.INTERPERSONAL ORIENTATTONS AND THE SELECTION OF SENSITIVE PEOPLE TM: for the Degree of M. L. I. R. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY JACK L. SHOOK 1971 VIII?- I l' .. me & sans I“ II anon smumv me. If ‘ LIBRARY BINDERS +_ ‘Lgsmnyumusg E. Q “V ‘5' "R BINDING BY ABSTRACT INTERPERSONAL ORIENTATIONS AND THE SELECTION OF SENSITIVE PEOPLE By Jack L. Shook Sensitivity to people was defined as the ability to predict another person or group's feelings, thoughts, and behavior. This ability was conceptualized as being composed of four independent com- ponents, Observational Sensitivity, Nomothetic Sensitivity, Idiographic Sensitivity, and Theoretical Sensitivity. Instruments were selected to measure the first three components. Correlates of sensitivity were identified and the following hypotheses advanced: l. The higher the belief in consideration, the more sensitive. 2. The higher the belief in initiation of structure, the more sensitive. 3. The more other-oriented, the more sensitive. 4. The more objective the orientation, the more sensitive. 5. The more intelligent, the more sensitive. 6. Traits of personality are related to sensitivity. All but hypothesis four were partially confirmed. The problem of the selection of sensitive people was discussed and some examples were cited. Areas for future research were indicated. INTERPERSONAL ORIENTATIONS AND THE SELECTION OF SENSITIVE PEOPLE By IJ Jack L? Shook A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF LABOR AND INDUSTRIAL RELATIONS School of Labor and Industrial Relations 1971 To Peggy and Douglas ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS My deepest appreciation is expressed to Dr. Henry Clay Smith, committee chairman, for his guidance, support, and an occasional kick. My thanks also to Drs. Thomas Patten and Michael Moore for their help- ful criticisms and suggestions. A special thanks is extended to my wife and son for just being there. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . HISTORY. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Components of Sensitivity . . . . . . . . . . . Observational Sensitivity. . . . . . . . . Nomothetic Sensitivity . . . . . . . . . . Idiographic Sensitivity. . . . . . . . . . . Theoretical Sensitivity. . . . . . . . . . . Correlates of Sensitivity . . . . . . . . . Intelligence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Leadership Attitude Scales . . . . . . . Subjectivity versus Objectivity. . . . . . . Self versus Other. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Personality Traits . . . . . . . . . . . . . PROBLEM C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O Hypotheses Tested . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . mmOD C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Subjects. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Measurements of Sensitivity . . . . . . . . . . . The Case of the Instructor (SMITH) . . . Knowledge of Men and Women . . . . . . . . . The Case of Zolton . . . . . . . . . . . . . Intercorrelations of the Measures of Sensitivity. Reliabilities of the Measures of Sensitivity. . . Measurements of Interpersonal Orientation to People Leadership Attitudes . . . . . . . . . . . . Self versus Other Orientation. . . . . . . . Subjectivity versus Objectivity. . . . . . . Intercorrelations of the Measures of Interpersonal Orientations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Reliabilities of the Orientation Scales . . . . . iv Page iii vi vii 13 l4 l4 14 15 15 l6 16 18 18 18 19 19 20 20 Measures of Intellectual Achievement. Measurements of Personality . RESULTS. . . DISCUSSION . The Problem of Selection. Future Research SUMMARY. . . BIBLIOGRAPHY . APPENDICES 20 22 26 3O 3O 32 34 35 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Intercorrelations among the measures of senSitiVity. O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O 0 0 l7 2. Reliabilities of the measures of senSItiVity. O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 l7 3. Intercorrelations among the orientation to people scales 0 O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 21 4. Reliabilities of the orientation scales . . . . . . . . . . 21 5. Intercorrelations among the measures of intellectual achievement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23 6. Reliabilities of the intellectual achievement measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23 7. Protebob personality inventory. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 8. Reliabilities of the Protebob person— ality inventory. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 9. Intercorrelations among the personality scales I I O O O O I C O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O 25 IO. Correlations among the measures of sensi— tivity and other available measures. . . . . . . . . . . 