FACULTY INFLUENCE AND PROFESSSONAL PARTICIPATIOR OF DOCTORAL STUDENTS Yhosis for the Eton?» a? M. A. MICHIGAN STATE UNSVERSIT’Y Jam Pea“ 3963 n mum; “L“!L‘W H1111 ['1‘ [will Illlllllilljflll w ? “My, gm wk- -.’.f“;:, “‘qu FACULTY INFLUENCE AND PROFESSIONAL PARTICIPATION 0F DOCTORAL STUDENTS By John Pease A THESIS Submitted to the College of Social Sciences Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Sociology and Anthr0pology 1963 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to thank Professor David Gottlieb for serving as my major professor. He has not only guided the writer, but his writings have suggested the focus of the thesis. I would also like to thank Professors Allan Beegle and-William Faunce for serving as members of my thesis committee, and Dr. Allan Tucker for his assistance in obtaining financial support for the study. Leah (who treats all this as a harmless game) and Barbara (who sometimes wishes that harmless games would have an end) have made the task an easier one. To them and to Professor and Mrs. Limpus, I would like to publicly express my sincere appreciation for their help and encouragement. my debt to them will always be a source of pleasant embarrassment. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS . . TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION: THE PROBLEM . . . . . . . . II PRELIMINARY CONSIDERATIONS: DISCUSSION OF PREVIOUS SMESO . . C . Q C C O C . O C III THE SAMPLE, FIELD PROCEDURES AND DESIGN OF ‘ANALYSISoooo‘ooooooooooooo IV FACULTY INFLUENCE AND PROFESSIONAL PmTICHATION C . o . . . -. . C . C O O C V rSUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS , ,,. , , , , , BIBLIOGRAPHY. ... . . . o . APPENDICES. . ... ... APPENDIX 1.». APPENDIX II 0 APPENDIX III. APPENDIX IV . APPENDIX V. iii ii 16 39 63 7O 73 74 -92 .93 95 96 TABLE 10 11 12 l3 14 15 16 LIST OF TABLES RETURNS OF THE MAILED QUESTIONNAIRE FINAL RETURNS OF THE MAILED QUESTIONNAIRE COMPARISON OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY TERM OFFICIALLY GRADUATED COMPARISON OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY YEAR OFFICIALLY GRADUATED COMPARISON OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY SEX COMPARISON OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY DEGREE COMPARISON OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY COLLEGE COMPARISON OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY DIVISION OF STUDY DESCRIPTION OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY DEPARTMENT AND DIVISION FOR THE TRADITIONAL ARTS AND SCIENCES DESCRIPTION OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY DEPARTMENT AND DIVISION FOR_THE PROFESSIONAL FIELDS PARTICIPATION IN PROFESSIONAL ACTIVITIES DURING THE PERIOD OF DOCTORAL TRAINING PARTICIPATION IN PROFESSIONAL ACTIVITIES BY AVERAGE NUMBER OF MEETINGS WITH MAJOR PROFESSOR PER TERM PARTICIPATION IN PROFESSIONAL ACTIVITIES BY NUMBER OF MEETINGS WITH FULL GUIDANCE COMMITTEE PARTICIPATION IN PROFESSIONAL ACTIVITIES BY DISCUSSION OF PROGRAM WITH MEMBERS OF GUIDANCE COMMITTEE ON AN INDIVIDUAL BASIS PARTICIPATION IN PROFESSIONAL ACTIVITIES BY NUMBER OF COMMITTEE MEMBERS MET WITH INDIVIDUALLY PARTICIPATION IN PROFESSIONAL ACTIVITIES BY OPPORTUNITY To DISCUSS CAREER PLANS WITH MEMBERS OF THE FACULTY iv PAGE 19 20 22 23 24 25 25 26 29 3O 41 43 44 47 49 51 TABLE 17 18 '19 20 PARTICIPATION IN PROFESSIONAL ACTIVITIES BY NUMBER OF FACULTY MEMBERS KNOWN ON'A.PERSONAL BASIS PARTICIPATION IN PROFESSIONAL ACTIVITIES BY FACULTY ENCOURAGEMENT INTERRELATIONSHIPS OF DOCTORAL STUDENT PARTICIPATION IN PROFESSIONAL ACTIVITIES INTERRELATIONSHIPS OF FACULTY ENCOURAGEMENT FOR DOCTORAL STUDENT PARTICIPATION IN PROFESSIONAL ACTIVITIES PAGE 53 57 60 62 APPENDIX II III IV LIST OF APPENDICES AN APPROXIMATION OF THE FIELD QUESTIONNAIRE AN APPROXIMATION OF THE COVER LETTER USED IN THE MAILING AN APPROXIMATION OF THE COVER LETTER USED FOR THE 1962 GRADUATING GROUP AN APPROXIMATION OF THE COVER LETTER USED IN THE FOLLOW-UP MAILING A RESEARCH NOTE ON THE NO ANSWER vi PAGE 74 92 93 95 96 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION: THE PROBLEM ('A number of studies have recently appeared in the literature of sociology concerning the socialization of the adult in the context of the American graduate school.1 These studies have demonstrated that students experience changes in their attitudes, values and behaviors, and that these changes are related to the social system of the graduate school. Quite broadly, the concept of socialization refers to the processes by which an individual learns the ways of a social group so that he can function within that group. It includes the learning and internalizing of apprOpriate norms and attitudes along with the neces- sary knowledge and skills. In contradistinction to the psychiatric and psychological conceptualizations, socialization in the perspective of the sociologist does not end at the time of puberty. 1James A. Davis with David Gottlieb, Jan Hajda, Carolyn Huson and Joe L. Spaeth, Stipends and Spouses: The Finances 2; American Arts and Science Graduate Students. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1962; David Gottlieb, "American Graduate Students: Some Characteristics of A5piring Teachers and Researchers," Journal 2f Educational Psychology, 53 (1961), 236-40, and; David Gottlieb, "Processes of Socialization in American Graduate Schools," Social Forces, 40 (1961), 124-31. 2 Recent articles2 have pointed out how, under the banner of Freudian doctrine, socialization has been restricted to the time of child deve10p- ment--the time when "the individual is originally inducted into the social organization".3 Emphasizing the time of childhood deve10pment in the socialization process is, for Parsons, prOper for two reasons. First, the child ". . . has so far to go, . . ."4 and second, There is reason to believe that, among the learned elements of personality in certain respects the stablest and most enduring are the major value- orientation patterns and there is much evidence that these are "laid down" in childhood and are not on a large scale subject to drastic alteration during adult life.5 Nonetheless, for most sociologists and especially those who work in the areas of adult socialization, careers, and occupations a fundamental assumption is that socialization occurs throughout life. . . . the personality is a growing, changing, plastic structure that is subject to modification throughout life by the social influences that surround the individual. . 2David Gottlieb, Processes pf Socialization in the American Graduate School. An unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, 1960, and Howard S. Becker and Anselm L. Strauss, "Careers, Personality, and Adult Socialization," The American Journal g£_Sociology, LXII (1956), 253-63. 3Gottlieb, Processes g£_Socialization $3 the American Graduate School, 22, cit., p. 2. 4Talcott Parsons, The Social System. Glencoe: The Free Press, 1951, p. 208. 51bid. . 6Gottlieb, Processes g; Socialization in the American Graduate School, 22. cit., p. 1. 3 Just as the new recruit is socialized into the army, the groom into a life of marriage, and an employee into the job situation, the graduate student is socialized into his professional field. Although the socialization of the adult differs from that of the child, in that the adult has already develOped a somewhat established character structure, the processes are much the same. The socializer teaches, serves as a model, and invites participation. Individuals being socialized learn and internalize the new expectations and develop and modify their self- conceptions by role-taking, observation, and participationu“I . Perhaps, the most useful definition of adult socialization is that made by Merton. Socialization . . . refers to the learning of social roles. In its application to the medical student, socialization refers to the processes through which he develops his professional self, with its character- istic values, attitudes, knowledge, and skills, fusing these into a more or less consistent set of diaposi- tions which govern his behavior in a wide variety of professional (and extra-professional) situations. Socialization takes place primarily through social interactions with people who are significant for the individual--in the medical school, probably with the faculty members above most others 7 Thus, the concept of adult socialization is seen as the process whereby the individual deve10ps or modifies his concept of self through the acquisition of personality characteristics vis-a-vis contacts with "significant others". As Gottlieb has pointed out, Merton's conceptual- ization of socialization is one which assumes that the 7Robert K. Merton, "Socialization: A Terminological Note," in Robert K. Merton, George Reader and Patricia L. Kendall (editors), The Student Physician: Introductory Studies in_the Sociology 2£_Medical Education. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1957, p. 287. 4 . . professional self is an important aspect of the total self, and that the professional school and the personnel directly'and indirectly associated with it are significant influences in the formation of the professional self.8 Moreover, Merton assumes that in the context of the professional school the most important "significant other" for the student is that of the graduate faculty. Merton's conceptualization also notes that the processes of socialization occur both formally and informally. . direct learning through didactic teaching of one kind or another, and indirect learning, in which attitudes, values and behavior patterns are acquired as byproducts of contact with instructors and peers, with patients, and with the members of the health team. It is the latter, the less conSpicuous learning process, with which Merton is primarily concerned. Indirect learning is frequently and more easily neglected and is viewed as being acquired as a byproduct of contact with the faculty, among others. Elkin, too, emphasizes the "informal" process of socialization and further notes that the persistence of any on-going social group, in its existing form, is continually dependent on socialization, . on the learning by new recruits and participants of both the necessary skills and appropriate attitudes and sentiments. The infantry recruit should learn not only the basic skills of fighting, he should also know 8Gottlieb, Processes g£_Socialization in the American Graduate School, 92. cit., p. 3. 9Robert K. Merton, "Some Preliminaries to a Sociology of Medical Education," in Merton, Reader and Kendall, 22. cit., p. 41. 5 the traditions and slang of his unit, and believe that commands should come through military hierarchy.10 Q In learning to become a professional, the student formally learns the skills of the field. He is both formally and informally introduced to the "code of ethics", ideals, norms and protocols of the profession. At much the same time, he is informally socialized through interaction with the faculty. Through stories, observations, imitation, and inter- action with these ”significant others", he deve10ps a conception of his professional selfqt The Problem The task of this research is to explore one small part of the graduate student socialization process-~that of the "professionaliza- tion" of the doctoral student. Specifically, we shall explore the role the faculty plays in accounting for the professional participation of doctoral students. we shall investigate, describe, and analyze how students of varying degrees of faculty encouragement and varying degrees of faculty contact participate in the professional activities of their reSpective fields. We shall describe how students who par— ticipate and students who do not participate in these various activities compare in terms of their relationships with the faculty. It is the thesis of this research that in the social system of the graduate school the faculty constitutes a "significant other" for the graduate student and that much of the student's professional behavior is 10Frederick Elkin, The Child and~Society: The Process 2f_Socializa- tion. New York: Random House, 1960, pp. 101-102. 6 a result of his interaction with the faculty. we will utilize the con- ceptual framework described by Merton and Gottlieb. "That is, casting the faculty in the role of the socializer and the student in the role of the socializee, we want to see if the faculty constitutes a significant influence for the student."11 We hypothesize that the participation in professional activities by the graduate student during the period of his doctoral training is re- lated to (a) the amount of contact he has had with the faculty and (b) the amount of encouragement he received from the faculty. The more contact a student has with the faculty and the more he is encouraged by the faculty towards a professional activity, the more the student will tend to participate in that activity. The reader is asked to bear in mind that this is not a study of the total graduate student socialization process, but an exploratory inves- tigation of one part of the socialization process—-the graduate student as a professional and the influence of the faculty in this part of his socialization. We are not, of course, pr0posing that the faculty is the sole influence in the life of the graduate student for previous studies have shown that among other factors, peers, spouses, and finances have a' direct influence on him.12 The focus of this research is the influence of the faculty. 11Gottlieb, Processes gf Socialization in the American Graduate School, 22, cit., p. 4. 12See, for example, William Erbe, "Gregariousness, Group Member- ship, and the Flow of Information," The American Journal 2f Sociology, LXVII (1962), 502-16 and, Davis, 2531;, 22. cit. 7 It.is also important that the reader keep in mind that our data covers only the more structural and behavioral aspects of the student- faculty relationship and does not directly deal with attitudes, values, cognitions or beliefs. Moreover, we are asserting that the faculty socialize, that they impart their values to the student and yet we know that reciprocity is characteristic of all interaction. Most of all, we should be cognizant of the fact that student conformity to various 'faculty influences does not guarantee that socialization has occurred. It may simply be that the student recognizes the path of least re- sistance to the degree and, not that he has internalized the attitudes and values of the faculty.