THE IMPACT OF SOAP OPERAS ON VIEWERS Thesis for the Degree of M. A. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY ' MITZI BOND. l 9 75 WWIHHIIWHWH“Wilflfllflw 3 1293 10 3:. V". >' ' f‘ :“f‘ 9 I'm. 3 4.1 T ' ' t L' 5."; I" _ . " 3" ' i no..- ,.. .,.:‘ .gv.oa «Jiva . r- . "'. 4“ a. T .V‘. k” c r , e . ., . 4' '. I' .‘:l. .1 'v ' V *3 g 3. ‘3‘“ . m .3... e 305')" ‘ L ‘ ' wsfififlp Ava-m 13 '65 ' 453.5 4' $1.5. a“. ‘y ABSTRACT THE IMPACT OF SOAP OPERAS ON VIEWERS BY Mitzi Bond The purpose of this study was to examine viewers' and nonviewers' perceptions of the reality of soap opera characters and content, to explore viewers' degree of interaction, identification, and involvement with soap operas, and to identify functions satisfied by watching daytime serials. Four hypotheses were tested concerning the rela- tionships among ten variables. The independent variable was viewing; the intervening variable was perceived reality; and the dependent variables were knowledge of program con- tent, involvement, interaction, identification, companion- ship, escapism, boredom, and social utility. The following are hypotheses tested in this study: H1: The higher the amount of viewing, the greater the perceived reality. H2: The higher the amount of viewing, the more knowledge the viewer will have about program content. Mitzi Bond H3: The higher the amount of viewing, the greater the viewer's involvement, inter- action, and identification. H4: The higher the amount of viewing, the more functions watching soap operas will satisfy. A telephone survey was conducted in Lansing, Michigan, in August, 1974. Five hundred fifty numbers were randomly chosen from the Lansing telephone directory and questionnaires were completed by 272 respondents. Hypotheses were confirmed for the following cri- terion variables. As the amount of viewing increased, (1) perceived reality increased, (2) knowledge of soap opera content increased, (3) involvement increased, (4) identification increased, (5) interaction increased, and (6) social utility increased. As an intervening variable, perceived reality shows that as the amount of viewing time increases, and perceived reality increases, the viewer will have (1) more interac- tion with soap opera characters and content, and (2) more relief from boredom from the serials. Hypotheses were not confirmed for an increase in companionship, escapsim, and relief of boredom as viewing increased. Accepted by the faculty of the Department of Communication, College of Communication Arts, Michigan State University, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Master of Arts degree. Dirgfit of TEFsis Guidance Committee: W42 , Chairman é/Wm/ I L. / THE IMPACT OF SOAP OPERAS ON VIEWERS BY Mitzi Bond A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Communication 1975 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Deepest appreciation goes to the author's adviser, Dr. Bradley Greenberg, whose time, patience, and criticisms were most valuable in conduction of the research and in preparation of the thesis. Special recognition goes to two other committee members, Dr. Erwin Bettinghaus and Dr. Edward Pink, for their assistance and review of the thesis. The author also appreciates the time and energy given by Barbara Brooks, Robert McPhee, Sharon Reinert, Cheryl Oliver, Jan Bidwell, Viola Morris, Stephanie Robinson, Gloria Bettinson, and Katherine Smith in col- lection of the data. An inspirational thank you goes to Sherrie Mazingo and a special thank you to David Anderson. ii LIST OF Chapter I. II. III. TABLE OF CONTENTS TABLES . . . . . . . . . INTRODUCTION . . . . . . TV Soap Opera History. Production Costs . . Literature Review. . Soap Opera Reality and METHODS. . . . . . . . . Pretest and Questionnaire Administration . . Gratifications. Respondents and Their Community. Demographics and Viewing Operational Definitions. Independent Variable Perceived Reality. . Knowledge. . . . . . Involvement. . . . . Identification . . . Interaction. . . . . Functions. . . . . . Analysis . . . . . . RESULTS. . . . . . . . . Perceived Reality. . Knowledge. . . . . . Involvement, Interaction, and Identification . . iii Page 23 25 25 27 29 29 36 37 40 40 42 44 46 46 48 49 TABLE OF CONTENTS (cont'd.) Chapter Page Boredom, Companionship, Escapism, and Social Utility . . . . . . . . . . 50 Perceived Reality Correlations . . . . . 51 Partial Correlations . . . . . . . . . . 54 One-Way Analysis of Variance . . . . . . 56 IV. SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . 60 Study Weaknesses . . . . . . . . . . . . 64 Directions for Future Research . . . . . 65 New Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67 Postscript . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69 APPENDICES A. SOAP OPERA QUESTIONNAIRE . . . . . . . . . . 70 B. SOAP OPERA STUDY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80 C. ONE-WAY ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE . . . . . . . . 91 D. CURRENT SOAP OPERA (JANUARY 1975) 1973 NIELSEN SOAP OPERA RATINGS. . . . . . 93 BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95 iv Table 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. LIST OF TABLES Demographics of Viewers and Nonviewers (Chi-Square Test). . . . . . . . . . . . Demographics and Amount of Viewing (Chi-Square Test). . . . . . . . . . . . . Perceived Reality Correlations (Total Sample) Perceived Reality Correlations (Viewers) . . Perceived Reality Correlations (Nonviewers). Perceived Reality Item Frequency . . . . . . Perceived Reality Score Frequency. . . . . . Knowledge. . . . . . . . . . . . .-. . . . . Involvement, Identification, and Interaction (Intercorrelations). . . . . . . . . . . . Involvement, Identification, and Interaction (Frequency). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Functions. 0 O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Viewing and Perceived Reality. . . . . . . . Viewing and Involvement, Interaction, and Identification . . . . . . . . . . . . Viewing and Functions. . . . . . . . . . . . Page 26 28 31 32 33 34 35 38 39 41 43 47 50 51 LIST OF TABLES (cont'd.) Table 15. Perceived Reality Correlations (High and Low PRTV Viewers). . . . 16. Perceived Reality Correlations (Shows—Episodes) . . . . . . . . . l7. Partial Correlations (Viewing by Perceived Reality) . . 18. Partial Correlations (Viewing by Dependent Variables, Controlling for Perceived Reality) 19. Overall Correlations . . . . . . . . Cl. One-Way Analysis of Variance (Viewing-Shows). . . . . . . . . . C2. One-Way Analysis of Variance (Viewing-Episodes) . . . . . . . . D1. Current Soap Operas (January, 1975). D2. 1973 Nielsen Soap Opera Ratings. . . vi Page 52 53 55 55 58 91 92 93 94 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Soap Operas are the literary products of the electronic media. They are simply literature to be seen and not to be read. These serials have central characters with whom the viewer can often identify, whose life could be followed, whose problems could be seen, and with whom the viewer could establish something like a friendship. Their attraction or addiction is based on audience curi- osity about and interest in what will happen to the various characters. James Thurber's definition of soap operas is ‘perhaps the one most often quoted: A soap opera is a kind of sandwich, whose recipe is simple enough, although it took years to compound. Between thick slices of advertising, spread twelve minutes of dialogue, add predicament, villainy, and female suffering in equal measure, throw in a dash of nobility, sprinkle with tears, season with organ music, cover with a rich announcer sauce, and serve five times a week. The dictionary yields a more precise definition: "soap opera--a radio or television serial drama performed usually on a daytime commercial program and chiefly char- acterized by stock domestic situations and often melodra- matic or sentimental treatment." However, soap operas really need no definition. Any television viewer and most nonviewers are familiar with the daily daytime dramas portraying domestic dilemmas. TV Soap Opera History1 This study is concerned with the television serials, the first of which was introduced in 1946. 'A single epi- sode of Big Sister was televised and a full-scale serial, A Woman to Remember, aired in 1947. Both failed. Three years later CBS experimented with The First Hundred Years.' It missed its mark by 99 years. There were many obstacles that denied success to the early television soap Opera. First there was cost. Producing a serial for television was more complicated than producing one for radio. The weekly cost of producing lMadeleine Edmondson and David Rounds, The Soaps: Daytime Serials of Radio and TV (New York, 1973), pp. 130- 140. a fifteen-minute daily television serial was $8650, as opposed to $3500 for a radio serial.- Production also pre- sented problems. Places and people were now right there on the television screen, so sets had to be built and actors appropriately costumed. All props mentioned in the script actually had to be on the set. TV soaps were too expensive to produce until CBS experimented with a new length--the half hour. Production costs for one half-hour episodes were far lower than those of two fifteen-minute episodes for different shows, and the amount of time available to be sold for commercials was un- changed. Lengthening the soap episode therefore made sense for the network, and it turned out to have a delightfully unexpected side effect as well--viewers approved the new length. The very early television serials were-almost in- distinguishable from radio serials. The visual aspect of the new medium turned out not to be as crucial as had been feared. Soaps had always been largely domestic so it was possible to limit most of the action to easily-built indoor sets. Early television soaps even kept the announcer, that useful intruder who was so helpful in-commenting on the action and directing the attention of the audience to the most fruitful themes of future interest. Production Costs2 The average soap Opera in 1973 cost about $60,000 a week to produce (The Young and the Restless is the most ex- pensive at $70,000) compared to an average $100,000 for a single half-hour in prime time. To update Thurber's defi- nition of soaps, the serials consist of 22 minutes of dia- logue (with the exception of Another World which expanded to 60 minutes 6 January 1975). The director of a solid running TV soap makes $2000 a week and the head script writer makes about $3000 a week. In 1970, CBS's two-decade hold on daytime TV rat- ings was an accepted fact of life supported by the Nielsen ratings. That year CBS racked up $162 million in daytime sales compared to NBC's $100 million and ABC's $85 million. However, during the next three years, NBC and ABC intro- duced new soaps of their own and turned the competition into a three-way race. From January to May 1973, CBS 2"Real Drama in Daytime: The Networks' Battle for Dominance," Journal of Broadcasting, 2 July 1973, p. 17. chalked up $68 million in daytime sales, NBC $61 million, and ABC $47 million. (One of the reasons for ABC's rela- tively