‘- 33%;? .v....a..t‘¢~.<$um 5:de . Pl- . .I 3 )1 %, fir mm?» ‘ u"... .110: “gm. . vi.“ 1:... 3.2. .1». 153.! .1...» .v: (.27 I- LIBRARY ' I t Michigan 5'0“ a University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled INSPIRING THE LIFE OF THE MIND: AN EXAMINATION OF THE ROLES OF RESIDENTIAL COLLEGE ENVIRONMENTS AND MOTIVATIONAL ATTRIBUTES IN PROMOTING UNDERGRADUATE STUDENTS’ INCLINATION TO INQUIRE AND CAPACITY FOR LIFELONG LEARNING presented by Jody E. Jessup-Anger has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Ph. D. degree in Higher, Adult, and Lifelong Education Modem 61.56:” Major Professor’s Signature film’- qr 071qu Date MSU is an Afiinnative Action/Equal Opportunity Employer PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5/08 K lProj/Acc8PrelelRC/Date0ue indd INSPIRING THE LIFE OF THE MIND: AN EXAIVIINATION OF THE ROLES OF RESIDENTIAL COLLEGE ENVIRONMENTS AND MOTIVATIONAL ATTRIBUTES IN PROMOTING UNDERGRADUATE STUDENTS’ INCLINATION TO INQUIRE AND CAPACITY FOR LIFELONG LEARNING By Jody E. Jessup-Anger A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Higher, Adult, and Lifelong Education 2009 ABSTRACT INSPIRING THE LIFE OF THE MIND: AN EXAMINATION OF THE ROLES OF RESIDENTIAL COLLEGE ENVIRONMENTS AND MOTIVATIONAL ATTRIBUTES IN PROMOTING UNDERGRADUATE STUDENTS’ INCLINATION TO INQUIRE AND CAPACITY FOR LIFELONG LEARNING By Jody E. J essup-Anger As postsecondary education is promoted as necessary for participating in the 21St f_ century knowledge economy, academics, policymakers, and the public have voiced _. concerns about the quality and coherence of undergraduate education (AAC & U, 2007; Barr and Tagg, 1995; U. S. Department of Education, 2006). Critics point to the Size, scope, and multiple missions of large, public research universities as contributing to students’ feelings of anonymity, disengagement, anddisconnection from faculty (Astin, 1993; Boyer, 1987; Gaff, 1970; Gamson, 2000; Guskin, 1994; Hawkins, 1999). University administrators increasingly turn to residential colleges and other types of living-learning programs to address the size and scale conundrum facing large research universities. By creating smaller enclaves of students living together initially, taking part in a shared educational endeavor, and using resources within their environment that stress academics (Inkelas, Zeller, Murphy, & Hummel, 2006), administrators and faculty purport to create the atmosphere of a small liberal arts college while still offering students the resources of a large university (Magolda, 1994; Schuman, 2005). Despite this claim, virtually no attention has been paid to whether and how these environments promote values associated with a liberal arts education, including whether they deepen students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Identified by the Center of Inquiry in the Liberal Arts at Wabash College as one distinctive outcome of a liberal arts " ”"4 L1 ‘ ’II‘SI‘J‘H‘ 9’ 35' ' "' I' ‘maybrunldkbrgr‘b“:3“. :;‘e‘,,ii'!u.-U In! I a” .. . education, having a deep inclination to inquire suggests that a student has a strong desire to learn and continues to pursue intellectual growth. Closely connected to students’ inclination to inquire, is their capacity for lifelong learning, defined by Hayek and Kuh (1999) as students’ ability to “‘learn to learn’ and interact effectively with others in a complex, information-based society” (p. 4). In the current study, I used Moos’ social-ecological framework, which accounts for environmental context and individual characteristics (Moos, 1976, 1979, 1986), to examine how students’ attributes (including their motivation and other sociodemographic characteristics) and residential college environments were associated with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. With data collected from over 1800 undergraduate students in 24 residential colleges at 10 research universities across the United States, I used hierarchical linear modeling techniques to ascertain the statistically significant individual, contextual, and cross-level associations of variables with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. I found that a statistically Significant amount of variation students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning was attributable to students’ residential college environment and that much of the variation was explained by the liberal arts emphasis of the residential college environment. Specifically, an ethos marked by academic challenge and high expectations was associated with a deepened inclination to inquire and an ethos marked by out-of- class interactions with faculty was associated with a deepened capacity for lifelong learning. Furthermore, students’ motivational attributes and desire to obtain more than a bachelor’s degree were also associated both outcome variables. Implications for theory, research, practice, and policy are discussed. DEDICATION I dedicate this dissertation to Eric and Olivia who bring me a constant source of joy. iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS If ‘it takes a village to raise a child,’ it takes an entire city of support to birth a dissertation and a child at the same time. I would like to express my gratitude to the faculty, friends, and farmly who have provided cOuntless hours of advice and support as I indulged in this academic undertaking. First, to Kris Renn, my advisor, thank you for stepping me through a large portion of the writing process while on sabbatical. I appreciate the countless drafts you read while traversing across the world or recovering from jetlag. Thanks too for bringing stories of your adventures to our meetings. At times they were much more exciting than my struggles with data collection. To Kim Maier, thank you for your patience as you helped me to learn a new method of data analysis. I appreciate your willingness to meet, to challenge mythinking, and to be candid about the balance of family and academe. You are a wonderful inspiration. To Marilyn Amey and Ann Austin, thank you for your time, thoughtful questions, and guidance in conceptualizing the study. I aspire to teach as well as each of you. Thank you to the other faculty in the Higher, Adult, and Lifelong Education program for contributing to my knowledge of higher education. From deepening my understanding of the rOle of postsecondary education in society, to bolstering my skills in conceptualizing and designing a study, the education you have provided is first-rate. Thank you to Matt Wawrzynski for taking me on as a research assistant four years ago despite my limited knowledge of quantitative methods. Thank you to John Dirkx for inviting me to participate on your research team examining the impact of study abroad. And, thank you to Jim Fairweather for serving as my ‘dissertation psychologist’, meeting with me throughout the proposal and writing process, and raising good questions when I started to get stuck. I am fortunate to have an amazing family and group of fiiends. To the rest of the cohort, thank you for providing a wonderfully supportive learning environment. I learned more because of each of you. To the yoga moms, thank you for candidly sharing your struggles with motherhood and advice for overcoming the bumps along the way. To Okemos Kids Club, thank you for showing such love for Olivia that I never hesitated to leave her in your care. To my parents, thank you for believing in me and raising me in a way that has enabled me to believe in myself. Mom, I appreciate your willingness to fly across the country on short notice to help with Olivia so that I could have more time to write. You always did it with such little hesitation that I was comfortable asking again. To Olivia, thank you for providing me with a deeper sense of purpose and focus. When I think back on this time, I imagine that one of my most lasting memories will be caring for you. I cannot tell you how much joy you have brought to our lives. Finally, to Eric, my partner, fiiend, and colleague, thank you for journeying with me. I have no doubt that I learned more, enjoyed the process more, and ate better because of you. Thank you for deepening my joy in life. We did it! vi TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .............................................................................. xi LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................ xii CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION .............................................................................. 1 Purpose and Research Questions .................................................... 7 Conceptual Framework .............................................................. 8 The Environmental System ................................................. 11 The Personal System ......................................................... 12 Mediating Factors ............................................................ l3 Coping and Adaptation ...................................................... 13 CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW ..................................................................... 15 Historical and Contemporary Role of Residential Colleges ............ 15 Historical Overview and Modern Day Challenges ............. l6 Outcomes of Residential Colleges and Living-Learning Communities ................................................ 18 Conditions that Promote Students’ Inclination to Inquire and Capacity for Lifelong Learning ................................... 25 Examining Students’ Inclination to Inquire ..................... 25 Empirical Studies Related to Students’ Inclination to Inquire ....................................................... 26 Historical Overview of Lifelong Learning ...................... 29 Recent Empirical Work on Lifelong Learning .................. 32 Relevant Motivation Theory ................................................ 37 Existing Studies Taking an Ecological Approach ........................ 39 Summary of Literature Review ..................................................... 41 CHAPTER 3 STUDY DESIGN .............................................................................. 42 Research Design ...................................................................... 43 Population, Sample, and Participants .............................................. 43 Data Collection Instruments, Variables, and Materials .......................... 52 Independent Variables ....................................................... 52 Pre-College Characteristics ....................................... 52 Motivation ............................................................ 53 Discipline ............................................................ 55 Liberal Arts Experiences .......................................... 56 Dependent Variables ......................................................... 62 Inclination to Inquire ............................................... 62 Capacity for Lifelong Learning ................................... 64 vii Data Analysis Procedures ............................................................ 66 Limitations ............................................................................. 75 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS ....................................................................................... 77 Research Question One: Does students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning vary across residential college environments? ......................................................................... 77 Research Question Two: How are students’ sociodemographic characteristics and motivation related to their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning? ............................. 79 Sociodemographic Variables ....................................... 82 Pro-College Characteristics........................... ............. 84 Motivation, College Experiences, and Aspirations ............. 85 Liberal Arts Experiences ........................................... 86 Summary of Within-Residential College Model Findings ...................... 89 Research Question Three: Does the association between students’ sociodemographic attributes, pre-college characteristics, and motivation and their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning vary across residential colleges? ........... 90 Research Question Four: How is the environmental context of a residential college related to students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning? ......................................... 94 Environmental Liberal Arts Experience Variables ............. 95 Disciplinary Focus .................................................. 100 Summary of Between-Residential College Model Findings .................... 101 Research Question Five: Does the association between student- level motivation and students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning differ by residential college- level liberal arts experience variables? ................................... 102 Summary of Results .................................................................. 104 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS ...................................................... 107 Discussion of Findings ............................................................... 110 Null Model .................................................................... l l l Within-Residential College Model ........................................ 113 Sociodemographic Attributes ..................................... 1 13 Pre-College Characteristics ............................... 1 17 Motivation, College Experiences, and Degree Aspirations ......................................... 1 18 Individual Liberal Arts Experiences ..................... 120 Between-Residential College Model ...................................... 123 Inclination to Inquire ............................................... 124 Capacity for Lifelong Learning ................................... 127 Revisiting Moos’s Social Ecological Framework ........................ 129 viii The Environmental System ......................................... 130 The Personal System ................................................. 132 Mediating Factors ...................................................... 132 Efforts at Adaptation ................................................. 133 Student Stability and Change ....................................... 134 Implications for Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy ........................ 135 Implications for Theory ..................................................... 135 Implications for Research ................................................... 136 Implications for Practice ..................................................... 139 Student Advising and Support ..................................... 140 Faculty and Student Interaction ................................... 142 Implications for Policy ....................................................... 144 Suggestions for Future Research .................................................... 146 Conclusion ............................................................................. 148 APPENDIX A: SURVEY ..................................................................... 150 APPENDIX B: VARIABLE INFORMATION ............................................ 166 APPENDIX C. FACTOR ANALYSIS OF MOTIVATION (GENERAL NEEDS SATISFACTION) SCALE .......................................................... 173 APPENDIX D: FACTOR ANALYSIS OF LIBERAL ARTS EXPERIENCE SCALE-ACADEMIC CHALLENGE AND HIGH EXPECTATIONS ...... 175 APPENDIX E: FACTOR ANALYSIS OF LIBERAL ARTS EXPERIENCE SCALE-DIVERSITY EXPERIENCES ........................................... 178 APPENDD( F: FACTOR ANALYSIS OF LIBERAL ARTS EXPERIENCE SCALE-INTERACTIONS WITH PEERS ....................................... 180 APPENDIX G: FACTOR ANALYSIS OF IN CLINATION TO INQUIRE SCALE ................................................................................. 182 APPENDIX H: FACTOR ANALYSIS OF CAPACITY FOR LIFELONG LEARNING SCALE ................................................................. 184 APPENDIX 1: BIVARIATE CORRELATIONS OF LEVEL-IVARIABLES ........ 186 APPENDIX J: BIVARIATE CORRELATIONS OF LEVEL-2 VARIABLES ...... 190 APPENDIX K: FULL MODEL OF INCLINATION TO INQUIRE .................. 192 ix APPENDIX L: FULL MODEL OF CAPACITY FOR LIFELONG LEARNING... 199 REFERENCES ................................................................. _ ................ 2 06 LIST OF TABLES Table 3.1: Sample and Distribution Information ............................................. 47 Table 3.2: Descriptive Information of Analytic Sample ..................................... 49 Table 3.3: New Scales for Good Teaching and High Quality Interactions with Faculty ................................................................................... 59 Table 4.1: Null Models: Partitioning Variance in Inclination to Inquire and Capacity for Lifelong Learning ....................................................... 78 Table 4.2: Association between Students’ Sociodemographic Characteristics, Motivation, and College Experiences and their Inclination to Inquire and Capacity for Lifelong Learning ...................................................... 81 Table 4.3: Deviance, Chi—Square Change, and Significance of Random Level-One Slopes .................................................................................... 91 Table 4.4: Association between Environments Marked by Liberal Arts Experiences and Students’ Inclination to Inquire ................................................. 97 Table 4.5: Association between Environments Marked by Liberal Arts Experiences and Students’ Capacity for Lifelong Learning ..................................... 98 Table 4.6: Deviance, Chi-Square Change, and Significance of Cross-Level Interactions between Motivation (Student) variables and Liberal Arts Experience (Environment) variables ................................................ 103 xi LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1.1: Adaptation of Moos (1979) Model of the Relationship between Environmental and Personal Variables and Student Stability and Change. . 11 Figure 5.1: Relationship between my Research Questions and Moos (1979) Model of the Relationship between Environmental and Personal Variables and Student Stability and Change ......................................................... 130 xii CHAPTER ONE Introduction In the report Reinventing Undergraduate Education: A Blueprint for America ’s Research Universities, the Boyer Commission on Educating Undergraduates in the Research University (1998) called for research universities to redesign their undergraduate curriculum systematically with an eye toward coherence and excellence. The report was issued on the heels of growing concerns regarding fiagmentation of undergraduate education in the United States, which many educators argued negatively influenced motivation, persistence, and graduation rates of undergraduate students (Barr & Tagg, 1995; Boyer Commission, 1998; Duderstadt, 2000). The call was especially important because, next to community colleges, undergraduate students attending nonprofit institutions are educated most often at research universities (National Center for Educational Statistics, 2006). Often the sheer size of these institutions detracts fi'om students’ ease in finding the sense of connection that they might at a smaller school (Boyer, 1987; Gaff, 1970; Hawkins, 1999). Among the recommendations of the report was a call for research universities to “cultivate a sense of place” by creating shared rituals, nurturing community spirit within the residence halls, forming collaborative study groups and project teams to customize the community, and using co-curricular activities to bring students together (Boyer Commission, 1998, p. 34). The development of living-learning communities is one promising practice that addresses the size and coherence conundrum at research universities. These communities consist of a cohort of participants who “(1) live together on campus, (2) take part in a shared academic endeavor, (3) use resources in their residence environment designed specifically for them, and (4) have structured social activities in their residential environment that stress academics” (Inkelas, Zeller, Murphy, & Hummel, 2006, p. 11). Residential colleges are a subset of living-learning communities that are comprehensive in form, incorporating the community-oriented ethos, academic emphasis, and co- curricular focus throughout undergraduate students’ entire educational experience, and typically culminating in the attainment of a degree. In their ideal form, these communities cohere and integrate students’ intellectual and personal lives (Smith, 1994). Smith explained that “well developed residential colleges address critical higher education issues: involvement, community building, diversity, retention, bridging the gap between . ideal academic standards and actual student performance, fostering faculty and student interaction” (p. 242). Robert O’Hara, an evolutionary biologist and residential college advocate, explained his afi'mity for their structure, stating I have spent many years living and working in residential college settings as a graduate student and a faculty member, and 1 have also had the contrasting experience of being an undergraduate in a big university without residential colleges. I am convinced that small residential colleges within a large university provide the best educational environment for students and faculty alike. (O’Hara, n. d., 11 1) Among the claims of many residential colleges at research universities is that they emulate the atmosphere of a small liberal arts college with their intimate feel, commitment to high quality undergraduate education, and holistic approach to learning, but boast the resources of a large university, which include comprehensive research and library facilities (Magolda, 1994; Schuman, 2005). The website of Lyman Briggs College (LBC), a residential college at Michigan State University, for example, makes the following claim, “All under one roof, LBC encompasses physics, chemistry, biology, and computer laboratories; classrooms; faculty, administrative, and academic support staff offices; student residences; a dining hall; and a convenience store. . .. LBC offers the benefits of a small, liberal arts college with the resources of a great research university” (Lyman Briggs College Website, n.d., 1] 1). Implicit in the comparison of residential colleges to small liberal arts colleges is an assumption that these small enclaves within research institutions are devoted to the liberal arts ideal, which consists of a clearly defined mission promoting students’ intellectual development and values congruent with their mission, including a commitment to holistic student development, to democratic ideals, and to the creation of lifelong learners (Hawkins, 1999; Hirt, 2006; Kuh, Kinzie, Schuh & Whitt, 2005; Michalak & Robert, 1981). The goals of the University of Michigan’s Residential College, which include the desire to “foster a genuine appreciation and lifelong passion for learning; not merely individual quests for knowledge, but preparation and encouragement that lead to effective and responsible engagement in the real world,” illustrate the presence of the liberal arts ideal within the community (University of Michigan Residential College website, 11. d., 1] 2). Also implied by the comparison of residential colleges to small liberal arts colleges are assumptions regarding quality of instruction and importance placed on the development of intellect. Pascarella, et a1. (2005) examined the environmental differences in liberal arts institutions, regional institutions, and research universities and found that liberal arts college environments in general generate greater student-faculty contact, active learning/time on task, expectations, quality of teaching, and a supportive campus environment. A recent working definition of liberal arts education proposed by Blaich, Bost, Chan, and Lynch (2004) supports this finding, highlighting the conditions they believe encapsulate liberal arts education, including, 1. An institutional ethos and tradition which places a greater value on developing a set of intellectual arts, than professional or vocational Skills. 2. Curricular and environmental structures that work in combination to create coherence and integrity in students’ intellectual experiences. 3. An institutional ethos and tradition which places a strong value on student- student and student-faculty interactions both in and out of the classroom. (p. 2) There is growing evidence demonstrating positive learning outcomes associated with living-learning communities and residential colleges (Pascarella & Terenzini, 2005), including greater academic achievement (Inkelas & Weisman, 2003; Pasque & Murphy, 2005), social integration and involvement (Pike, 1999; Stassen, 2003), and intellectual engagement (Pasque & Murphy, 2005). Less attention has been paid to whether and how these environments foster the transformative educational outcomes espoused (and often achieved) by liberal arts colleges (Kuh, et al., 2005; Pascarella, et al., 2005), including Whether they deepen students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Identified by the Center of Inquiry in the Liberal Arts at Wabash College as one distinctive outcome of a liberal arts education, the inclination to inquire [S]uggests a strong desire to learn, to ask questions, and a willingness to consider new ideas. Such learning involves taking initiative, not being satisfied with a . quick answer, and intrinsic motivation for intellectual growth. These learning traits lend themselves to a lifelong pursuit of knowledge and wisdom. (Center of Inquiry in the Liberal Arts at Wabash College Website, n.d., 1i 3) Closely connected to students’ inclination to inquire is their capacity for lifelong learning, which is defined by Hayek and Kuh (1999) as students’ ability to “‘learn to learn’ and interact effectively with others in a complex, information-based society” (p. 4). Whereas a deepened inclination to inquire promotes a value for continuing to pursue knowledge, a robust capacity for lifelong learning provides students the tools to act upon their value for inquiry. Examining students’ inclination to inquire and their capacity for lifelong learning is more relevant (and interesting) as access to postsecondary education widens and student demographics become more diverse. Whereas historically upper-class White men pursued a bachelor’s degree to claim their place in civic life (Thelin, 2004), today access has widened and myriad students pursue postsecondary education for a variety of reasons. Many people believe that participation in postsecondary education is becoming de facto compulsory because national and individual economic prosperity are increasingly tied “to the attainment of high levels of knowledge and skill, and to the ability to continue learning over a lifetime” (Association of American Colleges and Universities (AAC & U), 2007, p. 7). Colleges and universities are being called upon to provide students with the knowledge and skills necessary to successfully navigate an increasingly complex and globally focused world, and also to promote an education- focused mindset that will prepare students for a volatile and evolving workforce requiring continuous learning (AAC & U, 2007). A survey by the Bureau of Labor Statistics (2006) indicated that people born in the late baby boom years (1957 to 1965) changed jobs 10 times from the time they turned 18 until they turned 40. With the advent of the global knowledge economy, it is probable that younger workers will not only change jobs as often, but also require retraining within their positions. Compounding the difficulty of the charge directed at colleges and universities to develop students who possess an inclination to inquire and the capacity for lifelong learning is the reality that Students entering postsecondary education are more disengaged academically than ever before, with a greater number reporting frequently being bored in class and studying less during their last year in high school (Astin, Oseguera, Sax, & Korn, 2002). Furthermore, as access to postsecondary education widens, more students view a college education merely as the next logical step in their educational trajectory (AAC & U, 2007), necessitating that more attention he paid to understanding the interaction between students’ motivation and their collegiate environment, and specifically how to create environments that foster and develop students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. The size and scope of many large research universities, with the accompanying research pressures and emphasis on graduate study, limit the extent to which they focus on undergraduate education (Boyer, 1987; Jerome, 2000). Large classes often curb faculty and student interaction and constrain the range of assignments possible (Boyer, 1987; Guskin, 1994). Students also feel more anonymous in large environments, and report less willingness to get involved than they might in a smaller environment (Astin, 1977; Gamson, 2000; Guskin, 1994). Despite the educational limitations of large research universities, they remain a likely destination for many students to begin or complete their baccalaureate education because of their relative affordability and diversity in educational offerings. Consequently, it is vital that faculty and administrators at these large institutions find ways to create environments that help students feel connected to their curricular and co-curricular pursuits, thereby deepening their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Although residential colleges offer one way of addressing the scale conundrum, it remains unclear how these environments interact with the students within them to influence their inclination to inquire and their capacity for lifelong learning. Purpose and Research Questions The purpose of this study was to examine how students’ motivational attributes and environmental contexts facilitated or impeded students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning in residential college settings. Using data collected from over 1800 students in 24 residential colleges, 1 investigated the following questions: 1. Does students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning vary . across residential college environments? 2. How are students’ sociodemographic characteristics and motivation related to their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning? 3. Do the associations between students’ sociodemographic attributes and motivation and their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning vary across residential colleges? 4. How is the enviromnental context of a residential college related to students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning? Specifically, 3. Environmental Liberal Arts Experience variables — (good teaching and high quality interaction with faculty, academic challenge and high expectations, diversity experiences, and peer interactions) b. Disciplinary Focus 5. Does the association between student-level motivation and students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong differ by residential college- level liberal arts experience variables (good teaching and high quality interaction with faculty, academic challenge and high expectations, diversity experiences, and peer interactions)? Conceptual Framework In their comprehensive review of research related to college impact, Pascarella and Terenzini (2005) encouraged researchers to acknowledge the multitude of factors affecting student change, and to adopt broader conceptual models which might “more fully account for the multiple sources of influence,” instead of relying upon a single disciplinary perspective or dimension of students’ experiences (p. 630). Their call echoes that of human development researchers, who in the mid-1970s began to shift their perspectives and research designs beyond traditional experimental models involving only a subject and experimenter, instead expanding their gaze to include the social contexts in which development occurs and those that influence development indirectly (Bronfenbrenner 1979; Moos & Insel, 1974). Influenced by Kurt Lewin’s proposition that behavior is a fimction of a person and an environment (1936), two complementary human ecology models developed virtually simultaneously. The ecology of human development arose out of developmental psychology, specifically from Urie Bronfenbrenner’s work exploring infant and adolescent development. Bronfenbrenner (1979) emphasized the importance of studying human development in the context of “the actual environments, both immediate and remote, in which human beings live” (p. 12). His theory stressed the importance of studying development within the context it occurs, and specifically how biological factors — including physical characteristics and genetic propensities — interact with the “immediate environment, and the way in which this relation is mediated by forces emanating from more remote regions in the larger physical and social milieu” (Bronfenbrenner, 1979, pp. 12-13, italics in the original). Simultaneous to the emergence of the ecology of human development, social ecology, “the multidisciplinary study of the impact that physical and social environments have on human beings” arose out of Stanford University, and held three basic assumptions: (1) Human behavior cannot be understood apart from the environment in which it finds its expression. Accurate predictions about behavior simply cannot be made only from information about the individual; information about the environment is essential. (2) Physical and social environments must be studied together since neither can be fully understood without the other. . . [and] (3) Social ecology has an explicit value orientation in that it attempts to provide knowledge relevant to promoting maximally effective human functioning. (Moos & Insel, 1974, p. ix-x) Whereas the ecology of human development emphasized the interaction of direct ' and indirect environmental effects on biologically determined development (Bronfenbrenner, 1979), social ecology theory placed more emphasis on the immediate physical environment as a mediator of development, and underscored the importance of creating an environment that promotes effective human functioning (Moos & Insel, 1974). For the current study, I use the social ecological framework because of its explicit acknowledgement of the importance of the immediate environment. However, I also incorporate into my research design Bronfenbrenner’s notion of environments as multiple, interacting, and influential even when not directly interacting with the student, the environment as representing an ‘ethos,’ so to speak. Rudolph Moos (1976, 1979, 1986) developed a social-ecological framework to evaluate educational settings. His work stemmed from his observations that by focusing only on personal traits (including students’ demographic characteristics, motivation, or values) or environmental settings (including a residential college environment), researchers could not adequately account for variations in behavior (Moos, 1979). Instead, Moos believed it important to consider both the social-environmental and physical-environmental variables together (Moos, 1976). Moos’s (1979) model, “notes the existence of both environmental and personal systems, which influence each other through selection factors...[and] mediating processes of cognitive appraisal and activation or arousal (motivation)” (p. 4). These mediation processes typically arise when the environment necessitates a response and result in efforts at adaptation and use of coping skills. The initiation of adaptation efforts may change both the environmental and the personal systems, and ultimately determines stability or change in student behavior. A visual representation of the model is depicted in figure 1.1 and each variable in the model is described below. 10 A Environment # system ‘ 7 Student Stability Cognitive Activation. gem at & or Arousa optotion c Appraisal :5 :3 and :9 hango Coping Personal system A A Figure 1.1: Adaptation of Moos (1979) Model of the Relationship between Environmental and Personal Variables and Student Stability and Change The Environmental System. Moos (1979) described four major domains of variables within the environmental system, including “the physical setting, organizational factors, the human aggregate, and social climate” (p. 6), each of which can potentially influence educational outcomes directly or indirectly thrOugh interaction with the other environmental variables. The physical setting includes the physical design and architecture of the environment. In a residential college setting, the physical setting may include the building in which the college is housed, the presence or lack of study and gathering spaces, and the amenities provided within the college. Organizational factors include such dimensions as size of the residential college, faculty-student ratio, and offerings provided to students in the way of co-curricular activities. The human aggregate is comprised of the total characteristics of students in the setting, and may include “age, ability level, socioeconomic background, and educational attainment” (p. 8). Faculty and staff characteristics may also be part of the human aggregate. Moos found the human 11 aggregate pertinent to the environmental system because of the “notion that most of the social and cultural environment is transmitted though other people,” and the implication that “the character of an environment depends in part on the typical characteristics of its members” (p. 8). Also included in the human aggregate would be the collective attitudes of students, and their collective beliefs about the environment as promoting or thwarting their educational pursuits. The fourth domain, social climate, is inferred by the “continuity and consistency in otherwise discrete events” (p. 10). Within a residential college, the social climate would be the integrating features of the environment, including the overall attitude of students toward one another with regard to learning. In addition to serving as a domain of the environmental setting, Moos viewed the social climate as a mediator of the other environmental variables. The Personal system. AS might be expected, individual characteristics that assist in explaining students’ responses to an environmental context comprise the Personal System. “Background and personal indexes include age, sex, ability level, interests and values, ego strength and self-esteem, and preferences for such coping styles as active engagement in the environment, tension reduction and exploration” (Moos, 1979, p. 11). Other personal factors considered within the Personal System include attitudes, expectations, and roles. Moos explained that “People who have more responsible organizational roles (such as administrators, professors, and teachers, as compared with high school and college students) tend to perceive educational settings more positively,” and furthermore, “Expectations of new environments can influence both an individual’s choice and later perception of an environment” (p. 11). 12 Mediating Factors. Moos (1979) identified two factors that mediate the interaction between the Environmental System and the Personal System, namely 1) Cognitive appraisal and 2) Activation or arousal. Cognitive appraisal is the process by which an individual evaluates the environment as “being either potentially harmful, beneficial, or irrelevant (primary appraisal) and his or her perception of the range of available coping alternatives (secondary appraisal)” (p. 11). Activation or arousal occurs when an individual appraises the environment as needing a response, which in turn “prompts efforts at adaptation, or coping, which may change the environmental system (students decide to use a recreation room as a library or study hall) or the personal system (students seek and obtain information that changes their attitudes or expectations)” (p. 12). Coping and Adaptation. Moos (1979) explained that although situations chosen to study coping and adaptation usually involve major life changes including death, financial disaster, and serious illness, more common transitions and everyday Situations including the transition from adolescence to adulthood also demand coping responses. Moos also noted coping and adaptation are not only mediators of outcomes, but also can become outcomes, depending on the interest of the investigator. The social-ecological framework provided a general model for examining the process of person-environment interaction. It is relevant to the current study because it considers the dialectic relationship between person and environment, with each potentially transforming the other. In other words, the model allows for the possibility that motivated students transform their environment into a setting in which a deepening inclination to inquire and an expanding capacity for lifelong learning are the norm, and 13 also the possibility that converging environmental factors transform students toward a deeper inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. By using an ecological approach, not only was I able to examine how students’ motivation influenced students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, I was also be able to examine the interaction between students’ motivation and their environments, exploring how the interactive effects evoke or inhibit students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. In chapter two, I situate the study in existing literature related to residential colleges, the growth of students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, motivation theory, and ecological approaches to postsecondary educational research. In chapter three, I explain the research design, detailing the methods by which I carried out the study. In chapter four, 1 detail the findings of the study. Finally, in chapter five, I discuss the findings of the study in light of existing research, the implications of the findings for research and practice, and detail future research. 