27 ll. Correlations among the orientation to people scales and the measures of intellectual achievement and person— ality scales . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 vi Appendix LIST OF APPENDICES Case of the instructor Knowledge of men and women Case of Zolton Orientations to people The Protebob personality inventory vii INTRODUCTION The concern with understanding people has increased considerably in the past two decades. This can be evidenced in the rise of sensi- tivity training groups such as T-groups and in the popularity of the social sciences among college students. The ability to understand people has been named empathy, social perception, ability to judge people, etc. In this study, it is referred to as sensitivity to people. The purpose of this study was two-fold. The first was to identify and measure several components of sensitivity and their correlates. The second was to present a method of selecting a sensitive person. As used in this study, sensitivity refers to the ability to predict an- other person or group's feelings, thoughts, and behavior (Smith, 1966). HISTORY A sensitive person and his ability to understand people has been given several names. Dymond (1950) used the term "empathy" to describe this ability. For Dymond, empathy was the "imaginative transposing of oneself into the thinking, feeling, and acting of another." The accu- racy score for empathic ability was the sum of all errors made in pre— dicting the behavior of several persons. Dymond saw empathy or sensi- tivity to people as a unidimensional ability. Later conceptualizations of sensitivity mathematically dissected the score one received into error components. (Cronbach, 1955; Bronfenbrenner, ggngl, 1958; Cline, 1960; Smith, 1966) Cronbach felt that the total score may be misinterpreted due to error tendencies within the judge. He statistically isolated five main components which may affect one's score. Elevation (E) was conceived as the way the rater used the response scale, i.e. whether he tended to rate others high or low. Differential Elevation (DE) was the tendency to spread the ratings among those rated. Stereotype Accuracy (SA) was the ability to predict the norms of a group of persons. Differential Accuracy (DA) was the ability to predict differences between the persons rated. Assumed Similarity (AS) was considered the rater's tendency to project his feelings, thoughts, or behavior into the person being rated. Although all of these were components of the sensitivity score, later studies recognized the components of Stereotype Accuracy and Differential Accuracy as being "types" of sensitivity. The other 2 dimensions were to be reckoned with by techniques of test construction. (Cline and Richards, 1960; Grossman, 1963; Johnson, 1963; Shears, 1967). Bronfenbrenner,_g£ a; (1958) referred to his two types of "social perception ability" as sensitivity to the generalized other and interpersonal sensitivity. In addition, he conceptualized four types of prediction: first person--the judge (A) predicts what the person being rated (B) thinks of him (A); second person--(A) predicts what (B) thinks of himself; third person-- (A) predicts how (B) feels towards another person (C); and non-personal--(A) predicts (B's) feel- ings about other objects and concepts which do not refer to particular individuals or groups. Finally Bronfenbrenner added a time dimension to sensitivity. He believed that a sensitive person not only is aware of differences among people or groups but also of variation within a person or group through time. This addition of the time axis has the effect of putting all of the indices of sensitivity into three dimen- sional space. Smith (1966) divided the concepts of Cronbach and Bronfenbrenner into the perceiver and the perceived. Level and spread were viewed as habits of the perceived. Stereotype Accuracy and Differential Accuracy were knowledge of the person judged. An interaction variable was added to the schema. It consisted of empathy, previously called projection or assumed similarity and observation. An even finer breakdown of sensitivity was presented by Warr and Knapper (1968). They chose to view "person perception" in terms of cognitive elements and processes. Although they attempt to theorize the whole perceptual process, many of their conceptualizations are not currently measurable and will not be considered here. 4 It is evident that when we speak of a sensitive person we are not referring to a unidimensional process. A sensitive person can be accu- rate in his stereotypes, his observations, his "individual" perceptions, etc. It has been generally recognized that these components are inde- pendent. (Cline and Richards, 1960) Therefore, he can be accurate in one component but not in another. As mentioned earlier, some of the components are considered errors in sensitivity rather than components differentiating types of sensitive people. Included among the errors in sensitivity are level, spread, and