|| | 1 l i I l '! WW“! I ‘ f E 1i THE iNFiUENC‘E C??? DC-GMA‘HSM AND RIGEMYY GEN PLEfigSQ-i‘éfi-NG Thesé: {For $315 Degree a? M A. MlCHiGA’N STATE UNIVERSITY Robert Paul Beach @964 . "3V 2 9 2001 THE INFLUENCE OF DOGMATISM AND RIGIDITY ON REASONING ABSTRACT To test the relationship between synthesis and analy- sis and induction and deduction with dogmatism and rigidity, Rokeach's Dogmatism Scale and the Gough—Sanford Rigidity Scale was administered to 3&1 undergraduates at Michigan State University. The 20 students scoring highest and the 20 students scoring lowest on the Dogmatism Scale were selected for individual testing. Both groups of subjects were further divided into two groups of ten each on the basis of their scores on the Rigidity Scale. The first task consisted of a set of twenty-five analogies emphasizing induction, and another set of analogies emphasizing deduction. The subject's per- formance on this task was measured in terms of the relative amount of time Spent on the preparation period of the problem and on the solution phase of the problem as well as the number of errors made. The second problem was the Denny Doodlebug Problem with separate measures of analysis in terms of the time required to overcome the beliefs of the problem and the number of beliefs the sub- ject could overcome by himself, and synthesis measured in terms of the number of minutes required to solve the problem after each of the beliefs was overcome. The general conclusions of this study are that: (l) in a problem solving situation in which the primary task of the subject is to integrate various beliefs of component parts of the problem Open subjects are superior to closed subjects; (2) in a problem solving situation in which the task of the subject is to analyze or discover the parts of the problem, non-rigid subjects are superior to rigid subjects; (3) there is a tendency for subjects who excel in synthesis to also excel on inductive reasoning; (b) there is a tendency for subjects who excel in analysis to do well on problems emphasizing deduction. / . Appr oved Mk/égidaz Date 7%; as; we! THE INFLUENCE OF DOGMATISM AND RIGIDITY ON REASONING by Robert Paul Beech A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 196M Page 32 Table XIII. BREATA Kean Solution Time for the Denny Doodlebug Problem Following the First, Second, and All Three Beliefs. Group First Second All Three Belief Belief Beliefs High Dogmatism-High Rigid 21.83 15.95 8.65 High Dogmatism-Low Rigid 28.36 24.48 14.90 Low Dogmatism-Eigh Rigid 19.22 12.95 5.19 Low Dogmatism-Low Rigid 11.31 8.31 1.86 High Dogmatism 25.095 20.215 11.795 Low Dogmatism 15.265 10.630 3.525 High Rigid 20.525 14.45 6.920 Low Rigid 19.835 16.395 8.uoo Page 39 Chi Square =.226 Page #0 Chi Square = 1.034, p less than .15 one tailed test Page 41 Chi Square = 1.096, Page 42 Chi Square = .510 Page b# p less than .70 Page 44 Chi Square = .083, p less than .80 Page 50 In Table XXXIV, the time taken to solve the Doodlebug Problem after all three beliefs had been overcome for the Nonrigid group = 2.6 INTRODUCTION. METHOD. . RESULTS . DISCUSSION. SUMMARY . REFERENCES. APPENDIX. TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES Table I. Comparison of Means and Standard Deviations on the Dogmatism Scale. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . II. Comparison of Means and Standard Deviation on the Rigidity Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . III. Comparison of the Mean Number of Errors made on the Problems Emphasizing Induction. . . . . . IV. Analysis of Variance of the Errors made on the Problems Emphasizing Induction . . . . . . . . . V. Comparison of the Mean Preparation Index on the Problems Emphasizing Induction . . . . . . . . . VI. Analysis of Variance of the Preparation Index on the Problems Emphasizing Induction. . . . . . VII. Comparison of the Mean Number of Errors made on the Problems Emphasizing Deduction . . . . . . . VIII. Analysis of Variance of the Errors made on the Problems Emphasizing Deduction . . . . . . . IX. Comparison of the Mean Preparation Index on the Problems Emphasizing Deduction . . . . . . . X. Analysis of Variance of the Preparation Index on the Problems Emphasizing Deduction. . . . . . XI. Analysis of Variance of the Errors made on Problems Emphasizing Induction and Deduction . . XII. Analysis of Variance of the Preparation Index on the Problems Emphasizing Induction and Deduction . XIII. Mean Solution Time for the Denny Doodlebug Problem Following the First, Second, and All Three Beliefs. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . XIV. Analysis of Variance of the Time Required to Solve the Denny Doodlebug Problem after the First Belief . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . XV. Analysis of Variance of the Time Required to Solve the Denny Doodlebug Problem after the Second Belief. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Page 16 18 2h 25 25 26 27 28 28 29 30 31 32 33 33 Table XVI. XVII. XVIII. XIX. XX. XXII. XXIII. XXIV. XXV. XXVI. XXVII. Analysis of Variance of the Time Required to Solve the Denny Doodlebug Problem after All Three Beliefs. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Comparison of Mean Number of Minutes Taken to Overcome the First Belief, the First Two Beliefs, and All Three Beliefs . . . . . Analysis of Variance of the Time Required to Overcome the First Belief . . . . . . . . Analysis of Variance of the Time Required to Overcome Two Beliefs. . . . . . . . . . . Analysis of Variance of the Time Required to Overcome All Three Beliefs. . . . . . . . Analysis--The Number of Beliefs Overcome by the Subject in the First Fifteen Minutes of the Experiment--Rigid vs. Non-Rigid . . . Analysis-~The Number of Beliefs Overcome by the Subject in the First Fifteen Minutes of the Experiment--Open vs. Closed ... . . . Synthesis and Induction-~The Time Required to Solve the DD Problem After All Three Beliefs Had Been Overcome and the Number of Errors Made on the Problems Emphasizing Induction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Synthesis and Induction--The Time Required to Solve the DD Problem after All Three Beliefs Had Been Overcome and Preparation Index on the Problems Emphasizing Induction. Synthesis and Deduction--Time Required on the DD Problem after All Three Beliefs Had Been Overcome and Number of Errors Made on the Problems Emphasizing Deduction . . . . . Synthesis and Deduction-~Time Required on the DD Problem After All Three Beliefs Had Been Overcome and Preparation Index on the Problems Emphasizing Deduction.. . . . . . . Analysis and Deduction-~Comparison of Number of Beliefs Overcome in First Fifteen Minutes of DD Problem and the Number of Errors Made on Problems Emphasizing Deduction. . . . . . Page 34 35 36 36 37 38 39 ho bl #2 1+2 ’43 Table XXVIII. XXIX. XXX. XXXI. XXXII. XXXIV. XXXV. XXXVI. Analysis and Deduction--Comparison of Number of Beliefs Overcome in First Fifteen Minutes on DD Problem and Preparation Index for Problems Emphasizing Deduction. . . . . . . . . . . . ... Analysis and Induction--Comparison of the Number of Beliefs Overcome in the First Fifteen Minutes of the DD Problem and the Number of Errors Made on the Problems Emphasizing Induction. . . . . . Analysis and Induction--Comparison of the Number of Beliefs Overcome in the First Fifteen Minutes of the DD Problem and the Preparation Index on the Problems Emphasizing Induction . . . . . . . Correlations Between Induction, Deduction, Analysis and Synthesis . . . . . . . . . . . . . Ability to Analyze: Comparison Between Rigid and Non-rigid Groups and Between Closed and Open Groups on Mean Number of Minutes Taken to Overcome the First Belief, The First Two Beliefs, and All Three Beliefs . . . . . . . . . Ability to Synthesize: Time Taken to Solve the DD Problem after the First, Second, and Third Beliefs were Overcome by Rigid and Non-rigid Groups and by Closed and Open Groups . Synthesis: Mean Time Taken to Solve the DD Problem after the First, Second, and Third Beliefs Had Been Overcome. . . . . . . . . . . . Analysis: Mean Time Taken to Overcome the First Belief, the First Two Beliefs, and All Three Beliefs. O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O Page ’43 M4 nu 45 ’49 50 51 51 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The author wishes to express his sincere appreciation for the time and effort so willingly given in the comple- tion of this study committee. In addition, Dr. Donald Johnson and for the verbal deduction, and for preparation of the Special thanks for to Dr. Milton Rokeach, Chairman of the the author wishes to convey thanks to for the use of his serial-exposure box analogies emphasizing induction and his constructive criticisms in the manuscript. To Dr. Bertram Karon, his invaluable assistance in the for- mulation of the analysis of this study. THE RELATIONSHIP OF DOGMATISM AND RIGIDITY ON REASONING There is a general distinction made by psychologists and others interested in the study of problem solving and the process of thinking between reasoning from a part to a whole, and reasoning from the general to the particular. The former frequently is referred to as inductive reasoning and the latter as deductive reasoning. Clarke, for example, states that "The term logic. . .is usually employed in a more restricted sense. . .means either the process of making generalizations (inductive logic), or the process of drawing from assumptions inferences which are implicit in them but which are not obvious (deductive logic)(1929, p. 77)." Similarly, Miller differentiates between the two forms as follows, "Deduction is that form of thinking in which an in- dividual (case) which is problematic is interpreted and con- trolled by referring it to some concept, or law, which is, for the time being at least unquestioned. Induction is that form of thinking in which a concept or law, which has become problematic, is reconstructed through an investigation and analysis of individuals." (1915, p. 233) More recently Vinacke describes the same two processes as: "Deduction, crudely defined, is reasoning from the general to the par- ticular, or, more Specifically, from stated premises to a pr0per conclusion; induction, similarly, signifies reasoning from the particular to the general." (1952, p. 76) The distinction between induction and deduction appears to be primarily linked to the nature of the problem more than in the person attempting to solve the problem. If the problem is one which requires the application of a known principle or generalization to a Specific case, then the thought process is deductive. If the problem is one for which an example has been provided and the problem solver must find the general rule which is Operating, the reasoning is inductive. The question then arises whether there are individual differences in problem solving which are Specific to the kind of problem being worked upon or are persons who are capable of one kind of reasoning also capable of the other equally well? Further, if there are individual differences with reSpect to the ability to solve inductive or deductive problems, what other variables are associated with these individual differ- ences? One technique which was employed to study such re- lationships was factor analysis. Of the factor analytic studies on problem solving which attempted to separate these two functions, the results have not been conclusive. Thurstone factor analyzed 57 tests taken by 250 college students and identified an induction factor, a restrictive thinking factor, and a deduction factor (1938). However, a similar factor analysis of 60 tests taken by 710 eighth-graders found only the induction factor (Thurstone and Thurstone, 19u1). Additional factor analytic studies by Holziner and Harman failed to confirm the separation of inductive and deductive reasoning (1938). One of the most complete studies of reasoning using the method of factor analysis is that done by Guilford and others at the University of Southern California (Guilford, Comrey, Green, and Christensen, 1950; and Guilford, Green, and Christensen, 1951). It was hypothesized that the ability to graSp a system of relationships in its totality, or to see trends in a series of objects, to identify a relationship in a variety of settings would constitute inductive reasoning ability. The reasoning ability tested in syllogisms was felt to be deduction. Factor analysis of 3% tests of 283 subjects identified a general reasoning factor of prime importance while deductive‘reasoning was found in tests of reasoning, inference, syllogisms, and false premises, but the tests which were used to define this factor were multiple-choice and true-false form. No general induction factor was identified. Another approach has been to analyze a the process of problem solving into various phases. Dewey described five such separate stages in the problem solving sequence as follows: "(1) a felt difficulty; (ii) its location and definition; (iii) suggestion of possible solution; (iv) development by reasoning of the bearings of the' suggestion; (v) further observation and experiment leading to its acceptance or rejection; that is, the conclusion of belief or disbelief." (1910, p. 72) From such an analysis of the problem solving process it is possible to formulate different methods of studying the reasoning process. First there is the possibility Of constructing problems in which the various phases may be measured separately, such that an objective record may be made as the problem solver passes through each successive phase. Another alternative which is also based on the analysis of the problem solving process into its component parts consists of constructing problems in which the diffi- culty of the problem lies primarily in one phase. Perfor- mance on this kind of a problem may be compared with performance on another problem for which the difficulty lies in some other phase. One of the major benefits of using a method based on the analysis of the parts of the problem solving process into separate phases is that it is possible to take into account the fact that in most problem solving behavior, both an inductive and a deductive Operation are involved. Johnson in referring to problems of logical relations states that: "Forming concepts, principles, and patterns of personality traits may be called inductive tasks in that the particulars are organized into patterns... But we do not observe these cognitive patterns di- rectly; we test for the pattern by asking the thinker to use it in some particular way, that is, by a de- ductive process...problems that are called deductive, e.g., the syllogism, always include an inductive step because the particulars of any problem have to be organized before any conclusions can be produced." (1955.p- 2M3) Following Dewey's analysis of the problem solving process, reason may be thought of as the solution of problems by logical Operations. More specifically, the Operations which the reasoner goes through in order to solve a par- ticular problem may be thought of as follows: first, the reasoner organizes the problem situation and the wanted solution as well as he can. He then produces reSponses and finally judges the acceptability of these reSponses. Induction is the process of organizing the particulars of the problem situation into a conceptual pattern. An in- ferenceisofthe form. If thus and so then . Deduction is the process Of fitting a solution pattern to the inference thus organized. It is reasonable to assume that individual differences may be found at any point in the sequence of logical Operations. Consider the following analogy: large is to small as expensive is to fine, post, run, time, cheap First the relationship between large and small must be discovered. Second, expensive must be placed into this relationship with a place for the other term. Third, the