hymyam> cmyyyyyamyy mcoo amyy myamymm> IQawiflmoo nmxzmTHoych Ill'l\ III!" -II ameCwync j %Homoywo CHQQMQQ> “InuilINIm UNIDNVH. .wyymymv mom Hyw ymymmao mmm .mmyymmym ymcoyymmsmmo mayvom 0y cm>y0>ay yamEmaSm m>yyommaaw yo mmymmo pmymmyw may yo mmnmoma mmqma ycyom I y m ayayyz yamEmmymm 0y mmumymy mm: myymyynm may .Emyy wyay co .mwoo mmymmym m 0y yamEmmywm mm: mao myay ymmmxm mEmyy yym co myymyypo myyyyamyymma Amway yamoyoyyv .mymmm may :0 mym>my m30ypm> may ym ma0m may 0y a0m m.pmcpmm mwmz :ymE may yo mwyymmym may m0 aoyymmmyma yymay mayamyme .yamsmwSm m>yy0mwasm m mme mpmvoo .mymmm may wayuyyyy: cH .ymamym mumma cam amma away mwyywmym mwmy u o mpoyomyyom mwmaymw .nmxmym magma 0cm :mma n y mnoymymao wqmywzma ammmnyymz n a mgaympmao 00yymym m0y>nmm .mnoyomy myxmym 90y poymymmo ymyyoa n m maoyymyompom wmmyay 90m amsmmymm .mqua0m= oysm u : mamammywoyoam ymmmmo .hammeom acyymyyommcmyy 90y xymyo mymy n m whammeom mmmyw 90m yoymEyymm ymoo am0y>ymm maymyyam>om 90y nmwmame yyvmpo u o mymwmama yamyasooom ywoo m>yyoeoysm .pmagoz coyym>pmmcom yyom u h mzcmmfioo myyyomym pom ycmmm maywmaogom .mcmmeoo yawn moyaoyymmym pom ymmcywam mmymm n m mamammmy yooamm mamym .ymmaywam ymoyEmao wayyHSmcoo u m .moyoy gym 90m poymspymcy yymyoyym .yomaamyayymasm yooamm myyasa no ycmymymmm I oy myommmmoym mwoyoymom .ymmzmy m yo mwyywmym may a yy mmmzzmy m away mwyymmna mace u my "mam .m mmmom mymmm may .yamoyogy .o wcyyym> ha ammoym>mo moo my ammo mwyymmym ymaoyymmsooo m0 mpsmmme maym Ammyymmym amoc :omymm mwyymmym yma0yymasmmo awya myay mmoo yma: «mayo; comamm myay ymcoyymasmoo n8 3 :03 fl .. 8 $8 $2: ~38: byemm as» my 23 3 205mm? . msymym myeoaommIOymom %ammpmoemm amyyyyyamyy maoo Emyy myamyym> anommpmw In; mayfioo myawdym o szymyCH . > I\r|‘ " ‘I-III III" U zayyncoIIbm myam, 174 pmayo u H mm mycmymm ayoa ayy3 om>yy ma amymoyway yamaaommmy u N wm>yy 50% 0a an2 ayyz mamy myyemm x903.y0c mmoo pmayoe u N msymym mm mEnyyqu u m wayoz %may 00 cmyyo ycmamonEm mEnyyysy wxy03.pmayos u a 30m «mayo: myyemm 990m cy 0a: m.ymayoz mm mfimgSma yayemm my I mo maymyaxompov o>yy so» om sag: gpyz mNym yyyemm ymmyo no Nmmy u o Nomo ymmzm: so ymymy p0 mmmy u y w.%yyemm 900% m0 ymmh may my ymaz A0 r2mm y 90 o u o wmyyemm om mymo : 0y N u y 950% my m>ma :0» 0a mymm mama 30m A0 mmmoam N 0y y u o magma 30a .mmm mH om mmcoaa m 0y m n y choammymy m m>ma myyemm maom mmoa AA 980.» m 0y m u o Nm>ma mmsoa no ycmEypmmm msm meooy by 0y oy u y m.>yyemy ysoz mmoa meooy Name 30: Am Axmazyv Amsymym yogum mzmyy mach maoymmmmmoa II amya 0y Boyv : I o mzb xom mumoom 02ynzam wm meszH “N onmmm> cmyyyyyamyy 0000 I Emyy myamyam> muowmymm Ima maywoo myamypm> mmvsmymyay WMMMWyCCUIINN myflmfi 175 prm u a coy 50a u N Nyyym m 90 moa m 50% my< xmm ayyao %yco my yamaaommmy u y ayyam ymmvyo u N Uyyam mymaye u m oyyam ymmmazoz wmmmm yymay mum yma: 0cm m>ma 30% 0o wmm my yammcommmy u : mymymym mam mamayoya home 30: ymoyo ayyym mmyyyyyamyy mooo EmyH myamyym> mhowmymo Ima maycou myamyym> mums mymyay l‘IiII‘I ammayy200II>N myamy APPENDIX B INDEXING PROCEDURES ... '4‘ IXDSXING PROCEDURES From the original Wisconsin questionnaire of 212 items, 90 were originally tapped for the purposes of the present study. These 90 items were coded into 145 different initial variables potentially use- ful to test the hypotheses outlined in chapter II. An a priori decision was made to group items into indexes where such a procedure was warranted. Of the 62 final variables in this study, 13 were indexes reated by summing one or more of the original items. The indexing procedure generally involved two steps: 1) first a logical decision was made on what items seemed to be tapping the same behaviors; and 2) the logical decision was tested by computing Pearson product 9 men at correlation matrixes for each set of items that might form an index. Zechs, standard deviations and code ranges of the original items were also examined so that constructed indexes would be based on items having roughly the same variance and contributing roughly the same weight to the