THESE \ MTJBRARY 20061 Michigan State University This is to certify that the thesis entitled CONGRUITY THEORY: THE RELATION BETWEEN CONTEXT TYPE AND ADVERTISING APPEAL presented by Caitlin Michelle McLaughlin has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Master of degree in Advertising, Public Relations Arts and Retailing ’ Major Professor’s Signature gin/9912247 Date MSU is an affirmative-action, equal-opportunity employer PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5/08 K:IProj/Acc&Pres/ClRC/DateDuetindd CONGRUITY THEORY: THE RELATION BETWEEN CONTEXT TYPE AND ADVERTISING APPEAL By Caitlin Michelle McLaughlin A THESIS Submitted To Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Advertising, Public Relations and Retailing 2009 Abstract By Caitlin McLaughlin Congruity theory has been studied many times in the advertising literature, and overall can generally be defined as a theory related to how similarity between stimulus objects affects individuals’ processing of the stimuli. The condition of moderate congruity between stimulus objects has commonly been found to be significant in affecting consumers’ attitudes (Dahlen, 2005; Mandler, 1982; Meyers-Levy et al, 1994; Moore et al, 2005), a concept which this study attempted to explore further. The study utilized three levels of congruity (congruent, moderately incongruent, and incongruent) between the context type and the appeal of the commercial used in order to determine if a case of moderate congruity existed for this type of match-up, and if so, how did this perception of moderate congruity affect consumers’ attitudes. The results indicated that no condition for moderate congruity existed for the group studied, suggesting that the manipulation of moderate congruity in this form may be too subtle to affect consumers’ beliefs. Acknowledgements Thank you to my wonderful thesis advisor, Dr. Keith Adler, who got me through my first years at Michigan State University and supported me through it all. Thank you to Dr. Mira Lee, who provided me with many of my human subjects, and thank you to my committee members, Dr. Bruce Vanden Bergh and Dr. Charles Atkin, for their support and dedication in helping me complete this project. iii Table of Contents ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .................................................................... iii LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................... v CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION ............................................................. 1 CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW ..................................................... 3 Congruity Theory ....................................................................... 3 Context Congruity ...................................................................... 6 Product Congruity ...................................................................... 12 Advertiser Congruity ................................................................... 15 Commercial Component Congruity ................................................ 17 Conclusion ............................................................................... 19 CHAPTER 3: RESEARCH QUESTION ................................................... 21 CHAPTER 3: HYPOTHESES ................................................................ 21 CHAPTER 4: METHODOLOGY ............................................................. 22 Experimental Design .................................................................. 22 Variables ................................................................................. 23 Program Selection ..................................................................... 24 Experimental Procedures ............................................................ 25 Recruitment Technique ............................................................... 27 CHAPTER 5: RESULTS ....................................................................... 29 Description of the Sample ............................................................ 29 Manipulation Checks .................................................................. 30 Research Question Testing .......................................................... 33 Hypothesis Testing ................................ . .................................... 34 CHAPTER 6: DISCUSSION .................................................................. 39 APPENDICES .................................................................................... 43 Appendix A: Survey Instrument ..................................................... 43 Appendix B: Pretest Questionnaire ................................................ 53 Appendix C: Commercial Storyboards ............................................ 55 BIBLIOGRAPHY ................................................................................. 60 iv List of Tables TABLE 1: EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN ................................................... 22 TABLE 2: GENDER DISTRIBUTION OF SAMPLE .................................. 29 TABLE 3: AGE DISTRIBUTION OF SAMPLE ......................................... 30 TABLE 4: MEAN CONGRUITY PERCEPTION SCORES OF GROUPS ....... 33 TABLE 5: POST HOC ANALYSIS OF CONRUITY PERCEPTIONS ............ 33 TABLE 6: MEAN AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR ATTITUDE TOWARD THE BRAND ............................................................... 34 TABLE 7: MEAN AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR ATTITUDE TOWARD THE AD ...................................................................... 34 TABLE 8: MEAN AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR PURCHASE INTENTIONS .............................................................................. 34 TABLE 9: MEAN AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR BRAND RECALL 34 TABLE 10: MANOVA: CONGRUENT VS. INCONGRUENT MANIPULATION EFFECT ON DEPENDENT VARIABLES .......................................... 35 TABLE 11: CHI-SQUARE: CONGRUENT VS. INCONGRUENT MANIPULATION EFFECT ON BRAND RECALL ................................ 36 TABLE 12: MANCOVA: MODERATE INCONGRUENT MANIPULATION EFFECT ON AAD CONTROLLING FOR COMMERCCIAL TYPE ........... 36 TABLE 13: MANOVA: MODERATE INCONGRUENT MANIPULATION EFFECT ON DEPENDENT VARIABLES .......................................... 37 TABLE 14: CHI-SQUARE: MODERATE INCONGRUENT MANIPULATION EFFECT ON BRAND RECALL ........................................................ 38 Chapter 1: Introduction For years advertisers have been conducting marketing research in order to determine which combination of factors (eg. commercial theme, spokesperson, music) creates the most desirable response in consumers. Appeal type has been one of the most studied in academic advertising effectiveness research, with multiple studies conducted concerning the effect each type of appeal has on different brand images and product categories. Congruity has been a popular topic in communication research for many years now due to its connection to key measures of advertiser success, including purchase intentions, attitude toward the ad, attitude toward the product / brand, and brand recall (Aylesworth & MacKenzie, 1998; Chandon et al, 2000; Dahlen, 2005; Heckler & Childers, 1992; Horn & McEwen, 1977; Kamins, 1990; Kamins & Gupta, 1994; Kamins et al, 1991; Lee & Mason, 1999; Meyers-Levy & Stemthal, 1993; Meyers-Levy & Tybout, 1989; 1997; Meyers-Levy et al, 1994; Osgood & Tannenbaum, 1955; Rifon et al, 2004; Russell, 2002; Sharma, 2000). Based on analysis of the above-cited articles, congruity can be defined as “a situation in which there is a match between two factors within an experience, reducing cognitive dissonance when processing the experience or advertisement.” Many different types of congruity have been explored over the years (self congruity, product congruity, advertiser congruity) and each of these has yielded useful and interesting results to guide advertisers when creating advertising campaigns. One example of such useful results relates to the attractiveness of models in advertisements: a congruity study found that spokesperson attractiveness was only important to consumers when the product being sold was related to physical beauty (Kamins, 1990). Congruity is often first conceptualized as a dichotomous variable (congruent vs. incongruent situations) to determine if consumer perceptions were affected by a particular ‘type’ of congruity. Congruent situations have been defined as those in which the content of the ad falls within the normative scope of the consumer’s previous understanding and does not violate previous perceptions (Mandler, 1982). Incongruent situations were defined as those in which an incongruous piece of material within the ad or situation did not fit with the consumer’s preconceptions and understanding, and therefore the consumer was required to expend effort to adjust his or her perceptions to make sense of the incongruous occurrence. Once this relationship has been determined, oftentimes the concept of ‘moderate congruity’ has been introduced to determine if this can maximize the positive effects of both congruity and incongruity (Dahlen, 2005; Mandler, 1982; Meyers-Levy et al, 1994; Moore et al, 2005). Based on analysis of multiple articles on congruity theory (Dahlen, 2005; Heckler & Childers, 1992; Meyers-Levy et al, 1994; Yih & Mason, 1999), moderate incongruity in advertising can be defined as a situation in which cognitive elaboration is required to make sense of the meaning of the communication. The study described in this paper introduced the concept of moderate incongruity into the domain of matches between commercial appeal type and the theme of the context in which it was placed (e.g. humorous context) This study attempted to re-examine some of the effects of moderate congruity on advertising effectiveness. Chapter 2: Literature Review Congruitv Theorv Congruity was first formally articulated with the statement “changes in evaluation are always in the direction of increased congruity with the existing frame of reference” (Osgood & Tannenbaum, 1955). The theory stated that congruity became relevant whenever a message related more than one object of judgment. When both objects of judgment were neutral, the movement in attitude was determined solely by the nature of the message / assertion. The total amount of pressure to change an attitude was determined to be equal to the difference between the attitude toward the objects of judgment and the attitude with the maximum congruity between the assertion and the objects of judgment. This pressure was distributed between the objects of judgment, with the least amount of pressure on the most firmly held beliefs, and vice versa. If incredulity was activated by a particular assertion, however (the assertion was unbelievable because it was so incongruent with current beliefs) this would dampen the effects of pressure to solve the incongruity. Following this theorization, an experiment was conducted to determine the actual effects. The experiments found that in all cases, the predicted direction of change in attitudes (positive, negative or no change) was observed, providing support for this congruity theory. One criticism of Osgood’s theory related to its theoretical base, and talked of the assumption inherent in the schema theory that every linguistic unit was a ‘sign’ for some other implicit meaningful thing or state of affairs for the listener (Edwards, 1972). This assumption has not been