WWWWWI N I HWIWHI ‘ (lHHlleWliHl l0 'UBRARY M'ChIgan State l'fiE‘meI\;+\l v1 ”VV'OIL Y This is to certify that the dissertation entitled IMPACT OF SELF ON ATTITUDES TOWARD LUXURY ITEMS AMONG TEENS IN BRAZIL presented by LUCIANA DE ARAUJO GIL has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the PhD. degree in Retailing ajor Professor's Signature ?« 8 ~ 100? Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Employer PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5/08 K:IProj/Acc&Pres/ClRC/DateDue.indd IMPACT OF SELF ON ATTITUDES TOWARD LUXURY ITEMS AMONG TEENS IN BRAZIL By Luciana de Araujo Gil A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Retailing 2009 ABSTRACT IMPACT OF SELF ON ATTITUDES TOWARD LUXURY ITEMS AMONG TEENS IN BRAZIL By Luciana de Araujo Gil The main purpose of this study was to increase our understanding of teenagers’ self and attitudes toward luxury brands in a developing country. We analyzed how social consumption motivations affect teenagers’ attitudes toward luxury brands, how teens’ self concepts can influence social consumption motivations and whether peer pressure affects this relationship; also we examined how materialism influences teenagers” social consumption motivations and attitudes toward luxury brands. We explored the cross-cultural applicability of scales created in the US, for use with Brazilian teenagers. Our total sample consisted of 558 teenagers between the ages of 12 and 19 (grades 7 through 12) from three different high schools in Brazil. We tested hypotheses with structural equation modeling. One of the key contributions of this study is that our results demonstrate that materialistic orientation is a powerful force in developing more positive attitudes towards luxury brands among Brazilian’s teenagers. Also, Brazilian teenagers that have self-beliefs that are clear, confidently defined, internally consistent, and stable have a stronger tendency to resist social motivations to consume, because the clearer they are about themselves the less they attend to external sources and stimuli. In addition, the desire for wealth and material possession is positively related to collective incentives to consume, which is consistent with existing literature. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................. v LIST OF FIGURES ................................................................................ vi CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION ................................................................. 1 Statement of the problem ....................................................................... 4 Objective of the study ........................................................................... 5 CHAPTER 2. LITERATURE REVIEW11 Conceptual Framework ...................................................................... 11 Self ........................................................................................... 12 Materialism ................................................................................. 15 Social consumption motivation ........................................................ 16 Attitude toward luxury .................................................................... 17 Peer pressure .............................................................................. 19 Hypothesis development ..................................................................... 22 Self dimensions! social consumption motivation, and peer pressure........22 Social consumption motivation and attitude toward luxury ..................... 28 Role of materialism ........................................................................ 30 CHAPTER 3. RESEARCH METHODS ...................................................... 34 Research design ............................................................................... 34 Sample ....................................................................................... 35 Instrument ................................................................................... 36 Measures37 Reliability and validity ..................................................................... 41 Procedure ........................................................................................ 42 Preliminary tests ................................................................................ 43 Data analysis ..................................................................................... 45 CHAPTER 4. RESULTS ......................................................................... 46 Sample demographics ........................................................................ 46 Purification of the scale and measurement model ...................................... 48 Collinearity analysis for self concepts ...................................................... 51 Manipulation check on luxury ................................................................ 51 Structural equation model .................................................................... 52 CHAPTER 5: CONCLUSION .................................................................. 58 Discussion ....................................................................................... 58 Implications ..................................................................................... 60 Suggestion for future research and limitation .......................................... 64 APPENDICES ..................................................................................... 71 APPENDIX A ....................................................................................... 71 Survey items .................................................................................... 71 Assent form ...................................................................................... 77 iii Letter of support ................................................................................. 78 Parent consent form ............................................................................. 79 APPENDIX B .................................................................................... 81 REFERENCES ................................................................................. 91 iv LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Top 10 countries where teens live- Average weekly teen spending (USS) ....................................................... 6 Table 2. Pre-test results ......................................................................... 44 Table 3. Demographics variables ............................................................. 