poor showing is that it only sends out five hours of daytime programming, whereas NBC and CBS each sends out six hours.) The cost-per—minute figures (i.e., how much an ad— vertiser must pay for a one-minute commercial) depend on the ratings of the soap opera and on the demographic break- down of those ratings. By 1973 the most lucrative serial on the air was CBS's As the World Turns, now in its twen- tieth year, which was priced at $19,600 a commercial minute. NBC's Another World, The Doctors, and Days of Our Lives were priced at $13,600 a minute, $12,400, and $12,200 re— spectively. For ABC, General Hospital costs $12,400 a minute compared to $11,200 for All Minhildren and $10,800 for One Life to Live. Advertisers have to pay more to get their messages on soap operas than on game shows because the serials attract a higher percentage of women between the ages of 18 and 49--the most desirable demographic target for daytime sponsors (and for nighttime sponsors). Literature Review One of the earliest studies concerning soap operas was conducted by Herta Herzog (1942) in Iowa, New York, and Pittsburgh. A preliminary study based on 100 intensive interviews suggested three major types of gratification experienced by listeners to daytime serials: l) emotional release--the chance to cry because of happy or sad events in the characters' lives; 2) wishful thinking--the listeners "drowned" their troubles in listening to the events por- trayed in the serials; and 3) sources of advice--the ser- ials teach the listeners appropriate patterns of behavior and how to handle situations that may turn up in their own lives. Herzog found that the less formal education a woman has, the more she is likely to consider these programs helpful and that less-educated women have fewer sources from which to learn "how to win friends and influence people" and are therefore more dependent upon daytime serials for this gratification. Sociologist Nora Scott Kinser (1973) attempted to dissect soap opera characters to show why viewers get mes- merized. Millions are intimately involved with the lives and problems of their favorite assortment of soap opera heroes, heroines, villains, and villainesses. They write letters to fan magazines anxiously inquiring about the per- sonal lives of the stars, threatening mass defection should Mark marry Susy and leave Mamie, weeping over the death of a special character, and pouting about an actor who has been replaced. Sometimes their passions even spill over into real life. Eileen Fulton, who plays Lisa Shea of Ag the World Turns, once fled in terror from the appliance section of a large department store after watching a taped segment of her show and listening to women customers mutter how much they hated Lisa and wanted to kill her. On another occasion a woman asked her if she was Lisa Shea. When she said "yes" and began searching for a pencil to write an autograph, the fan began cursing Lisa and beating her with a purse. How do the soap operas entice such audience involve— ment? To answer this, Kinser developed a typology of soap opera characters. Kinser typology: characters good bad good good-bad good-bad characters bad bad-good bad-good In most soap operas the good-good is usually a motherly- grandmotherly type to whom all the other characters tell their respective tales of woe. The typical bad-bad is an out-and-out thoroughly mean witch who always causes trouble and ruins the lives of the other characters. The good-bad is a baddie who turns out to have a good side while the bad-good is a goodie with a bad side. Part of the fun of soap operas lies in the fact that the audience usually knows that the supposed bad-bad is really a bad-good or a good-bad long before the good-goods realize what is happen- ing. But the immense popularity of the soaps is a complex affair. While their lusty plots titillate fans' daydreams, their chaos and affliction make the viewer's dull life seem well—ordered and safe by comparison. Is it any wonder fans shrieked when the Senate Subcommittee on the Watergate pre- empted their soaps? LaPota and LaPota (1973) suggest the study of the soap opera in the classroom as an attempt to broaden the spectrum of teaching and learning strategies and to promote the discovery of a whole new vein of human skills. The daytime serial is a true literary product of the electronic media. Some authorities believe the soap Opera is already the literature of millions of Americans. Estimates run from 10- to 30-million viewers in the United States who daily watch one or more daytime serials. That is a very respectably sized audience that depends on nonprinted material to experience another man's version of the human experience. And that is what literature basically is. LaPota and LaPota make three functional observa- tions about soap opera content. Values perceived in the soap opera.--(l) It re— flects a particular aspect of contemporary American life: primarily the white, Protestant, middle to upper middle class life style. (2) It is valid to say there is no "typ- ical" soap opera viewer because of the wide spectrum in age group, education, and economic standing represented in the audience. (3) The traditional values of the American de- mocracy are upheld. Reliance on the judicial system, stress on law and order, belief in free enterprise, and duties and responsibilities of citizenship are reflected frequently in the episodes. Any character who breaks the law is eventually punished. (4) The American Judaic- Christian moral values are mirrored. Sin is to be avoided, but if that is not possible the commission of sin is always punished in some way; virtue is rewarded in the long run. 10 (5) The economic value system in the soap opera is a dis- torted picture of the American world of work. The soap opera as a tool for social and-political change.--The soap Opera has been a demythicizer and breaker of subject matter taboo. Apparently producers and writers of soaps are making conscious efforts to explore objec- tively certain conservative political and social convic- tions (e.g., abortion, atheism, alcoholism, sex discrimi- nation). Characters with points of view and attitudes in opposition to the conservative are presented as sincere, thoughtful people operating from their own individuality, rational convictions, and value systems. The soapAOpera as an art form.--(l) The structure of the soap opera is that of continuing episodes without beginning, middle, and end. It maintains its unity through a rather permanent cast of characters, permanent settings, and a theme or variations of one theme. (2) The form of the soap opera is unique in three ways: the emphasis on dialogue instead of action, the "slower-than-life“ pace and movement, and the reviewing of what-happened-yesterday that often occupies as much as five of the approximate 22 minutes of plot time. (3) Because of its leisurely pace, the soap opera is allowed time for detailed develOpment of 11 characters, numerous subplots and incidental action, and the minutiae so often found in real life. (4) Conflict in all ranges of human problems and emotions keeps the plot and subplots constantly at a boil. (5) The standard lit- erary devices most frequently used in the soap opera are the flashback, interior monologue, and dream sequence. Figurative language is confined mostly to dramatic irony. (6) There is little humor in the lives of the characters. Although tragedy does occur, the survivors usually manage to work out satisfactory lives eventually. Katzman (1973) examined the size and character— istics of the soap opera audience, the situations the soaps portray, and the characters that populate them, and dis- cusses some potential implications of all those. On the average, every adult viewer in the United States sees two hours of soap Operas-every week. The growth in viewer-hours can be explained almost totally as a function of the steady growth in the number of homes with television and the growth in the number of minutes of serials broadcast each day. This indicates an "elastic demand" for soap operas: as the population grows and the number of homes with television increases, a fairly stable proportion of the new potential viewers will turn to the 12 daytime serials. As new soap operas are added to daytime schedules each one seems able to attract an audience with- out taking viewers away from other serials. This elastic demand appears to hold even when the networks schedule three serials in the same time period. Sometimes when three soaps compete with each other, they reach 30 per cent of all households with television, a rating as high as the highest rated prime time program. Katzman found 71 per cent of the serial audience composed of adult females. Education of the head of house- hold was inversely related to the tendency to watch soap operas; the serials were most popular among the low-income, low-education groups. The most typical viewer of daytime serials was a southern or midwestern woman from a large household with relatively low educational and income levels. The aLmost-realism of the characters and themes, the repe- tition due to slow pace, and the extremely large number of hours spent viewing soap Operas indicate that these shows have great potential power. They can establish or rein- force value systems. They can suggest how people should act in certain situations. They can 1egitimize behavior and remove taboos about discussing sensitive such topics as drugs and premarital sex. 13 Edmondson and Rounds (1973) looked at the soap opera viewer. They found that in 1972, 96 per cent of all American households had television sets and 53 per cent of them were color. Thirty per cent of all television house- holds watch daytime television with 18.6 million viewers tuned in. Chances were four to one that the viewer was a woman since fewer than 20 per cent of daytime viewers are men. Seventy-six per cent of nonworking women watch TV in the daytime and 54 per cent of the working females still find time to view the set during the day.- Sixty-five per cent of all women watch some daytime television--a per— centage that has remained quite constant since 1968.’ The "average" woman watches television Monday through Friday between 10 a.m. and S p.m. foreaneaverage of over seven hours each week. WOmen between 18~and‘50 prefer soap Operas to game shows, and they watch them a little over three times a week, more.in the fall, a little less during the winter, and the least during the summer. There has been no consistent mode of studies on soap operas. It is a much unexplored field. The only viewer-sample study was done by Herzog over 30 years ago. The Katzman study was solely a content analysis of char- acters and content. The other studies and articles were 14 4 simply discussions by sociologists and psychologists about some aspect of the serials. Soap Opera Reality and Gratifications Dr. Louis I. Berg, a New York psychiatrist and writer, was a man who hated soap operas. His crusade against them began in 1941, when he developed the suspicion that certain symptoms manifested by his patients, certain disturbing relapses, might have their origin in addiction to radio serials. Setting out to test his hypothesis that such symptoms as tachycardia (rapid pulse rate), arrhythmia (distorted heart beat), emotional instability, and vertigo (fear of heights) might be produced by soap operas, he found just what he had set out to look for. After a daily dose of two leading exemplars, Right to Happiness and Women in White, Dr. Berg tested his own blood pressure and found it rising. Serials, he concluded on this evidence, are dangerous, especially to the middle-aged woman, the ado- lescent, and the neurotic. They furnish to those unfortu- nate addicts "the same release for the emotionally dis- torted that is supplied to those who derive satisfaction from a lynching bee, who lick their lips at the salacious 15 scandals of crime passionnel, who in the unregretted past cried out in ecstasy at a witch burning." The subject of all this tempestuous verbiage was then, as now, a humble art form, meek and unconfident, looked down upon by the cultivated and even sometimes by its own creators. But though it has often been snubbed and publicly humiliated, it has always been beloved by millions who have found it indispensable to their private well being.3 What is it in soap operas that can arouse such loyalty from their audiences and such opposition from their critics? What is the secret of their addictive powers that cause viewers to become so involved in the lives of the characters and their problems? Such questions as these will be partly explored in this study. The specific purposes of this study were to examine viewers' and nonviewers' perceptions of the reality of soap opera characters and content, to explore viewers' degree of interaction, identification, and involvement with soap operas, their knowledge of program content, and tc-examine four functions (companionship, boredom, escapism, and 3Edmondson and Rounds, p. 15. 16 social utility) possibly satisfied by watching daytime serials. The amount of viewing is the independent variable in this study. The dependent variables are-knowledge of soap opera content; interaction--stimulation of the viewer by characters and/or content of the programming'which moti- vates the viewer to make a response, comments on or dis- cussion of the program with others; identification--imagin- ing oneself in the place of a soap opera character; and involvement--how important watching soaps are to the viewer. Four other variables are possible functions satisfied by watching soap operas: companionship, relief of boredom, escapism, and social utility. Perceived reality (the ex- tent to which the viewer thinks that program content and characters resemble peOple and events in real life) is the intervening variable in that it may affect the relationship between the independent and certain dependent variables. Perceived reality is a variable explored to deter- mine its potential impact on the viewer. According to Greenberg and Reeves (1974), general use of television is strongly related to general measures of perceived reality of television. One may anticipate that people who watch the medium with some regularity will generate stronger 17 estimates of perceived reality. This media realism has been researched in connection with television impact on children (Greenberg, 1973; Reeves, 1974; Greenberg and Reeves, 1974; McLeod, Atkin and Chaffe, 1971; Ward, 1971; Greenberg, 1971; Greenberg and Dominick, 1970) adults (Dervin and Greenberg, 1973; and Greenberg, Vlahos and Ericson, 1971), aggression stimulation (Feshback, 1971), and violence (Gerbner, 1969; Dominick, 1972; and Clark and Blankenburg, 1971). The bulk of this research was con- cerned with the effects of prime time programming. Identification, interaction, and involvement are three variables postulated by Rosengren and Windahl (1972) as important in examining mass media consumption and mo- tives. They examined mass media content, identification with the content, and interaction (imaginary) with the same content. Identification and interaction take place between the individual and a media character. Identification is defined as "imagining oneself in the place of another person," and interaction is defined as "mutual stimulation and response." The relation between interaction and iden- tification defines the degree of involvement (with mass media content). The authors argued that the need for interaction may be satisfied by almost any type of media 18 content. They divided media content into (1) fictional and (2) as supplying explicit and concrete information (fac- tual). Any media content can be either or both or none. The cross-break between factual and fictional content yield the values of another variable--degree of reality proximity of media content. Their findings supported the following hypotheses: 1) There is a positive correlation between the degree of involvement and amount of consumption; and 2) a high degree of involvement will tend to go together with preference for and consumption of content with low degree of reality proximity (where identification and interaction are easier to establish). Functions satisfied by soap opera viewing intro- duced here are relief of boredom, escapism, social utility, and companionship. Danowski ("Functions and-Gratifications of Soap Opera Viewing: Some Operationalizations," 1973) discussed operationalizations of nine functions and grati- fications of soap Opera viewing. The four functions pre- sented here are taken from Greenberg, 1973 (learning, re- lief of boredom, relief of stress habit, identification, social utility, stability, arousal/emotional release, and para-social interaction) and from the eight clusters of 19 responses as to why people watch television also demon— strated by Greenberg (1973). Knowledge of program content and characters has been a sparsely researched variable. Questions of this nature have usually been reserved to measure perceived reality of a specific character, mentioning the character by name, but not asking specific questions about characters and events in programming. Perceived reality is dependent upon viewing for its existence and measurement. We posit that it determines how susceptible the viewer is to interaction, identification, and involvement. The closer to reality the viewer be- lieves the content and characters to be, the mOre the viewer will become involved with the programming. Perceived reality also determines how likely one is to feel some type of gratification through exposure to the-medium. Therefore, viewing predicts the seven variables as mediated by per- ceived reality. The following are original hypotheses for this study. H1: The higher the amount of viewing, the greater the perceived reality. The greater the amount of time and the more repeated the exposures, the more likely 20 the viewer will see the medium's content as being very much like real life. H2: The higher the amount of viewing, the more knowledge the viewer will have about program content. The greater the length of time the viewer is exposed to soap Operas, the more familiar he/she will become with the content. H3 : The higher the amount of viewing, the greater the viewer's involvement. H3a : The greater the perceived reality, the 1 greater the viewer's involvement. H3b: The higher the amount of viewing, the greater the viewer's interaction. H3b . The greater the perceived reality, the 1 greater the viewer's interaction. H3c: The higher the amount of viewing, the greater the viewer's identification. H : The greater the perceived reality, the greater the viewer's identification. As the viewer spends more and more time-watching the serials, he/she will develOp more and more interest (becoming more curious) in the lives of the characters and their situation, empathizing to the point of placing one- self in similar circumstances and discussing such events and alternatives with others who share the same curiosity about soap operas. H4 The higher the amount of viewing, the more companionship the viewer will seek from soap Operas. a: 21 H4a : The greater the perceived reality, the 1 more companionship the viewer will seek from soap operas. H4b: The higher the amount of viewing, the ’ more the viewer will seek escapism in soap operas. 4b : The greater the perceived reality, the 1 more the viewer will seek escapism in soap Operas. H40: The greater the amount of viewing, the more relief from boredom the viewer will seek from soap operas. H4C : The greater the perceived reality, the 1 more relief from boredom the viewer will seek from soap operas. H4d: The greater the amount of viewing time, the more social utility the viewer will find in soap operas. H4d : The greater the perceived reality, the 1 more social utility the viewer will find in soap Operas. As the viewer becomes addicted to the serials, he/she will retreat to them as a break in the daily rou- tine, to replace absent company, and to observe social behaviors. 22 a.mmanmflnm> cow3uoa mumflxm coeumamuuoo m pmcu mpmoatcfl waso m3OHH¢ usmfio>ao>sa cOHDMOAMHusOOH soauomumusfl s 7 SOHOMHQ monocuommm A moccasocx \ ; 1\ asses»: AmGOHuocsmv HMHOOm Emflmmomm EOOOHOQ magmGOHcmmEoo M muwamwu to>awoumm Ocaxma> unsoem "muozv CHAPTER II METHODS This study was drawn entirely from original data collected in August, 1974, in the greater Lansing, Michigan, area. The methods used in data gathering and analysis will be discussed in the following order: 1) pretest and ques- tionnaire administration, 2) the respondents and their community, 3) operationalization of the independent vari- able, 4) operationalization of the dependent variables, and 5) statistical analyses. :re\1feSt and Questionnaire wstration Pretest telephone interviews were first conducted 7 August 1974. Twenty-five telephone numbers were randomly Selected from the Michigan State University telephone di- rectory. Eleven of the 25 completed interviews. Answers to questions concerning involvement, identification, and lntereaction determined the coding categories and wording 23 24 for the final instrument. The four functions measured in the final questionnaire were those most frequently men- tioned by respondents in the pretest (Appendix A). The final questionnaire was administered 12-15 August 1974. Five hundred fifty names were randomly drawn from the Lansing, Michigan, telephone directory. Nine trained interviewers made calls from 12 noon until 4 p.m. each day, amassing responses from 272 people, 157 of who were viewers and 115 of whom were nonviewers. Telephoning results were 272 completed calls (49%), 188 refusals (21%), 128 no-answers or busy signals (23%), 30 disconnected numbers (5%), and two miscellaneous. Seventy-three of the 272 completed interviews were call- backs of previous busy or no-answer numbers. There were 390 contacted respondents of whom 70% yielded interviews. The questionnaire took approximately 20 minutes for completion and all questions were read to the respondent and answers recorded by the interviewer. Only measures of perceived reality and demographic information were obtained from nonviewers. The first person who answered the tele- phone was interviewed unless that person declined in favor of someone else in the household who did watch soap operas. After the interviewer read the introduction and the 25 instructions for question one, the respondent indicated whether or not he/she watched daytime television and/or soap operas. The entire questionnaire is in Appendix B. Respondents and Their Community The total sample consisted of 55 males and 216 females, ranging in age from the teens to the eighties with over half the sample in their thirties or younger. All the respondents came from the Lansing, Michigan area, including East Lansing, Meridian, Haslett, and Okemos. People in the sample came from a variety of socioeconomic backgrounds with a fairly even spread among income and education. Lansing is located in the central part of Michigan and has a large number of industries. Its suburbs offer faculty, staff, and students from Michigan State University and other professional people. Consequently, respondents come from a variety of backgrounds. Demographics and Viewing Table 1 shows that both viewers and-nonviewers in this sample were predominantly female (87% and 65% TABLE l.