EVALUATION OF A SENSITIVITY TRAINING PROGRAM WITH A COMPONENT CRITERION Thesis for the Degree of 'M. A. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY JOHN R. MIETUS 1969 3 129 Michigan State University III III" II II IIIIIIIIIIIQIIIIIIILHIIIILIIIIIIILIIII L LIBRARY , . - t \ "’(.Jw Jy.’ '— A‘L“ A) 6' ? f'J'fi" ..'.' 1' — r'? /Ja Q70 APR2 22005 - 0411 08 X' ABSTRACT EVALUATION OF A SENSITIVITY TRAINING PROGRAM WITH A COMPONENT CRITERION BY John R. Mietus The purposes of this study were threefold: (1) to construct a component criterion of sensitivity to a single person, (2) to evaluate the effectiveness of a sensitivity training program which gave diagnostic feedback of predic- tive errors, and (3) to investigate specific individual differences in the impact of training. Sensitivity was defined as the ability of an indi- vidual to predict what another person said, felt, and did about himself and others. The components of sensitivity were postulated to be Stereotype, Level and Empathic Ac— curacy. The criterion instrument was a paper and pencil di- agnostic test of accuracy in judging one individual. It consisted of a written description of the individual and seventy-two MMPI statements which the individual answered. The judge's task was to predict how the criterion individual answered these statements. John R. Mietus The training program utilized feedback, diagnosis of errors, and practice on specific principles. It con- sisted of training sessions, administered to a Fall 1968 sophomore level personality theory class, interpOlated be- tween a pre- and a post-test on the criterion. No control group was used. A correlational analysis and matched t- tests were used to study training effectiveness. The major results were as follows: Training involving diagnostic feedback in judging persons will increase the sensitivity of trainees to a person not used in the practice sessions. Trainees who were initially low in sensitivity made the greatest gains as a result of training. Observant individuals made great- er gains in stereotype accuracy. The trainee's course grades, scores on a sensitivity to persons measure, moti- vation to understand others, and sex were not related to training gains. Suggestions for the improvement of the criterion, as well as the training materials and procedures were made. It was further suggested that the long range effects of training and the use of smaller training groups be investi- gated. Approved* EVALUATION OF A SENSITIVITY TRAINING PROGRAM WITH A COMPONENT CRITERION BY John R: Mietus A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1969 4992/0 4-2 9—47 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I wish to express my sincere appreciation to Dr. Henry Clay Smith for his guidance and patience. I also wish to thank Drs. James S. Karslake and John H. Wakeley for their suggestions and criticisms. ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS . LIST OF TABLES . . LIST OF APPENDICES INTRODUCTION . . . HISTORY . . . . . Sensitivity and The Training Problem The Centrality of Feedback The Insufficiency of Feedback Individual Differences in the Impact of Training PROBLEM . . . . . METHOD . . . . . . TABLE OF CONTENTS Its Components The Development of the Criterion The EXperimental Design Measures of Individual Differences Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure Measure of of of of of of Scholastic Ability Sensitivity to People Self-centered Orientation Psychological—mindedness Level of Aspiration Observational Accuracy Training Methods and Materials RESULTS 0 O O O 0 Effectiveness of Training Individual Differences in the Impact of Training iii Page ii vii 10 12 24 Page Differences in Scholastic Ability Personality Differences Motivational Differences Differences in Observation Sex Differences DISCUSSION 0 O O O O O O O O O I O O I O O O O O O 35 Individual Differences Differences in Scholastic Ability Differences in Personality Sensitivity Motivational Differences Observational Differences Sex Differences Who Can Best be Trained? Comments About Principles of Training Training Materials The Criterion Instrument The Experimental Design SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 5 BIBLIOGRAPHY O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 47 APPENDICES O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O 5 1 iv Table 1. 10. LI ST OF TABLES KR-20 reliabilities of the criterion on the pretest (N=101) . . . . . . . . . . Intercorrelation of the scores on the criterion for the pretest (N=l45) . . . Intercorrelation of the scores on the criterion for the post-test (N=l45) . . Sample sizes and sex of trainees . . . . . Gains on five criteria of predictive ac- curacy (N=l45) o o o o o o o o o o o o 0 Analysis of gains (post-test minus pretest) by pretest scores . . . . . . T-tests of differences in training gains of a high scholastic ability group minus the gains of a low ability group when matched on pretest criterion ' scores (N=61 pairs) . . . . . . . . . . T-tests of differences in training gains of a high scoring group minus the gains of a low scoring group on the leader- ship and personality scale when matched on pretest criterion scores (N=61 pairs) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . T-tests of differences in training gains of a high self-centered orientation group minus the gains of a low self- centered orientation group when matched on pretest criterion scores (N=58 pairs) . . . . . . . . . . . . . Correlations of level of aspiration with pretest scores and gains in scores as a result of training (N=l45) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Page 15 16 16 18 25 26 28 29 31 32 Table 11. 12. 13. Page T-tests of differences in training gains of a high aspiration group minus the gains of a low aspiration group when matched on pretest cri- terion scores (N=56 pairs) . . . . . . . 32 T-tests of differences in training gains of a high observation accuracy group minus the gains of a low ob- servation accuracy group when matched on pretest criterion scores (N=44 pairs) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 Summary of evidence for hypotheses . . . . 36 vi Appendix A. B. C. LIST OF APPENDICES Page The Criterion Instrument . . . . . . . . . 51 Measures of Individual Differences . . . . 54 The STOP Scale The Human Relations Test The First Impressions Test Some Training Instruments . . . . . . . . 61 The Case of Naomi Warren The Ruth Case The Self Image of the College Man The Case of the Young Psychologist, Morgan Johnson vii INTRODUCTION In the last decade, many have tried to train per- sons to become more sensitive, i.e., to better "predict what an individual will feel, say and do about you, himself, and others" (Smith, 1966). Few of these efforts have been objectively evaluated; those that have been evaluated, have generally produced negative results. The purpose of this study was to develop a sensitivity training program that stressed immediate feedback and diagnosis and to develop an evaluation criterion that would permit a component analysis of the effects of training. It also has had the aim of ex- ploring variables related to individual differences in the impact of the training. HISTORY After defining sensitivity the following discussion considers (l) the results of previous evaluations, (2) the importance of feedback in training, the absence of feedback in most situations, the insufficiency of feedback and what further principles of training are needed, and (3) individ- ual differences influencing the impact of training. Sensitivity and Its Components Sensitivity, as defined above, has three components: Stereotype Accuracy, Level Accuracy, and Empathic Accuracy (Smith, 1968). Stereotype Accuracy refers to the predictive accu- racy of the responses of most members of the group in which the person judged is placed. There is some evidence that individuals can more accurately predict the responses of the person judged on the basis of the group to which he be- longs than on fuller information about the person judged himself (Stelmachers and McHugh, 1964). The second component is Level Accuracy, the specific tendency of an individual to rate the person judged as good or had, high, low, or medium. A third component is Empathic Accuracy, the tend- ency of the individual to correctly assume that the judged person's thoughts, actions, and feelings are similar to his own. These components interact: people evaluate more positively those to whom they assume more similarity (Byrne and Clore, 1967): stereotype groups may have high or low evaluations: and people pay more attention to those to whom they feel similar. The Training Problem The outcomes of general training programs have varied almost as much as the training programs themselves. In general, results have been inconclusive or negative (Crow, 1957, Taft, 1955). Literature reviews have shown that training, when equated with amount of course work in psychology or psychological experience, has not led to im- provement in predictive ability (Grossman, 1967; Smith, 1966). Specifically designed training programs have not fared much better. Methodological problems in evaluating training effects and constructing valid criteria are numer- ous (Anderson, 1963; Carron, 1964; Miles, 1960). Some training programs have resulted in decrements in predictive accuracy after training (Crow, 1957; Taft, 1955). The Centrality of Feedback Recently a number of studies have exhibited posi- tive training gains. A common factor in these programs is feedback to the trainees of their predictive accuracies. Murray and Deabler (1958) had fifteen psychologists match figure drawings with five diagnostic labels. After each judging, the judge was given information on the cor- rect diagnosis and allowed time to study the material. Re- sults indicated learning progress. Oskamp (1962) compared inexperienced undergraduates with experienced clinicians on a task of predicting, on the basis of an MMPI profile, whether a patient was hospitalized for psychiatric or non-psychiatric reasons. He was able to train the undergraduates, who were initially inferior to the experienced clinicians, to attain a level of accuracy equal to that of the clinicians. He felt that the use of objective predictive tasks accompanied by immediate feed- back and specific training was responsible for the gains. Newton (1965) used a different type of feedback to obtain increased accuracy in a multiple-cue inference task. The judges were told the cue validities and their own one utilization coefficients. Accuracy increased in spite of a three week interval between task and feedback. Kepes (1965) conducted eight one-hour training ses- sions designed to increase Individual Accuracy. The training stressed knowledge of results, practice, and par- ticipation. He found the individuals who score low on the pretest improved. It was suggested that these persons make larger errors in their implicit personality theories, in stereotyping, and in assuming similarity. Grossman (1967) conducted a somewhat similar train- ing program. He ran five one-hour training sessions inter- polated between a pre- and post-test on a training criteri- on. His program stressed feedback, practice, participation, systematizing the prediction process, and using an explicit empirical personality theory. When compared to a control group, the trainees demonstrated a significant improvement in Individual Accuracy, the ability to differentiate between individuals when group membership cues are reduced to a minimum. Sechrest, et. a1. (1967) gave undergraduates the task of interpreting short sentence completion protocols. Those students who received feedback performed better than those who did not. The authors questioned whether the dif- ference was not due to the feedback's increasing the stu- dent's motivation, rather than being due to the additional informational cues present. Watley (1968) studied the effect of providing imme- diate feedback training to educational counselors who were found in a previous study (Watley, 1966) to predict educa- tional criteria (freshman and overall college grades) at high, moderate, and low levels of accuracy. The counselors were divided into an immediate feedback group and a no- training group. Compared to the no-training group, the im- mediate feedback had no observable effect on the initially high and moderate forecasting accuracy groups. However, the low accuracy group increased its predictive accuracy to a level comparable to the counselors who initially predicted at the highest level of accuracy. Watley felt that the in- itially high judges may have been already predicting at the limits of currently possible accuracy. His 1966 study showed that the superior judges were better able to under- stand and deal with abstract concepts and were possibly more compulsive. Intelligence may also have accounted for the difference. Spier (1969) studied the effect of the initial ac- curacy of judges on stereotype accuracy training improve- ment. He found the high initial accuracy group decreased slightly on a post-test, the moderate pretest accuracy group increased slightly, and the low initial accuracy group in— creased substantially on the post-test. He hypothesized that ceiling effects or regression to the mean might be the most plausible explanation for the phenomenon. Feedback, then, is important for increasing sensi- tivity. However, as to why and when it is effective, and what kinds of feedback are most effective in certain situ- ations, there is some dispute (Ammons, 1956; Miles, 1960; Todd and Hammond, 1965). Feedback may decrease sensitivity in situations when things appear to be correlated when in fact they are not (Chapman and Chapman, 1967; Chapman, 1967). Feedback in addition to providing one information also may focus the trainee's attention on the person he is attempt- ing to understand. It also may increase the trainee's mo- tivation and acceptance of attitude change through the de- velopment of game-playing attitudes and self-competition (Sechrest, 1967: Smith, 1966). However, many situations in which it is important for one to understand others and make predictions do not allow the individual knowledge of his performance. The classroom teacher finds it difficult to profit from experi- ence. He almost never learns of his long range effects up- on his students, and he finds it difficult to trace his short term effects to the practices from which they presum- ably arose (Skinner, 1968). The clinician and the personnel interviewer find it difficult to improve their predictive accuracy. According to Goldberg (1968), "for learning to occur, some systematic feedback regarding the accuracy of the judgmental response must be linked