47 Table 4. Measurement model results ....................................................... 50 Table 5. Hypotheses - parameters .......................................................... 55 Table 8.1 Scale items and factor analysis ................................................. 81 Table 8.2 Scale items and measurements ................................................ 83 Table 8.3 Measurement model standardized estimates ................................ 85 Table 8.4 Collinearity analysis - Coefficients ............................................. 87 Table 8.5 Descriptive Statistics .............................................................. 88 Table 8.6 Top ten luxury brands chosen by our sample ............................... 90 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. Conceptual Model ................................................................... 12 Figure 2. Research Model ..................................................................... 33 Figure 3. Final structural model .............................................................. 57 vi CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION The economic impact of the expenditure patterns of today’s global teenagers is documented frequently, however, little, if any research specifically evaluates teens' attitudes toward luxury brands in developing countries, such as Brazil; our study reverses that trend. 8y gaining a more complete understanding of the teen market in developing countries, local and global firms (such as Apple, Dell, Maybelline, Levi’s, and Nintendo, etc.) will be better equipped to effectively target this consumer sector. Teenagers’ consumption of luxury brands exists not only in affluent countries but also in less developed countries such as Brazil (Moses 2000; Troiano 1997) where the teenage population comprises roughly 10% of the whole country's population (IBGE Brazilian Census 2000), and represents an attractive market for many companies. Recent market research predicts that the younger generation represents the most important segment that will influence the global luxury market over the next decade (Unity Marketing Inc. 2007). For teenagers, particularly in affluent countries, the ownership of luxury brands is important as a means of facilitating friendships with others and achieving popularity (Ruffin 2007; Wooten 2006), and given that teens are becoming more globalized, this trend is expected to be observed in emerging markets as well, like Brazil. The emphasis on luxury brands is increasing in emerging markets, whereas in the past, luxury companies would focus only on established markets (Allen 2007). A luxury brand is defined as a high-quality, scarce, high-priced, or rare brand (Kapferer 1998; Mortelmans 2005). A study of 20 countries (European countries, United States, Australia, Canada, New Zealand and Hong Kong) by Dubois, Czellar and Laurent (2005) found that consumers in 19 countries define luxury and its value in similar terms. Another study found that middle-class teens from 26 different countries, show an astonishing similarity in ambitions and they crave a comfortable life, indicating that possession of the latest consumer goods is important, and because of their age, peer-pressure is significant (Gallant 1995). Brazil presents some characteristics that make study of the teen population intriguing. The Brazilian emerging economy shows signs of an advanced economy, but has not yet fully demonstrated itself to be a developed country. A less developed country, also called a third world nation, is defined by a low standard of living, an undeveloped industrial base, a low per capita income, and a moderate to low Human Development Index score (HDI- is an index combining normalized measures of life expectancy, literacy, educational attainment, and GDP per capita for countries worldwide) (Mouton and Waast 2006). However, Brazil is a paradoxical country because despite its strong annual GDP growth of 5.4% (Cia.gov 2008), poverty levels remain above 30% (Cia.gov 2008). Brazil’s rapid emergence as a global consumer power makes it an important case study of consumer trends in a developing country. An indication of Brazil’s growth is that its stock market rose more than 70 percent in 2007, while maintaining a steady inflation rate of 4.2 % per year (Cohen 2008). From 2000 to 2008, Brazil’s luxury market growth was 35% (Strehlau 2008). Specialists from Goldman Sachs even believe that Brazil will become part of the largest world economies before 2050 (Keston 2007). The Brazilian teen population (15-19 year olds) represents 10% (17.8 million) of the total population (slightly higher than USA and Europe, but about the same as India and China), with 77% living in urban/suburban areas. Fifty percent of Brazilian teenagers are middle class (IBGE Brazilian Census 2000; Moses 2000). Furthermore, in an analysis of the “Top ten countries where teens live” by the United States Bureau of the Census, Brazil is in the fifth position in terms of number of teenagers living in the country, (International Data Base 1999 as cited in Moses 2000). Brazil ranks among the top three of 23 countries (among the top are the United States, India and Japan) in teen spending of $1 billion or more annually (Moses 2000). Research shows that Brazilian teenagers have been increasing their purchases of luxury brands (Saad 2007), and their yearly general expenditures represent 10% of the country’s gross domestic product- GDP (Bruin 2003). In a cross-cultural study among eight other countries, Brazilian teens were rated as the number one group that most enjoys shopping (Quart 2003). In the case of Sao Paulo state, the largest economy of South America, 78% of the teenagers prefer spending on clothes and accessories rather than food, snacks and footwear (Brasil News 2000). Brazilian teens’ perceptions of luxury are important to study because of their increasing preferences for luxury brands and because of the increasing attention that Brazil is receiving as a player in the global economy. Statement of the problem Adolescence is often a complicated, transitory period, usually characterized by rapid biological, social and cognitive growth. Teens deal with a contradictory ideology; they want to create an individual identity but at the same time, still connect with their own group of friends (Gulland 2006). In this phase, teenagers are continually learning how to behave appropriately in a new situation or phase of their lives (Steinberg 1993). Not surprisingly, periods of transition are often accompanied by the need for a variety of products or services to ease