empathy. What remains is the components of sensitivity recognized in this study as important to identifying and selecting sensitive peo- ple, i.e. stereotype accuracy and individual accuracy. Both are modi- fied by the time axis of Bronfenbrenner presenting a third component. Sensitivity based on these components is guided mainly by the implicit personality theories which the person uses to predict behavior. (Smith, 1966) To improve sensitivity, Smith (1968) suggested that training pro- grams stress the use of explicit theories which could be tested and improved through direct feedback. The use of explicit personality theories in correctly predicting behavior is the last component of sen- sitivity. We thus have four components which are postulated to be relatively independent measures of sensitivity to people. In a forth— coming book, Smith refers to them as Observational, Nomothetic, Idio- graphic, and Theoretical sensitivity. Each of these components will be considered separately below. Components of Sensitivity Observational Sensitivitye—"the ability to look at and listen to another person and remember what he looked like and said," has had few studies concerning sensitivity directed toward it, although it is an important concept. Gestalt psychologists have shown that what is obser- vable in objects is not always perceived. That is, we tend to see the unified whole rather than the specifics in a situation. Smith (1968) referred to this unified whole as the expressive quality in a person. That is, when we observe people, we have a tendency to perceive their goodness or badness and then evaluate them based on our perceptions. Body and facial cues have been related to judgements of people (e.g. Thornton, 1944; Secord, 1958; Argyle and Dean, 1965). Ekman (1957, 1964, 1965a, 1965b) has studied judgemental accuracy based on observations of head and body cues. Harris (1962) and Bruni (1963) developed a test of observational accuracy. In their studies they used color and sound motion pictures developed by Cline (1960). The subjects were asked to view a film of several persons being interviewed. After viewing the film, they were presented with a questionnaire which tested their knowledge of the interviewee's appearance, actions, and content of conversation, and also how he believed the interviewees answered a series of questions (sensi- tivity). They found that sensitivity was related to accurate observa- tion. As a component of sensitivity, observation is especially important to situations in which the perceiver has low acquaintance with the individual perceived. That is, he has little knowledge of the person's background, group membership, etc. It is in this context that we use the term Observation Accuracy in this study—-the ability to correctly predict the behavior of others in low acquaintance situations. Nomothetic Sensitivity is "the ability to learn about the typical member of a group and to use this knowledge in making more accurate predictions about individuals in that group." For years stereotypes have been considered inaccurate and a hinderance to the problem of social perception (Harding,1968). However, several studies have shown stereotypes provide at times more accurate judgements of individuals when known than in their absence (Tagiuri, 1968). Some studies have even found that judges were more accurate when only the stereotype informa- tion was known rather than when additional information about the per- ceived individual was provided (Stelmachers and MCHugh, 1964). Stereotypes pervade all of our perceptions of people. Several studies have shown that by altering information such as race and ethnic group membership inferences made about people can be changed (Warr and Knapper, 1968). Sappenfield (1969) found that even personal acquaint- ances are perceived stereotypically. In recognition of the importance of stereotypes, Zavala (1960), Silkner (1962), and Johnson (1963) have developed tests to measure stereotype accuracy concerning groups. Their scales were derived from information about a particular group ascertained by the Strong Voca- tional Interest Blank. Their scales tested one's knowledge of several groups: men, women, young, old, executives, unskilled labor, etc. One <3f the important conclusions that arose out of their studies was that stereotype accuracy is not a generalizable ability as others (Traverse, 1941) had believed. That is, one may be very accurate in perceiving men in general but not psychologists. A later study by Spier (1969) has also confirmed this finding. In addition, Spier found that train- ing and improvement in one stereotype did not generalize to other stereotypes. Idiographic Sensitivity—-"The ability to use increasing exposure to and information about a person in making increasingly accurate pre- dictions about him." This component of sensitivity has been studied very little. One of the main reasons is that