WI 1 WI 312% WWNWIllilllHHWIW: LIBRARY MIChIQaH State niversity Z 03"] I This is to certify that the thesis entitled COMPUTER-MEDIATED PERSUASION IN ONLINE REVIEWS: STATISTICAL VERSUS NARRATIVE EVIDENCE presented by SEOYEON HONG has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the M. A. degree in PUBLIC RELATIONS /%Z~W Major'Professor’s Signature OB/ILl/éloo? Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Employer .o—.---.-.-.-u-o--o--n-c---n-o-u---ugc—n—-- — PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5/08 K'lPrqlAcc8Pres/ClRC/DateDue.indd COMPUTER-MEDIATED PERSUASION IN ONLINE REVIEWS: STATISTICAL VERSUS NARRATIVE EVIDENCE By Seoyeon Hong A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Public Relations 2009 ABSTRACT COMPUTER-MEDIATED PERSUASION IN ONLINE REVIEWS: STATISTICAL VERSUS NARRATIVE EVIDENCE By: Seoyeon Hong The current study examined persuasive effects of online reviews about a product considering summary of statistical ratings as a type of statistical evidence and consumer comments as a type of narrative evidence. Study 1 used a (statistical and narrative i review) X 2 (negative and positive review) between subject design. Study 2 used a 2 (statistical review valence: positive and negative) X 2 (narrative review valence: positive and negative) between subject design. In both studies, the effects of online reviews were examined on the credibility perception of the reviews, attitudes about a product, and intention to purchase the product. Findings indicated that statistical negative reviews were perceived to be the most credible. On the other hand, statistical reviews and narrative reviews did not differ in affecting attitudes about the product and intention to purchase the product. Additionally, the current study found that the vividness of individuals’ perceptions of each review type and valence had varying effects on review credibility, attitudes about the product, and intention to purchase the product. This thesis is dedicated to my families: mother, Soonsup Lee, my father, Sanggi Hong, my aunts, Hiosup Lee and Yangsup Lee, and my grandmother, Youngsoon Kim who have given endless love, support, and encouragement to me TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES v CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION 1 CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEWS 3 CHAPTER 3: THEORETICAL BACKGROUND 8 CHAPTER 4: STUDY 1 15 STUDY 1 METHOD 15 STUDY 1 RESULTS 19 CHAPTER 5: STUDY 2 27 STUDY 2 METHOD 27 STUDY 2 RESULTS 30 CHAPTER 6: SUMMERY OF FINDINGS - 35 CHAPTER 7: DISCUSSION 38 CHAPTER 8: CONCLUSION 44 APPENDICES 50 APPENDIX A. Background Information 50 APPENDIX B. 2 x 2 design 52 APPENDIX C. Vividness / Credibility / Attitudes about a Product / Purchase Intention 56 REFERENCE A ' 61 Table 1. Table 2. Table 3. Table 4. Table 5. LIST OF TABLES Demographic Information - Study 1 45 Online shepping Tendency - Study 1 46 Demographic Information - Study 2 47 Online shopping Tendency - Study 2 48 Comparisons of Experimental Conditions in Study 1 and Study 2 ------------- 49 INTRODUCTION Word of mouth (WOM), as a type of interpersonal communication, is defined as all forms of information exchanged among people regarding products, or services (Bass, 2004; Biyalogorsky, Gersyner, & Libai, 2001 ). It can be simply described as buyers saying to each other “if you buy, I will buy” (Arndt, 1967a, p.295). People often follow their peer group’s opinions because they are concerned about what others may think of them, how others react to their behavior, and how credible the information they obtain fiom others is (Arndt, 1967b). WOM is a persuasive and distinctive marketing tactic because of its informal and non-commercial characteristics. Brooks (1957) claimed that WOM is a key factor in interpersonal and social influence networks and is powerful and relevant to the difiusion of information. Consumers often have difficulty in evaluating products because of their limited sphere of experience, and they perceive a high risk in purchasing (Heywood, 1989). Therefore, they rely on the information fi'om previous users of the product to mitigate the risk. Walker (1995) demonstrated that more than 40% of Americans seek others’ opinions when they need shopping advice. WOM recommendations are perceived to be more credible (Dellarocas, 2003) and receive more attention than other types of recommendation (Amdt, 1967; Ditcher, 1966; Silverman, 1997). Because WOM messages originate from “people like me” who have no personal interest in recommending a particular brand or a product, WOM is perceived to be unprejudiced (Allsop, Bassett, & Hoskins, 2007). As a result, people tend to rely heavily on WOM when they make purchasing decisions (Davis, Guiltman, & Jones, 1979; Richins & Root-Shaffer, 1988). After the earliest research of Katz and Lazarfeld (1955) that claimed a sigrificant role for WOM as a personal influence, the importance of WOM has been examined with various products or services such as searching for a physician (Coleman, Katz, & Menzel 1957; F eldman & Spencer, 1965), new fabric (Beal & Rogers, 1957 ), information about farming practice (Katz, 1961), razor blades (Sheth, 1971), air conditioners ONhyte, 1954), and automobiles (Newman & Staelin, 1972). In particular, WOM is considered a major drive in the diffusion of information (Brown & Reingen, 1987) due to its influence on a person’s decision to adopt an innovation (Rogers, 1995; Williams, Strover, & Grant 1994). Considering the significant influence of WOM on purchasing, WOM is “the most powerful force in the marketplace” (Silverman, 2001, p. 47). LITERATURE REVIEW Traditional WOM versus Electronic WOM Since the appearance of the Internet, a number of scholars have underlined the role of WOM in the online sphere (e. g., Goldenberg, Libai & Muller, 2001; Hennig- Thurau et al., 2004; C. Park & Lee, 2009; D.-H. Park & Lee, 2008). Electronic WOM (eWOM) refers to “any positive or negative statement made by potential, actual, or former customers about a product or company, which is made available to a multitude of people and institutions via the Internet” (Hennig—Thurau et al., 2004, p.39). Although Hennig-Thurau et a]. (2004) claimed that the concepts of eWOM and traditional WOM are similar, when it comes to speed, convenience, one-to-many reach and the lack of pressure in face-to-face communication, electronic WOM is more influential than traditional WOM (Chatterjee, 2001; C. Park & Lee, 2009; Phelps et al., 2004). Dellarocas (2002) also claimed that eWOM is different from traditional WOM in its bi- communicational possibilities, low cost when conducted via the Internet, the company’s ability to control and monitor the operation, and new potential in the online situations. Knowing the importance of eWOM, companies are currently putting forth enormous efforts to initiate WOM online (Schwartz, 1998). Researchers have identified five different characteristics useful in distinguishing eWOM fiom traditional WOM (Chatterjee, 2001; Chen & Xie, 2008; Hennig-Thurau et al., 2004; Sun, Youn, Wu, & Kuntarapom, 2006). The first characteristic is the endless boundary: eWOM communicators can reach far beyond geographical limitations, because consumers all over the world are accessible via the Internet. The second is the written sphere: unlike traditional WOM which consists of spoken word exchanges, electronic WOM takes place with written words. This makes eWOM more prevalent than traditional WOM, because the written word is more logical than the spoken. The third characteristic emphasizes the communication with anonymous people: consumers share their opinions with other anonymous consumers on the Internet, while traditional WOM occurs between friends or relatives. The fourth is directness: eWOM allows a seller to effectively and directly initiate and broadcast consumer reviews with the help of advancement in technologies. The last characteristic is volume: information available online is much more voluminous in quantity compared to that of traditional WOM. On-Line Consumer Review as a Tool of eWOM With the growth of online commerce, seeking online opinions of other consumers is increasingly becoming a part of purchasing behavior (Pitta & Fowler, 2005). Technological developments in web sites provide unprecedented opportunities for customers to express their opinions about companies, products, and services in the form of online reviews (Evans, Wedande, Ralston, & Van't Hul, 2001). Online consumer reviews have been treated as special type of WOM in many studies (e. g., Chatterjee, 2001; Chen & Xie, 2008; Godes & Mayzlin, 2004; Hu, Liu, & Zhang, 2008; D.-H. Park & Lee, 2008). Chen and Xie (2008) discussed the implications of online customer reviews for marketing strategies and recommended that the sellers need to develop unique response system in online reviews. The study of DH. Park and Lee (2008) reported that the number of reviews affects the perceived review informativeness and popularity of the product. Online customer reviews significantly impact both companies and customers. For example, online reviews for movies significantly affect the revenue of the weekly box office (Liu, 2006). Out of consumers who visit online malls, 50% consider reviews an important factor in their decision (Piller, 1999). Chevalier and Mayzlin (2006) found that online book reviews are integral sources in book sales. Knowing the significant impact of customer reviews, companies such as eBay.com and Arnazon.com take action to increase the benefits of using online reviews (Melink & Alm, 2002). Online reviews can provide indirect experience before consumers make a purchase decision (Park, Lee, & Han, 2007) and work as free ‘sales assistance’ to the e-consumers utilizing them as a new part of marketing communications (Chen & Xie, 2008; Wernerfelt, 1994). Online consumers utilize online reviews in order to learn about products and reduce the uncertainty risks and transaction cost (Hu, Liu, & Zhang, 2008; D.-H. Park & Lee, 2008). Consumers frequently use online review that consumer- generated information is more credible than seller-created information (Wilson & Sherrell, 1993). Also, consumer- generated information is more interesting to the online consumer compared to marketer- generated information (Bickart & Schindler, 2001). People consider consumer reviews created by users to be for the benefit of other users, so their information will contain more possible usages (Chen & Xie, 2008). Additionally, online reviews enable buyers to overcome the limitations of an online store; it is impossible to see, touch and smell the product (Chatterjee, 2001; Chen & Xie, 2008; Park, Lee, & Han, 2007). In other words, online review meets consumers’ need as they are looking for recommendations about purchasing a product. Therefore, the online review as a form of eWOM communication has become a crucial source for both consumers and marketers. The feedback system that eBay employs is an excellent example of online consumer review (Chevalier & Mayzlin, 2006). The feedback system allows consumers to acquire verifiable information about sellers who may be unknown in cyberspace. Sellers can build their reputation by increasing the number of transactions they make on eBay (Resnick & Zeckhauser, 2002). A number of studies have examined the efficiency of the eBay feedback system. For example, Resnick, Zeckhauser, Swason, and Lockwood (2006) showed that users have stronger willingness to purchase the product from a seller with a high reputation rather than a seller with a newly created account. Additionally, positive feedback increased the price of products (Dewan & Hsu, 2004; Kalyanarn & McIntyre, 2001; Mcdonald & Slawson Jr., 2002; Melink & Alm, 2002) while negative feedback decreased the price (Kalyanam & McIntyre, 2001; Melink & Alm, 2002) and probability of sale (Melink & Alm, 2002). Computer-Mediated Persuasion O’Keefe (2004) pointed out that despite today’s widespread use of computers, few studies investigated the aspects of “computer mediated persuasion.” Moreover, researchers have emphasized the necessity of considering the influence of computer mediated persuasion on the use and effectiveness of evidence (Reynolds & Reynolds, 2002; Seiter & Seiter, 2005). Therefore, the current research aims to fill the gap between persuasion research in offline and online environments and improve understanding of the persuasion effects of online evidence. Drawing from the literature on WOM and persuasion, this study seeks to build a connection between them in the context of online reviews. The present study compares the two types of online reviews: narrative and statistical evidence. Online reviews can be classified into two types of feedback. One is a statistical feedback (e.g., 999 customers out of 1000 satisfied with the product) and another is a narrative feedback (e.g., I strongly recommend this camera. When I went to Sea World at night, it was easy to take a picture in the darkness with its amazing flash function). The feedback system in eBay provides both types of feedback on the same web page. Inconsistencies between the statistical and the narrative feedback can occur. Even when the statistical feedback is very positive, the narrative feedback can be negative for the same product. Then, one may ask which kind of feedback is more likely