the transition (Hartman et al. 2006). Not much is known about the spending motivations of this young group of powerful consumers, who just a decade ago, were not perceived to be particularly economically important (LaFerle, Edwards, and Lee 2000). While teens do not earn large salaries themselves and teens have little income compared with adults, they do receive money from allowances or from temporary jobs and they have more disposable income than adults because most adolescents do not have to pay health insurance, credit card bills, mortgages/rent, supermarket or even utility payments (Zollo 1999). Consequently, teens’ consumer behavior has gained importance as a research topic of study due to teenager’s disposable income expenditure levels. Adolescence implies the realization that teens experience different role requirements that often require different behaviors (Hopson and Adams 1976). According to Moses (2000), teenagers’ spending focuses on specific brands that their peers use, sometimes called the “right brands.” Teens’ identities are still being shaped at this stage in their lives, so having the brands that are considered to be the “right” ones represent a way to fit in the “right” group (Moses 2000, p.27). Also, adolescents may seek material possessions for a sense of security when they face stressful situations (Chang and Arkin 2002). For those reasons, teenagers may not consider the financial consequences of their purchases nor their impact on the family budget; this may become problematic over time (Bristol and Mangleburg 2005) since 46 percent of teens do not keep any record of their spending (Hovanesian 1999). Marketers recognize that teens represent a target audience in which consumption patterns are always changing and, for that reason, teens are difficult to evaluate as a target market (Fields 1999). Despite their changing consumption patterns, teenagers continue to increase their buying power (Hovanesian 1999; MMR 2000) which places them among today’s most desired consumers. In addition, according to the spokeswoman for YM magazine (now teenvogue.com), teenagers represent one of the most influential trendsetter groups in society (as cited in MMR 2000). Objectives of the study According to Peter Zollo [of Teenage Research Unlimited (cited in MMR 2000)], the reason companies focus on teenage wishes and needs might be related to the fact that teens shop more and save less than adults (Villaire 2004). In fact, some teens exhibit a tendency to spend a lot of money, especially those Who have been raised during prosperous times. Due to the growing spending power of teenagers, global marketers are now trying to understand teenagers’ wishes and needs in order to relate to such an attractive consumer base. Recent data show that teens around the globe spent more than US$169 billion in 2004 (Teenage Research Unlimited 2004). It is remarkable to observe that weekly teen spending in Brazil exceeds levels found in Sweden, especially if one considers the difference in poverty levels of both countries: Brazil 31%, Sweden 2.9% (Cia.gov 2008; Schmitt 2000) (Table 1). Table 1. Top 10 Countries Where Teens Live- Average Weekly Teen Spending (U83) 1- Norway $ 49.70 2- Sweden $ 41.70 3- Brazil 3 41.30 4, Argentina 5 40.50 5- Hong Kong $ 38.00 6- United States 5 37.60 7- Denmark 3 37.40 8- Singapore $ 34.10 9- Greece 5 32.90 10- France 5 31.30 Source: (Moses, 2000) Brand loyalty is a behavior found more often in teenagers than adults and teens often avoid taking risks in appearance-related products due to the importance of these products to perceptions of self (Zollo 1999). Branded products are seen as less risky because they are easily recognizable by their friends and they serve as a status symbol; this combination makes them a safe choice for teens (Bearden and Etzel 1982; Wooten 2006). Appealing to teenagers is an attractive business strategy because branded product companies hope that teens choose their products as lifetime brands. As Silverrnan (1998) points out, companies should focus on the teen market given that they are already influencing a huge part of consumer behavior in general. Peter Zollo of Teenage Research Unlimited (cited in MMR 2000) presents some major reasons why teenagers are such an essential market segment and why companies are strengthening their efforts to reach them: teens' growing disposable spending power, their increasing spending of family money, and their rising influence on family purchases. Zollo found that teens are trendsetters in certain categories (such as electronics). Thus, teens are flexing the power of the economy and, according to Tevlin (1999), the growth of teenagers’ consumption power could be attributable to both peer-pressure and money from parents. Earlier studies explored “self” as a predictor of a variety of human behaviors, such as motivation, purchase intention, cognition, brand and advertising attitude (Grubb and Grathwohl 1967; Markus and Kitayama 1991; Sirgy et al. 2008; Tsai 2006). It is well known that adolescents often pass through a stage of identity crisis (Erikson 1959). Some authors hypothesize that adolescents seek self identity through acquiring and accumulating selected consumption objects (Belk 1988). Peer pressure may play an important role in a teenager’s life, leading to a series of specific behaviors, such as what to eat, how to dress and how to speak. Research suggests that teens create shared meanings with their friends, so that their interpretation of reality is reasonably consistent with the interpretation of the reality of their peers (Solomon 1983). Teenagers use their peers as a mirror to align their values, discussing their personal reflections with friends, while looking for support and mutual understanding (Youniss and Smollar 1985). Therefore, any consumer behavior research related to teens should investigate the influence of peers. In seeking to explain how teens’ views of themselves and how the impact of peer pressure affect interest in and attitudes towards luxury brands, we find that consumers’ social motivation and materialism are important factors in making purchasing decisions (Heaney, Goldsmith, and Jusoh 2005). In addition, the level of materialism among adolescents is very high (Bristol and Mangleburg 2005; Larsen, Sirgy, and Wright 1999; Roedder-John 1999). Given that consumers attempt to gain acceptance into social groups through the products which they purchase (Vigneron and Johnson 2004), the findings related to materialism might explain why the luxury brand market has become increasingly important to teens in recent years. In the current