empirically tested, however. Another major problem with Osgood’s theory was the assumption that each linguistic unit had only one meaning when utilized in a certain way, which was almost certainly not the case. Another criticism was that it was unclear how scientists should be able to measure the implicit responses, which introduced the problem of measurement in this theory. In a later work, Osgood et al (1957) identified some of the limitations in measuring congruity. One of these limitations had to do with the use of contradictory language within an assertion (eg. the lazy athlete) and unknown effects this would have on the valence of thoughts toward the object. Some other limitations included the unknown qualities related to the intensity of assertions, the believability of assertions, and the relevance an assertion had to an individual’s own life experiences. In order to explain the relationship between congruity and the change in consumers’ perceptions of the ad / brand, the psychological construct of a schema was extended to become a part of the congruity literature. Schemas were defined as previously held representations that aid in determining perceptions, actions, and thought (Mandler, 1982; 1993). A precursor to schema theory, however, should be acknowledged when dealing with schemas. Piaget’s work on perception stipulated that learning was an essential part of perception, and hence the acknowledgement that ‘the grass is green’ was a result of learning to perceive both the color green and the object grass (Edwards, 1972). By acknowledging the importance of learning in perceptual processes, Piaget created the foundation for schema theory. Mandler asserted that individuals in our culture sought to understand their environment, and as such if two objects belonged to different, unlinked schemas, individuals would always seek to find a connection between the two objects (1982). Mandler argued that congruent ads were more easily comprehensible by the consumer because they required less elaboration to relate to the existing schema; incongruent ads required more elaboration in order to reconcile the ad message with the pre-existing attitudes about the product/brand (Mandler, 1982). Therefore it has been argued that congruent information would result in moderately positive attitudes about the ad because it would require very little elaboration and would do very little to change the current schema; incongruent information would lead to stronger evaluations (negative or positive) about the object of consideration because the object required more cognitive elaboration. The direction of the attitude change with incongruent information was likely to depend upon the amount of cognitive resources required to process the new information, as compared to the cognitive resources available. If there was a moderate level of incongruity, then the individual evaluating the object would be able to determine a solution which connected the two concepts, and in the process of solving this incongruity would undertake an extensive elaboration process concerning the concepts in the ad, thereby improving the memory of the ad. This would also lead to improved attitudes toward the ad, product and advertiser because the respondent would have a positive reaction to being able to solve the discrepancy. Mandler hypothesized that in the case of high incongruity individuals became frustrated because of their inability to solve the mystery of how to connect the two concepts, and would have a general feeling of frustration toward the ad which could transfer to evaluations of the product and advertiser. lncongruity was more appreciated in some instances than other instances: with high-risk situations, congruent information was likely to produce more positive results, whereas lower-risk situations were more acceptable for incongruent information (Campbell & Goodstein, 2001). Authors argued that this trend could be due to individuals’ needs to feel comforted and able to process all information for situations in which there is a higher personal or financial risk involved, whereas consumers could be entertained and intrigued by incongruity in a situation in which the risk was minimal. Context CW The effectiveness of advertising has been demonstrated to be affected by several factors, but three main categories (Pelsmacker et al, 2002) of factors are: characteristics of the stimulus itself (Cho, 1995; De Pelsmacker & Geuens, 1996; De Pelsmacker & Van den Bergh, 1998; LaTour & Henthome, 1994; Leather et al, 1994; Stewart & Koslow, 1989; Weinberger & Gulas, 1992), characteristics of the individual (Chaudhuri & Buck, 1995; & Buck, 1995; De Pelsmacker & Van den Bergh, 1996; Geuens & De Pelsmacker, 1997, 1999; Moore & Harris, 1996; Moore et al, 1994; Smith et al, 1995; Vakratsas & Ambler, 1996) and situational factors. One of these situational factors that has remained a mystery for researchers is the media context and its congruity or contrast with the style and content of the ad (Pelsmacker et al, 2002). One study attempted to determine the interaction of context-theme congruence and context—involvement in order to determine how these factors combined to affect audience members’ attitude toward the ad (Pelsmacker et al, 2002). This study determined that the interaction of the independent variables did have a significant impact on evaluations of the ad. Low-involvement individuals (those who were primed to have a low level of involvement in the program) found congruent commercials to be significantly more Iikeable than incongruent commercials, and high-involvement individuals (those who were primed to have a high level of involvement in the program) found incongruent commercials to be significantly more Iikeable than congruent commercials. Similarly, low- involvement individuals perceived congruent messages to be clearer than incongruent commercials, and high-involvement individuals perceived incongruent commercials to be clearer than congruent commercials. The authors argued that the high involvement individuals were stimulated by the incongruity of the message, whereas the low involvement individuals found the congruent advertisement easier to process. Another interesting finding within this study was that positively appreciated contexts (articles or television shows rated positively on a three-item scale including: interesting, boring, would see it again) resulted in the commercials being rated as more Iikeable, more informative, and clearer than commercials placed within a more negatively rated context. Positively appreciated contexts also led to higher recall with age as a moderator: people in older age groups demonstrated this effect, whereas people in younger age groups did not show a context appreciation effect when recalling the ad. This study was impressive in the variety of factors considered, but due to the huge scope of the project it was difficult for the researchers to focus on any one aspect enough to deeply understand it. As such, this study does not include a discussion of the moderate congruity condition on any of these dependent variables. Another approach to congruity theory deals with the mood induced by the context and the mood of an advertisement placed within that context (Kamins et al, 1991). This study found that individuals exposed to a happy ad (an ad in which all actors were smiling and talking about positive changes in the healthcare system) within a happy context had higher commercial effectiveness ratings, more positive attitudes toward the ads, and higher likeability ratings for the ad than individuals exposed to the happy ad in the sad context. Conversely, individuals who viewed a sad ad (an ad in which actors are crying and discussing issues of despair) within a sad context had higher commercial effectiveness ratings, more positive attitudes toward the ads, higher likeability ratings for the ad, and a significantly higher intention to use the service than individuals who viewed the sad ad within the happy context. In another study of commercial-context congruity, involvement type was studied to determine if affective and cognitive involvement had different effects on recall of the advertisement (Sharma, 2000). This study utilized moderate levels of involvement for all of its experimental evaluations, manipulated by instructing participants to attempt to come to a ‘basic' understanding of the program. Individuals that were manipulated to be cognitively involved in the program (these participants were asked to focus on the issue inherent in the program) recalled commercials with a cognitive appeal better than commercials with an affective appeal. Similarly, individuals who were in the affective involvement manipulation (these participants were asked to role play and identify with the characters) recalled commercials with an affective appeal better than commercials with a cognitive appeal. This study utilized only a moderate level of involvement when discussing the effects of involvement type I appeal type on recall. It also ignored several other key advertising effectiveness dependent variables, including purchase intentions, attitude toward the ad and attitude toward the brand. Further research has studied the effects of congruity on creative media choices, such as advertising on eggs or using stickers on elevators (Dahlen, 2005). Creative media choice, when congruent, implicitly communicated the message being portrayed about the product, reducing the need to explicitly advertise the product. An example given of a creative media choice was advertising a Curious George movie on a banana: this, upon further elaboration, would make sense to the consumer, but it is a medium in which individuals would not expect to see an advertisement. This study compared traditional media advertising (ads within a newspaper) with congruent vs. incongruent creative media choices (as determined by a pretest in which individuals rated the ‘fit’ of objects to be used as possible creative media choices with product categories). This study found that brand associations, ad credibility, and brand attitude were higher within the congruent creative media choice than in an incongruent creative media choice or within a traditional media choice. There was no difference in effectiveness between an incongruent creative media choice and a traditional media choice. Although this study was extremely interesting in its implications, it utilized photographs of the creative media choice rather than actual examples of the creative media choice for consumers to see firsthand. Also, the creative media choice for one of the product categories was a sticker placed on an elevator, which may have reduced the comparability between the two situations. An experiment that was conducted utilizing product placement discovered that congruence required a trade-off between recall and attitude toward the ad / placement (Russell, 2002). This study defined congruent product placements as those that matched modality (visual vs. auditory product placement) and the degree of plot connection. For a product to justify an auditory product placement, researchers determined that it needed to have a higher plot connection to reduce suspicion and increase persuasion. Incongruent product placements had higher recall scores, but persuasiveness was lower. Specifically, it was determined that products whose names were actually heard by consumers had high recall scores, no matter how integrated the product was in the plot. For products that were merely seen by consumers, recall was higher for products that were better integrated