--Demographics of Viewers and Nonviewers (Chi-Square Test) 26 Total . . Nonv1ewers Viewers Sample Sex: male 55 36 19 female 216 79 137 x2 = 14.96, p < .001, df = 1 Age: teens 41 8 33 20s 79 32 47 303 36 16 20 403 29 19 10 503 + 80 39 41 x2 = 16.67, p < .01, df = 4 Education: below high school 64 14 50 completed high school 88 34 54 some college 55 28 27 college degree 35 25 10 graduate or professional 22 13 9 degree x2 = 28.64, p < .001, df = 4 Income: student 26 6 20 below $5,000 18 12 6 $5,000-$10,000 33 15 18 $10,000-$15,000 65 27 38 $15,000-$20,000 35 15 20 over $20,000 21 13 8 retired 34 ll 23 x2 = 13.11, p < .05, df = 6 Marital Status: single 93 37 56 married 158 69 89 widow l8 8 10 x2 = 0.4329, p < .90, df = 2 27 respectively); 34% of the viewers completed high school compared to 30% for nonviewers; and 40% of viewers and 49% of nonviewers had incomes of more than $10,000. Half of the viewers and 35% of the nonviewers were below age 30, and 60% of both were married. Table 2 indicates that the "typical" soap opera viewer is a female of no particular age group whose head- of-household is in the middle income bracket (between $10,000 and $20,000), and has a high school education. Katzman (1973) characterized the "typical viewer" as a southern or midwestern woman from a large household with relatively low educational and income levels. Size of household and area of the country were not examined in this study. Operational Definitions The amount of viewing was used as a predictor for nine variables: perceived reality (which was also used as a mediator between the independent variable and seven other dependent variables), knowledge of soap opera content and characters, involvement, interaction, identification, companionship, boredom, escapism, and social utility. 28 TABLE 2.--Demographics and Amount of Viewing (Chi-Square Test) 1-2 Shows 3-4 Shows 6+ Shows Sex: male 10 7 2 female 45 48 44 x2 4.54, p < .20, df = 2 Age: teens l4 l6 3 203 16 l7 13 308 6 6 8 40s 4 2 4 50s + 12 l3 16 x2 = 12.36, p < .20, df = 8 Education: below high school l4 l9 17 completed high school 15 19 20 some college or more 23 15 7 x2 = 10.25, p < .05, df = 4 Income: student 9 8 3 below $10,000 11 3 10 $10,000-$15,000 11 13 14 $15,000 or more 9 11 8 retired 8 8 7 x2 = 13.65, p < .10, df = 8 Marital Status: single 20 27 9 married 30 27 32 x2 = 9.35, p < .01, df = 2 29 Demographic variables (age, sex, marital status, income, and education) were also examined as predictors of these dependent variables. Independent Variable The amount of 'viewing time was measured by the number of different soap operas viewed per week and by the total number of episodes viewed per week. Respondents were asked which shows they watched and how many times a week they watched. This was done to establish a compara— tive frequency of how often each show was viewed weekly. (The correlation between watching shows and episodes is .77.) Viewers were also asked "How long have you been watching soap operas?" to determine the amount of viewing in years. This correlated with shows and episodes as .24 and .27, respectively. Perceived Reality Perceived reality was measured by reading the respondent nine statements to which he/she answered "agree, disagree, or not sure." 30 "Soap operas on TV tell about life the way it really is." "The same things that happen to people on soap operas happen to people in real life." "The places I see in soap operas are just like places in real life." "People in soap operas are just like people in real life." "Families on soap operas are just like families in real life." "Doctors on soap operas are just like doc- tors in real life." "Marriages on soap operas are just like marriages in real life." "Problems people have in soap operas are just like problems people have in real life." "Is there any soap opera character who reminds you of yourself?" Scores for the individual items were summed for a total perceived reality score. Measures were coded as agree = 3, not sure = 2, disagree = l. The variable "character like self" was dropped from the perceived reality index because of its poor correlations with the other variables. The final index was composed of eight variables. Intercorrelations among perceived reality items in the total sample (Table 3), viewers (Table 4), and 31 TABLE 3.--Perceived Reality Correlations (Total Sample) (N = 272) O m H m Hr-l O O O O O O o O m m E D m (n m O O -a H «0 O 0 4J (D H H O ~r-l H (00) H c o o 44 p H .o H.x O O O o E o H o O a .u > r1 O O o O H .2 q 0 an a. m m o 2 n. L) a Life .32 .33 47 .43 .29 .40 .29 .04 .68 (X'= 1.6) Events .18 .32 .26 .16 .34 .38 .04 .60 (X'= 2.2) Places .38 .41 .29 .22 .21 .08 .60 (X = 2.0) People .38 .30 .43 .44 -.01 .75 (X = 1.9) Families .40 .49 .36 .05 .75 (X = 1.7) Doctors .30 .19 .02 .34 (Y = 1.5) Marriages .36 -.06 .66 (X = 1.8) Problems -.006 .60 (X'= 2.3) Character Like Self .15 (X = 1.2) (ifi= 16.2) 32 TABLE 4.--Perceived Reality Correlations (Viewers) (N = 157) O H m o O O m o O m m g m m O O -H H O O JJ (D H H 0 ml H H c o o. -H H H .o O O O o g o H o H > H O o O H o m m m m o z m a Life .34 .44 42 44 .24 31 .33 70 Events .25 .36 .29 .16 .42 .50 .61 Places .40 .47 .28 .31 .30 .65 People .56 .24 .45 .39 '.72 Families .45 .51 .35 .73 Doctors .32 .17 .53 Marriages .38 .73 Problems .62 (Y = 15.6) nonviewers (Table 5) are fairly consistent with the excep- tion of the variable "doctors." This variable correlates higher with the other perceived reality variables among nonviewers than it does among viewers and the total sample- 33 TABLE 5.--Perceived Reality Correlations (Nonviewers) (N = 115) Events Places People Families Doctors Marriages Problems Total Score Life .29 .15 55 .40 .40 .26 .21 .61 Events .12 .26 .21 .19 .25 .25 .53 Places .37 .30 .34 .13 .13 .49 People .60 .41 .43 .51 .80 Families .33 .45 .37 .72 Doctors .31 .26 .61 Marriages .37 .63 Problems .62 (X = 14.2) Table 6 shows that viewers have high perceived reality (above the scale's-midpoint)"fortproblemsx(X==2.5)r events (Y = 2.3), places (Y = 2.1), and.people (Y’= 2-0). Nonviewers have high reality perceptions for only events (X = 2.1) and problems (X = 2.0). TABLE 6.--Perceived Reality Item Frequency Viewers (N 157) 34 Nonviewers (N 115) Viewers Nonviewers 3 2 l 3 2 1 Life 37 31 88 (X = 1.7) 18 19 80 (X 1.5) Events 80 27 39 (X = 2.3) 51 28 36 (X 2.1) Places 69 32 54 (X = 2.1) 35 41 35 (X 1.9) People 68 22 66 (X = 2.0) 34 26 55 (X 1.7) Families 53 22 81 (X = 1.8) 21 30 62 (X 1.5) Doctors 30 28 98 (X = 1.6 10 42 65 (X 1.5) Marriages 47 32 74 (X = 1.8) 30 26 59 (X 1.7) Problems 99 ~31 26 (X = 2.5) 50 32 31 (X 2.0) Dividing perceived reality scores into Low PRTV (range eight to 15) and High PRTV (range 16 to 24), Table 7 shows that 48 per cent of the viewers and 64 per cent of the nonviewers fall into the low perceived reality cate- gory. The mean perceived reality score for the two groups is not statistically different, 15.6 for viewers and 14.1 for nonviewers. 35 TABLE 7.—-Perceived Reality Score Frequency Viewers (N = 157) Nonviewers (N = 115) Viewers Nonviewers Total Score Frequency Percentage Frequency Percentage 8 10 6% 7 6% 9 5 3% 6 5% 10 8 5% 4 3% 11 8 5% 6 5% 12 22 14% 18 - 15% 13 7 5% 11 9% 14 6 4% 12 10% 15 10 6% 13 11% 16 12 8% 12 10% 17 13 8% 4 3% 18 9 6% 6 5% 19 5 4% 3 3% 20 11 7% 1 1% 21 8 5% 5 4% 22 10 6% 4 3% 23 5 3% — - 24 7 5% 3 3% 2’: 15.6 X = 14.2 s.d. = 4.75 s.d. = 7.94 36 Knowledge The variable knowledge about soap opera characters and content was measured by having respondents answer ques- tions about the serials he/she claimed to watch. The ques- tionnaire contained three questions about each of the 14 soaps. 02 For example: The Young and the Restless Who is the father of Sally McQuire's baby? Who is responsible for Leslie Brooks' emotional breakdown? What is Brad Elliot's real profession? All My Children 3. Who is the father of Tara Martin Tyler's son Phillip? Who killed Jason Maxwell? Why did Ann Martin abruptly leave Pine Valley? Another World ' Who caused Alice Frame's emotional breakdown? To whom was Rachael Frame married when Jamie was born? Why was Steven Frame sent to prison? The possible range for correct answers per show was from 0 to 3. The possible range for total correct answers was from 0 to 42. 37 Table 8 shows that viewers averaged 2.1 correct answers per show with 72 per cent of the sample between two and three correct answers per show. The highest number of total correct answers was 30, by one viewer who watched 10 soaps. Forty-six per cent of the viewers answered 10 or more questions correctly but only 12 per cent correctly answered 15 or more questions. Involvement Involvement was measured by asking the respondent the following two questions. An index was then formed by summing the two responses. "How important are the soap operas to you would you say?" 3 very important 2 somewhat important not very important "How involved do you get watching your favorite soap Opera?" 3 very involved 2 somewhat involved 1 not very involved Intercorrelation among involvement items is .46 (Table 9); however, frequencies and mean indicate viewers have little involvement with the serials (Table 10). TABLE 8.--Know1edge (N = 157) Correct Answers Frequency Total Number Frequency Per Show Correct Answers .0 7 0 7 .l l 1 6 .3 3 2 13 .4 l 3 27 .5 4 4 14 .6 l 5 8 .8 l 6 7 1.0 11 7 1 1.2 1 8 8 1.3 4 9 9 1.4 l 10 4 1.5 6 11 8 1.8 2 12 10 2.0 27 13 2 2.1 l 14 5 2.2 6 15 9 2.3 5 l6 4 2.4 4 17 2- 2.5 15 18 2 2.6 5 19 2 2.7 7 20 2 2.8 10 21 l 2.9 l 23 1 3.0 32 28 2 30 1 X'= 2.1 (s.d. = 8.78) X'= 8.6(s.d.==9.l6) 39 Nb. hm. mm. mm. BZH mm. on. hm. NH. he. he. mm. OHOHOOHOQO HOOQO MHH03 av. NN. coHuOsuHm HOHHEwm ca maOm OchOEH mm. mm. usOEO>Ho>sH om. mm. mHOHOOHOso o» HHmu Nm. mHOHOOHOto HOOQO hHHos mHOHOOHOso OH xHOu xHosuOc OH cOuuHH3 mm. OEOHQOHQ OatcOn HOHOOHOBO mO MHOw OchOEH we. msflnouO3 COflHUNHOT-ZHH UGO HHOBHOO OH sOuuHHz OOHHOOHOHHH OOHHOOHHm HOHHEHO cw MHOm OchOEH HOHOOHOBO mO maOm OchOEH HOHOOHOOO OxHH On aOHumonHuamoH mchOHOS OOOOHHOQEH ucOEO>Ho>cH zv AOGOHHOHOHHOOHOHGHV .GOHHOOHHHHOOOH .usOEO>Ho>cHII.m names IRE Lu — VI). 