9 v— .— g . ¢ ' I"—-‘.“ 0---...“ "s- .. wow—w”. .fl‘qu‘VM‘VC-c CW. 0 0 O > . ‘ . . » GNITIVE COMPLEXITY AND' THE. STIMULUS _. . ‘ ' ; _- ~ , PERSON JUDGED: A 'R‘E-DMMINATI'ON or ~ VALENCE AND FREQUENCY or m ' , Thesis for the Degree'of M A. ' . -' '- . '- _ _ ' MICHIGAN 8 TE UNIVERSITY- - ‘ .. ~ . IRENE IMANNé I ‘_ I _ - ..- A . . .h" d. I . . . . . O . . . I . . a!) \ I > '. I. 'l O I ~ .1 o . . I . I I o o . .. . I’vll't .X iv' I a ‘ . . I I I . . . D . . . O . . ' I . . . . . O I. O ‘~ ' _ _ . _, ‘. - o I I S C aqggsgsghfls’fi "t0 0 . . n l o I 0 c I l . . - o . a - . M I . \ o \ _, I . o . . 0'1“‘O.' . {'tstigflsmh. I'- l o a c o 0.. u o - Mi!!! . _ . .. v , . l‘ r. .. -‘. '- - ’-. .u. ‘ . . - .p.~ .. t . . . . . , . . .-- un-v . . - ‘c . . _ ~"" . . . .- . v. . . . _ “'f" .-o t .g , o . ' ' C 0-. o . - 0.. ‘a ’ - .. _- r . . . ' . _ ' .n —. - . .. ’ ‘ . ' ..- -‘~. . . ., . -. -—‘-’r. ' _ ' ’ I: o . _ ‘,- . I - . _ — -- - A v . . ‘ . ' cg... . . ... rook .- ,. ’ . . , - on . . n.n .4- ... .‘. u....-. .n“ . O-C-‘Has'k‘ \“- ,‘.'.l.'2._.‘.'-'.!;-vau" M IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII WWW \HWWHWMWWW \zmi. In 3., 3 1293 10600 4116 .. ,.‘ n K 0’“: .’ \ :4 . ‘~ '1. 3 g , . é .. ‘fn ‘ . :3 z. «.1... ”J...- . It: "‘_ I.“- :3} ‘ [i . 3 v.21,” 9": . a - 7‘ £8“??- " ‘ ' ' ' - . ’1‘? 3 '49.. I . . A"??? 0 9 it: ;-:1 ABSTRACT COGNITIVE COMPLEXITY AND THE STIMULUS PERSON JUDGED: A RE-EXAMINATION OF AFFECTIVE VALENCE AND FREQUENCY OF INTERACTION BY Irene T. Mann This study re-examined research regarding complexity of response as a function of the stimulus person judged. More specifically, it dealt with the vigilance hypothesis, the frequency of interaction hypo- thesis and the neutral affect hypothesis, which have made differential predictions regarding complexity of response. The study attempted to show that results supporting the three interpretations were related to the measure of cognitive complexity employed. Accordingly, two different measures of complexity were used and also two sets of role figures, yielding four groups. It was hypothesized that the vigilance effect would occur only when evaluative traits were used in adminis- tering the Rep Test measure of complexity; in using nonevaluative traits on the Rep Test and another measure of complexity, H/Hmax, no such vigilance effect would be found. Assumptions regarding the affective valences of the ten role figures conventionally presented in the Rep Test were also examined. Finally, given that the vigilance effect was found to be measure-specific, this study examined supportive evidence for the frequency of interaction hypothesis and the neutral Irene T. Mann affect hypothesis. Four groups of 30 participants each were tested. Participants in Groups 1 and 2 responded to Bieri's ten role figures, and the Rep Test and the H/Hmax measure of complexity, respectively, were administered. Participants in Groups 3 and 4 responded to six role descriptions that varied in terms of affective valence and know- ledge of the person; the Rep Test and the H/Hmax measure of complexity, respectively, were administered. Results generally supported previous assumptions regarding the affective valence of the ten role figures; other predictions were also confirmed. Results for the Rep Test using evaluative traits supported the vigilance hypothesis. For the Rep Test using nonevaluative traits, no strong pattern of response emerged. Results for the H/Hmax measure of complexity supported the frequency of interaction hypothesis. Well known persons were more highly dif- ferentiated. The data indicated no support for the neutral affect interpretation. However, great variability in the strength and dir- ection of relationships between complexity and the variables of main interest, liking and knowledge of the person, for the separate role figures and individual participants was noted; thus, overall trends were quite weak. Results in this area of research depend on the mea- sures used, masking to a great extent variance which is not accounted for by the variables chosen for study. The domain specificity notion was briefly discussed. However, this notion did not fully account for a subsequent analysis which indicated low generality of complexity of response for individual participants and across role figures. A more exact definition of complexity of response in this context needs to be developed. COGNITIVE COMPLEXITY AND THE STIMULUS PERSON JUDGED: A RE-EXAMINATION OF AFFECTIVE VALENCE AND FREQUENCY OF INTERACTION BY h u“ U" 0‘ Irene T. Mann A THESIS Subndtted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1976 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to gratefully acknowledge those persons who participated in my study, for without them it would not have been possible. I would also like to express my deep appreciation to Dr. Barbara Riemer, Dr. James Phillips, and Dr. Ellen Strommen for their guidance and support. Special thanks also go to Bill Brown for computer assistance and Sue Weesner for secretarial assistance. Lastly, my mother, my brother Joe, and my friend Deb deserve my deepest gratitude for their loving support. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . CHAPTER I 0 INTRODUCTION 0 O O C O O O O 0 Two Measures of Complexity . . . . . Comparison of the Two Measures . . . Vigilance-Survival Hypothesis . . . . Justification Hypothesis . . . . . . Frequency of Interaction Hypothesis . Neutral Affect Hypothesis . . . . . . The Rep Test: Methodological Issues Aim of the Present Study . . . . . . CHAPTER II. METHOD . . . . . . . . . . . Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . Materials . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Data Preparation . . . . . . . . . . Data Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . CHAPTER III. RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . Group 1 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Group 2 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Group 3 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Group 4 . . . . . . . .'. . . . . . . Groups 3 and 4 Correlations . . . . . Overall Comparison . . . . . . . . . CHAPTER IV. DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . Interpretation . . . . . . . . . . . Implications . . . . . . . . . . . . smary o o o o o o u o o o o o o o 0 LIST OF REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDICES O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O 0 iii Page iv 10 ll 14 16 20 25 25 25 27 28 29 31 31 37 44 45 46 47 48 48 56 56 59 62 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1 Mean Ratings and Scores for Group l Role Figures . . . 33 2 Stepwise Regression Analysis for Group 1 . . . . . . . 34 3 Correlations Among Ratings and Complexity Scores for Group 1 Role Figures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 4 Mean Ratings and Scores for Group 2 Role Figures . . . 4O 5 Stepwise Regression Analysis for Group 2 . . . . . . . 41 6 Correlations Among Ratings and Complexity Score . for Group 2 Role Figures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 7 Group 3. Means and Standard Deviations of Complexity Scores ' Evaluative Traits . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45 8 Group 3. Means and Standard Deviations of Complexity Scores - Nonevaluative Traits . . . . . . . . . . . . 46 9 Group 4. Means and Standard Deviations of H/Hmax Scores . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 iv LIST OF APPENDICES Appendix Page A PROVIDED BIPOLAR CONSTRUCTS ON THE REP TEST . . . . 62 B SAMPLE PAGES AND SCORING GRIDFORM FOR THE REP TEST GROUPS 1 AND 3 O I O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O 6 3 C SAMPLE SHEETS AND INSTRUCTIONS--GROUPS 2 AND 4 . . 66 D CORRELATIONS FOR INDIVIDUAL PARTICIPANTS IN GROUP 1 68 E ADDITIONAL DATA FOR GROUP 2 (BIERI'S TEN ROLE FIGURES AND H/Hmax MEASURE) . . . . . . . . . . . . 69 F ADDITIONAL DATA FOR GROUP 3 (REICH'S SIX ROLE , FIGURES AND REP TEST) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72 G ADDITIONAL DATA FOR GROUP 4 (REICH'S SIX ROLE FIGURES AND H/Hmax MEASURE) . . . . . . . . . . . . 74 H OVERALL CORRELATIONS FOR EACH GROUP . . . . . . . . 78 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION A great deal of human activity is spent in getting to know other persons and thinking about them. The area of person perception or social cognition analyzes such activity. Research in this area has generally focused on the "processes by which man comes to know and think about other persons, their characteristics, qualities and inner states,“ [Tagiuri, 1969, p. 429] and more specifically on the characteristics of the perceiver that might affect such processes. One important characteristic of the per- ceiver is degree of differentiation or the "tendency to make fine distinctions among people and thus to perceive them as different from one another" [Shrauger and Altrocchi, 1964, p. 292]. Bieri [1955] defined differentiation as the number of constructs or attri- butes constituting a given cognitive structure. The cognitive struc- ture or domain of interest was the interpersonal construct system. The cognitively complex person utilizes a more highly differentiated construct system than a cognitively simple person. It has been shown that cognitively complex persons and cognitively simple persons per- form differently when given impression formation tasks [Nidorf and Crockett,l965; Mayo and Crockett, 1964; Shrauger, 1967], in discrimin- ating behavioral stimuli and making judgments [Bieri, Atkins, Leaman, Miller and Tripodi, 1966], and in predicting how others will respond [Shrauger and Altrocchi, 1964]. Complexity has not only received consideration as a personality variable; researchers have also examined complexity of response to various stimuli. Many researchers have focused on complexity of re- sponse to stimulus persons differing in affective valence. This work has been approached in the following ways: 1) in person perception, a concern with the differentiation skills of the perceiver, and the per- ceiver's use of personal constructs; 2) cognitive complexity as a characteristic of cognitive structure and its generality across and within domains; 3) perceptual defense and vigilance applied to the perception of persons; 4) affective arousal and its effect on discrimin- ation of persons. A fifth approach is information theory. Researchers studying