SPATIAL, STRUCTURAL, AND BEHAVIORAL CHARACTERISTICS IN THE OVERLAPPING HOME RANGES f _ 0F SONG AND FIELD SPAR-ROWS _ ' Thesis for the Degree of M. S. . MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY. , ALBERT AULEITE - 1977 kt ~‘~fl'b"~'-.‘—1)-'\,; .‘fifi '3 v7 '89”: ., .J AISWISSAI: "my“ 3 . ABSTRACT SPATIAL, STRUCTURAL, AND BEHAVIORAL CHARACTERISTICS IN THE OVERLAPPING HOME RANGES OF SONG AND FIELD SPARROUS By Albert Aulette This study examined the extent and consequences of interspecific home range overlaps between Song and Field Sparrows, revealing general patterns of coexistence during the breeding season. Two old fields on the campus of Michigan State University served as the study sites during the summer of 1975. Three classes of data were collected: spatial locations of the two co-occurring species, their activities, and habitat structure measures. The locations and activities of each individual were plotted on scaled, gridded, transparent field maps. By overlaying the maps, locations and activities occurring in the home range overlap areas of each species were contrasted with the patterns in the non-overlapping areas. The analyses used revealed several sets of differences between the Song'and Field Sparrows. The Field Sparrow's home range is from l.? to 2.5 times as large as that of the Song Sparrow (both fields considered). The overlap zone comprised 25% of the Song Sparrow's home range area, and about 12% of the Field Sparrow's home range area. In each of the two fields, Overlap and Overlap Proportion Indices indicated that both species spent a disproportionate amount of time within the overlap areas, yet maintained a high degree of spatial Albert Aulette separation there. Overlap and non-overlap areas for each species were shown to differ in at least several classes of vegetation structure. The two species differed further in their utilization of the home range zones. Host of the activities and associated substrates were observed more frequently in the overlap area relative to the non-overlap area in the Song Sparrow. The Field Sparrow also showed activities and substrates which were more frequent in the overlap area, but fewer categories were involved. When contrasts were made between the two species within the overlap zone, the Song Sparrow predominated in terms of both activity and substrate usage patterns. While not a test fer competition between the species, this study demonstrated spatial, structural, and behavioral differences which may allow the two species to coexist. SPATIAL, STRUCTURAL, AND BEHAVIORAL CHARACTERISTICS IN THE OVERLAPPING HOME RANGES OF'SONG AND FIELD SPARROHS By Albert Aulette A THESIS Submitted to Hichigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Department of Zoology 1977 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I thank the members of my guidance committee, Dr. Stephen N. Stephenson, Dr. William E. Cooper, and the chairperson, Dr. Donald L. Beaver. Special thanks goes to Dr. Beaver, not only for invaluable assistance on the thesis, but for continuous encouragement. I wish to thankIJudy Aulette for not typing any of the manuscript, and for several theoretical and practical ideas for this study. Finally, the works of Woody Allen have helped me to keep my work in the proper perspective. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS ListofTables......... ListofPigures ........ Introduction.......... Species and Study Area . . . . . Methods ............ Remtaoooeoooooooo muuuionoooooooeooo 8m 0 O O O O O O O O O O 0 Suggestions for future Research Literature Cited 0 o o o o o o 0 iii IE” "iv 13 22 A2 53 5s Table l. T‘bl. 2. Table 3. Table H. T‘blc 50 Tabl. 6 o Tabla 7 e thC 80 Table 9. Tabl. 10 0 LIST OF’TAHLES Sizes and weights of the Song and Field Sparrow . . Activities and associated substrates recorded for C‘ChBPOCiCBoaeoooeooooeoeooeoo Vegetation classes which were measured and used in the analysis of home range structure . . . . . . . mmwuocover'alnea(n) oeeeooeooao Area (3(- 1 Signof overlapping and non-overlapping (exclusive) me ranges of Field and Song Sparrows Analysis of variance table for the average values for the difference between exclusive Song Sparrow areas and overlap areas and between exclusive Field Sparrow areas and overlap areas within CEChfi-Oldoa-aooaeoaoeeeoeeoeeo Analysis of variance table for overlap index values on species pairs within each field . . . . . . . . Overlap proportion index (I) . . . . . . . . . . . Analysis of variance table for the overlap proportion indices of individual Song and ”Oldswrousaeooaoeeoeooaaeoee Avian species seen on or near the home ranges OfthGSOngandHoldspmrrow. o a e e o a o o e 0 iv 15 16 17 25 26 28 32 52 Figure 1. figure 20 Figure 3. figure “a Figure 5. Figure 6. LIST OF'FIGURES Water Quality Management Project Area . . . . Two selected study sites within the Water Quality Management Project Area . . . . . . . Example of an observationvarea curve . . . . Diagrammatic representation of home range overlaparealooeooeooeaooaaoe Significant vegetation differences for three contrasts of home range zones . . . . . . . . Significant activity and substrate differences 12 19 2h 35 INTRODUCTION Even to the most casual observer it is apparent that animals utilize space in precise and characteristic