w'wfi V." 4 ‘- 0 D - O. ..-.,ooooq-1-!‘.\tk1?-. ;§‘. ~-$1‘« .a, COMMUNICATION PATTERNS OF MOTHERS IN MOTHER-(HELD ENTERAC'E’EQN WETHIN THE HOME That: for “1.0 Degree af M. A. MECBIGAN STATE UNWERSETY Angeia Remsberg Smith 1968 a li' um; Wily/13!!!!” E”! Q ”Sn in W6 n; my a L = “12513:: :3; TH ESIS UQiVCISiL-y ABSTRACT COMMUNICATION PATTERNS OF MOTHERS IN MOTHER-CHILD INTERACTION WITHIN THE HOME by Angela Remsberg Smith This study attempted to identify verbal communication pat- terns of mothers during mother-child interaction within the home and to examine the relationship of the patterns to situ- ational factors. Communication is one of the central elements in home management frameworks. For example, the functions of planning, controlling and evaluating cannot be carried out among several family members without communication. In the management process, communication serves to inform family members of the patterns of behavior expected of each. In this way, the interplay of role responsibility and communication net— works contributes to the development of family members. In order to better understand this interplay, the study of the relationship between verbal family communication and the characteristics of the home environment is needed. Taped conversations were collected of nine mothers with their preschool children while each dyad engaged in four dif- ferent shared activities within their homes. The tapes were later transcribed and eight five-minute samples of the mothers' communications to their children were categorized using the Angela Remsberg Smith coding system described in Magrabi gt _l. ("Framework for Studying Family Patterns," J. of H.E., Vol. 59, Nov. 1967, 714-719). Biographical data and descriptions of situational factors in the family were obtained by interviews. A resource was defined in this study as communication content a sender used to influence another person or persons-- fact, preference, direction, and motivation. Each resource was further coded as to the mode of communicative exchange. The mode indicated the manner in which the resource was transmitted to the receiver--offering, seeking, accepting and not accepting. The sums of the frequencies for each mother in each of these sixteen communication categories were obtained and these were correlated with selected situational variables using the method of stepwise addition of variables to form a least squares equation. The mean frequency of the message units delivered by the mothers to their children, when defined as to modes were: offering--69 percent; seeking--20 percent; accepting--8 percent; and not accepting--5 percent. When message units were defined as to resources, the frequencies were: fact--52 percent; preference--7 percent; direction or command--58 percent; and motivation or encouragement-~5 percent. Situational factors were found to be related to specific categories of the mothers' communication. Those factors re- lated to all the modes were: (1) number of community moves Angela Remsberg Smith since marriage, (2) amount of time spent by mothers in house- hold duties including such activities as meal preparation, dishwashing, bedmaking, and tidying up; and (3) amount of help with household duties. Those factors related to all the resources included the same three factors that were related to the modes plus the factor of family size and composition. The less frequently related variables for the modes and for the resources were: (1) mother's educational level, (2) father's daily hours from home because of employment, (5) family income, and (4) frequency of eating dinner together. Combination of the mode—resource designations resulted in the situational variables clustered both by the frequency and the similarity of occurrence into two groups. Those factors most frequently related to the communication categories were: (1) family size and composition, (2) number of community moves since marriage, (5) amount of time spent by mothers in household duties, (4) amount of help with household duties. Less frequently related factors with the categories were: (1) mother's educational level, (2) father's daily hours from home because of employment, and (5) frequency of eating dinner together. COMMUNICATION PATTERNS OF MOTHERS IN MOTHER-CHILD INTERACTION WITHIN THE HOME BY Angela Remsberg Smith A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Home Management and Child Development 1968 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The author wishes to express her sincere appreciation and gratitude to Dr. Frances M. Magrabi, Associate Professor of the Home Management Department, for her guidance and encourage- ment during the course of this study and the preparation of this manuscript. Her time and sincere concern have been generously given. Appreciation is also expressed to Mrs. Marjorie Heifner, Research Assistant of the Home Management Department, and to Miss Jo Lynn Cunningham, Graduate Assistant, for their helpful suggestions and for their assistance in determining intercoder reliability. Gratitude is expressed to Dr. Beatrice Paolucci and Dr. Gertrude Nygren for their review of the manuscript and their suggestions. Gratitude is also expressed to my husband, Dudley, for his many helpful suggestions and his empathic understanding throughout the period of this study. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS. . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF TABLES. . . . . . . . . . . . Chapter I. II. III. IV. INTRODUCTION. . . . . . . . . Purpose of the Study. . . . Assumptions . . . . . . . . Definition of Terms . . . . REVIEW OF LITERATURE. . . . . Communication as a Process. Styles of Communication . . Coding System Deve10pment . Conclusions From the Review MATERIALS AND METHODS . . . . Method. . . . . . . . . . . Detailed Descriptions of Measures Employed Description of Subjects . . Collection of Data. . . . . FINDINGS. . . . . . . . . . . of Literature Distribution of Message Units Test of Independence of Categories. Test of Relationship Between Communication Categories and Situational Variables. The Relationship of Situational Factors to the Modes . . . . . . . . The Relationship of Situational Variables to the Resources. . . . . The Relationship of the Situational Vari- ables of the Mode-Resource Categories iii Page ii \IUI U'l tP-IP‘CN l-‘ 10 16 16 17 22 26 52 52 54 55 4O 44 46 TABLE OF CONTENTS — Continued Chapter Page V. CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS. . . . . . . . . 52 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52 Limitations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55 Implications for Future Study . . . . . . . 57 LITERATURE CITED. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 APPENDIX. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65 iv Table 1. 2. 5. 10. 11. 12. LIST OF TABLES Age of Children. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Annual Family Income . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Distribution of Message Units of Mother's Communication to Children During Home Activities as Related to the Categories of Resources and Modes. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Relationships of Situational Variables with Multiple Regression Correlational Coefficients (R) to the Mode-Resource Categories. . . . . . . The Relationship of Situational Variables With the Modes Communicated from Mothers to Their Children During Shared Home Activities . . . . . The Relationship of Situational Variables and Their Component Parts with the Modes Communi- cated from Mothers to Their Children During Shared Home Activities . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Relationship of Situational Variables with the Resources Communicated from Mothers to Their Children During Shared Home Activities . . . . . The Relationship of Situational Variables and Their Component Parts with the Resources Communi— cated from Mothers to Their Children During Shared Home Activities . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Sum and Mean Distribution of Message Units in Each Communication Category . . . . . . . . . Bibliographical and Descriptive Information About Families of the Sample . . . . . . . . . . The Number of Message Units for Each Mother in Each Communication Category. . . . . . . . . . . The Relationship Between Each Mode—Resource Cate- gory with Each Situational Variable Based on Simple Correlations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Page 23 25 55 56 41 45 45 47 49 67 7O 71 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Communication is an integral part of any management process and is recognized as one of the central elements in home manage- ment frameworks. Bettinghaus (1964, p. 50) at a 1964 Home Management Conference stated, I am convinced that communication is vital to the Home management task, and also convinced that we can develop an adequate theory of communication to the home management situation. Liston, at the same meeting, categorized communication as one of the interaction processes involved in management by the family unit. \She conceptualized it as a process involving transmission of a message from one individual or group of individuals to another individual or group of individuals. Also it is defined as the process of making common or of exchanging subjective states such as ideas, sentiments, and beliefs usually by means of language, though also by visual representation, imitation, and suggestion (Liston, 1964, p. 68). Miller (1966, p. 92), on the other hand, has narrowed the con- cept for research purposes to the study of behavioral situations in which the source transmits a message to the receiver with the conscious attempt to affect the latter's behavior. This definition refers predominately to verbal communication rather than to both verbal and non-verbal behavioral. In developing a conceptual framework which defines the place of communication within the home management process, scientific efforts need to be focused on empirical research of communication patterns which will describe: 1. the people involved in the home management process; 2. the kinds of people involved as receivers or sources of communication; 5. the content and frequency of messages; and 4. the relationship of communication as a management process to the other basic components of family management--va1ues, goals, resources, family, manager, environment, and problem situation. Research of this type needs to be conducted within the home to preserve as much as possible the natural setting of the situ- ation. Up to this time, though, a major problem in this area of research has been the development of coding systems applic- able to family interaction as it relates to management. Human resources are recognized as a basic component of family management. This concept refers to the energy, inter- ests, abilities, skills, knowledge, and attributes of indi- viduals. In the family the mother plays an important part in her child's development and resource use through her manipula- tive role during mother-child interaction within the home environment. The following study attempted to identify communication patterns of mothers during mother-child interaction within the home and to examine their relationship with situational factors which were selected on the basis of studies in the review of the literature and the availability of data. The verbal communication of each mother to her preschool child or children while participating in four shared activities was tape- recorded within the home during Fall, 1966, and Winter, 1967. These conversations were later coded on two bases--the salient resource and the mode of transmission. The coding system utilized was eSpecially adapted to evalu- ate verbal communication. With the use of this instrument, the frequency with which a specific resource was utilized with a specific mode was found for each mother. This was considered to be her communication pattern. The frequency of messages in each mode-resource category was then correlated with specific situational variables. These situational variables were com— posed of bibliographical data and descriptions of factors in the home environment. Purpose of this Study The two main purposes of categorizing the mothers' com- munication to their children were: 1. to explore ways of coding tape-recorded verbal com— munication of mother to child while they participated in shared activities in the home. 2. to identify if the elements of communication patterns of mothers were related to selected situational variables. Previous studies by Bernstein (1965), Hess and Shipman (1965), Merrill (1946L Olim, Hess and Shipman (1965), and Walters, Conners and Zunich (1964) lead one to believe that situational variables would be related to a valid categoriza- tion of communication. The objective of this study was to examine whether or not selected situational variables were related to specific categories of verbal communication. Because of its exploratory nature, no specific hypotheses were postulated. Assumptions Three assumptions underlying this study were: 1. communication is a symbolic system which expresses the roles and statuses of family members. 