CONNECTING THE WRONG DOTS: CAN THALAMO -CORTICAL DYSCONNECTIVITY EXPLAIN ALTERED COROLLARY DISCHARGE IN SCHIZOPHRENIA? By Beier Yao A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Psychology Ð Master of Arts 2018 ABSTRACT CONNECTING THE WRONG DOTS: CAN THALAMO -CORTICAL DYSCONNECTIVITY EXPLAIN ALTERED COROLLARY DISCHARGE IN SCHIZOPHRENIA? By Beier Yao Corollary discharge (CD) signals are Òcop ies Ó of motor signals sent to sensory areas to predict the impending input. Because they are used to distinguish actions generated by oneself versus external forces, altered CD has been hypothesized to result in the commonly -observed agency disturbances in schizophrenia patients (SZP). Behavioral evidence for altered CD in SZP has been observed i n multiple sensorimotor domains, including the oculomotor system; however , its exact neural underpinning is unknown. One oculomotor CD pathway identified in primates projects from motor neurons in the superior colliculus (SC) to visual neurons in the front al eye fields (FEF) via the mediodorsal thalamus (MD T). The current study aimed to examine the structural connectivity of MD T-FEF pathway in SZP and whether it relates to oculomotor CD abnormalities. Twenty -four SZP and 22 healthy controls ( HC) underwent d iffusion tensor imaging (DTI) , and a large subset of those individuals also performed the blanking task, an eye movement task that measures the influence of CD on visual perception . Probabilistic tractography was used to identify white matter tracts connec ting FEF and MD T. Microstructural integrity of these tracts was compared across groups and correlated with behavioral indices of oculomotor CD from the blanking task and symptom severity. We found that SZP had compromised microstructural integrity in MDT -FEF pathway . This hypoconnectivity was correlate d with both impaired oculomotor CD signals and more severe positive symptoms in SZP. These data suggest that the MDT -FEF pathway may serve an important role in transmitting oculomotor CD signals , which in turn may relate to positive symptom manifestation in SZP. !iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The author would like to thank Dr. Katy Thakkar for her guidance and support, Dr. Jason Moser and Dr. Mark Becker for their helpful feedback, Lara Rısler for sharing her analyses results, Jingtai Liu for his assist ance o n 3D tract visualization, Livon Ghermezi for visually inspecting part of the imaging data, Xiaochen Luo for providing constructive distractions and meaningful companionship, and all the participants for their partic ipation . !iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ..v LIST OF FIGURES ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ... Évi INTRODUCTION ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ..1 Corollary discharge abnormalities in psychosis ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ.3 Altered thalamo -cortical connectivity in schizophrenia ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ.5 METHODS ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ É.7 Overview ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ É.7 Participants ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ É..7 Blanking task ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ É..8 Image a cquisition ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ É..10 Regions of interest ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ÉÉ11 Preprocessing ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ É11 Probabilistic tractography ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ÉÉ.12 Statistical analyses ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ÉÉ14 RESULTS ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ.16 Participant characteristics ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ É.16 Hypoconnectivity in MDT -FEF pathway ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ É.16 Relationship with oculomotor CD ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ÉÉ17 Relationship with clinical symptoms ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ É.. É.. 18 DISCUSSION ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ...19 APPENDICES ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ É..24 APPENDIX A: Tables ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ÉÉ..25 APPENDIX B: Figures ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ÉÉ.27 APPENDIX C: Supplementary Results ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ É37 REFERENCES ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ÉÉ.41 !v LIST OF TABLES Table 1 . Demographic information ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ...25 Table 2 . Mean microstructural integrity measures (FA, MD, RD, AD) in the MDT -FEF tract by hemisphe re ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ.ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ...26 Table S1 . Demographic information of the subset sample with behavioral data ÉÉÉÉÉÉ .39 Table S2 . Mean microstructural integrity measures (FA, MD, RD, AD) in the MDT -FEF tract by hemisphere in the subset !sample with behavioral data ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ.ÉÉ40 !vi LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 . Blanking task ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ. 27 Figure 2 . Blanking task perceptual judgment ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ...28 Figure 3 . Blanking task pe rformance in chronic SZP and HC ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ÉÉ.29 Figure 4 . Blanking task performance and symptom severity in chronic SZP ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ..30 Figure 5 . Group -averaged blanking task per formance in chronic SZP and HC ÉÉÉÉÉÉ ...31 Figure 6 . Probabilistic tractography results of the MDT -FEF pathway in both groups ÉÉÉ... 32 Figure 7 . 3D Probabilistic tractography resul ts of the MDT -FEF pathway in HC ÉÉÉÉÉ ...33 Figure 8 . Group differences in the MDT -FEF tract microstr uctural integrity by hemisphere É .34 Figure 9 . Scatterplots of JND against MDT -FEF tract -specific indic es of microstructural integrity ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ................................ .............................35 Figure 10 . Scatterplots of PANSS positive symptoms scores against MDT -FEF trac t-specific indic es of microstructural integrity .ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ..36 !1 INTRODUCTION It has always been a challenge to come up with a mechanistic model of schizophrenia that explain s its wide range of symptoms on both phenomenological and neuro bio logical levels. One recent theory that attempted to address this problem was the prediction error (PE) model of psychosis (Corlett, Honey, & Fletcher, 2016; Fletcher & Frith, 2009; Gray, Feldon, Rawlins, Hemsley, & Smith, 1991) . Simply put, we form a model of the world that is based on sensory inputs and expectancies, and we update our model of the world whenever we encounter a PE Ð a mismatch between our prediction and actual sensory input , to make the model closer to the ever -changing reality . However, individuals with schizophrenia (SZP ) may have excessive false PEs that constantly prompt them to update a perfectly working model. Because these abnormal PEs were due to systematically altered predictions rather than real changes in the environment , the model can never be updated satisfacto rily, hence leading to unusual sensory experiences commonly seen in SZP . To break the PE model down, basic predictions rely on simple correlations between commonly co -occurring stimulus -stimulus pairs (e.g. police cars by the roadside predict accidents ) an d action -stimulus pairs (e.g. speaking predicts hearing your own voice ). This study focus ed on the latter. Predictions about our own actions are established via corollary discharge (CD), or efference copy, signals in our brains . CD signals are , in essence , ÒcopiesÓ of motor signals containing information about the intended movement (e.g. direction, amplitude, velocity, etc.) . They originate from the same brain areas as the motor signals do but are then sent to sensory areas , where a prediction of the sensor y consequences of the intended movement can be computed . This match between predict ed and actual sensory inputs is argued to be the basis of our sense of agency - the subjective sense of being in control of the actions we produce. !2 However , an altered CD si gnal could lead directly to a wrong (or absent) prediction and hence a PE. One direct consequence then could be misattributing self -generated actions to external forces . Interestingly, almost all first rank symptoms of schizophrenia (Schneider, 1959) Ñsymptoms which were argued to be pathognomonic to schizophrenia Ñcould be conceptualized as agency disruptions, e.g. alien control delusions , auditory hallucinations (misattributing oneÕs sub vocal voices as someone else speaking; Green & Kinsbourne, 1990) . Therefore, altered CD has been theorized as the basis for these agency disruptions in schizophrenia (Feinberg, 1978) . There is indeed a growing evidence base that CD is impaired in SZP in multiple sensory modalities (Blakemore, Smith, Steel, Johnstone, & Frith, 2000; Ford & Mathalon, 2012; Shergill, Samson, Bays, Frith, & Wolpert, 2005) . A thoro ugh understanding of how CD signals go awry in the brains of SZP s can shed light