to the particular cue configuration which led the clinician to make that judgment." But in clinical practice, feedback is infrequent, and if it occurs at all, it does so after a long time interval, thus making it hard to associate the initial one configuration with the feed- back. The Insufficiency of Feedback While the application of the feedback principle is necessary, it may be insufficient. The application of other principles also seems necessary for learning to take place. It is quite important that the trainee diagnose his errors on each of the components (Grossman, 1967; Kepes, 1965; Smith, 1966). The student should be given explicit guidelines for change (Kepes, 1965). His training should be structured so that resistance to change is decreased. Practice materials should move from the less stressful to the more stressful (Smith, 1966). The training should combine theoretical principles with practice in applying them (Grossman, 1967). And the training should attend to whole persons, not to components of a person. Individual Differences in the Impact of Training Persons differ in their ability to understand others and to profit from training in sensitivity. The more intelligent the person is, the better he understands others (Smith, 1966). Motivation to understand others is a factor in sen- sitivity. Orientation towards others can be divided into first-, second-, third-person, and non-personal types (Bronfenbrenner, 1958). The less oriented a person is to- wards others and away from himself, the less mature he is (Linden, 1965). He should not be as capable of making gains as a result of training as the more other-oriented person. The person who tends to perceive others in terms of their internal psychological state, that is, their thoughts, feelings, desires, etc., has been found in a past study to be no more sensitive in Stereotype Accuracy than persons who are oriented towards another's physical appearance, his social stimulus value, or his actions (Mullin, 1962). Bronfenbrenner (1958) suggested that the ability to accurately perceive the stimulus field in a social setting influences sensitivity. Significant positive correlations between sensitivity and observational accuracy have been found by Harris (1962) and Grossman (1963). Past studies suggest there is no sex difference in interpersonal sensitivity, or at best a slightly superior ability in women (Grossman, 1963, 1967; Taft, 1955). PROBLEM The study was designed to determine whether feed- back and diagnosis of errors in the trainee's predictions about individuals and groups would produce improvements that would generalize to other individuals and groups not included in the training. In addition to giving the trainees feedback and di- agnosis of errors, other principles of training were used. The trainees were taught to increase their motivation for a high level of accuracy in understanding others. Trait, dee velopmental, and interpersonal theories of personality were presented to the trainees, and they were told to judge in- dividuals with these in mind. They were taught to note em- pathic differences and to compensate for emphathic biases. They learned to discount in judging a person whether they like or dislike him. Finally they were taught that in judging an individual, they should apply not only the typi- cal man's theory of himself, but should also apply the judged individual's theory of himself. The following is hypothesized: l.- Training involving diagnostic feedback in judging 'persons will increase the sensitivity of trainees to.a_person not used in the practice sessions. 10 11 In addition to investigating the above hypothesis, the study also had the aim of exploring variables related to individual differences in the impact of training. Therefore, the following are hypothesized: 2. The lower the initial sensitivity of the trainee, the greater his gains. The higher the scholastic ability of the trainee, theggreater his gains. The more sensitive the personality of the trainee, the greater his gains. The next three hypotheses refer to the relationship between motivation to understand others and the effects of training. 5. The stronger the self-centered orientation of the trainee, the less his gains. The more psychologically-minded the trainee, the greater his gains. The higher the trainee's level of aspiration, the greater his gains. The more observant the trainee in a natural set- ting, the greater his gains. Females make greater gains than males. METHOD The research was divided into phases as follows: (1) development and testing of the training criterion; (2) selection and development of instruments for measuring in— dividual differences in scholastic ability, personality, motivation, and observation; (3) development of the sensi- tivity training sessions within the design. All subjects used in these phases were undergraduates in a personality course . The Development of the Criterion As previously indicated, the components of sensi- tivity are postulated to be stereotype, level, and empathic accuracy. Whereas instruments have been constructed to measure these components individually (Grossman, 1963, 1967: Johnson, 1963; Lynch, 1968; Price, 1969), none have attempted to measure total sensitivity to a single person using all these components. This was the goal for the con- struction of this criterion. Furthermore, as the training aims were to increase stereotype, level and empathic accu- racy to diverse individuals, the criterion provided a test of generalization of training. 12 13 The initial form of the instrument was constructed during the first months of 1968. It consisted of a written description of an actual person and three scales measuring stereotype, level, and empathic predictions respectively. The instrument had multiple choice responses and the scales were derived from items of the Minnesota Multiphasic Per- sonality Inventory. The-individual, hereafter referred to as the person judged, took the MMPI twice, and twenty-three items to which he answered in the same direction as at least two-thirds of a norm group of men (Rorer, 1963: Goldberg, 1963), as well as twenty-three items to which he answered in the opposite direction as the norm group, were selected to comprise the stereotype scale. The level scale consisted of fifty-six items each of which was made up of a pair of statements, one statement being more desirable than the other based on a study by Messick and Jackson (1962). The judge's task was to predict to which of the two statements the person judged answered "true." Finally, the empathy scale consisted of fifty-six neutral items to which the judge was to predict the person judged's response and also to indicate his own response. Total accuracy scores were obtained by summing the scale accuracy scores. The instrument was administered to 71 members of a sensitivity training class. A point-biserial item-total correlation, and item difficulty and discrimination analy- see were conducted. The stereotype and level scales were reduced to forty items each, and the empathy scale was 14 dropped from the test. Empathy is now measured by the stereotype and level scales, with the judge reSponding to each of the items as in the original instrument, but also indicating his own response to the statement. This form was administered to a group of fifty-eight persons. A fur- ther item analysis was conducted and the instrument revised and shortened to seventy-two items (see Appendix A).- The following scores were computed for the criterion: 1. Total Accuracy: the total number of correct respon- ses that a trainee makes. 2. Stereotype Accuracy: the number of correct re- sponses on the Stereotype subscale that a trainee makes. 3. Level Accuracy: the number of correct responses on the level subscale that a trainee makes.. I 4. Assumed Similarity: the number of times the trainee- makes the same response as he indicated the person judged made. 5. Actual Similarity: the number of times the trainee makes the same response-as the person judged actually made. 6. Errors in Assuming Similarity: the difference be- tween a trainee's Assumed Similarity and Actual Similarity scores. 7. Errors in Level: the total number of incorrect un- desirable responses a trainee makes. As stated earlier, the level section of the instrument contains items each of which is made up of a pair of statements, one statement 15 being more socially desirable than the other. If the trainee incorrectly indicates that the person judged made an undesirable response to the item, this is considered to be an error in level. The reliability of the instrument was computed by Kuder—Richardson formula 20. Table 1 shows the reliability of the subscales. Table l.--KR-20 reliabilities of the criterion on the pre- test (N=101) _ Scale Reliability Stereotype Accuracy .47 Level Accuracy .63 Total Accuracy .65 Errors in Assuming Similarity .65 Errors in Level .46 Reliabilities of this size are marginally adequate for measuring group gains. Tables 2 and 3 indicate the intercorrelations of the scores of the instrument on the pretest and on the post- test for the total sample. The tables show Stereotype Accuracy to be relative- ly independent of Level Accuracy, especially on the pretest. 16 Table 2.--Intercorrelation of the scores on the criterion for the pretest (N=l45) Stereotype Level Total Errors in .Errors Accuracy Accuracy Accuracy Assuming in Similarity Level Stereotype Accuracy 1.00 Level Accuracy + .27 1.00 Total Accuracy + .79 + .81 1.00 Errors in Assuming Similarity - .26 - .17 - .28 1.00 Errors in Level - .10 - .82 - .59 - .10 1.00 Table 3.--Intercorrelation of the scores on the criterion for the post-test (N=145) Stereotype Level Total Errors in Errors Accuracy Accuracy Accuracy Assuming in Similarity Level Stereotype Accuracy 1.00 Level Accuracy + .42 1.00 Total Accuracy + .82 + .85 1.00 Errors in Assuming Similarity - .35 — .18 - .31 1.00 Errors in Level - .40 - .83 - .72 + .23 1.00 17 Both Stereotype and Level Accuracy scores are highly corre- lated with Total Accuracy: this is reasonable, as the Total Accuracy score is the sum of the Stereotype and Level Accu- racy scores. Level Accuracy has high negative correlations with Errors in Level, which suggests that both scores are measuring somewhat the same thing. Errors in Level is in- versely related to Total Accuracy. The Errors in Assuming Similarity score shows a small negative correlation with all three accuracy scores. The Experimental Design The design of the study consisted of the adminis- tration of a pretest and post-test on the criterion with controlled intervention in the form of training sessions between testings. Training sessions were conducted by the course instructor, and were 50 minutes in length. No con- trol group was used. The trainees in the study were Michigan State Uni- versity students enrolled in a Fall 1968 Psychology of Per— sonality class. Table 4 presents the sex and sample size breakdown of the group. An initial analysis of all the variables was done by a simple correlational analysis run on the Michigan State University Control Data Corporation 3600 data proces- sing equipment. An analysis of difference scores was done 18 Table 4.--Sample sizes and sex of trainees Subjects N Male 78 Female 67 Total 145 by matched t-tests. Because of the suspected relationship between pretest scores and gains, the sample was divided into high, medium, and low initial score groups for the tests of hypotheses. Measures of Individual Differences Measure of Scholastic Ability The course grade in the Psychology of Personality class was chosen as the measure of scholastic ability. It was based on the total points an individual obtained on two multiple-choice tests. Measure of Sensitivity to People The Personality and Leadership Scale is a paper and pencil test consisting of selected items from a test of leadership attitudes and a personality inventory. The measure was developed by Smith and Mietus. The Protebob 19 Personality Inventory and a measure of consideration and responsibility (Dore, 1960) were administered to students in aHWinter 1968 Psychology of Personality Class. From this sample were picked the tests of students who scored consist- ently high on a battery of prediction tests and the tests of a low scoring group of students. These students were matched for sex and grade point average. The items in the leadership scale and the personality inventory were item analyzed. The discriminating items formed the November 1968 Personality and Leadership Scale. This scale was giv- en in the Fall of 1968 to the trainees in this study. The KR-ZO reliability of this form is .56. Subsequent to this study, prediction tests used during that quarter were employed to select a high group which was matched with a low group of similar sex and course grade. Another item analysis was conducted and for- ty-eight items were selected for inclusion in the STOP scale. These items were chosen for both their discrimina- tion values and their internal consistencies, and were ar- ranged in order of their discrimination values. Students in the Winter 1969 personality class took the STOP scale. Groups scoring high and low in the Bill Wilkins, Diagnostic Test of Empathy, and Mr. W tests were separated and matched for sex but not for grade point average. These student's tests were item analyzed. The twenty-four items included 20 in the REVISED STOP scale are those that stood up under this analysis and are arranged in order (Appendix B). The next three measures relate to a trainee's mo- tivation to learn about others. Motivation here was not thought to be only one's level of aspiration. It was also considered to be a function of how much satisfaction an in- dividual obtains in human relations, his level of maturity, his interest in the psychological states of others, and whether he relates to other persons in terms of himself or the other person. Measure of Self-centered Orientation The Human Relations Scale (Appendix B) was a meas- ure of self-centered orientation developed by Linden (1965). It measured whether an individual's social orientation was first-person oriented or other-oriented. The instrument used in this study is a 35 item revision of the original. Its KR-20 reliability coefficient is .82 based on this sample. Measure of Psychological- mindedness Mullin (1962) developed a measure of psychological- mindedness which he called the First Impressions test (Ap- pendix B). It measured an individual's tendency to respond about.another person's internal psychological states, that is, his desires, feelings, or thoughts, rather than his 21 social stimulus value, his actions, or his physical appear- ance. The-present shortened test form has a KR-20 relia- bility of .82 based on the trainee's scores. Measure of Level of Aspiration Level of aspiration was measured by the difference score between each trainee's initial Total Accuracy score on the criterion and the goal he set for the post-test. For example, a trainee scored 44 on the criterion pretest, prior to taking the post-test he indicated he would attempt to reach a score of 55 on the post-test. Plus eleven is his level of aspiration. Measure of Observational Accuracy The measure, termed Observations of Mrs. D., Mrs. N., and Mrs. P., presents the trainees with filmed inter- views of three persons. While viewing the films the trainees are instructed to note their similarity to the in- terviewees. They are not informed that they will be tested afterwards on their ability to observe physical character- istics and events accurately. After filling out a short form containing questions about similarity, the trainees are given another test booklet consisting of forty-eight objective questions about what they saw during the films. 