I I IIIHIHIN NH ADULT RESPONSES TO INFANT CUTENESS p4 _s._s _.{ I .m\]_.\ Thesis for Hm Degree of M. A. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY Katherine Ann Hildebrandt I976 L II III II III III I \ \IIIIIII I ABSTRACT ADULT RESPONSES TO INFANT CUTENESS By Katherine Ann Hildebrandt Lorenz first hypothesized that physical and behavioral characteristics of both human and nonhuman infants act as innate releasers of caregiver approach. Several studies are reviewed which indicate that adults do in fact prefer, and show a stronger positive emotional response to, pictures and drawings of babies as compared to pictures and drawings of adults. The purpose of the present study was to investigate the effects of different configurations of infant facial features, categorized according to perceived cuteness, on college students' behavior. Forty college students from introductory psychology classes (20 males and 20 females) were tested individually. Half of the participants observed six photographs of 4-month—old infants and half observed six photographs of 8-month-old infants. Pairs of photographs were presented via slides. During Parts l and 3 or the experiment, each pair consisted of two copies of the same infant. In this way, each of the six photographs was presented individually until the participant pressed a button to remove the photographs from the screen. During these presentations, recordings were made of looking time (the amount of time the participant looked at each pair before removing them from the screen), smiling (measured electromyographically <3 2: ,) Katherine Ann Hildebrandt from the zygomaticus muscle of the cheek), and skin conductance responses (measured from the palm of the left hand). During Part 2 of the experiment, the six photographs the participant had just seen during Part 1 were presented in all possible paired combinations. Paired looking time was defined as the average percent of time spent looking at each of the six photographs during this part of the experiment. After completion of all three parts of the experiment, participants were asked to rank prints of the six photographs previously seen in their order of perceived cuteness. A 2 x 2 x 2 (sex of participant x age of infant x cuteness ranking of photograph) multivariate analysis of variance was performed on the four dependent variables: looking time, smiling, SC, and paired looking time. Two levels of cuteness were established by considering the average value of each dependent measure for the two cutest photographs and the average value for the two least cute photographs. The only significant effect was that of cuteness. Post hoc univariate analyses revealed significant effects of cuteness for looking time and paired looking time. Photographs ranked cuter were looked at longer in all three segments of the experiment. Looking time, smiling, and SC were further analyzed for the effects of repeated presentations (Parts 1 and 3). Looking time significantly declined from Part l to Part 2, and the decline was larger for the least cute photographs than for the most cute photographs. Smiling did not change from Part l to Part 3. The number of SCRs occurring declined from l28 during Part l to 57 during Part 3 (out of a total of 240 slide presentations during each part). Katherine Ann Hildebrandt The observation that participants looked longer at infant photographs which they ranked higher in cuteness suggests that adults may vary their looking at real infants as a function of perceived cuteness. Of course, many cues other than facial features may contribute to the perceived cuteness of a real infant. However, if looking behavior is affected by cuteness, in whatever way cuteness is defined by the individual, it is not unreasonable to expect that this variation in adult behavior may influence the quality of adult—infant interaction. The results of the present study suggest that the infant's physical appearance and the adult‘s perception of cuteness be included in future studies of adult-infant interaction, particularly with respect to the organization of mutual visual regard. ADULT RESPONSES TO INFANT CUTENESS By Katherine Ann Hildebrandt A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1976 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to thank Hi Fitzgerald for his advice and assistance; Ellen Strommen, Gary Olson, and Lauren Harris for their interest; Tom Hildebrandt for his support and patience; and my assistants for their competent help. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ................................. _...... ........ iv LIST OF FIGURES ................. ....... ................. ..... vi INTRODUCTION ................................. ................ 1 Adults' responses to pictures of babies The importance of infant physical appearance to social relations METHOD .............................................. . ........ 6 Participants Stimuli Apparatus Dependent variables Procedure Data Scoring RESULTS ...................................................... 14 DISCUSSION ................................................... 2l APPENDIX A - SUPPLEMENTARY TABLES ............................ 26 APPENDIX B - PARTICIPANT FORMS AND INSTRUCTIONS .............. 30 APPENDIX C - PILOT STUDY ..................................... 34 REFERENCES ................................................... 46 iii Table Al. A2. A3. A4. C1. C2. C3. C4. C5. C6. C7. C8. LIST OF TABLES Number of participants assigned to each experimental condition .......................................... Order of presentation of stimuli ............ . ...... Average rankings of each infant photograph by male and female participants ................... ......... Multivariate tests of the effects of sex of participant, age of infant in photographs, and cuteness rankings of photographs on looking time, smiling, SCR, and paired looking time ....... ,. Multivariate tests of the effects of sex of participant, age of infant in photograph, cuteness rankings of photographs, and time seen (Part 1 or Part 3) on looking time and smiling ............................................ Number of participants assigned to each conditiOn .. Proportion of participants choosing each photograph as cuter than each other photograph of 4-month—olds Proportion of participants choosing each photograph as cuter than each other photograph of 8-month—olds Rank order of photographs .. ........................ Measurements and ratings of facial features in photographs of 4-month-old infants ................. Measurements and ratings of facial features in photographs of 8-month—old infants ................. Correlations between cuteness rankings and faCia] features 00000000000000 , 0.0.0.1000... 00000000000 Features of the photographs mentioned as being most important when rating cuteness ................ iv Page ...... 12 ...... 26 ...... 27 QO‘QQQ 28 ...... 29 ...... 37 ...... 39 ...... 40 ...... 4T ...... 42 ...... 42 Table Page C9. Cuteness ratings (expressed as proportions) of photographs of 4—month-old infants chosen for use in subsequent study ................ .............. 43 ClO. Cuteness ratings (expressed as proportions) of photographs of 8—month-old infants chosen for use in subsequent study‘ ....................... . ..... . 44 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page I. Time spent looking at infant photographs at each cuteness rank ......... . ............. . ................ l6 2. Percent increase in smiling during presentation of infant photographs at each cuteness rank ............... l6 3. Magnitude of SCRs to infant photographs at each cuteness rank ........................................ l7 4. Average percent of paired looking time spent looking at infant photographs at each cuteness rank ...................................................... l7 5. Interaction between cuteness ranking and time seen for looking time ..................................... l9 vi INTRODUCTION Lorenz (l943; l950) first hypothesized that physical and behavioral characteristics of both human and nonhuman infants act as sign stimuli (or innate releasing mechanisms) that elicit caregiver approach and physical contact. The term commonly used to refer to these infant characteristics, babyishness, is defined by a number of component cues including a relatively small size, a plump body with short limbs and large head, soft skin, certain behaviors such as awkward movements and crying and characteristic facial features (Hess, l970). Prominant facial features include a short face in relation to a high and protruding forehead, large eyes placed in the middle of the face, rounded cheeks, and a small nose, mouth and chin (Bradshaw, 1969; Bradshaw & McKenzie, l97l; Gardner & Nallach, l965). Doll manufacturers, cartoonists, and illustrators of children's books often exaggerate these features in order to create "cute" baby animals and children. Infants whose features are more ideally "babyish" might be expected to be perceived as cuter than infants whose features are less "babyish." According to Lorenz's hypothesis, these infants should also elicit the strongest approach responses from caregivers and other adults. Although infants who are not "cute" may also elicit approach responses (for example, because they are "beautiful," or because they evoke pity), the present study is concerned only with the effects of perceived infant cuteness on adult behavior. (How individual variations in infant facial features affect adults' perceptions of cuteness is discussed in Appendix C.) 1 2 Adults' responses togpictures of babies Studies of human adults'responses to pictures of babies and to pictures of adults indicate that women show stronger verbal preferences for pictures of babies (Fullard, Reiling, Love, & Fowler, l975) and toddlers (Beier, Smock, Izard, & Tougas, 1957) than men do. In one study, pairs of pictures, one of an adult and one of a baby of the same species, were shown to adult men and women who were asked to judge which picture they liked more (Cann, T953, cited in Hess, l970). Nomen preferred the baby picture more often than the men did, regardless of their marital or parental status. However, men who were fathers or whose wives were pregnant preferred the baby pictures more often than other men did. In contrast, Berman (1975) found no sex difference in college students' ratings of pictures of nonhuman primate infants and adults, although, once again, infants were rated as more attractive than adults. This finding may reflect minimal differences in attitudes and experience with infants between males and females in the college environment. Children also respond positively to pictures containing infants (Bernick, 1966), and after puberty, show an increase in verbally expressed preferences for infants over adults (Fullard et al., l975). Pupil dilation is another possible index of preference. According to Hess (l965). pupillary dilation reflects the degree of interest or positive emotional arousal elicited by pictures. Hess and Polt (l960) used pupillary dilation to compare the responses of men and women to a picture of a baby and a picture of a woman holding a baby. It was found that women showed greater pupillary changes in response to these pictures than men did, indicating a greater preference. In a second study, Hess (l970) found that the magnitude of pupillary 3 dilation corresponded to the degree of babyishness conveyed by stylized drawings of human and animal faces. It seems then, that adults prefer, and show a stronger positive emotional response to, pictures and drawings of babies as compared to pictures and drawings of adults. Moreover, it appears that these tendencies are stronger in adult females than in adult males. But do individual differences in infant facial features influence responses to infants? Evidence suggests that they do. Two studies have employed a rating technique whereby adults were asked to judge drawings of infant faces which varied systematically in their component features. In general, one particular variation of each facial feature, such as eye position or eye size, was rated as significantly more attractive than other variations of that feature (Brooks & Hochberg, I960; Sternglanz, Gray & Murakami, l974). Interestingly enough, males in the Sternglanz et al. (1974) study gave significantly higher attractiveness ratings to all the pictures than females did, although the specific feature variations that resulted in the highest ratings were the same fbr both sexes. The amount of prior experience with children did not affect the ratings. Studies like these are informative, but do not answer the more basic question of whether adult responses to line drawings are predictive of their responses to real infant faces. Since adults often base their impressions of other adults and children on such features as general physical attractiveness, pupil size, facial angle, iris color, and eye contact (Dion, l974; English & Palla, l97l; Hicks, Reaney, & Hill, l967; Jones & Moyel, l97l; Stass & Willis, l967; Nalster, Aronson, Abrahams, & Rottmann, 1966) it seems likely that adults also 4 will be influenced by physical features of infants. Evidence indicates that this is so. The importance of infant physical appearance to social relations There is some evidence that the quality of parent-infant interaction is affected by the subjectively rated cuteness of the infant; parents of cuter newborn infants engage their infants in more eye-to-eye contact, and hold and kiss their infants more than do parents of less cute infants (Parke & Sawin, l975). However, since cuteness in this study was not objectively defined, and ratings were made by individuals other than the parent, the Parke and Sawin results must be regarded as suggestive. Certain facial expression changes, as well as invariant physiognomic characteristics seem to be important determinants of caregiver responsive- ness to infants. The smiling behavior of the infant is particularly important in maintaining caregiver proximity and positive social interaction (Bowlby, l969; Gewirtz, l968). Eye contact and widening of the eyes are also effective in encouraging proximity and caregiving from adults (Bennett, 1971). Stern (1974) has observed that infants influence adult behavior by initiating and terminating eye contact with them. The purpose of the present study was to determine whether or not adults' behavior is differentially affected by the degree of cuteness perceived in photographs of infants. Although still photographs eliminate much of the variation inherent in facial expression, they provide an effective means of presenting infant facial features. The effects of facial expression are not completely eliminated so perceptions of cuteness are most likely based on the combined influence 5 of facial features and facial expression, as no doubt they are in everyday interactions. The adult behavioral responses chosen for study include looking, smiling, and the skin conductance response (SCR). It was hypothesized that infant photographs ranked cuter would elicit longer looking, more smiling, and larger SCRs than infant photographs ranked less cute. METHOD Participants Forty-seven college students from introductory psychology classes participated in the study. Most of the students received extra credit for their research participation. The data from seven research participants had to be discarded because of equipment problems or experimenter errors, leaving a final sample of 40 (20 men and 20 women). The average age was 20.7 years, with males significantly older than females (22.l years and l9.3 years respectively; t(38)=2.55, E.< .02). There were no differences between males and females or between participants viewing older and younger babies in the average amount of contact they currently had with infants. Thirty of the 40 participants stated that they had had regular contact with infants at some time since they entered high school. The number of participants with previous infant contact did not differ significantly for the sexes or for any of the conditions to which participants were assigned. The only restriction placed on research participants was that they be able to see reasonably well at a distance of three feet without eye glasses. This restriction was necessary, since eye movements could not easily be observed in participants wearing glasses because of reflections from the lenses. Stimuli Six photographs of 4-month-old infants and six photographs of 8-month-old infants were used as stimuli. These photographs were chosen from a larger collection of infant photographs on the basis of cuteness ratings obtained in an earlier investigation (see Appendix C). Each participant observed photographs from only one age group. Apparatus Pairs of photographs of infants were presented via slides, rear projected on a 48.5 by 41.5 cm screen set into one wall of a sound-attenuated booth containing a comfortable arm chair. Two Kodak Carousel slide projectors placed outside the booth were regulated by Hunter timers. During the experiment the participant's visual fixations were recorded by an observer watching corneal reflections through a .64 cm hole placed 8.16 cm below the rear projection screen. The observer pressed two silent buttons in response to the participant's eye movements, one button when the right photograph was fixated and the other button when the left photograph was fixated. Visual fixation data were recorded on the event marker channel of a Grass Model 7 polygraph located in an adjacent room. Slide changes were also recorded polygraphically as were several physiological dependent