- Consequently, we shall necessarily be some- what conservative concerning the inferences we make about socialization. CHAPTER II PRELIMINARY CONSIDERATIONS: A DISCUSSION OF PREVIOUS STUDIES1 There have been relatively few studies that have been concerned with the student-faculty relationship in the area of graduate student sociali- zation. The few that have been done, however, have had direct influence on this study. These studies come from two major sources: the study of the medical student by Robert Merton and associates of the Bureau of Applied Social Research,2 and the study of the arts and science student by James Davis and associates of the National Opinion Research Center.3 “The first of these, the study of the medical student is only partially reported. A collection of articles, edited by Merton, Reader and Kendall, present initial findings of the research, along with a description of the setting and theoretical orientation of the project. Most of the articles focus on the process of occupational decision: making. One of these, an article on "The Decision to Study Medicine," 1For a complete review of the literature on adult socialization, see Gottlieb, Processes 2£_Socialization ig_the American Graduate School, 22. Cite, pp. 4'53. 2Merton, Reader and Kendall, 92, 215. For a methodological review, see James A. Davis' review article in The American Journal of Sociology, LXIII (1958), 445-46. A more substantive review is Howard Becker's in the American Sociological Review, 23 (1958), 336-37. 3Davis, t al., 22. cit. 9 analyzes correlates of early and late decisions to enter medical school. 4 _ A..- “...—mag...- WI» 3*“ -. Mn... ...—...... .,_ n.---- -1: MMmaM—M‘ , VIP-"‘5'." ”WWW '- Rogoff finds that "the more contact with physicians (in the family),t ‘.-_— ......- nw-w— rooA~ 1.. .‘ earlier interest was aroused"5 and that an early decisionm to study medi- -u--4.+~H-.-., M . ....— w cine is generally a more enthusiastic one. Those who decided early --o— .-“ ‘n‘ ._._....41 “"'-..‘ .1... 7 reported that their significant reference group was their family, whereas students who decided on medicine after entering college reported more social support from their peers. Thielens' article6kcompares entrants to medical school with entrants ...-1 ...«u- “M to law school and shows that the decision to study-medicine is made __ ...-a .n . .uux earlier than the decision to study law. The_author describes three factors which he thinks contribute to the difference: course requirements for admission to medical school are more extensive than those for admission to law school, medical entrants have had more contact with representatives of their future profession, and ,‘ physicians have higher standing in the community i and larger incomes than lawyers.7 ..40"-‘\ 'I‘lv- 0 One article in this collection that Specifically discusses the sig- nificance of student-faculty relationships is Kendall and Selvin's "Tendencies Toward Specialization in,Medica1 Training."8 -They-found that students are increasingly likely to express a preference for specializa- tion as they progress through their training. Among first year students, 4Natalie Rogoff, "The Decision to Study Medicine,” in Merton, Reader and Kendall, 22, cit., pp. 109-29. 5Ibid., p. 128. 6Wagner Thielens, Jr., "Some Comparisons of Entrants to Medical and Law School," in Merton, Reader and Kendall, 22, cit., p. 130-52. 71bid., p. 151. 8Patricia L. Kendall and Hanan C. Selvin, "Tendencies Toward Specialization in Medical Training" in Merton, Reader and Kendall, 22. cit., pp. 153-78. lO 35fipercent expect to go.into Speciality practice, 41 percent of the second year students, 56 percent of the third year students and 74 percent of the fourth year students.9 The authors suggest the reason that a high per- centage of first year students choose general practice is because they know so little about their own capacities and the requirements of the "Preference” for general practice is their way Of specialities. Their avoiding a premature commitment. The fourth-year students, on the other hand, show a marked prefer- ence for speciality practice and Kendall and Selvin hypothesize that this preference reflects the advanced students' awareness of the horizons of their professions and their own limitations. Thus, they choose to narrow the area in which they will commit themselves to competence. During the fourth year the student actually applies for and receives his internship assignment. During this time when final commitments are made, Kendall and Selvin suggest that a selective process is set in motion.10 Many students change their preferences by the time they formally make appli- cation for internships. The authors present data which shows that there is a tendency for the better students (as measured by cumulative grades) to request and receive the Speciality internships.11 The authors sug- gest that the better jobs await those students who have completed Speciality internships and that most of the students know this but only the better students can count on getting good recommendations from the 9Ibid., p. 156. 101bid., p. 166. 111bid., pp. 169-70. 11 faculty. The poorer students recognize this, and alter their expec- tations to be more consistent with what they think their chances are.12 The-students are assisted in their evaluations of their pro- fessional self-image by discussions with the faculty. "During the course of these disCussionS, students may be discouraged from some of the plans which they have developed and encouraged in others."13 Although Kendall and Selvin's suggestions of faculty influence are largely Speculation, they do hypothesize the kinds of relationships in which we are interested and will be investigating. ‘ Another article in The Student Physician which is relevant to the prOposed research is "The DeveIOpment of a Professional Self— Image."14 In this article, Huntington presents data which shows how the medical student develops an image of himself asa doctor, rather than student, as he goes through his professional training. She finds that the student alters his professional image as he interacts with persons in his role-set who have varying expecta- tions of him. 121bid., p. 170. 13Ibid., p. 171. 14Mary Jean Huntington, "The Deve10pment of a Professional Self-Image," in Merton, Reader and Kendall, 22. cit., pp. 179-87. 12 SELF-IMAGES AND ATTRIBUTED IMAGES OF FIRST-YEAR STUDENTS IN DIVERSE ROLE RELATIONSHIP315 Percentage Percentage who who thought of thought others defined In their themselves as themselves as dealings with: doctors doctors Faculty 2 2 Classmates 3 0 Nurses 12 8 Patients 31 75 (N-162) (N-162) Although these data are not conclusive, they do suggest that in the environment of the professional (graduate) school, the faculty becomes an important reference group for individuals in training. It is here that the student, to a large extent, deve10ps his professional self-image and obtains a definition of his profession. The other major study which has produced data relivant to the re- search prOposed here, is the National Opinion Research Center's national J survey of arts and science graduate students. This study has reported a relationship between faculty encouragement and the faculty rating of the students'ability.16 15Adapted from Huntington, ibid., p. 182. 16Adapted from table 9.16 in Davis, §£_al., 22, cit., p. 274. l3 Faculty Ability Encouraged Rating (percent) (N) High and middle 77 (1491) Low 55 ( 731) They also found a relationship between faculty encouragement and the type of jobs that students eventually obtain. Students who are high on our faculty encourage- ment index are more likely to get academic jobs, even controlling for the major predictor variables. Whether such students are more highly motivated or whether the professors who encouraged them hustled around to get them jobs, we do not know.17 It would seem that students who are defined as the better students ability-wise are not only encouraged by the faculty during the time of their training, but are also Sponsored in the job market. The writings from the Chicago study whigh have most relevance for '._ this study are those on the Processes gf_Socialization in the American Graduate School.18 Gottlieb focused on faculty influence in the changes of student preferences for teaching and research. His purpose was to ascertain whether the faculty had any influence in student changes in values, and to investigate this influence from the standpoint of adult socialization. "We want to see if the faculty constitutes a significant reference 17Ibid., p. 118. 18Gottlieb, Processes gf_Socialization in_the American Graduate School, gp. cit. 1 \\ 14 group for the student in deciding his career."19 The major hypotheses ~....——- ..—._...-. .—.._ of the research were:20 1. When the orientation of the student and faculty are in Substantial agreement, the student orien— tation will be reinforced and will be less amenable to change than the student whose orientation is similar to that of the faculty. 2. The student whose orientation is different from the faculty will tend to change in the direction of the faculty. The highest rates of career preferences change will take place among those students with the highest degree of interpersonal contact with the faculty. Data are Run—....“ . presented which support each of the above hypotheses. He shows that changes in career preference are related to (a) the passing of time, \ (b) departmental climate, and (c) integration with the faculty.21 He \ ézjound that "The pe0p1e who are integrated with the faculty change more j {than those who are not, and this effect accelerates with the passage of l atime up to the fourth year of graduate study.”22 He also found that Students reapond quite markedly to faculty encouragement.23 ,u- I I In an article entitled "The Development of Identification with an I iOccupation,”24 Becker and Carper discuss how student-faculty relationships 19Ibid., p. 6. 201bid., p. 7. 21Gottlieb, "Processes of Socialization in American Graduate Schools," 22, cit., p. 131. 221bid., p. 127. 23Ibid., pp. 130-31. 24Howard S. Becker and James W. Carper, "The Development of Identification with an Occupation," The American Journal of Sociology, LXI (1956), 289-98. 15 play a major part in the acquisition and/or maintenance of Specific kinds of occupational identities among graduate students in physiology, philoso- phy and mechanical engineering. In their discussion of the physiologists, Becker and Carper observed that He (the student) is often able to model his behavior after that of a professor or of an ideal constructed of the characteristics of several professors, learning through observation of them thI kind of tasks which physiologists in fact perform. 5 Another way in which the faculty's relationship to the student affects the latter's occupational identity is through the pattern of sponsorship, "a means by which persons low in an occupational hierarchy 5 are recommended by more highly placed persons for better positions."26 As such, the sponsorship pattern involves reciprocal obligations between the parties, for if the Student fails he will have embarrassed his sponsor. When a person is Sponsored into a first position in the work world after leaving graduate-school, he feel obli- gated to act as a true member of the occupation and to remain within it, because of the trust placed in him by his Sponsor. The creation of this obligation solidifies occupational attitudes and loyaltieS--the individual . feels that he must remain what he has become in order not to let down his sponsor--and thus Strengthens identification with occupational title and ideology.27 25Ibid., p. 292. 26Ibid., p. 297. 27Ibid., p. 298. CHAPTER III THE SAMPLE, FIELD PROCEDURES AND DESIGN OF ANALYSIS The sample for this study consists of 358 individuals who of- ficially received a doctorate (Ph.D. or Ed.D.)1 from Hypothetical University during the calendar years of 1960, 1961 and for the period January 1 to March 13 (winter quarter), 1962. It should be emphasized that this is a very select group.2 The title of this thesis, "Faculty Influence and Professional Participation Among Doctoral Students," is somewhat misleading. A more accurate title would be "Faculty Influence and Professional Participation Among Successful Doctoral Students at Hypothetical U.” Hypothetical U. is a large midmwestern university located in the town of Lazarsville, four miles from the city of Stoufferton. Lazarsville, a town of roughly 25,000, is almost entirely a college and residential community. Stoufferton, the state capital, is a relatively large industrial city of approximately 100,000. Employment in Stoufferton is mainly with the automobile industry and with the state civil service. 1During the period of January 1, 1960 to March 13, 1962 the University did not grant any D.V.M. or D.B.A. degrees. 2Preliminary analysis of data from a national survey reveals that attrition for students enrolled at the post-master level is upward of 25 percent. The Graduate Student Attrition Project, directed by David Gottlieb, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan. 16 l7 Hypothetical U. was founded in the mid-nineteenth century, and it ranks as one of the nation's ten largest educational institutions with about 4,000 of its 24,000 students enrolled in the graduate school. The graduate school at Hypothetical U. has eight colleges and 66 departments offering degree programs in 275 different fields. Degrees that may be earned on the doctoral level are the Doctor of Philosophy (Ph.D.), Doctor of Education (Ed.D.), Doctor of Business Administration (D.B.A.), and Doctor of Veterinary Medicine (D.V.M.). The enrollment for the fall term of 1961—62 for the total graduate school was 3,596. Of this total, 63 percent were studying on the master's level and the remaining 37 percent were doctoral candidates. The University grants approximately 200 doctoral degrees each year. The listing and procurement of the sample was completed in two stages because the study began before the (March) 1962 group had graduated. Consequently, stage one will refer to those persons who graduated in the calendar years 1960 and 1961 and stage two will refer to those who graduated in 1962. The list of the sample for stage one was obtained from the records of the Office of the Registrar. A total of 417 persons had officially received the doctorate during the calendar years of 1960 and 1961. Of this number 50 had foreign addresses and 367 had American addresses. On January 24, 1962, a COpy of the field questionnaire,3 a cover letter,4 and a self-addressed envelOpe were mailed first—class to each 3An approximation of the field questionnaire used in this study appears in the appendix. 