up!" RECREATION AREA “DAY-USE" INVESTIGATION TECHNIQUES: A STUDY OF SURVEY METHODOLOGY WITHIN MICHIGAN STATE PARKS Thesis for the Degree of M. S. MICHIGAN. STATE UNIVERSITY DOUGLAS MELVIN CRAPO 1969 W4“ .1.‘ 7‘.- U IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII L _ ;-~ I THESIS 3 1293 10456 . Y . . .-- 0‘ t, _ ,_ . |f..‘ t! 6' 6"?" ’2 IV} ‘t‘ ’ 'I ‘1’" lel .1 (504.. 1:. ‘4 ”a'ximl‘wim‘.um. III ' 7 "‘ ABSTRACT RECREATION AREA "DAY-USE" INVESTIGATION TECHNIQUES: A STUDY OF SURVEY METHODOLOGY WITHIN MICHIGAN STATE PARKS By Douglas Melvin Crapo This study was concerned with designing and testing a data-collection system that would gather accurate information about recreation area ”day-users. " "Day-users" were considered to be those individuals who used recreation areas for a single day or a portion thereof. Since it was impractical to separate "day-users" from ”campers" in the data-collection stage, information was gathered from each group and then separated during analysis. The information collected was in three general categories: socio-economic characteristics of the users, user "origin" and travel characteristics, and activity characteristics while in the area. Limitations imposed upon the data collection system were that it must be inexpensive, it must yield accurate data, and it must be implemented into the existing administrative framework of Michigan State Parks. Douglas Melvin Crapo The common methods of data collection were reviewed and the self-enumerative questionnaire was chosen as being "best, " considering the limitations outlined above. However, as is the case with most self—enumerative questionnaires, there is a vital problem of survey bias resulting from non—response. Essentially, this study deals with measuring the bias effects of non—response within a sample, and with testing various methods used to attempt to reduce non-response. A careful investigation of sampling techniques and question- naire construction was conducted. On the basis of this investigation, an initial questionnaire and dispersal-retrieval system were designed. The questionnaire had an attractive format and appeared to be brief (although it did contain a substantial number of questions). Ques- tionnaire distribution was through hand-out by the contact station ranger at the area entrance. Retrieval of questionnaires was accomplished by means of "voluntary deposit" into a collection box placed near the area exit. A representative sample of all Michigan parks and recreation areas was then chosen for the testing. In the ”seasonal" phase of the study, a random systematic sampling procedure was employed to distribute questionnaires to I every nth vehicle entering the 'seasonal" sample parks during the months of July and August. Of the total number of questionnaires Douglas Melvin Crapo handed out, approximately 63 per cent were returned. However, only 39 per cent were totally completed and were included in the analysis. Each sample park was then visited by interview teams and information was obtained from nearly all park users (hereafter called "parameter data"). The information collected by ”voluntary deposit” throughout the summer months was then compared to ”parameter data. " It was found that at the 95 per cent level of con- fidence the accuracy of the ”voluntary deposit" information was very high. Travel information (characterized by large standard devia- tions) was the only data that showed an accuracy problem. The "experimental" study phase was concerned with identi- fying and testing numerous variables that might affect the amount of non-response. Testing of each variable was carried out for at least two days in all of the "experimental" sample parks. Each variable was tested individually, and the results of that particular variable were evident through changes in the ques- tionnaire response rate. It was found that the ”questionnaire" which had the greatest amount of response possessed the following characteristics (ranked in descending order of importance). (1) The questionnaire was handed out at the entrance to the recreation area and was collected by means of "voluntary deposit" in a box at the recreation area exit. Douglas Melvin Crapo (2) A pencil was supplied with the questionnaire in order to facilitate completion. (3) The questionnaire was printed on a heavy card-stock. (4) The color of the questionnaire was, in order of preference, blue, white, brown, tan, yellow and orange. (5) The questions concerned with activity analysis were constructed so as to be "closed" or "structured. " The use of a simple chart was most effective. The small difference in amount of return between the "regular" length questionnaire and the ”post card" length indicated that, as long as the questionnaire appeared to be of short length, response rate was unaffected by amount of content. The printing of a pre-paid address on one side of a "post card" type questionnaire did not appear to increase the number of questionnaires returned when a "voluntary deposit" collection system was used. The inclusion of a slotted hole in the questionnaire did not appear to increase the response rate enough to warrant its addi- tional expense. The data collection system designed and tested in this study may be used as a framework for future investigations of recreation area users. RECREATION AREA ”DAY-USE" INVESTIGATION TECHNIQUES: A STUDY OF SURVEY METHODOLOGY WITHIN MICHIGAN STATE PARKS By Douglas Melvin Crapo A THE SIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Department of Resource Development 1969 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to express my sincere appreciation to all those who have assisted me with the preparation of this thesis. I particularly wish to thank Dr. Michael Chubb, thesis advisor and Director of the Recreation Research and Planning Unit, for his generous assistance in all phases of the study. His enthu- siastic encouragement, conscientious guidance and liberal allotment of financial aid were largely responsible for the success of this project. Special thanks also go to Louis F. Twardzik (Chairman, Department of Park and Recreation Resources), Raleigh Barlowe (Chairman, Department of Resource Development), and Harry Webb (Department of Sociology). In addition to their suggestions and assistance with this study, they have also had a profound influ- ence upon my professional education and life. Mr. William Colburne (Division of Recreation Resources and Planning) and Mr. Paul Rearick (State Parks Division), together with other staff members from the Michigan Department of Natural Resources, aided in study design and data collection. The close ii cooperation received from all Department of Natural Resource employees was most gratifying. Thanks are also extended to: Dennis Gilliland (Department of Statistics and Probability), for unending patience and guidance during the statistical design and analysis phases. Willard G. Warrington and the staff of the evaluation ser— vices unit, University College, for assistance in data processing. Lauren P. Brown, David Marquette and the staff of Univer- sity Mimeograph and Printing Service, for quality printing under the most trying circumstances. John Kohmetscher, Charles Poland, and the staff of Appli- cations Programming, Computer Laboratory, for computer pro- gramming and analysis assistance. Ronald Hodgson, Julia Jones and other graduate students and student assistants, for unselfish assistance in the design and implementation of the study. William Lontz and the staff of the Office of Planning Co- ordination, Governor' 3 Office, State of Michigan, for assistance in study design and the arrangement of additional funding. Lastly, I wish to gratefully acknowledge the assistance of my wife, Jan. Her untiring assistance and loyal support were a constant aid during this investigation. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGMENTS..................ii LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES Chapter I. THE PROBLEM AND DEFINITIONS OF TERMSUSED................1 Introduction Statement of the Problem Significance of the Problem Limitations of the Study Assumptions Hypothesis Sub -Hypothe s e 5 Definitions II. REVIEW OF METHODS OF DATA COLLECTION................13 Introduction Survey Bias Available Methods of Data Collection Observation Use Advantages Disadvantages Characteristics of Data Obtained Through Questioning iv Chapter Page Personal Interview Uses Advantages Disadvantages Questionnaires Uses Advantages Disadvantages Design of the Questionnaire Correction of Non-Response Comparison of the Personal Interview and Questionnaire Survey Methods III. DESIGN OF A DATA COLLECTION SYSTEM..................46 Determination of the Desi red Information Determination of the "Best" Survey Method Design of the Survey Instrument and Dispersal-Retrieval System Sampling Choice of a Population Sample Choice of Sample Parks and Recreation Areas Design of the Dispersal-Retrieval System Questionnaire Design IV. ACCURACY OF COLLECTED INFORMATION...............64 Preliminary Preparations Questionnaire Design Sample Size Sampling Method Implementation of the Continuous Data Collection System Collection of Parameter Information Implementation of Parameter Information Collection Treatment of Collected Data Analysis of Data Results and Conclusions Chapter V. DETERMINATION OF "BEST" QUESTIONNAIRE AND DISPERSAL— RETRIEVAL SYSTEM Determination of Variables Which Might Influence Questionnaire Response Means of Choosing "Best" Alternatives Within Each Variable Sampling Method Implementation of the Testing Schedule Analysis of Data Results and Conclusions VI. RECOMMENDATIONS General Recommendations Recommendations to the Michigan State Park System SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY APPENDICES Appendix A: "Seasonal" Questionnaire Appendix B: Weekly Report Form Used in "Seasonal" Sample Parks . Appendix C: "Short-Form" Questionnaire Used as the Interview Schedule Appendix D: Optical Scan Forms Used in Coding Appendix E: Typical "Experimental" Sample Park Test Instructions Appendix F: Description of Various "Experimental" Questionnaires Appendix G: ”Experimental" Questionnaires Handed Out, Returned, and Completed vi Page 91 115 123 128 130 131 132 134 136 145 LIST OF TAB LE S Table Page 1. Sample Parks of the Michigan State Park System Included in This Study . . . . . . . . . 58 2. Number of Questionnaires Handed Out, Returned, and Judged Complete within ”Seasonal" SampleParks 83 3. Number of Completed Questionnaires Obtained from "Seasonal" Sample Park Users During Parameter Information Collection . . . . 84 4. Variables from "Day-User" Questionnaire Information Which Show a Significant Difference in a Comparative Analysis . . . . . 86 5. Variables Which Might Affect Questionnaire Response and Which Were Capable of Being Tested in This Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93 6. Schedule of "Experimental" Tests . . . . . . . . . 101 7. Results of the Testing of "Experimental" Variables..................107 vii Figure 10. 11. LIST OF FIGURES Interview Structuring and Interview Depth . Michigan State Parks and Recreation Areas Indicating Sample Park Locations Vehicles Waiting to Enter Dodge Brothers #4 State Park Contact Station Rangers Handing Out Questionnaires Handing Out Questionnaires While Remaining Within the Contact Station . View of Motivational Sign and Pneumatic Traffic Counter Hose From the Contact Station Sign Warning Motorists of Their Approach to the Deposit Box Deposit Box and Sign Pneumatic Traffic Counter Modified for Systematic Sampling Use Workers at the Study Center Preparing Experimental Questionnaires for Shipment to Parks Activity Analysis Questions from Ques- tionnaires in Experimental Test #2 viii Page 23 58 72 72 73 73 74 74 75 75 141 Figure 12. Back View of "Post Card" Questionnaires Used in Experimental Tests #5 and #8 13. Front View of ”Post Card” Questionnaires (2—4-1, 2—4-2, 2-4-3) Used in Experimental Test #5 14. Front View of "Post Card" Questionnaires (2-5-1, 2-5-2, 2-5-3) Used in Experimental Tests #5 and #8 ix Page 143 144 144 CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM AND DEFINITIONS OF TERMS USED Introduction In early 1968, authorities in the Michigan Department of Conservation expressed a need for detailed information about the Michigan State Park "day-user."1 In the past, limited information about campers had been collected while data on ”day-users” was virtually nonexistent. The desired information was of the type that would expose trends in recreation area usage. These trends would then be used in the planning process, to ensure that state parks provide the types of activities and facilities that appear to be most desirable. Since "trends" are changes over time, collection of trend information must take place over an extended time period. Thus, the means of A "day-user" is an individual who uses a recreation area for the day or a portion thereof. He does not reside within the recreation area during usage. collecting the desired information would be through a continuous data collection system. An investigation into the design of such a system revealed that there were too many ”unknowns" to be able to both design and implement a data collection system of the desired scale within one season. This study represents a compromise from the above situation--the first step in the eventual implementation of a continu- ous data collection system throughout all state parks and recreation areas in Michigan. Statement of the Problem The problem is to design and test a system to collect recreation area "day-use" information on a continuous basis within Michigan State Parks. Specifically, the problem is to design a data collection system using a questionnaire survey instrument and then test the system for accuracy of collected information. The questionnaire survey instrument has many inherent problems when accurate information is required. Can these prob- lems be eliminated or at least decreased to the point at which col- lected information is still of value to administrators? Significance of the Problem The predicted increase in outdoor recreation participation, the fixed amount of land available for outdoor recreation facilities, and the expected growth in population size combine to focus attention upon a large increase in demand for outdoor recreation opportunities. ”One thing is clear; the conventional approach to providing outdoor recreation is not adequate for present needs, and it will certainly not be adequate for the future. "1 These facts point out the importance of good planning and administration of recreation areas. But adminis- tration depends upon accurate data to provide an information base for decision making: It is a tribute to the recreation leaders of the past that they were able to accomplish so much without the support of syste- matic research. . . . As a major activity in contemporary social life, and as a broad field of public and private enterprise, outdoor recreation has had relatively little systematic study. It is generally agreed that much of the research completed [to date has used questionable survey methods and such low levels of statistical confidence as to render the collected data suspect. Although these studies are of value in delineating problem areas and in investi- gating new survey research techniques, the data collected is often little better than no data at all. Unfortunately, regardless of past inaccuracies, recreation survey results have been used in the decision-making process. 1Outdoor Recreation Resources Review Commission, Out- door Recreation for America (Washington, D. C. : Government Printing Office, 1962), p. 81. 2Ibid. , p. 183. As administrators recognize the value of research, the demand for accurate data is increased. Sound planning, facility and program development, budget preparation, and economic analysis all require basic data about the recreation user and the recreation site. After the conceptual framework is designed for a particular problem, and the appropriate information collected and presented in an effective form, alternative choices can be formulated from the data and the most appropriate decision can be made. Some of the specific problems facing recreation agencies in Michigan are out- lined in Michigan' 5 Recreation Future. 1 Among those mentioned are the lack of funds, the increasing pressure on resources, the insufficient planning for recreation use, the low level of appreciation of values of recreation, and the inadequate facilities to meet the demand. In the past, the majority of outdoor recreation research has been centered around the camper. The problems presented by camp- ground use occupied a disproportionate amount of a recreation manager' s time. As a result there has been a great deal of interest in collecting and analyzing information about the camper. He, of all park visitors, is one of the few with a fixed location for part of his 1Michigan Department of Conservation, Michigan' 5 Recrea- tion Future (East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan Department of Con- servation, 1966), pp. 16—17. visit, and a campground provides a definite population from which to draw samples. Most campers are gregarious and loquacious, hence, good research subjects. Indeed, the main problem encoun- tered in obtaining information from campers is in terminating the interview. ”Day-users" have frequently been ignored--probably because of the many problems that they present to survey research. In com- parison with campers, "day-users" are mobile-~creating problems in filling the sampling frame if information is to be collected at one location. Either the information must be obtained at the site or a record of attendance must be made (i. e. , recording license plate numbers) so that contacts can be made at a later date. It is the general impression of recreation authorities that "day-users" show a higher degree of motivation than do campers. The amount of time available to pursue recreational activities is more limited for the "day-user”--the recreation day must also include time for travelling to and from the activity area. Therefore, the ”day-user" is gen- erally anxious to begin his activities, and during participation he dislikes being interrupted. At the completion of the activities, he is usually in a hurry to return to his place of residence. Yet, in most outdoor recreation areas, ”day-users" com- prise the majority of recreation visits. In 1967, "day-users" accounted for 67 per cent of the total park visits within the Michigan State Park System. Campers comprised the remaining 33 per cent. There are two principal ways in which outdoor recreation data can be gathered: Special surveys which are designed for one specific problem or need and are generally short-term studies; and continuous surveys which are designed to collect data over an ex- tended period of time. Continuous data collection surveys are the only means of exposing usage trends. By relating trends to changes in society, predictions about future demand and usage can be made. Because of its ease of administration, the questionnaire has been used more than any other survey method. 1 The questionnaire, however, has several problems associated with it which must be overcome in order to obtain accurate data from its usage. These problems are outlined in Chapter II, and several solutions are sug- gested. As the volume of information collected increases, it becomes increasingly more important to put collected data into a form which permits rapid analysis. Also, the involvement of public departments, 1Within the context of this study, "questionnaire" is inter- preted to mean the printed, self-administered questionnaire. "Interview" is interpreted to mean the verbal interview in which there is both an interviewer and respondent. The printed format for the interview is referred to as the "interview schedule” or "schedule. " such as utility and transportation agencies, in the planning process forces recreation planners to put their data into a form that can be used by the other agencies. The increase in popularity of data processing/computer types of data analysis and storage is with good reason. Information in this form can be adapted to a number of uses and can be made available to a number of nonrelated agencies. The ultimate choice of what data is required and how it will be collected rests with the administrator. Administrators and decision—makers must be cognizant of the relative advantages of data collection methods, and the limitations of the collected data. Limitations of the Study The following are limitations of this study: 1. This study will be limited to a representative sample of Michigan State Parks and Recreation Areas. 2. It will investigate only those individuals entering a Michigan State Park or Recreation Area by means of an authorized entrance; specifically, an entrance that is regularly supervised by Michigan State Park staff. 3. It will be limited to the period July 1, 1968, to Septem- ber 5, 1968. 