:02: s ”1..., .. put» "an nun-1:." . 1' pulvuuuc "flung“ : 1;... . Ptuntuz 1 In... .3. v m n .2 A m. . v _..-..n..q ur. ”V... up". LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled EMPIRICAL ESSAYS IN FAMILY STRUCTURE AND EARLY CHILD OUTCOMES presented by TERRY-ANN L. CRAIGIE has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Ph.D. degree in Economics ajor Professor’s Signature /6/ 6 5/1, June 12, 2009 Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Employer PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5/08 K'lProlecc&Pres/CIRC/DaleDue,indd EMPIRICAL ESSAYS IN FAMILY STRUCTURE AND EARLY CHILD OUTCOMES By Terry-Ann L. Craigie A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Economics 2009 ABSTRACT Empirical Essays in Family Structure and Early Child Outcomes By Terry-Ann L. Craigie This dissertation empirically explores the issues surrounding family structure in the United States and its consequences for the outcomes of young children. It highlights the instability hypothesis, which holds that the father’s sporadic presence in the household lowers cognitive performance and exacerbates behavioral problems in young children. In addition, the de-institutionalization of marriage and the family in recent years has made the study of committed unmarried couples relevant to the discussion of family structure and child wellbeing. As a result, adverse outcomes for young children and factors linked to family dissolution among married and cohabiting couples are studied in detail. The first chapter investigates how a father’s presence in the household affects child cognitive performance as measured by the revised version of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-R). By meticulously defining all possible forms of paternal presence, while holding mother’s presence in the household constant, the model distinguishes between stability and family structure effects of paternal presence. The empirical findings show that cognitive outcomes are statistically similar for children in stable single-parent and stable two-parent households. However, unstable family structures, characterized by a father’s sporadic presence in the home, are shown to have adverse effects on cognitive performance compared to the stable single-parent family structure. The profound implication of these findings is the importance of family stability relative to family structure in producing positive child cognitive outcomes. The second chapter empirically tests the long-held view that parental incarceration negatively impacts child wellbeing. Stemming from the findings of the first chapter, all absences are not created equal. As such, the study will distinguish between the effect of a father’s incarceration on the cognitive and behavioral development of the pre-school aged child and the effect of his absence in general. The findings suggest that when both incarceration and absence are treated as endogenous in the model, where identifying instruments are used for both in instrumental variables (IV) estimation, the effect of paternal incarceration is not observed to be statistically different from the effect of his subsequent absence. The third chapter investigates the factors that influence the probability of family dissolution and explore whether the hazard of dissolution is characterized by duration dependence. Unlike previous works, this study goes beyond the examination of unions formed through marriage only, in order to observe unions formed through cohabitation as well. Factors such as age, race, education, religion and cohabitation are shown to significantly influence the risk of union dissolution. Religion and religiosity are shown to be especially important to union survival among cohabiting couples relative to married couples. The study finds no evidence of duration dependence among unions once marital status and other indicators of relationship quality are controlled for in the model. Copyright by TERRY-ANN L. CRAIGIE 2009 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to express my love and appreciation for my Rock and Shield, Jesus Christ. Without His strength and guidance, I would not have been able to successfully complete this journey. In addition, I extend my sincerest gratitude to Dr. Jeff B. Biddle who has been an excellent dissertation advisor. His guidance and instruction throughout this process was invaluable. Dr. Stephen Woodbury and Dr. Thomas J eitschko were more than committee members, but excellent counselors as well. Thank you all for transcending your generic graduate school roles to being strong supporters of my academic and professional choices. Dr. Tom Luster — you are gone but not forgotten. I am grateful to Barbara Ames, Todd Elder, Steven Haider and Gary Solon for helpful comments on my dissertation. Thanks to the secretaries, professors and students of the economics department. To my classmates — you pushed me to the pinnacle of my potential (and sanity). To my academic mentors — Charles Becker, Elizabeth Elmore, Reza Ghorashi, Sonia Gonsalves, Kenneth Harrison, Janice Joseph, Melaku Lakew and Linda Nelson — I would not have reached this milestone if you had not recognized that I was a diamond in the rough. To Stephen Davis and Paulette Forbes-Igharo — I have not forgotten your listening ear and tolerance of my whining. Thank you all for your support and encouragement in the mountains and valleys of this journey. To my parents and siblings -— thank you for your patience during my most stressfirl times and your forgiveness when I forgot your birthdays — I love you all. Thanks to my extended family for your prayers and continued support. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................. vii LIST OF FIGURES ................................................................................... ix CHAPTER 1 EFFECTS OF PATERNAL PRESENCE AND FAMILY INSTABILITY ON CHILD COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE ........................................................................................ 1 1.1 Introduction .................................................................................. l 1.2 Literature Review ........................................................................ 3 1.3 Simple Theoretical Framework ....................................................... 6 1.4 Econometric Approach ................................................................. 8 1.5 Data Description ...................................................................... 10 1.6 Results .................................................................................. 20 1.7 Summary ............................................................................... 25 CHAPTER 2 PATERNAL INCARCERATION, PATERNAL ABSENCE AND EARLY CHILD DEVELOPMENT .................................................................................... 28 2.1 Introduction ................................................................................ 28 2.2 Background ............................................................................ 30 2.3 Data and Variable Descriptions ..................................................... 32 2.4 Econometric Issues and Methods ................................................... 37 2.5 Results .................................................................................. 46 2.6 Summary ............................................................................... 49 CHAPTER 3 AN ANALYSIS OF THE CAUSES AND CORRELATES OF FAMILY DISSOLUTION ..................................................................................... 5 1 3.1 Introduction ................................................................................ 51 3.2 Literature Review ..................................................................... 52 3.3 Data ..................................................................................... 57 3.4 Econometric Specification .......................................................... 61 3.5 Results .................................................................................. 63 3.6 Summary ............................................................................... 67 APPENDICES ...................................................................................... 70 Appendix A .................................................................................. 112 Appendix B ............................................................................... 114 Appendix C ............................................................................... 1 15 BIBLIOGRAPHY .................................................................................. 116 vi LIST OF TABLES Table 1 Summary Statistics ....................................................................... 70 Table 2 Comparison of the Standardized PPVT-R Score by Family Structure ............ 73 Table 3 Summary Statistics of Independent Variables by Family Type .................... 74 Table 4 Replication of Columns (1) & (2) of Tables 3 & 4 of Lang and Zagorsky (2001) 6 ......................................................................................................... 7 Table 5 Estimated Effects of Paternal Presence on Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-R) Scores ................................................................................... 78 Table 6 Estimated Effects of Paternal Presence on Boys’ Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-R) Scores .............................................................................. 80 Table 7 Estimated Effects of Paternal Presence on Girls’ Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-R) Scores .............................................................................. 82 Table 8 Robustness Checks ....................................................................... 84 Table 9 Summary Statistics ....................................................................... 86 Table 10 Summary Statistics by Incarceration History ....................................... 88 Table 1 1 Average Non-Traffic Stops by City and Race/Ethnicity ........................... 90 Table 12 OLS Estimates of the Marginal Impact of Paternal Incarceration on Outcomes92 Table 13 Estimates of the Marginal Impact of Paternal Incarceration Ex Post by Father’s Race/Ethnicity ....................................................................................... 95 Table 14 OLS and IV Regressions of the Effect of Incarceration Ex Post on Child Outcomes ............................................................................................ 96 Table 15 Summary Statistics ..................................................................... 98 Table 16 Linear Probability Estimates of the Correlates of Union Dissolution .......... 100 Table 17 Linear Probability Estimates for Low-Educated and Black Mothers ........... 102 Table 18 Linear Probability Estimates for Couples Cohabiting and Married at Baseline ...................................................................................................... 104 vii Table 19 Logit Estimates of Union Dissolution .............................................. 106 viii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1.1 ........................................................................................... 108 Figure 2.1 .......................................................................................... 110 Figure 3.1 .......................................................................................... 111 ix Chapter 1. Effects of Paternal Presence and Family Instability on Child Cognitive Performance 1.1. Introduction Non-traditional and single-parent family structures are a growing phenomenon in the United States. According to the US. Census Bureau Current Population Reports, in 1996, 25.4% of all children under eighteen had only one parent in the household. This figure rose to 27.3% in 2002; during this period, over 80% of single-parent family households were headed by single mothers. The issue therefore remains as to how children are being affected by the growing trend of family structures, in which the father is seldom in residence. This study will examine how paternal presence in the household and stability of the family structure impact the child’s cognitive development. The fundamental identification problem in answering this question is that unobserved characteristics such as parental values, preferences and innate ability are potentially correlated with both paternal presence and child outcomes — a situation which could severely bias the estimated effect of paternal presence (Lang and Zagorsky, (2001) and Painter and Levine, (2000)). The problem can be addressed by including numerous family background and individual covariates to attenuate omitted variable bias and subsequently make causal inferences (Antecol and Bedard, (2007); Lang and Zagorsky (2001); Painter and Levine (2000)). I employ this approach to address the identification problem using data from The Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS), which provides very rich data on family structure as well as a plethora of family background, household and individual correlates. Prior studies have focused on the outcomes of adolescent children and the outcomes of adults who grew up in single-parent households (Antecol and Bedard, (2007); Corak, (2001); Lang and Zagorsky, (2001); Painter and Levine, (2000); Sandefur and Wells, (1997)). However, there is still much to learn about the impact of family structure on outcomes for young children, particularly pre-school aged children. Parental investments during early childhood years may significantly impact the brain development of the child, thus affecting cognitive skills and accordingly, human capital accumulation (Heckman (2000); Ruhm, (2004)). It is therefore imperative to investigate how the family setting affects early cognitive development due to the momentous impact this may potentially have on skills of the future labor force. The outcome variable used to evaluate cognition is the revised version of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-R), as it conveniently serves as a measure of cognitive ability and academic readiness. Unlike Antecol and Bedard (2007) and Lang and Zagorsky (2001), the study finds no statistically significant effects of paternal presence when the indicator is defined as a continuous variable. However, once the model meticulously specifies all family structure types brought about by variations in paternal presence, the stability effect is clearly observed. First, the study finds that child cognitive performance within the stable two-parent family struCture is not statistically different from performance within the stable single-parent family structure. Second, unstable family structures, where paternal presence in the household is sporadic, yield more negative outcomes for the child than the stable single-parent household. In general, children of unstable families score on average about 1/5 of a standard deviation lower than children of stable single-parent families. Two-parent families are not shown to necessarily yield better cognitive outcomes than single-parent families and as such, the family structure effect is not substantiated by this study. The main implication of these findings is that when it comes to the cognitive development of pre-school aged children, the stability of the family structure is more important than the family structure type. The paper is organized as follows. Section 1.2 provides a brief review of past works that examine the effect of paternal presence in the home. Section 1.3 describes a simple theoretical fi'amework from which the model specification was derived. Section 1.4 discusses the econometric issues associated with measuring the effect of paternal presence. Section 1.5 gives the data description and descriptive statistics of the variables used in the model. Section 1.6 discusses the OLS regression results and robustness checks; Section 1.7 concludes with a summary of the findings and policy implications. 