2. verbal communication can be differentiated into cate- gories for analysis. 5. the communication pattern of a mother to her child is not influenced substantially by the nature of the activities in which they jointly participate. Definition of Terms The definitions of the terms as Operationalized in this study were: 1. Communication—~an interaction situation in which the source (person speaking) transmits a message to the receiver with the conscious attempt to affect the latter's behavior. 2. Communication pattern-~relative frequency of resource category and mode of communication as coded by the coding system of Magrabi §;_§_, (1967). 5. Resource-~content a sender uses to influence another person or persons—-fact, preference, direction, or motivation. 4. Mode--the manner in which the resource is transmitted to the receiver—-offering, seeking, accepting, or not accepting. 5. Message unit-~each attempt to transmit a single thought or idea (one stimulus) from mother to child. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURE Communication as a Process Communication is one of the processes of management. As a process it is composed of interactions which are events by which one party tangibly influences the overt action or state of mind of the other (Loomis, 1960). The purpose of communication is to elicit a specific response from a particu- lar person or group. For the family communication conveys meanings of social acts. Through this process, information, decisions, and directions are transmitted among actors which affect the ways in which knowledge, opinions and attitudes are formed or modified by interaction. Since every social act must have at least two people, communication represents a symbolic interaction from which the individual acquires in- sight and knowledge of his role (Berlo, 1950). Communication is recognized as one of the central elements in home management frameworks. For example, the functions of planning, controlling, and evaluating cannot be carried out among family members without communication. During the manage- ment process, communication serves to inform family members of the patterns of behavior expected of each. Liston's (1964) conceptual model of home management interlaces communication with the development of individual family members. This development takes place when responsibility is assumed by a family member. It is through role playing that family members learn the cultural patterns associated with their respective roles. Sabin (1954) defines role as a "patterned sequence of learned activities or deeds performed by a person in an inter- action situation" and Linton (1945) defines role as a combi- nation of cultural patterns associated with a particular position or status within the social system. Roles and statuses are important in communication because they identify the participants and convey both verbal and non-verbal messages. These messages serve as instructions from the receiver to the sender concerning how the receiver should be addressed and from the sender to the receiver concerning how the verbal message should be interpreted (Ruesch, 1955). In Liston‘s (1964) model of home management, communica— tion networks within and outside the family serve to transmit information about behavior expected of persons playing each particular role. These unique patterns of role responsibility for each family affect the ways of getting decisions made and carried out when various problem situations occur. Communica- tion serves to inform the members of the overt patterns of thinking, feeling, and acting that are expected of each member and as a result, according to Liston, characteristic patterns for each family are formed and can be identified. Interpersonal tactics are employed by family members as attempts to change existing roles, statuses, or rules, while the manipulation of communication content is used by family members as a control technique. Efforts are made both to sustain existing relation- ships and to maintain and clarify self-identity. Styles of Communication Bernstein (1965) identified two styles of communication found in families--public (or restricted) and formal (or elaborated). He suggested that they have important implica- tions in child development. Each family in society has a de- rivative of one of the styles. The restrictive style of communication consists of short, simple, often unfinished sentences. The intent of these messages is to promote soli- darity or to reduce tension. On the other hand, the elaborate style of communication is individualized; the message is specific to a particular situation, tOpic, or person. Messages of this type are more differentiated and precise than messages of the restrictive style; as a result, they permit a wider and more complex range of thought. Bernstein went on to illustrate the meshing of social interaction and language style by the distinctions he made between two family types—-those oriented toward control by social norms, or by statues, and those oriented toward persons. In status oriented families, role characteristics and expectations serve as guides for behavior. The behavioral norms, which are stressed through the restrictive style of verbal communication, rely upon the status of participants, or a behavioral norm for justification. Verbal exchange is inherent in the structure; it regulates it and is regulated by it. In the person oriented family, the unique characteristics of the family members modify status and role demands which affect interaction. Behavior is justified in terms of preference, personal reactions and sub- jective states. A wide range of linguistic and behavioral alternatives in interpersonal exchange is evident. With Bernstein's concepts of language style and family style as a foundation, Olim, Hess, and Shipman (1966) conducted a study to observe the relationship of language style to family type. They found that families oriented to control by status appeal, or role designations, used predominately the restric- tive style of communication. The children in this situation learned best by rote—learning techniques and tended to be compliant. Olim, Hess and Shipman (1965) continued the above research using samples from different socio—economic populations. The results of their studies showed evidence that the restrictive style of communication was used extensively in lower class families. The elaborate style, on the other hand, was used mostly in middle and upper class families. Within the indi— vidual families, the association between language and social structure emerged in terms of principles that governed decision-making activities. These activities, in turn, helped to regulate the nature and amount of social exchange. Walters, Conners and Zunich (1964) had earlier investi- gated the relationship of mother-child interaction to the lower socio-economic class in the study, "Interaction of Mothers and Children from Lower Class Families"; they found that mothers and their children had less interaction than those middle and upper class subjects who participated in an earlier study by Merrill (1946). The middle and upper class subjects used direct- ing behavior four times as frequently, helping behavior nine times as frequently, structuring behavior five times as fre- quently and teaching behavior more than three times as frequently as did the lower class mothers (Walters §£_al,, 1964). Based on the earlier research, Hess and Shipman (1965) postulated that the two types of language styles affected the children's patterns of cognitive development differently. Since these patterns of cognitive activity are socialized in early experience in the home, especially with the mother, they be- come the basis upon which further cognitive development proceeds. The researchers, in addition, considered the mother a teacher and viewed interaction between the mother and child as a teaching- learning situation (Hess and Shipman, 1967). If, then, the growth of c0gnitive functioning is mediated by experience, the strategies that a mother employs are likely to have consequences for the ability of the child to graSp or learn a lesson in any Specific teaching. 10 In a study now in progress, Georianne Baker (1965) has integrated Hess and Shipman's cognitive theory within the process of home management. Her objective is to delineate empirically the way resources present within the environment of preschool children are organized and utilized for educability, that is, the goal of preparing children to participate in the future school situation. One part of the study is to analyze the mother's communication in an attempt to acquire the mother's verbal reactions to a set of drawings or photographs which represent selected resource utilization actions. The reactions are to be coded to form a subscore of the Home Resource Pattern Scale. Coding System Development Developing coding systems to analyze communication between family members which are meaningful to home management pro- cesses and which can be applied objectively to family integra— tion situations has been one of the real problems in research. In 1964, Bettinghaus observed that in every book and article he had read in home management, communication was given the same level of importance as any other element of the management process; yet, none of the sources made use of communication theory in any fashion which helped to understand the process of home management. He further stated that in the home manage- ment area attempts made to fit management models to the home management situation had failed. In the area of the social 11 sciences, similar problems of model adaptability have occurred, especially in researchers' attempts to analyze communication. Heuristic communication models have been developed by Berlo (1961), Morris (1964), Reusch (1955), and Miller (1966). In addition, several researchers have made attempts to Specifically classify communicative acts. These classifications may be applicable to home management models. Bales (1950) has developed an interaction process analysis system. This framework divides communication into twelve cate- gories. Each single act spoken or gestured is placed in the most appropriate division. An index of supportiveness is then computed for each speaker. Kenkel and Hoffman (1956), using Bales' framework, focused on the communication between husband and wife in a hypothetical decision situation. Strodtbeck (1954), also using Bales' model, studied the family as a three person group. He described the communication of mother, father, and son during discussions of hypothetical situations. This appears to be the only completed study found during the review of literature which had been Operationalized in the home; all the rest were conducted in the laboratory. Longabaugh (1965) divided each communicative message into (1) resources salient in the interaction and (2) modes of transmission which were defined as: 12 Salient resources Primary modalities Fact Offering Information Seeking Support Depriving, i.e., taking away, withholding Accepting Not accepting, i.e., ignoral, rejection The resources were defined as information, support, and