cognitive structure have adopted measures of amount of infor- mation to assess dimensionality in terms of distinctions yielded by a system of groupingscnrcategories [Scott, 1962, 1969]. The first approach was briefly introduced. In regard to the second approach, generality of cognitive complexity has been the major issue. Crockett [1965] reviewed research offering some support for the following hypotheses: l) complexity in one domain does not neces- sarily imply complexity in other domains; 2) persons for whom inter- personal relations are functionally important have more complex systems than individuals for whom interpersonal relations are less important; 3) an individual may show differential complexity with respect to dif- ferent categories of persons depending on the extent of interaction with them. The latter notion underlies the frequency of interaction hypothesis, which indicates that one is more complex in responding to well known and liked persons since these persons are socially close and more familiar. Crockettfisconclusions contradicted the findings of Miller and Bieri [1965] and Irwin, Tripodi and Bieri [1967], who found that par- ticipants responded to negative role figures in a more complex manner. Their studies displayed a concern with perceptual effects and discrimination. The vigilance hypothesis, called upon to explain greater differentiation of negative role figures, was borrowed from notions of perceptual defense and vigilance in the area of perception [Erdelyi, 1974], and applied to person perception, offering a ready- made and perhaps counter-intuitive explanation for obtained results. Research representing the fourth approach has dealt with affective arousal and how this affects judgments and discriminability of cues [Bieri, 1967]. One view holds that an increase in affective arousal decreases the individual's discrimination of stimuli. This view served as the basis for yet another hypothesis, the neutral affect hypothesis, which makes predictions contrary to both the vigilance hypothesis and the frequency of interaction hypothesis. The neutral affect hypothesis suggests that persons of neutral affect will be perceived in a more complex manner. Liked or disliked persons are more arousing affectively and are less well differentiated or discriminated. The three hypotheses mentioned, the vigilance hypothesis, the frequency of interaction hypothesis and the neutral affect hypothesis, posit different processes underlying responses to stimulus persons when responses are examined in terms of degree of differentiation or complexity. They also make differential predictions regarding which stimulus persons will be more highly differentiated. In one case, negative persons will be more highly differentiated, in another case positive persons, and in another, persons of neutral valence. All three hypotheses have found some support. The hypotheses were de— rived from different approaches, as outlined above. There have also been different approaches to the measurement and definition of come plexity in this context. This problem of defining and measuring complexity is a major concern for the research area. Bieri [1961] pointed out that it is unclear whether cognitive complexity is a differentiation concept exclusively or relates to organizational properties of the cogni- tive system as well. A factor analysis c>f several widely used mea- sures of complexity in the interpersonal domain and several related. measures by Vannoy [1965], revealed that cognitive complexity is not a unitary trait as measured by the various paper and pencil tests. vannoy contended that existing measures tap different variables or aspects of complexity; these aspects have not as yet been clearly defined. Given this state of affairs, one must proceed with extreme caur tion in this area; an imprecise definition of complexity permits dif- ferent approaches to its measurement. The Role Construct Repetory Test (RCRT or Rep Test), as outlined by Kelly [1955], and subsequently modified by Tripodi and Bieri [1963] and Bieri, et a1. [1966], has been one of the most widely used measures of complexity. In addition to providing a method to assess an individual's cognitive complexity in the interpersonal realm, the Rep Test has been used to measure complexity of response to role figures differing in affective valence. The work of Scott [1962] represents a slightly different approach to the measurement of complexity than that of the Rep Test. The approach was derived from information theory [Attneave, 1959]. The partici- pant sorts a number of items; an index, based on the dispersion of objects over a set of distinctions yielded by the category system, is then computed. Reich [1969] utilized such a task in assessing come plexity of response to persons differing in affective valence. Results of studies in this area do not overwhelmingly confirm any one of the hypotheses. It is extremely difficult to determine the validity of the processes posited because researchers have not maintained comparability of method and complexity measures. There appears to be some relation between the measure and the results sup- porting a particular hypothesis, such that certain measures consis- tently yield certain results. The aim of the present study was to confirm this. To this end, two different measures of complexity were examined, Bieri's Role Construct Repetory Test and information theory's index of relative entropy of H/Hmax. The two measures will be briefly discussed; then research findings relevant to them as dependent mea- sures in examining complexity of response to stimulus persons will be presented. Two Measures of Complexity Rep Test The Rep Test is based on Kelly's theory of personality [1955] which assumed that every person has a number of personal constructs to cognize and perceive others. A construct is defined as a dimension for construing the way in which persons are alike and different. Examples of bipolar constructs are "considerate-inconsiderate" and "dominant—submissive." The Rep Test taps the individual's system of constructs, and then requires the participant to judge a number of persons on these dimensions. When constructs are provided for the participant it is assumed that they are representative of the per- son's own constructs [Bieri, et al., 1966]. Tripodi and Bieri [1963] reported that providing constructs yielded results similar to results found when the procedure involved use of the participants' own constructs. Bieri, et al. [1966] described the scoring of the Rep Test. Participants are presented with a 10 X 10 grid. Each of the ten columns is identified by a certain role figure. Ten rows of bipolar constructs are provided. The participant uses a six-point Likert scale to rate the ten role figures on the first construct, then rates all ten on the second construct, and so on, for a total of 100 ratings. Complexity is measured by comparing each rating in a row with the rating directly below it for the same person and thereon for each rating in the column, yielding 45 comparisons and for the entire grid, 450 comparisons. A score of one is given for exact agreement of rat- ings on any one person. Thus, the highest possible score is 450, indicating simplicity or minimal differentiation. The role figures commonly used are: Self, Mother, Father, Friend of the Same Sex, Friend of the Opposite Sex, Boss, Person You Dislike, Person You'd Like to Help, Person With Whom You Feel Most Uncomfortable and Person Difficult to Understand. H/Hmax This measure of complexity was derived from information theory [Attneave, 1959]. Participants list and group adjectives describing others. The number of adjectives written and the number of categories created are counted. For example, one might write seven adjectives (jealous, stingey, realistic, thoughtful, modest, kind, considerate) and place the first three in one group and the last four in another group. A measure of dispersion, H, of the items over the group com— binations is calculated. The ratio, R, or relative entropy is obtained by expressing H in ratio to the maximum H possible with a certain number of categories (i.e., Hmax is obtained when all categories have equal frequencies). The ratio R or H/Hmax varies between 0.00 and +1.00, regardless of the number of categories [Scott, 1962]. As H/Hmax approaches +1.00, complexity of response is greater. H/Hmax corrects for verbal fluency; the measure is most useful when either the number of stimuli or the number of categories varies across parti- cipants. If a person creates a large number of categories and dis- tributes the items equally over all categories, there is a high degree of uncertainty and a low degree of structure, hence more ambiguity and greater complexity of response. H/Hmax is a measure of categorization. It assesses departure from distributional equality and is not concerned with the content of the response; it taps a purely structural property [Scott, 1962]. If items are distributed equally over the categories, the amount of infor- mation yielded is low, and it is uncertain, given any one item, which category it belongs in. This uncertainty connotes greater ambiguity and hence complexity of response. Glixman [1965] used H/Hmax as a measure of the degree of structure in a domain. Scott [1966] spoke of H as a measure of cognitive dimensionality; "the number of groups- worth of information can be represented as the dispersion of the. objects over the set of distinctions yielded by the category system" [p. 408]. Comparison of the Two Measures The two measures appear to require different types of responses from1participants. The Rep Test requires the individual to use con- struct dimensions to discriminate among persons. H/Hmax has little to do with how construction dimensions are used to discriminate among. others. With the H/Hmax measure, once the participant has written adjectives to describe a certain person, the adjectives become the stimuli. Although the individual may have the stimulus person in mind when creating the groupings, it might also be the case that the participant responds solely to the adjectives. The adjectives for each role figure are grouped separately by the participant, so that each role figure serves as a separate domain. This does not serve to assess how well the individual is discriminating or differentiating gmggq_others. Although Reich [1969] states that "discrimination in cognitive research typically is regarded as being the process in whidh the person uses a set of independent dimensions or characteristics to respond to stimuli, responding differently to the different classes but similarly to those stimuli within each class," [p. 107] it is not exactly clear how H/Hmax measures this. Irwin, Tripodi and Bieri [1967] pointed out that "such a task [written descriptions of persons] makes no assessment of how functionally different the constructs may be inasmuch as the subject is not required to actually use these dimen- sions to discriminate between individuals," [p. 446] which is what the Rep Test procedure purportedly accomplishes. The lack of relationship between various measures of complexity leads one to suspect that the measures are not convergent. Miller [1969] found nonsignificant correlations of .009 for males and -.113 for females between Bieri's and Crockett's procedures. Wilkins and Epting [1974] reported a correlation of -.03. Vannoy [1965] reported a correlation of .06 between the Rep Test and number of categories used to group 12 significant persons. The so-called "measures of complexity" appear to tap different aspects of what might be called complexity. Vigilance-Survival Hypothesis Miller and Bieri [1965], using the Rep Test, found that parti- cipants were less complex in judging persons who were close to them and toward whom they were expected to relate positively (Self, Mother Father, Friend of the Same Sex, Friend of the Opposite Sex) than more socially distant persons toward whom the participant was expected to relate negatively (Persons You Dislike, Person You'd Like to Help, Boss, Person With Whom You Feel Most Uncomfortable, Person Difficult to Understand). A "vigilance" hypothesis was advanced to explain these results, i.e., differentiation serves an adaptive function for antici- pating and identifying the behavior of more remote and threatening persons. Differentiation helps to clarify the nature of the threat. 1b Irwin, Tripodi and Bieri [1967] obtained similar results using the same technique to measure differentiation and two different sets of stimuli. In Study I participants provided names for four liked housemates (same sex), four housemates of neutral valence (same sex), and four disliked housemates (same sex). In Study II partici- pants provided names for eight role categories: closest friend (male and female), person you admire (male and female), person you find hard to like (male and female) and person with whom you feel most uncomfortable (male and female). Positive figures were differentiated significantly less well than negative figures. Also, positive figures were differentiated less well than neutral figures and neutral figures were less well differentiated than negative figures. Results supporting the vigilance hypothesis were also reported by Miller [1968] and Wilkins, Epting and VanDeRiet [1972]. In both studies the stimuli were the ten role figures presented by Miller and Bieri [1965]. Soucar and DuCette [1971] found greater differentiation of disliked political figures than liked political figures, and Soucar [1970] reported higher complexity scores among high school students for disliked teachers than for liked teachers. Justification Hypothesis A justification hypothesis was advanced by Koenig [1971] which makes the same prediction as does the vigilance hypothesis, i.e., nega- tive persons will be more highly differentiated than positive persons. However, the justification hypothesis posits a different underlying mechanism for this effect. According to Koenig, there is "a trait in our culture and perhaps others as well, which stresses being friendly, 11 and liking other people" [p. 385]. Dislike for someone requires justification; one should be able to provide good reasons for dislike. This implies that negative persons will be more highly differentiated and discriminated, so that, if required, one can justify one's dis- like. Since justification for liking is not expected, positive per- sons would not be as highly differentiated as negative persons. Soucar and DuCette [1972] demonstrated that differentiation can be manipulated in an experimental session by asking participants to defend their choices of liked and disliked persons. When participants were asked to justify their choice of a liked person, differentiation (as assessed by the Rep Test) of these persons increased. Results were less clearcut when participants were asked to justify their choice of disliked persons, primarily because of problems with the scale used. The authors concluded that complexity may not be influr enced by affect per se, but may be influenced by committing oneself to a statement concerning like or dislike; the threat of postdecisional justification increases complexity of response. Finally, Koenig and Seaman [1974b] examined both the vigilance and justification effects. The authors concluded that they were un- related, but both contributed to increased complexity of response. Frequency of Interaction Hypothesis Contrary to results supporting a vigilance or justification inter- pretation, Supnick [1964; as reported by Crockett, 1965] found that participants used more constructs to describe persons they liked than to describe persons they disliked and to describe peers than to describe 12 older persons. She posited a"frequency of interaction" hypothesis to explain the results, i.e., participants differentiated more highly the persons they knew well or liked than less well known or disliked persons because they came into more contact with the former persons and avoided the latter persons. Her resultso ow.HH mq.qH mN.m n~.o mm.q n¢.q euwz .mEoodD Hmmm cow comumm mw.a no.8H mo.m oa.m om.m Aa.N swam on mega p.sow aomumm mm.oa NH.NH mm.q oo.m mn.q mm.m tcmumumpsa cu pacofimmwa compom nn.w CH.SH mm.q up.“ om.c mo.w mxHHmHn sow cowumm mm.oa nw.ma oo.m o~.o mw.q oq.¢ mmom mm.oa mo.om mm.~ mm.N 0H.m nw.a xmm muumommo ozu mo pcoaum OH.0H ma.ma mq.N nn.m mo.N om.a wow meow can mo pcmaum mm.m mm.wa mm.~ mm.~ ow.H uwnumm mm.m mq.ma wo.m mm.H mw.H umnuoz mm.oa m~.N~ oq.~ m~.~ mamm mufimue .Hm>mcoz wuamua .Hm>m mwpmHBocM acme coauom wdwxaq 1%ufixmaaaoo thuflondEoo oo>ao>nH luwuaH H manna woudwam oaom H acouu you monocm can mwcfiumm can: 34 ummu pmawmunosu .mo. v a « ,NNo. ooo. oNo. ooo. oHo. Hoo. unmao>ao>uH .o moo. oHo. omo. moo. ado. moo. cowuumumuaH .m ooH.- «om. ooh. ooo. oHo. ooo.- .wanxwa .N HHH. ooH. omo.~ mHo. mHo. «AH. «womasoae .H mufiwua m>fiumaam>ocoz thuwxmamsou Noo.- moo. woo. woo. HoH. ooodu wwwuasoae .4 AAA. ooo. moo.H moo. ooo. ooH.- unmam>ao>aH .m BAH. Hoo. AHN.¢ oHo. soo. Hoo.u cowuomumuaH .N mmo.- ooo. oHH.AH oko. oko. «okm.u manage .H muamufi o>fiumsam>m muom mucmoHMAcwwm Housm cu m wwcmno mumcvm m u mamafim mumsvm m Imuwxoamaoo H macho you mwmhamc< doommouwmm mmasdmum N magma 35 relationships were between liking and involvement ratings, r = .729, and interaction and involvement ratings, r = .736. (See Appendix H.) Complexity on Evaluative Traits The relationship between liking and complexity was not consistent for all of the role figures (see Table 3). For four of the role figures, Friend of the Same Sex, Friend of the Opposite Sex, Person You Dislike, and Person Difficult to Understand, there was essentially no relation- ship; for the other role figures there were negative correlations between liking and complexity. Self (r.= -.454) and Mother (r = -.362) displayed the strongest relationships. The negative relationship indi- cated that participants were less complex in responding to liked per- sons and were more complex in reponding to disliked persons. The average of these correlations (using an r to z transformation; [McNemar, 1962]) was -.l66, a significant negative relationship. In examining the correlations for individual participants, most were negative; about 16% were positive. (See Appendix D for these correla- tions.) The relationship between knowledge ratings and complexity scores was weak. For five of the role figures the relationship was essentially zero; for the other five role figures the correlations were nonsigni- ficant and positive. The average correlation was essentially zero. For the individual participants' correlations, the majority were non- significant and negative, but approximately 25% were positive, indica- ting a fair amount of individual variability in this regard. Tenta- tively, it appeared that while for individual participants the response to less well known persons was more complex, for certain individuals 36 the relationship was reversed, and persons known slightly were less well differentiated than well known persons. Complexity on Nonevaluative Traits The relationship between liking and complexity was not consistent for all of the role figures (see Table 3). For two role figures the relationship was zero; for the remaining role figures equal numbers showed positive and negative correlations. The stronger relations, however, were positive, indicating more complex responses to liked persons. The average of these correlations was essentially zero. The correlations for individual participants were both positive and nega- tive, although the negative correlations outnumbered the positive (see Appendix D). Again, this suggested a trend opposite to that trend suggested by the correlations for some of the role figures. The relationship between complexity and knowledge was only slightly more consistent. For five of the role figures the correlation between knowledge ratings and complexity scores was positive, indica- ting more complexity in responding to better known role figures. The average correlation, however, was still essentially zero. For individual participants, both a positive and negative relationship between the two variables appeared to be Operating, indicating, again, individual variability in response. The relationship between the two complexity scores (one on evalu- ative traits, the other on nonevaluative traits) was nonsignificant for each of the role figures. For Mother, Friend of the Opposite Sex and Person You Dislike, the correlation between the two scores was low and negative; for the other role figures the correlation was zero or 37 positive. The correlations for individual participants were both positive and negative. Conclusion Thus, for complexity on evaluative traits, a vigilant or justi- fication effect appeared to occur; participants seemed to be respond- ing to negative role figures in a more complex manner. However, for individual role figures and individual participants such a relation- ship often was not apparent. For complexity on nonevaluative traits, relationships were not at all clear cut. However, the trend appeared to be opposite to the trend in results for complexity scores on evalua- tive traits. For nonevaluative traits, the tendency to be more complex in responding to well liked and well known role figures was very weak and tentative at best, again hiding a great deal of variability both for the role figures and for individual participants. Complexity scores on evaluative and nonevaluative traits were not related. Group 2 Participants in Group 2 responded to Bieri's ten role figures. Number of adjectives, number of categories and H/Hmax scores were examined. Positive and Negative Role Figures Mean liking ratings for the role figures showed a pattern similar to that for Group 1: Boss, Person You Dislike, Person Difficult to Understand and Person With Whom You Feel Most Uncomfortable were rated more negatively than the other role figures. In general, although the pattern was not as consistent as it was for Group 1, participants also 38 on u z ummu tmafimulosu .mo. v a « «no. moo. «we. owe. «oca.l mcoaumamuuoo. owmuo>< oAH. moo. ooo.