final index. The correlation matrixes for this index construction Operation were computed by deleting all respondents with non-responses on one or both of the items involved in each individual correlation. To establish a criterion for significance a conservative baseline n of 200 was selected to compensate for the varying n's for items. .Actual n's for the items used in indexes varied between 229 and 252. At an n of 200, the critical value of r at p1(.05, using the r to z trans- f rmation test of significance, is .14 (McNemar 1962). All tables of correlation matrixes which follow use this criterion for significance. Aipendix C reports the code ranges, means, standard deviations, and non- ressonses counts for all final indexes. 177 the five items are listed in Appendix A. Table 28 below reports the original code ranges, means, standard deviations, and non-response counts for the five items. Table 29 reports the Pearson product moment correlations between the five items. Table 28: Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-response Counts For The Five Items Used To Create Two Indexes Of Permissiveness Item Code range Meana s.d. Non-responses Hsa-s on reetdays 0 - 8 “.18 2.09 23 hours on wdehends . o - 8 n.94 ‘ 2.28 22 Tell where are after school 1 - 2 1.16 .37 0 Tell Iherc are in evening 1 - 2 1.06 .23 1 Tell where are on recknds 1 - 2 1.13 .33 l a For all five items, the means, medians, and modes were equal. Table 29: Matrix Of Pearson Product Moment Correlations Between The Five Items Used To Create Two Indexes 0f Permissiveness (1) (2) (3) (u) (5) (1) Hours on weekdays - (2) Hours on weekends .53 — (3) Tell where are after school .11 .1u - (4) Tell where are in evening .09 .06 .27 - (5) Tell where are on weekends .11‘ .09 .MS .32 - Since the correlations for the five items clustered into two major groups, two separate indexes of parental permissiveness were computed. A 179 respondent hours -- was created by summing original scores for items 1 and 2 with any respondent with a non- rl' .’ J" (D reasonse on one or both items being recorded as a non-response on the new index. The second index -- parent knowledge of respondent whereabouts -- 1'53. U) created by summing the original scores for items.3, 4,.and 5 with any respondent with a non-response on two or more of the items being ecorded as a non-response on the new index. A respondent with a non— response on only one item had the original mean for that item plugged in for the computation of his final index score. 'ee items.in the original questionnaire seemed to tap various of child-parent communication. The original wording and codes ‘ . 1; (J (A 0) f1} () ’(1 car the three items are listed in Appendix A. Table 30 below reports 133 original code ranges, means, standard deviations, and non-response counts for the three items. Table 31 reports the Pearson product moment correlations between the three items. Table 30: Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, And Non-response Counts For The Three Items Used To Create An Index Of Child-Parent Communication Item Code Range Meana s.d. ' Non-responses Talk to parents on problems 1 - 3 2.57 .60 0 Parents ask R.opinion 1 - 3 2.54 .67 0 Parent reaction if R comes in late 1 - 3 2.10 .82 6 2? all three items, the means and medians were equal but the n Igginal distributions were skewed with a discrepant mode. ’4 C’) O . son Product Moment Correlations Between The ems Used To Create .n Index of Child-Parent (1) (2) (3) (1) Talk to parents on problems - (2) Parents ask R opinion .29 - (3) Parent reaction if R comes in late .22 .13 - To create the child-parent interaction index, scores on the three items were summed. Any respondent with a non-response on only one of the th.ee items had the rounded off mean value for that item plugged into his final index scores. Respondents with non-responses on two or more of the original items received a non-response on the final index. 