PERCEWED PARENTAL REJECTION AND SCHOLASTIC ACHIEVEMENT Thesis for The Degree of M. A. TMICHI‘GAN STATE UNIVERSITY Jane-t Landis Summers 1962 i; L I B R A R Y Michigan State University IIIIIII TATE RRRRRRRRRRRRRRRR IVTE;I_J RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to LIBRARIES remove this checkout from “ your record. FI__N__ES W1” be charged if book is returned after the date 2 2 CW aw stamped below. ABSTRACT PERCEIVED PARENTAL REJECTION AND SCHOLASTIC ACHIEVEMENT by Janet Landis Summers This study analyzed the relationship between perceived parental rejection and scholastic achievement within a theoretical framework of deprivation and compensation. Data obtained by the administration of paper—pencil questionnaires to a sample of 293 undergraduates enrolled in basic social science courses at a large state university were analyzed to test three major hypotheses: 1. There will be a low positive relationship between certain of the background items—~namely, socio— economic status, religion, parental valuation of education--and scholastic achievement. There will be an inverselassociation between perceived parental acceptance and scholastic achievement. The inverse relationship between perceived paren— tal acceptance and scholastic achievement will be more definite when sooio—economic level is controlled, and will be strongest in the high socio—economic group. Janet Landis Summers The findings yielded support for the first hypothesis; in addition, a low positive association resulted between sex and achievement with females being the higher achievers, and a positive relationship between year in school and achieve- ment with freshmen being the higher achievers. Hypothesis two was refuted: there was a tendency for perceived parental acceptance to relate to high scholastic achievement, and for perceived parental rejection to relate to low achievement. These tendencies were stronger for mother acceptance than for father, and were especially strong among females. In light of the findings in relation to hypothesis two, the third hypothesis was inapplicable. Perceived parental accep_ tance and scholastic achievement were unrelated in the high socio_economic group; in the low socio-economio group there was a low positive association between the two variables. The findings were analyzed from a deprivation and compen» sation framework. It was concluded that a large state- supported university, such as that used for this study, is not the type of institution which would tend to draw a pre~ ponderance of academically oriented students~mstudents whose orientation is other than academic would tend to compensate for feelings of parental rejection by seeking gratification in areas other than high scholastic achievement. At a school with a high proportion of academically oriented students, it is expected that perceived parental rejection would be related to high scholastic achievement. The results from the present sample seem to suggest that perhaps one factor in adequate per- formance is a feeling of acceptance in the family of orien- tation. PERCEIVED PARENTAL REJECTION AND SCHOLASTIC ACHIEVEMENT By Janet Landis Summers A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Sociology and Anthropology 1962 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to express my indebtedness to my husband, Worth, and to my parents, Judson and Mary Landis, for their encourage— ment and moral support throughout the various stages of my graduate training. I want to thank my brother, Judson R. Landis, for suggesting the topic of this study. I want also to indicate my gratitude to Dr. Melvin Segal, Dr. Golda Ross, Dr. Orden Smucker, Dr. Ruth Useem, and Dr. Arthur Vener for their assistance and COOperation in the gathering of data for the study. Finally, I wish to thank the members of my guidance committee, Dr. Wilbur Erookcver, Dr. Charles Ramsey, and eSpecially Dr. David Gottlieb who, as thesis advisor, has given generously of his time and advice. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. Chapter I. INTRODUCTION TO THE PROBLEM. II. METHODOLOGY III. FINDINGS: BACKGROUND ITEMS AND SCHOLASTIC ACHIEVEMENT . . . . . . IV. FINDINGS: INTEGRATION WITH PARENTS AND SCHOLASTIC ACHIEVEMENT. V. FINDINGS: SOCIO-ECONOMIO LEVEL, INTEGRATION WITH PARENTS AND SCHOLASTIC ACHIEVEMENT VI. SUMMARY, INTERPRETATICNS_ AND CONCLUSIONS REFERENCES APPENDIXES APPENDIX A APPENDIX B APPENDIX C iii Page ii Table 10. ll. 12. 13. 14. LIST OF TABLES Marginals for Integration with Parents Categories Obtained by Method 1 and by Method 2 . . . . . . . . . Pattern of Childhood- Adolescent Happiness and Integration with Parents Socio-Economic Status and Scholastic Achievement Socio-Economic Status and Scholastic Achieve— ment for Males and Females Religious Preference and Scholastic Achievement Parental Valuation of Education, and Scholastic Achievement Scholastic Achievement for Males and Females Year in School and Scholastic Achievement Integration with Parents and Scholastic Achievement Integration with Parents and Scholastic Achieve— ment for Freshmen and SOphomores Integration with Parents and Scholastic Achieve— ment for Males and Females Patterns of High Integration with Father and with Mother for Males and Females . Father Integration in Relation to Mother Integration . . . . . . . Integration with Father and High Achievement, Integration with Mother and High Achievement, for Males and Females . . . . . . iv Page 23 26 32 33 all Al 43 AA 146 1+7 48 Table Page 15. Pattern of Childhood-Adolescent Happiness and Scholastic Achievement. . . . . . . . 52 16. Socio-Economic Status and Integration with . Father for Males and Females., . . . . . 56 17. Socio-Economic Status and Integration with Mother for Males and Females. . . . . . 58 18. Integration with Parents and Scholastic Achievement by Socio-Economic Level . . . 60 19. Socio-Economic Status and Parental Valuation of Education . . . . . . . . . . . 62 20. Integration with Father for Males and Females. 83 21. Integration with Mother for Males and Females. 83 22. Integration with Father and Scholastic . Achievement for Males and Females . . . . 8A 23. Integration with Mother and Scholastic Achievement for Males and Females . . . . 8A LIST OF FIGURES Figure l. Nye Scale of Perceived Parental Rejection with Response Values . . . . . 2. Integration with Parents Scores Yielded by Methods 1 and 2.. . . . . 3. Breakdown of Integration and Achievement Variables versus Combined Variables vi 71 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION TO THE PROBLEM The purpose Of this study is to examine certain rela- tionships which may exist between perceived parental rejec— tion and scholastic achievement as measured by expressed grade point average. Our theoretical framework is that Of deprivation and compensation: when an individual is deprived in one area he will seek out gratification in another area. The key point is that unsatisfactory interpersonal relations in child- hood produce insecurity and anxiety which are compensated for by a quest for power, recognition, and success. The expected institution for the initial satisfaction of the individual's needs for acceptance and recognition is the family of orientation. Because this is the initial group to which one relates himself, a lack Of a feeling Of security and acceptance within this group is an especially vital factor in the development Of the individual. The need for acceptance and recognition is developed in children to the extent that they feel anxiety when they sense that there is a lack Of love and acceptance from the family. The individual who feels unu accepted will strive to overcome his anxiety, will strive to 2 be accepted and recognized elsewhere. Horney,l in particular, has suggested that the quest for power is frequently used as a compensatory means Of attaining reassurance against the anxieties produced by unhappy childhood experiences. Because Of the widespread acceptance of high aspiration and high achievement as values in American society today, becoming a high achiever (at least in some socially accepted area) is one legitimate means to Obtaining gratification and of compensating for the emotional deprivation felt in the family. It is recognized that there are avenues to gaining acceptance other than that Of high academic achievement, how- ever, our concern is only with the latter. Further, we are saying nothing about how much this compensatory seeking Of gratification is internalized or recognized by the individual, rather, we are only interested in the external manifestations of deprivation in interpersonal relations leading to a seeking out of gratification in other areas. Several investigations have been conducted which bear on some of the assumptions of this operational framework. Lasswell hypothesized that power is pursued as a means of compensation against deprivation-—"power is expected to over— come low estimates of the self, by changing either the traits Of the self or the environment in which it functions."2 He 1Karen Horney, The Neurotic Personality Of Our Time (New York: W. W. Norton and Company, 1937), pp. 162-187. 2Harold D. Lasswell, Power and Personality (New York: W. W. Norton and Company, 1937, p. 39. 3 points out that the self includes symbols of identification, so in this sense grades in school would be included as a part of the self. His sub-hypothesis is that power is resorted to when it is expected to contribute more than any alterna- tive value to overcoming or obviating deprivations of the self. He expects this to be especially the case in middle—class homes--"we know that middle-class homes are hothouses Of ambition, holding their children to high standards Of achieve— ment, thus providing the tension between indulgence and deprivation so congenial to the accentuation of power."3 His major hypothesis is supported. The intensive study Of infancy and childhood . . . has underlined the decisive importance Of the early years in shaping the structure Of personality. . . . The data go in the direction toward which we have been pointing. The accentuation of power is to be under- stood as a compensatory reaction against low estimates Of the self. . . . At the same time adverse estimates Of the self must not be overwhelming, or the resort to power will Be blocked by sentiments Of utter hopeless- ness. . . . Kausler and Trapp, on the other hand, found results which conflict with those of Lasswell. They tested the null hypothesis that there is no relationship between level Of achievement motivation and level Of manifest anxiety drive. The null hypothesis was rejected--”the results clearly reflect a significantly negative relationship between 3Ibid., p. 47. 