ways. Spatial patterns are manifestations of the more subtle biotic and abiotic interactions that occur between individuals and their environment. In this sense, locally sympatric species have gained attention recently as the subjects of scientific investigations into the nature of such phenomena as resource partitioning, competition, and coexistence. If two or more species co-occur in a particular area, the survival of their populations (and, hence, their coexistence) may in some manner depend upon the spatial extent of the local areas of co-occurrence. If significant areas of spatial overlap are evident, it becomes interesting to examine the physical and biological structure of such areas along with the modes of behavior exhibited in these zones in order to reveal possible ecological factors contributing to their coexistence. Territoriality in birds is one of the most familiar and thoroughly studied systems whereby space is partitioned within a particular habitat. The interest in such a system lies in the fact that it may serve to completely separate the ecological activities of individuals (Cody, 197A). In some cases two sympatric species which exhibit only intraspecific territoriality may have territories that spatially overlap and are jointly used within a local habitat. This 1 2 study investigates such a situation (with the modifications presented below) and attempts to explain the patterns feund in terms of coexistence mechanisms. Territorial behavior, especially in birds, has been described frequently since the classic book by Reward (1920). Although the range of these behavioral interactions varies greatly between species, the territories themselves have popularly been defined as defended or exclusive areas (Nbble, 1939). Once established, a territory entitles an individual to exclusive use of its resources relative to conspecifics (Pitelka, 1959). Hence, the appeal of territories for resource partitioning and coexistence studies is evident. These concepts, however, require repeated observations of those sometimes rare and subtle actions termed "defense" in order to delimit a particular territory. To avoid these problems some researchers have re-defined the territorial concept and for research purposes have presented such terms as "total utilized area" (Stenger and Falls 1959), "activity space" (Stenger Weeden, 1965), "maximum territory," and "home range" (Odum and Kuenzler, 1955). All these terms are based on the combined sightings of individuals engaged in a variety of activities within a certain area and time span. The "defended" territory is hereby considered only as some subset of a'more broadly defined area to which an individual (or pair) is confined during the breeding season. The term "home range" appears frequently in the scientific literature, and refers to that broad area to which animals of a species regularly confine their activities. Rather than basing an investigation of coexistence mechanisms on spatial patterns resulting 3 from the rather specific behaviors called defense, home ranges may delimit more ecologically complete areas. Territoriality, then, is the behavioral phenomenon that initially distributes birds over a habitat; it is in this organizational sense that territoriality is discussed in the remainder of this study. However, once the spatial arrangements have been established behaviorally, home ranges can then serve as the units of investigation. The results of this study are based“ on measurements of home ranges. As will be seen below, these "home ranges" may differ only minimally in size from their respective "territories." The resources actually encompassed by a territorial boundary vary depending upon the type of territory (Hinds, 1955). The type-A territory of Nice (l9#l) is used for mating, nesting, and.feeding of both adults and young, and is typical of many passerine species. In such instances determining the limits of a territory at a particular point in time simultaneously delimits the resource space. The type-A territorial system is characteristic of the species in the study; and, since birds in this system rarely venture beyond the limits of their "defended” area, a territory may have essentially the same area as a home range (Brown, 1959). The interest in the territorial (home range) system as a space and resource partitioning mechanism lies in the fact that such a system is assumed to have survival value for local populations of a species. Intraspecific territoriality is an adaptation selected for in the course of evolution because it aids individuals in competing for the requisites for reproduction or survival (Brown and I, Oriana, 1970). The aid is rendered through the actual reduction of competition by spatial separation within the habitat (MacArthur and Wilson 1967). Now consider the case of two intraspecifioally territorial bird species coexisting in a local habitat throughout a breeding season. The resource utilization patterns exhibited will depend upon the ecological similarity between the two species. If ecologically very similar, they might show interspecific territoriality and separate spatially over the habitat. The mechanisms involved here seem to be no different than those of intraspecific territoriality (Hinds, 1956). This situation has been described fer many