9 Ischerness index no itdms in the original questionnaire seemed to tap the degree of family group activity or family togetherness. The original wording and codes for the items are listed in Appendix A. Table 32 below reports the original code ranges, means, standard deviations, and non-response counts for the items. The Pearson product moment correlation between the two items was.43. Table 32: Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-response Counts For The Two Items Used To Create An Index Of Family Togetherness Item Code ranges Meana s.d. Non-responses 9r] -anily participation in 0 activities 01 - 10 6.1% 1.83 ily works on group projects 1 - 3 1.92 .68 5 (D 1 a. '1'" 4....1 ”'1 a For both items, the means, medians and modes were equal. can which activities his family did together. The second item came .etimes-often” response to whether his family worked .er. In order not to give the work on projects on undue eight in the final index, the item was converted to a ”yes-n0" basis by recoding an original response of "sometimes" or ”often" as "yes" and (code 1) and an original response of "never" as "r 0" (code 0). The original scores on item 1 and the recoded scores on item 2 were then summed to create the.family togetherness index. Any respondents with a non-response on one or both items was recorded as a non-response on the new index. 3 hone respon nsibilities index Fourteen items coded from the original questionnaire asked the raspondent the frequency with which he handled various home" responsibilities such as housekeeping and yard care.. The original wording and codes for the items are listed in Appendix A. Table 33 below reports the original code ranges, means, standard deviations, *nd non-response counts for the 18 items. Table 3a reports the.‘ correlation matrix for the set of items. chile 33~ Code Ranges. Means, Standard Deviations, And Non-Response Counts For Tne 14 Items Used To Create And Index Of Tespoident tone Responsibilities. Itena Code range Meana s.d. Non-responses (1) hop for Food 1 - 3 1.06 .55 0 (2) Take care younger children " .96 .88 l (3) Clean own room " 1.72 .u8 0 (a) Help clean whole house ” 1.19 .69 u (5) Wash family's clothes " .36 .56 u (6) Wash own clothes " .50 .65 3 (7) Iron family's clothes " .su .64 u (8) Iron own clothes " .88 .78 3 (a) Do yard work " Lush .70 1 (10) Cook meals " .88 .59 3 (11) Wash the car " 1.07 .67 l (12) Wash the windows " .89 .68 2 {13) Take out garbage " 1.38b .75 1 '5+) Shovel snow " 1.60 ‘ .64 1 A. DI All but three of these items had equal means, medians, and modes.. The rest narktd with a subscript "b") had roughly equal means and medians but disc epant modes. Table 34: Matrix Of Pearson Product Moment Correlations Between The 14 Items Used To Create An Index Of Respondent Home Responsibilities (1) (2) (3) (9) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) (ll) (12) (13) (14) 3 D \_,»z\_~lvaV\-’vv . N \1 r .07 .07 .27 -.02 .39 - -.04 .21 .34 - ' .19 .05 .18 .28 .73 - .13 .08 .23 .38 .H6 .H4 - .18 .27 .33 .36 .53 .69 - .00 —.01 —.19 -.11 -.14 -.23 -.24 - .12 .21 .23 .30 .28 .21 .21 .02 — .02 .15 .01 .11 .06 -.10 -.O7 .86‘ .22 - .13 .26 .17 .20 .20 .12 .16 .23 .34 .95 - .03 .08 .05 —.02 -.08 -.17 -.18 .31 -.O6 .15 .23 - .03 .02 —.18 -.08 -.06 -.26 -.22 .66 .01 .38 .22 .35 - AI—‘Ar‘ofif\ P~ .\ (... \J L! (.11 42‘ (U .1 ( I") I I00!“ Hcocncocowi- C , .5 /‘\ 7' “". 3 .\ J I —~ \/ V' ’... \'.J ;/ o c o 0 O (”J 1"" C) I 183 (I {a E) C t (' 0 C) 'O I“ :5 ..Q '-g-type tasks and one set of highly interrelated 'ardwork and heavier-type tasks. Approximately seven items fit into seven into the second. SinCe it seems logical that girls w ould score more on the first type of task and :oys more on the second, it was decided not to separate the 14 items Lnto two separat e indexes but to sum across all 14 items. Any respondent dth non-reSponses on-three or less of the original items had the fi‘.‘ ~- . x. at; ded off mean values for those items added to his final index :-;.