u .‘..‘:.\W|‘l ‘l ,.......x;._. a ., ix: 23%.”.3 flan. “Ea r 3*“: . a, a“. V :. 7.1.1.: A.» .1: ‘xl‘. 1..., 0.32.1“: '13.. figfiafinfiw 1 a». f 0;}? LTBfi—A RY " " Mich? State UniverSIty This is to certify that the thesis entitled The Effectiveness of Melodic Intonation Therapy and Therapeutic Singing on Functional Communication Skills for Adults with Expressive Aphasia presented by Yea Ju Rhee has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Master of Music degree in Music Therapy /'l F4 Ola'jor’Professor’s Signature Date MSU is an Affinnative Action/Equal Opportunity Employer ‘U-O-I-Une-a-g- - —.-o-I---o---o-q-—o-a-c-e-I-----o-o->-.—-— II--— —i-—--—--A----—---—~—.o-- PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5/08 K:/Proj/Acc&Pres/ClRC/Date0ue indd TITLE: THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MELODIC INTONATION THERAPY AND THERAPEUTIC SINGING ON FUNCTIONAL COMMUNICATION SKILLS FOR ADULTS WITH EXPRESSIVE APHASIA By Yea Ju Rhee A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements For the degree of MASTER OF MUSIC Music Therapy 2009 ABSTRACT By Yea Ju Rhee The study of music and speech for persons with expressive aphasia by Sparks and Holland attracted a fair amount of critical attention in the 19705. The effectiveness of Therapeutic Singing (TS) and Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) in improving the speech production of persons with expressive aphasia has been established in subsequent research since that time. Based on the present research, the reframing study of Modified Melodic Intonation Therapy (Baker, 2000), which is the systematic combination of Melodic Intonation Therapy and Therapeutic Singing, gives convincing answers to some of this study's research questions regarding whether or not the addition of TS to MIT helps persons with aphasia improve their speech production. Two case studies were conducted to investigate the effectiveness of MIT and TS to improve functional communication skills. Eight variables of spoken language were compared: total length of samples, total number of utterances, total number of words, average length of utterances, average number of words per minute, total number of items named, total number of prompts per item named, and scores for MIT performance. The results showed that MIT and TS interventions helped the subject who had low functioning speech to improve functional communication skills in both the long term and short term. On the other hand, the subject who had high functioning speech skills showed positive numerical gains on some of the variables in functional communication, but there were not statistically supported differences between pre and post-tests in the short-term. Copyright by Yea J u Rhee 2009 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The author gratefully acknowledges all those who helped in the realization of this study - to participants and their wives for delightful cooperation, their enthusiasm, and for making this study existed. I would like to thank committee members, Professor Frederic Tims, for his grateful feedback, and Professor Hwan Jung for all his guidance in the statistical analysis of my data to help this study show reasonable data sets. A special thanks to my consultant, Professor Katie Strong, for providing expert advice in the speech and language pathology fields and the help in recruiting participants in the Aphasia Support Group around Lansing. I am forever grateful to my supervisor, Roger Smeltekop, for his immeasurable support throughout my six years as a music therapy student and for his ongoing lessons as the role model in my life. Finally, very special thanks to my husband for all his support through love and patience to finish my music therapy degree, and to my parents for the special care of my twins with uncountable grace, and to my son and twins for waiting for mommy’s time. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ........................................................................................................... vi LIST OF FIGURES ....................................................................................................... viii CHAPTER I 1 INTRODUCTION ....................................................................................................................... 1 STATEMENT OF PURPOSE ............................................................................................................. 4 CHAPTER II 5 LITERATURE REVIEW ............................................................................................................ 5 CHAPTER III 18 METHOD .................................................................................................................................. 18 Subjects .................................................................................................................. 1 8 Setting and Apparatus ............................................................................................ 22 Research Design .................................................................................................... 23 Procedure ............................................................................................................... 24 Measurement and Analysis .................................................................................... 40 CHAPTER IV 45 RESULTS .................................................................................................................................. 45 Findings and Analysis .................................................................................................... 45 Spontaneous Speech Measurement ........................................................................ 46 Naming Measurement ............................................................................................ 64 MIT Measurement .................................................................................................. 70 CHAPTER V 71 DISSCUSSION .......................................................................................................................... 71 Summary and Conclusion .............................................................................................. 7] Implications for Interventions ........................................................................................ 80 Implications for Future Researchers ............................................................................. 83 APPENDICES 86 Measurement F orms & M T Hierarchy ......................................................................... 87 Picture Stimuli Sheets for Naming Test ........................................................................ 97 F lyer & Consent Form ................................................................................................. 113 ASHA levels of Comprehension, Expression, Fluency ................................................. 116 Raw Data & Statistics Data ......................................................................................... 119 REFERENCES- 127 LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Subjects Description .......................................................................................... 18 Table 2. L’s Linguistic Contents ..................................................................................... 26 Table 3-1. MIT Hierarchy Level I ................................................................................... 28 Table 3-2. MIT Hierarchy Level II ................................................................................. 31 Table 3-3. MIT Hierarchy Level III ................................................................................ 33 Table 3-4. MIT Hierarchy Level IV ................................................................................ 34 Table 4—1. Scoring Hierarchy of Spontaneous Speech Measurement ............................. 41 Table 4-2. Scoring Hierarchy of Naming measurement ................................................. 42 Table 4-3. Scoring Hierarchy of MIT Measurement ....................................................... 43 Table 5-1. & 5-2. L’s Paired t-test on Total Length of Samples ..................................... 47 Table 5-3. & 5-4. R’s Paired t-test on Total Length of Samples .................................... 49 Table 6—1. & 6-2. L’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Utterances ............................... 50 Table 6-3. & 6-4. R’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Utterances ............................... 52 Table 7-1. & 7-2. L’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Words ...................................... 54 Table 7-3. & 7-4. R’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Words ..................................... 55 Table 8-1. & 8-2. L’s Paired t-test on Average Length of Utterances ............................ 57 Table 8-3. & 8-4. R’s Paired t-test on Average Length of Utterances ............................ 59 Table 9-1. & 9-2. L’s Paired t-test on Average Number of Words per Minute .............. 60 Table 9-3. & 9-4. R’s Paired t-test on Average Number of Words per Minute .............. 62 Table 10-1. & 10-2. L’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Items named ........................ 64 Table 10-3. & 10-4. R’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Items named ........................ 66 vi LIST OF TABLES (continued) Table 11-1. & 11-2. L’s Paired t-test on Average Number of Prompts per Item ........... 67 Table 11-3. & 11-4. R’s Paired t-test on Average Number of Prompts per Item ........... 69 Table 12. Summary of the data result in t-test ................................................................ 72 Table 13-1. & 13-2. The second-half period in L’s paired t-test .................................. 107 Table 14-1. & 14-2. The first-half period in L’s paired t-test ....................................... 108 Table 15-1. & 15-2. Independent t-test for L’s Goal 111 ............................................... 110 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. One Group pre-test-post-test Design ................................................................. 23 Figure 2. Subject L’s Sessions Diagram ........................................................................... 24 Figure 3-1. Total Length of Sample in L’s Spontaneous Speech Test ............................. 47 Figure 3-2. Total Length of Sample in R’s Spontaneous Speech Test ............................. 48 Figure 3-3. Total Number of Utterances in L’s Spontaneous Speech Test ....................... 50 Figure 3-4. Total Number of Utterances in R’s Spontaneous Speech Test ...................... 52 Figure 3-5. Total Number of Words in L’s SP Test .......................................................... 53 Figure 3-6. Total Number of Words in R’s SP Test ......................................................... 55 Figure 3-7. Average Length of Utterances in L’s Spontaneous Speech Test ................... 57 Figure 3-8. Average Length of Utterances in R’s SP Test ................................................ 58 Figure 3-9. Average Number of Words per Minute in L’s SP Test .................................. 60 Figure 3-10. Average Number of Words per Minute in R’s SP Test ................................ 62 Figure 4-1. Total Number of Items Named in L’s Naming Test ...................................... 64 Figure 4-2. Total Number of Items Named in R’s Naming Test ...................................... 65 Figure 4-3. Average Number of Prompts per Item Named in L’s Naming Test .............. 67 Figure 4-4. Average Number of Prompts per Item Named in R’s Naming ...................... 68 Figure 5—1. MIT Scores of Percent in L’s MIT Test ......................................................... 70 viii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Music is a method of communication that is both nonverbal and verbal. Music helps people who have difficulty speaking their minds mentally or physically, in order to give them a voice in a nonverbal musical medium. On the other hand, music also helps the people who have a functional difficulty in verbal expression, in order to improve their ability to communicate by training them with musical cues, such as rhythm and melody. When I visited the meeting of the aphasia support group as a volunteer, an intriguing observation was that persons with expressive aphasia could sing with steady rhythmic patterns and speech production without long pauses between words, although they couldn’t speak when they tried talking to someone. This research will seek to provide important information to help the persons who have fimctional difficulty in producing language expression by demonstrating the effectiveness of music in functional . communication through singing. There are different conditions that contribute to functional language difficulty, and stroke is one of the major causes. According to data from the American Heart Association and the National Institute of Health, 600,000 to 750,000 strokes occur in the US each year. A stroke, which causes damage to the posterior part of the frontal lobe in the left hemisphere of the brain, results in language disorders including Broca’s aphasia (expressive) and Wemicke’s aphasia (receptive). Expressive aphasia is characterized by disturbed prosody, effortful and slow speech with long pauses between words, poor sentence construction, difficulty naming objects, word-fmding difficulties, and poor word repetition (Baker & Tamplin, 2006). Speech and music share neural networks in the brain because music and language both use structured patterns of pitch (melody), duration (rhythm), and intensity (dynamics) (Patel, Peretz, Tramo, & Labreque, 1998). According to the research (Borchgrevink, 1982; Zatorre, Evans, & Meyer, 1994), pitch in melody is processed only by the right hemisphere, but rhythm is processed by the left hemisphere, so music including rhythm and pitch stimulates both right and left hemisphere in the brain. This rationale would support the idea that singing which includes pitch and rhythm could activate both hemispheres in the brain; music would be beneficial to persons who have Broca’s aphasia with damage to expressive speech functions by stimulating the left side of their brains. Neurologic Music Therapy (NMT) is one of the dominant music therapy approaches, which is based on a neuroscience model of music perception and production and the influence of music on functional changes in nonmusical brain and behavior fimctions (Thaut, 2005). NMT has several standardized techniques in three different areas of functional training: 1) Sensorimotor Training, 2) Speech and Language Training, and 3) Cognition Training. The dominant domain is the Sensorimotor area in NMT training, which is well established by plenty of research, live demonstration with patients, and collaboration with physiological experts. In recent years, several studies have attempted to find the reasons for the treatment effect of MIT and establish standardized techniques in Speech and Language Training and Cognition Training. According to the present research by speech and language pathologists, rhythmic stimulus can be an important musical element to activate the human nervous system. For this reason, rhythmic instruments were used to stimulate the subject by rhythmic beats in the present study. In NMT, Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) is one of the most important techniques of Speech and Language Training. MIT was established first from the speech pathologist, Sparks, in thel97 Os, and later the speech pathologist, Galloway, made the first contribution that connected music therapists and speech therapists by investigating the effectiveness of MIT in speech functions (Galloway & Kraus, 1982). This technique proved the power of musical elements, such as rhythm and melody, in brain functions (Sparks, Helm, & Albert, 1974). While I participated in the NMT training in 2009 at Colorado State University, music therapists mentioned that research on the Speech and Language domain is still in its early stages, as the brevity of the bibliography attests in the Music Therapy field. I have observed from small workshops to national conferences in music therapy that many music therapists use NMT techniques without clear recognition of what techniques they are using, such as MIT, Musical Speech Stimulation (MU STIM), Rhythmic Speech Cueing (RSC), Vocal Intonation Therapy (VIT), Therapeutic Singing (TS), Oral Motor and Respiratory Exercises (OMREX), Developmental Speech and Language Training through Music (DSLM), and Symbolic Communication Training through Music (SYCOM). This study will be intended to broaden our horizons of understanding of the effectiveness of functional communication through music in the Speech and Language Training area of music therapy. STATEMENT of PURPOSE With the intent of identifying effective music therapy techniques, the purpose of this study is to ascertain the effectiveness of Therapeutic Singing and Melodic Intonation Therapy in improving the functional communication skills of adults with expressive aphasia. The goals for subject L’s case study, who had a lower level of fimctional communication skills, are as follows: (1) to give specific information whether Therapeutic Singing (TS) combined with Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) helps adult clients who are diagnosed with expressive aphasia as measured by the quality of their functional communication skills; (2) to gain some information as to whether MIT treatment itself helps clients improve the quality of their functional communication skills; (3) to compare the effectiveness of TS combined with MIT and MIT alone in increasing fimctional communication skills; and (4) to investigate whether TS enhances the benefits of MIT treatment. The goal for subject R’s case study, who had a higher level of functional communication skills, is as follows: 1) To acquire information as to whether Therapeutic Singing (TS) itself helps clients improve the quality of their functional communication skills. CHAPTER II LITERATURE REVIEW This literature review reflects information in the following areas: 1) music and brain; 2) stroke and aphasia; 3) music and speech; 4) melodic intonation therapy (MIT) and therapeutic singing (TS); 5) modified melodic intonation therapy (MMIT); and 6) measurement tools for functional communication skills. Music and Brain Throughout brain science research over the past two decades, the element of music has emerged as a factor in answering the question of what stimulates brain functionality. Sacks (2006) addresses this question by stating that music provides one of the most powerful sources of auditory stimulation in the human brain. Also, evidence from developmental studies indicates that music can increase auditory cortical functions of the brain (Engineer, et al., 2004). The belief that music is processed only in the right hemisphere has been rejected by researchers (Hachinski & Hachinski, 1994). They found musical activity involved almost every region of the brain that was known, and almost every neural subsystem. They also insisted that different neural locations throughout the cortex handled different aspects of the music. Actually, the brain uses functional segregation for music processing, and employs a system of feature detectors whose job is to analyze specific aspects of the musical signal, such as pitch, rhythm, tempo, timbre, and harmony (Levitin, 2006). These cerebral locations in the brain are not restricted to only the right hemisphere as past information suggests (Prior, Kinsella, & Giese, 1990). Stroke and Aphasia According to data from the American Heart Association and the National Institute of Health, 600,000 to 750,000 strokes occur in the US each year (Schlang, Marchina, & Norton, 2008). Moreover, according to the World Health Organization, Cerebral Vascular Accidents (CVA) affect 15 million people worldwide each year, with five million people left permanently disabled. A CVA, commonly known as a stroke, results in the rapidly increasing loss of brain functions when a part of the brain is deprived of blood flow, and brain cells subsequently die. Overall, CVA affects 11% of females and 8.4% of males (Mackay & Mensah, 2004). The most well-known Neurogenic speech disorder, aphasia, is most often caused by a cerebral vascular accident. People who have experienced a CVA are known as having a left CVA or right CVA, depending on the site of the lesion. The patients with a left CVA experience problematic physical functions and communication. The left CVA causes the most apparent impairments of the motor control and sensory experience of the right side of the body and causes a right hemi-paresis. Furthermore, a left CVA also causes language disorders including Broca’s aphasia (expressive) and Wemicke’s aphasia (receptive) (Baker & Tamplin, 2006). Expressive aphasia is caused by damage to a region of the inferior left frontal lobe, and it is also called motor aphasia, non-fluent aphasia, or Broca’s aphasia. This results in the disorders of speech production characterized by slow, laborious, and non- fluent speech. People with expressive aphasia have difficulty saying basic words with semantic meaning. On the other hand, people with receptive aphasia, otherwise known as sensory aphasia, fluent aphasia, or Wemicke’s aphasia, show poor speech comprehension and production of meaningless speech (Goodglass & Kaplan, 1972). According to several examinations of auditory comprehension, a person with Broca’s aphasia shows understanding and retention of spoken language that is essentially normal in a variety of contexts (Sparks & Deck, 1986). Sparks also indicates that the examination of verbal expression provides a profile of the good candidate for Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT). He states: 1. “Almost no responses occur in confrontation naming, responsive naming, word and phrase repetition, or sentence completion. However, an occasional response will be poorly articulated but accurate enough to indicate correct encoding of the target wor .” 2. “Articulation of stereotype phrases will be precise and easily transcribed into phonetic symbols. On the other hand, any attempt to duplicate or initiate propositional language results in slurred articulation and phonemic substitutions.” 