into the plot than for products that had no real connection to the plot development. The trade-off discussed in this paper was found in the persuasiveness of the different product placement types: for auditory product placements a higher plot connection was more persuasive for consumers, and for visual product placements a lower plot connection was more persuasive for consumers. As such, the more ‘congruent’ product placements (those product placements in which the level of plot connection ‘fit’ with the type of placement it was given) performed better and were more persuasive for consumers. Moderate incongruity was not assessed in this experiment. Another product placement study was conducted utilizing videogames as the context (Lee & Faber, 2007). This study measured many other factors, but 10 determined that brands that were incongruent with the theme of the game were recalled better than congruent brands. The qualification that inexperienced players experienced this same effect but to a lesser extent indicated that individuals utilized much of their cognitive resources while playing the videogame, and thus only surprising contextual factors were likely to be noticed and l or remembered by players. A novel context congruity study was done in which respondents were given information concerning the type of business that previously occupied the building in which a new company was supposed to be opening (Meyers-Levy & Stemthal, 1993). The information given to respondents on the former tenant concerned how elegant / casual the company had been, as well as product category (restaurant vs. clothing store). The experiment studied the effect this information had on evaluations of the new tenant for individuals with high and low need for cognition. Individuals with a high need for cognition demonstrated an assimilation effect for situations with a product category overlap (if the previous tenant had been elegant, the new tenant was perceived to be more elegant than if the previous tenant had been a casual establishment) and a contrast effect for situations with low product category overlap (if the previous tenant had been perceived to be elegant, the new tenant was perceived to be more casual than if the previous tenant had been perceived to be casual) as these individuals attempted to compensate for the bias introduced by the previous tenant’s image. For individuals with a low need for cognition, the assimilation effect was observed with or without product category overlap. 11 Prodgct Conmlitv Product congruity has been related to another area of congruity research in which the product is evaluated in terms of the appropriateness for certain product category claims, appeal types, etc. This further has provided evidence for the effects of congruity on the key indicators of advertising success. Another research project dealt with congruity as it related to sales promotion type and type of brand equity (Chandon et al, 2000). This research project determined that nonmonetary sales promotions were more effective than monetary sales promotions for hedonic products while monetary sales promotions were more effective than nonmonetary sales promotions for utilitarian products. This indicated that there was a ‘benefit congruency’ effect in which individuals responded better to sales promotions that reflected the benefits derived from using the product. This study indicated that, for products with a hedonic value, consumers thought of it as ‘cheap’ to use only monetary incentives, thus reducing the hedonic value that consumers achieved when purchasing the product. As such, the incentives given for these products should have been aimed at increasing hedonic value. For utilitarian products, since money was an extremely utilitarian possession, individuals saw a monetary incentive as increasing the utilitarian value of any product being purchased. In a famous congruity study, expectancy and relevancy was studied to determine how these factors affected memory for verbal and visual information (Heckler & Childers, 1992). These authors argued that incongruent information would result in greater elaboration and thus would be better recalled. This experiment manipulated the content of advertisements to have expected and 12 unexpected visual or auditory content as well as relevant versus irrelevant visual or auditory content (auditory content being ad copy rather than voice). This experiment determined that recall of visual content was greater for unexpected (incongruent) content than for expected (congruent) content but ad recognition was unaffected. Relevant information was better recalled than irrelevant information. An extension of the expectancy and relevancy literature integrated humor to determine if humor had a significant impact on the relationship between expectancy, relevancy and attitude toward the ad / brand (Yih & Mason, 1999). This study found that unexpected (incongruent) information that was relevant resulted in more positive attitude toward the ad than did expected (congruent) relevant information but no difference was present for attitude toward the brand. Unexpected-irrelevant advertisements resulted in less favorable attitudes toward the ad and attitudes toward the brand than did ads with expected irrelevant information, but attitudes were improved if humor was used in the ads. Although attitudes were different between conditions, ads with unexpected relevant information had the same number of favorable ad and brand cognitions as the expected relevant condition. In general, it was determined that incongruent (unexpected) components resulted in more positive ad attitudes, but only when the incongruent components were also relevant. This seemed to fit with the moderate congruity literature which indicated that moderate congruity was the most effective at improving consumer perceptions. Irrelevant and unexpected information was extremely incongruent and had negative responses, while relevant and unexpected information was the most positively evaluated and could 13 be operationalized as a moderately congruent manipulation. Furthermore, the expected and relevant information was positively viewed, but still did not perform as well as the ‘moderate’ condition of relevant-unexpected information. In a similar vein of congruity research, an experiment was done to determine the effects when respondents were given a description of a product which identified it as being a member of a certain product category, and then exposed the individual to an incongruent or congruent product description of a product meant to be within that product category (Meyers-Levy & Tybout, 1989). This was operationalized by utilizing the ‘beverage’ (superordinate product category) and the ‘soft drink’ (basic product category) and giving respondents a list of product attributes that created a congruent or incongruent fit with the soft drink product category. Individuals who were told that the product was a soft drink were likely to find the situation more or less congruent depending upon the list of product attributes given. This experiment’s results indicated a higher level of recall for incongruent product categories, but did not result in a significant change in attitudes. A similar study was conducted later in which need for cognition was introduced as an additional variable (Meyers-Levy 8. Tybout, 1997). Before respondents were exposed to a product description and taste test, all subjects were asked to read a set of thoughts (either negative or positive) and respond to them as a primer. This was intended to induce a mood prior to the individuals responding to the actual stimulus. Following this primer individuals were exposed to a manipulation similar to that identified in the previous study. It was found that respondents that were low in need for cognition (as measured by a need-for- 14 cognition scale) exhibited moderately positive responses regardless of the experimental treatment (information congruent or incongruent with schema activated). For respondents with a high need for cognition, assimilation and contrast effects were observed. For these high cognition individuals, situations in which the product description was congruent with the product category identified exhibited a contrast effect (they attempted to negate the impact of the mood induced by the primer and thus overcompensated in the other direction) due to their ability and need to cognitively process further their motivations for responding to stimuli in a particular way and the excess of cognitive resources available for the task. For situations in which the product description was incongruent with the category, an assimilation effect was observed for individuals with a high need for cognition (cognitive resources were fully utilized determining why the description was incongruent with the expected product description and thus no resources were available to compensate for the mood induced by the primer). Advertiser Congruitv Companies have often been concerned with their brand image and how consumers perceived their company. As such, companies have often attempted to use imagery and messages that fit with the image that the company was attempting to project, in order to strengthen the associations between the brand and certain other nodes in consumers’ memory. This is how advertiser congruity has often been described. Congruity has also been found to be important when a company was considering sponsoring a specific event (Rifon et al, 2004). In a study of 15 sponsorships, congruity was defined as a situation in which a sponsor that was related somehow to the medium (e.g. health-related sponsors for breast cancer research) sponsored a website. Attribution theory was utilized in this study to determine the effects of different sponsorships by determining why respondents believed the sponsor had placed an advertisement on the site. In the congruent condition, significantly more people inferred an altruistic motive than in the incongruent situation. The congruent situation also resulted in more positive sponsor attitudes and stronger credibility perceptions. Further analysis (using a step-down of F-values, an ANCOVA of the effects of congruence on credibility perceptions with altruism motives as a covariate, and finally another ANCOVA of congruence effects on sponsor attitudes with altruism attributions and sponsor credibility as covariates) revealed that altruistic attributions mediated the relationship between congruency and the change in attitudes and credibility perceptions. Another study of advertiser congruity studied the congruity between a brand extension and the parent brand product category (Meyers-Levy et al, 1994). This was manipulated by pairing a book topic with different well-known brands in order to create situations with high congruity, moderate congruity, and high incongruity. These levels of congruity were established in a pretest in which respondents were asked how likely each company was to create a brand in a particular product category. The moderate congruity condition was found to be preferred over both the congruent and highly incongruent conditions. Another analysis of brand name congruity was conducted utilizing websites as the context (Moore et al, 2005). This study utilized a pretest to 16 determine names that were either congruent, moderately congruent, or incongruent with the product type (eg. Upper Newberry Pet Center was incongruent with the apartment product category). Participants had higher recall and recognition scores for incongruent brand names than for congruent brand names. Conversely, attitude toward the ad was more favorable in the congruent brand name condition than the incongruent brand name condition. In a further investigation, the moderately congruent and the congruent condition were both found to have more positive attitudes toward the ad than the incongruent condition. Commercial Commnent Congm Congruity’s importance has been further demonstrated by within- commercial factors. One study of model-spokesperson congruity determined the relationship between types of product (attractiveness-related vs. non- attractiveness related) and model attractiveness (Kamins, 1990). This study found that attractiveness did not have any effect on brand attitudes, ad attitudes, credibility or purchase intention when the product was unrelated to attractiveness (eg. a personal computer). For products that were related to attractiveness, however (eg. a sports car or beauty product), an attractive model was significantly more effective than an unattractive model. Although attractive models have generally been utilized in advertising and it has been said that these models were always more effective, this research suggested that model attractiveness was only important when consumers related it somehow to enhancing their own physical beauty. 