4An approximation of the cover letter used in the mailing appears in the appendix. 18 person with a foreign address (50) who had officially received a doctor's degree from Hypothetical U. during the calendar years 1960 and 1961. On January 25 and 26, 1962, a COpy of the field questionnaire, a cover letter, and a Stamped, self-addressed envelOpe were mailed first- class to each person with an American address (367) who had officially received a doctor's degree from Hypothetical U. during the calendar years 1960 and 1961. Consistent with the best dictates on maximizing returns from a mailed questionnaire,5 each questionnaire was hand processed;6 each cover letter was individually typewritten and personally signed on letterhead stationary of the Office of Research DevelOpment, Hypothetical U.7 Addresses were individually typewritten on each envelope and each envelOpe was then hand stamped with small denomination stamps.8 Thirty days after the original mailing, a "follow-up" mailing was sent. The returns, as of that day, were 56 percent for the total sample-~62 percent for American addresses. 5Mildred Parten, Surveys, Polls and Samples: Practical Procedures. New York: Harper and Brothers, 1950, pp. 383-402. 6D. S. Longworth, ”Use of a Mail Questionnaire,” American Sociological Review, 18 (1953), pp. 310-13. 7William J. Goode and Paul K. Hatt, Methods in_Social Research. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc., 1952, p. 179. 8Longworth, gp. cit. 19 TABLE-I RETURNS OF THE MAILED QUESTIONNAIRE (One Month After Original Mailing) Address Returns . Group (percent) (N) American Addresses 62 (367) Foreign Addresses 12 ( 50) Totals 56 (417) The follow~up.included a brief cover letter,9 another COpy of the questionnaire, and a stamped, self~addressed envelOpe. It was sent only to those persons who had American addresses and, as of February 26, 1962, had not returned the first questionnaire. A follow-up was not sent to persons with a foreign address because it usually takes thirty days or more just for first-class mail to reach respondents in foreign countries. A total of 138 of the original reSpondents were included in the follow~up. On May 1, 1962, approximately three months after the first mailing, coding began and returns for the mailing at that time were 80 percent of the total sample; 88 percent for American addresses and 30 percent for foreign addresses. 9An approximation of the cover letter used in the follow-up mailing appears in the appendix. I‘l'..ll{{[[tl1 20 TABLE 2 FINAL RETURNS OF THE MAILED QUESTIONNAIRE Address Returns 10 Group (percent) (N) American addresses 88 (363) Foreign addresses 31 ( 54) Totals 80 (417) During the course of the mailing, it was learned that eight persons included in the original listing of this part of the sample did not re- ceive a doctorate, but a Diploma of Advanced Graduate Study. The Diploma of Advanced Graduate Study is usually referred to by the ab~ breviation "D.A.G.” and is sometimes referred to as the "six year degree" (estimating four years for the bachelor's, one year for the mater's and one year for the D.A.G.). The only requirement for this degree is successful completion of 45 hours of class work beyond the master's degree, It requires no language, no research and no thesis. By definition, persons with a Diploma of Advanced Graduate Study were not part of the sample and were eliminated from further analysis. The total number of individuals in this part of the sample, 1960 and 1961, is 409. The number of individuals for whom we have completed questionnaires is 327--80 percent. The list of the sample for stage two was obtained from the Office of the Registrar during the first week of March. Persons graduating in 10Address changes that occurred since the first mailing necessitated adjustments in these figures. 21 1962 were given a questionnaire and a cover letter11 along with the usual official forms which must be completed in order to formally qualify for graduation. On March 20, 1962, one week after graduation and three weeks after the official deadline to complete application for graduation, 65 percent of the 37 persons graduating had returned their questionnaires. On that same day, March 20, 1962, a copy of the questionnaire, a cover letter and a Stamped self-addressed envelOpe were sent to each of the remaining 13 persons who had graduated but, as of that day, had not returned their completed questionnaire. On May 1, 1962, coding began and the reSponse from this part of the sample was 84 percent. In summary, the selected sample N for this study is 446; the reSponse is 358-~80.2 percent of the sample. A_Comparative Description of the Sample and the Response In an attempt to ascertain the representativeness of the sample, this section compares the-original sample with the reSponse by term officially graduated, by year officially graduated, by sex, by degree earned, by college, and by department and division. Table 3 shows the proportion of the sample and the reSponse for each of the nine different graduating groups included in this survey. 11An approximation of the cover letter given to individuals in this group appears in the appendix. 22 TABLE 3 COMPARISON OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY TERM OFFICIALLY GRADUATED Term Sample Reaponse Graduated (percent) (percent) Winter, 1960 6 6 Spring, 1960 14 13 Summer, 1960 11 12 Fall, 1960 12 10 Winter, 1961 10 11 Spring, 1961 13 14 Summer, 1961 ll 12 Fall, 1961 l4 14 Winter, 1962 8 9 Totals 99 101 (N : 446) (N 3 358) For two of the graduating groups-~Winter, 1960 and Fall, l96l--the proportion of the sample and proportion of the reSponse are the same. In five of the nine groups (Summer, 1960; Winter, Spring and Summer, 1961; and Winter, 1962), the reSponse is over-represented by one per“ cent. The only group where the reSponse representation varies more than one percent from that of the sample is in the Fall, 1960 group where the response is under-represented by two percent. Generally, the more recent graduates are somewhat over-represented. In Table 4 the data are categorized by year graduated rather than term 23 graduated. The 1960 graduates constitute 43 percent of the sample and 40 percent of the reSponse, a difference of three percent. The 1961 graduates, on the other hand, constitute 48 percent of the sample and 51 percent of the reSponse, an over-representation of three percent. The 1962 gradu- ates (only one graduating group) constitute one percent less in the sample than in the reSponse. TABLE 4 COMPARISON OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY YEAR OFFICIALLY GRADUATED Year Sample Response Graduated (percent) (percent) 1960 43 40 1961 48 51 1962 8 9 Totals 99 100 (N : 446) (N : 358) Why do the more recent graduates return better than graduates of some time ago? Part of the answer may be found in previous researches which have shown that higher returns are realized from those for whom 12 the organization is a significant reference group and from those who 12Carl F. Reuss, "Differences Between Persons Responding and Not Reaponding to a Mailed Questionnaire," American Sociological Review, 8 (1943), pp. 433—38. Also, Richard F. Larson and William R. Catton, Jr., "Can the Mail-back Bias Contribute to a Study's Validity?," American Sociological Review, 24 (1959), pp. 243~45. 24 are characterized as having the more ”extreme" Opinions on the subject under investigation.13 Taken in aggregate, one might expect the above characteristics to be more predominant among the recent graduates, and thus expect the recent graduates (1961 and 1962) to have a higher per- centage of returns. TABLE 5 COMPARISON OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY SEX Sex Sample ReSponse (percent) (percent) Male 94 94 Female 6 6 Totals 100 .100 (N : 446) (N 2 358) When we compare the reSponse and the original sample in terms of sex, we see that there is no difference in terms of representa- tiveness. l3Daniel Katz and Hadley Cantril, ”Public Opinion Polls," Sociometry, 1, (1937), pp. 155—79. Also, Parten, op, cit., p. 391. 25 TABLE 6 COMPARISON OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY DEGREE Degree Sample ReSponse (percent) (percent) Ph.D. 89 89 Ed.D. 10 11 Totals 99 100 (N = 446) (N : 358) Table 6 shows that by degree earned the Ph.D.‘s constitute the same prOportion of the response as they do the sample and the Ed.D.‘s are slightly overmrepresented in the reSponsew-a difference of one percent. TABLE 7 COMPARISON OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY COLLEGE College Sample ReSponse (percent) (percent) Agriculture 27 26 Business and Public Service 6 6 Communication Arts 3 3 Education 24 26 Engineering 5 4 Home Economics - - Science and Arts 33 32 Veterinary Medicine 2 2 Totals 100 99 (N : 446) (N = 358) 26 Table 7 shows the percent of the sample and percent of the reSponse for each of the eight colleges. Four of the colleges, Business and Public Service, Communication Arts, Home Economics and Veterinary Medicine, are equally well represented. Three colleges, Agriculture, Science and Arts and Engineering, are slightly under-represented--a difference of one percent--whereas the college of Education constitutes 24 percent of the sample and 26 percent of the reSponse. TABLE 8 COMPARISON OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY DIVISION OF STUDY Division of Study Sample ReSponse (percent) (percent) Humanities 6 7 Social Science 11 9 Biological Science 10 10 Physical Science 13 12 Traditional Arts and Sciences, All Divisions 39 38 Agriculture 27 26 Education 24 26 "Other Professional” 10 9 Professional Fields, All Divisions 61 62 Totals 100 100 1“ N : 446) (N : 358) 27 Table 8 shows that the traditional arts and sciences comprise 39 percent of the sample and slightly less, 38 percent, of the response. By division, the Biological Sciences constitute the same prOportion in each category. The division of Humanities is slightly over-represented, a difference of one percent, whereas the division of Physical Sciences is under-represented by the same margin. The division of Social Sciences constitutes 11 percent of the sample but nine percent of the reSponse, a difference here of two percent. For the professional fields which constitute 61 percent of the sample and 62 percent of the reSponse, both the divisions of Agriculture and "other professional” are under-represented by one percent. The division of Education is over-represented by two percent. We have now presented a descriptive comparison of the sample and the reSponse for each of the known sample values. We have seen that most of the variables under scrutiny were proportionately the same for the reSponse as they were for the initial sample. The largest discrepancy between the sample and the reSponse was for "year graduated” where we observed that the recent graduates constitute three percent more of the reSponse than they do of the sample. As most of the differences are quite small, no correction will be made for the sample—response discrepancies. However, the reader is asked to keep these differences in mind as we proceed with the analysis. Table 9 shows the distribution of departments in each of the divisions of the traditional arts and sciences and Table 10 does the same for the professional fields. Using the traditional categoriesl4 14Bernard Berelson, Graduate Education in the United States. New York: McGraw—Hill Book Company, Inc., 1960. 28 of classification, the divisions of the traditional arts and sciences are Humanities, Social Science, Biological Science and Physical Science. The divisions of the professional fields are Agriculture, Education and "other professional". The division of "other professional" includes the reSpondents of Business Administration, Home Economics, Veterinary Medicine and Engineering. As these latter four areas are all profes- sional and since the combined N is relatively small, we have grouped them together under the division heading of "other professional". Since the number of cases in each department is quite small, the data are reported in raw numbers, rather than percentages. 29 TABLE 9 DESCRIPTION OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY DEPARTMENT AND DIVISION FOR THE TRADITIONAL ARTS AND SCIENCES Department Sample Response and Division (N) (N) Humanities: English 5 5 General Communication Arts 2 2 History 2 2 Music 3 3 Philosophy 3 3 Speech 10 10 Totals 25 25 Social Science: Economics 6 3 Political Science 7 6 Psychology 21 15 Social Science Divisional Major 2 l Sociology and AnthrOpology l3 9 Totals 49 34 Biological Sciences: Anatomy 2 1 Botany and Plant Pathology 11 8 Entomology 5 3 Microbiology and Public Health l4 l4 Physiology and Pharmacology 3 3 Zoology 9 7 Totals 44 36 Physical Sciences: Chemistry 34 27 Geography 4 3 Geology 2 2 Mathematics 5 4 Physics and Astronomy 9 5 Statistics 3 1 Totals 57 42 30 TABLE 10 DESCRIPTION OF THE SAMPLE AND RESPONSE BY DEPARTMENT AND DIVISION FOR THE PROFESSIONAL FIELDS Department Universe Sample and Division (N) (N) Agriculture: Agricultural Chemistry 6 5 Agricultural Economics l7 16 Agricultural Engineering 7 5 Animal Husbandry 15 12 Dairy 5 4 Farm CrOps 5 3 Fisheries and Wildlife ll 8 Food Technology 10 10 Forest Products 2 1 Forestry ll 9 Horticulture 12 8 Poultry Science 5 5 Soil Science 15 8 Totals 121 94 Education: Adult and Continuing Education 27 24 Agricultural Education 1 1 Business and Distributive Education 1 1 General School Administration 54 45 Guidance and Counseling 15 15 Industrial Education 1 1 Physical Education 4 4 Social-PhiloSOphic Foundations of Education 4 4 Totals 107 95 "Other Professional:” Business-Administration 15 13 Engineering: Applied Mechanics Chemical Engineering Civil and Sanitary Engineering Electrical Engineering Mechanical Engineering Home Economics Veterinary Medicine \I UINNCDi—‘Nm I U1D-‘IC\I Totals 43 32 31 The data—gathering instrument for this study was a 20-page questionnaire consisting of approximately 90 questions, many with sub- parts. Although most procedural statements on the use of the mailed questionnaire advise that " . . . it is usually unwise to expect returns from a questionnaire which requires much more than ten to 25 minutes to complete . . . ",15 Sletto's study has shown that with some groups the "shorter the questionnaire the higher the returns" axiom does not hold.16 Using three groups of university alumni of 100 persons each, he mailed questionnaires of ten pages, 25 pages and 35 pages in length and found no significant difference in returns from the three groups. This does not, however, necessarily mean that there is no threshold (high or low) for length. LThe questions were designed to elicit information on items including: A. The Independent Variable - 1. Faculty encouragement How much encouragement did you receive from faculty members of your department to attend professional meetings? How much encouragement did you receive from faculty members of your department to present papers at profes- sional meetings? How much encouragement did you receive from faculty members of your department to make professional contacts with important scholars or researchers of your field within the university, but outside-of your department? 