species including Red-winged and Yellow-headed Blackbirds (Orlans and Wilson, l96h), Red-tailed Hawks and eagles (Fitch, gt_gl, 19H6), Eastern and Western Meadowlarks (Lanyon, 1956), and thrushes (Horse, 1971). If there are important ecological differences between the two species such that it is no longer necessary to maintain interspecifically exclusive areas, their territories may overlap locally. Most species respond to such situations of potential competition with relatively specific resource utilization patterns (Niell, 1975). The term "ecological isolation," coined by Moreau (1948) and exemplified by Lack (1971), describes the evolution of the different utilization patterns which allow local coexistence. Ecological isolation may take place along a number of niche dimensions. Communities containing ecologically similar species which do not show interspecific territoriality have been examined with respect to general ecological isolating factors (Moreau, l9h8; Pitelka, 1951; 5 Schoener, 1968; Lack, 1971; Pianka, 1973; Cody, 197“). On a finer scale than most of the previous investigations into coexistence patterns in birds, this study examines the occurrence and extent of interspecific home range overlaps by two species of sparrows in old fields. Spatial and behavioral patterns of individuals and microhabitat structure are compared between overlapping and distinct portions of the home ranges to determine the way these birds use the habitat. While not a test for competition between the species, this study may allow generalizations to be made about potential competition and coexistence mechanisms. SPECIES AND STUDY AREA The two species observed were the Eastern Field Sparrow (Spizella pusilla pusilla (Wilson)) and the Mississippi Song Sparrow (Melospiza melodia euphonia (Wetmore)). The Eastern Field Sparrow is one of two subspecies in North America, and breeds from central Minnesota, north-central Michigan, and southwest Quebec south to eastern Texas, southern Mississippi, and southern Georgia (A.O.U., 1957). It is fairly common in old fields and bushy hedgerows. The Mississippi Song Sparrow, one of 31 subspecies north of Mexico, breeds from northern Wisconsin, northeastern Michigan, and western New York, south through southeast Minnesota, Iowa; southwest Kentucky, western North Carolina to northern Georgia, and northwestern South Carolina (A.O.U., 1957). It is locally abundant in gardens, fields, and forest margins. The breeding ranges of the two subspecies overlap over much of the Midwest with Michigan being near the northern extent of the range of each. Both species belong to the order PasserifOrmes and family Fringillidae. Although the Song Sparrow is heavier, both birds appear similar in size in the field (Table 1). They are both strongly territorial (Nice's type-A) and share a similar breeding schedule in the study area. The two sparrows can commonly be found in local sympatry. The study was conducted from June 9 to September 9, 1975, 6 7 within the Water Quality Management Project Area in Ingham County on the campus of Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan (Figure 1). Data were collected from two selected fields (Figure 2). Fields I and II are 2.55 and 5.50 hectares in size, respectively, and are separated by a 275 meter extent of deciduous woods. Each field is bordered by either a narrow dirt road, a woodlot, or hedgerows, and is comprised of typical old field successional vegetation. Field I is at a slightly higher elevation than Field II, slopes to the south, and contains a low central area with trees and shrubs. Field II is slightly rolling but of uniform vegetative composition. Field I has been fallow fer about 7 years, and Field II for considerably longer (no exact determination). Table 1. Sizes and weights of the Song and Field Sparrow. Linear Measurements1 Song Sparrow Field Sparrow bill to tail length 16.00 cm. 1#.45 cm. distance from bend of wing to end of longest primary 6.#0 6.55 tail length 6.65 6.b8 bill length 1.24 0.91 Weights average weight 21.} gm.2 12.6 gm.3 l ZNice , 1937s 3‘Wetherbee, 1934. Based on American Museum of Natural History specimens (Chapman, 1966). Figure 1. Water Quality Management Project Area. The two study fields are located within the broad area delimited by the dashed lines. 10 r 03¢ .jioz‘m \ K a has n 0 ~ s. mesa mass. mmamm ~50 N as «2 «mm #\ .rts E: 5:65; 3.23 oz: 8mlap Sparrow J 1 Field I I singing foraging perchpsing foraging perching perching perohpsing ‘3 aggression- 33 display 3 ;preaning perchpslann t perchpfood feeding young t O grsmnoid graninoid ii fcrb forb 1; shrub shrub A tree tree 5: post Field 11 singing singing * perehpfood t foraging foraging perching perching V perch-sing perch-sing 33 flying feeding young s :3 preening perchsfood sir graminoid post 3 graminoid forb I: forb +3 shrub I a: tree i: post nest associated Ginsu: foraging perohpsing aggression- display a perchsalann forb shrub tree post “lens perchpsing perchnalann * perchpfood fbrb shrub * post 41 the Song Sparrow overlap. However, when the Song and Field Sparrow are compared to one another within this overlap zone (Contrast 3), it is clear that the Song Sparrow predominates with respect to several activities and substrates. With the exception of nest associated behavior, the Field