variables. Dependent variables Looking time. Looking time (the amount of time spent visually fixating a stimulus) has been widely used to study the responses of adults, children, infants, and animals to visual stimuli varying 8 on a number of dimensions (Berlyne, 1960; Fantz, 1961; Leckart & Faw, l968). Complexity (Leckart & Bakan, 1965; Wohlwill, 1968), novelty (Berlyne, 1958), incongruity (Berlyne & Lawrence, 1964; Connolly & Harris, 1971), and verbally expressed preferences for visual stimuli (Day, 1966) have all been found to affect adults', as well as children's, looking time. When the variables determining the information value of stimuli (predominantly complexity and novelty) are equated, looking time is primarily related to the affective value of the stimulus contents (Faw & Nunnally, 1967), with positive stimuli looked at longer than neutral stimuli. Since photographs of strange infant faces are reasonably similar in their levels of complexity and novelty, adults' looking time was expected to be positively related to their attractiveness. In addition to being related to attractiveness of visual stimuli, looking time has been suggested as an index of approach and avoidance behavior (Webb, Matheny, & Larson, 1963). Since eye movements often are not consciously controlled, they may be a more sensitive indicator of an individual's preferences than overt verbal expressions. Looking behavior also has been found to be consistent both within sessions and between sessions (7-10 weeks apart) for individual research partici- pants (Leckart & Bakan, 1969) and therefore is; a potentially useful indicator of adult preferences for photographs of infants. Smiling. The zygomaticus muscle, which extends between the corner of the mouth and the cheekbone, is essential to the production of a smile (Webb, 1974). Changes in this and other muscles can be measured electromyographically, and even sma11,visua11y non-observable changes can be detected (Schwartz, Fair, Greenberg, Friedman, & Klerman, 1973). The frontalis muscle, which runs vertically across 9 the forehead, shows little change in activity when a happy facial expression is made (Schwartz et al., 1973). An increase in activity from both of these muscles indicates a general activation of facial muscles or electrical interference with the signals, whereas an increase in activity from the zygomaticus but not the frontalis indicates smiling. Skin conductance. SCRs are often used as a measure of emotional arousal. For example, Berlyne, Craw, Salapatek, and Lewis (1963) found that the amplitude of galvanic skin responses increased with incongruity and novelty of visual displays. Larger galvanic skin responses occurred in response to photographs considered to be aversive (dead bodies) in another study (Geer & Klein, 1969). Yet another investigation (Smith & Sloboda, 1968) discovered no systematic relationship between the type of picture viewed (pleasant, unpleasant, neUtral) and log conductance change scores. Since none of the photographs of infants should be considered aversive by adults, it was hypothesized that larger SCRs should occur when the cuter pictures were being viewed. Procedure The experimental procedure was first explained to the participant (Appendix B), questions were answered, and signatures were obtained on the standard Department of Psychology Research Consent Form (Appendix B). The participant's left palm, left cheek and left forehead were cleaned with 70% ethanol and allowed to dry prior to electrode attachment. Two silver/silver chloride electrodes with contact areas of .78 cm2 were then attached to the hypothenar eminence of the left hand approximately 1.5 cm apart (see Bundy & Fitzgerald, 1975). 10 The electrolytic medium used was a Unibase preparation (Lykken & Venables, 1971). These electrodes were used to measure SC changes. Four Grass miniature silver electrodes (contact area = .12 cm2) were attached to the participant's face in order to measure facial expression changes electromyographically. Beckman Offner Paste was used as the electrolyte. Two electrodes were placed side-by-side over the zygomaticus muscle and the other two electrodes were placed side-by-side over the frontalis muscle of the forehead. Grounding was provided by a metal plate covered with Offner Paste and attached to the participant's left wrist. After the electrodes were attached, participants were seated inside the experimental booth and asked to relax for a few minutes while electrode connections and equipment functioning were checked. When everything was in order, the instructions for Part 1 of the experiment were given via an intercom located inside the booth (see Appendix B for instructions read to the participants). When the experimenter was certain that the instructions were understood, Part I began. Part 1 consisted of the presentation of a control photograph (from the appropriate age group) paired with itself followed by each of the six stimulus photographs paired with themselves. The purpose of the control photograph was to prevent large initial responses from being included in the main analysis. The participants controlled the length of time each pair was on the screen by pressing a button attached to the chair inside the booth when they were tired of looking at a particular pair. The next pair was then automatically projected after a 15 second blank slide interval (used to allow physiological responses to return to baseline). 11 After all six stimulus pairs were shown, Part 2 began. Participants were instructed over the intercom to simply look at the pairs of photographs during this part of the experiment. All possible pairs (30) of the six stimulus photographs were shown for 10 seconds each with a blank slide interval of 1-2 seconds between each pair. Part 3 was the same as Part 1. In Part 4 participants were asked to rank prints of the six photographs previously seen in their order of cuteness. Participants were asked questions about their prior experience with infants and then were debriefed. Debriefing consisted of an explanation of the experiment in more detail. Polygraph records were shown to interested participants, and those who wished to receive a copy of the general results of the experiment were asked to fill out an envelope with their name and address. Slides within both age groups were presented in two different orders (List 1 and List 2). One order was the reversal of the other (see Appendix A, Table A1). Experimental conditions and the number of participants assigned to each are shown in Table 1. Participants were systematically assigned to a condition depending on their sex and order of testing. Eye movements were recorded by one of five different observers. A double-observer system was used to determine inter-observer relia- bilities. Two observers simultaneously recorded the looking behavior of a single participant who looked at approximately 18 pairs of slides for eight seconds each. The amount of time recorded for each side of each slide was correlated for each pair of observers. These correlations averaged .85 with a range of .69 to .94 and are consistent with general findings in this area.) 12 Table 1 Number of Participants Assigned to Each Experimental Condition Infant Age Group Order of Presentation Males Females 4-Month List 1 5 5 List 2 5 5 8-Month List 1 5 5 List 2 5 5 Data scoring Looking time, smiling, and SC were scored during Parts 1 and 3. The looking time score for each slide was the total amount of time spent fixating the slide before the participant pushed the button to remove the slide from the screen. The smiling measure consisted of the percent change in average zygomaticus EMG activity from the 5-second preslide interval to the slide-on interval, scored using the integrated signal. Segments of the integrated recording which were affected by artifacts or general facial movement were not scored. These segments were identified by visual inspection of the raw EMG signals from the zygomaticus and frontalis muslces. The last second of each slide-on interval was discarded due to equipment ertifact. The magnitude of the first SCR beginning during the five seconds following slide onset was measured for each slide. Because of the preponderance of non-responses (scored as zero) the SCRs for each slide in Part 1 and Part 3 were combined for most analyses. 13 During Part 2 only paired looking time was scored. (The term "paired looking time" will be used to distinguish this variable from the looking time measured during Parts 1 and 3.) The percent of time spent looking at each slide in each pair was first calculated, and then the average percent of time spent looking at each of the six slides was determined. RESULTS Rank orders of the photographs (obtained during Part 4) were significantly intercorrelated (E [18]=.54, p.