consumer behavior literature, only a few studies exist that relate to teen consumer behavior (Hartman et al. 2006; Lueg et al. 2006; Tootelian and Gaedeke 1992), and how peer pressure plays a role in teens’ decision-making (Elliott and Leonard 2004; Huebner and Mancini 2003; Mangleburg, Doney, and Bristol 2004; Shoham and Dalakas 2006). However, there are studies that address teens’ levels of materialism (Banerjee and Dittrnar 2008; Flouri 1999; Roberts, Manolis, and Tanner 2006; Schaefer, Herrnans, and Parker 2004), teens' attitudes toward prestige or luxury items (Beaudoin and Lachance 2006; Flouri 1999; Lachance, Beaudoin, and Robitaille 2003; Nelson and McLeod 2005; Wooten 2006), teens' social consumption motivation (Moschis 1981; Moschis and Churchill 1978), and finally, studies addressing teens' self aspects (Huebner and Mancini 2003; Montemayor and Eisen 1977; Nelson and McLeod 2005). These studies suggest that there are logical connections between the constructs of interest in our research. Rather than trying to explain teens’ attitudes toward luxury by examining the influence of external factors such as advertising and promotions, our study focuses on internal factors of self, materialism, peer pressure and social consumption motivation. The overarching objective of our research is to investigate how perceptions of the self impact teens’ attitudes toward luxury brands in a developing country and to provide theoretical contributions to the field of teenager’s consumer behavior. Using a framework that represents a sum of interrelated fields such as consumer psychology, sociology and marketing, the specific objectives of our study are 1) to increase our understanding of teenager's attitudes toward luxury brands in a developing country, 2) to understand the impact of self on social consumption motivations among teenagers, 3) to investigate how peer pressure affects teenagers’ consumption motivations, 4) to analyze how social consumption motivations affect teenagers’ attitudes toward luxury brands, 5) to examine how materialism influences teenagers’ motivations and attitudes toward luxury brands, and 6) to explore the applicability of scales created in US with Brazilian teenagers. The following sections of the dissertation are organized as follows: Chapter II presents a comprehensive literature review followed by hypotheses. Chapter III offers a discussion of methods, followed by a summary of the pre-test results. Data analysis and hypothesis testing are discussed in Chapter IV and in Chapter V conclusions, discussion, limitations, and suggestions for future research are presented. 10 CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW In presenting a review of the literature relevant to our study, first, we present and discuss the conceptual framework related to self, materialism, social consumption motivations, attitude toward luxury, reference group theory and peer pressure. We follow with a detailed examination of the hypothesis development for the study. The last section of this chapter provides the research model that is the basis for the research, focusing on Brazilian teenagers. Conceptual Framework Conventionally, marketers segment luxury consumers in terms of demographic characteristics (e.g., social class) which are not strong descriptors of the differences among luxury consumers (Dubois and Duquesne 1993). Marketers need to understand the impact of social and personal influences on individual tastes and preferences in order to more richly describe luxury consumers. Our study does not offer an overall evaluation of teenager consumer behavior related to luxury brands, but rather proposes a framework intending to investigate the areas of reference group (peers and social groups in general), self and materialism which have been identified as relevant for influencing teenager’s attitudes towards luxury brands. Figure 1 depicts our conceptual model. We first discuss the exogenous variables of self, peer pressure and materialism, followed by a discussion of how social consumption motivation influences attitudes towards luxury brands. 11 Figure 1. Conceptual Model Social consumption motivations Attitude toward luxury brands Peer pressure Materialism + D Exogenous Variables . “MUD Endogenous Variables The connection between self and consumer behavior has been investigated for more than five decades (Grubb and Grathwohl 1967). Available data strongly support the role of self as an element that helps to establish human behavior, hence, representing a promising area for marketing research (Markus and Kitayama 1991; Tsai 2006). Campbell, Trapnell, Heine, Katz, Lavallee and Lehman (1996) illustrate that early researchers treated self-concept as a unitary entity, a stable, comprehensive view of the self. Contemporary researchers, on the contrary, rely on a multi-dimensional, dynamic construal of the self-concept. For that reason, for our research, we propose to use three constructs about self in an attempt to better capture such a complex aspect of an individual’s life. There are several constructs related to self-concept; for instance there are ideal self, actual self, ought self (Higgins et al. 1986; Sirgy 1982), possible self (Markus and Wurf 1987), self-congruity (Grzeskowiak and Sirgy 2007; Sirgy et al. 2008), academic self-concept, social self-concept, and physical self—concept (Shavelson, Hubner, and Stanton 1976). The other dimensions of self that relate most directly to our study are: self-concept clarity, independent self-construal, and interdependent self-construal. According to parents, concepts such as independency and interdependency are very important during the teenager phase (Bristol and Mangleburg 2005). Other aspects of self such as ideallpossible/academic self may be too complex for our teenage sample and may be less relevant to the study of luxury consumption. Self- concept clarity is a concept interrelated with the concepts of identity and self-consistency (Gergen and Morse 1967) and it indicates an organized structure about the self that is stable and consistent over time. In other words, it is what or who an individual is, perceived by himself, during a period of time (Campbell et al. 1996). 