NW mWWWWWWNM Wavelength ——> Status —> Success 0.36 54.5% 81.0% 846% Figure 5. Path analysis of the relationship of the predictor variables (age, %fat, % water, and wavelength) with male territorial status and mating success. This path diagram represents an hypothesis of causation for evaluation. Single- headed arrows represent a causal relationship between two variables while double-headed arrows denote correlations without causation. The degree to which the variation in a variable is accounted for (indicated below diagram) can be calculated by summing the products of each path coefficient with the respective variables’ correlation coefficient (Li, 1981). 45 %Fat 0.37 0.35 0.28 Territorial 0.92 . Mating %Water —> Status Success -0.23 Age I 84.6% 39.1% Figure 6. Alternative path diagram resulting from the exclusion of wavelength as a predictor variable. 46 Table 7. Correlation and path coefficients for calculation of the contributions of predictor variables to predicted variables in Figures 5 and 6. Path Correlation Path Coefficient Territorial Status to Mating Success 0.92 0.92 Wavelength to Territorial Status 090 -0.90 %Fat to Wavelength -0.57 -0.46 %Water to Wavelength -0.48 -0.32 Age to Wavelength 0.36 0.36 %Fat to Territorial Status 0.51 0.37 %Water to Territorial Status 0.44 0.27 Age to Territorial Status -0.36 -0.23 47 DISCUSSION Territorial male Q. maculata were smaller in terms of both size and body mass, younger and more successful during mating attempts than nonterritorial males. Marden and Wage (1990) concluded that size and mass are not important in damselfly aggressive interactions. Path analysis of the data supported this conclusion. That territorial males were smaller than nonterritorial males is simply be an artifact of the negative relationship between day of emergence and hence, age, with size. That territorial males are younger than nonterritorial males, and that the 143 males exhibiting both territorial and nonterritorial behavior began as territorial and switched to nonterritorial behavior, is Similar to other examples of age related behavior patterns in odonates (T subaki and Ono, 1987; Campenella and Wolf, 1974). Forsyth and Montgomerie (1987) also demonstrate that in Q. maculata the probability of adopting an alternative reproductive behavior such as sneaking increases with male age. Successfully mated male Q. maculata are younger and smaller than unsuccessful males. This is most likely because females mate almost exclusively with territorial males, which tend to be younger and smaller than nonterritorial males. Territorial male Q. maculata are able to control female access to oviposition sites, exchange matings for guarding (Waage, 1979; 1982) and exclude nonterritorial males. This study demonstrates an age-related color change that is also related to territorial behavior in male Q. maculata. Territorial males exhibited significantly Shorter wavelength coloration than nonterritorial males. Wavelength was also shown to increase with both Size, body mass and age. Again, the relationship with size is most likely an artifact of the relationship 48 between size and day of marking resulting in older males being larger than younger males. Since size is a component of mass, body mass is also confounded by the Size/emergence clay relationship. Therefore, as Q. maculata males age, their color changes from short to long wavelength and many switch from territorial to nonterritorial behavior. The importance of fat or energy reserves in male Q. maculata aggressive interactions has been suggested in a number of studies (Forsyth and Montgomerie, 1987; Marden and Waage, 1990). Marden and Waage (1990) show that winners of escalated contests among male Q. maculata have significantly more fat or energy reserves than losers. They also show that young territorial males have significantly higher fat levels than old territorial males. The present study indicates that the wavelength of male color is also related to fat, as well as water content in this species. Short wavelength males had a significantly higher fat and water content per unit body mass than long wavelength males. Path analysis of the hypothesized causal relationships among the variables predicts that male Q. maculata mating success is affected most directly by male territorial status (Waage, 1973, 1979). Male territorial status is affected most directly by male wavelength. The variables age, fat and water, which have previously been shown to vary with male territorial and aggressive behavior (Forsyth and Montgomerie, 1987; Marden and Waage, 1990), make their greatest direct contribution to male wavelength variation rather than to male territorial behavior directly. The variables day of season, male size (forewing length), body mass, and year do not make a significant direct contribution to any other variables. These variables may affect the predicted variables but only through their relationships with the other more important predictor variables of age, fat and water. 