4. This study will be conducted within the normal limita- tions upon recreation area user surveys: a) Although the number of people surveyed is generally large, the amount of money budgeted for research is small. A desirable data collection method maximizes the benefits for all financial inputs. b) Visits to recreation areas are generally made with enjoyment as the purpose. Any interruption for information collection must be made as pleasant and as short as possible. c) Information collection must take place as soon after the experience as possible. This is especially true when objective recall data is sought. Errors in recall of information are difficult to evaluate, and such errors should be decreased by collecting the desired information immediately upon completion of the activity. d) It is generally desirable that data collection systems be made to work within the existing administra- tive and operational framework of the recreation area. In Michigan State Parks, the staff are usually operating near maximum load without the additional burden of a survey. This is especially important when a continuous data collection system is being considered. 5. This study will consider only those data collection sys- tems that will function within the limitations outlined. Assumptions The following assumptions are made within this study: 1. It is assumed that all Michigan State Parks and Recrea- tion Areas fall within the classes outlined in the classification of outdoor recreation areas proposed by Clawson in Land and Water for Recreation. 2. It is assumed that previous attendance figures for all State Parks and Recreation Areas are accurate. 3. It is assumed that through intensive research during the heaviest use period of the recreation system, a system can be designed that could be operated throughout the total recreation year. 4. It is assumed that with all the information on hand, a correct judgment can be made as to which method of data collection is "best. " 5. It is assumed that when objective questions require answers of recall, both interview and questionnaire techniques will produce similar data to the same questions. Hypothe sis Accurate data about the socio—economic characteristics and usage patterns of Michigan State Park users can be collected on a 1Marion Clawson, Land and Water for Recreation (Chicago: Rand McNally and Company, 1963), pp. 13-16. 10 continuous basis by means of a questionnaire survey instrument and a random systematic sample. Sub -Hypothe S e s 1. Through a process of trial and selective elimination, a questionnaire and dispersal-retrieval system can be designed which will obtain a greater number of usable completed responses than other questionnaires tested under similar conditions. 2. Questionnaires returned within the proposed dispersal— retrieval system will contain information that does not differ sig- nificantly from the parameters of the park -using population of the Michigan State Park System. Definitions The following are definitions of terms used within this study: accuracy: when dealing with a sample, this term refers to the close- ness to the actual population parameter. administrative framework: the overall policy, planning, design and operation relating to a particular area. M: having the highest degree of desirability, suitability or advantage. This judgment is made after considering several 11 factors, among them cost, information desired, and the existing administrative framework within which judgment is to be made. contact station: a booth designed to control and regulate the vehicular traffic entering and leaving a specific area. ”day-user": any person entering through the recognized entrance into a Michigan State Park who is not included in the follow- ing two categories: 1. camper--users who camp within the park overnight. 2. official and service entrants--individuals performing a recognized service within the park (1. e. , park staff, Coast Guard, police, concession employees, telephone repairman). fixed alternative ("closed" or "poll") questions: those questions which offer the respondent a choice among two or more alternatives. interview: is a face-to—face interpersonal role situation in which one person (the interviewer) asks a person being interviewed (the respondent) questions designed to obtain answers per- tinent to the purposes of the research problem. 12 open end questions: are those that supply a frame of reference for respondent' 8 answers, but put a minimum of restraint on the answers and their expression. repetitive entry: a vehicle entering a particular park two or more times in a single day. state park or recreation area: any area administered and maintained by a state park and recreation agency for the recreation of the public. vehicle: any type of self-propelled, wheeled conveyance (i. e. , car, bi cycle, motor cycle). Chapter II will examine the methods of data collection com- monly used in recreation area user investigation. The uses of each method, together with its advantages and disadvantages, will be reviewed. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF METHODS OF DATA COLLECTION Introduction In the past, the particular survey research method used seemed to be dictated by "availability of subjects and expense rather than suitability of the technique to the criteria the writer had in mind for the evaluation. "1 Even present research within the field of out- door recreation apparently is "dictated more by administrative con- venience . . . than by meaningfulness of data collected. " Before the particular method of data collection is chosen, the researcher and administrator must "weigh" each method and find the one which most closely fits the requirements of the research situation. Perhaps a combination of methods would yield the most 1W. E. Deming, ”On Errors in Surveys, ” American Soci- ological Review, 9 (1944), pp. 359-369. 2Marion Clawson and Jack L. Knetsch, "Recreation Re- search: Some Basic Analytical Concepts and Suggested Framework for Research Problems, " Proceedings of the National Conference on Outdoor Recreation Research. Ann Arbor, Michigan, 1959, p. 15. 13 14 accurate information. The steps in deciding which research technique would be most applicable in a given situation are as follows: 1. Decide what information is desired. 2. What special characteristics does the desired informa— tion reflect? Can the information be obtained through solicitation of objective facts or is the respondent forced to state attitudes and opinions ? 3. Consider the administrative framework within which the research is to be attempted. What funds are available? What period of time is available for the data collection and analysis? What staff will be available and what is the level of competence of the staff? 4. Weighing all of the advantages and disadvantages of each technique, and considering the data collection process within the administrative framework, decide which technique or which combina— tion of techniques will best fill the research requirements. Survey Bias The possible sources of bias within sampling surveys are numerous and there has been much investigation concerning them. Deming lists thirteen major types of bias together with an even larger 15 number of possible sources of error. 1 An excellent discussion on the sources of error in surveys is also presented by Cochran, who concluded that the potential value of a research project is dependent upon the care given to selecting, evaluating and controlling sources of variation. 2 In most surveys, one of the most important bias sources is due to non—response. A later section within this chapter will discuss this problem in detail. Motivation of the subject is extremely important to all sur- vey research. Unless the subject wants to cooperate and is made to feel that the information he has to offer is important, the validity of the collected data is suspect. "When the circumstances in which the report occurs leave us to suppose that the subject' 3 motivation or the pressures to which he is exposed are such as to prevent a candid report, we are not likely to give it much credence. "3 Very little is known about the motivational process itself and this area is in need of further research. Cannell and Fowler suggest that there is no 1Deming, op. cit., pp. 359—369. 2William G. Cochran, Sampling Techniques (New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1964)--Chapter 13 deals with all possible error sources and contains a summary on page 389. 3Claire Selltiz, et a1. , Research Methods in Social Rela- tions (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1967), p. 237. 16 value in research which does not attempt to interest or motivate the respondent in the survey. 1 Available Methods of Data Collection Several methods are available for data collection, among them personal interviews, observation, questionnaires, telephone surveys, and review of records that have already been established. Telephone surveys have little to recommend them beyond speed and, in some cases, low cost. If most respondents are located in the same general area, as is the case with some regional park authorities, telephoning might be useful in obtaining information. However, telephone surveys are usually "limited by possible non- response, uncooperativeness and by reluctance to answer more than simple, superficial questions. "2 Review of already collected records usually does not yield the type of information sought in recreation research. Possible uses of records are to validate responses which have been collected else- where, and to act as a source for addresses of individuals to be 1Charles F. Cannell and Floyd J. Fowler, "Comparison of a Self-enumerative Procedure and a Personal Interview: A Validity Study, " Public Opinion Quarterly, 27 (Summer, 1963), 263. Fred N. Kerlinger, Foundations of Behavioral Research (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1967), p. 397. 17 contacted for future research. Usually outdoor recreation agencies do not have these records available and must go to a secondary source to obtain the desired information (i. e. , Department of State). If, for instance, one wishes to obtain a random sample of vehicles entering a park and then contact them later by mail or tele- phone, the process becomes complicated and costly. First, a random sample must be chosen. Second, the vehicles within the sampling frame must be identified and the license plates must be recorded. Third, the license plate numbers must then be forwarded to the De- partment of State. Fourth, the Department of State identifies the owner of the vehicle--this process usually takes a period of months. Fifth, the vehicle owner must then be contacted by mail or telephone to elicit his cooperation in the research. One must also keep in mind that the vehicle registrations at the Department of State are not always up to date and that a certain percentage of the sampling frame will be lost. A description of the uses, advantages and disadvantages of the three most popular methods of survey research follows. Observation Use Observation reflects the network of actions and reactions among group members, or the objective behavior of individuals. As 18 a scientific tool, observation can be an accurate means of measuring actions to the extent that it: (1) serves a formulated research purpose, (2) is planned systematically, (3) is recorded systematically and related to more general propositions rather than being presented as a set of interesting curiosa, and (4) is subjected to checks and controls on validity and reliability. 1 The type of observation that is most commonly used in recreation research is non-participant observation. In this method, the observer watches and records behavior patterns and does not attempt to participate in or interfere with the group. This type of survey research can produce information about how people use the recreation area. Questions such as "what is the amount of use of an area (visitor-load) at a specific time?", ”what is the size of the visitor group?", "what are the activities being participated in?", or ”what is the length of stay?" can be answered through use of the ob- servation method. The National Parks Service of Canada has made extensive use of observational techniques in order to obtain data for the above que stions. Advantages 1. Observation can uncover patterns of action that the participants themselves do not fully comprehend. An observer is 1Selltiz, op. cit., p. 200. 2Specific surveys using the observational method are listed in the Bibliography of this study. 19 unbiased and can offer an objective interpretation of group behavior. 2. Behavior is recorded as it occurs. The researcher does not have to depend upon people' 5 retrospective or anticipatory reports of their own behavior. 3. Observation, if the subject is unaware that he is being watched, yields information that is not influenced by anything except the person's own orientations. Direct questioning of an individual will often influence his behavior after the questioning situation. Disadvantages 1. Highly structured and methodical observations seem suitable only for fairly small groups, although an observer (or better, several observers) can effectively record impressions of the "social atmosphere” or ”general behavior" of somewhat larger assemblages. The limitation of lack of funds for most recreation research will generally ensure that the use of observation within recreation areas will be confined to large groups, thus reflecting general behavior only. 1Matilda White Riley, Sociological Research: A Case Approach (New York: Harcourt, Brace and World Incorporated, 1963), p. 186. 2Selltiz, op. cit., p. 201. 20 2. This method is applicable to action taking place only in the present. It obviously cannot be used to refer to periods prior to the inception of the research; and to extend obser- vation into the future in order to trace a gradual develop- ment of some social system property over time requires a considerable investment of research time. This technique is obviously unsuitable for a continuous data collection system. 3. The areas used for outdoor recreation are usually large, with many diverse activities. Several observers would be needed to obtain a general overview of behavior on the entire area. As we have seen, observational techniques are primarily 'directed toward describing and understanding behavior as it occurs. They are less effective in providing information about a person' s beliefs, feelings, motivations, anticipations, perceptions or future plans; and certainly they provide no information about past behavior or private behavior. To obtain such information, the interview and the questionnaire have been used. Characteristics of Data Obtained Through Questioning Data from questioning reflects the subjective network of orientations and interpersonal relationships; for example, the under- lying ideas and feelings of the members, their dispositions to act 1Riley, op. cit., p. 186. 21 toward others and to define and evaluate these others in various ways. It must be kept in mind that responses to questions may be viewed as resulting from the specific item content of the various questions and the stylistic orientation of the respondent to the ques- tioning situation. 1 In questions about actions or factual data the validity of response is only as good as the recall accuracy of the respondent. Therefore, answers to questions about actions are indirect reports of the action, described by the respondent as he perceived and evaluated it. In addition to the questioning situation, characteristics of the respondent himself will also influence response. The motivation or interest of the subject towards the survey is the most important factor in the majority of studies. 2 Gergen found that age influenced response, especially when opinions were desired. Differences in response of the aged were hypothesized to be due to: lack of familiarity with issues, lack of interest, more constraint in re- sponses, more complex opinions than younger people, less capability of fitting them into forced choice categories, and less willingness to 1Kenneth J. Gergen and Kurt W. Bach, "Communication in the Interview and the Disengaged Respondent, " Public Opinion Quarterly, 30 (Fall, 1966), 385. 2Cannell and Fowler, op. cit., p. 261. 22 prov1de answers on topics he is not familiar With. Education of respondent is also an important influence upon answers to survey questions. Personal Interview Interviews may be classified according to the "depth" of "scale" various aspects of the information desired. An attempt to interview is contained in Figure 1. As one would suspect, the easiest information to obtain is that which is the most "shallow. " Information about occupation, age, and amount of park usage can be obtained from a highly structured interview--one which requires a relatively less-skilled interviewer. At the other end of the "depth" scale is that information concerned with attitudes, feelings and motivations. Interviewing is not as simple a skill as one might first sus- pect. In order to gather reliable information, all interviewers must use the same techniques and interview situations. This necessitates an adequate training program, adequate skilled interviewers, and supervisory control for all interviewers. 1Gergen, op. cit., p. 395. 2Cannell and Fowler, op. cit., p. 261. "Depth" Level Kind of Information SoughL la. "Objective Facts" "Surface" fl 0‘ "Deep" "Personal" l’acts Opinions on relatively non- emotional ”topic Attitude toward relatively "ego-important" topic Feelings (or sentiments) Motivational analysis Pre-conscious . . . unconscious. . . Ve ri fi cation of 23 Validity Interviewer Skill Needed no hi: 0-: e3 “.320 sung we, DOC/i Examples or Characteristics Easier —9 6-— More Difficult Less % 6— Greater More Highly Structured <———-— Unstructured Occupation. home owner- ship, residence, car registration. etc. Law violations, age. income. sex life, etc. What do you think of UN; of Congo; school-bond issue: much legislation, etc. Some relevant aspects: intensity; interrelations among attitudes: involv«-- ment; self-concept: reference groups: ambiva- lence. Often have not previously been clearly verbalized; ambivalence; appeals to symbols of consensus. l. e. , that which "lies behind" opinions, attitudes, feelings: personal goals highly complex relationship to social goals often not clear: not too much ve rifled scientific knowledge avail- able for ”practical" use. Source: Donald W. Olmsted, from material distributed during Sociology 492, Methods of Social Research, Michigan State University, East Lansing, April, 1968. FIGURE 1. -- Interview Structuring and Interview Depth 24 Use The interview has three main purposes. First, it can be used as an exploratory device to help identify variables, to suggest hypotheses, and to guide other phases of the research. Second, it can be used as the main instrument of the research. In this case, the interview schedule will include questions designed to measure the variables of the research. The "Visitor Use Study in New Jersey State Parks" exemplifies this type of interview usage. 1 Third, the interview can be used to supplement other data collection techniques; to validate collected information, to follow up unexpected results, and to further investigate the motivations of respondents and their reasons for responding as they do. Advantages 1. Since the interviewer and the person interviewed are both present as the questions are asked and answered, there is opportunity for greater flexibility in eliciting information. Problems in question interpretation can be noted and corrected immediately. 1New Jersey Department of Conservation and Economic Development, New Jersey State Park Visitor Use Survey 1966-1967 (Trenton, New Jersey: Department of Conservation and Economic Development, 1968). 25 2. The interviewer has the opportunity to observe both the subject and the interview environment. Subjective evaluations can be made without the subject being aware. 3. The personal interview usually yields a high percentage of returns, for it is difficult for people to refuse the interviewer. 4. It can be made to yield an almost perfect sample of the general population because practically everyone can be reached by and can respond to this approach. 5. The information secured is likely to be more correct than that secured by other techniques when "feelings" or motivations are sought, since the interviewer can clear up seemingly inaccurate answers by explaining the questions to the informant. If the latter deliberately falsifies replies, the interviewer may be trained to spot such cases and use special devices to get the truth. 2 6. Visual material to which the informant is to react can be presented. 7. Return visits to complete items on the schedule or to correct mistakes can usually be made without annoying the informant. Thus greater numbers of usable returns are assured than when other methods are employed. 1Selltiz, op. cit., p. 238. 2Mildred Parten, Surveys, Polls and Samples: Practical Procedures (New York: Harper and Brothers, 1950), p. 80. 26 8. The interviewer may catch the informant off guard and thus secure more spontaneous reactions than would be the case if a written form were mailed out for the informant to mull over. 9. The interviewer can usually control which person or per- sons answer the questions, whereas in mail surveys several members of the household may confer before the questions are answered. Group discussions can be held with the personal interview method if desired. 