control. The modes of transmission were defined as offering, seeking, depriving, accepting, and not accepting. The direct measure- ment of rewards and costs occurring could then be grossly counted. Direct measurement of quantity brings the variables to the Operational level where they can be used for experimen- tation. Magrabi gtwal. (1967) adapted Longabaugh's framework by modifying the categories of salient resources to make them more appropriate to the study of family activity patterns within the home. The salient resources in this revised coding system in- cluded fact, preference, direction or command, and motivation or encouragement. This framework was designed to code only verbal behavior as collected in the home by use of a tape re- corder. This approach to collecting family communication dif— fered from the other studies found in the search through the literature in two ways: 1. It referred to activities observed within the home. 2. The definition of communication included only verbal communication; it did not include non-verbal. Hess and Shipman (1967) developed a coding system in which they identified relationships between socio-economic 15 characteristics and the cognitive elements in maternal behavior of four socio-economic groups of mothers and children during laboratory observations. Results indicate that mothers dif- fered little in the affective elements of their interaction with their children. The gross differences appeared in the frequen- cies of patterning stimuli by the mother which organized informa- tion for the child and in the cognitive environments which they represented. An analysis of maternal techniques of conversing with their children utilized a laboratory procedure in which mothers were asked to teach their children three Specified tasks. Two tape recorders were used to record the mother's verbal behavior during the interaction with her child instead of coding during the observation. One tape recorded the sound while a hidden observer recorded behavioral observations on another tape at the same time. The transcripts containing the verbalizations of the subjects and the observer's descriptions of the mothers' non-verbal behavior were divided into message units and were coded by a category system. The categories were informing, motivating, orienting, seeking physical feedback, seeking verbal feedback, positive reinforcement and negative reinforcement. Conclusions From the Review of Literature Since the mother and child may react differently in a laboratory setting than in the home atmosphere, it is important to observe interaction in the home, but there are several 14 problems related to coding mother-child interaction outside the laboratory. The amount of control the researcher has over the environment and the subjects is greatly reduced. This affects the methods of coding possible and the type of mother- child interaction that will be observed; yet deSpite these weaknesses, the home setting provides a more natural environ- ment for the interaction. Since the observer cannot remain hidden, he must either code behavior during the observation, in the presence of the participants, or record it for later transcription. The Specific coding categories used in a research design will depend on the researcher's definition of communication and one's objectives of the study. Hess and Shipman (1965) used a definition of communication which included both verbal and non-verbal behavior. Miller (1966), on the other hand, defined communication as the study of behavioral situations in which a speaker through his message to a receiver consciously tries to affect his behavior. This definition refers to pre— dominately verbal communication; non-verbal communication is on the perifery of the model. Using this definition it would be easier to identify and observe a subset of phenomena unique to communication and measureable in objective terms than when the definition would include both verbal and non-verbal stimuli. The Miller criterion also makes tape recording sessions of family communication feasible and once this verbal communication is recorded, it can be stored until coding is possible. 15 A well-defined coding system would enable the researcher to analyze the data objectively and the use of tapes would enable the researcher to review the communication several times, if necessary, in order to code the verbal interaction and to check intercoder reliability. A study in which the conversation of mother to child dur- ing the participation in shared home activities would be tape- recorded, coded, and analyzed would introduce a new approach in gathering data about family interaction within the home based on objective, measureable criteria. It would also offer a new criterion for evaluating conversational patterns of mothers to be correlated with situational variables. This analysis would be a useful addition of information for obtain- ing a greater understanding of the management process in the home. Hess and Shipman (1967) found in their study of socio- economic levels that the association between language and social structure emerged in terms of principles that govern decision- making processes, which in turn help to regulate the nature and amount of social exchange. Other researchers had previously found similar relationships between communication content of mothers and their socio-economic levels. Based on these studies, one concludes that communication and family situational variables are not independent. Therefore, they need to be studied as they occur in the home environment. CHAPTER III MATERIALS AND METHODS Method For this study mother—child activities within homes were used as behavioral situations. An interview method was uti- lized to obtain biographical data and descriptions of situational factors surrounding the home environments. Conversations be- tween mothers and children during four shared home activities were obtained by taped recordings during home visits and were later transcribed. The data for this study were selected from the questionnaires and taped conversations collected in a pilot study.1 Random samples of mothers' conversations directed to their children during shared home activities were coded using the system developed by Magrabi §£_al, (1967). For each mother the number of message units in each conversation category in the coding system was correlated with selected situational variables. The Situational variables examined in this study were chosen on the basis of characteristics of social structure. 1"Use of Space, Material and Communication Among Family Members Performing Family Activities." Michigan Agricultural Experiment Station Project No. 786. 16 17 Those factors which emerged during the review of studies by Bernstein (1965), Hess and Shipman (1965), Merrill (1946), Olim 35 El. (1965), and Walters §£__l, (1964) were: family size and composition, educational level of mother, and family income. Other variables included were factors thought by the researcher to control the pattern of family life and inter- action among members. They included the father's daily hours from home because of employment, time allocated by mother for household duties, amount of help mother received with house— hold duties, frequency of eating dinner together, and the number of community moves since marriage. Detailed Descriptions of Measures Employed Interview schedule and method. A three part interview schedule was used to obtain background information about the families. Information obtained from the questionnaire for this study pertained to: 1. family Size and composition, 2. educational status of wife, 5. husband's daily hours from home because of employment, 4. family income, 5. number of community moves since marriage, 6. time allocated for household duties by the mother, 7. amount of help given the mother with household duties, 8. frequency of eating dinner together. The questionnaire was precoded to facilitate punching the data onto IBM cards. Coding_categories and definitions. Magrabi §£._l, (1967) revised Longabaugh's model by modifying the categories in salient resources and in modalities (modes). The framework was 18 designed to code only verbal behavior collected within the home by use of a tape recorder. The revised resources and modes were: Salient resources Primary modalities (modes) Fact Offering Preference Seeking Direction or command Accepting Motivation or encouragement Not accepting This framework described verbal communication as an ex- change process of resources. A resource was defined as com- munication content a sender used to influence another person or persons; the possible resources were: fact, preference, direc- tion, or motivation. The definitions of the resources were: 1. Fact--that which is believed by the speaker to be of actual occurrence and existence, reality as an event, statement of a thing done or existing, indisputable statement—-not an evaluation or preference. Preference--that which conveys the act of preferring (a value judgment) or opportunity of choosing--either explicitly offered or implied. Usually found within messages that ask questions or offer a choice between two or more things. Command or direction--that which implies action--to direct authoritatively, order, require, guide, manage or bring into common action. Stimuli which contain "shall we" if a definite command or guidance is implied. Questions seeking from another ”what to do next" when one is at the end of an activity. Statements which infer self direction (e.g., I will go now.). Instances when the source calls a person by name in order to gain their attention; this is at the beginning of a message unit. Motivation or encouragement-—that which is intended to inspire, hearten, and/or cheer up an individual or in- cite satisfying relationships. Stimuli are mostly spontaneous, exuberant statements, not questions or mundane or expected reinforcements, such as "O.K.," "all right," "yea," and "unhuh." Most message units are of the offering mode. 19 In the communication process one person will instigate a message when he makes use of a particular resource in an at- tempt to influence another. The other party will then react by responding to the resource already made salient. As a result of the interaction, rewards and costs are incurred for the actors. Rewards are the pleasures, satisfactions and gratifications the person enjoys; or the provision of means whereby a drive is reduced or a need is fulfilled. Costs, on the other hand, are the factors that operate to inhibit or deter the performance of a sequence of behavior. The reward less the cost is the psychic profit. This is the necessary consequence for the parties in order for an exchange to continue. For each resource there were four modes of communicative exchange between persons. The mode was used to convey the resource as it was transmitted to the receiver. The possible modes were offering, seeking, accepting, or not accepting. They are defined in this study as follows: 1. Offering~~act of one offering. 2. Seeking-~act of inquiring or searching. Stimuli of this mode are usually phased in the form of questions. 5. Accepting--act of receiving (with a consenting mind) a thing or idea offered; to admit or agree to; to receive with favor; to approve; to confirm. 4. Not accepting--act of rejecting or declining; to refuse; disagree to; receive with disfavor; disapprove. Modes play an important role in transmitting the resource from a sender to a receiver and vice versa. Possible ways of communication transmission are: 20 If a person (sender) offers a resource to a receiver, the latter can either accept the resource or he can "not accept" the resource, in which case he either will reject or ignore it. If the sender seeks a resource from a receiver, the latter can either respond by offering the resource or he can ignore the request. Accepting--act of receiving (with a consenting mind) a thing or idea offered; to admit or agree to; to receive with favor; to approve; to confirm. Not accepting--act of rejecting or declining; to refuse; disagree to; receive with disfavor; disapprove. Modes play an important role in transmitting the resource from a sender to a receiver and vice versa. Possible ways of communication transmission are: 1. If a person (sender) offers a resource to a receiver, the latter can either accept the resource or he can "not accept" the resource, in which case he either will reject or ignore it. If the sender seeks a resource from a receiver, the latter can either respond by offering the resource or he can ignore the request. If the receiver accepts an offered resource by the sender, he can continue the communicative interchange by either offering a resource to or seeking a resource from the original sender. On the other hand, if a receiver does not accept a resource offered by the sender, he can initiate further communication by either offering a resource to or seek- ing a resource from the sender. 