4Ibid.,pp. 52—53. A the two sets of scores. . . ."5 That is, high manifest anxiety drive was related to low achievement motivation, and low manifest anxiety, to high achievement motivation. Further, in line with the assumption that excessive ambition is a way of compensating for a low level of self- esteem, that power is sought as insurance against an under- lying feeling of worthlessness, Cohen6 investigated goal~ level setting within a level-Of—aspiration framework by studying feelings of adequacy and self—acceptance. He found that both very high goal setting and very low goal setting were related to self-rejection, to a basic feeling of in- 7 security. 8 Along this same line, Wylie reviews numerous studies on level of self—esteem and achievement, some Of which have conflicting results. Some investigators reported no relation~ ship between self-esteem and academic achievement; others found none between self-regard and n Achievement (i.e. achieve— ment motivation). Where relationships were found between self-esteem and achievement, they were usually direct rather than inverse. 5D. H. Kausler and E. P. Trapp, ”Relationship Between Achievement Motivation Scores and Manifest Anxiety Scores,” Journal of Consulting Psychology, 22: 448-4502 6Louis D. Cohen, "Level-Of-Aspiration Behavior and Feelings Of Adequacy and Self-Acceptance," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49: 84-86. 7Ibid., p. 86. 8Ruth c. Wylie, The Self Concept (Lincoln: The Univer— sity of Nebraska Press, 1961), pp. 224-246. 5 The conflicting results Of the above investigations indicate a need for some measure which would discriminate between two types Of individuals: (1) those whose level Of anxiety is so high or who have such extremely adverse esti- mates Of self that they are incapacitated, or, in Lasswell’s words, are blocked from pursuit of power by feelings Of utter hopelessness; and, (2) those who have a high level of anxiety and a low estimate Of self but not so low that it prevents them from attempting to attain power and recognition. There has been an abundance of research in recent years on the relationships between child—training practices and achievement motivation. The results of these investigations are in general agreement that severity of independence training in childhood is significantly related to achievement. That is, the earlier and more severe the independence training in childhood, the higher the n Achievement.9 These findings 9See, for instance; Elizabeth M. Drews and John E. Teahan, ”Parental Attitudes and Academic Achievement,” Journal of Clinical Psychology, 13: 328~332; G. A. Friedman, "A Cross: Cultural Study Of the Relationship Between Independence Training and n Achievement as Revealed by Mythology” (unpub-w lished A. B. thesis, Harvard University, 1950), cited in Mc- Clelland, Studies in Motivation (New York: Appleton~Century— Crofts, Inc., 1955), pp. 411—413; David C. McClelland, ”Meas- uring Motivation in Phantasy,” Studied in Motivation, ibid., pp. 401-413; F. L. Strodtbeck, "Family Interaction, Values and Achievement" in D. C. McClelland, et al, Talent and Society (New Jersey: D. Van Nostrand Compafiy,—Inc., 1958), pp. 135~ 194; M. Winterbottom, "The Sources of Achievement Motivation in Mothers' Attitudes Toward Independence Training,” in Mc- Clelland, et al, The Achievement Motive (New York: Appleton- Century—CrOItET Inc., 1953), pp. 297—304. 6 tend to confirm the assumption that unsatisfactory inter- personal relations in early childhood produce insecurity and anxiety which are compensated for by a seeking Of power, recognition, and success. In the present study, rather than being concerned directly with what training practices are related to later achievement, we are interested in what feelings of the sub- ject about the interpersonal relations in his family are related to later achievement. Research has been conducted by some investigators whose approach is directly relevant to ours. Dynes, Clarke, and Dinitzlo initiated a project to find if individuals with high aspirations are characterized by greater difficulty in their interpersonal relations within the family Of orientation than are those with lower aspira- tions. They found a positive relationship between unsatis~ factcmw interpersonal relationships (as perceived by the subject) in the family of orientation and high occupational aspiration. Ellis,ll testing the hypothesis that a sense Of inade- quacy develops early in life during a childhood lacking in warmth and affection, studied sixty unmarried career women, comparing the successful with the nonsuccessful. She found that the most successful women indicated that they felt 10Russell R. Dynes, Alfred c. Clarke, and Simon Dinitz, "Levels of Occupational Aspiration: Some Aspects of Family Experience as a Variable,” American Sociological Review, 21: 212—215 (1956). 11Evelyn Ellis, ”Social Psychological Correlates Of Up~ ward Social Mobility Among Unmarried Career Women,” American Sociological Review, 17: 558-563 (1952). 7 rejected both by their parents and by their community. Evidence is consistent with the theory that upward social mobility is likely to be an outgrowth of basically neurotic drives resulting from unsatisfactory early primary group relations, and that mobility leads to a continuation of superficial, impermanent primary group relations and other overt manifestations Of emotional maladjustment . . . A comparison of mobile and non-mobile [successful and nonsuccessful] women reveals that significantly larger proportions Of mobile individuals had experienced both rejection by parents and by the over—all community during childhood.12 One Of the most comprehensive studies Of family relation- ships and achievement is reported in The Achievement Motive.13 As a part of their investigation the authors examined the child-rearing practices attributed to parents by sons with varying n Achievement. The most significant finding is that perceived severity of upbringing or ”felt lack Of love" is associated among college male students with high n Achievement. The largest single correlation involves the rejection attriw buted to the fathers by their sons; that is, sons who felt their fathers had rejected them had higher n Achievement scores than those who felt their fathers had loved and accepted them. The authors point out that, "these data may have more bearing on the ways sons with high achievement motivation perceive their parents than on the way in which they were actually brought up."lu The authors sum up their findings as follows: lerid., p. 563. l3Mcclelland, §§_§; , The Achievement Motive, pp. 275-318. 14lbid.’ p. 280. (underlining this author‘s.) 8 College males who give evidence Of being very "close" to their parents in their admiration of them and perception of them as particularly loving and helpful do not for the most part score high on n Achievement. On the contrary, it is the students who see their. parents as "distant"—-unfriend1y, severe, unsuccessful—~ who have high n Achievement scores.i Similar studies by McClelland are reported in Studies of Motivationl6 with results which support the above findings. Robinowitzl7 examined the ways in which a group of high school students with high achievement-re1ative-to-ability differed from three control groups on several variables, two Ofwhich were the subject's perception of his acceptance by his family and by his peers. His data suggest a relationship between achieving beyond the level of expectancy and holding an ambivalent evaluation Of ones own family and peer accept- ance. He infers from this that the subjects who are unsure Of their own acceptance by family and peers seem to be seeking a more secure status by means Of academic achievement beyond the level of expectancy. Friend and Haggard, with a sample Of 80 men and women between the ages of 16 and 36, found that family integration and attitude toward father are important in occupational adjustment but make little difference in achievement-—that is, those high in adjustment had closely knit, strongly unified l51bid., p. 281. 16McClelland, §£_§l , Studies in Motivation, pp. 389— 397. l7Ralph Robinowitz, "Attributes of Pupils Achieving ‘ Beyond Their Level of Expectancy,” Journal of Personality, 24: 308—317 (1956). 9 families and more affection for the father. On the other hand, antagonism for the mother was positively associated with both occupational adjustment and achievement.18 Those high_in adjustment and achievement had a more positive feeling for the mother and less sense of rejection than did the lows. Although these findings conflict with those of other investigators, Friend and Haggard make the important point that, "whether or not a worker makes special demands ofthe job or needs Special appreciation and status, depends upon the amount Of deprivation during his early life: that he goes after the identical and specific satisfactions in his work which were denied him years before."19 A preliminary investigation was made Of those rela~ tionships which might exist between interpersonal relation~ ships within the family and scholastic achievement with a sample Of 60 undergraduate students enrolled in introductory and advanced sociology courses at a large state university. The instrument and methods of analysis differed from those in the present study. The sample was divided into a high achievement group (reported grade average of A or B) and a low or average achievement group (reported grade average of C, D, or F), and into a group who perceived their relation- ships with the family as being satisfactory (i.e., feeling close to, accepted by the parents, and remembering childhood and adolescence as being happy), and a group who perceived l8Anne Roe, The Psychology Of Occupations (New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1956), pp. 128429. 