1.2. Literature Review Child outcomes are not only shaped by the genetic endowments of parents, but also the allocation of resources within the household. Parents have genetic endowments such as health and intelligence that are considered heritable and thus, are passed on to children directly (Haveman and Wolfe, (1995); Scott-Jones, (1994)). Therefore, a child will inherit intellectual and health endowments from his/her parents regardless of the family structure. However, parental genetic endowments also affect child outcomes by influencing the level and allocation of resources within the household. Family dissolution ultimately influences the resources devoted to child development. A highly intelligent and healthy father living in the household could significantly increase household income and subsequently the investments of both time and goods devoted to the child (Haveman and Wolfe, (1995)). The mother could also increase her time allocation within the household and her interaction with the child as a result (Scott- Jones, (1994)). These arguments suggest that paternal absence could have deleterious effects on the cognitive performance of the child. Furthermore, the timing of paternal absence may also have varying effects (Haveman and Wolfe, (1995); Seltzer, (1994)). Using sibling comparisons, studies have shown that children exposed to paternal absence for a longer period of time experience more pronounced negative effects (Ermisch and Francesconi, (2001); Sandefirr and Wells, (1997); Sutton-Smith et al., (1968)). However, the assumption must be made that siblings respond to paternal absence in the same way and that parents treat all children equally. There is also the selection problem associated with using sibling comparisons - it limits the analysis sample to families with multiple children (Sigle-Rushton and McLanahan, (2002)). Other studies examine and exploit the reasons for paternal absence. Divorce for instance, as a cause of paternal absence, is much more endogenous than paternal loss through death (Corak, (2001); Lang and Zagorsky, (2001)). Divorce or separation may be caused by pre-existing factors and consequently, father absence would be endogenous in the model. Paternal ab'sence through death, on the other hand, is arguably more exogenous since it is not expected to be correlated with pre-existing factors’. Lang and Zagorsky (2001) exploit the exogenous variation provided by paternal death and concluded that this event decreased the probability of a son being married. It is traditionally believed that paternal presence in the household yields positive repercussions for family and child outcomes. However, it has been shown that father presence may not be as important as previously thought (Corak, (2001); Lang and Zagorsky, (2001)). Lang and Zagorsky (2001) found that when family background and individual characteristics were controlled for, there was not much evidence of the positive impact on outcomes that one would expect (with the exception of father’s death lowering the chances of the son being married). In particular, paternal absence had only modest effects on child cognitive ability as measured by the Armed Forces Qualification Test (AF QT). Using a similar methodology however, Antecol and Bedard (2007) buttressed the traditional hypothesis on the importance of father presence, concluding that children were indeed “better off” the longer the biological father lived in the household. They found that an additional 5 years living with a biological father reduced the probability of outcomes such as smoking, drinking, convictions, marijuana use and pre-marital sexual activity. Recently, there have emerged works that examine the stability of the family structure. Cavanagh and Huston (2006) showed that family instability was strongly associated with teacher and observer reports of child behavioral problems. Fomby and Cherlin (2007) bolstered these findings, noting that multiple family transitions produced .1 If father’s death is due to risky lifestyle choices such as dangerous occupations, criminal activities, unhealthy eating or drinking, death is arguably no longer an exogenous event. more negative developmental outcomes than stable two-parent and even stable single- parent family structures. Similarly, Osborne and McLanahan (2007) concluded that partnership instability moderately contributed to behavioral problems in young children up to three years old. Cavanagh and Huston (2006) hinted at the importance of unraveling family structure as a dynamic process rather than observing it in its discrete form. Instead of examining paternal presence as a continuous variable with a unique effect, the purpose of this study is to explore the possibility of multiple effects on the child’s cognitive development by meticulously detailing all family structure types generated from variability in paternal presence over time. 1.3. Simple Theoretical Framework The model is based on the following production function: Yi = F (Tia Pia Hi: Xi) (1) where Y denotes the child’s PPVT-R score as a measure of child output, T is a vector of variables modeling family structures, P is a vector of parental attributes affecting the productivity of time inputs, H denotes measures of household income and X is a vector of individual and family background covariates affecting performancez. The family structures are depicted as a tree diagram, in which the mother’s presence is held constant while the father’s presence is allowed to vary (see Figure 1.1). Binary variables are created to represent each form of paternal presence. Paternal 2 Leibowitz (1977) employs a similar theoretical framework to show the effect of quality of time inputs on child output measured by the PPVT. presence is specified in this way to examine how the stability and presence of a father impact the child’s cognition simultaneously. These issues for children in their early developmental stages of learning (pre-school) have yet to be critically analyzed together, and this model specification will allow me to do exactly this. It should be noted that these measures do not speak to the quality, but rather to the quantity of time the father spends in the home. Nevertheless, we expect that paternal presence (whether through marriage or common-law union) will have a positive impact on child cognitive ability. It is also important to reiterate that if the father is not consistently present in the home, a negative disruptive effect may ensue. The child’s adjustment to untimely paternal shifts into or out of the household could detract from the quality and quantity of interaction time between the parent and the child (Amato and Booth, (1991); Cherlin, (1978); Seltzer, (1994)). In addition, family disruption may cause stress for parents as well as the child, generating parental aggravation and even child behavioral problems (McLanahan, (1985); Sandefur and Wells, (1997); Wu, (1996)). Parental attributes such as schooling and substance abuse, P, affect time inputs and child cognitive ability and should be extensively controlled for in order to reduce omitted variable bias. In addition, higher household income, H, is assumed to have a positive effect on the child’s PPVT-R score because more goods and services that foster educational development can be purchased (Leibowitz, (1977)). Individual and family characteristics, X, include the child’s birth order, sex, race/ethnicity, father figures present in the home and household size. (See Table 1 for the full list of control variables.) 1.4. Econometric Approach The production function (1) given above can be estimated as: Yr :2 5k Tki + Pi I31 “I“ Hi [32 + Xi [33+ Hi + 3i (7—) =1 where Y denotes the child’s PPVT-R score and T is the set of m family structure types engendered by variability in paternal presence; 5k shows the effect of different family structures on cognitive performance. Father’s time in the household as well as parents’ education and income are potentially correlated with time-invariant and unobserved innate ability, parental values and preferences (captured by It). Since the Fragile Families dataset includes the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale — Revised (WAIS-R3) scores for both parents, I argue that these scores can be used as proxy variables for parents’ cognitive ability. In addition, the dataset supplies several proxy variables for parental values and preferences (see Table 1 (section D) for the complete list of proxy variables, Z). If these variables are valid proxies for unobserved characteristics, listed above, the OLS estimator, 5, will be arguably unbiased: 5 is expected to be upwardly biased if unobserved heterogeneity is not effectively addressed. The methodology of dealing with omitted variable bias in this way is formally known as the Proxy- Variable OLS Solution (Wooldridge, (2002) pg. 63-64). I argue that the proxy variables for parental ability, tastes and preferences, Z, are valid in that they are redundant (i.e. they can be ignored as long as u and the 3 The questions are acquired from the Similarities subtest expected to measure verbal concept formation and reasoning abilities (Wechsler, (1981)). independent variables are directly controlled for) and once they are accounted for in the model, yield no correlation between u and the independent variables. Put simply, once Z is incorporated into the model, the endogenous variables and Z should not be correlated with 8. The reduced-form model becomes: Yi =33ka Tki + Pi (11 + Hi 0£2 + Xi 0£3 + Zr (14 + Vi (3) where Z represents the proxy variables for innate ability, parental values and preferences usually unmeasured in previous studies. Prior studies have exploited variation fi'om parental loss through death as well as sibling composition to attenuate omitted variable bias (Lang and Zagorsky, (2001); Sandefur and Wells, (1997)). However, as discussed in section 1.2, using these methods may introduce other sources of bias into the model. Exploiting sibling comparisons, for instance, requires the assumption that siblings receive equal parental investments; moreover, the analysis sample is restricted to only those families with multiple children (Sigle-Rushton and McLanahan, (2002)). Paternal death may also be endogenous in the model if death is caused by endogenous factors such as lifestyle and occupational choices. Furthermore, it cannot be used to examine multiple effects of paternal presence. If the main observed and unobserved characteristics can be directly controlled for in the model using a rich set of control variables along with the proxy-variable OLS solution, then arguably the “true” impact of father’s presence on child cognitive performance can be isolated. The FFCWS aptly offers a wealth of data in which once unobserved and unmeasured characteristics can now be directly controlled for in the model. Even though this econometric method is not as elaborate as those employed in previous studies, omitted variable bias will be effectively attenuated without introducing other sources of bias. 1.5. Data Description The Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS) supplies rich and detailed information on family structure, family characteristics and conditions. It follows a sample of approximately 5,000 children born between 1998 and 2000. Follow-up interviews were conducted at one, three and five years thereafter. For this analysis, I will only use data from the baseline, the one-year and three-year follow-up interviews. The baseline interviews of both parents occurred shortly after the child was born, when both parents were likely to be present in the hospital for the birth of their child. As a result, the study was able to interview about 75% of all unmarried fathers in the sample - the cohort that is usually under-sampled in many surveys. Moreover, because both parents were interviewed at the baseline, data on missing fathers are also made available through the mother’s responses. The FFCWS uses stratified random sampling as the means of recruiting parents to be a part of the sample p0pulation. Of all 77 large cities in the United States with a population of 200,000 or more, 16 of these were randomly sampled. It is important to note that there are 20 cities that comprise the total FFCWS sample. However, 4 were 10 non-randomly selected as they were of special interest to specific foundations of this Study“. The cities were stratified according to policy milieu and labor market conditions. All cities were scored on their welfare generosity, the strength of the child support system and the state of the labor market. The indicators for welfare generosity are measures of the dollar value of monthly welfare payments; the strength of the child support system was determined by paternity establishment and the number of AFDC cases given awards and payments; labor market conditions of a city were primarily determined by unemployment rates. For welfare generosity, the stringency of the child support enforcement system and labor market conditions, cities were scored then ranked in categories of strong, moderate or weak. A city is classified as ‘extreme’ if it ranked in the strong or weak categories (top or bottom quartiles of total points respectively) for all three policy regimes. Cities that had extreme values fell into one of eight possible cells. For instance: (i) generous welfare, stringent child support enforcement, and strong labor market; (ii) generous welfare, stringent child support enforcement, and weak labor market; (iii) insufficient welfare, stringent child support enforcement and strong labor market, etc. This represents different combinations of extreme forms of welfare, child support, and labor market regimes. Within each of these eight ‘extreme’ cells, 1 city was randomly chosen and over—sampled, providing about 325 births to the sample population. These cities were over-sampled in order to maximize variation in policy regimes. In total, there were 12 cities over-sampled — the 8 ‘extreme’ cities and the 4 4 These cities include: Detroit, MI; Oakland, CA; San Jose, CA; Newark, NJ 11' selected for specific foundationss. Approximately 100 births were sampled from each of the other 8 ‘non-extreme’ cities with moderate policy regimes or labor market conditions; this helped facilitate the detection of non-linearities as well as create a nationally representative sample of non-marital births. After the cities were selected, hospitals were then sampled such that each city sample was representative of non-marital births for that city. However, sampling I hospitals within each of the twenty cities was not uniformly executed. First off, there were five cities in which there were only 5 birthing hospitals or less: Oakland, CA; Austin, TX; Newark, NJ; Richmond, VA and Corpus Christi, TX. Consequently, all hospitals in these cities were sampled. By contrast, the other 15 cities had more than 5 birthing hospitals. For 12 of these, the hospitals were rank-ordered such that the hospital with the most non-marital births was sampled first; sampling would continue in descending order until the sample was representative of the non-marital births for that city — usually about 75%. Hospitals in New York, NY and Chicago, IL, were not rank-ordered using this ru1e6. These cities had numerous birthing hospitals and consequently, there was less need to secure the participation of any one hospital. Only hospitals with over 1,000 non-marital births per year were randomly sampled in New York, NY and Chicago, IL. The sample frame for each hospital was a list of beds in the maternity ward. The design of the Study was to over-sample non-marital births and as such, non-marital births were sampled until a pre-set quota, based on 1996-1997 non-marital birth rates in 5 These 8 extreme cities are: Indianapolis, IN; Austin, TX; Boston, MA; Santa Ana, CA; Richmond, VA; Corpus Christi, TX; Toledo, OH; New York, NY. 6 Hospitals in Philadelphia, PA, were also not rank-ordered using this rule. 63% of non-marital births to city residents were sampled from six of the eighteen hospitals. 12 each hospital, was reached. Marital births were also sampled until a pre-set quota was reached. Some parents were inevitably ineligible to be a part of the Study: (i) those parents under 18 in hospitals where interviewing under—18 parents was prohibited; (ii) fathers who were dead at the time of the child’s birth; (iii) mothers who were not sufficiently fluent in English or Spanish to complete the interview; (iv) those parents who intended to give the child up for adoption. There are implications for the use over-sampling of non-marital births on the estimation results. Not only will it affect the generality of the findings, but also the efficiency of the estimators. I have included city indicators in each regression in order to control for the idiosyncratic differences among cities, upon which over-sampling was based. Over-sampling is not expected to affect the consistency of the estimators so long as I have specified the model correctly. 1.5.1. Description of Variables in the Model The child outcome that will be examined in this study is the revised version of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-R). The PPVT-R has two aims: (1) to test the respondent’s receptive vocabulary capabilities for standard English and (2) to test the respondent’s verbal ability7. The PPVT-R is also often used as a measure of academic readiness for pre-school aged children and hence is salient to examine. Even though the PPVT-R is useful in measuring English Language proficiency and can even be usefirl to test respondents with mental and language impediments, one caveat is that it only serves as a reliable indicator of verbal ability for those living in an 7 The PPVT-R is administered by the examiner, selecting a ‘picture plate’ which shows four different black and white images. The examinee must choose the image that best describes the stimulus word spoken by the examiner. American Guidance Service, Inc. ' http ://www.state.tn.us/education/c i/cistandardsZOO 1/1a/cik32tssesmentfolder/cik3 rapeabodypicture.htrn 13 environment where English is principally spoken. For instance, the PPVT—R scores of Hispanic and Latin-American children in the sample may not be reliable indicators of their cognitive skills. Consequently, the language chiefly spoken in the household must be controlled for (in some form) if the PPVT-R is to accurately measure the verbal ability of these childrens. For the test, the child has to identify the picture that best describes the noun or the verb spoken by the examiner (Jeruchimowicz et al., (1971)). The PPVT-R is generally administered to individuals over the age of 2.5 years. The data on the PPVT-R are provided in the 36-month In-Home Longitudinal Study of Pre-School Aged Children (a module of the FF CWS). As a result, only a single cross-section of the data can be used for the purpose of analysis. This immediately reduces the analysis sample to only 2,368 respondents. The average age of the child at the time the test was administered was approximately 38 months, underscoring the importance of controlling for as many factors influencing the child’s cognitive performance as the data will allow. Table 1 shows the summary statistics of all the variables included in the model. The outcome measure is the child’s PPVT-R standardized score and the independent variables include measures of paternal presence, parental attributes, income, family background, household conditions and proxies for parents’ ability, values and preferences. The standardized form of the PPVT-R score was chosen because it adjusts for the mental age-score of each child. 