< .02 for participants viewing photographs of 4-month-olds; E:[18]=.47, p,< .05 for participants viewing photographs of B—month-olds), although there were considerable individual differences. There were 18 different orders for the 20 participants viewing photographs of 4-month-olds and 16 different orders for the 20 participants viewing photographs of 8-month-olds. The average ranks for each photograph were significantly correlated for males and females viewing photographs of 4-month-olds (p£4]=.86, p_< .05) and for males and females viewing photographs of 8-month-olds (3I4]=.85, p'< .05) (see Appendix A, Table A2), indicating that individual variations in rankings were not systematically related to sex of participant. Because of the variability in cuteness rankings, further analyses relate participants' responses to their own rankings of the photographs rather than the actual photographs viewed. Single factor analyses of variance with order of presentation as the independent variable were performed for looking time, smiling, and SC. Looking time and smiling were further analyzed for interactions between time seen (Part 1 or Part 3) and order of presentation. There were no significant effects of order. Accordingly the order of stimulus presentation to which a participant was assigned was excluded from subsequent analyses. 14 15 A 2 x 2 x 2 (sex of participant x age of infant x cuteness ranking of photograph) multivariate analysis of variance was performed on the four dependent variables: looking time, smiling, SC, and paired looking time. Two levels of cuteness were established by considering the average value of each dependent measure for the two cutest photographs and the average value for the two least cute photographs. Data for Parts 1 and 3 were combined for looking time, smiling, and SC. An alpha level of .05 was distributed among the separate tests in this analysis and the multivariate analysis reported below. The only significant multivariate E'was that for cuteness (E[4,33]=l7.65, p_< .05, see Appendix A, Table A3). Variations in looking time, smiling, SC, and paired looking time as a function of ranked cuteness are illustrated in Figures 1 through 4. Post hoc univariate analyses revealed significant effects of cuteness for looking time (E[1,36]=24.65, p.< .05) and paired looking time (f[1,36]=41.54, JB>< .05). The computer program used for this analysis constructs a correlation Inatrix for the variables under consideration. Three significant positive <:orrelations were found in the 28 which were calculated. Average amount of looking was correlated at p(38)=.41 (p_ < .05) with the (lifference in looking between the most cute and the least cute photographs. Siimilarly, average amount of smiling was correlated at [(38)=.84 (1;‘< .01) with the difference in smiling between the most cute and the 1east cute photographs. Individuals who looked at the photographs Tionger overall showed a larger differentiation in looking between most (:ute and least cute photographs than individuals who looked less overall. ‘The same type of relationship held for smiling. However, since the *correlation between looking and smiling was -.19 (n.s.), the participants Looking Time in Seconds Percent Change in Smiling 14 13 12 11 10 7O 60 50 40 30 20 10 p — - - b .4 N 00 .h 01 0‘ Ranked Cuteness Figure 1. Time spent looking at infant photographs at each cuteness rank. L Ranked Cuteness Figure 2. Percent increase in smiling during presentation of infant photographs at each cuteness rank. Magnitude of SCRs in Micromhos Percent Paired Looking Time 14 12 10 ON-QOSCD 60 55 50 45 40 - P p - -l N w .h 0'1 03 Ranked Cuteness Figure 3. Magnitude of SCRs to infant photographs at each cuteness rank. Ranked Cuteness Figure 4. Average percent of paired looking time spent looking at infant photographs at each cuteness rank. 'q 18 who looked for a long time were not necessarily those who smiled a great deal. The third significant correlationwas that between paired looking time and the difference in smiling between the most and least cute photographs (p_[38]=.49, p_< .01). A 2 x 2 x 2 x 2 (sex of participant x age of infant x cuteness ranking x time seen) multivariate analysis of variance was performed on looking time and smiling to assess the effect of repeated presentations (Part 1 and Part 3). The results of this analysis are presented in Appendix A, Table A4. Significant multivariate E_ratios were obtained for cuteness ([[2,35]=16.19, p_< .05), time seen (£12,35]=12.61, p_< .05), and the interaction between cuteness and time seen (fI2,35]=7.99, p_< .05). The effect of cuteness could be attributed to looking time (fIl,36]=23.32, p.< .05), as in the first analysis. The time seen effect also was due to looking time (EI1,36]=25.92, p_< .05). Looking time decreased from 13.9 seconds per slide during Part 1 to 8.9 seconds per slide during Part 3. The significant interaction between cuteness and time seen, again due to looking time ([11,36]=13.82, p_< .05), indicated that the amount of decline in looking time was dependent on cuteness level. As illustrated in Figure 5, the decline in looking was less for the cuter photographs. Because SORs occurred to only 38.54% (185/480) of the photograph presentations, an analysis of variance was deemed inappropriate to determine the effect of repeated presentation. There was a significant decline (£f7.22, p_< .001) in the rate of response from Part 1 (53.33%; 128/240) to Part 3 (23.75%; 57/240). 15 14 13 12 ll 10 Most Cute Least Cute Looking Time in Seconds WONWO Part 1 Part 3 Time Seen Figure 5. Interaction between cuteness ranking and time seen for looking time. Aflthough the purpose of the control slide was to eliminate large initial responses from being included in the main analysis, how participants responded to this slide was of interest since Berlyne (1958) has found that degree of novelty and length of looking are positively related. The control slide was seen twice, once at the beginning of Part 1 and once again at the beginning of Part 3. Based on Berlyne's findings, it was predicted that the second presentation of the control slide would elicit prolonged looking, since it would be relatively novel compared to the other pictures, each of which had been presented 11 times prior to the second presentation of the control slide. aalfi‘ 20 Separate 2 x 2 x 2 (sex of participant x age of infant photograph viewed x time seen) repeated measures analyses of variance were performed for looking and smiling at the control slide. There were no significant effects or interactions for either analysis. Wilcoxon matched pairs signed-ranks tests were then performed to determine whether or not looking and smiling at the control slide changed relative to looking and smiling at the other six slides. The basic datum for this analysis was the number of slides looked or smiled at less than the control slide during Part 1 and during Part 3. The change in rank was significant for looking time (gf3.44, p < .001, one-tailed test) with a larger number of slides being looked at less than the control slide during Part 3 than during Part 1. There was no significant change in rank for smiling. A 83R to the control slide occurred for half the participants iduring both Part 1 and Part 3. This frequency of response was significantly higher than the frequency of response to the other six slides during Part 3 (x2[l]=15.22, p < .001). DISCUSSION The hypothesis that college students' behavior is differentially affected by the degree of perceived cuteness of infant photographs was confirmed for looking time. Photographs ranked as cuter were looked at longer. It is possible that looking preferences were elicited by something other than perceived cuteness, and that the participants' perceptions of cuteness were directly determined by how long they looked at a particular photograph. However, this explanation seems unlikely because of the short lengths of time involved and because of the general comment by many participants that they had looked longer at the cuter babies. Smiling increased when the photographs were on the screen (Figure 2), indicating that smiling is a likely response to a photograph crf an infant. Before accepting this conclusion, however, it is necessary tn) determine whether or not an increase in smiling is a general response t1) any photograph. The amount of smiling was not significantly related tr) ranked cuteness of the photographs. It is possible that with a Wider range of cuteness, a relationship between smiling and cuteness MKNJld be evident, although it is also possible that smiling is a general Ii'eSponse to infants regardless of their cuteness. The high positive coY‘r‘elation between average amount of smiling and the difference in Sm‘iling between the most cute and least cute photographs indicates that