22 Training Methods and Materials The training sessions used the case study method. The study of a whole person allows the student the fullest knowledge of the interaction effects in his judging. Accu- racy in the judging process seems to be dependent upon in- teractions between the components of sensitivity, stereo- type, level, and empathic accuracies, and interactions be- tween the person judged, the judge, and the judging situa- tion (Smith, 1966). In the case study method, the princi- ples of feedback, diagnosis of errors and practice were used. In a typical training session, the trainees are given a specific principle to apply. They are presented with training materials (Appendix C), and given information about the case study. They take a pretest, get practice and feedback in.a practice section of the materials, take a post-test, and receive feedback on the post-test. In this manner they practice applying principles, get feedback and learn what errors they are making. Further elaboration and examples of the training materials used in these sessions can be found in the works of Price (1969) and Spier (1969). Other training is directed wholly to giving the trainee information pertaining to his leveling errors, empathic errors, or stereotyping errors. Materials used in 23 these training sessions are diagnostic in nature, and pro— vide feedback of a general kind to the trainee. RESULTS The results of this study will be presented in two sections. The first deals with the overall effectiveness of the training program and presents results concerning the principal hypothesis: Training involving diagnostic feedback in judging persons will increase the sensitivity of trainees to a person not used in the practice sessions. The second section deals with individual differences in the impact of training. It presents results concerning variability in training gains as a result of initial accu- racy. It also gives data pertaining to hypotheses about the effects of training upon intellectual efficiency, personal- ity sensitivity, motivation, observant behavior, and sex. Effectiveness of Training Hypothesis 1. Training involving diagnostic feedback in judging persons will increase the sensitiv- ity of trainees to a person not used in the practice sessions. Table 5 presents an analysis of training gains on five criteria of predictive accuracy. T-tests of the 24 25 Table 5.—-Gains on five criteria of predictive accuracy (N= 145) ar— _ ' 4 —_—== Criterion Post-test Pretest Difference t Stereotype Accuracy 23.5 22.0 +1.5 3.1** Level Accuracy 20.7 20.1 +0.6 1.2 Total Accuracy 44.1 42.0 +2.1 2.6** Errors of Level 7.7 8.4 -0.7 2.3** Errors in Assuming Similarity +5.8 +6.8 -l.0 1.0 ** = significant at the .01 level of significance (one-sided test) differences between scores on the pretest and the post-test are significant for the Total Accuracy score and the Stereo- type Accuracy score, but not for the Level Accuracy score. Also there is a significant decrease in the Errors of Level score. Individual Differences in the Impact of Training Hypothesis 2. The lower the initial sensitivity of the trainee, the greater his gains. Table 6 gives a training gains analysis for each of the criterion scores: the analysis divides the training sample into thirds based on each criterion score. A gain 26 'Table 6.--Analysis of gains (poet-test minus pretest) by pretest scores Criterion Lower 3rd Middle 3rd Upper 3rd Total Stereotype Accuracy +4.0** +1.3* -0.7 +1.5** Level Accuracy +3.1** -0.3 -l.0 +0.6 Total Accuracy +5.4** +2.6** -0.9 +2.1** Errors of Level +0.7* 0.0 -2.7** -0.7** Errors in Assuming Similarity -3.3** -1.0 +4.0** -l.0 * = significant at the .05 level of significance (one-sided test) ** = significant at the .01 level of significance (one-sided test) score was obtained by subtracting each trainee's pretest score from his post-test score. The scores reported are the mean gain scores. The N's for each of the scores ex- cluding the Errors in Assuming Similarity are: lower 3rd= 41, middle 3rd=53, upper 3rd=51, and Total Samp1e=l45. For the Errors in Assuming Similarity score, the N's are: low- er 3rd=48, middle 3rd=49, upper 3rd=48, and Total Samp1e= 145. The lower third of the sample exhibited significant gains in accuracy, but made more errors in assuming simi- larity to the criterion individual. The middle group in- creased its Total Accuracy through a gain in Stereotype Accuracy. Finally the upper third showed slight decreases 27 in predictive accuracy, a significant reduction of its Er- rors in Assuming Similarity, and an equally significant re- duction in its Errors of Level. An interpretation of these results is found in the Discussion section. Differences in Scholastic Ability Hypothesis 3. The higher the scholastic ability of the traineeyythe greater his gains. All pretest criterion scores are significantly cor- related with course grades. The course grade correlates +.26 with Stereotype Accuracy, +.28 with Level Accuracy, +.34 with Total Accuracy, -.17 with Errors of Level, and -.15 with Errors in Assuming Similarity. Regarding gain scores, only the Gains in Errors of Level score correlated significantly with course grades. An analysis to determine the effect of course grade upon training gains was made in the following manner: A trainee with a high course grade and a trainee with a low course grade were paired on the basis of their having es— sentially the same pretest Total Accuracy criterion score. This matching was done throughout the range of pretest Total Accuracy scores. These paired individuals were then grouped into high, middle, and lovaretest Total Accuracy score groups. T-tests of the differences in training gains between the high and low course grade trainees for each of the criterion scores and for the upper, middle, lower, and 28 total groups were then performed. Results for the total group are presented in Table 7. Table 7.--T-tests of differences in training gains of a high scholastic ability group minus the gains of a low ability group when matched on pretest cri- terion scores (N=61 pairs) Criterion Criterion Criterion Score §td. Deviation Mean High Low High Low Difference t Stereotype Accuracy 4.1 5.0 +1.6 +1.0 +0.6 0.7 Level Accuracy 3.6 5.3 +1.0 -0.3 +1.3 1.6 Total Accuracy 5.8 8.9 +2.9 +0.6 +2.3 1.6 Errors of Level 2.5 3.1 -0.8 -0.1 +0.7 1.3 Errors in Assuming Similarity 6.3 9.7 +0.4 -0.2 +0.6 0.4 Only the results for the total groups are reported in the tables for the following hypotheses. Those gains for the upper, middle, and lower pretest score groups which are significant will be noted in the text.' The upper third of the sample exhibited a signifi— cant difference between the high and low scholastic ability groups in Total Accuracy (t=+2.1, significant at .05 level). 29 The table shows no significant differences, although the t's of the Level and Total Accuracy scores approach significance Personality Differences Hypothesis 4. The more sensitive the personality of the trainee, the_greater his gains. Neither pretest nor gain scores, with the exception of the Stereotype Accuracy gain score, which correlation is -.18, is significantly correlated with the scores from the test of a sensitive personality. Table 8 shows that there Table 8.—-T-tests of differences in training gains of a high scoring group minus the gains of a low scoring group on the leadership and personality scale when matched on pretest criterion scores (N=61 pairs) - _—= Criterion Criterion Criterion Score Std. Deviation Mean High Low High Low Difference t Stereotype Accuracy 4.7 4.5 +1.7 +1.5 +0.2 0.2 Level Accuracy 4.5 4.7 +0.5 +0.8 -0.3 0.4 Total Accuracy 7.3 6.7 +2.1 +2.1 0.0 0.0 Errors of Level 2.7 2.7 -0.4 -0.8 +0.4 0.7 Errors in Assuming Similari- ty 8.7 7.6 +0.2 +0.8 -0.6 0.4 30 are no significant differences between trainees who score high and those who score low on the scale, when matched for pretest Total Accuracy score in the manner previously des- cribed. Motivational Differences Hypothesis 5. The stronger the selffigentered orientation of the individual, the less his gains. Self-centered orientation is not related to pretest Accuracy scores, but is significantly correlated with pre- test Errors in Assuming Similarity (-.24) and with pretest Errors of Level scores (-.l6). Self-centered orientation correlates with the Errors in Assuming Similarity gains score (-.18), but not significantly with any other training gains score. Table 9 shows there were no significant dif- ferences between the gains of a high and a low self-centered orientation group when matched for pretest Total Accuracy scores 0 Hypothesis 6. The more psychologically-minded the trainee, the greater his gains. No relationship was found between psychological- mindedness and gain scores. A barely significant relation- ship was found between psychological-mindedness and pretest Level Accuracy (-.l6), and pretest Errors of Level (+.l6). Because of the lack of relationship indicated, no further analysis was attempted. 31 Table 9.--T-tests of differences in training gains of a high self-centered orientation group minus the gains of a low self-centered orientation group when matched on pretest criterion scores (N=58 pairs) Criterion Criterion Criterion Score Std. Deviation Meant; High Low High Low Difference t Stereotype Accuracy 4.4 4.6 +1.5 +1.5 0.0 0.0 Level Accuracy 4.0 4.4 +0.3 +1.1 -0.8 1.0 Total Accuracy 6.2 7.0 +1.8 +2.5 -0.7 0.6 ------------—--—---------_--------------------------------- Errors of Level 2.6 2.8 -0.3 -0.9 +0.6 1.0 Errors in Assuming Similarity 8.2 8.2 +1.1 +1.6 -0.5 0.9 Hypothesis 7. The higher the trainee's level of aspiration, the greater his gains. Pretest Accuracy scores are negatively correlated with level of aspiration; the error scores are positively correlated (Table 10). Table 11 shows the difference in the gains of high and low level of aspiration trainees when matched for pre- test Total Accuracy scores. The only significant differ- ence in gains was in the Level Accuracy score of the upper third of the sample (t=+2.3, .05 level of significance). 32 Table lO.--Correlations of level of aspiration with pretest scores and gains in scores as a result of train- ing (N=145) Stereotype Level Total Errors Errors in Accuracy Accuracy Accuracy Of Assuming Level Similarity Pre-test -.31** -.4o** -.44** +.3o** +.l8* Gains +.10 +.07 +.l4 .00 -.15 » II significant at the .05 level of significance. ** = significant at the .01 level of significance. Table 11.--T-tests of differences in training gains of a high aspiration group minus the gains of a low aspiration group when matched on pretest crite- rion scores (N=56 pairs) Criterion Criterion Criterion Score Std. Deviation Mean High Low High Low Difference t Stereotype- Accuracy 4.7 4.3 +2.1 +1.3 +0.8 0.8 Level Accuracy 4.2 4.9 +1.1 -0.1 +1.2 1.3 Total Accuracy 6.8 8.0 +2.6 +1.3 +1.3 0.9 Errors of Level 2.7 2.7 -0.9 -0.5 -0.4 0.8 Errorssin Assuming Similarity: 9.1 8.4 -0.6 +1.0 -1.6 1.0 33 Differences in Observation Hypothesis 8. The more observant the trainee in.a natural setting, the greater his_gains. Gains in Stereotype Accuracy are significantly cor- related with scores on the test of observation (+.21). Table 12 indicates the differences in the gains of high and low observation accuracy trainees when matched on pretest Total Accuracy scores. The high observation accuracy group gained significantly more than the low group in Stereotype Accuracy. Table 12.-—T-tests of differences in training gains of.a high observation accuracy group minus the gains of a low observation accuracy group when matched on pretest criterion scores (N=44 pairs) Criterion Criterion Criterion Score Std. Deviation Mean Difference t High Low High Low Stereotype Accuracy 4.3 3.5 +2.2 +0.2 +2.0 2.3* Level Accuracy 4.0 4.6 +0.5 +0.9 —0.4 0.5 Total Accuracy 5.3 5.9 +2.6 +0.8 +1.8 1.5 Errors of Level 2.3 2.7 -0.4 -0.4 0.0 0.0 Errorsvin Assuming Similarity 7.7 8.3 -l.6 +1.2 -2.8 1.6 *significant at the .05 level of significance (one- sided test) 34 Sex Differences Hypothesis 9. Females make greater gains than males. No significant sex differences were found on any of the criterion gain scores. DISCUSSION Table 13 presents a summary of the results concern- ing the hypotheses tested. As indicated in Table 13, the hypothesis regarding the effectiveness of the training was strongly supported.. Three of the hypotheses regarding individual differences in the impact of the training were supported. The data did not support five of the hypotheses tested. No strong relation- ships were found between gains as a result of training and the sensitivity of the personality of the trainee, and the sex of the trainee. . The findings of the study in regard to training ef- fectiveness are consistent with previous studies ofya simi- lar nature. The training results indicate that training which stresses feedback, diagnosis of errors in judging, and practice on specific principles does generalize to other individuals not included in the training materials. The studies of Kepes (1965) and Grossman (1967) are most rele- vant to the present one. Both found some training gains in second person sensitivity. The principles of training used in each study were feedback, participation, and practice on specific principles. 35 36 Table 13.--Summary of evidence for hypotheses W Hypotheses Evidence 1‘? 1. Training involving diagnostic feedback in judging persons will increase the sensi-' tivity of trainees to a person not used in the practice sessions. ++ 2. The lower the initial sensitivity of the trainee, the greater his gains. ++ 3. The higher the scholastic ability of the trainee, the greater his gains. ? 4. The more sensitive the personality of the trainee, the greater his gains. 