49 Path analysis predicted that age, fat and water content are better indicators of color or wavelength than they are of territorial status directly. Color or wavelength is a better indicator of male territorial status than are age, fat and water content combined. Path analysis has organized the many pairwise comparisons of this and previous studies into a coherent picture of male Q maculata territorial and mating behavior. Path analysis has indicated the relative importance of variables having significant direct effects on territorial and mating behavior (age, fat and water content) while eliminating from the analysis the variables whose affects occur only through their interaction with other more important variables (wing length, body mass, day of season, year). Color is reliably related to the measures of body condition (energy reserves) which have been shown to affect the outcome of male Q maculata interactions. Age and energy reserves account for 54.5% of the variation in wavelength which in turn accounts for 81.0% of the variation in territorial status. Without wavelength, our accounting of the contributions to territorial status is only 39.1%. Thus, wavelength greatly increases the ability to predict male territorial status. The high predictability of territorial status from wavelength alone (81.0%) suggests that color is a reliable signal or index of fighting ability. Color may be used to assess energy reserves while the significant positive relationship between wavelength and fat content maintains a level of reliability or honesty in the signal. Future research must focus on testing whether or not Q maculata make use of the information provided by color. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The exchange of information via signals is often a means by which opponents in agonistic encounters can assess each other’s resource holding potential or fighting ability. I investigated the role of color as a signal of fighting ability in males of the territorial damselfly Qeloptem mgchata. l accomplished this by elucidating the mechanism of color production and color change to reveal the kinds of information provided by color. I also conducted field observations to establish the relationship of color with territorial and mating behavior. In my study, I was able to demonstrate that the bright blue and green colors of male Q maculata are caused by constructive interference by thin lamellae in the epicuticle. The lamellae consist of alternating layers of electron lucent chitin and electron dense melanin. Male Q maculata change from blue to green as they age. Fat levels; however, are more important than age for determining the wavelength of color reflected. This is the first study to demonstrate a multilayer interference reflector in an odonate cuticle. It is also the first study to address the role of color and color change in a natural population of Q maculata. Field data and path analysis indicate that territorial male Q maculata are younger, have higher percent body fat, and are more successful at mating than nonterritorial males. These results are similar to those reported in other studies of Q magglata. What is new and exciting about the present study is that color, 50 51 a variable previously unmeasured in Q maculata populations is revealed to be the best predictor of territorial behavior, with territorial males reflecting shorter wavelength color than nonterritorial males. Furthermore, color is shown to be reliably related to the measures of body condition (fat or energy reserves) known to affect the outcome of Q maculata aggressive interactions. These results indicate that color provides reliable information by which male Q maculata can assess each other’s fat levels or energy reserves as they indicate fighting ability. What remains to be shown is the extent to which males utilize the information provided by color. LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES Alcock, J. 1987. The effects of experimental manipulation of resources on the behavior of two Calopterygid damselflies that exhibit resource-defense polygyny. Can. J. Zool., 65:2474-2482. Anderson, T.F. and AG. Richards. 