3. “Effort at self-correction is often vigorous. This is to be expected in an aphasic who is acutely aware of making errors in his verbal output. Unfortunately, the product is not improved by this effort.” (p. 255) There are useful selection criteria for adult aphasia patients, with the best candidates for MIT including: “(a) no evidence of bilateral brain damage, (b) average-to- high receptive abilities, (c) poor repetition of single words, ((1) non-fluent verbal production with inadequate articulation agility and effortful ignition of verbal communication, and (e) a well motivated, emotionally stable patient with an appropriate attention span” (Benson, Dobkin, Rothi, Helm-Estabrooks, & Kertesz, 1994, pp. 566- 568) However, Sparks et a1. (1986) also show that the other three kinds of aphasia are not receptive to MIT as a form of treatment. There are three types of poor candidates for MIT treatment: Wemicke’s, T ranscortical, and Global aphasia. People with these types of aphasia are very difficult to engage in MIT and achieve minimal therapeutic success. Music and Speech According to Sacks (2008), speech is not just a succession of words in the proper order. It contains inflections, intonations, tempo, rhythm, and melody. Speech and music both are dependent on phonology and articulatory mechanisms that are elementary in other primates, and for their appreciation both are dependent distinctly on human brain mechanisms dedicated to the analysis of complex, segmented, rapidly changing streams ofsound. Brain processing of music and speech overlaps, and there has been extensive research to figure out how music stimulates speech. Belin (1996) insists that musical intervention has been used as part of speech therapy in stroke rehabilitation. There is evidence that music therapy practice is effective in alleviating speech disorder problems, such as Broca’a aphasia (Naeser & Helm-Estabrooks, 1985). King (2007) also gives convincing information of the effectiveness of music in brain functionality. This is important for music therapists to know and use in reviewing the definitions and characteristics of adult onset aphasia, as well as apraxia and dysarthria. Jackson (1871) characterized propositional speech from so called automatic speech. He stated that even when propositional speech became seriously damaged, automatic speech could be preserved in aphasia. Hobson (2006) gives a rationale for applying music therapy methods to communication disorders, as well as definitions and descriptions of several speech disorders relevant to music therapy practice. He notes that people with aphasia are able to sing familiar songs but are not able to engage in conversations. The classical interpretation of this observation is that singing familiar songs would depend on right hemisphere functions, whereas propositional speech would depend on left hemisphere functions. But it 'has not been substantiated by quantitative data yet (Hebert, Racette, Gagnon, & Perets, 2003). Sacks (2008) pointed out that singing familiar songs was a form of automatic speech as well as cursing or reciting a poem. With this apt observation, Sacks (2008) reformulated the question of music therapy: A person with aphasia may be able to sing or curse or recite a poem but not to utter a propositional phrase. The question of whether singing has any use in the recovery of speech, then, can be formulated another way: can language embedded in unconscious automatism be “released” for conscious, propositional use? (p. 218) This shows an important role of music, especially singing, for speech therapy as a “trigger”. In his view, it could be possible that by re-experiencing language even though it is a kind of wholly automatic language embedded in music, cortical areas previously inhibited but not damaged can be de-inhibited (Sacks, 2008). Melodic Intonation Therapy Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) was introduced in 1973 through successful research using three chronic non—fluent aphasics (Albert, Sparks, & Helm, 1973). MIT is a treatment technique for the rehabilitation of expressive (Broca’s) aphasia. There is also support for its use with apraxia. Fitting candidates for MIT are clients with left-side stroke lesions in Broca’s area, which is responsible for encoding speech production, or clients with lesions interrupting the nervous connections between Wemicke’s and Broca’s area. MIT utilizes a patient’s unimpaired ability to sing to make speech production possible. The rationale is based on a hemispheric transfer of speech functions from the left-hemisphere (Wemicke’s area) to its right-hemisphere homologue during encoding of speech into singing (Albert, et al., 1973; Helfrich-Miller, 1994; Keith & Aronson, 1975; Overy, Norton, & Ozdemir, 2005; Popovici, 1995; Sparks, et al., 1974). Several published reports over the years have outlined a programmed technique for MIT,(Helm-Estabrooks & Albert, 1991; Sparks & Holland, 1976) which include relatively strict criteria for patient selection. Functional sentences or utterances in MIT are translated into a song by translating the speech inflection patterns into musical rhythm and intonation. Therefore, it is technically more accurate to refer to the sung utterances not as songs, but as melodic intonations. Singing is reduced to Sprechgesang (speech singing) and finally to normal speech pattern in later steps of the therapy. The important characteristics are the rhythmic and melodic elements for the therapeutic stimulus (Thaut, 2005). The range of musical notes is limited to representing only slightly greater inflection than the pattern of normal speech. MIT works in a gradual progression and is designed to lead the client through a sequence of therapy steps, which gradually increases the length of intoned sentences, decreases dependence on the therapist by fading hand tapping and singing, and increases the client’s independence from intonation. The client is guided through a sequence of four progressive levels of difficulty, each containing a series of discrete steps. A criterion of 90% correct responses at each level is needed to ensure gradual progression, as well as the advancement up to the next level. In each step, target sentences or phrases are practiced until the level is 10 completed or failure occurs for that particular sentence (Sparks & Deck, 1986). Based on the supportive literature and clinical observation, the song strategy proves helpful for patients with left frontal lobe damage or bilateral damage that leaves singing centers of the right temporal lobe relatively intact (Lucia, 1987) . According to research from Emory University, neurological development is steady, but critical periods of rapid cognitive growth are known to occur. Musical techniques such as MIT could be beneficial to children suffering speech and language problems due to some subtle cerebral dysfimction, since the child’s brain possesses resilience and plasticity to some developmental point (Galloway & Kraus, 1982). According to the research (Albert, etal., 1973), MIT is effective for 75% of adults with non-fluent aphasia, and for 25% of people with severe non-fluent aphasia with little change in their ability to communicate verbally in meaningful ways. There are currently very few treatment techniques for in use of language therapy, and many are not uniformly practiced to allow for consistent measurements. MIT can satisfy consistency requirements to offer beneficial variation for research-level studies (Benson, et al., 1994). Therapeutic Singing Both singing and speech are essential tools for human expressive communication in both nonverbal and verbal ways. Singing has been described as an “automatic speech skill” which generally leads to functional speech treatment (Lucia, 1987). According to Thaut (2005), therapeutic singing (TS) refers to an unspecific use of singing activities in groups to facilitate initiation, development, and articulation in speech and language, as well as to increase functions of the respiratory apparatus, used with a variety of neurological or developmental speech and language dysfunctions. (S. Jackson, Treharne, & Boucher, 1997; Thaut, 2005) Singing has also been recommended as a therapeutic intervention specifically for people with Broca’s aphasia. (Benton, 1977; Gerstrnan, 1964; Gleason & Goodglass, 1984; Keenan, 1987). According to the research, while examining the preservation of singing in 24 patients with Broca’s aphasia, 87.5% of the subjects produced accurate melodies, and 57% sang with correct diction (Yamadori, Osumi, Masuhara, & Okubo, 1977). Singing was. recommended for the rehabilitation of expressive deficits as early as 1953 (Vargha & Gereb), when it was observed that many aphasics could sing the words of previously learned songs better than they could speak. Speech therapists are looking for new interventions to use with speech-disordered patients. Singing can be a valuable tool in speech rehabilitation because of its similarities to speech production. Singing is a musical behavior which shares with the speech elements of fundamental frequency, fundamental frequency variability, vocal intensity, rhythm or rate, and diction (Cohen, 1992). The use of singing has been successfully demonstrated to facilitate speech development with speech-disordered clients (Crocker, 1958; Darrow & Starker, 1986; Lathom, Edson, & Toombs, 1965; Leung, 1985; Ogden, 1982; Vanderark, 1986). Another research study (Keith & Aronson, 1975) was conducted with an adult woman with severe expressive aphasia. After a month of traditional speech therapy, her progress had been minimal, so the researchers decided to give her a one-hour singing therapy session once a week. After a short period of treatment, the patient was able to initiate speech with song-like phrases. Two months later, she had progressed to the point where she could eliminate the singing entirely from her speech. Differences between MIT and TS At this point the author needs to give convincing answers to the questions regarding the differences between MIT and TS. Therapeutic singing (TS) has distinct melodies and melodic intonation therapy is based on the spoken prosody of verbal utterances, so MIT has a limited range of sung notes and is comfortable for the untrained voice of adults.(Sparks & Holland, 1976). MIT, when using an intoned utterance which resembles a familiar song, may produce disastrous results. The familiar melody will stimulate a recall of the words of that song. According to the article by Sparks (1974), researchers selected for their client the target phrase, “meat and potatoes.” Then researchers asked the client to use a melody that resembled a popular song, “I love my baby, my baby loves me”. When the target phrase was presented and the aphasia client was asked to join in, the client altered it to, “Meat and potatoes, my baby loves me”. For this reason, MIT prohibits the use of familiar or popular melody lines in order to sing target phrases. Therapists must create a new melody prosody. On the other hand, for TS it is valuable to use popular or common songs which the clients already know and prefer to sing in order to improve their utterances of speech by the possible stimulation of the brain regions for long term memory. Modified Melodic Intonation Therapy Modified Melodic Intonation Therapy (MMIT) makes use of the strong association between MIT and TS. Baker (2000) has developed a modified version of MIT technique for working with persons with non-fluent expressive aphasia. In this technique, target phrases are more melodic than Sprechgesang (speech song), and the natural pattern of speech inflection is revised to create a more musical structure. The phrases chosen in modified MIT (MMIT) should be meaningful for functional communication (Baker & Tamplin, 2006). MMIT phrases are composed of a limited range of pitches within an octave to ensure that the patients are able to sing them comfortably. Two criteria prescribe the choice of musical phrases: first, the phrase is essentially different from the phrases preceding and following it; and second, the creation of a melodic phrase that can be easily encoded in memory and then produced on the researcher’s requests (Baker & Tamplin, 2006). Baker designed a procedure with six steps involved in the process: 1) Client sings familiar songs; 2) Therapist sings and plays functional phrases to clients; 3) Therapist sings the phrases; 4) Therapist withdraws participation; 5) Client practices independent production of learned words and phrases. (Musical cues may be provided initially and later faded); and 6) Client practices use of the phrases out side of music therapy context (pp. 148-149). In the first step clients are encouraged to sing familiar, well-known songs. It provides clients with immediate positive feedback of verbal output, motivation, and distraction when progress is slow and difficult. The second step involves the introduction of a small number of meaningful phrases set to simple melodies. However, it is important that phrases used should not resemble familiar melodies, as this might result in the accidental generation of non-propositional speech comprising the original lyrics of the song. The third is that clients are directed to sing the phrases with the therapist. And then, a therapist gradually withdraws participation and clients encourage to sing the phrases unaided in the fourth step. In the fifth step a therapist asks clients questions to test independent word generation. When this step has been completed, new phrase or short sentence can be introduced with new melodic phrases. The final step involves the use of the target words in normal conversation outside of the music therapy session (Baker & Tamplin, 2006). Measurement tools for functional communication skills Lezak (Lezak, Howieson, Loring, Fischer, & Hannay, 1995) suggests that a review of client’s language and speech functions to indicate whether communication problems are present will include examination of the following aspects of verbal behavior: 1) Spontaneous speech; 2) Repetition of words; 3) Speech comprehension; 4) Naming; 5) Reading; and 6) Writing. The Multimodal Communication Screening Test for Aphasia (MCST-A) is one of 3 criterion-referenced assessment tools in the Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC) Assessment Battery for Aphasia (AAC-ABA) developed by K. Garrett & J. Lasker. The MCST-A is designed to systematically assess whether people with severe aphasia can use alternative or augmentative modalities to communicate via alternative pictorial symbols. The second tool is Systems Trials protocol for Aphasia (AAC-STA), and the third tool is AAC Categorical Assessment (AAC-CAT) which will aid in determining whether the person with aphasia can benefit from partner dependent or independent alternative communication strategies (Garrett & Lasker, 2005). MCST-A will be used to modify the Naming measurement picture sheets in order to investigate the effectiveness of functional communication in this research. Another clinical application for measurement is the Aphasia Diagnostic Profiles (ADP) by Nancy Helrn-Estabrooks. ADP is a systematic method of assessing language and communication impairment associated with aphasia. Designed to meet the needs of medical settings, it creates profiles that explain critical areas of the client’s performance. Nine brief subtests create composite scores. It indicates overall severity and specific strengths and weaknesses. The severity profile identifies the client’s strongest response modalities. Error profiles identify the communication value of a client’s response. Behavioral profile indexes the client’s overall social-emotional state during testing. This assessment takes 40 to 50 minutes, but the tool will be revised to 10 minutes for pre-post tests in the current study. With the intent of identifying effective music therapy techniques, the purpose of this study is to ascertain the effectiveness of Therapeutic Singing and Melodic Intonation Therapy in improving the functional communication skills of adults with expressive aphasia. The two case studies were conducted with subjects who were in different level of functional speech. The goals of for subject L’s case study are as follows: (1) to determine whether Therapeutic Singing (TS) combined with Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) helps adult clients who are diagnosed with expressive aphasia as measured by the quality of their functional communication skills; (2) to determine whether MIT treatment 16 itself helps clients improve the quality of their functional communication skills; (3) to compare the effectiveness of TS combined with MIT and MIT alone in increasing functional communication skills; and (4) to investigate whether TS enhances the benefits of MIT treatment. On the other hand, the goal for subject R’s case study is as follows: 1) To give information whether Therapeutic Singing (TS) itself helps clients improve the quality of their functional communication skills. CHAPTER III METHOD Subjects Two adults with diagnoses of expressive aphasia with different levels of functional speaking were selected to participate in this study. One was described by the speech pathologist as high level and the other as low level in functional speaking. The subjects were recruited with the help of speech-language pathologists from the Aphasia Support Group in the Lansing area, explaining the research study in the group meeting. Announcements were also placed in the e-mail newsletters of the Aphasia Support Group (see Appendix 9- Flyer). Three restrictions were imposed on selecting the subjects: 1) Diagnosis of expressive aphasia caused by CVA; 2) Adult males; 3) No other therapy received, such as speech or physical therapy, during the course of this study. There are some differences in the two subjects. L is considered to have a the low level of functional communication skills, while R is considered to have a high level of functional communication skills, which means is that R did not need to receive MIT treatment because he was capable of success in the final level of MIT treatment. (see Table 1) Table 1 Subject Description Subjects Treatments Age Gender Diagnosis 32:38:): *SLC *SLE Fluency Expressive Aphasia L TS & MIT 62 Male 1 year Level Level Mild Apraxia 6 3 Lev“ 2 Expressive Aphasia Level Level R TS 53 Male Right Dysfunction 7 years 7 5 Level 5 (* SLC — Spoken Language Comprehension, * SLE - Spoken Language Expression) Subiect L - Low Functional Communication Skill On May 09, 2008, L had a sudden onset cerebrovascular accident (CVA) from a blood clot in the left parietal lobe. It resulted in expressive aphasia, mild apraxia of speech and the loss of memory fimction, both long-tenn and short-term. His spouse stated that his stroke was caused by high cholesterol and the distribution of stress. He was hospitalized after onset of the stroke and had undertaken speech therapy and physical- occupational therapy, but he wasn’t receiving any other treatments while this research treatment was being provided. In the area of Speech and Communication, L showed some limited characteristics in functional communication skills: 1) non-fluent, effortful, slow, halting and uneven speech; 2) limited word output, with short phrases and sentences; 3) misarticulated and distorted sounds; 4) agrammatical speech; 5) impaired repetition of words and sentences; 6) impaired naming; and 7) poorer production of speech than comprehension. The Spoken Language Comprehension (SLC) level was rated at a high level 6. According to the FCMs in America Speech and Hearing Association (ASHA), the level 6 in SLC states that “the individual is able to understand communication in most activities, but some limitations in comprehension are still apparent in vocational, avocational, and social activities. The individual rarely requires minimal cueing to understand complex sentences. The individual usually uses compensatory strategies when encountering difficulty” (See APPENDIX 11). On the other hand, the Spoken Language Expression (SLE) level was rated at a low level 3. According to the FCMs in ASHA, a level 3 in SLE means that “the communication partner must assume responsibility for structuring the communication exchange, and with consistent and moderate cueing, the individual can produce words and phrases that are appropriate and meaningful in context” (See APPENDIX 12). Furthermore, L was rated at the low level 2 in Fluency. According to the FCMs in ASHA, a level 2 in Fluency refers to Speech that is “functional most of the time, but labored in many day-to-day situations due to extended disruptions of speech flow which sometimes renders the individual difficult to understand. Participation in vocational, avocational, and social activities requiring speech is reduced overall. Listener discomfort is evident throughout conversational interactions” (See APPENDIX 13). In the area of Sensorimotor skills, his fine motor function on the right side was not fully stable, but he was able to ride a bike by himself. There also was limited control and ability in the facial muscles because he was diagnosed with mild apraxia. Therefore, the limited usage of tongue and mouth muscles caused limited accuracy of pronunciation in vowel sounds and especially in consonant sounds. In the area of cognitive function, there was a slight loss of memory functions. His wife stated that during the year after onset of the stoke, his short-terrn memory was coming back, but some of his long term memories were still blank. Subject R - High Functional Communication Skill R had a second stroke (CVA) seven years ago, with his first stroke (CVA) occurring twelve years ago. He was diagnosed with acute expressive aphasia and right side dysfunction. He had received speech, physical, and occupational therapies after the onset of the stroke, but he hasn’t been given any other therapy for several years, and there was no input from other therapies during the music therapy intervention. 20 In the area of Speech and Communication, L showed some limited characteristics in functional communication skills: 1) fluent, effortful, slow; 2) limited word output; 3) no impaired repetition of words and sentences; and 4) poorer production of speech than comprehension. The Spoken Language Comprehension (SLC) level 7 was higher than subject L’s. According to the F CMs in ASHA, a level 7 in SLC states, “the individual’s ability to independently participate in vocational, avocational, and social activities are not limited by spoken language comprehension. When difficulty with comprehension occurs, the individual consistently uses a compensatory strategy” (See APPENDIX 11). On the other hand, the Spoken Language Expression (SLE) level 5 was higher than L’s. According to the F CMs in ASHA, a level 5 in SLE indicates “the individual is able to successfully initiate communication using spoken language in structured conversations with both familiar and unfamiliar communication partners. The individual occasionally requires minimal cueing to frame more complex sentences in messages. The individual occasionally self-cues when encountering difficulty” (See APPENDIX 12). Furthermore, The Fluency level of 5 for subject R was higher than that of that of subject L. According to the FCMs in ASHA, a level 5 in Fluency indicated that “Speech is functional for communication, and fluency can be maintained in some situations. Self-monitoring is inconsistent. The frequency and severity of disruptions of speech flow within problem situations is distracting some of the time. Speech difficulties are noticeable when they occur, and sometimes limit vocational, avocational, and social activities requiring speech in problem situations. Listeners are occasionally aware of fluency difficulties relative to particular situations” (See Appendix 13). 