0 5. The stronger the self-centered orientation of the individual, the less his gains. 0 6. The more psychologically-minded the trainee, the greater his gains. 0 7. The higher the trainee's level of aspiration, the greater his gains. 0 8. The more observant the trainee in a natural setting, the greater his gains. ? 9. Females make greater gains than males. 0 ++ = statistically significant support. ? = inconsistent or insignificant support. 0 = no support. The size of the training effect is difficult to measure with the present experimental design. The lack of a control group precludes dividing the gains into those due to actual training and those due to practice effects.- 37 Furthermore, as the reliability of each of the subscales of the criterion is marginal, random error could be expected to play its part in confounding the results. Individual Differences Gains and Initial Sensitivity In explaining the training gains, individual varia- bility based upon initial sensitivity must be considered. The third of the sample which scored lowest on the pretest criterion made substantial gains in the two accuracy com- ponents, while the upper third of the sample actually suf- fered a slight and insignificant loss in accuracy on the post-test. Why this is so may have a variety of explana- tions. The most probable factor involved is regression to the mean. Without a control group, the amount of variance due to regression cannot be measured. However, in examining Table 6, it can be seen that the Total Accuracy gains of the lower third of the sample were quite large (mean gain of +5.4), those of the middle third of the sample were still large (mean gain of +2.6), and those losses of the upper third were insignificant (mean gain of -0.9). The total sample exhibited a statistically significant mean gain of +2.1 (t=2.6). The gains of the low and middle group far exceed the losses of the upper initial accuracy group. 38 The slight losses of the upper group might be due to an interaction of three factors. Ceiling effects might be operating to prevent this superior group from making further gains. The tendency for the scores of this group to regress to the mean may be nullified by the effects of practice on the criterion. The large gains of the initial low and moderate ac— curacy groups might be accounted for in a similar manner. Regression to the mean, practice effects, and training ef- fects all might act to increase the trainee's scores. Pos- sibly knowledge of the pretest criterion score indicated to the members of the low group that they had to make rela- tively many changes in their responses on the post-test. They might be more open to changing their perception of the criterion individual and more motivated to use the knowledge gained in the training sessions. Grossman (1967) thought that the low scorers made relatively large errors in stereo- typing, in assuming similarity, in their level accuracy, and in their implicit personality theories. Possibly the train- ing's major impact lies in helping the low scorers to cor- rect their gross errors. There are many possible explanations for the varia- bility in gains as a function of initial accuracy. Which ones are operating and to what extent are not known from the data of this study. 39 Differences in Scholastic Ability It was found that course grade is correlated with pretest criterion score, but not with gains in training. Intellectual ability, as measured by course grade, is a significant factor in sensitivity. The more intellectually able trainees are already scoring high on the criterion pretest, and not likely to exhibit post-test gains. The nature of the sample must be considered. The range of in- telligence is restricted, as all trainee were college students. Perhaps with a more random sample of intellectu- al ability the relationship of gains with scholastic abil- ity would be more rigorously tested. Differences in Personality Sensitivity No relationship of training gains to scores on a test of a sensitive personality was found. That the scale is an untried instrument must be considered foremost in evaluating this finding. Given a more reliable and valid instrument, further investigations into the nature of this possible relationship should be made. Motivational Differences The degree of self-centered orientation of a trainee is not related to his ability to profit from train- ing designed to increase second-person predictive accuracy. The hypothesis regarding the relationship between 40 psychological-mindedness and gains in predictive accuracy as a result of training is not supported. This is consist- ent with previous findings of Mullin (1962). The hypothe- sis concerning the relationship between level of aSpira- tion and gains in training is not supported, although the results indicate a trend in the hypothesized direction. Further investigation of this relationship with more re- fined instruments is recommended. In general, level of motivation as measured by self-centered-orientation, psychological-mindedness, and level of aspiration to increase post-test criterion per- formance was not found to be substantially related to gains as a result of training. Observational Differences This hypothesis is partially supported. Trainees scoring high on a test of observation made greater gains in Stereotype Accuracy than low scorers. This is somewhat consistent with the literature. Harris (1962) found ob- servational accuracy to be related to some types of stereo- type accuracy. Grossman (1963) found a significant corre- lation between observational accuracy and interpersonal sensitivity free of stereotype judgements. Sex Differences No sex differences in training gains were found. Past studies suggest there is no sex difference in 41 interpersonal sensitivity ability, or at best a slightly superior ability in women (Grossman, 1963, 1967; Taft, 1955). Who Can Best be Trained? The data of this study suggests that there are no major limitations upon who can be trained. There remains some question about how much the already sensitive person can profit from training. The level of self-centered-ori- entation of‘a trainee, the psychological-mindedness, the as- piration, the personality characteristics, and sex do not influence his trainability. To a certain extent, the_more observant trainee will make greater training gains. And the more intelligent trainee will enter the training situ- ation at a higher level of sensitivity and reach the limits of sensitivity that present training methods can help him attain sooner than the less intelligent trainee. Comments About Principles of Training The principles used in the sessions are effective. Giving trainees knowledge of their performance and showing them where they made incorrect predictions on practice ma- terials has increased their sensitivity to at least the criterion individual. 42 The duration between making the prediction and the feedback should be made as short as possible. In this program, the intervals ranged from very short, on the training materials, to rather long, a week or two, on the diagnostic materials. Ideally, diagnosis of errors would be made shortly after the prediction process, when the ma— terial is still fresh in the trainee's mind. The present study used one very large training group. It is thought that more efficient training could be done using small training groups, where individual prob- lems could be spotted and corrected. Training Materials A simplification of the scoring procedure on the training materials should be made. At present, the trainee has to invest quite a bit of energy in understanding and maintaining response methods. The present training materials are quite diverse in methods of presenting subjects and methods of measuring sensitivity. The trainee's reactions to types of training materials and methods of presentation should be further examined, as should the effectiveness of these various types of presentation. 43 The Criterion Instrument At present, the criterion instrument requires the judge to make predictions about one individual. It seems that the presentation of more than one individual would be beneficial, if the individuals were clearly differentiated. Reliability of the instrument is marginal. A fur- ther refinement should be made. Predictive validity of the instrument is unknown. This should be researched. Attempts at establishing construct validity should be con- tinued. As on the training materials, the scoring procedure is somewhat confusing; a simplification would be in order. The Experimental Desigp As indicated in the preceding discussion, the ex- perimental design lacked the ability to isolate variance due to practice effects, regression to the mean, and the marginal reliability of the instruments. Future studies should include a control group receiving no training. The realistic problem is to obtain comparable training and control groups. The long-range effectiveness of the training should be studied. Perhaps a small representative sample of the 44 training group could be re-tested on the criterion instru- ment at a future time. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The purposes of the present study were threefold: (l) to construct a component criterion of sensitivity to a single person, (2) to evaluate the effectiveness of a sen- sitivity training program which gives feedback and diagno- sis of errors, and (3) to investigate specific individual differences in the impact of training. Sensitivity in this study is the ability of an in- dividual to predict what another person said, felt, and did about himself and others. The components of sensitiv- ity are postulated to be Stereotype, Level, and Empathic Accuracy. The criterion instrument is a paper and pencil di- agnostic test of accuracy in judging one individual. It consists of a written description of the individual and seventy-two MMPI statements which the individual answered. The judge's task is to predict how the criterion individual answered these statements. The training program utilized feedback, diagnosis of errors, and practice on specific principles. It con- sisted of training sessions, administered to a Fall 1968 Psychology of Personality class, interpolated between a 45 46 pre- and a post-test on the criterion. No control group was used. A correlational analysis and matched t-tests were used to study training effectiveness. On the basis of the data reported, the following conclusions can be made: Training involving diagnostic feedback in judging persons will increase the sensitivity of trainees to a per- son not used in the practice sessions. I Trainees who are initially low in sensitivity will make the greatest gains after training. It is suggested that the cause of this lies primarily in regression and ceiling effects. The more observant trainee makes greater gains in Stereotype Accuracy. The trainee's course grades, sensitivity of his personality, motivation to understand others, and sex are not related to training gains. It is recommended that refinements be made in the instruments used in this study, that more immediate feed- back be given on predictive errors, and that control groups be used in further studies. It is further suggested that the long range effects of training and the use of smaller training groups be investigated.‘ BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Ammons, R. B. Effects of knowledge of performance: A survey and tentative theoretical formulations. J. Gener. Psychol. 1956, 54, 279-299. Anderson, W. How effective is human relations training for the first line supervisor? Personnel, 1963, 40' 62-650 Bronfenbrenner, V., Harding, J., and Gallwey, M. The measurement of skill in social perception. In McClelland, D.C. (ed.). Talent and Society. New York: Van Nostrand, 1958, pp. 29-108. Byrne, D.-and Clore, G. L., Jr. Effectance Arousal and at- traction. J. Person..Soc. Psychol., 1967, g, (4 whole No. 638). Carron, R. J. Human relations training and attitude change: A vector analysis. Personnel Psychol., Chapman, L. J. Illusory correlation in observational re- port. J. Verbal Lrng. & Verbal Behav., 1967, g, Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P., Genesis of popular but erroneous psychodiagnostic observations. J. Ab- norm. Psychol., 1967, 12, 193-204. Crow, W. J., The effect of training upon accuracy and var- iability in interpersonal perception. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol., 1957, 55, 355-359. Dailey, C. A. An experimental method for improving inter- personal understanding. Psychol. Reports, 1963, 13, 240. Dore, R. The development and validation of forced-choice scales measuring attitudes toward leadership methods. Unpublished Master's thesis, Michigan State University, 1960. 47 48 Goldberg, L. R. A model of item ambiguity in personality assessment. J. Educ. Psychol. Meas., 1963, 23. Goldberg, L. R. Simple models or simple processes? Some research on clinical judgements. American Psychol- Grossman, B. A. The measurement and determinants of inter- personal sensitivity. Unpublished Master's thesis, Michigan State University, 1963. Grossman, B. A. Evaluation of a training program to im- prove the ability to differentiate between people. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1967. Harris, W. The relation of observational to inferential accuracy in judging people. Unpublished Master's thesis, Michigan State University, 1962. Johnson, R. L. Correlates of a test of group sensitivity. Unpublished Master's thesis, Michigan State Uni- versity, 1963. Kepes, S. Y. Experimental evaluations of sensitivity training. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1965. Linden, J. The self—centered orientation in interpersonal relationships. Unpublished Master's thesis, Mich- igan State University, 1965. Lynch, P. Development and evaluation of an empathy train- ing program. Unfinished Master's thesis, Michigan State University, 1968. Mager, R. F.‘ On the sequencing of instructional content. Psychol. Rpts., 1961, 9, 405-413. Messick, S. & Jackson, D. N. Desirability Scale values and dispersions for MMPI items. Psych. Rpts., 1961, 8, 409-414. Miles, M. B. Human relations training: processes and outcomes. J. counsel. Psychol., 1960,.1, 301-306. Mullin, J. A., Empathic drive and its influence on accura— cy in judging people. Unpublished Master's thesis, Michigan State University, 1962. 49 Murray, D. C. & Deabler, H. L. Drawings, diagnoses, and the clinician's learning curve.