17 In another study of model congruity, celebrity spokespersons were compared to expert spokespersons for different types of products (Kamins & Gupta, 1994). This study compared advertisements in which the spokesperson was deemed to be congruent (celebrity endorsing a product in which they were viewed to have a certain amount of expertise) vs. incongruent (endorsing a product unrelated to their area of expertise). Results showed that the higher the degree of expertise (congruity), the greater the believability of the spokesperson whether or not the spokesperson was a celebrity. Although this study showed interesting results related to celebrity appropriateness, it could also be argued that this was an issue of source credibility, and thus this study should be utilized with caution in congruity research. Further research discovered that models could also be congruent with the cognitive age of the individuals viewing an ad (a version of self-congruity), which can affect evaluations of the product and brand (Chang, 2008). Congruity was defined as a similar ‘cognitive age’ as the perceived age of the model in the advertisement. This type of congruity led to higher levels of ‘for me’ identification, greater self-brand affinity, higher self-referencing, and more favorable brand attitudes. Another within-commercial component that has been evaluated for congruity dealt with music-brand congruency (Lavack et al, 2001). Congruity was determined by pretests in which respondents were asked to determine how appropriate the brand was for different types of music (e.g. classic rock, blues). Congruity was found to significantly improve attitude toward the ad and attitude toward the brand when high-cognition ad copy was utilized, while congruent 18 music did not have any effect on these attitudes when low-cognition ad copy was utilized. Conclusion Context-commercial congruity has been explored in many different ways: context theme congruence and context involvement (Pelsmacker et al, 2002); congruity between the mood of the context and the mood of the advertisement (Kamins et al, 1991); affective vs. cognitive involvement congruity between involvement in the context and involvement in the commercial (Sharma, 2000); creative media medium choice and product category congruity (Dahlen, 2005); product placement type (auditory vs. visual) and level of plot connection (Russell, 2002); congruent vs. incongruent product types within videogames (Lee & Faber, 2007); and the congruity between the images of two businesses that resided in the same space at different time periods (Meyers-Levy & Stemthal, 1993). All of these studies indicated that congruity between the context and the advertisement had some impact on the effectiveness of the advertisements. These studies also indicated that consumers prefer to understand why certain choices were made, but do enjoy the process of determining what the relationship between the two objects are, but only if the process ends with positive results (with an understanding of why the choice was made). These studies indicated that individuals did undertake this elaboration process, indicated by the increased recall for incongruent or moderately congruent advertisements. However, the attitude was consistently negative for incongruent advertisements because the individuals did not like discrepancies between what was expected and what occurred that were unsolvable. As such, uncertainties about the moderate 19 congruity operationalization have allowed study for all types of congruity, to determine if moderate congruity is possible and, if so, what effect it has on attitude toward the ad, attitude toward the brand, product recall, and purchase intentions within this particular domain of congruity research. 20 Chapter 3: Research Question RQ1: Are there discemible differences in effort / perceived fit for the consumer between high, moderate, and low congruity in advertising theme and context theme congmity situations? Chapter 3: Hypotheses H 1 a: A congruent ad (appeal matches context) will be more effective at inducing positive brand attitudes than an incongruent ad. H 1 b: A congruent ad (appeal matches context) will be more effective at inducing positive purchase intentions than an incongruent ad. H10: A congruent ad (appeal matches context) will be more effective at inducing positive attitude toward the ad than an incongruent ad. H2: An incongruent ad (appeal does not match context) will be more efi'ective at inducing brand recall than a congruent ad. H33: A moderately incongruent ad will be more effective at inducing positive attitude toward the ad than an incongruent or congruent ad. H3b: A moderately incongruent ad will be more effective at inducing positive brand attitudes than an incongruent or congruent ad. H3c: A moderately incongruent ad will be more effective at inducing positive purchase intentions than an incongruent or congruent ad. H3d: A moderately incongruent ad will be more effective at inducing brand recall than an incongruent or congruent ad 21 Chapter 4: Methodology aperimentilDesigg Table 1: Experimental Design Drama I Emotional Comedy Context Context Emotional Appeal Group 1 (Congruent) Group 2 (Incongruent) Comedy Appeal Group 3 (Incongruent) Group 4 (Congruent) Informative Appeal Group 5 (Moderate) Group 6 (Moderate) This study was created to determine the effect of a match-up between the theme of a commercial and the theme of the context within which it was placed. This experimental design introduced the concept of moderate congruity, which was new to commercial appeal-context theme congruity research. A drama and a comedy context were chosen because following an analysis of the Simmons 2004 data, these two types of programs were both popular amongst the college-age demographic group. Following this decision, commercials were chosen to fit with these two types of appeals. An informative appeal was chosen to describe the commercial in the moderate congruity condition, although the commercial was chosen based on neutral ratings for both emotional warmth and humor. lnforrnative appeals were also deemed to be generally appropriate within any context, but did not seem to be particularly appropriate for either of these types of programs. 22 This experiment measured the effect of appeals placed in different types of programming on college students and how congruence or ‘advertising fit’ affected the audience’s reaction to the commercials. The same set of two ‘dummy’ commercials (one advertisement for a computer and one advertisement for pizza) was placed within two different programs (That 70’s show and Grey’s Anatomy), with one test commercial for a little-known but available coffee brand (Tim Horton’s) with high, low, or moderate theme congruity with the context programming placed within the commercial block. In order to control for the order effect, the test commercials were all placed at the beginning of the three- commercial block. Six different experimental conditions were used in this experiment, with three different levels of congruity (three commercials) and two different types of programming (humor and emotional / drama). Online data collection was utilized in order to increase response rates. A central website was used to redirect respondents to one of six online questionnaires. Before the actual questionnaire could be accessed, respondents watched a 13-minute segment of a television program with a commercial block near the middle of the programming. The commercial block was placed near the middle of the program to increase realism and to ensure that individuals were fully engrossed in the program before and after viewing the commercial. Variables This experiment utilized the manipulation of the context-commercial theme congruity as an independent variable, with six different manipulations which were condensed into three different conditions (congruent, moderately congruent, incongruent). The dependent variables utilized in this experiment included 23 purchase intentions, attitude toward the ad, attitude toward the brand, and brand recall. All scale items have been included in the questionnaire, located in Appendix A. Recall was measured by giving the respondents the product category (coffee) and asking them to name the coffee brand. Coders then read each individual response and determined whether or not the individuals had correctly identified the brand. Attitude toward the ad utilized a previously used scale created by Susan Tai (1999) which was a 7—point semantic differential scale, and included items such as clearI unclear, interesting I uninteresting, and appealing I unappealing. Attitude toward the brand also used a scale created by previous researchers (Spears & Singh, 2004). This was also a 7-point semantic differential scale, which included items such as bad I good, favorable I unfavorable, and appealing I unappealing. Purchase intentions were measured using one 7-point semantic differential scale item, with anchors of definitely won’t purchase I definitely will purchase from Tim Horton’s. EggLaJn Selection Through an analysis of the Simmons 2003 television ratings as they related to demographics, a humorous program was selected from the humor context. The television show was selected because of high ratings within the 18- 24 demographic and a fairly even gender preference distribution. The drama was selected based on analysis of the fan bases on applications within the social networking site Facebook (Addicted To application and Become a Fan). Although 24 the gender distribution was biased toward the female demographic for the drama program, the chosen episode appealed to both genders. For the dramatic program, Grey’s Anatomy was chosen due to its popularity on the Addicted To feature. The Addicted To Grey’s Anatomy feature had 114,901 fans as of November 14, 2008. Because Facebook was most avidly used by the chosen sample frame of undergraduate students at universities, this was a positive indicator that individuals within this demographic enjoyed the Grey’s Anatomy program. Although other demographic groups had begun utilizing the social networking site Facebook, the individuals most likely to explore its features were still within the target market of undergraduate students. For the humorous program, That 70’s Show was chosen due to high ratings