40 Identification Identification was measured-by three questions from which an index was formed (yes = 2, no_= 1). "Are there any characters you would like to be like?" "Do you ever imagine yourself as one of the characters?" "Do you ever imagine yourself in a similar situation that a particular character faces?" The mean for identification was 3.8 (Table 10) although it correlates well with involvement and interac- tion (Table 9), and very well among its separate items. Interaction Interaction was measured by four questions'which were also summed for an index. "Do you ever think about telling some char- acters how to handle their problems?" "Have you written to the network or any place about a show?" "Do you ever find yourself talking to the characters while the show is on?" The interaction mean for viewers was 5.5 and interaction correlated well with both involvement and identification. 41 1 TABLE 10.--Involvement, Identification, and Interaction (Frequency) (N = 157) Involvement (X = 3.5) value frequency 2 50 3 31 4 37 5 22 6 14 Identification (X'= 3.8) value frequengy 3 74 4 47 5 28 6 4 Interaction (X = 5.5) value frequency 4 3S 5 44 6 39 7 35 8 0 Correlations Among Indices identification interaction invOlvement .37 » .50 identification .36 ,42 Functions The four functions were operationalized as follows and indices were formed for each. Response categories were coded as agree = 3, not sure = 2, disagree = 1. Boredom--"Watching soap operas helps me to pass the time." "I watch soap operas because I have nothing better to do." These two items correlate .15. The first correlates insignificantly with the boredom index (r = .09) and the second correlates highly .78.. This index correlates with the companionship, escapism, and social utility indices .20, .18, and .55 respectively, indicating that viewers who seek relief from boredom from the serials are also more likely to find social utility in the soaps as well. Companionshi -—"I watch soap operas when there's no one to talk to." "Watching soap operas makes me feel less lonely." These two items correlate .43. The first corre- lates with the index .26 and the second .23. 'Companionship correlates with boredom, escapism, and social utility .20, .28, and .32 respectively (Table 11). 43 TABLE ll.--Functions (N = 157) Boredom (X = 3.0) Companionship (X'= 4.1) value frequency value freguengy 2 78 2 44 3 8 3 9 4 52 4 37 5 9 5 10 6 6 6 53 Escapism (X'= 3.8) Social Utility (X'= 2.6) value frequengy, value frequency 2 57 l S 3 10 2 100 4 36 3 21 5 14 4 13 6 36 5 15 Correlations Among Indices companionship escapism- boredom .20 .18 companionship .28 escapism social utility .55 .32 .21 44 Escapism--"Watching soap operas helps me to escape from my daily routine." "Watching soap-operas takes me away for a little while." These two items correlate .45 and with‘thetindex.29 and .24 respectively. Escapism correlates~witheboredom, companionship, and social utility .18, .28, and .21 respec- tively. Social Utilitye-"Do you get any useful information from the soap operas you watch?" "Watching soap operas helps me to deal with other people." (The first question was coded as yes = 1 and no = l; the second as agree = 3, not sure = 2, disagree = 1.) These two items correlate .41 and .72 and .92 with the index. Social utility correlates with boredom,es- capism, and companionship .55, .21, and .32 respectively. Analysis All questionnaire data were transferred to com- puter cards for analysis using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences. In testing the hypotheses, four statistical analyses were used: the Pearson product moment 45 correlations, the chi-square test, partial correlations, and one-way analysis of variance. CHAPTER III RESULTS The results of this study are presented according to each hypothesis.4 Perceived Reality, H1: The higher the amount of viewing, the greater the perceived reality. Table 12 shows this hypothesis is confirmed. Overall perceived reality scores correlate positively with the number of different shows (.24), the number of epi- sodes (.16), and the number of years the viewer has watched soap operas (.26). All three viewing measures correlated consistently best with perceived reality measures of life (r = .24, .29, and .28), people (r = .24, .29, and .21), problems (r = .22, .21 and .21), and families (r = .19, .26, and .25). f 4For viewers (N = 150, i 5), correlations above .14 are significant at p < .05 and correlations above .18 are significant at p < .01. For nonviewers (N = 115) correla- tions above .18 are significant at p < .05 and correlations above .20 are significant at p < .01. 46 47 TABLE 12.--Viewing and Perceived Reality— (N = 157) Viewing Perceived Reality shows episodes years Life .24 .29 .28 Events .11 .06 .12 Places .04 .08 .24 People .24 .29 .21 Families .19 .26 .25 Doctors -.02 .03 .10 Marriages .10 .15 .19 Problems .22 .21 .21 Index .24 .16 .26 Edmondson and Rounds (1973) found that the viewer spent seven hours a week with the serials. This study showed viewers watched an average of 3.8 different soaps a week and 14.0 episodes (seven hours), and had watched daytime dramas for over eight years. Perceived reality was higher for older viewers (r = .25) and viewers of lower education (r = -.18). For nonviewers, perceived reality was higher for older viewers 48 (r = .34). There were no significant differences across sex, income, and marital status for viewers-and none across sex, income, marital status, and education for nonviewers. Perceived Reality Index Demographics . . Viewers nonV1ewers Sex -.12 .17 Age .25 .34 Marital Status -.01 .08 Income -.02 .00 Education -.18 -.11 Knowledge H2: The higher the amount of viewing time, the more knowledge the viewer will have about program content. This hypothesis is true for viewing time measured by the number of different shows watched correlated with the total number of correct answers (r = .47); and by the number of episodes correlated with correct answers per show and total correct answers (r = .20 and r = .44, respec- tively. However, the number of years watching soap operas correlates -.02 with average correct answers and .06 with total correct answers--both insignificant. 49 Average Correct Total Correct Demographics Answers Per ShOW' (r = .49) ' AnSWers Sex .35 .25 Age .00 ‘ .00 Marital Status .07 .02 Income .07 —.03 Education .13 .14 Viewers who had more knowledge about program con- tent were females with low perceived reality levels. There were no differences in age, income, marital status, and education. Involvement, Interaction, and Identification H3: The higher the amount of viewing, the greater the viewer's involvement, identification, and interaction. Table 13 shows viewers who have higher amounts of viewing have greater involvement (r = .46, r = .46, and r = .13), identification (r = .19, r = .14, and r = .11), and interaction (r = .21, r .28, and r = .02). Involve- ment was greater for lower-income (r = —.16) and lower- educated viewers (r = -.17). Identification was greater (among males (r = —.15), single viewers (r = -.19), and \riewers (r -.25). Interaction was greater for single \riewers (r -.23), and lower-income viewers (r = —.16). 50 TABLE 13.--Viewing and Involvement, Interaction, and Identification (N = 157) Involvement Interaction Identification Shows .46 .21 .19 Episodes .46 .28 .14 Years .13 .02 .11 Demographics Involvement Interaction Identification Sex .02 -.03 —.16 Age -.03 -.14 -.09 Marital Status -.17 -.24 -.14 Income .00 -.17 -.22 Education -.17 -.10 -.10 Boredom, Companionship, Escapism, and Social Utility H4: The higher the amount of viewing, the more functions watching soap operas will satisfy. Viewers with higher amounts of viewing found grat- ification for social utility (r = .20, .23, and .15), but not for companionship, boredom, and escapism (Table 14). Only less-educated viewers sought relief from boredom iflrrough soap operas (r = -.25) and found social utility in 'the soaps (r = -.25). Escapism seekers were older (r == .31) and married (r = .18). 51 TABLE 14.--Viewing and Functions (N = 157) Boredom Com anionshi Esca ism -Social 9 P p Utility Shows .03 -.02 .11 .20 Episodes .02 -.03 .13 .23 Years .03 .12 .11 .15 Social Demographics Boredom Companionship Escapism Utility Sex -.10 -.02 -.03 -.08 Age .03 .06 .32 .15 Marital Status -.12 .04 .18 .07 Income -.04 -.03 .05 .04 Education -.25 .04 -.08 -.25 Perceived Reality Correlations Viewers were divided into two groups according to their perceived reality scores (range: eight to 24). High PRTV viewers (N = 81) were those whose scores were between 16 and 24 (Table 15). Low PRTV viewers (N = 76) were those whose scores were for eight through 15. Viewers with high perceived reality of television had more knowledge of soap operas in terms of correct answers per show (r = .18 and .25) than did viewers with low perceived reality of television (r = .11 and .23). However, the latter group had better correlations between 52 TABLE 15.—-High and Low PRTV (N = 157) Shows Episodes Years Hi PRTV Lo PRTV Hi Low Hi Low Knowledge answers per show .18 .11 .25 .23 .09 -.02 total correct .31 .77 .29 .72 .02 .15 Involvement .48 .43 .57 .34 .15 .10 Identification .11 .34 .06 .28 .14 .14 Interaction .23 .28 .27 .28 .01 .14 Boredom .10 -.07 .08 -.06 .09 -.10 Companionship .02 .01 .15 -.14 .19 .08 Escapism .08 .16 .20 .07 .11 .09 Social Utility .25 .13 .31 .12 .23 .01 viewing and total correct answers across all shows with r = .77 and .72 compared to r = .31 and .29 for the former group. High PRTV viewers found more involvement (r = .48, .57, and .15), interaction (r = .23 and .27), companionship (r = .15 and .19), and social utility (r = .25, .31, and .23) in soap operas. Low PRTV viewers found more identi— fication (r = .34, .28, and .14). Boredom was insignifi- cant for both groups while escapism was significant with episodes (r = .20) for high PRTV viewers and with shows for low PRTV viewers. ’53 NH. Hm. mH. mH. mm. om. suHHHuo HmHuom no. om. Ha. OH. mo. Ha. EmfloOomm ea.1 ma. NH. Ho. me. no.1 mflnmsowsOmEou $0.1 mo. mo. no.1 oa. mo. EOUOHom mm. hm. mm. mm. mm. mm. COHHOOHOHGH mm. mo. ea. em. HH. ma. OOHHOOHMHHOOOH em. am. me. me. we. we. HOOEO>Ho>cH mm. mm. we. we. am. he. mHO3ch HOOHHOO HOHOH mm. mm. om. Ha. ma. oa. sonm HOQ mHO3ch OOOOazosM >Bmm on >Bmm Hm HHOHO>O ..>Bmm on >Bmm Hm HHOHO>O mOOomHom macaw AsmH u zv AOOOOOHQM1m3oan msoHHOHOHHOU muHHOOm OO>HOOHOm11.mH mqmde 54 This analysis provides support for perceived re- ality of television as an intervening variable. Respondents with greater amounts of viewing had higher perceived re— ality scores; high PRTV viewers found more involvement, companionship, and social utility in the serials. Thus, perceived reality mediates the relationship between the independent variable and four of the dependent variables. Partial Correlations Variables examined were amount of viewing (separ- ately for shows, episodes, and years), perceived reality (index), involvement, identification, interaction, boredom, companionship, escapism, and social utility. Table 17 represents analysis done by correlating viewing and per- ceived reality and partialing out the dependent variable. Table 18 represents analysis done by correlating viewing time with the dependent variable and partialing out per- ceived reality. Table 17 indicates a fairly strong relationship be- tween viewing and perceived reality, and that the other dependent variables have little effect on this relation- ship. TABLE 17.