-. Tezpcndents with four or more non-responses were recorded as - -reoponses on the final index. ’. o 0 efficndent knowlecce of family Operation Eight items in the original questionnaire tapped the respondent's about his family's household Operation. A typical item Zed ”do you know how your home is heated?" and was coded simply as 5.- uJ answer" or "don't know". For the four items that were used Ithis index, the original wording and codes are listed in Appendix A. tie 35 below reports the original code ranges, means, standard =fiations, and non-response counts.‘ Table 36 reports the correlation a. 3, '2-‘39K o -‘:le as. God: Ran,rs, Loans, Standard Deviations, and Non-response Cosnts For Tze Four Items Used To Create An Index Of Respcntcnt \LOLlcdge of Family Operation 2:3; Code range Meana s.d. Non-responses Pa-e:t no7c; worries O - l .40 .ug 5 it:il' use of credit 0 - 1 .35 , .48 0 P:r-nt debts O - 1 .33 .u7 17 Fanily health insurance 0 - 1 .35 .u7 2 e For all four lLCWS the means medians and modes were equal. Table 36: Matrix Of Pearson Product Moment Correlations Between The Four Items Used To Create An Index Of Respondent Knowledge Of Family Operation (1) (2) (3) (4) 1) Pzrert men 3 wcrries - ;~} Panil; use of cr-edit .17 - {1) Parent d3-ts .18 .29 - ;;) Family health insurance .17 .28 .37 - To compute the index of respondent knowledge of family operation, ;;;res on the four items in table 35 were summed. Any respondent with non-reSponse on only one of the original items had the rounded off {U :ean value for that item added to his final index score. Any respondent Vith more than one non—response was recorded as a non-response on the final index. 185 -tens in the originrl questionnaire tapped aspects of ' 3 I no original wording and codes for these firms are lisced in r~nCnalx A. able 37 below reports the original are ranges, means, standard deViations, and non—response counts for flmse items. The two items were correlated.17. fible 37: Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, And Non-response Counts For The Two Items Used To Create An Index Of Parent Gregariousness lien Code range Meana s.d. Non-responses Participation in com nunity. activities . 1 - 4 2.57 .87 6 1E-iti:g friend: and ' relatives 1 - 2 2.52 .55 3 r.) or both items, means, medians, and modes were equal. To create this index, scores on the two items were summed with any mayor de nt having a non-response on one or both of the Original items :ecorded as a non-reSponse on the final index. -xsendant cr-~*riousness index Five items in the original questionnaire tapped various aspects he respondent's gregariousness.' One of the original items 5- a ~£sure of whether the respondent felt he has a close friend -- showed Lnxie variance and was deleted. The original wording and codes for tfiirenaining four items is listed in Appendix A. Table 38 below 186 e;orts the code rances, means, standard deviations, and non-response counts for the four items. Table 39 below reports the correlation matrix. T:ble 33. Code angcs, beans, Standard Dev ia ations, And Non-response Counts For The Four Items Tapping rd Respondent Gregariousness ‘Item Code range Meana s.d. Non-responses II ave a bunch of friends 1 - 2 1.69 .A6 3 Visit at friends ho...es 1 — 3 2.58 .53 5 Friendss stay at R home 1 - 3 2 O2 .63 3 Organizat io ons belong to 1 - 7 2.08 1.37 7 a . . For all four items, means, medians, and modes were equal Ta.le 30: Hatrix Of Pearson Product Moment Correlations Between The Four Items Tapping Respondent Gregariousness O (1) (2) (3) (4) (1) Have a bane h of friends - (2) Visit at £1 iends homes .21 - (3} Friends stay at R home .18 .42 - (4) Organize tions belong to .28 .12 .1u - ecause the item measuring the number of organizations to which the respondent belong was not significantly related to one (and related only barely to another) of the other three items tapping more informal behaviors, the organization measure was excluded from this index. However, it was left in the final analysis for this study and is !.l ’1: sted in Appendix A as-version 1 of the variable, respondent