- ooo.. HoH.- gun: .oaouao down now somuom How. moH. oHN. oko.n ooo.- (mama on mega v.90w somumm who. omm. Ham. mom. (mono. oamumumoa: on “Hoodoofio commas ooH.- Hem. Nam. Hmo.n Noe. mxAHmHo cow cowumm me. mnH.| Hmc. «No. Hom.l mmom NSN.- ooo.- AAN.- ooH. ooo. woo muamoaao no oamfium mew. nos. wees. ooN. ooo.- xmm memo mo ocmaum moo. Hmo. HoH.- okH. oo~.- “means ~mH.- ooo. Hmo.n m-.- :mom.- Hugues mom. soo.- ooH.- ooo.- «mmo.- wamm .Hm>mcoZI.Eoo o .Hm>mu.aoo .Hm>oCOZI.Eoo .Hm>osozl.aou o .3oae o wafixaq .Hm>MI.aou w .BOaM .Hm>m1.eoo o waexfia newsman mHom H ozone How mouoom hufimeosou can wwcaumm wcoafi msowumamuuoo m manna 39 gave these role figures low ratings in terms of interaction, involve- ment and knowledge. Boss, Person You Dislike and Person With Whom You Feel Most Uncomfortable were the role figures with the lowest means for number of adjectives, number of categories and the H/Hmax scores. The latter indicated that participants were somewhat more complex in responding to positive and well-known role figures. (See Table 4 for the mean ratings.) Overall Analysis For this group a negative correlation signified that as complex- ity increased there was increased liking, or the converse. Number of adjectives correlated negatively and significantly with ratings of . liking, interaction, involvement and knowledge. Participants wrote more adjectives for persons they liked, interacted frequently with, were involved with and knew well. The same pattern of correlations held for number of categories and H/Hmax scores. A stepwise multiple regres- sion analysis showed that for number of adjectives, the interaction variable explained 7% of the variance while the other three variables contributed only another 2%. For the number of categories, the inter— action variable acounted for 4% of the variance and the remaining variables contributed 2%. In regard to H/Hmax, involvement contributed 5% of the variance; the other variables accounted for an additional 1%. (See Table 5.) The correlations among the ratings of liking, interaction, involve- ment, and knowledge for each of the role figures were positive. The overall correlations among the ratings were all positive and significant, with the strongest relationships between liking and involvement (r = .709) 40 moo. 0H.~ m<.o wH.q on.< mw.¢ mm.m menu: A Hamum>o mum. mn.H nm.m oa.c mm.n mn.o oo.m :uHS .msoocp Hook cow somuom emu. o~.~ o~.m nc.m mq.q mo.q mc.N can: ou oxfiq p.3ow somwwm «no. o~.~ ma.o nm.m o¢.q m~.q no.m ensconced: cu uacuammwa somuom Hum. ow.H nn.m mm.m sq.m no.n mm.h mxwamfio ‘ now common owe. no.H no.m oo.n 5H.n nm.m mm.¢ mmom me. mm.~ om.n oo.~ mo.m nm.~ mo.H Rom ouamommo mo panama Ono. no.~ no.0 nn.m oa.m no.m NH.~ Rom mEMm mo oceans nmn. mm.~ ow.o o~.m ow.~ nn.H nonumm own. mH.N ca.n om.N MH.N mo.H “memo: mmn. oq.~ om.n no.~ om.~ maom xmsm\m moauowoumo mm>auoont< omega acme cognac .Awwfiqu mo Honssz mo Honacz Iaoax I~>Ho>uH lumuaH o maoma mousmfim oaom N macaw mom mouoom tam mwcfiumm one: ammo toawauloau .no. v a a 41. .. oao. so. ooo. ono. «aoa.- amongsoce .o oko.- ooo. no. ~oo. ooo. «ooo.- coauuauuuoo .n koo.- man. so. ooo. kno. .oo~.- mousse .~ ooo.- goo. on.HH omo. «no. .o-.- uooau>aopua .~ NEH ooo. How. oo. ooo. Noo. «no~.- unusu>qo>ua .4 «no.1 own. on. Noo. ~oo. wo~u.: «woodsoau .n ANA.- oAH. on.~ «no. ooo. «no~.- mowed; .N nn~.- moo. on.oH ooo. ooo. «o-.- cowuuuuooas .H nuuuowouuu uo nonasz ooo.: are. co. «co. . soc. cama.n mcfixua .Q mmo. who. mu. ooc. mac. «wm~.| unfilu>do>um .n omH.I mmo. as.n oao. «mo. «enN.1 cavedaouu .N cn~.1 coo. an.sn who. Qua. «th.| acquuuuuuau .H oo>wuuofiv< uo amass: noon oucuuwuwswum yucca cu m uumuao ouasam m u omwmwm vacuum a N macaw pom mwmhama< scammmummm mmwsdmum m manna 42 and interaction and involvement (r = .764), as in Group 1. (See Appendix H.) Number of Adjectives and Number of Categories See Appendix E for a discussion and presentation of data for number of adjectives and number of categories. H/Hmax The relationship between H/Hmax and liking ratings was not con- sistent for all of the role figures; for three role figures, Person You Dislike, Person Difficult to Understand and Person You Would Like to Help, the correlation was essentially zero; for two role figures, Self and Boss, the relationship was positive, and for the remainder a negative relationship between H/Hmax and liking was indicated. The latter negative relationship suggested that the greater the liking, the higher was H/Hmax, and the greater the complexity of response. The average of these correlations was essentially zero. For individual participants, the correlation between the two variables was negative in approximately 80% of the cases. Knowledge ratings and H/Hmax were negatively correlated for four of the role figures and positively correlated for three others. The negative correlations seemed to be slightly stronger than the positive correlations. For about 60% of the participants, the correlation between knowledge ratings and H/Hmax scores was negative. Those par- ticipants who indicated greater knowledge of the stimulus person responded more complexly than those indicating lesser knowledge. Thus, data for Group 2 indicated that liked and well known persons 43 Table 6 Correlations Among Ratings and Complexity for Group 2 Role Figures Score ‘1 Liking & Know. & E H/Hmax H/Hmax ? 3 Self .109 .292 % Mother -.435* -.364* 2 Father -.304 -.257 Friend of Same Sex —.276 -.096 4 Friend of ' Qpposite Sex -.213 -.257 . Boss .397* .048 4 Person You i Dislike -.034 .080 j Person Difficult 7 to Understand .054 .183 ' Person You'd Like to Help -.075 .199 Person You Feel Uncomf. With —.168 —.134 Average Correlations -.098 -.032 1 * p < .05, two-tailed test N = 30 44 were responded to in a more complex way than disliked and less well known persons, supporting a frequency of interaction interpretation. Group 3 Participants in Group 3 responded to six role figures: Person You Know Well and Like, Know Well and Dislike, Know Well and Feel Neutral Toward, Know Slightly and Like, Know Slightly and Dislike, Know Slightly and Feel Neutral Toward. Complexity scores came from the Rep Test, using evaluative and nonevaluative traits. Manipulation Check Mean ratings on liking, interaction, involvement and knowledge of each of the role figures served as a check. For liking and know; ledge, mean ratings followed the pattern indicated by the role figure descriptions to a satisfactory extent. Interaction and involvement ratings were less clearcut. (See Appendix F.) Evaluative Traits An analysis of variance performed on complexity scores on evalua- tive traits as the dependent measure showed a significant main effect for Affect (F(2/56) = 17.115, p < .0005). Participants were significantly more complex in responding to disliked persons (M = 13.02) and neutral persons (M = 15.38) than in responding to liked persons (M = 19.18), as shown by a Newman-Keuls test with p < .01. The former two means did not differ significantly from each other. There was also a signi- ficant Sex X Affect interaction (F(2/56) = 3.201, p < .05). Females were significantly more complex than males in responding to the neutral role figures. For males, the means for liked and neutral persons did 45 not differ significantly from each other; females were significantly more complex in responding to the neutral figures than to the liked role figures. (See Table 7 for cell means.) Tafle7 Group 3 Means and Standard Deviations of Complexity Scores Evaluative Traits Liked Disliked Neutral Participants Role Figures Role Figures Role Figures Male M 18.97 M 12.93 M 17.40 16.43 SD 6.15 SD 5.56 SD 7.78 Female M 20.77 M 13.10 M 13.37 15.74 SD 6.49 SD 7.13 SD 4.90 19.87 13.02 15.38 Nonevaluative Traits The analysis of variance for complexity scores on nonevaluative traits resulted in no significant interactions or main effects. How- ever, the main effect for Affect approached significance (p < .10). Inspection of means showed that the trend was toward greater differ- entiation of disliked persons, followed by liked and neutral persons. (See Table 8.) Group 4 Participants in Group 4 responded to six role figures. Number of adjectives, number of categories and H/Hmax scores were the depen— dent measures. 46 Tafle8 Group 3 Means and Standard Deviations of Complexity Scores - Nonevaluative Traits Role Figures Liked Disliked Neutral Kn W 11 M . 