13 The central point of independent self-construal is the perception of the self as an object separated from others, where a sense of autonomy, internal rights, convictions and goals are maintained (Markus and Kitayama 1991). In other words, it is a self view that emphasizes uniqueness. Lastly, interdependent self stresses the relationships with others; it is the dimension of self that desires to maintain connections and harmony and it operates under the premise that the boundaries between the self and others are open and flexible (Cross and Madson 1997). The three dimensions of self are connected with social influences, consumption and materialism; objects (such as clothes, watches etc.) are sometimes used purposively to express aspects of the self to others (Douglas and Isherwood 1979) and materialistic individuals are more likely to use possessions to express characteristics of personal success and status (Richins and Dawson 1992). In summary, the self is the way a person views himself (Krech, Crutchfield, and Ballachey 1962, p. 495-496). The self regulates intentional behavior and permits the person to function efficiently in his social world (Banaji and Prentice 1994; Markus and Wurf 1987). An individual's evaluation of himself greatly influences his behavior, and therefore, the more valued the self, the more organized and constant his behavior becomes (Grubb and Grathwohl 1967). Consequently, this theoretical framework about self informs our investigation of teenagers’ attitudes toward luxury, first bemuse teens’ self perceptions are formed during adolescence and secondly, because teens’ attitudes toward luxury l4 might be a way to express who they really are inside, to the outside world, rather than the attitudes being solely a result of environmental/cultural influences. Materialism Materialism is broadly defined as placing a value on possessions (Richins and Dawson 1992) or the belief that possessions will bring happiness (Belk 2001). It is an orientation which views material goods as important for personal happiness and social progress (Ward and Wackman 1971). Although authors define materialism differently, their definitions share the following concept: that consumption is related to more than the instrumental or functional value of objects, suggesting that individuals seek a relationship with objects whereby they can improve themselves in some way. As proposed earlier by James (1890), a person’s definition of himself is comprised of everything that he can call his. Although possessions communicate personal values such as materialism (Dittrnar, Beattie, and F riese 1995), there are very few studies relating materialism with luxury product consumption (Richins 1994a; Wong and Ahuvia 1998). A study by Richins (1994b) found that materialists were more likely to consume in public if a brand was perceived as expensive in relation to a similar product, creating a link between consumption and personal values. Some studies found high levels of materialism among adolescents (Fields 1999; Roedder-John 1999). This is not surprising since a frequent theme of the modern consumer culture is that happiness can be purchased at the mall, on the lntemet, in a catalog, or on the l-phone (Kasser 2002). The literature on 15 materialism offers a framework that helps researchers understand how consumers become attached to their belongings (Belk 1985; Vigneron 1998). Attitude toward luxury is related to materialistic consumer behavior, because materialistic-oriented consumers rely heavily on external cues (Richins and Dawson 1992) and they understand that possessions (such as luxury brands) signal who they are and what their status or position is (Belk 1985; Douglas and Isherwood 1979). To summarize, materialism is “a value that guides people’s choices and conduct in a variety of situations, including, but not limited to, consumption arenas” (Richins and Dawson 1992, p. 307). The materialism approach uses a framework that can facilitate the examination of teens’ attitudes toward luxury, because many consumers appear to emphasize image and use material possessions such as luxury brands to portray images that display success and status (Richins 1994a; Wang and Wallendorf 2006). The materialism framework has significant implications for consumption meanings, self-identity (Zinkhan 1994), peer relationships (Banerjee and Dittrnar 2008) and teenagers (Schaefer et al. 2004). Materialism affects what consumers expect from their possessions and which products they believe can fulfill their desires (Richins 1994a). Social consumption motivations Notably, within a given society an individual identifies more with some people than others, thus seeing himself as being more similar to some individuals than others. In general, people feel more comfortable associating with others 16 who are somewhat like them: “Many consumption decisions are intended, consciously or subconsciously, to signal one’s place in the social continuum. Clothing, hair styles, club memberships, automobiles, and housing neighborhoods all have some component of social identification associated with them” (Friedman and Grilo 2005, p.2). Consequently, individuals who exhibit social consumption motivation believe that it is important to know what others think about different brands and products and also what kinds of people consume certain brands and products (Prendergast and Wong 2003) in order to blend in better within their own society. Social consumption motivation is the level of importance that individual consumers place on what other consumers think or do prior to purchasing a product (Moschis 1981). Social consumption motivation is related to the wish to make an impression on others, mainly by showing that one can afford high priced and prestigious products (Prendergast and Wong 2003). Particulariy, the issues related to social consumption from the teenage perspective are related to the fact that social relationships, including those with parents, teachers, other adults and friends are influenced by social consumption (Moschis and Churchill 1978). Attitudes toward luxury brands Attitudes in general are an essential matter to study in a consumer behavior setting. Since early 1960, studies investigate how attitudes are developed, established and most importantly, how they can influence human behavior (Katz 1960; Smith et al. 2008). Attitude is commonly defined as an 17 individual preference (Bass and Talarzyk 1972), and a predisposition of the individual to evaluate some symbol or object of his world in a favorable or unfavorable manner (Katz 1960). Attitudes are important for marketing studies because they represent an effort in trying to predict an individual’s behavior and intentions (Ajzen 1991 ). From a historical point of view, the concept of luxury is associated with unproductive behavior and tension amongst members of society, and in modern times, luxury is something inessential but conducive to pleasure and comfort, something expensive or hard to get; luxury is associated