I}! M ml \ l 1 ill. I ’lHl l i i I «I: CCINFCIRIWTY EN CQQPERATNE AND CQM?ETi‘.‘l\’E GRQUPS Thesis {or Hug Degree of M. A. MICHEGAN STATE UNEVERSITY Stanley A. Smith 1961 nst[mmmummmum“:WWW:nu L». 3 3 1293 10539 8261 L I B R A R Y Michigan State University MSU RETURNING MATERIALS: P1ace in book drop to mamas remove this checkout from “ your record. FINES M” be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. 'flybl kt " I 3;, H CONFORMITY IN COOPERATIVE AND COMPETITIVE GROUPS BY Stanley A . Smith AN ABSTRACT Submitted to the College of Science and Arts Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1961 Approved CFM19{61 a I Stanley A. Smith ABSTRACT This investigation reports an experiment designed to find the effects of varying set, in terms of different degrees of competition and cooperation, upon conformity. In other words, under what conditions of competition and c00peration is conformity more strongly represented? Three groups of subjects were tested on an aptitude-type test in which the experimental situation was designed to produce conformity. The three groups involved were a C00perative, Competitive and Control Group A. A fourth group (Control Group B) took the test independently. This eliminated all possibility of conformity, and made known the objective level of clarity or ambiguity (difficulty) of the test items. The major findings of this experiment are as follows: 1. The hypothesis has been supported that people in a Cooperative Group setting (i. e. working towards a common goal) will conform more than people in a Competitive Group setting (i. e. working for an individual goal). 2. People are generally competitive when placed in a group setting. Individuals in the CooPerative Group were also found to compete to some extent. Competition in the CooPerative Group was probably not directed at others, but towards some personal standard, e. g. , doing well. ~ Competition for Control Group A was evidenced by the fact that the means of the correct answers for both the Com- petitive Group and-Control Group A. were almost identical. 3. Individuals who knew an answer to a particular question gave it, while individuals who were not sure or did not know an answer to a particular question tended to go along with Stanley A. Smith the group answer. This tendency was found in all experi- mental situations (i. e. , regardless of rewards, and group atmosphere). . When subjects did not know an answer to a question, there was a greater incentive to follow the group in the'COOperative setting, and a lesser incentive to follow the group in the c ompetitive s etting . CONFORMITY IN COOPERATIVE AND COMPETITIVE GROUPS By Stanley A . Smith A THESIS Submitted to the College of Science and Arts Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1961 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The author wishes to express his sincere gratitude to the chairman of his committee, Dr.‘ Charles F. Wrigley, for his invaluable professional assistance, and generously giving of his time and effort in seeing this thesis to a speedy completion. The author is also indebted to Dr. Earl R.‘ Carlson, for his many helpful suggestions and constant encouragement, and for suggesting to the author the main hypothesis of this thesis. A debt of gratitude is owed to Mr. James Mathie, a fellow graduate student, who devised and built the apparatus and graciously loaned it to the author. . The author would also like to express his appreciation to Dr. Gerald F. King for serving as a committee member. ii Dedication To my beautiful and loving wife Carole, whose encouragement, patience and under-- standing have contributed much in seeing this endeavor through to its completion, this thesis is dedicated. - Now Jodi can get to know her father. TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER Page 1. INTRODUCTION .................... 1 Organization of the Thesis ......... . . . . 1 2. FACTORS AFFECTING THE DISPLAY OF COMPETITION AND COOPERATION ....... 5 Effects of COOperative and Competitive Sets . . . 6 Hypothesis .................. . . . 7 3. METHODOLOGY ............. . ...... 8 Experimental Changes in Instructions ....... 9 Apparatus . ...... . .............. 9 Procedure ............... . ..... 11 4. RESULTS . . . . ............ . . ..... 13 Analysis of Results ....... ’ .......... 16 5. DISCUSSION .................... . . 21 Limitations of the Study .............. 24 6. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS ............ 25 REFERENCES .......................... 27 APPENDICES .......................... 30 A - Instructions ....... . . . . ........... 31 B - Answering Positions of Subjects . . . . ....... 34 C - Frequency Distribution of Individual Scores for the Forced Critical Items ...... . ...... . . . 35 D - Test Questions . . .................. 36 E - Answer Sheet . . . .................. 40 iv 'LIST OF TABLES TABLE Page 1. Mean and Standard Deviation for the Critical Items. . 14 2. Answers Given to All Items Broken Down into Their Respective Groups of Subjects ............. 15 3. Means of Correct Answers of the Twenty Critical Items .......................... 17 4. Critical Items Ranked in Terms of Difficulty ..... 18 5. Means for the Ten Easier and the Ten Harder Questions ........................ 19 6. Answering Positions of Subjects ............ 34 7. . Frequency Distribution of Individual Scores for the Forced Critical Items ....... . ...... . . . 35 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION This investigation reports an experiment designed to find the effects upon conformity of varying subjects'set, in terms of different degrees of competition and cooperation. . The aim is to find under what conditions of competition and c00peration, conformity is the more strongly represented. ‘ Conformity is defined as the tendency of members of a group to shift in attitudes and behavior in the direction of the majority or modal or statistically most usual behavior. ‘ Set is defined as the readiness to reSpond in a certain way, due to the individual's stimulation by a variety of external and internal factors e. g. , motivation and attitude. . COOperation is defined as work performed together for a group goal. - Competition is defined as behavior intended to show oneself superior to other group members. Organization of the Thesis The present chapter concerns itself with analyzing conformity in terms of stimulus ambiguity, group goals and motivational factors. The following chapter presents the factors affecting the