among the market and a small gender skew (based on the Simmons 2003 ratings). In the sample males were 9% more likely to watch the show than females. Young adults were more likely than any other demographic to watch the show, with 6.89% of them watching the television show regularly. Mmental Procedures Since this experiment was testing for a context effect on commercial effectiveness, a pretest was required. This pre-test asked students to evaluate the commercials on the two different types of appeal used (please see Appendix B for further information) in order to ensure that commercial appeal type was agreed upon by the audience, and to ensure comparability for each commercial that was actually tested (commercials with similar scores on the appeal type that they represent were selected to be the test commercials utilized). Based on these pre-tests, three commercials for the same brand were selected in order to 25 represent a humorous appeal, an emotional appeal, and an informative appeal (used to represent the moderate congruity condition). The experiment took place online with a random assignment of six different online questionnaires (please see Appendix A for further information). This experimental design tested the effectiveness of congruity manipulations by measuring consumers’ brand recall, attitude toward the brand, attitude toward the ad, and purchase intentions for the three different conditions. More information was collected in order to ensure that any other variables that could have possibly affected these key variables were available for use as covariates. Respondents were also asked about the perceived ‘fit’ between the commercial and the program in order to ensure that the congruity manipulation was successful. Individuals evaluated each commercial on humor, emotional warmth, and inforrnativeness in order to ensure that this manipulation was also successful. The commercials were all for the same brand (Tim Horton’s) which was available in the geographic area of study but for which brand attitudes and purchase intentions were still largely undefined. The brand was selected based on availability of appropriate commercials as well as the relatively unknown quality of the brand (individuals had very little experience with the brand prior to participating in the experiment). Dummy commercials were utilized but were for products that were typically appealing to members of the youth demographic and for fairly familiar brands. See Appendix C for storyboards of the three experimental commercials used in the study. Two different types of programming were used to test the context effect on appeal effectiveness: a comedy (That 70’s Show) and a drama (Grey’s Anatomy). 26 The programs that were used had to appeal to both male and female young adults to eliminate gender bias when respondents reacted to the commercials. Although these programs were of different lengths (one was a half hour show, another was an hour-long show), the programs were edited to ensure that that the manipulations were approximately the same length. Although in each group some of the respondents were likely to have seen the program before, if they were true fans of the program they would still have been engaged in the program and if they were not fans they would have been unlikely to remember the program even if they had seen it before. Recruitment Technique Subjects were solicited from large undergraduate advertising classes. Some students received extra credit points for participation in the research. Because the experiment needed to be representative of the general student population, this experiment included only undergraduate students (the most representative sample of the general public within a university environment). Individuals participating in the experiment were told that the purpose of the experiment was to test individuals” response to the program as well as the advertising within the programming. This ensured that they did not skip the commercials but did not focus solely on the commercials either. Professors from the advertising department were asked to agree to award a bonus point in return for a student taking part in the study. Professors that agreed to this sent out an email invitation to participate in the study to their students with a link to the informed consent page of the experiment. From that link, respondents were randomly redirected to one of six experimental conditions. 27 Following completion of the survey students were redirected to a central webpage not associated with the research data collection in which they were asked to fill in their name, student number and class, in order to record extra credit points. Respondent confidentiality was maintained by disassociating the subjects’ names from the data collection questionnaires. A copy of the data collection questionnaire can be found in Appendix A. 28 Chapter 5: Results QescLiption of the Sampl_e The sample of 202 students was comprised entirely of individuals between the ages of 19 and 24. Only 1.5% of the sample was under the age of 20, and 93.5% of the sample was between the ages of 20 and 22. Male respondents comprised 34.2% of the sample, which demonstrated a slight skew towards the female gender in the sample. All respondents were members of at least one advertising concentration course in a large Midwestern university of the United States. Gender distribution was not significantly different for the three different congruity conditions tested (X2 (2) = 1.794, p > 0.05). This indicated that the sample compositions for gender were the same. Table 2: Gender Distribution of Samfi Male Female Congruent 38.10% 61.90% Moderate 36.60% 63.40% Incongruent 27.90% 72.10% Age distribution was not significantly different for the three different congruity conditions tested (X2 (10) = 7.524, p > 0.05). This indicated that the sample compositions for age were the same. 29 _'l'_a_ble 3: Age Distribution of Sample 19 20 21 22 23 24 Comment 1.60% 46.00% 31 .70% 14.30% 4.80% 1.60% Moderate 0.00% 33.80% 43.70% 18.30% 4.20% 0% Incongruent 2.90% 42.60% 36.80% 13.20% 4.40% 0% Manipulation Checks This section of the results is concerned with determining which other factors measured had an effect on the dependent variables of brand recall, attitude toward the ad, attitude toward the brand, and purchase intentions. Those findings that were significant were used as covariates when the hypotheses were tested in order to control for any effects that these variables might have had on the dependent variables. A series of one-way ANOVAs and chi-square tests were conducted in order to determine if the show type, commercial type or television show involvement significantly affected the dependent variables of brand recall, attitude toward the brand, attitude toward the ad, or purchase intention. Show type, commercial type and TV involvement were analyzed in order to ensure that these manipulations did not significantly affect the dependent variables, and that any difference in perception was due to the experimental manipulation rather than these differences. The series of one-way ANOVAs and chi-square tests based on show type indicated that show type did not have a significant effect on congruity perceptions (F(1, 200) = 1.289, p > 0.05), did not have a significant impact on attitude toward the brand (F(1, 193) = 1.359, p > 0.05), did not have a significant impact on 30 attitude toward the ad (F(1, 193) = 3.804, p > 0.05), and did not have a significant impact on brand recall (x2 (1) = 2.305, p > 0.05). This series of tests indicated that show type did not have an effect on our dependent variables, and thus did not interfere with the congruity effect discussed in the hypotheses. Show type did have a significant impact on purchase intention, however (F (1, 199) = 4.682, p < 0.05). The data indicated that individuals who viewed the drama (Grey’s Anatomy) were slightly more likely than individuals who viewed the comedy (That 70’s Show) to recall the brand of interest. This finding indicated that should significant results be found for attitude toward the ad, show type would need to be controlled for to ensure that this was not the reason for the significant effects. The series of one-way ANOVAs and chi-squares based on commercial type indicated that commercial appeal did not have a significant impact on congruity perceptions (F(2, 199) = 2.255, p > 0.05), did not have a significant impact on attitude toward the brand (F(2, 192) = 0.486, p > 0.05), did not have a significant impact on purchase intention (F(2, 198) = 1.338, p > 0.05), and did not have a significant impact on brand recall (x2 (2) = 0.248, p > 0.05). Commercial appeal type did, however, have a significant impact on attitude toward the ad (F (2, 192) = 10.918, p < 0.001). Following a series of post-hoc tests it was determined that a commercial with a dramatic appeal (M = 41.61) was preferred to a commercial with a humorous appeal (M = 36.79) or the informative appeal (M = 36.32). No significant difference existed between the humorous and informative commercials’ attitude toward the ad. Similar to other results, should attitude toward the ad be significantly affected by the congruity manipulation, commercial 31 type would need to be controlled for to ensure that this does not confound the results. Further analysis of the one-way ANOVAs and Chi-squares determined that involvement in the television show had no significant effect on perceptions of congruity (F(1, 198) = 1.807, p > 0.05), had no significant effect on attitude toward the ad (F(1, 193) = 0.693, p > 0.05), had no significant effect on purchase intention (F(1, 197) = 0.004, p > 0.05) and had no significant effect on brand recall (x2 (29) = 24.213, p > 0.05). Television involvement did, however, have a significant effect on attitude toward the brand (F (1, 191) = 6.405, p < 0.05). Following the use of a regression analysis, however, the R2 = 0.032, however, so this was determined to be too small of an effect to be deemed a confounding variable. A one-way ANOVA was run in order to determine if congruity perceptions differed for the different congruity manipulations utilized. The results indicated that congruity perceptions did differ for the three conditions (F (2, 199) = 19.473, p < 0.001). A post-hoc analysis was utilized to determine which conditions were significantly different, and it was determined that the congruent condition was significantly different from the incongruent condition (p < 0.001) and the moderately congruent condition (p < 0.001) but that the incongruent and moderate condition were not significantly different (p > 0.05). 32 Table 4: Mean Congruity Perception Scores of Gr0t_1p_s_ Mean SD Congruent 8.921 2.892 Moderately Incongruent 6.761 2.405 Inconment 6.382 2.186 Table 5: Post Hoc Analysis of Congruitv Perceptions Congruent Moderate Incongruent Congruent p-value - 0.000 0.000 Mean Difference - 2.16 2.538 Moderate p-value 0.000 - 0.646 Mean Difference 2.16 - 0.378 Incogqruent p-value 0.000 0.646 - Mean Difference 2.538 0.378 - Research Q_ugstion Testing Based on the above discussion of the respondents’ perceptions of the congruity condition, the research question was tested. This research determined that although congruity perceptions did exist for theme congruity, congruity in this case was a bifurcation between ‘congruent’ and ‘not congruent’ commercials. Moderately incongruent appeals were viewed to be equally incongruent as the extremely incongruent appeals by respondents. The above table (table 5) summarizes the post-hoc analysis conducted for this research question. It includes p-values as well as the mean differences in congruity perceptions for each set of congruity manipulations. 