with consumer behavior (Dubois and Laurent 1994; Hader 2008; Mortelmans 2005; Seringhaus 2005). “Luxury products are scarce, i.e., they are limited in production and distributed highly selectively...however it is not sufficient that a product is not massively produced, it also needs to be subjectively rare” (Mortelmans 2005, p. 505). A recent study establishes that luxury from the customer point of view is a “promise”, it is a brand's commitment that the customer will be taken care of in precisely the way he expects, whether purchasing a $20 million yacht, a diamond encrusted cell phone, or a $20 chocolate bar (Hader 2008). The target group for luxury brands has to believe that they belong to a select group of people who are able to buy these types of brands. Therefore, most luxury product companies strictly guard their distribution, setting up a sophisticated network of boutiques all over the world (Mortelmans 2005; Ruffin 2007; Seringhaus 2005). Previous studies found that consumer’s attitudes in general are multidimensional (Batra and Ahtola 1991), and the same applies to attitudes 18 toward luxury brands. While Dubois and Laurent (1994) identified four dimensions of luxury brands, the scope of attitudes toward luxury brands in our study is limited to two dimensions: evaluative attitudes toward luxury brands and interest toward luxury brands. Evaluative attitudes toward luxury brands means what an individual thinks about luxury brands, in other words, their general opinion about luxury brands. Interest toward luxury brands implies that a brand is considered to be significant for an individual, meaning that an individual thinks luxury is important and/or is curious about luxury brands (Dubois and Laurent 1994). There are two reasons we limited the scope of study to two dimensions rather than four as proposed by Dubois and Laurent (1994). The other two dimensions are named personal rapport with luxury and mythical/symbolic values attached to luxury, which imply a personal affinity with luxury and a personal knowledge of the supposed value attached to luxury. These two dimensions assume a level of experience with luxury products, which teenagers are not likely to have obtained. The second reason is that we wish to focus on general attitudes toward luxury brands that a broad base of teenagers can possess. Peer Pressure Peer pressure and social consumption motivation are derived from reference group theory. The ways in which individuals perceive different circumstances and interactions are often influenced by the groups with whom these individuals are associated, known by most theorists as reference groups. Reference groups expose people to behavior and lifestyles, influence self- concept development, contribute to the formation of values and attitudes, and 19 generate pressure for conformity to group norms (Bearden and Etzel 1982). What an individual thinks other people think about him influences, in some manner, the way that individual perceives or feels about himself. The focal idea of reference group theory is based on the principle that individuals adopt the standards of others they consider significant as a basis for making self- appraisals, comparisons, and ultimately choices (Dawson and Chatrnan 2001; Venkatesan 1966). Reisman and Roseborough (1955) as well as Parsons, Bales .and Shils (1953) speculate that children learn from their peers important elements of consumption, such as how to select a product and how to shop. Previous studies also indicate that at different stages of socialization, there may be a stronger interest in this process of “learning to consume”, such as with adolescents (Campbell 1969; Park and Lessig 1977; Wooten 2006). In fact, previous findings confirm that adolescent peer and social groups are particularly significant sources of consumption influence (Bristol and Mangleburg 2005; Campbell 1 969). Peer pressure takes place when an individual actively encourages or insists that a person from the same group acts or thinks in a similar way as him, whether or not the person actually wants to do it (Wells 2006). Peer pressure becomes an important issue and a strong force in preteen and teen years, because there is a strong desire to “fit in” (Jacobs 2005; Santor, Messervey, and Kusumakar 2000). As stated by Jacobs (2005, p. 1) “As social beings, individuals want the sense of belonging to a group and fear being an outcast.” In other 20 i— "'W“ words, for better or for worse, peer groups have strong power over adolescents’ socialization and self identity (Jacobs 2005). Peers can facilitate new forms of behavior from a consumer purchasing perspective (Bearden and Etzel 1982). These behaviors often reflect a need for exclusivity in products and services, such as those offered by luxury brands that may not be easily available to the general public (Bearden and Etzel 1982; Ruffin 2007). Therefore, a combination of factors that demonstrate preferences towards luxury brands and easily recognizable products is preferred, especially among teenagers (Bearden and Etzel 1982; Wooten 2006). Boume and Francis (1957), introduced the “theory of reference group” as the basis of the decision-making process for luxury items. Decades later Bearden and Etzel (1982) found that the conspicuousness of a product was positively related to the receptiveness of the individual’s reference group influence and also that publicly consumed luxury products (such as cars, purses, watches) were more likely to be classified as conspicuous products rather than privately consumed luxury products (such as a refrigerator, sofa, make up) which were less likely conspicuous, or unlikely to be seen and observed by members of one’s reference group. Reference group theory prompts the marketer to look at not only how a consumer uses their products, but the environment in which the product is selected and used. For the purpose of our study, reference group theory, as demonstrated through social consumption motivation and peer pressure, helps to explain teenager attitudes and consumer behaviors by illustrating that teens are 21 immersed in a reference group much of the time, even when they are making very basic, daily decisions. In the next section, using the theoretical underpinnings from self (Gudykunst et al. 1996; Sirgy 1982), materialism (Banerjee and Dittrnar 2008; Goldberg et al. 2003; Sirgy 1998), and reference group theory (Bearden and Etzel 1982; Boume and Francis 1957; Park and Lessig 1977), we propose a research model for teenager’s attitudes toward luxury brands that relates materialism, peer pressure, self perceptions, including T self-concept