display of competition and cooperation. Chapter 3 presents the methodology employed in the present investigation. The questioning method, apparatus, procedure and subjects are contained therein. .Chapters 4 and 5 present the results and discussion, respectively, of this investigation. Finally, Chapter 6 summarizes the major findings. Conformity phenomena demonstrate that groups exert pressure upon their members to conform to their practices and policies. The question that arises now is: Under what circumstances is conformity more likely to occur? Cartwright and Zander (1960 p. 173) summarize the evidence by stating: . the tendency of a person to accept others' opinions is stronger where the following conditions are present. . . . (a) the quality of the evidence presented by others is convinc- ing . . . (b) the quality of the evidence being judged is unclear or involves ambiguous distinctions; (c) the discrepancy between his own Opinion and opinions of others is large (but not too large); (d) the confidence he has in the correctness of his own per— ception is low; and (e) he knows that others are aware that his opinions differ from theirs. Deutsch and Gerard (1955) agree with Asch (1956) that conformity is more likely to occur when a person is required to state his views publicly rather than privately, when a member of a group rather than a non-member, and when he is not urged by others to hold on to his own beliefs. Samelson (1956) demonstrated that subjects who were given the opportunity of explaining their cognitive conflict showed less conformity. Jackson and Saltzstein (1958) state that conformity behavior is greater where group members are interdependently working toward a common goal, rather than a situation in which there is no common goal, or the task is not relevant to group achievement. Kidd (1955) has shown that the more successful the members of the group in reaching a group goal, the greater will be the conformity. Sherif (1936), Asch (1956) and Crutchfield (1955) pioneered in devising laboratory techniques for the measurement of conformity. Sherif (1936) showed how a subject's reports of the movement of a pinpoint of light (actually stationary), could be altered by his hearing other subjects' judgments of the same autokinetic effect. Asch (1956) reports a series of experiments in which groups were given the task of comparing the lengths of successive sets of lines. All the members except one were instructed by the experimenter to reSpond erroneously. The single uninstructed member was then confronted by a situation in which all other members of the group were perceived to be in error. Two-thirds of the uninstructed subjects gave the correct response even though in a minority, and one-third of the subjects gave responses which corresponded with the erroneous reports of the unanimous majority. A main deficiency of Asch's procedure was the need for the experimenter and also his confederates to devote an hour or so to the testing of each subject. . Crutchfield (1955) devised equipment to dispense with the confed- erates, and to enable a set of five subjects to be exposed to group pressure simultaneously. The defect of his technique is the expense involved in building the electronic equipment. Mathiel devised cheaper equipment, financially within the reach of almost any experimenter, which would serve the same function as Crutchfield' s . - It is difficult to isolate all variables dependent upon conformity. The ambiguity of the stimuli, the motivations of the individual and the group task (e. g. group goals) are among the more important, and these will be considered in turn. Deutsch and Gerard (1955) in studying conformity have distinguished between normative and informational social influences. A normative social influence may be defined as an influence to conform with the positive expectations of another. An informational social influence may lUnpublished study, 1959. The apparatus is described in Chapter 3. be defined as an influence to accept information obtained from another as evidence about reality. Using normative and informational influence as a guide we can better understand the experiments that were conducted in conformity. Sherif's (1936) study falls under the heading of informational influence. The subjects in this investigation were willing to accept the judgments of others as valid. The subjects were motivated to be accurate. The stimulus was highly ambiguous, namely, that of a pinpoint of light in a pitched black room. In other words, this was clearly an unstructured situation. On the other~hand, Asch's (1956) investigation falls under the heading of normative influence. Subjects were influenced to conform with the positive expectation of other group members. The stimuli were unambiguous, namely, lengths of pairs of lines. Subjects were motivated to be accepted by other subjects as well as the experimenter. Conformity as evidenced from the work of Asch (1956) and Sherif (1936) can be produced under a variety of circumstances. Group goals and the motivations of an individual are among the more important related variables. With this in mind we can turn our attention to the motivations and group goals associated with cooperation and competition. CHAPTER 2 FACTORS AFFECTING THE DISPLAY OF COMPETITION AND COOPERATION What makes people cooperative or competitive is partly a function of individual personalities, partly a function of cultural expectencies, and partly a function of the social situation, with all three interrelated (Stendler, 1951). Philp (1940) lists three interweaving factors that determine with whom people will as a rule compete or cooperate: (a) The degree of likeness of the individuals concerned; (b) Their knowledge of each others' skills; (c) The general conditions under which competition and cooperation occur. One of her major findings was that children are more quiet when compet- ing or cooperating with strangers. . Greenberg (1932) suggests four factors affecting the display of competition in children: (1) The degree of the child's understanding of the idea of excelling; (2) The degree of the child's ability to dominate the material; (3) The educational factors in the situation; (4) The individual temperament as a competitive factor. Deutsch (1949b) states that personal needs can function only if they are relevant to the objective situation. Competing behavior involves seeing the objective situation as relevant to the personal need to win or for prestige. Only personal activities, therefore, can