‘ J. project. tech- niques, 1958, 22, 415-420. Newton, J. R. Judgement and feedback in a quasi-clinical situation. J. person. soc. Psychol., 1965, 1, 336-342. Oskamp, S. The relationship of clinical experience and training methods to several criteria of clinical prediction. Psychol. Monogr., 1962, 76, No. 28 (Whole No. 547). - Price, K. Is the favorableness of interpersonal evalua- tions a general trait? Unfinished Master's thesis, Michigan State University, 1969. Rorer, L. G. The function of item content in MMPI re- sponses. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Uni- versity of Minnesota, 1963. Sechrest, L., Gallimore, R., & Hersch, P. D. Feedback and accuracy of clinical predictions. J. Consult. Psychol., 1967, 31, 1-11. Skinner, B. F., The technology of teaching, New York: Ap- pleton-Century—Crofts, 1968. Smith, H. C. Sensitivity to people. New York: McGraw- Hill, 1966. Smith, H. C. Bill Wilkins as a model for sensitivity training. Paper delivered at the 76th American Psychological Association Convention, San Fran- cisco, California, 1968. Spier, M. Evaluation of a stereotype accuracy training program. Unfinished doctoral dissertation, Mich- igan State University, 1969. Stelmachers, Z. T. & McHugh, R. B. Contribution of stereo- typed and individualized information to predictive accuracy. J. Consult. Psychol., 1964, 28, 234-242. Taft, R. The ability to judge people. Psychol. Bull., 1955, 52, 1-23. Todd, F. J. & Hammond, K. R. Differential feedback in two multiple-cue probability learning tasks. Behav- ioral Science, 1965, 19, 429-435. 50 Watley, D. J. Counselor variability in making accurate predictions. J. Counsel. Psychol., 1966, 13, 53- 62. Watley, D. J. Feedback training and the improvement of clinical forecasting. J. Counsel. Psychol., 1968, 1.2, 167-171. APPENDICES APPENDIX A JM/HCS June 1968 Michigan State University THE CASE OF BILL HILKINS Bill is a 25 year old graduate student in.psychology who answered the statements in a long personality inventory on two different occasions. The items in this test are those which he answered in the same way on both occasions. This is a test of your ability to predict his responses. Thumbnail sketch of Bill: Bill is from a midwestern upper-middle class family. He is married, and has one child. Bill attended Catholic schools until he came to graduate school. He admires his father for his success and level- headedness. His parents stress achievement, and Bill is very ambitious. Bill attained high status in his college organizations, and in the service was in a responsible position. He joined ROTC in college partly because of patriotism, partly because being an officer would improve his status. He takes little time off for recreation. However, he does enjoy mixing with people. Bill's health is good, although he does have allergies and frequent tension headaches. During unfavorable service assignments, Bill often felt vaguely ill and went to the dispensary. He considered a number of other professions before deciding on psychology. He is materialistic, yet moralistic. He has masculine interests, is restrained and controlled. Bill appreciates artistic subjects. He has sometimes alienated peeple who thought he was too self-confident. He enjoys hearing about shrewd deals, is not above manipulating individuals to achieve his ends, but doubts he would do anything he considers immoral in this respect. He is happy. He philos0phizes with himself quite a bit, and is not very interested in current events. Bill is interested in what other people think and do, but does not care to change them; he considers peOple basically honest, good-natured, but not above little wrong doings. Part A: 41,039" ‘1‘ 2L séb‘w Bill's responses to the following statements are the same as the responses of men- in- general. A large number of men took the same personality inventory Bill did,- For each of the statements below at least two- thirds of the men answered in the same way that he did. Your task is to predict how Bill responded to the state- ments and also to indicate hOW'ZQE would respond. Mark "1" if you think Bill answered true to the statement and you think the state- ment is true or more true than false of yourself. (TT) Mark "2" if you think Bill answered false to the statement and you think the state- ment is false or more false than true of yourself. (FF) Mark "3" if you think Bill answered false to the statement and if you think the statement is true or more true than false of yourself. (FT) Mark "4" if you think Bill answered true to the statement and if you think the state statement is false or more false than true of yourself. (TF) . I have had very peculiar and strange experiences. . I do not always tell the truth. . I am troubled by discomfort in the pit of my stomach every few days or oftener. . I have often wished I were a girl. (Or if you are a girl, I have often wished I were a boy.) F 5. I am easily downed in an argument. 114‘“ UNI-4 V 4.x (over) \. 0" 12. 13. Ar 14. 15. “‘1\ k 17. - 2 - My table manners are not quite as good at home as when I am out in company. I would like to be a florist. Criticism or scolding hurts me terribly. - Sometimes I feel as if I must injure either myself or someone else. I used to keep a diary. I have been inspired to a program of life based on duty which I have since carefully followed. What others think of me bothers me. When someone says silly or ignorant things about something I know about, I do not try to set him right. I am not very religious (less than most people). A person should try to understand his dreams and be guided by or take warning from them. I can be friendly with pe0p1e who dorthings which I consider wrong. I have never done anything dangerous for the thrill of it. Part B: \: -- -' ' I “’3 ‘ ‘ a. " "r. " .f 1‘ '\ 't 1' Bill's responses to the following statements are the OPPOSITE of those of men- in-general. That is, if Bill answered "true”, at least two-thirds of the men would answer "false" and viceversa. Your task is to predict how Bill responded to the statements and also to indicate how you would respond. Mark this section in the same manner as you did in Part A "1" (TT), "2" (FF), 713. T319. 7'20. F 21. F 22. f‘23. 7.24. F'ZS. F’26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. *AW\*\T) I'I' 4u3n (TF), "4" (FT). I do not try to correct people who express an ignorant belief. I have spells of hay fever or asthma. Children should not be taught all the main facts of sex. I enjoy the excitement of a crowd. , I usually "lay my cards on the table" with people that I am trying to correct or improve. I like to let people know where I stand on things. I am afraid of fire. I strongly defend my own opinions as a rule. During one period when I was a youngster I engaged in petty thievery. I seldom find it necessary to stand up for what I think is right. At times my mind seems to work more slowly than usual. I pray several times a week. Any man who is willing and able to work hard has a good chance of succeeding. I am entirely self-confident. I tend to be interested in several different hobbies rather than stick to one of them for a long time. When I get bored I like to stir up some excitement. I like or have liked fishing very much. I try to remember good stories to pass them on to other people. When I leave home I worry about whether the door is locked and the windows closed. - 3’- Part g C 1 a" . ' ':1...-'=-- fi-r Bill responded "true" to one of the statements in each of the following pairs and "false" to the other. Consider both statements in each pair, and then Mark "1" Mark "2" Mark "3" Mark "4" if you think Bill answered tEue to the FIRST statement (1) of the pair and if you think the first statement (1) is true or more true than false of yourself. (TT) if you think Bill answered false to the FIRST statement of the pair and if you think the first statement is false or more false than true of yourself. (FF) ' if you think Bill answered false to the FIRST statement of the pair and if you think the first statement is true or more true than false of yourself. (FT) if you think Bill answered true to the FIRST statement of the pair and if you think the first statement is false or more false than true of yourself. (TF) ‘ ‘ .F 31. (1) People often disappoint me. (2) I wake up fresh and rested most mornings. f: 38. (l) I must admit that I have at times been worried beyond reason over something that really did not matter. (2) Sometimes at elections I vote for men about whom I know very little. 7' 39. (l) I would like to hunt lions in Africa. (2) I find it hard to make talk when I meet people. F. 40. (l) I wish I were not so shy. (2) I seldom ask people for advice. F 41. (l) I have had periods in which I lost sleep over worry. (2) I am neither gaining nor losing weight. F‘ 42. (l) I liked "Alice in Wonderland." (2) I do not mind seeing women smoke. . 7 43. (1) I prefer work which requires close attention, to work which allows me to be careless. (2) During one period when I was a youngster I engaged in thievery. f’ 44. (1) In school I found it very hard to talk before the class. (2) I practically never blush. r 45. (l) I would like to be a singer. _ (2) I do not like to be with a crowd which plays jokes on one another. ? 46. (1) When in a group of people I have trouble thinking of the right things to talk about. (2) Something exciting will almost always pull me out of it when I am feeling low. 1’ 47. (l) I think nearly everyone would tell a lie to keep out of trouble. (2) Sometimes I get so excited I find it hard to go to sleep. ‘A 48. (l) I would not like to belong to several clubs or lodges. . (2) Some of my family have quick tempers. F: 49. (1) The man who provides temptation by leaving valuable property unprotected is about as much to blame for its theft as the one who steals it. (2) I can read a long time without tiring my eyes. 50. (1) At times I feel like picking a fist fight with someone. (2) I like to flirt. ‘ 51. (1) I have never felt better in my life than I do now. (2) Once in a while I think of things too bad to talk about. (1) It does not bother me that I am not better looking. (2) I like to poke fun at people. (over) ‘I 53. (1) (2) 754. (1) (2) 55. (1) (2) F 56. (1) (2) 7' 57. (1) (2) ~r' 58. (1) (2) 59. (1) (2) 60. (l) (2) / 61. (1) (2) f 62. (1) (2) 7 63. (1) (2) ’T 64. (1) (2) T 65. (1) (2) 66. (1) (2) F 67. (1) - (2) r 68. (1) (2) F 69. (1) ~\ ‘I jI ~\ (2) 7 70.. (1) (2) '7 71. (1) (2) T 72. (1) (2) 31 6/7/68 - 4 - I have very few fears compared to my friends. I frequently find myself worrying about something. I have not had to be rough with people who were rude or annoying. I am often inclined to go out of my way to win a point with someone who has opposed me. I have periods in which I feel unusually cheerful without any special reason. Sometimes when embarrassed, I break out in a sweat which annoys me greatly. Several times I have been the last to give up trying to do a thing. It is unusual for me to express strong approval or disapproval of the actions of others. I daydream very little. I frequently notice my hand shakes when I try to do something. There are certain people whom I dislike so much that I am inwardly pleased when they are catching it for something they have done. I very seldom have spells of blues. I do not think I feel more intensely than most people do. My mother or father often made me obey even when I thought it was unreasonable. It is always a good thing to be frank. At times I have worn myself out by undertaking too much. I very much like hunting. I don't blame anyone for trying to grab everything he can get in this world. . . If'I were an artist, I would not like to draw flowers. If I were in trouble with several friends who were equally to blame, I would rather take the whole blame than to give them away. I like to talk about sex. . I have often felt that strangers were looking at me critically. When I was a child I didn't care to be a member of a crowd or gang. When I was a child, I belonged to a crowd or gang that tried to stick together trough thick and thin. I do not have a great fear of snakes. I have difficulty in starting to do things. I am not apt to hide my feelings to the point that people may hurt me without knowing it. I am always disgusted with the law when a criminal is freed through the arguments of a smart lawyer. It makes me uncomfortable to put on a stunt at a party even when others are doing the same sort of things. I like to cook. At times I have very much wanted to leave home. I have never been in trouble with the law. Sometimes without any reason or even when things are going wrong I feel excitedly happy, "on top of the world." I dream frequently. It is not hard for me to ask help from my friends even though I cannot return the favor. I have never had any breaking out on my skin that has worried me. I like tall women. I am very careful about my manner of dress. Most nights I go to sleep without thoughts or ideas bothering me. A large number of people are guilty of bad sexual conduct. APPENDIX B STUDENT NUMBER H. C. Smith January, 1969 FTHE STOP SCALE This training session is designed to increase your understanding of a group that will be described later. THE SEPARATE ANSWER SHEET. When First, however, ANSWER EACH OF THE STATEMENTS BELOW ON Mark "1” if you think the statement is "true" or more true than false as far as you are concerned. Mark ”2” if you think the statement is "false" or more false than true as far as you are concerned. the group is identified, circle the statements as follows: TT true for you and true for the group. FF false for you and false for the group. TF true for you but false for the group. FT false for you but true for the group. Return the materials at the end of the exercise. SCORES Exer A Exer B Exer C IMPROVEMENT Assumed similarity(TT+FF) Actual "(X'd TT+FF) ' . DIFFERENCE Empathy(A-C) ACCURACY Total (C-A) EXERCISE A F'TT FF TF FT 1. Some people I know can look forward to a happier life than I can. F7 TT FF TF FT 2. I really don't like to drink alcoholic beverages. 7’TT FF TF FT 3. I am not particularly methodical in my everyday life. F TT FF TF FT 4. It is as important for a person to be reverent as it is for him to be sympathetic. ' 7’TT FF TF FT 5. I always keep control of myself in an emergency situation. F‘TT FF TF FT 6. I am somewhat more shy than the average person. FTTT FF TF FT 7. I am seldom extremely eXcited or thrilled. 7’TT FF TF FT 8. I am more interested in general ideas than specific _ . facts. 