21 The individual message units were defined as each attempt to transmit a single thought or idea (one stimulus) from one ‘person to another. It may have consisted of one word, "No"; several words, "In a Minute"; or a complete sentence, "I don't think we will want to do that yet." A message was considered extraneous material and coded as "9" if it was uninterpretable either because it could not be distinguished when transcribing the tapes or could not be coded because of a lack of understanding of the situation. A common occurrence was the reSponse "yes" at the beginning of an activity. Without the knowledge of the previous communication, it was not possible to interpret the salient resource. The following responses were also coded as "9" if the coder was not able to determine to what they referred: "okeydoke," "all right," "there," "w00ps,“ "oh," and "well." If the message was not considered extraneous material, it was coded with respect to the resource and the mode utilized. Examples of message units categorized into each of the sixteen cells, as Shown below, are in the appendix, pp. 64-66. RESOURCES Fact Preference Direction Motivation Offering m . m Seeking Q a 2 Accept ing Not Accepting 22 Intercoder reliability. The reliability of the coding was measured by calculating the percentage of agreement between two persons independently coding two five—minute samples of data immediately preceding the coding of the data reported here. The formula used was: Number of agreements between two coders = x 100 Number of message units per five minute sample Intercoder reliability The testing for intercoder reliability was done by the researcher and one other graduate student in the Department of Home Management who had not used the coding system before. The coding system was explained to the graduate student and the terms were defined and discussed. One two-minute and one five- minute practice sessions were carried out immediately before the reliability coding. The intercoder agreement for the five- minute practice session was 85.6 percent. The intercoder reliability of the first five-minute coding session was 76/85 or 91.5 percent, while for the second five- minute session it was 40/45 or 95.0 percent. The mean inter- coder reliability was 92.2 percent over both sessions. Description of Subjects The study was confined to conversations of nine mothers who had a child enrolled in Michigan State University's Labora- tory Preschool during Fall, 1966, and Winter, 1967. The non- random sample was chosen on the basis of availability of subjects 25 with preschool children. The data collection took place during shared home activities of the mother and the preschool child who was attending Laboratory Preschool. Other family members, in addition, participated in some of the activities. (Refer to Appendix, pp. 67-69, Table 10, for a resume of the sample characteristics.) Family size and composition. The families ranged in size from 4 to 8 members with a mean of 4.6 members. Three families did not have fathers present in the home. In one case the mother and father were divorced; in another they were separated; and in another the mother had remained unmarried. The age of the husbands ranged from 28 up to and including 42 years with a mean of 55.2 years. The age of the wives ranged from 24 years up to and including 42 years with a mean of 51.1 years. Two—thirds of the homemakers were in the 28 to 55 age range. There were 27 children in the sample families. The ages ranged from 7 months up to and including 15 years. A resume is shown in Table 1. There were 16 males and 11 females. Fifteen children were preschoolers. Table 1. Age of Children Age Group, Number of Children 7 to 12 months 2 2 years 2 5 to 5 years 11 6 to 10 years 11 15 years 1 Total 27 24 Educational level. All the mothers had at least a ninth grade education while two had some college; two had bachelor's degrees; and two had advanced degrees. Of the six fathers present in the home, only one had at least a ninth grade education; the remaining five had at least some college. Of these five, two fathers had bachelor's de- grees and two fathers had advanced degrees. Employment status. Only two women were employed. One was employed in the clerical field 4 to 8 hours daily. The other was employed as a service worker one day weekly. One other woman was a graduate student. There was no indication of the amount of time she spent outside the home at school daily. All of the fathers present in the home were employed. The hours daily away from home because of employment ranged from less than 6 hours to more than 12 hours. The mean time away from home for the husbands ranged from 6 to 11 hours. One husband was a carpenter, another was a salesman and the other four were classified as professional. In the pro- fessional group, two of the men held doctorates, one was a lawyer and one was a graduate student. Family income. The family income ranged from $5,000 to $20,000 per year as shown in Table 2. The three fatherless families received $4,000 or less annually while the families with fathers present had incomes of at least $6,000 with 5 of the 6 families receiving $9,000 or more annually. 25 Table 2. Annual Family Income Annual Family Income Number of Families 2,000 to $4,000 5 6,000 1 9,000 . 1 10,000 to $12,000 5 Over $12,000 1 Total 9 Type of dwelling. The residences of the entire sample were similar in that all the families were living in Single family dwellings. It was the one independent variable con- sistent for all observations. Amount of help with household duties from family members. The amount of daily help given the mother with ten household duties varied among the families. The duties selected for the study were: 1. dishwashing 2. meal preparation 5. family laundry 4. bedmaking 5. daily tidying up 6. weekly cleaning 7. chauffeuring family 8. yard work and outside house care 9. caring for family members 10. caring for pets Father's assistance ranged from 5 to 6 duties with a mean of 5.1. Children's help ranged from 1 to 7 duties with a mean 26 of 5.7. The combination of all help given the mother included husbands, children and hired help. The number of duties that family members and hired help assisted the mother in completing or in completing without the mothers' help ranged from 5 to 9; the mean was 5.9 different duties. Frequency of dinner eaten together. Five of the families always ate this meal together, two usually, and two seldom. Collection of Data The Michigan State University Laboratory Preschool was contacted in the fall of 1967 in order to obtain names of families with at least one child in Laboratory Preschool during Fall, 1966. .After Laboratory Preschool's recommendation of thirty families in the Greater Lansing Area, contacts to elicit cooperation and to set times for interviews were made with the mothers of these families until ten mothers were obtained. On the basis of family income, five of the families were considered to be at a middle socio—economic level and five at a lower socio-economic level. Thus, the sample was non-randomly selected, based on the availability of a preschool child and the willing— ness to participate. Data collection from one of the families of the lower socio-economic group was not completed because of a death in the family. Data for the study were obtained through personal inter— views with each homemaker either during the initial visit to her home or on subsequent visits when the activities were 27 tape-recorded. During the interview the following procedures were followed: 1. Biographical information was secured by open-ended questions. 2. Check lists pertaining to the household routine were completed. 5. Precoded questions concerning other situational vari- ables were answered. Collecting the data served a two—fold purpose. First it gave information to further describe the sample. Second, it served to establish general rapport between the observer and the respondent. The interviews and the taped recordings of the mother—child activities within the home were conducted in the morning or afternoon, depending upon the convenience of the homemakers and the schedule of the researcher. No weekend activities were collected. The mothers were free to choose any home activities to do with their children during the observations; suggestions such as cookie baking, meal preparation, and bed making were made by the researchers. Actual activities included during the taped sessions were: cookie making, tree trimming, bed making, meal preparation, dressing, story telling, playing with toys, feeding pets, packing away groceries, and making valentine cards. The length of the home activities varied from 11 minutes to 45 minutes with a mean of 25.1 minutes. For six families, each activity was recorded on a different day. Two families were observed only twice with the collection of data of two 28 separate activities at each observation. For one family, a single observational visit was made, at which time all four activities were recorded in a single morning session. Coding procedure. Before coding the taped-recorded activi- ties within the home, the data were transcribed from tapes to paper by research workers who were associated with the pilot study. A "+" was put in the appropriate column to the left of each message unit to indicate which person was Speaking and a "-" was placed in the appropriate column indicating to whom the conversation was directed. The data were transcribed from the tapes to coding sheets during Spring, 1967 through Fall, 1967. For this study the transcribed conversations were divided into five—minute units for each mother—child activity and eight five-minute samples of conversation were chosen for each mother based on the following criteria: 1. In order for a five minute sample to be eligible, the mother must have transmitted at least ten message units per five—minute period. 2. Two five-minute periods were randomly selected, using a table of random numbers, from all the eligible five— minute samples for each of the four activities. There were a total of eight five-minute samples or 40 minutes of conversation selected for each mother. 5. The eight five-minute samples for each mother were coded using the coding system described earlier. The specific resource used was determined first; then the mode of transmission was selected. 29 Both mothers and children's communication were coded in order to examine the mothers' communication in the context of the interaction situation but only the mothers' conversations were analyzed. Conversations of the mothers to the observer were not coded but were considered irrelevant to the study of home activity patterns. All data were coded within six days after determining intercoder reliability. The sum of the frequencies for each of the sixteen possible mode-resource dyads of all eight samples were found for each mother and were used as variables indicating communication pattern. 