be satisfactory (Lewis 1944). Cooperative groups have been shownto be more highly task motivated than competitive groups (Deutsch, 1949b). . The term "promotively interdependent" is used to describe the individual in the cooperative groups. His results indicate that these interdependent subjects were more highly motivated toward the assigned group task than the less promotively interdependent members of the competitive groups. It has been hypothesized that individuals will cooperate rather than compete when cooperation is perceived as a means toward achieving shared goals (Allport, Murphy and May, 1937). Effects of Cooperative and Competitive Sets Cooperative and competitive sets could have differential effects on both the extent to which group members will accept the judgments of older members as valid, and the motivation to comply with the expecta- tions of others. The effects will tend to supplement each other in lead- ing to greater conformity in cooPerating than competing groups. Effects on the perception of group members: In the C00perative Group the goal is dependent upon the actions of others as well as him- self. The instructions specify that effective coordination amongill members is required for sucpess.‘ Since competence of others is required for success, then each group member in the cooperative setting is motivated (by induction) to achieve the group goal. Hence the member is likely, selectively to perceive the judgments of others dur— ing the course of the early task judgments and to come up with an impression of others as relatively competent and accurate. ‘ In the competitive situation, the subject is rewarded only if the others are not competent. Therefore, his need for success could lead to selective interpretation during the early judgments of others as less competent and accurate. Effects on the motivation to comply: In the Cooperative Group a member knows that he will be accepted by others if he "helps, " and rejected if he "hinders" the group. When all other group members answer in an identical fashion, the member may be hypothesized to be not only less certain of his own judgment, but also to face the likeli- hood of rejection if incorrect. ‘ If he conforms and is incorrect, on the other hand, he does not risk rejection. Thus, the COOperative Group member, as contrasted with the Competitive Group member both is more strongly motivated for acceptance, and risks less through conforming. Hypothesis The hypothesis of the present study is accordingly as follows: ' People in a cooperative group setting (i. e. where a common group goal is stressed) will conform more than people in a competitive group setting (i. e. where an individual goal is stressed). CHAPTER3 METHODOLOGY The general scheme of the experiment was to try to change the subjects' set toward cooperativeness or competitiveness by varying the instructions, and then to find the effects of this in an aptitude test situation designed to induce and measure conformity behavior. The four experimental groups were: 1. The C00perative Group. A group of .30 53, tested in sub- groups of five, was told that the group with the highest percentage of correct answers in the test situation would win a prize. The intention was to induce a cooPerative set in the SS. 2. The Competitive Group. Another group of 30 55 was told that the person with the highest percentage of correct answers would receive a prize, in the hOpe of inducing a competitive set. . 3. Control Group A. ,This group of 30 88 took the aptitude test under the same conformity conditions as the two preceding groups. ~ However, no prize was offered to these 55, i. e. , no attempt was made to induce any particular set. I 4. Control Group B. Each member of this group of 86 Ss answered the aptitude test items independently, i. e. , without exposure to the conformity-producing situation, and with no possibility of being influenced in responses by others... The purpose of testing this group was to determine the level of objective clarity or ambiguity (difficulty) of the test items. 'All 58 in the experiment were volunteer male students enrolled in psychology classes. By restricting 53 to males, it seemed that the subgroups would be more homogeneous and sex differences in con- formity would be eliminated. (For sex differences, see Beloff (1958), Crutchfield (1955), Mouton (1957), and Nakamura (1958). Experimental Changes in Instructions The oral instructions for the COOperative Group included the following paragraph: It is important that you do well as a group. I am running other groups in this experiment that are similar to this one. The group with the highest percentage of correct answers will receive a prize. The prize will consist of $5. 00, which will be divided equally among the members of the winning group. ‘It is important to remember that this is an experiment in group dynamics, and therefore, the group as a whole must do well. "It is not important how well each of you does as an individual. What is important once again is that as a group you do well. The oral instructions for the Competitive Group included the following paragraph: It is important that each of you does your very best. I am running other groups in this experiment that are similar to this one. The individual with the highest percentage of correct answers will receive a prize. The prize will consist of $5. 00.. For this reason it is to your benefit to do as well as you can because one of you might receive the sum of $5. 00 for your participation in this experiment. Apparatus The apparatus for inducing conformity was designed by Mathie. 1 It consists of five cubicles arranged on tables in a straight line.2 One subject is instructed to sit in each cubicle. Before each subject is an answer sheet, and a pack of fifty numbered index cards, with a question and five answer choices, lettered A through E, on each card. 1James P. Mathie. Unpublished study, Michigan State University, 1959. 2Reference is made to Figure 1. ___ 11:; 10 msumummfiw .H 0&9th 11 On each card a number in the upper right hand corner instructed each ‘5 upon the order in which he is to answer each question. 