it is highly impractical for most researchers to have subjects interact for great lengths of time. Most of the past studies have had interactions of less than an hour. In the case of the Cline films, observations only last five min— utes. There have been a few studies, however, that have measured parts of this component. For instance, Dymond (1954) measured sensitivity in married couples. Gage (1958) correlated teacher effectiveness and "understanding" of pupils. The importance in the understanding of this component may be most realized in studies such as the present one, i.e. selection of sensitive people. As an example, clinical psychologists need to be proficient in idiographic sensitivity. Whether this ability can be taught is under conjecture. Several studies cited by Smith (1966) have shown that psychologists who are supposedly trained in this art do no better, and sometimes worse, than the untrained in accurately perceiving others. Therefore, selection of those with high idiographic sensitivity may be necessary before entering graduate training in clin- ical psychology. Since time and money would prevent a lengthy acquaint- Ennce period between the perceiver and the perceived, other selection scales which correlate with idiographic sensitivity and yet are easy to administer would be more practical. Theoretical Sensitivity is "the ability to select and use theories to make more accurate predictions about others." As mentioned above, we tend to use implicit personality theories when we judge others. The foremost problem with these theories is that we use them unconsciously, and therefore they are unverifiable. Smith (1966) suggested that sensi- tivity can be improved by selecting an explicit theory, thereby provid— ing feedback as to our theoretical accuracy. Francher (1966), in a study using Harvard undergraduates who had taken a course in personality, found that those who use an explicit personality theory (in this case a trait approach) were more accurate predictors. Grossman (1967) tried to improve the sensitivity of a group of undergraduates by instructing them on the use of a five trait theory of personality. In this case, the theory accounted for little improvement. However, this result may have been due to difficulties in getting people to accept and use a new theory as in previous studies, and general difficulty in improving sen- sitivity with any kind of training. This component, because of its stress on training, will not be con- sidered further in this present study. Correlates of Sensitivity In the foregoing sections, we were concerned with defining the term sensitivity as it was used in this study. In considering the further problem of the selection of sensitive people, we were concerned with two aspects of the problem. The first was to ascertain the qual- ities and interpersonal orientations that are recognizable or measurable in a sensitive person. The second was to develop instruments which are relatively easy to administer, making it practically feasible to select sensitive people from large samples at different times. In this section, the correlates of sensitivity are discussed. The methods section will deal with the selection instruments. Intelligence Smith (1966), in reviewing twenty studies occurring between 1915 and 1965, concluded that "intelligence is the most certain correlate of sensitivity," having a median r of .30. Although other measures of intelligence have been positively related to sensitivity (course grade, GPA, quantitative, etc.), verbal ability has received by far the most support. (Dymond, 1950; Johnson, 1963; Grossman, 1963, Bruni, 1963). _L§adership Attitude Scales Fleishman (1953), in constructing a test to measure differential leadership styles, isolated two major factors. "Consideration" was "the extent to which the leader was considerate of his workers feelings" éind reflected a human relations aspect of leadership. "Initiation of 10 Structure" was "the extent to which the leader defined or facilitated group interactions toward good attainment" (planning, scheduling, criticizing, etc.). In a review of past studies, Korman (1966) found no consistent trends in the interaction of the two dimensions, i.e. they are independent. He suggested that situational variables are act- ing as moderators. Greenwood (1969) comes to a similar conclusion but also adds that both are independent of intelligence. Dore (1960) constructed a leadership attitude scale based on four attitudes isloated by the Survey Research Center of the University of Michigan. The sub—scales were measurements of Employee—Orientation (E0), Delegation of Authority (DA), Differentiated Role (DR), and Creates Teamwork (CT). Based on the intercorrelations of the scales, he con- cluded that the scales measure two independent attitudes, one involving E0, DA, and CT, and the other, DR. It was concluded that these were similar to the Fleishman factors. He