7 Analysis of data. The data were analyzed using descriptive and statistical techniques.l Biographical information such as ages of children, education level of mother, family income, and dwelling was used descriptively. Analysis of variance was used to find if any means of the message units were significantly different from one another. Duncan's multiple range test then isolated those means which were significantly different from one another. The LSADD Routine was used to identify those situa— tional variables which werezmost highly related to the conver- sation categories. Analysis of variance of message units was computed con— sidering mother, mode, resource, and all possible two way interactions as sources of variance. Means significantly dif— ferent from one another on the basis of the F test were 1Dr. Charles Cress, Statistican for the College of Agri- culture served as a consultant in suggesting ways of analyzing the data. 50 identified using Duncan's (1955) multiple range test. Differ- ences were concluded to be significant at the five percent level. The family background and situational responses of the interview and the sum of the frequencies of the communication patterns were punched on IBM cards. The LSADD Routine, using a CDC 5600 computer at Michigan State University, was used to develop the stepwise addition of variables to form a least squares equation.1 In the LSADD Routine one dependent variable is Specified for regression with several independent variables. Independent variables may be "forced" into the regression analysis initially if the experimenter desires to do so. Usually this "forcing" technique is limited to those variables known or thought to influence the dependent variables. Additional independent variables may be specified or designated for possible inclusion or consideration in the regression analysis; in which case, an arbitrary maximum significance level is stated. When the LSADD Routine is initiated, all independent variables are scanned and the single variable that is contribut- ing the most to the regression analysis is selected. Then the regression equation is computed regressing the selected inde— pendent variable with the dependent variable. This process 1Computer programs written and developed by Mary E. Rafter and William Ruble of the Michigan State University Computer Laboratory, Agricultural Experiment Station, were utilized in analysis of variance and regression analyses. 51 is repeated again selecting from the remaining variables a second independent variable that contributes the most to the regression. Additional independent variables are considered and included in the order in which they contribute to the over- all regression. When the significance level of the regression about the mean reaches the level originally specified by the researcher, the program is completed for the dependent variable under consideration. However, the researcher must evaluate the regression equation developed by this system as to the validity of the variables included or not included. Additional programs may be necessary to develop a more accurate regres- sion equation once the initial equation has been proposed by the LSADD Routine. Because this was an exploratory study with limited data, the analyses were not carried beyond the point of identifying variables which appeared to be most highly significant. The number of message units in each communication category was considered, in turn, as a dependent variable. For each dependent variable, all the situational variables (independent variables) were listed for consideration and possible inclusion in a regression analysis. Because of this study's exploratory nature, there was no prehand knowledge or basis to indicate which independent variables should have been forced into the regression. Therefore, no situational variables were forced into the equation and all were Specified (listed) as possible factors in the program. The Significance level was specified as P ’<' 0.20. CHAPTER IV FINDINGS The relationship of the situational factors to the com- munication categories will be discussed in the following order: (1) distribution of message units, (2) test for independence of categories, (5) test of relationships hetween communication categories and situational variables, (4) relationships when the message units were categorized as to modes, (5) relation- ships when the message units were categorized as to resources, and (6) relationships when the message units were categorized as to mode-resource interaction. Distribution of Message Units A total of 2,644 message units, or a mean of 295.7 message units for each forty minute sample of communication, were spoken by the nine mothers to their children during shared home activities. The message units were analyzed as to their distribution to both the mode and resource categories. Each mode was composed of the four resources—~fact, prefer- ence, direction, and motivation. Communications of the offer- ing mode were found to comprise a mean of 458 message units per mother or 69 percent of the total communication. The seek- ing mode accounted for a mean of 129 message units per mother 52 55 or 20 percent of the total while the accepting mode repre- sented a mean of 55 or 8 percent of the total. The not accept- ing mode accounted for a mean of 19 or 5 percent of the total number of communications from mothers to children (Table 5). Table 5. The Distribution of Message Units of Mothers' Communi— cation to Children During Home Activities as Related to the Categories or Resources and Modes.1 Total Mean Number of Number Communication Message Per Percentage Categories Units Mother of Total Mode2 Offering 1,854 458 A 69 Seeking 515 129 B 20 Accepting 220 55 C 8 Not accepting 75 19 C 5 Resource3 Fact 1,566 544 a 52 Preference 177 44 b 7 Direction (or command) 1,016 252 c 58 Motivation (or encourage» ment) 85 21 b 5 Total 2,644 611 1Means followed by the same case letter are not significantly dif- ferent from one another but means followed by different case letters are significantly different from one another (P < 0.05). 2Composed of four resources—~fact, preference, direction, moti— vation. 3Composed of four modes—-offering, seeking, accepting, not accepting. 54 Each resource was composed of the four modes which were offering, seeking, accepting, and not accepting. For the resource of fact, the mean number of message units delivered by the mothers was 544 or 52 percent of the total resource communications. For the resource of preference a mean of 44 message units or 7 percent of the total communications were transmitted this way. For the resource of direction (or com— mand) a mean of 254 message units or 58 percent of the total message units were recorded, and for the resource of motivation, 21 communications or 5 percent of the total message units were of this type. Test of Independence of Categories Several significant differences were found between message units when they were categorized as to the four modes and then as to the four resources; although for the mode categories no significant differences were found between the mean frequencies of accepting and of not accepting. This is not too surprising, for the differences between these two modes is one of degree of attitude rather than a completely different manner of transmit- ting contents. When the accepting and not accepting modes were considered as one, significant differences were found between the mean frequency in this combined category and the modes of offering and of seeking. This indicated that the three major divisions into which the message units were categorized were significantly different from one another. 55 For the resource categories, no significant differences for the mean frequency of the message units were found between preference and motivation; yet each of these were significantly different from the communication frequencies of fact and of preference. These results indicated that the categories set up by the model 'were defining the message units into independent groups. Longabaugh (1965), in his model, incorporated fact and preference into one large resource category he called informa- tion. The data of this study supported the feasibility of separating this resource in order to examine the informational content of communication in greater depth and it also lent sup- port to the content validity of the model. Test for Relationship Between Communication Categories and Situational Variables Using the regression technique, the difference in the number of message units in each category between mothers was shown to be related to specific situational variables. Some situational variables were related to more communication cate— gories than others; however, all the situational variables were used in explaining the differences in the frequency of message units in one or more categories. The relationship of the situational factors to message units when they were separated into mode categories and then into resource categories was abstracted from the regression equations (Table 4). To determine what situational factors were related to the message units of a mode, the variables in the i- ..\. 56 .EOQm mm mQHQMHHM> MO fiOHHHUUm Cu. @DU M GA... mmmmmHUCH mmHMUHUCH ”HQ . . H mm. wEoucH haflfimm hm. mmmauumfi mucflm mm>oE mpHcDEEOU mo .02 am. mmHmE mo .02 mm. mam: umrumu mo unsosa mm. mmfluso oaonmmson mcflummovm SH ucwmw mEHu EDEASAE #02 we. SOHumummmum HmmE SH ucmmm 08H“ ESEmeE md. SOHpmummmHm HmmE mm. maflmp OEOS Eoum muson m.um£umm SH “comm mEflu ESEmez mm. mpmoH nmm3 .02 mm. mmMHHHmE mocflm mm>OE muHSDEEOU .02 em. mam: Hmsumm mo pcsofid mm. coaumnmmmum HmmE Hm. mmflgso oaozmmson SH ucmmm mEHu EDEHMME SH “comm mEHp ESEHSHZ mcflummood aw. coaumummmum hm. SOHDMHManm HmmE Home CH ucmmm mEHu EDEHSHE SH ucmmm mEHu EDEwaE on. mmfluso paozmmson Hm. Hmnummou HOSSHU mcflumm mo wocmsvmum SH ucmmm mEHu ESEHSHZ mcflxmmm mm. mEOUcH MHHEmm mm. Hm>ma Hmcoflumuzpm m.mma3 0.6 SOflpmummmHm Home SH ucmmm mEHu EDEHSHE mm. memoH gums mo .02 mm. damn umsumu mo “csoea mm. mmHmE mo .02 mm. mam: UHHSU mo HSSOES mm. mam: Ham mo ucsoea mm. mmflusp eaormmsos msgnmuuo hm. mmmauumfi mocam wm>OE muflcsfifioo mo .02 SH ucmmm mEHu ESEmez Om. coaumummmum Home SH “comm oeflu EDEHSHZ Sn. mHmQEmE %HHEMM Hmuoe me. mmflusp paonmmson SH “comm mEHu Enfiflxmz mm. Hm>ma HMSOHDMUDUm m.mmH3 Md mocmummmum m< pomm mopoz mMUMDOmmm Hmmflnommumo mousommmlmpoz gnu ou Amv mucmfloflmmmoo HMSOHumamHHOU coammmummm mamfluasz flufl3 mmHQMHHm> HMSOHDMSDHm mo mmHQmSOHpmHmm .6 mHQme 57 Sm. mam: Ham mo HSSOdemcflummoom mSOHum>umeo Ham now 0 mm. >HHEMM SH .0: Hmuoa uoz mm. sflgmv mm. mEoocH madamm 0&0: Eoum mason m.n0£umm mm. coaumummmum Home CH ucmmm mEHu EDEflxmz Sm. mmmanuma hm. wmfluso paonmmson :H Human mafia EDEASHZ mocflm m0>oE muHSSEEOU mo .02 «m. mmmauume mocflm wm>oE huHcDEEoo .02 mm. Monummou mcflummood mm. Hw>ma HMSOHumozpm m.mma3 umccdo mcflumw mo mocmsvmum mm. mam: UHSQU mo ucsofid an. mam: UHHSU mo unsofig hm. mmHuDU UHonmmson SH ucmmm 08H» ESEmeE em. Hm>ma HMCOAuMUDUm m.mma3 oo.a mmama mo .02 mm. mmfluzp UHOS Immson SH ucmmm mafia EDEASHZ mm. mam: aggro mo unsoam mm. mmMHHumE woman mm>OE muHSSEEOU mo .02 mm. “wrummou mm. mam: Ham mo ucsofig Hmccflo mcfipmm mo mocmsqmum mcflxmmm am. 0mMHHumE mocflm m0>oE muflcsfifioo .02 mm. mmflusp GHQ: Nb. mmHmE .oz Iwmsoc SH pcmmm mEHu ESEmeE «m. mam: UHHSU mo DSDOES 5w. H0>0H Hmc0flumospm n.0MH3 mm. mmmauumfi mocam mmDOE muflcsafioo mo .02 mm. Hm>ma HmSOHumospm m.mMH3 mm. Sagan 0&0: Eoum muzon m.um£umm mm. mmmaunme mocflm mm>oE muflcsfifioo mo .02 mm. memE mo .02 mm. mmflusp UHonmmson SH ucmmm mfiflu EDEHCHZ ms. mmflusp oaoz mcflummmo mm. waflmp 0&0: Eoum mason m.um£umm Immson CH ucmmm mEHu EDEmez mm. mmflusp UHozmmso: SH ucmmm mEHu EDEmeE mg. coaumummmum mm. SOflpmummmum Home SH ucmmm mafia EDEmeE HmmE SH ucmmm mEHu ESEHSHZ Md coaum>fluoz m4 COHpumnfln mmpoz mmoéommm 00:CHDCOO I w magma 58 four mode-resource categories in which the mode under consider- ation was a part were examined. All variables found to be related to at least one mode-resource dyad in which the mode was a part were considered related to the mode. For example, to determine if wife's educational level was related to the mode of offering, all the situational variables related to offering fact, offering preference, offering direction and offering motivation were scanned to determine if wife's educa- tional level was listed. Since it was related to the communi- cation dyads of offering fact and offering direction it was considered related to the mode of offering. A similar technique was used to determine what situational factors were related to the resources. All variables which appeared more than once in the regression equations in which the mode-resource dyad had the resource under examination as part of it were considered related to the resource. For example, to determine if wife's educational level was related to the resource of fact, all situational variables in the regression equations of offering fact, seeking fact, accepting fact and not accepting fact were scanned. Because wife's educational level appeared twice (offering fact, seeking fact), it was considered related to the resource of fact. The situational factors of (1) family size and composition, (2) time allocated for household duties, and (5) amount of help with household duties were subdivided into component parts to Show their relationship to the modes and the resources. 