19Ibid., p. 129. 10 the relationships as being unsatisfactory. Additional factors in the social milieu of the subjects were examined such as sex, religion, year in school, social class, and value the family places on education, and these showed no relationship to achievement. When the ”satisfaction with family relation- ships" groups were examined for their levels of achievement, the high satisfaction group had significantly more low achievement (76%) than high achievement (24%) whereas the low satisfaction group showed an equal amount of high and of low achievement. These results might be interpreted to mean that in a family which provides emotional support and acceptance, the need to seek gratification and recognition elsewhere is not developed. The present study is intended to examine further the relationship between perceived parental rejection and scholastic achievement. The three purposes, with the second and third being the major ones, are: 1. To examine certain factors in the subject's social milieu-—such as, sociO-economic status, religion, parental valuation of education, sex, year in school--for their influence on achievement. 2. To examine the relationship between perceived parental acceptance or rejection and scholastic achievement. 3. TO make the same examination as in number (2), within each sociO—economic group. 11 For the purposes Of this investigation it is hypothesized that: The There will be a low positive relationship between some Of the background itemse-namely, socio-economic status, religion, and parental valuation Of educa- tion—-and scholastic achievement. There will be an inverse relationship between feeling accepted by parents and high scholastic achievement. The inverse relationship hypothesized in number (2) will be (a) more definite when sociO—economic status is held constant, and, (b) strongest in the high sociO—economic group. rationale behind hypothesis 2 has been discussed. The general hypothesis in most studies of social class and achievement is that academic performance will be positively related to social class--that more high achievement will occur in the middle class than in the lower class. We hypothesize, however, that at the college level, if a relationship between sociO—economic status and achievement exists, it will be slight. It is Anne Roe states, still not literally true that anyone who wishes to can get a college education in this country, but it is more nearly true than it used to be . . . . Expectation Of college attendance varies with the social class position of the family. . . . after students get to college the percentage graduating is very similar in all groups except for those from farm homes. College students, after all, comprise a limited group and are already selgcted on a social class basis to a considerable extent. 0 2OIbid., pp. 104—107. l2 Washburne found that sociO-economic status, as measured by education and occupation(s) Of parents, was n9£_signifi— cantly related to the academic performance of college students. He concludes that ”perhaps the part played by sociO-economic status as regards higher education is limited to the deter- mination Of Opportunity to attend school, and the development Of levels Of aspiration such that the student is motivated to attend when the Opportunity is Offered."21 Washburne's results seem to indicate that once the student gets_tg college, the social class of his family has very little to do with his achievement. 22 With a sample of high school sophomores, Rosen found that members Of the middle class tend to have considerably higher need achievement scores than individuals in the lower social strata, and that the middle class is characterized by a larger proportion of persons with achievement oriented values than are the lower social stratai'Subjects with high ____7 ,1 achievement motivation scores were proportionately more likely to achieve grades of "B” or better than were those with low motivation scores,whereas achievement value orientation was not related to academic achievement but was related to edu- cationaleaspiration. (Rosen concludes that the relationship between academic achievement and social class reflects to a 21Norman F. Washburne, "SociO—economic Status, Urbanism, and Academic Performance in College," Journal of Educational Research 53: 137 (1959). 22Bernard C. Rosen, "The Achievement Syndrome: A Psy- chocultural Dimension of Social Stratification," American Sociological Review, 21: 203—211 (1956). l3 considerable extent social class differences in achievement motivation. Finally, Strodtbeck, from a study of 1,151 boys Of pre- college age, reports a definite positive relationship between being an over-achiever and being of higher socio~economic status.23 The latter two investigations cited indicate a positive relationship between social class and achievement at the pre- college level. They seem to get at the way in which and the level at which this relationship operates. Middle class individuals are likely to be both higher aspirers and higher achievers than are those in the lower class. This differ- ence is especially evident at the premcollege level because a college sample is a select one in terms of motivation and value orientation. Only thOse individuals, regardless Of sociO-economic status, who are both high aspirers and high zrhievers can go to college. SO, although the middle class is characterized as having higher levels of aspiration and achievement than is the lower class, we could assume that the lower socio_economic level students who are in college have high levels of aspiration in order to be there, and will, therefore, achieve at almost as high a level as the higher sociO—economic level. It is expected that the extent to which a subject's parents value education (i.e. encouraged him tO attend college) 23Strodtbeck, op. cit., pp.l35—l94. 14 will vary with sociO-economic level. An investigation by Gottlieb in which the question was asked, "Who would you say played the most important role in helping you to decide to attend college?", reveals that middle and upper class boys were more likely to mention parents than were lower class boys and lower class boys selected teachers and guidance counselors more frequently than did middle and upper class boys.2u However, we might expect that the lower socio~economic status college students gained at least some Of their motiva- tion through encouragement to attend college received from their parents. Gottlieb found that in each social class it was the high achievers who had received parental encouragement to attend college.25 In light Of this, we would expect a rela- tionship between parental valuation of education and scholastic achievement, however, because Of the relationship between socio— economic status and parental valuation Of education, and of the only slight relationship between socio~economic status and achievement at the college level, perhaps the relationship will not be strong. Although we would not expect a strong relationship between socio—economic status and achievement, because of the differoing behavioral patterns at different sociO-economic levels——these patterns influencing the subject's perception of 2“David Gottlieb, "Social Class, College Students and the Ideal College Professor" (paper read at the Annual Meetings of the Ohio Valley Sociological Society, April 21, 1961). (Egrthcoming School Review, The University of Chicago Press, 19 2 . 25lbid., p. 5. *— 15 his acceptance or rejection by his parents--we expect to find a stronger relationship between perceived rejection and scholastic achievement when sociO-economic level is controlled. [Allison Davis cites research on child training practices which supports the view that "the middle-claSs child undergoes in his cultural training a more depriving attack upon his sources of organic and emotional support than does the lower-class child.”26 Middle-class parents "who have been taught by their own parents to strive for early and fast attainment as an absolute 'gOOd' in their part of society, very Often sacri— fice their children's happiness, their basic adequacy in facing the realities Of later life, by training them too early and tOO severely for 'achievement'."EZ/,Following this line of argument, we expect the relationship between perceived parental rejection and scholastic achievement to be stronger in the higher sociO—economic groups. Most discussions Of the relationship between religion and achievement are based upon Max Weber's study of the influ— ence of a religious ethic upon social activity. Following Weber's hypothesis that the spirit of modern capitalism is intimately connected with the Protestant Ethic, McClelland, Rindlisbacher, and DeCharms hypothesized that there will be an association between ”the new 'spirit of capitalism,' 'the irrational sense Of having done his job well' with an increase in achievement motivation (n Achievement) and the Protestant 26Allison Davis, Social Class Influences Upon Learning (Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1960), pp. 18-19. 27lbid., p. 22. 16 emphasis on 'self-help' for salvation with an increased stress on independence training for young children."28 That is, they hypothesize that Protestants will have higher achievement motivation than Catholics and that Protestant families will emphasize independence training more. Winterbottom29 and McClelland and Friedman30 have demonstrated an empirical con— nection between emphasis on independence training and n Achievement. In addition to their main hypothesis, McClelland g§.