also found some evidence that the scales were related to the stereotype accuracy between men and women. Johnson (1963) found a significant relation with the same scales. Gross- man (1963) found a positive relationship between the leadership scales and the ability to judge people. However, in all of the above cases, erratic relationships occurred among leadership attitudes and the cri- terion scales. It is believed that this may be due to the unreliability of some of the measures. The correlation of the leadership scales with sensitivity makes some intuitive sense. For to believe in the delega- tion of authority and to be employee oriented indicates confidence in interpersonal abilities. In assuming a differentiated role, the leader 318 socially perceptive to his special role in the group which demands EUJ understanding of the group. It must be remembered that the scales ask “flhat one prefers to do, not what the situation commands. ll Subjectivity versus Objectivity Based on a previous study by Wolin (1954), Mullin (1962) conducted an investigation of what he termed Empathic Drive (subjectivity) and its relationship to sensitivity. Empathic Drive was the tendency to respond to a person's internal psychological states. The test that he developed to measure Empathic Drive required the subjects to watch a series of silent films depicting a person being interviewed (Cline, 1960). After the film stopped, the subjects were asked to fill out a questionnaire, picking a statement in each item that was most like their impression of the interviewee. Each item contained one statement re— flecting a psychological state, e.g. is satisfied with life and three others reflecting the physical, actional, or characterological aspect of the person. He found that people vary consistently in their saliency of Empathic Drive. He also found a low but negative correlation between stereotype accuracy (mendwomen) and interpersonal accuracy. In a later study, Meitus (1969) also found these low but again negative relation- ships, which indicates that objectivity or the tendency to notive veri- fiable facts may be related to sensitivity. Mullin found that Empathic Drive was not related to the personality variables measured or to intel- ligence. He also found a positive relation between Empathic Drive and Employee Orientation (consideration). Self versus Other Bronfenbrenner (1958) and Jones and Thibaut (1955) have defined the ways in which people orient themselves to others. Linden (1965) developed a test to measure these orientations and studied how they are related to sensitivity. The orientations thus defined were: first per- son, "What can he do for me?"; second person, "What does he think of 12 himself?"; third person, "What do his friends think of him?"; and non- personal, "How much does he weigh?" The test consisted of fifty-two multiple choice questions concerning the way the subject feels the per- son in a hypothetical situation responded. Each alternative represented one of the four orientations. Linden found that people were either first person oriented with a high reliability, or they were split among the other orientations caus— ing low reliabilities. No significant relation was found between the orientations and sensitivity. However, it was suggested that given im- proved reliabilities, the relationship of self (eog involvement) versus other orientation may be similar to that of subjectivity versus objec— tivity. Personality Traits Several studies have indicated a relationship between personality indices and sensitivity. All results are not comparable due to diff- erent measures of sensitivity and personality. Smith (1966) cited some studies that are comparable and came up with this profile: sensitive people are "more intelligent, more tolerant, and above all, more inde- pendent but responsible and considerate in their relations with others." Shears (1967) in attempting to verify the personality correlates using the Protebob Personality Inventory (Linden, 1965) found virtually no relationship. Continued research needs to be done in this area using similar criteria and measurement to get meaningful interpretations. PROBLEM This study had two objectives: one, to identify and measure the components of sensitivity and their correlates; and two, investigate the problem of selecting a sensitive person. Hypotheses Tested The hypotheses presented below are based on the correlates of sen- sitivity previously discussed. 1. 