21 In the area of Sensorimotor skills, there was a weakness in right side vision and limited horizontal range of vision in the right eye. R had difficulty in reading letters line by line because of the loss of vision. The fine motor skill was functioning quite well, but the tactile sensation in his right hand somewhat dysfunctional. In the area of cognitive function, his memory had returned since the onset of the stroke. His long term and short term memory was satisfactory, but there was a memory loss of specific professional skills in his occupation. This study was explained to each subject and the general content was reiterated to the subjects. Test measurements were presented briefly by using a Macintosh notebook. After subjects verified their agreement, their guardians and subjects were asked to sign the informed consent. (See Appendix 10 — Consent form) Setting and Apparatus The study was conducted in an available meeting room to which subjects had access, such as the living room or dining room at the subject’s homes. The materials and equipment used in the study were: response sheets, lyric sheets, pencils, castanets, a keyboard, a stop watch, a iMovie software, and PowerPoint software in the Macintosh notebook. While testing with the Spontaneous Speech Measure, iMovie software in the Macintosh notebook was used to record the subject’s production of speech, and while testing with the Naming Measure, PowerPoint software was used to randomize 90 pictures to form each sixteen-picture test. A Samsung stop watch was used to measure the given minutes. During MIT Treatment, castanets were used to help subjects and 22 therapists make rhythmic stimulus with one hand, instead of working on hand tapping with both hands. Lyric sheets and castanets were also used for the TS treatment. Lam—Wise A pre-experimental design consisting of a one-group pre-test-post-test study was used to administer the procedure. The subjects were pre-tested, received treatments, and then post-tested in each session (Albert, et al., 1973). A benefit of this design is the inclusion of a pre-test to determine baseline scores (2005), a useful design when there are not enough subjects to provide a control group. (See Figure 1) Figure 1. One Group pre-test-post-test Design vmrfilfi»y¢n3za “law-r. Pretest www— '— (Source: Dooley, 1990) There were eight dependent variables for subject L who had low functioning expressive aphasia, and seven dependent variables for subject R, who had high functional speech because this subject did not need to receive MIT treatment: 1) Total length of samples; 2) Total number of utterances; 3) Total number of words spoken by subjects; 4) Average length of utterances; 5) Average Number of Words per Minute in the Spontaneous Speech assessment of the Functional Communication Measure; and in the naming test: 6) the number of items named by a client; and 7) the number of prompts per item named given by a therapist in the Naming Assessment of the Functional Communication Measure (FCM); and in MIT test; 8) the percent scores collected by the 23 Melodic Intonation Therapy Measure; these variables were obtained from the pre-test, post-test, and MIT test, revised with the support of a speech-language pathologist and based on Aphasia Diagnostic Profiles (ADP) and Functional Communication Measures (FCMs) from the Adult National Outcomes Measurement System (NOMS). ELM The intervention period for the two subjects consisted of sixteen individual sessions, three to five times a week for 80 minutes, including pre and post-tests. The period for subject L (low function) was divided into two different terms of interventions of eight sessions each (16 sessions in all): 1) MIT in the first period of 8 sessions; 2) TS combined with MIT in the second period of 8 sessions (see Figure 2. Subject L’s Sessions Diagram). On the other hand, Subject R (high function) received TS itself for all 16 sessions because he could complete the final level of MIT at the outset and did not require MIT treatment. He could repeat items in normal speech patterns from the beginning of the sessions. When the researcher gave signals, he replied with appropriate and accurate sentences. Figure 2. Subject L ’s Sessions Diagram session ltl scx‘sinl‘. 15 ~.t‘\\.il)il lb sr-ssron 2 session it ‘ MIT '15 '18 erl' MIT 24 Subject L ’s Intervention The intervention strategies consisted of the original MIT protocol (Albert, et al., 1973) in the first period of sessions and TS protocol preceding MIT treatment in the each of second period of sessions. After pre-testing the Spontaneous Speech Test for 13 min. and the Naming Test for 5 min. before every session, a form of Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) was introduced, and L worked on the steps of each level of MIT for around 30 min. for eight sessions (from session 1 to 8). After half way through, by the eighth session of the procedure, Therapeutic Singing (TS) treatment was added from sessions nine to sixteen after the pre-test measures. Therefore, subject L received the pre- test, TS, MIT, and the post-test during the second period of sessions. There were two techniques used during his intervention: 1) Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT); 2) Therapeutic Singing (I S), described in the following paragraphs. 1) Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) The primary goal of MIT for non-fluent aphasics is the basic recovery of verbal utterances with the accurate use of language. There is also research evidence for its use with apraxia (Galloway & Kraus, 1982). Therefore, Subject L is a good candidate to practice MIT, as a subject who suffered from both non-fluent expressive aphasia and mild apraxia, to improve functional communication skills. There are four levels of MIT hierarchy and it includes three to four steps in each level. Subject L started his first sessions on level I and II and was capable of improving to level III at the end of the session. Before interventions began, the relevant sample materials of MIT needed to be collected. 25 Linguistic Contents The selection of linguistic content is important, depending on the severity of the aphasic’s communication inability and the usefulness of the verbal material (Sparks & Deck, 1986). Subject L’s sentences were chosen by the suggestions of his wife and himself about things as basic as family relationships, personal needs, and his background information. Content was subsequently selected regarding the complexity of syllables in each level, but some level II sentences were used in level III. According to the Sparks’ article (1986), meaningful stimulus samples for the aphasic who has no impairment other than phonological errors should focus on producing more intelligible speech. Subject L’s collection of contents, however, was more focused on the usefirlness to his daily life. Regarding the ability to produce utterances, less important words, such as prepositions, were eliminated from the contents. The phrases and sentences are illustrated in Table 2. Table 2. L ’s Linguistic Contents Subject L’s Sample linguistic contents for Level II What time? My name is L My wife is Marlene Take Sadie walk I had a stroke Today is Friday Good girl Sadie I had a dog What time want eat? One, two, three I love you, Marlene I have trouble speaking Cup of coffee Thanks for being I work at GM Go to sleep One, two, three, four I can understand you Jump Sadie My lovely wife Happy birthday to you Stay with me I love hunting The love in my life Thank you I love my country Thank you so much One O’clock God bless you 1 love you Time for lunch Subject L’s Sample linguistic contents for Level IH Happy birthday to you Go to bed at night 1 love to go hunting My name is L I love my wife, Marlene There will be a baby deer My wife is Marlene 1 have had a stroke 1 saw a deer this morning One or two babies I can understand you I’ve been walking with Sadie I saw female deer I love to take a bike I go to the gas station I mow this morning 26 What follows is the original MIT hierarchy from level I to level IV (see Appendix 2)(Sparks & Deck, 1986), and also the subject L’s MIT description for a non-fluent aphasia. MIT Hierarchy Level I The goal of the first level is the establishment of the client’s comfortable adaptation to the technique by learning intoning, hand tapping, and response to hand- signal controls of the therapist. The linguistic contents of level I were used from the sample materials in level 11 depending on the subject’s comfortable adaptation. When the researcher gave a stimulus by humming the sentence prosody and asking subject L to join humming in unison, he produced some unintelligible output instead of simply humming, but the researcher accepted his production as a humming response because the muscle movement of his tongue and face was restricted for speaking due to the symptoms of mild apraxia. For a convenient provision of the rhythmic stimuli, a castanet was used to produce rhythmic beats instead of hand tapping. In the beginning of the session, subject L utilized his more comfortable left-hand because he had a right-side weakness. On the first day of the sessions, the researcher repeated this level as long as L felt comfortable intoning, hand tapping, and responding to hand-signals. Subject L took about 5 to 10 minutes in the beginning of MIT treatment, then proceeded onto the next level as soon as he was comfortable with the first level goals. No scoring took place at this level. See more detailed procedures in table 3-1. 27 Table 3-1. MIT Hierarchy Level I LEVEL I — learn intoning and hand tapping, response to hand-signals Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Hums melody twice with (HT) (HT) Hand Tapping . Unison humming with (HT). Unison (Together) Response (Client) (C) Fades participation but continues (HT) Score and Progression No score. Proceed to next step 11. MIT Hierarchy Level II The goal of the second level is the aphasic subject’s immediate repetition of the target intoned sentence and the response to a question. Level II is a four-step procedure. He had remained at this level II protocol for thirteen sessions, and then proceeded to the next level III for sessions fourteen to sixteen. Subject L spent 20 to 30 minutes for level 11 of MIT technique in each session. For more detailed information on each of the four steps, scoring procedures are in Table 3-2. The researcher needed to modify the melody pattern on the target sentence whenever the subject was unable to produce verbal utterance of the melody. In other words, once the researcher modified the melody pattern on the target sentence, the change of melody stimulated the subject to produce clearer utterances. According to the book, Rhythm, Music, and the Brain (Thaut, 2005), musical elements as reinforcement are classified by three aspects; 1) Spatial (Pitch, Dynamic, Sound duration, Harmony); 2) Temporal (Tempo, Meter, Rhythmic pattern, Form); and 3) Force cueing (Dynamic, Harmony, Timbre, Tempo). This modification of melody pattern is included in the area of spatial cueing, such as pitch. There is research evidence based on the effect of musical cues for persons with aphasia (Cohen & Ford, 1995). Namely, when the researcher modified the melody with high pitch intonation on the last word, subject L produced 28 clearer utterance, so the change of pitch stimulated his speech production. For example, the melody for the phrase (go to sleep) was changed, so that the last word (sleep) had a higher pitch than before. For more information about the modification of melody patterns, refer MIT article (Sparks & Deck, 1986). The linguistic contents of level II was selected from the contents of subject L’s daily-based usage. The numbers of syllables were in the range of two to six syllables for a sentence or a phrase. His first sentence was selected from the sentence with the most usage, such as Take Sadie Walk, which was reduced from full sentences such as I love to take Sadie out for a walk, depending on the level of his functional communication skills. Six to eight sentences were used for this level II procedure from the sample materials for linguistic contents (see Table 2). There are five specific techniques of level II technique: 1) Hand Tapping; 2) Control by hand signals; 3) Fading Participation; 4) Unison Repetition; and 5) Verbal Cueing. Hand Tapping with a Castanet Rhythmic stimulus has proved to be an effective musical element to activate the htunan nervous system through several decades (Sacks, 2006). The castanets were used to stimulate the subject by rhythmic sound. While humming the melody, the researcher started making the rhythmic beats with the castanet, and then asked the subject to join making the rhythm first with the castanet alone and then in unison with the intoned sentence with the castanet. Subject L used his more functional left hand for the castanet at the beginning of the sessions, and the researcher also encouraged him to gradually use his right hand depending on his adjustment. The researcher asked the subject to turn a little 29 bit in his chair, so that the performance was visible to each other, the researcher and the subject. Control by Hand Signals Using a castanet for hand signals was an effective way to control the subject’s response as a nonverbal means. It was used in many steps when the researcher asked the subject to join in humming or intoning sentences by hand signals. It was also used to guide the subject on when to take turns, such as the subject needed to listen or to take turns with the researcher for the repetitions in step 3. Also the castanet was held up and forward toward the subject to produce one strong rhythmic sound. This way the signals were useful in enforcing latency and inserted delay for responding, if used consistently (Sparks & Deck, 1986). Fading Participation & Immediate Repetition Level II of the hierarchy involved fading the participation of the researcher in step 2 and the immediate repetition by the subject after the researcher had just presented in step 3. If the researcher faded her participation in order to allow the ‘solo’ performance, subject L sometimes stopped producing the intoned sentence at early stages in sessions, but he did gradually keep going to perform solo by making beats with his castanet. When the subject stopped his vocal production, the researcher needed to rejoin in unison because it was evident that he was not ready to proceed to step 3 of solo repetition on his own. Verbal Cuing The use of verbal cuing is shown in step 3 and step 4 in Level II, and the cue has an effect on counting scores (see Table 3-2). If he wasn’t able to repeat the intoned 30 sentence in step 3 as a solo, the researcher gave verbal cuing of the initiation of the utterance in the target sentence. L needed the cuing in most sentences in steps 3 and 4. Table 3-2 MIT Hierarchy Level II LEVEL 2 — Ability repeating intoned sentences immediately, Response to a question Step Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression 1.Hums melody with (HT), 2.1ntones sentence with (HT), 3.Signals (C) to join in unison intoning of sentence Intone sentence together with (HT) - Unison Acceptable- 1 point. Proceed to Step 2, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. Step Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression Same as Step 1 But fades participation except (HT) Intone sentence with (HT) gradually Solo Acceptable- 1 point. Proceed to Step 3, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. Step Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression 1.Signals (C) to listen 2.1ntones sentence with (HT) 3.Signals (C) to repeat, Cueing for initiation of utterance if necessary Repeat intoned sentence with (HT) immediately Acceptable— 2 point without cue, 1 point with cue Proceed to step 4, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. Step Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression 1.1ntones question, “What did you say?” 2.Signal (C) to repeat Cueing for initiation of response if necessary Repeat intoned sentence with (HT) Acceptable— 2 point without cue, 1 point with one Proceed to step 1, for next sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. (Table.3-2. MIT Hierarchy Level 11) 31 MIT Hierarchy Level III The goal of the third level is the aphasic’s ability to delay responses and respond to less specific questions. Through this level of training, the subject was able to respond with the intoned sentence even if the researcher signaled to wait for 2 to 3 seconds. Backup The important technique, Backup, was introduced in level III and IV, which was the means of attempting indirect correction of errors by repeating the previous step. This manner of correcting errors is meaningful, as the subject may or may not be aware of the purpose of this procedure, but his failure is not drawn to his attention (Sparks & Deck, 1986). If the subject presented an unacceptable response in step 2, he was guided to return to the previous step of listening to the intoned sentence and following in unison, followed by a retrial of the repetition after a l or 2 second delay. This backup procedure was used in level III and IV. There are three steps in the procedures for level III. The subject received this level III protocol from the 14th to l6tb sessions and was unable to precede to the last level of MIT. L took 20 to 30 minutes for the level HI techniques in each session. He showed comfortable adaptation in intoning sentences with gradual solo performance after the researcher removed her unison participation, but he was challenged by repeating intoned sentence after the 1 or 2 second delay in step 2. For more detailed information on each of the three steps in Level III procedures and scoring are in Table 3-3. 32 Table 3-3. MIT Hierarchy Level III LEVEL 3 — Ability to the delay of responses, Response to specific questions Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Step Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression 1.1ntones sentence with (HT), 2.Signals (C) to join in unison sentence 3. Fade participation except (HT) Intone sentence with (HT) gradually Solo Acceptable- 1 point. Proceed to Step 2, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Step Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression 1.Signals (C) to listen 2.1ntones sentence with (HT) 3.Signals (C) to repeat after 1 or 2 second Delay 4. If (C) fails, Backup step 1 (B) Back up Repeat Intoned sentence with (HT) afier Delay If fail, backup step 1 and retrial Acceptable- 2 point without (B), 1 Point with (B) Process to Step 3, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Step Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression l.Intone a related question (e. g. what kind of, How many) 2.Signals (C) to answer 3. If (C) fails, Backup step 2 (B) Back up Appropriate Answer (Intoned or Spoken) If fail, backup step 2 and retrial Acceptable- 2 point without (B), 1 Point with (B) Process to Step 1 for next sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. MIT Hierarchy Level IV (Table.3-3. MIT Hierarchy Level 111) The goal of this last level of MIT is the aphasic’s ability to return to normal speech prosody, which is facilitated by a technique called Sprechgesang (speech-song). In this technique the melodic line remained the same as the intoned sentence of the preceding step, but the pitches of intoned words were replaced by the variable pitch of natural speech, retaining the tempo, rhythm, and stress of the target sentences. More 33 complex and longer sentences are used as the target materials in this level. There are four procedural steps in level IV, but subject L was unable to precede to this level. For more detailed information on each four-step procedure and scoring, see Table 3-4. More descriptive explanations of MIT procedures can be found in the literature (Sparks & Deck, 1986). Table 3-4. MIT Hierarchy Level IV Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Step 1 Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression LEVEL 4 — Ability of Normal Speech 1.Signal (C) to listen 2.1ntone sentence with (HT) 3.Present it twice in Sprechgesang 4.Signal (C) unison Sprechgesang of sentence (HT) 5. If (C) fails, Backup to present in Sprechgesang 6. Retrial No. 4 Sprechgesang of sentence with (HT) Acceptable- 2 point with Sprechgesang. - 1 point with Backup - Proceed to Step 2, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Step Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression 1.Signal (C) to listen 2.Present sentence in Sprechgesang with (HT) 3.Signals (C) to repeat after 2 or 3 second Delay 4. If (C) fails, Backup to Step 1 same sentence Sprechgesang of sentence with (HT) after 2 or 3 second Delay. Acceptable- 2 point without Backup - 1 point with Backup - Proceed to Step 3, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. (Continued) 34 Table 3-4. MIT Hierarchy Level IV (Continued) Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Step Response (Client) (C) 3 Score and Progression 1.Signal (C) to listen 2.Present sentence once in Sprechgesang 3.Present sentence twice in Normal Speech Prosody 4.Signals (C) to repeat after 2 or 3 second Delay 5.If (C) fails, Backup to Step 2 same sentence Repeat sentence in Normal Speech Prosody after2 or 3 second Delay, no (HT) Acceptable- 2 point without Backup - 1 point with Backup - Proceed to Step 4, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Step 4 Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression 1.Ask question about substantive information on the same sentence. 2. If fail, Backup step 3 same sentence and retrial 3. Ask question about associative information 4. If fail, No backup and retrial if fail one. Answer appropriate responses 3 point — bonus point, one or more associative responses 2 point — without Backup, substantive content 1 point — with Backup. Proceed to next sentence. Seven Principles There are seven important principles of speech-language therapy involved in MIT that the researcher should remember while leading MIT hierarchy (Sparks & Deck, 1986): 1) gradual progression of increased length and difficulty of task; 2) indirect attempts to correct by the ‘backup’ technique; 3) repetition; 4) timing and controlled latency; 5) avoidance of practice effect; 6) attention to the purpose of the subject’s verbal utterance; 7) frequency of sessions. 35 2) Therapeutic Singing (TS) According to Thaut’s book (2005), Therapeutic Singing (TS) is a reinforcing and supporting technique for speech and language development, used with a variety of neurological or developmental speech and language dysfunctions. This technique is able to be used for a broad range of therapeutic goals, and also to support the goals of other specific therapeutic techniques, such as Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT), Vocal Intonation Therapy (V IT), musical speech stimulation (MUSTIM), and so on. Modified Melodic Intonation Therapy (MMIT) is the modified technique of MIT by combining the Therapeutic Singing technique in MIT procedure. The Therapeutic Singing (TS) technique was added to subject L’s intervention before conducting MIT in the second period of sessions, from the 9th to the 16’” Subject R, who was in high level of functional communication skill, utilized only TS without MIT. There are different hierarchies of treatment outlined by the researcher, depending on the level of speech and language for each subjects L and R. Therapeutic Singing Hierarchy [or subject L 1. Stimulate memory function - Therapist (T) play a familiar song with keyboard - Client (C) listen to the song - Acceptable — (C) respond to the song as familiar - Unacceptable — (C) don’t respond at all. Use next song 2. Initiation of Utterance - (T) sing the song by accompanying on the keyboard. - (T) invite (C) unison singing to the song. - Acceptable — (C) produce utterances (unclear, humming acceptable) - Unacceptable — (C) don’t respond at all. Retrial step 1. 3. Adjustment of syllables and Repetition - (T) sing in unison by adjusting to a comfortable beat of song for (C) - (T) repeat the song twice in unison - (C) sing the song in unison with 20% or more accurate syllables produced. - Acceptable - (Full participation required, unclear production acceptable) 36 - Unacceptable - (C) did not respond at all. Retrial step 2. - If Second retrial fails, proceed to next song material 4. Fade (T) participation - (T) withdraw participation and encourage (C) to produce utterances - (T) check (C) accuracy of production while (C) sing SOLO - Acceptable -— (C) sing alone with 20 % or more production of lyrics - Unacceptable — (C) terminate participation, Retrial step 3. 