3 . . own e 10 0 M 9 67 M 10 53 10.08 SD 3.79 SD 2.87 7 SD 4.44 Known Slightly M 11.17 M 8.73 M 11.50 10 47 SD 6.65 SD 3.37 SD 5.04 10.60 9.20 11.02 Manipulation Check Mean ratings for liking and knowledge were consistent with the role figure descriptions. (See Appendix G.) Number of Adjectives and Number of Categories A discussion and presentation of the data for number of adjec- tives and number of categories as dependent measures appear in Appendix G. H/Hmax There was a significant main effect for Knowledge (F(l/28) = 16.633, p < .01). Participants were more complex in responding to well known persons (M = .76) than to persons known slightly (M = .55). (See Table 9.) Groups 3 and 4 Correlations The pattern of correlations among the ratings and complexity scores for the role figures in Groups 3 and 4 (shown in Appendices 47 Table 9 Group 4 Means and Standard Deviations of H/Hmax Scores Role Figures Liked Disliked Neutral Known Well M .792 M .804 M .685 760 SD .333 SD .295 SD .424 Known Slightly M .513 M .593 M .560 555 SD .467 SD .437 SD .467 .653 .698 .623 F and G) showed much variability, a pattern similar to the pattern found for the role figures in Groups 1 and 2. This indicated that the relationship between complexity of response and liking or know- ledge ratings was not consistent for all of the role figures. Overall Comparison Comparison of Groups 1 and 3 in terms of their respective over- all correlations (see Appendix L), showed that none of the correlations differed significantly from each other. For Groups 2 and 4, one of the comparisons showed a significant difference. For Group 4 the correlation between liking and H/Hmax was .068, while for Group 2 the correlation between liking and H/Hmax was -.209. The two corre- lations differed significantly (p < .05). CHAPTER IV DISCUSSION Interpretation 1) Results from Groups 1 and 2 (in both cases Bieri's ten role figures were presented to participants) indicated that the role figures previously assumed by researchers to be positive or negative in affec- tive valence seemed to have similar valences for the participants. In ranking the role figures in terms of their mean liking ratings, Mother, Father, Self, Friend of the Opposite Sex and Friend of the . Same Sex were the five most positively rated role figures while Boss, Person you Dislike, Person You would Like to Help, Person Difficult to Understand and Person With Whom You Feel Most Uncomfortable were the five least positively rated role figures, thus offering some support for prior assumptions with a college sample. However, although the latter five.role figures were rated less positively than the former, they were by no means rated negatively. Except for Person You Dislike, which received the highest ratings of dislike (8.03 and 7.93), the other four "negative" role figures were actually rated more neutrally than negatively. Such a pattern might indicate a response bias on the part of the participants to use more often the positive end of the ten point scales. Or it might indicate that only one of the role figures was actually negative in valence for the participants. The only conclusion permissible is that in terms of rankings, the role figures fell into the positive and negative subsets assumed in prior 48 49 research. 2) The results for Group 1 (Bieri's ten role figures and the Rep Test) indicated that a vigilance or justification effect did occur when evaluative traits were used in administering the Rep Test, as predicted. The prediction was based on Shepherd's observation [1972] that the tendency to use positively evaluated words more readily than negatively evaluated words (the Pollyanna effect) operates in complet ing the Rep Test. Positively evaluated words would be assigned to positive persons, producing redundancy in response and yielding a low differentiation score, while both positively and negatively evaluated words would be assigned to negative role figures, producing higher differentiation scores for the negative persons. When nonevaluative traits were used, a vigilance effect did not appear, and in fact a very weak relationship between knowledge and complexity and liking and complexity emerged, opposite to that of the vigilance effect, i.e., participants tended to be more complex in responding to role figures they rated as liking and knowing well than to role figures they rated as disliking and knowing only slightly. Thus, in comparing results for evaluative and nonevaluative traits on the Rep Test, it was obvious that, as Shepherd [1972] reported, results were markedly different. Only in using evaluative traits was the vigilance effect supported. Shepherd's nonevaluative traits and the additional nonevaluative traits used in this study were from the middle range in favorability on Anderson's list of personality-trait words. In rating persons of positive or negative valence on these non- evaluative traits, the participant would not find words ordinarily applied to positive or negative persons. This was demonstrated by Lott, 50 Lott, Reed and Crow [1970]. The tendency to use certain words to describe liked persons and certain words to describe disliked persons would be thwarted. Participants confronted words that perhaps they applied to neither liked or disliked persons and hence may have answered in a random manner or made inferences regarding traits they ordinarily did not apply to the persons they were rating. If parti- cipants were answering randomly, one would expect lower numerical scores or greater complexity of response; this was supported by the the data. The mean complexity score for nonevaluative traits was much lower than the mean complexity score for evaluative traits in Group 1. Results for Group 3 (Reich's six role descriptions and the Rep Test) offered partial confirmation for Group 1 results. For complexity on evaluative traits, there was a main effect for Affect; negative (and neutral) role figures were differentiated to a greater extent than liked role figures, again supporting the vigilance or justification hypothesis. The analysis for nonevaluative traits however, indicated only weak support for such an effect. 3) Results for Group 2 (Bieri's ten role figures and the H/Hmax measure of complexity) showed a relationship similar to that found in Group 1 for nonevaluative traits, i.e., as liking and knowledge of the person increased, complexity of response also increased. This did not support a vigilance or justification interpretation. The relationships were weak; the overall regression analysis showed a very low percentage of variance accounted for by the variables and the direction and strength of the relationships for individual role figures and individual partici- pants varied to a great extent. Although relationships were weak, they 51 tended to support a frequency of interaction interpretation. For H/Hmax, Reich obtained a main effect for Affect; Group 4 data showed a main effect for the Knowledge variable in this study. AThus, whereas Reich found support for the neutral affect hypothesis using H/Hmax (neutral persons were more highly differentiated than either positive or negative persons), this study found support for the frequency of interaction hypothesis using H/Hmax. Clearly the Knowledge variable was important. For number of adjectives and number of categories, the Knowledge by Affect interaction indicated that Person Known Well and Liked and Person Known Well and Disliked received significantly greater numbers of adjectives and categories than the Neutral role figures or the Known Slightly role figures. The data for H/Hmax showed a similar pattern (although the interaction was not significant). Thus, this study found no support for Reich's contention that the greater the involvement, the less the discrimination of stimuli. The opposite was fOund; the greater the involvement, the greater was the discrimination of stimuli. Indeed, in Group 2 (Bieri's ten role figures and the H/Hmax measure of complexity), Involvement was an important variable (indicated by the overall regression analysis), and the relationship was such that as involvement increased, complexity of response also tended to increase. This study found support for the frequency of interaction hypothesis for both complexity of response and quantity of responding. 4) In comparing overall correlations for Groups 2 and 4, it was found that the relation between liking and H/Hmax was essentially zero for Group 4 while for Group 2 the liking variable correlated significantly with H/Hmax such that as complexity of response increased, ratings of 52 liking increased, or the converse. In Group 4, the analysis of variance produced a main effect for the Knowledge variable. However, eta2 was only .06. For Group 2's overall regression analysis, the Knowledge variable entered the equation only on the last step (Involvement was the first variable to enter the equation accounting, however, for only 5% of the variance). The only difference between Groups 2 and 4 were the role figures presented. For each of the Group 4 role figures, participants briefly described their relation to the person they had thought of. An examination of these descriptions revealed that in nearly all cases peers were selected to fit the role descriptions. Bieri's ten role figures included older persons (such as Mother, Father, Boss). Supnick [1964;as reported by Crockett, 1965] found that parti- cipants used more constructs to describe peers than to describe older persons. A tentative explanation might be that in responding to peers, knowledge was the more important variable in influencing complexity of resonse, while in responding to both older and younger persons, the involvement variable was more important. Perhaps relationships with peers are similar in affective experience but differ quantitatively (i.e., in how often the person is seen and how well the person is known) while for older persons the role expectations andfibehaviors required result in stronger and qualitatively different affective re- lationships. Such an interpretation awaits further testing. However, the overall conclusion is that the presentation of two different sets of role figures did not markedly affect results, as predicted. Finally, the Sex X Affect interaction for complexity scores on evaluative traits in Group 3 was somewhat similar to the tendency 53 reported by Irwin, Tripodi and Bieri [1967], that "females compared to males tended to differentiate more among neutral and negative figures, the latter sex difference being statistically significant" [p. 446]. In this study, females differentiated neutral role figures to a signi- ficantly greater extent than did males. However, for negative role figures, males and females did not differ significantly in complexity of response. The latter differs from the results of Irwin, Tripodi and Bieri [1967]. Shepherd [1972] also reported a Sex X Valence interaction; females were less complex in responding to positive role figures but more com- plex in responding to negative role figures than were males. Irwin, et al., suggested that females have a greater need to depend