on key mechanism s of the disorder and help develop more targeted interventions. One potential neural pathway transmitting CD signals associated with saccadic eye movements has been identified in recent primate studies: neurons in the mediodorsal thalamus (MDT) seem to play a crucial role in relaying CD signals originating from movement neurons in the superior colliculus (SC) to the visual neurons in the frontal eye fields (FEF; Sommer & Wurtz, 2002, 2004, 2008) . Results from humans with specific MDT lesions support this finding (Gaymard, Rivaud, & Pierrot -Deseilligny, 1994; Ostendorf, Liebermann, & Ploner, 2010) . A large body of animal physiology work support s the notion that the thalamus may play a more general key role in transmitting CD sig nals. The branching axons innervating thalamus enable the distribution of an identical message to both motor centers and sensory areas, making them a strong candidate for CD circuits that permit sensory predictions of almost all motor actions (Sherman, 2016) . If this is the case, then thalamus essentially helps modulate all cortical !3 processing of motor commands. In other words, thalamo -cortical connectivity could play a crucial role in abnormal transmission of CD signals. Interestingly, there has long been a model of schizophrenia as a Òmisconnection syndrome Ó, in which thalamus is a key link (Andreasen, 1999). Altered CD could be a direct consequence o f misconnection in neural circuit s and could be a more proximal mechanism of schizophrenia symptoms. Though there is mounting behavioral evidence for altered oculomotor CD in SZP (See Thakkar , Diwadkar, & Rolfs , 2017 for review ), no study to date has studied whether abnormalities in the MDT -FEF pathway contribute to this. Corollary discharge abnormalities in psychosis To date , most of our understanding of CD in human and non -human primates come s from the oculomotor system. Current evidence indicates that we rely on CD signals for different oculomotor processes such as maintain ing visual stability (i.e. perceiving a stable and continuous world de spite constant eye movements; Cavanagh, Hunt , Afraz, & Rolfs, 2010) and generat ing rapid successive goal -directed saccades (i.e. preparing a second saccade command while executing the first saccade ; Hallett & Lightstone, 1976a, 1976b) . Likewise , a large body of evidence for CD abnormalities in psychosis also comes from the oculomotor system (see Thakkar et al. , 2017 for review ). One commonly used task to assess visual stability is the blanking task, which measures visual perception shortly following an eye movement (Figure 1; Deubel, Schneider, & Bridgeman, 1996) . In this task, following a period of fixation, a visual stimulus appear s at a peripheral location on the screen (at the pre -saccadic location ), and the participant is instructed to look at the stimulus. Once the participant initiates a saccade, the stimulus disappear s for a brief moment and reappear s at a new location ( post -saccadic location ) slightly bac kward or !4 forward relative to the pre -saccadic location . The participant is then asked to judge in which direction the stimulus jumped relative to its pre -saccadic location. Because saccadic eye movements are often imprecise, the saccade typically fall s short or long of the target. Therefore, the participant cannot rely on the saccade landing site to inform them of the pre -saccadic location to make an accurate judgment on the post -saccadic location. For example, if the stimulus jumps backward by 0.5 ¡ and the saccade falls short of the target by 1 ¡ (i.e. 0.5 ¡ short of the post -saccadic location), it will appear that the stimulus jumps forward relative to the saccade landing site (see Figure 2 for an illustration). The participant has to ÒknowÓ by exactly h ow much the saccade f ell short or long of target in order to remap the actual pre -saccadic location correctly, which requires access to information stored in a CD signal. In other words, if CD signals are intact, there should be no correlation between the actual saccade landing site and the participant Õs perceptual judgment Ðsaccades fall ing short or long of the target are random errors that have no relationship with the pre -saccadic location, and hence should have no influence on the judgment of post -sacca dic location either. Indeed , this is exactly how healthy people perform on the task (Collins, Rolfs, Deubel, & Cavanagh, 2009) . However, if CD signals are compromised, the participant may not have access to enough information about the spatial accuracy of the saccade. In such a case, the participant can only rely on the actual saccade landing si te to make the perceptual judgment - there is no information that would prompt the participant to think that the saccade was not precise. Therefore, when CD is impaired, one would expect to see a correlation between the saccade landing site and the partici pant Õs perceptual judgment because as the saccade falls increasingly short of the target, there is a greater chance that the post -saccadic location will be forward of the saccade landing site. This has indeed been observed in humans with MDT lesions (Ostendorf et al., 2010) and in primates whose MD T was !5 temporarily inactivated (Cavanaugh, Berman, Joiner, & Wurtz, 2016) , providing more evidence to suggest that MDT is a key relay station for CD signals related to saccadic eye movements . Recently, Rısler and colleagues (2015) examined CD in SZP using the blanking task. SZP exhibited reduced precision of perceptual judgments, a s evidenced by flatter psychometric curves plotting performance against target displacement . That is, their perceptual judgments were less sensitive to the actual target displacement. On a group level, t hey found a correlation between saccade landing site and perceptual judgment in SZP, but not in HC (Figure 3) , consistent with previ ous literature of impaired oculomotor CD in SZP. That is, SZP with shorter mean saccade amplitudes made more forward judgements. Although this relationship did not manifest on the single -subject level for most participants, SZP with more severe pos itive sy mptoms showed a greater reliance on saccade landing sites (Figure 4) , supporting the theory that abnormal CD is a key mechanism of psychosis (Feinberg & Guazzelli, 1999) . Altered thalamo -cortical connectivity in schizophrenia There is growing evidence that thalamo -cortical connectivity is impaired in schizophrenia and is closely related to symptomatology and functioning. Recent structural imaging studies found white matter alterations in thalamo -cortical circuits across differ ent illness stage s (see Canu, Agosta, & Filippi, 2015 for review ), which are consistent with altered functional connectivity in thalamo -cortical circuits (see Pergola, Selvaggi, Trizio, Bertolino, & Blasi, 2015 for review ). More specifically, pathways involving MDT and its project ion sites in frontal cortex seem to have the strongest support for being crucial to the pathophysiology of schizophrenia, though not many studies have examined individual thalamic nuclei (Anticevic, Cole, et al., 2014; Giraldo -Chica, Rogers, Damon, Landman, & Woodward, 2018; Shepherd, Laurens, Matheson, Carr, & Green, 2012; Woodward, Karbasforoushan, & Heckers, 2012). Importantly, thalamo - !6 frontal hypoconnectivity has also been found in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis and bipolar patients with a history of psychosis (Anticevic et al., 2015; Anticevic, Yan g, et al., 2014) , ruling out medication effects or illness chronicity as main contributors and suggesting that thalamo -frontal hypoconnectivity may relate to psychosis in a trans -diagnostic manner . Attesting to their clinical relevance, these white matt er alterations are further correlated with clinical symptoms , cognitive impairments, and treatment outcomes (see Canu et al. , 2015 for review ). Relevant to this study, there is evidence that SZP with passivity symptoms (i.e. symptoms related to a sense of losing agency over oneÕs body/thoughts, directly reflecting altered CD) have more compromised white matter integrity in thalamus, relative to SZP without passivity symptoms (Sim et al., 2009) . This provides indirect evidence for our hypothesis that impaired thalamo -cortical structural connectivity may underlie the CD abnormalities in schizophrenia, which we propose could be a key mechanism of symptom expression of the disease. In this study, we used diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and the blanking task to study white matter connectivity and oculomotor CD in SZP , respectively . DTI i s an imaging method commonly used to assess structural connectivity in human brains. Specifically, we tested the following three hypotheses: (1) there will be abnormal structural connectivity in the MD T-FEF pathway in SZP relative to HC ; (2) connectivity of the MD T-FEF pathway will be positively correlated with oculomotor indices of CD signal integrity; (3) the integrity of MD T-FEF white matter pathway will also be correlated inversely with positive symptom severity in SZP . !7 METHODS Overview Participants performed the blanking task and underwen t DTI . Putative white matter tracts connecting our regions of interest (ROIs) were computed using p robabilistic tractography , an analysis technique that reconstructs anatomical pathways (presumably white matter tracts) between any given