7‘TT FF TF FT 9. Compared to your own self-respect, the respect of others means little. - FITT FF TF FT 10. In a discussion, I tend to lose interest if we talk . about serious literature F7TT FF TF FT 11. I have sometimes corrected others, not because they . were wrong, but only because they irritated me. 7'TT FF TF FT 12. I am a fairly impulsive person. After you have answered the questions above and on the other side for yourself and after the group is identified and you have circled the answers above, cal- culate the following scores and put in the table: ASSUMED SIMILARITY. ACTUAL SIMILARITY. DIFFERENCE. ACCURACY. Total number of TT and FF responses circled. Total number of X's in the TT and FF columns. Assumed similarity score minus actual similarity score. Total number of X's inside circles. (over) FTT 7—TT F'TT 7'TT fTTT FTTT F7TT 7'TT 7‘TT 7‘TT FTTT 7'TT FF FF. FF FF FF FF FF FF FF FF FF FF TF TF TF TF TF TF TF TF TF TF TF FT- FT FT FT FT FT FT FT FT FT FT FT l3. 14. 15. 16. wl7. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. EXERCISE B I have quite a few fears about my future. Artistic experiences are of great importance in my life. I consider most matters very carefully before I form an opinion. I almost never lose my head. .I dislike it when I am with people a great deal. I like to visit exhibits of famous paintings. I like to have my life so arranged that it runs smoothly without muqh change in plans. Radical agitators should be encouraged to make public speeches. I am extremely active in my everyday life. I am always taking on added social reSponsibility. I generally keep in the background at social functions. I am quite self-confident. EXERCISE C In each question below are two statements of things that a work leader can do. Choose the one you feel it is more important for him to do. Mark ”1” if you think alternative (1) is more important. Mark ”2" if you think alternative (2) is more important. It is more important for a leader: /11 .211 .211 «211 /11 )11 /11 /11 Ill /11 /11 0111 22 22 22 22 22 22 22 22 22 22 22 22 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 21 21 21 21 21 21 21 21 21 21 21 21 HCS:Ceb 12-12-68 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. (1) To take an interest in the worker as a person. (2) To maintain sensible standards of performance. (1) To set up most projects himself. (2) To let his workers make all routine daily decisions. (1) To give instructions on just the way to do each job. (2) To let workers take time out from the monotony. (1) To see that the group produces. (2) To let the workers generally decide how to do their work. (1) To teach his workers new things. (2) To work along with his men as much as possible. (1) To explain carefully each worker's duties to him. (2) To spend some of his time helping to get the work done. ‘(1) To meet with the workers to consider proposed changes. (2) To pitch right in with the workers to help make changes. (1) To explain the duties of each worker's job to him until he really understands them. (2) To pitch right in with the workers. (1) To be a skill trainer. (2) To set a good example by working hard himself. (1) To make it clear that he is the leader of the group. (2) To have workers settle by themselves problems they meet on the job. (1) To be respected for his knowledge of the work that the group does. (2) To pass along to his workers information from higher management. (1) To realize that a worker generally knows when he is doing poor work without being told. (2) To explain in detail to each worker the duties of his job. HUMAN RBLNTIONS SCALE This is a scale measuring beliefs about how people react in different situations. There are no right or wrong answers. In many cases it may be difficult to choose an answer, but please mark a choice for each one. The Case of Hans: The place: Munich, Germany. The time: 1922. Hans Meyerhoff, a poor shopkeeper, has been invited to a secret meeting of a small organization headed by Adolf Hitler. Hans is bewildered throughout the meeting. 1. Hans becomes enthralled with Hitler and tries to convince one of his customers, Rudolph, to join the Party. Why is Rudolph hesitant? 1. Hans, himself, doesn‘t know what.he is joining. 2. Hans and the rest will 000“ outgrow this craze. CE? I wonder why Hans wants me to join the Party.. 2. In time, however, Hans's friend, Rudolph Hess, joins the Party and becomes one of Hitler's most trusted aides. For some reason in the middle of World War II, Rudolph Hess flew alone right over London only to be shot down. What were Hitler's thoughts about this? (I). He did it to embarrass me before the world. 2. He did it to show the others he wasn' t a coward like they said. 3. He did it to prove to himself he is brave. 4. He did it in a moment of insanity. 3. Hans, however, remained far down the line in Party power. What does his wife think about this? 1. He must feel inadequate not to have been promoted any higher. (:2. I wonder if he thinks I' m.part1y responsible. 3. The other members don't respect him at all. 4. Hans's only daughter, Hilda, falls in love with one of the few Jews left in Munich. Hans of course is Opposed to the romance. What does her lover, Max think about Hans? 1. Hans is weak; even his friends in the Party don' t respect him. 2. He is only a poor, frightened shopkeeper. (g: I think he genuinely hates me. U1 Hilda and max elope, as a friendly guard lets them through a checkpoint. What was the guard thinking as they went through? 1. They will have few friends in allof Germany. (2: They will always be grateful to me for letting them out. 3. They know not what they do, only.of their mutual-love. The Case of Cardinal Vincengi: Cardinal Vincenzi is attending the Ecumenical Council in Rome. There is a question on the floor about which he has strong feel- ings, diametrically Opposed to the Pope. He is,in the process of preparing his argument. . 6. What is he thinking as he writes his speech? . l. The other Cardinals have a high regard for the Pope. (g: I must be careful not to arrouse the Pope's ire against me. The Case of the Babe: Besides being one of baseball's great heroes, Babe Ruth had a sincere interest in children.“ He once had an interview with Tommy Smith, reporter for his high school paper. 7. What was Tommy thinking during the interview? ($2 I hope he thinks I'm.doing a good job. 2. I wonder if he knows how admired he is. O ”--_1- bkl_‘- LA.“ -A‘11-0 fi-A.. CIA fi‘tln n; his: f‘m- 1‘h. fh‘g- -2- The Case of Martha: Martha is an orphan.' She is fifteen years old and is being con- sidered for adoption through a social work agency. The interested couple is talking with a social worker. 8. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. The social worker decides to recommend the adoption. What might she be thinking during her conference with her supervisor? L14 He seems to respect my views. 2. His experience makes him a keen judge of adoption cases. 3. He has a lot of confidence because of his wide experience. Her supervisor's thoughts? 1. She's done a good job of analysis. L21 She knows she has to convince me. 3. I've heard she is a very capable worker. Martha is adopted by the couple. What is the social worker thinking after her twelfth and final monthly visit? ' 1. Martha realizes she has never been happier. 2. Her parents and friends have grown to love her. (3. They all seemed terribly grateful to me. Two years later Martha falls in love with a college senior named Bill. What do her parents think about this? 1. At eighteen we should expect a girl to fall in love. 2. Bill seems to love her too; he treats her like a queen. 53a She doesn't need us like she used to. Martha talks to her social worker for advice about leaving her parents so soon. Martha's thoughts? ' l. I hope she doesn't think she made a mistake with me. .21 I hope she doesn't think I let her down. 3. A social worker would be a good person to talk to now. 4. She is respected by her colleagues; she must be a good worker. What is the social worker thinking? {I} Martha thinks I can give her some good advice. 2. She knows she needs advice. The social worker talks with Martha's parents. Their thoughts? 1. Martha seems to think a lot of her now. 2. She will be good to talk to now. Ci. She probably thinks we let her down as parents. Martha and Bill decide to get married. Her parents' thoughts now? 1. They make a great couple and have happy days ahead. 2. They know they made the right decision. {3. HOpe she still loves us. What is Bill thinking now? 1. Her parents still love her and understand her. 2. Our lives are just beginning. . g; I hope she loves me as much as I love her. The Case of Lou: Lou is the father of three college-age children. He has been acting rather cold toward his wife as of late. His wife is worried. They had always gbtten along well in their 26 years of marriage, and were able to discuss their problems with each other. -3.- 17. What might Sally be thinking? She is his favOrite childL' . & must be depressed because I left for college. ' 2. I guess adults have periods of depression just like us kids. 3. I hope he can talk it out with his friends. The Case of Albert: Little Albert is a schoOlboy in Germany. ‘Hé’is doing below aver- age work in math and sees his teacher for help. 18. What is Albert thinking during the conference? 1. He is one of my best teachers. . I wonder if he's interested in helping me. 3. I wonder what kind of teacher he thinks he is. 19. Poor Albert failed his math course. How did his teacher feel? 1. I hope this doesn't hurt his self-confidence too much. (21 I hope he doesn't feel resentful toward me for failing him. 3. He just doesn't have the ability to do math. 20. How did his teacher feel a few years later when his former student formulated an equation e - mcz, changing world history? 1. Einstein will go down as one of the great thinkers. CD. I wonder if he thinks I was a poor‘teaoher. 3. He is being hailed by all as our greatest physicist. The Case of Samuel Reshevsky: Mr. Reshevsky is a world champion chess player. He recently played 50 players simultaneously. . .1 21. What were his opponents thinking as they sat down to play him? 1. He is truly one of the world's great players. 2. He must know he's pretty good to play so many at once. CT. Does he really think that I'm.a challenge? Jfi. One of the players, a 15 year old boy, beats the Mhster.’ His name is Bobby ’ Fisher, current U. S. chess champion. As they played the second time, what was Reshevsky thinking? '"- .p 4 1.1 don' t think success has gone to Bobby's head. C2} He seems to look at me differently than he did the last time. 3. The audience seems really to like him, maybe for his youth. 23. What did Bobby think after he defeated the Master again?" 1. His one mistake at the end cost him the game. He must think I'm his equal now. The Case of Cathy: Cathy and her roommate are sophomores at a large-university.‘ They just had a fight about keeping the room neat, Cathy claiming her roommate is not neat enough. 24. What did Cathy think after talking to her housemother about it? 1. She really understands the problem.= 2. Any housemother would have trouble handling this kind of problem. 3. I can see why girls think she is so understanding. C3: I wonder what she thought of me and my side of the argument. 25. Cathy gets a new roommate; her thoughts upon meeting her? .3 1.1 hope she's more well- liked than my old roommate. fi’2. Anything will be better than the old situation. 1.3. I hope she realizes that her way will not always be the best. -4- The Case of Bob: Bob is a senior majoring in math and plans to go to graduate school next year. His math teacher, Mr. Lewis, is retiring. 26. His wife's thoughts about the news of her husband's retirement? 1. I'm proud of all the praise he's getting from.his colleagues. 2. He has a feeling of real satisfaction after these 30 years. 3. These next years might be a good change for him. ’4. Maybe he will need me more now the t. he is not working. 27. Mr. Lewis is replaced by a young Ph..D. fiShe is bright, good-looking and single. What is Bob thinking as she walks into class? 1. This should be an interesting course. 2. I wonder what the staff thinks of this new addition. ci. I hope she likes my work. 28. Bob goes to talk to her about his work. Her thoughts? 1. He seems upset at me for marking so hard. (2i He seems genuinely interested in improving his work. 29. Bob gets straightened out and ends up with an A in the course.' What are his thoughts now? L She thinks I really know the material.now. (2% The class ended up respecting her and liking her a lot. 3. This was a very beneficial course. The Case of Leon: Leon Winters is captain of his bowling team. His team loses its first three matches and he resigns as captian. Under his successor, Al, the team wins its next 4 games. 30. What are Al's thoughts now? 1. Leon must realize he's more of an asset to the team as a member than as captain 2. His bowling has improved lately, as has the teams'. (33. Heresents me for taking over his job. The Case of Jan: Jan is a high school dropout. He is seeing an advisor from the Poverty Program to try to get a job. 31. What is the advisor thinking as he talks to Jan? 1. He realizes he needs help. Q. He seems to think I can help him. 3. This program was designed to help this kind of boy. 32. _1Jan gets a job on a construction job. His foreman' s thoughts at first. ‘&fl?. This boy needs to gain some self- confidence. (21 He'll be depending on me to help get him started. 33. The foreman' s thoughts? 6 Jan will probably worry what I'll do to him about the fight. 2. I wonder if Jan thought it was wrong to have fought with Bret. 3. I wonder what the workers think of Jan. 4. These things happen on any job. The Case of Mr. Moore: Alan Moore is in the market for a new car. He is deciding be- tween a Lincoln and a Cadillac. 34. What might he be thinking as he is talking to one of the salesmen?" CI. I wonder if he thinks I'm.an easy customer to sell. 2- I've heard he's: a wall-respected salesman. -5- The Case of Ellen: Ellen has been dating a boy steadily for three months. both freshmen and have decided to stop seeing each other for a while. 35. How does Ellen feel? 1. It's best for both of us because we're too young to get serious. CZ: I hope he still likes me even though we're not dating. 3. I wonder how he feels about it. jhs 1-23-67 They are he ¥*\N‘- FIRST IMPRESSIONS H. C. Smith Fall, 1968 Directions: This is a study of the impressions people make on others. You will see three people in silent movies. Trygto form as life-like an impression of each as you can. The first will be of Mrs. P.: the second of Mr. W.: And the third of Mrs. N. As each film is finished the camera will be stopped. Then, in each of the groups of statementSIEmbered below pick the one that is like your impression of the person. Mark the nimber of this statement on the separate answer sheet. Work as rapidly as you can! THE CASE OF MRS. P. l. (1) is sincere (2) wearing a coat (3) fairly attractive. 2. (1) is about 40 years old (2) talkative (3) self-satisfied. 3. (1) did most of the talking (2) felt the inerview had been successful (3) is one who makes a good impression. 