14 CHAPTER TWO Literature Review The purpose of this chapter is to locate the current study within the context of existing literature. The chapter is divided into four sections that inform and situate the examination of how environmental contexts and motivational attributes facilitate or impede students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning in residential college settings. Section one situates the study in the context of residential college and living-learning community literature, examining the historical and contemporary role of residential colleges and living-learning communities in US postsecondary institutions; section two examines existing literature and empirical research outlining the conditions that promote students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning; section ' three reviews relevant motivation theory, outlining how students’ motivational attributes may influence their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning; and section four provides an overview of existing studies taking an ecological approach to understanding students and environments within postsecondary education. Historical and Contemporary Role of Residential Colleges in the United States Although residential colleges have been a part of the higher education landscape in the United States since Harvard was founded in 163 6, only relatively recently have there been attempts to assess the outcomes of these environments on students. This section situates the current study in the context of existing literature, detailing a historical overview of residential colleges in the United States and providing information about modern day challenges, while also reviewing recent empirical literature that illustrates some of the outcomes of these communities. 15 Historical Overview and Modern Day Challenges Much of the literature on residential colleges in the United States is descriptive, detailing their historical role and evolution over time (Duke, 1991; Gaff, 1970; Ryan, 1992, 1993; Smith, 1994). Ryan (1992, 1993) traced the foundation of residential colleges to the English College model of Oxford and Cambridge, where older fellows instructed younger ones about subject matter and character, expanding the content cOvered beyond the classroom and into students’ day-to-day lives. Although the structure of the early colonial colleges such as Harvard and Yale reflected a commitment to holistic education and character development with residential facilities and tutors to oversee day-to-day life, the expansion of higher education and the incorporation of the German model (with its nonresidential structure and intense focus on research and graduate training) raised questions about the fundamental purpose of higher education (O’Hara, 2001; Ryan, 1993), and the way it Should be structured. The early 19003 brought about intense debate about the purpose of higher education and a partial revival of the collegiate ideal, as college leaders including Woodrow Wilson, who was then the president of Princeton, encouraged movement away fi'om the laissez-faire elective system toward a more coherent curriculum (Ryan, 1992, 1993). At large universities, curricular innovation spurred initiatives that attempted to carve the campus into smaller communities. The Experimental College at the University of Wisconsin, developed in the early 19203 by Alexander Meiklejohn, was intended to assist students in actively exploring the values of democracy through an integrated curriculum that was designed to facilitate faculty—student interaction (Smith, 2001). Although the experiment was short-lived, it provided a template of integrating small 16 residential communities within research universities, a model that continues to be emulated today (Gabelnick, MacGregor, Matthews, & Smith, 1990; Matthews, Smith, MacGregor, & Gabelnick, 1997; Ryan, 1992, 1993). By the 19608, the rapid expansion of postsecondary institutions led many leaders at research universities to invest resources in creating smaller communities for undergraduates. Residential colleges gained momentum within a variety of different institutions, including Wayne State University, Wesleyan University, University of the Pacific, University of California San Diego, the University of Michigan, University of Massachusetts-Amherst, Michigan State University, and F ordham University, among others (Gaff, 1970). In addition, new institutions like University of California Santa Cruz and the Claremont Colleges were created that partitioned the larger campus into small enclaves, incorporating the residential college structure throughout the larger campus (Duke, 1991). Duke (1991) explored the historical evolution of residential colleges beginning with Harvard, Yale, and the University of Chicago and later colleges including the Claremont Colleges and University of California Santa Cruz. He argued that the English Oxbridge model, which provided a rationale and model for the development of residential colleges in the United States, was often used by administrators symbolically to gain support for these endeavors. However, the model was widely misunderstood by leaders who were advocating for reform, and as a consequence has resulted in difficulty maintaining the residential college structure at many institutions (Duke, 1991). The difficulty creating and maintaining residential colleges and other types of living-learning programs becomes evident when one examines the breadth of literature describing how to implement and sustain these communities (Gabelnick et al., 1990; Hart 17 & Smith, 1993; Hohenbary et al., 1993; Lenning & Ebbers, 1999; Macy, 1993; O’Hara, 2001; Shapiro & Levine, 1999; Smith, 1994). Common difficulties cited in the literature include recruiting faculty participation in the communities (Gabelnick, et al., 1990; Lenning & Ebbers, 1999), planning for sustained development of the communities (Lenning & Ebbers, 1999; Gabelnick et al., 1990), bridging the student affairs/academic affairs divide (Gabelnick, et al., 1990; Lenning & Ebbers, 1999; O’Hara, 2001), finding an administrative home for a community (Gabelnick, et al., 1990), and maintaining facilities conducive to community building (O’Hara, 2001). Despite these modern day challenges, recent empirical research details mostly positive outcomes associated with these communities on undergraduate students’ learning and engagement. Outcomes of Residential Colleges and Living-Learning Communities Interest in exploring the outcomes of residential colleges and living-learning communities has increased with the rise in the number of these initiatives on college and university campuses. Much of this existing research has focused on the outcomes of living-learning communities, which are often less comprehensive than residential colleges because they may only accommodate a student for one or two years, may offer less curricular integration across courses, or may focus solely on students’ extra- curricular experiences (Inkelas et al., 2004). Because living-learning communities are smaller in scope than residential colleges, positive gains found in these communities are also likely to be found in residential colleges; thus, it is important to include the empirical research on the outcomes of living-learning communities to see if it may elucidate the potential influence of these communities on students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. 18 One approach researchers have taken in examining the influence of living learning community environments on student outcomes is to group students from varying living-learning communities (typically on a single campus) together and compare them to peers in traditional residence halls (e.g., Pasque & Murphy, 2005; Pike, 1999; Pike, Schroeder, & Berry, 1997). Pike, Schroeder, and Berry (1997) examined the impact of one campus’s multiple living-learning communities on student persistence. They found that residing in living-learning communities had a significant direct positive effect on faculty-student interaction, social integration, and institutional commitment; however there was no direct or indirect effect of the living-learning community environments on students’ academic achievement, nor did living-learning community students’ reports of greater institutional commitment and social integration contribute significantly to their persistence. In light of Pike, et al.’s (1997) findings, Pasque and Murphy (2005) conducted a single institution study (at a different institution) examining whether participation in living-learning communities predicted academic achievement and intellectual engagement. They used linear regression analysis to ascertain whether there were differences in intellectual engagement and self reported GPA between living-learning community and traditional residence hall students, and found that living-learning community participation was a small, but significant predictor of both intellectual engagement and academic achievement. Several variables in the intellectual engagement factor of Pasque and Murphy’s (2005) study, including ‘eager to try new experiences’ and ‘enjoys courses that are intellectually challenging,’ allude to the possibility that living-learning community environments may promote students’ inclination to inquire, l9 however, because there are other variables included in the factor (for example, “discusses ideas or concepts outside of class” and “challenged by learning”) it is difficult to decipher the overall meaning of the findings for students’ inclination to inquire. In addition to exploring the role of living-learning communities in student persistence, Pike (1999) examined the differences in experiences and learning reported by students in living-learning communities versus their non-living-learning community peers, exploring Specifically whether the relationships between experiences and educational outcomes were the same for both groups. Using survey data from the College Student Experiences Questionnaire (CSEQ) coupled with institutional data, Pike developed ten scales, which measured student involvement in 1) the arts, 2) clubs and organizations, and 3) the residence halls; interaction with 4) faculty, and 5) peers; 6) topics of conversation; integration of 7) course information, 8) information in conversations; and gains in 9) general educatiOn and 10) intellectual development. Using a one-way analysis of variance, Pike found that students in living-learning communities had significantly higher levels of involvement, interaction, integration and gains than their traditional residence hall peers, with the effects of involvement and interaction being greater than those of integration and gains. Based upon these findings, Pike concluded that living-learning communities “exert a positive direct effect on day-to-day behavioral aspects of students’ college experiences and indirect effects on the integration of information and gains in student learning and intellectual development” (p. 280). Although these studies highlight some of the potential benefits of living-learning communities on student outcomes including greater faculty-student interaction, social integration, involvement, and institutional commitment, the inconsistency in findings 20 regarding the role of living-learning communities in promoting academic achievement (Pasque & Murphy, 2005; Pike et al., 1997) illuminates some of the methodological difficulties encountered when attempting to isolate the role of these environments in promoting student outcomes. One such difficulty occurs when different living-learning community environments on a single campus are grouped together without taking their varying organizational elements (such as quality of faculty/ student interaction, degree of challenge, and peer environments) into account, potentially mitigating or overstating the impact of the different environments. Another problem with using participant/non- participant designs to ascertain the influence of a living-learning community or residential college environment is the reality that most students self-select into these environments, and this motivation, if not examined and possibly accounted for, may confound the actual influence of these environments. A final difficulty with Pike, Schroeder, and Berry (1997) and Pike (1999) studies in particular is the fact that students were surveyed during the 10th week of their first semester on campus, which may have been an insufficient timeframe for the environment to affect student persistence, interactions, integration, and commitment. Recent literature on outcomes of living-learning communities and residential colleges has broadened to include multi-campus studies, including the National Survey of Living-Learning Programs (Inkelas et al., 2004) and others (e. g., Snider & Venable, 2000). In addition, researchers have delved deeper into examining differences among different communities within a Single institution (Inkelas & Weisman, 2003; Stassen, 2003). Furthermore, several recent studies have examined specific aspects of living- leaming community environments, including the nature of faculty-student interaction 21 (Cox & Orehovec, 2007), variation in alcohol consumption (Brower, Golde, & Allen, 2003) and student engagement (Zhao & Kuh, 2004). The National Survey of Living-Learning Programs, a multi-year, multi- institutional study fimded by the Association of College and University Housing Officers—Intemational (ACUHO-I) was initiated in 2002 to examine the impact of living- leaming programs on various student outcomes (Inkelas et al., 2004). Preliminary results from the project indicated that students in living-learning communities were more likely than non-living learning community peers to perceive a positive residence hall environment, have positive peer interactions, report a smooth transition to college, and persist. In addition, students in living-learning communities had greater gains in academic achievement and participation in civic engagement. On the other hand, there were no statistically significant differences between living-learning community participants and non-living learning community peers in appreciation of diversity, self- confidence, and cognitive development. Again, however, like Single institution studies that pool various communities and compare them to traditional residence halls, caution should be taken when interpreting the results of this multi-institution study because the tremendous variation in the structures of living-learning communities that participated in the study were not accounted for, and may result in the effectiveness of some communities being overstated and others understated. For example, of the 34 living-learning communities included in the study that were at research universities, 73% offered no courses for credit, 50% required no co-curricular activities, and 33% had no faculty involvement (Inkelas, et al., 2004). These variations in structure and offerings among living-learning communities 22 make it difficult to discern what aspects of the communities are making a difference for students, a point that will be discussed in more depth in chapter three. Variation in living-learning community outcomes becomes even more apparent by examining studies of different communities conducted on one campus. Inkelas and Weisman (2003) examined student outcomes among participant in three types of living- learning programs (honors, transition, and curricular) at one university in the Midwest. They sought to determine whether students from different living-learning communities were more involved in college activities and perceived their environments more positively. Specifically, they explored whether students in different environments exhibited more positive outcomes in their transition to college, their preferences for and enjoyment of challenging academic pursuits, and their interest and openness to learning new and different perspectives. The researchers also examined whether students in each living-learning community demonstrated greater gains in the domain of their particular community (i.e., honors, transition, curricular) than the other students did. They found that students in living-learning communities were significantly more involved in their residence hall environments and perceived them more positively than their non-living-learning community peers. There were variations in students’ perceptions of their environments based on their affiliation with a specific living-learning community. For example, students in the honors living-learning community were more likely to report enjoyment of academic pursuits than either of the other two communities or the control group. Likewise, students in the transition-focused community reported a smoother transition to campus than either of the other two living-learning communities and the traditional residence hall group. Students in the curriculum-based programs were more 23 similar to the traditional residence hall students than to the students in the other two communities, as they were less often engaged in out of class activities than students in the other two living learning communities. These findings illustrate that the mission and conteXt of the living-learning communities matter, as students in living-learning communities with a specific focus indicated satisfaction or gains in that particular domain. However, the study did not account for the variation within each community, rather it grouped the students from each community together and then compared across communities, ultimately disregarding individual variation. In summary, the existing literature on living-learning communities sheds light on some of the potential benefits students in these derive from participation, including greater interaction with faculty and peers, higher academic achievement, persistence, and overall satisfaction with their residence hall experience. However, less is known about how interactions between students and their environments may lead to these outcomes. Furthermore, no research exists that specifically examines whether living-learning communities are successful in promoting outcomes that are congruent with a liberal arts education, which many of them purport to emulate. The current study will begin to address this gap in the literature, but before doing so, it is important to understand the literature and research detailing the conditions that promote students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning in order to know what aspects of residential . college environments may foster them. 24 Conditions that Promote Students ’ Inclination to Inquire and Capacity for Lifelong Learning The inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning are different but related concepts. The first centers on students’ intrinsic tendency to engage in and enjoy thinking, whereas the second focuses on the skills necessary for a student to act upon their value for inquiry. Both are outcomes associated with a liberal arts education (Center of Inquiry in the Liberal Arts at Wabash College Website, n.d) in that they encourage students to build a foundation for continuous learning throughout their lifetime. This section situates them in existing theoretical and empirical literature. Examining Students ’ Inclination to Inquire Psychologists and higher education researchers continue to be interested in understanding and measuring the development of students’ inclination to inquire deeply into their life and academic pursuits (Biggs, 1993; Cacioppo & Petty, 1982; Cohen, Stotland, & Wolfe, 1955; Marton & Sttle, 1976, 1984). Drawing on the work of social psychologists Cohen, Stotland, and Wolfe (1955), Cacioppo and Petty (1982) “sought to identify differences among individuals in their tendency to engage in and enjoy thinking” (p. 116), which they labeled the ‘need for cognition.’ The researchers developed a measure of individuals’ need for cognition using a sample of faculty and factory workers from Iowa City. Subsequently, they tested the measure with different populations, examining the relationship between the need for cognition scale and cognitive style, test anxiety, intelligence, social desirability, dogma, and affective reactions to completing a cognitive task. They found that the need for cognition scale assessed one primary factor that was gender neutral, different from but related to cognitive style, related to 25 intelligence, unrelated to test anxiety and social desirability, and predictive of enjoyment of cognitive tasks (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982). Since the development of the original need for cognition scale, Cacioppo, Petty, and Kao (1984) developed and tested a Shortened version. In addition, Cacioppo, Petty, F einstein and Jarvis (1996) conducted a meta analysis of over 100 studies that used the need for cognition scale to examine individual differences. The researchers found that an individual’s need for cognition is somewhat stable in the short term, but not invariant because it can be developed or changed over time. Furthermore, one’s need for cognition is “derived from past experience, buttressed by accessible memories and behavioral histories, manifest in current experience, and influential in the acquisition or processing of information relevant to dilemmas or problems” (Cacioppo, et al., 1996). Because of the scale’s robust psychometric properties, and consideration of both dispositional and situational influences, it has been deemed as a “valuable tool for measuring the cognitive goals of a liberal arts education” (Brown & Rogers, 2005, 1] l), as it can be used to measure “the extent to which cognitive activities are desirable and important” to students (11 2)- Empirical Studies Related to Students ’ Inclination to Inquire Several higher education researchers have used the need for cognition scale specifically to examine students’ inclination to inquire. As part of their study examining the impact of liberal arts experiences on liberal arts outcomes, Seifert, et al., (2008) explored the extent that a liberal arts experience, (which they defined as “an institutional ethos that values student-student and student-faculty interaction within a supportive environment characterized by high expectations for developing the intellectual arts” [p. 26 108]) predicts liberal arts outcomes, including students’ ‘inclination to inquire and lifelong learning’ among others. Using a sample of 909 students from 4 institutional types (research university, regional institution, liberal arts college, and community college), they conducted ordinary least squares regression using a liberal arts experience variable as an independent measure of students’ experiences. They used liberal arts outcomes, including students’ inclination to inquire and lifelong learning as dependent variables. ’ The researchers used the aforementioned ‘need for cognition’ scale (alpha = .89) and the ‘positive attitudes toward literacy’ scale (alpha =.65), which measures students positive attitudes towards reading and writing, as measures of their inclination to inquire and lifelong learning. Among their findings relevant to the current study was that the liberal arts experience variable significantly changed the amount of explained variation in the inclination to inquire and lifelong learning, as the R2 change in need for cognition was .038, (s=.235) and the R2 change for positive attitude for literacy was .052 (p=.258). Also relevant to the current study was the finding that the liberal arts experience variable significantly changed the amount of explained variation in the liberal arts outcomes’ measures, a finding that will be discussed in more detail in the methods section. Although this study advances the notion that liberal arts experiences do indeed influence the development of students’ inclination to inquire, questions remain about whether these environments can be fostered intentionally at research universities and also what aspects of the environment are the most influential. Furthermore, the researchers acknowledged that their results may have been confounded by students’ pro-college tendencies toward liberal arts outcomes. In addition, the study did not differentiate between students’ 27 enjoyment of thinking, as measured through the need for cognition scale, and their capacity for lifelong learning, leaving questions about whether students possess the skills necessary to continue learning throughout their lifetime. Mayhew, Wolrriak, and Pascarella (2008) also used the need for cognition scale to examine how educational practices influence students’ development. Interestingly, they used the need for cognition scale as a proxy for ‘lifelong learning orientation,’ arguing that the scale provides a measure of students’ intrinsic cognitive motivation, which they deemed is a prerequisite for lifelong learning. They examined how curricular conditions and educational practices impacted the development of lifelong learning orientations in undergraduate students, specifically exploring how provisions of opportunities for reflection, active learning, and perspectiVe taking, influenced the students’ lifelong learning orientations in five different courses (including a philosophy, psychology, service-leaming, intergroup dialogue, and introduction to sociology course). Their longitudinal, comparative, research design used the short version Cacioppo and Petty’s (l 984) need for cognition scale to gauge changes in the need for cognition over the Course of a semester. The researchers found that the relationship between need for cognition and course enrollment was marginal, with enrollment in the Introduction to Sociology course having the most positive impact on students’ change in the need for cognition. Classroom Practices, on the other hand, significantly contributed to a change in the need for cOgnition, with negative interactions with diverse peers hampering growth in the need for Cognition most significantly, and positive interactions with diverse peers and instruction- based educational practices significantly promoting growth. 28 In summary, these studies illustrate the potential influence that environments and individual courses can have on students’ inclination to inquire deeply into their life and academic pursuits. However, more research is needed to determine how the interaction between students’ motivation and their environment influences their inclination to inquire. Furthermore, a finer empirical distinction should be made between students’ inclination to inquire and their capacity for lifelong learning, as evidenced by the literature, to which I now turn. Historical Overview of Lifelong Learning For the past half century, educatOrs, policy makers, and researchers have begun to focus more acutely on the concept of lifelong learning, examining its philosophical underpinnings and varying definitions (Dave, 1976; T uijnman & Bostrorrr, 2002), its practical application (OECD, 1996), and the roles of families, schools, and society in promoting it (Abukari, 2005; Chapman & Aspin, 1997; Cropley, 1978; International Labour Organization, 2000; Knapper & Cropley, 2000; McCombs, 1991; Smith & Spurling, 2001). Tuijnman and Bostrom (2002) traced the conceptual evolution of lifelong learning from its roots in the 196OS when it was called ‘lifelong education,’ through the 197 OS and 1980s when it was termed ‘recurrent education,’ to contemporary examinations, in which the label ‘lifelong learning’ emerged. The authors highlighted how the differing names and definitions of lifelong learning reflected societal trends, with ‘lifelong education’ reflecting the growing need and demand for firrther education, and ‘recurrent education’ focusing the scope of inquiry to the relationship between work and education, reflecting the economic difficulties of the 19805 (Tuijnman & Bostrom, 2002). 29 The contemporary notion of lifelong learning emerged in the 1990s, and is broader and more holistic, having been defined by the UNESCO Institute for Education (UTE) as “a process of individual learning and development across the life-span, from cradle to grave - from learning in early childhood to learning in retirement” (OECD, 1996). The UIE’s definition refers to education in formal settings (such as schools, universities, and adult education settings), and also informal settings (such as at home, at work, and in the community). Although educators, researchers, and policy makers have generally accepted this broader definition of lifelong learning, several have emphasized that the learning undertaken must be intended or planned (Smith & Spurling, 1999), rather than the “spontaneous, day-to-day learning of everyday life” (Knapper & Cropley, 2000, p. 11). Furthermore, these researchers have argued that lifelong learning has specific goals, and these goals are the reason that the learning is undertaken (Knapper & Cropley, 2000). In addition to examining varying definitions and notions of lifelong learning, researchers, educators, and policy makers have also explored the skills necessary for lifelong learning to occur. Cropley (1977) identified several specific skills of lifelong learners, including the ability to adapt previous learning to new tasks, to apply learning practically, to use media, to employ various learning strategies to different situations, and to think critically. More recent efforts at identifying skills that promote lifelong learning have revealed similar findings. Tuijnman, Kirsch, and Wagner (1997) argued that the skills necessary for lifelong learning include those dealing with abstract and symbolic information (both verbal and numerical), analytical skills, problem-solving skills, adaptation skills, and technical skills. Although their terminology is somewhat different, 30 many of the skills identified as necessary are similar the earlier ones posited by Cropley (1977). Researchers have also identified several psychological characteristics upon which lifelong learning is built, including awareness of the importance of lifelong learning, motivation to carry on the process of lifelong learning, belief in one’s ability to continue learning, and an ability to identify one’s own learning needs (Cropley, 1977; Knapper & Cropley, 2000; Tuijnman, Kirsch, & Wagner, 1997). Existing research has also explored the environmental conditions that promote lifelong learning. Chapman and Aspin (1997) examined the roles of schools, family, and the community in promoting lifelong learning. They emphasized that- schools should focus on providing a foundation for lifelong learning and cultivate students’ motivation for it. McCombs (1991) drew more specifically upon motivation and other psychological theory and argued that in order to promote lifelong learning, schools should infuse into the curriculum meaning and relevance, supportive climates that are conducive to cultivating personal relationships, a sense of control and personal choice in the learning process, and mechanisms for helping student to understand and work with their psychological tendencies. McCombs (1991) stressed the importance of examining lifelong learning not from a deficit perspective, where motivation for lifelong learning is a problem to be fixed, but to focus on the “inherent capabilities and higher levels of functioning within all persons that can be accessed by them if they are placed in supportive environments” (p. 122). Smith and Spurling (2001) echoed the perspective advanced by McCombs, arguing that the “general ethos of an institution is hugely important” to promoting lifelong leaming and underscoring the control that a 31 postsecondary institution has on aspects such as students’ peer networks, through the way it “structures groups of students, and the effort it makes to influence student culture toward mutual support and altruistic activity - by encouraging group learning rather than individualistic, competitive leaming” (p. 81). The existing philosophical and theoretical literature illustrates the evolution of lifelong learning to a broad and holistic concept rooted in formal educational settings as well as informal setting including the home, work, and community. The literature’s emphasis on the skills and psychological characteristics of lifelong learners, coupled with the conditions that promote lifelong learning provides a rationale for examining students’ inclination to inquire (a psychological characteristic) together with their capacity for . lifelong learning (a skill set), within the context of different residential college settings. More substantiation of the importance of studying as separate, yet conceptually related constructs is illustrated in recent empirical work. Recent Empirical Work on Lifelong Learning Perhaps as a result of the belief that postsecondary education increasingly plays a vital role in fostering the skills necessary for competing in the 21”1 century knowledge economy, there has been an increase in the number of empirical studies exploring different facets of lifelong learning, including what individual characteristics predict participation in lifelong learning (Gorard & Selwyn, 2005), how students’ capacity for lifelong learning has changed in the face of the 21”t century knowledge economy (Hayek & Kuh, 1998), and what environmental factors influence the development of varying lifelong learning orientations (Hayek & Kuh, 1999). 32 Gorard and Selwyn (2005) examined adults’ patterns of participation in lifelong learning in the United Kingdom. Using a survey designed to obtain adults’ varying forms of participation in lifelong learning and demographic information (including age, sex, occupation, nature of schooling, employment status, family background, and access to technology) the researchers sampled 1001 adults living in several different electoral wards throughout England and Wales. They found that many of the predictors of lifelong learning are categorized as “birth related,” including age (with younger adults participating more than older adults), educational level and occupation of parents, and geographic mobility (with those living further from their birthplace participating more). They also found that adults who undertook formal education in their adult years were also those who reported using intemet and computer technology. This research sheds light on some of the potential demographic characteristics that may influence one’s capacity for lifelong learning. However, questions remain as to whether the findings are relevant to students enrolled in postsecondary institutions in the United States. Perhaps more relevant to the current study because of their focus on college students, Hayek and Kuh (1998, 1999) conducted two studies exploring the capacity for lifelong learning in college seniors. The first study compared two cohorts of seniors, one from the 1980s (1984-1987) and the second fiom the 1990s (1994-1997), and examined the extent to which they developed the capacity for lifelong learning throughout college, whether the capacity for lifelong learning remained stable across the time periods even as the need for workers to participate in the knowledge economy increased, and which types of institutions better prepared students in developing the capacity for lifelong learning (Hayek & Kuh, 1998). Using data from 26,629 completers of the College Student 33 Experience Questionnaire (CSEQ) from doctoral granting universities, comprehensive colleges and universities, selective liberal arts colleges, and general liberal arts colleges, the researchers examined students’ scores on the “Capacity for Life-Long Learning Index, “a measure created by summing students’ responses to eleven Estimate of Gain items,” which included variables measuring the extent to which students believed they made progress in such areas as ‘general education,’ ‘synthesizing information,’ ‘analyzing quantitative problems,’ ‘writing,’ ‘getting along with others,’ and ‘learning on one’s own’ (Hayek & Kuh, 1999, p. 5) The researchers found that students’ capacity for lifelong learning remained stable or increased in the 19903 largely as a result of seniors’ self-reported gains in their ability to use computers. After the ‘computers’ measure was removed, students’ capacity for lifelong learning actually decreased from the 19803 to the 19903, with significant decreases occurring in seniors’ self-reported abilities in general education, analyzing quantitative problerrrs, getting along with others, understanding scientific and technological developments, synthesizing information, and leaming on their own. Despite these decreases, seniors reported significant increases in their ability to write and function as a team member. Hayek and Kuh also reported that students within Selective Liberal Arts institutions reported the highest capacity for lifelong learning in the 19803 and 19903, and with statistically significant increases occurring fi'om the 19803 to the 19903. The researchers acknowledged that their study failed to take into account students’ motivations, which may have influenced their gain in their capacity for lifelong learning. Furthermore, they argued that more research is needed in order to determine how learning communities and other interventions promote the acquisition of lifelong learning skills. 34 Hayek and Kuh’s second study (1999) examined how college activities and environmental factors influenced undergraduate seniors’ development of lifelong learning capacities. Using data from 17,541 CSEQ completers from 106 institutions (including doctoral granting, comprehensive, selective liberal arts, and general liberal arts) the researchers examined the areas that students with the greatest gains in the capacity for lifelong learning devoted their time and energy to, and the characteristics of students and institutions that are associated with above average gains in the skill areas comprising the capacity for lifelong learning. Using multiple regression and correlation analyses (with separate models run for gender, race, and socioeconomic status), coupled with calculating effect sizes, the researchers found that students’ major field resulted in medium to large effect sizes, with majors including engineering, physical sciences, and biological science scoring high, and arts, humanities, education, and foreign language scoring low. Furthermore, they found that certain clusters of activities and environmental factors influence students’ capacity for lifelong learning, including students’ overall satisfaction with college, the amount of effort they devote to classroom activities, the amount of effort they devote to science and technology, and an institutional environment that values critical, evaluative, and analytical performance. Surprisingly, the researchers found that students’ participation in formal extra-curricular activities and student-faculty interaction outside of class had low effect to no effect on students’ capacity for lifelong learning . These two studies are relevant to the currently study because they illuminate some environmental factors that influence students’ capacity for lifelong learning. Specifically, the finding that environments characterized as valuing critical, evaluative, and analytical 35 performance are most efi‘ective in promoting students’ capacity for lifelong learning provides credence to the notion that a residential college, with its emphasis on providing a small, liberal arts education with the resources of a research university, may be efl‘ective in promoting students’ capacity for lifelong learning. However, questions remainastowhetherresearchuniversifiescanbesuccessfidinemulafingthis small liberal arts atmosphere. Furthermore, more research is needed to understand the intersection of students and these environments, and specifically whether these outcomes are a result of the environment itself, or if they are successful in attracting a student already motivated toward developing the capacity for lifelong learning. In summary, the inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning are diflemnt but related concepts that convey students’ willingness and ability to engage in learning throughout their lifetime. The development of these capabilities is dependent on the interaction of individuals and their environments. More research is needed to understand how postsecondary institutions can promote conditions that encourage the development of students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, especially at large research universities, which face the dual challenge of size and scale with a large undergraduate student body and many (often competing) missions. Of additional importance is research that accounts for students’ self-selection into these environments, examining how their motivational attributes interact with their environments and whether these environments are effective in influencing students with varying motivation. Consequently, it is important to examine relevant motivation theory to get a sense of how students’ motivational attributes might influence students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. 