15Goode and Hatt, 22, cit., p. 170. 16R. F. Sletto, "Pretesting a Questionnaire," American Sociological Review, 5 (1940), pp. 193-200. The 32 How much encouragement did you receive from faculty members of your department to make professional contacts with important scholars or researchers of your field, in other universities? How much encouragement did you receive from faculty members of your department to publish in professional journals or magazines? Contact and ”integration” with the faculty Do you feel you had sufficient opportunity to discuss career plans with members of the faculty? How many faculty members of your department did you know well enough during your doctoral training with whom you felt you could discuss personal problems? How many times did you meet with your full guidance committee during your graduate training? Did you discuss your thesis or program with members of your committee on an individual basis? How many times in a quarter did you meet with your major professor, on the average? Dependent Variable - Professional behaviors During the period of your doctoral training did you attend one or more professional meetings? During the period of your doctoral training did you deve10p professional contacts with important scholars or researchers of your field within the university, but outside of your department? During the period of your doctoral training did you present one or more papers at professional meetings? During the period of your doctoral training did you deve10p professional contacts with important scholars or researchers of your field, from other universities? During the period of your doctoral training did you have any papers, articles, etc., published in professional journals or magazines? 33 The analysis of the data was facilitated by the use of the Hollerith punch card system.17 This system was deve10ped by Dr. Herman Hollerith during the late 1880's and was first used on a large scale basis in the United States Census of 1890. Following the collection of data, it was then coded into numerical form suitable for the Hollerith punch card. In order to afford some degree of confidence in the accuracy of the coding, it was decided to completely re-code a random sample of ten percent of all questionnaires. Questionnaires to be re-coded were selected by the use of random numbers.18 .All numbers smaller than our parameter (001-358) were rejected as were all numbers larger and all ties. On a column basis, the re-coding showed a coding error of four-tenths percent. The data were punched into two decks of cards which were then verified for accuracy of original punching. After the card-punching and verification the cards were "deck cleaned". The Design gj_Analysis The study will utilize the method of analysis variously referred to as "cross-tabulation analysis",19 "multi-variate analysis",20 and "survey 17Goode and Hatt, 92. cit., pp. 317-20. 18Hubert M. Blalock, Social Statistics. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc., 1960, p. 438. ‘19Hans Zeisel, Say I£_With Figures. New York: Harper and Brothers, 1957, pp. l3l-34. 20Paul F. Lazarsfeld, "Introduction: Multivariate Analysis," in Paul F. Lazarsfeld and Morris Rosenberg (editors), The Language 2f Social Research: A Reader in the Methodology 2£_Socia1 Research. Glencoe: The Free Press, 1955, pp. 111-12. 34 design analysis".21 As defined by Lazarsfeld, multimvariate analysis refers to " . . . the study and interpretation of complex interrelations among a multiplicity of characteristics".22 This method of analysis is an approximation of the controlled experiment which is so characteristic of the natural sciences. In the controlled experiment, the experimental group and the control group are, by definition, exactly comparable in all but one-reSpect-nthe experimental variable. As a result, experimental findings will be limited only by two elements: the statistical sampling error and the Special conditions which accompanied the particular experiment, that is, its particular time, place, surroundings, etc.23 The first limitation can be omitted by repetition of the experiment. The second, a more serious limitation, can be reduced by repeating the experiment under different conditions. Survey analysis retains the limitations of the controlled experiment and adds a few of its own. In survey analysis the "control group" and the "experimental group” are, just as in the controlled experiment, different in terms of the experimental variable. However, unlike the controlled experiment, the groups are not exactly alike in all other 21Paul F. Lazarsfeld and Patricia L. Kendall, "Problems in Survey Analysis," in Robert King Merton and Paul F. Lazarsfeld (editors), Continuities in Social Research: Studies in the Sc0pe and Method of "The American Soldier". Glencoe: The Free Press, 1950, pp. 133-96. 22Paul F. Lazarsfeld, The Language Qf_Social Research, op. cit., p. 111. 23Zeisel, op. cit., p. 132. 35 respects. Consequently, it is necessary to determine if the difference presumed to be caused by the experimental variable is, in fact, the result of a spurious factor. Thus, through a series of cross-tabula- tions, the investigator must control or "hold constant" possible spurious factors in the groups and observe the original relationship when these factors are ”controlled." This points to another limitation of the design of survey analysis; that is, usually the investigator is able to ”control” only four or five variables at a time because of the number of cases in his sample. Still another limitation of the survey analysis design is that of prOperly establishing the time sequence of variables. If the researcher asserts that "a" causes ”b", then he must demonstrate that "a" precedes "b" in time. As many researchers gather their data from one point in time, such demonstration becomes, at best, arduous. To combat this difficulty, the "panel study" design24 is used. The procedure here is to obtain data from two different points in time, the "before-and-after" technique. The third and most serious limitation of survey analysis is that it is usually post factum (and sometimes ad_hoc) and as such can never, at least theoretically, equal the degree of proof that the controlled experiment can. The implications of EQSt factum research have been clearly described by Merton.25 24Ibid., pp. 215-54. 25Robert K. Merton, Social Theory and Social Structure. Glencoe: The Free Press, 1957. 36 Post factum explanations remain at the level of plausibility (low evidential value) rather than leading to "compelling evidence" (a high degree of confirmation). Plausibility, in distinction to compelling evidence, is found when an interpretation is consistent with one set of data (which typically has, indeed, given rise to the decision to utilize one, rather than another, interpreta- tion). It also implies that alternative interpretations equally consistent with these data have not been system- atically explored and that inferences drawn from the interpretations have not been tested by new observations. The logical fallacy underlying the post factum explanation rests in the fact that there is available a variety of crude hypotheses, each with some measure of confirmation but designed to account for quite con- tradictory sets of affairs. The method of post factum explanation does not lend itself to nullifiability, if only because it is so completely flexible.26 Our findings will be reported in percentage form and the traditional tests of significance will not be used. The judgment not to use sig- nificance tests rests mainly on three points. First, "traditional tests of significance have been developed to study the probable cor- H27 rectness or incorrectness of single, isolated statements, such as, a is more likely than ”b" to cause x . Since the hypotheses of this research are interrelated so that no hypothesis can be viewed inde- pendently of other hypotheses, the use of significance tests would not be appropriate. What should be tested is not a relationship observed in a particular table, but the relationships between a whole series of related tables. A second basis for not using significance tests in this research is that "these tests presuppose that the units being studied were sampled randomly 26Ibid., pp. 93-94. 27Merton, Reader and Kendall, QB. cit. p. 301. 3 37 .from the p0pulations to which they belong."28 We have data from only one university, and in no way can this be considered a random sample. Third, the X2 test is designed to show whether a relationship between two measurements could have been due to chance. Since our indicators are simply crude measures of the variables to be tested, it would seem irrelevant to test for independence. For example, to show that ideologically sensitive men are more active politically, we use as an indicator the amount of talk about politics reported by the men. This is only a crude indicator of political activity and might easily fail to show the relationship if one actually exists. Thus, because the variables being related hypothetically are seldom the-same as the measures being related empirically, a test of inde- pendence appears irrelevant.29 Selvin has argued quite effectively that in nondescriptive and non- experimental research, tests of statistical significance are always inapplicable.3O The basic difficulty in design is that sociologists are unable to randomize their uncontrolled variables, so that the differences between ”experimental" and ”con- trol" groups are a mixture of the effects of the variable being studied and the uncontrolled variables or cor- related biases. Since there is no way of knowing, in general, the sizes of these correlated biases and their directions, there is no point in asking for the Probability that the observed differences could have been produced by random errors. The place for significance tests is after all relevant correlated biases have been controlled.31 281bid., p. 303. 29Seymour Martin Lipset, Martin Trow and James Coleman, Union Democracy. Garden City: Doubleday and Company, Inc., 1962, p. 484. 30Hanan C. Selvin, "A Critique of Tests of Significance in Survey Research," American Sociological Review, 22 (1957), pp. 519-27. 311bid., p. 227. 38 As we begin the analysis of the data, it should be kept in mind that several procedural limitations will restrict the impact of the findings. First, we do not have a panel study, the data has been collected from only one point in time. Secondly, we are asking individuals to recall their experiences and, as a result, the data will be somewhat distorted since peOple frequently tend to forget things over a period of time. Also, the reader should keep in mind that we have a very select sample--individuals who are recent re- cipients of the doctorate from one university. CHAPTER IV FACULTY INFLUENCE AND PROFESSIONAL PARTICIPATION We noted at the end of Chapter I that the purpose of this research is to ascertain the role of the faculty in accounting for professional participation among doctoral students. We hypothesized that a rela- tionship will be found to exist between faculty contact, faculty encouragement, and student participation in professional activities. To provide a test for our hypothesis, we asked each respondent to recall his graduate school experiences and to report whether he had participated in the various professional activities. Furthermore, we asked each to characterize his relationship with the faculty in six different situations and to report whether he had received Specific encouragement from the faculty to participate in the professional activities and, if so, how much. The data is presented in terms of the independent variables-- faculty contact and faculty encouragement. Tables are presented that compare the percent of participation for each of the five activities at the same time by each of the independent variables. By cross-tabulating on these dimensions, we will be able to see whether our hypothesis is borne out. Table 11 shows the percent of students who participated in each of the five activities. The activity in which the highest proportion (93 percent) of students report to have participated is that of attendance at meetings. That such a high prOportion attended these meetings is not 39 40 at all surprising, for many of the professional associations actively solicit student membership. Many have student honoraries and offer the student both membership and the official journal of the organization at a reduced price. The meetings provide the student with an opportunity to meet friends from other universities, to visit with former teachers, and to meet "important" personalities in his field. 0f no less importance is the fact that the meetings provide the student with information concerning his role as a member of a professional society. Forty percent reported that they presented papers at these meetings. During the initial stages of the student's graduate training, the meetings serve as a vehicle of socialization. The closer the student gets to obtaining the degree, the more the meetings serve him as a ,marketplace to seek and inquire about prospective employment 0p- portunities. Sixty-seven percent reported to have made contacts with important scholars and researchers of their field from Hypothetical U. (but not in the student's own department), 61 percent with professionals from other universities and 40 percent presented papers at professional meetings of their field. 