10. The personal interview may take long enough to allow the informant to become oriented to the topic under investigation. Thus, recall of relevant material is facilitated. 11. In unstructured interviews, questions about which the informant is likely to be sensitive can be carefully "sandwiched in" by the interviewer. By observing the informant' s reactions, the investigator can change the subject if necessary or explain the sur- vey problem further if it appears that the respondent is about to rebel. In other words, a delicate situation can usually be handled more effectively by a personal interview than by other techniques. 12. Also, in unstructured interviews the language of the survey can be adapted to the ability or educational level of the person interviewed. Therefore it is comparatively easy to avoid misinterpretations or misleading questions. 1Ibid. 27 Disadvantages 1. Compared to the observational method, the degree to which one can evaluate or predict behavior from interview data is, at best, limited. 1 2. The primary weakness is the interviewer himself. Unless the interviewer is competent and effective the reliability of data cannot be guaranteed. 3. The transportation costs and the time required to cover addresses in a large area may make the personal interview method less desirable than other data collection methods. 4. The human equation may distort the returns. If an inter- viewer has a certain economic bias, for example, he may uncon- sciously ask the questions so as to secure confirmation of his views. In opinion studies especially, such biases may operate. To prevent such coloring of questions, most opinion surveyors instruct their interviewers to ask the question exactly as printed on the schedule. 5. The organization required for selecting, training, and supervising a field staff is more complex than that needed for sur— veys conducted by other methods. 1Selltiz, op. cit., p. 201. 2Jackson, op. cit., p. 11. 3Parten, op. cit., p. 81. 28 6. It is usually claimed that costs per interview are higher when field investigators are employed than when telephone or mail surveys are used. This may not be true if the area to be covered is not too great. If the general public in a community is to be surveyed the costs of securing a representative sample by tele— phone or mail inquiries will probably equal or exceed the cost by the personal interview method, since in the end personal follow -up will be necessary to round out the sample. 7. If the interview is conducted in the home during the day, the majority of the informants will be housewives. If a response is to be obtained from a male member of the household, most of the field work will have to be done in the evening or on weekends. Since only an hour or two can be used for evening interviewing, the per- sonal interview method requires a large staff for studies requiring contacts with the working population. 2 Questionnaires More studies have been made with a questionnaire than any other instrument in the field of social investigation. However, as with all survey methods, questionnaires are impositions--to justify 1Ibid. 2 Ibid. , p. 82. 29 this imposition and to convince the individual that he should fill out the questionnaire, resistance must be anticipated and procedures must be designed to overcome it. Although questionnaire survey methods are generally grouped together for discussion, there are many differences among question- naire techniques which affect response. It has been found that the proportion replying to a questionnaire depends upon: (1) the popula- tion being surveyed--those individuals with a higher economic status and more education tend to have a higher rate of response than others;1 (2) the subject of the survgnas the interest of the respon- dent increases, so does the response rate. 2 Strong agreement or disagreement with the propositions about which respondents are being surveyed also increases response rate. 3 Subjects respond to ques- tions that call for factual answers more often than to those asking for evaluations; 4 (3) the sponsorship of the survey--”A survey with official backing will normally get a bigger response than one eminating from, say, a university or a research agency. "5 If the 1C. A. Moser, Survey Methods in Social Investigation (London: Heinemann Educational Books Limited, 1958), p. 181. 2Ibid. 3Ibid., p. 179. 4 . Jackson, op. c1t., p. 110. 5Moser, op. cit., p. 179. 30 sponsoring body is in some way connected with the population, this is the most favorable survey situation; (4) the questionnaire length;1 (5) the attractiveness of the questionnaire;2 (6) the ease with which the questionnaire can be completed and returned. It is apparent that all means must be employed to motivate the subject and to facilitate completion and return of the questionnaire instrument. Rewards have been investigated as a means of increasing response. A gift of a carborundum stone was made to respondents in a continuing study of recreation mobility in Ontario. It is pre- sumed that the gift aided in eliciting the desired cooperation. 3 In 1940, Hancock compared the effects of rewards on questionnaire return in a study of attitudes toward a retail store. The writer in this case established four groups: the first group received only a mailed questionnaire; the second, the questionnaire and twenty—five cents; the third group of subjects received the questionnaire and the promise that they would be sent twenty-five cents as soon as their completed questionnaire was received by the investigators; the sub- jects of the fourth group were interviewed with the same questionnaire 1Selltiz, op. cit., p. 241. 2Parten, op. cit., p. 391. 3Ontario Department of Lands and Forests, Ontario Park User Survey-—Methodology and Results (Toronto: Ontario Depart- ment of Lands and Forests, 1967), p. A—4, Appendix A. 31 schedule as used in the mail follow -up. For standardization, the interviewer asked the question just as it appeared on the instrument and then marked the form according to the response of the inter- viewee. The interview was much more productive in terms of the per cent of completion. The questionnaire alone led to a 9. 56 per cent return; the questionnaire and quarter brought in a 47. 2 per cent response; the questionnaire and promise of a quarter 17. 6 per cent; and the interview return was 85. 5 per cent. 1 However, if the reward is more than nominal, this may offset the chief advantage of the ques- tionnaire method--its cheapness. The manner in which the questionnaire is distributed and collected is a main influence on amount of return. A questionnaire survey of campers by Taylor in 1965 received a return of from 60 to 80 per cent when the form was handed out after camper registra- tion. When the questionnaire was handed out as an integral part of camper registration the response dropped below 40 per cent. The amount of response seems to improve as the "captivity" of the respondent increases. If the subject feels that he has a 1J. W. Hancock, "An Experimental Study of Four Methods of Measuring Unit Cost of Obtaining Attitudes Toward the Retail Store, " Journal of Applied Psychology, 24 (1940), pp 213-230. 2George A. James and Gordon D. Taylor, "Recreation Use Estimation in the United States and Canada, " XIV IUFRO-Kongress, section 26 (Munchen, 1967), p. 19. 32 definite responsibility to reply, or if he feels he will suffer social disapproval by not replying, the percentage of response seems to improve. A difficulty with mail questionnaires, unless they are sent to a highly motivated group, is that they do not inspire a feeling of ”captivity. " Uses The questionnaire can be put to the same uses as the inter- view, with the exception of an unstructured, in-depth examination of feelings or motivations. As the problems associated with the ques- tionnaire technique are decreased, the usage of this technique will become even more popular. Advantages 1. Several members of a population may be questioned simultaneously, with no increase in research staff. 2. Questionnaires tend to be less expensive to administer than interviews. 3. Questionnaires require less skill to administer than do interviews. 4. The impersonal nature of a questionnaire--the stan- dardized wording, order of questions, and instructions for recording 1Selltiz, op. cit., p. 238. ' 33 responses--can insure uniformity from one measurement situation to another. 1 5. Respondents have confidence in their anonymity and thus can feel freer to express views they fear might be disapproved of. 6. Questionnaires usually place less pressure on the sub- ject for immediate response. Thus, the subject may ponder a diffi- cult question before answering. 7. When information concerning several members of a group or consultation with records is required, questionnaires per- mit consultation and discussion during the completion. 8. The administration of a questionnaire survey can usually be adapted so that it doesn' t interfere with the operation of a recrea— tion area. 9. A mail questionnaire survey can obtain information from people located in scattered geographical areas. 10. The questionnaire may reach groups who are more or less protected from solicitors and investigators. In high-rent apartment houses or private homes where servants protect the occu- pants from solicitors and other doorbell ringers, for example, it is 11bid. 2 Cannell and Fowler, op. cit., p. 262. 34 often difficult for investigators to gain admittance. Of course, the fact that the questionnaire is received at the intended address does not guarantee that it will be filled in and returned. 11. Personal antagonism to investigators, which may lead to refusal to give the desired information, is avoided. 12. It is claimed that the mail questionnaire brings many more returns from the man of the house than does the telephone or personal interview method. Disadvantages 1. The most significant disadvantage is the vital question of non—response. The effects of failure to fill the sampling frame and the efforts to overcome this deficiency will be discussed in a later section. 2. The answers to questionnaires have to be considered as final. There is no opportunity to probe beyond the given answer, to clarify an ambiguous one, to overcome unwillingness to answer a particular question, or to appraise the validity of what the respon- dent said in the light of how he said it. 2 3. The questionnaire is inappropriate where it is impor— tant that the views of one person only are obtained, uninfluenced by 1Parten, op. cit., p. 95. 2Moser, op. cit., p. 177. 35 discussion with others, and where it is essential that one particular person in each household or group complete the questions. When socio—economic class is to be evaluated, information is desired about the ”head of the family" only. It is difficult to ensure that information collected through questionnaires applies only to the "head of the family. "1 4. Questionnaires can be considered only when the ques- tions are sufficiently simple and straight forward to be understood with the help of printed instructions. 5. Questions cannot be treated as independent, since the respondent can see all of the questions before answering. 6. There is no opportunity to supplement respondent' 8 answers with observational data, as there is with an interview. 7. Because most people would rather talk than write, questionnaires must be made very interesting to induce responses. 8. Mail returns from the last third of the respondents come in slowly; hence the mail survey must be spread over a relatively long period, if a high percentage of returns is to be se- cured. 1Ibid. 2Parten, op. cit., p. 96. 36 Design of the Questionnaire Questionnaire length is particularly important. Numerous experiments have found that shorter questionnaires are returned more frequently than lengthy ones. When planning questionnaires, it is desirable to use a color which will attract the recipient' s attention. In some marketing studies, yellow paper was found to have the highest percentage of returns and pink followed closely, but dark colors were not effective. The same investigator also found that when one subject was to re- ceive a series of questionnaires, alternating of lighter colors solici— ted more returns than the continual use of one color. 1 The arrangement of questions in order of interest also has an effect upon response. 2 Lacking the personality of an interviewer to motivate the respondent towards the survey, the questionnaire must be its own salesman. The first question should be easily answered and should have human interest appeal. It should be short, and preferably one that can be answered with a simple yes or no. An early interest is a good guarantee of completeness of response. A few simple questions at the beginning will give the respondent 1Ibid. , p. 161. 2Jackson, op. cit., p. 9. 37 confidence in himself which is invaluable for carrying the subject through later more complicated questioning. After rapport has been established, a few questions asking for the respondent' 5 advice are good for bolstering the ego and maintaining interest. 1 "General questions should precede specific ones, otherwise the respondent is likely to answer the general ones in terms of the earlier specific ones.” After questions are assigned specific wording, the follow— ing points must be checked: 1. Are questions specific enough? Do they ask exactly what is required? 2. Is simple language used? Any words that cannot be understood by the desired population must be avoided. 3. Ambiguity must be avoided. A response must mean one thing only. 4. The use of vague questions encourages vague answers. 5. Avoid leading questions. Wording should be entirely neutral and should not encourage the respondent to take a position in his answer. 1Parten, op. cit., p. 391. 2 Ibid. , p. 214. 38 6. Hypothetical questions will encourage an answer that is more a matter of philosophy or wish than fact. 7. One must be sure that he includes all answer possi- bilities in designing a precoded question. Respondents should not be forced into an answer category in which they do not belong. Whether or not one should use open end questions or fixed alternative questions depends upon the type of answer desired. Colfax found that the open end form resulted in more complete and unbiased reports from high school students concerning socio- economic questions. 1 However, response is higher for fixed alter- native questions than it is for open-ended questions. 2 Selltiz sums up advantages and disadvantages of both forms as follows: Fixed alternative questions have the advantages of being "standardizable, " simple to administer, quick and relatively inexpensive to analyze. . . . A closed question may help to insure that the answers are given in a frame of reference that is relevant to the purpose of the inquiry and in a form that is usable in the analysis. . . . Sometimes the provision of alternative replies helps to make clear the meaning of the ques- tion. . . . A similar function of alternative responses is to make clear the dimension along which answers are sought. . . . Fi- nally the closed question may require the respondent himself to make a judgment about his attitude rather than leaving this up to the interviewer or quoter. 1David J. Colfax and Irving L. Allen, ”Pre—coded vs. Open-ended Items and Children' s Reports of Father' s Occupation, " Sociology of Education, IL (January, 1967), 965. 2Jackson, op. cit., p. 110. 39 Closed questions are more efficient where the possible alternative replies are known, limited in number, and clear cut. Thus they are appropriate for securing factual informa- tion (age, education, home ownership, amount of rent, et cetera) and for eliciting expressions of opinion about issues on which people hold clear opinions. Open-ended questions are called for when the issue is com- plex, when the relevant dimensions are not known, or when the interest of the research lies in the exploration of a process or of the individual' 5 formulation of an issue. The closed question has the advantage of focusing the respondent' s attention on the dimension of the problem in which the investigator is interested; by the same token, it does not provide information about the respondent' s own formulation of the issue, the frame of ref- erence in which he perceives it, the factors that are salient for him, the motivations that underlie his opinions. When these matters are the focus of interest open-ended questions are essential. 1 An excellent description of the steps to be considered in constructing a questionnaire and interview schedule is contained in Appendix C of Research Methods in Social Relations by Selltiz. Within this appendix the points to be considered in training inter- viewers are also outlined. Correction of Non-Response Non-response is a problem because of the likelihood-- repeatedly confirmed in practice--that people who do not return questionnaires differ from those who do. The greater the propor- tion of non-respondents and the extent to which non-respondents 1Selltiz, op. cit., p. 262. 40 differ from those who did respond, the greater will be the bias from non- response . The most inexpensive means of dealing with non-response is by reducing it in the first place. The techniques to accomplish this have been discussed in previous sections. In spite of all efforts, however, non-response will usually occur in all survey research. Some individuals perceive survey research as an invasion of privacy and their hostility is evident in non-response to questionnaires and interviews. 2 Even though a respondent might want to help in giving information, he may be unable to do so. Psychologists have pointed out that we are not aware of many of our most important beliefs and motivations, and hence cannot report them. Most survey researchers will further reduce non-response by use of follow—up waves. Shafer doubled the response to his camper survey through the use of four follow-up waves. A some- what comparable mail back questionnaire survey by the Detroit 1Moser, op. cit., p. 128. 2Stanley D. Bachrack and Harry M. Scoble, ”Mail Ques— tionnaire Efficiency: Controlled Reduction of Non-response, ” Public Opinion Quarterly, XXXI (January, 1967), 267. 3Selltiz, op. cit., p. 237. 41 Metropolitan Area Regional Planning Commission obtained the following completed response: Initial Response 14.2% of Total Number Distributed Additional response following the 19. 3% of total number lst "follow-up" wave Distributed Additional response following the 15.7% of Total Number 2nd "follow-up" wave Distributed Additional response following the 9. 2% of Total Number 3rd "follow -up" wave Distributed Total 58. 4% Another survey within the same regional parks obtained 30. 8 per cent of the sampling frame in the initial response. Two mail follow -up waves produced an additional 28. 8 per cent of the sam- pling frame. 2 It seems apparent that the use of follow—up waves will increase questionnaire response significantly. However, follow-up waves are difficult to administer, expensive and time consuming. 1Detroit Metropolitan Area Regional Planning Commission, Home Survey of Regional Recreation Activities (Detroit: Detroit Metropolitan Area Regional Planning Commission, 1959), p. 19. 2Detroit Metropolitan Area Regional Planning Commission, Park Users Survey: A Survey of Persons Using Eight Selected Regional Parks in the Detroit Region (Detroit: Detroit Metropolitan Area Regional Planning Commission, 1959), p. 21. 42 A low response rate does not necessarily mean the research is of no value: . it would be quite wrong to imply that non-response vitiates the scientific nature of sampling. Mail surveys apart, it is usually possible to keep non-response down to a reasonable level and to estimate roughly what biasing effects it may have upon the results. 1 Statistical adjustments for non-response are outlined in Sampling Techniques by Cochran. Even with 100 per cent returns, the researcher must be cautious about accepting the responses from questionnaires or inter- views. 3 This is especially true when subject matter deals with social approval issues or issues of status. Comparison of the Personal Interview and Questionnaire Survey Methods Comparisons of the interview and questionnaire survey methods can be made on the following basis: 1. Amount of response--numerous studies have shown that the interview method obtains responses from a larger propor- tion of the sample. 1Moser, op. cit., p. 127. 2Cochran, op. cit., p. 71. 3Elwood Lewis Shafer, Jr. , "Effects of Sampling, Location, Period, and Method on Camper Survey Results" (unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, State University College of Forestry, Syracuse Uni— versity, 1966), p. 13. 43 2. Completeness of return--an interviewer can assure that all questions are answered completely. 3. Quality of answer--the presence of the interviewer generally insures that most answers are "usable. ” 4. Cost of method-~unless extensive callbacks are required, the questionnaire method is considerably less expensive. In a recent study, Shafer compared various methods of ob- taining Opinions (preferences) from campers. He used four tech- niques: personal interviews at the site; questionnaires to be completed and returned at the camping site; questionnaires mailed to the camper immediately upon completion of the camping experi- ence; and questionnaires mailed three months following the camping experience. Shafer used four follow-up waves in order to decrease the non-response to the questionnaire methods. He concluded that hand out and mailed survey procedures could be expected to produce about the same total response regardless of the intensity of sampling or the location, but sampling periods throughout the summer may have a Significant influence on return. 