2. The higher the belief in consideration, the more sensitive. The higher the belief in initiation of structure,_the more sensitive. The more other-oriented, the more sensitive. The more objective the orientation, the more sensitive. The more intelligent, the more sensitive. Traits of personality are related to sensitivi_y. l3 METHOD This study proceeded in two phases. The first phase concentrated on the selection and improvement by item analysis of the various instru- ments to be used in the second phase. The revised tests were then ad— ministered to a group of college students to test the foregoing hypo- theses. Subjects The subjects for both phases of the study consisted of undergrad— uate students in Psychology of Personality classes at Michigan State University during the terms Fall, 1970 and Winter, 1971. The Fall, 1970 class consisted of 250 students, 100 males and 150 females. The Winter, 1971 class had an enrollment of 200. However, as an indication of moti- vation to do well, only those students who took all of the tests were included in this study. The number of subjects used in the second phase was thus reduced to 67, 26 males and 41 females. The Measurements of Sensitivity A component approach to sensitivity consisting of four relatively independent parts was postulated. However, this study only proposed to measure three of them, Idiographic, Nomothetic, and Observational. l4 15 The Case of the Instructor (SMITH) To measure idiographic sensitivity, it was necessary to select a person who was known to all of the subjects for a relatively long period of time. The one person who best fit the requirements was the instruc- tor. All of the subjects were exposed to the instructor approximately 25 hours before taking the test. It is conceded that this was not a long time for an intimate acquaintance and interaction. However, it is a considerably longer acquaintance than that afforded by most sensiti- vity studies. The test was developed by having the instructor answer a series of questions dealing with his likes or dislikes of various subjects, inter- ests, and occupations. In addition, the instructor took a personality test which will be described later. His scores (percentiles) on the various personality scales were also included. The subjects were to predict how the instructor answered the questions. This test and all of the others were scored by adding the correct responses. The test and the correct answers are provided in Appendix A. Knowledge of Men and Women To measure nomothetic sensitivity, it was decided to use a group that all of the subjects were equally familiar with, in this case men and women. In other situations, different tests may be selected. For instance, if we were selecting inner city police officers, a test of the typical juvenile delinquent would perhaps be more appropriate. (See Appendix B) The test used in this study was originally developed by Zavala (1960) and refined by Silkner (1962). It consists of items taken from the Strong Vocational Interest Blank. The subjects were asked to l6 respond to each item as they thought over 50% of men and women in gen- eral did. See Appendix B for the test. The Case of Zolton As mentioned earlier, observational sensitivity includes the ability to accurately predict another's feelings, thoughts, or actions after only being exposed to him for a short time. The difficulty in measuring this aspect of sensitivity is that of providing a constant amount of cues for all observers. Cline and Richards (1960) minimized this by developing a set of sound color films of interviews with various persons. The interviews were not rehearsed. However, the interviewer intentionally probed the following areas: personal values, personality strengths and weaknesses, reaction to the interview, hobbies and activities, self conception, and temper. Each interviewee was studied in depth after the interview, includ— ing several paper and pencil personality tests and an intensive inter- view on his past history and personal habits. Five judging instruments were then derived. This present study used items from several of these instruments, adjective checklist, sentence completion test, behavior post—diction, and opinion prediction. (See Appendix C) Intercorrelations of the Measures of Sensitivity If the measures of sensitivity are properly related to the compon- ent theory, they should be relatively independent. A brief look at Table 1 will confirm this. We therefore, have three independent measures of sensitivity which when combined will, in addition, indicate one's general sensitivity. 17 TABLE 1 INTERCORRELATIONS AMONG THE MEASURES OF SENSITIVITY SMITH ZOLTON STEREOTYPE SZS—TOTAL SMITH __- ZOLTON .11 __- STEREOTYPE —.16 .18 _—_ SZS—TOTALl .3o* .40** .38** —-- Correlation of part with remainder. * P<< .05 ** P< .01 TABLE 2 RELIABILITIES OF THE MEASURES OF SENSITIVITY Internal Internal Consistency Consistency Fall 1970 WINTER 1971 SMITH .56 .59 ZOLTON .54 .53 STEREOTYPE (MEN & WOMEN) .63 .71 18 Reliabilities of the Measures of Sensitivity Table 2 indicates the reliabilities of the instruments for both phases of this study. The reliabilities are internal consistency measures based on the Kuder-Richardson Formula #20. As mentioned earl— ier, the tests were originally presented in Fall, 1970 and then item analyzed. According to Nunnally (1967), item analysis requires a mini- mum