Sparrow neither engages in any activity nor uses any substrate more often than does the Song Sparrow. The various analyses used above reveal several sets of differences between the Song and Field Sparrows. The Field Sparrow's home range is from 1.7 to 2.5 times as large as that of the Song Sparrow (Fields II and I respectively). The overlap zone comprises 25% of the Song Sparrow's home range area and about 12% (on the average) of the Field Sparrow's home range area. In each of the two fields, Overlap and Overlap Proportion Indicies indicated that both species spent a dis- proportionate amount of time within the overlap zones, yet maintained a high degree of spatial separation there. Overlap and non-overlap zones for each species were shown to differ in at least several classes of vegetation structure. The two species differed further in their utilization of the home range zones. Host of the activities and associated substrates were observed more frequently in the overlap area relative to the non-overlap area in the Song Sparrow. The Field Sparrow also showed activities and substrates which were more frequent in the overlap, but fewer categories were involved (especially in Field I). When contrasts were made between the two species within the overlap zone, the Song Sparrow predominated in terms of both activity and substrate usage patterns. DISCUSSION Home Range Sizes The home range sizes for the Song Sparrow in Fields I and II were .161 and .326 hectares respectively. No reference could be found in the literature on Song Sparrow home range sizes. However, several studies report areas in terms of territory size. Recalling the general similarity between the Song Sparrow's type-A territory and its home range, it may be possible to compare the areas of these two different systems. In habitats of weeds, shrubs, and trees in Ohio, Nice (1937) reported a mean territory size of .271 hectare (range .202 to .607) for an area well filled with Song Sparrows. Along Minnesota lake shores, territories averaged .202 hectare (Southers, 1960). Under the specialized conditions found on small islands, Song Sparrow territory sizes may reach a minimum of .016 hectare (Beer _e_t_ g, 1956). In mixed island and mainland sites in the Pacific Northwest, mean territory sizes of .029 to .328 hectare have been measured (Yeaton and Cody, 1974). The aVerage areas found above are within the size ranges presented in the literature. The home range sizes of the Field Sparrow were .h08 and .569 hectares in Fields I and II respectively. With the qualifica- tions noted above, these values are slightly below the .809 to 2.h28 hectare territory sizes reported for old field and bushy fencerow habitats by Walkinshaw (Bent, 1968). #2 43 The Song Sparrow's home range size in Field II is about twice as large as its size in Field I. Similarly, the Field Sparrow‘s home range is about l.h times larger in Field II. While no detailed between- field analyses were made, it is possible that the general differences in vegetation structure for the two fields could have had some effect (direct or indirect) on the size differences. Field I contained a large central area of shrubs and trees while Field II appeared uniformly covered with low herbaceous vegetation. A loose correlation may exist, then, between gross scale vegetation uniformity and.large home range size. The densities of the two sparrows (which will be discussed later in another context) could also explain the home range size differences but the direction of causation is uncertain. In Field II both species had larger home ranges and lesser densities than in Field I. The densities in males per hectare were 1.2% and 0.99 respectively for Song and Field Sparrows in Field II. The values in Field I were 1.98 and 1.#8 per hectare respectively (see below). Structural Characters From studies that have been made on habitat selection and territorial structure, it is currently thought that some birds rely on the overall pattern of vegetation structure, the physiognomic features of an area, in determining its suitability for breeding (Wiens, 1969). Thus, the individual species of plants may not be important in the initial selection of’a home range as long as the components of the habitat are of the prOper type (Cody, 1968; MacArthur and icArthur, 1961, MacArthur at 51., 1962; Beaver, 1976). yoreover, 4h if birds select their habitats through such structural cues, their response to and utilization of the habitat should be related in various ways to this structure (Wiens, 1969). This concept has been extended here to the analysis of overlapping home ranges. An effort was made to record all plant species and substrate characteristics in each home range, but certain species were subsequently eliminated from the data since it was Judged that they played very little role in the physiognomy and utilization of the habitats. While no claim is made to know the exact factors in this study that are selectively perceived by the birds, the contributors that were deemed important to home range structure in Fields I and II have been listed in Table 3. Grasses andhgpldenrods ‘coveredimost of both fields, and together with trees, whmnbs, and saplings. gave each'field'its'chamacteristic appearance. The other plants stood out in importance because they