1 In comparing other methods with the interview he found that answers obtained from the hand out method differed significantly from corresponding answers obtained from the interview, 19 per cent of the time. The mailed questionnaires 1Ibid., p. 168. 44 varied between 12 per cent and 24 per cent from interview results, depending upon the sample waves included in the analysis. Shafer concluded that: . . based on a complete overview of all these results, the hand out and mailed survey methods-~with three to four waves for non-respondents—-generally seemed to be reliable for obtaining information of the type requested in this study (opinions of the recreation site) if a 10 to 25 percentage point discrepancy from comparable personal interview values is acceptable. However, if only a 10 to 15 percentageggintflffemnfiaL can be tolerated, then the delayed mailed procedure is recom- mended, with the possible exception perhaps of questions that deal with large value categories. 1 Shafer found that the answers for sensitive questions such as income and expenditure patterns were the same for all methods, implying that the potential subject matter which can be accurately treated in future questionnaire surveys seems quite broad and varied. 2 He states that if interviews are out of the question, the delayed mailed survey should be used in preference to hand out or immediately mailed surveys. In obtaining information about issues involving social approval or disapproval, Knudson found questionnaire responses to 1Ibid., p. 168. 2Ibid., p. 174. 3Ibid., p. 178. 45 be more valid than interview responses. 1 The interviews appeared to provide information that has been revised to an assumed public standard or standard held by the interviewer. When information is desired about a person other than the respondent, Cannell and Fowler found that the most valid answers were obtained by the ques- 2 tionnaire method. These authors also found that: . in the interview procedure, education of respondent is the most important variable, and that motivation is a factor only within educational groups; whereas in self-enumerative procedures motivation is a more important variable, and edu- cation of respondent becomes a factor within the "better motivated" groups of respondents. When "objective recall" data is desired, with no attitudes or opinions called for, very little difference is found between responses from interview and questionnaire methods. 4 The following chapter will discuss preliminary aspects of study design. 1Dean D. Knudson, "Response Differences to Questions on Sexual Standards: An Interview-Questionnaire Comparison, " Public Opinion Quarterly, XXXI (January, 1967), 297. 2Cannell and Fowler, op. cit., p. 262. 3 . Ibid., p. 261. 4Ibid. CHAPTER III DESIGN OF A DATA COLLECTION SYSTEM Determination of the Desired Information Several meetings were held with representatives from interested Park and Recreation agencies to determine the type of information needed to indicate "day-use" trends. It was concluded that factual data about the "day-user" was required in three general categories; characteristics of the user, locational and travel char- acteristics, and activity and use characteristics. The total infor- mation outlined as being of value to planners and administrators was as follows: Characteristics of the User 1. Distinguish between the ”day-user" and the camper. 2. Group composition--what kind of group entered the park ? 3. Ages and sex of all individuals in the group? 46 47 4. What is the socio-economic level of the family of the driver of the vehicle? Travel and Locational Characteristics 5. What is the "origin" or residence of the user? 6. What distance is the "origin" of the user from the park? Activity and Use Characteristics 7. What ”type" of a visit is this? For example, one day event or part of a major annual vacation? 8. How often does the subject use the particular park? 9. How often does the subject use other parks? City parks? County and metropolitan parks? State parks? National parks? 10. When did the subject enter the particular park? 11. How much time did the user spend in the park? 12. What activities were participated in and for what length of time? Determination of the "Best'' Survey Method Authorities of the Michigan State Park system advised that a continuous data collection system must function within the following limitations: 48 1. No additional staff would be made available to supple— ment the present staff within each park and recreation area. During the summer season, a maximum of two staff members from division office would be made available for limited periods of time. 2. Since the process of obtaining names and addresses from vehicle registrations would be too costly and time consuming, no methods of dispersal—retrieval were to be considered that would have to use this information source. 3. Data was desired for each park and recreation area. 4. Funds for the research were extremely limited. 5. The data collection system should be put into operation as soon as possible. 6. Data collected must have a known accuracy that will permit it to be used in the planning and management processes. As previously outlined above, all of the desired information was "objective recall, " that is, no opinions or attitudes are sought. "Objective recall" data can be obtained through the use of either personal interview or questionnaire techniques. Considering the advantages and disadvantages of each method as outlined in Chapter II, the data required, and the limita- tions of the Michigan State Park system, the "best" survey technique was the questionnaire method. The one weakness of this technique 49 was the low rate of response, thus jeopardizing accuracy of infor- mation. If this deficiency could be surmounted, questionnaires could provide data of the desired accuracy. Design of the Survey Instrument and Dispersal-Retrieval System The testing of each sub-hypothesis involved different parks, sampling methods and administrative procedures. In order to avoid confusion, the development of the "best" questionnaire and dispersal- retrieval system (discussed in Chapter V of this report) is hereafter ' Testing collected question- referred to as the "experimental phase. ' naire data for accuracy (discussed in Chapter IV) is hereafter referred to as the ”seasonal phase" because the same type of questionnaire was distributed throughout the entire summer season. Although the test- ing of each phase was carried out in different parks, both phases were administered from the same research center during the same time period. Before implementing each phase, several common problems were encountered. The following section applies to both the "experi— mental" and "seasonal" phases of the study. Sampling If one were to survey every user entering every State Park or recreation area, a fantastic amount of funds (and staff, 50 administrative space, and analysis resources) would be required. Fortunately, a sample of visitors will yield the desired information within precise limits of accuracy. The greatest advantage of sam- pling is that it saves time, labor and money, allowing more "distance" for the research dollar. If the sampling procedure is correctly designed and is followed, the research limitations are minimal. In order to obtain a representative sample, that is, to ensure that all segments of the population thatuse the area are ' a random method of sampled according to their relative ”weights, ' sampling should be employed. The criteria of a random sample is that every subject in the population has an equal chance of being included in the sample. A random sample is difficult to obtain within a recreation area. It cannot be obtained by choosing individuals participating in certain activities (because responses will be biased towards that particular activity and other activities will be ignored), or by choos- ing vehicles within a parking lot (because not all vehicles are usually vehicles within a parking lot--some vehicles are parked along roadways, others can be taken to the activity site, i. e. , bicycle). Usually the only locations where truly random samples can be chosen are at entrances or exits. 1The disadvantages of sampling are outlined on page 111 in Surveys, Polls and Samples by Parten. 51 Random sampling is relatively simple. Either the use of random number tables or a systematic sampling system will yield a truly representative sample, assuming that "enough" sampling periods (of "long enough" duration) are used. If the area has two or more entrances or exits the task becomes more complicated. The proportion of total vehicle flow either into or out of the area must be calculated for each entrance or exit. A correspondingly proportionate amount of the random sample must then be obtained for each of the entrances or exits. For example, if a particular area has three entrance/exits, with proportions of total vehicular traffic of 20 per cent, 30 per cent and 50 per cent respectively, then 20 per cent of the total sample should be obtained at the first entrance/exit, et cetera. When a sample is used to represent the entire population, the science of statistics can be employed to make inferences about the population based upon information contained in the sample. Inas- much as populations are "characterized by numerical descriptive measures called parameters, statistical inference is concerned with making inferences about population parameters. "1 Typical population parameters are the mean, standard deviation, or the area between 1William Mendenhall, Introduction to Probability and Sta- tistics (Belmont: Wadsworth Publishing Company, 1967), p. 148. 52 two values of a random variable on a normal curve. Statistical sampling is an advantage over any other way of choosing a part of the population because, when the estimates of the population char- acteristics are made from the sample results themselves, the pre- cision of these estimates can also be gauged from the sample results themselves. An excellent description of all phases of sampling is included in Sampling Techniques by Cochran. Choice of a Population Sample The task of determining the exact sample size is more complicated than one would first suspect. The first difficulty con- cerns the precision required. Ideally, the decision maker in con- sultation with the researcher should decide what precision is needed in sample size calculations. In practice this is difficult to accom- plish: the administrator may not be able to make such a value judg- ment due to the complexity of the problem involved, or he may not fully understand many of the statistical implications of the experi— ment. Based upon the experience of Shafer, 1 the recommendation of Mendenhall and discuSSion With recreation authorities, it was 1Shafer, op. cit., p. 29. 2Mendenhall, op. cit., p. 158. 53 decided that sample size would be determined to ensure that estimation error for proportions is no greater than .05 at the 95 per cent confidence level. These statistical limits were established as the minimum acceptable, and whenever possible, the statistical standards were set to yield more precise results. Second, the decision on sample size will be largely governed by the way the results are to be analyzed, so that the researcher must at the outset consider, at least in broad terms, the breakdowns to be made in the final tabulations. He can then calculate what num- bers are needed in each subgroup to give the desired precision for that subgroup and hence, what total sample size is required. Since most surveys seek information on a number of different variables, the sample size must be calculated to yield the desired information for that "critical" question which has answers of least reliability. The M difficulty in choosing a sample size is that in order to make the required calculations, the variability of the population must be known. Once again, sample size for the entire study will have to be determined so as to provide an adequate number of responses to that particular question which will have the greatest diversity of response. Variance data is usually obtained from pre- vious survey research among the outlined population. In the case of this study, there was no previous research available upon which 54 to base calculations, so the maximum possible variance of any population was used in sample size calculations. Further discus- sion of the qualities looked for in choosing a sample can be found in Survey Methods in Social Investigation. The individual units that combine to form the sample are referred to as "sampling units." Usually, sampling units are speci- fied individuals (respondents), but this does not always have to be the case. If the sample size is large and there is not much diversity in the number of occupants, the sampling unit can also be a vehicle. In fact, using ”vehicles" instead of "individuals" will yield more data for analysis. (Information can be obtained about all vehicle occupants, not merely one person.) Because of the disadvantages of using individuals as ”sampling units, " this study will consider ”sampling units" to be vehicles. Although this study was designed to collect information about the "day-user" it was administratively unfeasible to separate "day-users" from campers at the data collection phase. Thus, the sample frame contains both "day-users” and campers, the separa- tion of which will take place in the analysis phase. 1Moser, op. cit., p. 121. 55 Choice of Sample Parks and Recreation Areas Each park and recreation area can be considered to be "individual" in that it has distinctive, unique features that attract a separate and distinct park -using population. 1 It makes little sense to describe an "average” user for the entire park system as the re- sult would be a nonexistent, stereotype individual that may create more managerial problems than were intended to be solved. Despite the fact that parks are "individual" and should be regarded as such, it is often necessary (usually because of insuf- ficient research funds) to use a representative sample of all parks. How then can the researcher choose a representative sample of all parks in order to enjoy the advantages of sampling previously dis- cussed? The answer is to stratify parks into similar groupings and then choose a park from each group to represent that particular strata. The problem arises as to how parks should be stratified. In his survey of campers, Shafer found definite changes both in amount of response and completeness of response from month to month dur- ing the recreation season, and from park to park. 3 Based on his study, Shafer recommended that parks should be stratified "on the 1Shafer, op. cit., p. 178. 21bid., p. 176. 31bid., p. 165. 56 basis of lake-acreage availability classes, tourist attraction densi- ties, park-accessibility groupings and environment variability cate- gories. "1 It has also been suggested that ”attractability indexes" be developed and used to categorize parks. Unfortunately there have been no suitable methods of classification developed to date, and further research is needed in this area. For lack of a better stratification scheme, the one proposed by Clawson was used for this study. 2 This stratification guide, while being so general as to not exclude any State park or recreation area, was specific enough to indicate definite boundaries for each of the three categories. The three classes of recreation areas proposed by Clawson are: 1. User-oriented areas—-located close to large population centers and characterized by a large annual attendance. 2. Intermediate areas--located within two hours driving time from major population centers and usually characterized by the diversity of activities offered. 3. Resource based areas—-generally located considerable distances from large population centers and characterized by a lack of intensive development of the site. 11bid., p. 5. 2Clawson, op. cit., p. 13. [\9 an: 57 Upon request, lists of park characteristics and past attendance figures for each State park and recreation area were made available to the research staff. A series of meetings was held with repre- sentatives of the Division of State Parks to "pair" parks for each of the two study phases. The parks outlined in Table 1 and Figure 2 were chosen as both representative of the park system and "admin- istratively feasible" enough to be included within the study. TABLE 1. -- Sample Parks of the Michigan State Park System Included in This Study Class "Experimental" Phase ”SeasonaP' Phase 1. User-oriented area 2. Intermediate area 3. Resource -based area Dodge Brothers #4 State Park Pinckney State Recreation Area Harrisville State Park Holland State Park Waterloo State Recreation Area Tawas Point State Park Design of the Dispersal-Retrieval System The limitations imposed by the Michigan State Park system and by the use of random sampling, drastically reduced the dispersal- retrieval options available. The only means of dispersal available that does not involve the use of additional staff members is by 58 SAMPLE PARKS O "SEASONAL" PARKS [:1 "EXPERIMENTAL" PARKS teenage @0 ® 6) ALPENA. 0 ® @ TRAVERSE. .@ Q cm® GRAYUNG 0 . HARRISV/LLE 574 r: PARK 0 @ @LUDINGTON o e@ 69 ' ® .9 s c...- o :USKEGON ® 9 TAWAS POINT STATE PARK ® .0, .GRAND @® HOLLAND STATE PARK Q) ”Nos 0 Q) Q _ . . LANSING 90' 00 6‘ 0 A 8R0 HERS STA TE PARK . 4 D TROIT O WATERLOO RECREATION AREA @m Pmcmsr RE I? .71., R .. e @@ FIGURE 2. -- Michigan State Parks and Recreation Areas Indicating Sample Park Locations 59 questionnaire hand out to designated sample frame vehicles at the area entrance by the contact station ranger. Retrieval options available are either deposit in a designated container within the recreation area or mail-back to a central location. Since the mail- back method is costly (at least seven cents per response), time consuming, and requires space on the questionnaire for the mailing address and postal permit number (thus reducing space available for questions), this method of retrieval is unsatisfactory. The use of follow-up waves could not be considered because there was no way of identifying and contacting sample frame members once they had left the park. Vehicle registrations could not be used, as previously explained. In summary, the questionnaire survey instrument was to be distributed to sample frame members by contact station rangers. Retrieval was to be by voluntary deposit in a collection box placed near the area exit. No follow-up waves were to be used. Questionnaire Design The questionnaire should be designed so that it will obtain the greatest response rate possible, with all answers completed and interpretable. To accomplish this, the survey instrument must appear to be so attractive that the respondent will want to complete 60 it, yet simple enough that all questions can be interpreted correctly and answers can be easily given. Although adaptations were to be made within each of the study phases, the following basic design principles were incorporated into all questionnaires constructed. Questionnaires were designed so as to appear brief. A single sheet of paper stock with maximum dimensions of 8% X 11 inches was to be used. This would necessitate photo-reduction of all printed material in order to fit within the size limitations. The paper stock also had to be sturdy enough so that the questionnaire could be filled out without requiring an additional supporting surface. In addition to the information previously outlined to be in- cluded on the questionnaire, space also had to be provided for the particular park identification number and the date on which the ques- tionnaire was handed out. These items were necessary in order to identify the questionnaires for later analysis. Campers were to be identified by their response to the question, ”Are you going to camp in this park tonight?" Since most parks do not contain concessions in which users can buy food staples, campers usually have to leave and re-enter the park at least once a day. In order to cover the situation of the camper returning to the park after buying breakfast requirements the day he is to break camp, an additional question, "Did you camp in this park last night?", was included. If the answer 61 to both of these questions was negative, the individual was a "day- user.” In addition to the traditional means of obtaining user origins (asking the respondent to write-in his town or city, county, and State of residence) a question requesting postal zip code numbers was included. If respondents would answer the zip code question, a more precise indication of origin could be made, i. e. , zip code area 48912 is a more precise area than "Lansing. " Socio-economic level is difficult to ascertain, because it is a composite measurement requiring evaluations from at least two different scales. Answers to the following questions could be com- bined to indicate socio—economic levels: 1. What is the age and sex of the "head of the family"? 