gregarious- T} .ess. Verson 2 of this variable is the index that resulted from sum- Ding scores: on the three remaining items in Table 39. In computing 187 this index, any respondents with non-responses on only one of the three crijinal items had the rounded off mean value for that item added to his final index score. Any respondent with non-responses 1‘" on two or more of the items was recorded as a non-responses on the Outside Home orient tion index {J Bight items drawn from the original questionnaire tapped the degree to which the respondent's family uses outside home resources elaxation, enterta nment, and education. One of the original —- frequency of attendance at taverns -- lacked variance and | l d SlutCCi. The seven remaining items are listed with their original ‘~fitga and odes in Appendix A. Table 40 below reports the. coin ranfes, means, standard deviations, and non—response counts for f'l ‘ the seven items. Table 41 reports the.correlation matrix. Table 40: Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, and Non-response Counts For The Seven Items Used To Create The Outside Home Orientation Index. Ltcm Code ranges Meana ‘s.d. Non—responses GO .ut driving 1 - 3 2.13" .63 4 Go to plays 1 - 3 1.69 .61 5 CF to concerts 1 - 3 1.64 . .64 2 &>out to dinner 1 - 3 2.24 .57 2 {b to sport events 1 - 3 2.06 .68 6 ‘25 .‘CO lectures 1 " 3 1.41 .57 7 CC. 0.1". picnics 1 - 2 1.66 .47 8 {U For all seven items, means, medians, and modes were equal. V Table, : :. :1 : natrix Cf Pea so; Product E-Zomen“ Correlations For The Seven Items Used To Create The Outside Home Orientation Index \ (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (1) «3:; out drivirg - (2) (3:; 1:5 plays .05 -- (3) -3 1:0 concerts .04 .46 - (4%) Go out to dinner .23 .27 .17 - (5) Go to sport events .18 .12 .05 .29 - (6) Go -0 lectures .08 .37 .51 .16 .08 - (7) so or: picnics .31 .02 .20 .19 .14 .14 - To create the outside home orientation index, (scores on the seven .4’3w‘ o ‘ lLL...S teem-re summed. ny respondent with non-responses on one or two of 3 had the rounded off mean values for those items added to his fine? ifi ~ i - - 1-c1 x score. Any respondents with three or more non—responses C 22;". ed as a non-response on the final index. (5: ~~\;‘:'~“ _ci-‘ attendance at church index ms drawn from the original questionnaire tapped the frequency Q ~fi, o o I Q -L cam ily's attendance at church. The original wordings and codes for 1*: - e 7e *‘C ems are listed in Appendix A. Table 42 below reports the COde I“: “ _ , ‘ ‘Nz; Q S , means, standard deviations, and non-response counts for the 3:130:13. ,.___‘ ‘- -L me two items were correlated .53. = Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, And Non-response Counts For The Two Items Used To Create The Frequency Of \\\Church Attendance Index Item Code range Meana s.d. Non-response trequm Q Setha by attend church to- Klbch regularly 1 - 2 1.64 .49 8 $1: / boath items, means, medians, and modes were equal. To create this index, scores on the two items were summed with respcndents ::ith non- esponses on one or both it recorded as non- Rine items drawn from the original questionnaire required the respondent to use his knowledge of'math. Two of these items were deleted because the items were too difficult and variance was low. Two additional items were deleted because they did not relate significantly either to each other or the remaining five items. The wording and codes for the remaining five items are listed in Appendix A. Table 43 below lists the code ranges, means, standard deviations, and non- response counts’for these items. Table 44 reports the correlation matrix. Table 43: Code Ranges, Means, Standard Deviations, And Non-response Counts For The Five Items Used To Create The Math Knowledge Level Index. Item Code range Meana s.d. Non-response Percentage problem 0 - 1 .30 .46 12 Principle problem 0 - 1 .28 .45 12 Interest rate problem 0 - 1 .43 .49 12 Finance charge problem 0 - 1 .10 .31 12 Late payment problem 0 - 