33 Hypothesis Testing These tables summarize the means and standard deviations for the different dependent variables under the three different congruity situations. Iable 6: Mean and Standard Deviations for Attitude Toward the Brand Mean SD Congruent 23.873 6.150 Moderately Incongruent 23.114 5.174 Incongruent 23.661 5.593 Table 7: Mean and Standard Deviations for Purchase Intentions Mean SD Congruent 0.670 0.475 Moderately Incongruent 0.580 0.497 Incongruent 0.690 0.465 _T_able 8: Mean and Standard Deviations for Attitude Toward the Ad Mean SD Congruent 39.050 9.486 Moderately Incongruent 36.328 5.445 Inconguent 39.471 7.064 Ia_ble 9: Mean and Standard Deviations for Brand Recall Mean SD Congruent 0.830 0.383 Moderately Incongruent 0.770 0.421 Incongruent 0.780 0.418 This table summarizes the results of a MANOVA comparing congruent and incongruent advertisement situations. 34 Fible 10: MANOVA: Cmgruent vs. Incongruent Manflation Effect on Dependent Variables Dependent Variable DF F Sil Attitude Toward the Brand 1 0.074 0.786 Purchase Intentions 1 0.016 0.900 Attitude Toward the Ad 1 0.135 0.714 *See tables 6 through 8 for means and standard deviations H1 a: A congruent ad (appeal matches context) will be more effective at inducing positive brand attitudes than an incongruent ad. This hypothesis was tested utilizing a multivariate analysis of variance. The congruity manipulation did not have a significant impact on attitude toward the brand (F (1) = 0.074, p > 0.05). Therefore, hypothesis 1a was not supported. H 1 b: A congruent ad (appeal matches context) will be more effective at inducing positive purchase intentions than an incongruent ad. Following further use of the multivariate analysis of variance procedure, the congruity manipulation was determined to have no significant impact on purchase intentions (F(1) = 0.016, p > 0.05). Therefore, hypothesis 1b was not supponed. H 1 c: A congruent ad (appeal matches context) will be more effective at inducing positive attitude toward the ad than an incongruent ad A multivariate analysis of variance procedure was used to determine if the congruity manipulation had a significant impact on attitude toward the ad, and findings were not significant (F (1) = 0.135, p > 0.05). Therefore, hypothesis 10 was not supported. 35 H2: An incongruent ad (appeal does not match context) will be more effective at inducing brand recall than a congruent ad. The congruity manipulation was tested utilizing a chi-square test to determine if it significantly impacted brand recall (X2 (1) = 0.435, p > 0.05). The results were not significant, and thus hypothesis 2 was not supported. Laple 11: Chi-square: Congruent vs. |ncong_rt_1ent Manipulation Effect on Brand Recall Dependent Variable DF X2 Sig Brand Recall 1 0.435 0.510 *See table 9 for means and standard deviations H3a: A moderately incongruent ad will be more effective at inducing positive attitude toward the ad than an incongruent or congruent ad. The moderate congruity manipulation was tested utilizing a multivariate analysis of variance (F (2) = 3.404, p > 0.035). Since an emotionally warm appeal was found to improve attitude toward the ad in pretesting sections, type of commercial was used as a covariate in a MANCOVA. This removed all significant effects of type of congruity on attitude toward the ad, and thus these results were not significant. Therefore, hypothesis 3a was not supported. @le 1; MANCOVA: Moderate lncomrpent Manipulation Effect on Agg conLrollingfor commercial type Dependent Variable DF F Sig Attitude Toward the Ad 2 2.221 0.111 *See table 8 for means and standard deviations The following table is a summary of the MANOVAs run to compare all three congruity conditions. 36 fine 13: MANOVA: Moderate Incongruent Maniwlationjfiect on Dependent Variables Dependent Variable DF F Sig Attitude toward the Brand 2 0.147 0.864 Purchase Intention 2 0.413 0.662 *See tables 6 through 7 for means and standard deviations H3b: A moderately incongruent ad will be more effective at inducing positive brand attitudes than an incongruent or congruent ad. The moderate congruity manipulation was further tested through the use of multivariate analysis to test this hypothesis (F (2) = 0.147, p > 0.05). These results were not significant, and thus hypothesis 3b was unsupported. H3c: A moderately incongruent ad will be more effective at inducing positive purchase intentions than an incongruent or congruent ad. A multivariate analysis of variance procedure was utilized to test the moderate congruity manipulation’s impact on purchase intentions, and findings were not significant F (2) = 0.413, p > 0.05). Therefore, hypothesis 3c was not supponed. H3d: A moderately incongruent ad will be more effective at inducing brand recall than an incongruent or congruent ad. The moderate congruity manipulation was tested utilizing a chi-square test to determine if this condition significantly impacted brand recall (X2 (2) = 0.622, p > 0.05). The results were not significant, and thus hypothesis 3d was not supponed. 37 Ia_ble 14: Chi-imam: Moderate Incongruent Magiwlation Effect on Brand Reg" Dependent Variable DF X2 Sig Brand Recall 2 0.622 0.733 *See table 9 for means and standard deviations These findings support the earlier findings of Pelsmacker et al (2002) that theme congruity had no significant effects on the key indicators of advertising success. 38 Chapter 6: Discussion This research addressed the effect of different congruity conditions (congruent, moderately incongruent, and incongruent) on advertising effectiveness measures, including brand recall, attitude toward the ad, attitude toward the brand, and purchase intentions. A previous study of advertiser appeal-context theme congruity was conducted (Pelsmacker, 2002). These findings replicate this previous study, and provide further support for the findings. Previous research focused only on the congruent versus incongruent manipulation, but the first set of hypotheses (H1a, H1b, H2) were tested in order to replicate these findings. Null findings are not often found in congruity literature, and thus further evidence for these unexpected results helps scientists to determine that this indeed is an area of congruity that has little impact on consumers’ perceptions of the brand or advertiser. The research question addressed in this study is the most interesting result in this research. Moderate congruity has been a key variable for much of the congruity literature, and has been previously shown to be an effective tool at improving attitudes toward the brand and attitude toward the ad without sacrificing brand recall. As this research indicated, consumers tend to perceive advertisements to be either congruent or incongruent in advertiser appeal-context theme situations. Advertising researchers, in the past, have been able to capitalize on moderate congruity in order to reduce the typical trade-off between attitudes (which tended to be positive for congruent situations) and brand recall (which tended to be the 39 best for incongruent situations). Findings tended to indicate that brand recall and attitudes were both improved in moderate congruity situations. These findings, however, would suggest that advertising researchers cannot rely on this in all types of congruity research. This lack of moderate congruity perceptions from the consumer may be due to the nature of this type of congruity. There are certain situations in which the perception of moderately congruent information is possible, but there are likely many other situations in which the differences between moderately incongruent information and incongruent information are too subtle and irrelevant to the consumer’s evaluation for the consumer to perceive the difference. As such, moderate congruity may only exist in situations in which it would be relevant to the consumer and consumers have the resources to perceive the difference. The lack of moderate congruity perceptions may also be due to other factors which affected the consumers’ evaluations. Some consumers may not have been coffee drinkers or may have been loyal to a particular coffee brand, and thus this may have affected the manipulation. Other factors that may have impacted the manipulation adversely include program selection, commercial selection, or the attention that respondents paid to the commercial in the artificial setting of the online experiment. There are limitations to this study’s generalizability as well due to the homogeneity of the sample (college students) and the product category utilized. As such, these and other factors may have masked the effects that moderate congruity could have had on the consumers’ perceptions. In future studies, 40 researchers should seek to understand whether or not there is a perception of moderate congruity in a more general population. Even if consumers are unable to consciously perceive differences between moderate incongruity and incongruity in this type of congruity research, it is possible that a moderately incongruent manipulation may still have an effect on the measures of advertising effectiveness. This is another area for further research. The moderate congruity manipulation in this research may have been confused by operational difficulties. When consumers were asked about congruity perceptions, at times other types of congruity may have interfered (such as product congruity, self congruity, advertiser congruity, etc) with their evaluations. Despite explicit instructions to evaluate the theme congruity, it is possible that this confounded the experimental results. By utilizing a rational appeal for the moderate congruity condition, a different cognitive set of considerations and evaluations may have been created. Both of the other commercials were based on affective responses, so by introducing a commercial which focuses on cognitive responses, the results may have been adversely affected. This research only studied one brand which did already do some small amount of advertising in the geographic area. An existing brand was used to increase realism and external validity, but further research could explore fictional or unknown brands, or sets of known brands in order define the parameters of the congruity variable. This research focused on television commercials to manipulate congruity perceptions, but future research may be needed to determine if the results for moderate congruity can be replicated in some form of 41 advertising aside from television commercials (e.g., print advertising or new media) The null findings in this research indicate that advertiser appeal-context theme congruity has no significant impact on measures of advertising effectiveness. Because incongruity was found to have no significant negative impact on advertising effectiveness measures, advertisers should be able to use the same appeal type regardless of the context without negative repercussions. This would allow advertisers to use appeals appropriate to the product category without concerning themselves with the impact the context will have on how the advertisement will be received by consumers. By recognizing this, advertisers will be able to use the same commercials in multiple contexts, thereby reducing the costs of creating advertisements. Advertisers will also be able to ensure that the most appropriate appeal is used for the target audience and product category without needing to consider what type of program it will be placed in. 42 Appendix A: Survey Instrument Commercial I