'A series of plywood squares with the letters A through E, one letter being on each square, is connected by hinges to the front end of the cubicle, which faces the experimenter. On another table, in front of the experimenter, but facing the SS is a rack, and twenty plywood lettered squares, viz. , four for each letter A through E. Procedure The E announces, "Question one. " The S designated to respond first for question one, i. e. , the S with a "1" on the upper right hand corner on his index card, raises the plywood lettered square which corresponds with what in his opinion is the best answer to the question. This response can be seen by E but not by other 58. E then places 83 reSponse on his rack, where all 83 can see it, and the first S to respond quietly lowers his card, so that no one but he and E know who has answered. When all 83 have answered the first question they are instructed to check their answer on the answer sheet in front of them. .Therefore, S may have showed E one letter, but have recorded a different letter on his answer sheet. 'It is not important what letter S raises. What matters is the letter which‘S indicates on his answer sheet. The remaining 88 see this 53 answer on the rack facing them, but they do not know who has answered. The S designated to answer second then raises a lettered sauare which in his Opinion is the best answer to that question. The third, fourth and fifth S follow in similar manner. ' In the instructions it has been stated that since no one follows the fifth S to answer, his response is not to be placed in the rack with the other four. - No problems were found in connection with this 12 instruction. After the four 58 responded, and saw their responses placed in the rack facing them, E waited for the fifth S to answer, and then E cleared the rack, and said, "Question two. " Each 8 remained in his answering position for a few trials. For example: The S in number one position remained there for four questions, and then switched to number four position for a further six trials. This was done so that 85 would get used to being in the same position for a period of time. The first twenty-five questions were not relevant to the conformity results since each S was in a different answering position, namely one through five. The conformity part of the experiment started with the 26th question. ' All 53 were now in the fifth or last answering position. By looking around indiscriminately, E put up four answers as before, but in this case there was no one answering in these positions. 'All four answers were identical (i. e. four A's, four B's, etc. ), but were wrong. The issue in these critical items was whether 83 would give what they considered to be the generally accepted answer, even though it was wrong. 6 ‘Items twenty-seven through thirty-five and forty-one through fifty proceeded‘in a similar manner. 'Items thirty- six through forty were neutral items in which 58 once again returned to having various answering positions. -For these twenty critical items, each S was faced with the situation in which all four other 53 appeared to have answered identically but erroneously. .Conformity was measured by the extent to which each 5 accepted these psuedo—conformist responses. CHAPTER 4 RESULTS Mean conformity scores for the four groups are given in Table l. The hypothesis that subjects with a cooperative set would conform more than those given a competitive set was confirmed. 7 Mean conformity scores were 11. 53 and 7. 30 respectively. The difference is significant at the . 01 level. ‘ Since the standard deviation for the Cooperative Group was only 4. 76, it will be seen that means differ by nearly one SD, so that a test of significance was hardly needed. ~ From Table .2 it will be seen that for every critical item the Cooperative Group conformed more than the competitive one. Control Group A had been expected to conform less than the Competitive Group, but the mean difference of . 10 was not significant, and indeed was in the opposite direction from that anticipated. Inclusion of the instructions ”to do your very best" does not seem to have made any difference. In other words, the uninstructed S seems to approach the task with much the same attitude as the one Specifically encouraged to be competitive. A third comparison of interest is that between the two control groups. ' In Control Group B, with no pressure to conform, there was a mean score of 2. 60 in the ”conformity reSponses“ to the 20 critical items. These represent guesses and random item responses, or items in which S believes the error response selected by other groups for conformity to be the right one. ' In Control Group A, the mean score is 7.40. This highly significant difference represents the effect of the confo rmity situation. 13 14 Table 1--Mean and Standard Deviation for the Critical Items CODtI'Ol Control COOperative Competitive Group A Group B Group Group Mean 7.40 2.60 11.53 7.30 Standard Deviation 3 . 70 l. 71 4. 76 3. 75 >'.< The t—values are: 3. 81 (Cooperative Group vs.- Competitive Group); 3. 72 (Cooperative. Group vs. Control Group A); 0.10 (Competitive Group vs. Control Group A); 6. 76 (Control Group A vs. Control Group B). A t-value of 2. 58 is significant at . 01 level. 15 ON MN we m m . ON N H >N O O wN om MN MO OH OH MN N. N OH OH 3 3 m we Om ON w MN m O OH O eN OH > h we wO 2 N. wN w O N n 2 S O e we HM me OH M Ma NH M N. ON O MN O Ow Om MN N. S e O : M OH OH O 5 me OM OM m: ON e ON m H wN e N wN we ON MM MN 0 O MN O O m; w m ea Me OM ew M MN NH m w 2 : OH OH O Ne MN. m O OH H MN OH O ON ON M NH He MN Om N. M .2 NH O : OH O O MN mM ON MO N O OH O O eN OH OH OH e eM mM HM ON NH N. 3 w m HN O M Ma MM be ON OH NH O OH e M MN NH O OH NM MN mm O O ON w H ON : M HN O HM NN mO O N. ON N. N ON MN O OH OH OM ON mm. 2 e 5 : H OH wN N OH O ON me NM e O 3 MN m e 3 MN m NH ON OH HO OH e NH wN N MN OH > ea 0 hN OH em NN w o NL H w HN N Na ON ON mo>flm SoounoO poouoh mo>flm poohpoO, eoouoh oozed “09:00 Ooopoh mo>flm goonnoU Ooonoh 1:33. Lemma?» ushofieu nanoujw nosuO quOO .350 .3330 m 95.20 Noun—GOO QSOHO o>SEoQEOO neponO o>flmuomooO < QSOHO HonusoO muooflnrdm mo mmdouu o>fioommom Sosa. oi: GBOQ GoMOHM maofi Sea 3 anti—U whoa/95w..- N 03mm. 16 These results are corroborated by consideration of the correct responses. Table 3 gives means for correct answers for the 20 critical items. Control Group B, answering without pressure to conform, had a mean score of 9.47 correct responses. This score fell to 6. 73 and 6. 33 for the Competitive Group and Control Group A (a non-significant difference) and to 5. 26 for the Cooperative Group. This decline in correctness of response is attributable to conformity pressure. ‘ Answers given by Control Group B may be assumed to indicate the difficulty of the items under "neutral" conditions. 