of five subjects per item. Our study falls short of the minimum, which may explain the lack of improvement between the two phases. The Measurements of Interpersonal Orientation To People The interpersonal orientation scales consist of five separate tests that were combined into a battery to be given at one time. The battery includes measures of consideration, initiation of structure, self versus other orientation, subjective versus objective orientation, each of which will be discussed separately below. LeadershipgAttitudes The leadership attitude scales of consideration and initiating structure were developed by Dore (1960). They consist of pairs of statements expressing the leadership method to be measured and the other statement of the opposite view. The original test was given in the Fall of 1970, consisting of 64 items, 32 for each leadership atti- tude. After an item analysis, each test was shortened to 16 items. This was done so the battery could easily be administered at one sitting. (See Appendix D) 19 Self Versus Other Orientation This test was originally developed by Linden (1965) and consisted of 52 multiple choice items. The subjects are presented with various hypothetical cases and are asked to pick one of four alternatives which most nearly expressed how he thought the hypothetical person would be- have in the ambiguous situation. The four alternatives represented different ways of orienting oneself to a situation. Linden referred to these as self-orientation, i.e. "What does this person think of me?"; other-orientation, "Why does this person behave as he does?"; social orientation, "What do others think of him?"; and non-personal—orienta— tion, or factual orientation. Linden's study showed that the self-orientation is mutually exclu— sive of the other types. The present study, therefore, paired the self— orientation with one of the remaining three orientations which were re- named as the other orientation. The pairing was based on the most dis- criminating items, based on an item analysis. In addition, the test was shortened to 28 items. (See Appendix D) Subjectivity Versus Objectivity This test first appeared in Mullin (1962). It was developed using the Cline films. The subjects are asked to view two silent films. Each film showed a person being interviewed. After each film, the subjects are asked to pick a statement which is most like their impression of the person being interviewed. The original test had three choices, one representing internal psychological states and the other two represent- ing external appearance, actions, and impact upon others. The present study reduced the choices to two, internal and exter— nal. The test was scored in the direction of objectivity. The total 20 score thus indicates whether a person tends to form immediate impres- sions of people based on internal and non—verifiable constructs or ex- ternal and objective facts. Again this test was shortened from Phase 1 to Phase 2. (See Appendix D) Intercorrelations of the Measures of Interpersonal Orientations Although it was not postulated that the different orientations are independent, the intercorrelations among the orientation scales (Table 3) indicates that they, for the most part, are independent. The only significant relationship was between consideration and initiating structure. The independence of these measures is especially helpful when we combine them and obtain a total score and later when relating them to the measure of sensitivity. The Reliabilities of the Orientation Scales The reliabilities were again computed using the Kuder-Richardson Formula #20. It can be seen (Table 4) that although the tests were shortened from Phase 1 to Phase 2, the internal consistency was not ad— versely affected. Only the objectivity test showed a considerable loss. Measures of Intellectual Achievement Although the subjects were not given an intelligence test, various measures were available which do indicate intellectual achievement. The easiest to obtain in this study was the course grade. The other measures included were scores on tests that all incoming freshmen are required to take, which include verbal comprehension, arithmetic TABLE 3 21 INTERCORRELATIONS AMONG THE ORIENTATION TO PEOPLE SCALES CONSID INIT-STRUC OTHER OBJECT ORIENT-TOT CONSID ——— INIT STRUC -.30** --- OTHER .25 -.16 --- OBJECTIVE .12 -.05 .21 --- ORIENT-TOTALl .20 .11 .11 .36** --- Correlations of part with remainder. * P <5.05 ** P <1.01 TABLE 4 RELIABILITIES OF THE ORIENTATION SCALES Internal Internal Consistency Consistency Fall 1970 Winter 1971 CONSIDERATION .85 .74 INITIATION OF STRUCTURE .70 .75 OTHER ORIENTED .78 .79 OBJECTIVITY .86 .70 ORIENT-TOTAL -—— .73 22 proficiency, and math (algebra) proficiency. The intercorrelations for the measures are reported in Table 5. As expected, they were all highly intercorrelated (Pomexm - Hmm>omezH om. No.- AH. Rmm. HH. oo. oH. mH.- oH. HMHmU< A