1.5.2. Measures of Paternal Presence 8 I include variables indicating whether the mother was interviewed in Spanish as well as parents’ region of birth as proxy variables for chief language spoken in the child’s household. 14 The analysis sample is restricted to those children who live with their mothers all (or most of) the timeg. This ensures that any disruptive effect from paternal movement is not conflated by the disruptive effect that will possibly ensue from maternal movement into or out of the household. However, this restriction may introduce bias from sample selection because there are idiosyncratic differences between mothers who are primary caregivers and mothers who are not. Nevertheless, the vast majority of mothers in the sample are primary caregivers to the focal child and so we can argue that any selection bias caused by this restriction would be inconsequential. The restriction reduces the analysis sample fi‘om 2,368 respondents to 2,202 respondents. The final sample used for analysis is 1,745 respondents due to missing data for many of the covariates. The central question needed to derive the family structure types is: “Has the biological father ever been present in the household? ” From this question, different measures of paternal presence can be determined (See Figure 1.1). From Figure 1.1, we get the following measures: 1) Biological father present in the home since child’s birth and married to mother 1) Biological father present in the home since child’s birth and cohabits with mother 2) Biological father is no longer present in the home and mother is now married to social father10 2) Biological father is no longer present in the home and mother now cohabits with social father 3) Biological father is no longer present in the home and mother is now single X) Social father is present in the home since child’s birth and married to mother X) Social father is present in the home since child’s birth and cohabits with mother 9 Ideally, I would like to restrict the analysis sample to children living with their mothers all the time. However, in the third-year follow-up interview, the mother is asked if the focal child lives with her “all or most of the time.” As a result, all primary caregivers are grouped together despite the implications for instability. To simplify the various measures of paternal presence, I define “social father” as a man (who is not the child’s biological father) living and romantically involved with the focal child’s mother. 15 4) Biological father has never been present in the home but the social father is now married to mother 4) Biological father has never been present in the home but the social father is now cohabiting with mother 5) Interim relationships 6) Biological father has been completely absent and mother has been single since child’s birth Since the FF CWS does not ask the mother about a possible social father in the home at the baseline, it cannot be observed whether the social father had been present in the home since the child’s birth. Therefore, the two measures associated with the social father’s stable presence in the household (X) cannot be directly specified in the model1 1. I define an interim relationship as a cohabitational relationship that initiated and/or dissolved between the baseline and third-year follow-up interviews. Interim relationships could potentially include any of following family transitions: {father present at birth, absent at one-yearfollow-up and returns by third-yearfollow- up; father absent at birth, absent at one-year follow-up and enters the home by the third-year follow-up; father absent at birth, present at one-year follow-up and third- year follow-up; father present at birth, social father present at one-yearfollow—up and father returns by third-year follow-up; father absent at birth, social father present at one-yearfollow-up, he then leaves and father enters the home by third-yearfollow-up; no father present at child 's birth, social father present at one-year follow-up and mother is again single by third-yearfollow-up} H These households would likely be captured in measures where the child’s father has never been present but the social father is currently present -— father’s presence can be determined at the baseline while the social father’s presence can only be determined in subsequent waves. 16 If we assume that the effects of marriage and cohabitation on early cognitive development are not statistically different from each other, these numerous measures can be condensed as follows12 : 1) Biological father has been present in the home since child’s birth (stable two-parent family structure) 2) Biological father used to be in the home but the social father is now present (unstable two-parent family structure) 3) Biological father used to be in the home but mother is now single (unstable single-parent family structure) 4) Biological father has never been present in the home but social father is now present (unstable two-parent family structure) 5) Interim relationships (unstable family structure) 6) Biological father has been completely absent and mother has been single since child’s birth (stable single-parent family structure) Consequently, the number of family structures specified in equation (3), m, is equal to 6. These family structures can be classified as: stable two-parent, stable single-parent, unstable two-parent and unstable single-parent households. 1.5.3. Descriptive Statistics Table 1 illustrates that the standardized PPVT-R scores range from 40 to 137 points and the mean for children in the analysis sample is approximately 86 points. This low mean can be attributed to over-sampling of large cities'3 but should not influence the regression estimates. 50% of children lived in stable two-parent households while 20% of children lived with their single-mothers since birth. Moreover, over 1% of children had biological fathers who left but have social fathers present in the household by the mother’s third-year interview; almost 11% of children had no social father present after their biological father left. By contrast, 6% of the children in the analysis ‘2 The assumption of no statistical difference between marriage and cohabitation fails if the interpretation of cohabitation varies among the mothers of the sample. '3 These cities include: Austin, TX; Baltimore, MD; Detroit, MI; Milwaukee, WI; Newark, NJ; New York, NY; Oakland, CA; Richmond, VA; San Jose, CA (Reichman et al. (2001)). 17 sample never had a biological father living at home but now have a social father present; 11% experienced numerous disruptions caused by interim relationships of the mother. These figures reveal that a large percentage of children had their biological fathers present at least partially; however, a much smaller percentage of children had social fathers to fill the role of the absentee biological father. Table 2 gives the mean standardized PPVT-R scores for each family structure type. The general score means in Panel A show that children of the stable two-parent family type have higher PPVT-R scores on average than children of stable single-parent or unstable family structures. This is what we would expect a priori. However, the means also indicate that children of stable single-mother households have higher scores on average than children of unstable households. This lends credence to the theory postulated by Sandefur and Wells (1997), Wu (1996) and McLanahan (1985) that the stress associated with family disruption creates adverse outcomes for the child. The means also bolster Fomby and Cherlin’s (2007) instability hypothesis, which posits that children’s developmental outcomes are worse if they experience multiple family transitions as opposed to living in a stable environment. The outcome means insinuate that it is better for a father to be at home all the time than be there intermittently or not at all; it is also better for a father not to be at home at all than to be there intermittently. The same pattern of results is also clearly evident in Panels B and C where the sample is split by gender in order to observe gender differences in the impacts of patemal presence. The results show that girls have higher average scores in general than boys as expected (Bomstein and Haynes, (1998)). 18 Table 3 illustrates other independent variables truncated by the following family types: stable two-parent family, unstable family and the stable single-mother family structure. The most striking characteristic is that predominantly (over 70%), black parents and their children represent the stable single-mother household. White and Hispanic parents largely belong to stable two-parent households, with over 30% of white parents and about 25% of Hispanic parents comprising this family type. Parents with at least college education largely constitute the stable two-parent family structure whereas over 25% of parents who were high school dropouts typify the unstable and stable single-mother family types. 72% of fathers and 39% of mothers of stable two-parent families report they use alcohol — more than any other family type. This is not as surprising as it would appear since most fathers and a large percentage of mothers belonging to other family types also use alcohol. Also not surprising is that the stable two-parent family structure is characterized by older parents and higher household income. Unstable family structures, by contrast, typically display the youngest parents, the most residential moves since the child’s birth, the shortest dating period before pregnancy and more mothers with mental or emotional problems compared to any other family type. For stable single-mother households, only 42% of children were breastfed, the lowest among all the family types. Parents associated with the stable single-mother household also have the lowest WAIS-R scores and household incomes. One caveat in relying on these summary statistics is that they may reflect the use of over-sampling by the FFCWS. Observing the race averages indicate that about 52% of mothers and 55% of fathers in the analysis sample are black (47% of mothers and 19 49% of fathers in the total sample are black). The national average of blacks in the US. population is only about 12%“. Consequently, over-sampling directly increased the percentage of blacks in the sample. This provides an explanation for why a large percentage of children lived in stable single-parent households and only a small percentage had social fathers in the sample. Black children are less likely to have a father present in the household than any other racial or ethnic group. Subsequently, the means in Table 1 will be skewed by the use of over-sampling but this should not bias the regression estimators. In fact, it makes for a policy-relevant sample, where the results will yield direct implications for social policy. 1.6. Results Table 4 presents estimates from a specification similar to those in columns (1) and (2) of Tables 3 and 4 in Lang and Zagorsky (2001). They use the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY) to examine how a parent’s absence during childhood affects outcomes in adulthood. I define father’s presence as the fraction of time the father has been living with the child, constructed as the total number of years the biological father has spent living with the child divided by the child’s age. Column (2) indicates that as father presence increases from 0 to 1, the child’s standardized PPVT-R score increases by 5.2 points (about 0.30 standard deviations) when only region-of-birth and interview- year dummies are incorporated in the model. However, when similar control variables to Lang and Zagorsky (2001) are included in Column ( 1), the impact of the fraction of time a father is present in the household is no longer statistically significant, with the point estimate falling to about 1.2 points. Father’s presence has a larger positive impact ‘4 US. Census Bureau, Census (2000). 20 on girls’ than boys’ PPVT-R scores in the simple regression model; however, the effect dissipates for both boys and girls once the relevant control variables are included, challenging the statistically significant, albeit modest effects found by Lang and Zagorsky (2001). However, if the effect of paternal presence varies by family structure type, then the small and statistically insignificant results displayed in Table 4 should not be surprising. Multiple effects subsumed in a single measure become conflated and thus yield a statistically insignificant estimate. Table 5 displays the results when the effect of paternal presence is allowed to vary by family structure. Column set (A) are regressions including only city, region-of- birth and interview-year dummies. Standardized PPVT-R scores are higher by about 4 points (0.24 standard deviations) for children of stable two-parent families relative to children of stable single-parent families. In addition, the stable single-parent family structure generates higher scores than if the father and/or social father were present for only a portion of the child’s life illustrated by measures (2) and (4). These results underscore the implications derived from the outcome means in Table 2 and lend credence to the instability and stress hypotheses posited by Fomby and Cherlin (2007) and Sandefur and Wells (1997). When exogenous variables (listed in Table 1) are included in the model in column set (B), the effect of a father’s stable presence is no longer statistically different from the effect of his stable absence. The negative effect caused by family disruption is still apparent, nevertheless. In the instance where the biological father has left and the social father is now living in the household, the child is at about a 10-point disadvantage 21 (about 0.60 standard deviations); similarly, where the father was never present but the social father is currently present, the child’s PPVT-R score is about 4 points (0.22 standard deviations) lower than in the stable single-parent case. Unobserved heterogeneity is expected to upwardly bias the father presence coefficients. Therefore, column set (D) includes proxy variables (listed in section D of Table 1) for parental values, preferences and ability. When these proxy variables are included in the model, the coefficients measuring the father presence effects in general become larger than the coefficients in column (C), where all independent variables except the proxy variables for unobserved characteristics are accounted for. Column set - (D) shows that family disruption lowers the child’s test scores between 2 and 9 points (between 0.12 and 0.54 standard deviations) relative to the stable single-parent case. The negative coefficient on father is no longer present but social father is now present has the largest magnitude ‘across the board’ of any unstable family structure defined. However, this family type also represents the fewest number of families (27 in total) compared to the other family types and as such, the large magnitude may just be a reflection of this small sample. Test scores when the father is completely present remains statistically similar to test scores when the father is completely absent, and thus the stability effect holds. What is also interesting is that family structures with two parents do .not necessarily yield better outcomes than those with one parent. In particular, unstable two-parent family structures adversely affect child cognitive performance relative to the stable single-parent household. Consequently, the family structure effect is not confirmed by these findings. 