R£OOGNITION OF SEX DIFFERENCES BY CHILDREN AGES NINE MONTRS TO THREE YEARS Thesis for 9116 Dogma cf M. A. MECHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY Eat-ham Sue Gibson 1965 SSSSSS ' WWW ABSTRACT RECOGNITION OF SEX DIFFERENCES BY CHILDREN AGES NINE MONTHS TO THREE YEARS By Barbara Sue Gibson Social interactionists propose that sexual distinctions are defined socially as well as biologically, and these dis- tinctions are learned through a process of social interaction. The focus of this study was to investigate young children's accuracy in distinguishing between the sexes. For the present study, three sets of sex differentiating criteria were selected, namely, sex-appropriate clothing, primary sex characteristics and secondary sex characteristics. The child's age, sex, social status and family characteristics were considered in relationship to the criteria employed in differentiating between the sexes. The relative age at which each criteria was differentiated was also investigated. Three sets of male and female dolls representing the three sets of criteria were employed to measure the children's accuracy in recognition of sex differences. The sets of dolls were presented to the children in a series of three scenes. In Scene I, the dolls were sex-appropriately dressed according to the currently accepted fashion. In Scene II, the dolls were undressed. In Scene III, the dolls were dressed identically Barbara Sue Gibson in slacks and shirts with hair and cosmetic differences according to current fashion. As each scene was presented, the child was asked to choose the "mommy" then the "daddy" doll. A questionnaire was deveIOped in order to obtain certain background information from the parents of the children inter- viewed. The instruments wereadministed to 73 children and the ir mothers . The hypothesis that the ages of the children would be positively related to accuracy in distinguishing between the sexes was supported by the data. The children of the study were found to accurately differentiate between the sexes as early as 12 months on the basis of sex-appropriate clothing, 1h months on the basis of secondary sex characteristics and 16 months on the basis of primary sex characteristics. All the children of the sample 30 months or older made accurate distinctions between the sexes on all the criteria. The hypothesis that the criteria of clothing differences would be used for distinguishing between the sexes at a younger age than the primary or secondary sex characteristics was supported. It can be seen from the information presented above that secondary and primary sex characteristics were employed only slightly later than differences in clothing. The hypothesis that social class would be negatively re- lated to accuracy in recognition of sex differences was re- Jected by the data. Contradictory to the hypothesis, social class was found to be significantly and positively related to Barbara Sue Gibson accuracy in sex recognition on the basis of sex-apprOpriate clothing. Social class was positively but not significantly related to accuracy in differentiating between the sexes on the basis of primary and secondary sex characteristics. The hypothesis that the sex of the child would be related to accuracy in recognition of sex differences was not supported by the data. In the overall accuracy of sex recognition, only slight differences were found between the sexes. The sex of the respondent's siblings was found to make no significant difference in the respondent's accuracy in sex differentiation of the three criteria investigated. Thus, the hypothesis that sex of siblings would affect accuracy of sex recognition was rejected by the data. RECOGNITION OF SEX DIFFERENCES BY CHILDREN AGES NINE MONTHS TO THREE YEARS BY Barbara Sue Gibson A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Textiles, Clothing and Related Arts 1965 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The writer owes a special debt of gratitute to L. Edna Rogers for her encouragement, assistance and criticisms throughout this investigation. Her help in the present study can never be adequately acknowledged. Sincere appreciation is extended to Dr. Mary Gephart, Dr. Elinor Nugent, Stephania Winkler, Barbara Loder and \ Eleanor Mullikin for their help and suggestions during the investigation. The writer is grateful to the Agricultural Experiment Station of Michigan State University for financial support of the research project. Cooperation extended by the children and_parents of the Jonesville Community School District is greatly appreciated. Finally, the writer wishes to acknowledge the members of her family, Patrick, Betty, Dianne, and Virginia Gibson for their time, efforts, and encouragement in the collection of the data and duplication of the thesis. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS ‘ Page ACKNOWLEDGMENTS LIST OF TABLES. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv Chapter I. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Statement of the Problem. . . . . . . . . . 5 II. REVIEW OF LITERATURE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15 III. METHODOLOGY. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17 Selection and Development of the Measure- ment Techniques . . . . . . . . . . . . 17 Selection of the Sample . . . . . . . . . . 21 Description of the Sample . . . . . . . . . 23 Pretest of the Instrument . . . . . . . . . 31 Administration of the Instrument. . . . . . 32 IV. FINDINGS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 Age and Recognition of Sex Differences. . . 38 Social Class and Recognition of Sex Dif— ferences. . . . . NS Sex of the Child and Recognition of Sex Differences . . N7 Sex Distinguishing Criteria and Recognition of Sex Differences. . . . 50 Family Characteristics and Recognition of Sex Differences . . . . . . . . 52 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58 V. SUMMARY, IMPLICATIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS. . . . 60 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 Implications. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65 Recommendations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66 BIBLIOGRAPHY. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67 APPENDIX A- Photographed Dolls. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70 APPENDIX 8- Interview Schedule. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75 iii Page APPENDIX C— Background Information Questionnaire . . . . . 78 APPENDIX D- Explanatory Letter . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 APPENDIX E- Age Distribution of the Sample by Number of Errors Made on Scene IV . . . . . . 8h iv Table 10. ll. 12. LIST OF TABLES Number by Sex and Percentage Distribution and Age . Number by Sex and Percentage Distribution and Sibling Composition Number and Percentage Distribution Sex and Social Class Level . . Number and Percentage Distribution Sex and Father's Education . . . . Number and Percentage Distribution Sex and Mother's Education . . . . Number and Percentage Distribution Sex and Mother's Ages. . . . . . . Number and Percentage Distribution Sex and Mother's Employment. . Number and Percentage Distribution Sex and Mother's Religion. . . . . Number and Percentage Distribution of the Sample of the Sample of the Sample of the Sample of the Sample of the Sample of the Sample of the Sample of the Sample Sex and Mother's Religious Participation . . . Number and Percentage Distribution Sex and Place Mother Lived Most of Number and Percentage Distribution Sex and Present Place of Residence of the Sample Her Life. of the Sample by by by by by Page 2h 2h . 26 27 27 28 . 29 29 30 30 31 Test-Retest Reliability Coefficients of Correlations Between Sex Recognition Errors Made on Criteria in Scenes I, pending Criteria in Scene IV by Sex and Scene. II and III and Errors Made on the Corres- 35 LIST OF TABLES - Continued Table 13. 11+. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 21+- 25. 26. Page Correlations Between Months of Age and Number of Sex Recognition Errors by Sex and Criteria . . . . 39 Age Distribution of the Sample by the Number of Sex Recognition Errors Made on the Criteria of Sex-Appropriate Clothing . . . . . . . . . . . . . N1 Age Distribution of the Sample by the Number of Sex Recognition Errors Made on the Criteria of Primary Sex Characteristics. . . . . . . . . . . . R2 Age Distribution of the Sample by the Number of Sex Recognition Errors Made on the Criteria of Secondary Sex Characteristics. . . . . . . . . . . NB Age Distribution of the Sample by the Total Number of Sex Recognition Errors Made on All Scenes . . . uh Correlations Between North-Hatt Social Class Ratings and Number of Sex Recognition Errors by Sex and Criteria . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . N6 Mean Sex Recognition Errors Made by the Sexes on Each Scene and On All Scenes . . . . . . . . . . . u? Percentage of Each Sex Making Sex Recognition Errors by Scene and On All Scenes. . . . . . . . . M9 Absolute and Medal Zero Sex Recognition Error Points for Boys and Girls by Criteria. . . . . . . 50 Analysis of Variance of Sex Recognition Errors by Scene . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 Mean Age Expressed in Months and Mean Total Sex Recognition Error by Sibling Composition . . . . . 53 Mean Age Expressed in Months and Mean Total Sex Recognition Error by Mother's Employment . . . . . Sh Mean Age Expressed in Months and Mean Total Sex Recognition Error by Girl's Usual Dress. . . . . . 55 Mean Age Expressed in Months and Mean Total Sex Recognition Error by Mother's Usual Dress. . . . . 56 vi LIST OF TABLES - Continued Table Page 27. Mean Age Expressed in Months and Mean Total Sex Recognition Error by Present Place of Residence . . S7 28. Mean Age Expressed in Months and Mean Total Sex Recognition Error by Stage in Toilet Training . . . 58 vii CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Physical differences between the sexes are biological givens which function across all cultures to place individuals into basic role categories. Males and females form distinct, biologically different, categories; yet maleness and female- Lrness are fairly arbitrary matters. From the great arc of human potentialities even in the area of sex differentiation, each society selects certain segments of behavior and ignores others.1 The behaviors Eppropriate for males in one society ihmay contain certain behaviors that are exclusively appropriate for females in another society.2 Sexual behavior is, indeed, a social as well as a physical phenomenon. Furthermore, sexual identification relies heavily on social clues rather than biological characteristics. In categorizing others into sex roles a wide variety of symbols, such as clothing, hair style, voice, and manner of behavior, are employed. Sexual identification is not an automatic, or instinctual matter; it relies on social learning. The ability 1Ruth Benedict, Patterns of Culture (New York: Mentor Books. 19h6), P- 35. I 2Margaret Mead, Male and Female (New Ybrk: Mentor Books, 1955). p- 15. 1 of a young child to distinguish between the sexes, which is perhaps the first major social categorizing he accomplishes, involves some degree of cognition. At birth a human infant is not really "human". He in- herits certain biological characteristics, such as sex, pig- mentation, potential for physical and intellectual development; but he does not yet possess the essential human quality of re- sponding to others in the manner expected by members of the society into which he is born.3 It is only when the child is aware that others are expecting certain behaviors from him, that he becomes "human" in the sense defined by his culture. An infant with adequate biological inheritance has the potential for learning the cultural patterns of any society.h The behaviors that an infant acquires are the ones that are presented to him in his social milieu. Through interaction with "culture interpreters", i.e., "agents of‘socialization"S or "significent others"6, the child's behavior is modified to conform to the expectations held by his fellow society members. By means of this process-~the process of socialization7--the infant gradually learns the ways of his society so that he can 3Robert E. Park, Principles of Human Behavior (Chicago: The Zalaz Corporation, 1915), p. 9} hFrederick Elkin, The Child and Society. The Process of Socialization (New York: RandbmgHouse, 1960), pf7. 