clarity, independent self-construal and interdependent self-construal, with social consumption motivation. Hypothesis Development This section is divided into three parts: 1) self dimensions and relationship to social consumption motivation and peer pressure, 2) social consumption motivations and attitude toward luxury, 3) role of materialism. The proposed research model (Figure 1), where constructs and hypothesized relationships are indicated, is presented at the end of this section. Self dimensions/social consumption motivation, and peer pressure Self has emerged as one of the most studied areas of psychology (Brewer and Hewstone 2004). Self-concept encapsulates personal traits and characteristiee like personality and self-perceptions. Self-concept is what comes to mind when we think of ourselves (Neisser 1993); it comprises how a person perceives himself, and it can reflect characteristics such as culture and social 22 level. It is important to note that most of the previous studies about self exist primarily in the psychological and sociological arena, and that studies connecting self with consumer behavior are recent and not large in number (Wong and Ahuvia 1998). During the adolescence years, the process of building one’s own character takes place. For that reason, examining self-concept can assist in understanding how attitudes and consumption represent a way to express a teen’s individual self to the outside world. The stability of the self-concept can be gauged with the construct called self-concept clarity, which represents the degree of consistency in that an individual perceives himself (Campbell et al. 1996). A well-developed self- concept is less susceptible to the influence of external factors. Individuals with a lucid self-concept deal better with stress and have more healthy behaviors such as positive self-talk and better mental skills (Campbell et al. 1996; Campbell 1990). Past research establishes a positive relationship between high self- concept clarity and some indices of psychological health and well-being such as high self—esteem and lack of mental problems (Campbell et al. 1996; Campbell 1990). The fact is that having a poorly developed or ambiguous self-concept may direct people to rely on, and to be very affected by, external sources such as peer pressure, mass media, social consumption motivations, and other values such as materialism that can influence individuals’ attitudes and decision making processes (Kernis et al. 2000). Social consumption motivation increases with age and maturity (Churchill and Moschis 1979) and although it plays a critical role in the development of 23 teenagers” sense of self, its relationship with self-concept clarity has yet to be clarified by empirical research. Previous findings show that self-concept clarity is positively related to self esteem (Campbell 1990; Erikson 1959) and individuals with high self esteem are less influenced by external sources. Campbell (1990) found that individuals with low self-concept clarity are more susceptible to and dependent on the social environment. This implies that teenagers with high self- concept clarity would construct their own behavior as being separate from the social context. We propose the following hypothesis: H1- Self-concept clarity is negatively related to social consumption motivation. It may be observed that obtaining the support of reference groups (peers), from a popularity standpoint is largely dependent upon the power of possessing specific luxury products (Wooten 2006) and teenagers often feel social pressure to conforrn to the group of peers with whom they socialize. Peer pressure to conforrn can influence how adolescents dress, what kind of music they listen to, and in what types of behavior they engage, including even risky behaviors such as using drugs, tobacco, alcohol, and engaging in sex (Robin and Johnson 1996; Santor et al. 2000; Wells 2006). Thus, peers can have a negative or positive influence on knowledge, attitudes, perceptions, beliefs and behaviors. The positive influence of peer pressure is demonstrated through peer education, as in groups like the Boy Scouts and Girl Scouts, 4-H, Boys & Girls Clubs, other activity clubs, and sport teams (Jacobs 2005). ‘A previous study found that support from peers is related to a positive sense of identity (Meeus, Oosterwegel, 24 and Vollebergh 2002), and that peer acceptance and academic performance are related to self-concept in early adolescence (Harter, Whitesell, and Kowalski 1 992). The intensity of peer pressure varies from situation to situation. Sometimes in the child and teenager segments, an individual gives in to peer- pressure because they desire to be liked or because they are concerned that other kids may ridicule them if they do not go along with the group (Wooten 2006). For instance, clothing is one of the most important status symbols for young individuals (Elliott and Leonard 2004). For many consumers, the desire to impress others, including peers and those in different social or age groups, is very significant; therefore, they will purchase clothing items that are associated with specific brands to achieve the support of others (Prendergast and Wong 2003). Most importantly, even across cultures, a strong correlation exists between the ways in which brands are perceived and the strong desire to own products from these brands (Prendergast and Wong 2003; Wooten 2006). As mentioned previously, peer pressure is a powerful force among teenagers because it has the power to change the way teens behave and care about external influences. Both peer pressure and self concept clarity are forces that emerge slowly from childhood to the teen years (Bachmann, John, and Rao 1993; Shavelson et al. 1976). We believe that peer pressure is a complex issue and can influence even high self concept ”clarity individuals. Furthermore, previous studies found that even teenagers with high self esteem and self confidence are subject to the effects of peer pressure (Michell and Amos 1997). 