36 Relevant Motivation Theory Among the shortcomings of existing literature examining the environmental influences affecting development of students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning (e. g. Hayek & Kuh, 1998; Mayhew et. al., 2008; Seifert, et al., 2008) is its failure to account for the varying motivations students possess in these environments. This oversight has resulted in enduring questions as to whether the positive outcomes of these contexts (which include selective liberal arts colleges, courses that promote active learning, etc.) are attributable to their design, or rather are a result of the type of student attractedtothe context. lnordertoclarifythe influenceofindividual studentsandtheir environments, it is important to take students’ motivation into account. The body of research on individual motivation is vast. Many motivational theorists conceptualize individual motivation as a product of expectancy and value reasoning within the broader social milieu (Brophy, 2004; Svinicki, 2004; Wigfield & Bccles, 2000). The expectancy component of the motivation equation explores students’ beliefs about their ability to perform a task and the rewards for doing so; whereas the value reasoning component explores students’ beliefs regarding the overall worth of the process and the reward (Brophy, 2004). The social milieu is the context in which the motivation occurs, which might be in a residential college, classroom, or work setting. Perhaps most relevant to the current study, self—determination theory (SDT) is a motivation theory incorporating aspects of both the value and expectancy portions of the equation and planting them squarely in an environmental context. With roots in the Aristotelian view of human development, which posited that people have an “active tendency toward psychological growth and integration,” SDT embraces the assumption 37 that people have innate, natural, and constructive tendencies to develop a unified and elaborated sense of self (Deci & Ryan, 2002, p. 3). Despite this integrative tendency, however, SDT also acknowledges “that there are clear and specifiable social-contextual factors that support this innate tendency, and that there are other specifiable factors that thwart or hinder this fundamental process of human nature” (p. 5). As a result, SDT predicts differing developmental outcomes based upon an individual’s perceptions of social-environmental conditions. Self-determination theory hypothesizes that among the social-environmental ‘ conditions affecting developmental outcomes are three basic or ftmdamental psychological needs that must be met in order for development to occur. These needs include autonomy, described as a sense of choice or control over one’s actions; competence, described as a positive feedback mechanism that signifies efficacy and improvement; and relatedness, described as “a secure relational base [which] provides a needed backdrop” for the growth of people’s personalities and cognitive structm‘es (Deci & Ryan, 2000, p. 235). A sense of well being results from a person’s basic needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness being met. Moreover, these basic needs serve as a fomdafion for supporting an individual’s internalimtion of motivation (Deci & Ryan, 2002). As a consequence, the more fulfilled students feel in having these needs met in a specific context, the more intrinsically motivated they will be. Self-determination theory is relevant to the cmrent study because it provides a mechanism for assessing students’ motivation within a residential college setting, which existing studies have failed to do. 38 Existing Studies T ala'ng an Ecological Approach The final aspect of literattu'c important to situating the current study in existing scholarship is an examination of studies that take an ecological approach to tmderstanding student development. As I discussed in the theoretical fi'amework, environmental considerations continue to play an ancillary role in much of the college student development literature, as many researchers continue to isolate outcome variables instead of acknowledging the role of environments and students interactions within their environments (Herzog, 2007; Kuh, 1995; Renn, 2004; Rem & Arnold, 2003). As illustrated bythe firstthree sections ofthe literaturereview, inordertoexaminetherole of residential college environments in promoting liberal arts outcomes like students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, it is essential to take students’ sociodemographic and motivation attributes into account in addition to their environmental contexts. Existing literatme approaching student development from an ecological perspective accounts for the confluence of factors that ultimately affect development. Early attempts to incorporate enviromnental considerations into student development typically centered on students’ interactions with their physical environments (e.g., Banning, 1989), or were focused on assessing the appropriate fit between students and a single environmental context like a residence hall or specific resource omce (e.g., Aulepp & Delworth, 1976; Schuh, 1990; Schuh & Veltman, 1991). Strange and Banning (2001) provided a comprehensive summary of the amalgamation of early environmental approaches to student development in their book, Educating by Design: Creating Campus Learning Environments that Work. Written to provide a framework for administrators and 39 student affairs professionals to build supportive learning communities, the book drew heavily from Moos’ theoretical perspective and examined campus environments, including the physical setting, demographic and psychosocial fit for students, organizatioml design, and social construction. Recent empirical literature has used Urie Bronfenbrenner’s human ecology model to examine the influence of peer and institutional culture in student development (c. g. Arnold, 1995; Guardia & Evans, 2008; Johnson, 2005; Renn 2003, 2004). Specifically, in the realm of race, ethnicity, and collegiate experiences, researchers have examined the roles of fratemity membership on ethnic identity development of Latino men (Guardia & Evans, 2008), the racial consciousness of White men attending a historically Black University (Peterson, 2006), and the experiences of low-income Latina women as they sought financial aid for college (V enegas, 2005). These studies found the Bronfenbrenner model useful in accounting for the myriad influences on college student development, which included familial and cultural norms in addition to institutional and personal factors. Drawing upon the studies each conducted earlier, Renn and Arnold (2003) advocated for researchers to consider taking an ecological perspective to understanding peer cultm'e, because While much is said about “peer pressure” and the influence of peer attitudes on a number of undesirable behaviors (binge drinking, sexual harassment and assault, incivility, cheating, etc.), there is surprisingly little recent research linking peer culture with college student behavior and outcomes. (p. 263) 40 The authors explained that the ecology model “takes into account the specificity of individual life history, the campus milieu, and the larger societal and historical context of development” and thus is “particularly well suited to understanding how individuals and their proximal environments interact to shape identities of individuals and of groups” (p. 273). Summary of Literature Review Existing literature on residential colleges, learning outcomes, motivation, and campus ecology illustrates both the potential of residential college environments in fostering students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning and the need for additional research on their effectiveness in doing so. With the number of residential colleges at large research universities increasing as administrators work to integrate and cohere undergraduate education, questions about the whether these communities promote the outcomes associated with the liberal arts education they try to emulate have barely been examined. Furthermore, existing approaches to researching outcomes of these communities continue to fall short because often researchers aggregate the data they collect to the environmental level, disregarding the individual differences of students in these environments and ignoring the varying motivations students bring to these environments. By following an ecological approach that better accounts for environmental contexts and motivational attributes, the current study examined whether residential colleges do indeed foster‘the development of students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. In chapter three, I explain the research design, detailingthemethodslusedtocarryoutthestudy. 41 CHAPTER THREE Study Design This chapter describes how I conducted the study. After reintroducing the research questions, I discuss my research design, sampling, and instrumentation. Then, I explain my analytic approach and detail how I addressed each of the research questions. I conclude by detailing the limitations of my data collection and analytic procedures. The goal of the study was to examine how students’ motivation and environmental contexts facilitate or impede students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning in residential college settings. Using data collected from over 1800 students in 24 residential colleges, I investigated the following questions: 1. Does students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning vary across residential college environments? 2. How are students’ sociodemographic characteristics and motivation related to their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning? 3. Do the associations between students’ sociodemographic attributes and motivation and their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning vary across residential colleges? 4. How is the environmental context of a residential college related to students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning? Specifically, a. Environmental Liberal Arts Experience variables - (good teaching and high qudity interaction with faculty, academic challenge and high expectations, diversity experiences, and peer interactions) b. Disciplinary Focus 42 5. Does the association between student-level motivation and students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong differ by residential college- level liberal arts experience variables (good teaching and high quality interaction with faculty, academic challenge and high expectations, diversity experiences, and peer interactions)? Research Design I adopted a quantitative, cross~sectional survey design to conduct the study. A quantitative design is appropriate when “examining the relationships between and among variables is central to answering questions” (Creswell, 2003, p. 153). The survey approach enabled me to identify students’ inclination to inquire and their capacity for lifelong learning in a numerical format (Creswell, 2003). The design was cross-sectional because data were gathered from students across different class years (Creswell, 2003). By using an ecological perspective for data collection and analysis, I examined the influence of both personal and environmental characteristics on students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. The data I collected were hierarchical in nature, with lower-level observations (i.e., students) nested within environments (i.e., residential college settings) (Kreft & De Leeuw, 1998). Because the study explored the influence of both individual (level one) and organizational (level two) characteristics on individual-level outcomes, I employed hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) to analyze the data I collected. Population, Sample, and Partich The population of students to whom the study was intended to generalize included all students in residential colleges located within large public research universities. 43 According to the ‘Basic Classification Description’ of the Carnegie Classification of Institutions of Higher Education, at the time of the study there were 136 public institutions classified as “Research Universities (very high or high research activity)” or “Doctoral Research Universities” that were also classified as “Large (including primarily nonresidential, primarily residential, and highly residential),” defined as those with Full- Time Equivalent enrollment of at least 10,000 degree-seeking students. By examining the websites of these 136 universities, and typing, ‘learning community,’ “living-learning community” and “residential college” separately into the search engine and then examining the websites to ascertain whether they met my definition of a residential college, I determined there were 32 degree-granting (major or minor) residential colleges located within 11 of the large, public research universities in my sampling fiame. Although all of the residential colleges in my sampling frame were degree granting and offered students a shared academic experience, variations remain in terms of their admission requirements and organizational structures (e.g., faculty-to—student ratio, how long students are required to live with the residential college, and whether the faculty had their tenure homes in the college). After identifying the 32 residential colleges appropriate for inclusion in the study, I contacted the deans of 31 of them and invited them to encourage their students to participate in the study. (Because I conducted a pilot of my survey at one residential college which had been established only 2 years prior, I excluded it from my sampling fiame.) Of the 31 residential colleges (located within 11 universities) contacted, 30 expressed initial interest in participating in the study. One university (with six residential colleges embedded in it) dropped out of the study before sending information out to students. In many ways, the university that dropped out of the study was similar to other universities in the sample, as it was a large, public research university. However, it was the only university in that region of the United States that ofi‘ered multiple residential colleges on a single campus, and therefore may have attracted a different type of student than the other residential colleges fi'om that region. I worked in partnership with deans and directors of the remaining 24 residential colleges at 10 universities to distribute information to students in a way that they deemed most appropriate. Most of the administrators with whom I partnered believed that the best way to distribute information about the survey was for a representative of the residential college to send the information about the study and a link to the survey out via email or listserv. Others preferred to send the information out in an on-line, weekly newsletter sent to students. Three institutions provided me with contact information for students and asked that I send information to them directly; of those three, two gave me contact information for all the students, and one selected students whom they described as ‘student leaders.’ The different modes of contacting students led to some variation in response rate, with the information being sent via newsletter leading to a smaller than average response rate, and the information and follow-up being sent by me leading to larger than average response rates. These alternative modes of contacting students did not lead to any anecdotal differences in the representation of the samples. As an incentive for completing the survey, I offered students the opportunity to receive one of five $100 gift cards to Amazon.com. The gift card information was in no way tied to students’ responses, as upon completion of the survey, a link took them to a 45 separate website where they provided their contact information for entry into the drawing. Administrators’ willingness to send follow—up reminders to students urging them to take the survey varied by residential college. Some sent one or two follow up emails to students, whereas others expressed concern that they not burden the email in-boxes of their students, or cause ‘survey fatigue,’ a word used by one administrator to describe the constant pleas they sent to students asking them to complete various institutional and national surveys. Also important to note because of its potential influence on survey response rate was that the IRB of one institution asked that the gift card incentive for participation not be used as it could be construed as a lottery and therefore was against state law and university policy. Given these constraints, I followed Umbach’s (2004) recommendations for improving the response rate of a web-based survey to the greatest extent possible, as I or an administrator sent an initial email inviting students to take the survey followed by several reminders, kept the survey relatively short, included a deadline for response to the survey and an estimated time to allot for completion, and ofl‘ered an incentive for completion. Information about the sample sizes and procedures is included in table 3.1. There were 2,202 students who responded to the survey with some information. Of these students, 391 did not complete at least 20% of the survey, did not indicate their affiliation to a particular residential college, or did not answer all the questions to at least one of the outcome variables and were subsequently excluded from the analysis. The final analytic sample was comprised of 1811 students affiliated with 24 residential colleges at 10 large, public research universities throughout the United States cousmsoe e83 a; E: a a 3:3: as, a as 8:853 seam emcee as: cogmga so \ a... 3 NS 3:2 2 E as Sensuous straw emcee $52 83338? as \ an a: E 838% 3 as 8:853 355 seem Ens: toefiasas as \ a» «S me 2:35 B as usages stem cease seem SEE negates .xa \ as a: on. «3:81 3 ans 85:85 3:5 emu—.8 omens assesses so \ as we 8 2982 3 as segues seam emcee uses: spa. 835583 a: 3.3 a am 8:02 3 use usages seam amaze Bananas Essa gasses an \ so :. 3 «ENE B as Suwanee 3:5 09:8 .520 easiest an \ so 5 a 8382 E as manages seam emcee €38 scanning; s: 3:: on 3 «RES 3 as nonsense seam amaze 3:an a 28 83538? as. \ a.” 8 a: $252 .3 as confines 53 a: 03:8 aegis“; ox sensuous as as \ an em 3 ES: 3 as gaseous seam 8:8538 a 09:8 assesses an \ so me 8 335: B as guesses spam 2:2 09:8 sausages .xa \ as 2 a m: 3232 3 as Senses 555 29m 09:8 canines. ease... as: 3.8 3. on @3an B as assumes spam sea—Ewes: ESE use—~58 mailer. anal—.8 E con—E:— in as: 38 £3238 :3 .3». cause: .59: 335:8 as. 8:85. E .32.. a as: .38. 2.2.5:: sz 8...»... assets... .3th _ «mo—.8 .6538.— aoeaaeea 85.3% as 038$ ”2 23. 47 5.88... .5 ass... as. u... a Bass swag. Base... 22.. a... as... Swans... 355... o: .23 689% 8 been 2: we flame 05 8883 damage? mum—9:8 o5 unaccep— 3 amazon. igua— aoeome can: in.— ofi 9.: 8 gee _ 3.553.. an \ so . :2 menu aamogm .25 woman—e MEX anemone $8 \ sum 2 a. amen :85 as sauce. a. 883:8 . Sac... 03:8 Ease .x... . tan. 8 S 23% =2... «.5 saves a. 303:8 . 352.3. esnwawhwocw s. w .38. m. e. .832 as 38.5. ass 38...”... .uses 55.31 s... \ a... m: .2 2.2.2.: 3 as aofiwaaM%m suzoo 822% .xa. \ an S. on. .2 Q8. m E as aofihuofimmaem 03.30% .x... :a. E am $58: .3 as aofihcommfinfi .330 2.33. :2 E... 8 ab. 8.33 .3 as 8%Nfi o: 2P so \ so 3 mm. 3.232 .3 as cogfifi owe—.8 .85.. so \ .x... 8 8. E25: .3 as éfifi 09:8 go ease 3 one. 48 The response rate for the final analytic sample was 5%. The analytic sample approximates the national sample of Hispanic or Latino students, American Indian or Alaskan Native students, Native Hawaiian students, and multiracial students attending large public research miversities (NCES, 2004). However, Black or Afiican American students are underrepresented by about 8%, White students are underrepresented by 10%, and Asian students are overrepresented by about 13% in comparison to national averages of students attending large public research institutions (NCES, 2004).In addition, women are overrepresented in the analytic sample by about 20%. I provide complete descriptive information, including mean, standard deviation, range of responses, and the number of responses for each particular question or scale in the analytic sample in table 3.2. Appendix B provides additional information on all the variables in the analysis not provided in table 3.2. Table 3.2: Descriptive Information of Analytic Sample RESIDENTIAL COLLEGE STUDENT CHARACTERISTICS (Level 1) VARIABLE NAME MEAN SD MIN. MAX N Sociodemographic variables Afiican-American 0.02 0.13 0 1 181 l Asian-American 0.21 0.41 0 1 181 1 Hispanic/Latino 0.07 0.26 0 1 181 1 White (reference group) 0.56 0.50 0 l 1811 International 0.02 0.14 0 l 1811 Native Am. or Alaskan Native 0 0.05 0 l 1811 Native Hawaiian or other Pac. Is. 0.01 0.08 0 1 1811 Multiracial 0.1 0.3 0 1 181 1 No response forrace 0.01 0.1 0 1 1811. Male 0.33 0.47 0 l 1809 Family Income 350-1 10]: (reference grow) 0.43 0.49 o 1 1740 Family income Below $50K 0.26 0.44 0 1 1740 Family Income Above $110K 0.31 0.46 0 1 1740 HS GPA 3.63 0.32 1.7 4 1749 Combined ACT Score 27.72 3.98 11 36 710 Combined SAT Score 1258.28 155.14 650 1600 1120 First generation 0.24 0.42 0 l 1713 49 Table 3.2 (cont’d). VARIABLE NAME MEAN SD MIN. MAX N College Experience and Motivation Variables Years lived in the Res. College 1.45 0.73 0 4 1774 Degree Asp. - Less than bach. 0.01 0.12 0 l 1802 Degree Asp. — bach. (reference group) 0.15 0.36 0 1 1802 Degree Asp-More than bach , 0.83 0.37 0 1802 Motivation 89.87 13.45 42 1 19 1737 Individual Liberal Arts Variables (centered and entered at level 1) Good Teaching and High Quality Interactions with faculty - Classroom Practices ' 42.42 6.42 11 55 1669 - Out of Class Interactions 17.03 4.03 5 25 1689 Acad. Challenge & Expectations 72.29 12.57 32 105 1531 Diversity Experiences 25.24 6.16 8 40 1606 Interaction with Peers 22.78 5.38 6 30 1628 Student Outcome Variables Capacity for Lifelong Learning 29.71 6.58 l l 44 1769 Inclination to Inquire 54.45 8.27 18 75 1737 RESIDENTIAL COLLEGE ENVIRONMENT CHARACTERISTICS (Level 2) Environmental Variables (group mean entered at level 2) Good Teaching & Quality Interactions with Faculty - Classroom Practices 42.45 1.70 40.16 46.93 24 - Out of Class Interactions 17.25 1.48 15.23 21.53 24 Acad. Challenge & Expectations 72.74 6.63 64.06 91.76 24 Diversity Experiences 25.29 2.00 21.53 31.18 24 Interactions with Peers 22.90 1.53 19.18 27.12 24 Discipline - Soc. Sci. 0.04 0.2 0 l 24 Discipline - Nat. Sci. 0.08 0.28 0 l 24 Discipline - Interdiscp. (reference grorp) 0.83 0.38 0 1 24 Discipline - Humanities 0.04 0.2 0 l 24 De Leeuw, Hox, and Dillman (2008) outlined four cornerstones of data collection, which include sampling (selecting a subset of the population), coverage (creating a sampling frame that is representative of the population), response (obtaining responses to 50 questions from all participants in a sample), and measurement (ensuring that the questionnaire measures what it intends to measure). The authors explained that three of the four cornerstones (including sampling, coverage, and response) rest on the notion that surveys collect data from a sample, or a fi'action of the population of interest. They stressed the importance of minimizing sampling errors by “sampling enough randomly selected units to achieve the precision that is needed,” coverage errors by ensuring that “every member of the population has a known and nonzero chance of being selected into the survey,” and nonresponse errors by ensuring that “everyone responds or if the respondents are just like nonrespondents in terms of the things we are trying to measure” (de Leeuw, Hox, and Dillman, 2008, p. 7). The primary data collection approach for the current study was census, as in most cases every member of the residential college was asked to participate in the study. This approach reduced the possibility of sampling and coverage errors. However, because not all deans and directors of residential colleges granted access to their students, there is still some coverage error, as 7 of the 32 residential colleges located in large, public, research universities in the United States are not represented in the sample. Furthermore, one dean limited the sample of students included to a handful of student leaders. Admittedly the largest source of data collection error in the current study is as a result of nonresponse. Because of the census approach taken, and the limited opportunities to send follow-up reminders encouraging students to complete the survey, response rates are low. However, the descriptive statistics of the sample indicate that there is a normal distribution of data. Furthermore, because the chance to win a gift card was used as an incentive to encourage participation, it is possible that some students who 51 optedtoparticipate inthe study didsotogainentry intothedrawing, while others wished to provide feedback about their residential college environment. Despite these facts, the possibility remains that there is bias in the sample. Certainly, selecting a random sample of students and diligently following up with them to ensure a high response rate would have been the most ideal data collection method. Practically, this method proved impossible because of privacy concerns. Several deans and directors of the residential colleges were unwilling or felt unable to share contact information of the students in their respective residential college. As a result, some caution should be used when generalizing the results of this study to the general population of residential college students at large research 1miversities. Data Collection Instruments, Variables, and Materials The survey instrument for the study was developed using a compilation of several existing surveys along with questions aimed at identifying demographic information of students in the sample. Survey items included were meant to operationalize the individual motivational attributes and environmental contexts that influence students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning (see Appendix A for complete survey). Independent variables. Predictor variables for the study included: (1) students’ pre-college characteristics (e. g., sociodemographic traits and academic preparation); (2) students’ motivation; (3) academic discipline; and (4) liberal arts experiences (see Appendix B for complete description of variables). Pre-college chmtm “es. I collected sociodemographic information to determine whether my sample was representative and also control for potentially conformding information. Specifically, I collected information on students’ sex, 52 race/ethnicity, parental education and family socioeconomic status; class year, high school GPA, pre-college test scores (ACT/ SAT), number of years living in the residential college, and degree aspirations. Mom 'on. Self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2000, 2002), which posits that when individuals satisfy their basic needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness they are more intrinsically motivated, served as a foundation for measuring students’ motivation. Specifically, I used the General Need Satisfaction scale (Gagne, 2003), which was adapted from a measure of needs satisfaction at work (Ilardi, Leone, Kasser, & Ryan, 1993), and measures the degree to which individuals feel that their choices and activities are selfidetermined (autonomy), the degree to which individuals feel a sense of eficacy in their activities (competence), and the degree to which individuals feel a sense of connectedness to others (relatedness). Gagné (2003), who used the scale to predict the extent to which satisfaction of basic needs will predict engagement in prosocial behavior with a sample of 121 college students, reported the following Cronbach alpha values for the subscales: autonomy (7 items) = .69; competence (6 items) = .71; and relatedness (8 items) = .86, and an overall alpha of .89. The correlations among the subscales were between .61 and .66 (Gagne, 2003). Questions on the General Need Satisfaction scale included item such as, “I generally feel free to express my ideas and opinions,” “Most days I feel a sense of accomplishment from what I do,” and “People in my life care about me.” To ensure the General Need Satisfaction scale measured specific constructs that were consistent with Self-Determination Theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2000, 2002) for my sample, 1 factor analyzed the variables associated with each subscale. I expected one 53 construct to emerge from the analysis and so used quartimax rotation in order to examine how highly the variables from the scale loaded on to that dominant factor. I began by conducting a principle components factor analysis of the 7-item Autonomy subscale of the General Need Satisfaction scale. I found that one factor explained 38.07% of the variance. However, two of the factor loadings on the rotated matrix were low (-.05 for “I feel pressured by life” [recoded] and .042 for “In my daily life, I generally do what I am told” [recoded]) and the reliability analysis indicated that the reliability would improve (from .706 to .721) or stay virtually the same (.706 to .697) with each of the items removed. I deleted these in turn and formd that for the S-item subscale, 50.72% of the variance was explained by the dominant factor. Factor loadings for the 5-item scale ranged from .818 to .541. Reliability of the 5-item scale was a = .75, compared to a = .70 for the 7-item scale. As a result of the improvement in construct validity, I opted to delete the two variables, and keep the 5-item scale. The scale’s constituent items are found in appendix C. I then turned my attention to the Competence subscale of the General Need Satisfaction scale. Again I conducted a principle components factor analysis with quartimax rotation of the 6-item scale. I found tint one factor explained 46.63% of the variance. However, two of the factor loadings on'the rotated matrix were low (.087 for “I have been able to learn interesting new skills recently,” and .129 for “People I know tell me I am good at what I do”). After deleting the first of the two low items, the dominant factor explained 49.69% of the variance and factor loadings improved, ranging from .832 to .536 with a reliability of the 5-item scale of a = .745 in comparison to a = .767 of the 54 6-item scale. Because of the improved factor loadings, I opted to use the S-item scale. The scale’s constituent items are found in appendix C. Finally, I examined the Relatedness subscale of the General Need Satisfaction scale, factor analyzing the 8-item scale using quartimax rotation. I found that one factor explained 50.45% of the variance, Two of the factor loadings on the rotated matrix were low (.294 for “I pretty much keep to myself and don’t have a lot of social contacts (recoded)” and .304 for “There are not many people I am close to”). After deleting the first of the two low items, the dominant factor explained 52.98% of the variance and factor loadings improved, ranging fi'om .800 to .569. The reliability of the 7-item scale was a = .828 in comparison to 0. == .842 for the 8-item scale. Because of the improved construct validity, I opted to use the 7-item scale. The scale’s constituent items are found in appendix C. Because I was most interested in ascertaining how different aspects ofthe environment influence students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, I used the overall motivation scale (which was comprised of the 3 subscales) in my analysis. The reliability of the overall scale was a =.884. In addition to individual independent variables (level one), I included several environmental variables (level two) as independent variables. Q'gipline. I included the disciplinary focus of the residential college in my analysis because past studies (Hayek & Kuh, 1999) indicate that it has a bearing on students’ capacity for lifelong learning, and specifically that students’ in science-oriented environments report a higher capacity for lifelong learning. I dummy coded variables representing the disciplinary focus of the residential college, including natural sciences, 55 social sciences, humanities, and other/interdisciplinary (reference). I chose to place this variable at level two of the analysis because I was interested in examining the influence of the cultural implications of an academic discipline within a residential college setting as opposed to the individual, psychological reasons that one might choose one discipline over another. Liberal m Experiences= On the survey, I included several subscales of a liberal arts experiences scale originally developed by Pascarella et al. (2005) and adopted by Seifert et al. (2008) and Blaich and Wise (2008) that was designed to measme the institutional practices and conditions that comprise the environment of effective liberal arts colleges. These scales were used in the model at levels one and two, with the level- one responses representing each individual students’ estimation of the liberal arts experiences, and level two representing the mean of all students in the residential college (so as to capture the overall ethos of the residential college). The liberal arts experience scale included aspects of the environment that are most closely aligned with the Center of Inquiry’s definitions of a liberal arts education (Blaich et al. 2004): positive and influential student-faculty contact; faculty interest in teaching and student development; instructional clarity, organization, and preparation; academic effort and challenge; degree to which the institution is supportive; positive influence of interactions and relationships with peers; integration of ideas through class activities and assignments; challenging classroom environment characterized by high expectations; instructor feedback to students; emphasis on higher-order examinations and assignments; frequency of engaging in cooperative learning activities; frequency of faculty contact; 56 frequency of student afl'airs contact; overall diversity experiences and interactions; academically meaningful out-of-class experiences; involvement with active learning; diversity courses; and out-of—class research with faculty member. (Seifert et al., 2008) Seifert et al. (2008) examined the extent to which the liberal arts experience variable predicted liberal arts outcomes, including moral reasoning, efl‘ective reasoning and problem solving, intercultrn‘al effectiveness, inclination to inquire and lifelong learning, well-being, and leadership. They found that for four of the six outcomes (representing 18 out of 20 separate measures), including intercultural effectiveness, inclination to inquire and lifelong learning, well-being, and leadership had a positive effect on the outcome variables. Although the liberal arts experiences variable failed to predict moral reasoning or effective reasoning and problem solving outcomes, for the purpose of the current study, the nonsignificant finding for these two dimensions of the liberal arts experience is irrelevant. In their longitudinal study of liberal arts education, Blaich and Wise (2008) reported that foursubscales of the liberal arts experiences scale were significant in predicting changes in students’ need for cognition at liberal arts colleges. The specific subscales they found to have a positive influence include, Good Teaching and High Quality Interactions with Faculty. (alpha = .92) A measure created by summing students’ answers to 23 questions about their experiences with “faculty interest in teaching and student development, prompt feedback, quality of nonclassroom interactions with faculty, [and] teaching clarity and organization” (Wabash National Study of Liberal Arts Education, 2008, p. l). 57 Academic Challenge and High Expectations. (alpha = .88) A measure created by summing students’ answers to 31 questions about their experiences with “academic challenge and effort, frequency of higher-order exams and assignments, challenging classes and high faculty expectations, [and]integrating ideas, information, and experiences” (Wabash Natiorml Study of Liberal Arts Education, 2008, p. 1). Diversity Experiences. (alpha = .80) A measure created by summing students’ answers to 9 questions about their “diversity experiences [and] meaningful discussions with diverse peers (Wabash National Study of Liberal Arts Education, 2008, p. 1). Interaction with Peers. (alpha == .85) A measure created by summing students’ answers to 9 questions about their extracurricular activities and the extent of positive interactions with peers (Blaich, personal communication, September 4, 2008). To ensure that the four subscales of the liberal arts experiences scale were strong and measured the constructs I intended them to measure, I factor analyzed (using principle components analysis) the variables associated with the scales and using these results as a guide, revised the subscales to increase construct validity. Because I expected one construct to emerge from each subscale I analyzed, I used quartimax rotation in each analysis in order to examine how highly the variables fiom the scale loaded on to a dominant factor. I began by examining the results of the factor analysis of the 23-item ‘Good Teaching and High Quality Interactions with Faculty’ scale from the Wabash National Study of Liberal Arts Education. Instead of finding one dominant factor, I found two factors explaining 40.19% and 11.63% of the variance respectively. In examining the rotated component matrix, I noted that three variables, including, “frequency that you 58 received prompt oral or written from faculty on your academic performance,” “frequency that faculty informed you of your performance in a timely manner,” and “frequency that faculty checked to see if you learned the material well before going on to new material” did not load well on either of the two factors. I deleted them and reran the analysis. Two dominant factors emerged again, explaining 42.26 % and 13.30 % of the variance. I noted that four variables loaded moderately well on both factors, including for example, “Most faculty with whom I have contact are willing to spend time outside of class to discuss issues of interest and importance to students,” “Most faculty with whom I have had contact are interested in helping students grow in more than just academic areas,” and “Most faculty with whom I have had contact are outstanding teachers.” I deleted these variables and reran the analysis. In the new model, which had 16 items, two factors explained 42.36% and 13.82% of the variance respectively. I examined the questions associated with the factors and noted that the first factor dealt with classroom practices, and the second dealt with out of class interactions with faculty. I conducted reliability analyses for each of the emergent factors and found them to be reliable (a=.91 l and .844 respectively). I present the new constructed independent variables related to teaching and their constituent items in table 3.3. Table 3.3: New Scales for Good Teaching and High Quality Interactions with Faculty Residential college students’ self-reports of ‘good Reliability Factor teaching and high quality interactions widrfaculty’ Loading Teaching - Classroom Practices 0 = .911 1. Frequency that faculty were well prepared for class .823 2. Frequency that the presentation of material was well organized. .783 3. Frequency that class time was used effectively. .781 4. Frequency that faculty had a good command of what they were teaching. .781 59 Table 3.3 (cont’d). Reliability Factor Loading 5. Frequency that faculty made good use of ‘ examples and illustrations to explain difficult points .741 6. Frequency that faculty gave clear explanations. .741 7. Frequency that course goals and requirements were clearly explained. .730 8. Frequency that faculty interpreted abstract ideas and theories clearly. .704 9. Frequency that faculty gave assignments that helped in learning the course material. .660 10. Frequency that faculty effectively reviewed and summarized the material. .655 l 1. Most faculty with whom I have had contact are genuinely interested in teaching. .535 Teaching - Out of Class Interactions with Students on = .844 1. My non-classroom interactions with faculty have had a positive influence on my intellectual growth and interest in ideas. .839 2. My non-classroom interactions with faculty have had a positive influence on my personal growth, values, and attitudes. .839 3. My non-classroom interactions with faculty have had a positive influence on my career goals and aspirations. .806 4. Since entering the residential college, I have developed a close, personal relationship with at least one faculty member. .712 5. I am satisfied with the opportunities to meet and interact informally with faculty. .640 I then turned my attention to the factor analysis results of the 30-item ‘Academic Challenge and High Expectations’ scale. As expected, one dominant factor emerged that explained 29.34% of the variance. However, upon examining the factor loadings on the rotated matrix, I noted that two factor loadings were particularly low, with “In a typical week, how many problem sets take you more than an hour to complete” loading at .045 and “During the current school year, about how often have you come to class about 60 completing readings or assignments (recoded)” loading at .015. The reliability analysis also revealed that reliability of the construct would improve with the items deleted (from .905 to .909 and .910) respectively. I deleted both and re-ran the analysis. Again one dominant factor explained most of the variance, but several factor loadings remained low and the reliability analysis indicated marginal improvement were items deleted. I deleted variables in turn until I was satisfied with the factor loadings of the variables and believed they were representative of ‘Academic Challenge and High Expectations.’ The final scale contained 21 items, with one dominant factor that marked students’ assessment of their experience as challenging explaining 36.91% of the variance. The factor loadings ranged from .718 to .387, with 20 of the 21 factor loadings above .4. Reliability ofthe 21-item scale was a = .912, in comparison to a = .950 for the 30—item scale. The scale’s constituent items are found in appendix D. Next, I factor analyzed the 9-item ‘Diversity Experiences scale.’ Again, I opted to use quartimax rotation because of my desire to ascertain how strongly the variables loaded onto one dominant factor. The loadings of the variable, “How often do you attend a debate or lecture on a cm'rent political/social issue?” were low (.177) and the reliability analysis indicated only a marginal change in reliability were the item deleted (from .841 to .838) so I deleted the variable and reran the analysis. The result was one dominant factor which explained 47.95% of the variance with factor loadings ranging from .824 to .302. Six of the eight items loaded on the factor above .4. Two factor loadings were low (.331 and .302 respectively), however, their associated variables dealt unequivocally with diversity experiences, and the reliability analysis indicated that the alpha-level would decline were the items deleted (fi'om .838 to .830 and .829 respectively). Because of these 61 reasons, I opted to retain them. The reliability of the scale was a = .838. The scale’s constituent items are found in appendix E. Finally, I factor analyzed the 9-item ‘Interactions with Peers’ scale. Again, I used quartimax rotation. The results of the analysis indicated that the variable “Most students in my residential college have values and attitudes different from my own (recoded)” did not load well on the dominant factor. Furthermore, the reliability analysis indicated that the reliability of the scale would improve substantially were the item not in the scale (fiom .821 to .836). I deleted this variable fi'om the analysis and reran the factor analysis. Two additional variables had low factor loadings, including “About how many hours a week do you spend participating in co-curricular activities?” (.242) and “Few of the other students I know would be willing to listen to me and help me ifI had a personal problem (recoded ” (.203). The reliability analysis indicated improvement in the scale if each item were deleted. I deleted these variables in turn. The resulting 6—item scale had one dominant factor accounting for 71.24% of the variance with factor loadings ranging from .911 to .623. Reliability of the 6-item scale was 0. == .910. The scale’s constituent items are fomd in appendix F. Dependent variables. The dependent variables of interest include students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. W. The inclination to inquire variable was operationalized using Cacioppo, Petty, and Kao’s (1984) short form of the Need for Cognition Scale, which was discussed in more depth in chapter two, and measures “an individual’s tendency to engage in and enjoy efi‘ortful cognitive endeavors” (p. 306). The short form contains 18 items, including items such as “I would prefer complex to simple problems,” 62 “I really enjoy a task that involves coming up with new solutions to problems,” and “I prefer my life to be filled with puzzles I must solve” (p. 307). Some of the tests administered to establish validity and reliability of the original longer form were discussed in chapter two. Other tests administered included a correlation analyses between individual items in the survey and the total score to measure internal consistency, and a factor analysis, which determined that one factor was dominant in the scale (Eigenvalue of 10.22, accounting for 30.1% of variance (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982)). Cacioppo, Petty, and Kao (1984) ranked the 34 items form the original scale in terms of the absolute value of their factor loadings in the experiment from which the original scale was developed. Then, they calculated Cronbach’s alpha as each successive item was addedandconductedaScreetesttodeterminethenumberofitemstoberetained. After they determined the shortened form would contain 18 items, they correlated students’ scores on the full scale with the shortened scale (r=+.95, p<.001). In addition, they performed one factor analysis of the students’ responses to the long form, and a second factor analysis, using the same data, but only including the 18 items on the short form. They found that the variance attributable to the factor in the short form was 37%, as opposed to 27% in the long form (Cacioppo, Petty, & Kao, 1984). Furthermore, the maximized Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was .90 for the 18-item scale, versus .91 for the longer scale. In order to ensure the construct validity of the Need for Cognition scale for the current study, I conducted a factor analysis with the items from the short-form used on the survey. I opted to use quartimax rotation because I believed that one construct would explain most of the variance and I wanted to see how highly the variables from the scale 63 loaded on to that dominant factor. I found that there was one dominant factor that explained 33.9% of the variance. However, upon examining the results of the rotated matrix, I found that three ofthe factor loadings were low (.153, .169, and .180). The questions associated with the low loadings included, “I feel relief rather than satisfaction after completing a task that required a lot of mental effort,” “I only think as hard as I have to,” and, “I prefer to think about small daily projects to long-term ones.” The reliability analysis indicated that the reliability of the scale would stay virtually the same with each of the three items deleted (changing from .879 to .878, .873, and .873 respectively). I deleted the items in turn and found that one dominant factor explained 36.8% of the variance. Factor loadings on the rotated matrix ranged from .688 to .277, with 11 of the 15 items loading above .4. The reliability ofthe 15-item scale was o=.s73, compared to a=..879 for the l8-item scale. I opted to retain the lS-item scale because of its stronger construct reliability and validity. The scale’s constituent items are found in appendix G. W The capacity for lifelong learning variable was operationalized using the Capacity for Life-Long learning index (Hayek & Kuh, 1999), which is a measure created by smnming students’ responses to 11 of the Estimate of Gains items on the College Student Experiences Questionnaire developed by Kuh, Vesper, Connolly, and Pace (1997). The “Estimate of Gain items represent the ability to “learn to learn” and interact effectively with others in a complex, information-based society, indicating the extent to which students have acquired continuous learning skills” (Hayek & Kuh, 1999, p. 4). According to Hayek and Kuh (1999) the index “is reliable (.84), with item-score correlations ranging fi'om .42 to .69 and item intercorrelations ranging from .22 to .56” (p. 4). The index includes items asking students to indicate the extent to which their college experiences have led to their progress in such areas as “writing clearly and effectively,” “understanding new scientific and technical developments,” and “ability to learn on your own, pursue ideas, and find information you need” (Hayek & Kuh, 1999, p. 29). (All items in the Capacity for Lifelong Learning index were used with permission from the CSEQ Assessment Program, Indiana University, Copyright 1998, The Trustees of Indiana University.) To ensure the construct validity and reliability of the Capacity for Lifelong Learning scale for the current study, I conducted a factor analysis with the items fi'om the scale. Again, I used quartimax rotation because of my expectation that there would be one dominant construct. I found that one construct explained 46.2% of the variance, and factor loadings on the rotated matrix ranged fiom .800 to .428. The reliability of the scale was (F .88. The scale’s constituent items are found in appendix H. Prior to administering the survey, I piloted it with two sets of individuals. The first set was comprised of colleagues with knowledge and expertise in postsecondary education and residential college environments. I asked them to provide me feedback about the clarity of the questions, ease of navigating the web-based survey, and any general observations they had. As a result of their feedback, I shortened several of the questions. The second set of individuals who took the pilot included students in a new residential college that was not included in the sampling flame of the study. Based on feedback from these students, I clarified several demographic questions, including parents’lfamily income, and how long students have resided in a residential college. 65 Data Analysis Procedures Many existing studies examining the impact of living-learning community or residential college environments on student outcomes approach these hierarchical environments in one of three ways. Often, they pool all students in various living- learning communities or residential colleges together, disaggregating differing environmental variables to the student level, and then comparing those students to students living in traditional residence halls. This pooled approach ignores between- group variation among the different living-learning community or residential college environments, resulting in the larger clusters dominating the coefficient estimates. Furthermore, if the dependence of various units within a cluster is not accounted for in the model, the variance will be smaller, which will result in smaller standard errors of parameter estimates and a greater chance that the estimates will be statistically significant (Krefi & De Leeuw, 1998). Another approach taken in existing research is to aggregate the observations within a living-learning community or residential college to the group level (level-2) and then compare them to other living-learning community or residential college environments. Although this approach preserves the variation between different living- learning community and residential college environments, it ignores within-group variation, ultimately disregarding such information (Krefi & De Leeuw, 1998). Furthermore, although this approach may illustrate that one environment is more effective than another for producing a certain outcome or students, it does not aid in explaining for what type of students the environment is more effective or why the environment is more effective (Kreft & De Leeuw, 1998). A third approach to examining the impact of various living-learning communities and residential colleges is to conduct multiple student level (level- 1) regression models, one for each environment within the study. Not only is this approach cumbersome when there are a large number of environments in the sample, it also multiples the likelihood of type I or type H errors because different models are fitted for each context, resulting in error structures not being specified properly (Kreft & De Leeuw, 1998). Furthermore, the results from the regressions are difficult to compare across different environments, as standard errors of the different models often vary considerably (Krefi & De Leeuw, l 998). Using Hierarchical Linear Modeling to analyze the data collected addressed the analytic challenges formd in many existing studies because it accormts for the existence of varying levels of data, allowing for level-two variables to explain between-group variance in the level-one intercept and for the exploration of cross-level interactions (Kreft & De Leeuw, 1998; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Hierarchical Linear Modeling was also appropriate given the nature of my conceptual framework, as it enabled me to examine how students’ sociodemographic characteristics and residential college environments were associated with their appraisal of the environment with regard to liberal arts experiences, their motivation, and the interaction of these elements in promoting or impeding their inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. I used SPSS 16 and HLM 6.02 software to assist with data analysis. I began my analysis by examining the descriptive statistics of the survey data, checking for missing data, assessing normality, examining outliers, and conducting bivariate correlations of the variables (presented in Appendices I and J). I also evaluated the reliability and construct 67 validity of the scales using confirmatory factor analysis, the results of which I discussed above. Then, I turned my attention to my research questions, which I used as a guide in constructing the multilevel model I used to analyze the data. Below I describe how I answered each research question. Does students ’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning vary across residential college environments? I began my analysis by creating models of each of the outcome variables with no predictor variables included in them. These one-way Analysis of Variance models with random effects, also often called the null models, allowed me to estimate the variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning at the individual level (level one) and across residential environments (level two), and to obtain baseline values of deviance, which I would subsequently use to assess model fit of subsequent models (Hox, 2002). Equation 3.1 displays the null model at level-one (the individual level) Yij =50} +rii (3.1) where Yy is the outcome variable (Inclination to Inquire or Capacity for Lifelong Learning) for individual i in residential college j, ,601- is the intercept for residential college j, and ry- is the level-one (individual) residual. Equation 3.2 displays the implied model at level-two (residential college level) [301° = 7’00 + “0 j (3.2) where intercept fig}- becomes the outcome variable, 700 is the grand mean efi‘ect (the fixed intercept at Level-2), and no} is the level-two residual. Equation 3.3 displays the combined level-1 and level-2 equation, 68 Y5 = 700 + “Oj + r5]- (3.3) where, again, Yij is the outcome variable (Inclination to Inquire or Capacity for Lifelong Learning) for individual i in residential college j, 700 is the grand mean effect (the fixed intercept at Level-2), and no} and "ij are the residuals at the residential college level and the individual level respectively. Partitioning the residuals into within-residential college and between-residential college enabled me to ascertain information about the degree of variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning within and across different residential college environments. I did so by determining the intra-class correlation, which is calculated by dividing the variance in level-two (between- residential college) residuals by the total variance of level-one and level-two. In short, the null model allowed me to determine that the means of students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning were statistically different across residential college environments. Furthermore, it provided me with a baseline from which to gauge the reduction in variance in subsequent models. How are students ’ sociodemographic characteristics and motivation related to their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning? In the second step of the modeling process, I created the within-residential college (also called the individual or level—one) models. I entered four blocks of variables into each model at level-one. The first block was comprised of dummy—coded race/ethnicity variables and sex. Because of insufficient numbers, for the analysis I combined the categories ‘American Indian or Alaskan Native’ students and ‘Native Hawaiian’ students into a category, ‘other.’ By doing so, I did not mean to imply that these two groups of students necessarily have similar experiences in college. Rather, because the low 69 numbers in both Categories limit any meaningful assertion, I opted to combine them in favor of parsimony of the statistical models. The second block was comprised of sociodemographic and pre-college characteristics (parents’ income and education, and students’ high school grade point average). I opted to exclude the ACT score variable fiem the analysis because it would have severely limited the sample size. I excluded the SAT scores variable because students’ responses to the survey indicated confusion regarding the question as a result of scoring changes initiated in 2005 and as a result I was not confident about the validity of responses. The third block was comprised of students’ college experience and motivation variables (including how many years they lived in the residential college, their motivation, and their degree aspirations). The fourth block of variables was comprised of _ the group mean centered liberal arts experience variables. I opted to enter these at level- one of the equation to capture students’ individual views regarding their liberal arts experiences. I centered them to reduce multicollinearity. Also, since each residential college’s mean of the liberal arts experience variables will be entered at level-2 of the model, it was important to account for individual students’ views so as to better interpret the environmental context. Equation 3.4 shows the equation with all four blocks of variables entered. 70 ,st (Intly- ) + [36}- (MulttRacij ) + [37 j (RaceOthg ) + ,st (NoRespRaceyo ) + ,69 1 (F lrstGeni j ) + ,6] 0 . (IncLowij ) + £1 1 j (IncHtghij ) + ,612] (HS'GPAiJ- - HSGPA» ) + ,613j (Motivationij — Motivations. ) + 3 4 ,614 J. (YrsLivedij ) + ,6151-(DgASpNOBaChg-)+ s16]. (DgAspMoBadrij ) + ( - ) ,617j (T eachClassij - T eachClass. ,- ) + [31 81- (TeachOutij - TeachOutoj ) + ,61 91- (Ac.Challenge,-j — Ac.Challenge, j ) + ,6201-(Diversityij - Diversity. j ) + ,6211- (Peersij — Peers. j ) + rij In equation 3.4, Y3]- was the outcome variable (students’ Inclination to Inquire or Capacity for Lifelong Learning depending on the model), the i subscripts indicate individuals whereas the j subscripts indicate residential colleges. The symbol - indicates mean of a context, so when a replaces the i, it indicates that it is the mean of individuals in group j, whereas when 0 replaces both i and j, it indicates the grand mean across residential college. In this model, I allowed only the intercept to vary across residential college environments. As is indicated in equation 3.4, I grand-mean centered the high school GPA variable and the motivation variable in order to make the intercept meaningful, as prior to transformation, a zero value for these variables is outside the range of probable responses. When grand-mean centered, however, the intercept can be interpreted as the adjusted estimate of the outcome for an “average” student — those who have mean characteristics (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). I also group-mean centered the liberal arts experience variables (Good Teaching & Quality Faculty Interaction - Classroom Practices; Good Teaching & Quality Faculty Interaction - Out-of-Class Interactions; Academic Challenge and High Expectations; Diversity Experiences; and Interactions with Peers). I chose to center these variables around the group mean in order to make the intercept meaningful and reduce the multicollinearity of the variables 71 (Raudenbush & Byrk, 2002). The individual-level coefficients were “fixed,” meaning that their slopes (or efi‘ects) were constrained to be the same across all residential colleges (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Does the association between students ’ sociodemographic attributes, pre-college characteristics, and motivation and their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning vary across residential colleges? In the next step of the modeling process, I examined each of the level-one predictors in turn at level-two of each model to see if there was any variation in the variable’s association with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning across residential colleges. I was curious to examine whether the association between the predictor variables and response variables remained constant across residential colleges (called a fixed effect) or whether the association changed significantly depending on the residential college context (called a random effect) (Raudenbush & Byrk, 2001). In order to determine whether there was variation in the association of each level-one variables and students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, I modeled the variance of each level-one variable in turn at level two and examined the change in deviance and the chi-square value to determine whether the change was significant. After determining which slopes varied significantly, I modeled the full within-residential college models. Then, using the deviance test as a guide, I deleted variables that were non-significant throughout the modeling process in order to obtain the most parsimonious models that explained the most variance. 72 How is the environmental context of a residential college related to students ’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning? (Specifically, Liberal Arts Experience Environmental Variables and Disciplinary Focus) With the level one model complete, I turned my attention to the group-level (i.e., the level-2 or residential college) model. I was interested in determining which of the group-level variables explained the between-residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Starting with the outcome inclination to inquire, I modeled the level-one intercept with two difi'erent sets of contextual variables, one with each residential college’s mean of the liberal arts experience variables entered in turn, and one with the disciplinary focus of the residential colleges. I then did the same for Capacity for Lifelong learning, modeling the intercept of the level-one model with the two sets of outcomes. Equations 3.5 and 3.6 show the level- two models, which I added separately to the previous equations. Lib. Arts Em .- floj = 700 + 701(GrpMnExTCIassj) + 702 (GIpMnExT Out I) + (3 5) 703 (GrpMnExChj) + 704 (GrpMnExDj) + 705 (GrpMnExPj) + qu . Discp. Focus : [30]- = 700 + 701(DiscSocScij) + 702 (DiscNatScij) + 703 (DisHumj) + 1401- (3'6) These models were random intercept models, since I allowed the intercept to vary relative to the contextual characteristics of the residential colleges. Specifically, intercept 700 was a function of the grand mean across all residential colleges on the outcome variable (inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning) as well as the group-level liberal arts experience and disciplinary focus variables plus the random error for a specific residential college. Any significant reduction in the between-residential college variance was due to the explanatory power of the group-level variables (Hox, 2002). 73 Does the association between student-level motivation and students ’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong dtfiizr by residential college-level liberal arts experience variables (good teaching and high quality interaction with faculty, academic challenge and high expectations, diversity experiences, and peer interactions)? In the final step of the modeling process, I examined the cross-level interactions between level-one and level-two variables. I was most interested in the roles of individual motivation attributes and residential college environments in promoting students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Therefore, I focused my attention on exploring whether the association between environmental liberal arts experience variables (level-two) and individual motivation (level-one) had any relation to students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. Having already modeled the level-one intercept of each outcome variable with the level-two liberal arts environment variables (which I described in Research Question Four) and obtained a baseline deviance score, I added each group-level liberal arts experience in turn to the slopes of two different level-one variables, starting with ‘Degree Aspirations — More Than Bachelor’s’ and then focusing on ‘Motivation.’ In order to determine whether the cross-level association between each individual-level variable and liberal arts environment variable was significant, I examined the change in deviance and chi-square statistic. Equations 3.7 and 3.8 show cross-level models, which I added separately to the equation with all environmental liberal arts variables included. Although the group-level variables are all included in the equations for simplicity of the presentation, I added them one at a time in order to ascertain whether the association of each one with the motivation variables was related to student’s inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. 74 Deg. Asp - MoBach : [39] = 790 + 791(GrpMnExTClassj) + 792 (thMnErTOutj) + 793 (GrpMnExChj) + 794 (GrpMnExDj) + (3.7) 795 (GmMnExPj )[+u9j] Motivation: [9131- = 7130 + 7131(GrpMnExTClassj) + 7132 (GrpMnExTOutj) + 7133 (GmMnExChj) + 7134(GmMnExDj) + 7135 (GrpMnExPj) (3-3) The ‘Degree Aspirations-More than Bachelor’s’ slope was random for ‘Inclination to Inquire’ and fixed for ‘Capacity for Lifelong Learning.’ The decision to set one slope as random and one as fixed was based on my discovery that the association between high degree aspirations and students’ inclination to inquire varied across residential college context (see question three). For the ‘Motivation’ slope, both s10pes were fixed. The examination of the cross-level interactions answers my fiml research question. Limitations Although careful stepswere takento ensurethatthedatalcollected and subsequently analyzed were reflective of students and their experiences in residential colleges, there are several limitations that are important to note. The first limitation relates to my data collection procedures, and specifically the likelihood of coverage error, sampling error and non-response error that occurred because of the way in which I collected my data. I discussed these limitations in detail earlier in the chapter, but raise them again to acknowledge that they may have implications for the external generalizability of the findings. The second limitation relates to my use of self-report data. Several researchers have raised questions about the validity of self-report data (Anaya, 1999; Gonyea, 2005; Pace, 1985; Pike, 1995, 1996). As a result, I have attempted to follow guidelines that increase the validity of the data, including: 1) ensuring that the survey questions address questions that respondents would know, 2) phrasing questions clearly and 75 lmambiguously, 3) referring to recent activities, 4) asking questions that merit a serious response by the respondents, and 5) ensuring that questions do not embarrass or threaten the respondents (Bradburn & Sudman, 1988; Converse & Presser, 1989; Gonyea, 2005; Pace, 1985; Pike, 1995). By following these guidelines, I sought to mitigate the concerns related to self-report data. In this chapter I outlined my research design, analytic approach, and limitations. In chapter4,ldetailtheresults ofthe study, answering eachresearchquestioninturnand providing a full description of the relationship between the predictor variables with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. In chapter 5, I discuss the results of the study in light of existing literature and discuss implications for theory, research, practice, and policy. 76 CHAPTER F OUR Results This chapter details the results of examining the association between students’ motivation and sociodemographic attributes, their residential college environments, and their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. I discuss the findings in the order of the research questions posed in chapter three and in the sequential order in which I added blocks of variables to the prediction models. I provide a full description of their association with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Researc 'on e' Doesstudents’ inc ' 'on ' ' or i for ' el 11mm vary across midgtifl college en_v_r_r2' nments? For this research questiOn, I was interested in the extent to which students’ afliliation with a particular residential college environment was associated with their inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. In other words, how much of the variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning was attributable to differences in their residential college environment. In order to determine the association between students’ inclination to inquire, capacity for lifelong learning, and their residential college environment, I conducted a one-way AN OVA with random efi‘ects (also called a null model) to partition the variance in the outcome variables (inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning) into between-residential college and within-residential college components. I found that a significant amount of the variance in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning was attributable to their residential college environment. Having found that the partitioning of variance to within-environment and between-environment components was significant, I calculated the intra—class correlation 77 (ICC), or the proportion of the variance in each outcome that was explained by the grouping structure (which in this case was the particular residential college environment) (Hox, 2002). I determined the ICC by dividing the between-environment variance by the total variance for each of the outcome variables. I found that the proportion of the total variance that existed between residential college environments was 8.8% for ‘inclination to inquire’ and 5.3% for ‘capacity for lifelong learning.’ I detail the partitioning of variance in the null model in Table 4.1. Table 4.1 .: Null Models: Partitioning Variance in Inclination to Inquire and Capacity for Lifelong Learning Capacity Inclination for Lifelong to Inquire Learning Null models Variance components - Between-res. college environments (intercept 6.097 2.322 Within-res. college environments 63.420 41.449 Reliabilities 0.829 0.752 ICC 0.088 0.053 The between-residential college variance in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning may seem inconsequential. However, as Seifert (2006) and Porter and Swing (1996) argued, higher education survey research often only explains about 30% of the total variance in a given outcome. Furthermore, since in studies of college immct the majority of variation in outcome is expected at the individual level (Pascarella & Terenzini, 2001 , 2005), even a small amount of variation at the environmental level merits exploration, as it may provide insight as to what aspects of the environment are influential to a given outcome. Also, since residential college environments are often resource rich and assumed to be more similar than different (Ryan, 1993; Smith, 1994), with missions, resources, and organizational structures 78 designed to promote curricular and co-curricular integration, the environmental variation is particularly interesting. Determining environmental influences on students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning will most certainly have implications for policy and practice. W Two How are WW For this research question, I was interested in examining the extent to which individual sociodemographic and pre—college characteristics, college experience, motivation, and aspirations, and students’ perceptions of liberal arts experiences were associated with their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning within residential colleges. Having decomposed the total variance into between-residential college variance and within-residential college variance, for this question I focused on the within-residential college variance, or how different individual attributes and experiences (as opposed to environmental attributes) were associated with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. With regard to my conceptual fi-amework, which was Moos’s social-ecological fi'amework (see Chapter 1), the within-residential college (level-one) model encapsulates the personal system (sociodemographic variables), students’ cognitive appraisal (individual liberal arts experience variables), and their activation or arousal (motivation, degree aspiration and college experiences) on their efforts at adaptation and coping (outcome variables). The stability or change is cross-sectional as it is measured by how the number of years lived in the residential college is associated with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. 79 Using HLM 6.02, I regressed the inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning outcomes on four blocks of variables to explore what relationship, if any, these student attributes had on their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. In the first model, I regressed the outcome variables on sex and race/ethnicity variables. In the second model, I added pre-college characteristics including parent/family education, income, and high school grade point average. In the third model, I added college experience, motivation, and aspiration variables including the number of years students lived within the residential college, their motivation as determined by the survey of basic needs, and their degree aspirations. In the fourth model, I added the group-mean centered individual liberal-arts experience variables (good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty (classroom practices and out-of class interactions with faculty), academic challenge and high expectations, diversity experiences, and high quality interactions with peers). These variables reflect the difference between students’ scores on the variables and the mean of all the students in the sample fiom the same residential college. In this section, I discuss the effects of each of these blocks of variables on students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Table 4.2 presents all the coefficients of the complete within-residential college models. Appendices K and L display the step by step modeling process for both inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning respectively, starting with the first within-residential college (level-l) model through the final between-residential college (level-2) model which includes the environmental characteristics. Also in the appendices are the within- and between- residential college variance explained calculations to which I refer throughout the results section. The within- and between- residential college variance explained calculations are 80 determined at level level-one by subtracting the within- residential college variance of a particular model fi'om the baseline within-residential college variance estimated in the null model and then dividing by the within-residential college variance in the null model 2 __ 2 (e. g. R2 = (0" ’b 2 a, / "0) where 032.”, is the level-one variance estimate fiom the null ar/b model (baseline) and a}, m is the level-one variance estimate fi'om the model in which the variance is explained (Hox, 2002). At level two, the between-residential college calculation is the same, except that level-two variance components are used in each of the calculations (Hox, 2002). Table 4.2 Association between Students’ Sociodemographic Characteristics, Motivation, and College Experiences and their Inclination to Inquire and Capacity for Lifelong I . g . Capacity for Inclination Lifelong to Inquire Learning Coefficient sig. Coefficient sig. Intercept 51.648 *" 26.050 “" Sociodemographic variables White (reference group) African-American -0.786 n. s. 0.048 n. s. Asian-American -3.280 "* 0.141 n. s. Hispanic/Latino -l.530 n. 8. 1.355 * International 4.528 " 1.543 n. s. Multiracial -0.457 n. 3. -0.770 n. s. Race-Other 3.632 n. s. 4.291 * No response 4.200 n. s. -l.074 n. 3. Male 1.629 ” 0.327 n. s. First-generation student -0.270 n. 3. 0.268 n. s. Family Income 850-110): (reference group) Family Income Below $50K -0.462 n. 5. 0.278 n. s. Family Income Above $110K 0.320 n. 8. 0.169 n. 3. HS GPA -0.176 n. s. . 4.059 A 81 Table 4.2 (cont’d) College Aspiration variables Motivation 0.138 "‘ 0.126 *" Number of Yrs Lived in RC 0.749 * 1.426 "* Degree Aspirations — - Bachelor's (reference group) - Less than Bachelor’s 0.193 n. 3. —0.052 n. s. - More than Bachelor's 2.748 *“ 1.844 *" Liberal Arts Experiences Teaching & Quality Interactions w/ Faculty Classroom Practices 0.184 “W 0.049 n. s. -0ut-of-Class Interactions 0.1 12 " 0.171 ** Acad. Challenge & Expectations 0.041 " 0.096 W" Diversity Experiences 0.137 " 0.077 " Quality Interactions w/Peers -0.175 “ 0.004 n. s. Variance Components Between-Residential Colleges (intercept) 3.015 ~"* 1.579 "* Between-Res. Colleges explained 0.506 0.320 Within-Residential Colleges 54.995 30.224 Within-Residential Colleges explained 0.133 0.271 Reliabilities Intercept 0.663 0.659 Deviance (FML) 7713.752 7201.752 "p<.l; *p<.05; "p< .01; m p<.001 Sociodemographic Variables The first block of sociodemographic variables, race/ethnicity and sex, explain little within-residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning (5.1% and 0.3% respectively). As noted in Table 4.2, for some students, race/ethnicity and sex had significant association with their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, even after family economic and educational circumstances, educational aspirations and motivation, and liberal arts experiences were 82 accounted for. Specifically, Asian-American students living in residential colleges reported a significantly lesser inclination to inquire than their White peers. Even after controlling for first-generation status, income, and high school grade point average, Asian-American students reported scores in the inclination to inquire scale that were 3.28 points lower tlmn their White counterparts. Despite their lesser inclination to inquire, there was no difference in Asian-American students’ capacity for lifelong learning. So, although Asian-American students may have reported a slightly lower proclivity toward or value for learning, their capacity for lifelong learning was not statistically different from that of their White peers. International students, on the other hand, reported a significantly greater inclination to inquire than their White (domestic) peers, reporting scores that were on average four points higher. However, International students’ capacity for lifelong learning was not statistically difi‘erent fiom White students.’ In terms of sex, male students also reported a significantly greater inclimtion to inquire than their female peers in residential colleges, reporting scores that were 1.6 points higher. However, like international students, male students did not report a statistically different capacity for lifelong learning than their female peers. So whereas male students’ value for or proclivity toward learning may have been slightly greater, their capacity for lifelong learning was not statistically different fiom their female peers. Hispanic/Latino students and students in the combined Native American and Alaskan Native ‘other’ category reported a significantly greater capacity for lifelong learning than their White counterparts, even after controlling for confounding influences and experiences. However, despite their greater capacity for lifelong learning, these 83 students’ inclination to inquire was not significantly different from their peers after controlling for confounding influences. Interestingly, although the sociodemographic variables explained little of the within-residential college variance, these variables account for 42.2 % of the between- residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire and 4.2% of the between- residential college variation in students’ capacity for lifelong learning. This finding suggests that the effect of race/ethnicity and sex that I detailed above may be more pronounced in certain residential colleges than others, with residential colleges that have a higher concentration of Asian-American students, Hispanic/Latino students, international students, or female or male students having greater variation fiom those with fewer students’ of color or an equal gender distribution. Pre—college Characteristics In addition to race/ethnicity and sex, the second block of variables on which I regressed the inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning variables dealt with students’ other sociodemographic characteristics and pre-college experiences. As noted in Table 4.2, these variables (which included parents’ education, family income, and high school grade point average) had very little association with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. The addition of these variables only explained .2% more of the variation in students’ inclination to inquire and 1.9% more of the variation in students’ capacity for lifelong learning. The only variable in this block that had a marginally statistically significant association with either of the two outcome variables was high school grade point average, which after controlling for confounding influences, was negatively associated with students’ capacity for lifelong learning. In the 84 model, this variable was grand-mean centered, and thus the interpretation of the finding is that for every point above the grand mean a students’ grade point average was, there was a decrease of -1 .059 in their capacity for lifelong learning score. Motivation, College Experiences, and Aspirations Whereas the sociodemographic and pre-college blocks of variables explained only 5.3% of the within—residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire and 2.2% of the within-residential college variation in students’ capacity for lifelong learning, the addition of the third block of variables bolstered the explained within-residential college variance to 12.5% and 20.7% respectively. Specifically, the motivation variable had a statistically significant positive association with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. For every point increase in students’ self-reported score on the basic needs survey (measuring students’ satisfaction in their sense of autonomy, competence, and relatedness), there was a small, but significant increase in their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Furthermore, students’ aspirations to obtain more than a bachelor’s degree were also positively associated with a greater inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, even after controlling for potentially confounding influences. Students who aspired to obtain more than a bachelor’s degree reported on average a 1.8 point greater inclination to inquire on the index and a 2.7 point greater capacity for lifelong learning. On the other hand, students’ aspirations to obtain less than a bachelor’s degree had no statistically significant association with their inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. However, because there were only 26 students in the sample who aspired to obtain less than a bachelor’s degree, caution should be used when interpreting this result, as it is possible 85 that if more students with low degree aspirations were included in the sample, a statistically significant association between the variable and students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning may have been found. The number of years students resided in the residential college environment also had a significant positive association with their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, as for every year increase in their residency was an associated increase of .75 points in their inclination to inquire and 1.4 points in their capacity for lifelong learning. What remains unclear, however, is whether the positive association is due to students’ interaction with their residential college environment over time, or rather is as a result of their maturation. Also noteworthy was how much variation the college experience, degree aspiration, and motivation variables explained across residential colleges. The addition of these variables to the model increased the between-residential college variation explained in students’ inclination to inquire by almost 26%, raising it to 67%. For the capacity for lifelong learning outcome variable, the addition of the college experience, degree aspiration, and motivation variables increased the between-residential college variance explained fiom virtually nothing to 46%. These findings suggest that the effect of college experiences, degree aspirations, and motivation that I detailed above may again be more pronounced in certain residential colleges than others. Liberal Arts Experience Variables The final block entered into the within-residential college model was comprised of the group-mean centered liberal arts experience variables. The entry of these variables at level one served two purposes; it enabled me to ascertain the association between 86 students’ individual experiences with their variables and their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, which I could then compare to the environment (level-two) results. In addition, the addition of these variables at level—one served to control for students’ individual beliefs about the environment, allowing me to hone in on the potential environmental impact. In reference back to my conceptual fiamework, these variables captured students’ cognitive appraisal of their environment. With the addition of the group-mean centered liberal arts experience variables (which included individual students’ perceptions of ‘good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty’ [both ‘classroom practices’ and ‘out-of-class interactions with faculty’], ‘academic challenge and high expectations,’ ‘diversity experiences,’ and ‘high quality interactions with peers’), the final within-residential college models explained 13.3% of the individual-level variation in students’ inclination to inquire and 27.1% of the individual-level variation in students’ capacity for lifelong learning. The ‘classroom practices’ component of ‘good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty’ had a positive association with students’ inclination to inquire, albeit small (for each point increase in students’ self-reported classroom practices, a corresponding .184 increase in their inclination to inquire). Interestingly, the ‘classroom practices’ component did not have a statistically significant association with students’ capacity for lifelong learning. So although students with a high inclination to inquire also reported that their instructors employed effective classroom practices, the high quality instruction they received did not immediately translate to students’ reporting a higher capacity for lifelong learning. That said, students’ out-of-class interactions with faculty were positively associated with their capacity for lifelong learning (.171 increase in capacity for lifelong learning for every 87 point gain on their out-of-class interactions scale), and marginally significantly associated with the inclination to inquire (.112 increase in their inclination to inquire for every point gain on their out-of-class interaction scale). Students who reported that their academic experience was challenging and that expectations were high also reported a greater capacity for lifelong learning. Furthermore, there was a marginally significant positive association between students’ reports of their academic experience as challenging and their inclination to inquire, however, in a practical sense the association was quite small (for each point increase in students’ self-reported academic challenge and high expectations, a corresponding .041 increase in their inclination to inquire). So, whereas students’ experience of their collegiate environment as challenging may have promoted the requisite skills for lifelong learning, it did not necessarily deepen their value for or proclivity toward learning. The positive association among students’ inclination to inquire, capacity for lifelong learning, and their reports of diversity experiences was small, but significant. Because the model is not causal, it remains unclear whether students who have a greater inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning are also students who would be more apt to seek out diversity experiences, or whether students’ exposure to diversity experiences was influential in promoting their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Modeling the group mean of the liberal arts experience variables at level-2 of the model (research question 4) will provide a better sense of how an overall environment or ethos favoring diverse interactions may facilitate or impede students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. 88 Finally, there was a negative association between students’ inclination to inquire and their interactions with peers. Again, the practical significance of the association was small (.175 point decrease in students’ inclination to inquire for every 1 point above the mean of their residential college they scored). The association between students’ capacity for lifelong learning and their interactions with peers was not significant. Again, because there is not a causal relationship between the variables, it remains unclear whether the deep and meaningful relationships with peers actually detract from students’ inclination to inquire or rather that students’ who value deep and meaningful relationships with peers might place that value over their proclivity toward learning. Summary of Within—Residential College Model Findings The within-residential college model, which included sociodemographic information such as students’ sex, race/ethnicity, parents’ education and income, high school grade point average, motivation, degree aspirations, years lived in the residential college and the group-mean centered liberal arts variables explained 13% of the variation within residential colleges of students’ inclination to inquire and 27% of the variation within residential colleges of students’ capacity for lifelong learning. The addition of the college experience, degree aspiration, and motivation variables explained the most within-residential college variation for both inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. These findings highlight the importance of considering such individual factors when examining the effectiveness of residential college environments. Consistent with ecology theory (Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Moos, 1979), although the addition of the college experience and motivation variables into the model explained more of the within-residential college variation, none of the significant associations were 89 overwhelmingly large. As a result, it is impossible to point to one individual attribute or experience to explain the variation in students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. Furthermore, much of within-residential college variance remains unexplained, as the final level-one model only accounted for 13.3% of the within- residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire and 27.1% of the within- residential college variation in students’ capacity for lifelong learning. These findings indicate that the individual characteristics that may be most closely associated with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning remain unknown. The amount of variation between residential colleges explained by the individual/within-residential college model is also noteworthy, as the final within- residential college model explained 50.6% of the variation between residential colleges in students’ inclination to inquire and 32% of the variation between residential colleges in students’ capacity for lifelong learning. As noted earlier, these findings indicate that many of the individual factors that are associated with students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning are concentrated in certain residential college environments, and support the findings that these environments vary fiom one another. Research (Mica Three; Does the asggiation between mg giodemomhic attributes, pig-college characteristics, and motivation and their inclination to m uire or wig for lifelong learning vg across residential colleges? For this research question, I was interested in examining whether the association of students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning and the within- residential college variables I explored in Research Question Two varied across residential colleges. In reference to Moos’ Social-Ecological Framework (discussed in chapter 1), in this question, I was examining the relationship of the arrows between the 90 environmental system and students’ cognitive appraisal, activation or arousal and the outcome variables. Such variation across residential colleges would indicate that the slopes of the predictor variables (the within-residential college/individual variables) and the response variables were random, in other words that they changed depending on the residential college context (Raudenbush & Byrk, 2001). Educational researchers are increasingly interested in examining these conditional effects (Pascarella, et al., 2005; Seifert, 2006). Within HLM, it is possible to examine conditional effects by randomizing the slope of a coefficient, as opposed to fixing it, which is done by modeling the variance of the level-one predictor at level-two (by randomizing the error term). Starting with students’ inclination to inquire, I modeled the variance of each level-one predictor in turn at level two and examined the change in deviance and the chi- square value to determine whether the change was significant. I then turned my attention to students’ capacity for lifelong learning, again modeling the variance of each level-one predictor in turn at level-two and examining the change in deviance and chi-square value to determine whether the change was significant. Table 4.3 presents the baseline deviance from the Full Maximum Likelihood model of the complete within-residential college model followed by the deviance, chi-square change statistic, and significance of the model with each the level-one predictor modeled in turn at level-two. Table 4.3. Deviance, Chi-Square Change, and Significance of Random Level-One Slopes Inclination to Inquire Capacity for Lifelong Learning Deviance (FML) Chi-Sq. Sig._ Deviance (FML) Chi-Sq. 8L (baseline) u0 7713.752 7201.752 Male ul 7713.792 0.040 n. 5. 7201.567 0.700 n. s. AfAm u2 7713.703 0.049 n. 3. 7200.963 0.789 n. s. AsAm u3 7709.843 3.909 n. 5. 7200.171 1.581 n. s. 91 Table 4.3 (cont’d)._ His/Lat u4 7713.785 0.033 n. 5. 7197.887 3.865 n. s. Intl u5 7713.123 0.629 n. s. 7201.217 0.535 n. s. MultiRac u6 7713.758 0.006 n. 5. 7200.864 0.888 n. s. RaceOth u7 7712.936 0.816 n. 3. 7201.573 0.179 n. s. ' NoRespRace u8 7713.557 0.195 n. s. 7200.792 0.960 n. s. HSGPA u9 7713.045 0.707 n. 5. 7200.81 1 0.941 n. s. NoBach u10 7713.434 0.318 n. 5. 7200.742 1.010 n. s. MoBach ull 7705.629 8.123 "' 7201.565 0.187 n. s. FirstGen u12 7713.310 0.442 n. 5. 7201.770 0.018 n. s. 1.0me u13 7711.759 1.993 n. 3. 7198.703 3.049 n. s. HiInc ul4 7713.583 0.169 n. 3. 7201.729 0.023 n. s. YrsLived u15 7712.453 1.299 n. 3. 7201.769 0.017 n. s. MO'I'IV u16 7713.690 0.062 n. 5. 7201.787 0.035 n. s. TeachClass u17 7713.275 0.478 n. 3. 7200.788 0.964 n. s. TeachOut u18 7713.025 0.728 n. 5. 7197.236 4.516 " AoChall ul9 7713.625 0.127 n. 5. 7201.041 0.711 n. 5. Diversity u20 7709.212 4.541 " 7198.930 2.822 n. 5. Peers u21 7709.644 4.108 n. 5. 7201.718 0.034 n. s. "p<.l; * p < .05; 9* p < .01; m p < .001 As illustrated in table 4.3, very few of the random level-one slopes significantly reduced the deviance score of the Full Maximum Likelihood models. Such reduction would have indicated that more variance was explained by randomizing the slope of the coefficient, and thus, that there was significant variation in the association of the variables across environments. Specifically, only the association between students’ aspirations to obtain more than a bachelor’s degree and their inclination to inquire varied by residential context at the p<.05 significance level. The variance in slope across residential colleges that was associated with obtaining more than a bachelor’s degree was 9.77. The other associations of the within-residential college/individual variables and students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning did not vary significantly based on residential college context. The marginal significance of the random level-one liberal arts experience variables will be explored in more detail in Research Question 92 Four, which examines the association of the overall group mean of the liberal arts experience variables and students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. Because the variance associated with the random slope of ‘degree aspirations- more than bachelors’ was small, I opted to keep the slope fixed for subsequent analyses in favor of model parsimony. In addition, I used the deviance score of the model and Akaike’s Information Criterion AIC (Akaike, 1987 as cited in Hox, 2002), which is calculated using the deviance and number of parameters, to guide the deletion of variables that were non-significant throughout all of the models so as to further increase the parsimony of the level-one model. I deleted the following variables fiom the inclination to inquire model: ‘degree aspirations-less than Bachelor’s’ and ‘family income-Below $50,000.’ Despite the fact that the variables ‘high school GPA,’ ‘family income-Above $150,000,’ and ‘first-generation student’ were non-significant throughout the models, I opted to keep them based upon the amount of deviance each explained in the final parsimonious model. Equation 4.1 shows the final level-one model for inclination to inquire. Inclolnqij == floj +fllj(MaIe,J-)+,sz(AfAmy)+fl3j(AsAmiJ-)+fl4j(His/Laty)+ ,st (Intlij ) + fl6j (MultiRacij ) + ,67 j (RaceOth?- ) + 381‘ (NoRespRaceij ) + fig j (F irstGeni j ) + ,610 .(IncHighU ) + 3111' (HSGPAij - HSGPA» ) + 6121(Motivationij —Motivation..)+ ,613j(YrsLived,-j)+ (4-1) ,614 j (DgASpMoBachij ) + B] 51- (TeachClass i]. — T eachCIass of ) + ,616 j (T eachOut ,3- — T eachOut .j ) + [917 ,- (Ac.ChalIenge ,3- — Ac.Challenge ,1 ) + ,618 j (Diversity ,3- - Diversity ,j ) + ,619 1 (Peers ,3- — Peers .j ) + rij For the capacity for lifelong learning model, I deleted the following variables: ‘male,’ ‘degree aspirations-less than Bachelor’ s,’ and ‘family income-Below $50,000.’ Even though they were not significant throughout the models, I opted to keep ‘family income-Above $150,000,’ and ‘first-generation student’ because of their influence on the 93 deviance score of the model. Equation 4.2 shows the final, parsimonious, level-one model for capacity for lifelong learning. CapoeLrnij = 1301 +fllj(AfAmgj)+fl2j(ASAmy)+fl3j(Hi’/Latfi)+fl4j(lnfly’) +flsj(MuItrRac ij)+ ,B6j(Race0th,j)+,B7J-(No Re spRacey)+ (4.2) fl8j(FirstGen ,1. ) + ,69j(IneHigh ,1. ) + ,Bloj(HSGPA ,1. — 2755543. ) + 311 j( Motivation ij - Motivation .. ) + ,6121.(YrsLived ij ) + fll3j(DgAspMoBac hij- ) + ,614j(TchCIass ij - TchCIass ej)+ fl15j(Tch0ut i]- - TchOut oj)+ ,616j(Ac.Chally- - Acffl ej) + ,Bl7j(Diversit)z ij — Diversity ,1. ) + ,6181-(Peersij — Peers 0] ) + rij Research Question F our: How is the environmental context of a residengl' college ;e_lated to students’ inclination to inquireand camcitv for lifelong learning (Specifically, Liberal Arts Exm’ence Environmental Variables and Disciernag’ Focus 1? For this research question, I was interested in examining the relationship between the characteristics of the residential college environment and students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. Specifically, I was curious about how an overall ethos marked by the liberal arts experiences (good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty; academic challenge and high expectations; diversity experiences; and quality interactions with peers) and disciplinary focus were associated with students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. With regard to my conceptual framework, which was Moos’s social-ecological framework (see Chapter 1), the between-residential college (level-two) model encapsulates the environment system, including the mean score of the liberal arts experiences in a residential college as well as disciplinary focus of the college on students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning (efforts at adaptation and coping). Having decomposed the total variance into between-residential college variance and within-residential college variance, for this question I focused on the between- residential college variation, or whether there was a relationship between the different 94 environmental attributes and students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. I focused my analysis on two sets of contextual characteristics: environments marked by the liberal arts experience variables and the disciplinary focus .of the residential college. Environmental Liberal Arts Experience Variables I began my analysis by focusing on liberal arts experiences. Whereas in the level- one model I focused on how individual students’ self-reported assessment of the liberal arts experience variables was associated with their inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning (thus capturing students’ cognitive appraisal of their environment), in this question I was interested in how an environment marked by an overall ethos of each of the liberal arts experiences (good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty; academic challenge and high expectations; diversity experiences; and quality interactions with peers) was associated with students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. I used the mean score of individuals within each residential college as a proxy for the ethos of each residential college environment with regard to the liberal arts experience variables. I added each contextual variable to the parsimonious level-one models in turn and then created a model with all of the contextual liberal arts experience variables. To aid in interpreting my findings, I used two tools, the between-residential variance explained calculation (which I also used in to interpret the level-one model) and each model’s deviance score and chi-square change statistic. A significant drop in the deviance score (and a significant chi-square change statistic) would indicate that the addition of the environmental variable was explaining the variation between residential college environments. Because I was entering variables one at a time, these scores 95 provided insight into the magnitude of the association between each of the environmental variables and the outcome variables. I present the coefficients of the association between the contextual liberal arts experiences and students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning, the variance and variance explained, the reliability, and the deviance and chi-square change statistics in Tables 4.4 and 4.5. As illustrated in table 4.4, the ‘classroom practices’ component of ‘good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty’ and the ‘academic challenge and high expectations’ group-mean variables explained the most between-residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire. The ‘classroom practices’ component dropped the deviance score by 13.806 and raised the between-residential college variance explained fiom 50.2% to 83.7%. The ‘academic challenge and high expectations’ variable dropped the deviance score by 13.273 and raised the between-residential college variance explained from 50.2% to 74.6%. Although the ‘out-of-class interactions with faculty’ component of the ‘good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty’ group-level variable was also significant when added on its own, its significance dwindled when the final model with all the group-level liberal arts experience variables. ' In the final model, which accounted for the overall ethos of all the liberal arts experiences, the ‘academic challenge and high expectations’ variable remained significant, the ‘classroom practices’ variable dropped to marginal significance, and the ‘out-of-class interactions with faculty’ variable dropped to nonsignficance. These changes in significance are likely a result of the high correlations between the level-two liberal arts experience variables (reported in Appendix J). The other two group-level 96 :5. V a 1.... 2o. V a a... ”no. V a s £.Vn_< m a a _ _ _ a: t «8.2 < an." .3. an: t «5.2 t 2:: r... 8%— 2.5% 82.520 R a a mu 8 mm «a assuage ”$.32. REE 34.2: 48.8: 03.32. cages 80:? as: 833: is $3 485 83 82 33 33 58.35 again: RS 23 2:. £3 83 55 3.2. 8:330 _ .30 sees; 85.: new: 23m 83m Swen 23a ”8.: .30 seas“? and :3 83 33 £3 53 83 8:33 .60 .em-§m . £3 t. 82 r... :3 t 2.2 t; :3 2.. e8... .2. Rod osv._oo.s¢-§um 3.3.—.5 .9: :3. a... 82 soon? .2: £130 .2 22. .2 £2 «88:89.» £939 9 «ad a... mmmd doame.EU.u3< .m d 8:. a... need mas—0.3.50. < ”and t. 23 so? seems? . e385; 83335 £30 £388. 3333;330 .. SEN t. :93. t. 82.. t. at: t. 49.3 .3. 2a.: 2: an: “9285 we Sm 52 mm as; + ma bemoan new $9.30 1% as... a new eoélmmm .882 dam... EBB a :23 + _ 93 + _ 35 so + £6 + :33 + _ .23 _ E3 _ .23 can 233 2 83:2: .383"... as 80883“ 32 335 .3 332 fineness“ 8058 83834 ”4.... 0:3 97 :5. Va .3... ”8. V a a... "we. V a e m—.Va< m _ _ _ _ _ a: a: Smfim 8a.: a... mN—S a:- egk 9...... mcmd— .md ~34 UOQSMQHIHUW EU 8 an an «N a an a cages; 83.8: 298:. 233:. 303: 3.3»: 8285 38.83 SE: 85.6: on; .33 ~33 38.3 2.2. 38.3 38.3 30235 835353: :3 :3 ES :3 E... 2.3 Ed 85293 .30 .3353 323 83.8 38.8 38.8 «8.8 $23. 32.3. .30 .8m.§§ E3 Rm... 38 84.3 83 82 and 8:335 .30 amazon _. 32 E: t... 22 at £2 2. £20 3... 2.2 t. #2 .Aauesé .30 .8353 00an> .3. a 83 83 3.8%; .35 £35 .3. a $3.3 . ”23 88380 £3.35 .33 :33. t 33 .38me .3033 a. no: I... mead mun—oeoéo. < :20. .3. 5.3 3.835 338230. e385; £88.23 £35 a «338.; 89:23am“.— E< .82.: - .530 ._ 33.3 8:: : 83.. t. 8:: : Se: < ”.33 t. 208 33235 #3. ea 22 1% use + .ww £2.95 0mm ofifi0lmm 3% 3 am 888... new 383 42mm 3.8: .3 :23 + _ 35 + _ .23 so 820+ :23 + _ 35 + _ .23 :93 .3.— mefiafi muses 3e N33.3.00 .9835 as eofifiaxm as. ES: 3 80.32 acoaaesam 3858 83832 ”2. as; 98 variables, ‘diversity experiences,’ and ‘interactions with peers’ were not significant when entered on their own, nor were they significant in the final model. In sum, for every point increase in an environment marked by academic challenge and high expectations (as measured by the group mean) there was an associated gain of .214 points in students’ inclination to inquire score. Likewise, although it was only marginally significant, for every point increase in an environment marked by effective teaching practices, there was an associated gain of .598 points in students’ inclination to inquire. Although these gains are modest, when one considers that individual’s responses to the liberal arts experience variables are already controlled for, they are noteworthy. Table 4.5 illustrates the association between the group-level liberal arts variables and students’ capacity for lifelong learning. All of the group-level liberal arts experience variables were significant when added on their own into the parsimonious level-one model, with the exception of the ‘classroom practices’ component of ‘good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty.’ The ‘out-of-class interactions with faculty’ component of ‘good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty’ clearly had the strongest association with students’ capacity for lifelong learning. When added to the parsimonious level—one model, ‘out-of-class interactions with faculty’ lowered the deviance score by 16.565. In addition, it raised the explained variance of the between- residential college model from 32% to 70.9%. The variable ‘interactions with peers’ was also significant when entered on its own, lowering the deviance score by 11 and the raising the explained variance of the between-residential college model to from 32% to 55.7%. However, when entered into the final model, ‘interactions with peers’ dropped to non-significance. 99 Interestingly, the ‘classroom practices’ component of ‘good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty’ rose to marginal significance in the model with all group-level liberal arts experiences included. The negative association between the ‘classroom practices’ variable and students’ capacity for lifelong learning in the final model is likely a result of the high correlations among the other level-two variables. Given the fact that the significance is marginal, and the negative association between the variables is small, the practical significance of the finding is limited. However, the finding that an environment marked by an ethos of out-of-class experiences with faculty is associated with students’ capacity for lifelong learning was significant, and can be interpreted as for every 1 point increase of the mean score of students’ out-of-class interactions with faculty there was a corresponding increase of 1.1 points in their self- reported capacity for lifelong learning. Although the gain is modest, when one considers that individual’s responses to the liberal arts experience variables are already controlled for in addition to the overall ethos of liberal arts experience, it is interesting that out-of- class interactions with faculty was still significant. Disciplinary Focus After examining the relationship of the environmental liberal arts experience variables and students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning, I’tumed my attention to exploring the relationship between the disciplinary focus of the residential college environment and students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. I was particularly interested in examining how the disciplinary focus of the residential college related to students’ capacity for lifelong learning, as past research indicated their might be a relationship but failed to control for students’ motivation (Hayek & Kuh, 100 1998). Unfortunately, the sample of residential college environments I collected did not include enough variation in disciplinary focus to make meaningful inferences about its association to students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning, as 20 of the residential colleges were classified as interdisciplinary/other, 1 was situated in the social sciences, 2 in the natural sciences, and 1 in the humanities. As a result, I was unable to run the analysis. Summary of the Between-Residential College Model Findings The between-residential college model, which included variables pertaining to an ethos marked by the liberal arts variables (good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty, academic challenge and high expectations, diversity experiences, and interactions with peers) explained 82.2% of the between-residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire (3 2% of which was explained by the environmental liberal arts variables). The model explained 77.5% of the between-residential college variation in students’ capacity for lifelong learning (45.4% of which was explained by the environmental liberal arts variables). The majority of the between-residential college variation explained for inclination to inquire resulted from the addition of an environmental ethos promoting ‘academic challenge and high expectations.’ For the capacity for lifelong learning outcome, out-of-class interactions with faculty explained the greatest amount of between-environment variation. These findings highlight the importance of an ethos marked by these experiences. Even though the environmental liberal arts experience variables explained much of the variation between residential colleges, their association with the outcome variables was not large. Furthermore, as illustrated by the first research question, the between- 101 residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire was 8.8% and for capacity for lifelong learning it was 5.3%, and so while these models explain a large amount of the variation in the between-residential college outcomes, their overall contribution to understanding the variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning is considerably less. I raise this point not to diminish the importance of the findings, but rather to make sure that the findings are set in a larger context. Does the association between student-level motivation and students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong differ by residential college-level liberal arts experience variables (good teaching and high quality interaction with faculg, academic challenge fli high expectations, diversity experiences, and peer interactiofl? In this final research question, I was interested in examining the interaction between individual motivation attributes and residential college environments in promoting students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Because these interactions are between level-1 (individual) and level-2 (contextual/environment) variables, they are often called cross-level interactions. With regard to Moos’s social- ecological framework (see Chapter 1), in exploring the cross-level interactions I was examining the relationship between the personal system and environmental system, which was illustrated in Moos’ model by the arrows connecting the two systems. Having already modeled the level-one intercept of each outcome variable with the level-two liberal arts environment variables (which I described in Research Question Four) and obtained a baseline deviance score, I added each group-level liberal arts experience in turn to the slopes of two different level-one variables. I started with ‘Degree Aspirations — More Than Bachelor’s’ and then focused on ‘Motivation.’ I determined whether the cross-level interaction between the individual-level motivation variables and environmental liberal arts experiences were significant by examining the 102 chi-square change statistic and drop in deviance score. Table 4.6 presents the baseline deviance from the Full Maximum Likelihood model of the between-residential college model with all the environmental liberal arts experiences included followed by the deviance, chi-square change statistic, and significance of the model with the cross-level interaction of the motivation variables and each environmental liberal arts experience variable. Table 4.6: Deviance, Chi-Square Change, and Significance of Cross-Level Interactions between Motivation (Student) variables and Liberal Arts Experience (Environment) variables Inclination to Inquire Deviance (FML) Chi-Sq. Capacity for Lifelong Learning Deviance (F ML) Chi-Sq. baseline 7684.737 7180.650 MoreBach/TeachPrac 7684.154 0.583 n. 3. 7180.650 0.000 n. s. MoreBach/OutofClass 7683.864 0.873 n. 5. 7180.225 0.425 n. s. MoreBach/Challenge 7684.574 0.163 n. 5. 7180.641 0.009 n. s. MoreBach/Diversity 7684.730 0.007 n. 5. 7180.648 0.002 n. s. MoreBach/Peers 7684.734 0.003 n. 5. 7178.848 1.802 n. s. Motivation/TeachPrac 7684.662 0.075 n. 3. 7173.960 6.690 * Motivation/OutofClass 7684.709 0.028 n. 3. 7179.963 0.687 n. s. Motivation/Challenge 7684.588 0.149 n. 5. 7179.488 1.162 n. s. Motivation/Diversity 7684.729 0.008 n. 8. 7180.400 0.250 n. s. Motivation/Peers 7684.691 0.046 n. 3. 7180.251 0.399 n. s. Ap<.l; It p<05; #11: p < .01; its”: P < .001 As illustrated in table 4.6, very few of the cross-level interactions between the individual motivation variables (level-one) and environmental liberal arts experience variables (level-two) significantly reduced the deviance score of the Full Maximum Likelihood models of students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Specifically, only the cross-level interaction between students’ motivation and the ‘classroom practices’ component of ‘good teaching and high quality interactions with 103 faculty’ was significantly related to students’ capacity for lifelong learning at the p<.05 level. The interaction effect was -.0203 7, meaning that a rise in teaching practices was associated with a very slight decline in students’ motivation. From a practical perspective, the interaction coefficient was so small as to be all but insignificant. The other cross-level interactions of motivation variables (level-one) and environmental liberal arts experience variables (level-two) and students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning were not significant across residential college context. Summary of Results The results of the current study indicated that a statistically significant amount of variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning existed across residential environments. Although proportion of variance in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning was relatively small (8.8% and 5.3% respectively), existing research has illustrated that even a small amount of variation at the environmental level merits exploration, as it may provide insight into what aspects of the environment are influential to a given outcome (Porter & Swing, 1996; Seifert, 2006). The within-residential college model explained 13% of the variation within residential colleges of students’ inclination to inquire and 27% of the variation within residential colleges of students’ capacity for lifelong learning. The addition of the college experience, degree aspiration, and motivation variables explained the most within- residential college variation for both inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. The amount of variation between residential colleges explained by the individual/within-residential college model is noteworthy, as the final within-residential 104 college model explained 50.6% of the variation between residential colleges in students’ inclination to inquire and 32% of the variation between residential colleges in students’ capacity for lifelong learning. As noted earlier, these findings indicate that many of the individual factors that are associated with students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning are concentrated in certain residential college environments, and support the findings that these environments vary from one another. Having built within-residential college model for students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, I turned my attention to examining whether the degree of association of these outcome variables and the within-residential college variables varied across residential college context. By modeling the variance of each level-one predictor variable in turn at level-two, I determined whether the slopes of the predictor variables were random, thus indicating that the association changed depending on residential college context. I found that only the association between students’ aspirations to obtain more than a bachelor’s degree and their inclination to inquire varied by residential college context at the p<.05 significance level. Furthermore, the variance in slope was minimal, and as a consequence I Opted to keep all of the slopes ‘fixed’ for my subsequent analyses. The between-residential college model, which included variables pertaining to an ethos marked by the liberal arts variables (good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty, academic challenge and high expectations, diversity experiences, and interactions with peers) explained 82.2% of the between-residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire and 77.5% of the between-residential college variation in students’ capacity for lifelong learning. The majority of the between-residential college 105 Lilli v -. 5L1. r 5w hail variation explained for inclination to inquire resulted from the addition of an environmental ethos promoting ‘academic challenge and high expectations.’ For the capacity for lifelong learning outcome, out-of-class interactions with faculty explained the greatest amount of between-enviromnent variation. These findings highlight the importance of an ethos marked by these experiences. Having estimated the level-one, within residential college and level-two, between- residential college models for students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, I examined whether the interaction between the group-level variables in each of the models and the motivation variables had a statistically significant association with the outcome variables. I found that there was a statistically significant negative cross-level interaction between students’ motivation and the ‘classroom practices’ component of the good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty scale. The effect was so small that there was little practical significance associated with this finding. Having examined the associations of the variables at all different aspects of Moos’ model, my findings are complete. I turn now to the discussion of my findings. 106 CHAPTER FIVE Discussion and Implications In this final chapter, I discuss the findings of the current study, situating them in existing research and revisiting their relevance to Moos’s model. Then, I suggest implications for theory, research, policy, and practice. I begin by revisiting the purpose of the study and research questions. Next, I provide a brief summary of the findings reported in Chapter Fourwhile discussing their relevance to existing higher educations research. I also discuss the findings in relation to Moos’s model, and highlight where the model is particularly useful in illuminating environmental and personal considerations. Then, I consider the implications of the findings for theory, research, practice and policy. I conclude by discussing questions for future research. As postsecondary education is promoted as a necessity for participation in the 21St century economy, academics, policymakers, and the public have all voiced concerns about the quality and coherence of undergraduate education (AAC & U, 2007; Barr & Tagg, 1995; Boyer Commission, 1998; U. S. Department of Education, 2006). Critics point to the size, scope, and multiple missions of large, public research universities as contributing to students’ feelings of anonymity, lack of engagement, and disconnection from faculty (Astin, 1993; Boyer, 1987; Gaff, 1970; Gamson, 2000; Guskin, 1994; Hawkins, 1999; Jerome, 2000). Although undergraduates may face additional challenges at large, public research universities than they might in a more intimate and undergraduate-focused setting, these institutions remain a likely destination for many students to begin or complete their baccalaureate education because of their size, relative affordability, and diversity in educational offerings. 107 University administrators increasingly turn to residential colleges and other types of living-learning programs to address the size and scale conundrum facing large research universities. By creating smaller enclaves of students living together initially, taking part in a shared educational endeavor, and using resources within their environment that stress academics (Inkelas, Zeller, Murphy, & Hummel, 2006), administrators and faculty purport to create the atmosphere of a small liberal arts college while still offering students the resources of a large university, including comprehensive research and library facilities (Magolda, 1994; Schuman, 2005). Despite the increasing popularity of residential colleges and other living-learning programs, research examining their effectiveness is limited. Many studies have focused on determining whether these environments are more effective than no intervention in promoting students’ persistence (Pike, Schroeder, & Berry, 1997), academic achievement (Pasque & Murphy, 2005; Pike, Schroeder, & Berry, 1997), and involvement and social integration (Pike, 1999; Pike, Schroeder, & Berry, 1997). Virtually no attention has been paid to whether and how these environments promote values associated with a liberal arts , education, including whether they deepen students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Identified by the Center of Inquiry in the Liberal Arts at Wabash College as one distinctive outcome of a liberal arts education, having a deep inclination to inquire would suggest that a student has a strong desire to learn and continues to pursue intellectual growth. Closely connected to students’ inclination to inquire is their capacity for lifelong learning, which is defined by Hayek and Kuh (1999) as students’ ability to “‘learn to learn’ and interact effectively with others in a complex, information- based society” (p. 4). Whereas a deepened inclination to inquire promotes a value for 108 continuing to pursue knowledge, a robust capacity for lifelong learning provides students the tools to act upon their value for inquiry. Another concern with existing research on residential colleges and living-learning communities is that much of it is plagued with problems of analysis, as researchers have often aggregated data they collect to the environmental level, disregarding the individual differences of students in these environments, which may include their motivation, sociodemographic characteristics, and experiences. By using an ecological approach that accounts for environmental context and individual characteristics (Moos, 1976, 1979, 1986), in the current study I sought examine how students’ attributes (including their motivation and other sociodemographic characteristics) and residential college environments facilitate or impede students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Specifically, I investigated the following questions: 1. Does students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning vary across residential college environments? 2. How are students’ sociodemographic characteristics and motivation related to their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning? 3. Do the associations between students’ sociodemographic attributes and motivation and their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning vary across residential colleges? 4. How is the environmental context of a residential college related to students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning? Specifically, 109 a. Environmental Liberal Arts Experience variables - (good teaching and high quality interaction with faculty, academic challenge and high expectations, diversity experiences, and peer interactions) b. Disciplinary Focus 5. Does the association between student-level motivation and students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong differ by residential college- level liberal arts experience variables (good teaching and high quality interaction with faculty, academic challenge and high expectations, diversity experiences, and peer interactions)? From October 2008 through January 2009, I collected data from over 1800 undergraduate students in 24 residential colleges across the United States to examine the association between their sociodemographic characteristics, college experiences and motivation, residential college environments, and their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. I used multilevel analysis (Hox, 2002; Krefi & De Leeuw, 1998; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002) to ascertain the significant individual, contextual, and cross- level associations of variables with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Discussion of Findings I frame my discussion of the findings using the different levels of analysis as a guide. I begin by addressing the null model, which partitioned the variance into within- and between-residential college contexts, and then focus more acutely on the individual/within-residential college (level-one) and environment/between-residential college (level-two) models of the associations between variables and students’ inclination 110 to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Throughout the discussion I connect findings from the current study to existing research. Null Model — Estimating Individual and Contextual Variation In fitting the null model, I was interested in determining the extent to which students’ affiliation with a residential college environment was associated with their inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. Conducting a one-way AN OVA with random effects enabled me to partition the variance in the outcome variables into within-residential college and between-residential college components. I found that a statistically significant amount of the variance in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning was attributable to students’ residential college environment. Specifically, the residential college environment accounted for 8.8% of the variation in students’ inclination to inquire and 5.3% of the variation in students’ capacity for lifelong learning. At first blush the amount of variation across environments may not seem noteworthy. However, when placed in the context that residential colleges are often considered more similar than different (Ryan, 1993; Smith, 1994), and that in collegiate settings more variation is found between individuals than between environments (Pascarella & Terenzini, 2001, 2005), it became clear that the variation across environments merited exploration. The differences in outcomes across environments held potential insight into what aspects of the environment are most closely associated with students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. The variation across environments also justified the use of a multilevel model to explore the associations between the variables. 111 In addition, the findings from the null model support existing literature calling for a need to take an ecological approach to exploring liberal arts outcomes, as the multitude of factors affecting student outcomes include both environmental and personal factors (Pascarella & Terenzini, 2005). These factors often are interacting and may influence one another (Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Moos, 1976, 1979, 1986). In emphasizing the need for an ecological approach to examining residential college environments, the findings of the null model also help to contextualize some of the mixed findings from existing studies. Specifically, those studies that aggregate the variance in student outcomes to the group-level and compare across environments (e. g. Inkelas & Weisman, 2003; Pasque & Murphy, 2005) may ignore the greatest source of variation, which are often individual characteristics and experiences (Pascarella & Terenzini, 1991, 2005). The results of the null model in the current study were consistent with college impact research (Pascarella & Terenzini, 1991 , 2005) in that there was much more variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning within residential colleges than between residential colleges. By failing to examine the variation within residential environments, existing research may overlook important factors affecting student outcomes, or may overstate the environmental impact on student outcomes. On the other hand, the variation between environments, although small, was significant, and lends support to the argument that residential college environments and living-learning programs are not all the same (Inkelas, Longerbeam, Leonard, & Soldner, 2005; Wawrzynski & J essup-Anger, in press), and thus caution should be taken when grouping students from these environments together without a strong rationale for doing SO. 112 Within-Residential College Model —— The Association of Individual Attributes with Students’ Inclination to Inquire and Capacity for Lifelong Learning In fitting the within-residential college (level one) model, I was interested in examining the extent to which students’ sociodemo graphic and pre-college characteristics, college experiences and motivation, and individual perceptions of their liberal arts experiences were associated with their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. By regressing the inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning outcomes on the four blocks of variables, I was able to determine how closely associated these variables were with the outcome variables. I found that the overall within-residential college (level one) model explained 13.3% of the within—residential college and 50.6% of the between-residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire. The model explained 27. 1% of the within-residential college and 32% of the between-residential college variation in students’ capacity for lifelong learning. I frame my discussion of how the findings of the within-residential college (level one) model relate to existing research using the blocks of variables I entered as a guide. Sociodemographic Attributes Although the addition of the sociodemographic attributes to the model explained little of the within-residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning (5.1% and .3% respectively), several of the significant associations between variables are worth discussing. First, it is noteworthy that Asian- American students scored on average 3.28 points lower on the inclination to inquire scale than their White peers despite there being no difference in their scores on the capacity for lifelong learning scale. One possible explanation for their lower average scores is that the 113 scale does not provide a true measure of Asian-American students’ inclination to inquire. In recent years, researchers have raised questions about the applicability of psychological models that were developed and tested using primarily White and often male students to women and students of color and have suggested alternative models (Cross & Fhagen-Smith, 2005; Josselson, 1987, 1996; Kim, 2001; Kodarna, McEwen, Liang, & Lee, 2001; Torres, 2003). In their critique and proposed alternative to Chickering’s model of psychosocial identity development, Kodarna, et al. (2001) contended that two external domains are necessary in a model that is inclusive of Asian- American students. These domains include the tensions between Western values and racism from the US and Asian values for family and community. The researchers argued that underlying many existing Western models are individualistic values, which promote independence and self-exploration, whereas underlying traditional Asian familial and cultural norms are collectivist values, which promote placing the needs of others above one’s self and striving toward interdependence (Kodama, et al., 2001). In examining the scale used to measure students’ inclination to inquire with an eye toward why Asian-American students might have scored lower than White students, I found several items that when set in the context of an individualistic versus collectivist mindset, may be biased toward an individualistic outlook on valuing thinking. Items such as ‘The idea of relying on thought to make my way to the top appeals to me,’ and ‘I like to have the responsibility of handling a situation that requires a lot of thinking’ may have been construed by some students as necessitating acting in a way that would set themselves apart from their peers, a notion that would likely be met with more apprehension in a collectivist culture. 114 In addition to Asian-American students scoring lower than their White peers, female students scored on average 1.629 points lower than males. The finding is especially interesting when one considers that the ‘need for cognition’ scale, which was used to measure students’ inclination to inquire was tested for gender-bias when it was developed (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982). However, the tests were done almost three decades ago, and student demographics have changed considerably since that time with more diverse students attending college for myriad reasons. Like the argument made for why Asian-American students as a group scored lower on the scale, a similar argument can be made for why female students scored lower than males. Several researchers have argued that gender and gender role socialization may play a part in cognitive (Belenky, Clinchy, Goldberger, & Tarule, 1986), epistemological (Baxter Magolda, 1992), moral (Gilligan, 1993), and identity development (Josselson, 1987) of women and that existing theories and models of development have historically overlooked these differences. Researchers posit that women often take a more relational, interpersonal approach to knowledge acquisition than their male peers, who often prefer a more impersonal, debate centered approach (Baxter Magolda, 1992; Belenky, et al., 1986). If these arguments are extended to women students’ value for learning and expression of their enjoyment of thinking, it is possible that some of the scale items are emphasizing a pattern of development that is slightly more appealing to most men than most women. Another possible explanation for the lower average scores of Asian-American and women students may be because both of these groups of students have historically had less access to higher education than their White and male peers (Thelin, 2004). As a 115 result, these students may be socialized by family and society to focus more on the outcome of their college years in terms of degree attainment for their future success as opposed to developing a value for thinking. I will discuss the implications of this possibility later in this chapter. A third interesting finding was that international students scored on average 4.5 points higher on the inclination to inquire scale than their White peers, even after accounting for such factors as family income and education, motivation, and degree aspirations. Although it is difficult to interpret the exact meaning of this finding because all international students are grouped together regardless of country of origin, it may be most indicative of the reality that international students often must overcome more obstacles to get into college in the United States than their White (domestic) peers, and as a result, may have a more pronounced or thought-out value for learning than their peers. This finding aligns with Pizzolato’s (2003) argument that high-risk students are often more self-authored than their peers because of the additional obstacles they have overcome in order to arrive at college. In examining the association of sociodemographic variables and students’ capacity for lifelong learning, two noteworthy results included the fact that Hispanic/Latino students scored 1.355 points higher, and Race-Other students (which was a category that combined Native Hawaiian and Native American students) scored 4.291 points higher on average than their White peers on the capacity for lifelong learning scale, even after accounting for potentially confounding variables. These results may be indicative again that these students have a clear sense of why they are in college (Pizzolato, 1993), and thus are making greater gains than their white peers, or 116 alternatively it could be that these students as a group entered with a lower benchmark from which to compare their progress than their peers, and thus are making greater gains. Finally, although the sociodemographic variables did not explain a large amount of variation within residential college environments, these variables did explain a considerable amount of the variation between residential college environments (50.6% of between-residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire and 32% of between-residential college variation in students’ capacity for lifelong learning). These findings indicate that many of the significant sociodemographic characteristics that are associated with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning are clustered in certain residential college environments. The findings support the argument that if residential college research is aggregated to the environmental level, ignoring individual/within-environment variation, there is a risk of overstating the environmental influence, when in fact the findings may stem from the characteristics of individual students who are attracted to and clustered in specific environments. Pre-college Characteristics The addition of the pre-college characteristics (which included parents’ education and income levels, and students’ high school grade point average) explained little of the within-college and between-college variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. The only marginally significant association was the negative association between students’ high school grade point average and their capacity for lifelong learning score. This finding seems antithetical to conventional wisdom, however, Seifert (2006) found similar results in her study of the effect of major on 21St century competency development. Seifert argued that in self-report studies of college 117 impact, students who come into college with higher grades may have a higher benchmark from which to compare their progress, and thus may report lower gains. In the case of the current study, that rationale makes sense, as the capacity for lifelong learning index measures students’ self-reported gains in areas associated with the skills necessary for lifelong learning. It could be that students who had a higher high school grade point average did not believe they had made as much progress in developing these skills as their counterparts who entered the residential college environment with lower high school grade point averages. The non-significant association between the parental income and education variables and students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning was somewhat surprising considering that Gorard and Selwyn (2005) found both of these attributes to be predictive of a commitment to lifelong learning, which they measured by one’s enrollment in formal education during the adult years. However, because Gorard and Selwyn’s study was conducted in the United Kingdom and used a different outcome variable, the dissimilar findings may be explained by contextual differences in postsecondary education between the US and United Kingdom. Access is much more limited in the United Kingdom and thus parental income and education are likely more important factors to continuing one’s education. Motivation, College Experiences, and Degree Aspirations The addition of the motivation, college experience, and degree aspiration variables to the model explained the most variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning of any of the level-one blocks of variables. Although the positive association between students’ motivation and their inclination to inquire and 118 capacity for lifelong learning was small, its significance supports McCombs (1991) assertion that to promote lifelong learning, educational settings should develop supportive climates conducive to cultivating personal relationships, a sense of control, and personal choice in students’ learning process. The positive association also extends the reach of self-determination theory to the development of lifelong learners. However, the small significance of the association merits further exploration to determine whether the instrument is effective in capturing students’ sense of autonomy, competence, and relatedness and also if the association deepens over time. When taken together, the significant positive association between students’ motivation, aspirations to obtain more than a bachelor’s degree, and their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning support Hayek and Kuh’s (1998) supposition that students’ motivation to learn may play a role in the development of their capacity for lifelong learning, and thus should be accounted for when examining factors that influence students’ capacity for lifelong learning. Furthermore, although small, the results of the varying slope for the association between students’ desire to obtain more than a bachelor’s degree and their inclination to inquire suggest that this association may be stronger in certain circumstances, a finding that ought to be examined further in future studies. The positive association between the number of years students lived in the residential college and their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning is indicative that these outcomes may deepen over time as students become more integrated into the collegiate setting. However, as I alluded to in chapter four, it remains unclear whether the positive association between years spent in the residential college and the 119 outcome variables is a result of students’ interaction with their residential college environment, or rather a product of their maturation. A longitudinal study of students who leave the residential college setting and those who stay would help to tease out these findings. Individual Liberal Arts Experiences Admittedly, I was most interested in the addition of the liberal arts experiences variables (good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty; academic challenge and high expectations; diversity experiences and quality interactions with peers) at level- two of the model because they were more indicative of the potential influence of the environment on the outcome variables. That said, the addition of the group-mean centered liberal arts experience variables at level one enabled me to ascertain the association between students’ individual experiences with these variables and their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, which I could then compare to the environment (level-two) results. In addition, the addition of these variables at level- one served to control for students’ individual beliefs about the environment, allowing me to hone in on the potential environmental impact. I discuss the associations between the individual liberal arts experience variables briefly here, reserving some of the discussion for their entry into the model at level-two. When entered into the model at level one, all of the individual liberal arts experience variables were at least marginally statistically significant in their association with students’ inclination to inquire. However, none of these associations was particularly large. The most significant positive associations between students’ inclination to inquire and the individual liberal arts experiences variables were the 120 ‘classroom practices’ component of the ‘good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty’ variable and the ‘diversity experiences’ variable. These results, in part, support research conducted by Blaich. and Wise (2008) who in their summary of the findings from the first year of the Wabash National Study of Liberal Arts Education reported that good teaching and diversity experiences were both positively related to growth in students’ need for cognition (or inclination to inquire) over the course of one year. However, Blaich and Wise also found Academic Challenge to be related to growth in students’ need for cognition, which was only marginally significant in the current study. Perhaps the most interesting finding was the statistically significant negative association between students’ ‘high quality interaction with peers’ and their inclination to inquire. The negative association was somewhat surprising given the weight of evidence that peers and co-curricular involvement play a pivotal role in positively influencing student learning (Astin, 1984; Kuh, Schuh, Whitt, & Associates, 1991; Pascarella & Terenzini, 1991, 2005). However, the finding extends that of Pike, Schroeder, and Berry (1997) who found that although living-learning community participation may heighten social integration and institutional commitment, it does little to enhance academic achievement, indicating that the role of peers might be more indirect. More research is warranted to tease out whether the negative association between high quality interactions with peers and students’ inclination to inquire is an indication that deep and meaningful relationships with peers actually detract from students’ inclination to inquire, or that students who value deep and meaningful relationships with peers might place that value over their value for learning. It is also possible that the scale used in the current study is 121 not reflecting accurately the association between peer relationships and students’ inclination to inquire. Similar to the small associations with students’ inclination to inquire, several of the individual liberal arts experiences had a small but significant association with students’ capacity for lifelong learning. Interestingly, aside from diversity experiences, which had a significant positive association with both students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, the most significant associations between the liberal arts variables and students’ capacity for lifelong learning were different from those that were significant with students’ inclination to inquire. Whereas the ‘out-of-class interactions with faculty’ component of the ‘good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty’ variable had a marginally significant association with students’ inclination to inquire, it was significant at the p<.01 level for students’ capacity for lifelong learning. This finding contradicts that of Hayek and Kuh (1999), who found no relationship between faculty- student interaction and students’ capacity for lifelong learning. The difference in findings is potentially explained by differences in the two samples. Existing research suggests that students in residential colleges or living-learning communities have more opportunities to interact with faculty than other students (Cox & Orehovec, 2007; Pike, 1999; Smith, 1994), which may have contributed to the significant positive association between out-of- class interactions with faculty and students’ capacity for lifelong learning. The differing significant associations between the individual liberal arts experience variables and the two outcome variables allude to the possibility that different environmental features are important in eliciting and supporting an inclination to inquire (value) and capacity for lifelong learning (skill set). The entry of the variables at level 122 two of the model provides additional substantiation of this claim. I turn to my discussion of the between-residential college model now. Between-Residential College Model — The Association of Residential College Environments with Students ’ Inclination to Inquire and Capacity for Lifelong Learning In fitting the between-residential college (level-two) models, I was interested in determining the extent to which various aspects of the residential college environment were associated with the outcome variables. Specifically, I was curious about how an overall ethos marked by the liberal arts experiences (good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty; academic challenge and high expectations; diversity experiences; and quality interactions with peers) and disciplinary focus were related to students’ inclination to inquire or capacity for lifelong learning. Because there was insufficient variation in the disciplinary foci of the residential colleges included in the sample to run the disciplinary focus component of the level-two model, my discussion in this section focuses solely on the addition of the environmental liberal arts to the models. By adding each of the environmental liberal arts variables to level-two of the models in turn before adding them all together, 1 determined the unique influence of each in explaining the between-residential college variance in the outcome variables. With the addition of the final model (which included all of the environmental liberal arts variables), I determined which associations remained significant despite the high correlations between the environmental (group mean) liberal arts experience variables (see Appendix J for level-two correlations). I begin the discussion of the associations between the outcome variables and the environmental liberal arts variables by focusing on the inclination to inquire models. I then turn to the capacity for lifelong learning 123 models. Throughout the discussion, I offer potential explanations for the findings and connect them to existing literature. Inclination to Inquire The between-residential college model explained 82.2% of the between- residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire (32% of which was explained by the environmental liberal arts variables). Although three of the environmental liberal arts variables were significantly associated with students’ inclination to inquire when entered individually into the level-two model (including both the ‘classroom practices’ and ‘out-of-class interactions with faculty’ components of the high quality interactions with faculty scale and the ‘academic challenge and high expectations’ variable), only the ‘academic challenge and high expectations’ variable remained significant at the p<.05 level in the final model. The association between the environmental ‘academic challenge and high expectations’ variable and students’ inclination to inquire was modest (.214 points), but noteworthy given that individuals’ reactions to the liberal arts experiences were controlled for at level-one of the model. There is extensive research supporting the notion that an environment marked by academic challenge and high expectations would enhance students’ inclination to inquire. Researchers examining undergraduate student success have found that students who are challenged in their academic environment report higher levels of development in a variety of areas (Astin, 1993; Chickering & Gamson, 1991; Cruce, Wolniak, Seifert, Pascarella, & Blaich, 2005; Kuh, Schuh, Whitt, & Associates, 1991; Pascarella & Terenzini, 1991, 2005; Seifert, 2006). It is worth mentioning that even in a residential college environment, which arguably is deliberate in its attempt to provide students’ with 124 a comprehensive undergraduate experience, there is still variation in students’ perceptions of the level of challenge provided by the environment. Faculty and administrators who desire to engage students more deeply in the life of the mind might focus their attention on increasing the level of challenge and the expectations they have of their students. Also interesting to note were the differences in the results of the group-mean centered individual liberal arts variables entered at level one of the model and an environment marked by these variables (as indicated by the group-mean) entered at level two. The fact that the ‘out-of-class interactions with faculty’ variable, ‘diversity experiences’ variable, and ‘quality interactions with peers’ variable were significant at level one of the model, but not in the final level-two model (which included all of the level-two liberal arts variables) may indicate that these factors are most effective in influencing students’ inclination to inquire only when they connected directly with students. On the other hand, the ‘academic challenge and high expectations’ variable, which was significant in both the level-one and level-two final models, and the ‘classroom practices’ variable, which was significant in the level-one model and marginally significant in the level-two model, may have a more distal impact on students’ inclination to inquire, potentially creating an overall ethos that deepens students’ inclination to inquire even when it does not directly engage a student. That rationale holds when one considers that the ‘classroom practices’ and ‘academic challenge and high expectations’ variables are less about relationships and more about students’ general sense of their environment. These findings are supported by Mayhew, Wolniak, and Pascarella’s (2008) findings that instruction-based practices, which were those that p 125 promoted active learning, discussion, multiple viewpoints, and self-reflection had an indirect effect on encouraging students’ inclination to inquire (a term they labeled need for cognition, which was true to the original name of the scale). Also important to examine was the non-significant associations between students’ inclination to inquire and environments marked by diversity experiences and quality interactions with peers. Neither of these two variables was significant when entered into the level-two model on their own, nor were they significant in the final model, which included all of the level two variables. Mayhew, Wolniak, and Pascarella (2008) found that negative interactions with diverse peers hampered growth in students’ inclination to inquire. The differences in findings could be attributable to differences in the diversity scales, as the scale used in the current study focuses more on the frequency of interactions with diverse peers and ideas as opposed to the quality of those interactions. It is possible that by focusing on how an environment marked by positive or negative interactions with diverse peers, as opposed to frequency of these interactions, I would find more of an association between an environment marked by students’ diversity interactions and their inclination to inquire. As with the negative association between high quality interactions with peers at level-one of the model, the non-significant association between students’ inclination to inquire and an environment marked by high quality interactions with their peers at level- two of the model was curious given the evidence that peers play a role in engaging students’ in learning, especially in living-learning communities (Wawrzynski, Jessup- Anger, Helman, Stolz, & Beaulieu, in press). More research is warranted to determine if there are particular interactions with peers that are not captured in the ‘high quality 126 interactions with peers’ scale, which might be more associated with a deep inclination to inquire. Capacity for Lifelong Learning Turning now to the association of the environmental liberal arts variables and students’ capacity for lifelong learning, the level-two model explained 77.5% of the between-residential college variation in students’ capacity for lifelong learning (45.4% of which was explained by the environmental liberal arts variables). With the exception of the classroom practices component of good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty, all of the environmental liberal arts experience variables were significant when entered on their own into the level-two model. However, in the final model, only the out- of-class interactions with faculty remained significant at the p<.05 level. This finding contradicts that of Hayek and Kuh (1999) who found that faculty-student interaction had little to no effect on deepening students’ capacity for lifelong learning. As I argued earlier when discussing the individual level variables, the difference in findings is potentially explained by differences in the two samples, as students in the current study might have had more opportunity to interact with faculty, especially outside of class because of their participation in the residential college. The marginally significant negative association between ‘classroom practices’ and students’ capacity for lifelong learning was puzzling. However, examining the scale items of the ‘classroom practices’ component in the context of their association to students’ capacity for lifelong learning raised questions about the fact these items were mostly focused on instructor preparation, organization, and clarity. A classroom environment marked by these standards is unlikely to translate directly into deepening 127 students’ ability to learn to learn and apply concepts independently, unless coupled with developing a mastery orientation for learning (Dweck & Leggett, 1988), deepening students’ appreciation for why the material is important (Brophy, 2004), and creating opportunities for active engagement with material (Chickering & Gamson, 1987, 1991). In fact, some of the practices might be associated with an instructor maintaining control over the classroom as opposed to developing autonomy in students (McCaslin & Good, 1992) The disconnect between these instructional practices and students’ capacity for lifelong learning may also explain the significant negative cross-level interaction (Research Question Five) between students’ motivation and an environment marked by these classroom practices. Although the cross-level interaction between the two variables was so small as to not have practical significance, it adds credence to questioning whether a different measure of classroom practices which might take into account practices more geared toward developing self-regulated learners might more effectively capture the relationship between classroom practices and students’ capacity for lifelong learning. I Similar to the changes in the significance of the association of the liberal arts experience and students’ inclination to inquire at level-one and level-two of the model, there were also changes in the significance of these variables and students’ capacity for lifelong learning at the different levels of the model. Specifically, only the ‘out-of class interactions with faculty’ component of the ‘good teaching and hi gh-quality interactions with faculty’ variable remained significant in the final models at both level one and level two. The ‘academic challenge and high expectations’ and ‘diversity experiences’ 128 variables, while significant at level-one of the model, were not significant at level two. These findings may indicate that for this particular construct, which deals with building skills as opposed to deepening a value, the relationships students develop with faculty are important in creating an environmental ethos that supports building a capacity for lifelong learning. Alternatively, it may suggest that students who are already committed to deepening their capacity for lifelong learning are especially likely to interact with faculty outside of the classroom. Revisiting Moos ’5 Social Ecological Framework The findings of the current study illustrate the importance of using an ecological approach to examining the impact of sub-environments on students, especially in residential colleges and living-learning programs, as students bring many socio- demographic and motivational characteristics into these environments that are instrumental to their learning and development outcomes. In this section, I frame the findings using Moos’s model as a guide, critique the model in light of the findings, and discuss where the model was particularly useful. Figure 5.1 provides a visual of Moos’s model, including an illustration of how my research questions pertained to each aspect of the model. I began my investigation by examining whether there was variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning across residential college environments (Research question 1). By conducting the one-way AN OVA with random effects, I partitioned the variance in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning into within-residential college and between-residential college components. 