brain regions based on a distribution profile of probable fiber orientations in each voxel (Behrens et al., 2003; Behrens, Berg, Jbabdi, Rushworth, & Woolrich, 2007) . The FEF was defined based on group -level functional MRI ( fMRI ) activation maps from the same subjects performing a different saccadic eye movement task in the scanner (Thakkar, van den Heiligenberg, Kahn, & Neggers, 2014) . The MDT was localized using the Harvard -Oxford thalamic connectivity atlas, a probabilistic atlas of 7 sub -thalamic regions segmented according to their white matter connectivity to cortical areas (Desikan et al., 2006) . ParticipantsÕ indiv idual probabilistic tracts in native space were normalized into a common space and averaged within groups. Then a threshold was applied to obtain final group tracts. Within each group -averaged tract, mean fractional anisotropy (FA) , mean diffusivity (MD) , axial diffusivity (AD), and radial diffusivity (RD) were then calculated for each participant. These indices of microstructural integrity of white matter tracts were used for further group comparisons and correlation analyses with behavioral indices of CD obtained from the blanking task, as well as symptom scores within the patient group. Participants Twenty -two antipsychotic -medicated SZP were recruited from a longitudinal study (Genetic Risk and Outcome in Psychosis (GROUP) Investigators, 2011) and an out patient psychi atric facility in The Netherlands. Schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder diagnoses were based on !8 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM -IV) criteria and verified with the Comprehensive Assessment of S ymptoms and History inter view (Andr easen, Flaum, & Arndt, 1992) or Schedules for Clinical Assessment for Neuropsychiatry, version 2.1 (Wing et al., 1990) . Chlorpromazine (CPZ) equivalent antipsychotic dosages were calcula ted for each patient (Woods, 2003) . Clinical symptoms were assessed with the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS ; Kay, Fiszbein, & Opler, 1987) . Twenty -four HC without a personal or family history of DSM -IV Axis I diagnosis were rec ruited via community adve rtise ments. Criteria for participant exclusion were a history of head trauma or neurologic al illness and recent substance abuse or dependence. All subjects gave written informed consent and were reimbursed for participation. The study was approved by the H uman Ethics Committee of the University Medical Center, Utrecht. Blanking task This task measure s the degree to which CD influences visual perception immediately following a saccade (Figure 1). Each trial start ed with the participant fixating on a red circle on a grey background in a dimly lit room. To reduce anticipation effect or stereotypical behavior (Collins et al., 2009) , the circle randomly appear ed at one of six locations (a combination of a 1¡ or !1¡ of visual angle displacement horizontally and a 0¡, 1¡, or !1¡ displacement vertically relative to the center of the screen) with equal probability . Once the eye tracker detect ed that the parti cipant ha d maintained fixation for 200 ms, the circle would turn black. After a random delay of 500-1000 ms, the circle reappear ed at a new location 10 ¡ to the left or right of the fixation position (pre -saccadic location ). The participant was instructed t o look at the stimulus as quickly as possible . Once the participant initiate d a saccade, the stimulus would disappear (i.e. blank) for 250 ms and reappear at a location (post -saccadic location ) somewhere between !3¡ to 3¡ (in !9 increments of 0.5 ¡) to the left or right of the pre -saccadic location and would stay on the screen for the remainder of the trial . The participant was then asked to report in which direction the stimulus jump ed relative to its pre -saccadic location by key pressing . Although participants indicated a left or right response, for analysis purpose s we recoded these responses: Òforward Ó means jumping away from the fixation position, and Òbackward Ó means jumping towards the fixation. The combination of 6 fixation positions " 13 post -saccadic locations (including 0 ¡ displacement) " 2 directions (left, right) result ed in 156 total trials. Duration of the task varied across participants and typically ranged from 15 to 30 min utes . See Rısler et al., 2015 for more details on the eye -tracking apparatus and task stimuli. Eighteen SZP and 17 HC reported in Rısler et al., 2015 also participated in this study and their data was used in the analyses involving blanking task performance. ParticipantsÕ performance on this paradigm was already analyzed and reported elsewhere (Rısler et al., 2015) . For this study , two measu res of CD were used in relevant statistical analyses: just noticeable difference ( JND ) and the loss slope. Briefly, for each individual, trials were collapsed across fixation positions and saccade directions. A logistic function was fit to the data plotting percentage of forward responses as a function of target displacement (i.e. the position of post -saccadic location relative to the pre -saccadic location ; see Figure 5 for examples ). A JND was derived from the function as the difference in target di splacements between the points where the function reached 50% and 75% of its full growth. This measure indicates individualsÕ sen sitivity to target displacement , which presumably relies on CD input to infer the pre -saccadic location. Therefore, smaller JND indicates greater CD integrity. Next, for each individual, trials were collapsed across target displacements . Landing site errors (i.e. distance between the actual saccade landing site and the pre -saccadic location) were divided into !10 multiple bins. Mean s accade landing site was then calculated for each bin. The loss slope was derived from a weighted linear regression where percentage of forward responses corresponding to each bin was fit against mean saccade landing site per bin , weighted by the number of trials per bin. Because individuals with intact CD signals should not rely on saccade landing sites to make perceptual judgments about target displacements, the slope should be almost zero in HC. More negative slope values indicate more reliance on landing site and therefore a larger loss of CD signals, hence the name Ò loss slope.Ó Image acquisition All DTI data were acquired at the University Medical Center Utrecht on a Philips Achieva 3T scanner (Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands) equipped with an eight -channel head coil allo wing parallel imaging. Two dif fusion images were acquired using singl e-shot echoplanar imaging sequences, consisting of 30 diffusion -weighted scans ( b = 1,000 s/mm 2) wi th noncollinear gradient direc tions and one im age without diffusion weighting (b = 0 s/ mm 2), covering the entire brain (Repetition Time (TR) = 7,057 ms; Ech o Time (TE) = 68 ms; field of view = 240 mm " 240 mm " 150 mm; in plane resolution = 1.875 mm " 1.875 mm; slice thickness = 2 mm; no slice gap; 75 axial slices; matrix size 128 mm " 99 mm). The diffusion weighted scans were measured twice, once with phase encoding direction r eversed (first scan posterior -anterior, second scan anterior -posterior), in order to correct for susceptibility induced spatial distortions (Andersson & Skare, 2002) . For registration purposes, a whole -brain three -dimensional T 1-weighted scan (200 slices; TR = 10 ms; TE = 4.6 ms; flip angle = 8¡ ; field of view, 240 mm " 240 mm " 160 mm; voxel size: 0.75 mm " 0.8mm " 0.75 mm) was acquired. !11 Regions of interest We included two regions of interest (ROI s) in each hemisphere : FEF and MD T. As FEF does not have clear anatomical boundaries, the group -level functional MRI ( fMRI ) activation maps from the same subjects performing a different saccadic eye movement task in the scanner (Thakkar et al., 2014) were used to define th is region . More specifically, FEF was cre ated based on areas that showed greater a ctivation on trials during which participants were making saccades versus where they were fixating (th reshold p < 0.001 uncorrected) across both groups . The thalamus was localized using the Harvard -Oxford thalamic connectivity atlas, which is a probabilistic atlas of 7 sub -thalamic regions segmented according to their white matter connectivity to cortical areas (Desikan et al., 2006) . MD T was operationally defined as the region of the atlas with highest probability ( # 25%) of connectivity to the prefrontal cortex. Both ROIs were limited to whi te matter structure only and did not include grey matter. Individual white matter masks were created by first segmenting the T1 image into grey matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid using SPM, and then extracting those voxels that had the highest probability of being identified as white matter. Preprocessing The diffusion -weighted scans were prepr ocessed and analyzed using FSL 5.0 (FMRIBÕs Software Library, www. fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). As DTI scans suffer from spatial dis tortions along the phase enc oding direction, two diffusion -weighted scans were acqu ired with reversed phase encod ing blips, resulting in pairs of images with distortions going in opposite directions. From these two images, the off -resonance field were estimated using a method similar to that de scribed by Andersson and Skare (2002) as imple mented in