§1., test Weber's suggestion that Jews like Protestants will have a more rational acquisitive approach to the world, with the expectation that Jewish parents also will emphasize independence training more than Catholic parents. Their findings indicate that Protestant and Jewish parents do expect independence earlier on the part of their children than do Irish— or Italian—Catholics.31 Strodtbeck, however, in a similar study, found three items in particular which were related to achievement: (1) A belief that the world is orderly and amenable to rational mastery, that, therefore, a person can and should make plans which will control his destiny; (2) A willingness to leave home to make one's way in life; and (3) A preference for individual rather than collective credit for work done.32 28McClelland gt 21,, Studies in Motivation, p. 391. 29w1nterbottom, op. cit., pp. 297—304. 3OFriedman, Op. cit., pp. 401—413. 31Mcclelland §t_a1 , Studies in Motivation, p. 395. 32Strodtbeck, op. cit., pp. 186—187. 17 These values correspond to Weber's characterization of the Protestant Ethic, but, Strodtbeck states, Our concern differs from Weber's in that the orien- tation in question is not presumed to affect all categories of Americans equally. Some elements are believed to be widely held, but the particularly dynamic element of substituting achievement for interpersonal gratification is believed to develop only in adult years, and only in people who have been in some sense specially prepared. Not only is it necessary that they hold cer- tain beliefs about the nature of the external system, but the implication of performance in it for interpersonal relations may also be involved. It is in search for leads in this connection that we turn to . . . those [items] relating to early training as a motivational preparation for achievement.33 In the latter analysis, Strodtbeck found that sons' achieve~ ment was related to the power balance in the family (i.e. democratic familyowrelatively powerful motherm-yields sons with higher achievement motivation), which was in turn related to social class. ”Socio-economic status affects socialization and the power balance in the family, both of ”3A Thus, it which are related to subsequent achievement. appears that what is often characterized as the Protestant Ethic is in fact the middle class ethic—~this ethic or set of values beingtmgrgrdeterminedlby social class than by religious background. It is because of this, as with socio~ economic status, that we do not expect a strong relationship between religion and achievement. 33Ibid., pp. 143-14u 341bid., p. 185. 18 In summary, this chapter has included an introduction to the problem of the study--the theoretical background, a review of the relevant literature, and a statement of our purposes and hypotheses. CHAPTER II METHODOLOGY The instrument used in this study was a questionnaire containing 35 items. (A copy of the questionnaire can be found in the Appendix A.) Twelve of the items were intended to obtain objective data on background information, the remaining items, to elicit subjective responses on feelings of parental acceptance or rejection and on feelings of happi— ness in childhood and adolescence. The major variable in the study, other than scholastic achievement, is parental acceptance or_rejection of the child a§_perceived by the child. Throughout the rest of this paper, this "perceived parental acceptance/rejection” variable will be referred to as ”integration with parents." Level of inte- gration may be defined, then, as the degree to which the sub- ject feels accepted or rejected by his parents. 1 originated a two-part scale to measure feelings of Nye acceptance and rejection between parents and children. The first part of the scale consists of a group of items to deter- mine to what extent the child accepted or rejected his parents, and the second part, a group of items to determine to what 1F. Ivan Nye, Family Relationships and Delinquent Behavior (New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1958), pp. 77~ 78. 19 20 degree the child felt that his parents had accepted or re— jected him. The latter group of items is the one used to determine integration in this study. Figure 1 presents these items. Ten of the items elicit responses indicative of per— ceived paternal acceptance, and 10 of perceived maternal acceptance. Three scores were obtained from the scale: (1) integration with father, (2) integration with mother, and (3) integration with parents. The first score was derived by totaling the response values on the 10 ”father items" and terming those with scores from 15-20, highly integrated with father; those from 10-14, middle integrated with father; and those from 0—9, low integrated with father. The second score was obtained in the same manner and with the same range of possible values for each integration group using the total response values from the 10 ”mother items.” 'Two methods were used for determining the integration with parents score. First (Method 1), the reSponse values for the total scale (all twenty items) were totaled with a possible score of O~MO. Those who scored 30-40 were classified as highly integrated with parents, those from 20—29 as middle integrated with parents, and those from 0-10 as low integrated with parents. By the second method (Method 2), those subjects who scored from 15-20 on the ten "father items" and who also scored from 15-20 on the ten ”mother items" were called highly integrated with parents, those who scored from lO-lu on the ”father items” 10. ll. 21 FIGURE 1 NYE SCALE OF PER’ SEIVED PARENTAL REJ-ECTION WITH RESPONSE VALUES My father is interested in what I do: (1) Always _§; 2) Usually _l} (3) Sometimes o _; (A) Seldom O_; 5 Never O My mother is interested in what I do: (1) Always _§; (2; Usually l_3 (3) Sometimes _g, (4) Seldom o; 5 Never 0 , My father ridicules my ideas: (1) Always C) ; eUsually _g_; (3) Sometimes o ; (A ) Seldom l ; (STTUev 2.. My mother ridicules my ideas: (1) Always O ; eUsually o ; (3) Sometimes o ; (u) Seldom l ; (”TTNev n_g_ My father encourages me to discuss my problems with him: (I) Always 2 ; (2) Usually l‘; (3) Sometimes O ; A Seldom _9l3 (5) Never 0 My mother encoura Always 2 ; ( Seldom_ ,( 3 me to discuss my problems with her: sually l ; (3) Sometimes o ; -ever 0 02 mm \J-Z/(D 'Z C1 think my father has my best interests at heart: ) Always 2 ; (2) Usually l ; (3) Sometimes O_; Seldom O_; _5) Never 9, t hink my mother has my best interests at heart: Always 2 ; (2) Usually l ; (3) Sometimes O ; Seldom O“; (5) Never O . 21 22 22 I think my father shows more interest in my brothers and sisters than he shows in me: (1) Always o ; (2) Usually O ; (3) Sometimes O ; (A) Seldom l _; (57_Never 2 ; (67 Usually shows more interest in me 2 ; (7) 11m“‘an only child _l_. '_—- I think my mother shows more interest in my brothers and Sisters than she shows in me: (1) Always O ; (2) Usually O ; (3) Sometimes O m; (A) Seldom l _; (57—Never _2_; '(6) Usually shows more interest in me 2 ; (7) I'm an only child _l__ '—__ Other fathers tend to Show more interest in their children than my father shows in me: (1) Completely agree 0 ; (2) Partially agree 0 ; (3) Undecided l ; (A) Partially disagree _§_; (5) CompIetely disagree _E::' 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. I'D I\) FIGURE 1-~Ccntinued Other mothers tend to show more interest in their children than my mother shows in me: (1) Completely agree O ; (2) Partially agree 0 ; (3) Undecided (u) Partially disagree _2_; (5) CompIetely disagree My father praises me when I do my work well: (1) Always 2 ; (2% Usually ; (3) Sometimes O ; ( 1 Seldom 0.; 25 6“ Never My mother praises me when I do my work well: (1) Always 2 ; (2) Usually 1.3 (3) Sometimes o ; (A) Seldom"d ; (5) Never cf Does your father ever seem to wish that you were a different type( of person? (1) Always O ; (2) Often O ; (3) Sometimes O ; (A) Seldom__l_; (5) Never _2_ Does your mother ever seem to wish that you were a different type of person? (1) Always O ; (2) Often 0.; (3) Sometimes _Q_; (A) Seldom_ 1 ; (5) Never 2 Do you think that your father tries to understand your problems and worries? (1) Always _2_; (2) Usually 1 ; (3) Sometimes O ; (A) Seldomm ;(5) Never _9_ Do you think that your mother tries to understand your problems and worries?‘ (1) Always 2 ; (2) Usually l ; (3) Sometimes _O ; (A) Seldc%m. O ;_T5) Never 0 My father says and does things that make me feel that I am not trusted: (1) Always .913 (2) Often_;g_; (3) Sometimes__g_, _A) Seldom 1.; 25) Never _2_ My mother says and does things that make me feel that I am not trusted: (I) Always O“; (2) Often 0.; (3) Sometimes .o , ,A) Seldom '1"; (5) Never :2:; 23 and also scored from lO_lA on the "mother items," middle integrated with parents, and those who scored from 0-9 on the "father items" and from 0-9 on the "mother items," low integrated with parents° Figure 2 illustrates the possible combinations of scores on the father items and scores on the mother items which would yield a certain integration with parents score by Methods 1 and 2. The total range of possible father score-mother score combinations which would fall into a given integration with parents category is greater by Method 1 than by Method 2. Thus, Method 2 yields purer categories, including only those respondents in a given integration with parents group who are of the same level of integration with one parent as they are with the other. AS is obvious from Figure 2, this is not necessarily the case when Method 1 is used. The marginals which result using Method 1 and using Method 2 are presented in Table 1. TABLE 1 MARGINALS FOR INTEGRATION WITH PARENTS CATEGORIES OBTAINED BY METHODS 1 AND 2 Method 1 Method 2 Integration Per Integration Per With Parents N Cent With Parents N Cent High . . . . . 91 31 High . . . . . 75 26 Middle . . . . 129 AA Middle . . . . 5A 15 Low . . . . . 73 25 Low . . . . . A7 16 Totals. . 293 100 Totals. . 