Context Evaluations Informed Consent Form You are being asked to participate in an experiment to examine the relationship between attributes of television programs and television commercials. You will be asked to evaluate certain characteristics of both the programs and commercials. Your participation is voluntary. You may refuse to participate or may refuse to answer some of the questions asked. You may discontinue participation at any time. You will be asked to watch a television program with commercials on from your home computer for about 15 minutes and then answer questions about the experience. Your responses will be confidential, and your privacy will be protected to the maximum extent of the law. Since you will be participating for credit or as part of a classroom research experience, you will be asked for your name and PID following completion of the questionnaire on a separate survey. Your questionnaire will be separate and have no personal identification on it. Identification numbers that cannot be connected to your personal information will be assigned to the collected questionnaires after completion. The only foreseeable risk is that your credit may be incorrectly assigned. If your credit is not recorded correctly, please contact the researcher listed below. If you have questions about this study, contact: Keith Adler, Ph.D., Department of Advertising, Public Relations, and Retailing, Michigan State University, 366 Communication Arts Building, East Lansing, MI 48824, (517) 353-3266, adler@msu.edu. If you have any questions about your role and rights as a research participant, or would like to register a complaint about this study, you may contact, anonymously if you wish, the Director of MSU's Human Research Protection Programs, Dr. Peter Vasilenko, at 517-355-2180, FAX 517-432-4503, or e-mail irb@msu.edu, or regular mail at: 202 Olds Hall, MSU, East Lansing, MI 48824. By clicking on the ’next page' button below, you are indicating your voluntary agreement to participate in this study. Please watch the following video before proceeding to the rest of the survey. You will be asked questions on the program and the commercials you viewed, with 43 emphasis on one commercial in particular. Thank you for your cooperation. Video Using an emotional warmth scale from 0 to 100, where 100 is very emotionally warm and O is not emotionally warm, how emotionally warm was the program you just watched? Using a humor scale from 0 to 100, where 100 is very humorous and 0 is not humorous, how humorous was the program you just watched? i— How appealing do you find this television program? (1) Unappealing (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Appealing F (“ t" (7 f" I” (" What is your opinion of this television program? (1) Bad (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Good (7 (" (‘ (" (" (7 I" How pleasant do you find this television program? (1)Unpleasant (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Pleasant (‘ (7 f” f“ (7 (" (7 How favorable do you find this television program? (1)Unfavorable (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Favorable (‘ t” t" (‘ F I" (7 44 How likable do you find this television program? (1)Unlikable (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7)Likable I" I" (" I" t" (‘ t" The first ad you viewed was a coffee ad. What brand of coffee was it selling? I The brand was Tim Horton's coffee. Had you heard of the Tim Horton's brand before watching this commercial? F Yes F No Have you ever made a purchase from Tim Horton's? is Yes c No Using an emotional warmth scale from 0 to 100, where 100 is very emotionally warm and 0 is not emotionally warm, how emotionally warm was the coffee ad you just watched? [._ Using a humor scale from O to 100, where 100 is very humorous and 0 is not humorous, how humorous was the coffee ad you just watched? I'— Using an informativeness scale from 0 to 100, where 100 is very informative and 0 is not informative, how informative was the coffee ad you just watched? l__ 45 How well would you say the coffee ad 'fits' with the program you were watching? c r“ r‘ r“ r (1) Fits very well r (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Does not fit How similar would you say the mood of the coffee commercial is to the program you watched? " (1)Not similar 8 (2) r (3) r (4) F (5) F (6) F (7) Very similar How interesting did you find the coffee commercial you viewed? (1) Interesting (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Uninteresting (‘ I" t" F f‘ F (7 How appealing did you find the coffee commercial you viewed? (1)Unappealing (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Appealing (7 f“ (" (7 t" F (7 How believable did you find the coffee commercial you viewed? (1)Unbelievable (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7)Believable (" (7 (" F t“ 4" (7 How attractive did you find the coffee commercial you viewed? (1)Attractive (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Unattractive t" t" (7 (‘ I" (7 I" How impressive did you find the coffee commercial you viewed? (1)1mpressive (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7)Unimpressive t" t" (‘ t" (" t7 (7 46 I-Iow informative did you find the coffee commercial you viewed? (1)Uninformative (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7)lnformative (7 f“ F t" I" (7 (~ How eye-catching did you find the coffee commercial you viewed? (1)Eye-catching (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Not eye-catching t" I" (" t" t" (7 (7 How clear did you find the coffee commercial you viewed? (1)Unclear (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Clear t" F F F f“ F ("‘ How acceptable did you find the coffee commercial you viewed? (1)Acceptable (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Unacceptable 1" (7 t" t" I" F (7 How appealing do you find this coffee brand? (1) Unappealing (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Appealing r r r r‘ c r“ r“ What is your opinion of this coffee brand? (1)1361d (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Good F F f” (" (7 F (" 47 How pleasant do you find this coffee brand? (1)Unpleasant (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Pleasant r‘ I" (" F (" (7 I" How favorable do you find this coffee brand? (1)Unfavorable (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) Favorable (7 I" (7 t“ I" ('~ I" How likable do you find this coffee brand? (1)Unlikable (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7)Likable (7 I" F (‘ f“ (‘ I" How likely are you to purchase products from this coffee brand in the future? r (1) Definitely won't purchase from Tim Horton's (2) Probably won't purchase from Tim Horton's (3) May not purchase from Tim Horton's (4) Don’t know if I will or won't purchase from Tim Horton's (5) May purchase from Tim Horton’s (6) Probably will purchase from Tim Horton's 333333 (7) Definitely will purchase from Tim Horton's How interesting do you find the specialty coffee shop product category? (1)lnteresting (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7)Uninteresting (7 I" f‘ t" I" f“ I" 48 The second ad you viewed was a computer ad. What brand of computer was it selling? The brand was a Dell computer. Had you heard of the Dell brand before watching this commercial? 6 (fl Yes No Have you ever made a purchase from Dell? r‘ Yes F No Using an emotional warmth scale from 0 to 100, where 100 is very emotionally warm and 0 is not emotionally warm, how emotionally warm was the computer ad you just watched? Using a humor scale from 0 to 100, where 100 is very humorous and 0 is not humorous, how humorous was the computer ad you just watched? i— Using an informativeness scale from O to 100, where 100 is very informative and 0 is not informative, how informative was the computer ad you just watched? [_ How well would you say the computer ad 'fits' with the program you were watching? I“ I" F F t" (3) <4) <5) (6) ‘i (1) Fits very well (2) (7) Does not fit 49 How similar would you say the mood of the computer commercial is to the program you watched? F (1)Notsimilar " (2) F (3) F (4) F (5) F (6) F (7)Verysimi|ar How likely are you to purchase products from this computer brand in the mmm? r“ (1) Definitely won't purchase from Dell (2) Probably won't purchase from Dell (3) May not purchase from Dell (4) Don't know if I will or won’t purchase from Dell (5) May purchase from Dell (6) Probably will purchase from Dell 333333 (7) Definitely will purchase from Dell How interesting do you find the home computer product category? (1)lnteresting (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7)Uninteresting I" I" I" (" I" (" I" The third ad you viewed was a pizza ad. What brand of pizza was it selling? The brand was Little Caesar's pizza. Had you heard of the Little Caesar's brand before watching this commercial? (I! Yes ("No 50 Have you ever made a purchase from Little Caesar's? (a Yes PNo Using an emotional warmth scale from 0 to 100, where 100 is very emotionally warm and O is not emotionally warm, how emotionally warm was the pizza ad you just watched? Using a humor scale from 0 to 100, where 100 is ven/ humorous and O is not humorous, how humorous was the pizza ad you just watched? I—_ Using an informativeness scale from 0 to 100, where 100 is very informative and 0 is not informative, how informative was the pizza ad you just watched? I—_ How well would you say the pizza ad 'fits' with the program you were watching? r“ c r r“ r‘ (1)Fits very well 8 (2) (3) (4) (5) (e) (7) Does notfit How similar would you say the mood of the pizza commercial is to the program you watched? (7 t" t“ I" t" r‘ (1)Not similar 5 (2) (3)‘ (4) (5) (6) (7) Very similar 51 How likely are you to purchase products from this pizza brand in the future? F (1) Definitely won't purchase from Little Caesar's (2) Probably won't purchase from Little Caesar's (3) May not purchase from Little Caesar's (4) Don't know if I will or won't purchase from Little Caesar's (5) May purchase from Little Caesar's (6) Probably will purchase from Little Caesar's 333333 (7) Definitely will purchase from Little Caesar's How interesting do you find the pizza product category? (1)lnteresting (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7)Uninteresting I" f‘ (7 I“ f‘ (" f‘ Please fill out the following information about yourself: Gender 5 Male Female Age F— 52 Appendix B: Pretest Questionnaire Programs and Commercials: A Study of Congruity Informed Consent Form You are being asked to participate in a study to evaluate television commercials. You will be asked to evaluate certain characteristics of each commercial after watching it in class. Your participation is voluntary. You may refuse to participate or may refuse to answer some of the questions asked. You may discontinue participation at any time. Your responses will be confidential, and your privacy will be protected to the maximum extent of the law. Since you will be participating for credit or as part of a classroom research experience, you will be asked to provide your name. Your questionnaire responses will be separated from your responses before any analysis begins. Identification numbers that cannot be connected to your personal information will be assigned to the collected questionnaires after you leave. The only foreseeable risk is that your credit may be incorrectly assigned. If your credit is not recorded correctly, please contact the researcher listed below. If you have questions about this study, contact: Keith Adler, Ph.D., Department of Advertising, Public Relations, and Retailing, Michigan State University, 366 Communication Arts Building, East Lansing, MI 48824, (517) 353-3266, adler@msu.edu. If you have any questions about your role and rights as a research participant, or would like to register a complaint about this study, you may contact, anonymously if you wish, the Director of MSU’s Human Research Protection Programs, Dr. Peter Vasilenko, at 517-355-2180, FAX 517-432-4503, or e-mail irb@msu.edu, or regular mail at: 202 Olds Hall, MSU, East Lansing, MI 48824. Name Date Signature 53 Commercial i: How emotionally warm do you find this ad on a scale of 0 to 100, with 0 being not emotionally warm at all and 100 being very emotionally warm? How humorous do you find this ad on a scale of 0 to 100, with 0 being not humorous at all and 100 being very humorous? How informative do you find this ad on a scale of 0 to 100, with 0 being not informative at all and 100 being very informative? 