'In Table .4 the critical items are ranked in terms of difficulty. Table 5 gives means for the ten easier and the ten harder questions. Since all t's were significant at the . 01 level this confirmed the expecta- tion that people who knew an answer gave it whereas those who did not know an answer went along with the group answer for all experimental groups. In other words, forcing was much more effective with the difficult items (as established by Control Group B) than with the easier items. 1 Analysis of Results 1. The Cooperative Group provided slightly fewer correct answers than either the Competitive Group or Control Group A. The big difference seems to have been with the answers not known for in this instance there was a greater incentive to follow the group. 2. The Competitive Group showed less incentive to follow the group when the answers were not known. 3. Control Group A, like the Competitive Group showed no observ- able difference in regard to being influenced by the group. Evidently the addition of prize money made no difference. This reduced incentive 1Reference is made to Table 4. 17 Table 3 --Means of Correct Answers of the Twenty Critical Items Cooperative Group 5. 26 Control Group A 6. 33 Competitive Group 6. 73 Control Group B 9. 47 18 Table 4-—CriticaI Items Ranked in Terms of Difficulty Item Control Cooperative Competitive *2: Control Group A Ranks Number GroupB Forced Correct Forced Correct Forced Correct l. 1. OOWKIO‘U'Ii-hwmm 34 30 27 29 31 35 26 49 46 42 44 28 43 33 32 48 45 47 50 41 65 65 61 59 58 56 54 53 45 44 38 37 35 31 29 26 23 15 13 8 10 13 16 14 11 19 2.]. 18 20 ll 24 18 19 21 23 14 16 ll 27 20 14 15 13 15 18 ll 8 10 p—a o—oNI OHNWO‘WU‘O‘IPH 6 7 14 ll 4 12 17 7 12 5 16 13 15 ll 10 9 9 8 20 13 15 16 12 15 20 15 r—tU'ICDi-bmmxlO‘HhF y—a i—Ii—t UixomO‘UJO‘OOOO-F 16 15 14 19 21 9 12 ll 13 10 woxom'slmme'IN 19 Table 5--Means for the Ten-Easier and the TenHarder Questions Mean of Mean of Easier 10 _Harder 10 * Questions Questions t Values Cooperative Group 4. 06 1. 20 5. 54 Control Group A 4. 66 l . 66 5. 82 Competitive Group 4. 66 2. O6 5. 54 Control Group B 6. 51 2. 96 :9: A t-value of 2. 58 is significant at . 01 level. 20 of the Competitive Group was more characteristic of subjects without set than the increased incentive of the Cooperative Group. 4. Control Group B answered more of the items correct than any other group, because they allowed themselves to be forced only 2. 6 of the choices as compared with Control Group A, and the Cooperative Group who allowed themselves to be forced 7.4 and 11. 53 respectively. CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION The results demonstrate that people placed in a situation believed to encourage c00peration conform more than those who believe them- selves to be in a competitive situation. Since the mean of conformity for the Competitive Group was almost identical with that for Control Group A (where no prize was involved although the experimental situation was identical), two explanations are possible. One, is that people tend to compete with one another when placed in a group atmosphere, even when not specifically instructed to do so. Another explanation is that E did not induce a competitive set. The first explanation appears more likely for the following reason. Competition may involve competing against others or competing against some personal standard (e. g. doing well). ‘In other words, the Ss in Control Group A might not have actually competed against each other, but rather due to a personal standard tried to do as well as they possibly could. This would explain the fact that regardless of the experimental group an individual is in, if he knows an answer to a particular question he is likely to record it as such on his answer sheet. If he does not know the answer, then he will go along with the group answer. It would seem logical for an individual in the C00perative Group who knows a correct answer to record it than to follow his group in recording a wrong answer, which will obviously hinder rather than help the group. ‘ In other words, putting down a wrong answer will reduce the percentage of right answers. Yet, of the 30 subjects in the C00perative Group hardly anyone seems to have approached the situation from such a logical point of view. These subjects could not give E an explanation 21 22 for their conforming, especially when conformity in this case (to an answer they knew was wrong) would hinder the group. By the same token, it can be said that if most people thought of this possibility, there might well have been no significant difference. In the competitive situation if an individual does not know an answer, his taking the group answer which he believes four other individuals have agreed upon would not give any one individual an edge on that particu- lar question. They would either all get it right, or all get it wrong. However, if an individual not sure of an answer dissented from the group answer he might be right, the group might be right or neither he nor the group .might be right. Therefore, he has a greater incentive to gamble in this situation by selecting his own answer, which will place him at an advantage over his competitors if it happens to be the right one. Since the mean of correct answers for the Cooperative Group was quite close to the mean of either the Competitive Group or Control Group A (no prize involved), it is reasonable to conclude that subjects who knew an answer gave it, rather than succumbed to the group answer they knew was wrong. That is to say, subjects in the Cooperative Group were competing with each other to a certain degree. Since the mean for the correct answers for the Competitive Group and Control Group A were similar (M = 6. 73 and M = 6. 33 respectively), these two groups both adapted a relatively competitive set. ‘In other words, when placed in a group situation individuals tend to try to do well. This analogy also holds true for the C00perative Group, but to a lesser degree, since the mean of 5. 26 is not as close to the mean of 6. 73 as the mean of 6. 