176 57 21+ ®H....O H moooooo + mo....o 0H....O H 0H...oo + 0H....O 30g mm....om u 3H....ofi + SH....oH mm....om u om....o + om....o oases: os....om n om....mH + om....mH o:....om u om....oa + om....oa swam Hmuoe whoom oAOOm mo Hmpoe whoom oAOom muzmpmm tempo: assume a sosooz senses spas oHanuom oHnannom ofioaw o9 manannom mananuom soaooawoosH m Conpmz H ponpoz N Qz< H mQOmBmE Mm QmQQMHN mmmoom mezmmdm EBHB ZOHB.H u x N mm mm ma Nznz 30A Hm mm Hm mmuz 30a ma Fm mm :muz manna: ma pm pm mmauz manna: ma no em mpuz swam AH mm mm Hmuz swam 30% GHOUHE Swflm Z mpcmhmm QQHZ 30% QHUUHZ Swfim Z mpcmhmfl SPH3 PC®E®>0H£O< PCQO flmm COHpMmepflH UC®E$>®H£O< pflmo fimm COHPGLWOPCH N Dospmz H Conpmz Bzmzm>MHmo¢ UHBmoaeo< oposoeoom eofioog ooeH oeoso>ofieoe eosgoogm eofioohwooeH WWWD hwm PCmO hmm mmmozommom Qz< zmzmmmmm mom Bzm2m>MHmo< Qz< mezmmoH£o< onEom coapmgonCH pCoEo>oHSo< mam: soapMLmopCH puma pom pcoo mom mHHMHIO¢ QZ¢ mfizmmdm 38H: ZOHB M 17 33 Table 13 shows the relationship between father inte- gration and mother integration for the total sample. It appears that knowledge of integration with one parent is a fairly good predictor of integration with the other parent. If the child is highly integrated with the father, there is a very good chance that he will be highly integrated with the mother, and if the child feels rejected by the mother, that he also will feel rejected by the father. On the other hand, if the child feels accepted by the mother, he does not neces- sarily feel accepted by the father, and if he feels rejected by the father, he does not necessarily feel rejected by the 47 mother. Thus, a knowledge of perceived father acceptance or of perceived mother rejection would be a fairly reliable indicator of perceived integration with parents. TABLE 13 FATHER INTEGRATION IN RELATION TO MOTHER INTEGRATION Per Cent Mother Integration Father Integration N High Middle Low High N=87 84 16 0 Middle N=lOA 38 53 9 Low N=102 15 39 46 x2 = 15A 61, df, 4, p <: .001. Table 14 shows the relationship between integration with father and high achievement, and with mother and high achievement for males and females. (The figures in this table are extracted from Tables 22 and 23 which appear in Appendix B.) For both males and females, the relationship with father seems not to effect the level of achievement; relationship with mother, however, tends to have an influence on achievement among both males and females. It seems that it is not a question of how much integration with mother the individual perceives but rather one of feeling some integra— tion versus feeling none. In the tables where there is a relationship between integration and achievement, middle and high integration tend to go together in contrast to low inte- gration. 48 ma mauz mm mmuz ma mzuz mm onuz 30H om omuz om mmuz mm ssuz om mouz ofioofiz mm msuz om sznz mm mmuz om oquz omfim honooz z gogoom z goeooz z coeoom z eofioogmooeH monEom wcH>mH£o< swam pcoo pom moamz wcfi>oano¢ nmam psoo pom mmqezmm oz< mmqmHmo< mon oz< mmmeoz meHs 26HemHmo< mon 62¢ momeem meHz oneamomezH :H mqm¢8 49 Although the differences on integration with either parent and achievement are not significant among males, middle or high integration with mother, and middle integration with father tend to relate somewhat to high achievement. Feeling too close to the father, or too distant from the mother seems to be most detrimental to male high achievement. Rosen states that in order for strong achievement motivation to develop, the boy seems to need more autonomy from his father than from his mother. He reasons that the authoritarian father may crush his son and in so doing destroy the boy's achieve- ment motive, whereas the dominating mother does not seem to have the same effect, possibly because she is perceived as imposing her standards on the boy, while a dominating father is perceived as imposing himself on his son.1 An intensely 'involved mother appears to promote the deveIOpment of achieve- ment motivation in boys and it is the authoritarian father, not the m0ther,who represents a greater threat to the boy and inhibits the development of achievement motivation.2 Although the tendencies are very slight, it appears that our results might indicate such a relationship among the high achieving males. Among females, achievement is significantly related to integration with mother; there is significantly more high achievement among those who feel either middle or highly inte- grated than among those who feel low integrated with mother. 1Bernard C. Rosen, "Family Structure and Achievement Motivation," American Sociological Review, 26: 575 (1961). 2Ibid., p. 577. 50 This is an expected result. Of all the parent-child relation- ships, that between the mother and daughter is the closest-- the atmosphere of this relationship, then,would be expected to be most influential in the behavior of the female. A basic assumption in this investigation is that it is the degree of integration which influences level of achieve- ment rather than level of achievement determining integration. Some investigators have argued that there is a causal relation- ship in the other direction, that it is the level of achieve- ment which determines degree of integration with family and peers. Strodtbeck, in a study of business executives, suggests that high achievement brings depersonalization of relationships which may cause anxiety, and, The fact that status, power, and related rewards exist as positive inducements to achievement makes possible a resolution to the dilema: the chronic achiever expiates depersonalization with more achievement. Perhaps, non- achievers reduce anxiety over low achievement by the cultivation of more gratifying interpersonal relations.3 Our hypothesis that high achievement may be a response to un_ satisfactory interpersonal relationships—-a seeking of gratifi- cation in the area of achievement to overcome feelings of deprivation gained in the family—mis in contrast to this. Although the hypothesis that there will be an inverse relation- ship between integration with parents and achievement has not been confirmed, an attempt has been made to pinpoint the causal factors in the relationship which has tended to result. As 3Strodtbeck, op. cit., p. 142. 51 will be recalled from the discussion of the research instru— ment, childhood and adolescent happiness ratings were signifi- cantly related to integration with parents. In order to determine the causal factors in the relationship between integration with parents and achievement, we have examined the patterns of childhood and adolescent happiness as they relate to achievement. Logically, there are four possible patterns: Childhood Adolescence A. Happy . . . . . . . Happy B. Happy . . . . . . . Unhappy C. Unhappy . . . . . . . Happy D. Unhappy . . . . . . . Unhappy According to our original hypothesis, we would expect that those highest in achievement would be found in groups C and D, however, in light of our findings as revealed here, we now would expect the high achievement to occur in groups A and B. Table 15 shcws the relationship between pattern of childhood-adolescent happiness and scholastic achievement. Group C will not be discussed, because of the small number of respondents it contains. Respondents in groups A and B are more likely to be high achievers than low; the achieve- ment percentages for group A are exactly the same as those for the high integrated with parents group in our previous analysis. At first glance, the results for group D appear to conflict with our previous findings, however, in addition 52 to there being the largest-percentage of high achievers in this group, there is also the greatest per cent of low achievers . Although the differences are not significant, we would speculate that the per cent of both high achievers and of low achievers being greatest in group D suggests that consistent unhappiness may effect different people differently; some seek out academic gratification, while others sacrifice grades and seek recognition in some other area. In addition, we would speculate that perhaps our results reveal that un— happiness in childhood and adolescence arouses anxiety which causes the individual to seek power and recognition, but at the same time, "adverse estimates of the self must not be overwhelming, or the resort to power will be blocked by ”4 sentiments of utter hopelessness. TABLE 15 PATTERN OF CHILDHOOD-ADOLESCENT HAPPINESS AND ACHIEVEMENT Pattern of Happiness Per Cent Achievement Childhood Adolescence N High Middle Low A. Happy . . . . Happy N=207 24 57 19 B. Happy . . . . Unhappy N235 2O 74 6 C. Unhappy . . . Happy N212 25 58 17 D Unhappy . . . Unhappy N=3O 3O 4O 3O x2 = 9.74, df, 6, NS. ”Lasswell, op. cit., p. 153. 53 We feel that the results for group D in Table 15 give us some support for our original hypothesis that unsatisfactory interpersonal relations in childhood produce insecurity and anxiety which are compensated for by a quest for power, recog- nition, and success. Students differ, our hypothesis appears to be true of some students and not of others. From our results on year in school and achievement, we speculate that the school at which our sample was drawn, is not the type of institution which has a preponderance of academically oriented A students, and that the low integrated students would tend to compensate for feelings of insecurity and to seek gratifi- cation in areas other than scholastic achievement. There may be campuses at which a greater proportion of academically oriented students would be found, at at such institutions we would expect to find more of a relationship between low integration and high achievement. Why does the relationship between unhappy childhood and adolescence and achievement differ from that between low integration with parents and achievement? The difference in the number of respondents included in each group must be ruled out as a reason for the disparity, because, although there are only 30 in the unhappy childhood—adolescence group, there are only 47 in the low integration with parents group when integration is determined by Method 2. One factor which might account for the variation in results is that the intent of the items in the integration index is not quite so obvious as is that of the items on childhood and adolescent happiness; ! 