41 TABLE 11 PARTICIPATION IN PROFESSIONAL ACTIVITIES DURING THE PERIOD OF DOCTORAL TRAINING Activity Participators (N) (percent) Attended professional meetings 93 (356) Developed contacts with important scholars and researchers of the field from Hypothetical U. 67 (357) DevelOped contacts with important scholars and researchers of the field from other universities 61 (357) Published in a scholarly or professional journal or magazine 48 (353) Presented one or more papers at professional meetings 40 (356) A little less than one-half, 48 percent, published during the period of their doctoral training. There are, to be sure, journals of varying degrees of quality, but our data does not distinguish the quality of the source of publication. In addition, we know only if the re- Spondent published one or more, and not how many. Consequently, we are not able to Speak of the quality or the quantity of publication for any ,of the respondents. When we cross-tabulate professional participation by the number of meetings the students have with their major professors, we find (Table 12), 42 contrary to our hypothesis, very little relationship. 0f the group that had the fewest number of meetings with their major professor and the group that had the highest number of meetings with their major professor, the same proportion attended professional meetings. Although participation in the other four activities is more charac- teristic of the group that had most meetings, the data is not con- sistent for any of them. For example, 48 percent of the students who met with their major professors five times or less, but only 44 percent of the students that met six to ten times published. 0f the students who had 11 to 20 meetings 55 percent published where— as only 52 percent of the students who had more than 20 meetings published. This same type of inconsistency appears with involvement in presenting papers and in meeting professionals of the field. It should be noted, however, that presenting papers at professional meetings appears to be related at the "extremes.” The group that had fewest meetings, had the smallest prOportion and the group that had most meetings had the largest proportion of students who presented papers. For the two middle groups, however, the trend doesn't hold. 43 Ammuamv mm an mm mm «m mmeflu om cmnu muo: Aom-asv mm 03 N3 om ca amass oN on as AmoHv as me mm o“ Ha mosau emu ou xam AmNH-m~HV as an as no so mama no amass p>am sz ponmwansm museum moguwmum>flca Hmzuo .D Hmoflumnuomhm mwawuooz mwcwuooz wo Hosanz noncommum muomucoo muomuaoo popcmuu< Hmcowmmomoum Hmzofimmomonm Acmumaaofiuumm umcu unmouomv EMMH mum mommmmomm MOH< Mm mmHHH>HHQ< A¢ZOHmmmhomm ZH ZOHHflm Aosv om me as me so upon AooH-amv on as me we ma apnea Aooa-mav as em as am as 03a Aee-mev as Na we Hm em use sz woSmensm mummmm mowuwmuo>wsb Honuo .D Hmofluonuomhm mwcfiuomz mwawuooz woucmmoum .muomunoo .muomuaoo omcnouu< mo HonEDZ Hmcofimmowonm Hmnowmmmmoum Awmummwowuumm umnu unmowmav MMHHHZZOU MUZHHu<.Afics Hoauo .D Hmowuwsuomhm mwnwummz nowmmsomfin wmucmmwum .muomucoo .muomuaoo popcmuu< Hmaowmmmmoum Honoflmmomoum AwoummHUfluHmm uMSu unmoummv mHmHQZH z< zo NMHHHZZOU mozHHu< A¢ZOHmmmmomm ZH ZOHHfis3 Hmnuo .D Hmoflumfiuomhm mwnwumoz xaamswfl>flvcH cmucmmmum .muomucoo .mquucoo cmvcmuu< Suez umz Amsowmmmmoum HmaowmmmHOHm mumnEmz mo amnesz Avmummwowuuma umau unmouomv MAAHQZH mHHz HMS mmmmzmz MMHHHZZOU ho mmmZDz Wm mMHHH>HHQ< A¢ZOHmmmmOMm zH ZOHfldeUHHmwcp Honuo .D HmowuwSuomzm mwcflumoz mamam Homumo vmuswmmnm .muomusou .muomucoo wovaouu<. mmsomfin 0H Hmconmmonm Hmaoammmwoum muwasuuoamo Acmumawofiuuma umnu unmonmmv NHADUHHQ< AwGD Hmsuo .D Hmowuonuommm mwawumoz nSOqM mumnaoz vmuammoum .muomuaou .muomuaoo wowaouu< muasomm Hmcoflmmmmowm ammowmmmmoum mo.umna=z Annamawowuumm umsu unmoummv mHmHHQ< A chconn< mom “cowumoav mafia Ho3m=m and can mucmpaonmou mnu mo unwouma oawz~ .> xwvsmmm< mom mmcowummsv manna Hm3mam uoa twp muamvsommmu.m£u mo unmouoe maunuuhuamsfia Aewv wa Awwv w Awav mm Awsv ww Amwv we paoz Aamv Hm Adqv w Aowv om Anmv on Asqv mm mfiom Aonv we Awwv ww Awaav we Awoav we Awwv ww “spasm oumuovoe < Aaaav «a Amwav aw Ashe ww Awwv ww Amwav ww amps uppuw a sz unmoumm AAZV unmouom sz unmoumm Nsz unmoumm Hsz udooumm vmzmwansm muoemm mowuwmum>flcn Hague .D HmoquSuommm mwcwummz unmsowmwsoocm vmuaommum .muomucou .muomuaoo pwvcmuu< huasumm Hmcowmmmmoum HmGOHmmmmon Awmumaaowuuwe umnu unmoummv HZMZMQHHu<.A¢ZOHmmmmomm 2H ZOHHfiab Hmnuo nmuons—coo Hmsowmmmmoum Aoemv mm me me nu «m .D Hmowuonuommm .muomucoo Hmaowmmmmoum Awwwv we we ew we -- wwcaupez wovsmuu< sz vmamwansm mumamm mmfiufimum>wcs Henuo .3 Hmowumnuomzm mwcwuomz vmuammmum .muUMuaou .muomusoo pepsmuu¢ amsowmmowoum Hmnowmmmmoum Awoumawowuuma umzu udmoummv mMHHH>HHu< A xwpsmam< mom “sowummsv «Han Museum uos was «unaccommmu mnu mo unmoume maflze .> xfivsmme< mam mmsowummsv «asap Hmsmsm so: was musmvsonmmu ecu mo unmonom centunhusmsam 1r -- Aawwv ow Aawwv mw Awawv ow Awwwv ww pmamaappm Aawwv aw -- Aawwv ww Aoawv ww Awwav OOH masses vmucmmoum Amomv Na Ammmv um I: Amemv em AOHNV mm mowuwmum>wsp Hmsuo .muomusoo Hmsowmmmmoum Awewv ow Awwwv we Awewv ww -- Aewwv ww .: Hmuwuwauoawm .muomunoo Hmsoammomoum Aomwv ww Aewwv He Aeewv ww Awewv ow -- wwaauomz vowsouu< sz advance msz usouuoa sz unmoume «sz unmouoa msz usouumm voSmHHnsm muommm mofiuwmuo>wsb Hosuo .D Hmowumauommm mwswummz vmuammosm .muomusoo .muomusoo pmvswuu< Hmsowmmmmoum Hm:0Hmmumonm Avmwmusoosm unmoumnv mMHHH>HHu< A¢ZOHmmmhomm ZH ZOHHin accounting for the professional participation of doctoral students. we hypothesized that participation in professional activities would be related to the amount of encouragement the student received from the faculty and the amount of contact he had with the faculty. As an indication of the student's professional participation, each student was asked whether he attended professional meetings of his field, presented papers at these meetings, made contacts with important scholars and researchers of his field (both from the university where he did his graduate training and from other universities), and if he published one or more papers in professional journals of his field. As an indication of the students' contact with the faculty, each student was asked to report the number of meetings he had with his major professor, the number of meetings he had with his full guidance committee, the number of guidance committee members he met with on an individual basis, if he felt he had sufficient Opportunity to discuss career plans with the faculty and the prOportion of faculty members in his department that he knew well enough with whom he could discuss personal problems. In Chapter IV we compared participation in professional activities by each of the six different measures of student-faculty interaction. 63 64 We have seen that no relationship exists between the number of meetings a. student has with his major professor and his participation in pro- fessional activities. Also, there is no relationship between his par- ticipation and the number of meetings he has with his full guidance -committee. We have pointed out that student-faculty interaction in these situations is required for both the-student and the faculty. The remaining five measures of student-faculty interaction were related to professional participation. None of these are required. The difference in participation by whether the student met with members of his guidance committee on an individual basis is quite -small--ranging from two percent for the activity of presenting papers to eight percent for establishing contacts with important professionals of "other" universities. When we compared participation by the number of committee.members the student met with individually, we found a greater difference. For attendance at meetings, the dif— ference was 11 percent. The difference between these groups for publication was 16 percent and for presenting papers it was 20 percent. The number of committee members a student meets with on an individual basis distinguished participators from non-participators best for the activity of meeting professionals of the field. Whereas 56 percent of those who met with one committee member made contacts with professionals from Hypothetical U., 88 percent did of those that met with five or more committee members. These percentages were 48 and 100 for those who made contacts with professionals from other universities. This would seem to suggest that making contacts with important scholars and researchers of one's field is perhaps more related to the number of faculty members that a student has contact with, than how well the 65 student knows the faculty member. When we cross-tabulated participation by_whether the student reported to have had sufficient Opportunity to discuss career plans with members of the faculty, we found that participation was more characteris- tic-of the students that reported that they had. The difference in participation varied, however, from three percent for attendance-at professional meetings to 25 percent for publication. Of the six measures of student-faculty contact, the one which best predicts participation in professional activities is the one which reports the proportion of departmental faculty that the-student knows on a personal basis. The difference between the group that knew all of the faculty members and the group that knew no faculty members of their department on a personal basis was ten percent for attendance, 40 percent for making contacts with professionals from Hypothetical U., 33 percent for making contacts with professionals from other universi- ties, 37 percent for presenting papers, and 36 percent for publication. Of all the variables that we have dealt with, the one which best predicts student participation in professional activities is that of encouragement from the faculty. In every case, we have seen that par- ticipation was more characteristic among those students who were encouraged to participate by the faculty than those who were not encouraged. Moreover, reSpondents who reported that they were encouraged were sub-grouped in terms of the amount of encouragement and here again we have seen that the group with the highest percentage Of participators was that which reported the most faculty encouragement. The group with the second highest percentage of participators was the 66 group characterized as receiving a "moderate amount" of encouragement and the group with the fewest participators was the group that re- ceived the least amount of encouragement. We then presented cross-tabulations which showed the interrelation- ships for participation in each of the activities by participation in each of the other activities. Similarly, we presented cross-tabulations which showed the interrelationships of faculty encouragement for each Of the activities. We pointed out that if a reSpondent received encouragement for one activity, the chances were that he received encouragement for each of the other activities. Additionally, we found that encouragement to attend meetings was received by the highest prOportion of students and encouragement to present papers was received by the lowest proportion. In summary, it appears that the faculty do influence the participa- tion of doctoral students in their respective fields. Not only do the faculty influence the amount of participation, but also the kind of participation. This influence is discerned in both the amount of contact the student has with the faculty and the encouragement he received from the faculty. Moreover, it appears that the processes of socialization in the graduate school are informal. The student's contact with the faculty in the formal and structured situation is not related to his professional particpation. However, participation is related to student-faculty contact where the interaction is informal and face-to- face. The strongest relationship observed between student participation and faculty influence occurs between participation in a particular activity and faculty encouragement to participate in that activity. 67 Here, we Observed that the more the encouragement, the more the parti- cipation for each of the five different indicators of professional participation. There are, to be sure, many limitations of this research. Without a doubt, the most serious of these limitations is that it attempts to speak of changes in behavior with data that is from just one point in time. Ideally, Of course, we should have had a panel study. Through- out this study we have assumed that faculty encouragement and faculty interaction determined the extent to which a doctoral student would participate in the professional affairs of his field. 'Although we have shown a relationship between encouragement and participation, we have not demonstrated that encouragement occurred before participation. However, we would argue, with Merton,1 that the consistency of the data not only makes such a relationship plausible, but, also reasonable- Another limitation of this study is that the data is not sufficient in scape. We do not, for example, have data on peer group influences. More important, we do not have sufficient data about the faculty. It is almost certain that an encouraging cue from some faculty members has a different impact on the students than the same cue from other faculty members. Although we were able to obtain data on the number of student- faculty relationships and, in some cases, the circumstances under which the relationship took place, we did not have data concerning the content of the interaction. Although we know that a particular student had an opportunity to discuss his career plans with a member of the faculty, 1Merton, Reader and Kendall, 22. cit., pp. 303—05. 