5Ibid., p. us. 6George Herbert Mead, Mind, Self and Society (Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, l93uf, pfh2. 7This process is not confined to childhood but operates throughout an individual's life. function within it.8 The significance of this discussion of socialization is that all identities, as well as sexual identity,which is the concern of this study, are socially defined, socially sustained and socially transformed. "In other words, identity is not something 'given', but is bestowed in acts of social recogni- tion. We become that as which we are addressed."9 Unaware of the sex category into which he is born, an in- fant learns to identify himself with a particular sex through interaction with those around him. His parents, his chief agents of socialization, define the situation for him by be- having towards him according to their conception of the cultural patterns appropriate for dealing with his sex. "As soon as the child has free motion and begins to pull, tear, pry, meddle, and prowl, the parents begin to define the situation through speech and other signs and pressures: 'Be quiet','Sit up straight', 'Blow your nose', 'Wash your face', 'Mind your mother',‘Be kind to sister', etc."lo Furthermore, by selected methods of dressing and grooming their offspring, parents im- pose, to a large degree. the infant's appearance upon him. 8Elkin, op. cit., p. h. 9Peter L. Berger, Invitation to Sociolo y: A Humanistic Perspective (New York: DoubIeday & Cempany, nc., I9637, p.99, 10W. I. Thomas, "Definition of the Situation" in Lewis Coser and Bernard Rosenberg, eds., Sociological Theory: A Book of Readings (New York: The MacMillan Company, 19577, p. 210. u Stone refers to appearance imposing behavior on the part of parents as "investiture". As Stone says, "...dressing the child in blue invests the child with masculinity; in pink with femininity. In this way, the responses of the world toward the child are differentially mobilized. CIn other words, the " appearance of the child provides clues of his sex to members of the society so that they can behave toward him in a manner appropriate to his sex.11) The world handles the pink-clad and the blue-clad child differently. The pink-clad child is identified differently. It is 'darling', 'beautiful','sweet', or 'graceful'; the blue-clad child is 'handsome', 'strong', or 'agile'. At a very early age the investiture of the child provides the materials out of which the reflected sexual id- entity and its qualifications are formed."12 From cultural pressures a child comes to identify himself -— with one of the sex categories; society does not allow him to remain uncommitted. "He cannot, at least publicly, identify himself with both or neither."13 He must make a choice. Lindesmith and Strauss outline three phases involved in sex identification: (1) recognition of sex differences, (2) learning the meanings of "male" and "female", and (3) V I I 11It is interesting to note how embarrassed one becomes when he misidentifies the sex of even a small baby. 12Gregory P. Stone, "Appearance and the Self," in Arnold M. Rose, ed. Human Behavior and Social Processes, (Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 19627: p. 105. 13Alfred‘R. LindeSmith and Anslem L. Strauss, Social Ps cholo Revised edition (New York: Henry Holt an n3 Company, Inc., IREZ), p. 321. S identification with one or the other.1u Certainly the first step in this process is a crucial one and it is this phase of sex-identification that is the focus of this study. Stone supports the importance of the recognition of sex differences by pointing out that, "The term 'identification' subsumes at least two processes: identification of and identification with . . . identification with one another, in whatever mode, 15 cannot be made without identification of one another." Statement of the Problem The major purposes of this study are to determine: (1) the age at which recognition of sex differences occurs and (2) the criteria used to discriminate between the sexes. The criteria used in this study for measuring sexual distinctions includes: (1) sex-linked clothes (clothing considered by our culture to be appropriate for the two separate sexes), (2) primary sex characteristics (genitals and breasts), and (3) secondary sex characteristics (hair and cosmetics). Since parents and siblings are the chief socialization agents in our society this study will also investigate the in- fluence of certain family characteristics in relation to child- ren's recognition of sex differences. The specific assumptions, objectives, and hypotheses guiding this study are as follows: 1”Ibis. 15Stone, op. cit., p. 90. Assumptions: 1. A number of criteria are involved in recognition of sex differences. The measuring instrument involving dressed and un- dressed dolls with primary and secondary sex charac- ‘ teristicS' provide a means by which the researcher may discover a child's awareness of sex differences. Objectives: 1. To determine at what age children are able to recog- nize sex differences. To determine the relationship between social class and the ability to recognize sex differences. To determine the relative importance of the differ- ent criteria utilized to recognize sex differences. General Hypotheses: 1. The age of the child will be positively related to recognition of sex differences. The social class of the child will be negatively re- lated to recognition of sex differences. The sex of the child will be related to recognition of sex differences. The type of sex distinguishing criteria will be re- lated to recognition of sex differences. Family characteristics will be related to recognition of sex differences. From these general hypotheses flow various empirical hypo- theses which were tested and reported in Chapter IV. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURE The first chapter introduced the framework of role theory which has provided the theoretical basis for this study. As previously stated, there are many aspects of sex-role learning. The first step in the process, awareness of sex differences, is the concern of the present investigation. Although several studies have been made of children's sex socialization, most researchers considered the aspects of sex- role preference and adoption. Only a few investigators have considered the step of recognition of sex differences by child- ren. Possibly sex recognition has been overlooked by research- ers because it was considered to be a given rather than a part of the sex-role identification process. Perhaps the first systematic contribution of knowledge about the age at which children recognize sex differences was made by Gesell and Ilg.1 From observations of hundreds of nursery school children, Gesell and Ilg developed a growth gradients chart to report the average age at which evidence of various learnings had been exhibited by children. A summary of their findings concerned with recognition of sex differen- ces is presented below by six month age intervals. "’ *7 vifiw fl '7‘ lArnold L. Gesell and Frances L. Ilg, Infant and Child in the Culture of Today (New York: Harper & Bros., PubliShers,l9u3). 7 8 Between ten and twelve months some children were observed handling their genitals when their clothes were off. At eigh- teen months the children made no verbal distinction between boys and girls, and all men and women were still referred to as "daddy" and "mommy". Twenty-four month old children called themselves by their own names. At this age they Could distinguish between boys and girls by the type of clothes and style of haircut. By the age of thirty months, the children distinguished between their mother, "mommy", and other "ladies" and between their father, "daddy", and other men. Other children were verbally distinguished as "boys" and "girls". At this age, the children knew which sex they were. Thirty month old children were interested in watching others in the bathroom, and they noticed differences in the urinating posture of men and boys as compared to that of women and girls. This dif- ference became a basic for distinguishing males from females, but it was not verbalized.2 By thirty-Six months of age, children verbalized about physiological differences between the sexes and about different postures for urinating. Conn and Kanner3 focused their research attention more specifically on children's recognition of sex differences. fiV— ‘Y‘ —v—r—v—w ———rv 2The author could not determine from the report given how this was measured. 3Jacob H. Conn and Leo Kanner, "Children's Awareness of SexlDéfferences", Journal of Child Psychiatry, Vol. I (19u7), pp. - - ‘ I ‘ I *’ I'fi - They used a doll—play interview method to obtain information from children about many aspects of sex-role learning. Two hundred children, 128 boys and 72 girls, between the ages of four and twelve years were interviewed. The children were patients of the Pediatric Department at the Johns Hopkins Hospital who had IQ's ranging from 71 to 13u.h During the interview each child was asked to differentiate between two child dolls dressed in sex-appropriate clothing. The child wasa asked first to tell whether the doll waé a boy or girl, then to tell how he knew it was a boy or girl. The responses were recorded verbatim by a stenographer. An analysis of the responses dealing with sex differentiation revealed that of 200 children, 150 named inequalities of clothing as being sex discriminators, 116 mentioned genital differences, 93 noticed differences in hair style, uh mentioned differences in urinating posture, and nine commented about nipples and breasts.S As a part of the above study, Conn interviewed each respondent's mother to obtain information about factors in j ‘r ~r “The mean IQ was 95 since slightly more than half the sample had below "normal" IQ's. The intelligence level of the sample limits the degree to which the findings may be generalized. 5Conn and Kanner summarized that hair differences were used as sex distinguishing criteria at a younger age than were clothing differences. However, these findings were based on a sample of five four-year-olds and fourteen five- year olds.- None of the four-year-olds mentioned clothing differences, but one mentioned hair differences. Three five- year-olds mentioned clothing differences and two mentioned hair differences. 10 the child's experience which might have affected his aware— ness of sex differences. Conn, from this information, hypothe- sized four types of factors affecting a child's awareness of sex differences. The first factor was the child's language development and his growing sensitivity about language des- cribing sexual body parts and sexual behavior. The second factor was the varying interest in body parts and sexual be- havior at different ages. The third factor was specific sexual experiences, 6.3., the opportunity to observe others or a new baby in the family. Lastly, the develOping aware- ness of certain genital sensations which encourage the child to explore the genital area was postulated to be a factor affecting a child's consciousness of sex differences. Children's accuracy in recognizing the sex of their age peers was investigated by Brieland and Nelson.6 The criteria upon which the child based his identification were also con- sidered. A sample of 90 children between the ages of two and I one—half and seven years was selected from children enrolled in University of Minnesota demonstration schools. Paired photographs of children unknown to the respondents were used to measure the recognition of sex differences. Ten photo- graphs of preschool children were paired so that a boy and a girl were presented to each of the following five categories: 6Donald Brieland and Lillian Nelson, "Age Trends in Sex Identification as Determined by a Picture Test", American Psychologist, Vol. VI, No. 7 (July, 1951), p. 309. (KESEract). 