--Partia1 Correlations 55 (Viewing by Perceived Reality) (N = 157) Control Variables Shows Episodes Years Involvement .26 .05 .33 Identification .31 .14 .25 Interaction .24 .16 .26 Boredom .24 .16 .27 Companionship .24 .16 .26 Escapism .23 .14 .25 Social Utility .19 .09 .23 TABLE 18.--Partial Correlations (Viewing by Dependent Variables, Controlling for Perceived Reality) (N = 157) Shows Episodes Years Involvement .40 .16 .05 Identification .04 .12 .44 Interaction .09 .07 .05 Boredom -.04 .06 .07 Companionship -.04 .11 -.37 Escapism .10 .19 .11 Social Utility .13 .19 .11 56 Table 18 shows that perceived reality mediates the relationship between (1) viewing (years) and involvement; (2) viewing (shows and episodes) and identification; (3) viewing (shows, episodes, years) and interaction; (4) viewing (shows, episodes, years) and boredom; (5) view- ing (shows and episodes) and companionship; (6) viewing (shows and years) and escapism; and (7) viewing (shows and years) and social utility. This analysis refers to the correlation between viewing time and each dependent vari- able after the portion in both that can be predicted from perceived reality has been eliminated. Thus, the above were no longer significantly correlated when perceived reality was removed. As an intervening variable, perceived reality shows that as the amount of viewing time increases, and perceived reality increases, the viewer will have (1) more interac- tion with soap opera characters and content, and (2) more relief from boredom. One-Way Analysis of Variance A one-way analysis of variance was done with two measures of viewing, the number of shows and the number of 57 episodes watched. Viewing was divided into low (1-2’shows or 1-7 episodes), medium (3-5 shows or 8—16 episodes), and high (6 or more shows or 17 or more episodes). Table C1 (Appendix C) shows that among respondents who watched a greater number of different shows, perceived reality, knowledge of soap operas content, involvement, identification, interaction, escapism, and social utility were higher. There were no significant differences across boredom and companionship. Table C2 (Appendix C) shows that among respondents who watched a higher number of episodes, perceived reality, knowledge, involvement, interaction, and social utility were higher. Relief from boredom was the same for medium and high viewers; identification was higher for medium viewers, and companionship was higher for low viewers. 58 HmH. u H .GOHHOOHOHGH x MHHHOOH OO>HOOHOQV mo. no. mo. >Bmm mcHHHOHusoo 1 mm. mm. >Bmm OH 1 mm. mm. >Bmm H: No. mm. Hm. GOHHOHOHHOO HOOHO1OHON OOHHOOHOHGH AwH. u H .coHHOOHMHHcOUH x AHHHOOH OO>HOOHOmv we. NH. no. >Bmm mcHHHOHHsoo 1 mm. em. >Bmm oH 1 Go. HH. >53 E HH. HH. mH. OOHHOHOHHOO HOOHOIOHON coHHOOHMHusOOH AHm. u H .HGOEO>Ho>cH x muHHOOH OO>HOOHOQV mo. OH. ow. >Bmm mcHHHOHHcoo 1 em. me. >Bmm OH 1 pm. we. >Bmm H: mH. we. we. OOHHOHOHHOO HOOHO1OHON HGOEO>HO>OH mHOOw mOOomHom mzosm ASH n E OGOHHOHOHHOU HHOHO>O11.mH MHmHB 59 H .mHHHHHs HOHOOO x HHHHOOH OO>HOOHOoV >emm mcHHHoupcoo >Bmm OH >Bmm Hg OOHHOHOHHOO HOOHO1OHON MHHHHHD HOHOOm AmH. u H .EmHmOOmO x MHHHOOH OO>HOOHOQV >emm mcHHHOHHcOO >Bmm OH >amm Hz OOHHOHOHHOO HOOHO1OHON EmHmOomm H .oHnmsoHcOoEoo x MHHHOOH OO>HOOHOQV >Bmm mcHHHOHHcoo >Bmm OH >emm Ha OOHHOHOHHOO HOOHO1OHON QHHOGOHOOQEOU Hem. u H .EOOOHOQ x muHHOOH OO>HOOHOQV >emm msHHHOHHGOO >Bmm oH >Bmm H: soHuOHOHHOO HOOHOIOHON EOOOHom mOOOmHmm A.O.ucoocuu.mH momma CHAPTER IV SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION The purpose of this study was to examine viewers' and nonviewers' perceptions of the reality of soap opera characters and content, to explore viewers' degree of interaction, involvement, and identification with soap operas, and to identify functions satisfied by watching daytime serials. Four original hypotheses were tested concerning the relationship between amount of viewing time and perceived reality, knowledge of serial content, involvement, identi- fication, interaction, boredom, companionship, escapism, and social utility. Correlations between the nine depen- dent variables and demographic variables (sex, age, marital status, and income) were also examined. The Summary of Obtained Effects shows that hy- potheses were confirmed for the following criterion vari- ables. As the amount of viewing increased, (1) perceived reality increased, (2) knowledge of soap opera content 60 61 increased, (3) involvement increased, (4) identification increased, (5) interaction increased, and (6) social utility increased. Hypotheses for companionship, relief of boredom, and escapism were not confirmed. This indicates that the serials mainly provide information and advice that is useful to the viewer in helping him/her to deal with other people. Perceived reality mediated only the relationships between viewing and interaction with partial correlation of r = .09, .07, and .05 (viewing and-interaction correlate .21, .28, and .02); and viewing and boredom with partial correlations of r = -.04, .06, and .07 (viewing and boredom correlate insignificantly). Therefore, viewers do not depend upon media realism before becoming involved with soap opera characters and the events in their lives. Respondents were asked to indicate their favorite soap opera and the overwhelming choice was ”The Young and the Restless," a CBS production that first aired two years ago. This is the only serial filmed in Hollywood (the rest are made in New York) and it is the costliest soap produced. (Soap operas average a $60,000 weekly cost while CBS puts out $70,000 for this serial.) Fifty-six percent 62 perceived reality 11 amount of interaction viewing g boredom V identification knowledge involvement social utility Summary of Obtained Effects (Note: Arrows only indicate that a correlation exists between variables.) 63 of the respondents chose "The Young and the Restless" over the other 13 serials broadcast daily and 71 per cent watch it each week (Appendix D). What makes this soap opera so popular? Fifty-three per cent of this sample were below 30 years of age. "The Young and the Restless" has a cast mainly composed of young people between the ages of 18 and 30 and most of the sub- plots centered around their problems. This serial has been most prolific in demonstrating physical affection between persons. Intimate scenes are becoming more common to the soaps now, but in the sixties, viewers were shocked to see that Joanne Tate and her husband on "Search for Tomorrow" in the same bed. This study demOnstrates that soap Operas viewing is no lOnger a common practice for only housewives or other persons confined to the home during the day. There is no longer a "typical viewer." People of all income levels and of all educational backgrounds watch the soaps. Why? From habit. Because their friends or relatives watch them. Because the serials are the only thing on television during the day. Or simply because the soaps stimulate curiosity and interest about other people's lives and problems that let the viewer forget his/her own troubles. 64 (Nielsen ratings for the serials are also in Appendix D.) Study Weaknesses Weaknesses in this study lay in the confined mea- “sures of perceived reality and gratifications. Respondents were offered simply a three-fOil choice of "agree, disagree, not sure." A wider spectrum for responses was needed for a more discriminatory measure. For example: "The same things that happen to people on soap operas happen to people in real life." strongly agree agree not sure disagree strongly disagree "Watching soap operas helps me to deal with other people." strongly agree agree not sure disagree strongly disagree Such operationalizations would have allowed for a finer discrimination of viewers' and nonviewers' level of perceived reality. 65 Directions for Future Research There has been no consistent mode of studies on soap Operas. It is a much unexplored field with vast po- tential. The only sample study of viewers was done by Herzog over thirty years ago. The Katzman study was solely a content analysis of characters and content. The other so-called studies and articles were simply discussions by social scientists about some aspect of the serials. Millions of viewers subscribe to the literature of one electronic medium. This vast addiction is worth in- vestigating to find what gratifications come from a daily dose of soaps, and how similar to real life viewers per- ceive serial content and characters to be. In January, 1975, NBC expanded "Another World" to 60 minutes. The network found that it is less expensive to produce one soap fOr an hour than to produce two 30-minute serials. This practice is a new trend worth investigating. Because of success with the hour-long serial, in April, NBC cancelled "How to Survive a Marriage" to expand "Days of Our Lives" to an hour, and CBS plans to stretch "Search.for Tomorrow" and "The Guiding Light" to 45 minutes. What is viewer reaction to the new serial length? Is the hour to 66 become the standard length for the daytime drama? Will viewers frequent the soaps less because of their extended length? The Herzog study examined only women who were con- fined to the home with easy access to the soaps. This study found a large number of teenagers and young adults (men and women) who were avid soap opera fans. This is an audience that has not been researched independently about soaps and deserves to be. Perceived reality of soap operas is a variable that could be researched again. A questionnaire completed by the respondent could contain more in-depth questions and provide for better measure of media realism. A possible study would be to examine the same vari— ables in this study among teenage viewers and viewers in their forties and older and to contrast the results. View- ing and age correlated .03 for shows and .20 for episodes. Age is insignificantly related to the number of different shows viewed, but correlated more positively with episodes because the older viewers have more time during the week to watch while younger viewers have more outside activ- ities, considering this study was done in the summer, in the middle of August, the height of vacation season. 