1 .71 .46 18 For all five items, means, medians, and modes were equal. ’J LC) 0 ‘sen Product Moment Correlations Between The iv: Items Used To Create The Math Knowledge Level Index (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (1) Percentage problem - (2) Principle problem .26 - (3) Interest rate problem .23 .61 - (4) Finance charge problem .50 .48 .37 - (5) Late ,ayment problem .26 .14 .17 .16 - To create the math index, scores on the five items were summed. Any respondent with a non-response on any of the five items was recorded as a non-response on the final index. Mean values were not filled in for any items on this index because the original coding operation eliminated many "blank" answers which ordinarily would have been coded -l as no —respondes. Thus, in the original coding, coders were instructed r1 (J rt "5 (D (a 1+ {u (.1 'i-correct" any blank answers fer respondents who had obviously asttempted to complete the entire math section. Family snooping orientation index Two items drawn from the original questionnaire tapped the degree 0 to which the family utilized a greater number of shOpping areas in the wankee area. Original wording and codes for the items are listed in Appendix A. Table 45 below lists the code ranges, means,' scandard deviations, and non—response counts for the items. The correlation between the two items was .24. 191 Table 45 Code Ranges, leans, Scandere Deviations, And Non-response Cour‘" For The Two Items been To Create The Family ShOpping Cl‘ien Lu.ti0og Emile... Item _ Code range Meana s.d. Non-response General shopping preference 1 - 3 1.76 91 6 Preference for clothin ng stores 1 - 3 1.73 71 2 For both items, means, medians, and modes were equal. To create this index, scores on the two items were summed with any res spo ondents having non-responses on one or both. items recorded as non- resvcns cs on the final index. Socio-econcmic status: possession index -4 o)‘ P. H (I rr ‘3' he original survey asked respondents to estimate their family income, few of the respondents were able to do so. Thus, this index was created in an attempt to tap a more ecologically and economically based measure of socio-economic status than is provided by measures of occupational status (although a measure of occupational status is part of the final analysis in the present.study). Six items from the original survey asked respondents for indications of the size of their homes, age of their newest car, and so on. Two of the' original measures (kind of dwelling and frequency of maid service) were deleted because of the lack of variance. The wordings and codes for the remaining feur items are listed in Appendix A. Table 46 below repOrts the code ranges, means, standard deviations, and non-response counts for these four items. Table 47 reports the q correla tion matrix. Table A5 Code Ranges, Means, And Standard Deviations For The Four Items Used To Create The Socio—Economic Status Index Of Possessions Item Code range Meana s.d. Non—response # of rooms in household 03 - 17 9.17 2.57 1 # of phones in household 00 - 08 2.29 1.27 n # cars in family 00 - 04 1.50 1.65 0 age of newest car 00 - 12 8.83 2.80 8 a For all four items, means, medians, and modes were equal. Table 47: Matrix Of Pearson Product Moment Correlations Between The Four Items Used In Creating The Socio-economic Status Index Of Possessions (1) (2) (3) (u) I (1) 7 of rooms - {2} of pho.es .47 (3) a of cars .25 .36 — (9) age rewest car .27 .29 .46 - Because of the varying code ranges of the four items involved, each item was recoded to provide comparable ranges. To accomplish this, a median split was made on the four items and low values were recoded to 0, high values to 1. summed to compute the final index. These recoded values of each item were then Respondents with a non-response on one item had the mean value for that item (recoded per the median split) added to his final index score. response were reCorded as non-responses on the final index. Respondents with more than one