to influence online consumers' attitudes about a product. THEORETICAL BACKGROUND Persuasion and Evidence McCroskey (1969) defined evidence as “factual statements originating fi'om a source other than the speaker, objects not created by the speaker, and opinions of persons other than the speaker that are offered in support of the speaker’s claims” (p. 170). Evidence includes relevant facts, opinions, and information that support the persuader’s assertion (O’Keefe, 2002). In general, evidence is employed to increase the persuasiveness of the message and provide argumentative support for the message. Evidence consists of the facts and opinions creating a logical basis for the argument (Abernathy, 1964; Cohen & Nagel, 1962). Past research has shown that use of evidence is more persuasive when message recipients have personal involvement in an issue (Cacioppo, Petty, Kao, & Rodriguez, 1986; Petty & Cacioppo, 1984; Petty, Cacioppo, & Goldman, 1981; Reynolds & Reynolds, 2002; Seiter & Seiter, 2005). Statistical versus Narrative Evidence The current paper examines two types of evidence: statistical and narrative evidence. Statistical evidence is one type of factual information (Baesler & Burgoon, 1994; Reinard, 1988), includes “quantified descriptions of events, persons, places, or other phenomena” (Church & Wulbanks, 1986, p. 108), and is characterized as proof in the form of summary information across a number of cases (Allen & Preiss, 1997). Dickson (1982) considered “a rate, frequency, proportion, percentage, probability, average, median, or other statistical parameter” as statistical information (p. 399). Persuasiveness of statistical evidence can depend on the number of observations, because a larger number of cases can indicate stronger objectivity. Moreover, statistical evidence has a direct impact on judgments and an indirect impact on attitudes (Boster et a1, 2000). Narrative evidence is a message that presents information in a personal format as opposed to a statistical format (Ameson & Query, 2001; Ellinson & Buzzanell, 1999; Vanderford, Smith, & Harris, 1992). Narrative evidence is defined as the “use of case stories or examples to indicate that the conclusion offered by the communicator is true” (Allen & Preiss, 1997). Narrative evidence includes testimonial assertions which Reinard (1988) referred to as judgments and opinions fi'om others, and also includes what O'Keefe (2002) refers to as description and information in detail about an instance or an event. Generally, it focuses on elaborating the example of an event and providing appealing detail, characters, and some plot (Greene & Brinn, 2003). A number of studies have been conducted to examine the effects of evidence. Particularly, the comparison between narrative and statistical evidence has received attention from researchers. Previous studies stated that narrative evidence is more persuasive (Kazoleas, 1993; Kahneman & Trversky, 1973; Nisbett & Ross, 1980; Nisbett & Borgida, 1975; Taylor & Tompson, 1982). On the other hand, Reinard (1988) showed that both statistical and narrative evidence are equally persuasive and memorable. Also, other studies reported that both statistical and narrative evidence produce persuasive effects equally and reported no difference between the two types (Iyengar & Kinder, 1987; Nadler, 1983; Reyna, Woodruff, & Brainerd, 1987). Therefore, as O'Keefe (2002) pointed out, it is unclear why and how one type of evidence is more effective than the other. Putting aside the debates, a number of studies have continually emphasized the superior effect of statistical evidence. A meta-analysis by Allen and Preiss (1997) showed statistical evidence to be slightly more effective. Correspondingly, statistical messages are found to be more effective for health information than narrative information but both statistical and narrative messages are better than none at all (Greene & Brinn, 2003). Kopfrnan, Smith, Yun, and Hodges (1998) noted that messages containing statistical evidence produced greater cognitive reactions while narrative evidence produced greater affective reactions. Moreover, Baesler and Burgoon (1994) found that both forms of evidence were initially persuasive when compared to no evidence at all; however; statistical evidence was more persuasive in the long term. Baesler and Burgoon (1994) pointed out the fact that only a few studies on statistical versus narrative evidence used representative forms of narrative evidence, and they criticized the absence of studies addressing vividness. Vividness is “information that’s emotionally interesting, concrete and imagery provoking, proximate in a sensory, temporal, or spatial way” (N isbett &Ross, 1980, p. 45). Narrative evidence is easier to imagine than statistical evidence because narrative evidence has vividness as a factor that bolsters the effect of persuasion. On the other hand, statistical evidence, considered to be more abstract than narrative, fails to facilitate involvement, increase interest, or engage attention. Baesler and Burgoon (1994) controlled vividness when comparing the two types of evidence and found that with vividness, statistical evidence was generally more persuasive and produced immediate, moderate term (48 hours) and long term (one week) persuasive effects. Considering the potential role of vividness as a confounding factor, it seems important to consider and 10 control the effect of vividness in order to conduct a fair comparison of statistical and narrative evidence. It is expected that statistical evidence is more compelling when vividness is controlled. Persuasive Effects In the current study, persuasive effects of online reviews will be assessed in three ways: credibility of reviews as evidence, attitudes about a product, and intention to purchase a product. Evidence can affect perceived credibility which will eventually increase persuasive effects (Reinard, 1988). Perceived credibility refers to “the judgments made by a perceiver (e.g., a message recipient) concerning the believability of a communicator” (O’keefe, 2002, p. 181). Although credibility often pertains to an information source or sender, online reviews as a form of eWOM are provided by anonymous and numerous people, rather than a particular one person. Thus, credibility can be also relevant for online. reviews. For example, F lanagin and Metzger (2000) examined credibility of Internet information. When individuals see online reviews of a product, the extent to which they trust the reviews may vary. Online reviews can be persuasive in terms of their effects on individuals' evaluations of the product and intentions to purchase the product to which online reviews pertain. Attitudes about a product and purchasing intention have been a popular outcome of interests in previous studies examining the influence of WOM (e.g., Bansal & Voyer, 2000; C. Park & Lee, 2009; Sen & Lennan, 2007; Wangenheim & Bayou, 2004; Ying & Chung, 2006). It is expected that different types of evidence can be perceived more or less trustworthy and believable, and can result in varying levels of attitudes and purchasing intentions. 11 Hypothesis l-a: Individuals will perceive online statistical reviews as more credible than online narrative reviews when vividness is controlled. Hypothesis l-b: Individuals will indicate more positive attitudes about a product after seeing online statistical reviews than online narrative reviews when vividness is controlled. Hypothesis 1-c: Individuals will indicate stronger intention to purchase a product after seeing online statistical reviews than online narrative reviews when vividness is controlled. Given that evidence means facts or opinions presented as proof for an assertion (Reynolds & Reynolds, 2002), evidence can also vary as negative or positive for a product at issue. Both positive and negative information can influence the attitude change, but the impact of negative messages on consumers is considerably more significant (Laczniak, DeCarlo, & Ramaswami, 2001). Satisfied consumers rarely mention their experience, but dissatisfied customers tell everyone (Holmes & Lett 1977; Swan & Oliver 1989). Additionally, research on WOM shows that negative WOM has larger impacts on purchasing decisions and on consumer behavior than positive WOM (Herr, Kardes, & Kim, 1991). The valence of evidence (i.e., negative and positive evidence) can be differentially persuasive when individuals consider purchasing or not purchasing a product. Of the past studies of evidence, most studies used negative messages to show the effectiveness of the evidence regardless of the comparison results. Only a small number of studies used positive messages (e.g., Hamill, Wilson, & Nisbett, 1980; Hoeken, 2001; Kahneman, & Tversky, 1973 ; Koballa, 1986). For example, the past studies comparing statistical and 12 narrative evidence used negative evidence or messages about topics such as crime rates (Caroll, I97 7), car accidents and safety belts (Kazoleas, 1993), electronic accidents (Wells & Harvey, 1977), car accidents and organ donation (Kopfrnan et al., 1998), pollution (N adler, 1982), failed refrigerators (Dickson, 1982), divorce and juvenile delinquency (Baesler & Burgoon, 1994), alcohol education messages (Slater & Rouner, 1996), and skin cancer (Greene & Brinn, 2003). Of the studies using negative evidence, seven studies concluded that statistical evidence has greater persuasive effects. Thus, it is possible that the difference between statistical and narrative evidence can be greater when the evidence is negative toward a product than when it is positive. Thus, the following hypothesis is advanced: Hypothesis 2: The difference between statistical online review and narrative online review containing negative information will be larger than the difference between statistical and narrative online review containing positive information. Overview of Study 1 and Study 2 Two studies were conducted to examine the persuasive effects of review type and review valence. Study 2 replicated study 1 with only one difference. Study 1 used a 2 (review type: statistical and narrative review) X 2 (review valence: negative and positive review) between subject design. Study 2 used a 2 (statistical review valence: positive and negative) X 2 (narrative review valence: positive and negative) between subject design. For example, participants in one of four experimental conditions of study 1 read positive statistical review, whereas participants in one of four experimental conditions read statistical positive review as well as narrative positive review. After reading reviews 13 about a product (a digital camera), participants in both studies indicated their credibility perception of reviews, attitudes about the product, and intention to purchase the product. 14 Study 1 Study 1 Method Participants One hundred eighty one students (68.51% women, age M = 22.35, SD = 3.09) enrolled at Michigan State University participated in the study in exchange of course credit. Of the participants, 70.4% were Caucasian, 8.3% were Asian or Asian American, 3.7% were Afi'ican American, 2.8% were Pacific islander, 0.9% were Hispanic, and 13.9% were other (i.e., unclassifiable, mixed, etc.). Tables 1 and 2 provide more detailed information about the demographic characteristics and online shopping tendencies of participants. Design and Procedure This study used a 2 (review type: statistical and narrative review) X 2 (review valence: negative and positive review) between subject design. The experimental materials included information about a product (a digital camera) and were designed following typical online review formats commonly found on eBay.com, for example. Statistical review included the number of ratings (e.g., positive review = 900 positive ratings out of 1000; negative review = 900 negative ratings out of 1000). For narrative review, examples of positive review included "Amazing quality" and "I will recommend this to everyone." Examples of negative review included "Disappointing quality" and "I won't recommend this to anyone." See appendix II for the experimental materials. This study also included a control condition that provided only the product information and no online reviews. In the control condition, participants answered questions only about their 15 attitudes towards the product and intention to purchase the product, not about review credibility and vividness. See appendix III for all the measurement items. Manipulation Check Review valence. For positive and negative reviews, 10 items (a = 0.99) assessed the valence of reviews on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). Example items included, "overall reviews are positive" and "almost all of reviews are recommending this product." A 2 (review type: statistical and narrative review) X 2 (review valence: negative and positive review) between-subject ANOVA was conducted on valence. Review type did not have a significant main effect, F (1, 141) = 0.40, p = .53, 112 = .00. Narrative review (M = 3.92, SD = 2.29) did not differ from statistical review (M = 3.85, SD = 2.04). Review valence had a significant main effect, F (l, 141) = 923.15,p < .001, 112 = .87. Positive review (M = 5.81, SD = 0.81) was perceived as more positive than statistical review (M = 1.82, SD = 0.80). The interaction between review type and valence was