1942. An electron microscope study of some structural colors of insects. J. Appl. Phys, 13:748-758. Banks, M.J. and DJ Thompson. 1985. Lifetime mating success in the damselfly Coenagrign puella. Anim. Behav., 33:1175-1183. Campanella, P.J. and LL. Wolf. 1974. Temporal Ieks as a mating system in a temperate zone dragonfly (Odonata: Anisoptera). I. Plathemis lydia (Drury). Behaviour, 51 :49-87. Charles, MS. and J.V. Robinson. 1981. A scanning electron microscope study of the blue reflecting particles in Enallagma givile (Hagen) (Zygoptera: Coenagrionidae). Odonatologica, 10:219-222. Chromartie, R.I.T. 1959. Insect Pigments. Ann. Rev. Ent., 4:59-76. Conrad, K.F. and G. Pritchard. 1989. Female dimorphism and physiological color change in the damselfly Argia viviga Hagen (Odonata: Coenagrionidae). Can J. Zool., 67:298-304. Cordero, A. 1990. The inheritance of female polymorphism in the damselfly lschnura graellsii (Rambur) (Odonata: Coenagrionidae). Heredity, 64:341-346. Crespi, B.J. 1989. Variable selection and size-related mating patterns in natural populations of Elaphrgthrips tuberculatus: a path analysis. Am. Midl. Nat., 122:142-150. Dawkins, MS. and T. Guilford. 1991. The corruption of honest signaling. Anim. Behav., 41 :865-873. Enquist, M. 1985. Communication during aggressive interactions with particular reference to variation in choice of behaviour. Anim. Behav., 33:1152-1161. 52 53 Fincke, OM. 1987. Female monogamy in lschnura verticalis Say (Zygoptera, Coenagrionidae). Odonatologica, 1 6:129-1 43. Forbes, MR. 1991. Female morphs of the damselfly Enallagma boreale Selys (Odonata: Coenagrionidae): a benefit for androchromatypes. Can. J. Zool., 69:1969-1970. Forsyth, A and RD. Montgomerie. 1987. Alternative reproductive tactics in the territorial damselfly Qiloptemg magflata: sneaking by older males. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol, 21 :73-81. Fox, HM. and G. Ververs. 1960. The Nature of Animal colors. London: Sidgwick and Jackson. Gardner, R. and MR. Morris. 1989. The evolution of bluffing in animal contests: an ESS approach. J. Theor. Biol., 137:234-243. Grafen, A. 1990. Biological Signals as handicaps. J. Theor. Biol., 144:517-546. Hammerstein, P. and GA. Parker. 1982. The asymmetric war of attrition. J. Theor. Biol. 96:647-682. Harper, D.G.C. 1991. Communication. In: Behavioral ecology: an evolutionary approach. (Ed. by JR. Krebs and NB. Davies) pp 374-397, Blackwell Scientific Publications, Cambridge. Harvey, |.F. and PS. Corbet. 1985. Territorial behaviour of larvae enhances mating success of male dragonflies. Anim. Behav., 33:561-565. Harvey, IF and K.J. Walsh. 1993. Fluctuating asymmetry and lifetime mating success are correlated in males of the damselfly Coenagrion puella (Odonata: Coenagrionidae). Ecol. Entomol., 18:198-202. Hasson, O. 1994. Cheating signals. J. Theor. Biol., 167:223-238. Hinnekint, B.O.N. 1974. An individual marking technique for Odonata. Odonatologica, 3:261 -263. Hinnekint, B.O.N. 1987. Population dynamics of lsghnuraQ elegans (Vander Linden) (lnsecta: Odonata) with special reference to morphological color changes, female polymorphism. multiannual cycles and their influence on behaviour. Hydrobiologia, 146:3-31. Hinton, HE. 1976. Recent work on the physical colours of insect cuticle. In: The insect integument. (Ed. by HR. Hepburn). pp 475-496. Amsterdam, Elsevier Scientific Publishing Company. Hinton, HE. and BF. Gibbs. 1971. Diffraction gratings in Gyrinid beetles. J. Insect Physiol., 17:1023-1035. 54 Huntingford, FA. and AK. Turner. 1987. Animal Conflict. London, Chapman and Hall. Jarvi, T. and M. Bakken. 1984. The function of variation in the breast-stripe of the great tit (flaws major). Anim. Behav., 327:590-596. Johnstone, RA. and Grafen, A. 1993. Dishonesty and the handicap principle. Anim. Behav., 46:759-764. Kingsolver, J.G. and D.W. Schemske. 1991. Path analyses of selection. Trends Ecol. Evol., 6:276-280. Kodric-Brown, A. 1985. Female preference and sexual selection for male coloration in the guppy Ma LBJLQIAIBLQI- Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 17:199-206. Kodric-Brown, A. 1989. Dietary carotenoids and male mating success in the guppy: an environmental component to female choice. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 25:393-401. Leimar, O. and M. Enquist. 1984. Effects of asymmetries in owner-intruder conflicts. J. Theor. Biol., 111:475-491. Li, CC. 1981. Path analysis: a primer. Pacific Grove, Boxwood. Marden, J.H. 1989. Bodybuilding dragonflies: costs and benefits of maximizing flight muscle. Physiol. Zool., 62:505-521. Marden, J.H. and P. Chai. 1991. Aerial predation and butterfly design: how palatability, mimicry, and the need for evasive flight constrain mass allocation. Am. Nat., 138:5-36. Marden, J.H. and J.K. Waage. 1990. Escalated damselfly territorial contests are energetic wars of attrition. Anim. Behav., 39:954-959. Mason, CW. 1926. Structural colors in insects. l. J. Phys. Chem., 30:383-395. Mason, CW. 1927. Structural colors in insects. II. J. Phys. Chem., 31:321-354. May, ML. 1976. Physiological color change in new world damselflies (Zygoptera). Odonatologica, 5:165-171 . Maynard Smith, J. 1982. Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Maynard Smith, J. and D.G.C. Harper. 1988. The evolution of aggression-can selection generate variability? Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 319:557-570. 55 Maynard Smith, J. and GA. Parker. 1976. The logic of asymmetric contests. Anim. Behav., 24:159-175. McVey, ME. 1985. Rates of color maturation in relation to age, diet and temperature in male Erythemis simplicicollis (Say) (Anisoptera: Libellulidae). Odonatologica, 14: 101-114. Munsell Color Company 1976a. Munsell Book of Color. Baltimore, Munsell/Macbeth/Kollmorgen Corp. Munsell Color Company 1976b. Dominant wavelength and excitation purity data. Baltimore, MunselI/Macbeth/Kollmorgen Corp. Neville, AC. 1975. Physical properties. In: Biology of the arthropod cuticle. (Ed. by AC. Neville). pp 319-374. New York, Springer-Verlag. Onslow, H. 1921. On a periodic structure in many insect scales, and the cause of their iridescent colours. Phil. Trans, 211:1-74. Richards, AG. 1951. The integument of arthropods. Minneapolis, University of Minnesota Press. Rohwer, S. 1982. The evolution of reliable and unreliable badges of fighting ability. Amer. Zool., 22: 531-546. Schultz, TD. and MA. Rankin. 1985. The ultrastructure of the epicuticular interference reflectors of tiger beetles (Cicindela). J. Exp. Biol., 117: 87- 110. Simon, H. 1971. The splendor of iridescence: structural colors in the animal world. New York: Dodd, Mead. Tsubaki, Y. and T. Ono. 1987. Effects of age and body size on the male territorial system of the dragonfly, Nannophya pygmaea Rambur (Odonata: Libellulidae). Anim. Behav., 35:518-525. Ueda, T. 1989. Sexual maturation, body colour changes and increase of body weight in a summer diapause population of the damselfly Lestes sponsa (Hansemann) (Zygoptera: Lestidae). Odonatologica, 18:75-87. Waage, J.K. 1972. Longevity and mobility of adult Calopteryx maculata (Beauvois, 1805) (Zygoptera: Calopterygidae). Odonatologica, 1:155- 162. Waage, J.K. 1973. Reproductive behavior and its relation to territoriality in Calopteryx maculata (Beauvois) (Odonata: Calopterygidae). Behaviour, 487:240-256. 56 Waage, J.K. 1979. Adaptive significance of postcopulatory guarding of mates and nonmates by male Calopteryx maculata (Beauvois) (Odonata: Calopterygidae). Behav. Ecol Sociobiol., 6:147-154. Waage, J.K. 1982. Sexual selection, ESS theory and insect behavior: some examples from damselflies (Odonata). Fla. Entomol., 66:19-31. Waage, J.K. 1987. Choice and utilization of oviposition sites by female Calopteryx maculata (Odonata: Calopterygidae). l. Influence of site size and the presence of other females. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 20:439-446. Waage, J.K. 1988. Confusion over residency and the escalation of damselfly territorial disputes. Anim. Behav., 36:586-595. Wagner, W.E. 1992. Deceptive or honest Signaling of fighting ability? A test of alternative hypotheses for the function of changes in call dominant frequency by male cricket frogs. Anim. Behav., 44:449-462. Whitfield, DP. 1987. Plumage variability, status signaling and individual recognition in avian flocks. Trends Ecol. Evol., 2:13-18. Wootton, J.T. 1994. Predicting direct and indirect effects: an integrated approach using experiments and path analysis. Ecology. 75:151-165. Zahavi, A. 1975. Mate selection-a selection for a handicap. J. Theor. Biol., 53:205-214. Zahavi, A. 1977. The costs of honesty (further remarks on the handicap principle). J. Theor. Biol., 67:603-605.