4. (l) moistens her lips (2) is glad to leave (3) experienced with small groups. 5. (l) feels inadequate (2) is facially expressive (3) average looking. 6. (l) is an anxious person (2) a pleasing person (3) a conservative dresser. 7. (1) considers the interview serious (2) is gregarious (3) shows signs of amusement. 8. (l) is a typical housewife (2) has family problems (3) is intelligent 9. (l) is seeking employment (2) responding quickly (3) an average dresser. 510. (l) is a modest dresser (2) active in the community (3) uncertain of her answers. 111. (1) gave short answers at first (2) enjoys her family (3) has dark "features. 312. (l) is amusing (2) a modest dresser (3) feels inadequate. ’13. (1) wants to make an impression on interviewer (2) is from a laboring class (3) has good posture. 114. (l) is one who would go to a neighborhood bar (2) feels self conscious (3) dressed in red. 315. (l) is verbal (2) a housewife (3) feeling under pressure. 216. (l) is using too much makeup (2) worries a lot (3) gabby. 217. (l) sat on edge of chair (2) became self confident as interview progressed (3) is a good listener. .18. (l) is worrying (2) moving her head (3) wearing a coat. 319. (1) is chatty (2) a good housekeeper (3) not sure of herself 320. (l) is a neat dresser (2) laughing often (3) indecisive. (over) I 27. I 28. 129. J 30. 2 31. 332. 3 33. I 34. 2 35. I 36. I37. 1_38. 239. 3 40. 3141. I 42. I 43. 5 44. 5 45. 1'46. THE CASE OF MR. W. (l) is unsure of himself (2) looks like a ”beat" (3) is wearing a striped sweater. (l) is thinking the questions through (2) wearing a sweater (3) talking fast. (1) is wearing glasses (2) somewhat perplexed (3) holding his chin. (l) is unmarriéd(2) often grasps his chin (3) is on guard most of the time. (1) is a "lost youth” (2) has little money (3) has little patience with his intellectual inferiors. (1) leaves smiling (2) is an attentive young man (3) is uncertain of the future. (1) does not trust the interviewer (2) could be a delinquent (3) smiles little (1) is confident in his opinions (2) of medium height (3) does not have much money. (1) was eager to leave (2) left quickly (3) is reliable. o (1) has been turned down by several fraternities (2) hopes he has put his story over (3) has black hair. (1) did most of the talking (2) felt under stress (3) smiled when smiled at. (1) is wearing a sweater (2) unable to project his personality (3) considers himself well informed. (l) rocked back and forth (2) has curly hair in the situation. (1) knows what's going on (2)needs a shave (3) is fluent in Speaking. (1) raises eyebrows (2) remains tense throughout interview (3) talks with vigor. (l) feels confused (2) raises eybrows when Speaking (3) had on a loud sweater. (1) feels a bit insecure (2) uses his hands in expression (3) is in'“ his early‘20's. (l) dresses poorly (2) feels he is being treated unjustly (3) is dark complected. (1) has a closed smile (2) thinks before answering (3) needs a haircut. (1) gives no details in his answers (2) changes his facial expression little (3) is self—concerned. (3) is earnestly interested THE CASE OF MRS. N. (1) has a low skilled job (2) is concerned with giving the right answers (3) wearing glasses. (1) feels she is religious (2) is in her sixties (3) left with an empty smile. (1) thinks out the answer to each question (2) has her hair combed back (3) is friendly. (1) wears a black coat (2) is well mannered (3) concerned with what is happening. (1) is a typical grandmother (2) is well mannered (3) interested in others. (1) puckers her lips (2) doubts future employment (3) gives answers of moderate length. 147. 3 48. 1 49. a 51. 352. 153. l 54. [55. 356. 57. 258. ‘359. 260. (1) could be a retired school teacher (2) felt she told everything necessary (3) facially expressive. (1) has her hair in a net (2) has a dry manner (3) does not care about wealth. (1) has animated hands (2) feels lonesome at times. (3) has a quiet nature. (1) is content with life (2) has a sense of humor (3) is dressed suf- ficiently well. (1) feels anxious (2) is elderly (3) smug. (1) visits her grandchildren from time to time (2) wonders what she'll do when she stops working (3) is in good health. (1) is wearing glasses (2) proud of her past (3) 50-60 years old. (1) is the easy going type (2) is wearing a coat (3) enjoys discussing her grandchildren. (1) didn't get much out of the situation (2) wears glasses (3) has a closed smile. (1) is a good homemaker (2) a neat dresser (3) is a satisfied person. (1) is elderly (2) unmoved by the situation (3) lower middle class. (1) could be a sales clerk (2) feels emotionally stable (3) is the resourceful type. (1) her hands became nervous (2) is the grandmother type (3) thought deeply on most questions. (1) clenched her hands (2) is not used to city life (3) is a stabilizing influence. HCS:ta:10-17-68 APPENDIX C THE CASE OF NAOME WARREN October, 1968 "Empathy", assuming similarity between ourselves and others, is the basic method we use in trying to understand other people. Consequently, under-‘ standhxgis determined by empathic accuracy, the accuracy of the assumptions of similarity or difference that we make. improve this kind of accuracy. Stugyfithe sketch of Naomi Warren. -She is a forty year old wife of a social science professor and the mother of three children who are now in college. Naomi is the eldest of four sisters all of whom like to write. Naomi has published several children's books, another sister has published a novel, another sister is a newspaper reporter, and the other sister writes poetry. Naomi's daughter is planning, also, to be a writer. Naomi plays tennis, skates, skiis, and generally enjoys the outdoors. She enjoys cooking but is casual about her housekeeping. The aim of the exercise is to She is a member of several civic groups but dislikes speaking before a group. Answer the following statements for yourself and Naomi. So that you can know whether the training has benefited you, answer the At the end of the session, you will be BEFORE PRACTICE Similar Dissim five statements below now. given time to answer them again. Circle your answers as follows: If you think the statement is true for you and you think Naomi answered true for herself. be read by the instructor. TT. FF. TF. FT. Finally, the correct answers will If you think the statement is false for you and you think Naomi also answered falSe. If you think the statement is true for you but you think Naomi answered false for herself. If you think the statement is false for you but you think Naomi answered true for herself. F TT FF F rr FF i3 TT FF F TT FF TTT FF- CORRECT TF TF TF AFTER PRACTICE Similar Dissim FT TT FT TT ‘ FT TT FT . .TT FT TT; ACTUAL SIMILARITY ASSUMED SIMILARITY FF TF FF TF FF TF FF TF FF TF BEFORE 1 FT 1. FT 2. FT 3. FT 4. FT 5. AFTER Most of the time I am extremely carefree and relaxed. I like to discuss my emotions with others. I am really only interested in what is useful. I like to make a very careful plan before starting in to do anything. Women should have as much right to propose dates to men as men to women. GAIN (over) Sensitivity Training Exercises Exercise A._ giggle your answers and what you think Naomi's answers were to the following three statements. ”X” the correct answer when it is read by the instructor. Record at the bottom of the page the number of your correct respOnses, the number that you assumed similarity on, and the number in which you were actually similar. F'TT FF TF FT 1. I have frequently assumed the leadership of groups. F TT FF TF FT 2. I like to have people aroumlme practically , all of the time. {TTT 'FF TF ' FT 3. There are few things I enjoy more than being a leader of people. Excercise B. After completing Exerecise A, answer the following statements and record your answers in the same way. F'TT FF TF FT 4. It is as important for a person to be reverent as it is for him to be sympathetic. FTTT FF TF FT . 5. I trust in God to support the right and condemn the'wrong F'TT FF TF FT 6. The idea of God means more to me than any other idea. 7‘TT FF TF FT 7. I think that cremation is the best method of burial. 7FTT FF. TF FT 8. In the long run science provides the best hope for solving the world's problems. T'TT FF TF FT 9. Radical agitators should be allowed to make public speeches. szxsi§s_fl. After completing Exercise B, continue with this exercise in the same way. F7TT FF ‘TF FT 10. I never complain about my sufferings and hardships. 7'TT FF TF FT 11. I am moderate in my tastes and sentiments. PITT FF TF FT 12. I Spend a lot cf time philosophizing with myself. F3TT FF TF FT 13. I tend to judge people in terms of their corcrete accomplishments. 7'TT FF TF FT 14. I like to be with people who don't take life toc seriously. I TT FF TF FT 15. I always.keep control of myself in an emergency situation. SL??¥4V?; A (3) B (6) C (6) CCRTLCF ASSUME) SIMILARITY ACTUAL SIMILARITY *— _ h “ fl * _ * * HCS:ta:lO-8-68 HOS/KP THE RUTH CASE October, 1968 This exercise is designed to improve your understanding of Ruth, of values of college students, and of similarities between your values and theirs. During the exercise you will read what Ruth said about herself, take a pretest, get practice, and take the endtest, and finally, find out how much you improved. You should complete the steps in order. FIRST, read the interview. It is Ruth's half-hour interview of herself. She answered a series of written questions covering her education, family, etc. She answered the questions by speaking into the microphone of a tape recorder while alone. A psychologist talked to Ruth after her self-inter- view to find out how she had felt and behaved in a variety of situations. Two of her friends were later interviewed to check on the accuracy of her report. The correct answer to each question is based upon information given by Ruth and her friends. The Interview 1. Educational and Vocational Goals. My educational goal is to receive a B.A. degree in liberal arts in ele- mentary education. I like teaching very much and if I don't get married then I'd like to make this my life's career. And,--sigh--I guess I'm progressing fairly well. 2. Family. I have two brothers, one older, one younger, and my mother and father. My mother and father have always encouraged me to go to school but this' year I haven't had as close a relationship to my mother and father as I have in previous years. My older brother is married and has one little boy and my younger brother is still in high school. I have a very fond, good relationship with my younger brother at home. And I've always been very very close to my older brother. I don't feel as close to him now as I did before since he is married, and I guess I desire a family of my own probably I'm a little bit jealous. 3. Interpersonal Relationships I think I get along fairly well with most people. I enjoy being around them. Nothing seems to shock me very much although I don't approve of-of people who talk vulgarly or--pause--or tell dirty stories and things like that although I don't show this when I'm around them. I've been associating with all kinds of different people in my work in a restaurant. 4. Relationships with Opposite Sex. Long pause--I think that my emotional and physical relationships with the oppoSite sex are probably just as normal as most people's (laugh) although I have been told that I'm very prudish in my--pause--uh, sexual relationships with others, but I guess I have very high standards, moral standards, (over) religious standards that I can't quite do away with even though I am older then -.-than most girls here at the college probably. 5. Financial Situation. Well,--sigh--right now (pause) I'm totally independent. I have no other income except what I make myself. I have good credit rating at my bank at home so I'm not too worried about that although I always have to plan care- fully where my money's coming from and so on. 6. Religious Views. I'm basically a pretty religious person although I don't attend church every Sunday; IiulProtestant and I'm taking a course in Catholicism. I'm going with a Catholic fellow and have been for the past four years and have thought about changing to the Catholic religion. I think most religions are alike--God plays an important part in my life. My folks have always taught me to be honest and truthful. 7. Abilities and Skills. Well, I have abilities along several different lines. I've had many many different jobs and I've always done well on these jobs. I'm somewhat of a perfectionist. I have experience to be a secretary and I've been a waitress and I've taught school. I like a job where I have some responsi- bility and--am free to make up my own mind about things and I've never had a problem about getting a job. 8. Personal Weaknesses. Pause--sighe-long pause-~I have a quick temper sometimes but I get over it easily. I have a tendency to blame other people sometimes for things that aren't their fault. And I am jealous of other people sometimes which I think is an undesirable trait. I'm a perfectionist which is annoying to other people sometimes. Sometimes I get angry with people and I won't speak to them. I'm just very quiet until I've thought it all over and decided it's alright. I like to be alone when I'm angry. I cry quite often. It's my outlet for my anger. 9. Personal Strengths. I always tell the truth. Uhm,--pause--I'm clean very clean person in mind and physically and I uh, I like people. I have ability to talk with most people and I'm-I'm.very dependable. If I'm.given a job to do I work ahead until it's finished no matter how long it takes me or to what extent it works a hardship to me. 10. Other Important Agpects of Life. I had a very happy early childhood. We were a very close family, did things together, we played lots of games together and we never had much money but we always had lots of fun and we always had a very good Christmas or special occasions. v PRETEST AND END TEST In each of the situations below, one of the two alternatives was "true" for Ruth and the other one was "false”. Circle in the pretest column the alternative that you think was correct. ' ‘ PRE- TEST END TEST A. Ruth characterizes herself as: 19(1; A person who likes to be "on the run" with activities. ,( 2. A person without much real ambition. B. Ruth characterizes her years in Junior High School as the ”roughest times". One of the primary reasons for this , she feels is: W (1; She was less mature physically than the other girls. 0 2. She was more mature. C. Ruth, along with a few other teachers, decided not to join the National Educational Association because it was expensive. Many of the older teachers were very bitter about this since they had always had 100% membership. When pressure was exerted on her, Ruth: ‘( l. Unhappily joined NEA but only after a long verbal battle with the older teachers. 