129 Res. Question 4 \ \ \ \ Res. Question 3 \ l‘ ‘ / I \\ / I \ \ . A I 1 \ t \\ system V fl \\ Res. Question 1 V \\ v Student \ Stability ‘ Cognitiv. Activation mm B .t ‘ | Ads tlon Appraisal :5 °' Arousa d 3:14:22.)an : Change ‘\ A Personal \\ : l‘ 1 system \ l / / / A 4 \ 5 ’ / Res. Question 1 4 \\ 1 7 1 / / A f \ l / / ““““ \ l /// ~ ‘ g & Res. Question 2 Figure 5.1: Relationship between my Research Questions and Moos (1979) Model of the Relationship between Environmental and Personal Variables and Student Stability and Change The partitioning of variance provided a foundation for examining the personal and environmental systems and their association with students’ appraisal of their environment, motivation, and inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, each of which I detail below. The Environmental System. To assess the environmental system (research question 4), I used the group mean of the liberal arts experience variables for each residential college. Having already controlled for students’ individual perceptions (cognitive appraisal) of the residential college environment at level-one of the model, in assessing the environmental system, I was interested in the overall ethos of the 130 environment specifically related to the liberal arts experiences. The liberal arts experiences focused most heavily on what Moos would call the organizational factors, human aggregate, and social climate of the residential college setting (these were described in more detail in chapter one). The physical setting was more implicit, and may have affected students’ assessment of their environment in terms of studying with peers, involvement in activities, and interacting with faculty. In addition to assessing how an overall ethos of the environment related to the liberal arts was associated with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, Moos’s model illustrated the potential that the degree of the association between students’ pre-college characteristics, motivation, and individual liberal arts experiences and the outcome variables may have varied across residential colleges (research question 3). Furthermore, it demonstrated the potential importance of exploring the cross-level interactions between the personal system and the environmental system (research question 5), as the environmental system may have had an indirect association with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning by way of their motivation and degree aspirations. For the most part there was little practical significance in the degree of variation in the association between students’ pre-college characteristics, motivation, and individual liberal arts experiences “and the outcome variables by residential college context (research question 3). Perhaps if there were more variation in the types of communities examined (e. g., living-learning communities in addition to residential colleges), the degree of these associations would have varied more widely across residential colleges. Likewise, there was little practical significance in the cross-level 131 interactions between the environmental variables and the motivation variables, as the only significant cross-level interaction was the negative association between students’ motivation and the ‘classroom practices’ component of the good teaching and high quality interactions with faculty scale (discussed in detail in chapter 4). Again, the minimal cross level associations were perhaps a result of the fact that there was not a tremendous amount of variation across the residential college contexts. These relationships seem more important to consider when the environmental contexts are more distinct. The Personal System. To assess the personal system, I built within-residential college/individual (level-one) models examining the association between students’ sociodemographic and pre-college characteristics and the outcome variables. Moos explained that such characteristics may influence the way that students may perceive their environments and respond to them. As I discussed in the findings, the first two blocks of variables I entered into the level-one model explained little of the variation within residential college environments. Moos’ suggestion that additional personal characteristics, including “attitudes[and] expectations” may provide additional insight into the personal system, and should be considered when investigating the roles of the personal system in future studies. Mediating Factors. I added two blocks of variables representing Moos’s mediating factors. The first was students’ cognitive appraisal of their environment, which Moos (1979) described as students’ evaluation of their environment as being “potentially harmful, beneficial, or irrelevant” (p.11). I deemed these the individual liberal arts experience variables and explored their association with students’ inclination to inquire 132 and capacity for lifelong learning at level one of the model. As I mentioned in my discussion of the findings, the addition of students’ cognitive appraisal of their environment enabled me to ascertain the association between students’ individual experiences with the liberalarts variables and their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, which I could then compare to the environment (level-two) results. Furthermore, the addition of these variables at level-one served to control for students’ individual beliefs about the environment, allowing me to hone in on the environmental system. The second mediating factor represented by the model was students’ activation or arousal, or what I deemed students’ motivation, degree aspirations, and college experiences. The addition of these variables to the model explained the most within- residential college variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning and was clearly an important consideration of the personal system. These variables were mediated to a lesser extent by the environmental system, as only the degree of association between students’ desire to obtain more than a bachelor’s degree varied by residential context (illustrated by research question 3, and also mentioned above). " Eflorts at Adaptation. Moos explained that common transitions and every day situations demand coping and adaptation responses, and that coping and adaptation are not only mediators of outcomes, but are also outcomes themselves. My interest was in examining students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong leaming in light of their personal characteristics and residential college environments. As a result, in the current study, the outcome variables (students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for 133 lifelong learning) are considered students’ efforts at adapting to the collegiate environment and preparing for lifelong learning. As such, they are associated with the environmental and personal system and mediated by students’ cognitive appraisal and motivation. Student Stability and Change. Assessing the stability or change of specific students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning was outside the scope of the current study. However, by including a cross-sectional sample, I was able to ascertain that there was a statistically significant association between students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning and the number of years they lived in a residential college. However, as I mentioned in my discussion, more research is needed to determine whether the association is as a result of the residential college environment or merely and indication of students’ maturation. Regardless, the findings illustrate that the students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning may deepen as they mature. In summary, Moos’s model provided a useful framework for examining the relationship between the environmental system, personal system, and students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. The model allowed for the consideration of a confluence of personal and environmental factors to be associated with students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. In addition, the inclusion of mediating factors (including activation or arousal and cognitive appraisal) was important as well, as there were statistically significant associations between these factors and students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Perhaps less important to the current study was the consideration that the degree of variation in the 134 outcome variables may have changed across environmental context and the exploration of cross-level interaction. However, if the residential college environments were more distinct from one another (as they often are in living-learning community research) these considerations may have been more important. Additional studies could also make better use of Moos’s focus on the stability or change of these outcomes over time by taking a longitudinal approach. With my discussion of the findings and their relationship to existing literature and Moos’s model complete, I turn now to the implications of the study. Implications for Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy In light of the findings of the current study and the relationship of these findings with existing research, I offer implications for theory, research, practice, and policy. I also detail suggestions for future research. Implications for Theory The findings of the current study offer an important contribution to higher education theory by reinforcing the calls of researchers (Pascarella & Terenzini, 2005; Renn, 2003, 2004; Renn & Arnold, 2003; Strange & Banning, 2001) to employ ecological models to understand student outcomes, especially when seeking to understand the influence of collegiate sub-environments on student learning and development. As was illustrated by partitioning the variance in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning into within-residential college and between- residential college components, there was a greater amount of variation within residential college environments than between them. F urtherrnore, almost half of the variation in the outcome variables between residential college environments was attributable to 135 individual sociodemographic characteristics that were clustered within certain residential college environments. If the different levels of analysis were not accounted for, the environmental influence may have been overstated, because of these clustered individuals. Likewise, by examining solely the characteristics that students bring to their sub-environment, researchers miss an opportunity to explore and make meaning of how different interventions are associated with deepening student outcomes. The ecological approach enables researchers to have the best of both worlds, in that they can examine and explore personal and environmental considerations together, acknowledging that these components are constantly interacting and informing one another. By employing an ecological approach, researchers will gain a better understanding of the influence of students and their environments in promoting student learning and development. Implications for Research The contributions of the current study to higher education research are also worth noting. First, the current study continues the departure away from much of the early research on living-learning communities and residential colleges, which sought to I ascertain whether these environments were more effective in promoting student outcomes than no intervention (i.e., Pasque & Murphy, 2005; Pike et al., 1997; Pike, 1999). The next generation of living-learning community and residential college research focuses instead on ascertaining which aspects of the residential college environment are most effective in promoting student outcomes and why. In addition, the study is the first to examine the effectiveness of these environments in promoting the liberal arts outcomes they purport to emulate. In part, the current study supports the notion that the more closely aligned a residential college is with promoting liberal arts experiences (such as 136 academic challenge and high expectations and out-of-class interactions with faculty) the more likely students are to report higher liberal arts outcomes (in this case, a deeper inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning). However, the current study also raises additional questions about whether the emphasis of residential college environments in promoting deep peer relationships actually translates to deeper student learning. More research is needed to understand the role of peers in these settings beyond that they provide students’ a sense of social integration. A second research implication advanced by the current study is the conceptual distinction between students’ inclination to inquire, or their value for learning, and their capacity for lifelong learning. The results of the current study indicate that these two outcomes are distinct and associated with different sociodemographic characteristics and liberal arts experiences. Thus, care should be taken to define them accurately when conducting research. As I discussed in Chapter 2, existing research using the ‘need for cognition’ scale has used different terminology to describe the outcome associated with the scale, including ‘need for cognition’ (Blaich & Wise, 2008; Cacioppo & Petty, 1982), ‘inclination to inquire and lifelong learning’ (Seifert, et al., 2008), and ‘lifelong learning orientation’ (Mayhew, Wolniak, & Pascarella, 2008). With the results of the current study illustrating that there are different sociodemo graphic characteristics, experiences, and environments associated with students’ inclination to inquire (value) and capacity for lifelong learning (skill set), it is vital that researchers reflect these differences when defining their constructs how they relate to a deepened value for learning and capacity for lifelong learning. 137 In addition to taking care when defining constructs related to students’ value for learning and capacity for lifelong learning, another implication for research is that the results of the current study raise questions about the ‘need for cognition’ scale itself, which was used to measure students’ inclination to inquire. The factor analysis (discussed in chapter 3) indicated that for the current sample, several factor loadings of the scale were surprisingly low (.153, .169, and .180), especially when one considers how much empirical testing of the scale has been done on the scale (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982; Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein, & Jarvis, 1996). However, in examining those items that were low (and which I ultimately opted to exclude), namely, “I feel relief rather than satisfaction after completing a task that required a lot of mental thought,” I only think as hard as I have to,” and, “I prefer to think about small daily projects to long-term ones (recoded),” I wondered whether students’ answers to these questions were reflective more of their attempts to cope with a demanding and intellectually-pressured collegiate environment and less about their enjoyment and value for thinking. In fact, one of the items, “I prefer to think about small daily projects to long-term ones (recoded),” is often included in time management advice that college and university administrators give to students (see for example, Campus Calm website, n. d.; George Washington University Academic Success Center, n. d.; University of Kansas Counseling and Psychological Services Website, n. (1). Another issue raised by the results of the current study about the need for cognition scale is whether the scale appropriately captures Asian-American and women students’ enjoyment of thinking. The questions raised about potential bias in the need for cognition scale, coupled with the low factor loadings of several items are especially alarming when one considers 138 that the scale is currently widely used in the Wabash National Study of Liberal Arts Education to conduct research and assessment on the extent to which a variety of postsecondary institutions are successful in developing students’ inclination to inquire. More research is warranted to determine whether the questions raised about the scale in the current study extend to the findings of the Wabash study, and if so, whether caution should be taken in interpreting the results of that study. Implications for Practice — Prospective Students, Administrators, and Faculty The results of the current study provide parents and prospective students with additional information to consider when making the decision to enter a residential college. First, the results illustrate that there is substantially more variation in students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning within residential college environments than between them. The implication of this finding is that in addition to choosing a residential college based upon its educational offerings, prospective students should get to know students living in the residential college and determine whether these students have a similar level of motivation and eagerness to learn as the prospective student. In addition, as parents and prospective students weigh the educational offerings of aresidential college, the results of the current study demonstrate the importance of looking for an environment that has is a scholarly ambiance with high expectations of students. Again, in addition to examining course offerings, prospective students may be able to gauge the atmosphere by talking to students living in the residential college in order to get a sense of their perceptions of the environment. 139 Finally, prospective students should meet the faculty in the residential college and ask them about opportunities for interaction outside of class. These opportunities might include working on research projects with faculty, participating in service-learning projects, or engaging in some other type of co-curricular learning opportunity. By having semi-structured activities to participate in with faculty, students may engage with faculty in a meaningful way. In addition to providing important information to students and parents, the results offer an important contribution to higher education administrative and faculty practice, both within residential colleges and within postsecondary institutions more broadly. There are several findings that offer clear evidence of areas in which concentrated attention could potentially enhance students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning, especially in the realms of student advising and support and faculty and student interactions. Student Advising and Support The current study offers evidence that residential college environments, to the extent that they provide a challenging academic atmosphere and offer out-of-class interactions with faculty, are associated with deepening students’ inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Advisers and others charged with helping students navigate their academic decision-making can use the results of the current study to assist students in developing a more nuanced understanding of residence college environments. For example, instead of offering a general recommendation that students join a residential college, advisors may give specific advice about how to make the most of a residential college experience, getting involved in activities that foster deeper levels of student and 140 faculty interaction or engaging in activities that provide a deeper level of intellectual challenge. In addition, if an institution offers multiple residential colleges, advisors can assist students in finding one that is the right fit, especially if the student has the desire to deepen their value for learning and capacity for lifelong learning. Ideally, students should be able to weigh the specific programmatic features of a residential college and evaluate how they align with their goals for undergraduate education. The results of the current study also highlight that Asian-American students and women students have a lesser inclination to inquire than their white and male peers. Although more research is needed to determine whether these findings are a reflection of a biased scale or truly a reflection that these two groups as a whole tend to engage in and enjoy thinking less, it raises questions about how the messages we convey to students about the purpose of a postsecondary education facilitate or impede their value for learning. When faculty members, advisors, and other university personnel appeal to historically underrepresented students by encouraging them to partake in activities for strictly utilitarian reasons (i.e., serve as an intern, volunteer, or study abroad solely to better their prospects for future employment), are there ramifications for what students take away from that activity? Are these messages more often conveyed toward students who have historically been excluded from postsecondary education? Brophy (2004) suggests that there is value in helping “learners begin to see the value in potential learning opportunities that they have not yet come to appreciate (and might never come to appreciate) on their own” (p. 258). In order to assist all students (and specifically Asian- American and women students) in deepening their value and engagement in thinking and learning, faculty and administrators must reflect on how educational practices and 141 activities they encourage students to get involved in promote a value for learning, and then work to connect those deeper values to students’ beliefs. Faculty and Student Interaction In addition to offering implications for student advising and support, the results of the current study also have significance for faculty and student interaction, especially related to motivation, creation an environment marked by academic challenge and high expectations, and out-of-class interactions with faculty. Although the association between students’ motivation and their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning was small, it was significant and provides evidence that the more students’ feel a sense of autonomy, competence, and relatedness in their educational pursuits, the deeper their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Existing motivation and teaching and learning literature sheds light on how faculty might deepen students’ beliefs in these areas, including by providing students’ with opportunities to take charge of their educational pursuits, coupled with enough support and direction to do so (Baxter Magolda, 2004; Brophy, 2004), providing appropriately challenging material so that students feel accomplished yet continually strive for excellence (Brophy, 2004; Chickering & Gamson, 1987, 1991; Svinicki, 2004), and by creating a classroom environment that fosters collaboration and encourages risk taking (Brophy, 2004; Svinicki, 2004). The results of the current study clearly indicate that an environment that is marked by academic challenge and high expectations is associated with a deepened inclination to inquire. As I indicated previously in this chapter, there is substantial 142 research supporting the positive influence a challenging academic environment has on a variety of student outcomes. Although research universities as a whole might be too large and have too many competing missions to provide a coherent and appropriately narrow message promoting academic excellence and high expectations, a residential college environment is more suited to maintaining a consistent, scholarly message across all aspects of the college. The first step in this process may be to ensure that there is a consistent and coherent message to convey, which means that residential colleges must examine their missions and educational purpose to determine if they promote academic challenge and high expectations (Kuh, 1999). After the message is determined, it should be conveyed through all aspects of the residential college, from admissions through graduation, by faculty, administrators, and students alike. Whereas an environment marked by academic challenge and high expectations was indisputably related to students’ inclination to inquire, out-of-class interactions with faculty were unequivocally associated with students’ capacity for lifelong learning. Even with individual experiences accounted for at level one of the model, the ‘out—of-class interactions with faculty’ variable rose to significance at level-two. As I discussed previously, because the model is not causal, it remains unclear if out-of-class interactions with faculty deepen students’ capacity for lifelong learning or rather if students’ who have a deeper capacity for lifelong learning are also inclined to interact with faculty outside of class. Regardless, it is clear that if the opportunity to interact with faculty outside of class presents itself, these students are likely to engage, and will likely benefit from these interactions. 143 Cox and Orehovec (2007) examined faculty-student interactions within a residential college environment and developed a typology detailing the nature of the interaction and extent to which it happened. The five types of faculty-student interaction ranged from disengagement to mentoring, with incidental contact, functional interaction, and personal interaction defining the middle. The authors argued that even in a residential college environment, which is marked by an expectation that faculty and students will interact outside of class, the greatest type of interaction between faculty and students is disengagement, as often faculty and students have little common ground on which to build a relationship. The authors suggested examining the cultural norms of the institution to determine how faculty-student interaction is valued. For example, are faculty rewarded for connecting with students out of the classroom? Is their sufficient time built into their positions to enable them to connect with students outside of class? Are there alternative activities competing for student time? Perhaps most important, are there common experiences through which faculty and students can connect? Faculty and administrators seeking to improve liberal arts outcomes in their residential college environments can examine their organizational structures to determine how deliberately they promote faculty and student interaction outside the classroom. Adding such initiatives as an undergraduate research or professorial assistant program may increase the opportunities for meaningful student-faculty interaction outside of class. Implications for Policy Finally, the contributions of the current study to higher education policy are also worth noting. The results illustrate that residential colleges, to varying degrees of success, were able to create environments marked by ‘academic challenge and high expectations’ 144 and ‘out-of-class interactions with faculty’ and that these qualities were associated with deepened inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Although the relationship between these environmental variables and the outcome variables was not causal, it offers specific areas that administrators and faculty might target should they desire to foster students’ proclivity toward and capacity for lifelong learning. Thus, one policy implication stemming from the results is that offering residential colleges as a way to scale down the size and scope of large research universities may be the first step in creating a more intimate environment. However, to create an environment that is marked by liberal arts experiences and promotes liberal arts outcomes, administrators and faculty must also ensure that these environments are challenging and offer ample opportunities for faculty and student interaction. Furthermore, given the finding illustrating importance of creating an environment marked by out-of-class interactions with faculty, a second policy implication stemming from the current study is that some faculty may be more successful in these environments than others. Specifically, faculty who see value in engaging students in co-curricular activities and desire to expand their teaching beyond the boundaries of the classroom may be especially suited to teaching students in a residential college setting. Those charged With recruiting faculty to teach in residential colleges should clearly indicate their interest in attracting faculty with these values to the residential college. They should also evaluate how they reward these values for promotion and tenure. Perhaps the most enduring policy question related to residential colleges is: Do they work? The results of the current study indicate that residential colleges, to varying degrees of success, do promote environments that are marked by liberal arts experiences. 145 Even after accounting for students’ sociodemographic and background characteristics, pre-college experiences, motivation, and individual perceptions of liberal arts experiences, these environments were associated with students who had a deepened inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. Suggestions for Future Research There is much more research to be done to understand whether and how residential college environments promote the liberal arts outcomes they purport to emulate, not the least of which is examining their association with other liberal arts outcomes. The Center of Inquiry at Wabash College has identified several outcomes associated with a liberal arts education (Blaich, et al., 2004) and recently launched the Wabash National Study of Liberal Arts Education to examine how these outcomes are fostered in liberal arts colleges and other types of institutions. To date, the study has not examined sub-environments, including residential colleges and living-learning communities. As these types of interventions continue to gain popularity at large public research universities, it is important that researchers incorporate them into their study designs. It is no longer possible to generalize that all students at large research universities have the same educational experience, as many of them are involved in initiatives that to varying degrees of success, scale down the campus and provide a more intimate educational experience. In addition to focusing on other aspects of the liberal arts, enduring questions remain related to how residential college environments promote students’ proclivity toward and capacity for lifelong learning. One of the most pressing questions remaining is whether the need for cognition scale, which was used to measure students’ inclination 146 to inquire, is effective in capturing students’ value for learning. The low factor loadings I discovered when analyzing the validity and reliability of the scale, coupled with its significantly lower association to Asian-American students and women students, highlight potential bias in the scale that should be investigated in other studies using the scale. Another question remaining at the end of the current study is whether there are more objective measures that might capture students’ capacity for lifelong learning. As I discussed in the limitation section in chapter three, there are legitimate concems raised about the use of self-reported data in measuring college impact (Anaya, 1999; Gonyea, 2005; Pace, 1985; Pike, 1995, 1996). Future research could ameliorate this self-report limitation by including reliable and valid objective measures detailing the gains students’ make in their capacity for lifelong learning. A final question raised by the results of the current study relates to the negative association between the level-one ‘high quality interactions with peers’ and students’ inclination to inquire. As I discussed previously in this chapter, given the weight of evidence suggesting that peers play a pivotal role in learning and development, and the assumption made by residential colleges and living-learning communities that peers are a vital part of students’ collegiate experience, more research should be done to tease out the role of peers in promoting a value for learning in residential college environments. Because the current study and previous studies attending to the role of peers in influencing student outcomes are both quantitative (Pike, Schroeder, & Berry, 1997), is possible that the constructs being used are not accurately reflecting the role of peer relationships in promoting these outcomes. A qualitative portrayal examining how 147 students’ make meaning of the role of their peers in deepening their value for learning might tease out the relationship further. Conclusion The purpose of the current study was to examine the ways in which students’ sociodemographic characteristics, motivation and college experiences, and residential college environments were associated with their inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. The results support the contention that students’ motivation is associated with a deepened inclination to inquire and capacity for lifelong learning. However, moving beyond motivation, the results illustrate that certain aspects of the residential college environment itself, namely the degree to which the environment promotes academic challenge and high expectations as well as faculty-student interactions outside the classroom, have potential to deepen these outcomes in the students’ that reside in them. The current economic climate is making college decisions even harder for students, while also highlighting the need for them to continue learning throughout their lives as they prepare to contribute to the 21St century knowledge economy. It is also forcing postsecondary institutions to take a hard look at the effectiveness of their programs in light of declining budgets. With these pressures in mind, the results of the current study indicate that postsecondary institutions can assist students in developing a value of learning and capacity for lifelong learning by focusing on creating challenging environments that are marked by deliberate opportunities for faculty and student interactions outside of class. 148 APPENDIX A SURVEY 149 Dear Participant: This survey is part of a research study designed to frnd out about students’ participation in a residential college, their motivation, and their feelings about leaming. Your participation will contribute to research on the residential college experience and assist college and university practitioners in understanding how to best support students. Should you complete the survey, you will be entered into a drawing to receive a $100 gift card to amazon.com. There will be 5 gift cards given out. The information you provide for the giftcard drawing will not be connected in any way to your survey answers. The survey should take about 15 minutes to complete. Your privacy will be protected to the maximum extent allowable by law. Your participation is confidential, completely voluntary, and you may choose not to participate. The IP address of your computer will not be linked to your survey. You may refuse to answer any particular question. All responses will be numerically coded for statistical analysis. Any identifiable information will be removed. You may discontinue your participation at any time. There are no known risks associated with participation in this study. You must be 18 years old to participate in this study. If you have any questions about this study, please contact Dr. Kristen Renn, Associate Professor in Educational Administration by phone: (517) 353-5979, email address: renn@msu.edu, or regular mail: 428 Erickson Hall, East Lansing, MI 48824-1034. If you have any questions or concerns about your role and rights as a research participant, or would like to register a complaint about this research study, you may contact, anonymously if you wish, Michigan State University Human Research Protection Program at 517-355-2180, FAX 517-432-4503, or e-mail irb@msu.edu, or regular mail at: 202 Olds Hall, MSU, East Lansing, MI 48824. Thank you for participating! 150 1. Sex: 0 Male 0 Female O Transgender 2. Citizenship: 0 US 0 Other: (Please list) 3. Race/Ethnicity (choose all that apply): I] African-American E] Native American or Alaskan Native El Asian American [I] Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander 13 Hispanic/Latino El White/Caucasian C] International 4. What university do you attend? 5. Class year: 0 Freshman O Sophomore 0 Junior 0 Senior 0 Graduate Student 6. Is this your first year at your current university 0 Yes 0 No 7. Did you transfer from another university, college, or community college? 0 Yes 0 No 8. What is your major? (If you haven’t decided, write undecided) 9. What residential college are you affiliated with? 10. Including this year, how long have you been affiliated with this residential college? 0 1 year 0 4 years 0 2 years 0 5 years 0 3 years O 6 or more years 151 ll. Including this year, how many ears have you lived in the residential college? 0 1 year 0 4 years 0 2 years 0 5 years 0 3 years 0 6 or more years 12. If your residential college did not exist, would you have attended the same university? O Yes 0 No If no, what college/university would you have attended? 13. What was your unweighted cumulative high school grade point average (gpa) on a 4.0 scale? 14. What was your SAT score? (write n/a if you did not take the SAT) Verbal Math 15. What was your ACT score? (Write n/a if you did not take the ACT) 16. What is the highest degree you plan to obtain in your lifetime? 0 Less than a bachelor’s degree 0 Bachelor’s degree (B.A., B.S., etc.) 0 Master’s degree (M.A., M.S., M.B.A., M.F.A., etc.) 0 Law Degree (J.D.) 0 Doctorate (Ph. D., Ed. D., M. D.) 17. What is the highest degree obtained by your mother/guardian? 0 Less than High School 0 High School Degree 0 Some college, no degree 0 College degree 0 Graduate or professional degree 18. What is the highest degree obtained by your father/guardian? 0 Less than High School 0 High School Degree 0 Some college, no degree 0 College degree 0 Graduate or professional degree 19. What is your parents’lguardians’ annual family income (approximately)? 0 Below $20,000 0 $20,001-$30,000 0 $30,001-$50,000 0 850,001-870,000 O' $70,001-$90,000 0 $90,001-$110,000 O $110,001-8130,000 O $130,001-8150,000 0 More than 150,000 152 20. Unique Identifier — (Will be used to identify your first set of answers, were you to fill out a follow up survey. You are under no obligation to fill out a follow up survey, regardless of whether or not you provide the information. - First two letters of the city you were born in - First two letters of your mother’s maiden name (if unknown, put UK) - First two letters of the name of your elementary (or primary) school - First two letters of your father’s first name (if unknown, put UK 21. Ifyou are willing to discuss your experiences in your residential college in more detail, please supply your email address (note, you are under no obligation to supply your email address: 22. The following questions ask about your preferences related to inquiry. There are no right or wrong answers, just answer as genuinely as possible. Use the scale below to answer the questions. Extremely Uncharacter- Neither Character— Extremely Uncharacter- istic of me Uncharacter- istic of me Characteristic istic of Me istic nor - of Me Characteristic l. I would prefer complex to simple problems. 0 O O O O 2. I like to have the responsibility of handling a situation that requires a lot of thinking. 