2. What is the education of the "head of the family"? 3. What is the occupation of the "head of the family"? 4. What total income did the driver' 3 family earn during last year? Difficulty was encountered in determining which would be the best method to obtain information about activities "participated in" while at the park. No concensus of opinion could be found as to which method would be best, so a pre-test was conducted with all proposed techniques. As a result of this pre-test an activity analysis 62 question was designed in which the various activities ”participated in" could be identified, and those activities in which the participant spent most time could be assigned a quantitative time measurement. As an additional aid to recreation area management, the question "What kind of additional recreation opportunities or facili- ties would you like to see added to the park?" was included. Also, it was felt that this question would be an "ego booster" for the respondent, helping to maintain interest for the activity analysis question that followed. In order to provide background information for later market studies, Michigan State Park authorities indicated that they wished to obtain guidance about why people actually chose a particular park. The question, ”Why did you choose this park rather than a different one?" was included as a means of outlining answer categories to be used in future studies. Throughout the entire survey, a central motivating theme and symbol were used. The symbol was a picnic table silhouette which was superimposed on all questionnaires, signs and collection boxes. It was hoped that respondents would subconsciously associate the questionnaire to the instructions on the signs and ultimately to the collection box, thus improving response. The theme, "by com- pleting and handing in this card you can help plan your parks" was 63 expressed in the questionnaire, in signs, and in the short verbal message given by the contact station ranger. CHAPTER IV ACCURACY OF COLLECTED INFORMATION The purpose of this study phase was to test sub-hypothesis two, that is, to check the accuracy of information collected by ques- tionnaire. In order to accomplish this, a continuous data collection system using a questionnaire survey instrument was designed and implemented. The data collected from this system was then com- pared to parameter data for the park-using population. Thus, the accuracy limits of questionnaire information were determined. Preliminary Preparations Que stionnai re De sign The requirements of the ”seasonal" questionnaire were that it must be attractive, distinctive, convenient to complete, and diffi- cult to forget or ignore. Respondents could not fill out the question— naire immediately upon receiving it at the area entrance because of the activity analysis questions. Motivational problems would be created because the respondent could not fill it out immediately while 64 65 interest was high; rather it had to be set aside until all activities had been completed. In order to overcome this difficulty and the ones previously outlined in Chapter III, a number of devices were employed. m, the questionnaire was printed on bright yellow 24 pound Carni- val Kraft stock. This was a bright, distinctive, relatively inexpen- sive paper stock with enough rigidity so that the questionnaire card could be completed without requiring an additional supporting surface during the writing. Second, the card was a single sheet of paper stock folded once to provide a brochure-like questionnaire with dimen- sions 5% X 8% inches. Thus, the questionnaire appeared to require little time for completion and the folded size required less supporting surface during completion. m, a slotted hole was drilled in the upper left-hand corner of the card, permitting it to be suspended from a vehicle instrument knob. This would aid response by keeping the questionnaire in view of the vehicle occupants, thus reducing forget- fulness and misplacement. Fourth, a golf pencil was supplied to re- spondents who indicated that they needed a writing instrument. A sample of the "seasonal" questionnaire is contained in Appendix A. Sample Siz e Since information as to the variability of the Michigan State Park and Recreation Area "day-user” was not available, sample size 66 . 1 . . was calculated uSIng absolute error and assuming the max1mum possible population variance. Calculation was as follows: The probability requirement Pr[1p-p1\<.05] 2.95 leads to lp-pl .05 P13 /p (1 -p) Sq /p (1 -p) 2 .95 n n Where p = unknown proportion to be estimated estimate of p ’U) H n = sample size .05 = absolute error .95 = confidence level Using normal approximations we have that n must satisfy 05 .. /p(1-p) 21.96 11 1As contrasted to relative error where the accuracy is specified in terms such as .05p and where lower bounds for p are needed in order to determine sample size. 67 fr? >/ m (39.2) n >/ 1538 p (1 -p) Since p(1-p)\<% Then n 3%,- - 1538 = 385 For each unit of analysis, 385 "day-use" respondents were required. In this study, each park and recreation area was a separate analysis unit. Also, the occupants of each vehicle using the area entrance were not necessarily "day-users" (they might have been campers or park staff). Therefore, a sample of at least 900 respon- dents was set for each park and recreation area to guarantee the necessary ”day—use" sample size. Sampling Method Systematic random sampling was chosen as the method to be used in this study phase because it could be implemented with least interference to normal recreation area operation. In system- atic sampling, every nth member of the population was included in the sample. For instance, to obtain a sample of 250 vehicles from a population of 1000, every fourth vehicle was selected. To ensure that every vehicle in the population had equal opportunity of being 68 chosen, the first sample vehicle had to be designated through the use of a random choice method (1. e. , random number tables, "drawing" from the sample space). After the initial vehicle had been identified, every fourth vehicle thereafter would automatically be included in the sample. Since contact station rangers do not have the time to count all vehicles entering the area, a simple means of counting vehicles and identifying sample members was designed. A pneumatic traffic counter was adapted, through the use of slot car gears, to count axles and then to ring a bell when the predetermined number of axles was reached. To determine the setting of a traffic counter, the following procedure was used: For each recreation area, the attendance for each summer month during the past three years was obtained. From this, a "conservative" estimated attendance for the months of July and August, 1968, was set. The required sample size was then divided into the estimated attendance to provide the systematic sam- pling frequency. Through experimentation with numerous gear ratios in the traffic counter, a setting for each individual park was obtained. For example, Holland State Park required a systematic sample of every fifty-fifth vehicle. A gear ratio to provide this exact sampling frequency could not be constructed, so one was constructed which 69 would yield a greater sample size than was actually required. In the case of Holland State Park the traffic counter identified each one hundred and fourth axle (fifty-second vehicle). Some vehicles entering recreation areas have more than two axles, i. e. , card with boats, trailers or campers. Since vehicles with more than two axles apparently do not enter recreation areas in a definite order or scheme of stratification, they do not jeopardize the random sampling system. 1 In fact, they increase the number of respondents surveyed, thus helping to guarantee the filling of the sampling frame. If the questionnaire was distributed to the vehicle that caused the traffic counter bell to ring, those vehicles having more than two axles would have an advantage. To overcome this problem, the second vehicle following the one that rang the bell was designated to receive the questionnaire. Implementation of the Continuous Data Collection Sgyglem One month before the system was scheduled to be put into effect, a meeting was held with all sample park supervisors, regional park supervisors, and representatives of the Recreation Resource Planning Division and State Park Division of the Michigan Department 1Cochran, op. cit., p. 230. 70 of Natural Resources. At this time, the study objectives were outlined, possible operational problems were discussed and solutions were proposed. Special emphasis was given to possible implications of the collected data for future recreation area management. Using the discussion from the meeting as a guide, written instructions were prepared for use within each sample park. These instructions were constantly updated as new problems were encoun- tered. A weekly report form, included in Appendix B, was also designed. This form, in combination with the regular weekly report forwarded to the State Park Divisional Office, provided the following information on a daily basis: number of visitors and vehicles enter- " number of ing the park, number of campers, and "day-users, people refused entrance to the park (usually because of a capacity crowd), number of questionnaires handed out, number of question- naires returned, the presence of any special events in the region of the park that might influence attendance, and the weather—-including amount of cloud cover, amount of precipitation, amount of wind, and maximum / minimum temperatures. Each sample park was visited by a study team member shortly before commencement of the data collection system. The park supervisors were asked to delegate one park staff member as the "contact man" for the study, and permission was obtained to work 71 directly with the delegated person. In most cases, the contact man was either the park supervisor or assistant supervisor. The con- tact person was given the responsibility for ensuring that all phases of the study were carried out correctly. If any problems arose that were not outlined in the written instructions or orientation discus- sions, study team members were to be immediately contacted by tele- phone. During the initial visit, study team members supervised the installation of the traffic counter, signs and collection box. The traffic counter was placed in the contact station and set with the pre- viously determined random starting position. A short distance past the contact station a sign was erected that asked respondents to complete the questionnaire and return it at the area exit. It was anticipated that as one occupant of the vehicle was accepting the questionnaire, the other occupants could read this motivating Sign. The ”deposit" box was placed near the park exit, usually on the pas- senger side of the roadway. A short distance in advance of the deposit box a sign, ”Please Deposit Completed Park Use Card Ahead, " was posted. At the collection box an additional sign, "Please Deposit Park Use Card Here, ” was erected. The tasks of the contact station rangers were demonstrated: the placing of park code num- bers and hand-out dates on each questionnaire to be done in the 72 FIGURE 3. -- Vehicles Waiting to Enter Dodge Brothers #4 State Park (A Typical Weekend Scene) FIGURE 4. -- Contact Station Rangers Handing Out Questionnaires (Standing Out- side the Contact Station During Heavy Traffic Flow) 73 FIGURE 5. -- Handing Out Questionnaires While Remaining Within the Contact Station (Flexibility Is An Asset) FIGURE 6. —— View of Motivational Sign and Pneumatic Traffic Counter Hose From the Contact Station 74 -- ' ' . ..‘u - .,T o ' .4 " ' i n 1‘ “_ 1“ .- “L ‘ Tit ‘ We! mom comma PARK-USE CARD AHEA'D FIGURE 7. -- Sign Warning Motorists of Their Approach to the Deposit Box (As Motorist Is Leaving the Park) PLEASE DEPOSIT COMPLETED PARK-USE CARD FIGURE 8. --Deposit Box and Sign (Located Near Park Exit) 75 FIGURE 9. -- Pneumatic Traffic Counter Modified for Systematic Sampling Use (Designed So As to Ring a Bell Every nfl Axle) . ’. 1 . g ‘ - ._ c . “- ‘ t.{'t ’ -- - 7 -' .;.¢ ’ FIGURE 10. --Workers at the Study Center Preparing Experimental Ques- tionnaires for Shipment to Parks 76 morning before the vehicular traffic became busy; the completion of all forms; the collecting, packaging, and mailing of returned ques- tionnaires; and the motivating message to be given when the ques- tionnaire was handed out was rehearsed, namely, "Good day, Sir. Would you please fill out this card and leave it in the box near the exit as you leave the park? It is important that you complete every question. Do you need a pencil? Thanks for your cooperation—- enjoy your visit. " Sample parks were re-visited by study team members throughout the duration of the study. Collection of Parameter Information Parameters of the park-using population were to be obtained by a combination voluntary "hand in"-interview during the latter part of August. Questionnaires were to be handed to all vehicles entering the park during the interview period. A "spotter” was to be located within viewing distance of the collection box and in a position to sig- nal to the interview team farther down the road. Those "vehicles" voluntarily returning questionnaires in the deposit box at the park exit were allowed to leave the park. The ”vehicles" not depositing questionnaires in the collection box were to be stopped and inter- viewed to obtain information for certain "key" questions. The com- bination of data collection methods outlined above should produce 77 near 100 per cent response from all recreation area users during the interview period (hereafter, termed "parameter" information). Implementation of Parameter Information Collection Supervisors of the "seasonal" parks were contacted two weeks before the parameter information was scheduled to be collected, and assistance, in the form of making park staff members available to aid in the interview program, was requested. It was proposed that the interview teams consist of five members: one person (pref- erably a park staff member) would hand out questionnaires to all vehicles entering the park; one person would be positioned about 50 "spotter" (again, preferably a yards past the deposit box to act as park staff member). Those vehicles not depositing questionnaires would be directed into an interview area; two persons would be con- ducting interviews; and one person would be supervising the opera- tion and relieving other team members. During the first interview day it became apparent that the outlined interview program was inadequate. Traffic flow at the area exit was erratic, requiring five or six interviewers at times. In order to accommodate the sporadic vehicular flow, a supervised completion of a "short-form questionnaire" was substituted for the 78 verbal interview. 1 Those ”vehicles" not depositing questionnaires in the collection box were directed into a designated area. The driver of the vehicle was then asked to complete the "short-form questionnaire. ” The questionnaire was collected when completed, the driver was thanked, and the vehicle was allowed to proceed. The success of this method led to one additional change--another collec- tion box was erected at the exit of the designated interview area and drivers were asked to deposit the "short—form questionnaire" when it was completed. At Holland State Park the intense vehicular traffic neces- sitated a further change. Even with three people handing question- naires to vehicles entering the park, there was a traffic line waiting to enter extending for one-half mile. Experimentation revealed that there was no possible means of dispersing questionnaires at the area entrance without creating a major traffic congestion. The problem at the park exit was similar to that at the entrance--too much traffic for the one-lane exit. Therefore, it was decided to use random number tables to choose a random sample at the area exit. Those "vehicles" included in the sample were directed into a designated 1A sample "short—form questionnaire" is contained in Appendix C. 79 area, asked to complete a questionnaire, and to deposit it in the collection box provided at the area exit. Treatment of Collected Data Collected data was to be placed on data processing punch cards for the reasons previously outlined. Two methods could be employed to accomplish this task. The first method is through key punch-—questionnaires have to be precoded by study team members, coded key punch tables must then be prepared, and finally, key punch operators use the prepared coded tables to punch the data processing cards. With this method there are three sources of human error-~the preceding, the preparation of coded tables, and the punching of the data processing cards. The second available method is through the use of optical scan sheets—-questionnaires must still be precoded by study team members, but instead of plac- ing the coded information in key punch tables it is put on I. B. M. optical scan sheets. The optical scan sheets can then be processed directly through a reading-card punch machine to produce the desired data processing cards. Using this method there are only two sources of human error (the key punch operator is eliminated), and because there are fewer human handling procedures the cost is less than the key punch method. These advantages were sufficient reason to use the optical scan method of data treatment in this study. 80 Special I. B. M. optical scan forms were designed and are included in Appendix D of this report. Analysis of Data Only those questionnaires which were entirely completed were coded and included in the analysis. If one question was not filled in, and it was not among those questions judged to be essential for analysis (i. e. , questions 1, 2, 4, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 21, 23, 24 were considered "essential"), the non—completion was regarded as an error and the questionnaire was coded. Parameter information was compared to the information collected by voluntary deposit of questionnaires by means of analysis of variance and chi square techniques. The responses from ques- tions possessing "continuous" answer categories were analyzed through the use of analysis of variance. The chi square technique was employed on the remaining data. Information from each sample "seasonal" park was analyzed in two different time periods: 1. Data collected from voluntary deposit1 throughout the entire testing season were compared to the parameter data. 1Data collected from the dispersal-retrieval system pre- viously outlined in the chapter is hereafter referred to as "voluntary deposit data. " Voluntary deposit data, therefore, consists of the completed questionnaires that were voluntarily deposited in the 81 2. Data collected from voluntary deposit during the week immediately prior to, and the week immediately following parameter information collection, were compared to the parameter information. In order to test the accuracy of the voluntary information, only the second time period comparison is needed. The first time period comparison was only intended to test the changes in use and user characteristics which occur throughout the duration of the sum- mer season. Within each of these time periods the "day-users" were analyzed separately from the campers. Results and Conclusions The random-systematic sampling procedure used in this study phase appeared to function successfully. Minor difficulties occurred but were solved with a minimum of trouble. Due to the difficulty involved in stopping sample vehicles as they enter the park (vehicles with season passes do not have to stop at the park entrance), from 3% to 15% of the sample vehicles were missed. Generally, this collection box near each ”seasonal" sample park exit (with the excep- tion of the data obtained during the few days that parameter informa- tion collection took place). 1At Holland State Park, the "voluntary deposit" system was not re-activated when the interview team completed the collection of parameter data. Data collection from voluntary deposit within the two weeks immediately prior to the parameter information collection were used in the comparison analysis. 82 proportion was under 5%, but as the amount of traffic increased, one might suspect that the number of ”missed" sample vehicles also increased. A summary of the number of questionnaires distributed and returned during the "seasonal” testing program is contained in Tables 2 and 3. It appears that there is an inverse relationship between the number of completed, returned questionnaires and the total number of people visiting the park. This may be due to the fact that unlike the low-use parks, the staff in the high-use parks did not have the time to deliver the required motivating message. Also, a small sample included in a large population tends to lose the feeling ' There is less risk of "social dis- of "supervised responsibility.