67 This study itself could and should be repeated. One basic criterion for research is that it can be repli— cated and this study owes such clarification to the serials. Viewer involvement and gratifications should be more deeply explored. Prime time programming, news telecasts, cartoons, and children's programming are all media literature that have been researched for violence, news effects, informa- tion gain, and other concepts. The same courtesy and recognition should be extended to the serials. New Hypotheses Four new questions for future research are pre- sented here from.which two new hypotheses are tested. "Who is your favorite soap opera character?" "Is this person just like someone you know in real life?" strongly agree agree not sure disagree strongly disagree 68 "Which soap opera character do you like the least?" "Is this person just like someone you know in real life?" strongly agree agree not sure disagree strongly disagree Hypothesis: Perceived reality will be greater for the viewer's favorite character than for the char- acter the viewers likes the least. "Is there any character who reminds you of yourself?" "If so, who?" "Do you think this person behaves very much like you do?" strongly agree agree not sure disagree strongly disagree "Is there any character who least reminds you of yourself?" "If so, who?" "Do you think this person behaves very much like you do?" strongly agree agree not sure disagree strongly disagree 69 Hypothesis: Perceived reality will be greater for a character who reminds the viewer of him/ herself. Postscript The grandmother of soap-opera scriptwriters is 74-year-old Irna Phillips. She has been writing scripts for 45 years, and like Homer and Shakespeare, is considered a bard of the mythology of her thmes. Mythology is the literature created by the nameless and the illiterate (which is how some critics classify soap opera writers), passed down from generation to generation and altered within each. Soap operas are American myth- ology. The Greeks followed the escapades of the gods and goddesses of Olympus, recorded by the likes of Homer--the Americans follow the heroes and heroines of the daily day- time dramas created by the likes of Ms. Phillips. APPENDIX A SOAP OPERA QUESTIONNAIRE APPENDIX A Mitzi Bond Soap opera questionnaire August 1974 Date: ID number: Hello, I'm from the Department of Communication at Michigan State University. We're doing a study on daytime television and if I may have a few minutes of your time, I'd like to ask you a few questions. 1. 01 02 03 04 05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 I will read a list of programs that are on television during the day. Please tell me which of the following shows you watch and about hOW’many days a week you watch them. Love of Life The Young and the Restless Search for Tomorrow Somerset All My Children As the WOrld Turns The Guiding Light Days of Our Lives The Edge of Night The Doctors Another World General Hospital How To Survive a Marriage One Life to Live 70 9. .1. .2. 1 i 2 2. 71 Total number of shows viewed per week: Total number of days viewed per week: Which is your favorite soap Opera? I have a few statements here on which I would like your Opinion. Please tell me if you agree, disagree, or are not sure about whether the statement is true. 3. K Soap operas on TV tell about life the way it really is. ___ agree ___ not sure ____disagree The same things that happen to people on soap Operas happen to people in real life. ___.agree not sure ____disagree The places I see in soap operas are just like places in real life. agree not sure disagree People in soap operas are just like people in real life. agree not sure disagree 10. 11. 72 Families on soap Operas are just like families in real life. agree not sure ____disagree Doctors on soap operas are just like doctors in real life. ___ agree ___ not sure ‘___'disagree Marriages on soap operas are just like marriages in real life. agree not sure disagree Problems people have in soap operas are just like problems people have in real life. agree not sure disagree Which soap Opera character most reminds you of yourself? I have a few questions to ask you about the soap operas you watch. (Only ask respondent questions about soap operas he/she watches. Refer to page 1.) 12. 13. 14. 15. 73 Love of Life With whom is Jamie Rollins having an affair? Who was responsible for the attempts on Bruce and Vanessa Sterling's lives? What happened to Dan and Rebecca Phillips? correct answers The Young and the Restless Who is the father of Sally McGuire's baby? Who is responsible for Leslie Brooks' emotional breakdown? What is Brad Elliot's real profession? correct answers Search for Tomorrow Who is responsible for Doug Martin's car accident? With whom is Scott Phillips having an affair? To whom is Eunice Martin married? correct answers Somerset Who did Tony COOper marry? Who headed the syndicate operation in Somerset to try and gain ownership of Delaney Brands? Who shot Andrea Moore? correct answers 16. 17. 18. 19. 74 All My Children Who is the father of Tara Martin Tyler's son Phillip? Who killed Jason Maxwell? Why did Ann Martin abruptly leave Pine Valley? correct answers As the World Turns Who has custody of Emily Stewart? Who was the father of Kim Dixon's child? What is wrong with Jennifer Hughes? correct answers The Guiding Light Who is Freddie Bauer's father? Who killed Charlotte Bauer? How many husbands has Leslie Bauer had? correct answers Days of Our Lives Who is Dr. Tom Horton's daughter—in-law? Who is Julie Bannings' grandfather? How many children do Tom and Alice Horton have? correct answers 20. 21. 22. 23. 09 75 The Edge of Night Who was the intended victim when Nicole Drake was killed? Who killed Babs Macelli? Who is working as an undercover agent in the syndicate for the Monticello police? correct answers The Doctors Who killed Dr. John Morrison? Who is Dr. Althea Davis' most recent husband? Who is Ann Larimer's former fiancee? correct answers Another World Who caused Alice Frame's emotional breakdown? To whom was Rachael Frame married when Jamie was born? Why was Steven Frame sent to prison? correct answers General Hospital Who is the father of Diana Taylor's baby? With whom did Teddy Holmes elope? Who married Dr. James Hobart after his car accident? correc t answers 24. 25. 13 76 How to Survive a Marriage How did David Bachman die? For whom did Larry Kirby leave his wife Chris? What is Dr. Julie Franklin's profession? correct answers One Life to Live Who is the father of the child Cathy Craig is carrying? Why did Victoria Burke divorce her second husband, Steve Burke? With whom is Vince Wolek in love? correct answers Total number of correct answers (12-25): 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. How long have you been watching soap operas? What is your favorite soap opera? Why? Who are your favorite soap opera people? Why? Which people do you like least? Why? Are there any character(s) you would like to be like? Who? DO you ever emagine yourself as one of the characters? If so, which ones? 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 77 Do you ever imagine yourself in a similar situation that a particular character faces? Please give an example. Do you talk about the soaps with friends ____ yes ____ no relatives ____ yes ____ no children ____ yes ____ no spouse _____yes ____ no anyone else yes ____ no Why do you watch soap operas? to learn to escape ____ to relax to be aroused as a habit to pass time for companionship for entertainment Why did you first begin to watch soap operas? Why do you watch the particular soaps that you do? Do you get any useful information from the soap operas you watch? How is it useful to you? Was the information you got socially useful? Did any information or opinions you heard on soap operas change your Opinions about any topic or issue? (Ask for examples.) 41. 78 Do you sit and watch the stories or do you have the TV on while you do other things? sit and watch stories have TV on other (specify: ) I have a few more statements here on which I would like your opinion. Please tell me if you agree, disagree, or are not sure about whether the statement is true. 42. 43. 44. 45. Watching soap operas helps me to pass the time. agree not sure disagree Watching soap operas helps me to escape from my daily routine. agree not sure disagree Watching soap operas helps me to learn more about people. agree not sure disagree I watch soap Operas because I have nothing better to do. agree not sure disagree 46. Watching problems. 47. Watching 79 soap Operas helps me to deal with my own agree not sure disagree soap operas entertains me. agree not sure disagree 48. Sex of respondent: male female 49. What is your age? 50. What is your marital status? single married 51. What is the approximate income of the head of your household? 52. What was student below $5000 $5000-$10,000 $10,000-$15,000 $15,000-$20,000 over $20,000 the last grade of school you completed? below high school completed high school some college college degree graduate or professional degree APPENDIX B SOAP OPERA STUDY APPENDIX B Mitzi Bond Soap Opera Study August 1974 Interviewer: ID number: Date: Telephone number: Hello, I'm from.the Department of Communication at Michigan State University. We're doing a study on daytime televi- sion and if I may have a few'minutes of your time, I'd like to ask you a few questions. 1. I will read a list of programs that are on television during the day. Please tell me which of the following shows you watch and how many days a week you watch each one. 0 1 .2. 2. i 53. 01 Love of Life 02 The Young and the Restless 03 Search for Tomorrow 04 Somerset 05 All My Children 06 As the World Turns 07 The Guiding Light 08 Days of Our Lives 09 The Edge of Night 10 The Doctors 11 Another World 12 General Hospital 13 How to Survive a Marriage 14 One Life to Live 80 81 Number of different shows viewed per week: Total number of episodes viewed per week: I have a few statements here on which I would like your opinion. Please tell me if you agree, disagree, or are not sure about each statement. 2. Soap operas on TV tell about life the way it really is. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree 3. The same things that happen to people on soap operas happen to people in real life. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree 4. The places I see in soap operas are just like places in real life. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree 5. People in soap operas are just like people in real life. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree 6. Families on soap operas are just like families in real life. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree 7. Doctors on soap Operas are just like doctors in real life. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree 82 8. Marriages on soap operas are just like marriages in real life. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree 9. Problems people have in soap operas are just like problems people have in real life. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree 10. Is there any soap opera character who reminds you of yourself? 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree I have a few questions to ask you about the soap operas you watch. (Only ask respondent questions about soap operas he/she watches. Refer to page 1.) ll. 01 Love of Life 1. With whom is Jamie Rollins having an affair? 2. Who was responsible for the attempts on Bruce and Vanessa Sterling's lives? 3. What happened to Dan and Rebecca Phillips? 12. 13. 14. 83 Who is responsible for Leslie Brooks' emotional breakdown? Who is responsible for Doug Martin's car accident? With whom is Scott Phillips having an affair? 02 The Young and the Restless 1. Who is the father of Sally McGuire's baby? 2. 