0 O O O O 3. Thinking is not my idea of fun 0 O O _ O O 4. I would rather do something that requires little thought than something that is sure to challenge my thinking abilities. O O O O O 5. I try to anticipate and avoid situations where there is likely chance I will have to think in depth about something. 0 O O O O 6. I find satisfaction in deliberating hard and for long hours. 0 O O O O 7. I only think as hard as I have to. O O O O O 8. I prefer to think about small daily projects to long- terrn ones. 0 O O O O 9. I like tasks that require little thought once I’ve learned them. 0 O O O O 10. The idea of relying on thought to make my way to the top appeals to me. O O O O O 153 Extremely Uncharacter- Neither Character- Extremely Uncharacter- istic of me Uncharacter- istic of me Characteristic istic of Me istic nor of Me Characteristic 11. I really enjoy a task that involves coming up with new solutions to problems. 0 O O O O 12. Learning new ways to think doesn’t excite me very much. 0 O O O O 13. I prefer my life to be filled with puzzles I must solve. O O O O O 14. The notion of thinking abstractly is appealing to me. O O O O O 15. I would prefer a task that is intellectual, difficult, and important to one that is somewhat important by doesn’t require much thought. 0 O O O O 16. I feel relief rather than satisfaction after completing a task that required a lot of mental effort. O O O O O 17. It’s enough for me that something gets the job done; I don’t care how or why it works. 0 O O O O 18. I usually end up deliberating about issues even when they do not affect me personally. O O O O O 23. In thinking over your experiences in college up to now, to what extent do you feel you have gained or made progress in each of the following areas? Indicate your response by checking the box that best describes your experience.* Very Little Some Quite a Bit of Very Much Progress Progress Progress Progress 1. Acquiring background and specialization for further education in some professional, scientific, or scholarly field. ’ O O O O 2. Gaining a broad general education about different fields of knowledge. 0 O O O 3. Writing clearly and effectively. 0 O O O 4. Understanding other people and the ability to get along with different kinds of people. 0 O O O 5. Understanding new scientific and technical developments. 0 O O O 154 Very Little Some Quite a Bit of Very Much Progress Progress Progress Progress 6. Ability to think analytically or - logically. O O O O 7. Ability to put ideas together, to see relationships, similarities, and differences between ideas. 0 O O O 8. Quantitative thinking — understanding probabilities, proportions, etc. 0 O O O 9. Ability to learn on your own, ‘ pursue ideas, and find information you need. 0 O O O 10. Acquiring familiarity with the use of computers. 0 O O O 11. Ability to function as a team member. 0 O O 0 *All questions in this section were taken from the Capacity for Lifelong Learning index and used with permission from the CSEQ Assessment Program, Indiana University, Copyright 1998, The Trustees of Indiana University. 24. Please read each of the following items carefully, think about how it relates to your life, and then indicate how true it is for you. Indicate your response by checking the box that best describes your experiences. Not Hardly Some Neither Some Often Very at all ever true what true or what true true true untrue untrue true 1. I feel like I am free to decide for myself how to live my life. 2. I feel pressured by life. 3. I generally feel free to express my ideas and opinions. 4. In my daily life, I frequently have to do what I am told. 5. People I interact with on a daily basis tend to take my feelings into consideration. 6. I feel like I can pretty much be myself in my daily situations. 0 O - O O O O O 7. There is not much opportunity for me to decide for myself how to do things in my daily life. 0 O O O O O O O O O O OO O O OO O O OO O O OO O O OO O O O O O O O O O O O O O 155 25. Please read each of the following items carefully, thinking about how it relates to your life, and then indicate how true it is for you. Indicate your response by checking the box that best describes your experience. Not Hardly Some Neither Some Often Very at all ever true what true or what true true true untrue untrue true 1. Often, I do not feel very competent. ‘ O O 2. People I know tell me I am good at what I do. 0 3. I have been able to learn interesting new skills recently. 0 4. Most days I feel a sense of accomplishment from what I do. 0 5. In my life I do not get much of a chance to show how capable I am. 0 6. I often do not feel very capable. O OO O O O O OO O O O O OO O O O OO O O O O OO O O O O OO O O O O 26. Please read each of the following items carefully, thinking about how it relates to your life, and then indicate how true it is for you. Indicate your response by checking the box that best describes your experience. Not Hardly Some Neither Some Often Very at all ever true . what true or what true true true untrue untrue true 1. I really like the people I interact with. O O O O O O O 2. I get along with people I come into contact with. O O O O O O O 3. I pretty much keep to myself and don’t have a lot of social contacts. 0 O O O O O O 4. I consider the people I regularly interact with to be my friends. 0 O O O O O O 5. People in my life care about me. O O O O O O O 6. There are not many people that I am close to. O O O O O O O 7. The people I interact with regulme do not seem to much like me. O O O O O O O 8. People are generally pretty friendly toward me. O O O O O O O 156 27. The following pertain to your experiences in your residential college. Click on the response that best describes your experience within your residential college. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Strongly Disagree Agree or Agree Disagree 1. Most faculty with whom I have had contact are genuinely interested in students. 0 O O O O 2. Most faculty with whom I have had contact are interested in helping students grow in more than just academic areas. 0 O O O O 3. Most faculty with whom I have had contact are outstanding teachers. 0 O O O O 4. Most faculty with whom I have had contact are genuinely interested in teaching. 0 O O O O 5. Most faculty with whom I have had contact are willing to spend time outside of class to discuss issues of interest and importance to students. 0 O O O O 6. My non-classroom interactions with faculty have had a positive influence on your personal growth, values, and attitudes. O O O O O 7. My non-classroom interactions with faculty have had a positive influence on your intellectual growth and interest in ideas. 0 O O O O 8. My non-classroom interactions with faculty have had a positive influence on your career goals and aspirations. O O O O O . 9. Since entering the residential college, I have developed a close, personal relationship with at least one faculty member. 0 O O O O 10. I am satisfied with the opportunities to meet and interact informally with faculty members. 0 O O O O 28. The following pertain to your experiences since entering your current college/university. Click on the response that best describes your experiences, particularly in the current school year. Very Never Rarely Sometimes Often Often 1. Frequency that faculty informed you of your level of performance in a timely manner. 0 O O O O 157 Very Never Rarely Sometimes Often Often 2. Frequency that you received prompt written or oral feedback from faculty on your academic performance.M O O O O O 3. Frequency that faculty checked to see if you learned the material well before going on to new material. . O O O O O 4. Frequency that faculty gave clear explanations. O O O O O 5. Frequency that faculty made good use of examples and illustrations to explain difficult points. 0 O O O O 6. Frequency that faculty effectively reviewed and summarized the material. 0 O O O O 7. Frequency that faculty interpreted abstract ideas and theories clearly. 8. Frequency that faculty gave assignments that helped in learning the course material. 9. Frequency that the presentation of material was well organized. 10. Frequency that faculty were well prepared for class. 1 1. Frequency that class time was used effectively. 12. Frequency that course goals and requirements were clearly explained. 13. Frequency that faculty had a good command of what they were teaching. 0 O O O O O O O O 0 00000 CO 000 O 0 000 OO O CO CO 000 The following questions pertain to your experiences in your classes. 29. In your experience during the current school year, about how often have you worked harder than you thought you could to meet an instructor’s standards or expectations?“ 0 Never 0 Rarely 0 Sometimes O Often 0 Very Often ** Items used with permission from Indiana University 158 30. Over the course of the current school year, how many assigned textbooks, books, or book- length packs of course readings have you/or will you read?“ 0 None 0 Between 1 and 4 0 Between 5 and 10 0 Between 11 and 20 0 More than 20 31. Over the course of the current school year, how many papers or reports between 5 and 19 pages did you write?** 0 None 0 Between 1 and 4 0 Between 5 and 10 0 Between 11 and 20 0 More than 20 32. In a typical week, how many problem sets of homework take you more than an hour to complete?“ 0 None 0 1-2 0 3-4 0 5-6 0 More than 6 33. Please click on the button that best describes your experience in your current college/university, particularly in your residential college courses. . Very Never - Rarely Sometimes Often Often I. How often have your examinations challenged you to do your best work?" 0 O O O O 2. How often does your residential college emphasize spending significant amounts of time studying and on academic work?“ 0 O O O O 3. During the current school year, about how often have you asked questions in class or contributed to class discussion?M O O O O O 4. In a typical academic year, about how often will you make a class presentation?" 0 O O O O 5. In a typical academic year, about how often will you prepare two or more drafts of a paper of assignment before turning it in?" O O O O O 6. During the current school year, about how often have you come to class without completing readings or assignments?" O O O O O ** Items used with permission from Indiana University 159 7. In a typical academic year, how often do exams or assignments require you to write essays? 8. Since entering college, how often have exams of assignments required you to use course content not presented in the course to address a ' problem? 9. In your experience since entering college, how often have exams or assignments required you to compare or contrast topics or ideas from a course? 10. Since entering college, how often have exams or assignments required you to point out the strengths and weaknesses of a particular argument or point of view? 11. Now often have exams or assigmnents required you to argue for or against a particular point of view and defend your argument? 12. How often have faculty asked challenging questions in class? 13. How often have faculty asked you to show how a particular course concept could be applied to an actual problem or situation? 14. How often have faculty asked you to point out any fallacies (erroneous beliefs) in basic ideas, principles, or points of view presented in the course? 15. How often have faculty asked you to argue for or against a different point of view? 16. How often have faculty challenged your ideas in class? 17. How often have students challenged each other’s ideas in class? Never 160 Rarely Sometimes Often Very Often 34. The following questions correspond to your experience in your current college/university, and particularly in your courses affiliated with your residential college. Please click on the button that best describes you revel of agreement with each statement. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Disagree Agree or Disagree 1. The courses have helped me understand the historical, political and social connections of events. 0 O O O 2. The courses have helped me see the connections between my intended career and how it affects society. 0 O O O 3. My out-of-class experiences have helped me connect what I have learned in the classroom with life events. 0 O O O 4. My out-of-class experiences have helped me translate knowledge and understanding from the classroom into action. 0 O O O 35. Please click on the button that best describes your experience in your current college/university, particularly in your courses affiliated with your residential college. Never Rarely Sometimes Often 1. During the current school year, about how often have you worked on a paper or project that required integrating ideas or information from various courses?“ 0 O O O 2. During the current school year, about how often have you put together ideas of concepts from different courses when completing assignments or during class?“ 0 O O O 3. During the current school year, about how often have you discussed ideas from your readings or classes with others outside of class (students, family, co-workers, etc)?" 0 O O O 4. During the current school year, how much has your coursework emphasized synthesizing and organizing new ideas, information, or experiences into new, more complex interpretations and relationships?“ O O O O Strongly Agree Very Often ** Items used with permission from Indiana University 161 . Very Never Rarely Sometimes Often Often 5. During the current school year, how much has your coursework emphasized making judgments about the value of information, arguments, or methods, such as examining how others gathered and interpreted data and assessing the soundness of their conclusions?" 0 O O O O 36. Please click on the button that best describes your experience since arriving in your residential college. Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very Often I. How often have you attended a debate or lecture on a current political/social issue? 0 O O O O 2. How frequently have you had serious discussions with student affairs staff (e.g., residence hall staff, career counselor, student union or campus activities staff, etc.) whose political, social, or religious views were different from your own? 0 O O O O 3. To what extent does you residential college emphasize encouraging contact among students from different economic, social, and racial or ethnic backgrounds?" O O O O O 4. In your experience in your residential college during the current school year, about how often have you had serious conversations with students of a different race or ethnicity than your own?“ 0 O O O O 5. In your experience in your residential college during the current school year, about how often have you had conversations with students who are very different from you in terms of their religious beliefs, political opinions, or personal ' values?" 0 O O O O 6. How often have you participated in a racial or cultural awareness workshop during this academic year? 0 O O O O ** Items used with permission from Indiana University 162 . Very Never Rarely Sometimes Often Often 7. How often have you had discussions regarding intergroup relations with diverse students (e. g., students differing from you in race, national origin, values, religion, political views) since you became a part of your residential college? 0 O O O O 8. How often have you had meaningful and honest discussions about issues related to social justice with diverse students (e.g., students differing from you in race, national origin, values, religion, political views) since you became a part of your residential college? 0 O O O O 9. How often have you shared personal feelings and problems with diverse students (e. g., students differing from you in race, national origin, values, religion, political views) which you became a part of . your residential college? 0 O O O O 37. About how many hours in a typical week do you spend participating in co-curricular activities (organizations, campus publications, student government, fraternity or sorority, intercollegiate or intramural sports, etc.)?** 0 0 hours 0 1-5 hours 0 6-10 hours O 11-15 hours 0 16-20 hours O 21-25 hours 0 26-30 hours 0 More than 30 hours 38. The following questions pertain to your experiences with peers in your residential college. Please click on the button that best describes the extent to which you agree with the following statements. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Strongly Disagree Agree or Agree Disagree 1. I have developed close personal relationships with other students since coming to this residential college. 0 O O O O 2. The student friendships I developed in my residential college have been personally satisfying. 0 O O O O ** Items used with permission from Indiana University 163 3. My interpersonal relationships with other students have had a positive influence on my personal growth. 0 4. My interpersonal relationships with other students have had a positive influence on my intellectual growth and interest in ideas. 0 5. The quality of my relationships with other students in my residential college have been friendly, supportive, and given me a sense of belonging." O 6. It has been difficult for me to meet and make friends with other students. 0 7. Few of the other students I know would be willing to listen to me and help me if I had a personal problem. 0 8. Most students in my residential college have values and attitudes different from my own. 0 ** Items used with permission from Indiana University 164 APPENDIX B» VARIABLE INFORMATION - 165 VARIABLES INFORMATION Dependent Variables Inclination to Inquire (InctoInq) — a scaled measure with a mean of 54.45 and a standard deviation of 8.27. The scale was comprised of 15 items detailing students’ self-reports of their enjoyment of thinking (see appendix G for all 15 scale items). The reliability of the scale was .873 Capacity for Lifelong Learning (CapLifeLrn) — a scaled measure with a mean of 29.71 and a standard deviation of 6.58. The scale was comprised of 11 items from the Estimate of Gains items on the College Student Experiences Questionnaire, which measured I students’ self-reported “ability to “learn to learn” and interact effectively with others in a complex, information-based society, indicating the extent to which students have acquired continuous learning skills” (Hayek & Kuh, 1999, p. 4). The scale was comprised of students’ self-reports of the impact of their collegiate experience on the following items: Ability to synthesize ideas; think analytically; learn on one’s own; function as a team member; understand others; write clearly; gain a broad education; become familiar with computers; acquire background information for further education; think quantitatively; understand new scientific developments. The reliability of the scale was .880. Independent Variables —Level One Socio Demographic Characteristics Sex (Male) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for women; 1 for men. Men comprised 32.6% of the analytic sample (n=591). Race/Ethnicity comprised a series of dichotomous variables with regard to the race/ethnicity to which students identified at the time they completed the survey. See below for the dummy variables entered into the analytic models. 166 Afiican-American (AfAm) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for non-Afiican American; 1 for African-American. 1.8% or 33 students in the sample identified as Afiican-American. Asian-American (AsAm) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for non-Asian American; 1 for Asian-American. 21.3% or 386 students in the sample identified as Asian-American. Hispanic/Latino (His/Lat) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for non- Hispanic/Latino; 1 for Hispanic/Latino. 7.1% or 129 students in the sample identified as Hispanic/Latino. White (white) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for non-White; 1 for White. 56.2% or 1017 students in the sample identified as White and this served as the reference group for all of the analyses. International (Intl) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for non-Intemational; 1 for International. 2% or 36 students in the analytic sample identified as International. Race-Other (RaceOth) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for non-Race-Other; 1 for Race-Other, which was a combination of Native American/Hawaiian and Hawaiian or Pacific Islanders. .8% or 15 students in the analytic sample identified as Race-Other. Multiracial (MultiRac) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for non-Multiracial; l for Multiracial. 9.8% or 177 students in the analytic sample identified as more than one race/ethnicity. 167 No Response for Race (NoRespRace) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for provided a response for race; 1 for no response for race. 1% or 18 students did not identify a race. Pre-College Characteristics Family Income was originally a nine-point ordinal variable ranging from 1 (=below $20,000) to 9 (above $150,000). For analytic purposes, I calculated the percentiles of the original scale and used the lower 25%, middle 50% and upper 25%. Famin Income below $50,000 (IncLow) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for family income above $50,000; 1 for family income below $50,000. 25.4% or 460 students in the analytic sample described their family income as below $50,000. Family Income between $50,000 and $110,000 (IncMed) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for family income below $50,000 or above $110,000. 41.1 % or 745 students in the analytic sample described their family income as between $50,000 and $110,000, and this served as the reference group for all of the analyses. Family Income above $110,000 (IncHi) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for family income below $110,000; 1 for family income above $110,000. 29.5% or 535 students described their family income as above $110,000. High School GPA (HSGPA) was a scale variable with a mean of 3.63 and a standard deviation of .321 F irst-Generation (F irstGen) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for non-First generation and l for first-generation. For the purposes of the current study, first generation was defined ‘neither of a students’ parents obtained a bachelor’s degree. 22.3% or 404 students disclosed that neither of their parents/guardians had a college degree. 168 College Experience and Motivation Years Lived in the Residential College (YrsLived) was a scaled variable with a mean of 1.45 and a standard deviation of .730. Degree Aspirations was originally a five-point ordinal variable, coded 1 for less than bachelor’s; 2 for bachelor’s; 3 for master’s; 4 for law degree; and 5 for Ph. D., Ed. D., or M. D. For analytic purposes, I recoded it to less than bachelor’s, bachelor’s and more than bachelor’s. Degree Aspirations — Less than Bachelor’s Degree (DgAspNoBach) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for aspiring to a bachelor’s degree or more; 1 for aspiring to less than bachelor’s. 1.6% or 26 students in the analytic sample aspired to less than a bachelor’s degree. mgree Aspirations — Bachelor’s Degree (DgAspBach) was a dichotomous variable, coded O for aspiring to obtain less or more than a bachelor’s degree; 1 for aspiring to obtain a bachelor’s degree. 15.3% or 277 students in the analytic sample aspired to obtain a bachelor’s degree, and this served as the reference group for all of the analyses. Degree Aspirations — More t_h,an Bachelor’s Degree (DgAspMoBach) was a dichotomous variable, coded 0 for aspiring to obtain a bachelor’s degree or less; 1 for aspiring to obtain more than a bachelor’s degree. 82.9% or 1499 students in the analytic sample aspired to obtain more than a bachelor’s degree. Motivation (Motivation) was a scaled measure with a mean of 89.87 and a standard deviation of 13.45. The scale was comprised of 17 items which detailed students’ beliefs about their level of autonomy, competence and relatedness, which serve as a foundation for motivation (see appendix C for all 17 scale items). The reliability of the scale was .884. 169 Individual Liberal Arts Experience Variables Good Teaching and High Quality Interactions with F acultv Classroom Practices (T eachClass) was a scaled measure with a mean of 42.42 and a standard deviation of 6.42. The scale was comprised of 11 items which detailed students’ beliefs about the preparedness of their instructors. The reliability of the scale was .911. Out-Of-Class Interactions with Faculty (TeachOut) was a scaled measure with a mean of 17.03 and a standard deviation of 4.03. The scale was comprised of 5 items which detailed students beliefs about their out of class interactions with their instructors. The reliability of the scale was .844. Academic Challenge and High Expectations (AcChallenge) was a scaled measure with a mean of 72.29 and a standard deviation of 12.57. The scale was comprised of 21 items which detailed students’ beliefs about the level of challenge in their collegiate environment (see appendix D for all 21 scale items). The reliability of the scale was .912. Diversity Experiences (Diversity) was a scaled measure with a mean of 25.24 and a standard deviation of 6.16. The scale was comprised of 8 items detailing students’ beliefs about their exposure to diversity in their collegiate environment (see appendix E for all 8 scale items). The reliability of the scale was .83 8. High Quality Interactions with Peers (Peers) was a scaled measure with a mean of 22.78 and a standard deviation of 5.38. The scale was comprised of 6 items detailing students’ beliefs about their interactions with their peers (see appendix F for all 6 scale items. The reliability of the scale was .910. Independent Variables —Level Two Environmental Liberal Arts Experience Variables 170 Good Teaching and High Quality Interactions with Faculty Clasgroom Practices (GrpMnExTClass) was comprised of the mean of the level- one ‘classroom practices’ measure for all students in a given residential college. The mean was 42.45 and standard deviation was 1.70. Out-Of-Class Interactions with Faculfl (GrpMnExT Out) was comprised of the mean of the level-one ‘out-of-class interactions’ measure for all students in a given residential college. The mean was 17.25 and standard deviation was 1.48. Academic Challegm and High Expectations (GrpMnExCh) was comprised of the mean of the level-one ‘academic challenges’ measure for all students in a given residential college. The mean was 72.74 and standard deviation was 6.63. Diversity Experiences (GrpMnExD) was comprised of the mean of the level-one ‘diversity experiences’ measure for all students in a given residential college. The mean was 25.29 and standard deviation was 2.00 High Quality Interactions with Peers (GrpMnExP) was comprised of the mean of the level-one ‘interactions with peers’ measure for all students in a given residential college. The mean was 22.90 and standard deviation was 1.53. 171 APPENDIX C FACTOR ANALYSIS OF MOTIVATION (GENERAL NEEDS SATISFACTION) SCALE 172 Scales for ‘Motivation’ Residential college students ’ self-reports of their Reliability Factor ‘General Needs Satisfaction ’ Loading Autonomy a = .746 1. I feel like I can pretty much be myself in daily situations. .81 8 2. I generally feel free to express my opinions and ideas. .775 3. I feel like I am free to decide for myself how to life my life. .730 4. People I interact with on a daily basis take my feelings into consideration. .664 5. There is not much opportunity for me to decide for myself how to do things in my daily life (recoded). .541 Competence a = .745 1. I often do not feel very capable. .832 2 Often I do not feel very confident. .792 3. Most days I feel a sense of accomplishment from what I do. .692 4. In my life I do not get much of a chance to show .631 how capable I am. 5. People I know tell me I am good at what I do. .536 Relatedness o. = .828 1. I really like the people I interact with. .800 2. .I get along with people I come into contact with. .778 3. People in my life care about me. .753 4. I consider the people I regularly interact with to .753 be my friends. . 5. People are generally pretty friendly toward me. .746 6. The people I interact with regularly do not seem .669 to much like me (recoded). 7. There are not many people that I am close to. .569 Overall scale for Motivation (1 =.884 173 APPENDIX D FACTOR ANALYSIS OF LIBERAL ARTS EXPERIENCE SCALE-ACADEMIC CHALLENGE AND HIGH EXPECTATIONS 174 Scale for ‘Academic Challenge and High Expectations’ Residential college students ’ self-reports of ‘Academic Challenge and High Enpectations ’ Reliability Factor Loading 1. How often have faculty asked you to argue for or against a particular point of view? 2. How often have exams or assignments required you to argue for or against a particular point of view and defend your argument? ’ 3. Since entering college, how often have exams or assignments required you to point out the strengths and weaknesses of a particular argument or point of view? 4. How often have faculty asked you to point out any fallacies (erroneous beliefs) in basic ideas, principles, or points of view presented in the course? 5. During the current school year, how much has your coursework emphasized making judgments about the value of information, arguments, or methods such as examining how others gathered and interpreted data and assessing the soundness or their conclusions?* * 6. How often have faculty challenged your ideas in class? 7. How often have students challenged each other’s ideas in class? 8. During the current school year, how much has your coursework emphasized synthesizing and organizing new ideas, information or experiences into new, more complex interpretations and relationships?” 9. During the current school year, about how often have you worked on a paper or project that required integrating ideas or information from various sources?* * 10. In your experience in your residential college, how often have exams or assignments required you to compare or contrast topics or ideas from a course? 11. How often have faculty asked challenging questions in class? ** Used with permission from Indiana University 175 a=.9ll .718 .713 .708 .696 .675 .666 .662 .661 .660 .632 .631 12. In a typical academic year, how often do exams or assignments require you to write essays? 13. During the current school year, about how often have you put together ideas or concepts from different courses when completing assignments or during class?” 14. The courses have helped me to understand the historical, political and social connections of past events? 15. How often have faculty asked you to show how a particular course concept could be applied to an actual problem or situation? 16. During the current school year, about how often have you discussed ideas from your readings or classes with others outside of class (students, family, co-workers, etc.)?** 17.During the current school year, about how often have you asked questions in class or contributed to class discussion?" 18. My out-of-class experiences have helped me to translate knowledge and understanding from classroom to action. 19. My out-of-class experiences have helped me connect what I have learned in the classroom with life events?” 20. Over the course of the current school year, how many papers or reports between 5 and 19 pages will you write?* * 21. In a typical academic year, about how often will you prepare two or more drafts of a paper or assignment before turning it in?M ** Used with permission from Indiana University 176 .610 .595 .591 .577 .573 .472 .470 .470 .425 .387 APPENDIX E FACTOR ANALYSIS OF LIBERAL ARTS EXPERIENCE SCALE- DIVERSITY EXPERIENCES 177 Scale for ‘Diversity Experiences’ Residential college students ’ self-reports of ‘Diversity Reliability Factor Experiences’ Loading a = .838 1. During the current school year, about how often have you had serious conversations with students of a different race or ethnicity than your own?” .824 2. During the current school year, about how often have you had conversations with students who are very different from you in terms of their religious beliefs, political opinions, or personal .782 values?* "' 3. How often have you had discussion about cultural differences with diverse students (e. g., students differing from you in race, national origin, values, religion, political views) since you became part of your residential college? .765 4. How often have you shared personal feelings and problems with diverse students (e. g., students different from you in race, national origin, values, religion, political views) since you became a part of your residential college? .760 5. How often have you shared personal feelings and problems with diverse students (e.g., students differing from you in race, national origin, values, religion, political views) since you became part of your residential college? .722 6. To what extent does your residential college encourage contact among students from different economic, social and racial/ethnic backgrounds?" .527 7. How frequently have you had serious discussion with student affairs staff (e. g., residence hall staff, career counselor, student union or campus activities staff, etc.) whose political, social, or religious views were different from your own? .331 8. How often have you/will you participate in a racial or cultural awareness workshop during this academic year? .302 ** Used with permission from Indiana University 178 APPENDIX F FACTOR ANALYSIS OF LIBERAL ARTS EXPERIENCE SCALE — INTERACTIONS WITH PEERS 179 Scale for ‘Interactions with Peers’ Residential college students ’ self-reports of ‘Interactions Reliability with Peers ’ a = .910 1. The student friendships I have developed in my residential college have been personally satisfying. 2. My interpersonal relationships with other students have had a positive influence on my personal growth, attitudes, and values. 3. I have developed close personal relationships with other students since coming to this residential college. 4. The quality of my relationships with other students in my residential college have been friendly, supportive, and have given me a sense of belonging.* * 5. My interpersonal relationships with other students have had a positive influence on my intellectual growth and interest in ideas. 6. It has been difficult for me to meet and make friends with other students. ** Used with permission from Indiana University 180 Factor Loading .911 .893 .886 .865 .853 .623 APPENDIX G FACTOR ANALYSIS OF INCLINATION TO INQUIRE SCALE 181 Scale for ‘Inclination tolnquire’ Residential college students ’ self-reports of their Reliability Factor ‘Inclination to Inquire ’ Loading a = .873 1. I would prefer complex to simple problems .688 2. I would prefer a task that is intellectual, difficult, and important compared to one that is somewhat important but doesn’t require much thought. .68] 3. I like to have the responsibility of handing a situation that requires a lot of thinking. .677 4. I prefer my life to be filled with puzzles I must solve. .65 1 5. I find satisfaction in deliberating hard and for long hours. .648 6. The idea of relying on thought to make my way to the top appeals to me. .631 7. I really enjoy a task that involves coming up with new solutions to problems. .623 8. The notion of thinking abstractly is appealing to me. .553 9. I usually end up deliberating about issues even when they do not affect me personally. .475 10. Learning new ways of thinking doesn’t excite me much (recoded). .444 1 1. Thinking is not my idea of fun. (recoded) .405 12. I try to anticipate and avoid situations where there is likely a chance I will have to think in depth about something (recoded) .360 13. I would rather do something that requires little thought than do something that is sure to challenge my thinking (recoded). .350 14. I like tasks that require little thought once I’ve learned them (recoded) .300 15. It’s enough for me that something gets the job done; I don’t care how or why it works (recoded) .277 182 APPENDIX H FACTOR ANALYSIS OF CAPACITY FOR LIFELONG LEARNING SCALE 183 Scale for ‘Capacity for Lifelong Learning’ Residential college students ’ self-reports of their Reliability Factor ’Capacityfor Lifelong Learning’ ** Loading or = .880 1. Ability to put ideas together, to see relationships, similarities, and differences between ideas. .800 2. Ability to think analytically or logically. .762 3. Ability to learn on your own, pursue ideas, and find information you need. .738 4. Ability to function as a team member. .718 5. Understanding other people and the ability to get along with different kinds of people. .718 6. Write clearly and effectively. .711 7. Gaining a broad general education about different fields of knowledge. .642 8. Acquiring familiarity with the use of computers. .609 9. Acquiring background and specialization for further education in some professional, scientific, or scholarly field. .553 10. Quantitative thinking — understanding probabilities, proportions, etc. ‘ .475 11.Understanding new scientific and technical developments. .428 ** All items used with permission from the CSEQ Assessment Program, Indiana University, Copyright 1998, The Trustees of Indiana University. 184 APPENDIX I BIVARIATE CORRELATIONS OF LEVEL-l VARIABLES 185 8o. 8... 5.8. .238. Sumo. £6. .8. EEO .8. 90.8. E... :80. £8... $3... 8.8. Ca... 58.. $8.. 58.. E8... Arr!- .88.... 68...- 38.. ......t. .88. 8. to. ERN- :8. Can. .338- .:.48. ES. . $.23 .25.». . From. 58.. 8o.- 3...- 8...- 8o.- .8- CE..- ”.8 8o.- ?.3~. r38.- 38.- .28:- Ar..:- 8.... ..8. £8.- 5< (new... 8.... 8o. 3... «8.. «N...- 33. 8.... E..- AstSf 8.... 80.. F3:- . moo.- m8.- :0... 0N0.- Cup-yo.- v3.- 30.. Swansoz awo- 3°. wvor m 90. u mmo. . GOO.- V3.1 #3.. 08.. 08. NNor og- moo. onor ONO. N8. 30.. NS.- 0N0.- AlevMOFf Goo.- 8......o v8.- v8.1 Aevfigf Asivuhmr one.- Ono.- ‘— Anvkgf Aiiv POOP Ply wk 9 .1 m3... mNo .- 0013—32 92.8.. 08.: NewMWIO. ALVA-yo. alvfiaf .8- 2... 2o ALE-o. 2o. .3... 9... 8o. - 2o. 3... 3.... 8o.- ..8. N8.- Nuo. 38. 8o.- €5.- 35. .8. 2.... Fa:- ouo. 8...- 8o.- Ema. so. ESQ 8.... F33. 38. Fia- ouou r38.- io. - 8...- 2o.- 8.... ..r3 - CE? 8...- 8o.- . FE... ES... 2? 8o.- 2... 8. .3. .33.: Atvono. omo. . Automo- Aivhm r- moo. Emma. - Aavomo.. r 30.. - .33. 3.3 lore... 32.8.8.8... 8. 3 9m. Enos-.830 233:. flood 3.92,... .35 .2. 39.8.: 83.0283.- 5.32.0.2 853 3822 Cum... 8J1 F38. 2o. - F3:- 2... CE...- 2... Cs..- 8.... onw 2.... .ri..- 8o.- r..8~- 2... c8.- 8... 8...- 8... 8.... v8.- 53... to.- .r:8. $8.... .8- so. :5. ...o8. 8.8. 9.... 8o.- 8.... 8.. - fr:- ....2.. cm...- ...~8.- 3...- Cox. - N3. - EE- 8... - CE..- 3 8. - CS..- 80. - . Exo- .:.E... F «5. m8.- .1|l E : «ta. : 83. t 2.3. : 2.3. can as, gags E30 :3: a BE 1 53 z. 83 : on; aosgmbmpaé < :66 < Sod < «36 < :3. 3038.5 :2: Ba casino 28083 a... «F to a... :3 .3. :3 .3 2 2. Sega 223?? . 2... 8rd 1:. hard 1.. 8rd :5 mum—6 «602 809.20. 238 a? 2:835 £30 .3: 8.. uses; 88 53.2% 5.2 .32.: .322...— s. 83 i :3 z. 23 :. Ea €22.25 85 2o: - EOEQE 89o ..................... ..-..- muo_£8mfia§-§fia&<8aon «warm?» 3.332% - 38.33 ”EMS . Sod . 2o... . 83 _. 32 um a 35 as» a. 2:52 2:. word c 12.6 23 wad 2.3 «2.: 859562 8.3.: 8.5.1.”. $2.6 14% $8.16 d5 36 ma .8... ago #5 .032 + _ .23 8:5 + _ E3 5 a .332 + _ .23 c 3.: _ .23 h .032 "n 3932 guy— 3...: =5— ...3... 8 53:3. 195 .2.. and. 6.... 8...? .o... 3.20. $5 m: .2. 8nd .2. 8...... .2. 9...... 22.352. 8.8... 3.5.... .................. .52 322... 8.8... has... E3... 2.2%... 3.2.3 3.8... $3.. .2. 32.. .2. 8...... .2. 82.. .83. ”$2.8 8.3.0892... .2. 8.... z .8. .. 83 2.2 6... 306 .3. «mm... a... 03+ 8%.... .2. o5... .2. .8... .2. one... 350.08.. .2. find. .2. 2...... .2. 8...... 38......8 . 8v... . to... . 53 182.5... .2. 5... a... new... .2. 3m..- o§.§%.m coo N86- :0 hmrdn 3i 3N”: gag-flaunt. .2. 8“,... .2. 83. .2. 8...... §Ea<.§...< «9.39% 3293.3.» 8.3.: 83...... 3......8333 .2. :3. .2. En... e8. 5.58.9.3... $.30 ..m... .2. 92.. .2. «no... fifiuaxmb.§>.a . in... 2338...... ..m... 9... .9330 0.588... d... 8.6 Saunas... 3.0.»?50 - < 8m... 888... 8.33.0. 5.8.... 5.... 2....85... $.30 a... ..s. 2.232 8.... .9...... 3.8.8.. 2 8.3.3325 323.5 . .2.- .2. to...” z. 5.9 .923... [.23. a2 a... law 28.. + id 5.25 .2.... .2... .... . .96.. 5. + . .25.. .23: =3. 8.3.2.. s 3252.. + . .26.. .252 a .25... n: .032 196 :5. v a :3 ”S. V a go ”we. v a a u_.Va < o8.m 83 83 ,3 : 893 < 83 «B. F 3.3% 23$ .5 Sega 98.2 893 $55 3. 52. S. F E. .2 Stat 32.: 8.5:: :13 wood ..86 «8235 82.3.53 5.: 2:. 2:. 8398 E230 auufimoaééa 85.3 2.3m n83 amazon auscuoméfia ~35 :2. 83 3.5 8:335 83:8 3823.; . 33 .2. 08d 2.. 3o.» Agé 88:8 gagsméozom 85:33.5 ousat¢> .. 83. : 9:6. .. 3.3. 28.. a? 808235 £50 .55 t 83 .. 83 a 83 82.32% £35 < ~36 < Sad ¢ Sad 88383. .3: us ”95:20 o§§< a... :3 .2. up 3 .2. a. to 8888.5 393.30 . t. 33 i 83 z. 33 802 .8235. has... 5? flags £30 a“: as 353 88 «sarong .5 .23: 1:22....— ................ ....... fi.»§m§5§.§§%<8&5 «85.6%.: aS~33m - Eonuhmuw 3&3 . 83 . 8nd . $3 on 5 85 50> a. 35:2 2.. 83 t. 83 .1 83 83.9.2 83...: 818...”.— 32.5 d5 32 [am 98 + % £65 429— a: .Em =. _ .23 § + _ .23 :3: =3. 8.3.5 8 San-=2: + _ E3 .082 5 382 “2 E52 197 APPENDIX L FULL MODEL OF CAPACITY FOR LIFELONG LEARNING 198 < 96.7 .m .: ~25. .3. and