non- APPENDIX C CODE RAISES, MEANS, STANDARD DEVIATIONS, AND NON-RESPONSE CO TITS LISTED BY VARIABLE CATEGORY AND VARIABLE NAME. . .11 as cede IJTjCS, means, standard dev1ations, and non—response counts, listed by variable category and variable name. Code Number of Variable category and name Range MeanC s.d. non-responses Parent 7“elia use Amount of parent television viewing 0- 3 2 0.8 0 Variety of parent te ev ision viewing 0- 8 3 1.5 2 Number of TV sets in household 0- 2 2a 0.5 3 Amount of parent radio listening 0- 3 2b 0.8 0 Variety of pa mr tradio use 0- 6 2 1.3 u Number of ne wsprers subscribed to ---- --- --- ---- Version 1: weeklies 0- 5 l 0.7 0 Version 2: dailies 0- 4 l 0.7 O Xu.ber of magazines parents read 0- 8 3 1.6 5 Pa rcnta l sources of news 1- 2 l .5 1 number of phonographs owned 0- 8 2 1.2 2 F"‘“*”“* “od‘a Use .'snnt cf,t pendent radio use 0- H 2b 1.1 u Variety of respondent radio use 0— 5 1a 0.9 6 Vaflvfy of res mp endent newspaper use 0- 8 5 1.7 l _ “fer of magazines respondent reads 0- 8 2 1.6 8 Variety of respondent book preferences l-ll 4 1.9 l Penjezi;:nt nreference for comics l- 2 l 0.5 l Resp one nt library use 0- 2 l 0.6 5 Fresno: ey respondent movie going 0- u 2b 0.9 2 Variety respondent record preferences 0- 7 3 l.” l Nadia respondent would miss most l- 2 2 0.5 R spo ondent media credibility ratings —--- --- --- ---— Version 1: Most believed media 1- 2 2 0.5 2 Version 2: Least believed media l- 2 l 0.3 2 Family Cohesiveness Parental permissiveness ---- --- --- ---- Index l: restriction on hours l-l6 9 3.5 33 Index 2: knowledge R whereabouts 3- 6 3 0.7 l Child-parent communication index l- 7 6b 1.2 0 Fa: ily togetherness index l-ll 7 1.9 13 Parent orientation 0- 2 l 0.9 17 Parent domination ---- --- --- ---- Version 1: who pays bills 0- 2 l 0.5 3 Version 2: who pays allowance 0- 2 l 0.6 12 Reependent' 3 home responsibilities index 0—26 1” u.o 3 Acapondcnt's kn nowledge of family Operation index 0- H l 1.2 0 Iarent-child agreement on TV shows 0- 4 lb 0.9 4 dhile all variables are measured continuouslv. the annrenriate measure 0F Code Number of :ri:.1e cats o‘y and name Range Mean s.d. Non-responses Co*men 1' '"“‘f2:cld‘ Parent gregarious.css index 2- 7 5b 1.1 9 Respondent gro~111 usncss --—- --- --— -—-- Vex sion 1: or; aniz ations 1- 7 2 1.3 7 Version 2: peer index 3- 8 6 1.2 3 Outs ideehome orientation index 7—19 13 2.3 4 Length of time in community 0-14 7b 4.6 13 Frequency of attendance at church index 2- 5 5a 1.0 13 Respondent knowledge local 8 state figures 0- 8 4 1.9 2 Self Orientation banner ofr espondent hobbies 0- 6 2b l.u 5 Nimber of pa arent hobbies 0- 6 lb 0.9 29 Respondent knowledge level 5 ---- --- --- ---- Version 1: TV characters 1- 2 l 0.5 2 Version 2: Ad slogans 0-10 7a 1.9 2 Veruibn 3: Math index 0- 5 2b 1.“ l8 Respondent's outside home employment 1- 3 2b 0.7 2 In so“ of hours respondent studies 0- 8 3 1.9 18 Freqtercz respondent studies in library 0- 4 l 1.1 2 3:1: “or Or_cntation ”ether or money worries ---- --- --- ---- Ver51on 1: parents 0- 5 1 0.9 7 Vcr31on 2: respondent 0— 8 l 1.5 u Spend-save orientations ---- --- —-- ---- Version 1: parents 1- 3 2 0.9 7 Version 2: respondent 1- 3 3a 0.7 10 Family use of credit 0- 3 1b 1.1 8 Attitude toward credit 1- 3 2 0.6 u Vari iety of respondent spending 0-15 7b 3.1 l Pam y shopping orientation index 2- 6 3b 1.3 8 central tendency depends, of course, on the shape of the distribution of r: pentes. Those “mean” values follo~ed by a subscript "a” actually represent :h mole; and median value. Those "mean" values followed by a subscript J represent the ”I :3 mean and median value. edians, and modes were the same. For all other variables, Code Range Mean s.d. Number of Non-reSponses lenders“ v Socio-economic status Version 1: Version 2: Family Size Hother's employment status F‘nily Type Birth orcer Sex occupational prestige possession index 0- u 24.2-81.4 Moon 50 9.8 Ol—‘OOOi—‘F‘ .p HICHIGQN STQTE UNIV. LIBRQRIES MW 102 Illllllill |||||l||||| lill‘llH ll 93 69 2 7574