FSL (Smith et al., 2004) . Next, the 30 diffusion -weighted images from each phase -encoding direction were realigned to the b0 image !12 using affine registration, and eddy current correction was applied. The eddy -corrected scans with opposite phase encoding blips were then com bined into a single corrected image using the previously estimated off -resonance field. A brain mask was created for the mean b0 image and applied to all di ffusion -weighted images. DTI analyses must be performed in native space as diffusion gradients are specified in this space; however, ROIs were created in standard (Montreal neuroimaging; MNI) space. To transform the ROIs into each subjectÕs native space, the anatomical T1 -weighted volume was realigned to the mean b0 -weighted image and subsequently normal ized to MNI -space using the unified segmentation algo rithm as implemented in SPM8 (Ashburner & Friston, 2005) . The inverse warping parameters from this step were used to transform ROIs from MNI space to native space (Neggers, Za ndbelt, Schall, & Schall, 2015) . Probabilistic tractography We performed probabilistic tractography between MD T and FEF in both hemispheres from both directions (i.e. MD T to FEF and FEF to MD T), each serving as both a seed (start point) and a target (end point), since diffusion MRI cannot distinguish between forward and backward projections. The distribution profile of probabilistic connectivity i s computed by iteratively sending out 5000 streamlines from the seed area, going through all probab le principal diffusion directions in each voxel until it was determined impossible to continue . Only streamlines that reach ed the target successfully were included in the analysis , and streamlines were not allowed to continue after reaching the target. Two crossing fibers per vo xel were allowed. In addition , the mid-sagittal plane was used as exclusion mask to avoid streamlines travelling falsely into the other hemisphere (Landman et al., 2012; Mori & van Zijl, 2002; Mori & Zhang, 2006) . Upon examination of preliminary tractography results, an axial plane located one voxel beneath MDT !13 was added as another exclusion mask to avoid streamlines travelling in the opposite direction of FEF and looping back on themselves. For each subject , a Òcon nectivity valueÓ was generated for each voxel at the end of the tractography process by computing the number of streamlines going through the specific voxel. Therefore, higher values indicate a higher probabilit y that the voxel belongs to the tract of inte rest . In order to perform group analyses, individual tractography results were transformed to MNI spac e. To make sure only white matter structures were included in the analyses, each individualÕs whole brain white matter map was used as a mask to preserve connectivity values in these structures only. After that, these values were averaged across participants within each group . Connectivity values are not only determined by actual structural connectivity, but also influenced by other factors like size of the seed ROI (i.e. bigger seeds send out more streamlines) . Thus, higher connectivity values in a given tract do not necessar ily indicate a larger probability of a n actual structural connection. That is, the number of streamlines passing through a given voxel cannot be directly compared across seed/target combinations. Therefore, to avoid potential biases, we did not use the same absolute value for threshol ding each group -averaged tract but use d the top 0.27% ( 3$ above mean in normal distribution ) of all voxels as the threshold instead. After applying the thres hold, these group tracts were binarized into masks per tract. Masks from both directions ( i.e . FEF to MD T and MD T to FEF) were considered equally accurate and combined to achieve a single mas k for the MD T-FEF pathway per each group . For each participant , mean FA, MD, AD, and RD within each of the group -thresholded tracts were extracted. FA is currently the most widely used measure of anisotropy, highly sensitive to microstructural integrity of white matter tracts . Higher FA value means higher anisotropy and !14 lower diffusivity (e.g. better microstructural integrity) . However, FA value contains no information on the orientation of anisotropy and thus can be difficult to interpret in i solation . A decrease in FA could be caused by multiple factors: white matter neuropathology, fiber crossing, normal aging, etc . (Alexander, Lee, Lazar, & Field, 2007) . Therefore, it is recommended to u se multiple measures for better understanding of the microstructure of white matter . Specifically, RD appears to be a more sensitive indicator of myelination, while AD seems to indicate axonal degeneration (Song et al., 2002) . MD seems to be best at assessing white matter maturation and aging (Abe et al., 2002; Snook, Plewes, & Beaulieu, 2007) . Higher values for RD, AD, and MD mean higher diffusivity (e.g. poorer mic rostructural integrity) . Statistical analyses Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 20.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY) . First, to examine group difference s of tract locations , a probability value was calculated for each voxel by dividing the number of tracts going through the voxel by the total number of tracts in the MD T-FEF pathway in each hemisphere . This value indicates the probability of the specific voxel belonging to the actual MD T-FEF structural connection. Then between -group t -tests were performed on probability values at each voxel on the whole brain level with family -wise error correction using SPM . Next, to compare microstructural integrity of MD T-FEF white matter tracts , repeated measures AN OVAs were conducted on each of the four microstructural integrity measures (FA, MD, RD, and AD), including diagnostic group as a between -subjects variable and hemisphere as a within -subject variable . Since we we re interested in the specific infl uence of structural connections between MD T and FEF on behavior, mean integrity measures over the whole brain were regressed out of the tract measure s that did not differ between hemispheres . For measures with a significant !15 hemisphere effect or hemisphere by group interaction, mean integrity measur es over each hemisphere were regressed out of the corresponding tract measures. Thes e standardized residuals were used for all the correlation analyses. All standardized values are 0 when equal to the whole brain average of the corresponding measure. For s tandardized residuals of FA , higher values indicate more anisotropic voxels and lower more isotropic voxels (i.e. more impaired integrity) . For standardized residuals of MD, RD, and AD, higher values indicate more diffusive voxels, and lower values less di ffusive . We perform ed Pearson correlation analyses between two behavioral measures ( loss slope and JND ) and the standardized residuals . Using SZP data only, we conducted SpearmanÕs rank correlation analyses between PANSS subscale scores (positive and negative symptom s) and standardized residuals . To examine pote ntial confounding effects o f antipsychotic use, standardized medication dose was correlated with the behavioral measures and the microstructural integrity measures . !16 RESULTS Participant characteristics The SZP and HC were ma tched for sex, age, IQ, and handedness . See Table 1 for detailed participant characteristics. Only a subset of the sample (18 SZP, 17 HC) has both DTI and behavioral data. The two groups in this subset also did no t differ on sex, age, IQ, and handedness (Table S1) . Hypoconnectivity in MDT -FEF pathway The spatially normalized, group -averaged, and statistically thresholded MDT -FEF probabilistic tracts in both the left and right hemisphere are shown in Figure 6 and Figure 7. We performed a between -group t -test on whole brain probability values at each voxel. No significant differences were found between the SZP and HC at the family -wise error rate of 0.05 , indicating that the MDT -FEF pathway occupied the same spatial location in the brain. Next, we conducted four repeated measures AN OVAs where one of the microstructural integrity measures (FA, MD, RD, AD) was the dependent variable each time , using diagnostic group as a between -subjects variable and hemisphere as a within -subject variable (see Figure 8). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 2. Again, higher microstructural integrity is indicated by higher FA values and lower MD, RD, and AD values. For FA, MD, and RD, there was a signif icant effect of group such that SZP showed evidence of reduced microstructural integrity of the MDT -FEF pathway (FA: F(1,44) = 11.05 , p = 0.002 , partial !2 = 0.20 ; MD: F(1,44) = 4.69 , p = 0.036, partial !2 = 0.10; RD: F(1,44) = 10.88 , p = 0.002, partial !2 = 0.20) . There was no group difference in tract AD, F(1,44) = 0.001 , p = 0.98 , partial !2 = 0.00 . For FA and RD, t here was an additional group " hemisphere interaction effect (FA: F(1, 44) = 31.98, p < 0.001, partial !2 = 0.42 ; RD: F(1, 44) = 13.16, p = 0.001, partial !2 = 0.23), such that these !17 measures only differed significantly between groups in the right hemisphere (FA: t(44) = 5.86, p < 0.001; RD: t(44) = 4.63, p < 0.001; Table 2) . This group " hemisphere interaction effect was not significant for MD, F(1,44) = 0.42 , p = 0.52 , partial !2 = 0.01. Finally, there was no main effect of hemisphere for any of the measures. There were no correlation s between CPZ equivalent dose and any of the microstructural integrity measures (-0.44 < rs < 0.25, 0.07 < p < 0.98) . In the subsample of participants with behavioral data , there was still a main effect of group on FA, MD, and RD , such that patients showed reduced microstructural integrity . Complete results from this subsample are presented in Supplementary Results . Relationship with oculomotor CD Because we only found significant group differences in FA, MD, and RD, we performed Pearson correlation analyses between the standardized residuals of these three measures (after controlling for variance due to whole brain structural integrity) and two behavioral measures (loss slope and JND ). In SZP only, w e found a significant correlatio n between standardized resi dual of MD and JND (r = 0.57, p = 0.013 ) and a significant correlation between standardized residual of RD in the left tract and JND (r = 0.47, p = 0.049 ; see Figure 9). Combined, these results are cons istent with our predictions: higher JND, which is indicative of poorer CD signaling, is related to compromised white matter integrity in the MDT -FEF pathway in SZP. No correlations were found between the loss slope and the standardized residuals. No correlations were found between the blanking task measures and standardized residuals in HC. There were no correlations between CPZ equivalent dose and the behavioral measures in SZP (JND: rs = -0.31, p = 0.24; loss slope: rs = 0.42, p = 0.11). !18 Relationship with clinical symptoms Lastly , we conducted SpearmanÕs rank correlation analyses between PANSS subscale scores (positive and negative symptom s) and the three residual tract measures (FA, MD, RD) that were altered in SZP . We found a significant correlatio n between MD residual values and PANSS positive symptom score (rs = 0.43, p = 0.047 ) and between standardized residual of RD in the left tract and PANSS positive symptom score (rs = 0.48, p = 0.026 ; see Figure 10). No correlations were found between PANSS negative symptom score and the standardized residuals (MD: rs = 0.12, p = 0.58; left RD: rs = 0.08, p = 0.74) . Taken together, these correlations suggest that more severe positive symptoms in SZP, but not negative symptoms, were associated with compromised white matter integrity in the MDT -FEF pathway. !19 DISCUSSION In t his study, we examined whether SZP differed from HC on the microstructural integrity of the MDT -FEF pathway, and whether putative abnormalit ies were correlated with behavioral indices of oculomotor CD and psychotic symptoms. Using DTI and probabilistic tractography, we found that SZP and HC shared the same spatial location of the tracts connecting MDT and FEF. However, SZP had compromised microstructur al integrity of these tracts, such that they had lower FA and higher MD and RD than HC. This hypoconnectivity was found to further correlate with behavioral indices of reduced oculomotor CD signaling and more severe positive symptoms in SZP, indicating a p oten tial disease mechanism that has specific behavioral correlates and symptom implications. Our finding that SZP had reduced MDT -FEF structural connectivity is consistent with recent findings of white matter alterations in prefrontal -thalam ic pathways in SZP (Canu et al., 2015; Gi raldo -Chica et al., 2018) . This was also consistent with previous functional connectivity findings of reduced prefrontal -MDT connectivity in SZP (Anticevic, Cole, et al., 2014; Welsh, Chen, & Taylor, 2010; Woodward et al., 2012) and thalamo -prefrontal hyp oconnectivity in clinical high risk populations (Anticevic et al., 2015) . Notably , there have been consistent case reports on patients with MDT lesions experiencing psychotic -like symptoms such as hallucinations and delusions (Carrera & Bogousslavsky, 2006; Crail -Melendez, Atriano -Mendieta, Carrillo -Meza, & Ramirez -Bermudez, 2013; Zhou e t al., 2015) , supporting the link between thalamo -cortical communication and positive symptom formation. Besides decreased FA, SZP also exhibited increased MD and RD in the MDT -FEF pathway in current study. As a marker of normal aging (Abe et al., 2002) , our finding of increased MD in SZP provides indirect evidence supporting the accelerated aging theory of !20 schizophrenia (see Islam, Mulsant, Voineskos, & Rajji, 2017 for review ). In fact, a recen t study found that the rate of white matter decline was 60% steeper in SZP than in HC and the difference was present throughout the life span (Cropley et al., 2017) . On the neurobiological level, incre ased RD in SZP indicated that the hypoconnectivity might be related to myelin and oligodendroglia dysfunctions (Davis et al., 2003) . Somewhat surprisingly , we also found a significant group by hemisphere interaction effect in FA and RD. To date, very few studies have assessed the connectivity between specific thalamic nuclei and cortical areas in SZP. In the few studies that examined MDT -cortical functional connectivity, a group by hemisphere interaction was either not mo deled, not reported , or not found (Anticevic, Cole, et al., 2014; Welsh et al., 2010; Woodward et al., 2012) . One recent structural imaging study that found decrea sed prefrontal -MDT connectivity in SZP reported a more prominent difference in the left hemisphere (Giraldo -Chica et al., 2018) , contrary to our finding of a more prominent difference in the right hemisphere. However, there were a few ke y differences between their study and the current one. First, the SZP group in their study was approximately 10 years younge r and was earlier in the course of illness . Second , and perhaps more importantly , their main connectivity analyses used Òtotal connectivityÓ values (calculated by dividing the number of streamlines reaching a certain cortical area by the number of streamlines reaching all cortical regions from thalamus), rather than indices of microstructural integrity such as FA and MD . In other words, their group comparison revealed the differences in whole brain thalamo -cortical connectivity patterns (i.e. how thalamo -cortical connectivity was distributed among different cortical regions) , rather than the absolute difference on microstructural l evel within a specific thalamo -cortical pathway such as prefrontal -thalamic !21 connection . Clearly, more research is needed to address whether there is a hemispheric effect to the altered thalamo -prefrontal connectivity, or thalamo -cortical connectivity in ge neral, in SZP. The most novel and arguably most important finding of current study is that we found a relationship between the MDT -FEF structural connectivity and oculomotor CD signal integrity , mirroring findings from primate neurophysiology studies (Sommer & Wurtz, 2008) . In other words, one interpretation of the current findings is that due to reduced connectivity between MDT and FEF, oculomotor CD signals may have been lost or disrupted , resulting in poorer behavioral performance on the blanking task. Presumably, the other consequence of these compromised CD signals were frequent inaccurate predictions about visual inputs, which then led to constant false PEs that were ultimately Òresolved Ó by abnormal perceptual experiences. This could potentially explain the correlation between the MDT -FEF hypoconnectivity and positive symptom severity and the lack of correlation with negative symptom severity . Because whole brain white matter connectivity differences were already regressed out in the correlation analyses, this relationship between white matter integrity and positive symptom w as specific to the MDT -FEF path way rather than a result of general reduced structural connectivity . Taken together, the MDT -FEF white matter tracts seem to play a key role in transmitting oculomotor CD signals that may also explain some of the symptom manifestation in SZP. This hig hlights the importance of thalamo -cortical connectivity as a potential biomarker of and/or a treatment target for schizophrenia (Ramsay & MacDonald, 2018) . The in terpretation of current findings is limited by several factors. First , JND may not be the best indicator of CD signaling in the blanking task because it is also affected by general sources of noise (e.g. in visual input or in effector output) . Indeed, it has been found that SZP have a wide range of visual processing deficits (reviewed in Yoon, Sheremata, Rokem, & Silver, !22 2013). A more sensitive index of CD signaling would be the loss slope obtained from the blanking task. However, we did not find any correlations between the loss slope and the microstructural integrity indices. One potential explanation is that SZP may fail to encode the pre -saccadic target locations properly due to impaired spatial working memory (Lee & Park, 2005), resulting in noisier perceptual judgments on the single -trial level. Consequently, due to a relatively small trial number , the regression analyses may not be powerful enough to obtain the most accurate estimates of loss slopes. Future studies utilizing more trials may help resolve this issue . Second , we would expect that a ltered CD should most directly relate to symptoms explained by agency loss; however, we did not include measures that assessed agency disturbances directly. Future studies should employ a more comprehensive and detailed measure on agency disruptions to tes t a more fine -grained hypothesis