significant, F (1, 141) = 5.97, p = .01, n2 = .006. For negative review, statistical review (M = 1.94, SD = 0.84) did not differ from narrative review (M = 1.70, SD = 0.74), t (68) = 1.26, p = .21. For positive review, narrative review (M = 6.02, SD = 0.71) was perceived as more positive than narrative review (M = 5.62, SD = 0.85), t (73) = 2.22, p = .03. For statistical review manipulation, positive review (M = 5.34, SD = 1.14) was perceived as much more positive than negative review (M = 2.10, SD = 1.18), t (168) = 18.18, p < .001, n2 = .66. Positive review rating was significantly higher than the midpoint (4) of the scale, t (88) = 11.05, p < .001. Negative review rating was significant 16 lower than the midpoint of the scale, indicating that negative review was perceived as negative, t (80) = -14.49, p < .001. For narrative review manipulation, positive review (M = 5.81, SD = 0.80) was perceived as much more positive than negative review (M = 1.82, SD = 0.73), t (168) = 34.10, p < .001, 112 = .87. Positive review rating was significantly higher than the midpoint (4) of the scale, t (85) = 23.07, p < .001. Negative review rating was significant lower than the midpoint of the scale, indicating that negative review was perceived as negative, t (83) = -25.07, p < .001. Vividness. The vividness imagery was rated with items modified and adapted from Nisbett and Ross (1980) and Baesler and Burgoon (1994). Each two types (personal and general) of four items (vividness, emotiveness, concreteness, and imaginativeness) assessed vividness on a 7-point scale (1 = not at all, 7 = very much). These eight items for statistical vividness had a reliability of a = 0.93 and narrative vividness had a = 0.90. A 2 (review type: statistical and narrative review) X 2 (review valence: negative and positive review) between-subject ANOVA showed that review type had a significant main effect, F(1, 141) = 18.74,p < .001, n2 = .12. Narrative review (M: 4.64, SD = 0.97) was considered more vivid than statistical review (M = 3.82, SD = 1.30). Review valence did not have a significant main effect, F (l, 141) = 0.09, p = .76, n2 = .00. The interaction between review type and valence was not significant, F (1, 141) = 1.51, p = .12, n2 = .015. Measurements of Dependent Variables Review credibility. F lanagin and Metzger (2000) identified 5 items popularly used to measure information credibility (biased-unbiased, trustworthy-untrustworthy, accurate- 17 inaccurate, believable-unbelievable, and complete-incomplete). These five items (a = 0.80) were used in the current study (e.g., "To me, overall reviews are ..... ") with a 7-point scale (e.g., 1 = extremely unbelievable, 7 = extremely believable). Attitudes toward the product. Attitudes were measured with three items (a = 0.97) (good, favorable, and desirable). Participants were asked to indicate their feelings about this product on a 7-point scale (1 = extremely bad, 7 = extremely good). Behavioral intention to purchase the product. Purchasing intention was measured with 5 items (a = 0.96). Example items included "If I am going to buy a new digital camera in the future, I intend to buy this produc " and "If I am going to buy a new digital camera in the future, I would not buy this product" (reverse-coded). l8 Study 1 Results Hypothesis 1 Hypothesis l-a predicted that online statistical review would be more credible than online narrative review when vividness was controlled. The hypothesis was tested with a 2 (review type: statistical and narrative review) X 2 (review valence: negative and positive review) between-subject ANCOVA with vividness as a covariate. The results showed that vividness was not significant, F (1, 140) = 0.20, p = .65, n2 = .00. Review type had a significant main effect on review credibility, F (l , 140) = 9.32, p = .003, n2 = .06. Statistical review (M = 4.78, SD = 1.00) was perceived as more credible than narrative review (M = 4.29, SD = 0.77). Thus, the data were consistent with Hl-a. Additionally, review valence had a significant main effect on review credibility, F (1, 140) = 9.43, p = .003, n2 = .06. Negative review (M = 4.78, so = 1.05) was perceived as more credible than positive review (M = 4.33, SD = 0.74). The interaction effect of review type by valence was not significant, F ( 1, 140) = 1.43, p = .23, 112 = .009. Hypothesis 1-b predicted that attitudes about the product would be more positive after seeing online statistical review than online narrative review when vividness was controlled. The hypothesis was tested with a 2 (review type: statistical and narrative review) X 2 (review valence: negative and positive review) between-subject AN COVA with vividness as a covariate. The results showed that vividness was not significant, F (1, 140) = 1.89, p = .17, n2 = .004. Review type did not have a significant main effect on attitudes about the product, F (1, 140) = 0.84, p = .36, n2 = .00. Statistical review (M = 3.64, SD = 1.72) and narrative review (M = 3.83, SD = 1.49) did not differ from one another. Thus, the data were not consistent with H1 -b. 19 Additionally, review valence had a significant main effect on attitudes about the product, F (1, 140) = 341 .46,p < .001, n2 = .70. Positive review (M = 5.03, SD = 0.74) resulted in more positive attitudes about the product than negative review (M = 2.34, SD = 1.01). The interaction effect of review type by valence was not significant, F (1, 140) = 0.81, p -—- .37, n2 = .00. Hypothesis 1-c predicted that intention to purchase a product would be stronger after reading online statistical review than online narrative review when vividness was controlled. A 2 (review type: statistical and narrative review) X 2 (review valence: negative and positive review) between-subject ANCOVA with vividness as a covariate was conducted. The results showed that vividness was not significant, F (1, 140) = 0.07, p = .78, n2 = .00. Review type did not have a significant main effect on behavioral intention to purchase the product, F (1, 140) = 0.001, p = .98, n2 = .00. Statistical review (M = 3.27, SD = 1.64) and narrative review (M = 3.27, SD = 1.44) did not differ from one another. Thus, the data were not consistent with H 1 -c. Additionally, review valence had a significant main effect on intention to purchase the product, F (1, 140) = 192.95, p < .001, 112 = .57. Positive review (M = 4.40, SD = 1.02) resulted in greater intention than negative review (M = 2.06, SD = 0.99). The interaction effect of review type by valence was not significant, F (1, 140) = 1.29, p = .26, n2 = .004. Hypothesis 2 Hypothesis 2 predicted that the difference between statistical and narrative online review containing negative information would be larger than the difference between 20 statistical and narrative online review containing positive information. The hypothesis was tested with one way AN OVAs for each of the three dependent variables. Review credibility. A one way AN OVA comparing the statistical negative, narrative negative, statistical positive, and narrative positive conditions showed that the experimental conditions had a significant main effect on credibility perception of review, F (3, 141) = 7.59, p < .001, n2 = .14. A post hoc comparison using Tukey showed that statistical negative review (M = 5.11, SD = 1.20) was perceived as more credible than narrative negative review (M = 4.43, SD = 0.72) and that statistical positive review (M = 4.48, SD = 0.65) did not differ from narrative positive review (M = 4.17, SD = 0.80). There were no significant differences among narrative negative, statistical positive, and narrative positive reviews. Attitudes toward the product. A one way ANOVA comparing the statistical negative, narrative negative, statistical positive, and narrative positive review conditions and the control condition showed that the experimental conditions had a significant main effect on attitudes about the product, F (4, 176) = 92.71, p < .001, n2 = .68. A post hoc comparison using Tukey showed that statistical negative review condition (M = 2.16, SD = 1.09) and narrative negative review condition (M = 2.53, SD = 0.90) did not significantly differ fi'om one another and that statistical positive review condition (M = 5.00, SD = 0.82) and narrative positive condition (M = 5.06, SD = 0.64) did not significantly differ fiom one another. Statistical and narrative negative review conditions differed significantly fiom statistical and narrative positive review conditions. The control condition (M = 4.16, SD = 0.76) differed significantly from both negative and positive review conditions. 21 Because statistical and narrative review conditions did not differ fiom one another, both conditions were combined across negative and positive review conditions. Which of negative and positive review conditions affected attitudes to a greater extent was examined. As expected, attitudes about the product were less positive in the negative review condition (M = 2.34, SD = 1.01) than the control condition (M = 4.16, SD = 0.76), t (104) = 9.52, p < .001, point-biserial r = .68. Attitudes about the product were more positive in the positive review condition (M = 5.03, SD = 0.74) than in the control condition (M = 4.16, SD = 0.76), t (109) = 5.77, p < .001, point-biserial r = .48. Comparing the two correlations (.68 versus .48) indicated that the extent to which the negative review condition decreased attitudes about the product compared to the control condition was greater than the extent to which the positive review condition increased attitudes compared to the control condition, 2 = 2.20, p = .03, two-tailed. Intention to purchase the product. A one way ANOVA comparing the statistical negative, narrative negative, statistical positive, and narrative positive conditions and the control condition showed that the experimental conditions had a significant main effect on behavioral intention to buy the product, F (4, 176) = 48.17, p < .001, n2 = .52. A post hoc comparison using Tukey showed that statistical negative condition (M = 1.96, SD = 0.96) and narrative negative condition (M = 2.16, SD = 1.02) did not significantly differ fi'om one another and that statistical positive condition (M = 4.49, SD = 1.11) and narrative positive condition (M = 4.30, SD = 0.91) did not significantly differ from one another. Statistical and narrative negative conditions differed significantly from statistical and narrative positive conditions. The control condition (M = 3.20, SD = 1.07) differed significantly from both negative and positive conditions. 22 Because statistical and narrative review conditions did not differ fiom one another, both conditions were combined across negative and positive review conditions and comparisons were made in order to examine which of negative and positive review conditions affected behavioral intention to a greater extent. As expected, intention to purchase the product was weaker in the negative review condition (M = 2.06, SD = 0.99) than in the control condition (M = 3.20, SD = 1.07), t (104) = 5.49, p < .001, r = .47. Intention to purchase the product was higher in the positive review condition (M = 4.40, SD = 1.02) than in the control condition (M = 3.20, SD = 1.07), t (109) = 5.49, p < .001, r = .48. Comparing the two correlations (.47 versus .48) indicated that the extent to which the negative review condition decreased behavioral intention compared to the control condition did not differ from the extent to which the positive review condition increased behavioral intention compared to the control condition, 2 = 0.09, p = .93, two-tailed. Additional Analyses. The aforementioned ANCOVA and ANOVA results for hypotheses 1 and 2 indicated that vividness was not a significant covariate and that inclusion or exclusion of vividness as a covariate did not change the findings regarding the effects of evidence type on review credibility, attitudes about the product, and intention to purchase the product. In order to further examine the role of vividness, however, exploratory analyses were conducted. Moderated multiple'regression analyses examined the possibility of vividness as a moderator for the effects of review type and review valence on review credibility, attitudes about the product, and behavioral intention to purchase the product. Each of the two categorical variables was dummy-coded; statistical review = 0 and narrative review = 1 for review type, negative review = 0 and positive review = 1 for review valence. One 23 continuous variable, vividness, was mean-centered and multiplied by review type and by review valence to create second-order predictors (i.e., review type X vividness and review valence X vividness) and a third-order predictor (i.e., review type X review valence X vividness) for testing interactions. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted with the first-order predictors in the first block, the second-order predictors in the second block, and the third-order predictor in the third block. For any significant interactions, simple regression analyses were conducted to probe the interaction pattern. Review Credibility The overall model with all the predictors was significant, F (6, 138) = 7.49, p < .001 , adj. R2 = .21. The first-order predictors in the first block of the regression model explained a significant amount of variance in review credibility, F (3, 141) = 7.12, p < .001, adj. R2 = .13. Among the first-order predictors, vividness was not significant, [3 = - .05, t = -0.61, p = .54, sr (semipartial correlation) = -.05. On the other hand, review type was significant, [3 = -.25, t = -2.96, p = .004, sr = -.23, indicating that statistical review was perceived as more credible than narrative review. Review valence was significant, [3 = -.24, t = -3.10, p = .002, sr = -.24, indicating that negative review was perceived as more credible than positive review. The two second-order predictors accounted for a significant amount of variance in review credibility, F change (2, 139) = 3.31, p = .04, chhange = .04. The interaction of review type by vividness was not significant, B (unstandardized coefficient) = 0.23, t = 1.74, p = .09, sr = .13. The interaction of review valence by vividness was significant, B = 0.23, t = 1.98, p = .049, sr = .15. Simple slope of vividness was negative for negative review, b = -0.13, p = .22, whereas simple slope of vividness was positive for positive 24 review, b = 0.07, p = .34. This second-order interaction was qualified by a third-order interaction, which clarified that the interaction of review type by vividness varied with review valence. The third-order predictor also accounted for a significant amount of variance in review credibility, Fchange (1, 138) = 13.66, p < .001, chhange = .08. The interaction of review type and review valence with vividness was significant, B = -0.93, t = -3.67, p < .001, sr = —.27. For statistical review, simple slopes of vividness were negative for negative review, b = -0.3 7, p = .01, and positive for positive review, b = 0.16, p = .049. For narrative review, simple slopes of vividness were positive for negative review, b = 0.31, p = .009, and negative for positive review, b = -0.12, p = .44. Attitudes toward the Product The overall model with all the predictors was significant, F (6, 138) = 33.80, p < .001, adj. R2 = .58. The first-order predictors in the first block of the regression model explained a significant amount of variance in attitudes about the product, F (3, 141) = 64.80, p < .001, adj. R2 = .57. Among the first-order predictors, vividness was not significant, [3 = .02, t = 0.41, p = .68, sr = .02. Review type was not significant, [3 = -.01, t = -0.12, p = .91, sr = -.01. Review valence was significant, B = .76, t = 13.92, p < .001, sr = .76, indicating that attitudes about the product were more positive in the positive review condition than in the negative review condition. The two second-order predictors did not account for a significant amount of variance in attitudes, F change (2, 139) = 2.60, p = .08, chhange = .015. The third-order predictor also did not account for a significant amount of variance in attitudes, Fchange (1, 138) = 0.11, p = .74, chhange = .00. Behavioral Intention to Purchase the Product 25 The overall model with all the predictors was significant, F (6, 138) = 61 .91, p < .001, adj. R2 = .72. The model with the first-order predictors was significant, F (3, 141) = 116.43, p < .001, adj. R2 = .71. Among the first-order predictors, vividness was not significant, B = .07, t = 1.51, p = .13, sr = .07. Review type was not significant, B = .04, t = 0.85, p = .40, sr = .04. Review valence was significant, B = .84, t = 18.53, p < .001, sr = .84, indicating that behavioral intention to purchase the product was greater in positive review condition than in negative review condition. The two second-order predictors accounted for a significant amount of variance in attitudes, Fchange (2, 139) = 3.54, p = .04, chhange = .014. The interaction of review type by vividness was significant, B = -0.32, t = -2.43, p = .02, sr = .11. Simple slope of vividness was positive for statistical review, b =.12, p = .21, whereas simple slope of vividness was negative for narrative review, b = -.17, p = .15. The interaction of review valence with vividness was not significant, B = 0.12, t = 0.97, p = .33, sr = .04. The third- order predictor also did not account for a significant amount of variance in attitudes, Fchange (1, 138) = 1.42. p = .24, chhange = .003. 26 Study 2 In study 1, participants read only one type of reviews, either statistical or narrative. But because most Web sites in reality present both types of reviews, study 1 might have lacked realism. As a way to replicate study 1 and also to increase realism, study 2 was designed using the same materials as study 1, except that participants in study 2 read both types of reviews. Study 2 Method Participants One hundred forty five students (67.59% women, age M = 22.70, SD = 3.35) enrolled at Michigan State University participated in the study in exchange of course credit. Of the participants, 76.5% were Caucasian, 8.2% were Asian or Asian American, 3.5% were African American, 1.2% were Hispanic, and 10.6% were other (i.e., unclassifiable, mixed, etc.). Tables 3 and 4 provide more detailed information about the demographic characteristics and online shopping tendencies of participants. Design and Procedure This study used a 2 (statistical review valence: positive and negative) X 2 (narrative review valence: positive and negative) between subject design. Unlike study 1 where participants read either statistical or narrative review only,_all participants in study 2 read both statistical and narrative review. For example, participants in one of four experimental conditions read statistical positive review and narrative negative review and evaluated each review on review valence and vividness. Then, participants indicated their credibility perception, attitudes about the product, and intention to buy the product. The 27 experimental materials and measurement items used in study 2 were identical to study 1 (see appendices A, B, and C). Manipulation Check Review valence. Ten items assessed the valence of reviews (or = 0.99 for statistical review and a = 0.99 for narrative review) on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). Statistical review positivity was not significantly correlated with narrative review positivity, r (168) = .08, p = .29. For statistical review manipulation, positive review (M = 5.34, SD = 1.14) was perceived as much more positive than negative review (M= 2.10, SD = 1.18), t (168) = 18.18,p < .001, n2 = .66. Positive review rating was significantly higher than the midpoint (4) of the scale, t (88) = 11.05, p < .001. Negative review rating was significant lower than the midpoint of the scale, indicating that negative review was perceived as negative, t (80) = -14.49, p < .001. For narrative review manipulation, positive review (M = 5.81, SD = 0.80) was perceived as much more positive than negative review (M = 1.82, SD = 0.73), t (168) = 34.10, p < .001, n2 = .87. Positive review rating was significantly higher than the midpoint (4) of the scale, t (85) = 23.07, p < .001. Negative review rating was significant 0 lower than the midpoint of the scale, indicating that negative review was perceived as negative, t (83) = -25.07, p < .001. Vividness For statistical vividness, the eight items had a reliability of or = 0.89 and narrative vividness had 01 = 0.92. Items (e. g., "In general, to what extent were these messages vividly presented?" and "To you, personally, to what extent were these messages concretely presented?") used a 7-point scale (1 = not at all, 7 = very much). Correlation 28 between statistical review vividness and narrative review vividness was small but significant, r (168) = .28, p < .001. T-tests were conducted to see if statistical review and narrative review differed in vividness. The results showed that narrative review (M = 4.73, SD = 1.02) was perceived as more vivid than statistical review (M = 3.67, SD = 1.05), paired sample t (169) = 11.16, p < .001, n2 = .42. For the vividness rating of narrative review, negative review (M = 5.06, SD = 0.88) was perceived as more vivid than positive review (M = 4.40, SD = 1.04), independent samples t (168) = 4.46, p < .001, n2 = .11. For vividness rating of statistical review, negative review (M = 3.72, SD = 1.08) and positive review (M = 3.62, SD = 1.01) did not differ, independent samples t (168) = 0.65, p = .52, n2 = .00. Measurements of Dependent Variables Five items (a = 0.84) assessed review credibility (e.g., "To me, overall reviews are ..... ", 1 = extremely biased, 7 = extremely unbiased). Three items (a = 0.98) assessed attitudes about the product (e.g., 1 = extremely unfavorable, 7 = extremely favorable). Five items (0. = 0.95) assessed intention to purchase the product (e. g., "If I am going to buy a new digital camera in the future, I intend to buy this product"). 29 Study 2 Results Overview Moderated multiple regression analyses was conducted to examine the persuasive effects of statistical review valence and narrative review valence as well as to test a possibility of vividness as a moderator for the effects of statistical review valence and narrative review valence on review credibility, attitudes about the product, and behavioral intention to purchase the product. Each of the two categorical variables was dummy- coded; statistical negative review = 0 and statistical positive review = 1 for statistical review valence, narrative negative review = 0 and narrative positive review = 1 for narrative review valence. Two continuous variables (statistical review vividness and narrative review vividness) were mean-centered and multiplied by statistical review valence and by narrative review valence to create second-order predictors (i.e., statistical review vividness X statistical review valence, narrative review vividness X narrative review valence). Additionally, a product term of statistical review valence X narrative review valence was created as a second-order predictor. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted with the first-order predictors in the first block and the second- order predictors in the second block. For any significant interactions, simple regression analyses and post hoc comparisons were conducted to probe the interaction pattern. Review Credibility The overall model with all the predictors was significant, F (7, 159) = 12.48, p < .001, adj.R2 = .33. The first-order predictor in the first block of the regression model accounted for a significant amount of variance in review credibility, F (4, 162) = 13.74, p <. 001, adj.R2 = .24. Among the first-order predictors, vividness of statistical review was 30 inversely related to review credibility, B = -.28, t = -4.00, p < .001, sr = -.27, and vividness of narrative review was positively related to review credibility, B = .39, t = 5.19, p < .001, sr = .35. Valence of statistical review was also a significant predictor, B = -.19, t = -2.73, p = .007, sr = -.19, indicating that statistical negative review was perceived as more credible than statistical positive review. On the other hand, valence of narrative review was not a significant predictor, B = -.12, t = -1 .71, p = .09, sr = -.12. The second-order predictors in the second block of the regression model explained a significant amount of variance in review credibility, F change (3, 159) = 8.31, p < .001, chhange = .10. The interaction of narrative review valence with narrative review vividness was not significant, B = 0.03, t = 0.20, p = .84, sr = .01. However, the interaction of statistical review valence by statistical review vividness was significant, B = 0.41, t = 3.49, p = .001, sr = .22. Simple slope of statistical review vividness was negative for statistical negative review, b = -0.45, p < .001, whereas simple slope of statistical review vividness was close to zero for statistical positive review, b = 0.01 , p = .89. This finding indicated that for statistical negative review, the more vivid the review was, the less credible the review was. Additionally, the interaction of statistical review valence by narrative review valence was significant, B = 0.93, t = 3.83, p < .001, sr = .24, indicating that statistical negative review coupled with narrative negative review (M = 5.16,, SD = 1.24) was perceived as more credible than statistical negative review coupled with narrative positive review (M = 4.241,, SD = 0.73), statistical positive review coupled with narrative negative review (M = 4.33b, SD = 0.79), and statistical positive review coupled with narrative positive review (M = 4.36b, SD = 0.75). Attitudes toward the Product 31 The overall model with all predictors was significant, F (7, 159) = 23.04, p < .001, adj.R2 = .48. The first-order predictor in the first block of the regression model accounted for a significant amount of variance in attitudes about the product, F (4, 162) = 34.93, p <. 