5. Correct with articulated speech - (T) distinguish the unclear errors between consonant and vowel errors - (T) ask (C) to imitate the oral movement of intoned vowel sounds on unclear words - (T) ask (C) to imitate the oral movement of consonant sounds on unclear words - (C) correct his utterance of words by training the oral motor movement 6. Unison again — (T) encourage (C) to sing in unison again with instrumental accompaniment This hierarchy was designed by the researcher and focused on the goals based on his weaknesses in speech skills. Because of the symptoms of acute expressive aphasia and mild apraxia, two primary goals were founded; l) to increase verbal utterance through the song lyrics; 2) to improve the accurate speech articulation by focusing on a number of syllable, consonant, and vowel sounds. This procedure was generally utilized for subject L’s TS based on his goals. An average of four songs were used in each session, and he had the repeated usage of song materials with one or two new songs added in each session. As a result, subject L increased the production of a number of words through song lyrics and also gradually improved the accuracy of verbal utterances of syllables, consonant and vowel sounds. By repeating the song materials from sessions the 9th to the 16th, he accomplished 100% of word production with accurate verbal syllables and vowel sounds in lyrics, except for accuracy of consonant sounds. His improvement was usually ordered: first, gradually producing the accurate number of syllables by singing on vowel sounds. Second, gradually improving the clarity of vowel 37 sounds in matching rhythm and beat of the target song materials, and finally, producing the exact consonant sounds. While he participated in this hierarchy, his slurred speech was greatly reduced. The list of song selections was taken from the books: The Great Family Songbook (W eisssman, 2007); America’s All-Time Favorite Songs (Stone, 2009); The Best Songs Ever (Leonard, 1985); Rise Up Singing (Patterson, 2004). The song materials were given to subject L to assist in making his own song profiles, which were used for the second period of the sessions. Songs were listed such as Home on the Range, God Bless America, My Country ’ T is of Thee (America), If You 're Happy, I ’ve Been Working on the Railroad, MyBonny Lies Over the Ocean, etc. Therapeutic Singing Hierarchy (or subject R 1.Reading lyrics - Therapist (T) ask Client (C) to read the lyric on the song sheet. - Client (C) read the lyrics on the sheet. - Acceptable — (C) reads the lyrics with (T)’s prompts (Verbal or Physical) - Unacceptable -— (C) stops to read with frustration 2.Listening Stimulation - (T) ask (C) to listen to the song with sight-reading by matching the rhythmic beat with castanets simultaneously. - (T) make the rhythmic beat with a castanet while listening. - (C) listen to the song with sight-reading and playing the castanet simultaneously. - Acceptable — (C) listening with sight-reading and making rhythmic beats with the castanet. - Unacceptable — (C) don’t respond with the sight-reading and making beats 3.Adjustment to the song - repeat #. 2 ' - (T) focus on (C) verbal outputs 4.Unison singing with accompaniment on the keyboard - (T) encourage (C) unison singing by accompanying on the keyboard. - (C) sing in unison with sight-reading and matching the rhythmic beat with the castanet 38 - Acceptable — (C) sings the lyric with 80% of accuracy - Unacceptable — (C) shows frustration and stops singing 5.Fade (T) participation - (T) withdraw participation and encourage (C) to sing alone - (T) check (C) accuracy of production while (C) sings SOLO - (C) sing alone by matching the rhythmic beat of the song with the castanet - Acceptable — (C) sings alone with 80 % or more accuracy of lyrics - Unacceptable - (C) terminates participation This hierarchy was designed by the researcher and focused on the goals based on his areas of weakness in both speech fimctions and other dysfunctional areas. Because of his loss of range of vision, he had feelings of frustration whenever he tried to do any reading. Two primary goals were established in the TS intervention: 1) to improve the reading skill without frustration; 2) to improve accurate speech articulation by matching the right rhythmic beats of the song. This procedure was generally utilized for subject R’s TS intervention based on his goals. An average of four songs was used in each session, along with repeated usage of the same song materials. Several new songs were gradually added. As much as repeating the materials were important, steps 1 and 2 were skipped and the intervention procedure and time consumption for the song material was reduced. If new song material was added, the new material started from step 1. As a result, subject R improved the reading skills without having frustration. Through repeating the song materials from sessions 1 to 16, be accomplished steady reading skills without any prompts or terminations while performing the song materials. While he participated in this hierarchy of treatment, his slow speech with unmatched rhythmic beats and his reading failures were reduced. Subject R’s song materials were collected from the Beatles CD Album to make his own song profile, which were used for the full period of sixteen sessions. Songs were 39 listed such as Let it Be (Beatles), A Hard Days Night (Beatles), A Day in The Life (Beatles), Venus (Frankie Avalon), Look What You ’ve Done to Me (Boz Scaggs), Imagine (John Lennon), Woman (John Lennon), Hey, Jude (John Lennon), Yesterday (John Lennon), Everybody Talking (John Lennon), and so on. WI—m—WM There were two measuring procedures used for data collections: Functional Communication Measure (F CM) used in both the pre-tests and post-tests and Melodic Intonation Therapy Measure (MITM). On the pre and post-test, F CM was used to examine whether two musical interventions (TS & MIT) had effectiveness in improving functional communication skills. FCM was composed of two different parts: Spontaneous Speech Measurement (SPM) and Naming Measurement (NM). See the measurement 1 forms in Appendix 4 (SPM) and Appendix 6 (NM). According to aphasia assessment (Lezak, et al., 1995), there are six examination areas that are aspects of verbal behavior: 1) Spontaneous Speech; 2) Repetition of words; 3) Speech Comprehension; 4) Naming; 5) Reading; and 6) Writing. A review of language and speech functions indicates whether communication problems are present in those six areas (Lezak, et al., 1995). In order to reduce the measuring spans during each session, two examinations, l) Spontaneous Speech and 2) Naming, were chosen from among the six examination areas above to make up the Functional Communication Measurement (FCM). 4o Spontaneous Speech Measurement & Naming Measurement in F CM In the Spontaneous Speech Measurement, five variables were collected through each pre-test and post-test: 1) Total Length of Samples; 2) Total Number of Utterances; 3) Total Number of Words; 4) Average length of Utterances; and 5) Average Number of Words per Minute. For more information about the Scoring Hierarchy of the Spontaneous Speech, see Table 4-1. The video recorded data was analyzed by utilizing the i-movie program in the Macintosh notebook for the 2 to 3 hours of time used in each session. Before the pre-test, subjects were asked to choose the questions they would like to answer, but the set time to answer was no longer than 13 minutes. The same questionnaire was conducted in both the pre-test and post-test. The list of questions was made from the materials in the Aphasia Needs Assessment (Garrett & Beukelman, 1995) and the Aphasia Diagnostic Profiles questions (ADP)(Helm-Estabrooks, 1992). Table 4-1. Scoring Hierarchy at Spontaneous Speech Measurement Scoring Hierarchy of Spontaneous Speech Measurement in F CM 1. Total Length of Samples Sum of Seconds, including both intelligible and unintelligible words The unspoken times were deleted after 10 second without subject’s responses. 2. Total Number of Utterances Including paraphasic errors The effort in seconds of words produced was included in the utterances. If the subject stops to speak by saying ‘1 don’t know’, then the utterance was ended as counting an utterance. 3. Total Number of Words Counting Total Number of Words produced for 13 minutes. 2-1. Average Length of Average number of words for three longest intelligible phrases Utterances e.g.) l. I like a ball (4 words) * .me too (2 words) 2. I want to go home (5 words) 3. I love peanut (3 words) *. Lovely (l word) = 4+5+3/3 = 4 avera e len of utterances 3-1. Average Number of Words Total number of words are divided the average length of samples. per Minute It means the Speed of SpeakirE 41 On the other hand, the Naming Test content was composed of the picture stimulus sheets, which was edited from the picture materials in the Boston Naming Test (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 2001), the Aphasia Diagnostic Profiles (ADP) (Helm- Estabrooks, 1992), and the Test of Adolescent/Adult Word Finding (TAWF) (German, 1992). A total of 90 pictures were selected from the references with the assistance of a speech and language pathologist. A total of sixteen different picture sets were manipulated by randomizing the order of ninety pictures, which was made by using the PowerPoint slideshow program in the Macintosh Notebook. A total of five minutes was given to the test, and the same ordered set of pictures was used both pre and post-test in each session. In the naming test, two variables were measured: 1) Number of items named by subjects; 2) Average Number of prompts per item named. Subjects L and R each had a different scoring hierarchy, according to the level of speaking. See table 4-2. Table 4-2. Scoring Hierarchy at Naming measurement Scoring Hierarchy of Naming Measurement in F CM 1. Number of Items * Subject L —- Counted the total number of items named, including writing named by subjects letters * Subject R — Counted the total number of items named with verbal speaking. 2. Average Number of Counted the total number of prompts given by the researcher, then divided into Prompts per item #.1 above (Total number of Items named) named "‘ Subject L - Included the prompts in Appendix 7 (Hierarchy of Word Retrieval Prompts for the Client with Aphasia) and written prompts by correction of the letter errors. Unacceptable — if no response after four prompts given, pass to the next picture. * Subject R — Included the verbal prompts in Appendix 7 Unacceptable - If no response after 5 seconds, given a prompt, then if no response, given another prompt, then after 5 seconds, pass to the next picture On the other hand, MIT measure was conducted while the subject was participating in MIT treatment procedure. There are brief guidelines for the scoring 42 hierarchy in Table 4-3 to illustrate management of errors in the subject’s response (Sparks & Deck, 1986). Table 4-3. Scoring Hierarchy at MIT Measurement Scoring Hierarchy of Melodic Intonation Therapy (M11) Measurement Level 11 Scoring Examples description Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4 First sentence: Subject succeeds in all steps. Maximum scores attained l l 2 2 Second Sentence: Succeeds in all steps but requires a cue to initiate response in Steps 3 and 4. 1 1 1 1 Third Sentence: Succeeds in Steps 1 and 2, requires a cue to initiate response in Step 3, and fails Step 4 because of an unacceptable response 1 l 1 0 after backup Fourth Sentence: Succeeds in Steps 1 and 2, requires a cue to initiate response in Step 3, and requires a backup to initiate Step 4. 1 l l 1 Fifth Sentence: Succeeds in Step 1, fails in Step 2. Progression stopped 1 0 _ - and no scores given for Steps 3 and 4. Total 18/24 (75%) 5/5 4/5 5/8 4/6 (Continued) Level III Scoring Examples description Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 First sentence: Subject succeeds in all steps. Maximum scores attained 1 2 2 Second Sentence: Succeeds in all steps but requires backups for Steps 2 l 1 l and 3. Third Sentence: Succeeds in Steps 1, requires a backup to initiate Step 2, Succeeds in Step 3 without backup. I l 2 Fourth Sentence: Succeeds in Steps 1, requires a backup in Step 2 because Of a a a a l l 0 inaccurate response, fails to initiate response in step 3 after a backup. Fifih Sentence: Succeeds in Step 1, fails to repeat accurately in step 2, and fails again after a backup. Progression stopped and no score may be given for l 0 - step 3. Total 15/23 (65%) 5/5 5/ 10 5/8 (Table 4-3 — Scoring Hierarchy of MIT Measurement (Sparks & Deck, 1986) 43 Table 4-3. Scoring Hierarchy at MIT Measurement (Continued) Level IV Scoring Examples description Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4 First sentence: Succeeds in Steps 1, 2, and 3, requires a backup for one 2 2 specific question in step 4, but answers all associative questions. Second Sentence: Succeeds in Steps 1, 2, and 3, requires a backup for Step 2 2 4, fails to answer any associative question. Third Sentence: Succeeds in Steps 1 and 2, requires a backup to initiate normal prosody in Step 3. Succeeds in Step 4 but no bonus because of 2 2 failure on last associative question. Fourth Sentence: Succeeds in Step 1, requires backups for Steps 2 and 3. 2 1 Succeeds with bonus in Step 4. Fifth Sentence: Requires a backup to succeed in Step 1 and then succeed . 1 2 in subsequent steps. 2 2 Total 37/45 (82%) 9/10 9/10 8/10 ”/15 (Table 4-3 — Scoring Hierarchy of MIT Measurement (Sparks & Deck, 1986) 44 CHAPTER IV RESULTS Findings and Analysis Through the Functional Communication Measurement (FCM) and Melodic Intonation Therapy Measurement (MITM), data on eight variables were collected. There are two different measurements in the Functional Communication Measurement; First, Spontaneous Speech Measurement; second, Naming Measurement. Five variables were collected in the Spontaneous Speech Measurement (SPM) and two variables were collected in in the Naming Measurement (NM), and one variable was collected in the Melodic Intonation Therapy Measurement (MITM). There were four problems for subject L’s case study: (1) to investigate whether Therapeutic Singing (TS) combined with Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) helps adult clients who are diagnosed with expressive aphasia as measured by the quality of their fimctional communication skills; (2) to investigate whether MIT treatment itself helps clients improve the quality of their functional communication skills; (3) to compare the effectiveness of TS combined with MIT and MIT alone in increasing functional communication skills; and (4) to determine whether TS enhances the benefits of MIT treatment. On the other hand, there was only one goal for the subject R’s case study; 1) to investigate whether Therapeutic Singing (TS) itself helps clients improve the quality of their functional communication skills. 45 In order to select an appropriate statistical test in these two case studies, descriptive statistics was conducted after collecting the data on the eight variables. There are two methods of evaluating the data: first, the analysis of the short-term effect and second, the analysis of a long-term effect of the interventions. In order to prove the short- terrn effect, paired t-tests were conducted to compare and contrast the differences between pre and post-test variables. The subjects received the pre-test, interventions, and then post-test in each session, so this t-test could indicate the short-term effect of the intervention by comparing pre and post-test data. Second, a long-term effect of intervention was evaluated by comparing the means of eight variables. The long-term effect is shown by whether or not the means of second- half sessions are higher than the means of first-half sessions. The first—half data collections from the 1St to the 8th sessions were calculated as the means for the first-half, and the means for the second-half data were calculated from the 9’h to the 16th sessions. Spontaneous Speech Measurement The results for the Spontanous Speech Measurement were determined by collecting data on five different variables: 1) Total Length of Samples (See Figure 3-1 and 3-2); 2) Total Number of Utterances (See Figure 3-3 and 3-4); 3) Total Number of Words (See Figure 3-5 and 3-6); 4) Average Length of Utterance (See Figure 3-7 and 3- 8); 5) Average Number of Words per Minute (See Figure 3-9 and 3-10). For more information about raw data, See Appendix 14-1 and 14-2. 46 . Total Length of Samples L's Spontaneous Speech Test 1. Total Length of Samples H N H O A. Total Length of Sample QN-Fa\m I T I I T T T r I I I I I T 1 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10111213141516 Sessions "' 1-pre +1-post (Figure 3-1. Total Length of Sample in Us Spontaneous Speech Test) Paired Samples Statistics Std. Error Mean 11 Std. Deviation llean Pair Pre_TesLl 6.7114 16 152915 .38229 1 Post'l‘estl 8.1625 16 181219 15305 (Table 5-1. L’s Paired t-test on Total Length of Samples) Paired Samples Test Figure 3-1 shows the increasing tendency in post- test data for subject L’s total length of samples. There was a long-term effect of the intervention demonstrated by comparing the means between the first-half and the second-half sessions. Paired Differences Mean Std. Error 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference Std. Deviation llean Lower Upper 1 d1 Sig. (2-tailedl Pair Pre_TesL1 - l PosLTesLl -l.45111 164604 .41151 -2.32822 -.57399 -3.526 15 .003 (Table 5-2. L’s Paired t-test on Total Length of Samples) The mean for the second-half sessions (the 9’“ — the 16’“) on the pre-test (M=7.6) was higher than the mean for the first-half sessions (the 1S” — the 8th) on the pre-test (M=5.8). Also, the mean of the post-tests of the second-half sessions (M=9.4) was higher 47 than the mean of the post-tests of first-half sessions (M=6.9). It indicated that the total length of samples increased on the second-half sessions. Therefore, it is shown that the intervention of MIT and MIT combined with TS had a positive effect on the improvement of total length of samples as a long-term effect. In order to investigate whether there was the short-term effect on the sample length, a paired-samples t-test was conducted to compare pre-test and post-test from the first session to the sixteenth session. There was a statistically significant difference in the scores for pre-test (M=6.71, SD=1.52) and post-test (M=8.16, SD=1.82); t(15)=-3.526, p = 0.003 (p<0.05) (See Table 5-1 & 5-2). These results suggest that each intervention does have an effect on the difference between pre and post-test. Therefore, the results indicate that with both the interventions of MIT and MIT combined with TS, the total length of samples increased. Fi e 3-2 shows there was R's Spontaneous Speech Test gm 1. Total Length of Samples the tendency of slightly v, 12 d) ' ' - 2' 10 higher pornts on post tests in: 8 than on the pre-tests in the o 6 El: R’s total length of samples. 5 4 0 :3: 2 There was a slight difference a 0 I I I I l I I I T I T 1 l 1 ‘ ‘ on R’s long-term effect of 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 910111213141516 Sessions intervention by comparing "A'- 1-pre +1-post the mean of the first-half and (Figure 3-2. Total Length of Sample in R’s Spontaneous Speech Test) the second-half of sessions. 48 Paired Samples Statistics Std. Error Mean ll Std. Deviation Mean Wesu 6.1885 18 139134 .34784 1 PosLTesLl 7.4896 16 1.58987 .3974? (Table 5-3. R’s Paired t-test on Total Length of Samples) Paired Samples Test Paired Differences 95% Confidence Interval of the Std. Error Difference , llean Std. Deviation llean Lover Upper t d! Sig. (2-tailed) Pair Pre_TesL1 - 1 PosLTesLl -1.30104 2.03471 .50868 -2.38526 -.21682 -2.558 15 .022 (Table 5-4. R’s Paired t-test on Total Length of Samples) The mean of second-half sessions on the pre-test (M=6.02) was slightly lower than the mean of first-half sessions on the pre-test (M: 6.36). There were no improvements on the pre-tests. On the other hand, the mean of the post-tests of the second-half sessions (M=7.74) was slightly higher than the post-test mean of first-half sessions (M=7.23). It indicates that the total length of samples increased during the second-half sessions on only the post-tests. Therefore, it is shown that the intervention of TS had a positive effect, though less than the L’s improvement of total length of samples, as a long-term effect. In order to investigate whether there was a short-term effect on the sample length, a paired-sample t-test was conducted to compare pre-test and post-test from the first session to sixteenth session. There was a statistically significant difference in the scores for pre-test (M=6. l8, SD=1.39) and post-test (M=7.48, SD=1.58); t (15)=-2.558, p = 0.022 (p<0.05) (See Table 5-3 & 5-4). These results suggest that each intervention does 49 have an effect on the difference between pre and post-tests. Therefore, the total length of samples increased through the intervention of TS alone. 2. Total Number of Utterances L's Spontaneous Speech Test 8 2. Total Number of Utterances :40 E35 0 3:30 225 220 315 S10 2 5 E o 13 1 2 3 4 s 6 7 8 9 10111213141516 Sessions '4' Z-pre +2-post (Figure 3-3. Total Number of Utterances in Us Spontaneous Speech Test) Paired Samples Statistics Std. Error Mean— ll Std. Deviatifl Mean Pair Pre_Test_2 18.1260 16 3.44238 .86060 1 PosLTesL2 23.5625 16 6.81145 1.70286 (Table 6-1. L’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Utterances) Figure 3-3 shows the increasing tendency in post- test data in L’s total number of utterances. The long-terrn effect of the interventions was shown through comparing the means between the first-half and the second-half sessions. Paired Samples Test Paired [finances 95% Confidence Interval ofthe Std. Error Diffusin— Mean Std. Deviation Mean Lower Upper t df Sig. (2-tailedl Pair Pre.Test_2- _ 1 PosLTesL? '5.43750 7.00446 1.75112 -9.16991 -l.70509 '3.105 15 .007 (Table 6-2. L’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Utterances) The mean. for second-half sessions on the pre-test (M=l 7) was lower than the mean for first-half sessions (M=19.25). There was no improvement on the pre-test. On the other hand, the mean of the post-tests of second-half sessions (M=24.12) was higher than the post-test mean of first-half sessions (M=23). It indicates that the total number of utterances increased on the second-half sessions on only the post-test. Therefore, it is shown that the intervention of MIT and MIT combined with TS had a positive effect on the improvement of the total number of utterances on the post-tests as a long-term effect. In order to investigate whether there was the short-term effect on number of utterances, a paired-samples t-test was conducted to compare pre-tests and post-tests from first session to sixteenth session. There was a statistically significant difference in the scores for pre-test (M=l8. 12, SD=3 .44) and post-test CM=23.56, SD=6.81); t(15)=-3.105, p = 0.007 (p<0.05) These results suggested that there was a short-term effect of the interventions shown by the difference between pre and post-tests data. Therefore, the results suggested that when MIT or MIT combined with TS were conducted, the total number of utterances increased on the post-tests. 