11 (l) clothes; (2) nude, half view front; (3) nude, back view; (A) nude, full view front; and (5) nude, full view front with sex inappropriate hair style and body type. As each photo- graph was shown to a child individually, he was asked whether the photograph was of a boy or a girl. Relevant comments _ were recorded. Brieland and Nelson's findings were as follows: (1) The number of errors made in the identification of each pair of photographs decreased as the age of the respondent increased. (2) Three times as many errors were made on the sex-inappropriate pair of pictures as on the four other pairs. (3) Comments indicated that hair style was the dominant sex differentiating criterion for the children of every age.7 (u) The genitals were an increasingly important sex criterion for the older children. Katcher8 employed a similar method for measuring the dis- crimination of sex differences. His sample consisted of 266 nursery and elementary school children ranging in age from three to nine years. The sample was selected from two socio- economic groups with 116 subjects in the "Higher" socio- economic group and 150 in the "Lower" group. The research instrument measuring Sex recognition was composed of sex-paired drawings illustrating segments of a —fi 7Four out of five of the photographs showed nude indivi- duals which perhaps accounts for the mention of other criteria more often than clothing. ‘ 8Allan Katcher, "The Discrimination of Sex Differences by Young Children", Journal of Genetic Psychology, Vol. LXXXVII (1955). ‘pp. 131-1113. I l2 clothed and nude adults and children. Each pair of drawings was identical in every respect except the single sex differ- entiating variable of hair, clothing, breasts, or external genitals. As each child was shown a pair of segments, he was asked to discriminate between the male and female. Katcher found the following: (1) The subject's age was the most important variable influencing the number of errors made. Errors on all cues decreased as age increased. (2) The least number of errors was made in identifying the sex differentiat- ing characteristics of clothing. Next in order of errors were hair, genitals, and breasts. (3) Fewer errors were made on child and same-sex cues than on adult and oppositeysex cues. (H) No significant differences were observed in the total errors made by boys as compared to girls; however, younger girls were significantly more accurate than younger boys in differentia- ting both boys' and girls' genital cues. (5) No significant differences were found between the socio-economic groups, yet in all age groups the higher sooio-economic groups were more accurate than the lower ones. (6) There were no significant differences between the errors made by one—parent and by two- parent children. Similarly, having opposite sex siblings had no special effect upon the number of errors made in discrimi- nating Opposite-sex genital cues. (7) Stanford-Binet IQ were available for MB of the 266 respondents. For those RB there was no significant relationship between IQ and number of errors made. 13 Baxter9 studied 120 nursery school children in East Lansing, Michigan between the ages of two and one-half and five years. A doll-play interview was deve10ped by a committee of two sociologists, Dr. Arthur Vener and Dr. Joanne Eicher, and two graduate assistants, Hazel Ogilvie Baxter and Audray Weese. The measuring technique consisted of questions about three sets of adult male and female dolls exhibiting the following characteristics: (1) nudity, showing primary sex characteristics; (2) sex-apprOpriate dress; and (3) sex- inappropriate dress. As each set was shown individually to a child, he was asked to choose the "mommy" and the "daddy" doll and to tell how he knew the difference. The choices anH responses were recorded by a second person. No errors in choice of dolls were made when the doll's showing nudity or sex-appropriate dress were presented. Twenty-six of the 120 children made errors in choice between the dells dressed in sex-appropriate clothing. Yeunger children showed a slightly greater tendency to make errors than the older children. There seemed to be no relationship between the child's sex and his ability to distinguish between the sexes. Baxter determined the relative importance of the criteria by noting the frequency with which they were mentioned by the children. An analysis of the responses "showed that when the child was 9Hazel Ogilvie Baxter. "Sex Identification by Children Ages Two and One- Half to Five Years", (Unpublished Master's thesis, Dept. of Textiles, Clothing and Related Arts, Michigan State University, l96u.) 1'4 presented with the nude dolls, the primary sex characteristics were the most important indicators of sex differences for the children in the sample; and when presented with the clothed dolls, clothing became the most important distinguishing criteria."10’11 In all scenes, differences between hair style and cosmetics were the second most important criteria and differences in general body configuration were the least im- portant criteria. Baxter found that children without siblings tended to make more errors than children with either same or Opposite sex siblings, but almost no difference existed be- tween the mean number of errors made by the children with same and children with opposite sex siblings. No reference was made to the ages of the children in the three sibling categories.12 In an exploratory study of very young children's sex learnings, Hartman and Purse13 interviewed a quota sample of 50 children between ten and twenty-nine months of age who were living in married housing units on the Michigan State University campus. The dolls showing nudity and sex- apprOpriate dress of Baxter's instrument were used to measure 10Ib1d., p. as. 11This finding is not surprising in view of the logic that the most obvious difference would be mentioned most frequently. 12Baxter, op.cit., p. 28. 13Catherine Hartman and Marilee Purse, "Sex Identifi- cation by Children Under Two and One-Half Years", (Unpub- lished term paper, Department of Textiles, Clothing and Re- lated Arts, Michigan State University, l96u). 15 accuracy of sex differentiation. The children were asked to tell which was the "mommy" and which the "daddy" of each set and how they knew the differences. They found that of the 28 children who were over 21 months only four made errors in differentiating between the sexes; these four were boys. Twelve months was the earliest age at which any indication ' was made of recognition of sex differences. When the child- ren were presented with the sex-appropriately dressed dolls, clothing differences were mentioned most frequently and hair style differences were mentioned next often. When presented with nude dolls, primary sex characteristics were mentioned most frequently and again, hair differences next often. No differences between the boys' and girls' sex distinquishing responses were evident from the sample. Summary Relatively little research has been done concerning the recognition of sex differences by young children, even though writers agree that this is a crucial learning in the child's development. Based on the research that has been done, clothing was found to be one of the most important criteria children used for distinguishing between the sexes. Other criteria men- tioned most often by children were hair style, primary sex characteristics and urinating posture. 16 Age was positively related to accuracy in recognizing sex differences. Contradictory findings were reported con- cerning the relationship between the respondent's sex and his accuracy in recognizing sex differences. In several of the studies, various background factors were hypothesized to have an influence upon accuracy; however, neither sex of siblings nor number of parents present seemed to affect the children's accuracy in differentiating between the sexes. CHAPTER III METHODOLOGY The concern of this chapter is to clarify the measure- ments involved in the present study and to describe the sample selection procedures and the characteristics of the sample. These methodological considerations will be presented in the following order: (1) selection and development of the measurement techniques, (2) description of the community set- ting, (3) selection of the sample, (H) description of the sample, (5) protest of the instrument, and (6) administration of the instrument. Selection and Development of the Measurement Techniques Sex Recognition Instrument Baxter's study,1 a predecessor to the present one, successfully employed a doll-play technique which consisted of asking children to distinguish between male and female dolls and to tell which criteria they employed in making the distinctions. Other investigators, including Ammons,2 lBaxter, Op. cit. 2R. B. Ammons, "Reactions in a Projective Doll-Play Interview of White Males Two to Six Years of Age to Differences in Skin Color and Facial Features", Journal of Genetic PsychOIOgy, Vol. LXXVI (1950), pp. 323-3ul. 1? 18 Conn3, and Hartman and Purse“, have also had success in using dolls with children to obtain responses about sex-role sociali- zation. Since this study is an extension of Baxter's work, the initial development of the instrument has been done.5 But be- cause the children of the present study were younger than those studied by Baxter, the instrument needed to be modified slightly. The main limitation on the use of an interview method with young children is their limited ability to com- municate. Young children have a larger passive than active 6 vocabulary , i.e., they can understand more language than they can express verbally themselves. This makes it possible to ask children questions which they can understand but not answer verbally. The ability of young children by the age of twelve 7 months to give or take objects on command enables the use of an interview method which consists of the movement of objects in response to verbal commands. hf va— 3Jacob Conn, "The Child Speaks to the Psychiatrist: An Introduction to the Method of the Interview", Occupational Therapy, Vol. XVII, pp. 231-2uu. LLHartman and Purse, 0p. cit. 5The present study is part of a larger study of "Sex-Role Identification by Pro-School Children" supported by the Agri- cultural Experiment Station of Michigan State University. 6Dorothea McCarty, "Language Development in Children", in L. Carmichael, ed., Manual of Child Psychology, 2nd ed. (New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 19H6I, pfFE67. 7Gesell and Ilg, op. cit., p. 3H2. l9 Caucasoid models of commercially produced, molded, rubber dolls with sex characteristics were used in the study. They were described by the manufacturer as: Rubber doll family with sex features.... Complete body details, including sex features. Weighted feet make them stand readily. Wired from head to foot. Washable. Fully clothed. Easy to dress and undress. These rubber dolls can be bent and will hold their pose in any position. Used extensively by trained thera- pists in child guidance clinics and by child psychiatrésts to reveal difficult emotional problems. In four different "scenes" presented to the children, a total of three male and three female dolls were used. Each of the first three scenes employed two dolls, one male, one female, and one main set of criteria for distinguishing between the sexes.9 In Scene I the criteria used were tradi- tional male and female clothing. The dolls' hair and facial features were painted to look the same. The male doll was clothed in a suit; the female in a dress. Painted on "shoes" were also a discriminating factor with the male doll's shoes being larger than the female's. Scene II presented the criteria of primary sex characteristics. The dolls' hair and facial features were, again, painted to look alike. The "shoes" that had been painted on by the manufacturer were painted over so the dolls were completely undressed. In Scene 111 the criteria shown for distinguishing between the sexes were hair J L v—fi , —..,.— Viv—7‘7 fiv 8Creative Playthings, Inc., Catalog, Princeton, New Jersey, 1960, p. 26. C)In all scenes, the dolls' body structures were slightly different for the sexes. 