33 is for Control Group A. This point of view was also presented by Coleman, _e_t _a_1. , (1958), and many other researchers. Their contention is that an individual will be better able to resist conformity pressure from the group if he is certain of the correct answer. External information (looking at other 23 people's answers) will become important only when the individual is unfamiliar with the correct answer. ‘ If an individual is certain of the correct answer, he will be better able to respond in terms of internal cues (aptitudes, motivations, etc.) rather than rely on external infor- mation such as c0pying someone elses answer in the face of uncertainty. During the course of the experiment it was disclosed to E that sometimes people knew the right answer to a particular question, but nevertheless, went along with the group answer even though they knew the group was wrong. This phenomenon occurred in all groups except Control Group B who took the test independently. This is one explanation why Control Group B answered correctly more of the critical items than the other three groups. The Ss inControl Group B were comparable to the Ss in the other groups, so this would discount the fact that 53 in Control Group B were of a different population. This finding seems to contradict an earlier assertion that correct answers will be recorded correctly in any group. However, the finding that when S knew an answer he gave it, and when S did not know the answer he tended to follow the group answer is supported by the results. Therefore, the verbal reports of 85 must not be taken as a fact that can be statistically verified. -In other words, what 58 said they did was not corroborated with the results of what they actually did. ‘ A discussion of the time element involved in the testing situation is now in order. , Control Group B handed their papers in when they were finished. - No time limit was enforced. Most 85 in this group took the test home and handed it to E the following day. Control Group A, the Cooperative Group and the Competitive Group also had ample time, with no limit enforced, for taking the test in the experimental situation. .For the first 25 items the Ss in these three groups answered the items at their own speed. . For the remaining critical items E used as a guide the time it took for that particular group 24 to answer questions 1 through 25. For example: If a given group took half an hour to answer questions 1 through 25, E allowed at least another half hour to answer the remaining 25 questions. ‘ Indeed, since the re- maining 25 questions were more difficult, E usually allowed an extra ten to fifteen minutes beyond that. Upon questioning the 53 after the experiment, hardly anyone reported having been rushed. The average time for a group of five 85 taking the test in the experimental situation was an hour and fifteen minutes, with a range from fifty minutes to an hour and a half. Limitations of the Study The statistical testing indicates that the results can be generalized to other comparable groups with relatively small risk. However, the results can not be generalized to non-comparable groups. Another limitation of the study involves the stimulus material. The degree of ambiguity of the test items must be considered in regard to generalization. Different questions involving different degrees of ambiguity might give an investigator quite different results. CHAPTER 6 SUMMAR Y AND CONC LU SIONS Three groups of 30 subjects each were tested on an aptitude—like test in an experimental situation intended to produce conformity behavior. It was assumed that a person's set could be changed by varying the instructions by so doing inducing in the subject a Spirit of either co- operativeness or competitiveness. The ‘three experimental groups were therefore (a) the C00perative Group, (b) the Competitive Group, and (c) Control Group A (tested in the conformity situation with no Specific set induced). A fourth group Control Group B, consisted of 86 subjects. The major findings of the thesis are as follows: 1. The hypothesis has been supported that peeple in a Cooperative Group setting (i. e. working towards a common goal) will conform more than people in a Competitive Group setting (i. e. working for an individual goal). 2. PeOple are generally competitive when placed in a group setting. It was found that individuals in the Cooperative Group were competing to some extent. Competition for the Cooperative Group was probably not directed to others, but directed to a personal standard, e. g. , doing well. , Competition for Control Group A was evidenced by the fact that the means of the correct answers for both the Competitive Group and Control Group A were almost identical. 3. Individuals who knew an answer to a particular question gave it, while individuals who were not sure or did not know an answer to a particular question went along with the group answer. This was true regardless of the experimental situation the individual was in (i. e. , regardless of rewards, instructions and group atmosphere). 25 26 4. When 55 did not know an answer to a question there was a. greater incentive to follow the group in the c00perative setting, and a lesser incentive to follow the group in the competitive setting. REFERENCES Allport, G., Murphy, G., 81 May, M. Memorandum on-research in competition and €00peration. New York: Social Science Research Council, 1937. Asch, S. ~Studies of independence and conformity: l. A minority of one against aunanimous majority. Psychol. Monogr., 1956, 70, No. 9 (Whole No. 416). Bass, B. Leadership, psychology and organizational behavior. ~New York: Harper and Brothers, 1960. 'Beloff, H. Two forms of social conformity: Acquiescence and Conventionality. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1958, 56, 99-104. Cartwright, D., 81 Zander, A. Group dynamics. (2nd ed.) Evanston, Illinois: Row, Peterson and Co., 1960. Coleman, Janet F., Blake, R., & Mouton, J. Task difficulty and conformity pressures. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1958, 57, 120-122. Crutchfield, R. S. Conformity and character. 'Amer. Psychologist, 1955, 10, 191-198. De Charms, R. Affiliation motivation and productivity in small groups. J. abnorm. soc.-Psychol., 1957, 55, 222-226. Deutsch, , M. A theory of co-operation and competition. Hum. -Relat. , 1949, 2, 129-152 (a). ~ Deutsch, M. An experimental study of the effect of cooperation and competition upon group process. Hum. -Relat. , 1949, 2, 199-231 (b). Deutsch, M. , 81 Gerard, H. -A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgment. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol. , 1955, 51, 629-636. Greenberg,- P. J. .Competition in children: an experimental study. Amer. J. Psychol., 1932, 44, 221-248. 27 28 Grossack, M. M. Some effects of c00peration and competition upon small group behavior. J. abnorm. soc.'Psychol., 1954, 49, 341-348. Hardy, K. R. The influence of affiliative motivation and social support upon conformity and attitude change. — Dissertation Abstracts, 1954, 14, 1106-1107. Jackson, J. M., 81 Saltzstein, H. D. The effect of person-group relationships on conformity processes. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1958, 57, 17-24. Kidd, J. S. , 81 Campbell, D. .Conformity to groups as a function of group success. J. abnorm. soc.»Psychol., 1955, 51, 390-393. Lewis, H. B. An experimental study of the role of ego in work: I. The role of ego in cooperative work. J. exp.“ Psychol. , 1944, 34, 113-126. Maller, J. B. Cooperation and competition: An experimental study in motivation. New York: Teachers Coll. , Columbia Univer. , No. 384, 1929. Mizuhara, Taisuke 81 Tamai, Syusuke. -Experimental studies of c00peration and competition. Jap. J.- Psychol. , 1952, 22, 124-127. (Psychol. Abstr., 27:p. 519) Mouton, J. Psychological aspects of conformity behavior of men and women. Dissertation Abstracts, 1957, 17, 2697-2698. Nakamura, C. Y. Conformity and problem solving. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1958, 56, 315-320. Phillips, B. N., 81 D'Amico, L. A. Effects of cooperation and competition on the cohesiveness of small fac‘e-to-face groups. J. educ. Psychol., 1956, 47, 65-70. Philp, Alice. Strangers and friends as competitors and c00perators. J. genet.-Psychol., 1940, 57, 249-258. Samelson,. F. Group pressure and incongruity in the cognitive field as determinants of conformity., Dissertation Abstracts, 1956, 16, 2541-2542. 29 Sherif, M. The psychology of social norms. New York: Harper, 1936. Stendler, Celia, Damrin, Dora and Haines, Aleyne. Studies in co- operation and competition: I. The effects of working for group and individual rewards on the social climate of children's groups. J. genet. Psychol., 1951, 79, 173-197. Stogdill, R. M. Individual behavior and group achievement. New York: Oxford University, 1959. Whittemore, I. C. The influence of competition on performance: An experimental study. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1924, 19, 236-253. APPENDICES 30 APPENDIX A Instructions This is an experiment in group dynamics. Each of you has a set of 50 index cards, each card having one question and five possible answers. The possible answers are lettered A through E. ‘1 will first read out the number of the question. The question number is found at the bottom of the index card. In the upper right hand corner you will find a number indicating the order in which you are to answer that particular question. - At the front of your box you will find the letters A through E to correspond with the possible answers. After I have read the question number, the person with the number one in the upper right hand corner of his index card will quietly lift the letter in front of him which corresponds to the letter of his answer. This is done to show me what his answer is. 1 will then put his answer up in this rack. - After I have put his answer in the rack he will quietly lower his letter. Then the person with number two will answer and I will put his answer in the rack. Then number three, four and five will respond in the same manner. ~ Since there is no one more to answer after the fifth person, I will not place his answer on the rack. You must be very quiet when you raise and lower your letter so that no one will know who has answered. You will know the answers of those who answered before you, but will not know who gave the answer. Similarly, others will know what you answered but will not know that it was you who gave the answer. - After all five‘people have answered I will clear this rack. -At this time you will record your answer on the answer sheet in front of you by placing an X after the letter correSponding to your answer. 31 32 Please do not try to discover who is giving whichenswerand please do not talk during the experiment. Your answers will be kept strictly confidential. No one will see your answer sheets. - If a question arises during the experiment, please raise “your hand and'I will come to you. Do not mark up or do any figuring on the question cards. * It is important that you do well as a group. I am running other groups in this experiment that are similar to this one. The group with the highest percentage of correct answers will receive a prize. The prize will consist of $5. 00, which will be divided equally among the members of the winning group. It is important to remember that this is an experiment in group dynamics, and, therefore, the group as a whole must do well. It is not important how well each of you doesfi‘as an individual. What is important once again is that as a group you do well. . Are there any questions ? The only change of instructions for the Competitive Group is found in the last paragraph, marked by an asterisk. - All other instructions remain identical. * It is important that each of you does your very best. I am running other groups in this experiment that are similar to this one. The individual with the highest percentage of correct answers will receive a prize. The prize will consist of $5. 00. . For this reason it is to your benefit to do as well as you can because one of you might receive the sum of $5. 00 for your participation in this experiment. - Are there any questions 7 The instructions for Control Group A were identical to the instruc- tions for the Cooperative and the Competitive Groups except for the last paragraph. . Neither paragraph as marked by an asterisk was included in the instructions for Control Group A, since no prize was involved for 33 this group. The last few sentences of instruction for Control Group‘A are as follows: Please do not try to discover who is giving which answer and please do not talk during the experiment. Your answers will be kept strictly confidential. No one will see your answer sheets. 'If a question arises during the experiment, please raise your hand] and I will come to you. VDo not mark up or do any figuring on the question cards. Are there any questions? No formalized set of instructions was given to Control Group B. ~ Subjects were instructed to answer the questions given to them on mimeographed Sheets as best they could. They were told that the ques- tions were previously used in an experiment, and it was their task to answer them truthfully and independently, so that a level of difficulty could be ascertained from their answers to the questions. APPENDIX B Table 6 --Answering Positions of Subjects w Question Numbers 1-5 6-10 11-15 16-20 21-25 26-30 31-35 36-40 41-45 46-50 1 5 3 3 l l 5 5 4 5 5 2 3 5 2 2 2 5 5 5 5 5 Subjects 3 2 2 5 4 4 5 5 2 5 5 4 4 4 4 5 3 5 5 1 5 5 5 1 1 1 3 5 5 5 3 5 5 Table 6 shows the number of times each of the five subjects remained in their reSpective answering positions. . The question numbers are found at the top of the table. The numbers 1 through 5 representing the five subjects are found at the left of the table. For questions 26 through 35, and questions 41 through 50 all subjects were in the fifth or last answering position. - All other positions were distributed randomly. 