68 we were not able to know the dialogue of the discussion. However, we found that the more contact the student had with the faculty the more he participated,and, it would seem that if a student had an unpleasant or discouraging experience with a member of the faculty he would tend to have few contacts with that faculty member. Moreover, we found that encouragement from the faculty was the variable most associated with participation; since we also found that contact was positively associated with participation, it seems reasonable that the contacts the student had with the.faculty were positive and encouraging for him, especially in the high contact group. Also, we were not able to know whether the student or the faculty member initiated the interaction. It should also be pointed out that no deviant case analysis has been done. We have, as purposed, described student participation in professional activities by their relationships with the faculty. Yet, although we have seen that the hypothesized relationship between parti- cipation and faculty encouragement and contact does exist, we also found that some students of high encouragement did not participate and some students of low encouragement did participate. Also, as we have noted, our data comes only from those doctoral students who earned the degree. We do not have data from persons who attended at the doctoral level but did not earn the degree. Furthermore, we only have data from one university. It may be that student—faculty relationships vary by the size of the school, control of the school, school policy, etc. Another limitation of this study is that no multi-variate analysis has been completed. The reason for this is that the working N for this 69 study was only 358 and many times we reported on groups that had an N of less than 25. Were we to introduce test variables, the size of the cells would be too small to be reliable. However, we do not mean to suggest by this that other variables would not affect the data. Finally, the reader should again be reminded that the student's conformity to the cues of the faculty does not necessarily mean that the student has internalized the values which have prescribed the behavior. BIBLIOGRAPHY Howard 3. Becker and James W. Carper, "The DevelOpment of Identification with an Occupation,” The American Journal 2f Sociology, LXI (1956), 289-98. Howard S. Becker and Anselm L. Strauss, "Careers, Personality, and Adult Socialization," The American Journal of Sociology, LXII (1956), 253-63. Bernard Berelson, Graduate Education in the United States. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc., 1960. Bernard Berelson, "Graduate Education Today," The Doctorate in Education, 3 (1961), 113~133. Hubert M. Blalock, Social Statistics. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc., 1960. W. B. Brookover, "Sociology of Education: A Definition,” American Sociological Review, 14 (1949), 407-15. Irvin L. Child, ”Socialization," in Gardner Lindzey (editor), Handbook 9f_Social Psychology. Cambridge: Addision~Wesley Publishing Company, Inc., 1954, 655~92. James A. Davis with David Gottlieb, Jan Hajda, Carolyn Huson and Joe L. Spaeth, Stipends and Spouses: The Finances of American Arts and Science Graduate Students. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1962. H. A. Edgerton, S. H. Britt and R. D. Norman, "Objective Differences Among Various Types of Respondents to a Mailed Questionnaire," American Sociological Review, 12 (1947), 435-44. Frederick Elkin, The Child and Society: The Process of Socialization. New York: Random House, 1960. William Erbe, ”Gregariousness, Group Membership, and the Flow of Information,” The American Journal of Sociology, LXVII (1962), 502-16. Abbott L. Ferriss, "A Note on Stimulating Responses to Questionnaires," American Sociological Review, 16 (1951), 247-49. William J. Goode and Paul K. Hatt, Methods in Social Research. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc., 1952. 70 71 David Gottlieb, ”American Graduate Students: Some Characteristics of Aspiring Teachers and Researchers,” Journal of Educational Psyw chology, 53 (1961), 236w40. David Gottlieb, "Processes of Socialization in American Graduate Schools,” Social Forces, 40 (1961), 124~3l. David Gottlieb, Processes gj_Socialization in American Graduate Schools. An unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, 1960. Neal Gross, "Sociology of Education,” in Robert K. Merton, Leonard Broom and Leonard S. Cottrell, Jr., (editors), Sociology Today: Problems and Prospects. New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1959. Mary Jean Huntington, ”The DevelOpment of a Professional Selfwlmage," in Robert Merton, George Reader and Patricia Kendall (editors), Th3 Student Physician: Introductory Studies_ig the Sociology 9f Medical Educgtion, Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1957, 179-88. Herbert H. Hyman, Survey Design and Analysis. Glencoe: The Free Press, 1955. Daniel Katz and Hadley Cantril, "Public Opinion Polls,” Sociometry, l (1937), 155~79. Patricia Kendall and Hanan C. Selvin, ”Tendencies Toward Specialization in Medical Training,” in Robert Merton, George Reader and Patricia Kendall (editors), The Student Physician: Introductory Studies in the Sociology of Medical Education. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1957, pp. 153w87. Richard F. Larson and William R. Catton, Jr., "Can the Mail—back Bias Contribute to a Study‘s Validity?,” American Sociological Review, 24 (1959), 243~45. Paul F. Lazarsfeld, ”Introduction: Multivariate Analysis,” in Paul F. Lazarsfeld and Morris Rosenberg (editors), The Language gf Social Research: A Reader in the Methodology of Social Research. Glencoe: The Free Press, 1955, lllu12. Paul F. Lazarsfeld and Patricia Kendall, "Problems of Survey Analysis,” in Robert K. Merton and Paul F. Lazarsfeld (editors), Continuities ifl Social Research: Studies ig the Scepe and Method of ”The American Soldier." Glencoe: The Free Press, 1950, pp. 133~96. Seymour Martin Lipset, Martin Trow and James Coleman, Union Democracy. Garden City: Doubleday and Company, Inc., 1962. D. S. Longworth, ”Use of a Mail Questionnaire,” American Sociological Review, 18 (1953), 310~l3. Robert K. Merton, Social Theory and Social Structure. Glencoe: The Free Press, 1957. 72 Robert K. Merton, "Socialization: A Terminological Note," in Robert K. Merton, George Reader and Patricia L. Kendall (editors), Thg Student Physician: Introductory Studies la the Sociology_2£ Medical Education. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1957, 287-93. Robert K. Merton, ”Some Preliminaries to a Sociology Of Medical Educa» tion,” in Robert Merton, George Reader and Patricia Kendall (editors), The Student Physician: Introductory Studies 33 the Sociology 9f Medical Education. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1957, 3-80. Robert K. Merton, Samuel Bloom and Natalie Rogoff, "Studies in the Sociology of Medical Education,” Journal g£_Medica1 Education, 31 (1956), 552-65. Theodore M. Newcomb, ”Attitude DevelOpment as a Function of Reference Groups: The Bennington Study,” in Eleanor E. Maccoby, Theodore M° Newcomb and Eugene L. Hartley (editors), Readings in Social Psyu chology. New York: Henry Holt and Company, 1958, 265~75. Talcott Parsons, The Social System. Glencoe: The Free Press, 1951. Mildred Parten, Surveys, Polls and_§3mple§: Practical Procedures. New York: Harper and Brothers, 1950, 383w402. Carl F. Reuss, ”Differences between Persons ReSponding and Not ReSpond- ing to a Mailed Questionnaire," American Sociological Review, 8 (1943), 433-38. Natalie Rogoff, ”The Decision to Study Medicine," in Robert Merton, George Reader and Patricia Kendall (editors), The Student Physician: Introductogy Studies ifl_the Sociology 2: Medical Education. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1957, pp. 109~29. Hanan C. Selvin, "A Critique of Tests of Significance in Survey Re- search," American Sociolggical Review, 22 (1957), 519-27, R. F. Sletto, ”Pretesting a Questionnaire," American Sociological Review, 5 (1940), 193~200.. Wagner Thielens, Jr., "Some Comparisons of Entrants to Medical and Law School,” in Robert Merton, George Reader and Patricia Kendall (editors), The Student Physician: Introductory Studies in Ehg Sociology gf_Medical Education. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1957, pp. 131-52. Hans Zeisel, Say I; With Figures. New York: Harper and Brothers, 1957. APPENDICES To provide that the individuals and the university which actually participated in this study remain anonymous, we have not included actual COpies of the instruments which were used. We have, however, included an approximation of the instruments. The questionnaire, presented as Appendix I, differs from the one actually used in only two major ways: First, the university name has been changed and second, the original questionnaire was mimeographed. The cover letter used in the mailing, Appendix II, was individually typed on letterhead paper. The cover letter used for the 1962 graduat- ing group, Appendix III, appeared as a single page letter and was mimeographed. The cover letter used for the follOWJUP, Appendix IV, was also mimeographed. All cover letters used in the study carried the name of the university which actually participated. 73 74 APPENDIX I OFFICE OF THE GRADUATE SCHOOL HYPOTHETICAL UNIVERSITY Lazarsville, U.S.A. For office use only Male Sex: Name Female Department 1. Please list below the colleges and universities you have attended, beginning with your first undergraduate school and including your present school. Dates of Major field Degree College or University Attendance of Study Received 2. If you have interrupted your studies (except for summer periods) since you first began as a freshman in college, please indicate what you were doing "in between" (e.g., military service, physical illness, full-time employment). If you were working full time, indicate what sort of job you held. Please indicate by an X in the right hand column if at that time you would have preferred to stay in school. Reasons for Interruption of Study Would Have Dates Job (SPECIFY) Other (SPECIFY) Preferred School 75 When did you first seriously consider going into your current field of study? (Circle one number). Before entering high school. . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 During high school . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 During the first two years of college. . . . . . . . 3 During the junior year of college. . . . . . . . . . 4 During the senior year of college. . . . . . . . . . 5 After being out of college . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 Have you seriously considered any other field or career since you entered graduate school? Yes . . . . . . . . . 1 NO. . . . . . . . . . 2 If "yes” what field or fields? Please think back to the time when you first definitely de- cided to go to graduate school. Circle the number of the statement which comes closest to describing your career plans at that time. Definitely committed to the field and a preference for a Specific type of job in that field . . . . . . l Definitely committed to the field, but no preference for a Specific type of job in that field . . . . . . 2 Trying out the field of see if it might lead to a desirable career . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 Other (SPECIFY). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 Please write the number of the statement in Question 5 which comes closest to describing your situation right now. 76 7. Listed below are some of the things you might have considered when you weighed the advantages and disadvantages of different graduate schools. Please circle the appropriate number in each row in terms of the importance of each factor to you at the time you decided to go to Hypothetical University. One of the Most Impor- Quite Fairly Not Reason tant Reasons Important Important Important Reputation of institution . . . . 1 2 3 4 Particular man I wanted to study with. . . . . . . . 6 7 8 9 Reputation of Department. . . . . 1 2 3 4 Ease and Speed in getting degree. . . 6 7 8 9 Opportunities for teaching experience 1 2 3 4 Opportunities for research experience 6 7 8 9 Chance of getting a better job in the long run. . . . . . l 2 3 4 Housing . . . . . . 6 7 8 9 Scholarship or Assistantship . . . l 2 3 4 Not wanting to cut home ties . . . . . 6 7 8 9 Other important reasons (SPECIFY) 8. Which single factor in question 7 do you consider most important to your choice of Hypothetical University? 9. 10. 11. 77 Looking back, do you think you made the best decision by choosing Hypothetical University for your graduate training? I definitely made the best decision by coming here . l I am pretty sure I made the best decision in coming here. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 I am pretty sure I should have gone elsewhere. . . . 3 I definitely made a poor decision in coming here . . 4 Given your current knowledge about Hypothetical University and your department would you still select this school for your doctoral training if you had to make the choice once again? (Circle one number.) Yes. . . . . . . . . . . . 1 No . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 If ”no" to Question 10, what graduate school would you attend? Why would you choose this school? If a close relative or friend was interested in entering your field and wanted to attend Hypothetical University, what advice would you give him? 78 12. In terms of general reputation among experts in the field, how would you rate your department? Among the five best in the country . . . . . . . . . . 1 Among the top 20 departments, but not among the 5 best . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Not among the top 20 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 I have no idea at all . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 13. How would you rate the training opportunities in your depart- ment for a student who is interested in. (Circle one number in each row.) TRAINING OPPORTUNITIES Excellent gppg j§i£_ .2923 Teaching only . . . . 1 2 3 4 Teaching and research 6 7 8 9 Research. . . . l 2 3 4 Applied areas of the field . . ... . . . . 6 7 8 9 14. DO you feel you have had sufficient Opportunity to discuss your career plans with members of the faculty? (Circle one.) Yes. . . . . . . . . . . . 1 No . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 15. How many faculty members of your department did you know well enough, during your doctoral training, with whom you felt you could discuss personal problems? (Circle one.) All of them. . . . . . . . 1 Many of them . . . . . . . 2 About half of them . . . . 3 Very few of them . . . . . 4 None of them . . . . . . . 5 79 16. As far as graduate training, all in all how would you rate your department's faculty in respect to the following . (Circle one in each row.) Excellent ,Eplp 222p Sensitivity to student needs. . . 1 2 3 Knowledge of their field. . . . . 5 6 7 Teaching ability. . . . . . . . . 1 2 3 Awareness of current trends in the field. . . . . . . . . . . 5 6 7 Publishing productivity . . . . . l 2 3 Research skills . . . . . . . . . 5 6 7 Helpfulness in obtaining jobs for the doctorate recipients. . . . . l 2 3 17. From your own experiences and from what you have heard, how would you say Hypothetical University compares to other universities in respect to the following . Excellent Good Fair Poor Housing for graduate students . . 1 2 3 4 Concerts, foreign films, art fairs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 7 8 9 Stipends for graduate assistantships. . . . . . . . . . 1 2 3 4 Study facilities. . . . . . . . . 