reasoner tries out the offered solution words to see which one best fits the search model. If a measure of deductive ability is desired, it is necessary to con- struct items in which the third step is suffiently diffi- cult that some will fail at this point, while steps one and two are so easy that everyone will be successful at these points. If a measure of induction is desired, it is necessary to construct items such that SOme will fail on the first steo, that is in establishing the relationship, while there should be relatively little difficulty in {1 CI. (0 'U 1;) 8-7) two and three. (Tohnson, 1355, o 33, :‘d Johnson, in an attempt to study the determinants of preparation time, constructed a block of 25 analogies with the first pair of words more difficult than the second pair, hence emphasizing induction. Another block of 25 analogies was constructed with the second pair of words more difficult than the first pair, emphasizing de- duction. Using a technique of serial eXpOSUFC of the analogies, he found in connarix'lg preparation tirne between the two types of analogies hat log mean preparation time was greater for the analogies emphasizing induction, and log mean solution time was greater for those emphasizing deduction. Similarily, a time index of relative difficulty of preparation differentiated the two types of problems shgnificantly (Johnson, 1960). In further inv-stigation of the method of serial analysis of inductive and deductive reasoning using various formats for the presentation of the analogy, the preparation index, obtained by dividing preparation time by the total tine to get the preportion of he whole time that is spent on the first period,differentiated inductive from deductive problems thus Sipporting in general the distinction between these forms of reasoning, and the use Of serial presentation Of verbal analogies as a technique for studying these dif- ferent processes (Johnson, 1962). While the technique of comparing performance on differ- ent problems which emphasize different phases of the reason- ing process has been useful in the study of the problem solving, a number of studies have been done in which com- parisons were made between the subjects' performance on different phases Of the same problem. This has been one of the ways in which Rokeach has attempted to study the diStinction between Open and closed subjects. In discuss- ing the Doodlebug Problem he states that one of the reasons the problem is so difficult is that "the subject must first overcome not one but three currently held beliefs, and re- place them with three new ones. This is the analytic phase of the problem. . .He must then organize them together, or integrate them into a new system. This is the synthesizipg phase of the problem." (Rokeach, 1960, p. 173) The Problem is constructed so that it is possible to obtain five separate measures of the thinker's ability to analyze, all of which test in one way or another how long it takes the thinker to overcome the separate beliefs of the problem. Synthesis refers to how fast the thinker can integrate new beliefs into a new belief system. When the thinker finally states the solution to the problem, it is the end result of a prior process of synthesizing activity. In the Doodlebug Problem, there are three separate measures of synthesis or inte- gration. Another dimension in which the distinction between analysis and synthesis is profitable is in the study of perceptual tasks. Witkins and others have indicated that peOple differ in the extent to which their perception is analytical. This dimension of individual differences has been called "field-dependence-independence." The tendency toward an analytical or global way of perceiving characterizes a person's perception in a wide variety of situations, making for marked individual self-consistency. (Witkin et. a1. 1962, p. 58). The implication would appear to be that differences in analytic and synthetic thinking are general personality factors which are related to a number of other variables. From what has been mentioned above concerning individual differences in inductive and deductive reasoning ability, it might be reasonable that such differences would also be related to some general personality dimensions. One of the earlier personality variables which seemed to be related to a person's ability to reason was authoritarianism as measured by the F scale. Analysis of the ideological as well as the clinical material has suggested that ethnocentrism is related to stereotypy, rigidity, and concreteness in thinking (Adorno et. a1. 1950, P- 280). ROkeach, in an attempt to expand the measurement of authoritarianism to general authoritarianism develOped the Dogmatism scale which tested for general authoritarianism Of both the right and the left (Rokeach, 1960). With reSpect to the ability to solve abstract problems, Rokeach differentiates between the difficulties which arise due to the subject's resistance to change belief systems which is measured by the Dogmatism scale, and difficulties which are primarily due to the subject's resistance to change a single belief as measured by a Rigidity scale such as that of Gough and Sanford (1952). The referent of dogmatic thinking is within a total cognitive framework of ideas and beliefs organized into a relatively closed system; while rigidity refers primarily to the difficulties in overcoming single sets or beliefs encountered in attacking, solving, or learning Specific tasks or problems (Rokeach, 1960, p. 183). This dis- tinction between Dogmatism and Rigidity has been related to the above mentioned distinction between analysis and synthesis by Rokeach and others (1955). Persons who Score high on Dogmatism have difficulties with synthesis, while persons who score high on Rigidity have greater difficulty in analysis. These results have been replicated using the entire range of scores on the dogmatism scale (Fillenbaum and Jackman, 1961), although certain a5pects of the problem used to test these differences, namely the order of presentation Of the hints used in the Denny Doodlebug problem have been questioned (Lyda, L. and Fillenbaum, 1960), indicating that the construction of the problem used is Of great importance. The differences found between Open and Closed subjects who were either high or low on rigidity has 10 not only been found to Operate on conceptual problems such as the Denny Doodlebug problem, but has also been related to perceptual problems following Witkin (1950). Rokeach and Levy designed problems for which separate measures of analysis and synthesis in perception were possible which were found to be related.to dogmatism and rigidity. (Rokeach, 1960) Similarly, Huberman has reported differential behavior of extremely high and low dogmatic subjects on certain Kohs block tasks (1961). After having found dogmatism and rigidity to be re- lated to analysis and syntheSis in cognition and in per- ception, it would seem profitable if deductive and inductive reasoning could be related to analysis and synthesis. Upon examination of the processes which are measured by Johnson (1962) it is apparent that those problems which emphasize deduction do so by making it difficult for the subject to apply a general rule while in the inductive problems, it is difficult to produce the general rule or to state the re- lationship for Which the two words presented are examples. The process Of deduction is similar to analysis in that the subject is required to break the existing structure of the problem into its component parts in order to determine which of the alternatives Offered indeed fits as a part in the relationship, and which of the alternatives do not. Induction and synthesis if not altogether the same process at least share the common feature in that they are both concerned with putting the various parts of the problem together, or taking what has already been given 11 the various parts of the