51 Total Number of Utterances Sessions R's Spontaneous Speech Test 2. Total Number of Utterances 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 910111213141516 'i- 2-pre + 2-post (Figure 3-4. Total Number of Utterances in R’s Spontaneous Speech Test) Paired Samples Statistics Mean Std. Deviation Mean Std. Error Pair Pre_TesL2 12W) 16 250—768 .65192 1 PosLTesLZ 14.0000 16 3.74166 .93541 (Table 6-3. R's Paired t-test on Total Number of Utterances) Paired Samples Test Figure 3—4 shows the slightly increasing tendency in post-test data for R’s total number of utterances. There was a long-term effect in the interventions by comparing the means between the first-half and the second-half sessions. Paired Differences Mean Std. Deviation Std. Error 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference Mean Lower Upper t df Sig. t2-tailedl Pair Pre_TesL2 - 1 PosLTesLZ -1.50000 3.65148 .9128? -3.44674 .44574 -1.643 15 .121 (Table 6—4. R‘s Paired t-test on Total Number of Utterances) The mean for the second-half sessions (the 9m_ the 16*) on the pre-test (M=12.63) was slightly higher than the mean for the first-half sessions (the 1" — the 8m) on the pre- test (M=12.38). Also, the post-test mean for the second-half sessions (M=15.88) was 52 notably higher than the post-test mean for the first-half sessions (M=12.16). It indicated that the total number of utterances increased during the second-half sessions both pre and post-tests. Therefore, it is shown that the intervention of the TS had a positive effect on the improvement of total number of utterances as a long-term efi‘ect. In order to investigate whether there was the short-term effect on the number of utterances, a paired-samples t-test was conducted to compare the pre-tests and the post- tests fiom the first to the sixteenth sessions. There was no statistically significant differences in the scores for pre-test (M=12.5, SD=2.6) and post-test (M=l4, SD=3.74); t(15)= -l.643, p = 0.121 (p>0.05) (See Table 6-3 & 6-4). Although there was a slight difference between the means (pre = 12.5 and post = 14), this result indicated that the intervention did not result in a statistical difference between pre and post-test. Therefore, the result showed that the total number of utterances did not significantly increase through the intervention. 3. Total Number of Words ' Figure 3-7 shows an L s Spontaneous Speech Test 3. Total Number of Words increasing tendency in u, 800 E 700 L’s total number of 3 600 “5 500 words. The long-tenn l- 0 400 .9 S 300 effect of the E 200 intervention was shown 3 100 o [- 0 through comparing the 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 910111213141516 Sessions means between the 'i- 3-pre +3-post (Figure 3-5. Total Number of Words in L’s SP Test) 53 first-half and the second-half sessions. Paired Samples Statistics Std. Error Mean ll Std. Deviation Mean Pair Pre_TesL3 304.3750 16 15331139 38.32785 1 PosLTesL3 375.5000 16 106.62082 26.65521 (Table 7-1. L’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Words) Paired Samples Test Paired Differences 95% Confidence Interval of the Std. Error Difference Mean Std. Deviation Mean Lower Upper 1 df Sig. (2-tailed) Pair Pre_TesL3 - 1 PosLTesLS 71.12500 110.91310 27,72828-13022642 -12.02358 -2.565 15 .022 (Table 7-2. L’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Words) The mean for the second half-sessions on the pre-test (M=354.6) was higher than the mean for the first-half sessions on the pre-test (M=254). Also, the post-test mean for the second-half sessions (M=413.7) was higher than the post-test mean for the first-half sessions (M= 337.2). It is indicated that the total number of words increased through the second-half sessions for both the pre and the post-tests. Therefore, it is shown that the interventions of MIT and MIT combined with TS have a positive effect on the improvement of total number of words, as a long-term effect. In order to investigate whether there was a short-term effect on total number of words, a paired-samples t-test was conducted to compare pre-test and post-test data from the first session to the sixteenth session. There was a statistically significant difference in the scores for pre-test (M=304, SD=153) and post-test (M=375, SD=106) : t(15)= -2.565, p = 0.022 (p<0.05) (See Table 7-1 & 7-2). These results indicated that each intervention 54 does have an effect on the difference between the pre and the post-test scores Therefore, the total number of words increased through the interventions of MIT and MIT combined with TS. R's Spontaneous Speech Test _ 3. Total Number of Words F‘gure 3'8 Shows a 500 - - - _§ 450 slight increasrng O 400 ' a E 350 tendency in R 5 total 9 300 S 250 number of words. The '2 200 :I 150 long-term effect of the _Z_ 100 g 50 intervention was shown [_ 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10111213141516 through comparing the Sessions _‘__ 3-pre I 3-post means between the (Figure 3—6. Total Number of Words in R’s SP Test) first-half and the Paired Samples Statistics second-half sessrons. Std. Error Mean ll Std, Deviation Mean Pair Pre_TesL3 241.6250 16 76.98474 19.24618 1 PosLTesL3 280.6250 16 68.67981 17.16995 (Table 7-3. R’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Words) Paired Samples Test Paired Differences 95% Confidence Interval of the Std. Error Difference Mean Std. Deviation Mean Lover Upper t df Sig. t2-tailed) Pair Pre.TesL3 - L - . . 1 P MU est3 3900009 | 88.09843 22.02461 r85,94434 7.94434 1.741 15 .094 (Table 7-4. R’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Words) The mean for the second-half sessions on the pre-test (M=255) was higher than the mean for the first-half sessions (M=228). Also, the mean for the second-half sessions (M=322.6) was higher than the mean for the first-half sessions on the post-tests (M= 238.6). It is indicated that the total number of words increased through the second-half sessions in both the pre and the post-tests. Therefore, it is shown that the intervention of TS had a positive affect on the improvement of total number of words as a long-term effect. In order to investigate whether there was the short-term effect on the total number of words, a paired-samples t-test was conducted to compare pre-test and post-test data. There was no significant difference in the scores for pre-test (M=24l .62, SD=76.98) and post-test (M=280.62, SD=68.67); t (15)= -l .771, p = 0.097 (p>0.05) (See Table 7-3 & 7- 4). These results indicated that each intervention did not have a strong effect on the difference between pre-test and post-test. Although there was no statistical difference between pre and post-test, the post-test mean (M=280.62) was greater than the pre-test mean (M=241.62). Therefore, the intervention TS did not have statistically significant effect, but the total number of words increased on the post-tests as a short-term effect. 56 4. Average Length of Utterances L's Spontaneous Speech Test 4. Average Length of Utterances U3 0 U 1: t! In 0 1: :9 H- O r: O.) —l 0 DD 6 5 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 910111213141516 Sessions 'i- 2-1-pre +2-1-post (Figure 3-7. Average Length of Utterances in L’s Spontaneous Speech Test) Paired Samples Statistics Std. Error Mean II Std. Deviation Mean Pair Pre.Test_2_1 38.1375 16 20.73007 5.18252 1 96541755121 38.7750 16 11.99592 2.99898 (Table 8-1. L’s Paired t-test on Average Length of Utterances) Paired Samples Test Figure 3-5 shows there was no increasing short- terrn tendency in L’s average length of utterances as a result of the interventions. The long—term effect of intervention was shown through comparing the means between the first-half and the second-half sessions. Paired Differences 95% Confidence Interval of the Std. Error Difference Mean Std. Deviation Mean Lower Upper t df Sig. (2-tailed) Pair Pre_Test_2_1 - 1 PosLTesL2_1 -.63750 19.71662 4.92916 t11.14375 9.86875 -.129 15 .899 (Table 8-2. L’s Paired t-test on Average Length of Utterances) The mean for the second-half sessions on the pre-test (M=42.6) was higher than the mean for the first-half sessions (M=33.6). Also, the mean of the post-tests for second half sessions (M=39.8) is higher than the mean of the first-half sessions (M= 37.7). It 57 indicates that the average length of utterances increased in both pre and post-tests through the second-half sessions. Therefore, it is shown that the intervention of MIT and MIT combined with TS gave a positive effect on the improvement of average length of utterances as a long-term effect. In order to investigate whether there was a short-term effect on the sample length, a paired-samples t-test was conducted to compare the pre and the post-tests. There was no statistically significant difference in the scores for pre—test (M=38.13, SD=20.73) and post-test (M=38.77, SD=11.99) ; t(15)=-0.129, p = 0.899 (p>0.05)(See Table 8—1 & 8-2). These results indicat ed that although the pre and post means slightly increased, each intervention did not have a statistically significant effect on the difference between the pre-tests and the post-tests. Therefore, the intervention of MIT and MIT combined with TS did not affect the improvement on the average length of the three longest utterances as a short-term effect. R's Spontaneous Speech Test 4. Average Lengh of Utterances Figure 3-6 shows that there is no increasing tendency on the post- test data in R’s average length of utterances. Average Length of Utterances 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 910111213141516 S . There was no long—term CSSIOI'IS "‘- 2-1-pre z'l'pOSt effect of the (Figure 3-8. Average Length of Utterances in R’s SP Test) 58 intervention by comparing the means between the first-half and the second-half sessions. Paired Samples Statistics Std. Error Mean ll Std. Deviation Mean Pair Pre_TesL2_1 41.6125 16 16.81459 3.95365 1 PosLTesL2_1 43.8875 16 13.02955 3.25739 (Table 8-3. R’s Paired t-test on Average Length of Utterances) Paired Samples Test Paired Differences 95% Confidence Interval of the Std. Error Difference Mean Std. Deviation Mean Lovrer Upper t df Sig. (2-tailed) Pair Pre_TesL2_1- b 1 PosLTesLZJ -2.27500 23.22144 5.80536 14.64883 10.09883 -.392 15 .701 (Table 8-4. R’s Paired t-test on Average Length of Utterances) The mean for the second-half sessions on the pre-test (M=40.81) was lower than the mean for the first-half sessions (M=42.41). Also, the mean for the second-half sessions (M=43.81) was slightly lower than mean for the first-half sessions in the post- tests (M= 43.96). There was no progression in both pre and post-tests. It is indicated that the average length of utterances did not increased in both the pre and the post-tests through the second-half sessions. Therefore, it is shown that the intervention of TS did not have a positive affect on the improvement on the average length of three longest utterances as a long-term effect. In order to investigate whether there was the short-term effect on the sample length, a paired-samples t-test was conducted to compare the pre and the post-tests. There was no significant difference in the scores for pre-test (M=4l.61, SD=15.81) and post- 59 test (M=43.88, SD=13.02); t(15)=-0.392, p = 0.701 (p>0.05) (See Table 8-3 & 8-4). These results indicated that each intervention did not effect the difference between the pre-tests and post-tests. Although the mean for the post-tests (M=43.88) was slightly greater than the pre-test mean (M=41 .61), there is no statistically significant difference between the pre and the post-tests. Therefore, the intervention of the TS did not affect an improvement on the average length of the three longest utterances as a short-term effect. 5. Average Number of Words per Minute 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Average Number of Words per Minute L's Spontaneous Speech Test 5. Average Number of Words per Minute 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 910111213141516 Sessions '1“ 3-1-pre +3-1-post (Figure 3-9. Average Number of Words per Minute in L’s SP Test) Paired Samples Statistics Std. Error Mean ll Std. Deviation Mean Pair Pre_TesL3_1 44.4563 16 13.09488 3.27372 1 PosLTesL3_l 46.1562 . 16 8.23941 2.05985 (Table 9-1. L’s Paired t-test on Average Number of Words per Minute) 60 Figure 3—9 shows that there was no significant increasing tendency in L’s average number of words per minute. The long-term effect of the interventions was shown through comparing the means between the first and second-half session. Paired Samples Test Paired Differences 95% Confidence Interval of the Std. Error Difference Mean Std. Deviation Mean Lower Upper t df Sig. (2-tailed) 1;“ 1138;515:5833: -1.70000 8.65602 2.16400 -6.31247 291247 -.786 15 .444 (Table 9-2. L’s Paired t-test on Average Number of Words per Minute) The mean for the second-half sessions on the pre-test (M=45.2) was higher than the mean for the first-half sessions (M=43.8). On the other hand, the mean for the second-half sessions (M=43.7 9) was lower than that of the first-half sessions on the post-tests (M= 48.52). It is indicated that the average number of words per minute increased in the second-half sessions on the only pre-tests. Therefore, it is shown that the interventions of MIT and MIT combined with TS did not have a positive effect on both the pre and the post-test on the improvement in the average number of words per minute as a long-term effect. In order to investigate whether there was a short-term effect on the average number of words per minute, a paired-samples t-test was conducted to compare to pre- tests and post-tests from the first to the sixteenth session. There was no statistically significant difference in the scores for pre-test (M=44.45, SD=13.09) and post-test (M=46.15, SD=8.23); t(15)= - 0.786, p = 0.444 (p>0.05) (See Table 9-1 & 9-2). Although these results showed that the interventions do have no statistically significant effect on the difference between pre and post-tests, there was a slight increase in means (pre=44.45, post: 46.15). Therefore, the results indicated that MIT and MIT combined with TS did not have a strongly positive effect on the average number of words per minute. 61 R's Spontanous Speech Test 5. Average Number of Words per Minute 60 50 40 30 20 Average Number of Words per Minute 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 910111213141516 Sessions '1‘ 3-1-pre +3—1-post (Figure 3-10. Average Number of Words per Minute in R’s SP Test) Paired Samples Statistics Std. Error Mein II Std. Deviation Mean Pair Pre_TesL3_1 38.9375 16 7.48027 1.87007 1 PosLTesL3_l 38.0000 16 7.41791 1.85448 (Table 9—3. R’s Paired t-test on average number of words per minute) Paired Samples Test Figure 3—10 shows that there was no significant increase in R’s average number of words per minute. The long-term effect of intervention was shown through comparing the means between the first and second-half sessions. Paired Differences Std. Error 95% Confidence Interval of the Diffaence Mean Std, Deviation Mean Lover Upper t df Sig. (2-tailedl Pair Pre_TesL3_1 - 1 Poerest.3_1 33750 7.54214 1.88553 -3.08142 4.95642 .497 15 .626 (Table 9-4. R’s Paired t-test on Average Number of Words per Minute) The mean for the second-half sessions (M=42.01) was higher than the mean of first-half sessions on the pre-test (M=35.86). Also, the posttest mean for the second-half 62 sessions (M=41.52) was higher than that for the first-half sessions (M= 34.47). It is indicated that the average number of words per minute increased through the second—half sessions in both pre and post-tests. Therefore, it is shown that the intervention of TS had a positive effect to improving the average number of words per minute in both pre and post-test as a long-term effect. In order to investigate whether there was a short-term effect on the average number of words per minute, a paired-samples t-test was conducted to compare pre—test and post-test data from the first session to the sixteenth session. There was no statistically significant difference in the scores for pre-test (M=38.93, SD=7.48) and post-test (M=3 8, SD=7.41); t(15)= 0.497, p = 0.626 (p>0.05) (See Table 9-3 & 9-4). These results indicated that the interventions generated no statistically significant difference between pre and post-tests scores, as well as the difference of means in pre (M=38.93) and post- test (M=3 8). Therefore, the TS did not affect the average number of words per minute as a short-term effect. 63 Naming Measurement 6. Total Number of Items Named L's Naming Test 6. Total Number of Items Named 14 12 10 r T T T T 7— V Total Number of Items Named Sessions T T l' I '15- 1)-pre +1)-post 8 A 6~Mfiy ’75} Axel—~— 4 -——;.;-7».‘—4—»m ‘: x t l 2 0 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 8 910111213141516 (Figure 4-1. Total Number of Items Named in L’s Naming Test) Figure 4-1 shows the significant increasing tendency in L’s total number of items named. The long-term effect of the interventions was shown through comparing the means between the first-half Paired Samples Statistics and the second'half Std. Error sessions. Mean N Std. Deviation Mean '— Pair Pre_Test 4.5000 16 1.21106 .3027? 1 PosLTest 8.1875 16 204022 .51006 (Table 10—1. L’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Items named) Paired Samples Test Paired Differences 95%Conf1dence Interval of the Std. Error 01091911“ Mean Std. Deviation Mean Lower Upper t df Sig. (2-tailedl Pail PrLTest'PosLTesl -3.68750 1.30224 .32556 -4.38142 -2.99358 -ll.327 15 .000 (Table 10-2. L’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Items named) 64 The mean for the second-half sessions on the pre-test (M=5.12) was higher than the mean for the first-half sessions (M=3.88). Also, the mean for the second-half sessions on post-tests (M=9.75) was higher than that of the first-half sessions (M= 6.62). It indicates that the total number of items named increased through the second-half sessions on both pre and post-tests. Therefore, it is shown that the interventions of MIT and MIT combined with TS had a positive effect on the improvement of the total number of items named as a long-term effect. In order to investigate whether there was a short-term effect on the total number of items named, a paired-samples t—test was conducted to compare pre-test and post-test results through all sixteen sessions. There was a statistically significant difference in the scores for pre-test (M#.50, SD=1.21) and post-test (M=8.18, SD=2.04); t (15): -11.327, p = 0.000 (p<0.05) (See Table 10-1 & 10-2). These results suggest that each intervention does have an effect on the difference between pre and post-test scores. Therefore, the results suggest that when the interventions of MIT and MIT combined with TS were conducted, the total number of Items named increased as a short-term effect. Fi re 4-2 shows a sli tl R's Naming Test gu gh y 6. Total Number of Items Named increasing tendency in R’s total number of items named. The long-term effect of intervention was shown through comparing the means between the first half 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 910111213141516 Total Number of Items Named Sessions and the second half sessions. "A" 1)-pre +1J-post (Figure 4-2. Total Number of Items Named in R’s Naming Test) 65 Paired Samples Statistics Std. Error Mean II Std. Deviation Mean Pair Pre_Test.l 47.0625 16 7.46073 1.86518 1 PosLtesLl 64.0625 16 7.01872 1.75458 (Table 10-3. R’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Items named) Paired Samples Test Paired Differences 95% Confidence Interval of the Std. Error Difference Mean Std. Deviation Mean Lower Upper t df sen-511a) Pairl Pre_TesL1-Post.tesL. 4.00000 8.51665 2.12916 L11.53820 246180 -3.288 15 .005 (Table 10-4. R’s Paired t-test on Total Number of Items named) The mean for the second-half sessions on the pre-test (M=48) was higher than the mean for the first-half sessions on the pre-test (M=46). On the other hand, the mean of the post-test for the second-half sessions (M=51.75) was lower than that of the first-half sessions (M= 56.37). It is indicated that the total number of items named increased through the second-half sessions only on the pre—tests. Therefore, it is shown that the intervention of TS gave a positive effect on the improvement of the total number of items named only on the pre-test as a long-term effect. In order to investigate whether there was a short-term effect on the total number of items named, a paired-samples t-test was conducted to compare pre-test and post-test data for all sixteen sessions. There was a statistically significant difference in the scores for pre-test (M=47, SD=7.46) and post-test (M=54, SD=7.01); t (15)= -3.288, p = 0.005 (p<0.05) (See Table 10-3 & 10-4). These results indicated that each intervention does have an effect on the difference between pre and post-test scores. Therefore, the total number of Items named increased after the intervention of TS was conducted. 66 7. Average Number of Prompts per Item Named Average Number of Prompt per Item Named L's Naming Test 7. Average number of prompt per Item Named 2.5 A . . 