22 Tables 6 and 7 indicate that in the naive model, girls experience higher scores than boys when their fathers are completely present as opposed to completely absent. This is consistent with the outcome means discussed in the previous section. However, when the full set of variables are incorporated in the model, these gender differences are not as convincing. Depending on the family type, boys in unstable households score between 0.12 and 0.40 standard deviations lower than boys in stable single-mother households and girls in unstable households score between 0.01 and 0.80 standard deviations lower than girls in stable single-mother households when all variables are included in these regressions. These estimates are however, largely statistically insignificant due to small sample sizes. The only statistically significant estimate belongs to the unstable family structure in which the child’s biological father has left and the social father has entered the household. Boys living in this family setting score about 0.35 standard deviations lower than boys in stable single-mother households; similarly, girls belonging to this family type score about 0.75 standard deviations lower than girls living in stable single- mother households. This provides some, albeit weak evidence that during early childhood years, girls may in fact suffer more due to family disruption than boys. The stability effect on cognitive ability remains clearly evident since there is no statistical difference when the child lives in a stable two-parent home as opposed to a stable single-parent one. 1.6.1. The Resource Effect An unusual feature of this model is the modest mediating effect of resources in the child’s household. The resource effect is captured by household income per person 23 and shows that for an increase of $10,000, the child’s PPVT-R standardized score improves by 1 point (results not shown). This estimate is statistically significant at the 5% level but the magnitude (6% of a standard deviation) suggests that the resource effect is surprisingly not as important in this model as one would have expected a priori, particularly because all families in the sample reside in urban areas. Nevertheless, its inclusion bears a strong implication for consistency of the family structure estimators. Column set (E) in Table 5 shows that when the resource effect is unaccounted for, the estimated effect of each family structure type gets smaller (except for the family type in which the father is no longer present but the social father is)”, indicating upward bias. This reinforces the importance of controlling for as many factors as possible that concurrently influence child cognitive ability and family structure, ensuring unbiased estimators. 1.6.2. Robustness Checks As stated in section 1.5, twelve large cities were over-sampled. If I have specified my model correctly, over-sampling should not affect the consistency of the estimators. As a sensitivity check however, I generate weighted estimators using national sampling weights from the FFCWS. The results in Table 8 column set (B) show that the weighted estimators are not statistically different from the un-weighted estimators in column set (A)'6. '5 This family type includes only 27 observations compared to the other family types, which each have at least 100 observations. 6 The reason there is a difference in the sample size between the weighted regressions and the un- weighted regressions is that cities that were not randomly selected into the sample were not assigned weights. Of the twenty cities in the sample, Detroit, MI; Newark, NJ; Oakland, CA and San Jose, CA were selected based on special interest to some foundations. As a result, they were not assigned weights in the study. 24 The identification strategy employed also does not address heterogeneous family treatment effects. Despite mitigating unobserved heterogeneity, my methodology treats all families as identical except for time of paternal exit from or entry into the household. It is salient to note that two families can be identical based on observables at the time of the test despite one father’s exit from his family. By controlling for conditions preceding the father’s exit from or social father’s entrance into the household, families can be distinguished from each other beyond just the look of their present family structure. I incorporate in the full regression mother’s education since child’s birth, parents’ alcohol and drug use for all three waves, household income over all three waves and average number of adults and children in the household over all three waves. These results, as shown in column set (C), corroborate the main findings of the study. 1.7. Summary This paper adds to the literature by utilizing rich, policy-relevant data to examine the various effects of paternal presence and family instability on child cognitive development. The results show that when an exhaustive set of control and proxy variables are incorporated in the model, the pre-school aged child is not necessarily better off when the father is home all the time as opposed to never being home but he/she is certainly better off when the father is never home as opposed to being home on a temporary basis. The study was therefore unable to reject the conventional hypothesis that stable paternal presence yields better cognitive outcomes than stable paternal absence. The findings of Cavanagh and Huston (2006), Osborne and McLanahan (2007) and Fomby and Cherlin (2007) are endorsed by this study since a father’s partial 25 presence in the home stunts cognitive development relative to when he is not living in the household at all. The study also adds to this literature by reinforcing the stability effect: there is no statistical difference between the stable single-parent household and the stable two-parent household when it comes to child cognitive development; for children of unstable family structures, their PPVT-R scores are lower by 2 to 9 points (an average of about 1/5 of a standard deviation) relative to children of stable single- parent households. On the other hand, the family structure effect is not supported by these results — unstable two-parent households are found to'be worse than stable single- parent households as it pertains to child cognitive performance. If the proxy-variable OLS solution and extensive covariates sufficiently attenuate unobserved heterogeneity, the causal relationship between paternal presence and child cognitive performance is only apparent in the stability the father generates within the family setting. Paternal presence improves cognitive development through the stability of family structure — any type of paternal presence will not necessarily engender positive results for the child. The stress hypothesis postulated by Sandefur and Wells (1997), Wu (1996) and McLanahan (1985) gives us some insight as to why this might be the case: unstable family structures produce negative outcomes due to the stress and anxiety that accompany each transition. The study was not able to determine whether more family transitions yielded more adverse effects on early cognitive development. Moreover, the findings of Lang and Zagorsky (2001) and Antecol and Bedard (2007) are not reinforced by this study — the child does not seem to be better off as the father’s length of residence increases. However, it is important to note that I cannot 26 predict how the child would adjust to his/her family transitions over the course of his/her life. Since, the subjects of study are pre-school aged children (average age is 38 months), it cannot be determined whether the negative effects of family dissolution are short-lived or are improved over time. The child may be able to adjust to his/her family structure as time progresses but this clearly goes beyond the scope of this paper. Further, these effects may not extend to other child outcomes such as behavioral problems or substance abuse. Future research may study this in more rigorous detail. The main policy implication of the findings is the importance of encouraging family stability, as this should help improve the cognitive development of the children affected. There may also be implications for children parented by same-sex couples. If paternal presence only improves child cognitive performance through the stability of the family structure, it is quite possible that same-sex parents within a stable household may engender similar positive cognitive outcomes for their children as well. 27 Chapter 2. Paternal Incarceration, Paternal Absence and Early Child Development 2.1. Introduction By 2001, 2.7% of adults in the United States had been incarcerated, indicating a 50% hike since 1990. More interesting is the fact that 6 out of 10 inmates fi'om 1996 to 2002 were minorities: 40% were black and 19% were Hispanic”. Since statistics show that over 50% of all inmates grew up in a single-parent household and 46% had a family member who was previously incarcerated, the increase in incarceration rates could disproportionately affect minority children as well as their parents. This study will investigate the extent to which the incarceration of a central family member — the father -— influences developmental outcomes of young children. The fundamental identification problem associated with this research question is the difficulty in isolating the impact on childhood development of paternal incarceration from the impact of unobserved factors correlated with incarceration. For example, if a criminal offender lived in unfavorable circumstances ex ante, poor child outcomes may simply, be another result of the poor pre-existing conditions that also led to the father’s incarceration. This problem will be addressed using instrumental variables estimation and a differencing method. '7 Profile of Jail Inmates (2002), Bureau of Justice Statistics. 28 Another pertinent question inextricably linked to the research question is whether the effect of paternal absence due to incarceration is statistically different from the effect of his absence in general. The consensus among previous studies is that father absence has a negative effect on child outcomes (Antecol and Bedard (2007); Corak, (2001); Lang and Zagorsky (2001); Painter and Levine, (2000)). However, there is evidence that paternal absence does not always have a negative effect on child development. J afee et a1. (2003) argue that paternal presence may do more harm than good when the father exhibits anti-social behavior. In addition, other studies find that it is the instability of paternal presence (or absence) that engenders negative effects on child wellbeing (Craigie (2008); Cavanagh and Huston (2006); Fomby and Cherlin, (2007); Osborne and McLanahan (2007)). From these studies, the question arises: “Are all absences created equal?” Would absence due to incarceration be more detrimental because of the circumstances under which the father became absent? Or, would incarceration induce an improved family situation due to the timely removal of an anti-social presence from the household? The study will attempt to distinguish the impact of paternal absence from the impact of paternal incarceration as they relate to early child development. The data used to examine this problem is obtained from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS), which meticulously measures paternal incarceration as well as important early child developmental outcomes. Using these data, a prior study by Wildeman (2008) employed fixed effects estimation to show that paternal incarceration amplified aggressive behaviors in boys. In my study, paternal incarceration not only exacerbates aggressive behaviors and ODD symptoms in young children but also lowers 29 their cognitive test scores. However, when the effect of incarceration is effectively isolated from other factors with which it is correlated, the study finds no statistical difference between the effect of incarceration on child outcomes and the effect of paternal absence in general. The paper is organized as follows. Section 2.2 provides a brief review of past works that discuss parental incarceration. Section 2.3 gives the data description and summary statistics of the variables used in the model. Section 2.4 explains the identification strategies I use to estimate the effect of paternal incarceration. Section 2.5 discusses the results from the differencing and instrumental variables estimation methods. Section 2.6 includes a summary of the findings and policy implications. 2.2. Background The most apparent effect of parental incarceration is the strain on economic resources in the household. It is worth noting however, that the strain caused by incarceration does not only refer to economic capital, but social capital as well. Clearly, the structure and quality of family relationships are disrupted by incarceration. Non-resident parents are able to maintain frequent contact with children if they so choose. In the event of incarceration, however, children are more at a disadvantage since the avenue for frequent contact is physically obstructed. Moreover, the incarcerated parent may have been a serious drain on family resources prior to incarceration. He/She may even become incapacitated if the stable and supportive environment of the household is disrupted (e.g. due to violence, abuse or negligence). These hypotheses suggest that paternal incarceration may yield positive or negative outcomes for the child. 30 It can be strongly argued that criminal offenders and men predisposed to incarceration possess traits that frustrate family cohesiveness (Western, (2004)). Subsequently, weak attachment to the family would have occurred even if incarceration did not. Therefore, the factors and influences that predate paternal incarceration also help engender incarceration. Put differently, ex-post conditions are merely a continuation of the pre-incarceration situation. An attempt to simply compare children of incarcerated parents to children whose parents have never been incarcerated (but have similar background characteristics), will yield a biased incarceration estimator. This is because children of criminal offenders are exposed to unobserved factors, which not only increase the probability of parental incarceration, but also persist to adversely affect child wellbeing. Conversely, children whose parents have never been incarcerated are arguably less exposed to these factors and as a consequence, estimates from this simple method of comparison will not be consistent. A generally unexplored effect of parental incarceration is its effect on the parent- child relationship. Incarceration disrupts the attachment mechanism between the incarcerated parent and the child, especially at early ages. This could possibly lead to both short-terrn and long-term effects on the child’s well-being. Elicker et al. (1992) postulate that attachment of the child to the parent, particularly during infancy, increases social competence among peers. As a result, incarceration is expected to negatively affect child development due to its inherently disruptive nature. Despite a priori expectations of an adverse effect, there is still much to be determined about paternal incarceration and child outcomes. The views presented above indicate an ambiguous effect of paternal incarceration that could be negative, positive or 31 even zero. Previous works have shown that family instability produces negative childhood outcomes (Cavanagh and Huston, (2006); Craigie, (2008); Fomby and Cherlin, (2007); Osborne and McLanahan, (2007)). However, is the effect of instability from incarceration different from the effect stemming from other sources of disruption such as divorce or the death of a parent? I examine these differential effects to determine the influence of incarceration on early child cognitive and behavioral measures. 2.3. Data and Variable Descriptions The data for this analysis come from The Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS). It follows a sample of approximately 5,000 children born between 1998 and 2000. Parents were interviewed around the time of the child’s birth, with follow-up interviews occurring at about ages one, three and five years thereafter. Data have been gathered on not only the child’s developmental outcomes and characteristics, but also on family relationships and demographics of the parents and focal child. Because both parents are interviewed on these issues, the roles of both parents and particularly the father can be examined in detail. This proves to be especially important for the analysis, as it allows father characteristics possibly linked to his incarceration history to be incorporated in the model. Both parents report on the father’s past and current incarceration status in the FFCWS, and the data also include a wide array of child developmental outcomes useful for this study. (For more information on the FFCWS data, see chapter 1, section 1.5.) I will examine cognitive development measured by the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R) and behavioral problems displayed in the forms of aggression and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD). These outcome measures are 32 currently only given in the 36-month In-Home Longitudinal Study and as such, only a single cross-section of data may be used for estimation. 2.3.1. Early Child Development Measures i. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test - Revised (standardized) The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R) is administered to children over the age of two and a half years old to measure their verbal ability and English Language proficiency. For the test, the focal child must state the noun or verb which best describes the image given on a picture plate (J eruchimowicz et al., ( 1971)). The PPVT-R is also commonly used as a measure of academic readiness among pre-schoolers and is reliable even for those children with mental and language impediments. However, for children living in a household where English is not predominantly spoken, the PPVT-R does not reliably predict verbal ability. To control for this, I will incorporate in the model parents’ region of birth as well as an indicator for whether the mother was interviewed in Spanish. PPVT- R scores are also standardized to adjust for the chronological mental age-score of the child. ii. Aggression Aggressive behavior disorders in a child are shown by acts undeniably intended to hurt or destroy a person, animal or object (Grusec and Lytton, (1988); Maccoby, (1980); Shaw and Giovanelli, (2000)). However, to accurately diagnose aggressive behavior disorders, one must be able to determine intentionality (Shaw and Giovanelli, (2000)). There are nineteen acts of aggression and defiance listed in the In-Home Longitudinal Study (see Appendix B) but intentionality of these 33 acts is indistinct. Even though intentionality and thus a conduct disorder cannot be diagnosed in this instance, it does not negate the importance of examining aggression in children, since incarceration and criminal propensities are often preceded by such misguided behaviors during childhood (Robins, (1978); Sampson and Laub, (1992); Wildeman, (2008)). Thirteen of the acts of aggression are averaged to create an index, ranging from 0 to about 2, with 0 indicating the least aggression”. iii. Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) can be described as recurrent disobedient, defiant and aggressive acts, particularly towards those in authority (Greene at al. (2002)). Unlike the aggression outcome measure, ODD is an actual conduct disorder. The In-Home Longitudinal Study provides six symptoms of ODD (see Appendix B) that are requisite for the diagnosis of this type of conduct disorder. These symptoms are averaged to create the child’s ODD index, which ranges from 0 to 2, with 2 being the most defiant. Social factors such as poverty and family disruption are thought to increase the probability of diagnosis and will be incorporated in the model accordingly (Steiner et al. (2007)). 2.3.2. The Incarceration Measures The father’s incarceration history and status is inferred from information provided by both parents in the FFCWS. Mothers are asked about the fathers’ current incarceration status and whether the father has ever been incarcerated. Fathers are asked about their current incarceration status and also about their most recent incarceration and release. It '8 Note here that each individual measure ranges from 0 to 2 but averaging all measures creates a maximum of 1.92. 34 is important to note that mother and father interviews are conducted at different times, and hence, if one parent declares the father currently incarcerated but the other does not, this does not necessarily mean that either of the reports is inaccurate. The father could have been jailed and released prior to the other parent’s interview but this information is not easily verifiable”. As a result, a father will be coded as having been incarcerated after the child’s birth (i.e. incarceration ex post) if either parent reports in any interview that the father is currently jailed or if the father reports that he was recently incarcerated after the child’s birth. Father’s incarceration before the child’s birth (i.e. incarceration ex ante) is difficult to determine from the data. Since follow—up interviews do not occur annually, the jail and release period could easily have transpired between interviews, so that subtracting the sub-sample of ex post incarcerated fathers from the total sample of ever-incarcerated fathers will not yield the precise sample of fathers incarcerated ex ante. The father is therefore classified as incarcerated ex ante if the year of his incarceration and the year of his release were before the focal child’s birth year. 2.3.3. Summary Statistics The summary statistics for all the dependent outcomes and independent variables are presented in Table 9. The analysis sample is restricted to children living with their mothers all (or most) of the time due to the concern that mother’s unstable presence in the household could conflate any disruptive effect that ensues from the father’s incarceration or partial absence”. 19 There are questions on the time the father was jailed and released. However, these data are often missing in the FF CWS. 20 The analysis sample is further limited to children whose fathers have impulsivity (DDI) scores to ensure a more accurate picture of the families in the IV sample. 35 The average PPVT-R standardized score for children in the analysis sample is approximately 86 points. This score seems low but may be attributed to the over- sampling of large cities in the FFCWS (Reichman et al. (2001))21. Family dissolution, low education and economic adversity are reality for many children in large cities and may help explain their low scores. The child’s aggression measure is an index of thirteen acts of violent and aggressive behavior”. They are averaged to create an index from 0 to about 2 that increases with the level of aggression; the mean of this aggression index in the analysis sample is 0.58. Similarly, the oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) symptoms are averaged, range from 0 to 2 and increase with the level of defiance. The mean of this index is 0.63. Another important pattern displayed in the data is that parents of the focal child exhibit many characteristics associated with a high-risk environment. Of the fathers in the analysis sample, 38% were ever-incarcerated with almost 25% of these incarcerated since the child’s birth. Over 40% of fathers have been absent at least in part while most parents have a high school degree or less. Household income per person is about $8000 per year with a standard deviation of over $13,000. With the high probability of single- parenthood, low education and low household income, the children in the Fragile Families Study are, in general, at a greater disadvantage. However, these statistics do not give us a vivid picture of the differences between the child of an incarcerated father and the child whose father has never been incarcerated. 21 These cities include: Austin, TX; Baltimore, MD; Detroit, MI; Milwaukee, WI; Newark, NJ; New York, NY; Oakland, CA; Richmond, VA; San Jose, CA. 2 The six ODD items are a subset of the nineteen acts of aggression in the FFCWS. These items are excluded from the aggression index, leaving only thirteen. 36 Table 10 presents comparisons of variable means by father’s incarceration history. The child of an incarcerated father has a PPVT-R score that is about 4 points lower than the child whose father has never been incarcerated. Children of incarcerated fathers also display more aggressive and defiant tendencies as opposed to their counterparts. In addition to these developmental outcomes, it is evident that other characteristics of the child’s household vary by the father’s incarceration history. Incarcerated fathers and their partners are more likely to be black and less educated relative to non-incarcerated fathers; children of non-incarcerated fathers also live in households with over $4000 more income per person. 2.4. Econometric Issues and Methods 2.4.1. Omitted Variable Bias The simple model presented in equation (1) below, controls for exogenous individual characteristics, X, incarceration, I and absence, A, to explain child outcomes, Y. YI=B0+BIIi+B2Ai+XiB3+3i (1) The variable I, is not orthogonal to the error term, 8, since there are unobserved factors in 8 that simultaneously influence child outcomes and the probability of paternal incarceration. Factors such as innate ability, deviant tendencies, tastes and preferences may be correlated with both incarceration and child outcomes and constitute time- invariant unobserved heterogeneity, 9, in equation (2). An identification strategy must be employed to eliminate unobserved heterogeneity for an unbiased estimator y]. YI=Y0+YIII+72AI+XIY3+9i+Ili (2) 37 To eliminate the time-invariant unobserved factors captured by 9, a differencing strategy is employed to identify the marginal effect of incarceration ex post on the outcome measures of early child development. If 9 is interpreted as the father’s “criminal type” and directly controlled for in the model, the marginal impact of paternal incarceration may in fact be isolated. Juvenile offenses and incarceration prior to the child’s birth may well be a strong indicator of father’s criminal tendency as well as other unobserved characteristics associated with his incarceration ex post. As such, this indicator will be used, in essence, as a proxy for “criminal type”23. It is important to note however, that this is an imperfect proxy because “criminal type” may not have been revealed prior to the child’s birth. Further, the implicit assumption in using this strategy is that criminal propensities are fixed —- variability stemming from criminality as a response to events ex post are not considered (e.g. father’s unemployment occurring after child’s birth could be the cause of incarceration ex post). We also need to investigate whether absence due to incarceration engenders a different effect on child outcomes than paternal absence in general. For this reason, the model will directly control for father absence, to better isolate the marginal impact of paternal incarceration. Therefore, rewriting equation (1) to include juvenile offenses and incarceration ex ante, Ia, incarceration ex post, Ip, exogenous variables, X, and other independent variables, N, we get: 23 Grogger (1995) employs a similar differencing strategy to deal with unobserved heterogeneity in studying the impact of arrests on earnings and employment. For a sample of men arrested between 1972 and 1987, Grogger restricts the analysis sample to data on earnings and employment from 1980 to 1984. Labor market outcomes of men first arrested after 1984 are compared to a treatment group of men first arrested before 1985. Subsequently, men initially arrested in or after 1985 are similar in “criminal type” to those men initially arrested before this period, rendering comparison of their labor market outcomes unbiased. 38 Yr=(Po+\llIPI+7IIai+o= mm; 555 Cu _ o 3d :6 v9: 53:5 Bo: fl .5502 25 :5on 5w:o. o: m_ 55mm Am _ o 2 .o 5.0 32 5:855 Bo: m_ .5595 38m :5 255.5 5mg. 2. fl 55m... 3 _ o omd omd v2: :Em @250 3:5 25.5.5 55mm 2 Q .~ 3wa 28$ 5.5.3.85: 5,6233% 555% i S: 3. v0.3 Exam <22 88m 535.55% Mfr—5mm 9821-152 :23350 .Em 552 2233530 moumwfim E885 ._ 25¢. 70 WW HHMv—dv—Iv—d O (\O O ‘1’ th—av—tv—au—av—Av—t v-ir-‘r-i—fle—‘v—dv—flv—ir—fle—dv—iv—l 2 5559? 555v 255 55 we wfiwctpa: 55 5 8565335 5.5555 55 .8 52588582 55 mo cocoa—95 5.559: 55 555558 5.5:— mm OO—‘OOO P~cn OOOOOOOOOOOO—‘F‘OOOOOO Ned de 36 Ed Gd VON end 9: mad ONE amd NNd SH mad wed med Ned wed omd avd omd and wvd mvd de Nvd mmd mod ado ac; cud No.5 mmd mm; mod 36 mod mod dem hwfim end de mNd mod 26 ovd Ed mfio omd vwd :d mmd vowfi vow— vom— vow— vowfi vow_ vow~ vow_ vowfi vowfi vowfi vow— vow_ vowfi vow“ vow~ vow_ vow” vowfi vowfi vow_ vow_ vow“ vow“ vow_ vow— Uomm>01~mt0~aom oufioMS—an—H u~§U< gowns—003 I Mrmnd‘a NM :o55>55ww< 53:85 3.8555 #555 555 5553‘ 555533. 555m c2552; 5552555 5552 255535an~ 553$ 5555 #2-ng 5.555» 555.0. am-m~<3 5.55o2 RC steam .5 55.5555 53 EEO 5.62 5552532 Co 5552 $55.25 55585585 2055332 Ave 5 55585885 225532 525555 555525 .8 .856 5: 555% 5:5an 5285585 .5 3:58 me: 5552 5M5 5.5552 55 5.5552 5253 cum 5 3:525 5553 5552 5253 cam 5 555—25 5552 $555.5 8:525 555: 555m 3525 55.0.: 5523 355:5 555—95 553 55.25 555m 5:35p 5.5 55555 5w5=oo was 555m 5555 588 was .553 55555 Begum SE: mm: 553 35:8 5:: 588 .5: 5532 5:35p 5:5 55555 55:55 was 5552 55:55 588 mm: 5552 35.583 _ 535R 71 mBUHE ”859 5m [\v—qq—aw—n OOOOO omen mmd wad mmd on; 3.5 26 nod 26 5N5 vof veg vow— 5cm“ 5o? 555—5555 55.53 555m 358. 55% 5:5 .55 5954 5.5m 5.555 55.5 55555: 5 5552 55.3 555 55 55am 5555550 5.55 55 555 55 5552 5.555m 5852 5554 5.55am 525: 55: :55 55:0 @8555 65:85 5 25¢ 72 mBUmm 55G h: 55 2 5» mg 5.5m 5.555 55:5 E5552 555m 6 n: ow 2 mm mm 552555232 5555 G 5: m5 3 S G 5:555 35: 5 555m 555m :5 5:55.55 :555 55: .5: 555& Av m2 ow 2 5 55 53:5 35: mm 5552 5:5 5555555 5w:5_ 5: .5 555 “H Am 2: o5 w“ R. 2 5:55.55 35: 5 555m ~555m 55 5:5555 5m:5_ 5: mm 555 Am 52 CV C 55 35 55m 5.5550 55:5 5:555 555& A _ 52 a: ow 2 mm 32 5.:m 5.55:0 55:5 E5B< 555m 8 w: ow m _ an R: 5555:5523” 5555 G we ow 2 Na N5 E55555 35: mm 555m 555 :5 55555 .555 55: 5: 555m 55 n: 55 E «a 2: 53:5 35: 3 5552 5:5 5:555 555— 5: a 555& Am 55 m5 Z 55 .1 555555 35: 5 555m 555m :5 555555 5w:5_ 5: 5% 555& Am pm 2 ow 2 mm 53 5.:m 5.55:0 55:5 5:555 55552 2 5% a: o5 m 2 mm mom 5.:m 5.55:0 55:5 E53< 555m 8 w: ow 3 nu ma 325555—32 .5555 Am 3 _ ow m: N» 2 _ 5:555 35: mm 555m 555m 55. 5:555 :555 55: 55: 555m 9. mm“ ow 3 v» we 5_w:_m 35: mm 5552 5:5 5:555 5w:5_ 5: 5% 55552 An we 55 3 E. R 5:55.55 35: mm 555m 555m Sn 5:555 5w:5_ 5: a 555m AN mm _ 55 .2 5w com 5.5m 5.55:0 55:5 5:555 555m 2 33% x52 ...... 52 :osflgfl .55m :552 55553530 73 52 855 55.5 $.55 83 53.: 555 5.5555 :3 5.55 2.5 8.55 2.5 5535 555 5.5522 $3 553 :3 R3 23 353 353 5:5 5 3:85 83 5522 :3 553 :3 33 :3 «.53 233 555 a 8:25 5502 93 353 :3 553 23 23 55285 3:85 83 555: one 53 93 $3 :3 53 558:... was .555: :3 553 353 23 553 83 55555 $on5 555: :3 33 one 53 :3 53 8508...“ 555: :3 83 23 83 553 M53 5855 55 525% 555:8 3: as“: S3 :3 £3 23 :3 553 55:8 2:8 mg 555: 83 :3 93 :3 :3 353 585% 323. 55: £5 5255: 33 553 $3 :3 93 353 328 :5: 2:8 as 5555 23 83 £3 83 93 353 8&3 Ea 85% 055:8 8: 55o: :3 :53 33 353 :3 553 55:8 2:8 a: 5522 :3 553 :3 3.3 :3 353 525% 328 :5: 22 5502 :3 553 :3 23 :3 :53 328 :52 2:8 8; 5522 353 83 53 £3 :3 £3 555 5555 553 23 353 23 :3 5.53 3:85: 355: :3 2.3 :3 53 93 :3 5525 5555 23 .83 23 83 553 83 5:555 35555 5502 23 5:3 553 :3 :3 553 555 ”5502 83 23 553 :3 :3 553 2:82: 52522 53 58 :3 33 :3 R3 585 5502 one :53 one 33 one :3 252 mm: :5 5: 55.5 R: 2.5 555 55 am :82 am :82 55 :82 Smuz SW2 moouz 55:55:45. 555 m 5555 55555.5 55552 555533 555555 55.5..“2 5356 553. 35:55: 53 553553 55555555 .55 5555555 5585 .m 5355‘ 74 00.0 50.0 05.0 05.0 00.0 55.0 00.2 05.0 20.0 05.0 00.0 05.0 00.— 05.0 55.0 50.0 02.0 20.0 00.5 02.0 25.0 00.0 02.0 05.0 20.5 00.0 2.2 00.0 00.0 05.0 00.— 00.0 00.0 02.0 00.0 50.0 25.5 00.0 05.0 50.0 00.0 50.0 00.2 25.0 02.0 00.0 05.0 00.0 00.2 00.0 00.5 20.0 02.0 00.0 00.0 02.0 00.0 52.0 02.0 52.0 00.5 20.0 02.2 0 0.0 00.0 05.0 25.2 00.0 00.5 2.0 00.0 50.0 00.5 00.0 05.0 02.0 52.0 20.0 00.2 05.0 02.0 00.0 00.0 05.0 2.— 5.5.0 00.02 00.0 52.0 20.0 05.0 00.0 00.0 50.0 00.0 00.0 00.5 00.0 00.2 00.0 00.5 00.0 50.0 00.0 55.0 50.0 00.0 50.0 .030“; 55D 5055:0550 5.8.50 555m 355— :0» 58C. 828005. 08550055 5550 52800.55 05.52588 5502 038503 E 55:22 :53 5o: 20 5550 852.0050 :85 co Ho: fl 5852 5.50550 5852 555: 5.5.05.0 955: Ho: :05 BED 55525555 825555550055. 585.550 55.808 85.50 562m 8555808— 58.550 5.500 02.5555 5.555552 5.500 015$? 5.55032 05055555 555» EEO 5532 50552550 0o 50852 385515 585505885 20:55:03 A503 585505885 29.55503 2555.50 55.800 .5550 .550 585380 582885 8 5858 55: 550552 585580 5580.085 50 25858 55: .5502 25.8000 0 535.5 75 555.5- 555.5 555.5 35555 553 258 555 5552 2555.55 555. 0 555.55 555.5 555.5 555.5 E53 255 5558.5 53.5 5.552 A555. 0 555. 0 55. 0 555.? 555.5 55.5. 555585 35855 was 5555 855. 0 5555. 0 255:. 0 555.5- 555.: 55.: 6585 5555 5555 $55.55 8:55 855.55 . 5555- 555.5- 52.5- 555 5.5.55.5 255555 555.55 555.55 555.5 555.5- 555.5 555 5.52552 2555.: 555.55 555.0 555.5- 555.5 555. _- 255558 5555 A555. 0 55.55 2555.0 555.5. 555. _- R _ .5- 5555 555.5 55. 0 2555.50 855. 0 555.? 555.51 :5? 255525 5582 5555.0 @5550 055.: 555.5- 555.5- 5551 5555 5255: 555.55 055.55 555.55 555.5- 555.5 555.5- 25555555 25 E 525555 55 555.552 2555. 0 25550 955. 0 5555. 0 555.55 555.55 555.5 555.: 555.5 552 55.5 555: 58525 55 .255 255 5c 85585 25 55.50 58 555 555.55 855.55 255 5.50 5555.55 :55 555.5 555.: 555: 555.5 555.5 5&5 25 =5 .5552: as, 55>: 55.5 255.55 55.55 555.55 555.55 55.5 55.5 55.5 55.5 - - 05555 5525555 55 5555 A50 A55 A55 A50 A55 05 2.5me u% m>om -lCoomv. 5525505N 5§M§o ANV. 50 30 5.585523% 76 5528850 5555 0:5 05.55.00-85555 5555-35.55558 50258 5885555055 =< 5555085550 8 5.55 5555 0.5508555 «5:032 53555 ”5555 552 555: 55: 55: 5555 5555 5855.55.00 2.5 55.5 55.5 555 :5 55.5 525555 5 2555.: 2555.55 2555.: 55.: 555.5 555.5 58555 555 52555 555555 555 52555 5555.0 055.0 855.55 555.5 555.: 555.5 55558 5855 555 55555 A555. 0 55550 0 55.55 555. _- 555. 5- 552- 3255 555 5555 555 52555 A555. 0 555.55 555. 0 55.: 555.2 5_ 5.55 555555 555 52555 55:8 555 52552 A555. 0 555. 0 555.55 555.5 555.5 555 .5 555555 5855 555 52552 55525 5550 55.55 6.5585 5 5555 77 .0885 Boson—5: 335 .8523 gm _ 282. :0 Q :5 U .m 3288 5 :05: 35.3.? 35:00 2: =m ova—05 mcommmoumom mm .8552 3.8883 _ 28? .3 Q 98 U .m 803on E was: 83$“? 35:8 2.: E: 3:22: maoamouwom R .moumusm $883 _ 035% .«o D 98 m Eonoom E com: 853:? 3580 2: Eco 0:22: maofimohwom on .8333,“ $888 ~ 035. :0 m 5:08 E 3%.: 85st? 3280 msocowoxo 05 5:0 0:22: mcofimouwom mm .cEBoo 5E 2: E SHE mo :2on .85an 8.“ 35:8 :5 833.3 Egan _ 053. 05 E :82— 8559? 3.980 o: 0:22: acoumouwom 5m mBUmmnfimm 5e: 52: 59: 52: 52: 52: 52: 52: 53: 59: 885.530 5.8 Rd 25 Rd 3o 85 53 2d 2.0 :.o 583qu oZ 8% 02 we? 02 mo> oZ mo> oZ 8% :Emmo comwumbcouam $2.: 62.: 2:: as: can: son: 82.: A555: 35m: a2: 59%. 33- $2- 5N2- 82- 22. Rod- 5:. 82- 83- éficocgom 8:358 88.: 38.: $8.: $8.: $8.: 68.: 53.: Ex: 28.: 2%.: Samoa aoammbfiflioom £3- 585- 83- 584m- 55m- Q55- 29m- 53m- E3- ~25- 8388a :25 8852 beam? 83.: 83.: as”: 38.: $52: 552: 85m: $55.: 68.: 55.: ofiamaocmtofioz 82- 83- $2- $3- 83- .23- £3. 8:- 559°- :3- 2258:30choamtufimim can: 3%.: A552: $52: $50.: 83.: new: $3.: $55.: 88.: Egan 3825555568 53.”