‘ approval" resulting from not handing in the questionnaire (it is easier to lose oneself in the crowd). Parameter information collection was conducted on the dates outlined in Table 3. Since financial resources were limited and the amount of information that could be obtained at Tawas Point State Park was small, it was decided to curtail parameter information collection at that park and to move the interview team to Holland State Park. Shortly after the arrival of the team at Holland State Park, the weather became cold and windy, and the number of visitors declined drastically. However, over a period of five days, enough data were collected so that the desired analysis could be conducted. 83 .30 popes: mofimnsofimmav mo son -85: mg 0“ moawmcsoimosc pouoafioo Mo .8285: mg maimmfioo L3 poswwuno 93mm mum. A 3mm 53 SS 38. 3.8a 683860 can 3 was .3 .mm :3 $3 spam 83m eemzom so .5. S: s 3 Sam 33 spam 9.6% EBA page can .3 ma .3. .3 mg 32 mpp<6mmwmwmmmeB emwmfim emwmmmmwsa ”flaw emwwmwé ewmqwfiwe is H mo ponfisz mo ponfizz mo sonasz AwuwQ aowoamsmm wcfipfioxmv mxpwm onEmm :Hmsommom: 555». 339800 powpsh paw .posaaom J50 @0933 moawmacoflmosmu mo amnESZ I .m gamma. 84 TABLE 3. -- Number of Completed Questionnaires Obtained from ”Seasonal" Sample Park Users During Parameter Information Collection Park Date Voluntary Superv1sed DepOSIt Response Waterloo State Recreation Area August 18 231 70 August 19 141 37 TOTAL 372 107 Tawas Point State Park August 21 51 6 Holland State Park August 24 8 204 August 25 75 August 26 43 August 27 73 August 28 52 TOTAL 8 447 COMBINED PARKS TOTAL 431 560 85 Analysis of the collected data revealed that each park has user characteristics that are "individual" and characteristic of that park alone. It was also evident that even within each park, the characteristics change throughout the season. The visitation time during the season was shown to be closely related to the type of per- son who used the parks and the way in which the park was used. The accuracy of information collected by voluntary deposit "voluntary” data to the collected was determined by comparing the parameter information. If the assumption is made that the parame- ter information is valid, then any statistical difference between the two sets of data would indicate that the "voluntary" data was not accurate. However, because of the circumstances involved during the collection of the parameter information, a great deal of caution must be exercised in analytical interpretation. Information contained in the questionnaire yielded sixty- three different variables for comparative analysis. The results of this analysis indicated that the accuracy of ”voluntary" information was high. Even with the comparatively low amount of return, and with no "follow-up" waves to reduce non—response, the comparison analysis showed very little difference between ”voluntary" data and parameter data. At the 95% confidence level there was no significant difference between the two groups except for the few variables out- lined in Table 4. 86 TABLE 4. -- Variables from "Day-User" Questionnaire Information Which Show a Significant Difference in a Comparative Analysis (From Use of Both Analysis of Variance and Chi Square Techniques) Park Variables Tawas Point State Park None (low-intensity use) Waterloo State Recreation Area 1. How many miles, by the (intermediate-intensity use) most direct route, is this park from your home? Holland State Park 1. How many days have you (high-intensity use) used this park in 1968? 2. How much time did you spend traveling to this park today? 87 The subject matter area in which the voluntary deposit of questionnaires yielded the least accurate information about ”day— users" was in the characteristics of travel to the park. The "sea- sonal" questionnaire contained two questions requesting travel information: "How many miles, by the most direct route, is this park from your home?" and "How much time did you Spend travel- ing to this park today?" In data obtained at Holland State Park the "travel time" question proved to be significantly different in the com- parison of ”parameter" and "voluntary" information. The ”miles traveled" question showed a significant difference in information collected at Waterloo State Recreation Area. Surprisingly, at both of these parks only one of the two travel questions indicated a sig- nificant difference. The other travel question, in the case of both parks, provided responses which showed close agreement. There are two possible causes for the lack of accuracy in the "travel" questions. If the park-using population does not know (or care about) the distance traveled and time taken to reach the park, then the information they provide will not be reliable. Perhaps, when people are engaged in "day-use" activities they are more con- cerned with enjoyment than they are with travel measurements. The second possibility is that the population receiving interviews is more methodical (or accurate) in arriving at answers to the "travel" ques- tions. An inspection of analysis results shows that the average 88 information obtained by interview indicates less ”distance traveled" and ”time taken" than information obtained by voluntary deposit. The presence of the interviewer appears to ”trim down” measure- ments of the distance traveled. Since there was no "voluntary deposit" information available for Holland State Park after the collection of parameter information, one would expect the variable "How many days have you used this park in 1968?" to Show a lack of accuracy. In the other "seasonal" sample parks, the "voluntary deposit" information was "balanced" around the parameter information. This was accomplished by in- cluding in the comparison analysis "voluntary information" collected prior to, and immediately after parameter information collection. H H It was not possible to "balance voluntary data" at Holland State Park since no voluntary collection of data took place following parame- ter information collection. If the park using population consists of individuals who use the park often, an "unbalanced” time comparison would show a significant difference in response to this question. An examination of the data revealed that this, indeed, was the case. Parameter information showed a slight increase in the number of days the park was used when compared to ”voluntary deposit" data. This increase was enough to indicate a significant difference between the two sets of data. 89 It appeared that as the intensity of usage within a particular park increased, there was a decrease in the accuracy of information collected by means of the questionnaire system outlined. It was also noted that in all parks and in all time period comparisons, more variables within the "camper" group were significantly different than among the "day-user" group. This indicates that accurate "day- user” information can be obtained from a smaller sample size than can accurate information about campers. Therefore, when continu- ous data collection systems are being designed which will collect information from both campers and "day—users, " the sample size should be determined for camper information. The inclusion of a message asking respondents to mail the questionnaire back to a central point, if they have forgotten to deposit it while in the park, appears to increase response by less than 1%. Sub-hypothesis two, as previously stated, reads: Questionnaires returned within the proposed dispersal- retrieval system will contain information that does not differ significantly from the parameters of the park -using population of the Michigan State Park System. From the results of this study phase, it appears that this sub-hypothesis cannot be rejected. Repetitive Entries. -- The phenomenon of ”repetitive entries" was of more importance than had first been suspected. This practice 90 was more prevalent among campers than among "day-users. " The lack of a grocery store within a state park resulted in many campers having to leave the park in order to obtain food supplies. During the period when interview teams were in the parks it was noted that some vehicles left and re-entered the park more than six times per day. Indeed, within some state parks, the traffic on the main thoroughfare resembled a parade. Cars full of youngsters continuously cruised in and out of the park searching for some "action. " The sale and use of "season passes" for entry into parks encouraged the practice of leaving and re—entering. Since it was "repetitive-entry" vehicle during sur- impractical to determine the vey work, the results of all traffic-type surveys will over-represent the ”repetitive-entry" population. The extent of this bias will depend upon the relative size of the ”repetitive-entry" population, and the difference in characteristics between the general park-using popula- tion and the ”repetitive—entry" population. CHAPTER V DETERMINATION OF "BEST" QUESTIONNAIRE AND DISPERSAL-RE TRIEVAL SYSTEM The purpose of this study phase was to attempt to overcome the main problem of the questionnaire survey method--the vital prob- lem of non—response. To accomplish this end, those variables which might affect the number of questionnaires returned were identified. The variables were then tested, one at a time, in order to measure the effect of each on response rates. Determination of Variables Which Might Influence Questionnaire Response The many factors which combine to influence questionnaire response rate are complex and difficult to isolate for study. As in all research relating to motivations, the examination of these factors is still in a very elementary stage. Little is known other than the most elementary relationships involved, namely, response depends upon the population receiving the questionnaire, the appearance and content of the questionnaire itself, and the circumstances in which the 91 92 questionnaire is received and collected. These factors have been discussed in detail in Chapter II. In order to ascertain the variables that should be tested in this study, a review of factors that had influenced questionnaire response in the past was conducted. Also, a meeting with park and recreation authorities provided information on further factors which might affect amount of return. From these two sources, the vari- ables in Table 5 were outlined as being capable of being tested in this study. Means of Choosing "Best" Alternatives Within Each Variable Within the context of this study phase, "best" is interpreted to mean that means by which the greatest number of completed ques— tionnaire returns were obtained. To determine the "best" means of minimizing non—response, the alternatives within each variable being tested were outlined and questionnaires were designed utilizing each alternative. For example, if color of paper stock was the variable, the specific colors to be tested (1. e. , blue, white, yellow, etc.) were the alternatives within that variable. The other test variables which might affect response were held constant. Therefore, if the ques- tionnaires containing the alternatives of one variable were handed out in a random manner, one could state that the alternative which 93 .3388sz o: Bog 66:on oz E .m> mmofisfiowpfi so $3 8. one omcoamoanco: wswospop hammom 3:: Jenna oawmscofimmsv mo “sofiapmfi mg 89% mafiwccoflmmsv 9: economy. sosaoo tea soda: 5 Bo: pmfiofim 3 Cu acopcommop o5 poBOZw 30: pofiofim m sowam £293.“ 16.52“me UGJOQ em £3 pcm owcwao xoon Roman 90 p300 550.5 mafia 303mm 353 m0 somEmQEoo xooum ao>oo came x285 zoopm pamo :bwwom: AZ .m> spawn co m o m Messages? poaofioo «HEM Hmzsawv eased em x p cum :3 5: 3 86:6. :2; 2 x mm 6628:: p.28 36E E .m> “3235.3 :65 we x we 6 9S 6629 562.... :2: A A x mm a 28 ppEom 3 Aooswswoaag Ransom ofiwcsoflmosa mo>flmcaofi< .Ho soflaflomom £0.ng .8 63860 6.83 SEE ppm mmcoAmpm 855836650 Spas 232 533 SHARES, -- .m Humans mpgm mEH E @3on msfiom 94 .ommmmoa :fimoaop momma: m magnum”? 1:00 .566 aofio 9.3 690. came pogo.“ on“ so msoflmosv of .Ho poapmsvnoso bouwgwxoaam mi usfimusoo can one 8:? came “won :05 m X m m mafimcsofimosv on? 26.30 Rom: 3w .m> came :05 3 X mm poEoM enamosoflmosfio came poEob 3 .m msofimosv ofimm o5 . sopso sofimosc uneaomfim 0 mo mmsflopao “Genomic moat mo someano msofimosv mfimbwsm 533“; m0 mayo.“ “Genomflp oz.“ mo nomfiawmaoo soflmosv mwmbwsm 33306. am we 298m «coaomflm um “sousou ohmccofim mama ©3395 Zosom 02 A2 .m> Ugandan Sodom .200 oawmgofimosv fits poflamsm Sodom Q .m mo>3mcsofi< mo :oEQEomoQ 6333, UmsnficoU I. .m Himmdwh. ARE .5 ma Eugen mo msmoa Eco mg. was @95an ohms sages meson. fimoaom .mfixo mops pm paw mocmnpso . . . sown Ewe fixo mops sum psopcmm At am “so popcmm oEm pommoappwuflom Ema Iona fits 09$ :Pfimo amen: mm? mummacoflmosmu .m> .owmmmoE :fimomop ommoa: fits on? :ppmo “mom: was “the “a son. sotoofioo 5 opwwgoflmosg .mpgm mo omega :Hmcommom: fimoaop .oocwauco moss am #50983 3 A. 5 com: Hmsoflwesufimmpoamfip mo “80532 .sofimoofi pommoappmuoam one op % cocowcossoo m3 um peso one. .288 3500 so mops sown 2mg .8 tone mesa ofi $3 on ma xon cotbozoo 5 «Somme sonic pm xon sotoofioo a: fimomop Ann 3500 Sampsoamom .oEm pmmmosppmufiom .Emm Hm>oflpoa .oocmfism «m «noesmm At no.3 fits on? :ppmo Eon: was oawmqaoflmosg .ms .owmmmoS :fimoaop emmoaz fits on? :pawo Eon: was “the mesa Hm Non coflooHHoo efimccoflmosg .mp3»). mo ommnm :Hmsommom: E fimoaop .oosmfiso “m psopcmm 3 .H 5 pom: Hmsoflpopéwmaommwp Ho @0532 mpofioz ~m>oEuom1HmmsemmflQ mo>fimssofi< .Ho cothrnomem o3m€m> possthou I .m gamma. 96 achieved the highest response rate could be said to be ”best. " A description of all ”experimental" questionnaires is contained in Appendix F. In order to ensure that the results were due to the specific alternative tested and not due to chance, a "reasonably large number" of questionnaires were handed out and statistical techniques of sig- nificant differences were employed. The method used to determine whether one particular alternative was actually "better" than another was the ”fixed-pair" method of statistical significance: X. Pi = Kl- = proportion of the number of type i question- i naires that were completed and returned x = number of completed, returned question- naires of type i N = number of type i questionnaires handed out When comparing Pi and Pj , Pi - P3. is approximately normally dis- 2 2 2 tributed1 with mean Pi - Pj and variance / ij = &/ i + a/ j , where Pi(1 - P.) P.(1 - P.) A 2 [21: N. 1 is the variance of Pi and/ j = L N. J is the 1 J 1For proportions of the magnitude that will be estimated, 30 or more trials will ensure approximate normality. 97 variance of Pj . Using the fact that P(1 - P) \< :1— for all 0 \< P \< 1, we can conclude that é/ij \< —— + —— . A 95% confidence interval for P1 - Pj is Pi - Pj - 1. 96$sz + —L , * C 1 1 A . . . . . Pi - Pj + 1. 96\/4:I\Ti + 4N; and Pi is Significantly different from PJ. if this confidence interval fails to include 0. For example, with N = N. = 500, the 95% confidence interval is P. - P. - —1——Q§ 1 J 1 3 V 1000 A A 1.96 A A A A P-P+—— or Pi-P.-.062,Pi-Pj+.062 ,so i j V1000 3 there is a significant difference at the 95% confidence level if 113. - 13.1 > .062. 1 I It is known that the answer given to a question depends upon the respondent' s interpretation of the question and his ability to answer. Presenting the same question through different methods of data collection may result in different answers, depending upon the interpretation of the question in each context. There is a possibility that the population replying to a particular question on a blue ques- tionnaire may differ from those who answer the same question on a white questionnaire, even if the only difference between the two ques- tionnaires is the color of the paper stock. This variability in response 98 can be reduced by designing questions so as to be simple to answer and unambiguous. Sampling Method The sampling method used had to provide a sample that was representative of the park—using population. For comparison pur- poses, enough questionnaires had to be distributed so that the returns would not result in a bias due to choosing a small, select population as the sample. This problem was eliminated by printing and distrib- uting to the sample parks at least five hundred copies of each alter- native questionnaire. Since the different questionnaires had to be distributed in a random manner, they were collated together before shipment to the parks. The contact station ranger then handed out the questionnaires in order, beginning at the top of the collated stack. In an attempt to ensure that the sampling period provided results which were representative of the total park-using population, all tests were scheduled to include at least one weekday and one day of the weekend. Of the total questionnaires to be distributed in the test, 80 per cent were handed out on the weekend day and 20 per cent were handed out on the weekday. These percentages reflected the proportion of people visiting sample parks on weekdays and weekend days. 99 A further use of proportionate distribution was made in determining the exact number of collated questionnaires to be sup- plied to each park. A survey of past attendance records for the sample parks revealed that the total number of questionnaires in the test should be distributed as follows: Dodge Brothers #4 State Park--60%, Pinckney State Recreation Area--30%, Harrissville State Park——10%. The sampling periods during which questionnaires were to be handed out were selected as follows: For each test, the sampling days were divided into morning (8:00 a. m. to noon) and afternoon (noon to 10:00 p. m. ). The number of visitors entering parks during these time divisions was approximately equal. A random selection of the hours within each division was used to obtain the starting time for each sampling period. Questionnaires were then handed out until the supply was exhausted. In the case of inclement weather, sampling periods were adjusted so that questionnaire hand—out began as soon as the contact station was open. One questionnaire was handed to the occupants of each vehicle entering the area during a sampling period. There were two exceptions to this procedure. Due to the small number of question- naires to be distributed in Test Seven, every second "vehicle” enter- ing the area received a questionnaire during the sampling period. 100 The investigation of mail back return, which took place in Test Eight, was conducted with questionnaire distribution at the area exit. Dur— ing sample periods a park ranger was stationed at the area exit and handed prepaid, self—addressed questionnaires to all vehicles leav— ing the recreation area. Thus, the only means of returning the ques- tionnaire was by mail. Implementation of the Testing Schedule In order to complete the testing of all variables within the summer months, an intensive testing schedule was outlined. This schedule is contained in Table 6. As in the "seasonal” study phase, all park supervisors from the "experimental" parks attended an orientation meeting prior to the commencement of testing. Sampling and testing techniques were dis- cussed, and the importance of close cooperation was stressed. Study team members visited the ”experimental" sample parks to supervise the installation of signs and collection boxes. Placement of these boxes and signs was identical to that used in the "seasonal" study phase. A ”contact" man was chosen, and since this person had to assume a great deal of responsibility, much time was spent outlining sampling, testing and analysis procedures. The "contact" man was instructed to immediately contact study team 101 30: o: .m> 30: pofiOHm Axomoflflmfi pew 5-955 “Swag peso umom .m> fimsofi ._.H.m3mom__ Antenna 8.32 P86 Hop .9» £32 in??? S3 .3 DE. $882 33 .mm .33. seesaw xooum .8an mo soHoU $2 .5. .23. 5623.8 $2 .3. b3. seeps pomofioeo 30qu o: .m> Zosem 33 .NN ER. $882 $3 .AN 33. $856 msofimosv mwmbmsm btfipow “soaomfim $3 .3 .33. .smppsemm $3 .2 has .smetm ofion o: .m> 30: @39on xoopm peso :bsmosz .m> xoopm Paco :Ewflq: psmo pogomcs .m> pogoh $2 .3 .23. seesaw $2 .2 has .smepfimm 82 .2 .33. 582m Umpm OH. mOHQmwhm> moped Emu. Emma .gflemfitmmxmz so 6388..“ I .o mamas. m3: .3 “mums... $8..me 53ng 512393 .m> champ 333.23% I h. wmmfi .m 55.954 Swank w 102 .H. @3339» 5 303.3938 yo.“ mama m mm. Umms mpm? 335mg 23. .mfiwccofimwsw :HmGOmmmm: 8. mmcommmp 90 5mm. 33 .3. $395. .hqunm N. 32 .m “mama... .3233 mpmvuo c3525 Emummfim . O wmmfi .N pmfiwfiw Swank o Umummh. mmfinwflmxw mmme «mmH vmscficoU nu .w madmgfi. 103 members by telephone if there was any doubt about testing procedures. To ensure that the data collection system was functioning correctly, study team members were present in all sample parks during the first "experimental" test. Throughout the remaining tests unan— nounced visits were made to sample parks in order to assess the success of testing procedures. The weekly administrative procedure during the series of experiments was as follows: Questionnaires utilizing the alternatives to be tested on the following weekend were designed on Sunday. On Monday, the ”photo—ready" questionnaire designs were delivered to the printer who generally had the desired number of copies prepared before Tuesday afternoon. The questionnaires were then rushed to the study center where park identification numbers and the date were stamped on them. Following collation of the different questionnaires within each test, they were counted, sorted and prepared for ship- ment. A set of instructions outlining sampling procedures, question— naire identification, and the recording of results were enclosed with- in each shipment package. A set of typical park instructions is enclosed in Appendix E. The "experimental" sample parks received the packaged questionnaires on Thursday afternoon in preparation for hand-out during the testing periods. At the completion of each test, the returned questionnaires were forwarded by the park staff to the study center for analysis. 