20 and cosmetics. The female doll was given a wig and feminine facial cosmetics. The male doll's hair was painted in a popular masculine fashion. Both the doll's shoes were painted like the male doll's shoes in Scene I. They were dressed identically in blue slacks and white, long—sleeved shirts. Scene IV employed all six dolls used in Scenes 1, II, and III. The scenes are illustrated in Appendix A. The interview consisted Of asking the child as each scene was shown to choose the "mommy" doll. After the child responded, the dolls were placed together again and the child was asked to choose the "daddy" doll. This same procedure was followed for all the scenes. Clear, positive evidence, indicated by taking, point- ing, or verbally identifying, was required for a correct choice score to be given. NO response was considered tO be an error. In each scene the child's choice was limited by the set Of criteria present in the scene, thereby reducing the necessity for verbalization as to what was the basis of the choice. The interview schedule is presented in Appendix B. Backgpgund Information Instrument Certain factors in the child's environment such as the - Opportunity to Observe nudity and the type Of dress the child and mother wear, as well as parental characteristics such as education and religious affiliation and participation were hypothesized to be influential factors affecting the child's 21 sex-recognition responses. It also seemed logical that cer- tain developmental accomplishments of the child, e.g., toilet training, would be variables influencing the responses. A questionnaire was developed to gain information about these conditions and other background characteristics of the inter-- viewee and his family. See Appendix C for the questionnaire. Selection of the Sample As stated before, this study is a follow up of the Baxter study. The sample of the present study differs in that it is composed of younger age children with a wider range of social classes than were included in the Baxter study. In view of previous findings, it did not seem necessary to include in the sample children above the age» Of three years since they have been found at this age to discriminate between the sexes. On the other hand, nine months was believed to be the lowest age at which a child could understand the interview questions and communicate his doll choice. Thus the range of age selected for this study was nine months to three years. The community from which the sample was selected was Jonesville, Michigan, located in Hillsdale County in southern central Michigan. According to the 1960 census, persons in Hillsdale County of age twenty-five and ever had a median Of 11.0 years of schooling as compared to 10.8 years for the 22 state Of Michigan. The median age of Hillsdale County resi- dents was 28.2 years while 28.3 years was the median age for the state. Twenty-two and two tenths percent of the popula- tion moved into their present house in Hillsdale County after 1958. The mobility rate was twenty-one and seven tenths per— cent for the state of Michigan. Forty-three percent Of the employed males were farmers. Farm laborers and factory workers comprised another thirty-nine percent. The median Hillsdale County income was $u,9u0 per year, while the median state income was $6,256 per year. 'Twenty~six and six tenths 'percent Of the Hillsdale County residents, as compared to fifteen and seven tenths percent Of the state residents, had incomes under $3,000 per year. Only nine and eight tenths percent Of the Hillsdale County residents, as compared to seventeen and four tenths percent of the state residents had incomes over $10,000 per year. The median age, education and mobility rate for the residents of the community chosen for this study approximated the medians for the state. How— even, the median income for Hillsdale County residents was less than the median for the state. A list Of families with children between the ages of nine months and three years was Obtained from the Jonesville Community School District Census collected in May, l96u. Only Caucasoid children were included in the pOpulation. The families were stratified according to the ages of the children. A systematic sampling was done Of the age-strati- fied list Of families. One hundred thirty-five familes were 23 selected, giving 157 children in the sample since some families had more than one child within the desired age range. Of the 135 families selected, 33 refused to parti- cipatelo, 23 had moved out of town, four could not be con- tacted, six children or parents were seriously ill, two children were not the age reported in the census and hence fell outside our requirements, and one child did not speak English. Seventy-nine children from 66 families were inter- viewed. Of these 79, five were unOOOperative and one was reported by her mother tO have been mentally retarded. The final sample consisted Of 38 boys and 35 girls. Description Of the Sample The age and sex distribution of the sample is present- ed in Table 1. There was an approximately equal number of boys and girls in the sample. The boys were distributed quite evenly among all the age groups. However, seventy-four percent of the girls were concentrated in two age groups, 13 to 18 months and 25 to 30 months. f v— a? 7 fir W v. 10The sensitivity Of studies dealing with sex may account, in part, for the high rate Of refusals. 2h Table 1. Number and percentage distribution of the sample by sex and age Age in Months NO. % NO. % NO. 9-12 8 21 1 3 9 12 13-18 8 21 11 31 19 26 19-2u ' 5 13 2 6 7 10 25-30 7 18 15 H3 22 30 31-35 10 26 6 17 16 22 Totals 38 99* 35 100 73 100 *Due to rounding, the percentages do not add to 100 percent. As can be seen in Table 2, the sample exhibited a balanced distribution according to sibling composition. Table 2. Number and percentage distribution Of the sample by sex and sibling composition Boys Girls Total Sex of Siblings NO. % NO. % NO. No siblings 9 2H H 11 13 18 Sameésex siblings 13 3h 8 23 21 29 Opposite-sex siblings ' 5 13 15 N3 20 27 Both-sexes siblings ll 29 8 23 19 26 Totals 38 100 35' 100 73 100 25 Besides age and sex, social class was the other main independent variable. The child's social class rating was based on his father's occupation.11 Occupation has been found to be the best single index of Social class12, since it seems to be the one factor most highly correlated to all aspects of life style. The interpolated North-Hatt scale13 was used to give the occupations a numberical score. In addition, the sample was grouped according to Hatt's later findings about ranking Of occupationslu, i.e., the discovery that the continuum Of ranking was segmented. Three segments were represented in the present sample and will be designated as Upper Middle, Middle, and Lower Middle. The three social class levels were represented about equally by the sample. See Table 3. 11In two cases, due to divorce, the father was not present in the family. For these two cases, the social class level Of the child was based on the mother's occupation. Both mothers were working full-time in a factory. 12Joseph A. Kahl, The American Class Structure (New York: Rinehart & Company, Inc., 19577} ppi72-75. 13"The North-Hatt Scale" (interpolated by Russell Dynes, Ohio State University; Leslie Silverman, W. Roy Cook, and A. O. Haller, University of Wisconsin; Roy Buck, C. H. Brown, and George Lowe, Penn State University), (Mimeographed). 1Ll’Kahl, Op. cit., p.73. 26 Table 3. Number and percentage distribution Of the sample by sex and social class level m m Boys Girls Total Social Class Level NO. % - NO. % No. Lower middle uO-u9 2 5 1 3 3 M 50-59 1h 37 9 26 23 32 Middle 60-69 12 32 15 R3 27 37 Upper middle 70-79 9 2H 10 29 19 26 80-89 1 3 O O 1 1 Totals 38 101* 35 101* 73 100 *Due to rounding, the percentages do not add to 100 percent. The social class scores of those who refused to partici- pate in the study were very similar to those of the partici— pantslS‘ The mean social class score for the sample was 63, while the mean for the refusals was 6H. This indicates that the sample was not biased according to social class by the refusal Of certain respondents to participate in the study. The educational level Of the parents of the children in the study are presented in Tables u and 5. Three-fourths of the children's fathers and mothers had at least finished high school. An approximately equal number of mothers and fathers finished high school, but slightly more mothers than fathers had some further education beyond high school. 77 fir 15The father's occupation Of the refusals was Obtained from the summer l96u edition of the Hillsdale County Directory. 27 Table u. Number and percentage distribution of the sample by sex and father's education Boys Girls ' Total Education Completed NO. % NO. % NO. Some grammar school 1 3 l 3 2 3 Finished grammar school 1 3 3 9 u 5 Some high school 6 l6 6 17 12 16 Finished high school 22 58 21 60 H3 59 Some college or trade school A ll 3 9 7 10 Finished college 1 3 1 3 2 3 Some graduate school ' 3 8 O O 3 u Totals 38 102* 35 101* 73 100 *Due to rounding, the percentages do not add to 100 percent. Table 5. Number and percentage distribution of the sample by sex and mother's education Boys Girls Total Education Completed NO. % NO. % No. SOme grammar school 2 5 l 3 3 u Finished grammar school 1 3 l 3 2 3 Some high school 5 l3 7 2O 12 16 Finished high school 17 RS 20 57 37 51 Some college or tradeschoollj) 26 u 11 1h 19 Finished college 3 8 2 6 5 7 Some graduate school 0 O O O O O Yfi Totals 38 100 35 100 73 100 28 Further background information on the mothers of the children in the study is presented in Tables 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, and 11. With two exceptions, the children's mothers were between 21 and MO years Of age. Eighty-four percent of the mothers were full time homemakers. The great majority of mothers, eighty-two percent, were Protestant. Fourteen per— cent Of the mothers were Catholics. More than eighty per- cent of the mothers had lived most of their lives in a small town, village or farm. About fifty percent were currently living within the village limits of Jonesville and fifty per- cent in the surrounding rural area. Table 6. Number and percentage distribution of the sample by sex and mothers' ages Boys Girls Total Age in Years No. % NO. % No. Under 20 0 0 1 3 1 '1 21 - 30 3O 79 27 77 57 78 31 - no 8 21 o 17 1h 19 El — 50 ' 0 0 l 3 1 1 Totals 38 100 35 100 73 99* fl, *Due to rounding, the percentages do not add to 100 percent. 29 Table 7. Number and percentage distribution of the sample by sex and mother's employment Boys Girls Total Employment NO. No. % No. % Full-time homemaker 31 82 3O 86 61 8h Employed outside home 7 18 5 1h 12 16 Totals 38 100 35 100 73 100 Table 8. Number and percentage distribution of the sample by sex and mother's religion Boys Girls Total Religion NO. % NO. % NO. % Protestant 29 76 31 89 6O 82 Catholic 7 18 3 9 10 1h Jew O O O O O O Other 1 3 O O 1 1 None 1 3 l 3 2 3 Totals 38 100 35 101* 73 100 sDue to rounding, the percentages do not add to 100 percent. 30 Table 9. Number and percentage distribution Of the sample by sex and mother's religious participation Boys Girls Total Religious Participation NO. % ‘ NO. % NO. % Once per week 19 50 7 2O 26 36 Once or twice per month H 11 8 23 12 16 Sometimes 13 3H 18 51 31 H2 Never 2 '5 2 6 H 5 Totals 38 100 35 100 73 99* *Due to rounding, the percentages do not add to 100 percent. Table 10. Number and percentage distribution of the sample by sex and place mother lived most Of her life Boys Girls Total Place mother lived No. % NO. % No. % Farm or village (under 2,000) 16 H2 17 H9 33 H5 Small town (2,000 to 10,000) 11 29 15 H3 26 36 Small city (10,000 to 50,000) 1 3 O O 1 Medium city (50,000 to 200,000) 2 5 1 3 3 H Lar 6 city Tover 200,000) 8 21 2 6 10 1H Totals 38 100 35 101* 73 100 fi *Due to rounding, the percentages do not add to 100 percent. “'91 .3. r4 31 Table 11. Number and percentage distributiOn of the sample by sex and present place of residence M Boys- Girls Total Present Place Of Residence NO. % . N0. % NO. % Rural farm 7 l8 7 2O 1H 19 Rural non-farm 8 21 1H H0 22 30 Small town 23 61 1H H0 37 51 Totals 38 100 35 100 73 100 Pretest Of the Instrument Prior to the pretest of the instrument, the researcher and her adviser interviewed four children, ranging in age from one and one-half to three years, to acquire some initial experience in administering the instrument. 