participants who smiled a lot did discriminate between levels 0f'<:uteness in their smiling responses. A significant effect of 21 22 cuteness on smiling might be evident if only the data from participants who consistently smiled were considered. There was slightly more smiling to the cutest photograph than to the others (Figure 2). Another problem with the smiling measure can be traced to the instructions given to the participants (Appendix B). They knew their facial expressions were being monitored both electromyographically and supposedly by an experimenter's observation. They were told to try to keep their face in a natural position. Some participants may have tried harder to maintain a "straight face" than others, and the instructions in general may have eliminated much of the smiling which would have occurred naturally. Diverting the participant's attention away from his or her face may eliminate this problem. However, Schwartz et al. (1973) attached electrodes to other parts of their participants' bodies to help keep their attention away from their faces, and found no differences in pattern of responses from a condition where facial expression obviously was being monitored. SC also was unrelated to ranked cuteness. The major problem vvith this measure was the marked degree of response variability. SCRs occurred to only 38.33% of the presentations of photographs, arui there was a wide range in the magnitude of the responses. Apparently, wuatching slides of infants is not a very arousing experience, at least as reflected by SCRs. Repeated exposure to photographs of infants produced shorter vcfluntary looking times and a decrease in the number of SCRs from Part 1 to Part 3. There was not, however, a decrease in looking. Snriling, or the size of the SCR within Parts 1 and 3, implying that tihere was no rapid habituation of responses to the photographs. The control slide, which was relatively novel both times it was seen. 23 'was responded to in the same way both times. This indicates that the observed habituation of looking and SCRs from Part 1 to Part 3 was specific to the six infant photographs each participant was exposed to and not a generalized habituation to all infant photographs. The increase in rank of the control slide for looking time supports the general observation that novel stimuli are looked at longer than are familiar stimuli. There were no significant differences in the length of time photographs were looked at, in the amount of smiling, and in how the photographs were ranked for the two sexes. A more important consideration than sex when investigating responsiveness to infant photographs might be experience with infants. Participants in this study reported no sex differences in experience with infants. The observation that participants looked longer at infant photographs which they ranked higher in cuteness suggests that adults Inay vary their looking at real infants who differ in perceived cuteness. ()f course, with real infants there are many more cues than just “facial features that may contribute to a perception of cuteness. Eth if looking behavior is affected by cuteness, however it is defined tzy'an individual, this variation in adult behavior could affect the development of individual infants. Eye contact between an adult and an infant contributes to the atrtachment process (Robson, 1967), is related to visual fixation behavior of female infants (Moss & Robson, 1968), and shows a temporal Pairterning very similar to that of adult verbal conversation (Jaffe, Stern, & Peery, 1973). The amount of time a mother looks at her ifI‘f'ant has also been shown to be positively related to the infant's 24 total fixation time when shown a series of pictures (Noll, 1971), and the sight of an adult's eyes seems to be a "setting event" for infant conditioning (Bloom, 1974). Although Bloom (1974) found that an averted gaze was just as effective a catalyst for learning as was a direct gaze, in natural situations an adult's eyes are most likely to be visible to the infant when the adult is looking at the infant. Stern (1974) investigated the mutual gazing patterns of mother— infant dyads in detail. He found that the infant initiates and terminates 94% of all mutual gazes, which might imply that the looking behavior of the mother is irrelevant to the patterning of eye contact. However, he also noted that when the mother was gazing at the infant, the probability that the infant would initiate a gaze was increased and the probability that the infant would terminate a gaze was decreased. So, although the infant controls the exact timing of mutual gazes, the mother's looking behavior certainly influences this timing. Since the frequency and duration of mutual visual regard appears to be related to some aspects of infant development, and since an adult's looking at an infant seems to be influenced by how cute the infant is perceived to be, more information on how "cuteness" is defined would be enlightening. The amount of contact an adult has with other infants may determine how important perceived cuteness is to his or her behavior. Also, if repeated experience with an infant increases perceived cuteness, parents' behavior may be only minimally affected by their own infant's physical appearance. Parke and Sawin (1975) observed that parents of cuter newborn infants looked longer at their infants during the first few days of the infant's life, but whether or not this increased looking leads to an earlier or stronger than 25 average establishment of a pattern of mutual gaze is unknown. How the pattern of mutual visual regard develops from this point surely depends on a variety of factors. For example, both parental attitudes and sex of the infant have been shown to be relevant to the development of mutual gaze patterns (Moss & Robson, 1968). The results of the current study suggest that the physical appearance of the infant, and adults' perceptions of the infant's cuteness be included in future investigations of adult-infant relations, particularly in regard to mutual gazing behavior. APPENDICES APPENDIX A APPENDIX A SUPPLEMENTARY TABLES Table A1 Order of Presentation of Stimuli Infant Age Group 4 Months ‘ 8 Months Left Slide Tray Right Slide Tray Left Slide Tray Right Slide Tray Part 1 control control control control l4 l4 8 8 6 6 12 12 5 5 5 5 7 7 10 10 12 12 18 18 13 13 14 14 Part 2 12 7 12 8 14 13 14 18 7 5 8 5 6 12 10 12 5 l4 5 14 13 12 18 12 6 >5 10 5 12 14 12 14 13 6 18 10 14 7 l4 8 5 13 5 18 7 6 8 10 12 5 12 5 14 6 14 10 7 l3 8 l8 (slide trays are then changed sides for remaining 15 pairs) Part 3 Same as Part 1. ¥ Note. The order of presentation for half the participants was the reverse of that above. 26 27 Table A2 Average Rankings of Each Infant Photograph by Male and Female Participants Infant Age Group Photograph Males Females 4 Months 13 1.8 1.8 5 2.2 2.2 12 3.6 2.9 6 3.1 4.0 14 4.5 5.3 7 5.8 4.8 8 Months 5 1.3 2.9 8 2.5 1.9 10 3.1 3.3 14 4.1 3.7 12 4.2 3.8 18 5.8 5.4 28 Table A3 Multivariate Tests of the Effects of Sex of Participant, Age of Infant in Photographs, and Cuteness Rankings of Photographs on Looking Time, Smiling, SCR, and Paired Looking Time Source MShypothesis MSerror df I F P Sex --- --- 4, 33 .69 .6] Age --- --- 4, 33 .50 .74 Sex x Age --- --- 4, 33 1.08 .38 Cuteness --- --- 4, 33 17.65 .0001* Univariate Post Hoc Looking 376.69 15.28 1. 36 24.65 .0001* Smiling 2.59 .73 l, 36 3.56 .07 SCR 10.00 155.91 1. 36 .06 .80 Paired Looking 5336.10 128.46 1, 36 41.54 .0001* Sex x Cuteness --- --- 4. 33 .56 .69 Age x Cuteness --- -—- 4. 33 .94 .45 Sex x Age x Cuteness --- --- 4. 33 .84 51 *Significant at a = .05 29 Table A4 Multivariate Tests of the Effects of Sex of Participant, Age of Infant in Photograph, Cuteness Rankings of Photographs, and Time Seen (Part 1 or Part 3) on Looking Time and Smiling Source MShypothesis MSerror df ' F P Sex —-- --- 2, 35 .52 .60 Age --- --- 2, 35 .30 .74 Sex x Age --- --- 2, 35 1.82 .18 Cuteness --- --- 2, 35 16.19 .0001* Univariate Post Hoc Looking 1452.03 62.27 1, 36 23.32 .0001* Smiling 11.55 2.92 l, 36 3.96 .05 Sex x Cuteness --- --- 2, 35 .12 .88 Age x Cuteness --- --- 2, 35 .90 .42 Sex x Age x Cuteness --- --- ' 2, 35 1.39 .26 Time --- --- 2, 35 12.61 .0001* Univariate Post Hoc Looking 3422.50 132.04 1, 36 25.92 .0001* Smiling .21 2.19 l, 36 .10 .76 . Sex x Time ' -—- -—- 2, 35 2.35 .11 Age x Time ' --- --- 2, 35 .60 .56 Sex x Age x Time --- --- 2, 35 1.73 .19 Cuteness x Time --- --- 2, 35 7.99 .0014* Univariate Post Hoc Looking 191.84 13.88 1. 36 13.82 .0007* Smiling 1.43 1.86 l, 36 .76 .39 . 