upon others, and hence a greater ability to differentiate among potentially threaten- ing figures. However, this effect might well be due to the Pollyanna effect being greater for females than males (consistent with Warr, [1969]; as reported by Shepherd [1972]). Females would tend to rate positive figures uniformly favorably and negative figures less uniformly favorably than males. Lott, Lott, Reed and Crow [1970] found for one of the samples they tested that females tended to describe their acquaintances with adjectives from Anderson's list of personality-trait words which.were slightly higher in likableness value than did males. These explanations are not appropriate for the results of the present study; although female participants in Group 3 responded more complexly to neutral role figures than did the males, they did not respond in a more complex way to negative role figures. Other reported sex differences in complexity of response have been highly inconsistent. Supnick [1964; as reported by Crockett, 1965] 54 fOund a main effect for Sex; females used more constructs to describe persons than did males. Soucar [1970] and Koenig and Seaman [1974a] found main effects for Sex; males were more complex in their responding than were females. Other studies [Soucar and DuCette, 1971; Miller, 1968; Shepherd, 1972] reported no sex differences. Glixman [1965] found that females exhibited greater complexity of response than men; females used a greater number of categories than did males, contribu- ting to higher H scores. However, for H/Hmax, there was no effect for sex. The Sex X Affect interaction found in the present study does not correspond exactly to any reported result in prior research. No main effect or interaction for Sex was found in Group 4. Thus, there does not appear to be any ready explanation for the interaction found here. Thus, the predictions originally made in this study were generally upheld by the data. However, the data also showed that the overall relationships or trends masked a great deal of individual participant variability and variability in responding to the role figures. One might ask whether the role figures each constituted a separate domain for participants. In such a case one would not expect generality of complexity scores across these domains. To assess this, in a subsequent analysis, the role figures were ranked according to their respective complexity scores for each of the 30 participants in Groups 1 and 2; Kendall's coefficient of concordance was then computed on these 30 sets of rankings. For Group 1, the coefficient for complexity scores on evaluative traits was .171 (p < .01) and for nonevaluative traits, .065 (n.s.). For Group 2 H/Hmax scores, the coefficient was .099 55 (p < .01). These low coefficients support the notion of low consistency of response to the different role figures. The notion of the role figures constituting separate domains appears to be a plausible one. This study did not consider the complexity of the individual participants in the interpersonal realm. Were the more complex parti- cipants consistently more complex in their responding and were simple participants consistently simple in their responding? A subsequent analysis performed on the data for Groups 1 and 2 entailed ranking the 30 participants according to their complexity scores for each of the ten role figures and then computing Kendall's coefficient of con- cordance on these ten sets of rankings. The coefficients were, in. Group 1, .139 (n.s.) for the complexity scores on evaluative traits, and .172 (p < .01) on complexity scores for nonevaluative traits. For Group 2, the coefficient for H/Hmax scores was .272 (p < .01). These coefficients are quite low. Supnick [1964; as reported by Crockett, 1965] reported a coefficient of .604 for her fourteen participants; participants were ranked according to the number of interpersonal con- structs they used in describing eight stimulus persons. One might argue that generality of complexity scores would be expected to be stronger for Supnick's data than for this data, since verbal fluency probably contributed to the scores in Supnick's data, and a very fluent participant would probably be fluent‘ in all of his/her descriptions. However, the low generality of complexity for individual participants and across role figures here, seems to indicate low generality of any sort and may indicate randomness in the data. It calls into question the measures of complexity, the motivation of participants, the sample of persons tested, etc. In comparing mean number of adjectives, number 56 of categories and H/Hmax scores reported by Reich [1969] with the means from Group 4, one finds that in all cases the means were much lower for this study. Perhaps this lesser range of response affected overall results; perhaps it is indicative of the motivation of the persons tested, given that the H/Hmax task depends to a great extent on how much time and effort the participant is willing to spend in its comple- tion. Implications This study points to the necessity for examining differences in the measures used to assess complexity. The basic issue involves the definition of complexity or differentiation and how it pertains to_ person perception. What does it actually mean to differentiate among others or respond to them in a complex way? In a paper and pencil test of complexity the participant responds essentially to a verbal symbol of a person with whom.he/she interacts. This seems to be a very different situation than actual face-to-face interaction with others. The defini- tion of complexity is unclear in this context; studies arising from different approaches to its measurement have yielded conflicting results. At this point one cannot adequately examine the variables, such as affective valence or knowledge of the person, that might affect complexity of response when the notion of the latter is so unclear and when the measures available perhaps do not reflect what one might mean by the notion in actual interactions. Summary In summary, this study indicated no strong differences in results of two different sets of role figures. Participants were asked to 57 indicate their degree of liking for each of the ten role figures con- ventionally used in administering the Rep Test measure of complexity. When ranked, the ten role figures accurately reflected the positive and negative subgroups outlined in prior research. In addition, this study indicated that results supportive of either the vigilance or justification effect or the frequency of inter- action effect could be found depending on the measure of complexity used. On the Rep Test measure, the presentation of evaluative traits affected scores in such a way that a vigilance interpretation was pos- sible. When nonevaluative traits on the Rep Test were presented, results showed no strong relationship between complexity of response and liking or knowledge of the stimulus person. This might have occur- red because the nonevaluative traits were not traits ordinarily used to describe persons with strong affective valences. For the H/Hmax measure of complexity, knowledge of the person and degree of involvement were related to complexity of response. Well known persons and persons with whom participants indicated greater involvement were more highly differentiated.‘ Persons wrote a greater number of adjectives to describe well known and well liked stimulus persons, which also sup- ported a frequency of interaction interpretation. The conclusion drawn from the above results was that the vigi- lance effect appeared to be a very specific one that was found only when evaluative traits were presented on the Rep Test measure of complex- ity. When nonevaluative traits were used on the Rep Test and results for another measure of complexity were examined, no such effect was 58 fOund. Results generally supported the frequency of interaction hypothesis. This study also showed that in examining the relationship between complexity and the other variables for each of the role figures and for individual participants in two of the groups there was a great deal of variability in the strength and direction of these relationships, indicating that the chosen variables were not influencing complexity to the same degree, in the same way, for different individuals and in responding to the different role figures. The factors that might account for this great amount of unexplained variance remain unspecified at this time and await further investigation. Finally, the generality of complexity scores for this sample was quite low perhaps indicating that other (unknown) factors were affecting participants' responses in the experimental situation. LIST OF REFERENCES REFERENCES Anderson, N.H. Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1968, 9, 272-279. Attneave, F. Applications of information theorygto psychology, New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1959. Bieri, J. Cognitive complexity-simplicity and predictive behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psycholqur 1955, él, 263-268. Bieri, J. Complexity-simplicity as a personality variable in cogni- tive and preferential behavior. In D. W. Fiske and S. R. Maddi (Eds.), Functions of varied experience. Homewood, 111.: Dorsey Press, 1961. Bieri, J. Cognitive complexity and personality development. In 0. J. Harvey (Ed.), Experience, structure and adaptability. New York: Springer Pub. Co., 1966, pp. 13-37. Bieri, J. Attitudes and arousal: Affect and cognition in personality functioning. In C. W. Sherif and M. Sherif (Eds.), Attitude, ego-involvement and change. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1967. Bieri, J., Atkins, A. J., Briar, S., Leaman, R. L., Miller, H., and Tripodi, T. Clinical and social judgment: The discrimination of behavioral information. New York: J. Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1966. Boucher, J., and Osgood, C. E. The pollyanna hypothesis. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1969, 8, 1-8. Crockett, W. H. Cognitive complexity and impression formation. In B. A. Maher (Ed.), Progress in experimentalppersonality research. V01. 