problem. Rokeach, in Speaking about the distinction between these two interrelated aSpects of cognitive functioning states: "There are many ways of talking about these two aSpects: the resistance to change of systems of beliefs; rigidity and dogmatism; the analysis phase and the synthesis phase in thinking and perceiving; the overcoming of sets and the inte- gration of sets; the separation of an item from a field and its reconstruction into new fields. Perhaps we are also dealing with the process of deduction and induction." (Rokeach, 1960, p. 289) If the above description Of Johnson's measures of deduction and induction as being similar to analysis and synthesis in which the subject must make a judgment concerning a relationship between concepts holds true, then rather Specific hypotheses may be formulated re- garding the relationship between dogmatism with synthesis and induction and of rigidity with analysis and deduction. It is to be expected that those persons classified as dogmatic, that is resistant to change of belief systems, should have greater difficulty solving problems empha- lsizing induction than persons classified as Open or less resistant to change Of belief systems. Further, it is anticipated that those persons who are classified as being rigid Should have greater difficulty in solving problems emphasizing deduction than would persons classified as being more flexible. Third, it is expected that the ability to do well on problems emphasizing deduction should be re- lated to analysis as measured by the Denny Doodlebug problem. Similarly, it is expected that the‘ability to do well on problems emphasizing induction should be related to the ability to do well on conceptual synthesis. Hypotheses. The Specific hypotheses of this study are as follows: Concerning induction: 1. The Open group should solve inductive problems with fewer errors and faster than the closed group. 2. The rigid group should not differ from the non-rigid group with reSpect to this variable. Concerning deduction: 1. The Open group should not differ from the closed group with reSpect to this variable. 2. The rigid grOUp should make more errors and take more time to solve problems emphasizing this variable than should the non-rigid group. Concerning synthesis: 1. The Open group should be faster than the closed grOUp. 2. The rigid grouP should not differ from the non-rigid group with reSpect to this variable. Concerning analysis: 1. The Open group should not differ from the closed group on this variable. 2. The rigid group should be slower and discover fewer Of the beliefs without outside help than the non-rigid group. The relationship between analysis and deduction should be demonstratable in that those who do well on analysis should also do well on deduction. The relationship between induction and synthesis should be such that those who do well on synthesis should also do well on induction. NETHOD 13 Penulation. The subjects used in the eXperiment came from a pOpulation of over 300 students enrolled in either introductory psychology, education, or political science at Kichigan State University during the summer of 1963. '1" 4: .1, O Ufl mati~m Scale. he degree of Open.ess-closedness (w- L1 for each individual was measured by Rokeach's Dogmatism U) cale Form E1 which can be found in the Appendix. The (:1 ogmatism Scale (U0 items) along with the Cough-Sanford " scattered Rigidity Scale (22 items) and 2h "filler items throughout the scale to disguise its purpose was adminis- tered to the students in a classroom setting. To each of the questionnahwistatements the subjects reSponded indi- cating how strongly they agreed or disagreed by means of the following scale: +1 - I agree a little, +2 = I agree I! )n the whole, +3 = I agree very much, -1 I disagree a little, -2 = I disagree on the whole, -3 I disagree very much. The subjects were informed that the question- nairerwas part of a larger study and that they might be con- tacted later. The subjects were told before the question- naire was passed out that if they did not wish to partici- pate they could leave, although none Of the students did in fact leave. The following instructions were printed at the tOp of the questionnaire: 1For a full discussion Of the construction of this scale, see Rokeach (1960). 11 Rigidity Scale. The degree of flexibility in the ability to change single beliefs was measured by the Gough and Sanford Rigidity Scale which was mixed in with the Dogmatism Scale. The items of this scale can be found marked Rigidity in the Appendix. The instructions for the entire questionnaire were the same for all ques- tions, subjects indicating strong agreement with the item by marking +3 and indicating strong disagreement with the item by marking -3. The referents in this scale are for Specific tasks or habits rather than total belief systems. Rokeach has pointed out Hun;dogmatic and rigid think- ing are discriminable processes not necessarily indepen- dent processes. The correlation between the Dogmatism and \ Rigidity scales rangesfrom .37 to .55. (Rokeach, 13*0, p. 193). The correlation between the Dogmatism and Rigidity Scales for the subjects used in this eXperiemnt was .7 which is artificially high due to the process by which the subjects were selected for the eXperiment. The fact that these are two discriminably different processes has been supported by the results of two factor analyses, both in- dicating that the Dogmatism and Rigidity Scales are measur- ing essentially independent psychological dimensions (as reported in Rokeach, 1960). Subjects. Twenty students with the highest scores and twenty students with the lowest scores on the Dogmatism Scale were asked to come into the laboratory for individual 15 testing. They were told that they had been selected on a random basis in an attempt to know something more about the general pOpulation of students who were given the questionndre in class. Three of the high Scorers and two of the low scorers were not available for individual testing either because they were only attending the first five weeks of the summer session, or refused to make an appointment for individual test- ing. These were replaced by the students with either the next highest or lowest score. The two groups were then further divided into high and low scorers on the Rigidity scale. The subjects were selected in a we such that the eXperimenter did not know which of the resulting four groups the subject belonged to until after the in- dividual testing was completed. To quij dealing with negative scores, a constant of 160 was added to the Cogmatism score after algebrai-- cally summing the responses. Hence, scores on the Dogmatism Scale could range from +¥O for complete dis- agreement with each statement to +280 for complete agree~ ment to all statements. The range actually Obtained was from +85 to +l?~. \ As indicated in Table I below, low scorers on Dogmatism differ on the average about 62 points from the high dogmatic scorers. Low scorers on Qigidity differed from high scorers on the average of slightly under five points on dogmatism. Table I--Comparison of Means and the Dogmatism Scale Standard Deviations of Group High Dogmatism--High Rigidity High Dogmatism-~Low Rigidity Low Dogmatism-~High Rigidity Low Dogmatism--Low Rigidity High Dogmatism--combined Low Dogmatism--combined High Rigidity--combined Low Rigidityevcombined N 10 10 10 10 20 20 20 20 Mean 182.7 177.8 120.2 115.5 180.25 117.85 151.u5 1h6.65 Std. Dev 11.186 8.108 13.315 12.u21 9.835 12.762 3h.223 33.550 The scores on the Rigidity Scale were treated in a similar fashion in that a constant of 88 was added to each score to avoid dealing with negative scores. The possible range of scores after the constant has been added is from +22 to +15“, although in the sub- jects tested the obtained range was from +57 to +120. Table II below indicates that low dogmatic subjects differ from high dogmatic subjects on the average of about 17 points. Low scorers on Rigidity differ from high scorers on the average of approximately 23 points. Thus the precedure was successful in separating the groups on Rigidity but the relationship between the Dogmatism and the Rigidity scorers is also quite strong. 