35 1.82 .18 35 1.61 .21 Sex x Cuteness x Time --- —-— Age x Cuteness x Time --- -_- N N N so Sex x Age x Cuteness x Time --- --- *Significant at a = .05 APPENDIX B APPENDIX B PARTICIPANT FORMS AND INSTRUCTIONS Instructions to Participants We are interested in how adults respond to photographs of infants. In particular, we want to find out how pictures of different babies affect your general level of arousal (as measured by the galvanic skin response) and very small changes in facial expression (as measured by electromyography). To make these measurements I will attach two sensors to the palm of your left hand, a metal plate to your left wrist and four small sensors to your face. These sensors can pick up very small changes in skin sweating activity and muscle movement. These changes will be recorded by equipment in the next room while you look at photographs of babies. During the experiment you will be seated inside this booth and photographs of babies will be projected from back here onto the screen in front of you. Some of the time you will be asked to control slide changes by pushing a button on the arm of the chair inside the booth; at other times the slide changes will be automatic. You will be instructed over an intercom inside the booth as to exactly what you are to do. While you are looking at the photographs, an observer will be watching you through a small hole below the screen. He will be watching 3O 31 for any noticeable changes in facial expression. You should try to keep your face relaxed and neutral during the experiment; the sensors can detect small changes which you probably won't even notice. You should also try to keep your left hand as still as possible, without being uncomfortable, to make recording skin changes easier. If for any reason you decide not to participate in this experiment, just let one of us know and we will stop the experiment. The intercom inside the booth is connected to the next room so I will be able to hear you if you wish to stop. Do you have any questions? u—l O 32 Michigan State University Department of Psychology DEPARTMENTAL RESEARCH CONSENT FORM I have freely consented to take part in a scientific study being conducted by: Katherine Hildebrandt under the supervision of: H. E. Fitzgerald The study has been explained to me and I understand the explanation that has been given and what my participation will involve. I understand that I am free to discontinue my participation in the study at any time without penalty. I understand that the results of the study will be treated in strict confidence and that I will remain anonymous. Within these restrictions, results of the study will be made available to me at my request. I understand that my participation in the study does not guarantee any beneficial results to me. I understand that, at my request, I can receive additional explanation of the study after my participation is completed. Signed Date 33 Instructions Read to Participants During Experiment Part 1: During this part of the experiment you will see pairs of baby pictures; both pictures of the pair will be of the same baby. You may look at each pair as long as you'd like. When you are tired of looking at the pair you should press the button on the right side of your chair. Be sure you press the button only once. There will then be a blank slide interval of about 15 seconds followed by another pair of pictures. You should again look at the picture as long as you'd like, and press the button when you're tired of it. You should continue to do this with each pair ofpictures. Do you have any questions? (Wait for response.) The first pair will come on the screen in about 15 seconds. Part 2: During this part of the experiment you will see a number of combinations of the pictures you just saw. Slide changes will be automatic; all you have to do is look at the pictures. About half way through the series there will be a short break for a slide change. Part 3: For this part you are to do the same thing you did at the beginning of the experiment; look at each pair as long as you would like, then press the button when you are tired of a particular pair. Part 4: The last thing I'd like you to do before getting out of the booth is to smile broadly for a few seconds. 0K, thank you. APPENDIX C APPENDIX C PILOT STUDY Introduction The purpose of the initial study was twofold: first to investigate the relationship between rated cuteness of infant photographs and physiognomic characteristics of the infants' faces, and second to obtain a set of infant photographs varying in rated cuteness for use in a subsequent study. Previous studies of babyishness have typically employed a rating technique whereby adults are asked to judge drawings of infant faces which vary systematically in their component features. Simple drawings were rated as appearing "young" in two related studies if the eyes were high or wide, the nose short and the mouth narrow (Bradshaw, 1969; Bradshaw & McKenzie, 1971). The position of the eyes has also been found to affect ratings of cuteness, with eyes positioned slightly above the center of the face being rated cuter than eyes placed lower or higher on the face (Brooks & Hochberg, 1960). Gardner and Wallach (1965) even developed a super "babyish" profile by exaggerating real infants' facial proportions. An extensive investigation of the relationship between infant facial features and adults' ratings of attractiveness was conducted by Sternglanz, Gray and Murakami (1974). Eye height, eye width, 34 35 eye height and width together, iris size, and vertical feature position were systematically and individually varied. In general, one particular variation of each feature was rated as significantly more attractive than other variations of that feature. A composite drawing including the most highly rated facial features resulted in a face characterized by a high forehead and large eyes. The authors concluded that Lorenz's (1943) hypothesis that certain infantile facial features are attractive to adults was supported. The present study attempted to extend these findings to photographs of real infants. Method Participants Participants were 23 male and 20 female college students recruited from various sources, the majority from introductory psychology classes. The average age of the males was 21.96 years (§D_= 4.06 years) and of the females was 20.05 years (§D_= 1.79 years). The majority of the participants (33/43) stated that they currently had no regular contact with infants. Stimuli Stimuli were obtained by photographing l4 4-month-old infants A |><| N 19.13 weeks, _sp = .86) and 18 8-month-old infants (X = 35.29 weeks, C? II ___ 1.26). Infants were brought to the laboratory by their parents and photographed while seated in a high chair or in their parent's lap, at a time when their facial expression was judged to be relatively neutral. Each infant was photographed at least twice. Eleven photographs from each age group were chosen to serve as stimuli with photographic quality and neutrality of expression as the principle selection criteria. 36 Each photograph was independently rated or measured by two experimenters on each of the following characteristics: 1) sex (0 = female, 1 = male); 2) size of head (measured from the top of the head to the bottom of the chin with the projector at a set distance); 3) distance from top of head to line drawn between corners of eyes; 4) direction of gaze (0 = indirect, l = direct); 5) eye color (0 = brown, 1 = blue); 6) distance between inside corners of the eyes; 7) diameter of the iris (left and right averaged); 8) eye height (left and right averaged); 9) eye width (left and right averaged); 10) mouth (0 = open, 1 = closed); and 11) drooling (0 = no, 1 = yes). Measurements 3, 6, 7, 8, and 9 were made with the photograph adjusted so that the distance from the top of the head to the chin was 192 millimeters. All of the measurements were made to the nearest millimeter; disagreements between the two experimenters were resolved by averaging the two measurements. In no case did the two experimenters disagree by more than three millimeters. Procedure Each participant observed 30 pairs of infant photographs for 10 seconds each. The photographs were presented via slides rear projected on a screen set into one wall of a booth containing a comfortable arm chair. Participants were instructed to decide which photograph of a pair was cuter, and then to circle LEFT or RIGHT on a prepared form, corresponding to the side on which the cuter photograph appeared. Each participant rated pairs of photographs from one age group only. Thirty pairs were randomly chosen from the 110 pairs possible when 11 stimuli are used, with the constraint that when a pair was 37 selected, its left-right reversal was removed from the list. When 30 pairs were chosen, this group was designated List 1. List 2 consisted of the remaining possible pairs (left-right order again randomly determined) plus five of the pairs included in List 1. These five common pairs were included to make it possible to compare, at least