59 If at least one of the subcategories was related to a mode or resource, then the main variable was considered related to that mode or resource. For example, in Table 6 (p. 45), family size and composition is subdivided into number of males, number of females, and total number of family members. The subcategories-—number of males and total number of family members--showed relationships with the offering mode; therefore, the main family variable of family size and composition was considered related to the mode of offering as shown in Table 5 (p. 41). For the main variable of time allocated for household duties, subcategories of minimum and maximum times for both meal preparation and household duties were Specified. On the questionnaire, time allocations for these two duties were expressed as a range of time such as 15-50 minutes, 50-60 minutes, 1 hour to 2 hours. The minimum time of the duty was the first figure of the range that the mother had indicated such as 15 minutes if the mother had selected the time span of 15-50 minutes. The maximum time for the duty was the larger figure such as 50 minutes in the above illustration. If the duty was discussed without reference to time range (minimum or maximum) the relationship of both the minimum and the maxi— mum of the given duty to the modes or resources were combined. For example, in Table 8 (p.‘47),the minimum time spent in meal preparation was related to the resources of fact, preference and direction; and maximum time spent in meal preparation was 40 related to the resources of fact and motivation; the variable of time spent in meal preparation was considered to be related to all four resources. The same method of determining relation— ships applied when modes were considered. The Relationship of Situational Factors to the Modes The distribution of the situational factors indicated that all of them were related to modes of the mothers' communication to their children as shown in Table 5. Those factors related to all the modes (offering, seeking, accepting, and not accept— ing) were: number of community moves since marriage, time allocated for household duties, and amount of help with house- hold duties. Additional variables specifically related to the mode of offering were family size and composition, mother's educational level, and father's daily hours from home because of employment. For the communications of seeking, the other related variables were family size and composition, mother's educational level, family income, and frequency of eating dinner together; while for the communications of accepting, related situational vari— ables were mother's educational level, father's daily hours from home because of employment, family income, and frequency of eating dinner together. For the communications of motivation, the related variables were family size and composition, father's daily hours from home because of employment, and family income. 41 .SOAum>HuoE Ucm .SOHu00HHU .mocmnmmmum .uomm mo mmousommu msu mo UmmomfiouH N x Hmnummou Hmccflp mcflumm mo wocmsqmum x x x x meuDU oaonmmson SDHB mam: mo pSSOES x x x x mMADDU UHonmmson How topmooHHm mEHB x x x x 0mmauumfi moCHm mm>oE huHSDEEOU x x x mEOUSH maflemm x x x 080: Eoum muson waflmp m.um£pwm x x x Hm>0a HMSOHumospm m.uw£u02 x x x coauflmomfioo Ucm mNHm MHHEMm mcflummood uoz mcwummoofi mcflxwmm mcfiummmo mmHQMHHm> HmcoauMSDHm Hmpoz mmflufl>fluo¢ 050m cmumsm magnum cmupaaro uflmre ou mum:uoz Eoum UmDMUAGSEEOU mmpoz mnu £pfl3 mmHQMHHm> HMQOHumsuflm mo QHSmSOHumHmm was .m magma 42 There were differences in the relationship of the com- ponent parts of the situational variables with the modes. An examination of how the three component parts of family size and composition were related to the modes indicated that the number of females in the family did not appear to be related to any of them. The number of males, though, was related to all but the mode of accepting, and the total number of family members was related to the modes of offering and not accepting (Table 6). The variable of time allocated for household duties was composed of five situational variables as shown in Table 6. All five measures were related to both the modes of offering and accepting. The minimum and maximum times allocated for household duties, which included dishwashing, bedmaking, tidy- ing up and care of pets, were related to all four modes. The number of wash loads was related only to the two variables of offering and accepting. Laundry, unlike the other component variables is usually not a daily household duty. The relationship of the three component variables of the amount of help given the mother with household duties, to the modes, indicated that all three variables were related to the offering mode (Table 6). Total amount of help included father, children and hired help. 45 .coaum>wuoe paw .coauumuap .mocmummmum .uumm mo mmousomwu gnu mo UmmomfiooH x Hmnummou “macaw mcflumm mo wucmsqmum mUmoH SmMB >Hx003 .oz mmflusp UHOSmmDOS How mEau EDEmeE mmflusp paosmmson How mafia ESEASHZ .mmum Hmmfi How mafia EDEmez .mmum HmmE How mEHu EDEHSHZ “mmfluso UHosmmson How Umumooaam mEHB mam: Ham mo ucsosa mam: UHHSU mo unsofid mam: Hmnumm mo unsofid "mmfluso paonmwsos Spa? mam: mo unscam x x mmMHHHmE mUCHm mm>oE huHSDEEOU mo .02 XXX XXXXX X X X XXXXX XXX XX X XXX x mEOUcH maflfimm xuo3 mo mmsmomn 0&0: Scum muso: waflmp m.um£umm X XXXX X X x Hm>ma HMGOAuMUDUm m.umnuo£ x x Mafifimm SH .oc Hmuoe mmamfimm .02 x x x mmams .oz "coauflmomfioo paw mNHm maflfimm mcflummood uoz mcwummoog msaxmmm mcauwmwo mmHQMHHm> HMSOHDMSHHm mmpoz a mmflufl>flp04 mEom pmnmsm mSHHDD cmnpawno Mamas Op mumnuoz Eouw UmUMUHSDEEOU mmpoz mnu nuMB muumm ucmcomfioo uflmga paw mGHQMAHm> coaumsuflm mo OHSmSOHSMHmm 0:9 .m magma 44 The Relationship of Situational Variables to the Resources The distribution of the situational factors indicated that all of them were related to Specific resources of the mothers' communication to their children as shown in Table 7. Those situational factors related to every resource (fact, preference, direction, and motivation) were: family size and composition, the number of community moves Since marriage; the time allocated for household duties, and amount of help with household duties. Additional variables Specifically related to the resource of factual communication were mother's educational level and family income. For communications of preference, the other related variables were father's daily hours from home because of employment, family income, and frequency of eating dinner together. For communications of direction or command related situational variables were mother's educational level; father's daily hours from home because of employment, and frequency of eating dinner together; while for the resource of motivation, related variables were mother's educational level, father's daily hours from home because of employment, and family income. There were differences in the relationship of the com— ponent situational variables making up three larger classifi— cations. For the major variable of family size and composition, the relationship of the three component parts to the resources differed from one another as shown in Table 8. For example, 45 .mcwummoom uoc cam .mcaummoum .mcflxmmm .mcaumwmo mo moUOE usom mo UmwomeouH Hmnummou Hmccflp mcflpmm mo wocmsqmum mmflusp paonmmsos £ua3 mam: mo ucsoeg mmHuDU UHonmmson How Umumooaam mEHB X >< X X mmmauumfi wocflm mm>oE huwcsfisoo X X >< X mEoocfl maafimm X >< X X >< X X 080: Eoum musos maflmp m.u0£u0h x x Hm>ma HmSOHuMUDUm m.Hm£uoz X X >< X X >< X x x x cofluflmomfiou Ucm Guam %HHEmm coaum>fluoz coapomnfin mocmummmum puma moanmwum> HMSOADMDUHm J, Hmousomwm mmflufl>flu0¢ meow pmumgm magnum cmupaflno uflmnu on mumnuos Eoum UmHMUHSSEEoo mmousommm may SDAB mmHQMHHm> HMSOAUMDuflm mo QHQmSOHumHmm one .5 magma 46 the number of males in the family was related to all the re- sources; while the number of females did not appear to be related to any of the resources, just as it had not been re- lated to any of the modes. The total number of family members was related to the resources of fact and motivation. The variable of time allocated for household duties was composed of five variables as shown in Table 8. Time spent in meal preparation and household duties, which were daily activities, were related to all four resources. The number of wash loads was only related to the resource of preference; laundry is usually not a daily duty. The relationship of the three component variables of the amount of help with household duties to the resources is shown in Table 8. Total amount of help included father, children, and hired help. The amount of child help and amount of all help appeared related to the resources of direction and motivation whereas father help did not appear related to these resources, but was related to the resources of fact and preference. The Relationship of the Situational Variables of the Mode—Resource Categories The number of message units for each mother in each com- munication category is shown in the Appendix, Table 11, p. 70. The means for all mothers' message units in the categories of offering fact and offering direction were not significantly different from one another but they were significantly different 47 .mcflummoum pom .mcflummoom .mcaxwmm .mcflummmo mo mmGOE mnu mo pmmomaooH Hmccflp mcflumm mo mocmsvmum mpmoH £mm3 maxmm3 .oz mmflpsp UHozmmson Mom med» EDEHKME mmfiusp paosmmson mom mEHp EDEHSHZ .mmnm HmmE How mafia EDEwaz .mmum Hmme How mafia EDEHSAE “mmflusp paosmmson How pmumooaam mafia x x mam: Ham mo ucsofid x mam: UHHQU mo USDOE¢ mam: monumm mo unscad "mmausp paosmmson £ua3 mam: mo unsoad x x x mmmauuma mucfim mm>OE muHSDEEou mo .oz X X XXXX XXXX XX XX x x maoocfl waflfimm xno3 mo mmsmomn 0&0: Eoum mnson haflmp m.um£pmm X X HO>OH HMCOHHMUDUO m..H®£u.O—z. XXXXX X X x x maflfimw SH .0: Hmuoa mmHmEmw .oz mmamE .oz "coauflmomfioo paw mNHm waflfimm X X X X coaum>flu02 cowuomnfln mucmummmum pomm mmaflmwum> HMSOHumzuHm mwousommm H mmfluw>fluo< 080m Umumzm madman :mHUHHSU Hams» ou mnmfluoz Eouw pmumoHSDEEOO mmousowwm mnu nuHB muumm ucmcomfioo “doze paw mmHQMAum> HMSOHumsuHm mo mangOHumHmm one .m magma 48 from the message units in the category of seeking fact (Table 9). These three categories were significantly different from all message units in the other thirteen categories and no communi- cations in any of the other categories were significantly different from one another. Patterns in the relationships of some variables to the communication categories were evident from the simple corre- lations. A value of r 3'.50 was arbitrarily set as the criterion within which a variable was considered to be related to a mode-resource category. Wife's education appeared to be related to the communications in which the offering and accept- ing modes were in combination with resources (Appendix, Table 12, p. 71). The variable of time spent in meal preparation was related to all four of the mode-resource categories in which the mode of offering was a part. This variable was also related to three of the four possible mode-resource categories in each case where the resources of fact, preference, and direction were a part of the communication dyad. Father's help was related to three of the four categories in which fact was the resource while all help was related to three of the four categories