'This pro—pretest was conducted in Spartan Village on the Michigan State Univer- sity campus. This experience was helpful in refining the interview and questionnaire questions. The actual protest was conducted in Litchfield, Michigan, a town near Jonesville, where the study was done. Litchfield is only slightly smaller in population than Jonesville, and shares many of its characteristics. The occupations, median age, mobility rate, and median level of education of the Litchfield residents were essentially the same as those of the Jonesville residents. 32 The purposes of the pretest were (1) to test the instrument, (2) to provide a base of experience for the researcher, and (3) to investigate the willingness Of peOple in the area to participate in the study. Ten children be- tween the ages Of nine months and three years were randomly selected from the Litchfield School District Census which was taken in May, 196H. 0f the ten children selected for the pretest,“four were interviewed. Two children's mothers re- fused to participate in the study, one had moved, and three could not be located. One-third of those contacted for the pr test refused to participate, as compared to a refusal rate of one out of four for the main study. The children interviewed in the pretest varied in their responsiveness to the interview; however, all were relatively cooperative. The dolls seemed to be helpful in holding the children's attention. In fact, some children wanted to keep the dolls. All the mothers who agreed to participate in the study gave their full COOperation. Administration Of the Instrument A letter explaining the study was mailed to each family in the sample before they were contacted. See Appendix D for the letter. The researcher then called the mother Of each child to arrange for an appointment for the interview. Families without telephones were contacted in their homes 33 for an appointment. As in the Baxter study,16 two persons collected the data. One researcher interviewed the children ‘ while the other recorded the responses on the interview schedule. At the arranged time, the researchers went to the child's home for the interview carrying the dolls, prear— ranged by scenes, in a suitcase. One researcher began to talk with the child as the mother was given the background questionnaire to complete while the child was being inter— viewed. The questionnaire kept the mother occupied during the interview. The child was asked if he would like to play a game, and Scene I was removed from the suitcase and shown to the child. The suitcase was closed as each scene was shown to avoid interruption Of the interview sequence. The two dolls were held beside each other and the child was asked to take or to point to the "mommy" or "daddy" doll. As the response was made, it was recorded by the second person. The dolls were placed together again and the child was asked to take or give or point to the "daddy" or "mommy" doll. Then the dolls for Scene I were returned to the suitcase and the ones for Scene II were taken out. This procedure was followed for each of the first three scenes. For Scene IV all the dolls were randomly placed before the child so that all dolls could be seen equally well. The child was asked to select all the "mommy" dolls. They were placed together 16 Baxter, Op. cit. 3h again and this time the child was asked to select all the "daddy" dolls. The children were allowed to play with the dolls if they wished. This was a good way to Obtain responses to the fourth scene. Some children more readily separated the "mommies" from the "daddies" when given a reason for separa- ting them, e.g:, "All the 'daddies' are going to work now. Can you put them together?" Of the 79 children interviewed only five were uncooperative or unresponsive. With the mother's permission, a balloon was given to each child when the interview was completed. The balloon occupied the child's attention while the dolls were being returned to the suitcase. Reliability The reliability of a measuring instrument refers to the extent to which repeated applications to the same popu- lation will yield the same scores. The fourth scene in the interview schedule was used as a test-retest check for reli- ability Of the measuring instrument. When the first three scenes were administed, no indication was given to the child about the correctness of his choice. The fourth scene was administered directly after the first three. Thus, the possibility for learning to take place between the first and second administrations was kept to a minimum. Pearsonian correlatiOns were calculated between the errors made in each of the first three scenes and in the corresponding parts of 35 the fourth scene. These test-retest correlations are presented in Table 12. Table 12. Test-retest reliability coefficients Of correla— tions between sex recognition errors made on criteria in Scenes 1, II and III and errors.made on the corresponding criteria in Scene IV by sex and scene Scene I End II and III and I, II, III Sex IVrI IV—II IV—III and IV Boys .51 (.8H .;.72 .71 Girls .59 .9H .85 .89 Total .55 .89 ., .76 .79 The instrument was found to be sufficiently reliable for Scenes II and III with only a minimum reliability for boys on Scene 111.17 On Scene I, the reliability coefficients are relatively low for both boys and girls. However, the over all correlations indicate a fair amount Of reliability in View of the following considerations. Scene IV was a harder task than Scenes I, II, and 111 since the choice was Of three dolls from six rather than one doll from two. In addition, Scene IV was presented near the end of the respon- dent's attention Span. T fir l7Reliability correlations should approximate .80 for the instrument to be considered reliable. 36 Validity The validity of an instrument refers to the degree to which it measures what it was designed to measure. The validity of this instrument depends primarily upon two fac- tors. First is the ability of the child to understand the questions. This was hopefully facilitated by modification of the questions during the pretest to couch them in the types of vocabulary and sentence structure which could be under- stood by the children. The second factor affecting the validity of the in- strument is the possible difference in perceptual framework Of the interviewer and respondent. For example, the child may perceive differences between the two dolls but not at— tribute them to differences in sex. These kinds of problems are inherent in most social science research. One type of validation commonly used by social scientists is logical or face validity. A measuring instrument can be said to have face validity on the basis of the assumptions that (1) the behavior which appears to be involved in the test is the behavior it is actually measuring and (2) an adequate sample of the kind of behavior with which it is concerned has been included.18 It seems logical to assume that a test requiring a choice between two dolls, one manifesting male attributes 18Claire Sellitz, Marie Jahoda, Morton Deutsch, and Stuart Cook, Research Methods in Social Relations revised ed.,(New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1963), p. 165. 37 and another manifesting female attributes, is actually measur- ing sex discriminating abilities. Further, three sets of male-female attributes were included in the measuring instru- ment. \‘I‘lr CHAPTER IV FINDINGS The data collected in the manner described in the methodology chapter were statistically analyzed and the re— sults were organized and reported under the general hypo- theses of the study. The concern of this study was the re- cognition Of sex differences as it was affected by the variables of (1) age, (2) social class, (3) sex, (H) sex differentiating criteria and (5) family characteristics. Age and Recognition Of Sex Differences Age was hypothesized to be positively related to recognition of sex differences. The empirical measure of the recognition of sex differences was the number of errors made in the choice Of dolls. As explained previously, de- finite evidence of a correct choice was required for a correct choice score to be given. No response was scored as an error. The empirical hypothesis relating age and recognition Of sex differences was that the months Of age would be negatively related to the number of errors made. To test this hypothesis zero order correlations were figured between months and errors and first order partial correlations were calculated 738 39 to compare months and errors while controlling on social class.1 These correlations were done for total errors, as well as for each scene, for boys and girls and for both sexes combined. The results Of the cOrrelations are report- ed in Table 13. Table 13. Correlations between months of age and number Of sex recognition errors by sex and criteria* Boys Girls Total First ‘First First Zero Order Zero Order Zero Order Criteria Order Partial Order Partial Order Partial I Sex—apprOpriate clothing —.68 -.66 -.53 —.5H -.62 -.62 II Primary sex characteristics -.71 -.71 -.61 -.61 -.67 -.66 III Secondary sex characteristics —.7H -.75 —.56 —.55 -.6H -.6H Total -ux- -.80 -.83 -.72 -.72 -.77 -.78 (all criteria) f ‘ RAll values were significant at the .01 level. In all cases, age expressed in months was found to be negatively related to number of errors made. The overall partial correlation across all scenes between months and errors was -.76. For boys the correlation was -.83 and for the girls -.72. The correlations for each scene by total and 1The correlations between social class and age were .2H for the boys, .03 for the girls, and .13 for the total sample. to by sex showed a strong negative relationship between months of age and number of errors. Although all the correlations were highly significant, the boys exhibited a slightly stronger relationship between age and errors than the girls. After it had been established that the number of errors varied with age, a question was raised about what percentage of the variation in errors could be accounted for by the variation in age. The partial correlation coefficients were squared to measure the proportion of the total variation explained by differences in age. Fer the boys, age‘aocounted .for HH percent Of the errors made in distinguishing between the sexes by the clothing criteria, 50 percent of the errors made in distinguishing by the primary sex characteristics criteria, and 56 percent of the errors made in distinguish- ing by the secondary sex characteristics criteria. For the girls, age accounted for 29 percent of the errors made in distinguishing by the criteriarofvclothing, 37 percent by the criteria of primary sex characteristics, and 31 percent by the criteria of secondary sex characteristics. The largest difference between the sexes on the percentage Of variance in errors explained by age is in Scene III with the secondary sex criteria. The third area of interest in the relationship between age and sex recognition is the actual age at which children begin to recognize differences between the sexes. The age diatribution of the sample by number Of errors made is H1 presented separately for the three criteria in Tables 1H, 15 and 16. Table 1H. Age distribution of the sample by the number of sex recognition errors made on the criteria Of sex-appropriate clothing ——+ I , _ Numper Age in Months Errors 9-11 12-1h 15-17 18-20 21-23 2H-26_27-29'30-32 33-35 Sum V—W— O 0 3 5 9 3 9 9 8 11 S7 1 o 0 0 0 0 o 0 0 0 0 2 o o 0 3 0 1 0 0 0 16 Sum o 9 5 12 3 10 9 8 11 73 As can be seen from Table 1H, most of the children 15 months and Older could correctly distinguish between the sex- appropriately dressed dolls. The youngest age at which a child differentiated between the clothed dolls was 12 months. NO child 27 months or older made errors on the clothing criteria. _u2 Table 