2. New York: Academic Press, 1965, pp. 47-90. Erdelyi, M. H. A new look at the New Look: Perceptual defense and vigilance. Psyghological Review, 1974, 81, l-25. Glixman, A. F. Categorizing behavior as a function of meaning domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2, 370-377. Harvey, O. J., Reich, J. W., and Wyer, R. S. Effects of attitude dir- ection, attitude intensity, and structure of beliefs upon differ- entiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1968, 19, 472-478. 59 60 Irwin, M., Tripodi, T., and Bieri, J. Affective stimulus value and cognitive complexity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology; 1967, §, 444-448. Kelly, G. A. The psychology ofypersonal constructs. New York: Norton, 1955. Koenig, F. Positive affective stimulus value and accuracy of role perception. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 1971!.19' 385-386. Koenig, F., and Seaman, J. Comparison of cognitive complexity of male and female subjects responding to male and female stimuli. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1974, 38, 1345-1346. (a). Koenig, F., and Seaman, J. Vigilance and justification as explanations of complex cognition. Journal of Social Psychology, 1974, 23, 75-80. (b). Lott, A. J., Lott, B. E., Reed, T., and Crow, T. Personality-trait descriptions of differentially liked persons. Journal of Per— sonalityyand Social Psychology; 1970, lg, 284-290. Mayo, C., and Crockett, W. H. Cognitive complexity and primacy-recency effects in impression formation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1964, pg, 335-338. McNemar, Q. Psychological statistics. New York: Wiley & Sons, 1962. Miller, A. G. Psychological stress as a determinant of cognitive com- plexity. Psycholggical Reports, 1968, 2;, 635-639. Miller, A. G. Amount of information and stimulus valence as determin- ants of cognitive complexity. Journal of Personality, 1969, 37, 141-157. Miller, H., and Bieri, J. Cognitive complexity as a function of the significance of the stimulus object being judged. Psychological Reports, 1965, lg, 1203-1204. Nidorf, L. J., and Crockett, W. H. Cognitive complexity and the inte- gration of conflicting information on written impressions. Journal of Social Psychology, 1965, pp, 165-169. Reich, J. W. Affect and frequency of interaction as determiners of the complexity of interpersonal perception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1969, 28, 107-113. Rothman, L. K. The "vigilance hypothesis" and psychotherapists. Journal of CounselingyPsychology, 1973, 20, 169-175. Scott, W. A. Cognitive complexity and cognitive flexibility. Socio- 61 Scott, W. A. Structure of natural cognitions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychologx, 1969, ll, 261-278. Shepherd, J. W. The effects of variations in evaluativeness of traits on the relation between stimulus affect and cognitive complexity. Jourpgl of Social Psychology, 1972, 88, 233-239. Shrauger, S. Cognitive differentiation and the impression—formation process. Journal of Personality, 1967, 88, 402-414. Shrauger, S., and Altrocchi, J. The personality of the perceiver as a factor in person perception. Psychological Bulletin, 1964, 88, 289-308. ' Soucar, E. Students' perceptions of liked and disliked teachers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1970, 8l, 19-24. Soucar, E., and DuCette, J. Cognitive complexity and political pre- ference. Psychological Rsports, l971,_29, 373-374. Soucar, E., and DuCette, J. A re-examination of the vigilance hypo- thesis in person perception. Journal of Social Psychology, 1972, §__8_' 31-36. Tagiuri, R. Person perception. In G. Lindzey and E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social_psychology, Vol. 3. Reading, Mass.: Addison- Wesley, 1969. Tripodi, T., and Bieri, J. Cognitive complexity as a function of own and provided constructs. Psychological Rpports, 1963, l8, 26. Turner, R., and Tripodi, T. Cognitive complexity as a function of type of stimulus objects judged and affective stimulus value. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1965, 88, 182-185. Vannoy, J. S. Generality of complexity-simplicity as a personality construct. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 8, 385-396. Wilkins, G., and Epting, F. Cognitive complexity and categorization of stimulus objects being judged. Psychological Reports, 1971, 22' 965-966 0 Wilkins, G., Epting, F., and VanDeRiet, H. Relationship between repres- sion-sensitization and interpersonal cognitive complexity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1972, 88, 448-450. APPENDICES APPENDIX A PROVIDED BIPOLAR CONSTRUCTS ON THE REP TEST APPENDIX A PROVIDED BIPOLAR CONSTRUCTS ON THE REP TEST (MEAN LIKABLENESS RATINGS ACCORDING TO ANDERSON, 1968) (SCALE or 0-6) EVALUATIVE TRAITS (Shepherd, 1972) Sincere (5.73) Insincere (.66) Kind-hearted (5.14) Mean (.37) Interesting (5.11) Dull (1.21) Friendly (5.19) Hostile (.91) Trustworthy (5.39) Untrustworthy (.65) Additional traits: Open-minded (5.30) Intolerant (.98) Reasonable (5.00) Unreasonable (.97) Cheerful (5.07) Ill-tempered (.95) Thoughtful (5.29) Thoughtless (.77) Courteous (4.94) Rude (.76) NON-EVALUATIVE TRAITS (Shepherd, 1972) Talkative (3.52) Quiet (3.11) Methodical (3.25) Unmethodical (2.62) Impulsive (3.07) Cautious (3.34) Bold (3.36) ' Shy (2.91) Rebellious (2.58) Conventional (2.60) Additional traits: Outspoken (3.13) Reserved (3.48) Opinionated (2.57) Discriminating (2.83) Fearless (3.66) Hesitant (2.90) Suave (3.35) Blunt (2.87) Nonchalant (3.24) Serious (3.79) 62 APPENDIX B SAMPLE PAGES AND SCORING GRIDFORM FOR THE REP TEST--GROUPS 1 AND 3 APPENDIX B SAMPLE PAGES AND SCORING GRIDFORM FOR THE REP TEST--GROUPS 1 AND 3 Instructions At the top of each of the fbllowing pages you will find a word or phrase describing or indicating a person in a certain relation to you. You are to think of 92§_person whom you know who is appropriate and place this person's initials in the blank to the right. Then complete the page with that particular person in mind. You will first find a set of rating scales with an adjective at each end. Look at both adjectives first, and then decide how you would rate that person on a scale of l to 6. For example: ‘ Aggressive l 2 3 4 5 6 Passive If the person is more aggressive than passive, then circle 1, 2, or 3, depending on the degree of aggressiveness you perceive this person to have. Please do not omit any of the ratings. Answer as well as you can. Following the rating scales will be a few general questions: they are self-explanatory. Please check with the experimenter if you have any questions. Of course, all your responses will remain confidential. There is no time limit; work at your own pace. Thank you for your cooperation and time. 63 64 Person You Know Slightlypand Feel Neutral Toward Please rate this person: Sincere l Thoughtless Suave Courteous Dull Open-minded Friendly Discriminating Impulsive Mean Cheerful Quiet Bold Trustworthy Conventional Nonchalant Unreasonable Fearless Methodical P' P‘ h' h‘ h‘ k‘ F‘ P‘ k‘ P‘ P‘ P‘ h‘ h‘ ha h‘ h‘ k‘ b1 k) k: k) m: n: no a: k: u: n: k: k) k) k) k) n: k) h) k) n) u: u: u: u: u» u: U1 U1 U2 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 u: U1 U1 u: u: u: n. a. b» p. b. a. a, h. n. a. e. p. p. p. a. a. a. n, A. a U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 U1 Reserved How much do you like this person? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 like VEIY much How frequently do you interact with this person? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 frequently How involved are you with this person? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 very involved mmmmmmmmmmm-mmmmmmmmm Insincere Thoughtful Blunt Rude Interesting Intolerant Hostile Opinionated Cautious Kind-hearted Ill-tempered Talkative Shy Untrustworthy Rebellious Serious Reasonable Hesitant Unmethodical Outspoken 10 dislike very much 10 infrequently 10 not involved at all 65 SAMPLE GRIDFORM FOR SCORING THE REP TEST Sincere htless Courteous Dul Friendl Mean Cheerf Trustwor Unreasonable Suave Discriminat lsi Nonchalan r Methodi Reserved APPENDIX C SAMPLE SHEETS AND INSTRUCTIONS--GROUPS 2 AND 4 APPENDIX C SAMPLE SHEETS-~GROUPS 2 AND 4 Father How much do you like this person? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 like dislike very very much much How well do you think you know this person? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 very not well well at all How involved are you with this person? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 very not involved involved at all Person You'd Like to Help How much do you like this person? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 like - dislike very very much much How frequently do you interact with this person? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 frequently infrequently How involved are you with this person? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 very not involved involved at all 66 67 INSTRUCTIONS FOR PARTICIPANTS IN GROUPS 2 AND 4 Here are descriptions of six (ten) persons. I'd like you to think of six (ten) persons that you know that fit these descriptions. Please tell me the initials of the six (ten) persons you think of. Now, I'd like you to go back to each of the persons, one at a time and in the order they're arranged here, and list single word adjectives to describe each person, as many as you can think of. Write each adjective on a separate slip of paper. In the end you'll have six (ten) piles of adjectives, one pile for each of the six (ten) persons. When you have finished, please leave the piles underneath the appropriate card. Then call me and I'll introduce a second task for you. If you need anything, please call me. Take the stack of words for each of the persons you have described (in the correct order, of course) and look over the set of words you have used in describing each person. Then put together into groups the separate words which seem to go together. You may have as many or as few groups as you like, and you may have as many or as few words in a group as you like, so long as the words in each group belong together for one particular reason. If, after you have throught about the words, a few do not seem to belong with any of the others, you may put those into a group by themselves. Of course, your groupings may change from one person to another. In brief, then, you are to take the set of words describing a person, look over that set of words, and sort the words into whatever groups you wish. Please do this sorting and grouping separately for each of the six (ten) persons in the same order that you followed in describing them; For each person, after you have sorted the words into their separate groups, you should identify each group; do this by writing a brief de- scriptive word or phrase or sentence which describes that group of words, the reason you had for making it a group. Put the label slip with the reason on tOp of the group of words, then put a paper clip on that group so that the slips do not become separated. Do this labeling for each of the groups of slips for each of the persons you've described. Are there any questions about what you are to do? APPENDIX D CORRELATIONS FOR INDIVIDUAL PARTICIPANTS IN GROUP 1 APPENDIX D CORRELATIONS FOR INDIVIDUAL PARTICIPANTS IN GROUP 1 Liking 6 Knowledge & Liking & Knowledge & Com.