16 Table II--Comparison of Keans and standard Icviation on the Rigidity E‘cale Group N Teen Std.fcv. . - o - o 0 0 fl 0 K . 0 High 9 gmatism-—Uigh Rigidity 10 10?.6 v.15i High Cogmatism——Low Rigidity 10 86.7 7.25'2 Low' llxjnatism--Tfigfl1 Rigiditj' 10 92.2 7.800 Low Bogmatism-—Low Rigidity 10 69.6 8.072 High Dogmatism-—combined 20 09.15 13.639 . . . (‘1 r'\ ' I Low Dogmatism-—comb1ned 2O 00. O 14.1‘0 o a c ' o o ' C’ High Rigidity--comhined 20 100.00 11.2ld Low Rigidity-—combined 20 78.15 12.0U5 Trocedure. In the individual testins situation, the . , \ '9 students were given two kinds of problems to solve. First the :ndsjects wordnvitd1003 or very small distances, but not less than one inch per jung. 5. Joe cannot turn around. The Situation [Joe has been junping all over the place gettixg some exercise when his master places a pile of food three feet (jirectly west of him. Joe notices that the pile of food i.S a little larger than he. As soon as Joe sees all this f‘ood, he steps dead in his tracks facing north. After all T115 erercise Joe is very hungry and wants to get the food 3.5 quickly as he possibly can. Joe examines the situation and then says, "Darn it, I'll have to jump four times to Set the focal." firing ?roblem Joe Doojlcbug was a smart bus anj he was dead right Q VLt1 his conclusion. Why do you sunpose Joe Doodlebug had J. t30 take four Jumps, no more and no less, to reach the food? 10 \.J The hints which were given are as follows: 1. The facinf belief. "I'm $0115 to give you a hint: Joe does not have to face the food in order to eat it. (Repeat hint.) CI, T'll give you five minutes more." 2. llna<fiircctirni heliirf. "T'1J_;jive yetizLiother*liint: Joe can jump sideways and backwards as well as forwards. (Repea hiit) T'll give you five minutes more." 3. The movement helici. "Let's read the problem avain. L) (The eto ..‘ nfi - n h ‘(j ,'JO'\JAA-C~tl.:3;}1 .‘L . Onv/l ' .OJOJ1 .CU . . . -,-3 n ”‘0 1 .‘.l-.l(1 “H. ’1 O H A 0'4 I ..J H 4"." "x 3 k .3 ( J \ O (3 K J \\ \Q Total .91932 \I \D r“+!\r‘r-t. r. ml l‘AVLLV...‘ l_._‘. ...! 10 hypothesis that the Open subjects should be sunerior to the closed subjects and tlat the rigid subjects should not differ from the non—rt subjects is.supported by all three measures of synthesis on the Benny Doodlebug problem. Table XIII presents the mean solution times of the four groups cf subjects separately as well as for the his-.1 and 101: dogmatic subjects and the ri.f-:id and non- id subjects is consistent for all tLree measures as .‘ predicted while the difference between the ricid and non— \d (J rigid subjects is negligible. Table XIII. Fean Solution Time for the Penny Doodlebug Probler Following the first, Secon., and All Three Beliefs. GrOUp First Second All Three Belief Belief Eeliefs T o o o I o ' high Dogmatism-High hlgld 20.82 15.74 8.6% High Dogmatism-Low Rigid 20.37 23.99 14.15 Low Dogmatism-High Rigid T.02 12.95 5.t2 Low Dogmatisnhlxnr Tieid 11.61 €.b0 1.95 e1~h :ohnatism 21.595 19.515 11.,95 LOV Dogmat'sn 5.? 10.675 3.d20 High Rigid 19.92 1n.8u5 7.165 low fiigid 3.99 15.9»; 8.200 Table XIV below gives the summary table for the analysis of rarianee of the time required to solve the problem after the first belief had either been overcome by the subject or had been give: to the subject in the form of a hint. It may be seen that there is a sin- nificent difference between the high dogmatic and the low dogmatic subjects. Yo such difference exists between the rigid and the non-rigid subjects. Vowever, there is a significant interaction between Dogmatism and Rigi (L 'ity indiCating that the degree to which dogmatism determines the subject's performance for this measure of synthesis is influenced by the degree of rigidity for that subject. kl) \ 111 m 2 e of Lie lime fiequirel to jenny Ceadlebfiv Problem after tne ‘ n x G I) (7‘ ,4 (D l id . 1 '4 O 7 i i—J V L -f O “5 ,4 1 {J '1 L..— :3 O ‘v-s --fi.-‘y-\ -- -. I- l ...J C. .1 r4- \< ’3 t J H O J O 1...; O O U 0 fl 5 r-f- p.40 f. ’r ‘4 \ 1 k1 D \.l O i—J \JI \'\ .3 O 1 D .0 0 IA J O [—4 /(Q mgr) a c-L :3 (J J ‘\ J J J s E) d U H 0 J1 A v N .4 ‘i J ,. ad 1 f... I‘» k $ 4 .0 ('f‘ ,1 O .0 rod .4 .4 J H. v c r. L. (Id f—l \J U) P3 0 h’ d- :1 O with.tfiu3 tine requirwus‘ta -olve tin: eroble: sifter the ence between the coon and the closed ”rouge while there . \J . ‘ 1 -I u r\ . 1‘ _ ‘ I 1 _ d ,' V _ _ ’1 .fi ‘ _o N yo ’3 is no suca u1.fereace setueen t)? 1131d and no —r1g1 “ o P‘ ' ‘. "‘ \ ~ 5 . - v ‘ : J- : -‘- u grouns. L.avn, thelc 1s a 2:3n1f1ea2t interact101 between dogmatism and riridity. ’TablcsffV. .knalyhyis of“Vari;ntee CH7 the TTbne ihequirwxi to Felve the ienny Toodlebng Problem after the Lecont Telief. I 'oA r.) L) .1 ('1 o . I .- f“) o o L. o "1 H t.) ,1. 0 v‘ in O ['2 E) rt. id. (0 \l \J (0 \JJ 21\ H \l .\J O I J \ ) Ch H \I \1 ’\ O O H -’.. . O "' - 4-! :— Dogmatism x 37c.225 1 379.225 5.52 <’.0) Lligixiit3r ... . Q Q f tithin 3039.330 2 .3 0 ‘Total 2222 222 2: filTI lei to t] -8 ~ Table '1‘. ' ' v - . - . . N ‘ r T) ' '- al).;i: oi u;ruzztce of ‘tne ..ine .mxpiiree iolve he Tee‘v “oodlebuj Frohlen after l_'NJree "Pfi‘cfs. SSCRC; §um of d.L. Team P p Seuares Square ratio . ff ,a( a [I ‘f H.. _ O Do’fxzzatism 59).:7; 2~ 1 39.73625 11.01601 Rigidity 10.712?5 1 10.71225 .2] ... eogmatism x 22%.00625 1 228.0062; 19.51605 R’jidity .. C“ (K. ’ -' r? "‘73" ‘ thln 1C1?.1)CBOO 3’) you/‘41:!) Totgl 2655.15775 3? Analveie. The hypothesis that the rigid -ubject$ would he slower and discover fewer of the beliefs Ly themselves Ivoulxl be jeets we required 0 di b Table *TII gives conceptual analysis in terms 0 time required '3 V It may b 1. the closed for the it is belief '_\ L4 rid tile 1': .1. L11 ncni—ri tlia seen suoj second onlgr as a woulfi scever the a to overcome 4 L -‘ ,— enat. file d v (-0 zion-Ifigjid to be large enough to be the hypothes 4%.r1 LJLU gzwau;)e tile (:“P' lare from): 1 'V“ ”Al- ary 4" l 4. th orze, tlfO ifference e on f‘ -..‘J‘. ‘. LLL of subjects While there Open and closed sab- the time the eata concerning e average amount of 5!, Au , a1 all hree beliefs. between the and Open and becomes larger but even for the third between the "3 T‘ \Q. irst belief nd eigafi7icaut. ereaee between the rigid See“ that it consistently takes the rigid srbje eets lonjer to overcome each of the beliefs, nartieiilarly for the second and third beliefs fable KVTI. Congarison of Teen Number of Finutes Taken J to Tvercome the Tirst Telief, and the First Two Teliefs, and All Three Beliefs. Crown :irst First Two 111 Three Zelief Eeliefs Eeliefs High Bogmatis: Tigh “igid 9.72 15,60 ”2.90 High Dogmatism-Low Xijid 3.51 3.39 31.93 a v o o o 1" Low Cogmatism-gigh .igid 9°89 1€,15 23.91 Low Costaat" rn—Low igid (5.1:; 0.1;; 15 .90 High sor.atism C.115 1/,C05 22.b15 Q a Low Togmatism c.165 12.800 19,§0) Y_. ‘ ‘ j" m 0 . \ p Q - high i_id1t} 7.9” 15.275 23.H05 Low ‘.T1Ci 3 7.49 10.9? 18.?15 Table VVIII presents the summary table of the analysis of variance 0; the tiztie reqvired for either the sub to discover the first belief for himself or to have it given to him as a hint. It is apparent that the d'ffer- ences between the risid and the non-rigid subjects fall short of being statistic lly significant and that the differences between the Open and closed subjects is extrernely small. The hyoothesis is not confirmed by this data. \ .J U! to .\ Table XVIII. Analysis of Variance of the Time Requ1red to Overcome the First Relief. SOURCE Sum of Kean F o Squares‘ d.f. Square ratio Dogmatism 9.025 1 9.025 .2 ... Rigidity 53.82: 1 53.824 1.60 A 1 -v “ 1 A‘ a ‘ w“ ' o f‘ ‘ 1 r ‘ .Jul . L_tnr rll ”lac fillCLi ml Heex V.' "\ sfi C 13 ~11) . 1 me c ’7 (NW '. -' fi‘ ’ V1 ..1 , 0- . . - -.