3. What is Brad Elliot's real profession? 03 Search for Tomorrow 1. 2. 3. To whom is Eunice Martin now married? 04 Somerset 1. Who did Tony Cooper marry? 2. Who headed the syndicate operation in Somerset which tried to get ownership of Delaney Brands? Who shot Andrea Moore? 15. 16. 17. yes" . “ 84 Who is the father of Tara Martin Tyler's son Phillip? 05 All My Children 1. 2. Who killed Jason Maxwell? 3. Why did Ann Martin abruptly leave Pine Valley? Who has custody of Emily Stewart? Who was the father of Kim Dixon's child? What is wrong with Jennifer Hughes? 06 As the World Turns 1. 2. 3. 07 The Guiding Light 1. Who is Freddie Bauer's father? 2. Who kiIled Charlotte Bauer? How'many husbands has Leslie Bauer had? 18. 19. 20. 85 Who is Dr. Tom Horton's daughter-in-law? Who is Julie Bannings' grandfather? How many children do Tom and Alice Horton have? Who was the intended victim When Nicole Drake was killed? Who is working as an undercover agent in the syndicate for the Who killed Dr. John Morrison? 08 Days of Our Lives 1. 2. 3. 09 The Edge of Night. 1. 2. Who killed Babs Macelli? 3. Monticello police? 10 The Doctors 1. 2. Who is Dr. Althea Davis' most recent husband? Who is Ann Larimer's former fiancee? 21. 22. 23. 86 Who caused Alice Frame's emotional breakdown? To whom was Rachael Frame married when Jamie was born? Who married Dr. James Hobart after his car accident? For whom did Larry Kirby leave his wife Chris? 11 Another World 1. 2. 3. Why was Steven Frame sent to prison? 12 General Hospital 1. Who is the father of Diana Taylor's baby? 2. With whom did Teddy Holmes elope? 3. 13 How to Survive a Marriage 1. How did David Bachman die? 2. 3. What is Dr. Julie Franklin's profession? 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 87 14 One Life to Live 1. Who is the father of the child Cathy Craig is carrying? 2. Why did Victoria Burke divorce her second husband, Steve Burke? 3. With whom.is Vince Wolek in love? Number of correct answers per show (ll-24): How long have you been watching soap operas? years What is your favorite soap opera? (Only one please.) How important are the soap operas to you would you say? 3 very important 2 somewhat important 1 not very important How involved do you get watching your favorite soap opera? 3 very involved 2 somewhat involved 1 not very involved Are there any characters you would like to be like? 2 Yes 1 No DO you ever imagine yourself as one of the characters? 2 Yes 1 No 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 88 Do you ever imagine yourself in a similar situation that a particular character faces? 2 Yes 1 No Do you ever think about telling some characters how to handle their problems? 2 Yes 1 No Have you written to the network or any place about a show? 2 Yes 1 No Do you ever find yourself talking to the characters while the show is on? 2 Yes 1 No Do you ever worry about the characters in between episodes? 2 Yes 1 NO Total number of yeses (32-35): Why did you first begin to watch soap operas? Why do you watch the particular soaps that you do? Do you get any useful information from the soap Operas you watch? 2 Yes 1 No How is it useful to you? When your stories are on, do you give them full attention or do you do other things at the same time? 3 full attention 2 some of both 1 do other things 89 I have a few more statements here on which I would like your opinion. Please tell me if you agree, disagree, or are not sure about whether the statement is true. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. Watching soap Operas helps me to pass the time. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree Watching soap Operas helps me to escape from my daily routine. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree I watch soap operas because I have nothing better to do. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree Watching soap operas helps me to deal with other people. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree Watching soap operas takes me away for a little while. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree I watch soap operas when there's no one to talk to. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree Watching soap operas make me feel less lonely. 3 agree 2 not sure 1 disagree Record sex of respondent: 1 male 2 female 90 Just a few final questions 49. 50. 51. 52. 1 2 3 1l> U'I 0‘ \l l N w b U‘l What is your age? Are you single or married? 1 single 2 married What is the approximate yearly income of the head of your household? student below $5000 $5000-$10,000 $10,000-$15,000 $15,000-$20,000 over $20,000 retired What was the last grade of school you completed? below high school completed high school some college college degree graduate or professional degree THANK YOU VERY MUCH FOR HELPING ME! APPENDIX C ONE-WAY ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE TABLE C1.--One-Way Analysis of Variance (Viewing-Shows) (N = 157) Low Medium High (1-2) (3-5) (6+) F P < df Perceived Reality 14.3 16.2 16.7 3.987 .021 2/152 Knowledge answers per show 2.1 2.0 2.3 1.864 .159 2/151 total correct answers 4.8 7.7 14.9 19.281 .001 2/152 Involvement 2.8 3.7 4.1 16.736 .001 2/149 Identification 3.5 3.8 4.0 3.280 .040 2/148 Interaction 5.3 5.5 5.8 2.786 .065 2/148 Companionship 4.2 4.2 4.0 .384 .682 2/149 Boredom 3.1 3.1 3.1 .014 .986 2/149 Escapism 3.6 3.6 4.1 1.290 .278 2/149 Social Utility 2.3 2.6 2.8 2.988 .053 2/149 91 92 TABLE C2.--One-Way Analysis of Variance (Viewing-Episodes) (N = 154) Low Medium High (1-7) (8-16) (17+) F P < df Perceived Reality 14.4 15.5 16.9 3.812 .024 2/152 Knowledge answers per show 2.0 2.0 2.3 2.335 .100 2/151 total correct answers 5.0 6.9 13.9 16.640 .001 2/152 Involvement 2.7 3.5 4.3 22.809 .001 2/149 Identification 3.5 4.0 3.8 3.692 .027 2/148 Interaction 5.3 5.4 5.8 3.212 .043 2/148 Companionship 4.2 4.1 4.1 .051 .950 2/149 Boredom 3.0 3.1 3.1 .083 .921 2/149 Escapism 3.7 3.4 4.2 3.410 .036 2/149 Social Utility 2.3 2.6 2.8 3.928 .022‘ 2/149 APPENDIX D CURRENT SOAP OPERAS (JANUARY, 1975) 1973 NIELSEN SOAP OPERA RATINGS TABLE Dl.--Current Soap Operas (January, 1975) Serial Premiere Date NetwOrk ::;:I:tsg:w::s Search for Tomorrow 3 Sept. 1951 CBS 37% Love of Life 24 Sept. 1951 CBS 39% The Guiding Light 30 June 1952 CBS 36% As The World Turns 2 April 1956 CBS 54% The Edge of Night 2 April 1956 CBS 50% General Hospital 1 April 1963 ABC 14% The Doctors 1 April 1963 NBC 12% Another World 4 May 1964 NBC 15% Days of Our Lives 19 Nov. 1965 NBC 17% One Life to Live 15 July 1968 ABC 10% All My Children 5 Jan. 1970 ABC 12% Somerset 30 March 1970 NBC 7% The Young and the Restless 26 March 1973 CBS 71% How to Survive a Marriage* 7 Jan. 1974 NBC 16% *Canceled in April, 1975. 93 94 TABLE D2.--1973 Nielsen Soap Opera Ratings Time Serial Rating** A:::::CB 11:30 Love of Life (CBS) 6.9 31 12:00 The Young and the Restless (CBS) 5.2 21 12:30 Search for Tomorrow (CBS) 8.5 30 1:00 All My Children (ABC) 8.1 28 1:30 As The World Turns (CBS) 10.6 35 2:00 Days of Our Lives (NBC) 9.9 32 2:00 The Guiding Light (CBS) 8.3 28 2:30 The Doctors (NBC) 9.2 32 2:30 The Edge of Night (CBS) 8.0 28 3:00 Another World (NBC) 9.8 33 3:00 General Hospital (ABC) 9.6 31 3:30 Return to Peyton Place (NBC)* 7.2 24 4:00 Somerset (NBC) 6.8 21 4:00 The Secret Storm (CBS)* 6.5 ‘21 *Canceled in 1974. **The A. C. Nielsen Company is a research firm that conducts the audience-measurement research that is paid for by the networks, advertising agencies, and advertisers. The audience data comes in two primary forms: the rating and the share. The rating is a figure that indicates the per- centage of all existing TV homes actually tuned to a par- ticular program at a given moment. (Most soaps have a rating around 7.0 or 8.0 which means that seven or eight per cent of the homes equipped with television were tuned to that program.) The audience share is a figure indicat- ing the percentage of all homes actually using TV at a given time that are tuned to a particular program. The Nielsen ratings come from viewing information provided by 3400 television homes. BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Blumler, J., D. McQuail, and R. Brown, "The Social Origins of the Gratifications Associated with Television Viewing," in D. McQuail (ed.), Reader in Mass Communication, London: Penguin, 1972. Clark, D. G., and W. B. Blankenburg, "Trends in Violent Content in Selected Mass Media," Television and Social Behavior, Vol. 1, ed. G. A. Comstock and E. A. Rubinstein. U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, 1971. Danowski, James, "Functions and Gratifications of Soap Opera Viewing: Some Operationalizations" (1973). Dervin, Brenda, and Bradley Greenberg, "The Communication Environment of the Urban Poor," East Lansing: Michigan State University, Department of Communi- cation, 1972. Dominick, Joseph, and Bradley Greenberg, "Mass Media Func- tions Among Low-Income Adolescents," Use of Mass Media by the Urban Poor, ed. Bradley Greenberg and Brenda Dervin, New York: Praeger Publishers, 1970. Edmondson, Madeleine, and David Rounds. The Soaps: Day— time Serials of Radio and TV. New York: Stein and Day, 1973. Feshbach, Seymour, "Reality and Fantasy in Filmed Violencefl' Television and Social Behavior, Vol. III, ed. Murray, Rubinstein, and Comstock. U.S. Department of HEW, 1971. 95 96 Gerbner, George, "The Two Worlds of Violence:‘ Television and Reality," Mass Media and'Violence, ed: Lange, Baker and Ball. U.S. Government Printing Office, 1969. Greenberg, Bradley S., "Gratifications and Motivations of Television Viewing for British Children" (1973). Greenberg, Bradley and Joseph Dominick, "Television Be- havior Among Disadvantaged Children," Use of Mass Media by the Urban Poor, ed. Greenberg and Dervin, New York: Praeger Publishers, 1970. Greenberg, Bradley, P. Ericson, and M. Vlahos, “Children's Television Behavior as Perceived by Mother and Child," Television and Social Behavior, ed. Rubin- stein, Comstock and Murray, U.S. Department of HEW, 1971. Greenberg, Bradley S. and Byron Reeves, "Children and the Perceived Reality of Television" (March, 1974). Herzog, Herta, "Modivations and Gratifications of Daily Serial Listeners," Radio Research, 1942-1943, ed. Paul Lazarfield and Frank Stanton, New York: 1944. Katzman, Natan, "Developmental Interactions with the Ef- fects of Television" (1971). Katzman, Natan, "Television Soap Operas: What's Been Going on Anyway," Public Opinion Quarterly, #36, 1972-73. Kinser, Nora Scott, "Soap Sin in the Afternoon," Psychology Today, August 1973, pp. 46-48. LaPota, Margherite and Bruce LaPota, "Studying the Soap Opera," English Journal, April 1973. "Profile: Iona Phillips," Journal of Broadcasting, 6 November 1972, p. 75. "Real Drama in Daytime: The Networks' Battle for Domi- nance," Journal of Broadcasting, 2 July 1973, pp. 17-19. 97 Rosengren, Karl E., "Uses and Gratifications: An Over- view," Department of SociologYi University of Lund (mimeo), 1972. Rosengren, K. E. and S. Windahl, "Mass Media Consumption as a Functional Alternative," in D. McQuail (ed.), op. cit. HICHIGQN STATE UNIV. LIBRARIES ill||llllll|||||||1|||N||||1||||||||H||l|||||||||l||||l|l||||| 31293104228550