of the role that the MDT -FEF pathway plays in the PE model of psychosis. Nevertheless, altered CD has been posited as a general mechanism underlying hallucinations and delusions (Corlett et al., 2016) , which were both captured by the PANSS positive symptom score. Third, SZP in the current study were relatively high -functioning and exhibit ed a limited range of symptom severity. Future studies using a more clinically heterogeneous sample (e.g. recent -onset and unmedicated patients) could further inform the relationships between thalamo -cortical structural connectivity and clinical status in SZP. Lastly, we were not able to rule out potential con founding effects of antipsychotic treatment because current study used a medicated sample. Ho wever, the CPZ equivalent dose did not correlate with any of the behavioral or microstructural measures. We anticipate that findings from current study will pave t he way for more in -depth mechanistic questions on CD and psychosis . In terms of future directions, studies utilizing larger sample size are necessary to obtain adequate power for regression analyses in which white matter !23 connectivity indices, behavioral in dices of oculomotor CD, and symptom severity could be entered into one model, allowing us to test the hypothesis that structural connectivity in the MDT -FEF pathway directly mediates the relationship between oculomotor CD signaling abnormalities and positi ve symptom severity . Second, to better inform treatment development, a causal or directional relationship between white matter integrity, oculomotor CD signals, and psychotic symptoms is necessary. F uture studies could try to establish directions in the br ain -behavior correlations observed in current study by following clinical high risk populations over time and measuring their white matter integrity and behavioral indices of CD signaling before and after the onset of illness. Third, it is important to know whether altered CD is a ph enotype of psychosis in general or a specific biomarker of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. To this date , only one study has found that CD in the auditory domain was similarly impaired in SZP and bipolar patients with a history of psychotic features (Ford et al., 2013) . More replications in recent -onset affective and non -affe ctive psychotic disorders, clinical high risk population s, and unaffected first -degree relatives are needed to address this question. The p resence of altered CD in healthy relatives would provide ev idence that CD abnormalities represent vulnerability towards psychosis, but that other factors are involved in the expression of full -blown psychosis. In conclusion, we identified significant decreases in white matter connectivity in SZP relative to HC in the MDT -FEF pathway, which belongs to a key neur al circuit transmitting oculomotor CD signals previously established in the animal literature. We demonstrated that hypoconnectivity in the MDT -FEF pathway was correlated with impaired oculomotor CD signals a nd more severe positive symptoms in SZP. These findings have important disease mechanism and treatment implications. !24 APPENDICES !! !25 APPENDIX A: Tables Table 1. Demographic information . HC ( n = 24) SZP ( n = 22) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Statistic p Age (Years) 34.9 (8.1) 37.4 (7.8) t = -1.1 0.29 Sex (Female/Male) 11/13 5/17 !2 = 2.7 0.13 IQa 100.4 (13.8) 96.0 (12.8) t = 1.1 0.29 Education b 7.2 (1.3) 4.8 (1.7) t = 5.2 < .001 Handedness c 0.89 (0.41) 0.85 (0.45) t = 0.35 0.73 Illness Duration (Years) 14.2 (5.2) PANSS Positive 11.6 (5.2) PANSS Negative 13.1 (6.3) PANSS General 25.0 (7.9) CPZ Equivalent (mg) 281.3 (249.6) Notes: CPZ, chlorpromazine; HC, healthy control subjects; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; SZP, patients with schizophrenia. aBased on the Nederlandse Leestest voor Volwassenen. bEducation category: 0 = <6 years of primary education; 1 = finished 6 years of primary education; 2 = 6 years of primary education and low -leve l secondary education; 3 = 4 years of low-level secondary education; 4 = 4 years of averag e-level secondary education; 5 = 5 years of averag e-level secondary education; 6 = 4 years of se condary vocational training; 7 = 4 years of high -level professional ed ucation; 8 = university degree. cBased on the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory; scores range from 0 indicating complete left -handedness to 1 indicating complete right -handedness. !26 Table 2. Mean microstructural integrity measures (FA, MD, RD, AD) in the MDT -FEF tract by hemisphere. HC ( n = 24) SZP ( n = 22) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) FA Left 0.41 (0.02) 0.41 (0.02) Right 0.42 (0.02) 0.39 (0.02) MD Left 7.05 " 10 -4 (2.40 " 10 -5) 7.18 " 10 -4 (2.65 " 10 -5) Right 7.06 " 10 -4 (1.84 " 10 -5) 7.21 " 10 -4 (2.51 " 10 -5) RD Left 5.43 " 10 -4 (2.57 " 10 -5) 5.56 " 10 -4 (2.46 " 10 -5) Right 5.36 " 10 -4 (1.82 " 10 -5) 5.66 " 10 -4 (2.59 " 10 -5) AD Left 1.03 " 10 -3 (3.72 " 10 -5) 1.04 " 10 -3 (3.70 " 10 -5) Right 1.05 " 10 -3 (3.12 " 10 -5) 1.03 " 10 -3 (2.92 " 10 -5) Notes: AD: axial diffusivity ; FA, fractional anisotropy ; FEF, frontal eye fields ; HC, healthy controls; MD, mean diffusivity ; MDT, mediodorsal thalamus ; RD, radial diffusivity ; SZP, schizophrenia patients. !27 APPENDIX B: Figures Figure 1. Blanking task. Dotted circles represent gaze position s (Rısler et al., 2015) . !28 Figure 2. Blanking task perceptual judgment. Top: After the stimulus appears at the pre -saccadic location, a corollary discharge (CD) vector can be computed accordingly when preparing for the saccade initiation. In this example, the predicted saccade landing site (grey cross) will fall short of the targe t. Bottom: Upon saccade initiation, the stimulus will disappear and reappear to the left or right of the pre -saccadic location. If participant can use CD to remap the pre -saccadic location correctly, then vector a should match the actual stimulus displacem ent. Here in this example, the post -saccadic location is to the left of the pre -saccadic location, so the participant should judge the displacement as backward. However, if participant does not have access to complete CD information, saccade landing site m ay be used as a proxy of the pre -saccadic location. In this case, participant will perceive vector b as the target displacement and give a forward response instead (Rısler et al., 2015) . !29 Figure 3 . Blanking task performance in chronic SZP and HC. Mean saccade amplitude was correlated with judgment on pre -saccadic location of the stimulus in SZP, but not in HC (Rısler et al., 2015) . !30 Figure 4 . Blanking task performance and symptom severity in chronic SZP. When making perceptual judgments of target displacement, SZP with more severe p ositive symptom showed greater reliance on actual saccade landing site, consistent with less influence of CD signals (Rısler et al., 2015) . !31 Figure 5 . Group -average d blanking task performance in chronic SZP and HC. Mean percentage of forward responses averaged over each group as a function of target displacement. On the x -axis, negative values mean backward jumps (towards the fixation) and positive value s mean forward jumps (away from the fixation) relative to the pre -saccadic location (Rısler et al., 2015). !32 Figure 6. Probabilistic tractography results of the MDT -FEF pathway in both groups. Top panel: ROI masks projected onto a standard -space brain in multi -slice coronal view; bottom panel: group -averaged and thresholded white matter tracts in standard space. ROIs are color -coded as follows: FEF Ð green, MDT Ð yellow. Group tracts are color -coded as follows: HC tracts Ð red, SZP tracts Ð blue, overlapping areas Ð pink. Coordinates of each slice in standard space were presented in the middle. !33 Figure 7. 3D Probabilistic tractography results of the MDT -FEF pathway in HC. 3D-rendered representations of the group -averaged and thresholded white matter tracts reverse normalized and overlaid on an HC participantÕs white matter skeleton, then projected onto the same participantÕs structural image using AFNI FATCAT Visualization. Results are essentially indistinguishable in SZP. ROIs are color -coded as follows: FEF Ð red; MDT Ð yellow. Fiber tract reconstructions are colored according to the DTI visualization convention (blue: superior -inferior, green: a nterior -posterior, red: left -right). !34 Figure 8. Group differences in the MDT -FEF tract microstructural integrity by hemisphere. Top left: FA; top right: MD; bottom left: RD; bottom right AD . AD: axial diffusivity ; FA, fractional anisotropy ; FEF, frontal eye fields ; HC, healthy controls; MD, mean diffusivity ; MDT, mediodorsal thalamus ; RD, radial diffusivity ; SZP, schizophrenia patients. !"#$!"#%!"&!!"&'!"&&!"&$!"&%()*+,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,-./0+ 123!3&3%()*+,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,-./0+453$3'$%6,7!899'9&9%$'6,7!89()*+,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,-./0+ -59$$!7!!7!977!25()*+,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,-./0+ 6,7!89 !35 Figure 9. Scatterplots of JND against MDT -FEF tract -specific indices of microstructural integrity. Left: standardized residuals of MD acr oss hemispheres ; right: standardized residuals of RD of the left tract . These standardized values are 0 when equal to the whole brain MD or RD average, higher for more diffusive voxels and lower for less diffusive voxels. Smaller JND indicates more intact oculomotor CD signals. More compromised oculomotor CD signals correlated with more impaired white matter microstructure in SZP only. HC, healthy controls; JND, just noticeable difference; MD, mean diffusivity ; RD, radial diffusivity ; SZP, schizophrenia patients. !"