-/' v - -/' . ‘1 _, U \L 11.4 L~ lji) \' J "V‘ \f 1“ \. n ‘ r 0.1. ‘ _- r q .0 (7 : f3 3" drl‘ql‘:) _ \_K£ \ ». -_) ‘7 .4) J , 11" VI 1 J— . - / 'l '1 r) M 0‘] 1‘L.- ‘tc l T.\ (7‘ + ‘ t H, [I ‘r‘(‘ ‘1 (" ~5‘ ' l N ::L ~qwere z .b/ ,; P _ 1 '- o - I ‘ ‘- ‘1 — L! 1. L" ' - - .- .l t- -. - , 1.05, t1 .1 .10 L.-.O to: 19.. .0531. m’-~I-—‘——O - m“? mrt'vr-y- I: _ 9 \ 0 _ ,1 x . ' ' "W ’ '— uole .h. -. ”33tee215.(1:. Tedzetzona«TW 4‘..eQUIred 0e ‘49 .N" ) Y‘1 II A _ Q r\‘ hr 0 f‘ ‘ 11' ‘ . roslew reter .ul ,Jre\ ,ochls .a? con r‘- - . V 7‘ 1 ': 1- : J- _ W. at -. r re . exe'eo e .3 men‘ctu corn)» .po.e 1‘J 010v +3 ' -1 ° ,. . n ._ ‘ .. 1. ' , . 'm - . I . r‘. ‘1 - .lze e ,'1:1 o‘ee ; Tait 'o 1 .d’>\ ()9 t we rCHchLnT 1 . -1 o '_ . _ . . ‘ vfi~1euL/1‘ eflnetLOJ. —— n ~a , "\ *0 " I v v 4 " \ . L v- V -‘ u.;?1 o_ 34tes LeQ‘Lre! to olxe J. 7* ’ ' 1 , t, ’\ p _ ,‘ 1 1 1 1‘1 I 7~ ’ _ I" ‘ 1‘ 7 " ‘ _roele. “.ier .Ll .nree 01L“;S an ~03.1 ‘K'Tr‘cij ‘l 1‘ . n ‘I (30 f) ’3 r ( .' r n (1’ , .3 1| ‘- — .‘ . ,. u_/ g ‘) L“) Q ('1? \) i'.‘1(‘ p.30 ‘rfi w-x K ‘1, ,' 1' ‘—' r f‘,“ u-I /: 3-! r3 I'fv rre a 1‘303 “ole: ./M) c J , f - 1 r" 1’ f“ . r‘. '~‘~v- 71 1‘ «- v'r‘ f) [1’ I. r) "\ .L .3~V-L ‘- -\.)I ~ j‘) ‘4 I) 3 -1 F‘.‘ o I Q ’ ‘1 'q u‘ Ll —\lL‘.k‘\:~O : . I 'L.— .‘ P “I o l- c = 1 ~ 1 1 .1; *7 r.- v . .° 3 . ) les teej .{/ tee tallCU teat 1 r v --.. - r x 1‘ “ . " ‘\ L m‘ - 1 ‘ ‘1‘ r - :-~ .u ‘- ~ ‘Pfjj ,5 n ‘ '4zu,tlow. lne IU’MfitIC“‘ tlit ~Afl35eeas -‘ - ~ - L r r‘ 1 e. . - a, 1 r - " u ‘ -‘: ‘ . . :10 co 5311 or 1n0;ys1~ noutd 4.30 .0 R011 on flevuctJOT .-q 4— ‘ AA . .... ‘ .‘I, , _‘ _7 r _ ' i ’ 1 ‘_. 1‘: F-‘r- -' ‘.’I"TT" - ”Cu Tet eon;11 ‘. by two teta. 3510. _.‘fT eel '?!r ' ° __ , - , Y .' . belxw' "1\%) the rhiaeetefi.€recnw‘de1rhs and tJna cql smvtaro , , . . t‘ ‘ _ 1-1 , “fl _ . V.‘ _ . _ .., , , _‘ 1 . :L111}~.'s e~ :30t*1 one \re ;:.;t::w1 1 vlex :uxs tare 311<3rs .. . Lu , ~ , ‘ .1. ° '- 1 ‘ ' v I . ., 1 r‘ 1- r ,e H new“ on ‘re :mect:on nrooteud v1t1 {we nuaoer o. eelxets vr‘ 9“ - - . L h . ~ ‘ I‘ ' '1‘ - \ v . '4‘ I * A‘ o 1 q‘ ' ,‘ ' N f1 1‘ r.‘ oneteo e 1: tWe LerL ijteea .LH”&C£ or two e1Hy do leowv l 3 1+3 problem. In both comparisons, those who do well on deduction indicated by either a high preparation index or few errors, also seem to do better on analysis. How- ever, in neither comparison does the relationship between deduction and analysis reach statistical significance. As in the previous chi square analysis, for purposes of statistical evaluation, the table is reduced to a two by two contingency table. Table XXVII. Analysis and Deduction--Comparison of Number of Beliefs Overcome in First Fifteen Minutes of DD Problem and the Number of Errors Made on Problems Emphasizing Deduction. o Beliefs l Belief 2 Beliefs A11 3 Beliefs 0-5 Errors U 7 2 6 & above 9 6 Q 2 d.f. - 1 p less than .20 one tailed test Table XXVIII. Analysis and Deduction-~Comparison of Number of Beliefs Overcome in First Fifteen Minutes on DD Problem and Preparation Index for Problems Emphasizing Deduction. 0 Beliefs l Belief 2 Beliefs All 3 Beliefs P.I. below .325 u 8 3 5 P.I. above .325 9 5 3 3 Chi Square = 1.823 d.f. = 1. p less than .10 one tailed test By way of comparison to the relationships previously tested in Tables XXVII and XXVIII, Tables XXIX and XXX - o ' 1‘ I '* - v, . . ~ 1, '4' x \ or“_n't t.3 Olaorncd -rocHDRClo: bl; soc Ch ,analc r ~u n v - v v— 1 o . J . u- I 4* . . .. 1 . 1 . ...... anal}'. s o- lhl 's Klijl 11(RK3L107. o TWTlltl'Viul .. ‘ _ - J- 1 7 . 4_1 ‘ 0 . O a c L‘ lrod'c 0‘ jot con thV“ variables 32' it is QHLtC clccué that tfllore isszlo oxfi larities may be found with Specific changes in the administration of the uroblem. Table KIQIT below gives _ I the findings from a similar study reported by iokeach contrasting dogmatic thinking with rigid thinking (2 Reach, 1950, p. 188). Table YYYIT. Ability to Analyze: Comvarison Tetween ?igyid zine! Text-rig id (IrOLnis {MIC TTOtixaen Closed and 7pon Crouns on ”can Yumber of Tinutes Taken to Cvercome the First Telief, the ?irst Two Peliefs, and All Three feliefs. 1’ it] 71% o d ‘H'J‘: we All Three Groun First T ' efs :eliefs r-‘Cf‘ . . f F, (I Tigid 3.“ 1;.) 1-.0 W . o I .on-rigid 5.? 13.1 1 .4 0 FJ O an O (L \1\] (3 c H -J ‘J l—I ‘ J \D 15.; l It is seen that the closed and Open groups do not differ from each other in analytic thinking while the rigid group usually takes longer to analyze than the non-rigid group. The conclusions are sinilar to those reported from Table XXXIV. and the times are reasonably similar. Certain differences appear when comparing the results of these two studies on their measures of the ability to synthesize. Table below gives the results from Rokeach's study. Table XXXIV. Ability to Synthesize: Time taken to Solve the Doodlebug Froblen after the First, Second, and Third ?eliefs Were Cvercome By Rigid and Vonrigid Crouns and by Closed and Open Groups. GrOUp After First After Second After Third Belief Belief Belief Overcome Rigid 1h.5 7.2 3.2 Honrigid 15.1 6.6 2.8 Closed 15.2 7.7 3.3 Open 13.“ 6.1 2.4 While the general conclusions that the Rigid do not differ from the Nonrigid and that the Closed take longer to solve the problem than do Open subjects, the mean solution times are considerably shorter than those reported from the present study. However, it should be noted that whether or not a solution is reached in the study reported by Rokeach, the session was terminated 30 minutes after the problem was given to the subject while in the present study, the subject was permitted #5 minutes to solve the problem. In another_study done by Rokeach and Vidulich (as reported in Rokeach 1960), in which the subjects were given MO minutes to solve the problem, more compatible times are reported for synthesis in Table KKKV below while lower mean times for analysis (Table XXXVI) are reported. This second difference would appear to be due to another difference in adminis- tering the problem as the hints were given at the end of 10, 15, and 20 minutes. Table XXXV. Synthesis: Nean Time Taken to Solve the Doodlebug Problem After the First, Second, and Third Beliefs Tad Been OverCOme. Group After After After First Belief Second Belief Third Belief Closed—memory 21.9 16.0 11.3 Spen—memory 13.2 9.0 4.2 Table XXXVI. Analysis: Mean Time Taken to Overcome the First Belief, the First Two Beliefs, and All Three Beliefs Group First Pelief First Two All Three Beliefs Beliefs Closed-memory 3,8 9.7 13.3 Op n-memory 3.9 9.1 14.0 The general conclusion which comes from such coupar- isons is that the differences between dog.atic and rigid thinking are consistent, found reflect differences but the Specific differences in the testinr situation. \n Ix; 53 rt I x v .'\_ 7‘ \r U\,4.‘.4&Lh To test the relationship between synthesis and analysis and induction and deduction with dogmatism and rigidity, Pokeach's Dogmatism Scale and the Cough—Sanford Rigidity Scale was administered to 331 undergraduates at Tichigan State University. The 20 students scoring highest ans the 20 students scoring lowest on the Dogmatism Scale were selected for individual testing. Poth groups of subjects were further divided into two groups of ten each ¢ on the basis of their scores on the Pigid J ~1e. Ho ty Sc 9 The first task consisted of a set of twenty five analogies emphasizing induction, and another set of analogies emwhas'zing deduction. The subject's per- p—I formance on this taS' was measured in terms of the relative amount of time spent on the preparation period of the problem and on the solution phase of the problem as well as the number of errors made. The second problem was the Benny Doodlebug Problem with separate measures of analysis in terms of the time required to overcome the beliefs of the problem and the number of beliefs the subject could overcome by himself, and synthesis m asured in terms of the number of minutes required to solve the problem after each of the beliefs