in part, the pattern of preferences for participants rating List 1 and those rating List 2. The order of presentation of pairs in each list was randomized, except that the five common pairs were placed at the same points in each list (pairs 5, 6, ll, 13, and 25). See Table Cl for the number of participants assigned to each condition. After the participant finished the ratings he or she was asked how much contact he or she currently had with infants and what aspects of the photographs were most important in helping him or her decide which photographs were cuter. Table C1 Number of Participants Assigned to Each Condition Infant Age Group Order of Presentation Males Females 4-Month List 1 6 5 List 2 5 5 8-Month List 1 6 5 List 2 6 5 Results Cuteness Ratings The results of the cuteness ratings are summarized in Tables C2 and C3. Choices for each cell are expressed in proportions, since 38 the number of participants within each cell varied from 5 to 21. The sum of the proportions reflects the relative number of times each photograph was rated cuter than the other 10 photographs. A one-dimensional scaling procedure was applied to both grOUps of data (Guilford, 1954). A chi-square test of the internal consistency of the obtained scale values was significant (x2 [45] = 70.73, p_< .01 for 4-month-olds, x2[45] = 65.40, p_< .01 for 8-month-olds), indicating that more than one scale is involved. Due to the manner in which these data were collected, a multidimensional scaling procedure was not feasible. Therefore, the photographs were assigned ranks based on the magnitude of the sum of pr0portions for each photograph, resulting in the ranks shown in Table C4. There were no significant differences between ranks obtained for males alone and ranks obtained for females alone for either group of photographs. Table C2 Proportion of Participants Choosing Each Photograph as Cuter Than Each Other Photograph of 4-Month-01ds Photograph Photograph Chosen as Cuter Chosen as Less Cute 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 ll 12 13 14 10 36 64 27 09 .55 .55 xxx 40 48 73 50 11 40 1 00 60 00 .50 .30 50 xxx 70 91 36 12 64 60 36 18 50 .40 52 30 xxx 80 20 13 27 80 29 00 30 .40 27 .09 20 xxx 18 14 73 90 73 O9 86 .70 50 .64 80 82 xxx 39 Table C3 Proportion of Participants Choosing Each Photograph as Cuter Than Each Other Photograph of 8-Month-01ds Photograph Photograph Chosen as Cuter Chosen as Less Cute 5 6 8 9 10 12 l3 l4 l6 17 18 5 . . . . . . . . . 6 .36 xxx .82 .05 .45 .09 .27 .09 .36 .27 .09 8 . . . . . . . . . . 9 1.00 .95 .82 xxx .64 .73 .91 .91 .82 .82 .45 10 .55 .55 .55 .36 xxx .18 .45 .45 .55 .45 .09 12 .77 .91 .82 .27 .82 xxx .82 .36 .55 .91 .27 13 .55 .73 .73 .09 .55 .18 xxx .27 .68 .45 .36 14 1.00 .91 .73 .09 .55 .64 .73 xxx .82 .91 .27 16 .64 .64 .45 .18 .45 .45 .32 .18 xxx .45 .09 17 .64 .73 .41 .18 .55 .09 .55 09 .55 xxx .09 18 1.00 .91 1.00 .55 .91 .73 .64 :73 .91 .91 xxx Z 7.79 7.15 6.69 1.95 5.82 3.50 5.41 3.35 6.15 6.12 1.71 Table C4 Rank Order of Photographs 4-Month-01ds 8-Month-Olds Sum of Sum of Rank Photo Sex Proportions Rank Photo Sex Proportions 1 5 M 8.06 1 5 F 7.79 2 13 M 7.20 2 6 F 7.15 3 12 F 5.50 3 8 F 6.69 4 10 M 5.33 4 16 F 6.15 5 9 F 5.08 5 17 F 6.12 6 8 M 5.05 6 10 M 5.82 7 11 F 4.73 7 13 F 5.41 8 6 M 4.70 8 12 M 3.50 9 4 F 4.69 9 14 M 3.35 10 14 F 3.23 10 9 M 1.95 11 7 M .73 11 18 F 1.71 Binomial tests of the differences in ratings on the five common pairs between the two lists showed that there were no significant 40 differences for the 4-month-old lists and one significant difference for the 8-month-old lists (p_= .03 for the pair including photographs 16 and 13). Relationship Between Cuteness Ratings and Facial Features The measurements and ratings of the facial features of the infants in the 11 photographs in each group are shown in Tables C5 and C6 with the photographs arranged in rank order. Table C5 Measurements and Ratings of Facial Features in Photographs of 4-Month-01d Infants Facial Feature Photograph (See Text for Descriptions) 5 13 12 10 9 8 11 6 4 l4 7 1 1 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 0 0 1 2 209 228 226 242 245 248 200 250 290 200 254 3 121 107 105 112 113 114 103 115 104 113 121 4 1 0 1 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 1 5 0 1 0 0 0 1 0 O 1 0 0 6 27 34 32 34 34 30 33 33 34 36 36 7 13.015.015.014.013.013.517.5 14.915.517.014.0 8 12.011.5 13.012.0 11.012.013.5 11.0 13.010.0 10.0 9 21.0 25.0 23.5 22.0 23.0 22.5 25 5 21.0 26.0 25.5 23.5 10 1 0 0 O 0 0 1 0 1 1 O 11 0 1 1 1 0 0 1 1 0 1 1 The correlations between the ranks of the photographs and their scores on the 11 facial characteristics are reported in Table C7. Only one correlation is significant, that for the distance between the inside corners of the eyes of the 4-month-olds. Photographs of 4-month-old babies with less distance between the eyes were rated cuter than those with larger distances between the eyes. Since 41 this is the only significant correlation, and since the correlation for the 8-month-olds is in the opposite direction, this relationship is probably a chance finding. Due to the small amount of data available and the unsystematic appearance of graphs of facial features plotted against ranks of photographs, no further attempts were made to describe the relationship between these variables. Future studies are planned to further investigate this problem. Table 06 Measurements and Ratings of Facial Features in Photographs of 8-Month-01d Infants Facial Feature Photograph (See Text for Descriptions 5 6 8 16 17 10 13 12 14 9 18 1 0 O 0 0 0 1 0 l 1 l O 2 120 112 139 135 145 118 115 125 126 124 126 3 70 71 77 73 74 66 67 69 72 70 67 4 l l l l l O l 1 l 0 0 5 1 0 l O l 0 1 l l 1 1 6 21 20 29 28 27 18 22 20 18 20 20 7 9.0 9.0 8.0 9.0 8.010.011.0 9.0 9.0 9.0 10.0 8 6.0 8.0 7.0 6.5 6.5 6.5 8.0 7.0 7.0 6.5 7.0 9 14.5 15.0 14.5 14.0 14.0 15.0 17.0 15.0 14.0 14.0 15.0 10 0 0 l 0 0 O 0 0 0 0 0 11 0 O 0 O 0 0 O 0 1 0 1 The participants' statements about which features of the photographs were most important in helping them decide which were cuter are summarized in Table C8. The number of responses elicited from participants by the three experimenters were not significantly different (p = .93, 1.34. 1.20, p< .05). 42 Tab1e C7 Correlations Between Cuteness Rankings and Facial Features Facial Feature 4-Month 8-Month 1 -.23 .54 2 -.03 -.03 3 .08 -.45 4 -.02 -.58 5 -.06 .39 6 .65* -.36 7 .38 .38 8 -.49 .04 9 .28 -.07 10 .18 -.30 ll .18 .48 *p_< .05 Table C8 Features of the Photographs Mentioned as Being Most Important When Rating Cuteness Number of Responses 4-Month-01ds 8-Month-Olds Feature Males Females Total Males Females Total Total Facial Expression 9 7 l6 6 10 16 32 Eyes 6 8 14 10 8 18 32 Hair 4 6 10 9 3 12 22 Fatness 4 3 7 9 3 12 19 Facial Proportions 2 3 5 4 0 4 9 Ears 2 1 3 4 2 6 9 Complexion l 1 2 O 3 3 5 Nose 0 1 l 2 1 3 4 Drooling 2 0 2 l 0 1 3 Mouth 1 l 2 1 0 l 3 Eyelashes 0 3 3 0 0 0 3 Markings on Face 0 O O 2 1 3 3 Clothes 0 0 0 l 1 2 2 Miscellaneous 2 l 3 2 2 4 7 43 Choice of Photographs for Subsequent Study Six photographs from each age group were chosen for use in the subsequent study. Choices were based on the following criteria: 1. 3 photographs with sum of proportions greater than 5.0 (chosen as cuter than the other 10 photographs more than half the time) and 3 photographs with sum of proportions less than 5.0 were chosen. 2. At least one of each sex in the 3 photographs with sum of proportions greater than 5.0 and in the 3 photographs with sum of proportions less than 5.0 were chosen. 3. Higher ranked photographs which were chosen were rated cuter than all lower ranked photographs which were chosen at least 50% of the time. The six photographs chosen for each group and cell proportions (taken from Tables C2 and C3) are shown in Tables C9 and C10. Table C9 Cuteness Ratings (Expressed as Proportions) of Photographs of 4-Month-Old Infants Chosen for Use in Subsequent Study Photograph Chosen as Cuter Photograph Chosen as Less Cute 5 13 12 6 14 7 5 xxx .20 .40 .00 .10 .00 13 .80 xxx .20 .29 .18 .00 12 .60 .80 xxx .36 .20 .18 6 1.00 .71 .64 xxx .27 .10 14 90 82 80 .73 xxx .09 7 1:00 1:00 :82 .90 .91 xxx 44 Table C10 Cuteness Ratings (Expressed as Proportions) of Photographs of 8-Month-01d Infants Chosen for Use in Subsequent Study Photograph Chosen as Cuter Photograph Chosen as Less Cute 5 8 10 12 14 18 5 xxx .36 .45 .23 .00 .00 8 .64 xxx_ .45 .18 .27 .00 10 .55 .55 xxx .18 .45 .09 12 .77 .82 .82 xxx .36 .27 14 1.00 .73 .55 .64 xxx .27 18 1.00 1.00 .91 .73 .73 xxx Discussion Infant cuteness seems to be determined by a complex combination of many factors, including facial features, facial expression and individual differences in raters. Future studies utilizing a larger number of infant photographs and larger samples of adults are planned to attempt to determine exactly how these factors affect cuteness ratings. More saphisticated statistical techniques than those used here will be necessary. In sum, the question of what makes a baby cute has been answered only tentatively. The next question -- Does the degree of perceived cuteness of an infant affect adult behavior? -- was investigated in a subsequent study. 