!#!$%$#"%&%%&#%&'%&(%&)$&%*+,-.,/.012.3/240.5,63783+/,9+3:;<=;>?*@A!"!#!$%$#%&%%&#%&'%&(%&)$&%*+,-.,/.012.3/240.5,63783628+3+/,9+3B;<=; !36 Figure 10. Scatterplots of PANSS positive symptoms scores against MDT -FEF tract -specific indices of microstructural integrity. Left: standardized residuals of MD across hemispheres; right: standardized residual s of RD of the left tract. These standardized values are 0 when equal to the whole brain MD or RD average, higher for more diffusive voxels and lower for less diffusive voxels. Larger PANSS scores indicate more severe symptoms. More severe positive symptom s correlated with more impaired white matter microstructure. MD, mean diffusivity ; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale ; RD, radial diffusivity . !"!#$#"%#$#&"$"&'()*+),+-./+0,/1-+2)30450(,)6(0789:;''0<41-(-=/01>?<(4?10164,/!"!#$#"&#$#&"$"&'()*+),+-./+0,/1-+2)304503/5(0(,)6(0@89:;''0<41-(-=/01>?<(4?10164,/ !37 APPENDIX C: Supplementary Results Using the smaller subsample with behavioral data (see Table S1 for demographic information), the spatially normalized, group -averaged, and statistically thresholded MDT -FEF probabilistic tracts were very similar to those shown in Figure 6 and Figure 7. We performed a between -group t -test on whole brain probability values at each voxel. Again, no significant differences were found between the SZP and HC at the family -wise error rate of 0.05, indicating that the MDT -FEF pathway occupied the same spatial locat ion in the brain. Next, we conducted four repeated measures AN OVAs where one of the microstructural integrity measures (FA, MD, RD, AD) was the dependent variable each time , using diagnostic group as a between -subjects variable and hemisphere as a within -subject variable. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table S2. Again, higher microstructural integrity is indicated by higher FA values and lower MD, RD, and AD values. For FA, MD, and RD, there was a significant effect of group such that SZP showed evidence of reduced microstructural integrity of the MDT -FEF pathway (FA: F(1,33) = 23.03 , p < 0.001, partial !2 = 0.41 ; MD: F(1,33) = 6.61 , p = 0.015, partial !2 = 0.17; RD: F(1,33) = 19.61 , p < 0.001, partial !2 = 0.37) . There was no group difference in tract AD, F(1,33) = 0.34 , p = 0.57, partial !2 = 0.01 . For FA and RD, there was an additional hemisphere effect such that the MDT -FEF pathway in the right hemisphere showed lower microstructural integrity than that in the left hemisphere across groups (FA: F(1,33) = 9.56 , p = 0.004, partial !2 = 0.23 ; RD: F(1,33) = 4.26 , p = 0.047, partial !2 = 0.11). This hemisphere effect was not significant for MD, F(1,33) = 2.98 , p = 0.09, partial !2 = 0.08. For FA only, there was a group " hemisphere interaction effect, F(1, 33) = 7.97, p = 0.008, partial !2 = 0.19, such that the hemisphere effect was only significant in SZP, t(17) = 4.01, p = 0.001, but not in HC, t(16) = 0.20, p = 0.84 . This group " hemisphere interaction effect was not !38 significant for MD or RD (MD: F(1,33) = 0.00 , p = 1.0, partial !2 = 0.00; RD: F(1,33) = 3.58 , p = 0.07, partial !2 = 0.10). There were no correlations between CPZ equivalent dose and any of the microstructural integri ty measures (-0.49 < rs < 0.15, 0.06 < p < 0.77) . !39 Table S1. Demographic information of the subset sample with behavioral data HC ( n = 17) SZP ( n = 18) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Statistic p Age (Years) 34.1 (8.9) 36.7 (8.2) t = -0.9 0.37 Sex (Female/Male) 7/10 2/16 !2 = 4.1 0.06 IQa 99.2 (14.6) 95.8 (12.2) t = 0.7 0.48 Education b 7.2 (1.1) 4.9 (1.8) t = 4.6 < .001 Handedness c 0.85 (0.49) 0.93 (0.20) t = -0.7 0.49 Illness Duration (Years) 14.6 (5.4) PANSS Positive 11.4 (5.2) PANSS Negative 12.6 (6.1) PANSS General 24.6 (7.9) CPZ Equivalent (mg) 302.0 (257.7) !Notes: CPZ, chlorpromazine; HC, healthy control subjects; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; SZP, patients with schizophrenia. aBased on the Nederlandse Leestest voor Volwassenen. bEducation category: 0 = <6 years of primary education; 1 = finished 6 years of primary education; 2 = 6 years of primary education and low -level secondary education; 3 = 4 years of low-level secondary education; 4 = 4 years of averag e-level secondary education; 5 = 5 years of averag e-level secondary education; 6 = 4 years of se condary vocational training; 7 = 4 years of high -level professional education; 8 = university degree. cBased on the Edinburgh Handedness In ventory; scores range from 0 indicating complete left -handedness to 1 indicating complete right -handedness. !! !40 Table S2. Mean microstructural integrity measures (FA, MD, RD, AD) in the MDT -FEF tract by hemisphere in the subset !sample with behavioral data . HC ( n = 17) SZP ( n = 18) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) FA Left 0.43 (0.02) 0.41 (0.02) Right 0.43 (0.02) 0.39 (0.02) MD Left 6.98 " 10 -4 (2.15 " 10 -5) 7.16 " 10 -4 (2.42 " 10 -5) Right 7.01 " 10 -4 (1.92 " 10 -5) 7.20 " 10 -4 (2.30 " 10 -5) RD Left 5.27 " 10 -4 (2.22 " 10 -5) 5.54 " 10 -4 (2.24 " 10 -5) Right 5.28 " 10 -4 (1.87 " 10 -5) 5.63 " 10 -4 (2.31 " 10 -5) AD Left 1.04 " 10 -3 (3.65 " 10 -5) 1.04 " 10 -3 (3.50 " 10 -5) Right 1.05 " 10 -3 (3.31 " 10 -5) 1.03 " 10 -3 (2.95 " 10 -5) !Notes: AD: axial diffusivity ; FA, fractional anisotropy ; FEF, frontal eye fields ; HC, healthy controls; MD, mean diffusivity ; MDT, mediodorsal thalamus ; RD, radial diffusivity ; SZP, schizophrenia patients. !41 REFERENCES !42 REFERENCES Abe, O., Aoki, S., Hayashi, N., Yamada, H., Kunimatsu, A., Mori, H., É Ohtomo, K. (2002). Normal aging in the central nervous system: Quantitative MR diffusion -tensor analysis. Neurobiology of Aging, 23(3), 433 Ð441. Alexander, A. L., Lee, J. E., Lazar, M., & Field, A. S. (2007). Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain. Neurotherapeutics, 4(3), 316Ð329. Andersson, J . L. R., & Skare, S. (2002). A model -based method for retrospective correction of geometric distortions in diffusion -weighted EPI. NeuroImage, 16(1), 177Ð199. Andreasen, N. C. (1999). A unitary model of s chizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(9), 781Ð787. Andreasen, N. C., Flaum, M., & Arndt, S. (1992). The Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History (CASH). An instrument for assessing diagnosis and psychopathology. Archives of Gene ral Psychiatry, 49(8), 615Ð623. Anticevic, A., Cole, M. W., Repovs, G., Murray, J. D., Brumbaugh, M. S., Winkler, A. M., É Glahn , D. C. (2014). Characterizing thala mo-cortical disturbances in schizophrenia and bipolar i llness. Cere bral Cortex, 24(12), 3116Ð3130. Anticevic, A., Haut, K., Murray, J. D., Repovs, G., Yang, G. J., Diehl, C., É Cannon , T. D. (2015). Association of thalamic dysconnectivity and conversion t o psychosis in youth and young adults at elevated clinical r isk. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(9), 882Ð891. Anticevic, A., Yang, G., Savic, A., Murray, J. D., Cole, M. W., Repovs, G., É Glahn, D. C. (2014). Mediodorsal and visual thalamic connectivity differ in sch izophrenia and bipolar disorder with and without psychosis h istory. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(6), 1227Ð1243. Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2005). Unified segmentation. NeuroImage, 26(3), 839Ð851. Behrens, T. E. J., Berg, H. J., Jbabdi, S., Rushworth, M. F. S., & Woolrich, M. W. (2007). Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: What can we gain? NeuroImage, 34(1), 144Ð155. Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., Jenkinson, M., Johansen -Berg, H., Nunes, R. G., Clare, S., É Smith, S. M. (2003). Characterization and propagation of uncertainty in diffusion -weighted MR imaging. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 50(5), 1077Ð1088. Blakemore, S. -J., Smith, J., Steel, R., Johnstone, E. C., & Frith, C. D. (2000). The perception of self -produced sensory stimuli in patients with auditory hallucinations and passivity experiences: evidence for a breakdown in self -monitoring. Psychological Medicine, 30(5), !43 1131Ð1139. Canu, E., Agosta, F., & Filippi, M. (2015). A selective review of structural connectivity abnormalities of schizophrenic patients at different stages of the disease. Schizop hrenia Research, 161(1), 19Ð28. Carrera, E., & Bogous slavsky, J. (2006). The thalamus and behavior: Effects of anatomically distinct strokes . Neurology, 66(12), 1817Ð1823. Cavanagh, P., Hunt, A. R., Afraz, A., & Rolfs, M. (2010). Visual stability based on remapping of attention pointers. Trends in Cogni tive Sciences, 14(4), 147Ð153. Cavanaugh, J., Berman, R. A., Joiner, W. M., & Wurtz, R. H. (2016). Saccadic corollary discharge underlies stable visual p erception. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(1), 31Ð42. Collins, T., Rolfs, M., Deubel, H., & Cavanagh, P. (200 9). Post -saccadic location judgments reveal remapping of saccade targets to non -foveal locations . Journal of Vision, 9(5), 1Ð9. Corlett, P. R., Honey, G. D., & Fletcher, P. C. (2016). Prediction error, ketamine and psychosis: An updated model. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(11), 1Ð11. Crail -Melendez, D., Atriano -Mendieta, C., Carrillo -Meza, R., & Ramirez -Bermudez, J. (2013). Schizophrenia -like psychosis associated with right lacunar thalamic in farct. Neurocase, 19(1), 22Ð26. Cropley, V. L., Klauser, P ., Lenroot, R. K., Bruggemann, J., Sundram, S., Bousman, C., É Zalesky, A. (2017). Accelerated gray and white matter deterioration with age in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 174(3), 286Ð295. Davis, K. L., Stewart, D. G., Friedman, J. I., B uchsbaum, M., Harvey, P. D., Hof, P. R., É Haroutunian, V. (2003). White matter changes in s chizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(5), 443-456. Desikan, R. S., S”gonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., É Killiany, R. J. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of intere st. NeuroImage, 31(3), 968Ð980. Deubel, H., Schneider, W. X., & Bridgeman, B. (1996). Postsaccadic target blanking prevents saccadic suppression of image displacement. Vision Research, 36(7), 985Ð996. Feinberg, I. (1978). Efference copy and corollary discharge: implications for thinking and its disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 4(4), 636Ð640. Feinberg, I., & Guazzelli, M. (1999). Schizophrenia --a disorder of the corollary discharge systems that integrate the motor systems of thought with the sensory systems of consciousness. The Britis h Journal of Psychiatry, 17 4(3), 196Ð204. !44 Fletcher, P. C., & Frith, C. D. (2009). Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nature Revie ws. Neuroscience, 10(1), 48Ð58. Ford, J. M., & Mathalon, D. H. (2012). Anticipating the future: Automatic prediction failures in schizophrenia. International Journal of Ps ychophysiology, 83(2), 232Ð239. Ford, J. M., Mathalon, D. H., Roach, B. J., Keedy, S. K., Reilly, J. L., Gershon, E. S., & Sweeney, J. A. (2013). Neurophysiological evidence of corollary discharge function during vocalization in psychotic patients and their nonpsychotic first -degree r elatives. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(6), 1272Ð1280. Gaymard, B., Rivaud, S., & Pierrot -Deseilligny, C. (1994). Impai rment of extraretinal eye position signals after central thalamic lesions in humans. Experimental Brain Research, 102, 1Ð9. Genetic Risk and Outcome in Psychosis (GROUP) I nvestigators. (2011). Evidence that familial liability for psychosis is expressed as differential sensitivity to c annabis: an analysis of patient -sibling and sibling -control pairs. Archives of Gene ral Psychiatry, 68(2), 138Ð147. Giraldo -Chica, M., Rogers, B. P., Damon, S. M., Landman, B. A., & Wood ward, N. D. (2018). Prefrontal -thalamic anatomical connectivity and executive c ognitive function in schizophrenia. Biologi cal Psychiatry, 83(6), 509Ð517. Gray, J. A., Feldon, J., Rawlins, J. N. P., Hemsley, D. R., & Smith, A. D. (1991). The neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14(1), 1Ð20. Green, M. F., & K insbourne, M. (1990). Subvocal activity and auditory hallucinations: clues for behavioral t reatments? Schizoph renia Bulletin, 16(4), 617Ð625. Hallett, P. E., & Lightstone, A. D. (1976a). Saccadic eye movements to flashed targets. Vision Research, 16(1), 107Ð114. Hallett, P. E., & Lightstone, A. D. (1976b). Saccadic eye movements towards stimuli triggered by prior saccades. Vision Research, 16(1 ), 99Ð106. Islam, F., Mulsant, B. H., Voineskos, A. N. , & Rajji, T. K. (2017). Brain -derived neurotrophic factor expression in individuals with s chizophrenia a nd healthy aging: Testing the accelerated aging hypothesis of s chizophrenia. Current Psychiatry Reports, 19(7), 36. Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A., & Opler, L. A. (1987). The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for s chizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13(2), 261Ð276. Landman, B. A., Bogovic, J. A., Wan, H., ElShahaby, F. E. Z., Bazin, P. -L., & Prince, J. L. (2012). Resolution of crossing fibers with constrained compressed sensing using diffusion tensor MRI. Neu roImage, 59(3), 2175Ð2186. !45 Lee, J., & Park, S. (2005). Wor king memory impairments in schizophrenia: A meta -analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 599Ð611. Mori, S., & van Zijl, P. C. M. (2002). Fiber tracking: principles and strategies - a technical review. NMR in Biomedicine, 15(7 Ð8), 468Ð480. Mori, S., & Z hang, J. (2006). Principles of diffusion tensor imaging and its applications to basic neuroscience r esearch. Neuron, 51(5), 527Ð539. Neggers, S. F. W., Zandbelt, B. B., Schall, M. S., & Schall, J. D. (2015). Comparative diffusion tractography of c orticostriatal motor pathways reveals differences between humans and macaques. Journal of Neurophysiology, 113(7), 2164 Ð2172. Ostendorf, F., Liebermann, D., & Ploner, C. J. (2010). Human thalamus contributes to perceptual stability across eye movements. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(3), 1229Ð1234. Pergola, G., Selvaggi, P., Trizio, S., Bertolino, A., & Blasi, G. (2015). The role of the thalamus in schizophrenia from a neuroimaging perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 54, 57Ð75. Ramsay, I. S., & MacDonald, A. W. (2018). The ups and downs of thalamocortical connectivity in s chizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 83(6), 473Ð474. Rısler, L., Rolfs, M., van der Stigchel, S., Neggers, S. F. W., Cahn, W., Kahn, R. S., & Thak kar, K. N. (2015). Failure to use corollary discharge to remap visual target locations is associated with psychotic symptom severity in schizophrenia. Journal of Neurophysiology, 114(2), 1129Ð1136. Schneider, K. (1959). Clinical Psychopathology. New York, NY: Grune &Stratton. Shepherd, A. M., Laurens, K. R., Matheson, S. L., Carr, V. J., & Green, M. J. (2012). Systematic meta -review and quality assessment of the structural brain alterations in schizophrenia. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(4), 1342Ð1356. Shergill, S. S., Samson, G., Bays, P. M., Frith, C. D., & Wolpe rt, D. M. (2005). Evidence for sensory prediction deficits in s chizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(12), 2384Ð2386. Sherman, S. M. (2016). Thalamus plays a central rol e in ongoing cortical func tioning. Nature Neuroscience, 19 (4), 533Ð541. Sim, K., Yang, G. L., Loh, D., Poon, L. Y., Sitoh, Y. Y., Verma, S., É Pantelis, C. (2009). White matter abnormalities and neurocognitive deficits associated with the passivity phenomenon in schizophrenia: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychiatry Resea rch - Neuroimaging, 172(2), 121Ð127. !46 Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Johansen -Berg, H., É Matthews, P. M. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuro Imag e, 23(SUPPL. 1), S208 ÐS219. Snook, L., Plewes, C., & Beaulieu, C. (2007). Voxel based versus region of interest analysis in diffusion tensor imaging of neurodevelopme nt. NeuroImage, 34(1), 243Ð252. Sommer, M . A., & Wurtz, R. H. (2002). A pathway in prima te brain for internal monitoring of movements. Science, 296(5572), 1480Ð1482. Sommer, M. A., & Wurtz, R. H. (2004). What the brain stem tells the frontal c ortex. II. Role of the SC -MD-FEF pathway in corollary d ischarge. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91(3), 1403Ð1423. Sommer, M. A., & Wurtz, R. H. (2008). Brain circuits for the internal monitoring of movements. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 317Ð338. Song, S. -K., Sun, S. -W., Ramsbottom, M. J., Chang, C., Russell, J., & Cross , A. H. (2002). Dysmyelinatio n revealed through MRI as increased radial (but unchanged axial) diffusion of w ater. NeuroImage, 17(3), 1429Ð1436. Thakkar, K. N., Diwadkar, V. A., & Rolfs, M. (2017) . Oculomotor prediction: A window into the psychotic m ind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(5), 344Ð356. Thakkar, K. N., van den Heiligenberg, F. M. Z., Kahn, R. S., & Neg gers, S. F. W. (2014). Frontal -subcortical circuits involved in reactive control and monitoring of g aze. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(26), 8918Ð8929. Welsh, R. C., Chen, A. C., & Taylor, S. F. (2010). Low -frequency BOLD fluctuations demonstrate altered thalamocortical connectivity in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(4), 713Ð722. Wing, J. K., Babor, T., Brugha, T., Burke, J., Cooper, J. E., Gi el, R., É Sartorius, N. (1990). SCAN: Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry. Archives of Gene ral Psychiatry, 47(6), 589Ð593. Woods , S. W. (2003). Chlorpromazine equivalent doses for the newer atypical a ntipsychotics. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 64(6), 663Ð667. Woodward, N. D., Karbasforoushan, H., & Heckers, S. (2012). Thalamocortical dysconnectivity in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(10), 1092Ð1099. Yoon, J. H., Sheremata, S. L., Rokem, A., & Silver, M. A. (2013 ). Windows to the soul: vision science as a tool for studying biological mechanisms of information processing deficits in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychology, 4 , 681 . !47 Zhou, Y., Fox, D., Anand, A., Elhaj, A., Kapoor, A., Najibi, F., É Jaya m-Trouth, A. (2015). Artery of percheron infarction as an unusual cause of KorsakoffÕs s yndrome. Case Reports in Ne urological Medicine, 2015, 1Ð6.