. an”- 5.5. 83- 55:. $3.- 22:- N22- 33. 2:- Sffimoabwcsocmtofiaim 88.: $8.: $8.: 28.: $8.: $50.: 68.: 25.: :38 some :3- 555.? 23- 83- 83- 53.. £3 89° 23. $3 €35,220 852585 SEE: A“: 3 a: a: 3 3 A: S: 3 3 8285 .E 306.5? 83533—2 mofijmta> 385 02 20580 .uoxm £82.50 02 ”mm RA— omnv nmm— 5m< wokoum away/mac :on 93930> 282m 333m co 8:305 3583.? 38am BWMEHmm .m .2an 78 .3262 con: “kn—£56 2: 3:62 :23»:on ==m .bowoumo 3:388 05 mm G: 5:2 9220 3:6 3:83 329:8 985mm $855.8: 5 2w £95 235% finned $3885: 36 :5 30>-BBEBE 0:205 2338on Ex 5:53 W 22¢ 79 85:80 SEE—0 _ 050k .8 Q :20 U .m 08:000. 5 :03: 00320.3 80080 0.: :0 02:05 0880008.: N5 85:80. b08250 _ 050:- .8 U .80 m 08:08 E :03: 00328.» 800.80 05 2.8 0:305 accumfimé 25 0050330 E0883 2 030:- .8 M 8:00» 3 :80: 0050:? 8:80 «8:088 05 b8 0:205 0860008,: 05 .2: 28200 E 505 .8 8&2 .0808: 8.: 8.280 :5 00.50380 b05550 _ 030B 05 E :08: 0030280, .0280 02 0:205 0:20.80me 3 mgomm ”mama mg mg mg mg mg mg mg mg 08502030 mm: 8.: mm: mmd 2 .o \- 2 .0 mod 2.: ©0830m-m 02 00> 02 00> 02 00> oZ m0> £05 (8 :Em0m .3208: 2%.: 28.: Boa- $3- 2%.: 22.: $5.: 0%.: 2.8.: 2: 2.: $2- $0.: 28.: 83.: 58.0- 552. 950.: 2500.: 2%.: 2a.: 052- 23- 28.: 22.: 52.5- ~53- .G5o: 28.: wmad- 03.n- 68: 2%.: :3- $3- 23.: as: 98.: as: 30.0. 83. 50: 25%.: 82- 3.0- 68.: 28.: 35.: no: 28.: 232.: $2- 586 $05.: 655.: A58. : 258.: E:- 53.0.- 62: 22.: 052- EN- 2:: 252.: E:- 52. Gmw: 250.: 331 E5. 2505.: 285.: KEEOESSH «E085 G 80008 30: 3 8:08 300m 82 80008 802 8>02 was 0050a 95 29:0 302 2 8502 :8 E0000: 8w22 o: E 850m Am E0008 30: 0: 850m 300m :5 E0000: 00w8_ 0: £ 853 Am 3m: 33.: >2:- wvod wvwd Nwmd m 2 _.m 555 £05 0.220 0050 0:05.08 0050: C A: S G: E 25: 2: A: 22: 005mg; =< m030tw> 38.5 02 NBC £00080 0:0:0uoxm £00080 02 Sn :0 05m at. 0080m AMA->55: 000B aha—«5003’ 088E 38200: .mzom 8 0020000: 3:88: .8 0000.5: :00mfizmm .o 030:- 80 0003000 com: 030—56 08 02:00.0 :20000w00 ==m .bowgao 008288 0% 0m 8: 505 0.230 0050 00:00pm 0:03:80 0.850% 0000:8000: E 08 08000 8.50530 “030% 028830 38 was 80>-30MZ005 08205 0860003210. 80:8: 0 030.0 81 0300380 b08850 _ 030B .8 Q 28 U .m 0.83000 5 8000: 0033-8.» 8580 0:0 :0 00205 0.860003% 0 0030380 g0 _ 030B .8 U 98 m 083000 5 v0.0: 0030:? 80:80 05 38 02:05 0:2000uw0m 00 00303000 3085200 2 050R. .8 m 8:000 E @000: 0050:? 80080 0280808 05 >28 08205 0:2000uwox v 3. A C 5880 E 5.25 8 8&8 .0823 08 80:80 :5 0020830 0085820 _ 0302. 020 E 0000: 0050.5; 8.580 0: 09:05 0=om0000m0m 0v Em Gm mmd omd 02 00> 50.: 800.: 002- 2:0- 800: 200.: 000.0. 000.0- 220: 2.00.: 003. 000.0- 200: A005: 003 2- 000.:- 83: 600.: gm Gm de mmd 02 00> 800.: 2000.: 000.2. 003- 800.: 20:0: 002.0- 002.0- 2000.: 600.: 03.0- :03 88.3 2000.: 000.0- 00::- 202: 600.: Gm Gm mmd end 02 00> 200.: 2000.: 30.2- 000.2- 20:0: 200.: 000.0- 83w 800.: 2000.: $2.0 000.0 800.: 800.3 30.8- 22 2- 2.00.: 200.: 5w Gm 2.2 mfio 02 00> 23.0.: 800.: 200.2- 20.2- 28.: 20:: 000.0- 0:0. 20.00.: 2.00.: 003 000.0 50.: 6:: 80.0- 0:1:- 288 2000.: 085030020 megawm £05 .8 8&0]: .0808: 820800200 6:35 20 80008 Bo: 0m 005$ 32% :5 80008 c002 8000: 00; 850» 20 29:0 Bo: 0_ 8:82 0:0 80008 0082 o: 0_ 850m Am 80008 30: 0_ 850m 300m :5 80008 882 o: 00 850% Am 00:. 000.2- 000.2- 08.0- 08.2 30.2 000.0 003. €50.28853502305002 20: E G: A: 2.: A: A: 2: 00502802 =< 003010 > .085 02 N80 08.580 08:0moxm 08580 02 3A— 30 3m— 9.4 008,0m AMA->23 000H,ia_:nmoo>. .0883 38200: hrmg 0020005 108003 .8 080.03 80006301: .h 03I0H 82 000800 can: 050—820 08 00800.— 22000030 :25 58880 80203008 05 0m A8 503 08:20 0030 0020020 0008600 0.8fimm 000052089 E 08 08.8 «0882-80 annex 028830 38 can 80%-308805 08305 022000.230 =< m>>UnE 88D @083 0 050-0 83 .0020? 0050 =0 88 0:805 5050085 58 00203 0050 =0 88 5.5008: 05 5 080550 58 0:38 .8 855:: 090820 05 .0083 0050 =0 88 00: mg .0523 .555 0.550 0050 8500500 0.8585 00 :03 00 05005800 08:50.0 H 0.50-H. .8 Q 58 U .Q 008800 5 @000: 00558> 8580 05 :0 05:05 0.880030% 02 00> 820.: 0000.: 000.0- 000.2- 02.: 0002.: 002.0- 000.0- 2.00.: A000,: 000.0- 0:0- 2.000: A000: 000.:- 020.:- 5000: 600.: 02 00> 2000.00 2000.00 000.0 200.2 200.00 85.00 020.2 000.2 c.0000 200$ 00: 000.2 200.00 2000.00 020.02-000.02- .00000 800.: 02 00> 220.200.: 000.0- 000.0- 0000.: 0000.: 000.0- 10.0- 5000: 0000.: 002.0- 000.0- 2.00.: 0000.: 000.0- 000.0- 000: 200.: 9. .0550? maze-=00 5850: 5509 005503 08 850 “05005800 b08550 _ 050B .8 Q 58 U .Q 0083000 5 0000: 00550? 8580 05 :0 05:05 00500080M 00. 00500.. E: 80 55500 n 0.50-H- 00 0500 05 08 005000 0.:- .00:0.z£0 b05550 _ 030B .8 Q 58 D .Q 08800 5 0000: 0035:; 8580 05 :0 05:05 0.500080% 5. 0:8 00 02000 05:08:50m 2580.5 8 80008 30: 0_ 855 500m :5 500.00% 2005 00:0: 00; 850m 90 0550 30: 05 8:82 58 02000.3 8M=o~ 0: 05 850mm Am 80008 Bo: 05 855 500m :5 300.00%.00m80 0: 0_ 858m AN omwd- and- 02.n- Nwed- 20.6.- ommd- £55 0.550 0050 8000.5 858-.” 2 3: A00 2.: A: A: 20 0:508:00 880005 05503 5:58 Z W503 0.880080% ==m 000 00m $0.. I 050050 000E050M .m 053. 84 000800 :00: 05588 08 005000 05000800 :0: 008880 85:08 05 5 GV 555 0.550 0050 008050 088500 085$ 085005 5 08 08.8 585800 0.0507505. 000058080 5 08 0808 0.85800 8.50% ADV 58 $3 080 55500 5 0080008 005000800 05 5 000505 08 0050550 38 ”0055550 80%-3053005 05505 0005000800 =< .mBUmm 88G NmNH NmS mom _ 82 wow“ vow~ 05588050 wad mmd wmd 2nd _ Rd 5.2 008255 90.803 w 2an 85 Table 9. Summary Statistics Obs. Mean Std. Deviation Min --- Max (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) Dependent Outcomes PPVT-R 1541 86.374 (17.028) 40 137 Aggression 1986 0.579 (0.340) 0 1.923 ODD Symptoms 1999 0.631 (0.429) 0 2 Variables of Interest Incarcerated Ex Post 2549 0.097 (0.296) 0 1 Ever-Incarcerated 2549 0.377 (0.485) 0 1 Incarcerated Ex Ante 2549 0.076 (0.265) 0 1 Father Absent 2549 0.434 (0.496) 0 1 Exogenous Variables Male 2549 0.520 (0.500) 0 1 Birth Order 2543 2.092 (1.273) 1 13 Mother's Age 2549 28.351 (6.128) 17 48 Father's Age 2547 30.768 (7.045) 18 66 Mother Black 2549 0.466 (0.499) 0 1 Mother Hispanic 2549 0.235 (0.424) 0 1 Other 2549 0.185 (0.388) 0 1 Father Black 2549 0.485 (0.500) 0 1 Father Hispanic 2549 0.240 (0.427) 0 1 Other 2549 0.275 (0.447) 0 1 Mother Interviewed in Spanish 2549 0.073 (0.261) 0 1 ADHD Symptoms 2549 0.523 (0.574) 0 2 ADHD Symptoms (Missing) 2549 0.424 (0.494) 0 1 Mother was born in US. 2549 0.869 (0.337) 0 1 Father was born in US. 2549 0.804 (0.397) 0 1 Other Independent Variables Mother < High School Degree 2549 0.255 (0.436) 0 1 Mother has High School Degree 2549 0.313 (0.464) 0 1 Mother < College Degree 2549 0.233 (0.423) 0 1 Mother has at least College Degree 2549 0.136 (0.343) 0 1 Father < High School Degree 2549 0.229 (0.420) 0 1 Father has High School Degree 2549 0.348 (0.476) 0 1 Father < College Degree 2549 0.184 (0.387) 0 1 Father has at least Coll. Degree 2549 0.123 (0.328) 0 1 Household Income per Person (103) 2549 7.85 (13.57) 0 333.33 Household Income per Person (Missing) 2549 0.072 (0.258) 0 1 Father Smokes 2549 0.426 (0.495) 0 1 Father Smokes (Missing) 2549 0.069 (0.254) 0 1 Father Drinks 2549 0.696 (0.460) 0 1 86 Table 9 (cont’d) Father Drinks (Missing) 2549 0.169 (0.375) 0 1 Mother Smokes 2548 0.261 (0.439) 0 1 Mother Drinks 2548 0.344 (0.475) 0 1 Mother has Mental Problems 2549 0.049 (0.215) 0 1 Father has Mental Problems 2549 0.017 (0.130) 0 1 Father Figure Present 2549 0.109 (0.312) 0 1 Number of Residential Moves 2547 1.192 (1.310) 0 11 Child was Breastfed 2549 0.569 (0.495) 0 1 Mother's WAIS—R Score 2543 7.011 (2.572) 0 15 Father's WAIS-R Score 2524 6.664 (2.661) 0 15 Paternal Importance 2530 1.089 (0.158) 1 3 Either Parent Reports Aggravation 2549 0.217 (0.412) 0 1 Religiosity Measure 2549 4.229 (1.564) 0 7 Child will not have Father's Last Name 2549 0.104 (0.305) 0 1 Father's Name is not on Birth Certificate 2549 0.061 (0.239) 0 1 Father did not visit Hospital at Child's Birth 2549 0.099 (0.299) 0 1 Mother considered Abortion 2539 0.249 (0.432) '0 1 Father suggested Abortion 2538 0.080 (0.271) 0 1 Time Mother loiew Father prior to Pregnancy 2549 5.091 (4.791) 0 36 Instruments Father's Impulsivity 2548 3.019 (0.663) 1 4 Average Non-Traffic Stops 2549 0.406 (0.092) 0.125 0.8 Father's Biological Father Involved 2549 0.312 (0.464) 0 1 Father had Father-Figure 2549 0.668 (0.471) 0 1 Data: FFCWS 87 Table 10. Summgy Statistics by Incarceration Histog . Ever-Incarcerated Never Incarcerated Obs. Mean SD Obs. Mean SD Dependent Outcomes PPVT-R 677 83.780 (16.084) 864 88.406 (17.474) Aggression 788 0.639 (0.368) 1198 0.539 (0.314) ODD Symptoms 796 0.678 (0.454) 1203 0.599 (0.409) Variables of Interest Father Absent 960 0.646 (0.479) 1589 0.305 (0.461) Exogenous Variables Male 960 0.501 (0.500) 15 89 0.532 (0.499) Birth Order 956 2.259 (1.434) 1587 1.991 (1.154) Mother's Age 960 26.316 (5.276) 1589 29.581 (6.280) Father's Age 959 29.016 (6.384) 1588 31.826 (7.213) Mother Black 960 0.581 (0.494) 1589 0.396 (0.489) Mother Hispanic 960 0.204 (0.403) 1589 0.253 (0.435) Other 960 0.143 (0.350) 1589 0.210 (0.408) Father Black 960 0.605 (0.489) 1589 0.412 (0.492) Father Hispanic 960 0.218 (0.413) 1589 0.254 (0.435) Other 960 0.177 (0.382) 1589 0.335 (0.472) Mother Interviewed in Spanish 960 0.039 (0.193) 1589 0.094 (0.292) ADHD Symptoms 960 0.499 (0.565) 1589 0.538 (0.579) ADHD Symptoms (Missing) 960 0.446 (0.497) 1589 0.410 (0.492) Mother was born in U.S. 960 0.941 (0.236) 1589 0.826 (0.379) Father was born in U.S. 960 0.864 (0.343) 1589 0.768 (0.422) Other Independent Vflbles Mother< High School Degree 960 0.359 (0.480) 1589 0.193 (0.394) Mother has High School Degree 960 0.370 (0.483) 1589 0.278 (0.448) Mother< College Degree 960 0.182 (0.386) 1589 0.263 (0.440) Mother has at least College Degree 960 0.022 (0.146) 1589 0.205 (0.404) Father< High School Degree 960 0.334 (0.472) 1589 0.165 (0.371) Father has High School Degree 960 0.406 (0.491) 1589 0.313 (0.464) Father< College Degree 960 0.116 (0.320) 1589 0.225 (0.417) Father has at least College Degree 960 0.009 (0.096) 1589 0.191 (0.393) Household Income per Person (103) 960 4.38 (6.15) 1589 9.95 (16.16) Household Income/Person (Missing) 960 0.096 (0.295) 1589 0.057 (0.232) Father Smokes 960 0.599 (0.490) 1589 0.322 (0.467) Father Smokes (Missing) 960 0.083 (0.277) 1589 0.060 (0.23 8) Father Drinks 960 0.675 (0.469) 1589 0.708 (0.455) Father Drinks (Missing) 960 0.217 (0.412) 1589 0.140 (0.347) Mother Smokes 960 0.385 (0.487) 1588 0.185 (0.389) 88 Table 10 (cont’d) Mother Drinks 960 0.340 (0.474) 1588 0.347 (0.476) Mother has Mental Problems 960 0.066 (0.248) 1589 0.038 (0.192) Father has Mental Problems 960 0.017 (0.128) 1589 0.018 (0.132) Father Figure Present 960 0.122 (0.327) 1589 0.101 (0.302) Number of Residential Moves 958 1.531 (1.487) 1589 0.987 (1.144) Child was Breastfed 960 0.440 (0.497) 1589 0.647 (0.478) Mother's WAIS-R Score 959 6.766 (2.435) 1584 7.159 (2.640) Father's WAIS-R Score 950 6.409 (2.524) 1574 6.817 (2.730) Paternal Importance 955 1.094 (0.177) 1575 1.086 (0.145) Either Parent Reports Aggravation 960 0.251 (0.434) 1589 0.196 (0.397) Religiosity 960 4.453 (1.61 1) 1589 4.093 (1.519) Child will not have Father's Last Name 960 0.157 (0.364) 15 89 0.071 (0.257) Father's Name not on Birth Certificate 960 0.089 (0.284) 1589 0.044 (0.205) Father did not visit Hospital at Birth 960 0.172 (0.377) 1589 0.055 (0.229) Mother considered Abortion 956 0.333 (0.471) 1583 0.198 (0.398) Father suggested Abortion 956 0.106 (0.308) 1582 0.064 (0.245) Time knew father prior to Pregnancy 960 4.026 (4.035) 1589 5.735 (5.089) Instruments Father's Impulsivity 959 2.838 (0.721) 1589 3.128 (0.599) Average Non-Traffic Stops 960 0.431 (0.083) 1589 0.391 (0.094) Father's Biological Father Involved 960 0.371 (0.483) 1589 0.277 (0.448) Father had Father-Figure 960 0.663 (0.473) 1589 0.672 (0.470) Data: FFCWS 89 Table 11. Average Non-Traffic Stops by City and Race/Ethnicity Average Non-Traffic Black Hispanic Other Stops per City 0.43 City 1 0.46 0.43 0.29 (0.05) 0.38 City 2 0.39 0.40 0.32 (0.03) 0.43 City 3 0.48 0.21 0.27 (0.10) 0.44 City 4 0.47 0.38 0.30 (0.06) 0.32 City 5 0.34 0.30 0.24 (0.03) 0.34 City 6 0.41 0.36 0.13 (0.12) 0.44 City 7 0.47 0.43 0.37 (0.05) 0.43 City 8 0.38 0.48 0.29 (0.08) 0.47 City 9 0.57 0.54 0.34 (0.11) 0.47 City 10 0.50 0.50 0.38 (0.06) 0.33 City 11 0.33 0.35 0.26 (0.03) 0.36 mg 12 0.36 0.39 0.30 (0.04) 0.38 City 13 0.44 0.31 0.32 (0.06) 0.39 City 14 0.39 0.20 0.42 (0.05) 0.42 City 15 0.50 0.20 0.20 (0.13) 0.46 City 16 0.42 0.8050 0.47 (0.08) 0.48 City 17 0.48 0.55 0.46 (0.03) 0.39 City 18 0.30 0.43 0.21 (0.08) 0.39 City 19 0.42 — 0.37 (0.02) 0.35 City 20 0.31 0.6351 0.38 (0.09) Average Non-Traffic Stops 0.44 0.40 0.32 0.40 Per Race/Ethnicity (0.07) (0.08) (0.08) (0.09) 50 There are only 5 observations used for this mean (excluded from Figure 1.1). 51 There are only 8 observations used for this mean (excluded from Figure 1.1). 90 Table 1 l (cont’d) Data: FFCWS Notes: The table presents mean averages of non-traffic stops in each city by father’s racial or ethnic group. Due to privacy reasons, the author is unable to identify the cities in the sample by name. Standard deviations are in parentheses. 91 Table 12. OLS Estimates of the Marginal Impact of Paternal Incarceration on Outcomes PPVT—R AGGRESSION _O_D_12 (l) (2) (3) Incarcerated Ex Post -2.057 0.002 0.028 (1 .608) (0.03 9) (0.044) Ever-Incarcerated52 1 .869 0.050 0.001 (1.397) (0.029) (0.034) Father Absent -1 .097 0.027 0.046 (0.838) (0.017) (0.020) Mother Interviewed in Spanish 4087 -0.067 -0.065 (2.571) (0.043) (0.051) Male -2.033 0.036 0.034 (0.698) (0.014) (0.017) Birth Order -0.622 0.012 0.003 (0.334) (0.007) (0.008) ADHD Symptoms 0.362 -0.005 -0.034 (1.027) (0.020) (0.023) ADI-ID (missing) 0.758 0.003 -0.046 (1.180) (0.023) (0.027) Mother's Age 0.130 -0.001 -0.001 (0.1 18) (0.002) (0.003) F ather's Age 0085 -0.002 -0.002 (0.086) (0.002) (0.002) Mother Black -4.21 5 -0.068 -0. 153 (l .463) (0.032) (0.03 7) Mother Hispanic -3.598 0.027 -0.035 (1.473) (0.033) (0.03 8) Father Black -2.137 0.112 0.123 (1.495) (0.030) (0.03 7) Father Hispanic 0.023 0.035 0.029 (1.570) (0.031) (0.037) Mother Born in U.S. 3.427 0.170 0.093 (7.148) (0.070) (0.1 19) Father Born in U.S. 0.332 -0.007 -0.027 (1.615) (0.046) (0.051) Child was Breastfed 2.140 0.039 0.051 (0.769) (0.01 7) (0.020) Mother< High School Degree 0.557 0.019 0.027 (0.921) (0.021) (0.024) Mother< College Degree 3.809 0.018 0.015 (1.027) (0.021) (0.025) Mother has at least College Degree 9.841 0.014 0.016 (1.83 5) (0.028) (0.03 7) Mother's WAIS-R Score 0.374 -0.006 -0.001 52 This indicator excludes those fathers who though reported as incarcerated at some point, it could not be determined whether they were incarcerated ex ante or ex post. 92 Table 12 (cont’d) Father's WAIS-R Score Father< High School Degree F ather< College Degree Father has at least College Degree Father Smokes Father Smokes (Missing) Father Drinks Father Drinks (Missing) Mother Smokes Mother Drinks Mother has Mental Problems Father has Mental Problems Father Figure Present Household Income per Person/ 10,000 Household Income per Person (Missing) Number of Residential Moves Paternal Importance Either Parent Reports Aggravation Religiosity Child will not have Father's Last Name Father's Name is not on Birth Certificate Father did not visit Hospital at Child's Birth Mother considered Abortion Father suggested Abortion (0.160) 0.354 (0.149) -1344 (0.905) 0.178 (1.034) 0.891 (1.787) 0.633 (0.871) -2215 (1.753) 0.725 (1.212) 2.281 (1.660) 0.504 (0.908) 1.790 (0.797) 0.301 (1.497) -0.385 (2.082) 0.672 (1.203) 0.892 (0.446) -0.386 (1.431) 0.288 (0.275) 5.998 (2.216) -1.629 (0.868) 0.153 (0.238) 0.228 (1.162) —1.599 (1.492) 0.950 (1.264) 1.946 (0.827) 0.190 (1.230) 93 (0.003) 0.004 (0.003) 0.036 (0.021) 0.039 (0.021) 0.006 (0.028) 0.019 (0.017) 0.026 (0.047) 0.018 (0.021) 0.022 (0.034) 0.072 (0.020) 0.007 (0.016) 0.053 (0.035) 0.082 (0.066) 0.065 (0.026) -0.006 (0.005) 0.059 (0.029) 0.008 (0.006) 0.013 (0.046) 0.120 (0.018) 0.013 (0.005) 0.021 (0.029) 0.014 (0.036) 0.01 1 (0.029) 0.022 (0.018) 0.069 (0.032) (0.004) 0.001 (0.004) 0.027 (0.024) 0.015 (0.025) 0.006 (0.034) 0.010 (0.021) 0.043 (0.056) 0.038 (0.026) 0.009 (0.043) 0.066 (0.023) 0.026 (0.019) 0.072 (0.043) 0.168 (0.072) 0.021 (0.030) 0.001 (0.007) 0.097 (0.032) 0.004 (0.007) 0.050 (0.056) 0.1 12 (0.021) 0.020 (0.006) 0.029 (0.036) 0.031 (0.044) 0.001 (0.033) 0.006 (0.022) 0.093 (0.036) Table 12 (cont’d) Time Mother knew Father prior to Pregnancy 0.148 -0.001 -0.000 (0.089) (0.002) (0.002) R_Squared 0.23 0.12 0.1 1 Observations 1804 2143 2401 Data: FF CWS. Notes: All regressions include city, region-of-birth and interview-year dummies. Robust standard errors are in parentheses. 94 .5000 .6285 “Eamon 000500qu 000 600$ 050083.04 AMA—5%: 0056M I 000,—. b0500oo> 050E .6000?“ ”0050050 250 w530=0¢5 05 :0 0000 5 0050000005 .0583 .«0 00005 505505 05 5 80:20.50 030.600 00 0050 5 000% 5050:5000 0.0050.“ .3 0000055 80 005000535 .2 030B 5 0080—050 6005 05000500 05 00 003050.» 30500 0500 05 00505 0005000500 =< 000050800 5 5050 5000000 550% @593 50D mad _~.o cud Nine mum amv :38 $8.8 $3.8 80.0 50.0 :3- :38 $8.8 $8.8 $0.0 E; 80.0 a: _.8 :88 88.8 0:0- 08.0- 080. Ed omd mmd now 3.0 gm 0.3.8 888 05.0.: :00 ~80 08.0- 88.8 :88 5.0.8 80.0- $3 :2- $8.8 $8.8 0.8.8 8 3 :20- 0000 26 3.0 id 3: m2: 000 88.8 0.88 98.: :50 Rod 22. :88 $0.8 2%.: 30.0 5.0 80.0 800.8 a 3.8 306. 8 80.0 :00 so..- 00000005 005030000 500304 0055* VOHMHOOHNOELO>M “mom xm 0800000005 A8 98 S COO .OO< MACKE hoiao 8 5 A8 QDO .OG< M???