34 APPENDIX C Table 7 -- Frequency Distribution of Individual Scores for the Forced Critical Items __‘ __- Competitive . Cooperative . Control ‘ Control Group Group Group A Group B A 20 0 l 0 0 19 O 3 O 0 18 0 0 O 0 17 0 l O 0 l6 1 1 l 0 15 0 l 1 0 l4 0 2 l 0 13 2 4 O O 12 O 1 O 0 ll 4 4 2 0 10 2 4 4 0 9 2 1 2 0 8 2 1 3 0 7 4 1 4 0 6 5 1 0 5 5 1 2 3 l7 4 2 1 6 ll 3 l 0 1 24 2 2 0 l 18 1 l 1 1 12 O l 0 0 9 N=30 N=30 N=30 N=86 35 D-' 10. 11 12. 13. 14. APPENDIX D . 2isto4as4isto (A) 8 (B) 10 (C) 12 (D) 4 (E) 5 .. Cub is to bear as calf is to (A) leg (B) horse (C) cow (D) panda (E) deer . Tall is to short as die is to (A) dye (B) cast (C) live (D) expire (E) grow .. Leaf is to tree as hand is to (A) body (B) foot (C) handy (D) finger (E) give . Core is to Corps as sent is to (A) send (B) cent (C) sent (D) transmitted (E) sents .7 Book is to read as radio is to (A) Television (B) See (C) Listen (D) Marconi (E) Music . 1-3-5-7 is to 11-13-15-17 as 2-4-6-8 is to (A) 12.-13-14-15 (B) 10-12-14-16 (C) 8-6-4-2 (D) infinity (E) 12-10-9-7 . Cow is to cows as deer is to (A) deer (B) deers (C) deeres (D) bucks (E) herd . 1760 yards is to one mile as 220 yards is to one (A) quadrant (B) kilometer (C) fathom (D) furlong (E) octroi Five is to 4 as two is to (A) 6 (B) 8 (C) 4 (D) 3 (E) 7 .. Past is to "ed" as present is to (A) "in n (B) "tion" (C) "ex" (D) n h" (E) future ______g_ g Tibia is to Fibula as Radius is to (A) Ulna- (B) Femur (C) Ischium (D) Ilium (E) Tarsus Quarter is to football as Chukker is to (A) LaCrosse (B) Soccer (C) Rugby (D) Cricket (E) Polo One yard is to 36 inches as one fathom is to (A) 220 yards (B) one cubic rod (C) 16 feet (D) 6 feet (E) 100 centimeters . Horse is to sheep as mare is to (A) lamb (B) ewe (C) stallion (D) horse (E) sheep 36 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 37 Before is to Obstetrician as after is to (A) Pediatrician (B) Gynecologist (C) Osteopath (D) Ophthalmologist (E) Ortho dont1 s t 96 and 36 are to 12 as 84 and 7 are to (A) 2 (B) 6 (C) 3 _(_____D) 7 (E) 8 A robin is to a wren as a minnow is to a (A) Porpoise (B) Whale (C) Duckbill Platypus (D) Seal (E) Trout Flammable is to nonflammable as inflammable is to (A) fire (B) inoperative (C) nonflammable (D) incorrect (E) flammable 81 is to 9 is to 3 as 256 is to 16 is to (A) 8 (B) 4 (C) 2 (D) 6 (E) no answer Short is to longest as meek is to (A) mild (B) gentle (C) submissive (D) kind (E) modest Presley is to Rock and Roll as Goodman is to (A) Swing (B) Dixieland (C) Classical (D) Bop (E) Jazz .‘ Anode is to cathode as yes is to (A) certainly (B) doing (C) completed (D) stop (E) no Homo is to Latin as Anthropos is to (A) Spanish (B) Latin (C) Greek (D) German (E) Italian .HCl is to Hydrochloric acid as H2504 is to (A) Sulphuric acid (B) Hydrogen peroxide (C) Ether (D) Fire (E) Ammonia Washington D. C. is to the United States as Ankara is to (A) Chile (B) Iran (C) Turkey (D) Russia (E) Greenlandi< ‘A is to Z as alpha is to (A) epsilon (B) omicron (C) chi (D) pi* (E) omega Mark Twain is to Samuel Clemens as Saki is to (A) George Sands * (B) Ernest Hemingway (C) Joseph Conrad (D) Andre Malreaux (E) H. H. Munro One meter is to 39. 37 inches as one inch is to centimeter(s) (A) l (B) 5. 24 (C) 4. 52 (D) 10 ’5‘ (E) 2. 54 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 38 Distance in walking is to pedometer as density of milk is to (A) anemometer * (B) manometer (C) drosometer' (D) dosimeter * (E) lactometer Tack is to _ . _._. _._ as cat is to (A) ._._ _ __ (B) _._ ._ ._ (C) _._ _. _. * (D) _._. ._ __ (E) ._ _ __ Conceit is to egotism as Nationalism is to (A) Solecism ~ (B) Synchronism (C) Aphorism * (D) Chauvinism (E) Atavism 9-12-4 is to 25-30-6 as 9-6-18 is to (A) 25-20-100 (B) 9-5-20 (C) 25-22-66 (D) 18-12-36 x< (E) 6-3-15 Geologist is to rock as Ornithologist is to (A) Religion (B) Decoration (C) Bird (D) Star (E) Water* Pork Chop is to hill as Mannerheim is to (A) Line (B) Hill* (C) War (D) Canada (E) Win Pride is to Prejudice as Sense is to (A) Sensibility (B) Action (C) Knowledge (D) Ignorance (E) Hate Dunn is to Bradstreet as Merrill, Lynch, . Pierce, Fenner is to (A) Smith (B) Clark (C) Jones (D) Fox (E) Balser 6 a.m. is to 6 p.m. as betimes is to (A) anon (BJ vespertinal (C) matutinal (D) laconic (E) now 2 is to 4-2-4-2 as 3 is to (A) 9—3-6-3 (B) 6-3-9-3 (C) 3-9-6-3 (D) 6-3-3-9 (E) 3-6-6-3 -Abraham. Lincoln is to the "Railsplitter" as Martin Van Buren is to (A) The Kinderhook Fox (B) Old Man Eloquent (C) Squire of Hyde Park (D) Old Tip (E) Old Rough and Ready "Aida" is to "Sylvia" as "Burleske" is to (A) "C0ppelia" (B) ”Carnaval" (C) “Samson et Delila" (D) "Sinfonietta Giocosa" (E) "Idomeneo" * 5-7-9-2 is to 7-8-9-1 as 2-5-8-3 is to (A) 2-6-8-4 (B) 1-2-4-1 (C) 1-5-9-4 (D) 3-6-9-2 (E) 2-2-4-8 9.: 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 39 The Atlantic is to the Pacific as New Guinea is to - (A) Cyprus (B) Corsica (C) Cuba * (D) Greenland (E) Borneo Jocose is to melancholy as evergreen is to (A) carnivorous (B) perennial (C) deciduous (D) herbivorous (E) tree * ABC is to 2 as JKL is to (A) 5 (B) 4 (C) 6 * (D) 3 (E) none of the above s-b-l-i-b-r-q is to tangent as a-f-j-v is to (A) can't (B) belt (C) face (D) bomb * (E) bask .2-7-4-5-6-3 is to 3-6-5-4-7-2 as 4-7-6-3-8-1 is to (A) 3-6-5-8-1-4 (B) 1-3-6-5-7-6 (C) 4-7-6-3-8-1 (D) 3-1-5—7-2-2 >l< (E) 2-3-4-5-6-4 Jack of clubs is to 8 of diamonds as 7 of Spades is to (A) 4 of diamonds (B) 4 of Spades * (C) 2 of clubs (D) ace of clubs (E) 8 of hearts Appetizer is to after dinner drink as antipastois to (A) Hors D'Oeuvres * (B) Souffle (C) Demitasse (D) Suzette (E) Croutons 6-9-5-7-2 is to 6-11-9-13-10 as 4-7-8-2-6 is to (A) 4-6-8-3-5 (B) 5-8-9-3-7 (C) unsolvable * (D) 4-1—7-4-3 (E) 4-0-6-5-2 Legend: Underlined is the correct answer. * is the forced answer for the twenty critical items. D (1 if C APPENDIX E 26 E U it 1) 2'7 28 29 31 32 33 34 36 3’7 38 35 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 4'7 48 49 50 40 11‘11‘ J 12 14 15 16 1'7 18 19 2o 21 22 23 24 "I111111111)(