formed noticeably distinct patterns, and served as perch sites and protective cover for both species of birds. Vegetation height, the percentage of bare ground, and the presence of clumps and low patches also were considered important in the overall physiognomy, and are closely associated with foraging‘activity. Fro-{Figure 5 the structural make-up of the home ranges as well as many differences between overlap and non-overlap zones and between the non-overlaps of both species are apparent. It must be noted that there is no way to test here whether those statistically significant differences are of the magnitude that would constitute real differences for the birds. Separate studies of the Song Sparrow in Ohio, San Francisco, and British Columbia agree upon the species basic habitat requirements: 45 concealment and shelter, perches, singing posts, accessible ground, good light conditions under cover (dense but low vegetation or high thin canopy), and proximity t6 water (Bite, 1937; Marshall, 1948; Tompa, 1969). Nice states that trees, shrubs, and large weeds offer singing posts and look-outs for the male, while shrubs, grasses, and weeds provide both sexes with food, protection, and nest sites. All these requirements are met in each field of this study. The greater low grass cover in the overlap of Contrast 1 (Figure 5) may provide added protection for the Song Sparrow and account for its disproporo tionate occurrence in its overlap zones. In addition, low grass Slifilflcnted.aumajer foraging substrate. The greater goldenrod cover and vegetation height in the Song Sparrow non-overlap sons (Contrast 3) (also Contrast 1 for goldenrod) also indicates a potential for con- cealment. As compared to the overlap, the Song Sparrow's non-overlap with higher chickory and sapling cover values may offer relatively more perching, singing, and look-out posts. Moreover, those categories of structure which do not differ within contrasts may also provide many of the basic needs for the species. The literature is insufficient in its description of basic habitat requirements for the Field Sparrow. However, studies done in Earth Carolina, Texas, and”Hichigan report this species breeding in farmlands, over-grown pastures, and abandoned fields all containing grasses, forbs, and scattered shrubs and trees (Pulliam and Enders, 1971; Fretwell, 1959; Allair and Fisher, 1975: Evans, 1961+; Walkinshaw, 1968). Nice (1937) and Pulliam and finders found both Field and Song Sparrows to be locally sympatric, an apparently common situation in their 46 broadly overlapping ranges. Evidence such as this indicates that the two species may share many of their habitat requirements, with the exception that the Field Sparrow favors more of the open field type of habitat with scattered woody vegetation. Fbr example, consider the contrast between the Field Sparrow overlap and non-overlap for Field I (Figure 5). Shrubs and goldenrod cover are higher in the overlap, and both offer very good concealment (as well as a good foraging location). The non—overlap, on the other hand, contains more bare ground and sapling cover, two structural factors which are themselves scattered in their distribution over the field. This pattern then, seems to fit this rough habitat preference profile, and may account for the relatively large home range size of the Field Sparrow and the smaller percent of overlap with the Song Sparrow. A similar situation can be seen in Field II, where goldenrod cover and vegetation height in the overlap also offer the potential for increased concealment. The home ranges and overlaps of each species differ in one or more aspects of habitat structure. Nol,in order to generalize about the ecological relationships between the species, these structural characters of the habitat should be considered in association with the ways that they are used by their occupants. Features of the habitat such.as those discussed above nay affect the behavior of the birds or may determine differences in the supply of food available (Stenger and Falls, 1959). These possibilities are discussed below. #7 Activity and Associated Substrates A substantial potential for coexistence of the Song and Field Sparrow is manifest in the differences of home range utilization presented in Figure 6. Categories differ greatly between overlap and non-overlap for each species, especially in the Song Sparrow. Heavy use is made of the overlap zone, but according to Contrast 3, the two species may be able to reduce competition in the overlap zone through differential usage. The obvious activity category to consider when discussing coexistence is that of aggression.and/cr display. It is worth noting that the aggressive component of this activity category was in almost all cases the supplanting of the Field Sparrow by the Song Sparrow. Song Sparrows generally dominate most species that venture into their territory (Rice, 1937; Tbspa, 1964), while Field Sparrows are rarely aggressive toward other species (Bent, 1968). In fact, Nice states that the Field Sparrows were driven off by the Song Sparrows with special vigor, but they merely avoided the attacks and continued to nest in the midst of the Song Sparrows. This aggression and submission, rare as it was, might have served to spatially segregate the species within the overlap (especially in Field I - Contrast 3). not only, then, is there a low degree of association between the species in the overlap, but separation is also evident in their activities and use of substrates. The relationship between vegetation structure and utilization records is not entirely clear. If Figures 5 and 6 are compared, only a few related patterns can be seen.’ For example, in both fields the Song Sparrow spent lore time foraging and more time on graminoid 43 vegetation in its overlap relative to its nonpoverlap; and low grass, graminoid and a prime foraging substrate, was indeed more concentrated there. Similarly, in each field the Field Sparrow foraged more often and used forbs.more frequently in the overlap relative to its non-overlap; goldenrod, a forh and also a principle foraging substrate was more concentrated there. Other than these loose correlations, no consistent relational pattern can be seen. Several factors may be responsible: 1) most activities can take place in a variety of substrates; 2) some substrates might in fact inhibit certain activities; and 3) the two species may do different things on the same substrate. Also, some inconsistencies may arise by Jointly analyzing the two fields which differ sosewhat in their vegetational composition (although certain patterns are evident in both fields). Nevertheless, there are some structural and utilization differences between the species. In the study of grassland birds, Wiens (1969) concluded "absolute segregation is not required to permit the coexistence of two or more species since even slight differenses: in the frequency of utilization of a habitat component, or subtle differences in the degree of occupancy of a vegetation type, may be sufficient to allow continuing co-occupancy ofthe habitat especially if resources are abundant." It has been stated that animals partition environsental resources in three basic ways: spatially, trophically, and temporally (Pianka, 1973). Spatial factors have already been considered, but no statistical analysis of trophic factors were nade in this study. The gross observational data collected did suggest that both Field and Song Sparrows took similar food items, grass seeds and Lepidopteran larvae. “9 The Field Sparrow appeared in general to be more visible and "acrobatic" during foraging than the Song Sparrow, taking seeds and small insects fros plants while hanging in various positions. The smaller size and average weight of the Field Sparrow could explain'some of this apparent agility. On the importance of the trophic factor, Baker and Baker (1973) state "little doubt renains that at least for most birds food resources are the ultimate limiting constraint on population size," and this sentiment is echoed by other investigators (Lack, 1971: Edington and Edington, 1972, Cody, 1974). If food is in short supply locally, competition between the species could be reduced through: foraging behavior, kinds of food eaten, and differences in size and shape of the food eaten (Allair and Fisher, 1975). Pulliam and Enders (1971) detected slight differences between Field and Song Sparrows in each of the above three categories during the winter sonths despite a high degree of overlap. Also, in a study of Field, Chipping, and.Bachman's Sparrows in eastern Texas, Allair and Fisher (1975) found the Field Sparrow using higher perches to obtain seeds than did the other species. Thus, the potential for coexistence is present. 0n the other hand, Evans (196%) studied Field, Chipping, and Vesper Sparrows on an old field in southeastern“Nichigan and found no evidence of significant differences in the kinds of food eaten, nor any indication that food was in short supply during the breeding season. He concluded that the three species bred synpatrically without competitibn for food. This could well be the situation for the Song and Field Sparrows of this study. However, even under these circumstances it is possible that the species differ in their ability to locate the same food types. 50 The temporal factor in resource partitioning was not given detailed analysis in this study. Spatial and behavioral observations were not taken sisultaneously for each of the two species, and it was assumed that the patterns observed were the results of continuous and widely similar types of activity over the season. Indeed, in his study of several species of grassland birds, Hiens (1969) found them to be broadly overlapping in their seasonal use of the area, and saw no evidence of any temporal segregation. The response of the Song and Field Sparrows to habitat structure may depend upon both their population densities and the number and kinds of other bird species present as the potential resource competition becomes more severe or less severe (Viens, 1969). gist (395?) records San'lparrow densities of free 1.5.1” per hectare to 2.7 hiss per hectare in.a habitat of trees, shrubs, weeds, and gardens. The present study found 2.0 males per hectare on Field I and 1.2 males per hectare on Field II. Fretwell (1969) records Field Sparrow densities of fro- l.