“ 5:095m b_5m5m\000m 0.0050; 3.008 WWW—5000000005 _0580mww!00005m 35502 05 .50 8055a .2 030,—. 6 Q :0 000 .09. 0-55 0.8.0 95 0560030: >_ 000:: 5 08:05:55 00: 5 £000 00 0:0 0:0:0w0x0 on 8 0050000 5 8:030 :0500: Agnew 300: 0:0 .50 9:058 05 5:53 0080 0505-50: 0w0:0>0v w5an 05008 95 0:0 2an b_>m0_:aE_ 0.3500. 5 “0:0 50808005 30:80: 0:0:0wo0:0 :8 8:052:05 8:- 0.0 000.8 000.8 000.8 000.0 000.0 000.0- 08.8 00:08 0008 0.0.0- 000.0- :00 000.8 000.8 820.8 000.0 3.0.0 :00 2.00.8 8008 200.8 000.0 000.0 000.0- @008 000.8 200.8 000.0- 000.0- 3.0.0- 200.8 000.8 €008 000.0- 000.0- 3.00- :0008 800.8 200.8 2.0.0- 22 000.0 000.8 0000.8 000.8 00008 0000.8 000.8 200.0- 000.0- 000.0- 8008 2000.8 @008 000.0 000.0 000.0 50.8 020.8 50.8 000.0 :00 00.0.0 000.8 0000.8 0008 0:00 000.0 000.0- $008 0008 0000.8 30.0 0:0.0 000.0 0000.8 000.8 000.8 000.0 000.0 000.0- 088 0008 E08 080- 000.0- 000.0 2.008 8:08 8008 «mod Nth Sod :058 .00_w0_5n 5: 00: 003 0:3 8&8 :058 0 00: :0508 cc 0:0 0000—0—50 w5:=0 8: 02 5 00>—g5 b0> 003 :058 0003803 010500— 5 ”0:0 8:030 :058 0:0:0w00:0 :8 8:05.505 BC. .308» 300: 0:0 .50 0.:058 05 5:53 0080 0508-5: ow0:0>0v 3:80:— 3008 95 0:0 299 550595 0.5.00.8 8 ”0:0 5:800:35 _0::80: 0:0:0wo05 :8 00:05:50.5 8E- 00 9:8 000.8 :08 000.0 000.0 000.0 200.8 900.8 800.8 0002. 000.0- 000.:- A0008 300.00 50.8 000.0- 000.0- 000.0- 2.00.00 0000.00 88.00 000.0- 000.0- 000.0- 82: 0000.00 800.: 000.0 000.: 000.0 0000.: 800.00 08.: 50 000.0- :00 2.00. :0 800.000 05.8 5.0.: 000.3. 000.0- 0000.: 8 00028 8.00.: 03.00.000.00- 02.:- 0w< 000582 :00:O 5:5 0.02 5.050% 5 0035385 :0502 E50506 058:5»m 9.5000 05805.5 Q3900. 0:030. :0508 000m 00m 08038005 00.3 3.9 - cm.» 00.0 - ems No.0 - 505005-00 00.0-- 550303 - 30.: - - $0.: - - 3.. I . - 500800 .5508 I 020008-": 0w00m805m 00.3 00.»: - 30ng mud— - vwdm m2: - 5350800:— I 505008-00 0805-00500 A8 :00 E :00 :00 2.0 2.0 :00 20 002 002 ma—O v0>~ 00>— mAO 00E 22 WE - QQO ZO—mmfldww< mum->03 H050800 0:005:05 0050850 250 :0 000m :8 80808005 .80 008.5 05 .00 0:50-00:38 >8 0:0 m-HO .3 030B 96 £82823. m-HO Sm com: 8 29:8 mix—£8 >_ 2:- 86:33: mag—Eu of E wE>= Ho: mm on GE: >5 8 a “comp“ 3 809mm“? 8 353 6:5 05 .80 80:. .5 =a 80508 :2: FEB wE>= 82220 8 833me 8 29:8 £9298 2: .385588 E 08 80:0 83858 8.50% 83.556 :85 80 SEE Eco-an 8.5 .80» 0832: 8.288on =< 8802 may: ”88 88 88 88 88 88 88 82 82 £2 223388 - - 88 - - 88 - - :8 8:2ch 388 38.8 38.8 :88- 888 388. 32.8 38.8 68.8 82 8:8 9:8 388 38.8 28.8 888 888- 88.8. 888 88.8 38.8 888 888 83 38.8 38.8 88.8 888- 888- 88.8- 9.88 68.8 38.8 83.8- 838- 83- 38.8 38.8 38.8 :58- :88 :58- :88 $8.8 88.8 38.8 v8.8 888 38.8 32.8 38.8 :38 $38 58 38.8 38.8 38.8 888 .88 28.8 38.8 38.8 38.8 888 88 :88 38.8 38.8 38.8 88.8 88.8 888 38.8 38.8 38.8 888- 8.8. 8.8.8- 388 38.8 38.8 :88- :88- :88- 388 38.8 388 a; 838 82. 388 38.88 22.8 33.38.:- 838- 388 688 3:8 83. :88- 22- 388 88.8 G88 82. $38- 8%- 388 38.8 888 88.n- 83- 83.n- 38. 8 38.8 $8. 8 83-88.8- 88.8- ::.8 38.8 88.8 83- 888- 8.8- .m.D E Eon 83 3588 .m.D E Eon $3 .0522 2535 805mm xosm .5538 259-5 5522 gum—m 5502 ow< mmofiam 8.288 E 2%; 97 Table 15. Summary Statistics Variables % M20 .39. Min MA Union Dissolved at the End of Each Spell 5768 0.16 0.37 0 1 Duration of the Union at the Start of Spell 5768 4.90 3.89 0 27.33 0-1 Year Interval 5786 0.04 0.19 0 1 1-3 Year Interval 5768 0.33 0.47 0 1 3-5 Year Interval 5768 0.23 0.42 O 1 5-7 Year Interval 5768 0.14 0.35 0 1 7-10 Year Interval 5768 0.13 0.34 0 1 10-12 Year Interval 5768 0.05 0.22 0 1 12 Plus Year Interval 5768 0.07 0.25 0 1 Time between interviews (Months) 5768 19.53 5.61 3 37 Time-Invariant Age Difference 2267 2.50 4.76 -15 32 Age Difference Squared 2267 28.84 65.04 0 1024 Mother Black 2450 0.34 0.48 0 1 Mother White 2450 0.30 0.46 0 1 Mother Hispanic 2450 0.31 0.46 0 1 Other 2450 0.05 0.21 0 1 Less than HS 2450 0.34 0.47 0 1 High School 2450 0.23 0.42 O 1 Some College 2450 0.26 0.44 0 1 College 2450 0.17 0.37 0 1 Dad Very Involved 2450 0.40 0.49 0 1 Dad Little Involved 2450 0.35 0.48 0 1 Dad Not Involved 2450 0.23 0.42 0 1 Father Figure Present 2450 0.24 0.42 0 1 No Religion 2450 0.09 0.29 0 1 Catholic 2450 0.35 0.48 0 1 Jewish 2450 0.02 0.12 0 1 Muslim 2450 0.02 0.12 0 1 Baptist 2450 0.20 0.40 0 1 Other Christian 2450 0.32 0.47 0 1 Freq. of Church Attendance 2445 2.88 1.36 1 5 Mother’s Impulsivity Scale 2241 3.01 0.60 1 4 Father’s Impulsivity Scale 1817 3.06 0.65 1 4 Focal Child is a Boy 2450 0.52 0.50 0 1 First Child 2450 0.26 0.44 0 1 Mother's Age at Baseline 2450 26.38 6.08 15 43 Out of Wedlock Childbirth 2450 0.58 0.49 0 1 Marriage within 6 mths prior to birth 2450 0.02 0.14 0 1 Time-Varying Number of Children in HH 5754 1.78 1.33 0 10 Number of Adults in HH 5753 2.33 0.82 l 9 98 Table 15 (cont’d) Cohabit only 5768 0.45 0.50 0 1 Pre-Marital Cohabitation 5768 0.27 0.44 0 1 Married Only 5768 0.28 0.45 0 1 New Birth Shock 5768 0.76 0.43 0 1 Lagged Changes Residential Move 33 18 0.37 0.48 O 1 New Incarceration 3316 0.02 0.13 0 1 New Release 3316 0.02 0.13 0 1 Household Income (103) 3318 45.35 47.54 0 935.87 Change in Household Income (103) 3316 -O.61 73.73 -906.86 923.70 Data: FFCWS Note: The time-invariant summary statistics are calculated using the baseline sample only; all other summary statistics use the entire analysis sample. 99 Table 16. Linear Probability Estimates of the Correlates of Union Dissolution (1) (2) (3) (4) 1-3 Year Interval -0.031 -0.021 -0.007 (0.036) (0.035) (0.035) . 3-5 Year Interval -0.049 -0.018 0.013 0.031 (0.03 6) (0.03 6) (0.03 7) (0.022) 5-7 Year Interval -0.144 —0.082 -0.037 -0.015 (0.03 6) (0.03 7) (0.03 8) (0.024) 7-10 Year Interval -0.138 —0.056 -0.013 -0.002 (0.036) (0.03 8) (0.040) (0.026) 10-12 Year Interval —0. 191 -0.099 —0.043 -0.029 (0.03 7) (0.03 8) (0.040) (0.028) 12 Plus Year Interval -0.187 -0.082 -0.024 0.007 (0.03 7) (0.039) (0.041) (0.030) Interval Length 0.005 0.005 0.007 0.007 (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) (0.003) Age Difference -0.004 -0.004 -0.001 (0.002) (0.002) (0.002) Age Difference Squared 0.000 0.000 0.000 (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) Mother Black 0.086 0.068 0.052 - (0.016) (0.016) (0.020) Mother Hispanic -0.009 -0.017 0.004 (0.015) (0.016) (0.020) Other 0.009 0.014 -0.007 (0.021) (0.021) (0.025) Less than HS 0.044 0.036 0.015 (0.017) (0.017) (0.023) Some College 0.008 0.016 0.022 (0.016) (0.016) (0.021) College 0035 -0.006 0.014 (0.016) (0.016) (0.022) Dad Very Involved —0.009 -0.010 -0.017 (0.018) (0.018) (0.023) Dad Little Involved -0.018 -0.012 0.004 (0.016) (0.016) (0.022) Father Figure Present 0.000 -0.003 0.006 (0.016) (0.016) (0.021) No Religion . 0.047 0.040 0.032 (0.023) (0.023) (0.030) Jewish -0.002 0.020 0.053 (0.027) (0.027) (0.03 8) Muslim -0.021 -0.007 0.010 (0.040) (0.03 9) (0.055) Baptist 0.043 0.039 0.063 (0.019) (0.019) (0.025) Other Christian 0.008 0.017 0.009 (0.013) (0.013) (0.017) 100 Table 16 (cont’d) Freq. of Church Attendance 0.001 -0.007 -0.008 (0.004) (0.005) (0.006) Mother Impulsivity Scale -0.040 -0.036 —0.041 (0.010) (0.010) (0.013) Father Impulsivity Scale -0.041 -0.040 -0.034 (0.009) (0.009) (0.012) Focal Child is a Boy 0025 -0.024 -0.007 (0.011) (0.011) (0.014) First Child -0.021 -0.008 -0.000 (0.013) (0.014) (0.019) Mother's Age at Baseline -0.005 -0.005 -0.004 (0.001) (0.001) (0.002) Out of Wedlock Childbirth 0.032 0.026 (0.025) (0.027) Marriage within 6 mths prior to birth -0.020 0.002 (0.032) (0.043) Number of Children in HH 0.001 -0.001 (0.005) (0.008) Number of Adults in HH -0.008 -0.010 (0.008) (0.011) Cohabit only 0.094 0.084 (0.027) (0.030) Pre-Marital Cohabitation 0.01 1 0.009 (0.012) (0.016) New Birth Shock 0.016 0.000 (0.017) (0.020) Residential Move 0.017 (0.015) New Incarceration 0.508 (0.068) New Release 0.001 (0.047) Change in H. Income Def/ 10000 —0.004 (0.001) Observations 4108 4108 4095 2474 R-squared 0.03 0.09 0.1 1 0.14 Data: FFCWS Robust standard errors are in parentheses. The excluded category for the duration intervals is 0 to 3 years in column (4). Since lagged variables are included in this model, only 18 couples who have been together for less than one year are included in this regressiOn. Subsequently, using the 0 — 1 year interval as the sole excluded category will induce problems similar to a dummy variable trap. 101 Table 17. Linear Probability Estimates for Low-Educated and Black Mothers Mother Low-Educated Mother Black (1) (2) (3) (4) 1-3 Year Interval -0.009 0.012 (0.043) . (0.067) . 3-5 Year Interval 0.040 0.072 0.012 0.013 (0.047) (0.031) (0.070) (0.043) 5-7 Year Interval -0.036 -0.003 -0.028 0.012 (0.049) (0.03 7) (0.074) (0.052) 7-10 Year Interval 0.031 0.051 0.008 —0.032 (0.05 3) (0.044) (0.078) (0.056) 10-12 Year Interval -0.042 -0.040 -0.041 -0.069 (0.056) (0.043) (0.088) (0.070) 12 Plus Year Interval -0.017 0.047 -0.073 -0.046 (0.056) (0.050) (0.078) (0.059) Interval Length 0.007 0.009 0.01 1 0.01 1 (0.002) (0.004) (0.003) (0.005) Age Difference -0.008 -0.004 -0.005 -0.004 (0.003) (0.003) (0.004) (0.005) Age Difference Squared 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) Mother Black 0.076 0.045 0.000 0.000 (0.025) (0.03 3) (0.000) (0.000) Mother Hispanic -0.027 -0.030 0.000 0.000 (0.025) (0.03 1) (0.000) (0.000) Other 0.012 -0.055 0.000 0.000 (0.052) (0.05 7) (0.000) (0.000) Less than HS 0.038 0.019 0.039 0.075 (0.018) (0.024) (0.03 3) (0.044) Some College 0.000 0.000 0.002 0.061 (0.000) (0.000) (0.03 1) (0.03 9) College 0.000 0.000 -0.049 0.047 (0.000) (0.000) (0.037) (0.047) Dad Very Involved -0.018 -0.016 0.013 -0.025 (0.026) (0.034) (0.033) (0.042) Dad Little Involved -0.016 0.023 -0.016 0.013 (0.023) (0.030) (0.030) (0.039) Father Figure Present 0009 -0.002 0.009 0.012 (0.023) (0.030) (0.030) (0.039) No Religion 0.081 0.065 0.072 0.012 (0.035) (0.046) (0.056) (0.074) Jewish -0.093 -0.085 0.113 0.118 (0.039) (0.05 7) (0.149) (0.174) Muslim 0.092 0.158 0.056 0.108 (0.122) (0.168) (0.112) (0.145) Baptist 0.051 0.072 0.097 0.091 (0.029) (0.037) (0.041) (0.052) Other Christian 0.034 0.021 0.060 -0.008 (0.022) (0.028) (0.041) (0.052) 102 Table 17 (cont’d) Freq. of Church Attendance -0.009 -0.017 -0.017 -0.027 (0.007) (0.009) (0.010) (0.013) Mother Impulsivity Scale -0.050 -0.058 -0.052 -0.066 (0.014) (0.018) (0.020) (0.027) Father Impulsivity Scale -0.048 -0.044 -0.042 -0.036 (0.014) (0.018) (0.019) (0.024) Focal Child is a Boy -0.036 -0.008 -0.029 0.007 (0.017) (0.022) (0.023) (0.029) First Child 0007 -0.000 -0.018 -0.033 (0.024) (0.033) (0.037) (0.049) Mother's Age at Baseline -0.005 -0.005 —0.004 -0.004 (0.002) (0.002) (0.003) (0.004) Out of Wedlock Childbirth 0.051 0.053 0.035 0.026 (0.035) (0.039) (0.051) (0.057) Marriage within 6 mths prior to birth -0.035 0.046 -0.032 0039 (0.056) (0.083) (0.067) (0.088) Number of Children in HH 0.002 0.000 -0.008 -0.007 (0.008) (0.011) (0.010) (0.015) Number of Adults in HH -0.003 0.005 -0.015 -0.015 (0.009) (0.014) (0.020) (0.026) Cohabit only 0.050 0.015 0.147 0.124 (0.039) (0.045) (0.057) (0.065) Pre-Marital Cohabitation -0.023 —0.046 0.022 0.006 (0.024) (0.032) (0.03 1) (0.040) New Birth Shock 0.023 0.001 0.058 0.028 (0.027) (0.030) (0.036) (0.042) Residential Move 0.015 0.042 (0.023) (0.033) New Incarceration 0.466 0.442 (0.076) (0.090) New Release 0.060 0.091 (0.084) (0.1 18) Change in H. Income Def/ 10000 -0.004 —0.009 (0.002) (0.003) Observations 2093 1224 l 3 l 6 744 R-squared 0.09 0.14 0.10 0.18 Data: FFCWS Robust standard errors are in parentheses * significant at 5%; ** significant at 1% 103 Table 18. Linear Probability Estimates for Couples Cohabiting and Married at Baseline Cohabiting Married (1) (2) (3) (4) 1-3 Year Interval -0.016 -0.001 (0.039) . (0.086) . 3-5 Year Interval 0.003 0.041 0.011 -0.019 (0.043) (0.029) (0.089) (0.035) 5-7 Year Interval -0.054 -0.003 -0.024 -0.054 (0.048) (0.041) (0.090) (0.034) 7-10 Year Interval 0.006 -0.001 -0.022 -0.045 (0.055) (0.050) (0.091) (0.037) 10-12 Year Interval -0.018 -0.038 -0.053 -0.069 (0.089) (0.091) (0.091) (0.037) 12 Plus Year Interval -0.054 -0.008 -0.029 -0.035 (0.076) (0.088) (0.091) (0.039) Interval Length 0.007 0.008 0.006 0.007 (0.002) (0.004) (0.001) (0.003) Age Difference -0.007 -0.004 0.001 0.001 (0.003) (0.004) (0.002) (0.002) Age Difference Squared 0.000 0.000 -0.000 -0.000 (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) Mother Black 0.091 0.061 0.033 0.029 (0.026) (0.035) (0.01 8) (0.024) Mother Hispanic -0.012 0.008 0.004 0.010 (0.027) (0.035) (0.018) (0.026) Other 0.108 0.046 -0.019 -0.011 (0.063) (0.074) (0.014) (0.022) Less than HS 0.044 0.021 0.000 -0.013 (0.023) (0.031) (0.025) (0.034) Some College 0.034 0.059 -0.008 -0.012 (0.024) (0.032) (0.019) (0.026) College -0.024 0.034 -0.026 -0.018 (0.051) (0.063) (0.018) (0.025) Dad Very Involved -0.007 0.011 -0.009 -0.051 (0.028) (0.036) (0.021) (0.030) Dad Little Involved -0.013 0.035 —0.011 -0.033 (0.024) (0.032) (0.021) (0.029) Father Figure Present -0.025 -0.004 0.034 0.018 (0.024) (0.03 1) (0.020) (0.027) No Religion 0.056 0.039 0.038 0.042 (0.034) (0.044) (0.028) (0.03 8) Jewish -0.189 -0.193 0.009 0.044 (0.053) (0.076) (0.028) (0.041) Muslim 0.014 0.123 -0.023 -0.027 (0.116) (0.161) (0.031) (0.049) Baptist 0.074 0.1 17 0.006 0.01 1 (0.032) (0.042) (0.020) (0.027) Other Christian 0.046 0.030 -0.001 -0.006 (0.027) (0.034) (0.012) (0.017) 104 Table 18 (cont’d) Freq. of Church Attendance -0.014 -0.019 0.001 0.000 (0.007) (0.010) (0.005) (0.006) Mother Impulsivity Scale 0054 —0.062 -0.021 -0.023 (0.017) (0.022) (0.01 1) (0.015) Father Impulsivity Scale 0054 -0.052 —0.020 -0.015 (0.014) (0.019) (0.011) (0.015) Focal Child is a Boy -0.023 0.016 -0.024 -0.023 (0.018) (0.024) (0.01 1) (0.015) First Child 0019 -0.003 -0.004 -0.007 (0.024) (0.033) (0.015) (0.021) Mother's Age at Baseline -0.005 -0.003 -0.003 -0.004 (0.002) (0.003) (0.002) (0.002) Out of Wedlock Childbirth 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) (0.000) Marriage within 6 mths prior to birth 0.000 0.000 -0.011 -0.011 (0.000) (0.000) (0.035) (0.046) Number of Children in H 0.004 0.007 0.000 -0.005 (0.009) (0.013) (0.005) (0.007) Number of Adults in H -0.006 -0.007 .0009 -0.012 (0.009) (0.014) (0.011) (0.017) Cohabit only 0.078 0.078 0.000 0.000 (0.025) (0.026) (0.000) (0.000) Pre-Marital Cohabitation 0.000 0.000 0.008 0.007 (0.000) (0.000) (0.012) (0.016) New Birth Shock 0.015 -0.014 0.010 0.006 (0.029) (0.032) (0.018) (0.021) Residential Move 0.044 -0.014 (0.025) (0.016) New Incarceration 0.491 0.562 (0.074) (0.183) New Release 0.055 -0.030 (0.100) (0.03 7) Change in H. Income Def/ 10000 -0.007 -0.003 (0.002) (0.001) Observations 2102 1 192 1993 1282 R-squared 0.06 0.12 0.07 0.09 Data: FFCWS Robust standard errors are in parentheses * significant at 5%; ** significant at 1% 105 Table 19. Logit Estimates of Union Dissolution (1) (2) (3) (4) 1-3 Year Interval -0.223 —0.124 -0.062 (0.213) (0.232) (0.230) 3-5 Year Interval -0.346 -0.113 0.050 0.209 (0.220) (0.245) (0.246) (0.155) 5-7 Year Interval -l .169 -0.684 -0.389 -0.200 (0.249) (0.278) (0.280) (0.224) 7-10 Year Interval -1.097 -0.404 -0.125 -0.083 (0.250) (0.285) (0.295) (0.246) 10-12 Year Interval -1.914 -1.179 -0.803 -0.683 (0.383) (0.413) (0.425) (0.459) 12 Plus Year Interval -1.825 —0.976 -0.515 -0.140 (0.350) (0.389) (0.399) (0.371) Interval Length 0.040 0.047 0.056 0.063 (0.008) (0.009) (0.012) (0.021) Age Difference -0.038 -0.034 -0.011 (0.015) (0.015) (0.020) Age Difference Squared 0.002 0.001 0.001 (0.001) (0.001) (0.001) Mother Black 0.686 0.552 0.476 (0.123) (0.128) (0.171) Mother Hispanic 0.049 -0.013 0.171 (0.151) (0.158) (0.202) Other 0.119 0.187 -0.225 (0.290) (0.288) (0.452) Less than HS 0.264 0.205 0.087 (0.121) (0.124) (0.168) Some College 0.071 0.118 0.235 (0.128) (0.131) (0.172) College -0.862 -0.576 -0.218 (0.222) (0.236) (0.283) Dad Very Involved -0.085 -0.101 -0.212 (0.140) (0.141) (0.191) Dad Little Involved -0.162 -0.127 0.021 (0.124) (0.124) (0.163) F athcr Figure Present 0.029 -0.003 0.048 (0.122) (0.123) (0.159) No Religion 0.352 0.342 0.285 (0.180) (0.186) (0.25 8) Jewish -0.242 0.075 0.505 (0.553) (0.556) (0.556) Muslim -0.156 -0.053 0.296 (0.532) (0.541) (0.608) Baptist 0.383 0.369 0.570 (0.160) (0.164) (0.212) Other Christian 0.121 0.222 0.131 (0.138) (0.144) (0.187) Freq. of Church Attendance 0.007 -0.064 -0.079 106 Table 19 (cont’d) (0.037) Mother Impulsivity Scale 0312 (0.081) Father Impulsivity Scale 0310 (0.071) Focal Child is a Boy 0199 (0.093) First Child 0163 (0.118) Mother's Age at Baseline 0046 (0.012) Out of Wedlock Childbirth Marriage within 6 months prior to child’s birth Number of Children in HH Number of Adults in HH Cohabit only Pre-Marital Cohabitation New Birth Shock Residential Move New Incarceration New Release Change in H. Income Def/ 10000 Observations 4108 4108 (0.039) 0.294 (0.084) 0.314 (0.072) 0.209 (0.095) -0.063 (0.129) 0.039 (0.012) 0.324 (0.227) 0.1 19 (0.379) 0.014 (0.043) 0.041 (0.059) 0.792 (0.262) 0.221 (0.199) 0.068 (0.142) 4095 (0.051) -O.346 (0.109) 0.274 (0.097) 0.032 (0.125) 0.020 (0.172) 0.036 ' (0.016) 0.289 (0.255) 0.060 (0.451) 0.008 (0.063) 0.062 (0.088) 0.593 (0.296) 0.086 (0.233) 0.044 (0.166) 0.150 (0.128) 2.416 (0.414) 0.020 (0.451) 0.042 (0.010) 2474 Data: FFCWS Robust standard errors are in parentheses * significant at 5%; ** significant at 1% 107 «3563.5. ANNEQESQQ$NM~E 638% ntB @256 6688 2wfim soon was 5522 I o «sunfish» AhEukEEcR-SMFB 638th d=4$o=£ EPA-8:5 3 Bwfim mm “6508 28 8:450:08 “mom-28 8 Homo-a 5:8.“ 368 .585 98:86 8 Sumo-E 656-“ cc 8822: 33328.22 88.885 I m «6.586388. 