001, adj.R2 = .45. Among the first-order predictors, vividness of statistical review was not significantly related to attitudes about the product, B = .07, t = 1.14., p = .26, sr = .07, and vividness of narrative review was not significant, B = -.07, t = -1.16, p = .25, sr = -.07. On the other hand, valence of statistical review was a significant predictor, B = .37, t = 6.33, p < .001, sr = .36, indicating that attitudes about the product were more positive in the statistical positive review condition than in the statistical negative review condition. Valence of narrative review was a significant predictor, B = .55, t = 9.07, p < .001, sr = .52, indicating that attitudes about the product were more positive in the narrative positive review condition than in the narrative negative review condition. The second-order predictors in the second block of the regression model explained a significant amount of variance in attitudes about the product, F change (3, 159) = 4.33, p = .006, R2 change = .04. The interaction of statistical review valence by statistical review vividness was not significant, B = -0.25, t = -1 .51, p = .13, sr = -.08. The interaction of statistical review valence by narrative review valence was not significant, B = -0.62, t = -1.81, p = .07, sr = -.10. However, the interaction of narrative review valence by narrative review vividness was significant, B = 0.47, t = 2.55, p = .01, sr = .14. Simple slope of narrative review vividness was negative for narrative negative review, b = -O.3 7, p = .02, whereas simple slope of narrative review vividness was positive but not significant for narrative positive review, b = 0.10, p = .42. This finding indicated that for narrative negative review, the more vivid the review was, the less positive attitudes were. 32 Intention to Purchase the Product The overall model with all predictors was significant, F (7, 159) = 10.10, p < .001, adj.R2 = .28. The first-order predictor in the first block of the regression model accounted for a significant amount of variance in intention to purchase the product, F (4, 162) = 14.84, p <. 001, adj.R2 = .25. Among the first-order predictors, vividness of statistical review was not significant, B = .08, t = 1.1 8, p = .24, sr == .08, and vividness of narrative review was not significant, B = -.03, t = -0.46, p = .64, sr = -.03. On the other hand, valence of statistical review was a significant predictor, B = .25, t = 3.73, p < .001, sr = .25, indicating that intention to purchase the product was stronger in the statistical positive review condition than in the statistical negative review condition. Valence of narrative review was a significant predictor, B = .45, t = 6.28, p < .001, sr = .42, indicating that intention to purchase the product was stronger in the narrative positive review condition than in the narrative negative review condition. The second-order predictors in the second block of the regression model explained a significant amount of variance in intention to purchase the product, F change (3, 159) = 3.04, p = .03, R2 change = .04. The interaction of statistical review valence by statistical review vividness was not significant, B = 0.05, t = 0.25, p = .80, sr = .02. The interaction of statistical review valence by narrative review valence was not significant, B = -0.47, t = -1.25, p = .21 , sr = -.08. However, the interaction of narrative review valence by narrative review vividness was significant, B = 0.51, t = 2.52, p = .01, sr = .16. Simple slope of narrative review vividness was negative for narrative negative review, b = -0.33, p = .03, whereas simple slope of narrative review vividness was positive but not significant for narrative positive review, b = 0.16, p = .26. This finding indicated that for 33 narrative negative review, the more vivid the review was, the weaker intention to purchase the product was. 34 Summary of Study 1 and Study 2 Findings The two main hypotheses of the current study predicted stronger persuasive effects of statistical review than narrative review and expected the difference between statistical and narrative reviews to be greater when the reviews were negative than when they were positive. Individuals considered statistical review more credible than narrative review. Negative review was perceived as more credible than positive review. Statistical negative review was perceived as more credible than narrative negative review, while credibility of statistical positive review did not differ fi'om that of narrative positive review. The data were consistent with hypotheses 1 and 2 only for review credibility, but not for attitudes about the product and intention to purchase the product. Statistical review did not differ fiom narrative review in affecting attitudes about the product or intention to purchase the product. Compared to the control condition where individuals did not read any reviews, the extent to which negative review affected attitudes (in terms of decreasing positive attitudes) was greater than the extent to which positive reviews affected attitudes (in terms of increasing positive attitudes). But the extent to which negative reviews weakened intention to purchase the product did not differ from the extent to which positive review strengthened intention. For negative review, statistical review and narrative review did not differ in affecting attitudes about the product and intention to purchase the product. Similarly, for positive review, statistical review and narrative review did not differ in affecting attitudes about the product and intention to purchase the product. Study 1 of current research found that individuals Considered narrative reviews to be more vivid than statistical reviews and that vividness was a moderator for the effect of 35 review type and valence on credibility perception of review and intention to purchase the product. The more likely individuals were to perceive statistical negative review to be vivid, the less likely individuals were to perceive it to be credible, whereas the more likely individuals were to perceive statistical positive review to be vivid, the more likely individuals were to perceive it to be credible. On the other hand, the more likely individuals were to perceive narrative negative review to be vivid, the more likely individuals were to perceive it to be credible, whereas the more likely individuals were to perceive narrative positive review to be vivid, the less likely individuals were to perceive it to be credible. When it comes to purchasing intention, the more likely individuals were to perceive the statistical review to be vivid, the stronger were their intentions to purchase the product. On the other hand, the more likely individuals were to perceive the narrative review to be vivid, the weaker were their intentions to purchase the product. Additionally, study 2 of current research found that when statistical negative review was accompanied by narrative negative review, the reviews were perceived as the most credible, compared to any other combination of statistical positive review and narrative negative or positive review. The interaction of statistical review vividness with statistical review valence was significant for review credibility, whereas the interaction of narrative review vividness with narrative review valence was significant for attitudes about the product and intention to purchase the product. That is, for statistical negative review, the more likely individuals were to perceive the review to be vivid, the less likely individuals were to perceive it to be credible. For narrative negative review, the more likely individuals were to perceive the review to be vivid, the less positive individuals' 36 attitudes were about the product and the less likely individuals were to intend to purchase the product. 37 DISCUSSION The current research examined persuasive effects of online reviews for the categories of statistical versus narrative and positive versus negative reviews. Past research showed that the usefulness of negative reviews varied with product types (Sen & Lerman, 2007) and consumers' familiarity with a retailer (Chatterjee, 2001). The current study adds review type (summary of numeric ratings as a type of statistical evidence versus consumer comments as a type of narrative evidence) to the literature as another moderator of the effects of negative reviews on credibility perception of reviews. Statistical negative reviews were perceived as more credible than narrative negative reviews, possibly because a large number of ratings indicates greater objectivity, less bias, and numerous people who were familiar with the product. Sen and Lerman (2007) and Chatterjee (2001) argued that the stronger effects of negative reviews for utilitarian products and unfamiliar retailers could be because negative reviews for utilitarian products had greater objectivity and negative reviews of unfamiliar retailers helped with reducing uncertainties about risks. These reasons can be relevant for the stronger credibility of negative reviews containing statistical information, because statistical reviews may have heightened perceptions of objectivity and amount of risk information provided by experienced users of the product. Because previous research findings have shown greater persuasiveness of negative WOM over positive WOM (Herr et al., 1991; Larczniak et al., 2001), it was not surprising for the current study to find that negative reviews were perceived as more credible than positive review, that statistical negative reviews coupled with narrative negative reviews were the most credible, and that negative reviews affected attitudes 38 about the product to a greater extent than positive reviews did. Nevertheless, a reason for the current finding could be that a utilitarian product (a digital camera) was used in the current study. Sen and Lerman (2007) showed that consumers were more likely to consider negative reviews useful for utilitarian products (e.g., cell phones and digital cameras) than for hedonic products (e.g., movies and music CDs and DVDs), possibly because reviews for utilitarian products could be more objective and based on actual features and technical functions of the products, whereas reviews for hedonic products could be more subjective and based on individuals' personal preferences. It is possible that by considering negative reviews more credible than positive reviews, participants in the current study might have considered the negative reviews to be genuine concerns and sincere warnings expressed by previous buyers who hope others do not experience the same unpleasantness and disappointment. On the other hand, participants in the current study might have considered the positive reviews to be consiStent with the usual product information as advertised by manufacturers. Another reason for the current finding could be that participants did not know the company that made and sold the digital camera used in the current study (i.e., the digital camera was labeled as "Brand X Camera" with no specific information about who made the camera). Chatterjee (2001) reported that consumers who chose a retailer unfamiliar to them tended to look at more negative reviews than those who chose a familiar retailer, possibly because negative reviews could be more helpful for consumers to avoid risks with an unfamiliar retailer. Chatterjee (2001) firrther speculated that users of a familiar retailer could be more likely to dismiss negative reviews because they were already satisfied with the retailer. Thus, a future study may examine if the effects of negative 39 reviews on credibility perception vary with people's familiarity with manufacturers and sellers. Unlike the difference between statistical and narrative reviews for review credibility, the current study did not find many differences between statistical and narrative reviews for attitudes about the product and intention to purchase the product. One possible explanation could be that attitudes about the product and intention to purchase the product could be influenced by many factors other than the reviews which participants had just read. Naturally, positive reviews improved attitudes about the product and increased intention to purchase the product, while negative reviews decreased them. But attitudes about the product and purchase intention could have depended more on economic status, current necessity of a new product, and opportunities to compare various Web sites, reviews, and other similar products, rather than on how many others rated the product versus how others described their experience with the product. The current study measured individuals' evaluations of how vividly each review type was presented, predicting that vividness would explain differences between narrative and statistical reviews for their effects on credibility, attitudes about the product, and intention to purchase the product. Although narrative reviews were perceived as more vivid than statistical reviews, it was statistical reviews that were perceived as more credible regardless of vividness. The current findings indicated that vividness did not explain the persuasive effects of narrative reviews, and that vividness did not have direct effects on review credibility, attitudes about a product, and intention to purchase the product. 