15. Age distribution of the sample by the number of sex recognition errors made on the criteria Of primary sex characteristics +1 J 1 Number of Age in Months Errors 9-11 12-1H 15-17 18-20 21-23 2H-26'27-29 30-32 33-35 Sum 0 0 0 2 5 0 7 5 8 11 38 1 o 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 l 2 6 9 2 7 3 3 h 0 0 3h 1* Sum o 9 5 l2 3 10 9, 8 ,11 73 ‘1' Table 15 reveals the fact that no child over 30 months made errors in distinguishing between the sexes with the criteria of primary sex characteristics. Sixteen months was the youngest age at which children could differentiate between the nude dolls. After the age of 2H months, almost all the children could dis- tinguish between the sexes on this set Of criteria. From Table 16, it can be seen that no child 30 months or Older made errors in differentiating between the dolls on the basis Of secondary sex characteristics. Fourteen months was the youngest age at which a child correctly separated the dolls on the criteria of secondary sex characteristics. As with primary sex criteria, it was around the age of 2H months that the majority of the children could correctly distinguish male and female dolls on secondary sex characteristics. Comparing the ages at which the different criteria were used to distinguish 1&3 between the sexes, it can be seen that clothing was used at a younger age than were primary and secondary sex character- istics. Table 16. Age distribution Of the sample by the number of sex recognition errors made on the criteria of secondary sex characteristics Number of Age in Months Errors 9-11 l2-lH 15:17 18920 21-23 2H-26 27-29 30-32 33~35 Sum o 0 l 2 7 1 7 11 L13 1 0 0 1 o 0 0 0 1 2 6 8 2 5 2 3 O 29 Sum 6 9 5 12 3 10 ll 73 The data in Tables 1H, 15 and 16 are combined in Table 17 to show the age distribution of the sample by the total number of sex recognition errors made on all the scenes. HH Table 17. Age distribution Of the sample by the total number of sex recognition errors made on all scenes Nu§?°r Age by Months Errors 9—11 l2-lH 15-17 18920 21-23 2H-2627-29 30-32 33-35 Total 0 0 0 2 5 0 5 3 8 11 3H 1 O O O O 0 O O O O O 2 0 l l 2 1 H 5 0 0 1H 3 O O O O O O O O O O H 0 2 2 2 2 0 1 0 0 9 5 O O O O O O O O O O 6 6 6 O 3 O 1 O O O 16 Total 6 9 5 12 3 10 9 .8 11 ‘73 W Table 17 shows that no child 30 months or older made errors in differentiating between the sexes by any Of the three criteria. The youngest age at which a child could correctly distinguish all the dolls was 16 months. By approximately 2H months of age the majority of the children could accurately distinguish between the sexes on the criteria of clothing, and primary and secondary sex characteristics. The relationship between age and accuracy in recognizing sex differences can be explained best by interaction theory. Interactionists propose that people learn through reciprocal social encounters with others. It follows logically from this theory that older children should have greater accuracy than 115 younger children in recognizing sex differences due to their greater opportunity for social interaction. Social Class and Recognition of Sex Differences Social class was hypothesized to be negatively related to recognition Of sex differences. A North-Hatt rating of the occupation of the child's father was the empirical measure of social class. The empirical hypothesis relating social class and recognition of sex differences was that North-Hatt social status ratings will be positively related to the number Of errors made. TO test this hypothesis zero order correla- tions were calculated between North-Hatt ratings and errors and first order partial correlations were computed to com- pare North-Hatt ratings and errors while controlling on age. These correlations were done for total errors, as well as for each scene, for boys and girls and for both sexes com— bined. The results of these correlations are reported in Table 18. Looking first at the overall correlation of -.16 it can be seen that no significant relationship was found between social class ratings and number of errors for the total sample. However, there was a significant negative relationship for the tOtal sample between social class ratings and errors on the criteria of clothing differences, i.e., the higher the social class of the child, the fewer errors he made in differentiating between the sexes on this set of criteria. H6 Table 18. Correlations between NortheHatt social class rat- ings and number of sex recognition errors by sex and criteria * r 7 r - __Ip- Boys Girls Total I First First First Zero Order Zero Order Zero Order Criteria Order Partial Order Partial Order Partial I Sex-appropriate y "w V" clothing -.H1” -.35* -.25 -.27 -.3H““ -.33"” 11 Primary sex characteristics —.07 .1H -.19 -.22 -.12 -.OH 111 Secondary sex characteristics --08 -15 --O9 --08 --09 —.01 Totals -.20 -.02 —.2O -.26 -.2O -.16 (all criteria) * Significant at the .05 level **Significant at the .01 level Fewer errorsbwerelexpected'from the lower Sociél classr Childrentdue to the probability Of their living in more crowd— ed housing situations and having a greater OppOrtunity to observe nudity. This hypothesis was not supported by the data. As pointed out above, only one significant relation- ship was found; nevertheless, it is interesting to note that almost all of the correlations are in a negative direction indicating a slight tendency for errors to "go with" lower social class. One explanation might be the proneness of higher social class parents to be interested in all aspects of their children's education. This influence could be A7 reflected in the higher social class child's tendency to be slightly more accurate in recognizing sex differences than the lower social class child. Sex Of the Child and Recognition of Sex Differences The sex Of the child was hypothesized to be related to the recognition of sex differences. The hypotheses can be stated empirically as follows: (1) Girls will make fewer errors than boys. (2) Girls will recognize sex differences at a younger age than boys. The mean numbers of errors made by the boys and girls are presented in Table 19. The range of possible errors was 0 to 2 for each scene separately and from O to 6 for all scenes combined. Table 19. Mean sex recognition errors made by the sexes on each scene and on all scenes t,“ 1 ’— Mean Errors Boys Girls TotaI Scene N=38 N=35 N=73 Scene I .5 .3 .H Scene II 1.1 .8 1. Scene III .7 .9 All scenes 2.3 2.1 2. H8 The girls were found to make slightly fewer errors than boys; however, a t-test value of .3H revealed no significant difference between the mean total number of errors, 2.3, made by the boys, and the mean total number of errors, 2.1, made by the girls.2 Thus the hypothesis that girls would make fewer errors than boys was not supported. The data presented in Table 19 may be viewed in a dif- ferent way to compare the prOportions of boys and girls who made errors. 'These percentages are presented for each sex by scenes in Table 20. Table 20 again shows that a slightly smaller percentage of the girls than the boys made errors. A smaller percent- age Of the girls made errors on the criteria of sex-appro- priate clothing and primary sex characteristics; however, a smaller percentage of boys than girls made errors on the criteria of secondary sex characteristics. gAt;value of 2.0 was necessary to be significant at the .05 level with 35 degrees of freedom. A9 Table 20. Percentage Of each sex making sex recognition errors by scene and on all scenes Percentage Making Errprs BO 5 Girls, Total 11:; N=755f ,. N=73__ Scene %" Scene I 26 17 22 Scene II 55 H0 H8 Scene III 37 MS H1 All scenes 55 51 53 In order to compare the ages at which boys and girls recognized sex differences, two error points were established, namely the absolute zero error point and the modal zero error point. The absolute zero error point is the age after which no errors were made in distinguishing between the dolls. The modal zero point is the age below which almost all of the errors were made. The modal and absolute zero error points for boys and girls are shown in Table 21. In comparing the totals, no differences were found between the boys and girls in the age of recognition of sex differences. When the comparison was made by scenes, boys were found to recognize earlier on the criteria of sex apprOpriate clothing and secondary sex characteristics. NO difference was found between the sexes in their ages of 50 recognizing the criteria Of primary sex characteristics. Thus, the hypothesis that girls would recognize at a young- er age than boys was not substantiated. In fact, at least on two sets of criteria, boys recognized sex differences at a younger age than girls. Table 21. Absolute and modal zero sex recognition error points for boys and girls by criteria Boys Girls Absolute Modal Absolute Modal Zero Error Zero Error Zero Error Zero Error Criteria Point Point Point Point I Sex-appropriate clothing 19 12 2o 13* 11 Primary sex characteristics 29 25 29 25 III Secondary sex _ characteristics 22 22 29 29 Totals 29 29 29 29 (all criteria) *Because there were no 11 to 12 month old girls in the sample, this value is untenable. Sex Distinguishing Criteria and Recognition of Sex Differences The type Of sex distinguishing criteria presented to the respondent was hypothesized to be related to recognition of sex differences. Empirically it was predicted that the least errors would be made when clothing was the sex 51 distinguishing criteria presented and the most errors would be made when primary sex characteristics were shown. As presented in Table 19 on page H7, the respondents made .H, 1.0 and .8 mean numbers of errors respectively Idnrlclothing, primary sex characteristics and secondary sex characteristics were presented. The analysis of variance of errors among' the scenes is presented in Table 22. Table 22. Analysis of variance Of sex recognition errors by scene Source of Degrees of Sum of Means. . Variation Freedom Squares Square F Scenes 2 10.0 5.0 p' 1.85" Within 70 191.1 2.7 Totals 72 201.1 2.8 *Not significant The F-test showed no significant differences between 133 nman number Of errors made on the three criteria. However, in Table 20 on page H9, it is shown that 22 per- cent of the sample made errors on the criteria Of clothing, H1 percent made errors on the criteria Of secondary sex characteristics and H8 percent made errors on the criteria of primary sex characteristics. 52 Although the data seem to support the hypotheses that the least errors would be made on the criteria of sex-appro- priate clothing and the most errors would be made on the criteria of primary sex characteristics, the differences between the mean numbers of errors for the criteria were not statistically significant. In addition to the fact that fewer errors were made on the criteria of clothing, it can be seen from Tables 1H, 15 and 16 on pages 39, H2 and H3 that clothing and secondary sex characteristics were used as sex distinguishing criteria at a younger age than were primary sex characteristics. Perhaps the findings concerning the dominance of cloth- ing as a sex discriminator are not surprising in light of the fact that most social interaction involves clothed in— dividuals rather than nude ones. Likewise,.there is more Opportunity for the observance of secondary sex character- istics than primary. Family Characteristics and Recognition of Sex Differences The final general hypothesis of this study was that family characteristics besides social class and its close correlate, level Of education, would be related to recog- nition Of sex differences. The specific characteristics analyzed were sibling composition, mother's employment, child's usual dress, mother's usual dress, place of resi- dence, and stage of toilet training. 