in which direction was the resource. Theoretically all sixteen mode-resource categories were possible but in the small sample of this study no message units of not accepting motivation were transmitted from the mothers to their children. Five of the remaining fifteen categories had tWo situational variables related to them and the remaining 49 Table 9. The Sum and Mean Distribution of Message Units in Each Communication Category1 Resource Mode Fact Preference Direction Motivation 2 = 805 Z = 55 Z = 916 Z = 82 Offering __ __ __ __ X = 89.2ab X = 5.6d X = 101.7a X = 9.1d 2 = 585 Z = 106 Z = 24 Z = 2 Seeking _ _ _ _ X = 42.5c X = 11.7d X = 2.6d X = .2d 2 = 152 2 = 56 Z = 51 Z = 1 Accepting __ _. ._ ._ X = 14.6d X = 4.0d X = 5.6d X = 0.1d Not 2 = 48 Z = 2 Z = 25 Z = 0 Accepting __ __ __ '_ X = 5.5d X = .2d X = 2.7d X = 0d 1Means followed by the same case letter are not significantly different from one another but means followed by different case letters are significantly different from one another (P < 0.05). Total number = 2,644 Mean number = 295.7 50 ten categories each had four to seven factors related to them. -Those situational variables most often related to the mode- resource categories were: the size and composition of the family, number of community moves since marriage, time allo- cated for household duties and the amount of help with house- hold duties. All of these variables except community moves were related to daily operation of the house. -The less frequently occurring variables were: mother's educational level, father's daily hours from home because of employment, family income, and frequency of eating dinner together. Except for dining frequency, these latter factors were all related to the socio-economic level of the family. It was interesting to note that the variables most frequently related to communi- cation style pertained to daily operation of the house and that the less frequently occurring variables pertained to socio- economic levels of the families. Similarities were evident between several communication categories concerning their relationship to situational vari— ables; for example, almost all of the variables that were related to the communication categories of offering fact, Offer— ing preference, and offering motivation pertained to household Operations and help with household duties. Identical variables were related to offering preference and accepting preference, i.e., household Operations, amount of household help, and the number of community moves. All but two of the variables re— lated to Offering fact and not accepting fact pertained basically 51 to household Operation and the amount of household help. The situational variables related to Offering motivation pertained to household operation; those related to accepting motivation pertained to household operation and help with tasks. The results, then, indicated that all the selected situ- ational variables were related in some manner to mothers' communication to their children when: 1. Message units categorized as to modes were examined. 2. Message units categorized as to resources were examined. 5. Message units categorized as to mode—resource inter- action categories were examined. Therefore it appears that the coding procedure did identify variables indicating communication pattern. This is consistent with the findings of Bernstein, Hess and Shipman, Merrill, Olim §“_al., and Walters §£_ l. which would lead one to believe -—-— —-— that situational factors and communication are not independent. CHAPTER V CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS Summary The randomly selected communication samples of nine mothers to their preschool children during shared activities within the home were part of the communication samples used in testing a procedure to gather data about family activity patterns. All of the mothers whose conversations were used as data had children enrolled in Michigan State University‘s Laboratory Preschool during Fall, 1966, and Winter, 1967. In six of the families, father, mother, and children were present; in the other three families, the fathers were absent. The relative frequencies of the mothers' message units as they were categorized both as to the salient resources and the modes of transmission indicated that the mean percentage (or relative frequency) of message units delivered by the mothers to their children, when defined as to resource were: fact-~52 percent; preference—-7 percent; direction--58 percent; and motivation--5 percent. When the message units were defined as to mode, the percentages were: offering-~69 percent, seek— ing--20 percent; accepting-~8 percent, and not accepting—— 5 percent. 52 55 Significant differences between several means of the message units gave support to the coding system's successful separation of the mothers' communication to their children into independent components. The data also supported the feasibility of dividing Longabaugh's (1965) resource category of information into the two resources of fact and preference. Communication modes. The selected situational factors of the home environment appeared to be related to the communica— tive patterns. When the message units were categorized as to modes, those situational variables related to the communications of all four modes were: number of community moves since marriage, time allocated for household duties, and amount of help with household duties. Additional variables specifically related to the mode of offering were family size and composi- tion, mother's educational level, and father's daily hours from home because of employment. For the communications of seeking, the other related variables were family size and composition, mother's educational level, family income, and frequency of eating dinner together; while for the communica— tions of accepting, related situational variables were mother's educational level, father's daily hours from home because of employment, family income, and frequency of eating dinner together. For the communications of motivation, the related variables were family size and composition, father's daily hours from home because of employment, and family income. 54 Communication resources. When the message units were categorized as to the communications of the different resources, those situational variables related to all four of the re- sources were: family size and composition, the number of com— munity moves since marriage, the time allocated for household duties, and amount of help with household duties. Additional variables specifically related to the resource of factual communication were mother's educational level and family income. For communications of preference, the other related variables were father's daily hours from home because of employment, family income, and frequency of eating dinner together. For communications of direction or command, related situational variables were mother's educational level, father's daily hours from home because of employment, and the frequency of eating dinner together; while for the resource of motivation, related variables were mother's educational level, father's daily hours from home because of employment, and family income. Mode—Resource category. When the message units were desig- nated as to the Specific mode-resource categories of communica— tion, the situational variables were clustered both by the frequency and the Similarity of occurrence into two groups. The variables most frequently related to the communication pat- terns were: the size and composition of the family, number of community moves since marriage, time allocated for household duties and the amount of help with household duties. The less frequently occurring variables were: mother's educational 55 level, father's daily hours from home because of employment, family income, and frequency of eating dinner together. All Of the variables related to the mode-resource cate- gories of Offering fact, offering preference, and Offering motivation were similar and likewise those factors related to offering preference and accepting preference were alike. -The situational factors associated with Offering fact and not accepting fact were basically the same. Thus, patterns of variables related to some mode—resource categories appeared to be consistent for the same resource when in combination with different modes, or for the same modes when in combination with different resources. In one case, though, a combination of the Situational variables that appeared to be related to offering motivation and seeking motivation were found to be related to accepting motivation. Limitations 1. The first limitation was the small, non-random group of mothers from which the communication samples analyzed in this study were taken. Because of this limitation, it is not possible to generalize the results of this study to communi- cation patterns Of other mothers. 2. The second limitation was that one—third of the families had fathers absent from the home. This was a very important factor because of the small sample size. 5. A third limitation was inconsistency in data collection; the number of different days in which home activities were 56 taped for each mother—child dyad varied from one to four. The mother's communication to her child during four shared activities that were recorded in a single morning may have been different from that observed if activity periods had been collected on separate days, as was done for the other mothers. More diversity in individuals' temperament may be Observed with recording on separate days. .This diversity may be a major contributing factor in establishing reliable communication patterns between family members. 4. The diversity in the kinds of activities performed by the mothers and children was also a limiting factor. The participants were not asked to do Specific activities, and the activities during which communication was recorded were not the same for the group. Also, the number of children partici- pating in the activities varied. Generally one preschool child was present; yet in the activities of some families, two or more children were involved. 5. The presence of the observer during the Observation period may have influenced communication between the mothers and their children by altering the home environment. The ob- server's role was to follow the mother-child dyad throughout the home while they engaged in their shared activity. 6. A limitation occurring during the transcription of the dating was the inability to code some message units from the tapes because they were indistinguishable. This resulted in a loss of some of the data. 57 7. Another limitation was the small sample of message units used. Eight five-minute random samples, or forty minutes of each mother's conversation to her child, may not have been representative of the communication patterns char- acteristic of that mother. 