3 C2. Still refused to join NEA but felt guilty about it. D. Ruth reports she: ‘q l. Feels she sometimes talks too much. p (2} Would rather listen than talk. E. The first thing Ruth mentioned that she wanted in a husband was: & 1. One that loved children. a (21 One that could provide for her. GAIN (Number correct on post-test minus number correct on pretest). (over) EXERCISE A Our evaluations of the likelihood of a person engaging in desirable behavior heavily influences the accuracy of our judgments of him. The a accuracy of our stereopypes about‘what others will consiler desirable and the accuracy of our empathy also influence our evaluations and their accuracy. The purpose of this exercise is to analyze these influences on our judgments and to show how they interact with each other. For each of the five statements below answer questions (1), (2), and (3). (1) Do you think the first of the alternatives would have been less or more desirable for Ruth? Circle "L" for less and "M” for more. (2) Agroup of MSU undergraduates checked which alternative they thought was more desirable. Would you assume that your judgment ‘would agree with the judgment of the majority of this group? Circle ”Y" for yes and "N" for no in column-(2). (3) What do you think Ruth actually felt, said, or did? Put a circle in the space before your answer in column (3). YOUR ASSM STUD ACT ACT JUDM SIM? JUDG ‘ SIM? UAL (1) (2) (3) . F. Ruth lived on a farm for some time after she was born. According to a friend, the thing she hated about it was: L M. Y N Y N ____ Lila There was always too much work to do. 19 (2‘. She always had dirty feet. G. Ruth thilks it would be fun to: L M Y N r N pg}. Travel around the U.S. working just enough to pay expenses. (4 2. Go to Africa to work temporarily. H. When Ruth encounters discipline problem in school her initial reaction is to: L M Y N Y N Cilia Blame herself. H 2. Blame no one but try to deny the existence of the problem. I. Ruth feels she was in most of the high school organizations because of her artistic talents. She sees this as: L M Y N Y N b d’. A compliment. £1 2. Something which gave her an "in". J. Ruth feels she chose second grade to teach because: L M Y N Y N W l. The children accept more readily what the teacher has to say. ____ £12k When Ruth was in grade school she enjoyed the second grade most. TOTAL TOTAL Write "L" or "M" in the column headed ”stud judg" when the instructor ' reads the desirability judgments of the typical student. Write "x" in column (5) as he reads the answers for the ”actual" behavior of Ruth. Then count the following scores to analyze possible reasons for your error. EVALUATION ACCURACY. Write the number correct in (5). If you had errors, they may be due to the fact that you consistently judged that Ruth behaved in a less or a more desirable way than she did. Tb find out, count the number of times that your answer in column (5) corresponded to what you thought was the more desirable answer and the number of times to the less desirable (l). EMPATHIC ACCURACY. Compare your judgment in column (1) with the "stud judg" column. If your judgments agree, circle "Y" in the actual similarity column; "N” if your judgments disagreed. Then count the number of "Y"'s in (2) that agreed with Y's in the actual similiarity column. This is your empathic accuracy score, i.e., the correctness of your assumptions of similiarity. If it was low, it may be that you consistently assumed too much or too little empathy. Add the number of "Y”'s in column (2) and in the "stud judg"’oolumn and compare. STEREOTYPE ACCURACY. This is the accuracy of knowledge of the typical values of undergraduates. Count the ngmber of times that your "Y” or "N" agreed with the Y or N in the actual similiarity column. (over) . EXERCISE B This exercise follows exactly the same pattern. If you are in doubt about how to proceed, refer back to the steps in Exercise A. Try to apply what you learned about your judgments of Ruth. Remember, however, that even though she is the same person these are different situations. YOUR ASSM STUD ACT. ACT JUDG SIM? JUDG SIM? UAL (1) (2) (5) K. Ruth is reluctant to talk much about herself as a person. L M Y N Y N «40). She is afraid people will not like her. 672. She grew up in a family that is very reserved. L. Ruth feels her most successful year of teaching was her first. She feels it was because: L M Y N TN d a She was more strict with the children. a 2. She had a warmer relationship with her fellow teachers. M. Ruth sees premarital sexual relations as: L M Y N . Y N £21. All right for others but is unsure for herself. LIQD Not so bad if you have a definite plan of getting married. N. Ruth sometimes feels: L M Y N Y N (4CD. The only way she will get married is to become pregnant. 0 2. That her only real goal in life is to find a satisfactory husband. 0. There was a lot of talk at the restaurant where Ruth works about something she had done. The incident involved: L M Y N Y N w dl Ruth's necking with a Negro at an employee party. (I 2. Ruth's arguing with the manager. TOTAL TOTAL The instructor will read both the judgments of students and Ruth's actual answers as in the first exercise. Record and analyze your scores as in the first exercise. EVALUATION ACCURACY. EMPATHIC ACCURACY. STEREOTYPE ACCURACY. END TEST: When you have completed the analysis of your errors, return to the pretest end test page. Answer the same questions in the end test column again, recording your answers even if they are the same. Do you feel that you understand Ruth better and are more confident about your answers? If so, is the confidence reflected in greater ac- curacy? If.not, Why not? HCS:'ta THE SELF-IMAGE OF THE COLLEGE MAN H. C. Smith October, 1968 To understand a person or a group, we need to be able to predict accurately what they think of themselves. The accuracy of our predictions depends, in part, upon how accurately we can judge how much the person or group's image of themselves is like our image of ourselves. The purpose of this exercise is to increase both your understanding of what the typical college man thinks of himself (stereotype accuracy) and you ability to predict the degree of simialrity between yourself and him (empathic accuracy). Pretest and End Test Each of several hundred college mencmmpared himself to other college men on On each of the traits, the typical student rated himself as LOW (lower than three fourths ofccfllege men), as AVERAGE (in the middle half ofccllege men), or as HIGH (in the upper fourth of college men). the traits listed below. In the pretest column, first circle the "you” that corresponds to your rating of yourself (under the LOW column, if you would rate yourself in the bottom 25%, AVERAGE column, if you would rate yourself in the middle 50%, or in the HIGH column, if you would rate yourself in the upper 25%). Then circle the 'Snfinasyou think the typical student actually rated himseif. When the exercises are completed on the revers ent again. PRETEST Low Average High You You You (Stud‘ Stud Stud You You You (Stud Stud Stud You You You (SEW Stud Stud You You You Stud (ngd? Stud You You You Stud ”Sty Stud You You You Stud <fifl§§9 Stud You You You Stud Stud 6193 You You You Stud Stud Stud) You You You Stud Stud Qua) SCORES Pretest End test END TEST L93 Average You You Stud Stud You You Stud Stud You You Stud Stud You You Stud Stud You You Stud Stud You You Stud Stud You You Stud Stud You You Stud Stud You You Stud Stud Gain High You Stud You Stud You Stud You Stud You Stud You Stud You Stud You Stud You Stud ASSUMED SIMILARITY. judged that you and the typical student would have the same rating. ACTUAL SIMILARITY. STEREOTYPE ACCURACY. answers. 1. e side, you will rate yourself and the typical stu- TRAITS Timid Unpredictable Unrealistic Egotistical Talkative Aggressive Cooperative Friendly Cooperative The number of times that you When the instructor reads the correct answers, put an ”X". Count the number of times that yourcircle for yourself is in the same column as the "X". EMPATHIC ACCURACY. assumed and actual similarity. is "ON. The difference between A perfect score The number of correct (over) PRACTICE EXERCISES Exercise A Complete the following five ratings exactly as you did in the pretest as soon as you have finished it. The better you understand why you made the rating you did for college men and why you were wrong, if you were, the more accurate your image of the college man is likely to be. As an aid in under- standing the "why", jot down the reasons-for your answers before getting feedback. If your answer is wrong, jot down the explanation. that you think accounts for the rating that the typical student actually made. Also, calculate your assumed and the actual similarity scores to determine whether you are assuming too much or too little similarity. EXPLANATION OF ANSWERS LOW AVERAGE HIGH TRAIT Beforerfeedback After Feeback You You You 10. Rebellious -- gggd) Stud Stud You You You ll. Impractical _Sgud Stud Stud u You You 12. Irresponsible :Stud» Stud Stud (You , You You 13. Easily upset .StuHE Stud Stud gl‘0u You You 14. Socially poised QSEUd‘ Stud Stud ASSUMED SIMILARITY ACTUAL SIMILARITY DIFFERENCE STEREOTYPE ACCURACY Exercise B LOW AVERAGE HIGH You You You 15. Stubborn Stud (Stud‘ Stud You You You 16. Shy Stud ”StUd Stud You cu You 17. Affectionate Stud (Stud‘ Stud You You You 18. Serious Stud Ségdl Stud You You You 19. Imaginative Stud Stud Stud ASSUMED SIMILARITY ACTUAL SIMILARITY DIFFERENCE STEREOTYPE ACCURACY Exercise C LOW - .AVERAGE HIGH You You You 20. Adaptable Stud Stud (Stud . You You You 21. Wide range of Stud Stud fitudW interests You You You” 22. 'Liberal‘ Stud Stud (Stud You You You 23. Adventurous Stud Stud Stud . ‘ ' You 'You \Ybu_ 24. Trustful Stud ' Stud (Stud ’ ‘ ASSUMED SIMILARITY SEHSEFSEEMRHY ----STEREOTYPE ACCURACY H. C. Smith October, 1968 THE CASE OF THE YOUNG PSYCHOLOGIST, MORGAN JOHNSON The accuracy of our understanding of a man depends upon our understanding of the typical man (stereotype accuracy). It also, however, depends upon our understanding of the differences between groups of men (differential stereotype accuracy). In addition, of course, it depends upon skill in applying our knowledge to a particular man. This exercise is designed to improve your understanding of the interests of the typical man and the differences between his interests and the typical psychologist's, as well as your ability to apply this understanding in judging the interests of Morgan Johnson. ' Pretest and End Test The Interests of the Typical Man. The replies of several thousand business and professional men to the hundreds of items on the Strong Vocational Interest Blank were analyzed. To some of the interests, the majority of men said they liked the interest and to others the majority said they did not like the interest. In the pretest column (on the back page) headed ”most men” circle "Yes" if you think most men said they would like the interest and "No" if you think most men said they would 223 like the interest. The Different Interests of Psychologists. The replies of several hundred male psychologists were compared with the replies of men-in-general. To many of the interests, the replies of the psychologists were the same as those of the typical man. To some of the interests, however, more psychologists said they would like it. In the pretest column headed "more psychologists" circle "Yes” if you think more psychologists than men-in-general said they would like it and ”No" if you think fewer would. The Interests of Morgan Johnson. Morgan, a 23 year old graduate student in psychology, also completed the Strong Test. After reading the following sketch, answer "Yes" in the Morgan column if you think he said he would like the interest and ”No” if you think he said he would not like the interest. Morgan‘s parents died when he was four, and he and his younger brother were raised by permissive grandparents in Brooklyn. Of his childhood, Morgan said: "As I grew up, I always had the feeling that I was inferior to everybody else because I had no parents. In grade school, I was very loud and bOisterous and made persistent attempts to dominate my peers and to excel in everything I did.‘l Today he places stress on being a ”well-rounded scholar.” About his values, he now says: "I do not believe there are any determining forces in the universe that make us what we are; everybody rules his own destiny. I can think of nothing more important than being a good friend or having good friends, but I don’t think it is possible to have more than a few really close ones. I place little value on material things: cars, clothes, etc." - 2 - PRETEST END TEST Most More Morgan? Most More Morgan? Men? Psych? Men? Psych? ' eslm: GEEQNO {ES/No Yes No Yes No Yes No 1. Meeting new situations ’ejJNo Yes(:§)'YesGE: 'Yes No Yes No Yes No 2. Thirty peOple eSJNo es No Yes Jo; Yes No Yes No Yes No 3. Algebra esJNo eS No Yes ‘ Yes No Yes No Yes No A. Economics eS,No Yes(N5‘ Yes No Yes No Yes No 5. Saving money Yes(N3; Yes(N§§ Yes No Yes No Yes No 6. Drilling in a company Yes 'I'g§§EPk> Yes No Yes No Yes No 7. Politician Yes ‘ e ’No Yes No Yes No Yes No 8. Poet Yes . Yes Nd? Yes No Yes No Yes No 9. Auto Racer Yes io' Yes Io Yes No Yes No Yes No 10. Auctioneer Accuracy Score Pretest End Test Gain Men-in-general Psychologists Morgan Johnson EXERCISE A This exercise concerns the differences between the typical man and psychologist. Circle "Yes" if you think more psychologists and "No" if you think fewer psychologists than men-in- general said they would like the interest. es‘No ll. Carelessly dressed peeple es No 12. Absent-minded people es No 13. Orchestra conductor 1A. People who make fortunes in business 15. Regular hours of work 16. Golf EXERCISE B This exercise concerns the answers of Morgan. Circle "Yes" if you think he said he would like and "No" if you think he would not like the interest. "Psy" in ( ) indicates that more psychologists than men-in-general liked the interest. "Non” in ( ) indicates that more non-psychologists liked the interest. The figure is the percent of men-in-general who said they would like the interest. 17. Ph losophy (57% Psy.) 18. Literature (57% Psy.) _ 19. Sick people (20% Psy.) 'YesCEéZ 20. Auto repairman (19% Non.) YesCNgi 21. Regular hours of work (58% Non.) Yes<fi§> 22. Progressive people (85% Non.) fi\‘ Yes 0) 23. Quick-tempered peOple (7% Pay.) Yes Jo, 2h. Pet monkeys (8% Psy.) Yes& 25. Physics (58% Psy.) @es No 26. Driving an Automobile (77% Non.) No 27. Sporting pages (50% Non.) es'No 28. Fortune tellers (5% Non.) HICH G B A JIM/I?“ (Milkilfliiifl!