45 References Bradshaw, J. L. The information conveyed by varying the dimensions of features in human outline faces. Perception and Psychophysics, 1969’ g: 5'9. Bradshaw, J. L., & McKenzie, B. E. Judging outline faces: A developmental study. Child Development, 1971, 42, 929-937. Brooks, V., & Hochberg, J. A psychophysical study of cuteness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1960, 11, 205. Gardner, B. T., & Wallach, L. Shapes of figures identified as a baby's head. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965, 29, 135-142. Guilford, J. P. Psychometric Methods. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc., 1954. Lorenz, K. Die angeborenen Formen Moglicher Erfahrung. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 1943, 5, 235-409. Sternglanz, S. H., Gray, J., & Murakami, M. An ethological approach to adult preferences for infantile facial features. Paper presented at the 1974 meetings of the Animal Behavior Society, University of Illinois. REFERENCES REFERENCES Beier, E. G., Smock, C. 0., Izard, C. E., & Tougas, R. R. Responses to the human face as a standard stimulus: a reexamination. Journal of Consulting Psychology. 1957, 21, 165-170. Bennett, S. Infant-caretaker interactions.~ Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 1971, 19, 321—335. Berlyne, D. E. The influence of complexity and novelty in visual figures on orienting responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1958. 5.5.. 789-796. Berlyne, D. E. Conflict, Arousal and Curiosity. New York: McGraw- Hill. 1960. Berlyne, D. E., Craw, M. A., Salapatek, P. H., & Lewis, J. L. Novelty, complexity, incongruity, extrinsic motivation, and the GSR. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1963, pp, 560-567. Berlyne, D. E., 8 Lawrence, G. H. Effects of complexity and incongruity variables on GSR, investigatory behavior and verbally expressed preference. Journal of General Psyphology, 1964, 21, 21-45. Berman, P. W. Attraction to infants: Are sex differences innate and invariant? Paper presented at the 1975 meetings of the American Psychological Association, Chicago, Illinois. Bernick, N. The development of children's preferences for social objects as evidenced by their pupil responses. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago, 1966. Bloom, K. Eye contact as a setting event for infant learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1974, 11, 250—263. Bowlby, J. Attachment and Loss. Volume 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books, 1969. Bradshaw, J. L. The information conveyed by varying the dimensions of features in human outline faces. Perception and Psychophysics, 19699 £9 5’9. Bradshaw, J. L., & McKenzie, B. E. Judging outline faces: a developmental study. Child Development, 1971, 42, 929-937. 46 47 Brooks, V., 8 Hochberg, J. A psychophysical study of cuteness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1960, 11, 205. Bundy, R. 5., 8 Fitzgerald, H. E. Stimulus specificity of electrodermal recovery time: An examination and reinterpretation of the evidence. Psychpphysiology, 1975, 12, 406—411. Connolly, M. F., 8 Harris, L. Effects of stimulus incongruity on children's curiosity as measured by looking time and expression change. Psychonomic Science, 1971, 29, 232—234. Day, H. Looking time as a function of stimulus variables and individual differences. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1966, 22, 423—428. Dion, K. K. Children's physical attractiveness and sex as determinants of adult punitiveness. Developmental Psychology. 1974, 19, 772-778. English, R., 8 Palla, D. A. Attitudes towards a photograph of a mildly and severely mentally retarded child. Training School Bulletin, 1971, 99, 55—63. Fantz, R. L. The origin of form perception. Scientific American, 1961, 299, 66-72. Faw, T. T., 8 Nunnally, J. C. The effects on eye movements of complexity, novelty and affective tone. Perception and Psychpphysics, 1967, 2, 2634267. Fullard, W., Reiling, A., Love, C., 8 Shaw, F. An investigation of Lorenz's "Babyness." Paper presented at the 1975 meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, Colorado. Gardner, B. T., 8 Wallach, L. Shapes of figures identified as a baby's head. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965, 29, 135-142. Geer, J. H., 8 Klein, K. Effects of two independent stresses upon autonomic responding. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1969, 74. 237-241. Gewirtz, J. On designing the functional environment of the child to facilitate behavioral development. In L. L. Dittman (Ed.), Early Child Care: The New Perspectives. New York: Atherton Press, 1968. Hess, E. H. Attitude and pupil size. Scientific American, 1965, 212, 46-54. Hess, E. H. Ethology and developmental psychology. In Mussen, P. (Ed.), Carmichaels' Manual of Child 9§ycholpgy. Volume 1, 1970. Hess, E. H., 8 Polt. J. M. Pupil size as related to interest value of visual stimuli. Science, 1960, 192, 349-350. 48 Hicks, R. A., Reaney, T., 8 Hill, L. Effects of pupil size and facial angle on preference for photographs of a young woman. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1967, 29, 388-390. Jaffe, J., Stern, D. N., 8 Peery, J. C. Conversational coupling of gaze behavior in prelinguistic human development. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1973, 2, 321-329. Jones, 2. R., 8 Moyel, I. S. The influence of iris color and pupil size on expressed affect. Psychbnomic Science, 1971, 22, 126—127. Leckart, B. T., 8 Bakan, P. Complexity judgments of photographs and looking time. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965, 21, 16-18. Leckart, B. T., 8 Faw, T. T. Looking time: a bibliography. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1968, 21, 91-95. Lorenz, K. Die angeborenen Formen moglicher Erfahrung. Zeitschrift fur Tie1~psychologie, 1943, 9, 245—409. Lorenz, K. Physiological mechanisms in animal behavior. In Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology. No. 4. New York: Academic Press, 1950. Lykken, D. T., 8 Venables, P. H. Direct measurements of skin conductance: a proposal for standardization. Psychophysiology, 1971, 9, 656-672. Moss, H. A., 8 Robson, K. S. ‘Maternal influences in early social visual behavior. Child Development, 1968, 99, 401—408. Noll, R. L. The relationship between visual behavior of mothers and infant attentiveness. Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, 92(l-A). 250. Parke, R. D., 8 Sawin, D. B. Infant characteristics and behavior as elicitors of maternal and paternal responsivity in the newborn period. Paper presented at the 1975 meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, Colorado. Robson, K. S. The role of eye-to-eye contact in maternal-infant attachment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1967, 89 13-27. Schwartz, G. E., Fair, P. L., Greenberg, P. S., Friedman, M. J., 8 Klerman, G. L. Facial electromyography in the assessment of emotion. Paper presented at the 1973 meetings of the Society for Psychophysiological Research, Galveston, Texas. Smith, E. E., 8 Sloboda, W. Some relations between affect and recall: An exploratory study. Psychonomic Science, 1968, 12, 257-258. Stass, J. W., 8 Willis, F. N. Eye contact, pupil dilation, and personal preference. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 2, 375-376. 49 Stern, D. N. Mother and infant at play: the dyadic interaction involving facial vocal and gaze behaviors. In Lewis, M., and Rosenblum, L. A. (Eds.), The Effect of the Infant on its Caregiver. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1974. Sternglanz, S. H., Gray, J., 8 Murakami, M. An ethological approach to adult preferences for infantile facial features. Paper presented at the 1974 meetings of the Animal Behavior Society, University of Illinois. Walster, E., Aronson, V., Abrahams, D., 8 Rottman, L. Importance of physical attractiveness in dating behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1966, 4, 508-516. Webb, N. C. The use of myoelectric feedback in teaching facial expression to the blind. American Foundation for the Blind, Research Bulletin, 1974, 27, 231-262. Webb, W. W., Matheny, A., 8 Larson, G. Eye movements as a paradigm of approach and avoidance behavior. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1963. 19, 341-347. Wohlwill, J. F. Amount of stimulus exploration and preference as differ- ential functions of stimulus complexity. Perception and Psycho- physics, 1968, 4, 307—312.