40 The role of vividness varied across different review types. Vividness of statistical negative review was inversely related to review credibility, but vividness of statistical positive review was positively related to review credibility. Vividness of narrative negative review was positively related to review credibility, but vividness of narrative positive review was negatively related to review credibility. One possible explanation could be that individuals might have interpreted vividness of statistical negative review to be unreal (e. g., how can this many people react negatively about a product?, how can a product with this many negative review be still advertised for sale?) On the other hand, individuals might have interpreted vividness of narrative negative review as a serious expression of consumers' concerns about the product, whereas vividness of narrative positive review might have led individuals to question the real intention of the review comments. Sen and Lerman (2007) demonstrated that people were more likely to attribute product related motivations to negative review of a utilitarian product than that of a hedonic product. But their study did not differentiate numeric and narrative reviews for motive attribution. The current study findings may imply that people may make different attributions about negative and positive reviews when the reviews are in the forms of numeric ratings versus narrative comments. Another implication of the current findings regarding vividness is that individuals varied in their perceptions of the vividness of the same review. Such individual variations in vividness were differentially related to review credibility, attitudes about the product, and purchase intention across different types and valences of reviews. For example, the current study found that vividness of statistical review was positively related to purchasing intentions, but vividness of narrative review was negatively related to 41 purchasing intentions. Vividness of statistical negative review was inversely related to review credibility, but vividness of narrative negative review was inversely related to attitudes about the product and also inversely related to purchasing intentions. Because the current study did not examine what caused such individual variations of vividness of online reviews and subsequent variations in review credibility, attitudes about the product, and purchase intention, it is not easy to explain the findings concerning vividness. One speculation, however, may point to the trend that improvement in Web technologies enables online reviews to be presented with colorful graphics and/or in dynamic forms. How graphically and vividly Web sites present online reviews can vary as well. Individuals' experiences with various Web sites might have led to comparisons of the current study manipulation materials against the review formats of Web sites individuals were already familiar with. Thus, the current research materials might have seemed less vivid than usual for some participants and more vivid for others. Limitations of Current Research and Suggestions for Future Research A few limitations of the current study may provide suggestions for future research. First, only one product (i.e., a digital camera) was used in the current study. As previous research showed, product type can moderate the effects of eWOM (e.g., Sen & Lerman, 2007). Furthermore, the current study focused only on product review and not on retailer or seller review concerning shipping and handling quality, return policy, and customer service quality. Future research can examine whether the current findings can be generalized to online reviews of other types of product such as CDs and DVDs of music and movies and reviews of retailers or sellers. 42 The second limitation may pertain to the number of reviews shown in the statistical review versus the narrative review. Although the current study tried to prepare the equal base rate (i.e., the total number of reviews) in the statistical and narrative review conditions, participants in the current study might have not noticed it. For the statistical review, participants could easily notice that the rating summary was based on 1000 feedback ratings. For the narrative review, although a phrase, "1000 feedback received," was included at the top of 15 narrative review comments, the phrase might have been in too small of a font size for participants to notice. Thus, at this point, it is unclear whether participants in the narrative review condition counted or discounted the base rate when evaluating the review. Additionally, it can be asked if the number (15) of narrative review comments might have affected the current findings. Future research may examine if the varying numbers (e.g., 15 versus 30) of narrative review comments shown to participants make a difference and if there is a threshold of narrative review comments that consumers prefer to read before making a purchase decision. Third, the current study did not provide opportunities for people to choose which reviews they want to check. When the amount of reviews and feedbacks available is too large to check all, online consumers may chose to read only several review comments and their choices can be guided by various factors. Consumers may choose to read longer reviews because longer reviews are often perceived as more helpful (Chevalier & Mayzlin, 2006; Sen, 2008). Some consumers may prefer the reviews by someone with good reputation. For example, some Web sites provide a way for people to rate the review itself and some consumers may choose to read a review comment that many people already find usefirl. 43 CONCLUSION Advances in technologies and increases in online communication result in growing amounts and diversifying qualities of information available to consumers. Understanding how marketers and consumers adapt to any new modes of online communication and how they process various forms of online information can remain constant challenges as the speed of technology improvement increases. The current study findings can have some implications for scientific research, commercial practice, and uses of online reviews as a form of eWOM. Careful management of negative reviews can be a significant way to influence attitudes and intention to purchase about a given product. Effective control and presentation of numeric ratings as statistical reviews can be important ways to increase review credibility and attract consumers to check the reviews on a given Web site. Table 1 Demographic Information (Study 1) Category Number of Subject Percentage Gender (Age M = Male 57 31.5 22.35, SD = 3.09) Female 124 68.5 Grade Sophomore 4 2.2 Junior 69 38.5 Senior 51 28.5 Graduate 55 30.7 Ethnicity Caucasian/European 1 23 69.5 American Hispanic 1 0.6 Afiican American .8 4.5 Pacific Islander 4 2.3 Asian American 15 8.5 Mixed 1 0.6 Other 25 14.1 * n = 179 45 Table 2 Online shopping Tendency (Study 1) M Median SD Average spending 59.83 30 112.11 (per month) (U. S. dollar) (U. S. dollar) Average purchase 2.27 1 5.86 (per month) (times) (times) To what extent you believe online 5.45 6 0.97 review is useful (1 = lowest and 7 = highest) To what extend you believe it is useful 4.96 5 1.36 to rely on online reviews for purchasing decisions (1 = lowest and 7 = highest), 46 Table 3 Demographic Information (Study 2) Category Number of Subject Percentage Gender (Age M = Male 55 32.5 21.48, SD = 2.07) Female 114 67.5 Grade Freshman 2 1.2 Sophomore 37 21.8 Junior 50 29.4 Senior 73 42.9 Graduate 8 4.7 Ethnicity Caucasian/European 1 30 76.9 American Hispanic 2 1.2 Afiican American 6 3.6 Asian American 14 8.3 Mixed 4 2.4 Other 13 7.7 * n = 170 47 Table 4 Online shopping Tendency (Study 2) M Median SD Average spending 45.77 25 53.42 (per month) (U. S. dollar) (U. S. dollar) Average purchase 1.43 0.83 2.28 (per month) (times) (times) To what extent you believe online 5.49 6 0.93 review is useful (1 = lowest and 7 = highest) To what extend you believe it is useful 5.01 5 1.22 to rely on online reviews for purchasing decisions (1 = lowest and 7 = highest) 48 Table 5 Comparisons of Experimental Conditions in Study 1 and Study 2 R . Attitudes Intention to Cid?” about a Purchase a Credrbrlrty” ProductM Product” Statistical Negative Review coupled with Narrative Negative 39 5.16q (1.24) 2.15x (0.98) 1.933 (0.86) Review Statistical Negative Review only 36 5.1 1q (1.20) 2.16K (1.09) 1.96a (0.96) Narrative Negative Review only 34 4.43r (0.72) 2.53K (0.90) 217,002) Statistical Positive Review coupled with Narrative Negative Review 45 4.331r (0.78) 3.69y (1.34) 3.031, (1.40) . . 3.20bc Control group — no revrew included 36 -- 4.16y (0.76) (1 07) Statistical Negative Review 3 47 coupled with Narrative Positive 42 4.24r (0.73) 419,025) (1' 3;; Review ' Statistical Positive Review coupled 3.89“) with Narrative Positive Review 44 4'37’ (0'75) 49% (0'79) (1.09) Statistical Positive Review only 39 4.48r (0.65) 5.00Z (0.82) 4.49,, (1.1 1) Narrative Positive Review only 36 4.17,(0.80) 5.062 (0.64) 4.30,, (0.91) Note. Standard deviations are in parentheses. _ Means not sharing a same subscript within each column differ from one another at p < .05. ”ANOVA with credibility as the dependent variable, F (7, 307) = 7.24, p < .001, n2 = .14. ”ANOVA with attitudes as the dependent variable, F (8, 342) = 57.08, p < .001, n2 r .57. ”ANOVA with intention as the dependent variable, F (8, 342) = 28.96, p < .001, n2 49 II' .40. APPENDIX A Background Information Plege answer the following questions about yourselfi B1. Have you ever seen online reviews? (for example, feedback from Amazon or eBay) El Yes [:1 No B2. Have you ever bought any product online? Cl Yes D No B2a. If yes, how often do you shop online? times per D week El month Cl year B2b. If yes, how much do you spend while shopping online? $ per El week El month El year Please indicate the extent to which you agree or disagree with each sentence below. B3. I use online reviews to decide what I purchase with online shopping. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 B4. I use online reviews to decide what I purchase with offline shoppinL Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 B5. I prefer the website with online reviews rather than the website without online reviews. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 B6. I often check online reviews to get information about products. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Drsagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 B7. I have to check online reviews before any purchase is made. _ Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 B8. I usually check online reviews before finalizin my purchase. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 50 B9. I believe it is useful to rely on online reviews for purchasin r, decisions. Strong] . Somewhat Some hat Strongly Disagre: Disagree Disagree Neutral Agr‘Ze Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 BIO. I believe the information from online reviews are usefirl. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Please answer the following Questions about yourself D1. Age D2. Gender E] Male [:1 Female D3. Major D4. Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior Graduate D5. Please indicate your ethnicity (check one) Caucasian/European American Hispanic Afiican American Pacific Islander Native American Mixed (please specify Asian American Other (please specify Please go on to the next page 51 35 mm. oém .. i of... . .0 Erefiggaggimssm “Rama F o o 32 9 s; acreage; :5. ,, sex SEE—«Susana: .29on .3: Em 52% 38 com com 8 _. o m: So 33 ”as 33 .810 22E Em “£86 2 2:36 o 535 L 8.33 23.. MM. ESE 2 new .— 3350“ 35233333 ”$53.23? B 9 Egg 325.3 83 v5.29: :3. :5 . Ema.— .A “23%“; m .9 Ease Eaa 2: ass a 2 2 ea . 5 mm. s e 32% 3 m 2 3 £03 5 . 53 .3 2:. x M 2 $8 ham fiwnmomm .gEmom 3533.5 assuage senses ............ 33.2; 383 m 3.33833 33; m E . H. . .m 3.2m was: .3535 m a 5% e . . . , c “m. @533 5:: =2... .Efihm 53m ECU «QWEE o . . :1: P 52 Narrative Statistic Evidence .8 g. .2 ,. ea; . o o .2329 g s 2 as... 3.2... N. 2.5... o 58.... . ..,,~. .mfieoEm 1.2... H gogom . .39.... 3.5... 82 as 22. .. see a... a... so... avg—coo. 2.. m. 23 :23 2: go. .23. “8.33 3.8.. 3.533533. 3...... - e... w is. as“. 55.52 2 u. o> . E a 3.335. E... ”sausage... E802... Eggs 3...... 85.3. as .228 355.5 5...... “3.3. 32%? 3.... 3.. .5 3 x .8 2 x .5 z. ”.593. 33...... as... 3E“ 5.... a. 3 NE 3.2.. 5.3 a $2.5 wen—hug” “ESE—E so: 2&3: F. m at, 22.8 x23 3a3§fiaefis 331...... 5.2.3 .v 1, $55 Wages .3333 a 3...... Eu... 3 .5 Luv. E _. Em Eou§§6 53 Positive Narrative Evidence REViEW 2 The below is the part of feedback about the digital camera x from the previous buyers. 1” mm (viewing 1-25) Mm 0 Alazing quality. 0 This is not that heavy so that I can carry easily. 0 This is the one I an looking for. 0 Beautiful design! 0 It’s acceptable. 6Wenlusedthisatthebirthdayparty ‘ " everyone loved their face in the calera. 9 Nice one. I recouend this to my pals. ...~ --.......... -..-.. M- W..... 0 A11 functions are fantastic! . ....__..._..-..... .................. mmw- 0 This was ly anniversary gift and Iy husband loved it. It works well. ONOTBAD 0 I an pleased with the quality of the picture... 0 Useful product. I an happy with oy purchase. 0 This canera changed ny weekend in good way —-- -wflw. _. ._.. .4... .. -.. - fi- . 0 I will race-Tend this to everyone. 0 Nice appearance, this is what I wanted 54 From Buyer Buyer. whitefiioufi) Buyer. pmhalekw(203 *) Buyet: mor_chick87 (39 {k ) Buyer tennpwzons {in Buyer. siar¢20_3 ( 24 fl!) .........‘.......1.;"' "a...".....,4... ' ' "...J,...... 9.5 ,' “.....‘............5 Buyer: momma *) Buyer ruppel‘1969(104 fir ) Buyer. pdfner309 ( 3) Buyer; oolfer110751( 104 fi ) Buyer. 25101151 * ,1 Buyer. luypmmytibg * ) Negative Narrative Evidence Review 2 The below is the part of feedback about the digital calera x fron the previous buyers. 