were overcome. Ln The hypotheses tested were: I. The Open group should solve inductive problems faster and with fewer errors than the closed groun. The rigid group should not differ from the non-rigid group on this variable. This was supported by the data. II. The non-rigid groue should solve deductive problems faster and with fewer errors than the rigid group. The Open group shoulJ not differ from the closed group on this variable. There was an insignificant trend in the Opposite direction. III. The Open group should solve the synthesis part of the Denny Doodlebug problem faster than the closed group. There should be no difference between the riqid and the non-rigid groups on this variable. This was supported by the data but there was also a significant interaction between dog- matism and rigidity. TV. The non—rigid subjects should overcome more beliefs without outside assistance and faster than the rigid grOUp. There should be no difference between the Open and the closed groups. In general, this was subported by the data. V. There should be a relationship between the ability to solve deductive problems well and the ability to do well on analysis. A non-significant trend in the direction predicted appeared. VI. There should be a relationship between in— duction and synthesis such that those who do well on one task should do well on the other. A non-significant trend in the predicted direction was observed. The general conclusions of this study are that: (l) in a problem solving situation in which the primary task of the subject is to integrate various beliefs of component parts of the problem Open subjects are SUperior to closed subjects; ('2) in a problem solving situation in which the \n \A task of the subject is to analyze or discover the parts of the problem, non-rigid subjects are superior to rigid subjects; (3) there is a tendency for subjects who excel in synthesis to also excel on inductive reasoning; (4) there is a tendency for subjects who excel in analysis to do well on problems emphasizing deduction. Adorno, T.W., Frenkel-Erunswik, Else, Levinson, D.J., and Sanford, 9.3., (1950). The authoritarian :esonality. Yew York: Harper. Clarke, a.G. (1929). The Art of itraight Thinhing, A Primer of Scientific Tethod for social Ingyiry. few York: D. Appleton-Century Company. Dewey, John, (1910). How We Think. I'Tew York: D.C. Heath and Co., Publishers. Edwards, A.L. (1960). ngeriucntal Wesign Tn Pcycholooical Research. ‘l'ew York: Holt, Ileinhart, and inns-ton. Fillenbaum, S. and Jackman, A., (1051). Dogmatism and anxiety in relation to uriblem solving-—an extension of Rokeach's results. J. Abnorm. Soc. Dsychol., 63: 0rd, J. ., P.P.. (195 abilities: Guilf Comrey, A.L., Green, and Christensen. T“: T:‘ 1\.‘.’ O). A factor-analytic study of reasoning I. Uynoth0505 and descrififlon Of tCStFo Reports from the Psychological Laboratory, Los hngcles, University Cailford, J.P., of R Green, .F., Christensen, A factor-analytic study To. 1, Southern California. P.R.. (1951). of reasoning_abilities: II. . 11 1“ 1“»4.‘ .ihistration Reports from the no (A. of tests and r3sychological .L 1“ alysis of results Laboratory, 30. 3. Los Angeles, University of Southern California. Cough, H.G., and Sanford, R.¥., (1952). Rifiidity as a psychological variable. University Assessment Harvey, O.J., playing. 11a7 (07). Holzinger, K.J. two factor Huberman, J. of Rokeach's study on dogmatism. of California, and Research. Unpublished manuscript; Institute of Personality (1963). Cognitive determinents of role Technical Report 20. , and Harman, H.H., analyses, Psychometrika, Contract Jonr 3. (1938). Comparison of 3: 35-60. (1961). A re-examination of certain ascects {Tn publ i shed . A . Thesis, University of British-Colombia. 56 / pf/ /’ IV ’4 57 Johnson, D.E., (1955). The Psychology of Thought and Judflwent, Yew York: Harper and Brothers. Johnson, D.H., (1960). Serial analysis of think'ng. finn. H.Y. Acao. Sciences. 91: 66-75, Johnson, D.Y., (1961). Formulation and refermulation of figure concepts. The Amer. J. of Psychol. 7U: M18-U2b. Johnson D.T. (1962). Serial anal sis of verbal analos v 9 y t. problems. J. Educ. Dsychol. 53: 86-88. Johnson, 9.x., and Hall, E.R. (1961). Organization of relevant and irrelevant words in the solutiOn of verbal problems. J. Psychol. 52: 99—10%. Johnson, D.r., Lincoln, R.F., and Hall, E.R., (1961). Amount of material and time of preparation for solving nroblems. J. Psychol. 51: h57—Q71. Laffey, J.L., (1957). A theoretical and empirical analysis of loyalty' to and defection from a belief system. Unpublished H. A. Thesis. Tichigan State University Library. Lyda, L., and Fillenbaum, 8., (1960). Dogmatism and problem solvin5--an examination of the Denny Doodlebug problem. EcGovney, W.C. (1953). C gmatism and rigidity as determirantsc of cognition. Unpublished 7.A. Thesis. Michigan State University, Library. filler, I.E., (1915). The Psychology of Thinking. Iew York: The Xacmillan Company. Oram, A.. (1957). Some determinants of the formation and modification of new belief systems. Unpublished T A Thesis. Xiehigan State Vniversity Library. Rokeach, N., (195%). The nature and meaning of dogmatism. Psychol. Rev., 61: 191—20u. Rokeach, ‘3, C1960). The (Huni end Close] Tjrui. New Yorlu Basic Books. Rokeach, 1., FcCovney, W.C., and Denny, Y.?., (1955). A distinction between dogmatic and rigid thinking. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 51: 87-93. Swanson, T.S., (1958). Problem solving as related to ‘ dogmatism, depersonalization, and familiarity with chess. Unpublished TEA. Thesis. ITichigan State University Library. 58 Thurstone, L.L., (1938). Primayy mental abilities. Psychometric Monogr. Y . 1, Chicago, Fniversity of Chicago Press. Thurstone, L.L., and Thurstone, T.C., (1931). Tactorial studies of intelligence, Psychometric Tonogr., Yo. 2, Chicago, Yniversity of Chicago Press. Vidulich, R.Y., (1956). The integration of multiple sets / into a new belief system. anublished ?.A. Thesis. Tichigan itate T’niversity Library. Vidulicn, 3.1., (1060). A re-cxamination of the distinction between dogmatic and rigid thinkfirmg Unpublished Manuscript. Vidulich, R.‘., Adams, 3., and Creenbaum, 3., (1960). ?elief Congruence and Cognitive Isolation in “aired Associate- Learninw. Paper read at Southeastern Psychological Association, Atlanta, Georgia. Vidulich, P.I., and Kaiman, W.P., (1961). The effects 0f information source status and dogmatism upon conformity behavior. Unpublished Manuscript. Vinacke, W.F., (1953). The fsvchology of Thinkinh. Yew York: NcGraw—Till Book Company, Inc. T ‘l L 'alker, H.N., and Lev, J., (1953 a ). Stetistocal Inference. ‘En: Yorfiiz Holi:, Reiiniart, 1 rd Vfiniston. Witkin, U.A., (1950). Individual differences,in ease of perception of embedded-figures. J. Fersonolity. Within, H.A. (1962). PyycbOIOSicel differentiation: studies of develonemnt. Sew York: Wiley 59 APPENDIX Analogies Emphasizing Induction 1. ink cuttlefish sting bee 2. visit invade friend enemy 3. sturgeon caviar hen egg h. ascend decend up down 5. pregnant meaningless full empty 6. inauguration completion infant adult 7. retina cochlea eye ear 8. demise cadaver birth baby 9. statue sculptor book author 10. oxygen tank milk bottle 11. focus periphery center circle 12. total section book chapter 13. progeny ancestry forward backward 1h. numismatics hobby waltz dance 15. naive 50phistieated simple complex 16. flask stepper bottle cork 17. noxious injurious large big 18. tabby Spaniel cat dog 19. senility adulthood evening day 20. surplus sufficiency more enough 21. jury convince target aim 22. automatic V machine sour acid 23. regard eager danger afraid 2U. criminal prison bird cage 25. anecdote relate meat carve 10 \-J O\ I.) (Q 10 b.) 10 .