“ males per hectare to 3.7 sales per hectare in pastures and fields of Broomsedge and pine plantings. Bent (1968) gives densities in old fields of’O.17 to 1.95 males per hectare. This study found 1.5 males per hectare in Field I and 1.0 males per hectare in Field II. There is no way to know at this time whether the densities in Fields I and II are low enough to serve as significant coexistence aechanisms, but it appears that they are comparable to the general range of densities previously published. Other species can also have an effect on the local coexistence of the Song and Field Sparrow. They may compete for the same limited 51 resources, may affect the size of home ranges (Ieaton and Cody, l97h), or may act as predators keeping densities low (Connell, 1970). Table 10 lists those species which were seen regularly on or near the home ranges of the Song and Field Sparrows. The two sparrows, however, dominated both fields in terms of numbers of breeding pairs. Moreover, none of those species listed that nest on or near the ground did so within the home ranges of the two sparrows. The species composition here is not unusual, but the potential still exists for partial overlap in diet among some of the species (an important factor if food were found to be in short supply). so descriptions of Song and Field Sparrow home ranges in habitats lacking other bird species could be found in the literature. The two obvious interactions with other species were those of nest predation and parasitism. On at least three occasions eggs or nestlings were taken fros nests of both species presusably by avian predators. Also, Cowbird parasitism was widespread, and both sparrows were seen feeding the young of this species. The two sparrows are typically parasitized by the Cowbird, but the Song Sparrow appears in general to be the favored host (Rice, 1937). Obviously the Cowbird can affect the sparrows' pepulations, but the extent of this effect and its relation to coexistence patterns needs further study. Table 10. Avian species seen on or near the home ranges of the Song and Field Sparrows. Observations from both fields are combined. Cardinal (Cardinalis cardinalis) Indigo Bunting (Passerina 911212) American Goldfinch (m tristis) Eastern Meadowlark (Sturnella m.) Red-winged Blackbird (Agelaius phoeniceus) Co-on Crackle (Quiscalus guiscula) Brown-headed Cowbird (Holothrus 539;) Northern Oriole (Icterus gglbula) Harbling Vireo (£133 £123) Starling (Sturnus vulflis) American Robin (_Tu_1_'_¢_i_u_§ nigatorius) House Wren (Trogl‘ oytes 93293) Blue Jay (Cyanocitta cristata) Downy Woodpecker (Dendrocopos pubescens) Co-on Flicker (Colaptes 3M) Yellow-billed Cuckoo (Coccyzus americanus) SUMMARY Coexistence is the persistence of two or sore species in the same habitat, and such coexistence is achieved by the evolution of some mini-a1 degree of difference in resource use (Cody, 197k). This study has established, first, the gross pattern of resource partitioning through the measurement of structural components in overlapping Song and Field Sparrow home ranges. Differences in from 3 to 8 vegetation classes (out of’a total of 11 seasured) were found between areas of hose range overlap and non-overlap in both species. More structural differences were apparent in the contrast between the Field Sparrow overlap and non-overlap zones than that of the Song Sparrow. Second, the spatial and behavioral relationship to the habitat structure demonstrated several areas of separation in both occupancy and utilization between the species. Overlap Proportion Indicies greater than 1 indicated that both species made disproportionate use of the overlap zones. Moreover, Overlap Indicies of from 0.32 to 0.}? demonstrated small degrees ofsspatial'association between the species in this overlap zone. Many of the Song and Field Sparrow's activities and associated substrates were recorded with different frequencies in their zones of overlap relative to the non-overlap zones. within the overlap areas, the two sparrows differed in the frequencies of several important activities. Such separation along 53 l i l I III 54 more than one niche dimension has been found in similar studies of grassland birds (Wiens, 1969) and lizards (Pianka, 1971:). ' II III ‘ (I A! SUGGESTIONStFOR FURTHER RESEARCH It would clearly be interesting to analyze additional over- lapping home ranges of these species over several seasons and in other geographic areas. Spatial distributions of the sparrows plotted continuously and simultaneously for each overlap night show associations not discovered in the present study. More direct measures of resource partitioning and cospetition could be sade through trophic analyses and removal experisents if aatched sets of local overlap zones could be found. 55 \ I I I l til.‘ I LITERATURE CITED and! :lhlllq LITERATURE CITED Allair, P. M. and C. D. Fisher. 1975. Feeding ecology of three resident sympatric sparrows in Eastern Texas. Auk 92: 260-269. Aserican.0rnithologists' Union. 1957. Check-list of North American birds, fifth ed. Amer. Ornithol. Union, Baltisore. Baker, M. C. and A. E. M. Baker. 1973. Niche relationships among six species of shorebirds on their wintering and breeding ranges. Ecol. Monographs #3: 193-212. Beaver, D. L. 1976. Avian populations in herbicide treated brush field. 0 A“: $3-553 0 Beer, J. 3., L. D. Frenzel, M. Hansen. 