-Eva1. & Complexity- Complexity- Complexity- Complexity- Complexity- Evaluative Evaluative Noneval. Noneval. Noneval. -.407 -.277 -.521 -.356 .756* -.668 -.101 .011 .214 -.066 -.458 -.480 .214 .320 .030 -.l43 .218 -.308 -.520 -.483 .153 .106 -.406 .213 .204 -.563 ~.ll4 -.464 -.415 .133 .650 .155 -.185 .200 -.364 -.359 -.229 .299 .456 .461 -.706 -.556* -.529 -.199 .336 -.502 .421 .155 .151 .131 .456 .011 -.267 -.014 .169 -.405 -.020 -.351 .471 .282 -.396 -.487 .169 .284 .348 -.573 -.371 -.158 .035 .101 -.606 -.302 -.305 .068 .261 -.304 -.267 .245 .208 -.6S9* -.S76 -.389 .255 -.271 -.101 .468 .408 -.197 -.564 -.236 -.358 -.l74 -.453 -.482 -.386 .052 .186 -.516 -.573 -.475 -.584 -.313 .602 -.042 -.418 .330 .115 -.061 -.759* .064 -.237 -.l70 -.224 .116 .512 -.268 -.267 .205 -.353 -.306 -.816* -.665* -.312 -.141 .471 -.342 -.216 .436 .362 -.509 -.732* -.354 .351 .881* -.436 --040 -.319 .099 .281 -.539 -.554* . -.499 -.220 .337 .255 -.464 -.373 .311 .465 -.238 * p < .05, two-tailed test N - 10 68 APPENDIX E ADDITIONAL DATA FOR GROUP 2 (BIERI'S TEN ROLE FIGURES AND H/Hmax MEASURE) APPENDIX E DISCUSSION OF CORRELATIONS AMONG RATINGS AND COMPLEXITY SCORES FOR GROUP 2 ROLE FIGURES Number of Adjectives. The correlation between liking ratings and number of adjectives written in response to the stimulus person for each of the role figures was negative in all cases except three (Self, Boss and Person with Whom You Feel Most Uncomfortable); in such cases the relationship was essentially zero. For the former role figures, the negative correlation indicated that more adjectives were written in describing well-liked role figures. The correlation between liking and number of adjectives for each individual participant was a negative correlation except for four cases. The relation between number of the adjectives and knowledge ratings was a negative one for seven of the role figures, indicating that more adjectives were written as ratings indicated greater knowledge of the stimulus person. For all but five participants the correlation between number of adjectives and knowledge ratings was negative also. Number of Categories. The data for number of categories showed a pattern very similar to that for number of adjectives. 69 on n z umou poawwulosu .mo. v a « 70 «was. nmo.1 moo.1 «mum. «oo~.1 kmoH.1 msofiumamuuool mwmum>< «awn. me.1 mma. kooq. mmm.1 omo. suwz .maooss Home sow somumm «own. mwo. woa.1 «new. omo.1 oom.1 mama ou exam p.30» nomuom kmmm. 0mm. «no.1 «mas. omo. mmH.1 pamumuwpa: on uasofiwmwo domuwm «awn. oma. «Ho. «mmq. Nom.1 Hoa.1 mxHHmHo sow somuom «new. Hue. mmm. «mum. qwa.1 oqo. mmom «mow. «mwm.1 «Noc.1 «mum. «mqq.1 nqm.1 xmm mafimommo mo sunfish «mmm. AHA. mmo.1 «cam. mmo.1 mmH.1 sum 086m mo sunfish ammo. wm~.1 num.1 med. Hm~.1 mam.1 umnumm «cam. «mmq.1 qum.1 «mm. Nom.1 o-.1 umnuoz «Ham. mmo.1 oHH.1 mew. qu.14 oqo.1 wawm xmem\z .660 .oz .umo .oz xmam\: .ne< .oz yfie< .oz 8 .umo .02 a .3oax a wcfixaq a .fie< .oz 8 .3oas a masses mHMDUHm MACK N mDQmU mom mmmOOm MBHXMAQZOU 92¢ mUZHB¢m 02024 ZCHfiéflmmmco 71 CORRELATIONS FOR INDIVIDUAL PARTICIPANTS IN GROUP 2 Liking & * IIA P N .0 10 5, two-tailed test Liking & Knowledge & Knowledge & No. of Adj. & , No. of Adj. No. of Adj. H/Hmax H/Hmax H/Hmax _; l .667* -.365 -.453 -.486 -.393 -.116 .114 .090 .232 .128 -.054 -.353 -.118 -.169 -.248 -.096 -.542 -.259 -.547 .709 1 .763* -.082 .000 .000 .000 § -.460 -.349 -.448 .038 .126 2 -.582 -.375 -.818* -.122 .449 z -.477 -.703* .511 .523 -.030 i -.229 -.604 .193 -.021 .538 1 -.215 -.246 .001 .396 .075 g -.391 -.067 -.811* -.359 .560 g -.424 -.319 -.744* -.204 .424 ; -.016 -.123 -.524 .108 —.027 1 -.060 -.164 -.452 -.029 .388 -.292 -.819* .016 -.830* .781* ' -.753* -.689* -.150 -.410 .478 g -.342 .045 -.567 -.289 .634 ! -.524 -.701* -.330 -.724* .841* g -.776* -.597* -.198 -.283 .203 s -.825* -.816* .392 .299 -.070 . -.445 .205 -.197 .238 -.098 ; -.S87* -.805* -.156 -.427 .343 § -.241 -.767* -.117 -.320 .700* 9 -.813* -.733* .049 .253 .194 § -.336 -.524 .389 .031 .239 1 -.267 -.426 -.270 -.449 .876* g .161 -.545 -.060 -.133 -.061 ; -.355 -.389 -.023 .244 .462 z -.399 -.457 .129 -.058 .378 1 .253 -.204 -.270 -.230 .318 APPENDIX F ADDITIONAL DATA FOR GROUP 3 (REICH'S SIX ROLE FIGURES AND REP TEST) APPENDIX F MEAN RATINGS FOR GROUP 3 ROLE FIGURES Liking Interaction Involvement Knowledge Know Well & Like 1.5 2.17 2.00 1.63 Know Well & Dislike 8.03 5.90 6.53 3.70 Know Well & Feel Neutral Toward 3.93 4.43 5.33 3.27 Know Slightly & Like 2.73 3.77 5.17 5.13 Know Slightly & Dislike 7.93 7.17 8.37 6.37 Know Slightly & Feel Neutral Toward 4.60 4.90 6.83 6.73 Overall Means 4.79 4.72 5.71 4.47 72 73 om u z ammo ouaamauoSU .mo. v a « «ome. wno. HmH.- ka.- «858.- uumsoa Hmuusmz a saunwwam sous ems. wee. 465m. Heat: moo. mxsflmga a sauawfiam some New. mma. oAN. mma. ooo.: menu a sHuawuHm 366M «no. seen. 500. qu. moo. oumsoe Hmuusoz a saw: some mNN. mks. wme. meoq. mqfi. mxfiamua a HH63 some ems. soc. mm~.1 «80.- “Hm.1 we“; a HH63 266M .Hm>maoZI.Eoo .Hm>mco21.sou .Hm>maoZI.Eou .Hmbm1.800 .Hm>mI.Eoo a .Hm>mu.aoo a .3ocs a manage a .3oae a waaxsg mMMDUHh WHOM m m30m0 mom mmmOUm NBmequOU 92¢ mUZHfifim UZOEd WZOHfidqmmmoo APPENDIX G ADDITIONAL DATA FOR GROUP 4 (REICH'S SIX ROLE FIGURES AND H/Hmax MEASURE) APPENDIX G MEAN RATINGS FOR GROUP 4 ROLE FIGURES Liking Interaction Involvement Knowledge Know Well & Like 1.33 1.87 2.13 2.03 Know Well & Dislike 8.23 5.50 6.80 3.10 Know Well & Feel Neutral Toward 4.50 4.20 5.40 3.93 Know Slightly & Like 2.53 4.33 5.23 5.53 Know Slightly & Dislike 7.73 7.27 8.60 7.33 Know Slightly & Feel Neutral Toward 4.83 6.30 7.27 6.70 Overall ' 3 Means 4.85 4.92 5.91 4.79 ‘ 74 omuz ummu omafimulosu .mo. v a « 75 «kmm. NmH.- «ma. 4Aee. omo.- HOH.I HNH.- moo. uumsoa Hmuunmz a sauamfiam sons «Ame. sma.- owm. 4oem. qw~.u 88m. nos.- ham. «engage a manawgam 366x 48H“. mmo. «No.1 mwm. HNH. Nam. Nqo.- mwN.- mean a sauewfiam sons «H85. Hmm.1 omm.- 4amo. 4~m8.- mwm.- 4emm.- «aam.u eumson Hmuusaz a Hams 366s «886. moN.- mom. com. mNN.1 ems. on.- mmo. «shaman a Hams 366M 4Hmo. Noa.- mac. mam. «Ho.- Hoe. 46Hm.- eem.- mean a Hams 366M xmam\m .860 .oz .umo .oz xmam\m xmaz\m xmem\m .fiea .oz .naa .oz 8 .umo .oz 8 .366M 8 wanxfia a .ne< .oz 8 .3662 a wages; a .366“ w wafixfla mmmDOHm mqom v mDomU mom mmmOUm NEHxMAmEOU DZd mUZHfidm UZOEd mZOHEGAmmmOU GROUP 4 DISCUSSION OF NUMBER OF ADJECTIVES AND CATEGORIES Number of Adjectives. The analysis of variance showed a signi- ficant main effect for Knowledge (F(2/28) = 21.127, p < .01), Affect (F(2/56) = 9.746, p < .01) and a significant Knowledge X Affect interaction (F(2/56) = 6.4067, p < .01). Participants wrote signi- ficantly more adjectives for well known persons (M = 6.38) than for slightly known persons (M = 4.81). Significantly fewer adjectives were written for neutral persons (M = 4.67) than for liked (M = 6.38) or disliked persons (M = 5.733). The latter two means did not differ significantly. The means for the Know Slightly role figures were not significantly different from each other, whereas each of the means for the Know Well role figures differed significantly from each of the others [Newman-Keuls, p < .05]. The means for the two neutral role figures did not differ significantly from each other nor did they differ significantly from the means for the other Know Slightly role figures. Number of Categories. There was a significant main effect for Knowledge (F(1/28) = 28.64, p < .01). Participants created signifi- cantly more categories for well known persons (M = 2.44) than for slightly known persons (M = 1.89). Also significant was a Knowledge x Affect interaction (F(2/56) = 4.24, p < .05). The means for the Know Slightly role figures did not differ significantly from each other, while for the Know Well role figures, the mean for liked persons 76 77 (M 2.76) differed significantly from the mean for neutral persons (M 2.10), as shown by a Newman-Keuls test with p < .01. The means for the two neutral role figures did not differ significantly from each other and did not differ significantly from the means for the other Know Slightly role figures. (The interaction was very similar to the interaction indicated with the number of adjectives.) Group 4 Means and Standard Deviations of Number of Adjectives Role Figures Liked Disliked Neutral Known Well M 7.70 M 6.37 M 5.07 6 38 SD 3.67 SD 3.10 SD 2.57 '. Known Slightly M 5.07 M 5.10 M 4.27 4 81 SD 3.70 SD 2.95 SD 2.63 ' 6.38 5.73 4.67 Group 4 Means and Standard Deviations of Number of Categories Role Figures Liked Disliked Neutral Known Well M 2.77 M 2.47 M 2.10 2 44 SD 1.10 SD .82 SD .88 ' Known Slightly M 1.87 M 1.93 M 1.87 1 89 SD 1.07 SD .87 SD .86 ' 2.32 2.20 1.98 APPENDIX H OVERALL CORRELATIONS FOR EACH GROUP '78 .omauz .n:o«uadwuuoo qua non .OOMIZ .n:o«umaouuoo uoauo «do n you .Ohulz .ucwao>ao>=H ocq>~o>cu uneducaouuoo non .o-lz .coauouuoucu ocu>uo>cw uneducaouuoo noun .33 6033195 .3. v a. .94.. .3... AR... «3.. 21- So. gm .wmo. onun.1 c¢-.1 cuo~.1 mmo.1 .umu .oz .821 .39- .68... 311 .34 6: cmmm. cm~v. aowm. .3ocx save. «com. .HOSCH .hmv. .umucu be moozu «omm. Hod. -o.1 ovo.1 660.1 .~s>o:021.loo noo.1 caum.1 cooH.1 coon.1 .Ha>u1.lou chem. some. aomw. .305 «amp. «mom. .HoacH «mam. .uquH An macaw «Hon. coon. aama.1 am-.1 .non.1 cmo~.1 xusfi\= «mom. cmhd... choN.I cONN.I «mo~.I .350 .02 .391 .38.- .62.- 63... .6: 6: anew. anum. «mam. .xosx «vow. coon. .HO>CH doom. .uwu:H UN moozu Hmo. NHH. Hwo. woo. voo.1 .Au>0:021.lou ooo.: mo~.1 avo.1 «whN.I .Hu>u1.floo «one. «awn. came. .3ocu some. comb. .Ho>:H chmm. uoucn 0H macaw .uuU.oz .nv¢.oz .Hu>fl1.ioo .Ibzu .Ao>:H .umucn ocuxun macho gomw mug mom mZOHBngU EEO m Noun—29mg V "111111114 “111111101111? 04116