6 7 8 9 Research facilities . . . . . . . l 2 3 4 Library (Journals, references, etc.) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 7 8 9 General academic climate. . . . . l 2 3 4 18. As you think back, what kind of course do you believe was most valuable? Lecture. Seminars . . . . Individual reading . Can't decide . wav—I 80 19. From what you know, how important are the following criteria as the basis for assistantships, fellowships or scholarships awarded to graduate students in your department. order of importance 1, 2, 3.) Grades. . . . . . . Faculty personal impressions Financial need. . . . (Rank in 20. American graduate schools have been criticized and defended on a number of accounts. Listed below are some of the criticisms which have been made. For each, indicate whether you consider it valid or not for the department in which you completed your degree work. (Circle the apprOpriate number in each row.) It encourages over-specialization. It stifles student creativity. Training not really related to jobs students will get. . . . . . . . . Too many formal hurdles and iniw tiation rites which are not genuine training . . . . . . . Does not help students get desirable jobs . . . . . . . It accepts more students than it should . Admission standards are too low. It exploits its students by using them for cheap labor . . . . It rewards conformity, punishes individuality. Faculty members are more interested in research than they are in students. . Other (SPECIFY). . . . . . . . . Valid 1 6 Somewhat Valid 2 7 Not Valid 3 8 Dead Wrong 4 9 81 21. To what extent did any of the following factors affect the length of time it took you to get a doctor's degree? Length- Actually ened Time Length- Did Not Short- Not Consider- ened Time Lengthen ened Appli- ably A Bit Time Time cable Inadequate preparation before coming to Hypo- thetical University. . . . 1 2 3 4 5 Repeating work here you had already done . . . l 2 3 4 5 Passing foreign language requirements . . . . . . . 1 2 3 4 5 Being teaching assistant . l 2 3 4 5 Being research assistant . l 2 3 4 5 Having to work Off— campus while studying on campus. . . . . . . . . 1 2 3 4 5 Lack of financial pressure to get doctorate speedily, owing to continued GI benefits . . . . . . . . . 1 2 3 4 5 Being financially obliged to leave here in mid-course and work to earn money . . 1 2 3 4 5 Family obligations . . . . l 2 3 4 5 Preparation Of preliminary (or general) examinations. 1 2 3 4 5 Research for and writing of thesis. . . . . . . . . 1 2 3 4 5 22. Of all the above factors, encircle the one which probably was most important in lengthening the time it took you to get a doctorate. 23. If some other factor was more important than any Of the above, please note it here. 24. 25. 26. 27. 82 Which of the following comes closest to describing your career plans now that your studies are completed? (Circle one.) Position with academic institution . . . . . . . . . 1 Position with industry . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Position with federal or state government. . . . . . 3 Private practice . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 Other (SPECIFY) 5 Which of the following comes closest to describing your present job Situation? Definitely have a job. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . l Negotiating, looks good. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Negotiating, looks doubtful. . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 Have not really Started serious job hunting. . . . . 4 If you answered "1" (have job), how satisfied are you with the position? Very satisfied . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Satisfied. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Not satisfied. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 Very dissatisfied. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 There are many factors which might lead a graduate student to consider or actually drOp out of school. For example, some students have mentioned finances, poor grades, failure of graduate school to measure up to expectations, illness, faculty, marriage, military service, children and so forth. In as much detail as necessary, discuss the occasions when you seriously considered drOpping out or actually did drOp out of graduate school. What were the reasons, how did they come about, what did you do, with whom did you Speak? 83 28. If you actually did drOp out of school once you began your post-master’s work, discuss the factors which led to your returning for the completion of your doctorate. BACKGROUND INFORMATION 29. What is your age? 30. What is your marital status? Single, never married. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Married, no previous marriage. . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Married, a previous marriage . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 Separated or divorced. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 31. Number of children IF MARRIED 32. During your doctoral work, did your Spouse: Work full time. . . . . . . Work part time . Study for Bacheloris Degree Master's . . Doctor's. . . . . Non degree Housewife only. . . . . . Other (SPECIFY) 33. 34. 84 Which of the following would you say comes closest to describ- ing your Spouseas attitude during the time you worked on your doctorate? (Circle one.) She thought I Spent: Much too much time on my studies . . . . . . . . 1 Somewhat too much time on my studies . . . . . . 2 About the right amount of time on my studies . . 3 Somewhat too little time on my studies . . . . . 4 Much too little time on my studies . . . . . . . 5 Please discuss in as much detail as necessary the procedures by which you were assigned a major professor and a guidance committee. Discuss whether the choice was yours, the extent to which you were involved in the selection, and your general evaluation of the procedures which were followed. 35. 36. 37. 38. 85 From the date of your admission as a doctoral student, how long a period was it before you had a major professor? (Circle one.) During my first quarter. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 During my second quarter . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 During my third quarter. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 -After my first year but before my second . . . . . . 4 After the start of my second year. . . . . . . . . . 5 How long a period was it before you had a guidance committee? (Circle one.) During my first quarter. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 During my second quarter . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 During my third quarter. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 After my first year but before my second . . . . .4. 4 After the start of my second year. . . . . . . . . . 5 How soon after the selection of your major professor and your guidance committee did your committee hold its first meeting with you? (Circle the answer which comes closest.) About one month later. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . l About two months later . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 'About three months later . . . . . . . . . . . .‘. . 3 About four months later. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 About five months later. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 About Six months later . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 Between six months and a year later. . . . . . . . . 7 After a year . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 Please discuss your feeling about how your guidance committee [operated. Were you satisfied with their attendance and co- Operation? Was it difficult to get them together? Did they meet a sufficient number of times? 86 39. Please discuss your thesis tOpic and how it was selected. Was it something you were interested in and wanted to do or was it a ”practical choice”, i.e., something that came from a faculty member's research project? To what extent was your major professor involved in this decision? Were other faculty members involved? 40. How many times did you meet with your full guidance committee during your graduate training? (Circle one.) Once . . . . . . . . . . . l Twice. . . . . . . . . . . 2 Three times. . . . . . . . 3 Four times . . . . . . . . 4 Five times . . . . . . . . 5 Six times. . . . . . . . . 6 Seven times. . . . . . . . 7 Eight times. . . . . . . . 8 Nine or more . . . . . . . 9 41. How many members were there in your guidance committee? 87 42. Did you discuss your thesis or program with members of your committee on an individual basis? (Circle one.) Yes. . . . . . . . . . . . 1 IF YES 43. How many members of your guidance committee (excluding your major professor) did you meet with on an individual basis? (enter number here) IF YES 44. Of those you did meet with on an individual basis, how frequent were these meetings? (Circle one in each row for each guidance committee member included in question 43 above.) NUMBER 9].: TIMES First member . . . . . l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 or more Second member. . . . . l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 or more Third member . . . . . l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 or more Fourth member. . . . . l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 or more 45. How many times in a quarter did you meet with your major professor on the average? (Circle one.) Less than five times per quarter . . . . . . . . . . 1 Between six and ten times per quarter. . . . . . . . 2 Between 11 and 20 times per quarter. . . . . . . . . 3 More than 20 times per quarter . . . . . . . . . . . 4 88 46. To what extent did your department engage in any of the following activities? (Circle one number in each row.) Very Very Active Active Inactive Inactive Organized orientation programs for new graduate students. . . l 2 3 4 Organized informal activities for graduate students . . . . . . . l 2 3 4 Organized informal act- ivities for faculty- student get-togethers . l 2 3 4 Organized programs or seminars for graduate teaching assistants on teaching methods . . . l 2 3 4 47. Which of the above activities do you feel the department should organize? (Circle as many as apply.) Number 1 . . . . . . . . . 1 Number 2 . . . . . . . . . 2 Number 3 . . . . . . . . . 3 Number 4 . . . . . . . . . 4 All of the above . . . . . 5 None of the above- . . . . 6 48. In looking back on your eXperiences at Hypothetical University, what would you say the administration could have done to make your experience more meaningful in terms of social activites, graduate student organizations and married student activities? 89 49. In conducting research (laboratory, library, field, etc.) for your doctoral thesis, how much supervision were you given by your major professor (or thesis advisor if not the same person), and how much supervision would you have preferred? Supervision Given Supervision Preferred (Circle one.) (Circle one.) 1 Very close and con- 1 tinuous supervision 2 Close supervision but 2 not on a continuous basis 3 Continuous supervision, 3 but not very close 4 A moderate degree of 4 supervision 5 Very little supervision 5 50. If you were a major professor (or thesis advisor) and were directing the research of doctoral students in your department, how much supervision (both in frequency and attentiveness) would you give your students? (Circle one.) More than I received from my major professor . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Less than I received from my major professor . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 About the same as I received from my major professor. . . . . . . . . 3 51. How much Opportunity did you receive from your thesis advisor to incorporate your own ideas into the research design for your doctoral thesis? (Circle one.) Unlimited Opportunity . . . . . . . 1 Limited Opportunity . . . . . . . . 2 No Opportunity. . . . . . . . . . . 3 52. 53. 54. 55. 90 If you were a major professor (or thesis advisor) in your de- partment, how much opportunity would you give your doctoral students to incorporate their own ideas into the research de- sign for their doctoral thesis? (Circle one.) More than I received from my major professor . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Less than I received from my major professor . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 About the same as I received from my major professor. . . . . . . . . 3 During the period of your doctoral training, did you partici- pate in professional meetings? (Circle one.) I attended one or more professional meetings but did not present any papers at these meetings . . . . . . . . 1 I attended one or more professional meetings and presented one or more papers at these meetings . . . 2 I did not attend any professional meetings . . . . . 3 How much encouragement did you receive from faculty members of your department to attend professional meetings and to present papers? To Attend To Present Professional Meetings ngers (Circle one.) (Circle one.) 1 A great deal of encour- agement l A moderate amount of 2 encouragement 2 A small amount of en- 3 couragement 3 4 NO encouragement 4 During the period of your doctoral training, did you develop professional contacts with important scholars or researchers outside of your own department either in your own field or in related fields? Yes . . . . . . . . . 1 NO. . . . . . . . . . 2 91 56. If yes, were these professional contacts made with individuals? (Circle one.) In other departments of Hypothetical U.. . . . . . . 1 In other universities. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Both 0 0 O C O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O 0 O 0 O O O O 3 57. How much encouragement did you receive from faculty members of your department to make professional contacts with important scholars or researchers outside of your department? With Pe0ple in Other Departments of With People in Hypothetical University Other Universities (Circle one.) (Circle one.) 1 A great deal of encouragement 1 2 A moderate amount of encourage- 2 ment 3 A small amount of encouragement 3 4 No encouragement , 4 58. During the period of your doctoral training, did you have any papers, articles, etc., published in professional journals or magazines? (Circle one.) Yes 0 O O O O O O O O O O 1 59. How much encouragement did you receive from faculty members of your department to publish in professional journals or magazines? (Circle one.) A great deal of encouragement . l A moderate amount of encourage- ment 0 C O O O O O O I O O O C O 2 A small amount of encouragement 3 No encouragement. . . . . . . . 