8. The limitation of the situational variables--both availability and reliability--should be noted. In three of the families it could not be determined whether father figures were present in the homes. Also, several of the variables were chosen because they were thought to control the pattern Of family life and they were available. Finally, some of the information about the situational variables were obtained by self-report questioning and not by direct observation. Implications for Future Study Socio—economic environment. Findings from this study suggest that certain communication patterns Of mothers to their children appeared to be related to situational variables. Communication studies may identify differences between mothers of different socio-economic environments and/or styles of liv- ing in the frequencies of different kinds Of patterning stimuli Offered by these mothers to other family members. Language styles used by mothers may, then, reflect differences within as well as between socio-economic levels. Cognitive environment. If the growth Of cognitive func- tions is mediated by experience and if the mother is a child's 58 principle teacher for the first five years of his life, then the strategies that a mother uses through communication to manipulate the child will have consequences for the ability of the child to grasp or learn in later teaching situations. Decision making. If the association of language and situational factors do emerge in systematic relationships which have permanent effects on communication interaction, what ef- fects will these combination of factors have on decision making activities? How do these decision making activities in turn help to regulate the nature and amount of communicative exchange? Management style. Other useful studies would be those which would relate the communication patterns of homemakers to their styles of managing. Are their specific communications (mode-resource frequencies) that are characteristic of home- makers who are person centered and/or task centered? Do situ- ational characteristics related to person- or task-centeredness appear to transcend socio—economic boundaries? What communi- cative patterns are characteristic of effective managers or of ineffective managers? How are communicative patterns of family members related to the utilization and develOpment of their human resources in an effort to achieve family goals? Bell and Vogel (1960) point out that the motivational committment of family members for the performance of tasks is reflected in the amount of help actually given (or received) by family members. The present study showed a relationship 59 between the mode-resource categories of seeking motivation and accepting motivation with the situational variable--the amount of child help with household duties. The amount Of father's help, on the other hand, was related to the categories of Offering fact, not accepting fact, and accepting preference. Why was father's help related to the resources of fact and child's help related to the resource of motivation? A study which would analyze the communication patterns and techniques employed by family members to induce task committment as re- flected in the amount of family help actually given the mother would be another fruitful way of gaining a more complete under- standing of communication's relationship as a management process to the other basic components of family management-- values, goals, resources, family, management, environment, and problem situation. LITERATURE CITED Baker, Georgianne, "Patterning of Home Resources for Children's Early Cognitive Growth," Unpublished paper, Department of Home Management, Michigan State University, 1965. Also personal communication. Bales, Robert F., Interaction Process Analysis: A Method for the Study of Small Groups, Cambridge, Mass.: Addison Wesley Press, 1950. 1-84. Bell, Norman W. and Ezra F. Vogel (Ed), The Family, Glencoe, Illinois: The Free Press, 1960. 20. Berlo, David K., The Process of Communication, New York: Holt Rinehard and Winston, 1960. pp. 25-49, 190-215. Bernstein, Basil, "Family Role Systems, Communication, and Socialization," as cited by Hess, Robert D. and Virginia C. Shipman, "Early Experiences and the Socialization of Cognitive Modes in Children," Childgpevelopment, XXXVI (1965), 869—886. Bernstein, Basil, "Social Class and Linguistic Development: A Theory of Social Learning," pp. 288-514, as cited by Hess, Robert D. and Virginia C. Shipman, "Early Experiences and the Socialization of Cognitive Modes in Children," ' Childvpevelopment, XXXVI (1965), 869-886. Bettinghaus, Erwin P., "Communication: .An Integral Part of the Management Process," Conceptual Frameworks: Process of Home Management, (Proceedings Of a Home Management Conference, East Lansing, Michigan, June 17-20, 1964), 47-51. Duncan, D. 3., "Multiple Range and Multiple F Tests," Biometrics, XI (1955), 1-42. Hess, Robert D. and Virginia C. Shipman, "Early Experiences and the Socialization of Cognitive Modes in Children,“ Child Development, XXXVI (1965), 869-886. , "Early Blocks to Children's Learning," Growing for Parents Of School Age Children, XIX (1967), 12-17. 60 61 Kenkel, William F. and Dean K. Hoffman, "Real and Conceived Roles in Family Decision Making," Marriage and Family Living, XVIII (Nov. 1956), 511-516. , Longabaugh, Richard, "A Category System for Coding Interpersonal Behavior As Social Exchange," Sociometry, XXVI (1965), 515-544. Linton, Ralph, The Cultural Background of Personality, pp. 76-77, as cited by Magrabi 33 al,, "Framework for Studying Family Activity Patterns," Journal Of Home_§conomics, LIX (Nov. 1967), 714-719. Liston, Margaret I., "Management in the Family as Social Process," Conceptual Frameworks: Process oijanagement (Proceedings of a Home Management Conference, East Lansing, Michigan, June 17-20, 1964), 52-72. ~Loomis, Charles P., "Social Systems: Their Elements, Proces- ses and Patterns," Social Systems, New York: 'D. Van Nostrand Company, Inc., 1960, 1-56. Magrabi, Frances M., Beatrice Paolucci, and Marjorie E. Heifner, "Framework for Studying Family Activity Patterns," Journal Of Home Economics, LIX (Nov..1967), 714-719. Merrill, Barbara, "A Measure of Mother-Child Interaction," Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, XXXXI (1946), 57-49. Miller, Gerald R., "On Defining Communication: Another Stab," Journal of Communication, XXVI (1966), 88-99. Morris, Charles, signification and Significance, Mass.: The Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 1964, 87. Olim, Ellis, G., Robert D. Hess, and Virginia C. Shipman, "Maternal Language Styles and Their Implication for Children's Cognitive Development," (Paper read at the American Psychological Association Meeting, Chicago, September 5, 1965). , "Role of Mothers' Language Styles in Mediating Their Preschool Children's Cognitive Development," (Paper read at the American Educational Research Association Meeting, Chicago, 1966). Ruesch, Jurgen, "Syn0psis of the Theory of Human Communica- tion," Psychiatry, XVI (1955), 215-245. 62 Sabin, Theodore R., "Role Theory," p. 225, as cited in Magrabi g£_§l,, "Framework for Studying Family Activity Patterns," Journal of Home Economics, LIX (Nov. 1967), 714-719. Strodbeck, Fred L., "The Family As a Three-person Group," American Sociological Review, XXXIX (1954), 125-129. Walters, James, Ruth Connor, and Michael Zunich, "Interaction of Mothers and Children in Lower-Class Families," Child Development, XXXV (1964), 455-440. APPENDIX 65 64 Examples of Message Units From Each Of the Sixteen Message units enclosed by parentheses, Possible Mode-Resource Categories i.e., (. . .) indicate the message prior to the one presented as a category example. I. II. III. IV. VI. Offering fact A. "We can play it like the other records we got." B. "Maybe we should ask her if she knows how to do this." C. "You need a lot more." Offering preference A. "I bet that's what happened." B. "I think the red all by itself is very pretty and enough." C. ("See if there is anything you want.") "I want to keep this." Offeringgdirectigg A. "Cut it off." B. "Here." C. "Let me try it once." Offering motivation A. "Beautiful." B. "I'm sorry." C. "You boys are good bakers." Seeking_fact A. "Is it there yet?" B. "Do you have a big, long one?" C. "What color is that?" Seeking preference A. "Don't you want some cherry pie?" B. "Is that pretty good?" . C. l'DO you want to get down?" VII. VIII. IX. XI. XII. 65 Seeking direction A. "Should I pour it all in?" B. "What am I to do now?" C. "That fast?" Seekinggmotivation A. "Did I do that well?" B. "Don't you think I will make a good cook?" C. "Isn't my drawing pretty?" Accepting7fact A. ("I got hit.") "Yea." B. ("See a wolf likes to be hungry.") ll Oh. H C. ("It's falling apart right there.") "Yea." Accepting_preference A. ("It's a pretty green.") "Unhuh." B. ("I want to put blue in.") "All right." C. ("I want to put one of these Santa Clauses on the tree.") "00K." Accepting direction A. ("Just a little bit more.") "Yea." B. ("Put this in the refrigerator.") "All right." C. ("See?") "Yea." Accepting motivation A. ("Thank you.") "Your welcome." B. ("You've done a handsome picture.") "Thank you." C. ("You can do it if you put yourself to it.") "Yea." XIII. XIV. 66 Not acceptingifact A. ("You're making that all by yourself.") "No I'm not." B. ("That's Gunsmoke.") "That's not Gunsmoke." C. ("That's me.") "NO . H Not accepping_preference A. ("I think you will like that.") "What!" B. ("How about meat and Spagetti?") "Oh, I don't know, Tanya." C. ("You can have chocolate or vanilla.") "Forget it." Not accepting direction A. ("Can I do that, Mom?") "I'm going to try to cut it up into equal parts.“ B. ("Keep it on the paper plate.") "Not that one." C. ("You can put it on.") "Wait . H Not accepting motivation A. ("You are great.‘) "Be reasonable." B. ("Thank you.") "You're not welcome." C. ("That's a beautiful picture you did.") "It's awfull" 67 000.2% 99 0» m 0.0 N.N ¢.N 00002 000.0 um>o paw om 20000000000 0:02 s e m 09 Au0uc0mumov. 000.0 m o» 0 00900 0c02 m m m 0 000.0 um>o 0cm 0m AmHmcflmv Hmogumao e m 9 0 90699 99020 OOO.N u0>0 020 ON AO0UHO>HQV 2203 009>H0m m N H m u0>0 020 99:00:90“ 000.09 99 0» 09 900090009022 0:02 a m N 0 990000900 ooo.m o 2029 0009 900090009092 0002 e N N 0 A90230qv ooo.N9 m 09 w 902090009092 0002 e N N m 00900 ooo.N9 0205 90 N9 190099090 0:02 m N 0 N 000.09% m Cu 0 900090009092 0002 m a d 9 080009 29902 0502 2099022000 2099020000 0909E02 299502 :9 299802 209252 299502 8022 02:02 0.0009022 0.0293 299202 0090502 29.00902 299802 0.0009052 90 909822 90 909852 90 209552 092500 029 90 00999802 95090 00990890229 0>99©99000Q 0:0 9009£m0um092 .09 09908 68 0.0Nm 009 0.0 0.009 90 00002 0...... 009 0 009 00 090390 09 000 09m 0 009 02 020392 0 com 009 m 02 on 800900 0 00¢ 09N d 0N9 00 020392 0 090 mm 0 2.9 mm 92.0st m 00 09 0 009 00 800900 w 009 09 0 090 mm 090390 m 090 090 09 090 09 999...st N on... 009 0 009 00 09030 9 009950 0900 009952 0900 00009 00990902092 00990902092 299802 0 02 909852 I00502 00909 I00502 00902 0003 9002 09 9002 09 90990008, 299802 90020 0095092 90020 0095092 299003 0095092 0095092 900099 009902 8589902 8589092 90 909852 8589202 8589092 90 200050092 Al( |)) 005099000 I O9 09009 0.0 0.0 2.0 9.0 00002 9 00090 09092 n 0 20090900000 09 09050090 9 00090 09092 900000 0090 0 e 0 0 09050090 0. 0 900000 0090 9 9 20900900 0 .0 0 0009900 0800 2 2 900090>92V 0 009000 009000 0 00000>0< 00000>0¢ m m m 0 0 0009900 0800 009000 0009900 2 9 0 d 0 009000 0009900 009000 0009900 0 m m m 0 0009900 0800 009000 0009900 0 0 0 N 009000 0 00000>00 0009900 0000 0 0 m 9 00099902 009900502 009900502 00 09 2900 00090 00 90 00090 0000 909852 00090 00>02 0.0009052 0.0993 90 00090 00 2902 00 09 2909 90 00 09 299802 299058800 00992 90 .00909900 00909900 02902 9009 90 909852 .909902 009952 009952 I02 009952 090900502 90 909852 090900502 090900502 90 909852 90 909852 005099000 I 09 09908 70 The Number of Message Units for Each Mother in Each Table 11. Communication Category Number of Message Units in Each Category 0 0 0 0 o 0 o -H 0 ‘9 9 0 0 9 9 0 0 0 0 0 o 0 0 > c) 0 o 0 0 0 o -a 9 44 0 -H 0 o 04 c) 0 0 0 -H .9 c) 0 9 9 a) -a 9 0 0 9+ 9 9 0 0 :4 -a o 9 9 0 0 -H 9 0 0 :> ‘H a. 0 8 0 c) > 9 +J 0 +2 ‘9 0 -a +» u4 0 -9 0 0 :> 0 0 9 .9 tn 0 0~ 0 0 a) 9 9 +Jt9 0 0+ 0 9 -H 0 s: 0 0 0 {U 3-! W4 0 U (D H JJ ‘H at 'U E ".1 «4 .9 ".4 H4 2 '0 8 as 9 -H 0 .9 9 +’ 9 '9 Q. '0 E 0'1 0 01 0 0.. 04 04 Q. 0 0a In 0 0 0 0 0 a) 0 0 0 0 c: 0 0 03:0 0 0304 -H 'H -9 0 c) 0 0 -9 9a 04 -9 0 s: 0 0 44 9 9 9 (J 0 0 0 9 9 9 9 94-9 9 9+ 2 0. 2 cm 0 0 0 0 Mothers .93. .93. .3 3 0 0' '0 '33 8 8 8 3 9 9 9 9 9 94 t9 9 a) 0 0 a) 0 0 0 0 0 <3 0 0 <3 0 C) c: m a: to m «c 0 0 0 2: :0 z z 1 7O 5 88 2 7O 28 5 O 9 5 9 O 2 1 1 O 295 2 255 «1 75 9 76 7 1 O 47 O 8 1 15 O 5 O 492 5 92 14 175 52 54 25 9 O 26 15 16 O 6 O 4 O 444 4 78 7 112 9 6 5 2 O 9 2 9 O 4 1 4 O 248 5 155 5 105 19 58 18 5 O 14 7 7 O 6 O 2 O 561 6 81 1 95 5 52 8 2 2 14 5 O O 6 O 9 O 278 7 14 O 85 91 57 2 O O 1 O O O O O O O 158 8 50 O 120 4 59 6 O O 8 O 1 O 10 O 2 O 240 9 5O 0 67 5 51 9 O O 4 2 .1 O 1 O O O 148 Total = 2644 Mean = 295.7 71 Table 12. The Relationship Between Each Mode-Resource Category with Each Situational Variable Based on Simple Corre- lations1 W Situational Variables Mode-Resource Categories (D G 0:20 (IO-H 0049 0 0 s4+»0 0 0 L)2 ()0 o a) 0 c:o~4+Jwa0-9 0 004 0:20 awH4JL)0 9+4 0.9+J 0 004 9+J0 0sqm¢0 990 GMHJ 00>9208 0 0:> 9+J04ch004 +Jwa004 0 00 94J+H9o