1mhdndmamd (vicvang 1-25) Feedioct 0 Disappointing quality. {thie'i‘ssd heavy d... c... hottahyeasiiy. *5 hi; i... the mama... e... G hwful‘ design! ‘ at}; acceptable. "5h... I used thisat the birthday party. ’ ’ no one liked their face in the cawera. ° Bad one. I can’t recomend this to ly pals. 0 A11 functions are horrible! O This was my anniversary gift, but my husband hated it.It works badly. . -.-.-.... 7.....1 ..._. th..- quorum 0 I an not pleased with the quality of the picture- 0 Not useful product. I regret wy purchase. 9 This cawera ruined my weekend 0 I won’t recomend this to anyone. 9 Disappointing appearance, this is not what I wanted. 55 From Buyer Buyer caniesm (6) Buyer jimhaieitw( 2113 fir ) 4 1 Buyer. hogmmme :3; ) Buyer kennpttlz‘oi 36 fi) a... smzo_3(22.i 3%) Buyer aceohanshteuoms m Buyer gsharkytS) Buyer. 7442.38171120 {k > Buyer. susanandrandygs ( 50 *) .u.....hst..g,‘ ' B‘i‘lei NW911969(1104*)‘ 3W9" parlance ( 3 J Buyer. codefl10751 . 104 it } Buyer. 25m 151 *, Buyer lilgimmfl 169 fir ) APPENDIX C Vividness / Credibility / Attitudes about a Product / Purchase Intention Afler reading Review 1. Plea_se indicate the extent to which you agree or disagree with each statement below. MSl. Overall reviews are positive. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 M82. Almost all of reviews are recommending this product. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 M83. People who contribute reviews are mostly satisfied with this product. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 M84. Overall reviews are negative. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 M85. Almost all of reviews are not recommending this product. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 M86. People who contribute reviews are mostly unsatisfied with this product Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 M87. It seems that people generally recommend this product. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 M88. It seems that people do not generally recommend this product. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 56 Please indicate your feeling about Review 1 with each stgtement below. V68 1. In general, to what extent were these messages vividly presented? I Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 6—7 Very much ] VPSl. To you, personally, to what extent were these messages vividly presented? [ Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 5. 7 Very much j VG82. In general, to what extent were these messages emotionally presented? [ Not at all 1 2 3 4 5——-6————7 Very much 1 VPSZ. To you, personally, to what extent were these messages emotionally presented? [ Not at all 1 2 3 4 5——6—-—7 Very much VGS3. In general, to what extent were these messages concretely presented? [ Not at all 1 2 3 4 5—6—7 Very much ] VPS3. To you, personally, to what extent were these messages concretely presented? [ Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very much I VGS4. In general, to what extent were these messages imaginably presented? L Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 6———7 Very much I VPS4. To you, personally, to what extent were these messages imaginably presented? F Not at all 1 2 3——-4—--—5—--—6——7 Very much P81. How persuasive do you think these messages were? I Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 5 7 Very much | P82. To what extent do you think your opinions on this product were influenced by these messages? I Not at all 1 2 3 4 s 5 7 Very much ] 57 After reading Review 2, Please indicate the extent to which you agree or disagree with each statement below. MNl . Overall reviews are positive. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Dlsagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 MNZ. Almost all of reviews are recommending this product. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Dlsagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 MN3. People who contribute reviews are mostly satisfied with this product. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 MN4. Overall reviews are negative. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Dlsagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 MNS. Almost all of reviews are not recommending this product. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Dlsagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 MN6. People who contribute reviews are mostly unsatisfied with this product Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disa Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 MN7. It seems that people generally recommend this product. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 MN8. It seems that people do not generally recommend this product. Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Dlsagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 58 Please indicate your feeling gbout Review g with each statement below VGNl. In general, to what extent were these messages vividly presented? I Not at all 1 2 3 4 5———-6——7 Very much I VPNl. To you, personally, to what extent were these messages vividly presented? I Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 6—-—-—7 Very much I VGN2. In general, to what extent were these messages emotionally presented? I Not at all 1 2 3 4 5———-6—-——-7 Very much I VPN2. To you, personally, to what extent were these messages emotionally presented? I Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 6—7 Very much I VGN3. In general, to what extent were these messages concretely presented? I Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very much I VPN3. To you, personally, to what extent were these messages concretely presented? F Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 6—7 Very much I VGN4. In general, to what extent were these messages imaginably presented? I Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 6———-—7 Very much I VPN4. To you, personally, to what extent were these messages imaginably presented? I Not at all 1 2 3 4 5—-—6———7 Very much PNl. How persuasive do you think these messages were? I Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 5——-—7 Very much I PN2. To what extent do you think your opinions on this product were influenced by these messages? I Not at all 1 2 3 4 5 -6———-—-—7 Very much I 59 After reading both Review 1 and Review 2, what do you think about these reviews? Cl. To me, overall reviews are, Extremely Very Somewhat Neutral Somewhat Very Extremely Biased Biased Biased Unbiased Unbiased Unbiased l 2 3 4 5 6 7 C2. To me, overall reviews are, Extremely Very Somewhat Neutral Somewhat Very Extremely Trustworthy Trustworthy Trustworthy Untrustworthy Untrustworthy Untrustworthy l 2 3 4 5 6 7 C3. To me, overall reviews are, Extremely Very Somewhat Neutral Somewhat Very Extremely Inaccurate Inaccurate Inaccurate Accurate Accurate Accurate l 2 3 4 5 6 7 C4. To me, overall reviews are, Extremely Very Somewhat Neutral Somewhat Very Extremely Unbelievable Unbelievable Unbelievable Believable Believable Believable l 2 3 4 5 6 7 C5. To me, overall reviews are, Extremely Very Somewhat Neutral Somewhat Very Extremely Incomplete Incomplete Incomplete Complete Complete Complete 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 6O What do you think about this product? Please imag'ne you need to buy a new digital camera, then indicate your preference on the 71point scale. A1. Please indicate your feelings about this product, if it is on the market. Extremely Very Somewhat Neutral Somewhat Very Extremely Bad Bad Bad Good Good Good 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 A2. Please indicate your feelings about this product, if it is on the market. Extremely Very Somewhat Neutral Somewhat Very Extremely Unfavorable Unfavorable Unfavorable Favorable Favorable Favorable l 2 3 4 5 6 . 7 A3. Please indicate your feelings about this product, if it is on the market. Extremely Very Somewhat Neutral Somewhat Very Extremely Undesirable Undesirable Undesirable Desirable Desirable Desirable l 2 3 4 5 6 7 61 Please imagine thatjou may be looking to buy a digital camera in the near future and then indicate the extent to which you agree or disagree with each sentence below. Pleaie circle a number that best reflects your agreement level. BIl. If I am going to buy a new digital camera in the future, I intend to buy this product Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Dlsagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 BIZ. If I am going to buy a new digital camera in the future, I have no plan to buy this product Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Dlsagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 B13. If I am going to buy a new digital camera in the future, I have it in mind to buy this product Strongly . Somewhat Somewhat Strongly Disagree Dlsagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 B14. If I am going to buy a new digital camera in the future, I mean to buy this product Strongly Disagree Somewhat Neutral Somewhat Agree Strongly Dlsagree Dlsagree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 BIS. If I am going to buy a new digital camera in the filture, I would not buy this product Stron l . Somewhat Somewhat Stron I Disagfie: Dlsagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Agrcfey 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Thank you for participating in this study 62 REFERENCES Abernathy, E. (1964) .The advocate: A manual of persuasion. New York: Mckay. Allen, M., & Preiss, R. W. (1993). Replication and meta-analysis: A necessary connection. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 8, 9-20. Allen, M., & Preiss, R. W. (1997). Comparing the persuasiveness of narrative and statistical evidence using meta-analysis, Communication Research Report, 14, 125-1 3 1 . Allsop, D. T. Bassett, B. R., & Hoskins, J. A. (2007). Word-of mouth research: Principles and applications, Journal of Advertising Research, 47, 398-411. Amdt, J. (1967a). Role of product-related conversations in the diffusion of a new product, Journal of Marketing Research, 4, 291 -295. Amdt, J. (1967b). Word-of mouth advertising: A review of the literature. New York: Advertising Research Foundation. Ameson, P. A., & Query, J. L. (2001) .The case study: Revitalizing a nonprofit health organization. In 8. L. Kreps (Eds), Qualitative research: Applications in organizational life (2nd ed.) (pp. 151-169) .Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. Baesler, E. J ., & Burgoon, J. (1994). The temporal effects of story and statistical evidence on belief change. Communication Research, 21, 582-602. Bansal, H. 8., & Voyer, P. A. (2000). Word of mouth processes within a service purchase decision context. Journal of Service Research, 3, 166-177. Bass, F. M. (2004). A new product growth model for consumer durables. Management of Science. 50, 1825-1832 Beal, G. M., & Rogers, E. M. (1957) Informational success in the adoption process of new fabrics. Journal of Home Economics, XLIX, 630-634. Bickart, B., & Schindler, R. M.(2001). Internet forunls as influential sources of consumer information, Journal of Interactive Marketing, 15, 3, 31-40. Biyalogorsky, E., Gersyner, E., & Libai, B. (2001). Customer referral management: Optimal reward programs. Marketing Science, 20, 82-95. Borgida, E., & Nisbett, R. E (1977). The differential impact of abstract vs. concrete information on decisions. Journal of applied Social Psychology, 7, 258-271. 63 Boster, R, Cameron, K. A., Campo, 8., Liu, W.-Y., Lillie, J., Baker, R., & Yun, K. A. The persuasive effects of statistical evidence in the presence of examplars. Communication Studies, 51, 296-306. Brooks, R. C. Jr. (1957) .Word-of mouth advertising in selling new product. Journal of Marketing, 22, 154-161. Brown, J. J ., & Reingen, P. H. (1987). Social ties and word-of-mouth referral behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 14, 350-362. Bureau of Applied Social Research, Colombia University, On the Flow of Scientific Information. In the Medical Profession (2 Vols.), unpublished research reports, 1955. (James Coleman, Elihu Katz, and Herbert Menzel published in Book form) Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Kao, C. F., & Rodriguez, R. R. (1986). Central and peripheral routes to persuasion: An individual difference perspective. Journal of personality and Social Psychology, 51(5), 1032-1043. Chevalier, J. A., & Mayzlin, D. (2006). The effect of word of mouth online: Online book i reviews, Journal of Marketing Research, 43, 345-3 54. Charlett, D., & Garland, R. (1995), How damaging is negative word of mouth? Marketing Bulletin, 6, 42-51. Chatterjee, P. (2001). Online reviews-do consumers use them? Advances in Consumer Research, 28, 129-133. Chen, Y., & Xie, J. (2008). Word-of-mouth as a new element of marketing communication mix, Management Science, 54, 477- 491 . Cohen, M. R., & Nagel, E. (1962). An introduction to logic. New York: Harcourt, Brace &World. Czepiel, J. A. (1974). Word-of-mouth process in the diffusion of a major technological innovation. Journal of Marketing research, 1 I , 172-180. Davis, D., Guiltman, J. P., 82 Jones, W. H. (1970). Service characteristics, consumer search and the classification of retailing services. Journal of Retailing, 55(3), 3-23. Dewan, 8., & Hsu, V. (2004). Adverse selection in electronic market: Evidence from online stamp auctions. Journal of Industrial Economics, 52, 497-516. Dellacoras, C. (2003). The digitization of word-of-mouth: Promise and challenges of online feedback mechanisms. Management Science, 49, 1401 -1424. 