385658336863 63885» .83-30:08 Sum-BE“ E 8552 55mm Hogan-30:0.“ SPA .28 8 E328 Mafia-H 368 £83 8 Eamon .5568 mas-30:8 Sum-8:5 5 mg»... 85$ 28 9:3:8 Sow-0:0 “a Homo-E .856.“ 308 £83 “a “come-a 3656.8 mas-26:8 86.8-25 28 8:450:08 Rom-6:0 “a Homo-a .585 8 Eomna .868.“ mas-30:8 Hana-BE“ E 6.538 98 3-30:8 SPA-28 “a :5QO .585 8 :5me 65.8 ”ms-30:08 86.3885 5 6.838 28 8:86:08 Box-6:0 we. 86QO £85 8. #8on .556.“ 8832: 33.6.8232 85:: I 3% «6383.5. Asiansx Egon-PE 638858 Homo-E Bo: mm 558 868 :5 680: 65 E 8:328 soon 55: mm: .5588 868205 I v 73% 65 E flamingo 82H :83 FEE“. 85m 680: 65 E 8:388 soon was $568 Boom I X «6.585:- AmEQEEuméwfim 633th 2wEm Bo: mm .8568 :5 680: 65 5 Swan: 0: mm .5568 Eflwo—Bm I m «ES-63.5. 8328558363 83885» Homo-a Bo: mm 55$ 86% 2: :5 680: 66 E SwaB o: 8 .8568 88%an I N «386386. 828852268363 638m.» :83 FEE”. 8:8 080: 05 E 33.28 soon was .6568 Eomwo—omm I g ”36:08 8.. 88:68:06 on 98 390 chaos-cm Ema-8.8 =< 8.5232 we 85 "9.2.88 “4 «Ema 108 a. 3 ”35% Dana 5:35 0392: EEBE I BwEm _ v _ _ “528— _ 35:; _N_ #2528 _ _ 8:52— tnmuui :25 ~96 SQ 3.69% nix 23k .325 «635% :3: Q . 33H .325 E £28 352 2:985 :5: tufi 325% “N . u§H = 2:»qu =3: 2cQ 223m. Q . MEAN .325 . a. .253 a. 323 8:: 2525.5 32% . 32.5 @325 82.: 2.82% tsubuxrmsafi . 26.2325: 2: 5 258.5 :95 $>m .555" Esme—2m an: .: PEwE 109 Figure 3.1. The Hazard of Union Dissolution IO N.“ O O my O E OF‘ > u (0 F— . . O 0 ID 0‘ ' I I I I O 5 10 15 ttdum avgfail — Probability of Failure ttdum — Duration of the Union at a start of a Spell 111 Appendix A. Constructed Variables 1) Fraction of time Father is Present 2) Household Income/Person 3) Paternal Importance 4) Mental/Emotional problems 5) Other father figures present 6) Parents’ Drug Use 7) Parent Reports Parental Aggravation 8) Residential Instability Definition The total number of years father has spent living with the child divided by age of the chfld. Household income divided by household Size. Average of the questions reflecting the mother’s evaluation of the importance of the father’s involvement in the upbringing of the chfld. Likert scale: {(1) Very important, (2) somewhat important and (3) not important} How important is it for father to teach child about life? How important is it for father to provide direct care to child? How important is it for father to show love and affection to the child? How important is it for father to provide protection for child? How important is it for father to serve as authority figure and to discipline the child? The parent is characterized as having mental or emotional problems if he/she is taking medications for mental illnesses such as anxiety, depression or ADD. Defined as all men over the age of eighteen, living in the child’s household aside from the male spouse/partner. Parents’ level of smoking, alcohol consumption and illegal drug use over the all three waves. Both parents answer four questions on aggravation on a scale of 1 to 4 (1 being the most aggravated). He/She is classified as aggravated if he/she rates his/her aggravation as 1 or 2 on the aggravation scale. The total number of residential moves the child has experienced since birth. 112 Appendix A (cont’d) 9) Father Absent The child’s biological father is reported as absent from the child’s household in any wave. 10) Religiosity The number of times per week mother attends church. 11) Ever-Incarcerated This indicator excludes those fathers who are reported as incarcerated, but it could not be determined whether they were incarcerated ex ante or ex post. 113 Appendix B. Scales Documentation Aggression ODD ADHD DDI — Fafler’s Impulsivity Can’t wait turn Defiant Cannot concentrate I often say whatever comes into my head without thinking Demanding Disobedient Cannot sit still Often I don‘t think enough before I act Breaks others’ things Angry moods Quickly shifts actions I often say/do things without considering the consequences Easily frustrated Temper tantrums Demanding My plans fail because I fail to think them through first Gets in fights Uncooperative Gets into everything I often make up my mind w/o considering the situation No guilt after misbeh. Stubborn Can’t wait turn I get into trouble because I don't think before I act Hits others Hurts animals/people unintentionally Attacks people Punishment doesn’t change behavior Screams a lot Selfish/won’t share Wants a lot of attention Likert Scale: O-Not True O-Not True O—Not True l-Strongly Agree 2-Very True 2-Very True 2-Very True 4-Strongly Disagree Alpha on Full Sample: 0.88 0.77 0.72 0.84 Notes: The items are averaged to create each scale. 114 Appendix C. Religiosity — How often do you attend religious services? Likert Scale: 1 — Once a week 2 — Several times per month 3 — Several times per year 4 — Hardly 5 — Never 115 BIBLIOGRAPHY Amato, P., & Booth, A. (1991). The consequences of parental divorce and marital unhappiness for adult well-being. Social Forces 69(3), 895-914. American Guidance Service, Inc. (2001). Retrieved fiom http ://www.state.tn.us/education/ci/cistandards200 1/1a/cik3assesmentfolder/cik3ra peabodypicturehtm Antecol, H., & Bedard, K. (2007). Does single parenthood increase the probability of teenage promiscuity, substance use, and crime? Journal of Population Economics 20, 55-71. Balakrishnan, T.R., et a1. (1987). "A Hazard Model Analysis of the Covariates of Marriage Dissolution in Canada." Demography 24(3):395-406. Becker, G. S., Landes, E. and Michael. R., (1977). "An economic analysis of marital stability." Journal of Political Economy 85(6):1141-87. Bedard, K. and Deschenes, O. (2004). Sex preferences, marital dissolution, and the economic status of women. The Journal of Human Resources, 40 (2): 411 — 434. Bomstein, M., & Haynes, O. M. (1998). Vocabulary competence in early childhood: measurement, latent construction and predictive validity. Child Development, 69 (3), 654-671. Bumpass, L. and Lu, H. (1999). Trends in cohabitation and implications for children's family contexts in the U.S. CDE Work. Pap. No. 98- 15. Cent. Demography Ecol., University of Wisconsin - Madison. Bumpass, L. and Lu, H., (2000). Trends in cohabitation and implications for children’s family contexts in the United States. Population Studies 54(1): 29-41. Casterline, J .B., Williams, L. & McDonald, P., (1986). The age difference between spouses: variations among developing countries. Population Studies 40 (3): 353-374 Cavanagh, S., & Huston, A. (2006). Family instability and children’s early problem behavior. Social Forces, 85(1), 551-581. Cherlin, A. (1978). Remarriage as an incomplete institution. American Journal of Sociology 84(3), 634-650. Corak, M. (2001). Death and divorce: the long-term consequences of parental loss on adolescence. Journal of Labor Economics 19(3), 682-715. 116 Craigie, T. (2008). Effects of paternal presence and family stability on child cognitive performance. Center for Research on Child Wellbeing Working Paper Series, 2008-03-FF. Deaton A. (1997). The analysis of household surveys: a microeconometric approach to development policy. Baltimore and London: Johns Hopkins University Press for the World Bank, 67-72. Dickman, DJ. (1990). Functional and dysfunction impulsivity: personality and cognitive correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(1), 95-102. Elicker, J ., Englund, M., & Sroufe, A. (1992). Predicting peer competence and peer relationships in children from early parent-child relationships. Family-Peer Relationships: Modes of Linkage. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ermisch, J. F ., & F rancesconi, M. (2001). Family structure and children’s achievements. Journal of Population Economics 14, 249-270. Evenhouse, E., & Reilly, S. (2004). A sibling study of stepchild well-being. The Journal Human Resources 39(1), 248-276. Farrington, D. P. (1998). Predictors, causes and correlates of male youth violence. Crime and Justice, 24, 421-475. Finlay, K., & Neumark, D. (2008). Is marriage always good for children? Evidence from families affected by incarceration. NBER Working Paper Series, Vol. 13928 Fomby, P., & Cherlin, A.J. (2007). Family instability and child wellbeing. American Sociological Review 72 (April), 181-204. Freeman, RB. (1984). Longitudinal analyses of the effects of trade unions. Journal of Labor Economics, 2(1), 1-26. Geller, A., Garfinkel, I., & Western, B. (2006). The effects of incarceration on employment and wages: an analysis of the fragile families survey. Center for Research on Child Wellbeing Working Paper Series, 2006-01-FF. Glenn, N. D., & Kramer, K. B. (1987). The marriages and divorces of the children of divorce. Journal of Man'iage and the Family, 49, 811-825. Gottfredson, M.R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Grusec, J .E., & Lytton, H. (1988). Social development. New York: Springer-Verlag. 117 Greenberg, E. F ., & Nay, W. R. (1982). The intergenerational transmission of marital instability reconsidered. Journal of Maniage and the Family, 44, 335-347. Greene, R.W., Biederrnan, J ., Zerwas, S., Monuteaux, M.C., Goring, J. C., & Faraone, S.V. (2002). Psychiatric comorbidity, family dysfunction, and social impairment in referred youth with oppositional defiant disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry 15921214-1224. Grogger, J. (1995). The effect of arrests on the employment and earnings of young men. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 1 10(1), 5 1 ~71 . Grover, K. J ., et al. (1985). Mate selection processes and marital satisfaction. Family Relations, 34, 383-386. Haider, S., & Solon, G. (2006). Life-cycle variation in the association between current and lifetime earnings. American Economic Review, 96(4), 1308-1320. Haveman, R., & Wolfe, B. (1995). The determinants of children’s attainments: a review of methods and findings. Journal of Economic Literature 33(4), 1829-1878. Heckman, J. (2000). Policies to foster human capital. Research in Economics 54(1), 3-56. Hodges, G. (2006). The role of impulsive aggression in a cohort of suicide attempters. (Doctoral Dissertation: The University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas).Retrieved from http://edissertations.library.swmed.edu/pdf/HodgesG062 106/HodgesGayle.pdf J afee, S., Moffitt, T., Caspi, A., & Taylor, A. (2003). With (or without) father: the benefits of living with two parents depend on the father’s antisocial behavior. Child Development, 74(1), 109-126. J eruchimowicz, R., Costello, J ., & Bagur, J .S. (1971). Knowledge and action and object words: a comparison of lower and middle-class Negro preschoolers. Child Development, 42(2), 455-464. J uraj da, S. (2007). Notes on panel data econometrics. Unpublished Draft. Lang, K., & Zagorsky, J. L. (2001). Does growing up with a parent absent really hurt? The Journal of Human Resources 36(2), 253-273. Larson, J .H. and Holman, T.B., (1994). Predictors of marital quality and stability. Family Relations, 43 (2): 228-237. Leibowitz, A. (1977). Parental inputs and children’s achievement. The Journal of Human Resources 12(2), 242-251. 118 Lichter, D. T., Qian, Z. & Mellott, L. (2006). Marriage or Dissolution? Union Transitions among Poor Cohabiting Women. Demography, 43 (2): 223 — 240. Locander, .Sudman, S., & Bradbum, N. (1976). An investigation of interview method, threat, and response distortion. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 71, 269-75. Maccoby, EB. (1980). Social development. New York: Harcourt Brace. McLanahan, S. (1985). Family structure and the reproduction of poverty. American Journal of Sociology 90, 873-901. Menken, J ., et al. (1981). Proportional hazards life table models: An illustrative analysis of socio-demo graphic influences on marriage dissolution in the United States. Demography 18: 181- 200. Mumane, R., Maynard, R., & Ohls, J. (1981). Home resources and children’s achievement. The Review of Economics and Statistics 63(3), 369-377. Nock, S. (2002). "The social costs of de-institutionalizing marriage." in Revitalizing the Institution of Maniage for the Twenty-First Century, edited by A.J. Hawkins, L.D. Wardle, and DO. Coolidge, pp. 1-13. Westport, CT: Praeger. O’Connell, M. & Rogers, C.C. ( 1984). Out-of-wedlock births, premarital pregnancies and their effect on family formation and dissolution. Family Planning Perspectives, 16(4): 157-162. Osborne, C., & McLanahan, S. (2007). Partnership instability and child well-being. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69(November), 1065-1083. Painter, G., & Levine, D.I. (2000). Family structure and youths’ outcomes: which correlations are causal? The Journal of Human Resources 35(3), 524-549. Petry, N. (2000). Substance abuse, pathological gambling, and impulsiveness. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 63, 29—38. Reichman, N., Teitler, J ., & McLanahan, S. (2001 ). Fragile families: sample and design. Children and Youth Services Review 23(4/5), 303-326. Robins, L. (1978). Sturdy childhood predictors of adult antisocial behavior: replications from longitudinal studies. Psychological Medicine 8, 611-622. Rogowski, J. & Karoly, L. (2000). Health insurance and retirement behavior: evidence from the health and retirement survey. Journal of Health Economics, 19: 529 — 539. 119 Ruhm, C. (2004). Parental employment and child cognitive development. The Journal of Human Resources 39(1), 155-192. Sampson, R.J., & Laub, J .H., ( 1992). Crime and deviance in the life course. Annual Review of Sociology, 18, 63-84. Sandefur, G., & Wells, T. (1997). Using siblings to investigate the effects of family structure on educational attainment. Discussion Paper no. 1144-97, Madison, WI: Institute for Research on Poverty. Scott-J ones, D. (1984). Family influences on cognitive development and school achievement. Review of Research in Education I I , 259-304. Seltzer, J. (1994). Consequences of marital dissolution for children. Annual Review of Sociology 20, 235-266. Shaw, D.S., Gilliom, M., & Giovannelli, J. (2000). Aggressive behavior disorders. In C.H. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of Infant Mental Health, 2’” Edition (pp. 397-411). New York: Guilford Press. Sigle-Rushton, W., & McLanahan, S. (2002). Father absence and child well-being: a critical review. Center for Research on Child Wellbeing Working Paper Series, 2002-20-FF. Smock, P. (2000). Cohabitation in the United States: an appraisal of research themes, findings and implications. Annual Review of Sociology, 26: 1 — 20. Staiger, D., & Stock, J .H. (1997). Instrumental variables regression with weak instruments. Econometrica 65(3), 557-86. Steiner, H., & Remsing, L. (2007). Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with oppositional defiant disorder. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(1). Svarer, M. (2004). Is your love in vain? Another look at premarital cohabitation and divorce. The Journal of Human Resources, 39 (2): 523-535. Teachman. J ., (1982). Methodological issues in the analysis of family formation and dissolution. Journal of Marriage and Family 44: 1037-1053. Sutton-Smith, B., Rosenburg, G., & Landy, F. (1968). F ather-absence effects in families of different sibling compositions. Child DeveIOpment 39(4), 1213-1221. U.S. Census Bureau Population Census, (2000). Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/prod/2001pubs/c2kbr01-1 .pdf 120 U.S. Census Bureau Current Population Reports: Living arrangements of children (1996). Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/population/www/socdemo/ms-la.htrnl U.S. Census Bureau Current Population Reports: Living arrangements of children (2002). Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/population/www/socdemo/ms-la.html Vigil-Colet, A., & Morales-Vives, M. (2005). How impulsivity is related to intelligence and academic achievement. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 8(2), 199-204. Webster, P.S., Orbuch, T. L. & House, J. S., (1995). Effects of childhood family background on adult marital quality and perceived stability. The American Journal of Sociology, 101 (2): 404-432. Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler adult intelligence scale — revised (WAIS-R manual). Harcourt Brace J ovanovich: The Psychological Corporation. Western, B. and Lopoo, L. (2006). “Incarceration, Marriage, and Family Life.” Chapter 6 in Punishment and Inequality in America, New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. Western, B. (2004). Incarceration, marriage and family life. Retrieved fiom http://www.russellsage.org/publications/workingpapers/incarcerationman'iagefam ilylife/document Wildeman, C. (2008). Paternal incarceration and children’s aggressive behaviors: evidence fiom the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study. Center for Research on Child Wellbeing Working Paper Series, 2008-02-FF. Winkler, M. (2006). ADHD Symptoms ADHD Diagnosis — Diagnosis Criteria of ADHD. Retrieved from http://web4health.info/en/answers/adhd-diagn-dsm.htm Witte, A. (1980). Estimating the economic model of crime with individual data. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 94(1), 57-84. Wooldridge, J. (1999). Asymptotic properties of weighted M-estimators for variable probability samples. Econometrica, 67, 1385-1406. Wooldridge, J. (2002). Econometric analysis of cross-section and panel data. MIT Press. Wu, L. (1996). Effects of family instability, income and income instability on the risk of a premarital birth. American Sociological Review 61, 386-406. 121 3 1293 03062 9038