(I‘ to \J\ 1u1a105fix7s thm.asi7ixm~ (‘T‘x‘ yard write round shoe mea horse bottle kin Ln \ LJ (‘9 t.) double key win (“”0 La beginning foot letter square 011t txvo arm foot vugar cot milk Small least fork ground for thick fail Deduction m nocular safe drama tightrOpC acQuire omega swo rd seal circular creditor protein herbivorous V'C in perennial increase groom heavenly entry diverge solvent 60 binocular combination asset eject audixvzce acrobat dispose alpha dagger envelope cubical debtor (h:et elbow bulb carbohydrate carnivorous 'blood anrnlal converge i31rfl:rtw3t PSYCHOLOGY QUEST IONNA IRE ’1 10. 11. 12. 13. 11. 15. 16. The following is a study of what the general public thinks and feels about a number of important social and personal questions. The best answer to each statement below is your personal opinion. We have tried to cover many different and Opposing points of view; you may find yourself agreeing strongly with some of the statements, disagreeing just as strongly with others, and perhaps uncertain about others; whether you agree or disagree with any statement, you can be sure that many other people feel the same as you do. On the IBM answer sheet which you have been given, mark each statement in the space provided according to how much you agree or disagree with it. Please mark every one. Blacken spaces +3, +2, +1, -1, -2, or —3 depending on how you feel’in eadh case, using the following numbers: +3 I agree very much. -3 I disagree very much. +2 I agree on the whole. —2 I disagree on the whole. +1 I agree a little. -1 I disagree a little. Are there any questions? Please make no marks on this booklet. A person who thinks primarily of his own 17. It is only when a person devotes himself to happiness is beneath contempt. an ideal or cause that life becomes meaning- ful. I an often the last one to give up trying to do a thing. 18. If I could get into a movie without paying and be sure I was not seen I would probably The main thing in life is for a person to do it. want to do something important. 19. In this complicated world of ours the only Everyone at times thinks about things too way we can know what is going on is to rely bad to talk about. on leaders or experts who can be trusted. In a discussion I often find it necessary 20. I never miss going to church. to repeat myself several times to make sure I am being understood. 21. There are a number of persons I have come to hate because of the things they stand for. There is usually only one best way to solve - most problems. 22. I like to know some important people because it makes me feel important. lost people Just don't know what's good for them. 23. There is so much to be done and so little time to do it in. It is not always easy to tell the truth. - 24. I usually maintain my own opinions even In times like these, a person must be though many other people may have a differ— pretty selfish if he considers primarily his ent point of view. own happiness. 25. It is better to be a dead hero than a live I prefer work that requires a great deal of coward. ‘ ' attention to detail. 26. I do not like everyone I know. A man who does not believe in some great cause has not really lived. 27. A group which tolerates too much differences ' of opinion among its own members cannot lost people get angry sometimes. exist.for long. I'd like it if I could find someone who 28. I find it easy to stick to a certain sched- would tell me how to solve my personal ule, once I have started it. problems. 29. It is only natural that a person should I often become so wrapped up in something have a much better acquaintance with ideas I am doing that I find it difficult to turn he believes in than with ideas he opposes. my attention to other matters. 30. I gossip a little at times. Of all the different philosophies which ‘ exist in this world there is probably only 31. While I don't like to admit this even to one which is correct. myself, my secret ambition is to become a great man, like Einstein, or Beethoven, or I dislike to change my plans in the midst Shakespeare. of an undertaking. ‘ 32. I do not enjoy having to adapt myself to new and unusual situations. -2- 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. Even though freedom of speech for all groups is a worthwhile goal, it is unfortunately necessary to restrict the freedom of certain political groups. Some people vote for men about whom they know very little. If a man is to accomplish his mission in life it is sometimes necessary to gamble ”all or nothing at all." I prefer to stop and think before I act even on trifling matters. lost people Just don't give a "damn" for others. Once in a while I laugh at a dirty Joke. A person who gets enthusiastic about too many causes is likely to be a pretty "wishy- washy” sort of a person. I try to follow a program of life based on duty. To compromise with our political opponents is dangerous because it usually leads to the betrayal of our own side. At times I feel like swearing. If given the chance I would do something of great benefit to the world. I usually find that my own way of attacking a problem is best, even though it doesn't always seem to work in the beginning. In times like these it is often necessary to be more on guard against ideas put out by people or groups in one's own camp than by those in the opposing camp. I do not read every editorial in the news- paper every day. - In a heated discussion I generally become so absorbed in what I am going to say that I forget to listen to what the others are saying. I am a methodical person in whatever I do. Once I get wound up in a heated discussion I Just can't stop. I think it is usually wise to do things in a conventional way. There are two kinds of people in the world: those who are for truth and those who are against the truth. Iy table manners are not quite as good at home as when I am out in company. Ian on his own is a helpless and miserable creature. The United States and Russia have Just about nothing in common. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 62. 63. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. 76. -3- In the history of mankind there have probably been Just a handful of really great thinkers. I always finish tasks I start, even if they are not very important. The highest form of government is a democracy and the highest form of democracy is a government run by those who are most intelli- gent. I would rather win than lose in a game. The present is all too often full of unhappi- ness. It is only the future that counts. I often find myself thinking of the same tunes or phrases for days at a time. Unfortunately, a good many people with whom I have discussed important social and moral problems don't really understand what's going on. I have a work and study schedule which I follow carefully. Fundamentally, the world we live in is a pretty lonesome_place. I find it hard to make talk when I meet new people. It is often desirable to reserve Judgment about what's going on until one has had a chance to hear the opinions of those one respects. I usually check more than once to be sure that I have locked a door, put out the light, or something of the sort. The worst crime a person could commit is to attack publicly the people who believe in the same thing he does. I have never done anything dangerous for the thrill of it. In the long run the best way to live is to pick friends and associates whose tastes and beliefs are the same as one's own. I believe that promptness is a very impor- tant personality characteristic. lost of the ideas which get printed nowadays aren't worth the paper they are printed on. I am always careful about my manner of dress. It is only natural for a person to be rather fearful of the future. I always put on and take off my clothes in the same order. Iy blood boils whenever a person stubbornly refuses to admit he's wrong. When it comes to differences of opinion in religion we must be careful not to compro- mise with those who believe differently from the way we do. .e mam—,3. 2 1".D' «a BB 3m USE WU HICHIGRN STRTE UNIV. LIBRRRIES I III III II lllll ll 9 8 312 31044 4625