64 Dickson, P. R. (1982). The impact of enriching case and statistical information on consumer judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 8, 398-406. Ditcher, E. (1966). How word-of-mouth advertising works. Harvard Business Review, 44, 147-166. Dwyer, P. (2007). Measuring the value of electronic word of mouth and its impact in consumer communities. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 21 (2), 63-79. Ellinson, L. L., & Buzzanell, P. M. (1999). Listening to women’s narratives of breast cancer treatment: A feminist approach to patient satisfaction with physician- patient communication. Health Communication, 1 I, 153-183. Evans, M., Wedande, G., Ralston, L., & Van t Hul, S. (2001). Consumer interaction in the virtual era: some qualitative insights, Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal, 4, 150-159. Flanagin, A. J ., & Metzger, M. J. (2000). Perceptions of Internet information credibility. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 77, 515-540. Feldman, S. P. & Spencer, M. C. (1965). The Effect of Personal Influence in the Selection of Consumer Services, in Bennett, P. (Eds), Marketing and Economic Development, American Marketing Association, Chicago, IL, pp.440-52. Fotheringham, W. C. (1966). Perspective on persuasion. Boston: Alyn and Bacon. Gilly, M. C., Graham, J. L., Wolfinbarger, M. F ., & Yale, L. J. (1998). A dyadic study of interpersonal information search. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 26, 83-100. Goldenberg, J ., Libai, B.,& Muller, E. (2001). Talk of the Network: A Complex Systems Look at the Underlying Process of Word-of-Mouth, Marketing Letters, 12, 3, 21 1-223. Greene, K., & Brinn, L. S. (2003). Message influencing college women’s tanning bed use: Statistical versus narrative evidence format and a self-assessment to increase perceived susceptibility. Journal of Health Communication, 8, 443-461. Hamill, R., Wilson, T. D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1980). Insensitivity to sample bias: Generalizing fiom atypical cases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 578-589. Hennig-Thurau, T., Gwinner, K. P., Walsh, G., & Gremle, D. D. (2004) Electronic word- of-mouth Via consumer-opinion platforms: What motivates consumers to articulate themselves on the Internet?, Journal of Interactive Marketing, 18, 1, 38- 52. 65 Herr P. M., Kardes, F. R., & Kim, J. (1991). Effects of word-of-mouth and product attribute information on persuasion: An accessibility-diagnosticity perspective. Journal of Consumer Research, I 7, 454-462. Heywood, K. M. (1989) Managing word of mouth communications, The Journal of Service Research, 6, 287-303. Hilton, D. J. (1995). The social context of reasoning: Conversational inference and rational judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 248-271. Hoeken, H. (2001). Anecdotal, Statistical, and Causal Evidence: Their Perceived and Actual Persuasiveness, Argumentation, 15, 4, 425-437. Holmes, J. H., & Lett, J. D. (1977). Product sampling and word of mouth. Journal of Advertising, 1 7, 35-40. Hu, N., Liu, L., & Zhang, J. J. (2008). Do Online review affect product sales? The role of reviewer characteristics and temporal effects. Information Technology Management, 9, 201-214. E Iyengar, 8., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). News that matters. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kalyanam, K., & McIntyre, 8. (2001) Return on reputation in online auction markets. Santa Clara University, Santa Clara. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80, 237-251. Katz E. & Lazarfeld, P. F. (1995). Personal influence. Glencoe, 11: Free Press Kazoleas, D. C. (1993). A comparison of the persuasive effectiveness of qualitative versus quantitative evidence. A test of explanatory hypotheses. Communication Quarterly, 41, 40-50. Kiel, G. C., & Layton, R. A. (1981). Dimensions of consumer information seeking behavior. Journal of Marketing Research, I 8, 233-239. Koballa, T. R. (1986). Persuading teachers to reexamine the innovative elementary science programs of yesterday: The effect of anecdotal versus data-summary communications, Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 23, 5, 43 7-449. Kopfrnan, J i E., Smith, 8. W., Yun, J. K., & Hodges, A. (1998). Affective and cognitive reaction to narrative versus statistical evidence organ donation messages. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 26, 279-300. 66 Lazarsfeld, P. F ., Berelson, B. R., & Gaudler, W.(1948). The people ’s choice. New York: Columbia University Press. Laczniak, R. N., DeCarlo, T. E., & Ramaswamy, S. N. (2001). Consumers’ responses to negative WOM communication: An attribution theory perspective. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 1 I, 57-73. Liu, Y. (2006). Word of mouth for movies: Its dynamics and impact on box office revenue. Journal of Marketing, 70, 74-89. Martilla, J. A. (1971). Word-of-mouth communication in the industlial adoption process. Journal of Marketing Research, 8, 173-178. McCrosky, J. C. (1969). A summary of experimental research on the effects of evidence in persuasive communication. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 55, 169-176. McCrosky, J. C. (1967). The effects of evidence in persuasive communication. Western Speech, 31, 189-199. Mcdonald C. G., & Slawson, V. C. Jr. (2002). Reputation in an intemet auction market. Economic Inquiry, 40, 533-650. Melnik, M. 1., & Ahn, J. (2002). Does a seller’s reputation matter? Evidence fiom eBay auctions. Journal of Industrial Economics, 50, 337-350. Miller, G. (Victor D. Wall Jr.) (1972). Evidential attitudes and attitudes change. Western ' Speech, 36, 115-123. Nadler, M. A. K. (1982). Evidence usage in persuasion. Dissertation Abstracts International, 44, 910A. Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Nisbett, R. E., & Borgida, E. (1975). Attribution and the psychology of prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 932-943. O’Keefe, D. J. (2002). Persuasion: Theory and research (2nd ed.). Newbury, CA: Sage. O’Keefe, D. J .(2004). Trends and prospects in persuasion theory and research. In J. S. Seiter & R. H. Glass (Eds), Perspective on persuasion, social influence, and compliance gaining ( 31-43). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Park, C., & Lee, T. M. (2009). Information direction, website reputation and eWOM effect: A moderating role of product type. Journal of Business Research, 62, 61- 67. 67 Park, D.-H., Lee, J ., & Han, I. (2007). The effect of on-line consumer reviews on consumer purchasing intention: The moderating role of involvement. International Journal of Electronic Commerce, 11, 125-148. Park, D.-H., & Lee, J. (2008). eWOM overload and its effect on consumer behavioral intention depending on consumer involvement, Electronic Commerce Research and Applications, 7, 386-398. Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Goldman, R. (1981). Personal involvement as a determinant of argument based persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41 , 847-855. Piller, C. (1999) Everyone is a critic in cyberspace. Los Angeles Times, December 3, A1. Phelps, J. E., Lewis, R., Mobilio, L., Perry, D., & Rarnan, N. (2004). Viral marketing or electronic word of mouth advertising: Examining consumer responses and motivations to pass long email. Journal of Advertising research, 45, 333-348. Pitta, D. A., & Fowler, D. (2005) Internet Community forums: an untapped resource of consumer marketers. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 22, 265-274. Pollach, I. (2006). Electronic word of mouth: A genre analysis of product reviews on consumer opinion web Sites. Proceedings of the 36th Hawaii International Conference on System Science. Reynolds R. A. & Reynolds J. L. (2002). Evidence. In J. P. Dillard & M. Pfau (Eds) The persuasion handbook (pp. 427-444). Thousand Oaks CA: Sage. Reichheldm, F. F. (1996). The loyalty eflect. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Business School Press. Reinard, J. C. (1988). The empirical study of the persuasive effects of evidence: The status afier fifty years of research. Human Communication Research, 15, 3-59. Resnick, P., & Zeckhauser, R. (2002). Trust among strangers in intemet transactions: Empirical analysis of eBay’s reputation system. In M. R. Baye (ed.), The economics of the Internet and E- COMMERCE, Amsterdam, Elsevier Science, 11, 127-157. Resnick, P., Zeckhauser, R., Swanson, J ., & Lockwood, K. (2006). The value of reputation on eBay: A controlled experiment, Experimental Economics, 9, 79-101. Reyna, V. F., Woodruff, W. J ., & Brainerd, C. J. (1987). Attitude change in adults and adolescents: Moderation versus polarization, statistics versus case histories. Unpublished manuscript, University of Texas, Dallas. 68 Richins, M. L. (1983). Negative word of mouth by dissatisfied customers: a pilot study. Journal of Marketing, 4 7, 68-78. Richins, M. L., & Root-Shaffer, T. (1988) The role of involvement and opinion leadership in consumer word of mouth; an implicit model made explicit. Advertising Consumer Research, 15, 32-36. Rogers, E. M. (1995). Diflusions of Innovations (4‘h ed.). New York: The Free Press. Ryan, B., & Gross, N. (1945). The diffusion of hybrid seed corn in two Iowa communities. Rural Sociology, 8, 15-24. Sen, 8. (2008) Determinants of consumer trust of virtual word-of-mouth: An observation study from a retail Website, Journal of American Academy of Business, Cambridge, 14, 1. Silverman, G. (1997). How to harness the awesome power of word of mouth. Direct Marketing, 60(7), 32-37. Schwartz E. I. (1998). OK. Retailers, Why do your own marketing when you make 100,000 other web sites do it for you? New York Times, Aug 10, 3. Seiter. J. 8., & Seiter, D. R. (2005). Consumer Persuasion: The Use of Evidence When Negotiating the Price of a New Automobile, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 4, 686-696. Sen, 8. (2008). Determinants of Consumer Trust of Virtual Word-of-Mouth: An Observation Study from a Retail Website, Journal of American Academy of Business, Cambridge, 14, 1, 30-35. Sen, 8., & Lerman, D. (2007). Why are you telling me this? An examination into negative consumer reviews on the Web. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 21, 76- 94. Sheth, J. N. (1971). Word of Mouth in Low-Risk Innovations. Journal of Advertising Research, 113, 15-18. Stiff, J. B. (1986). Cognitive processing of persuasive message cue: A meta-analytic review of the effects of supporting information on attitudes, Communication Monographs, 53, 75-89. Slater, M. D., & Rouner, D. (1996). Value-affirmative and value-protective processing of alcohol education messages that include statistical evidence or anecdotes. Communication Research, 23 , 210-235. 69 Sun, T., Youn, 8., Wu, G., & Kuntaraporn, M. (2006). Online word of mouth : An exploration of its antecedents and consequences, Journal of Computer-mediated Communication, 11, 1 104-1 127. Swan, J. E., & Oliver R. L. (1989). Postpurchase communications by consumers. Journal of Retailing; 65, 516-533. Tan, C. T., & Dolich, I. J. (1983) .A comparative study of consumer information seeking: Singapore Versus U.S. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 11, 313-322. Taylor, 8. E., & Thompson, S. C. (1982). Stalking the elusive “vividness” effect. Psychological Review, 89, 155-181. F Udell, J. G. (1966). Prepurchase behavior of buyers of small electrical Appliances. Journal of Marketing, 30, 50-52. Vanderford, M. L., Smith, D. H., & Harris W. 8. (1992) Value identification in narrative ' discourse: Evaluation of a HIV education demonstration project. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 20, 123-160. Wagenheim, F., & Bayon, T. (2004). Satisfaction, loyalty and word of mouth within the customer base of a utility provider: differences between stayers, switchers and referral switchers, Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 3, 3, 211-220. Walker, C. (1995). Word-of-mouth. American Demographics, 1 7, 38-44. Wells, G. L, & Harvey, J. H. (1977). Do people use consensus information in making causal attributions? Journal of personality and social Psychology, 35 , 279-293. Wernerfelt, B. (1994). On the function of sales assistance, Marketing Science, 13, 68-82. Williams, F., Strover, 8., & Grant, A. E. (1994). Social aspects of new media technologies. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds), Media eflects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 463-482). Hillsdale: NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Wilson, E. J. & Sherrell, D. L. (1993) Source effects in communication and persuasion research: A meta-analysis of effect size, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 21, 2,101-112. Whyte, W. H., Jr. (1954). The Web of Word of Mouth, Fortune, 50, 140-143. Ying, H. L., & Chung, C. M. Y. (2006). The effects of single-message single-source mixed word-of-mouth on product attitude and purchase intention. Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics, 19, 75-86. 70 Zillmann, D., Perkins, J. W., & Sundar, S. S. ( 1992 ). Impression— formation effects of news varying in descriptive precision and exemplifications. Medienpsychologie, 4, 168-185. 71