104 Analysis of Data As questionnaires were returned to the study center they were examined for completeness of response. 1 Using the statistical methods previously outlined in this chapter, the significance of each variable was calculated. In order to determine whether the population completing one particular type of questionnaire differed from the population complet- ing another type of questionnaire (within the same experimental test), a comparative analysis was conducted. The responses to the five dif- ferent questionnaires in experimental Test 2 were compared using the analysis of variance and chi square techniques previously described in Chapter IV. The comparison would reveal differences in response between the different questionnaire types and would also reveal differences in response among the different "experimental" sample parks. 1The criteria used to judge completeness of response was the same criteria used in the ”seasonal" study phase. In Tests 1, 3, and 4 all questionnaires which were returned were judged to be complete. In these particular tests, the variables being investi- gated did not affect the actual content of the questionnaires. There- fore, there was no reason to suspect that there would be a difference between proportion of questionnaires returned and proportion of ques- tionnaires completed. 105 Results and Conclusions All of the experimental tests were conducted as scheduled, and no major difficulties occurred during the testing program. A summary of the number of questionnaires that were handed out, completed and returned is included in Appendix G. It is apparent that, similar to the results of the "seasonal” study phase, as the num- ber of people visiting the park increases the proportion of completed, returned questionnaires decreases. The average proportion of com- pleted, returned questionnaires to the number of questionnaires handed out was highest at Harrisville State Park (low-intensity usage). Pinckney State Recreation Area (intermediate-intensity usage) had the next highest proportion and Dodge Brothers #4 State Park (high— intensity usage) had the least number of questionnaires completed when compared to the total number of questionnaires handed out. Of the total number of questionnaires returned, approxi- mately one-quarter were incomplete. Therefore, they were not included in the analysis. Depending upon the degree of rigor used in judging completeness, from 10 per cent to 25 per cent of the total questionnaires returned in similar surveys will probably be lost from analysis. The percentage of questionnaires returned to number of questionnaires handed out varied from 48 per cent to 32.5 per cent 106 (excluding Test 8-—the mail-back test-—in which 15.5 per cent were returned). As the testing schedule progressed, the average number of returned questionnaires decreased slightly. This may be due to the length of the testing schedule. Since tests were conducted every week- end for five consecutive weeks, there is a good possibility that some park visitors received two or more questionnaires during the testing period. If an individual completed the first questionnaire he received, it is likely that the motivation to complete additional similar ques- tionnaires was less for each succeeding questionnaire. The proportion of questionnaires completed to the number of questionnaires handed out varied between 31. 5 per cent and 24. 5 per cent (excluding Test 8——the mail—back test--in which 15.5 per cent were returned and completed). The results of the significance testing of variables are ex- pressed in Table 7. All calculations were made at the 95 per cent con— fidence level following the procedure outlined earlier in this chapter. Of the total number of variables tested, only three alternatives exhib- ited a variance which was significant at the 95 per cent level of con- fidence. However, several other variables approached this signifi- cance level. The general conclusion one might draw from this is that park users appear to be of two general types when considering questionnaire completion: one type of park user will complete a 107 .coBmEommU ho.“ um momma oomo .omaommop mo mmouopmafioo .Hom cogfimxo who? mmfiwccofimosq @2338 mmmfi .mummp “nosvomnsm 5 .ouofldfioo omnmgmmoo who? 3 “won. 5 @2238 mmpwmccofimoao So “any «03 2: Song :Smop m 5 mooconmmfimo— .m 2an o“ ammo.“ ofisoonom ammo. Umfimumo hobo . mum; 30: m5 3mm pm: mum; 0 Z $-73 oz .m> .Q 3 wow. am pm: MHMHM 30: Q MYTH 69:on wnmufi . mum; Upmo mww m3: mfimm NINIH omgacb TN; 02 .m> .< H TH; . m-3-3 nnmo m5. 002 mHmm NIH; 6039.3 3-3-3 3.63 3.8 m .8 8.33800 So 8933 8383383 «m o mmfiwccofimosg mmfiwccoflmosg . o3m€w> pooh. :oEmHDonU mfiwcnofimosmv m mocmowficmwm go $5852 mo .HQQESZ $335.5 .afiqmfitmaxm: mo 3:83. we. .8 $3QO -- 3. mamafi 108 mu3q- m 33 3:35 wmm. mam S: NATN £953 HLVIN Undo menH oz .m> m fimcfl pom. Q33 gm N33“ __.3w3:wmm__ vmm. mm3 mam Nu3nm .3630 073 www. wm3 mam 3-3-m cofimmsv w 3&3». N: mam mn3um 3538.323 3033 wow. m33 gm N333 073 oz .m> m 2033 0mm. 3&3 gm 3-3-3» 3530a 3963 030mm 333800 30 nmccmm m mm .30 mmp3m§o33mms~u mmfimccofimmsg 330330333383 m3£m3pm> 59H. 3 3303335030 . . . whmccofimmsmv wucwowflcwfi . .30 umnanz .30 3933333373 UmfiGECOU .. n . N. WmewH 109 m m m . mumum c.3393 3&3 3mm oom3 3umum xown 332 w Ewe m mm? .m> um . 3 u 3.. m 93:36.3 3. 3% 3% ooo3 3.3.@ :3 3:333 3. 593. um 38 m-m-m 3 n 3. . u .. cam :3 USE: m3m mom 3m33 MIMIM 5953 v.30 pmom 073 .m> m . 33 3 w 5983 now m 5m N :pmngm: 3393 $8 m 30 832980 30 @3333 83833383 3m mmfiwcaofimmsg mmfimccoflmmsg . @3333? $93. 33033335030 ohmgoflmmnG mocmnvwficmfi 30 393333573 30 3238373 3.25380 -- .3. @3333. 110 3303333 mmm. 33 com 733-3-m oZ mew .m> m . Ummo3ocm 23 2% com 33.3.3” :0qu Wm E o3. $03 32.. N33 Emu 3-3-3 :35: mmW .m> 3 Hum $3. $33 82 313-3 3:8 T: .3333: 333 $3 .3 30 832388 :5 822; 8383383 3w mmfimgofimmsg mmfimccofimmng m3nm33w> KPH no 3333630 mfimccofimmsG mocmoafima .30 3938573 .30 3933333373 UmSGEGOU u: .3. HJm «mmH 383833830 9339338335330 mocmomamfi 30 3235373 30 pmngz 3305:3380 .1. .3. Mdmdfi. 112 questionnaire without being unduly influenced by the appearance and content of the questionnaire itself; the other type of park user tends to ignore the questionnaire in spite of different changes made in ques- tionnaire appearance and content. Based upon the results of this analysis, the questionnaire that would obtain the largest amount of response would have the fol- lowing characteristics (ranked in descending order of importance with regards to questionnaire completion): 1. The questionnaire would be handed out at the entrance to the recreation area and would be collected by means of a deposit box at the recreation area exit. 2. A pencil would be supplied with the questionnaire in order to facilitate completion. 3. The questionnaires would be printed on a "heavy" card stock. 4. The color of the questionnaire should be, in order of preference: blue, white, brown, tan, yellow, and orange. 5. The questions concerned with activity analysis should be constructed according to the following preference ranking: 2-—3-—5, 2--3--2, 2——1--1, 2—-3--4, and then 2--3--3. From these 1See Figure 11 for illustrations of the activity analysis questions. 113 results it appears that a closed, structured approach to collecting activity information would tend to obtain the largest amount of com- pleted response. The small difference in return between the regular length questionnaire and the post card length would indicate that as long as the questionnaire appears to be short in length, response will not be affected. When a "voluntary deposit" retrieval system is used in a park, the provision for pre—paid mailing of the questionnaire after leaving the park appeared to decrease the number of questionnaires returned. This could be due to the fact that respondents saw that they had an alternative way of returning the questionnaire. (It does not necessarily have to be completed while they were in the park. ) The questionnaire was then carried from the park and, in spite of good intentions to complete and return it, the card was then forgotten or mislaid. The inclusion of a slotted hole in the questionnaire did not appear to increase the response rate enough to warrant its additional expense. The results of the analysis of variance and chi square in- vestigation of Experimental Test 2 (determination of "best" activity analysis question) indicated that there were no significant differences 114 in the responses to the five different questionnaires. This compara- tive analysis was conducted to determine whether particular ques- tionnaires ”attract" responses from different segments of the park- using population. It appeared that despite the content and appearance of different questionnaires (if the compared questionnaires were reasonably similar), the populations completing the questionnaires possessed similar characteristics. Sub-hypothesis one is stated as follows: Through a process of trial and selective elimination, a questionnaire and dispersal-retrieval system can be designed which will obtain a greater number of usable completed re- sponses than other questionnaires tested under similar condi- tions. The results of this study phase indicate that sub-hypothesis one cannot be rejected. Testing of ten variables which might have affected response proved that by changing questionnaire design, con- tent and dispersal-retrieval, response rates can be altered. On the basis of the experimental testing of each variable, it was a simple task to choose the particular alternative that received the greatest number of completed responses. CHAPTER VI RECOMMENDATIONS The recommendations that follow apply generally to recreation survey research. Specific recommendations directed to the Michigan State Park system are included under a separate sub-heading later in this chapter. General Recommendations 1. Continuous data collection systems should be imple— mented in all park and recreation areas. As the number of potential recreation sites declines, and the use-intensity of present sites increases, more pressure will be brought upon park and recreation agencies to provide leisure opportunities. This increased pressure is already becoming apparent in the general interest displayed by the public towards park and recreation agencies, e. g. , public interest in the use of a recent Michigan recreation bond. Under these con- ditions, the park planners and administrators must have accurate data on which to base the decision-making process. Usually, the most valuable data is that which reveals "trends" in user 115 116 characteristics and characteristics of facility usage. This type of information probably provides the best present indication of probable future demand. 2. When recreation area "day-use" information is desired (on a continuous basis), a questionnaire data collection system of the type described in this study should be used. At the 95 per cent level of confidence, information collected by voluntary deposit of question- naires showed little significant difference from collected parameter information. The one area in which a small significant difference was noted was in the answers to questions concerned with travel time and distance. In spite of the inconsistencies noted in the answers to the travel questions, one trend was detected. Information collected by voluntary deposit of questionnaires showed slightly more ”travel time" and "miles traveled" than did parameter information. In future questionnaire surveys when travel information is desired, the respondent should be asked for both "mileage traveled" and ”travel time. " Based on the results of this study, if the response to one of the questions proves inaccurate, responses to the other travel question will likely yield the desired information. A questionnaire data collection system similar to the type described in this study can usually be implemented for a fraction of the cost of a comparable interview data collection system. 117 3. When "day-user" information is to be collected by means of a questionnaire, the questionnaire should be handed out at the area entrance, and collected by voluntary deposit at the area exit. This type of questionnaire dispersal-retrieval was more than twice as effective as a similar "mail-back” system tested. 4. All aspects of the survey system should be coordinated so as to provide a central theme or "motivational continuum" for the research project. If the same colors, language, survey symbols (e. g. , silhouette of a picnic table) can be used on all components of the survey system, the opportunity to associate components is in- creased. It is believed that the coordination of survey components was a major reason for the relatively high proportion of questionnaire completion in this investigation. 5. When designing a questionnaire and dispersal-retrieval system, every effort must be made to avoid possible non-response. Based upon the results of this analysis, the questionnaire that would obtain the largest amount of response would have the following characteristics (ranked in descending order of importance with regards to questionnaire completion): 3. A pencil would be supplied with the question- naire in order to facilitate completion. b. The questionnaires would be printed on a "heavy" card stock. 118 c. The color of the questionnaire would be, in order of preference: blue, white, brown, tan, yellow, and then orange. (1. Questions dealing with analysis of activities would be highly structured. Answer categories would be "closed, ” perhaps in the form of "simple" graphs. e. The questionnaire should "appear" to be of short length. The amount of material contained in a questionnaire does not seem to affect response as much as the appearance of being brief. 6. Better guidelines on the precision, reliability and validity of recreation research should be established. It is sug— gested that future recreation research be conducted at the 95 per cent level of statistical confidence. When calculating sample size, the estimation error should be no greater than .05 at the 95 per cent confidence level. Unless a concentrated effort is made to raise the standards of research within the field, even less attention will be paid to future research than has been the case in the past. 7. Whenever a data collection system utilizing a question- naire is being conducted, a further test (by means of the interview technique) should be made to determine the accuracy of collected information. In most circumstances, the amount of effort involved 119 in an additional interview accuracy-test is worth the trouble and cost. Too often, collected data is accepted as being 100 per cent accurate when this is in reality not the case. All data has limitations and if these limitations can be carefully and concisely outlined, the col- lected data can be used with more confidence. One interview-test per season should be sufficient to ascer- tain the accuracy of information collected by questionnaire. This is assuming that there are no drastic changes in the questionnaire con- tent or design, the questionnaire dispersal—retrieval, the park itself, or in the population using the park. 8. Further investigation should be made of the I. B. M. optical scan process. This method uses the reflection of light from a heavy lead pencil mark to produce a data processing (computer) card. Theoretically, any base material that does not also reflect ' If a questionnaire light could be used as a questionnaire "base. ' could be designed that could be read electronically, with no inter- mediate coding stage, survey research could be performed at a great financial saving. 9. Whenever possible additional staff members should be used to conduct survey research. It is suspected that one of the main reasons for the high proportion of returned questionnaires in the low- use parks was that the park staff member could devote more time 120 to "selling" each potential respondent on the importance of the research. Additional staff members, with sole responsibility to the survey research project, could take the time for an enthusiastic motivating message without worrying about the other responsibilities of a contact station ranger. Also, special staff members could be trained in research methods, thus helping to ensure the validity and reliability of collected information. 10. Survey researchers should give more attention to the determination of the method of data collection that best meets their needs. As more expertise in research methodology develops, and as more financial support is made available for research, this prob- lem will likely be eliminated. 11. Further research needs to be undertaken to check the validity of the collected information. Recommendations to the Michigan State Park System 1. There appears to be a growing trend to use state park and recreation areas throughout the entire year, rather than merely for the short summer season. In order to ascertain use and user characteristics of this segment of the park—using population, con- tinuous data collection systems should extend through the entire year. 121 2. The entrances and contact stations in the majority of state parks do not lend themselves to a user survey. There are usually no distinct separate entrance lanes for vehicles with season entry passes or for those "vehicles" purchasing daily entry permits. Traffic congestion makes traffic survey research difficult. All vehicles entering the park should come to a full stop. On the other hand, if a "vehicle" possessing an entrance permit can drive directly into the park without having to stop, any type of sampling or survey procedure is difficult. At present, the task of the contact station ranger is difficult even when a survey is not being conducted. When a Visitor is at the contact station purchasing a permit, the ranger has to virtually ignore the other vehicles driving directly into the park. 3. More attention should be given to the construction of adequate contact stations at park area entrances. If there were two entrance traffic lanes and the contact station was located between them, more control could be exercised over incoming traffic. Better communications between the park administrative office and the contact station should be established. In most parks, if any problems arise at the area entrance the ranger must leave the contact station to render assistance. Passing motorists must be depended upon to notify other rangers. If the door to the contact station was located either in the front or side, the ranger could 122 leave the station without losing sight of the traffic or the front counter (and his cash box) in the station. 4. More effective means of obtaining counts of all vehicles entering the park should be installed. A number of state parks do not have an accurate count of the number of vehicles admitted. Determination of sample size depends upon an accurate count of vehicles entering recreation areas. SE LE C TED BIBLIOGRAPHY SE LE C TED BIBLIOGRAPHY 1 Books and Periodicals Bachrack, Stanley D. , and Scoble, Harry M. ”Mail Questionnaire Efficiency: Controlled Reduction of Nonresponse. " Public Opinion Quarterly, XXXI (January, 1967), 265-271. (Q) Backstrom, Charles H. , and Hursh, Gerard B. Survey Research. Evanston: Northwestern University Press, 1963. Bauer, Raymond A. "Social Indicators and Sample Surveys. " Public Opinion Quarterly, XXX (February, 1966), 343-345. (C) Cannell, Charles F. , and Fowler, Floyd J. "Comparison of a Self-Enumerative Procedure and a Personal Interview: A Validity Study. " Public Opinion Quarterly, XXVII (1963), 250-264. (C) Clawson, Marion. Land and Water for Recreation: Opportunities, Problems, and Policies. Chicago: Rand, McNally and Company, 1963. , and Knetsch, Jack L. Economics of Outdoor Recreation. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins Press, 1966. Cochran, William G. Sampling Techniques. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1964. 1The letter in parentheses immediately following research reports refers to the research method used to obtain data, i. e. (O) = Observation, (1) = Interview, (Q) = Questionnaire, and (C) = Combination of research methods. 123 124 Colfax, David J. , and Allen, Irving L. "Pre-coded vs. Open—ended Items and Children' 5 Reports of Father' 3 Occupation. ” Sociology of Education, XL (January, 1967), 96-98. (C) Deming, W. E. "On Errors in Surveys. " Review, IX (1944), 359-369. American Sociological Doxiadis, Constantinos A. Emergence and Growth of an Urban Region. Detroit: The Detroit Edison Company, 1967. Festinger, Leon, and Katz, Daniel. Research Methods in the Be— havioral Sciences. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1966. Good, Carter V. , and Scates, Douglas E. Methods of Research. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, Inc., 1954. Gorgen, Kenneth J. , and Back, Kurt W. "Communication in the Interview and the Disengaged Respondent. " Public Ojlinion Quarterly, XXX (Fall, 1966), 385-387. (I) Kerlinger, Frederick N. Foundations of Behavioral Research. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1967. Knudson, Dean D. ”Response Differences to Questions on Sexual Standards: An Interview-Questionnaire Comparison. " Public Opinion Quarterly, XXXI (1967), 290-297. (C) Mendenhall, William. Introduction to Probability and Statistics. Belmont: Wadsworth Publishing Co. , 1967. Moser, C. A. Survey Methods in Social Investigation. London: Heinemann Educational Books Ltd. , 1958. Parten, Mildred. Surveys, Polls, and Samples: Practical Pro- cedures. New York: Harper and Brothers, 1950. Reissman, Leonard. Class in American Socieiy. New York: The Free Press, 1959. Riley, Matilda White. Sociological Research: A Case Approach. New York: Harcourt, Brace, and World, Inc., 1963. 125 Selltiz, Claire; Jahoda, Marie; Deutsch, Morton; and Cook, Stuart W. Research Methods in Social Relations. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1967. Sessions, Frank Q.; Epley, Robert J ; and Moe, Edward O. "The Development, Reliability, and Validity of an All-Purpose Optical Scanner Questionnaire Form. " Public Opinion Quarterly, XXX (1966), 423-428. (Q) Public Documents and Reports Chubb, Michael. Outdoor Recreation Planning in Michigan. Report for Michigan Department of Conservation. Lansing: Michi- gan Department of Conservation, 1967. Clawson, Marion, and Knetsch, Jack L. "Recreation Research: Some Basic Analytical Concepts and Suggested Framework for Research Problems. ” Proceeding of the National Con— ference on Outdoor Recreation Research. Ann Arbor, Michigan, 1963, 9-42. Detroit Metropolitan Area Regional Planning Commission. Home Survey of Regional Recreation Activities. Detroit: Detroit Metropolitan Area Regional Planning Commission, 1959. (Q) Park Users Survey: A Survey of Persons Using Eight Selected Regional Parks in the Detroit Region. Detroit: Detroit Metropolitan Area Regional Planning Commission, 1959. (Q) Hirsch, M. Lower Fort Garry National Historic Park Visitor Use Study. Canada: Department of Indian Affairs and Northern Development, April, 1967. (O) James, George A. , and Henley, Robert K. Sampling Procedure for *— Estimating Mass and Dispersed Types of Recreation Use on Large Areas. Asheville: U. S. Department of Agricul— ture, Forest Service, March, 1968. James, George A. , and Taylor, Gordon D. ”Recreation Use Esti- mation in the United States and Canada. " XIV IUFRO- KONGRESS. Section 26. (Munchen, 1967), 15-29. 126 King, David A. Characteristics of Family Campers Using the Huron- Manistee Natural Forests. St. Paul: U. S. Department of Agriculture, 1965. (I) Mutch, W. E. S. Public Recreation in National Forests: A Factual Survey. Forestry Commission Booklet No. 21. London: Her Majesty' s Stationery Office, 1968. (I) New Jersey Department of Conservation and Economic Development. New Jersey State Park Visitor Use Survey 1966-1967. State of New Jersey: Department of Conservation and Economic Development, 1968. (I) Nixon, H. N. Banff National Park: Aspects of Visitor Use. Canada: National Parks Service--Planning, Department of Indian Affairs and Northern Development, March, 1967. (I) Prince Albert National Park Visitor Use Survey. Canada: National Parks Service--Planning, Department of Indian Affairs and Northern Development, January, 1968. (I) RidingMountain National Park Visitor Pattern Survey. Canada: National Parks Service--Planning, Department of Indian Affairs and Northern Development, June, 1968. (C) North Central Texas Council of Governments. A Parks Appraisal. Texas: Texas Technological College, School of Agriculture, 1968. Ontario Department of Lands and Forests. Ontario Park User Survey-— Methodology and Results. Toronto: Department of Lands and Forests, 1967. (I) Outdoor Recreation Resources Review Commission. National Recrea- tion Survey. Study Report 19. Washington, D. C.: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1962. (I) Outdoor Recreation Literature: A Survey. Study Report 27. Washington, D. C.: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1962. Participation in Outdoor Recreation: Factors Affecting Demand Among American Adults. Study Report 20. Wash- ington, D. C.: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1962. (I) 127 The Quality of Outdoor Recreation: As Guidance by User- Satisfaction. Study Report 5. Washington, D. C.: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1962. (C) Taylor, Gordon D. Techniques in the Evaluation of Recreation Use. Ottawa: Department of Indian Affairs and Northern Develop- ment, 1967. Thorsell, J. W. A Trail Use Survey--Banff and Yoho National Parks. L/ Canada: National Parks Service--Planning, National and ' Historic Parks Branch, Department of Indian Affairs and Northern Development, February, 1968. Waterton Lakes National Park Visitor Use Survey. Canada: National Parks Service--Planning, Department of Indian Affairs and Northern Development, March, 1968. U. S. Department of Commerce. Tourism and Recreation: A State of the Art Study. A report prepared by Arthur D. Little, Inc. , Washington, D. C. : Government Printing Office, 1967. Other Sources Crane, Dale A. "Progress Report, Presented to Conference on Methods of Demand Analysis for Fishing, Hunting, and Other Outdoor Recreation Activities. " Washington, D. C. , February 27-28, 1967. Jackson, Robert Morton. ”Differential Value of the Mailed Question- naire and the Interview in a Follow-up Study of High School Graduates. " Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1959. Shafer, Elwood Lewis, Jr. "Effects of Sampling, Location, Period, and Method on Camper Survey Results. " Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, State University College of Forestry at Syracuse University, 1966. APPENDICES APPENDIX A ”SEASONAL" QUESTIONNAIRE 128 "Hakka “mmgbz HQOO vzgd 6qu EB undo—Auntflon 3E3 «on on olnl-I-lol-l "Ha—gm..— .I all. 1 .III' ...III .| "NA; «ma—0333:, Emu. Zn NEONA— firh ho a0< HE Hfi< B<§ V 35 SE; .550 D .3. 3:2,: 30 965 D .m one? 520.... 23 D .3 33:00 0.58 no 28 D .38 33.0 2960 one D .0 To... .ezo .53 .485. 8.650 55 8553 25 D A. 95.3» 3362:de D .0 £339 533 .3358 one D .a 88 “.85. ... “So—=3; ma. 5 $93 mzr flange ham 0253.393 mi. ..30 53:3 m 2. D 8.3 D 338an gm 8:. E 458 2. 968 no» mm... N ...... .. ...SD. EU «Eggumémzaagopowan — ....33 x52 my; m wmhazE no H.909 1 >420 3:2. ..3 ; .... $280.28 263335 :a 50 333.3 0305 330.38 36% 353333 05 83.5.3 05. E8»? fan .335 293:: 223 3 van: on :33 who: 9:.» 30.3 con-35.385 23.3. 33.3.5 35m .336 on: on? 0:300... 05 33930.3 3 3.3023 0.35 no one 0.3 :0.» ..tl. l4 4 «J. 1.1. 4 1 . .. . . . . .. . 1 Wmmx..s...r..w.1 . . . : . .. ...... i g... 5.: a * ...... . . . . ...........U..m,t¢f.. I. .... . p . £3523 . 2.4.... . 8.. Q E... 5 so 2 use. afiwn E... ”88 93305 6535 in .5285: 38m 558.: 95:5 88.58: 3.282 .2” 58m :5 9.35.4 v5 £83833 832833 63 s 33.2: 0303.3 Jena 05 89c .32: 6.30 «35 cor—.30 5353.303 023 so» 33 mic: as: mug nz< 59353.3 z< m><= Ed: 55> mo“. 92$: 3:23 .3... 932. .0 9:6: ..3 p.32. .a a 083.3. 35353333 .EEU< NE (.30 EDIE 28 9.2QO memo EDD? E3033 ho mag MHZ. zu— flg 0.0434 .28 Han. 88: NE. Emma 990330 ~50? EU< NDOh "Hm—.3. 2— NE .BSflm mafia—trauma mfloomm xom 5:. 2. 9:3 m—rh m> >032m .xmf. NIP w> mmOuwm kmnfi 4:2: mzo_._.wm:o oz_z_<2wm MIA. .50 4...... .82 on s mzo gamut—a < 25:. 5:93 3:24 BE. $003 so.» an E on 88.8586» Dd 3.6 Ba 88.3» D.» 88.8588 D .o m8.§».o8.2» D a «8.388....» D .n m8.3».o8.2» D6 88.3 5.5. D .n A08 185. «in: 35 g 45.2. mm... 8308a 5mm 03342 mi. ..3 .353 «— ouoE no S o— m.— v~ nu D D U U D NH «a an a m b m n v m N a DDDD DDDDDDDD .5... 86.8”“ D .u .Ed 85785 D .u .5... 8&é8a D 6 .Ed 85.86 D c 82-8.2 D .9 .53 36.6ch D .0 .58 85783 D J 38 x83. ..me a5. 552m :0» So was 955 m— D D D 32952 «— mefim 2 72:33:52 Ea E30 9— DUDE] DUDE] U D D Eu 3 a D D D U D D D D D D a a clam o a D Z unau on as munav uo>o onlfin own: 076 nn~ .Smhmhm xm 8:. Gum; DO? m>22 30: 32:“; £255 one :85. 9.555 53» .3 9.3:... 8:. .3 33.3200 22.5028 3 m5: ..3 xmmzpz 455,—. . 8:. .3 m5. 8:28.: 3mm 33mm Ewamzc. m2... .3 353 N— 2: 833. 833.600 away—Q 5:. .3 a 02:5 < mo... on ...SEE :3» .3 35:... E: 88 SE? —— nmmmamz... 03:3. D . Sam Bag .......... ... mo< mm 8. 22: D . s ...».Bfim E3» .3 9.5:: mm... .3 mo< mséuxoxmfi 92¢. xmm mm... a 955 e— 00:: uuuuuuuuuuuuuuuu p 83: 30.» 29: 3.3 9:. a .mesom sumac 302 m5. 3 .835 >24: 30: n3 :3: n.— :6: Tax? NIH. OZS< NEH. 835/2089. OEQDAOZ— EOE w><¢E 929nm 30% 8Q EB :02: 30: nnnnnnnnnnnnnnnnn N. MDOU .m—N £20.» 9 95.—3 33m 3530 .05 .3 .58. 62.82 3: 8223 «8.: 83.3 s 3.3: 3.meme E3» a 38.3 :3 3 .33 D A :3 ~74. D .u 8% 8-; U .u 25 a-» D 6 £96 8-: D .u 9.26 T" D .a :3... 1% 2-2 D .o {a 3.5 as 3 25 D .. “mam” a afim i=8 Dumb DO.” H><2 m>22 30: ‘— 2. 3:3. .343 D .m 9.333., .393... 3:32 a u and 3.8 32:25 0.35 .3 9.5 3 «.3 D 0 005353 a 3 tan D .o 35.3: 2.5 335 .338 a no En D a 53:33 5 3 tan D .u 3.5 .3 33:0 and 93 D J A23 3039 «ADOmU KEG? mow W29 m Emir ho BAU— aka—3 - - Continued APPENDIX A. APPENDIX B WEEKLY REPORT FORM USED IN ”SEASONAL” SAMPLE PARKS 130 down; Hwfio mmbv nonHqumem mans “503$..." 98 95» «>2: 95qu ME; 2H mnm¢ a a 2m , zu.mam¢u ‘eoo mnmgu u>4 m a 24‘ wa. .zH mamao .aao mam¢o w>4 m a 2m .zu.mnm¢u apo.mnm¢u w>4 m g _a<; .mm L E W25 So 858 92 m A In. . ; om4o Boo mom¢u u>4 m a :< «zzp 2H mammo, .933 nom¢ m 4 Arm 2H woman aoo momcu u>4 m a z< am: ...-.45 858 So man I 1 92 m a z“. zurmam4u sac mango u>4 m a xz< t 2H mom¢u aao mam4u u>4 a q :a 2H wom< . m A zqa. . auounccaz .¢ ...I .coo: nouns nuufidu 52.6 .n .. you: .q amouuflwo .m .3 mmuuo 05 can soon 0.3qu 3098 .. 3330: .m >258 m>¢uommuo>4 .m co>wooou nouco no nonauc uuooom yam“; .u uozonm ago“; .~ aauumm .~ munmam .~ 2.02 2" ocoz .H 53.8 ..n .733 ...” .600: uouuo can :00: 3800 v.33 counumuwmwooum nm>ou om ummumoc >43 “50 03.3: nun—do no none—s: vacuum 050.8 ”9.3 on ousuaummaoa m~_m m4¢z4xmmx 4<_um¢m APPENDIX C "SHORT FORM" QUESTIONNAIRE USED AS THE INTERVIEW SCHEDULE 131 010 YOU ow m ms PARK LAST mom? [] Yes [:]No AREYOUD some TO ow IN THIS PARK TONIGHT? Yes UHAT ARE THE AGES OF THE PEOPLE IN THIS VEHICLE? MALI: ____l -.—.-. ______I ‘___.D _____0 _____' :L‘IALE: -_‘_, ‘_ . , , , HOW MANY HOURS DID YOU SPEND IN THIS PARK TODAY? (Check a. [J 2 hours or less b. C] 2-4 hours c. [3 4-6 hours d. C] 6-8 hours e. [3 8-10 hours t. [J 10-12 hours 3. [J 12 hours or more One) WHERE IS roux PRESENT HON—E? (Exact street address not requireay Town or City I County State WHAT IS YOUR ZIP CODE? HOW MUCH TIHE DID YOU SPEND TRAVELING TO THIS PARK TODAY! (NOT INCLUDING "STOPOVER" TIME ALONG THE HAY.) Minutes HOW MANY DAYS HAVE YOU USED THE PARK IN 1968? a. [3 this is the first park e. visit b. C] 1-4 days c. [3 5-8 days d. E] 9-12 days h. C] 13-16 days [3 17~20 days [3 21-24 days E] over 24 days HOW MANY MILES. BY THE MOST DIRECT ROUTE, IS THIS PARK FROM HOME? Miles HHAT IS THE SEX AND APPROXIMATE AGE OF THE "HEAD OF YOUR FAMILY"? [3 male 9 . AGE. __ ________ years SEX. [3 female WHAT DOES THE "HEAD OF YOUR FAMILY" DO FOR A LIVING? WHICH OF THE ANSWERS BELOW BEST INDICATES THE TOTAL NUMBER OF YEARS OF EDUCATION COMPLETED BY THE "HEAD OF YOUR FAMILY"? (Check one answer) DDDUDDDDDUDU DDUDD KHICH OF THE FOLLOWING BEST DESCRIBES THE TOTAL INCOME OF YOUR FAMILY LAST YEAR (Check one) WHICH OF THE ACTIVITIES LISTED BELOW DID YOUR GROUP DO HHILE HERE? (Check all the boxes that apply). e. fl g. E] sightseeing from car only [3 walking to scenic points [3 picnicking [3 looking at plants, animals or birds for a hobby [] swimming E] wading [J sunbathing OTHER h. 1. j. k. E] water q. [3 games 8 skiing team sports C] skin or r. [j trail hikingl scuba diving 3. [] horseback E] notorboating riding [] sailing t. [J listening to 1. [] canoenng ll. n. 0- p. [j rouboating [j boat fishing [3 bank fishing museums or nature [3 fishing (wading) centers ranger talks u. E] taking guided tours v. [j visiting u. [j relaxing x. [3 photography (write in) a. 0 under $3,000 a. B 510,000 - $14,999 IL [J $3,000 - $5,999 f.[] $15,000 - $24,999 c. D $6.000 - $7,999 g. 0 $25,000 - and over d.C] $8,000 - $9,999 WHAT TIME DID YOU ENTER THIS PARK? a.[] 8:00 -10:00 a.m. (Check one) t>. C] 10:00-noon e.[] 6:00-6:00 p.m. c. E] noon-2 :00 p. m. f.EJ 6:00-8:00 p.m. d. [:1 2:00-4:00 p.m. 34:] 3:00-10:00 p.m. IN THE SPACES PROVIDED BELOW, WRITE IN THE FOUR ACTIVITIES YOUR GROUP SPENT THE MOST TIME DOING. NUMBER OF HOURS YOUR GROUP SPENT DOING EACH 0? THE ACTIVITIES. Activities ALSO WRITE IN THE Time Spent THANKS FOR (OUR HELP: HAVE AN ENJOYABLE & SAFE TRIP HOME LICENSE NUMBER PARK CODE NUMBER: '1 DATE: 1 IDENTIFICATION NUMBER APPENDIX D OPTICAL SCAN FORMS USED IN CODING 132 - I I? I N 1 I P .' 1’ A T I U N I N I N F ‘ ‘ I I 1 3 C S C 1 I 9 3 g g a 3 a g g 1 g g ‘ o 1 1 1 a s s 1 O I ‘ a 1 1 4L 0 L s 1 L 1 ‘ g 4 J 1 1 I 1 1 1 1 l x I L 1 1 1 1 4 1 1 I 1 ’ 0 1 1 L A j o 1 1 1 1 4 1 1 a ’ 0 j I 1 O I 0 1 3 9 : 'vm M (I'D WTWVIIT IV 7 :1uw—r III-I iii-T Tlun‘rr lerwr VII-r 1v—a‘w‘rr ' "I—"II" T‘I VIUV' # l A . l . L I . I t I ti 1: s - n I 1. rt " 1 -’ 0‘. OI, 0’. I.’ 'u '0’ ‘0. 'D. “0 3 ‘D ~10 'E‘ 0“ ’3‘ -” '3’ -“ -.. U“ 1 -5 -.. -11 .3. .as .1. .11 -00 -u no ‘ -1 -1o .11 -10 as -10 as 40 40 u» no no no 1 E1 A 1 ' CTT' I I a 1 1 1 0 s a 1 a 1 ’ L 1 1 4 s s 1 a a CVV‘I‘I'r '4 L 1 1 1 1 4 1 a 1] l a 1 1 1 1 s c 1 1 1 3 4L 4 1 j 0 -14 1 I 1 ’ o 1 -1 1 s s 1 1 w- v' j 't' cows 2 o 1 1 1 a s a 1 a 1 ' 1 1 1 4 1 0 1 a 1 ‘ a 1 .2 a 4 1 0 1 I 1 ’ a 1 A 1 1 a a tTVI'YIII TIWI: 'WILI‘U Tirvwr "0" 1a 1 L111 1 1111 1 1111 o cm 1' i o 1 1 1 0 1 1 1 1 1 a A a a 1 a 1 ’ a 1 s s j a a r LY 0 -1 1a 16 11. 0 -1 -1a - - 30 '1STITE Phil IS! ‘. NATIONAL PARK USE I .l 0.. up -¥ .“ . u n o o - H‘ . I I J J x "Uri! 71 7"“ l x Hunt: 1.1.? County 51111-3 O,'t‘UPATIONI 133 Iorurlrxcar1oa nun-Ia , 0 1 1 1 0 s s 1 a I o 1 a 1 1 s 1 a 1 1 1 0 s s 1 a 1 1 L 1 1 1 a a 1 a 1 L a a 1 L 1 4 a L x slcursrslus "nitric A-Ol‘ m .‘03‘ m 11cn1cs1ac LOOIIIs tug)” n. 0"." '- sulnnxuc vans-16 51.05. '8 P-DS‘ '- suunnrnxlc Infra 51111116 6-01” m I-O.‘ 1. 51111 01111115 nororlonrxuc l-OQ‘ us J-Ion m 31111.11”; cauotxuc l-lln "3 Plan I. aouaoarxuc soar rxsuxuc n-ulD VII "Na na aaar rxsnxnc rxsuxlls-lanlus 04,1111 m you-a ran sans; rIAIL 111111116 0-17. m I-IO. '- uoaszaaca 1101-6 ”1"l'l'6 3.1.1» us run us 10011: vxslrxlc u-n” I. v-um m IILAIXIIG PHOTOGRAPH! *2)” II. ]. orura TOTAbuACTTVT‘I'TII 7"“ 1 J L J— L l ac'rxvzrr 1 o 1 1 1 a 1 a 1 a 1 nouns ACTIVITY 1 O I. 3 ’ nouns 1 A L .1‘. J L L L I l L A 1' V TY , . . s . . HOURS I Ill J__I_m 1 2111 l 3111 C 4113 5. APPENDIX E TYPICAL "EXPERIMENTAL" SAMPLE PARK TEST INSTRUCTIONS 134 .cEaHoo awn» aw mvmm uxw: no sumo choowm .z.m oouoa um anonsumm can .z.m oouoa um mmowum mmxon uwmommv Eonm momma uumHHoo .xon .voumzmsxm ma aammsm us» Hausa mmvuaumm can hmcwum Ham mcumo #90 can: can amkum .z.< oouw um :fimwm I "maaconom vacuum: was» onHOM wmmoam .Homm ma Hmnummz map NH .vmumsmsxm mw aflmmzm Hausa use can: can Amumm ow. momma com saws unmum .z.m coum um canon Gamma u .voumzmaxm ma aammam may Hausa age can: can Amuom oma. mcuoo com :ua3 uumum .z.< ooum um gamma u .umm .cmumamcxm mw xammsm may Hausa use can: can .muom on. mouse oma nufis uumum .z.m oon~ an gamma a .voumzmaxm ma yammsm ms» ”was: use can: can ..muom omv moumo omH nu“: uumum .z.< ooua um canon . .flum no : osom unoccm: mwsu soaaom mmmmam .000 mg Monumm3 0:» NH .mufimccowumwsv no» man :ufis wcficcwmmn Hoouo cw use Ems» can: Ammqmz¢xm7mmmv ammoqozm .3OHHM ocwuuso 0:0 can xouHa mco nu“; uumno a m“ mm coflumusoumumuu .msonum xomHn n ma: nods: HHQSU 5 fl“ MN :ofiumosouenmu~ n.0wc mowuw>wuom -cfiwun mw :ofiu luosv mmnumnmum ”.muwuw>wuom 0:» gums. nu“; mafia nun vN counummaoj .vw can mm mcofiu omwsw mmzumumuw :..3Oamn vmcwp noun muommm ma» gowns. an“: mcfi=< o~ wash :H: sufi3 mcwm.>mcusumm :ofiuomaaoo 0:» GM oouwmom .umnummou cmama awn cm coHumoso can two Hanssc mg» mam8«m ma nHoo mcwmuam so» on omwaamsm ma Hnfl3 .vw can mm mcofip ma mash vacusuwu umnfiac was Wfiuwmccofiummav ucmumumwc 0>wm may Immsw manuauanm amoflum p30 amnzaapm mm: >qa v¢ mumguoum‘wmvon xmwm Amv vumnm mo mama mo ummmmc mnmum NIMIN Too Qmoz:oo muos ma cHso3 u“ .mconm an» minim Hummus umm: ummx 0n canon nowumEH0waw mfinu HH .umaamo on» o» cucusumu uwnfidc vums~ can use caucus Hogan: msu ommu mummam can ooum .wmocnm no cacosm on Haw; now momnm wéamsacm .mcfimcmg .muflmmo aowmfi>fio 0» sun» uumzuom Namum memmc can .805» Hman .mcumo no maccaa comm caucus momma owummam usm mmmwam H-H-~ .mcfiumm: guso can ooum conga; «on: map umnc: umnasc mnu umuzo rdonm H0 2 can use cmccmn no: Hawum n no mumm may ucaoo .mmuflmcco«ummaw mo umnEd: cmcusumm Anna» 0:» use new: on manwmmanfi ma can ooum ya “a .um>mzom .wmvnsumm can amcwum wmaHmm :0 momma Ham use can: on umsmuud F30 amaz35 mm: >65 Mocmuucm zc~H304: .3 mmDUHm m n m u N on Race Sm 3.0 N: m u m on .mfifioflwosG H .. H u m on EncoflmosG 8...: ...-no! 63-5... .35 33.2.5. 83m 555.: 033.3. 8883: 33.2 .3... 88». ...... 3.5:: ...: €338. 8:333. .3 = :15 0301 Jul. 05 Bob F)- E :5 vein-o b.8300- obqn 3.. a $5: 25 Him 92 5.252“. 5. w><= Ed... ”.2; «on. 8.25:. 8...... 5.822! £533 .3“. 3.35.9. 85m 535.: 5:5 22:23. .235. .8». ES... :5 2:53.... ...: €233. 8:353. .3 = ....E 0301 Jul. 05 59c .33.. End 35 var—.30 .3380- !in no» a “to: “:5 E5 mz< “1:25.15 5 m> Mun—.3: LO ZNQEPI. ...—:F 7.“ HHVS» O.rJ< .CZEOG mink PW»: ...:C. Hilde—v. QDCKU EEC? WEt.C.—.b< mph: m2... .6 “wk—K3 .BOJEE Quararim manna...“ NI... a QN 551333 3!... 25... 3953 «an a a ”15:3 98: 8 EDOKO “DO? SO EFU< 22:? MN local 1 US 52. nulllué BB. I _U ... Baal -- l D ... «8 Iggy—D HIP an #35 '8 In} 1. D .o :34 1.1.1.11... 33 D .0 {all 2 D .n 953' ......... 3.55.5 D ... 3331'! n- D .c 950.. ....... 1.05.33: D .u aim 3 males 3% non «.539. ”...... 89 CE? .52.. 5:. z. adv; :uFruch .Ehtsg Emits. on Bo E520 uhjazoo 5.0» o... .3 Sum 5.98 .30» b #5: EPEU... mu... 5?..- VN .5 375. .350 «5555.. D .u 33.9804. D ... . Annals. uEz-u D a a. D .— 5332 D... .32.... 45... D .o .33... :3 D .c 3312!?” D .6 .309. n .5. £23 ..a 2555. 8 BE - .0 E35... 9.35.. 5 F D .3 3563:. 95.2.. D ... 953 3.3.. 233 D... a: 00.... . 5.3 . u D _ .55... i .398. D .u ..‘E 8 355...: D .. as...» D ... 9.22:0... D ... .51: 1933.34. D .a 85.-€038 D A 35.x. 212.2...D; .33... 2:8. o. 2.3:. D A Esau 1:9. .3 sin D ._ 3:0 .23 Eco. v5 ...-Elm D ... :5!- ..3c! D .n 596 5:905:5- D .- ..:fi. :5 .38.. on. ... .86. a, 3...... 3...... on 55...... 5.0» Ba 33mm swam: WEE? 5F .5 5:5 MN ...: 3:3. .35». ...—3.35.... D .u Anna-i. union D .n 8:39! B .. ~55... :5: D .o 3555;. D .- use... :8 D ... act-3.30.. D .E .32.... u .5. . 5:2 ... 158.5 3.023.. D _ .55... .... ecu—:5. D .v 9.3.5835. D .x U552?— D. .- 23:9... ..... an: .3 $5.53.: “3.55; D . I 9:5. 322. 95.3 D ... 3...: 1135....6. .2... D .. 5...... D .. use... 19309.2. D .o man-.8508... D ... 350... 9.2:. 2!: D ... an. ..a. Econ. 0. 95:5. D .3 Bi: 3:0... 50 :0: D ._ Eco ado =33 va- nes}... D .0 :5!- »81.’ D .a SP: 3305...»... D .- ...a... as .33 as :- 126. $5.”... 5:...» on 5.9.0 58> Eo BS... out”... REFEU.‘ “:F .3 5:5 MN .A .52.. a..." 8 939. H... as 6.... as... 2.303 fink—do: :0 Ease...» 22:58”... ..owzm z< m><= Ed: «8» mo”. $.25: i ll gugua «3553:... —:gummm— «IN .N .Im ":13 lulu-om ..80 .953 a... «333.8489 Salami 5.52 £8 5 3.. «al.- 05 no .525 05 5 8th 030... .08- oaorm c.831— 480 fig O Q O «98.8.83. 0559”.“ hams-honflofl gnu-fio< 55739. oflzlfo£58t388n MN .0 .n 3:7... BE. 2. 859. mum 2 8.... .5» 5:03 359:. no Eamoaao 229:5”... q<= Ed: 55> «on. 8.25: '"DMM 9W )0 w In und- bm mm 110nm nan moqn on.“ mold ' 339 z 1- Iznoq 3.58839 asgué3gfiua Signing 33NMM§S8E§EGN .0 .& «gm BE 2 Gun—Q4 awn OF Hun—A 30k 383 §§O xomm :1 .NH mmDUHm mumum cam muvum mmuwmgoflmmng m-m-m 28 Néé 3:438:85 $5: .5: gm 82 5352”,. z< u><3 Hut. 23< a 2.359289. 29.340!— FOE wh<8fi at U: 0.... 944m><¢h Ola-Am 90» an flak IRS in! n ................. 9 “Eco Eu .50» a 955 N 85m 5550 Dru .8 nick 69:32 Us .923: .09:- 235 F 8.0: ~22qu .50» fl nuns? — zoCmmso >aw>m .So .3: um mutizt no m4aaou < >420 mug. 44 _l : 23.83 2823.6 :- :8 :c 2.105 3...! 30> mention I: .3593 v:- ESP? :3 .5: 9.5.3:: 2!. 8 v00: on :7 9.3.. 9:.— .5.» sonar—£5 .5. ing 38 use on: oat 9.9.99: 05 30.0.3... 3 c.8920. 035 .o 2! 93 30> 5. a. 3., +351!!! 5 E is. Humum cam Huwum mmfimzcoflmmna was: .5: gm oz< Wage—.5 z< w>.:l Lo Q 7.2:. .5 “.4.—Qua 5:. h: WMU< HIP m5 F; N .... D t... D .EEE 7.5 :2 ......E a as: .5» Sn — 3.0:mwso ..umsh ~30 .3: wm470 w¢<= Ed: :8» me $25: 656-6 H“ g ”a go< "a ho ID: Ego Agog; hoggagg .ESFEEEHAna—DOQO flsrggfllhl—E .38: 8990.: nus—DHIFE N E. 8:). has 0581' D .- RfluIDi . I231! D o 3!!)- D .. 38.023: .a 35530.» .5333 D 389.2 . 348.8. pinion-Inna... D...“ .25 .8 .155 13 . :2: D w .aia..ua18_ D ... iii—383.0 4 3:1. D J 50.8... D .0 35:13:80.. 23%... D .. 858 Indiana; .52.. 3823231.. U ... Eon- 350- no 51.. D é £8 .39 gal-ED... gal-5:0... Egg-D .n 7383305213 pg ".5 8 “92° “8% an 38mm ONES §U< HE no In: — 3:330 >¢w>w .50 .3: umI‘In Tests 1, 3, and 4 all returned questionnaires were In the remaining Tests, all returned questionnaires were individually examined for completeness of response. 146 Test Que stionnaire Number Number Number Identification Handed Out Returned Completed 4 3-1-1 274 158 158 3-1—2 273 147 147 3-1-3 273 159 159 3-1-4 273 141 141 3-1-5 273 157 157 3-1-6 273 153 153 5 4-1-1 387 124 102 2-4-1 387 114 93 2-4-2 387 166 138 2-4-3 387 112 98 4-1-2 387 119 97 2—5-1 387 104 80 2-5-2 387 122 102 2-5-3 387 137 128 6 5-1-1 396 168 128 5-1-2 396 156 122 5—1-3 396 172 123 7 5-1-1 500 171 123 2-1-1 500 178 126 8 2—5-1 500 71 66 2-5-2 500 74 74 2-5-3 500 86 86 HICHI RN S III/HIM!!! mil/I Iii! III/I 406 iIIIIIM L ““ 31293104 I'll W I