1956. Minimua space requirements of some nesting passerine birds. Wil. Bull. 68: 200-209. Bent, A. C. 1968. Life histories of'Morth.American cardinals, grosbeaks, buntings, towhees, finches, sparrows, and allies. U.S. Mat. Museum Bull. 237, part 3. Brown, J. L. 1969. Territorial behavior and population regulation in birds. Wilson Bull. 81: 293-329. Brown, J. L. and G. H. Oriana. 1970. Spacing patterns in mobile ”was A“. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 1: 239'2620 Cody, M. L. 197h. Cospetition and~the structure of bird communities. Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, M.J. 318 p. 3 1968. On the sethods of resource division in grassland bird communities. Am. Mat. 102: 107-1“?. Chapman, F. M. 1966. Handbook of birds of eastern North America. Dover Publications, Inc., M. Y. 581 p. Connell, J. H. 1970. On the role of natural enemies in preventing cospetitive exclusion in.some marine animals and in rain forest trees. Proc. Adv. Study Inst. Dynamics Numbers Popul. (Oosterbeek, 1970): 298-312. Edington, J. M. and M. A. Edington. 1972. Spatial patterns and habitat partition in the breeding birds of an upland wood. J. An. Ecol: 331.3% s 56 57 Evans, F. C. l96#. The food of Vesper, Field, and Chipping Sparrows nesting in an abandoned field in S. E. Michigan. Amer. Midl. 'Mat. 72: 57-75. Fitch, H. S., F. Swenson, D. F. Tillotson. 19h6. Behavior and food ghabits of the Red-tailed Hawk. Condor #8: 205-237. Fretwell, S. D. 1969. On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. III. Breeding success in a local population of Field Sparrows. Acta Biotheoretcia 19 3 “5‘52 0 Hinds, R. A. 1956. The biological significance of the territories of birds. Ibis 98: 3‘70'3690 Howard, H. E. 1920. Territory in bird life. J. Murray, London. 308 p. Kershaw, K. A. 1973. Quantative and dynamic plant ecology. Aaerican Elssvier Pub. Co., M.Y. 308 p. Lack, D. 1971. Ecological isolation in birds. Harvard Univ. Press, caridEO. H388. “'04 pa Lanyon, H. E. 1956. Territory in Meadowlarks, genus Sturnella. Ibis 98: 485-17890 MacArthur, R. H. and J. M. MacArthur. 1961. On bird species diversity. Ecology #2: 59t-598. MacArthur, R. H., J. H. MacArthur, J. Preer. 1962. On bird species diversity. II. Prediction of bird censuses from habitat measurements. Am. Mat. 96: 167-171}. MacArthur, R. H. and E. O. Milson. 1967. The theory of island biogeography. Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, H.J. 203 p. Marshall, J. T. 19h8. Ecological races of Song Sparrows in the San Francisco Bay region. Part I. Habitat and abundance. Condor 50: 193-215. Moreau, R. E. 19fi8. Ecological isolation in a rich tropical avifauna. Jo ‘31.. R010 17: 113'1260 Morse, D. H. 1971. The effects of the arrival of a new species upon habitat utilization by two forest thrushes in Maine. ‘Hil. Bull. 83: 57-65. Mice, M. M. 1937. Studies in the life history of the Song Sparrow. I. Trans. Linnean Soc. M.Y. h: l-2h7. . 1991.‘ The role of territory in bird life. Amer. Midl. Mat. 26 3 “14870 58 lie, M. H., C. H. Hull, J. G. Jenkins, K. Steinbrenner, D. H. Bent. Statistical package for the social sciences, supplement. McGraw Hill Book Company, New York, M.Y. 675 p. ' Hiell, U. E. 1975. Experimental studies of microcrustacean competition, community composition and efficiency of resource utilization. Ecology 56: 809-826. Moble, G. K. 1939. The role of dominance in the social life of birds. Au‘k %: 263.273. Odum, E. P. and E. J. Kuenzler. 1955. Measurement of territory and home range size in birds. Auk 72: 128-137. Orians, G. H. and M. F: Wilson. 1964. Interspecific territories of birds. Ecology #5: 736-745. Pianka, E. R. 1973. The structure of lizard communities. Ann. Rev. ROI. Syat. ‘7: 53-7“. . 197k. Miche overlap and diffuse competition. Proc. Nat. AC‘d. 8C1. UeSsAs 71: 21h1‘fl45. Pitelka, F. A. 1951. Ecological overlap and interspecific strife in breeding populations of Anna and Allen Hummingbirds. Ecology 32: 6k1-661. . 1959. Humbers, breeding schedule, and territoriality in ectoral Sandpipers of Morthern Alaska. Condor 61: 233-264. Puniaa, H. R. and F. Enders. 1971. The feeding ecology of five syspatric finch species. Ecology 52: 557-566. Ricklefs, H. E. 1967. A graphical method of fitting equations to growth curves. Ecology 48:978-983. Schoener, T. U. 1968. The Anolis lizards of Bimini: resource partitioning in a complex fauna. Ecology 49: 70h-726. Sokal, R. R. and F. J. Rohlf. 1969. Biometry. U. H.'Freeman and Co., San Francisco. 776 p. Suthers, R. A. 1960. Measurement of some lake shore territories of the Song Sparrow. Uil.Bu11. 72: 232-237. Stenger, J. and J. B. Falls. 1959. The utilized territory of the O'anirds W11. Balls 71: 1250 Stenger Meeden, J. 1965.' Territorial behavior of the Tree Sparrow. Condor 67: 193. 59 Tompa, F. 1962. Territorial behavior: the main controlling factor of a local Song Sparrow population. Auk 79: 687-697. . 196k. Factors determining the numbers of Song Sparrows Melogpiza melodia (Wilson), on Mandarte Island, B.C. Canada. cue 20010 PO”. 109: 1‘68. Wiens, J. A. 1969. An.approach to the study of ecological relationships among grassland birds. A.O.U. Ornith. Monog. Mo. 8. Hetherbee, K. B. 1939. Some measurements and weights of live birds. Bird Banding 5: 55-69. Ieaton, R. C. and M. L. Cody. Competitive release in island Song Sparrow populations. Theor. Pop. Biol. 5: h2-58. HICHIGQN STRTE UNIV. LIBRRRIES || 1|! | lllll 7I‘ll ||||7H|||||| 3129310