2 "1 '1 I". ’4 .f, It ‘ 1.5 I ‘l 7' 1‘: 5 8 \ ' )1 ~ 1 ' 8’ f 1 P UI O 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 8 910111213141516 Sessions "i' 2)-pre +2)-post Figure 4-3 shows the reducing tendency in post- test data for L’s average number of prompts per item named. The long-term effect of the interventions was shown through comparing the means between the first- (Figure 4-3. Average Number of Prompts per Item Named half and the second-half in L’s Naming Test) Paired Samples Statistics “35'0“" Std. Error Mean It Std. Deviation Mean Pair Pre_TesL2 1.2906 16 .60006 .16002 1 PosLTesLZ .5375 15 .26552 .06638 (Table 1 1-1. L’s Paired t-test on Average Number of Prompts per Item Named) Paired Samples Test Paired Differences 95% Confidence Interval of the Std. Error Difference ‘ Mean Std. Deviation Mean Lower Upper t df Sig. (2-tailed) Ii“ 52:55:32. .65312 70225 .17556 .27892 1.02733 3720 15 .002 (Tablel l-2. L’s Paired t-test on Average Number of Prompts per Item) The mean for the second-half sessions on the pre-test (M=1.03) was lower than the mean of first—half sessions on the pre-test (M=1.55) The goal in this variable is to decrease the number of prompts, so there was a slight improvement by reducing the mean 67 in the post-tests. On the other hand, the post-test mean for the second-half sessions (M=0.68) is slightly higher than that of first-half sessions (M=0.6). It indicates that the average number of prompts per item increased through the second-half sessions on the post-test. Namely, the pre-test results were an improvement, while the post—test increase was a slight regression. Therefore, it is shown that the intervention of MIT and MIT combined with TS did not have a significantly positive effect in decreasing the average number of prompts both on the pre and post-tests as a long-term effect. In order to investigate whether there was the short—term effect on the average number of prompts, a paired-samples t-test was conducted to compare pre-test and post- test results for all sixteen sessions. There was a statistically significant difference in the scores for pre-test (M=1.29, SD=0.60) and post—test (M=0.63, SD=0.26); t (15)= 3.72, p = 0.002 (p<0.05) (See Table 11-1 & 11-2). These results suggest that each intervention does have an effect on the difference between pre and post-test performance. Therefore, the results indicate that the prompts by the researcher were reduced in the post-test as a short-term effect. . F‘ 4-4 h th t R's Naming Test lgure S ows a 7. Average Number of Prompt per Item Named there was difference ,_, 0.16 2' 0.14 between pre and post- E E 0.12 ‘5 a 0.1 test data in R’s average 5 ‘z‘ 0.08 E E 0.06 number of prompts per 5 E 0.04 . E” =- 0.02 I items named. The long- : 0 < 1 2 3 4 s 6 7 8 910111213141516 tefineffectofthe Sessions interventions was "A" 2)-pre +2)-post (Figure 4-4. Average Number of Prompts per Item Named in R‘s Naming) 68 shown through comparing the means between the first half and the second-half sessions. Paired Samples Statistics Std. Error Mean ll Std. Deviation Mean Pair Pre_TesL .0394 16 .04404 .01101 1 PosLTesLZ .0400 16 .02608 .00652 (Tablel 1-3. R‘s Paired t-tcst on Average Number of Prompts per Item Named) Paired Samples Test Paired Differences 95%Conf1dence Interval of the Std. Error Difference Mean Std. Deviation Mean Lower Upper t df Sig. (2-tailed) Pair Pre_Test_2 - 1 PosLTesLZ '.00062 .03924 .00981 -.02153 .02028 -.064 15 .950 (Tablel 1-4. R’s Paired t-test on Average Number of Prompts per Item) The mean for the second-half sessions on the pre-test (M=0.03 l) was lower than the mean of first half sessions on the pre-test (M=0.047). Also, the mean for the second- half sessions (M=0.037) was lower than that of the first-half sessions on the post-test (M=0.042). There was improvement by reducing the number of the researcher’s prompts on both the pre and post-test. It indicates that the average number of prompts per item was reduced through the second-half sessions. Therefore, it is shown that the intervention of the TS gave a positive effect on reducing the average number of prompts per item as a long-term effect. In order to investigate whether there was a short-term effect on the average number of prompts per item, a paired-samples t-test was conducted to compare pre-test 69 and post-test data for all sixteen sessions. There was no statistically significant difference in the scores for pre-test (M=0.039, SD=0.044) and post-test (M=0.04, SD=0.026) ; t (15)= -0.064, p = 0.950 (p>0.05) (See Table 11-3 & 11-4) These results suggest that each intervention has no statistically significant effect on the difference between pre and post- test. The post-test mean (M=0.04) was slightly higher than the pre-test mean (M=0.039). Therefore, the results indicate that the prompts by the researcher were not reduced by the intervention, so there was no improvement on the average number of prompts as a short- term effect. Melodic Intonation T homer Measurement 8. MIT Scores of Percent I sessions more than in the L 5 Test first-half sessions. The mean 100 *5 90 for the second-half sessions E 80 a: 70 (M=75.1) was higher than '45 60 '3 50 the mean for the first-half g 40 U W 30 sessions =58.5). I: 20 (M 2 10 . . . 0 Therefore, it mdlcates that 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 9 1011 12 13 1415 15 there was an improvement in Sessions (Figure S-l. MIT Scores of Percent in L’s MIT Test) L S WT scores contmumg Figure 5-1 shows that there was the increasing tendency through the second-half in L’s MTT scores. To be exact, there was the tendency 53351095- toward an average rate of increase in the second-half 70 CHAPTER V DISCUSSION Summon and Conclusion The summary of results is shown in Table 12. There are two ways in which the data for the eight variables are interpreted: 1) Long-term effect; 2) Short-term effect. First, a long-term effect was measured by comparing the means of the first-half sessions with the means of the second-half sessions on both pre and post-tests (Independent t-test). If the second-half mean is higher than the mean of the first-half sessions, this result may indicate that the interventions in the second-half period of sessions have been effective. Table 12 shows a numerical increase, as ‘Yes’, and no increase, as ‘No’. Secondly, the short-term effect was measured by comparing the scores of pre-tests with those of post-tests (Paired t-test). Table 12 shows only whether or not there was a statistically significant difference between the pre-test means and the post-test means. If there was a significant difference (p values < 0.05), it is labeled as ‘Yes’, and if none, as ‘No’. 71 Table 12. Summagg at the data result in t-test Variables L R 1. Long-term: No test of Significance " Yes " indicates that Second-half means > 2 2 I I F i’ st-half means Long- Term Short Long- Term Short Term Term 2. Short-term: Test of Significance Pre Post Pre Post 1. Total Length of Samples Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes 2. Total Number of Utterances No Yes Yes Yes Yes No 3. Total Number of Words Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No 4. Average Length of Utterances Yes Yes No No No No 5. Average Number of Words per Minute Yes No No Yes Yes No 6. Total Number of Items Named Yes Yes Yes Yes No Yes 7. Average Number of Prompts per Item Yes No Yes Yes Yes No Named 8. MIT Scores of Percent Yes - - - (1. Long-term effect— Independent t-test, 2. Short-term effect — Paired samples t-test) L ’s Results The summary of results indicated that the music therapy interventions of Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) and Therapeutic Singing (TS) had a positive effect on L’s functional communication skill. In L’s long-term effects on pre-test measures, there was improvement in six variables: l.the total length of samples; 3.the total number of words; 4.the average length of utterances; 5.the average number of words per minute; 6.the total number of items named, and 7.the average number of prompts per item named, and no improvement on only one variable, 2.the total number of utterances. On the other hand, in the post-tests, there was improvement in five variables: l.the total length of samples; 72 2.the total number of utterances; 3.the total number of words; 4.the average length of utterances; and 6.the total number of items named, and no improvement on the two variables: 5.average number of words per minute, and 8.the average number of prompts per item named. It was shown that the means of eleven among the fourteen variables showed increases in both the pre-test and the post-test. Therefore, it might be indicated that there was a long-term effect on L’s functional communication skills through the last period of sessions. However, statistical significance was not achieved. Also, in L’s short-term effect, there were statistically significant differences in five variables: l.the total length of samples; 2.the total number of utterances; 3.the total number of words; 4.the total number of items named; and 5.the average number of prompts per item named, and no differences in two variables: l.the average length of utterance; and 2.the average number of words per minute. It was shown that there were statistically significant differences in most variables between pre and post-tests. Therefore, it is indicated that MIT and TS interventions helped L improve his fianctional communication skills in several areas as short-term effects. Discussion of L ’s Four Goal ’s Areas There were four goals targeted in L’s study. The first goal was to investigate whether or not Therapeutic Singing (TS) combined with Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) helps adult clients who are diagnosed with expressive aphasia as measured by the quality of their functional communication skills. For investigating the first goal, the paired t-test was conducted in the second-half sessions (9th - 16th sessions), which is the second period of sessions conducted with MIT combined with TS, by comparing the means of pre and post-tests. 73 Table 13-1 shows that the means for post-tests are distinctly higher than those of pre-tests in all seven variables (see Appendix 15-1). It supports that there was improvement on the post-tests in all seven variables after conducting MIT combined with TS through the second-half sessions. On the other hand, table 13-2 shows that there were statistically significant differences (p<0.05) in three variables: l.the total length of sample, 2.the total number of utterances, and 6.the total number of items named, through the second-half sessions, while there were no statistically significant differences (p>0.05) in four variables: 3.the total number of words, 4.the average length of utterances, 5.the average number of words per minute, and 7.the average number of prompts per item named. As a result, the increases in measured numerical quantities indicated that the interventions of MIT combined with TS had a slightly supportive effect in the Functional Communication Skills by increasing the means. The means for post-tests in all seven variables were higher than the means for pre-tests, although the results did not show statistically supported differences between the means of pre and post-tests. It is believed that the limited number of sessions (N =8) may be the reason for a lack of statistically supportive differences (See Table 13-1 in Appendix 15—1). The second goal was to explore whether MIT treatment itself helps clients improve the quality of their functional communication skills. For investigating the second goal, the paired t-test was conducted in only the first-half sessions (13’ — 8th sessions), in which only MIT intervention was conducted, by comparing the means for pre and post- tests. 74 Table 14-1 shows that the means for post-tests are also distinctly higher than the means of pre-tests in all seven variables. It supports that there was improvement on the post-tests after conducting the MIT intervention during the first-half sessions (See table 14-1 in Appendix 15-2). However, the results in table 14-2 (see Appendix 15-2) shows that there were statistically significant differences (p<0.05) in three variables: 3.the total number of words, 6.the total number of items named, and 7.the average number of prompts per item named during the first-half sessions, while, there were no statistically significant differences (p>0.05) in four variables: l.the total length of samples, 2.the total number of utterances, 4.the average length of utterances, and 6. Average number of words per minute (See Table 14-2 in Appendix 15-2). As a result, the means for post-tests improved more than those for pre-tests in all seven variables although the results did not show statistically significant differences on all seven variables (see Table 13-1). Therefore, MIT also had a positive effect on the post-test results, but did not have a statistically supported difference in all seven variables. The third goal was to compare the effectiveness of TS combined with MIT and MIT alone in increasing fiinctional communication skills. For investigating goal III, the independent sample t-test was conducted on sessions by comparing the two means of a variable: the numerical differences between the post-tests and the pre-tests in the first- half and the second-half sessions. (Post-test Means Minus Pre-tests Means (lSt — 8'“) vs. Post-test Means Minus Pre-test Means (9th —— 16th) (See table 15-1 in Appendix 15-3) Table 15-1 shows that the means in groups 2 (Post-test means minus Pre-test means in the second-half sessions) had more improvement than the means in groups 1 75 (Post-test means minus Pre-test means in the first-half sessions) on three variables: l.total length of samples, 2.total number of utterances, and 6.total number of items named, on the other hand, four variables showed no improvement: 3.total number of words, 4.average length of utterances, 5.average number of words per minute, and 7.average number of prompts per item named. Table 15-2, however, shows that there were no statistically significant differences between the first-half (MIT) and the second-half (MIT+TS) sessions (p>0.05) except the one variable: 6.the total number of items named. However, it is noted that the second-half (MIT+TS) sessions had higher means than those of the first-half sessions on three variables: l.the total length of samples, 2.the total number of utterances, and 6.the total number of items names. To be exact, the negative scores of Mean Difference in Table 15- 2 implied that the means of the second-half (MIT+TS) sessions were higher than the means of the first-half (MIT) sessions. On the other hand, the positive scores of Mean Difference showed that the means of the first-half (MIT) sessions were higher than the means of the second-half (MIT+TS) sessions on four variables: 3.total number of words, 4.average length of utterances, 5.average number of words per minute, and 7.the average number of prompts per item named. As a result, it can be shown that MIT combined with TS had a slightly positive effect on the improvement of functional communication skills on those four variables. Therefore, no clear trend can be attributed to the interventions. 76 The fourth goal was to determine whether TS enhances MIT treatment (See Figure 5-1 in Ch. 4). The results shows that MIT combined with TS interventions had a positive effect on MIT scores, more so than MIT intervention alone. As the result, it is considered that the TS intervention is beneficial in enhancing MIT treatment. As the evidence of its effectiveness, TS can support the goals of other techniques, not by taking their place as primary interventions, but as a separate technique in support of the other. For example, the primary goal of this study is improving the fimctional communication skills of the aphasic subjects, and MIT is one of the significant techniques for this goal in enhancing speech function. Significantly, the supportive role of TS could serve as an important reinforcement for improving the goals of MIT and this study. TS is the practice of combining elements of speech production and also incorporates emotional elements of speech and language. People who suffer from the symptoms of a stroke also suffer from the emotional wounds such as depression, and this emotional dysfunction may affect language and speech function negatively. A change in therapeutic focus by combining TS with MIT could provide a more beneficial intervention in the functional goals of this study, as well as the goals of MIT. R ’s Results R, who had high functional speech, received only the TS intervention, as has been mentioned in the Methods chapter, because he already had the ability to repeat full sentences, which represents the final stage of MIT. In R’s long-term effect, reflected in . the pre-tests, there was improvement in five variables: 2.the total number of utterances; 3.the total number of words; 5.the average number of words per minute; 6.the total 77 number of items named; and 7.the average number of prompts per item named, and no improvement in two variables: l.the total length of samples; and 4.the average length of utterances. On the other hand, in the post-tests, there was improvement in five variables: l.the total length of samples; 2.the total number of utterances; 3.the total number of words; 5.the average number of words per minute; and 7.the average number of prompts per item named, and no improvement in two variables: 4.the average length of utterances; and 6.the total number of items named. It was shown that the means of most variables were increased in both the pre-test and the post-test as a Iong-terrn effect, except in the average length of utterances. Therefore, it could be considered that there were possible long-term effects on R’s functional communication skills through the sixteen sessions although R did not show the improvement in the length of longest utterances. These results represent only numerical directions in improvement, as statistical significance was not established. In contrast, there was no statistically significant difference in the short-term effect. Only the mean of two variables (1. the total length of samples and 6. total number of items named) improved in the post-test. There was no short-term effect in five variables (2.the total number of utterances, 3. the total number of words, 4. the average length of utterances, 5. the average number of words per minute, and 7. the average number of prompts per item named). The result shows that even though the mean of post- tests is slightly higher than the mean of pre-tests, there was no statistically significant difference between pre and post-tests. Therefore, it is considered that the TS interventions may have helped R to improve functional communication skill as a long-term effect, but there seems to be no statistically significant short-term effect. 78 Specifically, R’s result showed there was no improvement at all in the average length of utterances both long-tenn and short-term. As discussed in chapter III, R had a high functional communication level, and most functional ability may have been recovered previously because the onset of his stroke was seven years ago. This fact may indicate that the TS intervention may not have a significantly positive effect on the short- term gains for a person who had been diagnosed with expressive aphasia several years ago and had already recovered speaking functions. The goal for R in this study was to gain information as to whether TS treatment itself helps clients improve the quality of their functional communication skills. Although the TS gave positive results on some variables in the functional communication measurement, R’s results did not show a stronger effect than L’s results. This may be explained by the fact that R already had recovered many fianctions during the seven years after onset of his stroke. Anecdotal Improvement L’s spouse stated at the middle of the research, “L is feeling like this is helping him!”, and he had started to ride a bike for 4 miles away from his home and communicated with the cashier of the gas station during the last period of sessions. This was his positive feedback regarding gains assessed through the Spontaneous Speech Measurement. One day during the last period of sessions, R mentioned that his brother said that R was talking more clearly than before by phone, and both his close friend and his wife stated that his speech function was more stabilized than in the past. 79 Im lications or Intervention Music is not a simple stimulus for activating brain function, but music is a complex combination of many supportive ways to trigger response to improve restricted functions in a human being. We cannot say that taking a dose of singing activities three times a day will help clients to return to a normal state of speech function. However, we can say that singing can trigger improvement in speech function by activating long-term memory, supporting states of emotion, and triggering unconscious automatic words. Jackson (1871) differentiated propositional speech from so-called automatic speech. He stated that even when propositional speech became seriously damaged, automatic speech could be preserved in aphasia. TS may be able to trigger unconscious memory for producing automatic speech. The findings of this study lent support to the idea that MIT and TS have positive effects on improving functional communication skills. Therapeutic Singing (TS) was added to serve a supportive role during the sessions in order to investigate whether TS would help to improve fimctional communication skills in L’s MIT interventions, as well as a main intervention in R’s sessions. The reason for investigating the combination of both interventions was that TS and MIT have reciprocally important roles. MIT has the benefit of focusing on the target phrases more than TS, and TS may be able to activate long-term memory functions through the usage of familiar songs, as well as support a state of pleasurable emotion to counteract depression. Singing also can help a person to engage actively in pleasure and emotional satisfaction. Investigating whether there is effectiveness in both MIT and TS can uniquely add to the body of knowledge in this area. 80 Since the 1970’s many researchers have been investigating whether or not singing can help enhance speech intelligibility, as well as the effectiveness of MIT in speech therapy. Studies until the 2000’s supported the effectiveness of MIT in speech production (Baker, 2000; Belin, et al., 1996; Bonakdarpour, Eftekharzadeh, & Ashayeri, 2003; Carroll, 1996; Galloway & Kraus, 1982; Laughlin, Naeser, & Gordon, 1979; Sparks & Deck, 1986; Sparks, et al., 1974), as well as the effectiveness of Therapeutic Singing in speech function (TS) (Cohen, 1992; Cohen & Ford, 1995; Cohen & Masse, 1993; Glover, Kalinowski, Rastatter, & Stuart, 1996; Keith & Aronson, 1975). Some recent, however, research studies showed that there was dissociation between singing and speaking (Hebert, et al., 2003; Peretz & Hébert, 1995; Racette, Bard, & Peretz, 2006). They stated that singing did not affect word production by producing normal speech. Even though the subjects produced more word utterances while singing, after repetition of the singing activity, speaking production with the same lyrics did not improve. Similar to these studies, when the researcher asked subject L to speak target phrases after MIT treatment with same materials, subject L could not speak most of the target phrases even though he had produced the target contents within the musical condition of MIT. As explanation, if a two-year-old toddler, for example, who has not started speaking yet, sings Twinkle Twinkle Little Star along with his mom, the toddler can then sing producing 60% of the lyrics. No matter how many words the toddler can produce while singing, can the toddler speak the lyrics without music? Of course, the answer is ‘No,’ because the toddler’s brain is not fully activated for speaking, even though spoken language comprehension may have started already. Even so, can we conclude there is dissociation between singing and speaking? 81 In contrast to Hebert’s study (2003), this study used, as pre and post-tests, different measurement tools within a non-musical condition. In the most of NMT intervention in Speech and Language Training, as well as Hebert’s research, word production has been measured within a musical condition. This study, however, has used measurement techniques designed after speech pathology tests of Functional Communication Measurement within non-musical conditions. This differs from other neurologic music therapy techniques. For a concrete example, MUSTIM is one of the effective NMT techniques, and the variables are measured by how many words the client produces during the MUSTIM intervention under musical conditions. For this reason, the measurement tools chosen and used in this study can be valuable methods for future researchers. Also, the number of numerical variables gave this study integrity, even though it developed into two case studies. The Paired t-test and Independent sample t-test was conducted on fourteen to fifteen variables. The rhythmic factor in speaking, emphasized by using castanets, also served as a memory reinforcement in this study. Resent research showed that the intoned melody was not the important factor in improving word production (Hebert, et al., 2003). The drum, however, played an important role for all children in increasing the length and clarity of response in one study (1996, Carroll). The amount of time consumed was one of the limitations to having a large subject pool in this study. In order to obtain the Spontaneous Speech Measurement data, it required two and one-half hours for each session, including driving to the subject's 82 location, as well as three hours for coding data. Using five and one-half hours for each session to obtain data on the fourteen to eighteen variables was prohibitive to having more subjects. However, this study gains credibility from the amount of date collected from the two subjects. There was also a limitation in the recruitment of aphasic participants who are at the same in level of speaking ability, because most of the clients who are diagnosed with cerebrovascular accidents (CVA) have combined disorders with other diagnoses. For example, L and R were diagnosed the same with expressive aphasia, but the researcher recognized that L also had motor functioning problems in using his oral muscles. It was discovered that L was also diagnosed with mild apraxia after engaging in several sessions. For this reason, recruiting a homogeneous pool of subjects is a most difficult step at the outset of the research. Although five subjects were recruited who were diagnosed with adult expressive aphasia, the study was limited to just two, due to the wide variation in speech abilities and the time consuming nature of the intensive sessions. Im lications or Future Research There are several suggestions for future research. First, the more sessions and longer period, such as a year, the better will be the results in data analysis. This study was conducted with restricted time line as a masters thesis, so subjects participated in a two- month time line. The speech and language pathologists recommended that they have sessions with persons with expressive aphasia for over a year in order to evaluate improvement. 