54 Appendix C: Commercial Storyboards Appendix C: Commercials Commercial 1: Emotional Appeal Music as man (grandfather) walks down the street with Tim Horton’s coffee Flashback: grandfather finds son playing hockey in the street Grandfather “You must study harder. Not just hockey all the time" (as they walk away together) Present day: grandfather walks into rink and sees caretaker Caretaker: “Hi Charlie, what are you doing here?" Grandfather (Charlie) “My grandson" Charlie hands coffee to caretaker “For you” Caretaker: “Gee thanks” Charlie climbs bleachers to sit next to his son. Son “Attaboy.” (cheering on his own son) “Dad. What are you doing here?” Charlie: “See Tommy" Hands coffee to his son “Double double” Son: “Thanks" 55 Flashback: Charlie comes in while his son is watching hockey and doing homework at the same time “You study. No hockey." See team (with Tommy) playing hockey Son: “He’s good.” Charlie: “Better than you." Son: sighs “How would you know?" Charlie: “I come watch.” Flashback: Charlie hiding around the comer watching his son playing hockey. Caretaker comes and brings him a coffee while he watches. Son: "Okay, what team did I play for?” Charlie pulls a hockey card out of his wallet with a picture of his son “You red wing” Son smiles Tommy does something well on the ice Charlie: Cheering “My grandson! My grandson!" Son: “Thanks for coming, Dad” Charlie: “Give me my picture back” Caption: Every cup tells a story. 56 Commercial 2: Humor Appeal Woman working at desk Man in break room: “I love fresh strawberries” as he and female co- worker eat strawberry tarts Woman in break room: “Mmmm-hmm” Woman working at desk stands up and begins to walk toward the break room and tarts when another man steps between her and the break room Man in office: “Hi Karen, 1 want to discuss the schedule of receivables with you, it won’t take too long.” (Karen is watching more and more people take away tarts from break room) “Okay good. Now before we start I should mention that one of our secondary objectives for this year was to. . .” Karen sees that there is only one tart left and cuts him off as she picks him up and heaves him over her shoulder as she runs toward the break room and picks up the last strawberry tart. Commentator: “Nothing can get between you and the plump and juicy fresh strawberries in Tim Horton’s delicious strawberry tarts. Just $1.49” 57 58 Commercial 3: Informative Man 1 eating soup in break room Man 2 walks in: “Mmmm, something smells.” Man 1: “Yeah, it's mushroom soup from Tim Horton’s” Man 2: “I'll have to get some" Man 1 taps spoon on the side of the bowl to wipe off excess before taking next mouthful Man 2: ‘Why are you eating your soup like that? Man 1: “Proper etiquette” Man 2: “For eating soup" Man 1: “Well when it’s made with three kinds of mushrooms and it’s this good, then yes. It’s a little thing called being refined” (shakes out ‘napkin’ to wipe his mouth) Man 2: “Yeah, and that’s a little thing called an oil rag.” (ream I‘ '~I Mushioffiuu Man 1 frowns as he looks at rag a Commentator: “Made with chiitake @ porcini and buck mushrooms it’s Tim Horton’s creamy field mushroom soup” Jingle: “Always fresh, always Tim Horton’ 59 Bibliography Aylesworth, Andrew B., Scott B. MacKenzie (1998). Context Is Key: The Effect of Program-Inducted Mood on Thoughts About the Ad. Journal of Advertising, 27(2), 17-31. Campbell, Margaret C., Ronald C. Goodstein (2001). The Moderating Effect of Perceived Risk on Consumers‘ Evaluations of Product lncongruity: Preference for the Norm. Joumal of Consumer Research, 28(3), 439-449. Chandon, Pierre, Brian Wansink, Gilles Laurent (2000). A Benefit Congruency Framework of Sales Promotion Effectiveness. Joumal of Marketing, 64(4), 65-81. Chang, Chingching (2008). Chronological Age Versus Cognitive Age for Younger Consumers: Implications for Advertising Persuasion. Joumal of Advertising, 37(3), 19-32. Chaudhuri, Arjun, Ross Buck (1995). Media Differences in Rational and Emotional Responses to Advertising. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 39(1), 109-125. Cho, Hyung (1995). Humor Mechanisms, Perceived Humor and their Relationships to Various Executional Types in Advertising. Advances in Consumer Research, 22, 191- 197. Dahlen, Michael (2005). The Medium as a Contextual Cue: Effects of Creative Media Choice. Joumal of Advertising, 34(3), 89-98. ' Edwards, Paul (1972). The Encyclopedia of Philosophy, Volume Five. New York: MacMillan Publishing Co., Inc. & The Free Press. Heckler, Susan E., Terry L. Childers (1992). The Role of Expectancy and Relevancy in Memory for Verbal and Visual Infomiation: What is Incongruency? Journal of Consumer Research, 18(4), 475-492. Horn, Martin I., William J. McEwen (1977). The Effect of Program Context on Commercial Performance. Journal of Advertising, 6(2), 23-27. Geuens, Maggie, Patrick De Pelsmacker (1997). Product Category Involvement and the Reaction of Polish and Belgian Consumers to Different Types of Advertising Appeals. European Advances in Consumer Research, 3, 33- 41. . (1999). Individual Differences and the Communication Effects of Different Types of Emotional Stimuli: Affect Intensity Revisited. Psychology and Marketing, 16(3), 195-209. 60 Kamins, Michael A. (1990). An Investigation into the “Match-up” Hypothesis in Celebrity Advertising: When Beauty May be Only Skin Deep. Joumal of Advertising, 19(1), 4-13. Kamal Gupta (1994). Congruence Between Spokesperson and Product Type: A Matchup Hypothesis Perspective. Psychology and Marketing, 11(6), 569-586. Kamins, Michael A., Lawrence J. Marks, Deborah Skinner (1991). Television Commercial Evaluation in the Context of Program Induced Mood: Congruency Versus Consistency Effects. Joumal of Advertising, 20(2), 1- 14. LaTour, Michael 8., Tony L. Henthorne (1994). Female Nudity in Advertising, Arousal and Response: A Parsimonious Extension. Psychological Reports, 75(3 pt. 2), 1683-1690. Lavack, Anne M., Mrugank V. Thakor, Ingrid Bottausci (2001). Music-Brand Congruency in Radio Advertising. American Marketing Association, 12, 314-315. Leather, Phil, Sally McKechnie, Manon Amirkhanian (1994). The Importance of Likeability as a Measure of Television Advertising Effectiveness. International Journal of Advertisin, 1 3(3), 265-280. Lee, Mira, Ronald J. Faber (2007). Effects of Product Placement in On-Line Games on Brand Memory: A Perspective of the Limited-Capacity Model of Attention. Journal of Advertising, 36(4), 75-90. Lee, Yih Hwai, Charlotte Mason (1999). Responses to Information lncongruency in Advertising: The Role of Expectancy, Relevancy and Humor. Joumal of Consumer Research, 26(2), 156-169. Mandler, George (1982). The Structure of Value: Accounting for Taste in Margaret S. Clark and Susan T. Fiske (Ed.), Affect and Cognition: The 17‘" Annual Carnegie Symposium (pp 3-36). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Meyers-Levy, Joan, Therese A. Louie, Mary T. Curren (1994). How Does the Congruity of Brand Names Affect Evaluations of Brand Name Extensions? Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(1), 46-53. Meyers-Levy, Joan, Brian Sternthal (1993). A Two-Factor Explanation of Assimilation and Contrast Effects. Joumal of Marketing Research, 30(3), 359-368. 61 Meyers-Levy, Joan, Alice M. Tybout (1989). Schema Congruity as a Basis for Product Evaluation. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(1), 39-54. (1997). Context Effects at Encoding and Judgment in Consumption Settings: The Role of Cognitive Resources. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(1), 1-14. Moore, David J., William D. Harris (1996). Affect Intensity and the Consumer’s Attitude Toward High Impact Emotional Advertising Appeals. Journal of Advertising, 25(2), 36-50. Hong C. Chen (1994). Exploring the Role of Individual Differences in Affect Intensity on the Consumer’s Response to Advertising Appeals. Advances in Consumer Research, 21, 181 -1 87. Moore, Robert 8., Claire Allison Stammerjohan, Robin A. Coulter (2005). Banner Advertiser-Web Site Context Congruity and Color Effects on Attention and Attitudes. Joumal of Advertising, 34(2), 71 -84. Osgood, Charles E., George J. Suci, Percy H. Tannenbaum (1957). The Measurement of Meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Osgood, Charles E., Percy H. Tannenbaum (1955). The Principle of Congruity in the Prediction of Attitude Change. Psychological Review, 62(1), 42-55. De Pelsmacker, Patrick, Maggie Geuens (1996). The Communication Effects of Warmth, Eroticism and Humour in Alcohol Advertisements. Journal of Marketing Communications, 2(4), 247-262. Pascal Anckaert (2002). Media Context and Advertising Effectiveness: The Role of Context Appreciation and Context / Ad Similarity. Journal of Advertising, 31(2), 49-61. De Pelsmacker, Patrick, Joeri Van den Bergh (1996). The Communication Effects of Provocation in Print Advertising. lntemational Journal of Advertising, 15(3), 203-221 . (1998). Advertising Content and Irritation: A Study of 226 TV Commercials. Joumal of lntemational Consumer Marketing, 10(4), 5-27. Rifon, Nora J., Sejung Marina Choi, Carrie S. Trimble, Hairong Li (2004). Congruence Effects in Sponsorship: The Mediating Role of Sponsor Credibility and Consumer Attributions of Sponsor Motive. Journal of Advertising, 33(1), 29-42. Russell, Cristel Antonia (2002). Investigating the Effectiveness of Product Placements in Television Shows: The Role of Modality and Plot 62 Connection Congruence on Brand Memory and Attitude. Joumal of Consumer Research, 29(3), 306-318. Sharma, Andrew (2000). Recall of Television Commercials as a Function of Viewing Context: The Impact of Program-Commercial Congruity on Commercial Messages. The Journal of General Psychology, 127(4), 383- 396. Spears, Nancy, Surendra N. Singh (2004). Measuring Attitude Toward the Brand and Purchase Intentions. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 26(2), 53—66. Smith, Stephen M., Curtis P. Haughtvedt, John M. Jadrich, Mark R. Anto (1995). Understanding Responses to Sex Appeals in Advertising: An Individual Difference Approach. Advances in Consumer Research, 22, 735-739. Stewart, David W., Scott Koslow (1989). Executional Factors and Advertising Effectiveness: A Replication. Journal of Advertising, 18(3), 21 -32. Tai, Susan HQ (1999). Advertising Ethics: The Use of Sexual Appeal in Chinese Advertising. Teaching Business Ethics, 3(1), 87-100. Vakratsas, Demetrios, Tim Ambler (1996). Advertising Effects: A Taxonomy and Review of Concepts, Methods and Results from the Academic Literature. M. S. I. Working Paper. Cambridge: Marketing Science Institute. Weinberger, Marc G., Charles S. Gulas (1992). The Impact of Humor in Advertising - A Review. Journal of Advertising, 18(2), 35-59. 63