53 Since age was found to be highly related to recogni- tion of sex differences the mean ages of the children in the categories were considered at the time comparisons were made between the categories of each family characteristic. Sibling Composition The mean ages and mean total errors are reported in Table 23 for the four sibling composition categories, i.e., no siblings, same-sex siblings, opposite-sex siblings, and both same and Opposite sex siblings. Table 23. Mean age expressed in months and mean total sex recognition error by sibling composition Sibling Category N ‘Mean Age. ~‘Mean Total Error None 13 20.5 2.2 Same-sex 21 22.9 p 2.5 Opposite—sex 20 22.7 2.5 Both sexes 19 25.0 1.6 The mean ages were sufficiently different to make com- parison among all the categories somewhat meaningless in terms of relating errors to sibling composition. However, the same-sex and Opposite-sex groupings were equivalent in mean age. Of these two categories, the mean number Of errors were the same. Thus, on the basis of this evidence the hypothesis that sex of siblings would affect the number Of errors made in distinguishing sex differences was not sup- ported. ELL Mother's Employment In Table 2H, the mean ages and mean total errors are reported for the children whose mothers were full-time home- makers and for the children whose mothers were employed outside the home. Table 2H. Mean age expressed in months and mean total sex recognition error by mother's employment Mother's Employment N Mean Age Mean Total Error Employed outside home 12 25.0 1.0 Full-time homemaker 61 22.5 2.H Even though there is only a 2.5 month age difference between the two groups, this difference could be sufficient to account for the mean error differences. Unfortunately, it is difficult to isolate the effect of age with only 12 cases of employed mothers. Child's Usual Dress It was hypothesized that the sex-apprOpriate or ins apprOpriate clothing investedu upon the child, i.e., the clothes in which he was usually dressed, would affect his recognition of Sex differences. Only girls were considered in this section since all the boys in the study usually were uStone, Op. cit. SS dressed in trousers. The girls, however, were fairly evenly divided according to their usual dress, slacks or dresses. The mean ages and mean total errors for the girls who usually were slacks and for those who usually were dresses are re- ported in Table 25. Table 25. Mean age expressed in months and mean total sex ' recognition error by girl's usual dress m ; Girl's Usual Dress N Mean Age Mean Total Error Slacks 15 22.7 2.5 Dresses 20 2H.7 1.7 A t-testS comparing the mean errors for the two cat- egories of usual dress showed no significant difference at the .05 level. Once again, the difference that does exist between the mean errors, though it is not significant, per- haps can be accounted for by the mean age difference between the two categories. Mother's Usual Dress It is probable that children come to associate the mother's general type of dress, i.e., slacks or dresses, with her sex. Thus, the mother's mode of dress was hypo- thesized to affect the child's recognition of sex 5The t value was .99 and a value of 2.1 was needed for significance at the .05 level with 17 degrees of freedom. 56 differences.6 The mean ages and mean total errors are presented in Table 26 for children whose mothers usually were slacks and for children whose mothers usually wore dresses. A Table 26. Mean age expressed in months and mean total sex recognition error by mother's usual dress v Mother's Usual Dress N Mean Age Mean Total Error Slacks H7 21.7 2.H Dresses 26 25.2 1.9 The mean ages were perhaps again sufficiently different to account for the difference in mean errors. Since the dif- ference in age was substantial a t-test was not calculated. Place of Residence For this study, three designations were used for place of residence. Small town was the designation given to the children living within the village limits of Jonesville. Rural non-farm was the classification given to children who lived outside the village limits but whose fathers did not farm. The children who lived outside the village limits and whose fathers were farmers were denoted as rural farm 6It is interesting to note that two—thirds of the mothers in the study usually were Slacksirather than dresses indicat- ing a changing trend in women's attire. 57 children. The mean ages and mean total errors for the three place Of residence categories are reported in Table 27. Table 27. Mean age expressed in months and mean total sex recognition error by present place of residence fi fr Place of Residence N Mean Age Mean Total Error Rural farm 1H 25.H 2.0" Rural non-farm 22 2H.5 1.6 Small town 37 21.1 2.7 Y— The mean age Of the town children was sufficiently less than the two categories Of rural children to account for the higher mean errors. The mean ages Of the two rural categories were similar enough for comparison. In this breakdown, the rural farm children, as a category, are slightly Older but have a larger mean error than the rural non-farm children. However a t-test7.comparing the mean errors revealed no significant differences. Stage in Toilet Training The mean ages and mean total errors for the children in each stage Of toilet training are reported in Table 28. 7The value of t was .65 and a value of 2.1 was needed for significance at the .05 level with 17 degrees of freedom. 58 Table 28. Mean age expressed in months and mean total sex recognition error by stage in toilet training f Stage in Toilet Training N Mean Age Mean Total Error Not started 2H 1H.9 3.8 Started 16 21.2 2.9 Not finished 33 29.6 0.7 The degree of toilet training is clearly related to age, and the mean ages Of the children in the three categories' were sufficiently different to account for the differences in mean errors. Once again, the small number of respondents in each category made it difficult to isolate the effect of age. Summary Age was found tO be highly and negatively related to number Of errors made in recognition of sex differences. Children recognized sex differences of clothing as young as 12 months, of secondary sex characteristics as young as 1H months and of primary sex characteristics as yOung as 16 months. NO child 30 months or older made any errors on any Of the three sets of sex distinguishing characteristics. Only on the criteria of clothing differences, was social class found to be significantly related to errors. This re- lationship was in a negative direction indicating that the higher the social status Of the child, the more accurate 59 was his recognition of sex differences on the set of clothing criteria. On the other two sets Of criteria, all but two Of the correlations between social class and errors were also in an inverse direction. The sex of the child seems to have little relationship to sex recognition ability. No Significant differences were found between the boys and girls on the mean number of errors made in distinguishing between the sexes. The boys Of this sample recognized sex differences at a slightly younger age than the girls on the criteria of clothing and secondary sex characteristics, but there was no difference between the ages of sex recognition for the boys and girls on the criteria of primary sex characteristics. Since the child's age was found to be highly related to sex recognition ability, it was manditory that this be taken into consideration when analyzing the family characteristics and their relationship to recognition of sex differences. In this study, sibling composition was the main family characteristic for which the effect Of the child's age could readily be controlled. The sex Of the respondent's siblings, that is, whether they were the same sex or the Opposite sex, was found to have no effect upon the respondent's accuracy in distinguishing sex differences. CHAPTER V SUMMARY, IMPLICATIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS Symbolic interactionists prOpose that the sex role, as well as other roles, are learned through a process Of social interaction. Two social psychologists, Lindesmith and Strauss, have suggested three steps in the sex-role identification process. These three stages are (l) the recognition of sex differences, (2) the understanding of tme Emanings Of male and female, and (3) the classifica— tion Of oneself as male or female.1 The first step of the sex-role identification process is the concern of this study. Previous researchers inves- tigating the recognition of sex differences have found age to be positively related to accuracy in differentiating between the sexes and to the number Of criteria mentioned as sex discriminators. The age at which recognition took place was found to be as low as two and one-half years but most of the studies have dealt with Older children. The presence or absence of Opposite-sex siblings or parents seemed to be unrelated to accuracy in distinguishing sex differences. The purpose of this study was to investigate sex re- cognition Of very young children who came from a range of social class backgrounds. Y— 7* ‘v' lLindesmith and Strauss, Op. cit, p. 321. 60 61 A random sample with a sampling rate of .83 was drawn from a population of families with children between the ages of 9 months to 35 months who lived within the school district Of a small industrial-farming community. Seventy-three children, 38 boys and 35 girls, were interviewed individually in their homes. The North-Hatt social status ratings of the children ranged from HO to 81. Three sets of male and female dolls were used as the instrument to measure the child's accuracy in recognizing sex differences. Each of the three sets of dolls presented a set of criteria for distinguishing between the sexes, i.e., sex-apprOpriate clothing, primary sex characteristics and secondary sex characteristics. The three sets of dolls were shown to the child in four scenes, and the child was asked to choose between the "mommy" and "daddy" doll. In Scene I the dolls were sex-appropriately dressed according to current fashion, in Scene II the dolls were nude, and in Scene III the dolls were dressed identically in slacks and shirts with hair and cosmetic differences according to cur- rent fashion. In Scene IV all six dolls Of Scenes 1, II and III were shown and the child was asked to choose all the "mommies" then all the "daddies". A lack of choice or in- correct choice was recorded as an error. The errors made in the choices of dolls were analyzed in relation to characteristics of the child including his age, sex, social class and family characteristics. The r--w 62 errors were also analyzed with respect to the criteria pre— sented in the scenes. The hypothesis that the ages of the children would be positively related to accuracy in distinguishing between the sexes was supported by the data. This finding was consistent with the other studies discussed in the review of literature. The children of this study were found to accurately differentiate between the sexes as early as 12 months on the basis of sex-appropriate clothing, 1h months on the basis of secondary sex characteristics, and 16 months on the basis of primary sex characteristics. All the child- ren of the sample 30 months or older made accurate dis- tinctions between the sexes on all the criteria. Hartman and Purse2 found most of the children 21 months or older in their sample could accurately distinguish between the sexes and most of the children under 1h months could not make accurate distinctions. From their studies, Gesell and 'Ilg3 found that most children could distinguish between the sexes on the basis of hair and clothing differences by the age of 2h months and on the basis of primary sex character— istics by the age of 36 months. Thus, it appears that children around the age of two years know the differences between clothed males and females. 2Hartman and Purse, Op. cit., p. 29. 3Gese11 and Ilg, op. cit., p.3uu. 63 The hypothesis that the criteria of clothing differ- ences would be used for distinguishing between the sexes at a younger age than the primary or secondary sex character- istics was supported. In the studies reviewed, clothing differences were also found to be one of the first sets of criteria used by young children to distinguish between the sexes. Secondary and primary sex characteristics were used ‘at approximately the same age a few months later than clothing differences were utilized. The hypothesis that social class would be negatively related to accuracy in recognition of sex differences was rejected by the data. Contradictory to the hypothesis, social class was found to be significantly and positively related to accuracy in differentiating between the sexes on the basis of primary and secondary sex characteristics. Katcher“ found no significant differences in sex recogni- tion between the social class groups he studied. However, he did find that the higher socio-economic groups were slightly more accurate than the lower ones which was con- sistent with the findings of the present study. The hypothesis that the sex of the child would be re- lated to accuracy in recognition of sex differences was not 2+Katcher, Op. cit., p. 135. 