5A knowing the purpose of the items may have influenced the respondents. Secondly,although pattern of childhood- adolescent happiness is significantly related to the integra- tion index, both childhood-adolescence happiness and integra— tion with parents are undoubtedly influenced by other factors. The factors which the two measures do not have in common are apparently related in different ways to scholastic achievement. This chapter includes an examination of the relationships between integration with parents and scholastic achievement for the total sample, for freshmen and sophomores, and for males and females. In addition, the patterns resulting between integration with each parent and achievement for males and for females were discussed. Subsidiary analyses included in the chapter showed the patterns of male and of female integration with father and with mother, the relation- ship of integration with father to integration with mother, and the ways in which patterns of childhood-adolescent happiness are related to achievement. CHAPTER V FINDINGS: SOCIO—ECONOMIC LEVEL, INTEGRATION WITH PARENTS, AND SCHOLASTIC ACHIEVEMENT Our primary concern in this chapter is with the rela- tionships between integration with parents and scholastic achievement within each socio—economic group. First, we will examine the patterns of integration with father and with mother for males and females within each socio-economic group, and second, the relationships between integration with parents and achievement at each socio-economic level. Table 16 shows the patterns of father integration for males and females at each socio-economic level. Male inte- gration with father varies with socio-economic level--males in the high socio-economic group are much more likely to be highly integrated with father than are males in the low socio- economic group. High socio-economic level males are about equally distributed among the three levels of integration with father, whereas low socio—economic level males are much more likely to be middle or low integrated with father than to be highly integrated with him. We would interpret this to mean that low socio—economic level college males do not want to be like their fathers, that in attending college, they are attempting to move away from the occupational status of their 55 56 .mz .m .co .mm.a u x Aoa..v.ov .mz .m .co .mm.: H x m m om mm H: mouz me o: AH oasuz god em mm mm mmnz mm mm mm mouz hmfim 3 m w 3 w oq Hoofiz 3 Am 2 w oq ofiooflz g Am 2 osooom hospmm spa: Coapmpwoch onEmm . pofipmm Spa: coaumsmoch mam: oHEocooonoaoom pcmo pom pcoo pom mmgoano< mdpmpm OHEocoomuoHoom 30g pcmo pom psoEo>oH£o< mSpMpm oHEocoomuoHoom swam pcoo pom mpcopmm spas coapmgwmch qm>mq onozoom-oHoom Mm ezmzm>mHmo< OHBmaqomom oz< mezmmI. l4 —l4 x2 = 9.00, or, 1. p <1 .01. by their values; thus, the low socio-economic status respon- dents who are highly integated with parents are slightly more likely to be low achievers than high achievers. If this were strictly the case, however, we would expect the most high achievement in the low sociomeconomic group to occur among those respondents who are low integrated with parents. As we have seen, this is not the case; those respondents who are of the low socioueconomic group and who are low integrated with parents tend to have more low achievement than do those in any other status—integration group. Perhaps there is a second factor operating here. These respondents have two major strikes against them for high achievement: they are low socio—economic class individuals competing in a middle class system, and they feel unaccepted by their parents. It seems that they have too many handicaps which prevent them from being high achievers. Finally, it appears that the relationship with parents most 63 conducive to high achievement in the low socio-economic class is one of middle integration--a relationship in which the respondent feels accepted by his parents but is not so close to them that he is overly influenced by their values, one in which he feels accepted by but remains autonomous from his parents. In this chapter we have dealt with socio-economic status, integration, and achievement. The patterns of inte- gration with father and with mother were examined for males and females in each socio-economic group. The results on integration with parents and achievement by socio-economic level were presented. In an attempt to interpret the findings, the relationship between socio-economic status and parental valuation of education was examined. Our final interpretations and conclusions will appear in Chapter VI. CHAPTER VI SUMMARY, INTERPRETATIONS, AND CONCLUSIONS The purpose of this study was to determine the effect of perceived parental rejection upon academic grade achieve- ment. The theoretical framework used is that of deprivation \ W and compensation, with emphasis upon the impact of unsatis- factory interpersonal relations in childhood on later aspects of behavior. More specifically, the position is taken that early parent-child tensions will produce insecurity and anxiety which may be expressed or compensated for by a quest for power, recognition, and success. Either implicitly or explicitly, investigators have examined the implications for achievement of relationship with parents and/or level of self—esteem. Numerous investigators report a relationship between high achievement and feelings of rejection and low level of self—esteem. This study represents a further effort to examine the effect of perceived parental acceptance or re- jection (i.e. integration with parents) upon scholastic achieve- ment as measured by reported grade point average. Data for the results reported here were obtained by the administration of paper-pencil questionnaires to a sample of 293 undergraduate students enrolled in basic social science courses at a large state university. 64 65 We took the position that (1) there will be a low positive relationship between certain of the background items-- namely, socio—economic status, religion, and parental valu- ation of education--and scholastic achievement; (2) there will be an inverse relationship between integration with parents and scholastic achievement; and (3) the inverse re— lationship between integration with parents and achievement will be more definite when socio-economic level is controlled and will be strongest in the high socio-economic group. The results obtained from the sample of college under- graduates in relation to the hypotheses may be summarized as follows: 1. The prediction set forth in the first hypothesis found some support in our analysis. There was a tendency for more high and less low achievement to occur in the high .... ..___~_ fl , socio-economic group; this tendency was stronger among '--————-—--" females than among males. Achievement varied slightly among the different religious groups, however, the ex— pected order (i.e. Jewish, Protestant, Catholic) did not hold in all cases. The order for high achievement was Protestant, Catholic, Jewish and the order for low achieve- ment, Jewish, Protestant, and Catholic. The group stating "none" as their religious preference had the least high and the most low achievement. There was a low positive relationship between achievement and parental valuation of educatiSRYIEhis relationships was stronger than that 66 between socio-economic status and achievement. Finally, a greater proportion of females reported high achievement and lesser rates of low achievement than did males, and freshmen were significantly more likely to be high achievers than were sophomores. 2. The prediction in the second hypothesis was not upheld; on the contrary, there were tendencies for the most high achievement to occur among those who were middle and highly integrated with parents, and the most low-achieve- mentl_among_those,whowwerewlowPintggggggdiy In particular, ,' trends appeared among both males and females for middle /,i < and high integration with/mother to relate to high achieve— \ , ~"ment.,,---and_low integration withgmother, to low achievement. -.,_ “__/ ""“"""’""‘_‘_*‘\\ -..——_. ..___ . This relationship was significant amongffemales. Father integration appeared not to influence achievement among either males or females. 3. In light of the results thus far, the predictions in the third hypothesis were inapplicable. The results for inte— gration with parents and achievement by socio—economic level showed no relationship between the two variables in the high socio-economic group. In the low socio-economic group there was a tendency for the most high achievement to occur among those who were middle integrated with parents and the most low achievement among those who were low integrated. As a part of the analysis, the relation- ship between socio-economic level and parental valuation education was examined with the result that the two are significantly related. /" 67 The tendencies which resulted between integration with parents and achievement are surprising in terms of the rela— tionships reported by previous investigators. We hypothesized that the two variables would be inversely related; and the . results indicated slight tendencies for them to be positivelyl related. A difference in the indices used to determine per- ceived relationship with parents must be ruled out as a possible reason for our results conflicting with those of others; some investigators1 reporting an inverse relationship between perceived parental acceptance and achievement have used items quite similar to the ones used in this study. Many of the studies in this area have used achievement motivation (n Achievement) rather than actual achievement, for their depen- dent variable. This cannot account for the difference in results, however, because other investigators have worked with actual achievement and have reported results similar to those obtained using achievement motivation. In addition, a rela- tionship between achievement motivation and actual achievement has been established. A more likely interpretation for the discrepancy of our findings with those of other investigators lies in the nature of the sample. We would speculate that a large state lDynes'et'al., op. cit., pp. 212-215; McClelland et al,, The Achievement Motive; Robinowitz, op. cit., pp. 308-317. 