4 WE SINCERELY APPRECIATE YOUR HELP. PLEASE RETURN THE QUESTIONNAIRE TO OUR OFFICE AS SOON AS POSSIBLE. 92 APPENDIX II HYPOTHETICAL UNIVERSITY Lazarsville Office of Vice President for Research DevelOpment School for Advanced Graduate Studies - Office of the Dean January 25, 1962 Dr. John Smith World City Anywhere Dear Dr. Smith: Having recently completed requirements for the doctoral degree at Hypothetical University, you are in a most favorable position to assist us in gaining insights and understanding of the graduate program at this institution. We are assisted through views and recommendations of faculty and academic administrators on a continuing basis as provided in established administrative arrangements. Their reactions are invaluable. However, the views and evaluations of those who have recently completed their graduate studies appear to us as constituting a further important and helpful source of information. Our purpose is not to seek fault or praise but to obtain candid evaluations based on personal experience and observations. We want an indication how students appraise their graduate education experiences, how they evaluate the graduate program at Hypothetical University and how they regard the effectiveness of the manner in which it was carried out. A questionnaire has been prepared by our office. We realize that you are busy and that asking you to complete a questionnaire may be an imposition. Your cooperation, however, is most important to our study and we would appreciate sincerely your taking time to respond to the ritems involved. Kindly rest assured that what you say will be treated in absolute confidence. Answers will never be read by anyone in your department or college. Any final report resulting from this survey will not identify individuals. May we thank you in advance for your consideration and assistance and ask that you return the completed questionnaire to our office as soon as possible in the enclosed stamped self-addressed envelOpe. If there are any comments you would like to make about things not covered in the questionnaire, please feel free to do so. Sincerely, Vice President and Dean 93 APPENDIX III HYPOTHETICAL UNIVERSITY Lazarsville Office of Vice President for Research DevelOpment School for Advanced Graduate Studies - Office of the Dean January 26, 1962 Dear Doctor: Having recently completed requirements for the doctoral degree at Hypothetical University, you are in a most favorable position to assist us in gaining insights and understanding of the graduate program at this institution. We are assisted through views and recommendations of faculty and academic administrators on a continuing basis as pro- vided in established administrative arrangements. Their reactions are invaluable. However, the views and evaluations of those who have recently completed their graduate studies appear to us as constituting a further important and helpful source of information. Our purpose is not to seek fault or praise but to obtain candid evaluations based on personal experience and observations. We want an indication of how students appraise their graduate educational experience, how they evaluate the graduate program at Hypothetical University and how they regard the effectiveness of the manner in which it was carried out. A questionnaire has been prepared by our office. We realize that you are busy and that asking you to complete a questionnaire may be an imposition. Your cooperation, however, is most important to our study and we would appreciate sincerely your taking time to reSpond to the items involved. Kindly rest assured that what you say will be treated in absolute confidence. Answers will never be read by anyone in your department or college. Any final report resulting from this survey will not identify individuals. As you probably know, the Graduate School at Hypothetical University is develOping more rapidly than almost any other segment of the University. Its increase in size constitutes an important response to the national need for more people with advanced graduate training of quality. We wish, therefore, to obtain as complete information as possible as we proceed in our develOpment of the graduate program. 94 May we thank you in advance for your consideration and assistance and ask that you return the completedquestionnaire.to our office as soon as possible. If there are any comments you would like to make about things not covered in the questionnaire, please feel free to do so. Sincerely, Vice President and Dean 95 APPENDIX IV HYPOTHETICAL UNIVERSITY Lazarsville Office of Vice President for Research DevelOpment School for Advanced Graduate Studies - Office of the Dean February 26, 1962 Dear Doctor: As you know, our office is conducting a study to evaluate the Graduate Program here at Hypothetical University. During the last week of January we sent a questionnaire asking for evaluations based on your own personal experience and observations. We realize that you are busy and that to complete a questionnaire is an imposition. Your appraisal, however, is most important to our study and we would appreciate sincerely your taking time to respond to the items involved. We are enclosing another c0py of the questionnaire along with a stamped selfmaddressed envelOpe and ask that you return the completed questionnaire to our office as soon as possible. Sincerely, Vice President and Dean 96 APPENDIX V A Research Note on the No Answer Because two of the questions dealing with encouragement had a high percent of "no answer" and since they are treated as independent variables, they are crucial to the analysis. We have written this note to argue the prOpriety for including them in this study. We maintain that the high rate of no answer to these questions does not bias the data. These no answer”re3ponses" are randomly distributed within the sample and are due to faulty question construction. Excluding the front sheet data, Openwended questions and questions that were applicable only in certain situations, there were a total of 93 questions in the questionnaire. In concerning ourselves with how to handle the "no answer"* we decided to follow the procedure outlined by Zeisel** for those questions where the-"no answer" reSponse was five percent or less. Of the 93 questions, 88 had a "no answer" response of five percent or less. These questions were cross~tabulated with several variables including age, degree obtained, sex, division of study, hiatus, etc., to determine if the "no answer" reSpondents were of a particular group. This was done for each of the 88 questions, and we found no homogeneity in the "no answer" reSponse. Thus, having some confidence that the "no answer" respondents for these questions were randomly *By ”no answer" we mean to include only times when the reSpondent did not answer a particular question to which he was asked to reSpond. **Hans Zeisel, Say I£_With Figures. New York: Harper and Brothers,1957. 97 distributed, we included the questions in our analysis. The ”no answers” were excluded from the tables and subtracted from the working N for that table. Had we included them in the base figure of the table, we would have superficially increased the reliability of that question. However, by completely omitting_the "no answer” category from the table assumes that they would distribute in exactly the same way as the group for ”no whom a substantive reSponse was known. The "save" here is that the answer” constitutes such a small percentage of the population that they wouldn't make any difference. Five questions had a "no answer" response higher than the arbitrary five percent level. The exact percent of ”no answers” for these questions was 8, 9, ll, 20 and 23 percent. In searching for some explanation of the high ”no answer" reSponse for these questions, we -observed that three of the five questions were set in a somewhat unusual format. The remaining two questions were also set in a format which is somewhat different from that which is standard for survey questionnaires. Hence, we began to suspect the "question format" as the cause of the high ”no answer" response for the five questions. The 93 questions of the questionnaire represent four different styles of question format. We shall identify each of them and refer to them as styles A, B, C, and D. FIGURE I Question Style "AV "How many members were there in your guidance committee? 98 The.first question style is the "fillwin” type which is quite standard in surveys. There were six questions of this type in the -questionnaire and the mean number of "no answer" response for this 'type-of question style was 4.1 which equals 1.1 percent of the-sample \(the-range was 10 which is 2.8 percent of the sample). FIGURE 2 Qpestion Style 9p" What is your marital status? Single, never married. . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Married, no previous marriage. . . . . . . . . 2 Married, a previous marriage . . . . . . . . . 3 ~Separated or divorced. . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 _Question type "B" is probably the most common style of question format that is used in questionnaires. A question,.followed by a series of responses and the reSpondent is merely to check or circle one or more codes correSponding to the responses under the question. There were 28 questions of this type in the-questionnaire. The mean number of persons failing to answer this type of question was 3.1 which equals .8 percent of the sample (the range was 19 which is 5.3 percent of the sample). 99 FIGURE 3 Question Style "9P How would you rate the training Opportunities in your department for a student who is interested in . . . (Circle one number in each row). TRAINING OPPORTUNITIES Excellent Good Fair Poor Teaching only . . . . . . . . . l 2 3 4 Teaching and research . . . . . l 2 3 4 Research only . . . . . . . . . 1 2 3 4 Applied areas of the field. . . l 2 3 4 In the third format style, a lead statement is made which is ap— plicable for each of the immediately following circumstances. The reSponses are also uniform in that the set of fixed alternatives for each question is exactly the same. The reSpondent then reads each individual "sub-question” and circles the apprOpriate reSponse that appears to the right of the question. There were 53 questions of this type, counting each item (”Teaching only", ”Teaching and Research", etc.) as individual questions. The mean number of persons failing to answer this type of question was 8.5 which is 2.3 percent of the sample (the range is 39 which is 10.8 percent of the sample). 100 FIGURE 4 Question Style “Q” "How much encouragement did you receive from the faculty members of your department to attend professional meetings and to present papers?” To Attend Professional To Present Meetings Papers (circle one) (circle one) 1 A great deal of encouragement l A moderate amount of encourage- 2 ment 2 3 A small amount of encouragement 3 4 No encouragement 4 The fourth question format style is quite similar to the previous one ("C”) in that a lead or introductory statement is made which is apprOpriate to more than one specific question and the categories of response are exactly the same for each of the questions. It differs from the third in that the categories of response are placed on the horizontal axis and the questions on the vertical axis--one on each side of the categories of answer. There were six questions with this particular question format style and the mean number of persons failing to reSpond to this type of question was 32.0 which is 8.9 percent of the sample (the range was 72 which is 20 percent of the sample). lOl TABLE 1 Mean Percent of Number of the sample with Question Number of "No answers" One or more Style Questions Per Question No response "A" 6 4.1 1.]. "B” 28 3.1 .8 "C" 53 8.5 2.3 "D” 6 32.0 8.9 From Table l we can easily see that there is a sizeable difference in the number of "no answer" responses which various question format styles yield. For each question of style "D" an average 32 persons failed to answer; 32 represents nearly nine percent of the total number of persons surveyed. Of the other three question styles, "C" had the highest percent of persons failing to respond, an average 2.3 percent of the total group questioned. The explanation for so many reSpondents failing to answer questions put in the format of "D" probably lies in their failure to see them. In the questionnaire, the place where the reSpondent was to indicate his ”choice of answer” to a question was to the right of the category of response, (the right side of the sheet) except where the questions were put in the format of "D”. Here, half of the time the respondent indicated his answer to the right of the reSponse category and half of the time to the left. When the respondent had to indicate his reSponse to the lgft of the category of reSponse the percent of "no answer” was nearly 11 percent higher than when it was on the right. 102 TABLE 2 Mean Number Percent of Place Number of "No answers” the sample of of Per with one or more Answer Questions Question no reSponse Right 3 12.6 3.5 Left 3 51.3 14.3 Each of the five questions which yielded more than five percent ”no answer" response were crOSSMtabulated with several control variables in order to determine if some particular group or category of persons were failing to respond. Control variables were department, division of study, type of degree obtained, sex, age, Specific time of graduaw tion and number of years taken to obtain the degree. In no case was the H 1 percent of no answer' response in any one category significantly different than any other. Also, we analyzed these data to determine if the same reSpondents were failing to answer the questions. We found that only 3.5 percent failed to answer three of the five questions and 4.9 percent failed to answer two of them. In conclusion, it appears that the reason for a high percent of ”no answer" to questions included in the Survey reported here was due to faulty question construction. When the place for the reSpondent to indicate his choice of answer was on the ”wrong” side of the page-- the left sidem-and when there is also, as usual, a reSponse requested to the right of the question, the percent of persons failing to answer the question on the left was 14.3. When the place is to the right side, the percent of persons failing to answer varied from .8 to 3.5 percent. HICHIGQN STRTE UNIV. LIBRARIES 31293103208603