6h supported by the data. In the overall accuracy of sex recognition, only slight differences were found between the sexes in the present study as well as the investiga- tions of Katchers, Baxter6 and Hartman and Purse7. Never- theless, the boys and girls of the Katcher study and of the present study showed similar trends in the comparative age of recognition on the different criteria. In the pre- sent study, the boys recognized differences in clothing and secondary sex characteristics at a slightly younger age than did girls and no differences were found between the sexes in age of sex recognition on the criteria of primary sex char- acteristics. In the Katcher study, the boys and:girls re- cognized sex differences at the same age on all criteria except the primary sex characteristics. On the primary sex criteria the girls distinguished between the sexes at a younger age. The hypothesis that family characteristics would be related to recognition of sex differences was only partially tested because the sample size made it difficult to isolate the effect on sex recognition of age from the effect of family characteristics. This was possible however for sibling composition. The sex of the respondent's siblings was found to make no significant difference in the res- pondent's accuracy in sex differentiation of the three 51b1d., p. 136. 6Baxter, op. cit., p. 27. 7 65 criteria investigated. Thus, the hypothesis that sex of siblings would affect accuracy of sex recognition was re- jected by the data. This finding was consistent with the reports of Katcher8 and Baxterg. Implications Clothing was found to be an important factor in dis- tinguishing between the sexes. It would be interesting to investigate the part clothing plays in other stages of the sex-role identification process. .For example, are girls who usually wear dresses able to identify themselves earlier and/or more completely with the female sex than are girls who usually wear slacks? Of the three criteria investigated, clothing was found to be the first criteria used by children to distinguish between the sexes. Another aspect which might be investigat- ed would be the part clothing plays in other role identifi- cation prbcesses. The new job role can be taken as an example. Are those new employees who are able to recognize the type of clothing appropriate to the new Job and who then "dress the part" better able to "play the role" than are those new employees who disregard clothing in the assumption of their new role? 8Katcher, op. cit., p. lul. 9Baxter, 0p. cit., p. 28. 66 Recommendations The present study has increased the understanding of the factors affecting recognition of sex differences. How- ever, the use of a larger sample would facilitate the possibility of controlling on age for comparisons between family characteristics and sex recognition. A measure of the child‘s language develOpment, especially his knowledge of the words "mommy" and "daddy", would be useful in analyzing the age of recognition and in validating the measuring instrument. It would be interesting to compare the results of this study with the results of similar studies done with children drawn from larger urban areas and from different ethnic, racial and cultural groups. In this study, the criteria of clothing represented vtraditional male and female dress. With the changing dress norms in our society, it would be relevant to investigate the accuracy in distinguishing between the sexes on the basis of clothing which is not so clearly feminine or masculine, e.g., women's stretch slacks and men's casual trousers. BIBLIOGRAPHY 6? BIBLIOGRAPHY Ammons, R.B. "Reactions in a Projective Doll-Play Interview of White Males Two to Six Years of Age to Differences in Skin Color and Facial Features," Journal of Genetic Psychology, LXXVI (1950), pp. 323-3u1. Baxter, Hazel Ogilvie. "Sex Identification by Children Ages Two and One-Half to Five Years," (Unpublished Master's Thesis, Department of Textiles, Clothing and Related Arts, Michigan State University, 1964). Benedict, Ruth. Patterns of Culture. New York: Mentor Books, 19u6. Berger, Peter L. Invitation to Sociology: A Humanistic Per- spective. New York} DoubIeday & Company, Inc., 1963. Brieland, Donald and Nelson, Lillian. "Age Trends in Sex Identification as Determined by a Picture Test," American Psychologist,VI, no. 7 (July, 1951), p. 309. (Abstract). Conn, Jacob. "The Child Speaks to the Psychiatrist: An Intro— duction to the Method of the Interview," Occupational Therapy, XVII, 1938, pp. 2317244. Conn, Jacob H. and Kanner, Leo. "Children's Awareness of Sex Diffegences," qurnal of Child Psychology, I (19u7), pp.l— 7. Coser, Lewis and Rosenberg, Bernard (eds.). Sociological Theory: A Book of Readings. New York: The Mac- MilIan Company, I957. Creative Playthings, Inc., Catalog, Princeton, New Jersey, 1960. Dynes, Russell, Silverman, Leslie , Cook. W. Roy, Heller, A.O., Buck, Roy, Brown, C.H., and Lowe, George. "The North-Hatt Scalefl. (Mimeographedfi Elkin, Frederick. The Child and Society: The Process of Socialization. New York: Random House, 1960. 68 69 Gesell, Arnold L. and Ilg, Frances L. Infant and Child in the Culture of Today. New York: Harper & Bros., Publishers, 19u3. Hartman, Catherine and Purse, Marilee. "Sex Identification by Children Under Two and One-Half Years," (Un- published term paper, Department of Textiles, Cloth- inguand Related Arts, Michigan State University, 19 . Hillsdale County Directory. Summer, 196M Edition. Kahl, Joseph A. The American Class Structure. New York: Rinehart & Company, Inc., 1957. Katcher, Allan. "The Discrimination of Sex Differences by Young Children," Journal of Genetic Psychology. LXXXVII (1955). pp. 1311iu3. Lindesmith, Alfred R. and Strauss, Anslem L. Social Psycho- lo . Revised ed. New Ybrk: Henry Holt and Company, 19EE. McCarty, Dorothea. "Language Development in Children," in L. Carmichael, ed., Manual of Child Psychology, 2nd ed. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 19u61, p.u67. Mead, George Herbert. Mind, Self and Society. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1934. Mead, Margaret. Male and Female. New York: Mentor Books, l955- Park, Robert E. Principles of Human Behavior. Chicago: 28182 Corporation, 1915. Selltiz, Claire , Jahoda, Marie , Deutsch, Morton, and Cook, Stuart. Research Methods in Social Relations. Re— vised ed. New York: HoIt, Rinehart and’Winston, Inc., 1963. Stone, Gregory P. "Appearance and the Self" in Arnold M. Rose (ed.). Human Behavior and Social Processes. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1962. Thomas, W. I., "Definition of the Situation," in Lewis Coser and Bernard Rosenberg (eds.). Sociological Theory: A Book of Readings. New York: The MacMillan Company, 1957. APPENDIX A Photographed Dolls 70 APPENDIX B Interview Schedule 75 76 Name . Code Sex Age Let's play a game. I have some dolls here and I want you to pick out the mommy doll and the daddy doll each time I show them to you. ““ -. '- I. SHOW DOLLS WITH TRADITIONAL DRESS W A. Give me the daddy doll. PLACE DOLLS TOGETHER AGAIN B. Now give me the mommy doll. Child's choice: Child's choice: 5} IL. SHOW DOLLS NUDE A. Give me.the mommy dOll. Child's choice: 0’ Q PLACE DOLLS TOGETHER AGAIN B. Now give me the daddy doll. *7 Child's choice: 9’9: III. SHOW DOLLS WITH MODERN DRESS A. Give me the daddy doll. Child's choice: 99) PLACE DOLLS TOGETHER AGAIN B. New give me the mommy doll. Child's choice: 99 '77 IV. SHOW ALL THE DOLLS Now here are all the dolls together A. Give me all the mommy dolls. 9% O” 9 9 9 B. Give me all the daddy dolls. 9 9 O" 9 ‘3‘? Residence: Rural-Farm Rural-Non farm Urban Who filled out questionnaire other than mother What child is wearing during interview What mother is wearing during interview APPENDIX C Background Information Questionnaire 78 79 GENERAL INFORMATION Name (Please Print) Age: Under 20 21 to 30 31 to NO Al to 50 Please give the name and birthday and circle "M" or "F" for sex Of all your children. NAME BIRTHDAY SEX o~ U1 #7 k» h) +4 I: :3 3 E: I: 3 *s w 11 as 'd w What is your husband's job or occupation? What is your job or occupation? A Circle the last year finished in school by your husband: 1 2 3 h 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1 2 3 u 1 2 3 u Grammar School High School College Graduate School Circle the last year in school you finished: 1 2 3 u 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1 2 3 u 1 2 3 u Grammar School High School College Graduate School In which one of the following social classes would you place yourself? Upper Class Upper Middle Class Middle Class Lower Middle Class Lower Class 10. 11. 12. 13. la. 15. 16. How large is the place where you have lived most Of your life? Farm or Village (under 2,000) Small Town (2,000 to 10,000) Small City (10,000 to 50,000)' Medium-sized city 80 Large City (over 200,000) What is your religious preference? Protestant Other Catholic Jew How Often do you go to the church or synagogue? None Once a week Once or twice a month Sometimes Never At the present time are you married , divorced ? . 9 separated 9 , widowed or other The following questions concern your child who is being interviewed. Does your child have a bedroom alone? Does your child have a bed alone? Does your child bathe alone? Yes Yes Yes NO NO NO Does your child have the Opportunity to Observe others nude? Yes NO 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 81 How much time during the day do you usually spend with your child? 1 to 3 hours A to 6 hours 7 to 9 hours All day What type Of clothing does your child usually wear? (for example: dresses, shirt and pants, or coveralls, etc.) What do you usually wear at home? Is your child toilet trained? Yes NO At what age did he (she) start toilet training? At what age did he (she) finish toilet training? APPENDIX D Explanatory Letter 82 83 Dear Parent: A research project is being conducted at Michigan State University concerning children's recognition of male and female role differences. This study attempts to discover when and how a child learns to distinguish between the sex-roles. The learning of sex differences is a first step, and a crucial step, in a child's socialization pro- cess. However, at the present, little is known about the kinds Of criteria young children use to determine these differences. In order to find out more about this process, we are re- questing your assistance in permitting your child to be included in the study. ,This will involve a twenty (20) minute interview, during which your child will be asked to distinguish between male and female doll—models. At that time the mother will also be asked to fill out a short information sheet. All information will be kept strictly confidential. Absolutely no names will be used in the analysis Of the data. - We will contact you by phone within the next month to arrange for an interview time at your convenience. Thank you in advance for your cooperation. Sincerely, (Mrs.) L. Edna Rogers Assistant Instructor (Miss) Barbara Gibson Graduate Assistant APPENDIX E Age Distribution of the Sample by the Number of Errors Made on Scene IV 314 85 Age distribution Of the sample by the number of errors made on Scene IV W Number ‘ of Age in Months Errors 9-11 12-1uP15417 18—2051-23 zu-ae 27-29 30-32 33-35 Sum O O O l 3 O 5 3 8 ll 31 1 O O O O O O 1 O O 1 2 O o O O o 3 O O o 3 3 O O O O O O l O O 71 u C o o 2 O O O O o 2 5 O O O O O O O O O .O 6 6 9 u 7 3 2 )1 o o 35 Sum 6 9 5 12 3 10 9 8 ll 73 HICHIGRN STQTE UNIV. LIBRQRIES lllll II ”I lllll Ill 9 7 312 31027 5453