2McClelland §t_§13, The Achievement Motive; Rosen, "The Achievement Syndrome: A Psychocultural Dimension of Social Stratification," pp. 203-211. 68 supported university, such as that used for this study, is not the type of institution which would tend to draw a pre- ponderance of academically oriented students. Students whose orientation is other than academic and who perceive parental rejection would tend to compensate for feelings of insecurity and to seek gratification in other areas-—for example, in peer relationships. At a school such as Oberlin, Antioch, or Reed which Imus a high proportion of academically oriented students we would expect to find a relationship between low integration with parents and high scholastic achievement. If we can generalize from the tendencies revealed in 1 this study, our findings seem to indicate that among respon- dents at the type of institution in which our sample was drawn, one necessary prerequisite for future adequate perfor— mance is feeling accepted in the family of orientation. It appears that the crucial relationships is that with the . mother;;perceived maternal acceptance influences level of Aachievément whereas perceived paternal acceptance does not, and this is especially true among females. It seems that rather than reacting against and trying to compensate for the treatment one has received and the attitudes one feels others hold about him, the individual tends to react in terms of the picture he has of himself-~this picture being formed initially in the family, and continuously through interaction with others. In light of this, the result that relationship with mother is more important than that with father is easily understandable. At this point in our society, it is 69 predominately the mother who has the job of child care and socialization; she is the one, rather than the father, who is in continuous interaction with the children and satisfies their needs. Because of the role of the mother, both male and female children tend to feel closer thPPP' Thus, it is 3 ‘\ -.__~_ _ ,_ . \‘—\ not surprising that perceived mhternal acceptance has a \\.. . ._ ,_____.__,/ h greater effect upon/performance than does thatffor,father,gj 1" _/ / We-offerwtngdifferent interpretations for the results obtained on integration with parents and achievement by socio- economic level. Tbe first is strictly in terms of the findings on integration with parents and achievement within each socio-economic group,without reference to previous results or to the methods of analysis used. The second interpretation is in reference to the methods of analysis we found it neces- sary to use, with emphasis upon the male-female patterns of integration with father and with mother in each socio-economic group which resulted in the previous analysis. We would speculate that a relationship between inte- gration with parents and achievement does not result in the high socio-economic group for the reason that education is such a part of the value system that this value overrides the influence which parental integration might have upon achieve- ment. In the low socio-economic group the value of education is not influential or pervasive enough to override the influ- ence of perceived parental acceptance; thus, a tendency results for integration with parents and achievement to be related. Those subjects in the low socio-economic group who are highly 7O integrated with parents are more likely to be low achievers than high. We Speculate that perhaps this indicates that being highly integrated with parents also means being more highly influenced by their values. Gottlieb,3 in a study of social class and college students, reports that only one- third of the lower class boys mentioned that parents had encouraged and helped to influence them to attend college; two-thirds mentioned non-familial sources of support and encouragement. If our intapretations thus far were strictly the case, we would expect the most low socio—economic level high achievement to occur in the low integrated group. That this is not so suggests that there is a second factor operating here. It appears that the low socio-economic, low integrated with parents group has too many strikes against them for high achievement. They are low socio-economic status individuals competing in a middle class system and they feel unaccepted by their parents. Thus, in the low socio—economic group, it seems that a relationship in which the subject feels accepted by, yet autonomous from his parents is the one which most conduces to high achievement. Although we offer the above interpretation of the results obtained for integration with parents and achievement within each socio—economic group, we cannot help but feel that the results were influenced to some extent by the methods of analysis we found it necessary to use. It will be recalled that in the analysis of integration and achievement our results 3Gottlieb, op. cit., p. 4. 71 became much clearer as we broke the total sample down into males and females, and as we broke integration with parents down into integration with father and integration with mother. When the relationships between integration with father and integration with mother and achievement for males and females were examined, definite patterns emerged. Further, when this same examination was made within each socio-economic group, these patterns varied with each of the four groups. When we looked at integration and achievement within each socio- economic group, it was necessary, because of the size of the sample, to combine the four groups into one, as in Figure 3. Instead of determining the relationship between integration with father and with mother and achievement for males and females within each socio-economic group, it was only possible to obtain that between integration with parents and achievement FIGURE 3 BREAKDOWN OF INTEGRATION AND ACHIEVEMENT VARIABLES 'VERSUS REQUIRED COMBINED VARIABLES Socio-economic Status Integration Achievement Integration Achievement Father . . . Male 1 Father Female 0 a c +— | 1 _ Mother . . . Male Paren. . . . Combinegeggjg Mother . . . Female for the total high socio-ecomomic group and for the total low one. By combining these groups into one, the differences be- tween the groups were obscured; some of these differences were 72 quite substantial. Had we been able to analyze the four groups separately, our results might have been quite dif- ferent. In conclusion, we suggest that a large public university, such as that used for this study, might not be the type of institution which would tend to draw an abundance of academ- ically oriented students--those whose orientation is other than academic would tend‘macompensateikm=feelings of rejec- tion by seeking gratification in areas other than scholastic achievement. Among academically orientedstudents, we expect that perceived parental rejection would be related to high scholastic achievement. Perhaps the major findings which can be abstracted from this study is that among students at a large state-supported school, a feeling of maternal acceptance is a factor in high scholastic achievement. This suggests that one prerequisite for future adequate performance is perceived acceptance‘vin the family of orientation. There remain many unanswered ques- tions, some of which might point the way for further research into the influence of interpersonal relations within the family and levels of self-esteem upon achievement. We might ask, what are the types of interaction, or factors in interaction, which most influence evaluation of self; in what way is eval- uation of self related to levels of aspiration and achievement; how do these relationships vary by social class. This study indicates the need in research for a measure of factors which could predict success. REFERENCES Cohen, Louis D. "Level-of-Aspiration Behavior and Feelings of Adequacy and Self-Acceptance,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49: 84-86 (1954). Davis, Allison. Social Class Influences Upon Learning. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1960. Davis, James, David Gottlieb, and Joel Spaeth. Stipends and Spouses. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1962. Douvan, Elizabeth. ”Social Status and Success Strivings," in John K. Atkinson, Motives in Fantasy, Action, and Society. New Jersey: D. Van Nostrand Co., Inc., 1958. Drews, Elizabeth M. and John E. Teahan. "Parental Attitudes and Academic Achievement," Journal of Clinical Psychology, 13 328-332 (1957). Duff, 0. Lee and laurence Siegel. ”Biographical Factors Associated with Academic Over- and Under-achievement," Journal of Educational Psychology, 51: 43-46 (1960). Dynes, Russell R., Alfred C. Clarke, and Simon Dinitz, ”Levels of Occupational Aspiration: ‘Some Aspects of Family Experience as a Variable,” American Sociological Review, 21: 212-215 (1956). Ellis, Evelyn. ”Social Psychological Correlates of Upward Social Mobility Among Unmarried Career Women,” American Sociological Review, 17: 558-563 (1952). Friedman, G. A. "A Cross-cultural Study of the Relationship Between Independence Training and n Achievement as Revealed by Mythology.” Unpublished A. B. Thesis, Harvard University. 1950. Cited inJHcClelland,Studies in Motivation. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, Inc., 1955: pp° 411-413, Gottlieb, David. ”Social Class, College Students and the Ideal College Professor." Paper read at the Annual Meetings of the Ohio Valley Sociological Society, April 21, 1961. Forthcoming School Review, The University of Chicago Press, 1962. 