83 A second recommendation is in the intensity of treatment. According to the original MIT protocols, it suggested a six-week intensive plan. At a rehabilitation center in Michigan, speech pathologists also meet stroke patients five times a week in an intensive plan. Subjects L and R had interventions around three to four times a week during the second period of sessions. For the future researcher, it is also recommended that a better way to measure would be to reduce the time consumed by examining only a pre and post-test at the first and last sessions. This would generate valuable data if gathered in long-term treatment over a year with an intensive plan of intervention. Pre and posttests might also be given at the beginning and end of each week. Also, length of time post stroke had an effect on results. R, whose stroke occurred seven years ago, had slightly more improvement than L, whose onset of stroke was a year ago. This can be a reference for future researchers in recruiting subjects. This research design may be a good model for a treatment plan, which was conducted in the order of pre-test, treatment, and posttest. Namely, each session was conducted in the order of conversation about favorite themes, musical intervention, and conversation on the same topic. This design may be a stable model for music therapy intervention. Lastly, even though the testing is time consuming, the seven different variables in functional communication skills can be good evaluation materials: 1.Total length of samples, 2.Total number of utterance, 3.Total number of words, 4.Average length of utterance, 5.Average number of words per minute, 6.Total number of items named, 7.Average number of prompts per item named. Those variables were selected with the 84 help of an expert in speech therapy, so they can be credibly used in the music therapy field. It would be encouraging news if persons with severe expressive aphasia could find the means to speak again. As to the question of whether singing has any use in the recovery of speech, the researcher would answer that language can be embedded with unconscious automatic words ready to be “released” for conscious uses, and music can trigger this unconscious automatism by stimulating the production of verbal utterances. This study is this researcher’s first step in learning the power of music in rehabilitation for persons with neurological conditions such as expressive aphasia. Through this first step of these two case studies, the researcher is convinced of the effectiveness of MIT and TS techniques to improve communication through speech for persons with expressive aphasia. 85 APPENDICES 86 APPENDIX 1 Table 1. Overview of Melodic Intonation Therapy and Scoring Criterion in. I. Hummmglmelody 2X (HT) ‘ 2. Humming in unison (HT) 3. Fade (HT) ' l. Humming (HT) . ‘ 2. Unison smgrngGJT) 0"}: . ' 3. Unison singing with fading (HT) 1., 4. Immediate repetition . 5. Response to a probe question 1. Introduction of item (HT) 2 Umsonwrthfad’mgy . . 3. Delayed repetition (HT) (Back up: unison with fitting) (Back up: Delayed repetition) 4 1. DelayedrepetmoMI-IT) . , :1 ' (Backup. Umsonwrthf' " ) ' ' " g *J-Noscore'.‘ ' 2. Introducing sprochgesang 3. Sprechgegang With mg”). . . :1 (Back up: Unison spreehg'esang) 4. Spoken repetition with delay . (Back up: Sprechgesang with fading) 5. Response to probe question (Back up: Delayed repetition) (HT: Hand Tapping) 87 APPENDIX 2 Melodic Intonation Therapy Measurement Form5 Name of Client: Session #. Date Pre—test / Post-test . 3. Unison singin y . * 4. Immediate repetition Q? 1. Hummmg melody 2X (HT) 2. Humming 1n unison (HT) 3 Fade (HT) 7 . . 1.Hummmg (HT) V 2. Unison Singing 5 Response to a probe question 1 1. Introduction of 1;th (HT) 2. Unison with fading (HT) 3. Delayed repetititm (HT) (330k up: unison With fading) - . No score No score. (Back up: Delayed repetition). , . 1. Delayed repetition (HT) (Back up: Unison With flitting) 2. introducing .sprechgesang (HT) 3. Sprechgesang with fading (HT) » V 4 (Back up: Unison Sprechgesang) 4. Spoken repetition with delay ' No score (Back up Sprechgesang with fading) . H 5. Response to probe question (Back up: Delayed repetition) Total scores : APPENDIX 3 Melodic Intonation Therapy (M11) Hierarchy LEVEL I — learn intoning and hand tapping, response to hand-signals Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Hums melody twice with (HT) (HT) Hand Tapping (T) fades participation but continues (HT) Response (Client) (C) Unison humming with (HT). Score and Progression No score. Proceed to next step 11. LEVEL 2 — Ability repeating intoned sentences immediately, Response to a question Stimulus (Therapist) (T) 1.Hums melody with (HT), 2.1ntones sentence with (HT), Step lgnals (C) to join in unison intoninj of sentence 1 Response (Client) (C) Intone sentence together with (HT) - Unison Score and Progression Acceptable- 1 point. Proceed to Step 2, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Same as Stet) ! . . Step But fades part1c1pation except (HT) 2 Response (Client) (C) Intone sentence with (HT) gradually Solo Score and Pro ression Acceptable- 1 point. Proceed to Step 3, same sentence g Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. 1.Signals (C) to listen Stimulus (Therapist) (T) 2.1ntones sentence with (HT) 3.Signals (C) to repeat, Step Cueing for initiation of utterance if necessary 3 Response (Client) (C) Repeat intoned sentence with (HT) immediately . Acceptable— 2 point without cue, 1 point with cue Score and Progressmn Proceed to step 4, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. . . 1.1ntones question, “What did you say?” Stimulus (Therapist) (T) 2.Signal (C) to repeat Step Cueing for initiation of response if necessary 4 Response (Client) (C) Repeat intoned sentence with (HT) Score and Progression Acceptable— 2 point without cue, 1 point with cue Proceed to step 1, for next sentence Unaccegable — Discontinue pflress for sentence. 89 APPENDIX 3 (continued) Melodic Intonation Therapy (M11) Hierarchy i LEVEL 3 - Ability to the delay of responses by fading Therapist’s participation Response to specific questions Step 1.1ntones sentence with (HT), Stimulus (Therapist) (T) 2.Signals (C) to join in unison sentence 3. Fade participation except (HT) Response (Client) (C) Intone sentence with (HT) gradually Solo Score and Progression Acceptable- 1 point. Proceed to Step 2, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. Step 1.Signals (C) to listen Stimulus (Therapist) (T) 2.1ntones sentence with (HT) 3.Signals (C) to repeat afier l or 2 second Delay 4. If (C) fails, Backup step 1, same sentence (B) Back up Response (Client) (C) Repeat Intoned sentence with (HT) after Delay Score and Progression Acceptable- 2 point without (B), 1 Point with (B) Process to Step 3, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. Step 1.1ntone a related question (e. g. what kind of, How Stimulus Thera ist many) ( p ) (T) 2.Signals (C) to answer 3. If (C) fails, Backup step 2, same sentence Response (Client) (C) Appropriate Answer (Intoned or Spoken) Score and Progression Acceptable- 2 point without (B), 1 Point with (B) Process to Step 1 for next sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. 90 APPENDIX 3 (continued) LEVEL 4 — Ability of Normal Speech - Step Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression 1.Signal (C) to listen 2.1ntone sentence with (HT) 3.Present it twice in Sprechgesang 4.Signal (C) unison Sprechgesang of sentence (HT) 5. If (C) fails, Backup to present in Sprechgesang 6. Retrial No. 4 Sprechgesang of sentence with (HT) Acceptable- 2 point with Sprechgesang. - 1 point with Backup - Proceed to Step 2, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. Step Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression 1.Signal (C) to listen 2.Present sentence in Sprechgesang with (HT) 3.Signals (C) to repeat after 2 or 3 second Delay 4. If (C) fails, Backup to Step 1 same sentence Sprechgesang of sentence with (HT) after 2 or 3 second Delay. Acceptable- 2 point without Backup - 1 point with Backup - Proceed to Step 3, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. Step Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression 1.Signal (C) to listen 2.Present sentence once in Sprechgesang 3.Present sentence twice in Normal Speech Prosody 4.Signals (C) to repeat after 2 or 3 second Delay 5.If (C) fails, Backup to Step 2 same sentence Repeat sentence in Normal Speech Prosody after 2 or 3 second Delay, no (HT) Acceptable- 2 point without Backup - 1 point with Backup - Proceed to Step 4, same sentence Unacceptable - Discontinue progress for sentence. Step Stimulus (Therapist) (T) Response (Client) (C) Score and Progression l.Ask question about substantive information on the same sentence. 2. If fail, Backup step 3 same sentence and retrial 3. Ask question about associative information 4. If fail, No backup and retrial if fail one. Answer appropriate responses 3 point — bonus point, one or more associative responses 2 point — without Backup, substantive content 1 point - with Backup. Proceed to next sentence. 91 APPENDIX 4 Spontaneous Speech Measurement Form On Functional Communication Measure (FCM) Name of Client: Sexism—nit. Date Pre-test / Post-test Subject is asked to choose the question from the questionnaire sheets Response limited within 5 min. Score Summary 1. Total Length of Samples 2. Total Number of Utterances 3. Average Length of Utterances 4. Total Number of Words Scaring Hierarchy 1. Total Length of Samples Sum of Seconds, including both intelligible and unintelligible words 2. Total Number of Utterances Including paraphasic errors 3. Average Length of Utterances Average number of words for three longest intelligible phrases e.g.) 1. I like a ball (4 words) * .me too (2 words) 2. I want to go home (5 words) 3. I love peanut (3 words) *. Lovely (1 word) = 4+5+3l3 = 4 (average length of utterances) 4. Total Number of Words Counting intelligible & correct words (Included Nouns, Verbs, Articles, Adjectives, Pronouns, and Preposition) 92 APPENDIX 4 (continued) Spontaneous Speech Measurement Form On Functional Communication Measure (FCM) Questionnaire Question 1: Number Of Sub'ect’s res onses Length Of Utterance J p Utterances Total: Total: 93 APPENDIX 5 Questionnaire sheet Spontaneous Speech On Functional Communication Measure (FCM) What would you like to talk about during conversations? _ a Funny stories about your children Your adventures as a young child/growing up Dating and getting married Being in the military Your worst jobs Your most important job/ career Moving or traveling Hobbies or unique interests P?°>‘.°‘S":“E“!° Family history/ ancestry/ genealogy u—d O . Local events p—s I—I . Current events . Sports I-I|—I| {AN . Favorite TV program i—s A . Politics/ the economy/ the government . Weather . Favorite meals/ restaurants u—II—It—I \IQLII . My house/ home town/ things to fix 18. My stroke and/ or other medical issues 19. Religion 20. Favorite movies (Revised from Aphasia Need Assessment, written by Kathryn L. Garrett & David R. Beukelman, (2005) 94 APPENDIX 6 Naming Measurement Form On Functional Communication Measure (FCM) flame of Client: Session #. Date Pre-test / Post-test Subject is asked to see the Picture Stimuli Sheets and to answer the names of pictures. Response limited within 5 min. The list of Name (Client’s responses) Number of Prompts (by Therapist) Total Number of Items Named: Average Number of Prompts pet Item Named: 95 APPENDIX 7 Hierarchy at Word Retrieval Prompts the Client with Aphasia Naming Measurement Form On Functional Communication Measure (F CM) ,9 Ask person to “say -Ask person to complete a sentence with phonemic cues “You sleep in a be -Less phonemic cues “You sleep in a b__” ~Verbal lead in without phonemic cues “You sleep in a _” State or demonstrate function “You sleep on it..motion sleep, it’s a _” -State function and supply a carrier phrase -Direct person to demonstrate function “Show me what you do with it.” oDirect person to state the fimction “What do you do with it? Request the name “What’s this?” 96 9, APPENDIXS 8 Picture Stimuli Sheets Naming Test On Functional Communication Measure (F C M 97 APPENDIX 8 Toothbrush 98 APPENDIX 8 (continued) _/ Canoe Stethoscope Unicorn Tripod 99 APPENDIX 8 (continued) Paint Bed Tree Pencil Whistle Scissors APPENDIX (continued) r/”i>> / ',,‘// (7.75:. ‘ x/ . \Kfitx/ \ J Saw Helicopter Broom Hanger Wheelchair 101 APPENDIX 8 (continued) Camel Mask Pretzel Racquet, Badminton 102 APPENDIX 8 (contmued) Globe Dart Harmonica Acorn Wreath Rhinoceros 103 APPENDIX 8 (continued) Igloo ' Dominoes Escalator Harp ' “ Knocker Bird, Pelican 104 (continued) " a... Accordion Asparagus T°ngs 105 APPENDIX 8 (continued) Trellis Ruler (( )) Antenna Statue in. Crutch Suspenders 106 APPENDIX 8 (continued) Calculator Palm, hand Microphone Dice Chopsticks 107 APPENDIX 8 (continued) 3» Eyebrow, eye Binoculars Flim Backpac 108 APPENDIX 8 (continued) Wishbone Propeller \e Pliers Dustpan Blimp Coffee pot 109 APPENDIX 8 (continued) Tambourine Spatula Nose Thumb Kite 110 a? APPENDIX 8 (continued) Gun, Pistol ill Key Scissors Tweezers Thermometer Ill APPENDIX 8 (continued) Typewriter Banjo Stool Candle Ladder Flashlight 112 APPENDIX 9 MUSIC HELPS APHASIA Research Project The effectiveness of Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) and “Tera peutic Singing (TS) has been established in past research since the 1970s in improving the speech production of persons with aphasia. Seeking individuals who have expressive aphasia to participate in research project. Participants would receive 16 sessions of music therapy targeted at increasing functional communication. Each session will include a brief pre- and post-test during session. Where: MSU Music Therapy Clinic Home visit Available Tuned: 50 min. 16 sessions Coat: Free Redearcber: Emily YeaJu Rhee Ifyou have questions, you are encour- aged to contact me, Mam 517 ~643- 0651at Michigan State Univ. Music Therapy Department "*1 113 APPENDIX 10 CONSENT FORM The Eflectiveness of Melodic In_tonation Therapy and Therapeutic Singing on Functional Communication Skills tor Adults with Expressive Aphasia You are invited to be in a research study of music therapy about the effectiveness of Melodic Intonation Therapy and Therapeutic Singing on Functional Communication Skills. You were selected as a possible participant because of your diagnosis of expressive aphasia. We ask that you read this form and ask any questions you may have before agreeing to be in the study. This study is being conducted by YeaJu Rhee who is a graduate student in the music therapy department at Michigan State University. Background Intormation The purpose of this study is to ascertain the effectiveness of Therapeutic Singing and Melodic Intonation Therapy in improving the functional communication skills of adults with expressive aphasia. The problems to be investigated by this study are as follows: (1) to determine whether Therapeutic Singing (TS) combined with Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) helps adult clients who are diagnosed with expressive aphasia as measured by the quality of their functional communication skills; (2) to determine whether MIT treatment itself helps clients improve the quality of their fimctional communication skills; (3) to compare the effectiveness of TS combined with MIT and MIT alone in increasing functional communication skills; and (4) to investigate whether TS enhances the benefits of MIT treatment. Procedures: If you agree to be in this study, we would ask you to do the following things: The procedures will consist of Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) in the first series of sessions and Therapeutic Singing (TS) combined with MIT treatment in the second series of sessions. For MIT treatment, you will be asked to sing and speak selected word phrases with a therapist, and for TS interventions, you will be asked to choose the songs that would like to sing, and participate in singing activity with the therapist. Previous music experience or training is not required for your participation in this study. Two measurement procedures will be used in a pre-test and post-test for each session: A Functional Communication Measure (F CM) and Melodic Intonation Therapy Measure (MITM). FCM will be composed of two different parts: Spontaneous Speech and Naming. In the Spontaneous speech part, you will be asked to choose the questions you would like to talk about and will have 5 minutes to talk in each pre and post-test. In the Naming part, picture stimuli sheets will be given to identify by name. The MIT measure will be conducted while subjects are participating in the treatment sessions. All sessions will be videotaped for the purpose of data collection. The intervention period will consist of between 16 and 20 individual or small group sessions, three times or five times a week for 50 minutes, including pre and post-tests. The period will be divided into two different terms of interventions of 9 to 11 sessions each: 1) MIT in first series; 2) TS combined with MIT in second series. For the purpose of gathering data, every session for every subject will be videotaped. In order to be included in this study each subject must agree to be taped 114 APPENDIX 10 (Continued) Risks and Benefits at being in the Studz The study has no known risks at all. Potential benefits are those that arise from participation of enjoyable musical experiences. There may be some gains in functional speech resulting from the treatments though they are not guaranteed. Confidentiality: The records of this study will be kept private. In any sort of report we might publish, we will not include any information that will make it possible to identify a subject. Research records will be stored securely in a locked area, and only researchers will have access to the records. The videotapes will be used for the purposes of research, and they will be erased after completion of this research project. Voluntary Nature at the Study: Participation in this study is voluntary. Your decision whether or not to participate will not affect your current or future relationship with any health care facilities or treatment providers. If you decide to participate, you are free to not answer any question or withdraw at any time with out affecting those relationships. Contacts and Questions: The researcher conducting this study is YeaJu Rhee. You may ask any questions you have now. If you have questions later, you are encouraged to contact me, 517-643-0631, Rheeyea@msu.edu at Michigan State University Music Therapy Department, or Associate Professor Roger Smeltekop, 517- 355-6753, Smeltokop3@msu.edu If you have any questions or concerns regarding this study and would like to talk to someone other than the researcher(s), you are encouraged to contact the Michigan State University’s Human Research Protection Program at 517-355-2180, Fax 517-432-4503, or e-mail irb@msu.edu or regular mail at 202 Olds Hall, MSU, East Lansing, MI 48824. You will be given a copy of this information to keep for your records. Statement of Consent: I have reviewed the above information. I have had opportunity to ask questions and have received answers. I consent to participate in the study. Signature: Date: Signature of parent or guardian: Date: Signature of Investigator: Date: 115 ET-.. '9‘ "E a APPENDIX 1 1 Spoken Language Comprehension ASHA. (2003). FCMs. National outcomes Measurement System (NOMS): Adult Speech-language Pathology User’s Guide. LEVEL 1: The individual is alert, but unable to follow simple directions or respond to yes/no questions, even with cues. LEVEL 2: With consistent, maximal cues, the individual is able to follow simple directions, respond to simple yes/no questions in context, and respond to simple words or phrases related to personal needs. LEVEL 3: The Individual usually responds accurately to simple yes/no questions. The individual is able to follow simple direction out of context, although moderate cueing is consistently needed. Accurate comprehension of more complex directions/messages is infrequent. LEVEL 4: The individual consistently responds accurately to simple yes/no questions and occasionally follows simple directions without cues. Moderate contextual support is needed to understand complex sentences/messages. The individual is able to understand limited LEVEL 5: The individual is able to understand communication in structured conversations with both familiar and unfamiliar communication partners. The individual occasionally requires minimal cueing to understand more complex sentences/messages. The individual occasionally initiates the use of compensatory strategies when encountering difficulty. LEVEL 6: The individual is able to understand communication in most activities, but some limitations in comprehension are still apparent in vocational, avocational, and social activities. The individual rarely requires minimal cueing to understand complex sentences. The individual usually uses compensatory strategies when encountering difficulty. LEVEL 7: The individual’s ability to independently participate in vocational, avocational, and social activities are not limited by spoken language comprehension. When difficulty with comprehension occurs, the individual consistently uses a compensatory strategy. 116 APPENDIX 12 Spoken Language Expression ASHA. (2003). FCMs. National outcomes Measurement System (NOMS): Adult Speech-language Pathology User’s Guide. LEVEL 1 : The individual attempts to speak, but verbalizations are not meaningful to familiar or unfamiliar communication partners at any time. LEVEL 2: The individual attempts to speak, although few attempts are accurate or appropriate. The communication partner must assume responsibility for structuring the communication exchange, and with consistent and maximal cueing, the individual can only occasionally produce automatic and/or imitative words and phrases that are rarely meaningful in context. LEVEL 3: The communication partner must assume responsibility for structuring the communication exchange, and with consistent and moderate cueing, the individual can produce words and phrases that are appropriate and meaningful in context. LEVEL 4: The individual is successfully able to initiate communication using spoken language in simple, structured conversations in routine daily activities with familiar communication partners. The individual usually requires moderate cueing, but is able to demonstrate use of simple sentences (i.e., semantics, syntax, and morphology) and rarely uses complex sentences/messages. LEVEL 5: The individual is successfully able to initiate communication using spoken language in structured conversations with both familiar and unfamiliar communication partners. The individual occasionally requires minimal cueing to frame more complex sentences in messages. The individual occasionally self-cues when encountering difficulty. LEVEL 6: The individual is successfully able to communicate in most activities, but some limitations in spoken language are still apparent in vocational, avocational, and social activities. The individual rarely requires minimal cueing to frame complex sentences. The individual usually self-cues when encountering difficulty. LEVEL 7: The individual’s ability to successfully and independently participate in vocational, avocational, and social activities is not limited by spoken language skills. Independent functioning may occasionally include use of self-cueing. ll7 APPENDIX 13 Fluency ASHA. (2003). FCMs. National outcomes Measurement System (N OMS): Adult Speech-language Pathology User’s Guide. LEVEL 1: Fluency is so disrupted that speech is often not firnctional for communication. Attempts at speech communication are extremely labored in all situations, which renders the speaker virtually unintelligible. Alternative means of speaking are used most of the time. Listeners avoid spoken interaction with the individual. LEVEL 2: Speech is functional most of the time, but labored in many day-to-day situations due to extended disruptions of speech flow which sometimes render the individual difficult to understand. Participation in vocational, avocational, and social activities requiring speech is reduced overall. Listener discomfort is evident throughout conversational interactions. LEVEL 3: Speech is functional. Dysfluencies are evident in all situations, but are particularly frequent in problem situations. Vocational, avocational, and social participation requiring speech is occasionally reduced overall, and significantly reduced within what the individual perceives as problem situations. Some listener discomfort is evident throughout interactions. LEVEL 4: Speech is functional for communication, but there is extreme situational variation. The frequency and severity of disruptions of speech flow within problem situation is distracting most but not all of the time. Vocational, avocational, and social participation requiring speech is limited most of the time in problem situations. Listeners are often aware of fluency difficulty. LEVEL 5: Speech is firnctional for communication, and fluency can be maintained in some situations. Self-monitoring is inconsistent. The frequency and severity of disruptions of speech flow within problem situations is distracting some of the time. Speech difficulties are noticeable when they occur, and sometimes limit vocational, avocational, and social activities requiring speech in problem situations. Listeners are occasionally aware of fluency difficulties relative to particular situations. LEVEL 6: Speech is functional for communication, and fluency can be maintained most of the time. Self-monitoring is consistent. Vocational, avocational, and social activities requiring speech is not restricted most of the time. Listeners are infrequently aware of fluency difficulties even in problem situations. LEVEL 7: Disruptions in speech flow do not call attention to the speaker, and participation in activities requiring speech is not limited. May include self-monitoring as needed. 118 APPENDIX 14 & 15 Raw Data Tables & Statistics Data Tables For L ’ Goal Areas 119 In APPENDIX 14 -1 Sub 'ect L ’5 Raw Data 0 Bi ht Variables Subject USS?!“ Session 1 2 3 4 13 14 15 16 l-pre 4 m. 4 m. 5 m. 6 m. 7 m. 6 m. 5 m. 6 m. 6 m. 6 m. 7 m. 6 m. 8 m. 6 m. 7 m. 10m. 27 s. 41 s. 36 s. 3 s. 47 s. 30 s. 19 s. 11 s. 4 s. 55 s. 50 s. 44 s. 17 s. 58 s. 17 s. 44 s. l-post 5 m. 8 m. 8 m. 6 m. 7 m. 6 m. 7 m. 6 m. 7 m. 10m. 6 m. 9 m. 9 m. 10m. 10m. 10m. 315. 65. 105. 255. 17s. 25. 44s. 95. 185. 36s. 445. 54$. 35. 34s. 135. 505. Z-pre 19 24 18 21 18 20 16 18 20 10 17 17 14 15 20 23 2-post 26 28 25 20 26 12 27 20 21 31 17 20 14 20 33 37 3-pre 115 208 225 191 435 288 258 313 314 141 258 208 389 348 418 761 3-post 242 411 385 307 405 293 359 296 399 303 208 355 379 531 577 558 2-1-pre 11 23.7 25 21.7 67 38 33.7 49 33 19.7 29.7 22 54.7 41 48 93 2-1-post 24.3 38 33.7 44 37 56 28 40.7 36 17.3 22 43 50.7 50 59.7 40 3-1-pre 25.8 44.4 48.9 31.6 55.9 44.3 48.5 50.6 51.8 20.4 32.9 30.9 47 50 57.4 70.9 3-1-post 43.9 50.7 47.1 47.8 55.6 48.6 46.4 48.1 54.7 28.6 30.9 35.9 41.9 50.3 56.5 51.5 1. Total Length of Samples, 2. Total Number of Utterances, 3. Total Number of Words, 2-1. Average Length of Utterances, 3-1. Average Number of Words per Minute Subectflamtb Session 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 1)—pre 3 4 3 4 4 3 6 4 4 4 6 S 4 5 6 7 1)-post 6 6 7 6 7 2)-pre 1 1.8 2.3 1.3 1.8 2 0.7 1.5 1.75 2 1 0.6 0.5 1.2 0.5 0.7 2)-post 0.5 0.7 0.7 1.2 0.6 0.2 0.6 0.3 0.7 0.7 0.5 0.6 1 0.6 1 0.3 1) Total Number of Items Named 2) Average Number of Prompt per ltem Named Suebzct [Us MW session 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 M1T(%) 53 48 58 67 65 58 63 56 65 62 80 82 75 81 70 86 Level 11 11 11 ll 11 11 ll 11 11 ll 11 11 11 111 111 111 120 APPENDIX 14 - 2 (Continued) Subiect R ’s Raw Data at Seven Variables Subject RSponIneousSpeeIhtestb Session 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 1'13"! 4 m. 6 m. S m. 6 m. 6 m. 6 m. S m. 8 m. 7 m. S m. 6 m. 4 m. 4 m. 4 m. 9 m. 4 m. 52 s. 25 s. 30 s. 33 s. 56 s. 29 s. 48 s. 20 s. 47 s. 54 s. 53 s. 33 s. 40 s. 49 s. 4 s. 28 s. 1-post 6 m. 5 m. 8 m. 9 m. 9 m. 6 m. 4 m. 6 m. 9 m. 7 m. 8 m 7 m. 8 m. 5 m. 6 m. 3 m. 515. Is. 42 s. 455. 355. 535. 205. 445. 505. 13s. ' 23s. 31 s. 13 s. 42$. 75. Z-pre 9 18 16 8 11 12 13 12 16 12 13 11 10 12 13 14 Z-post 12 14 10 7 16 11 11 16 19 19 15 13 11 12 19 19 3-pre 128 244 270 208 197 217 238 323 341 189 304 226 152 194 432 203 3-post 240 183 283 233 318 194 224 234 414 237 351 275 366 245 323 370 2-1-pre 29 40.3 36 45 28 43 35.7 82.3 47.7 31 50.7 39 23.7 .36.7 71 26.7 2-1-post 41.7 21 58 63 61 32 42 33 52 27 45.7 40.7 65.7 39.7 38 41.7 3-1-pre 26.3 38.0 49.1 31.8 28.4 33.5 41.0 38.8 43.8 32.0 44.7 49.7 32.6 40.3 47.6 45.4 3-1-post 35.0 36.5 32.5 23.9 33.2 28.2 51.7 34.8 42.1 32.8 43.9 37.2 43.0 39.4 48.2 45.6 1. Total Length ofSamples, 2. Total Number of Utterances, 3. Total Number of Words, 2-1. Average Length of Utterances 3-1. Average Number of Words per Minute ect 83 Test Session 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 1)-pre 43 46 57 54 35 53 43 38 53 44 48 SS 49 32 49 54 1)-post 46 53 63 63 46 59 52 69 52 43 49 SS 54 51 59 51 2)-pre 0 0.02 0 0.02 0.14 0.06 0.09 0.05 0 0.05 0.04 0 0.02 0.12 0.02 0 2)-post 0.09 0 0.03 0.02 0.07 0.03 0.06 0.04 0.04 0.05 0.06 0 0.04 0.06 0.05 0 1) Total Number of Items Named 2) Average Number of Prompt per Item Named 121 II 1 I e: L_. Subject L ’s Paired t-test Data For Goal I (Table 13-1 — The second-half period in L’s paired t-test) APPENDIX 15-1 Paired Samples Statistics Std. Error Mean 11 Std. Deviation Ilean— I.Total Length of Samples Pair TS-mffmj 7.6021 8 1.43475 .50726 1 rsrrrrrosu 9.4000 8 1.57765 .55768 2.Tota1 Number of Utterances Pair TSlll'I‘J’reJ 110000 8 4.07080 1.43925 New Number of Words “Istronst2 24.1250 8 8.35699 2.95464 Pair TSIII'I‘J’rej 354.6250 8 188.41591 66.61508 'I‘SIIl'U’osLS 413.7500 8 131.30200 46.42227 average Length of Pair 131111171621 42.6375 8 23.68127 8.37259 mm“ 4 TSMITJ’OSLZJ 39.8375 8 14.47490 5.11765 5.Average Number of words Pair TSIIII‘_Pre_3_1 45.1625 8 16.25677 5.74764 per Minute 5 ’I‘SIIITJ‘osLSJ 43.7875 8 10.98771 3.88474 6.TotalNumber 0mm, Pair Namingrre121911 5.1250 8 1.12599 .39810 Named 6 IIaminLPosLLZhalf 9.7500 8 1.48805 52610 I7.Average NiimbeirOf d Pair IlaminLPreJJhalf 1.0313 8 .57875 .20462 “"“P‘S P“ em 8““ 7 ”mustard: .6750 8 23755 06399 (Table 13-2 — The second-half period in L’s paired t-test) Paired Samples Test Pairedfierences 95" Confidence Interval of the Std. Error DiffHF-‘L Mean Std. Deviation Mean Lower Upper t df Sig. (Z-tailed) 1:" gmmijgfl -1.79793 1.79794 63567 3.30104 -.29481 -2.828 7 .026 g” fiflj‘ofiz -7.12500 7.88194 27866911371447 -.63553 -2.557 7 .038 1;“ Efigfifa 59.12500 13611491 48.12389-17291991 64.66991 -1.229 7 .259 1:“ Eligiiifilf 2.80000 22.94641 7.90029 L16.88123 2148126 .354 7 .788 E.“ 2536:5511 1.37500 8.39043 2.96646 -5.63957 8.38957 .464 7 .657 :9 fiflgg—tfl’ffl’ -4.62500 1.06066 .37500 -5.51173 -3.73827 -12.333 7 .000 1;“ gflgfifi’fh‘ .35625 .65978 23327 -.19534 .90784 1.527 7 .171 (Pair 1) 1.Total Length of Samples, (Pair 2) 2.Total Number of Utterances, (Pair 3) 3.Total Number of Words. (Pair 4) 4. Average Length of Utterances, (Pair 5) 5. Average Number of Words per Minute, (Pair 6) 6.Total Number of Items Named, (Pair 7) 7.Average Number of Prompts per Item Named. 122 (Table l4-1 — The first-half period in L’s paired t—test) l.Total Length of Samples 2.Total Number of Utterances 3.Total Number of Words 4.Average Length of Utterances 5.Average Number of Words per Minute 6.Total Number of Items Named 7.Average Number of Prompts per Item Named APPENDIX 15-2 Subiect L ’s Paired t-test Data For Goal II Paired Samples Statistics Std. Error Mean ll Std. Deviation Mean Pair MlT_Pre_l 5.857 8 1.06716 .377? 1 1111138581 6.9250 8 1.02412 .36208 Pair 14101882 19.2500 8 2.43487 .86086 100198512 23.0000 8 5.37188 1.89926 Pair lilT_Pre_8 254.1250 8 95.36462 33.71648 3 MIT_POSL3 337.2500 8 61.44626 21.72453 Pair liTI‘_Pr6_2_l 33.6376 8 17.71367 6.26273 4 MILPOSLZJ 37.7125 8 9.81114 3.46876 Pair MIT.Pre_3_1 43.7500 8 10.10078 3.57116 5 MIT_POSL3_1 48.5250 8 3.45243 1.22062 Pair iiaririrraPraLiirair 3.8750 8 .99103 .35038 6 MamingJJOSLLIhatf 6.6250 8 1.06066 .37500 Pair NaminLPrellhalf 1.5500 8 .53184 .18803 7 iiarrriraPosLLnraii .6000 8 .30237 .10690 123 APPENDIX 15-2 (Continued) Subiect L ’s Paired t-test Data F or Goal II (Table 14-2 — The first half period in L’s paired t-test) Paired Samples Test Paired Differences 95% Confidence Interval of the Std. Error Difference Mean Std Deviation Mean Lower Upper t Sig. (2-tailed) ‘Pairl MII‘_Pre.l-MII‘_PosL. -1.10429 1.51606 .53601 -2.37175 .1631? '2060 7 .078 P3112 Ml'I‘.Pre.2-111'I‘.PosL1-3.75000 6.04152 213600 8.80084 1.30084 -1.756 7 .123 P8113 1111‘.Pre_3-111'1‘_P05Ur83.12500 86.62800 30.62762-15554782 40.70218 -2.714 7 .030 Pair4 Ml'I‘_Pre.2_1-111T. PosL2_l -4.07500 17.51283 6.19172 318.71609 10.56609 -.658 7 .531 PM 03552-314” -4.77600 8.27919 2.92714 -11.69668 2.14658 -l.631 7 .147 Pairfi llamingJ’reJJhalf- P8117 NaminLPreJJhalf- ., llaminLPosLZlhalf .95000 .64807 .22913 .40820 1.49180 4.146 4 .004 (Pair 1) l.Total Length of Samples, (Pair 2) 2.Total Number of Utterances, (Pair 3) 3.Total Number of Words. (Pair 4) 4. Average Length of Utterances, (Pair 5) 5. Average Number of Words per Minute, (Pair 6) 6.Total Number of Items Named, (Pair 7) 7.Average Number of Prompts per Item Named. 124 APPENDIX 15-3 Subiect L ’s Paired t-test Data F or Goal 111 (Table 15-1. Independent t-test for L’s Goal 119 Group Statistics Std. Error Group 11 Mean Std. Deviation Mean l.Total Length of Samples 131le 1 8 1.1043 1.51606 .53601 2 8 1.7979 1.79794 .63567 Z-Total Number of Did 1 8 3.7500 6.04152 2.13600 Uttc’ances 2 8 7.1250 7.88194 2.78669 310““ Number of Words 12151.3 1 8 83.1250 86.62800 30.62762 2 8 59.1250 136.11491 48.12389 4-Average Length of Diff.2_1 1 8 4.0760 17.51283 6.19172 ”“6”““5 2 8 -2.8000 22.34541 7.90029 Words per Minute 2 8 -1.3750 8.39043 2.96646 6.Total Number of Items Naming_Diff_1 l 8 27500 '70711 25000 Named 2 8 4.6250 1.06066 .37500 7. Average Number of 11ammg_D1ff.2 l 8 -.9500 .64807 .22913 Prompts per Item Named 2 8 '.3563 .65973 .23327 Group 1 — Post-tests minus Pre-tests in the first-half sessions (lst— 8th) Group 2 — Post-tests minus Pre-tests in the second-half sessions (9th -l6th) 125 APPENDIX 15-3 (Continued) Subiect L ’s Paired t-test Data For Goal III (Table 15-2. Independent t-test for L’s Goal III) Independent Samples Test Lflene'sTestfor liqualibv of Variances t-test for 110116110 01 Means 95% Confidence 11118408101016 Mean Std. Error Difference F Sig. t df Sig.(2-tailed) Difference Difference Lower Upper m Equalvariances . 8551111841 .646 .435 -.834 14 .418 169364 .83149 '2.44701 1.05973 Equalvariances .. not assuned -.834 13.612 .419 '.69364 .83149 '2.48149 1.09452 0111] Equalvariances assumed 1.449 .249 1961 14 .353 '3.37500 3.51114 40.90565 4.15565 Equalvariances _ not assailed -.961 13.115 .354 '3.37500 3.51114 '1095363 4.20363 M3 Eqma’m’ 1.815 .199 .421 14 .680 24.00000 57.04349 98.34812 146.34812 assumed Equalvariances L‘ not assuned .421 11.871 .681 24.00000 57.04349-100.43654 14843654 Difl_2_1 Equal variances ., assailed .000 .998 .685 14 .505 6.87500 10.03753 -14.65337 28.40331 Equalvariances ., _ not assumed .685 13.244 .505 6.87500 1003453 14.76928 28.51928 0111.31 Equal variances mm .141 .713 1.476 14 .162 6.15000 4.16750 2.78839 15.0839 Equalvariances not assumed 1.476 13.998 .162 6.15000 4.16750 '2.78854 15.08854 liammLDile Equal‘it;names 2.011 .178 '4160 14 .001 -1.87500 .45069 284164 -.90836 assune Equalvariances .. notassumed 4.160 12.196 .001 -1.87500 .4506!) -2.85523 -.89474 “MLDM Eqmmc“ .015 .904 -1.816 14 .091 -.69376 32698 -1.29505 .10756 assumed Equalvariances . . . . notassumed 'l.8l6 13.996 .091 -.59375 32698-129507 .10/57 Group 1 — Post-tests minus Pre-tests in the first—half sessions ( 1 358‘“) Group 2 — Post-tests minus Pre-tests in the second-half sessions (9m-l6m) (Difference-l) l.Total Length of Samples, (Difi-Z) 2.Total Number of Utterances, (Diff-3) 3.Total Number of Words. (Diff-Z- l) 4.Average Length of Utterances, (Diff-3- l) 5.Average Number of Words per Minute, (Naming-Diff-l) 6.Total Number of Items Named, (Naming-Diff-Z) 7.Average Number of Prompts per Item Named 126 REFERENCES 127 REFERENCES Albert, M., Sparks, R., & Helm, N. (1973). Melodic intonation therapy for aphasia. Archives Neurology, 29(2), 130-131. Baker (2000). Modifying the Melodic Intonation Therapy Program for Adults With Severe Non-fluent Aphasia. Music Therapy Perspectives, 18(2), 110-1 14. Baker, & Tamplin, J. (2006). Music therapy methods in neurorehabilitation: a clinician ’5 manual. London: J. Kingsley Publishers. Belin, P., Van Eeckhout, P., Zilbovicius, M., Remy, P., Francois, C., Guillaume, S., et al. (1996). Recovery from nonfluent aphasia afier melodic intonation therapy: a PET study. Neurology, 47(6), 1504-1511. Benson, D., Dobkin, B., Rothi, L., Helm-Estabrooks, N., & Kertesz, A. (1994). Assessment: Melodic intonation therapy. Neurology, 44, 566-568. Benton, A. (1977). The amusias. Music and the Brain. London: William Heinemann. Bonakdarpour, B., Efiekharzadeh, A., & Ashayeri, H. (2003). Melodic intonation therapy in Persian aphasic patients. Aphasiology, 1 7(1), 75-95. Borchgrevink, H. (1982). Prosody and musical rhythm are controlled by the speech hemisphere. Music, mind, and brain: The neuropsychology of music, 151-157. Carroll, D. (1996). A study of the eflectiveness of an adaptation of melodic intonation therapy in increasing the communicative speech of young children with down syndrome. McGill University, Montreal. Cohen, N. (1992). The effect of singing instruction on the speech production of neurologically impaired persons. Journal of Music Therapy, 29(2), 87-102. Cohen, N., & Ford, J. (1995). The effect of musical cues on the nonpurposive speech of persons with aphasia. Journal of Music Therapy, 32, 46-46. Cohen, N., & Masse, R. (1993). The application of singing and rhythmic instruction as a therapeutic intervention for persons with neurogenic communication disorders. Journal of Music Therapy, 30, 81-81. Crocker, D. B. (1958). Using music in a speech therapy program. Music Therapy 195 7, 103-108. Darrow, A., & Starker, G. (1986). The effect of vocal training on the intonation and rate of hearin g impaired children's speech. Journal of Mu sic Therapy, 23(4), 194- 201. 128 Engineer, N., Percaccio, C., Pandya, P., Moucha, R., Rathbun, D., & Kilgard, M. (2004). Environmental Enrichment Improves Response Strength, Threshold, Selectivity, and Latency of Auditory Cortex Neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 92(1), 73- 82. Galloway, H., & Kraus, T. (1982). Melodic intonation therapy with language delayed apraxic children. Journal of MusicTherapy. 19(2), 102—113. Garrett, K., & Beukelman, D. (1995). Changes in the interaction patterns of an individual with severe aphasia given three types of partner support. Clinical Aphasiology, 23, 23 7-25 1. Garrett, K., & Lasker, J. (2005). The multimodal communication screening test for persons with aphasia (MCST-A): Retrieved. German, D. (1992). Test of adolescent/adult word finding: TA WF: DLM Teaching Resources/Taskmaster. Gerstman, H. (1964). A Case Of Aphasia. J Speech Hear Disord, 29, 89-91. Gleason, J ., & Goodglass, H. (1984). Some Neurological and Linguistic Accompaniments of the Fluent and Nonfluent Aphasias. Topics in Language Disorders, 4(3), 71-81. Glover, H., Kalinowski, J ., Rastatter, M., & Stuart, A. ( 1996). Effect of instruction to sing on stuttering frequency at normal and fast rates. Perceptual and motor skills, 83(2), 511-522. Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1972). The Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders: Lea & Febiger, US. Hachinski, K., & Hachinski, V. (1994). Music and the brain. CMAJ: Canadian Medical Association Journal, 151(3), 293. Hebert, S., Racette, A., Gagnon, L., & Perets, I. (2003). Revisiting the dissociation between singing and speaking in expressive aphasia. Brain, 126(8), 1838-1851. Helfrich—Miller, K. (1994). A clinical perspective: melodic intonation therapy for developmental apraxia. Clin Commun Disord, 4(3), 175-182. Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1992). ADP: Aphasia Diagnostic Profiles: Riverside Pub. Co. Helm-Estabrooks, N., & Albert, M. (1991). Manual of aphasia therapy: Pro-Ed. 129 Hobson, M. (2006). The Collaboration of Music Therapy and Speech-Language Pathology in the Treatment of Neurogenic Communication Disorders: Part 1— Diagnosis, Therapist Roles, and Rationale for Music. Music Therapy Perspectives, 24(2), 58-65. Jackson, J. (1871). Singing by speechless (aphasic) children. Lancet, 2, 430-431. Jackson, S., Trehame, D., & Boucher, J. (1997). Rhythm and language in children with moderate learning difficulties. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 32(1), 99-108. Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (2001). Boston Naming Test Record Booklet: Philadelphia: Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins. Keenan, J. (1987). First Aid for Aphasia: Home Exercises. Keith, R. L., & Aronson, A. E. (1975). Singing as therapy for apraxia of speech and aphasia: report of a case. Brain Lang, 2(4), 483-488. King, B. (2007). Language and Speech: Distinguishing Between Aphasia, Apraxia, and Dysarthria in Music Therapy Research and Practice. Music Therapy Perspectives, 25(1), 13-18. Lathom, W., Edson, S., & Toombs, M. (1965). A coordinated speech therapy and music therapy program. Journal of Music Therapy, 3, 118—120. Laughlin, S., Naeser, M., & Gordon, W. (1979). Effects of three syllable durations using the melodic intonation therapy technique. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 22(2), 31 1. Leonard, H. (1985). The Best Songs Ever: Hal Leonard Corporation. Leung, K. (1985). Improving the Use of Suprasegmentals with Severely Handicapped Children through Music and Movement. BC Journal of Special Education, 9(2), 1 3 1-143. Levitin, D. (2006). This is your brain on music: The science of a human obsession: Dutton Books. Lezak, M., Howieson, D., Loring, D., Fischer, J ., & Hannay, J. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment: Oxford University Press New York. Lucia, C. (1987). Toward developing a model of music therapy intervention in the rehabilitation of head trauma patients. Music Therapy Perspectives, 4, 34-39. 130 Mackay, J ., & Mensah, G. (2004). The Atlas of Heart Disease and Stroke: World Health Organization. Naeser, M., & Helm-Estabrooks, N. (1985). CT scan lesion localization and response to melodic intonation therapy. Cortex, 21, 203-223. Ogden, J. (1982). A Study of the Value of Musical Activities in Articulation Therapy. University of Kansas, Art and Music Education and Music Therapy. Overy, K., Norton, A., & Ozdemir, E. (2005). Activation of left inferior frontal gyrus afier melodic intonation therapy in a Broca’s aphasia patient. Program N. 595.7 2004 Abstract Viewer/Itinerary Planner. Society for Neuroscience. Washington, DC. Patel, A., Peretz, I., Tramo, M., & Labreque, R. (1998). Processing prosodic and musical patterns: a neuropsychological investigation. Brain and Language, 61(1), 123- 144. Patterson, P. B. A. (2004). Rise Up Singing. Bathlehem, PA: Sing Out Publications. Peretz, I., & Hébert, S. (1995). Music and the Mind Machine. The psychophysiology and psychopathology of the sense fo music: Music processing after brain damage: The case of rhythm without melody. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Popovici, M. (1995). Melodic intonation therapy in the verbal decoding of aphasics. Rom J Neurol Psychiatry, 33(1), 57-97. Prior, M., Kinsella, G., & Giese, J. (1990). Assessment of musical processing in brain- damaged patients: Implications for laterality of music. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12(2), 301-312. Racette, A., Bard, C., & Peretz, I. (2006). Making non-fluent aphasics speak: sing along! Brain, 129(10), 2571. Sacks, O. (2006). The power of music. Brain, 129(10), 2528. Sacks, O. (2008). Musicophilia: Tales of music and the brain: Vintage. Schlang, G., Marchina, S., & Norton, A. (2008). From Singing To Speaking: Why Singing may lead to recovery of expressive language function in patients with Broca' aphasia Music Perception, 25(4), 315-323. Sparks, & Deck, L. (1986). Melodic intonation therapy. Sparks, Helm, N., & Albert, M. (1974). Aphasia rehabilitation resulting from melodic intonation therapy. Cortex, 10(4), 303-316. 131 Sparks, & Holland, A. (1976). Method: Melodic Intonation Therapy for Aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 41(3), 287. Stone, A. (2009). America's All-Time Favorite Songs Amsco Publications. Thaut, M. (2005). Rhythm, music, and the brain: scientific foundations and clinical applications: Routledge. Vanderark, S. B., J. S Stewart, S. (1986). Utilizing music in speech therapy with a cerebral palsied girl. Contricutions to music education, 13, 1—6. Vargha, & Gereb (1953). Aphasie T herapie. Jene, Germany: VEB Fisher. Weisssman, D. F. a. D. (2007). The Great Family Songbook. New York: Black Dog & Leventhal Publishers, Inc. Yamadori, A., Osumi, Y., Masuhara, S., & Okubo, M. (1977). Preservation of singing in Broca's aphasia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 40(3), 221- 224. Zatorre, R., Evans, A., & Meyer, E. (1994). Neural mechanisms underlying melodic perception and memory for pitch. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(4), 1908-1919. 132 "11111131111111fllfllflujililflifllflllfil“