73 74 Horney, Karen. The Neurotic Personality of Our Time. New ‘York: W. W. Norton and Company, 1937. Kausler, D. H. and E. P. Trapp. "Relationship Between Achievement Motivation Scores and Manifest Anxiety Scores," Journal of Consulting Psychology, 22: 448- 450 (1958). Lasswell, Harold D. Power and Personality. New York: W. W. Norton and Company, 1948. McClelland, David 0. "Measuring Motivation in Phantasy," Studies in Motivation. New York: Appleton-Century- Crofts, Inc., 1955. ' McClelland, David C., John W. Atkinson, Russell A. ark, and Edgar L. Lowell. The Achievement Motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, Inc., 1955!. McClelland, David C., A. Rindlisbacher, and Richard DeCharms. ”Religious and Other Sources of Parental Attitudes Toward Independence Training," Studies in Motivation. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, Inc., 1955. Murray, H. A. Explorations in Personality. New York: Oxford University Press, 1938. Nye, F. Ivan. Family Relationships and Delinquent Behavior. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1958. "Adolescent—Parent Adjustment--Socio-economic Level as a Variable,” American Sociological Review, 16: 341—349 (1951). Robinowitz, Ralph. ”Attributes of Pupils Achieving Beyond their Level of Expectancy," Journal of Personality, 24: 308—317 (1956 Roe, Anne. The Psychology of Occupations. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1956. Rosen, Bernard C. HThe Achievement Syndrome: A Psycho- cultural Dimension of Social Stratification," American Sociological Review, 21: 203—211 (1956). . "Family Structure and Achievement Motivation,” American Sociological Review, 26: 574—584 (1961). Rosen, Bernard C. and Roy D'Andrade. ”The Psychosocial Origins of Achievement Motivation,” Sociometry, 22: 185- 218 (1959). 75 Shaw, Merville c. and John T. McCuen. "The Onset of Academic Underachievement in Bright Children," Journal of Edu- cational Psychology, 51: 103-108 (1960). Strodtbeck, F. L. "Family Interaction, Values and Achieve— ment," in D. C. McClelland, Alfred L. Baldwin, Urie Bronfenbrenner, and Fred L. Strodtbeck, Talent and Society. New Jersey: D. Van Nostrand Company, Inc., 1958. Taylor, Ronald G. "Personality Factors Associated with (Scholastic Achievement." Paper presented at the 1961 American Personnel and Guidance Association Convention. Washburne, Norman F. "Socio-economic Status, Urbanism and Academic Performance in College," Journal of Educational Research, 53: 130-137 (1959). Weiss, Peter, Michael Wertheimer, and Byron Groesbeck. "Achievement Motivation, Academic Aptitude, and College Grades," Educational and Psychological Measurement, 19: 663-666 (1959). Winterbottom, M. ”The Sources of Achievement Motivation in Mothers' Attitudes Toward Independence Training," in David C. McClelland, et al., The Achievement Motive. New York: Appleton-Cghffiry-Crofts, Inc., 1955. Wylie, Ruth C. The Self Concept. Lincoln, Nebraska: The University of Nebraska Press, 1961. S m I D N E P P. A 76 APPENDIX A 77 78 A study is being made of college students at this state university. As a part of this study we would appreciate receiving certain information from you about your background and some of your attitudes. This questionnaire is to be filled out anonymously and your full cooperation will be greatly appreciated. Please read each of the following questions carefully and check the one item in each question which best applies to you. In questions where there is no choice given but a space is provided, please answer as accurately as possible in this space. For questions 13 through 35, there are no "right" answers; in these please check (X) the one item in each question which best describes the way you feel. 10. 11. What is your age? Sex: (1) Male ,(2) Female Year in School: 9(1) Freshman , (2; Sophomore , (3) Junior“ 4 Senior , 5 Grad Marital status: (1 l) 21LSingle J (2) Married , (3) Divorced “, (4 ) Widowed . What is (or will be)your college major? Your approximate grade average in college is: (l) A , (a) 3+ . (3) B , 4) B— 4(5 0+ _: 6 C ; (7) C' 3 8) D 3A9 Your religious preference: (1) Protestant ( Catholic , (3) Jewish , (4) Other (Specify (5) None 2) ) Are you a United States citizen? (1) Yes , (2) No__ Where do you live now while attending college? (I) Dormitory , (2) Fraternity or sorority , 3 At home with my family , (4) Off-campus apartment , (5) Married housing . What is (or was) your father's occupation? Which one of the following best describes the number of years of school completed by your father: (1) Grade school or less , (2) High school , (3) Attended college but did not graduate , (4) Graduated from college , (5) Obtained a graduate or professional degree beyond college \‘7 \7 12. 13. l4 15. best l6. 17. 18. 19. 20. 79 Which one of the following best describes your family: (1) Both of my arents are alive and living to ether today , (2 ,M father is living , (3 My mother is living , (4 My parents are divorced or separated and I live with my father , (5) My parents are divorced or separated and I live with my mother Which of the following best deScribes the situation in your family when you were in high school: (1) It was naturally assumed that you would go to college‘“‘ , (2) If you wanted to go to college you were encouraged to do so by one or both parents, but it was not assumed that you would go , (3) It was not assumed that you would go to college and you received no encouragement from either parent when you decided to do so How would you rate the happiness of your parents' marriage: (1) Very happy , (2 Happy , (3) Neither happy nor unhappy , (4 Unhappy , (5) Very unhappy a. How would you rate your childhood: (1) Very happy , (2) Happy ~ , (3) Neither happy nor unhappy , (4 Unhappy , (5) Very unhappy b. How would you rate your adolescence. (1) Very happy , (2 Happy , (3) Neither happy nor unhappy , (4 Unhappy , (5) Very unhappy Which of the items in each of the following questions describes the way you feel in your family? My father is interested in what I do: (1)A1ways , 2 Usually , (3) Sometimes*Se1dom—. 5 Never . My mother is interested in what I do: (1) Always , (2) Usually , (3) Sometimes (4) Seldom , 5 Never . My father ridicules my ideas: (I) Always , (2) Usually , (3) Sometimes , (4) Seldom , (5) Never . My mother ridicules my ideas: (1) Always , (2) Usually , (3) Sometimes _, (4) Seldom , (5) Never . My father encourages me to discuss my problems with him: (I; Always , (2) Usually , (3) Sometimes , 4 Seldom , 5 Never . X7 \/ 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 30. 31. 32. 80 My mother encourages me to discuss my problems with her: 1) Always , (2 Usually , (3) Sometimes , (A) Seldom "“7"“, (5 Never ' . I think my father has my best interests at heart: (I; Always , (2; Usually , (3) Sometimes , 4 Seldom.____”, (5 Never . I think my mother has my best interests at heart: (I) Always , (2) Usually ,(3) Sometimes , 4) Seldom , (5) Never . I think my father shows more interest in my brothers and > sisters than he shows in me: (1) Always , (2) , Usually , (3) Sometimes *, (4) Seldom , ’ (5) Never , (6) Usually shows more interest in me; , - (7 I'm an only child “— I think my mother shows more interest in my brothers and sisters than she shows in me: (1) Always , (2) Usually , (3) Sometimes , (4) Seldom“ , (5) Never ' , (6) Usually shows more interest in me , 7 I'm an only child '_— Other fathers tend to Show more interest in their children than my father shows in me: (1) Completely agree , 2) Partially agree , (3) Undecided , (4) Partially disagree , (5) Completely disagrea::: Other mothers tend to show more interest in their children than my mother shows in me: '(1) Completely agree , (2) Partially agree , (3) Undecided , (4) Partially disagree ., (5) Completely disagree My father praises me when I do my work well: '(1) Always , (2) Usually , (2) Sometimes , (4) Seldom , (5) Never . My mother praises me when I do my work well: (1) Always , 2) Usually , (3) Sometimes , (4) Seldom , 5) Never . Does your father ever seem to wish that you were a different type of person. (1) Always , (2) Often , (3) Sometimes , (4) Seldom , (5) Never . Does your mother ever seem to wish that you were a different type of person: (1) Always . , (2) Often , (3) Sometimes , (4) Seldom ____, (5) Never Do you think that your father tries to understand your problems and worrieszl (1) Always , (2) Usually , (3) Sometimes , (4) Seldom , (5) Never . 81 Do you think that your mother tries to understand your_ problems and worries: (1) Always (2) Usually (3) Sometimes , (4) Seldom , (5) Never My father says and does things that make me feel that I am not trusted: (1) Always , (2) Often , (3) Sometimes , (4) Seldom , (5) Never My mother says and does things that make me feel that I am not trusted: (1) Always , (2) Often , (3) Sometimes , (A) Seldom , (5) Never THANK YOU VERY MUCH FOR YOUR COOPERATION APPENDIX B 82 83 TABLE 20 INTEGRATION WITH FATHER FOR MALES AND FOR FEMALES Per Cent Father Integration Sex N High Middle LOW Male N=l75 23 27 MO Female N=118 40 33 27 x2 = 10.43, df, 2, p < .01. Females are significantly more likely to be highly integrated with father than are males. [‘0 TABLE 1 INTEGRATION WITH MOTHER FOR MALES AND FOR FEMALES Per Cent Mother Integration Sex N High Middle Low Male N:l75 31 AA 25 Female N=118 6A 25 ll 2 X = 30.74, df, 2, p <: .001. Females are significantly more likely to be highly integrated with mother than are males. 8A 0cm LoSpoE Spa: COprpwoch ComZPon .monEom mcoEm pCmEo>mH£om Qfinmcoaumamp ocmoamficwam m ma whose .mo. am. e is .ee .Se.mH n mx .m2 .3 .6S .om.H u mx we mm ma mfinz ma mo ma meuz 30a S mm om omnz Hm mm mm SSuz manna: mH sm mm msnz ma mm mm mmnz ewfim sou manna: swam z seq mance: gwfim z tempo: spas pam8m>maco< mHmEmm pcmo pom meEo>oH£o< mam: pcoo pom coapmhmmch mMQ¢2mm mom Qz< mMQmHmo< Qz< mmmBOS mBHB ZOHBmaso< mHmEmm pcoo mom pcmEm>mH£o¢ mam: p:mo mom coapmpwmpCH mmn<2mm mom 92¢ mmfl<2 mom Bzm2m>MHmo¢ QZ< mmm8¢m EBHB ZOHB