25 Even though we previously proposed a negative relationship between self- concept clarity and social consumption motivation (as in H1), if there is a high level of peer-pressure, this negative relationship should be modified. We expected that there will be no effect of self-concept clarity on social consumption motivations. Therefore, we propose the following: H2- The impact of self concept clarity on social consumption motivation will be moderated by peer pressure. Everyone possesses dimensions of both independent and interdependent self construal because the self is a complex structure (Singelis 1994). Yet, individuals are inclined to use one dimension of self-construal more than the other to guide their behavior (Markus and Kitayama 1991). Independent self- construal “requires construing oneself as an individual whose behavior is organized and made meaningful primarily by reference to one’s own internal . repertoire of thoughts” (Markus and Kitayama 1991, p. 226). One can conclude that an individual’s behavior is based on his own independent self-construal if his own internal attributes (e.g., feelings and thoughts) determine or cause his own behavior (Markus and Kitayama 1998). In short, individuals with a prominent independent selfoconstrual exhibit a propensity to be individualistic, egocentric, autonomous, self-reliant, and self-contained, mainly because they prioritize personal goals over group goals and perceive a clear boundary that separates the seIf from others (Kitayama 2000; Singelis 1994; Triandis 1989; Tsai 2006). Clark (2006) conducted a study with university students in the US. and found that global independency is negatively related to social consumption 26 motivation. According to Wong and Ahuvia ( 1998) independent self-construal individuals prevail in Western cultures, where there is a belief that unique individuals are naturally distinct, consequently high independent self individuals are less susceptible to external influences in general, such as social pressures. Additionally, previous findings state that high independent self individuals are more focused on internal experiences (Abe and Bagozzi 1996). Based upon these empirical studies, suggesting that independent self individuals reject motivations that are socially generated, we propose the following hypothesis: H3- lndependent self-construal is negatively related to social consumption motivation. Markus and Kitayama (1991, p. 227) express that interdependent self- construal “entails seeing oneself as part of an encompassing social relationship and recognizing that one’s behavior is determined, contingent on, and to a large extent, organized by what one perceives to be the thoughts, feelings, and actions of others in the relationship”. The important tasks for individuals with elevated interdependent self-construal are: fit-in with their in-groups, act in suitable ways, promote their in-group goals, and conform to their social groups. Consequently, the behavior of individuals with a strong interdependent self is a reaction to others with who they are interrelated (Markus and Kitayama 1998). Previous research suggests a negative correlation between independency and social consumption motivations, which leads us to believe that conversely, interdependency is positively related to social consumption motivations (Clark 2006). Individuals with high interdependent self-construal think about themselves 27 in conjunction with others, and there is a sense that the self and others are highly intertwined (Singelis 1994). Thus, unlike the independent self-construal, interdependent refers to the view of the self as defined by assimilation with others rather than distinguishing the self from other people. In summary, interdependent people represent those who assign importance to their relationships with others and emphasize harmony and balance as comprised of social roles and relationships (Kitayama 2000; Swaminathan, Page, and Gurhan- Canli 2007; Tsai 2006). Hence, we hypothesize the following: H4- lnterdependent self construal is positively related to social consumption motivation. Social consumption motivations and attitude toward quugy Luxury is something that many individuals cannot easily afford; however, some consumers continue to make purchases of luxury products, regardless of their financial ability, because luxury products may provide a sense of power or control over others or because the possession of brands may be identified by their peers (Kapferer 1998; Prendergast and Wong 2003; Ruffin 2007). The peer impact is particularly advantageous for younger consumers, who often crave the attention and status, that luxury brands can provide (Wooten 2006). Many younger consumers search for products that they consider to be prestigious, through association with a specific designer, brand name, or an emerging trend; in many ways, these products fulfill some type of social requirement (Heaney et al. 2005). For those consumers with discretionary 28 income, a significant portion of these funds are often spent on goods that accommodate a social need or message (Heaney et al. 2005; Ruffin 2007). Social consumption motivation is related to the fact that individuals want to show symbols of their own consumption behavior to others. Bhat and Reddy (1998) argue that what a brand symbolizes is a significant component of consumer-buying behaviors, as brand image is significant in consumer decision making. The concepts of functionality and symbolism are identified as important to consumers as they make purchase decisions because they integrate specific criteria that may be exclusive to a specific product or brand (Bhat and Reddy 1998). Consumers are motivated by the wish to make an impact on others with their ability to pay for prestigious brands (Mason 1981) and social consumption motivation makes consumers more aware of social cues related to brand consumption. Martin and Turley (2003) established that social consumption motivation predicts the preference for design, variety, excitement, and ambience, while Clark’s (2006) findings show that social consumption motivation positively affects prestige sensitivity. Another study conducted by Clark, Zboja and Goldsmith (2007) showed that consumers’ attention to social comparison information has a positive influence on status consumption. These findings show that consumer interest (if a brand is considered important for an individual) and evaluation (what an individual thinks about a brand) of different brands often reflects a strong tendency to identify with the brand’s symbolic nature and to identify with their own social motivations. For this reason, we propose that social 29 consumption predicts the interest and evaluative attitude towards luxury brands. Therefore we hypothesize that: H5a- Social consumption motivation is positively related to interest toward luxury brands. H5b- Social consumption motivation is positively related to evaluative attitude toward luxury brands. Role of materialism Prendergast and Wong (2003) investigated the relationships of materialism and expenditure and social consumption motivations and expenditure, but they did not examine the relationship between materialism levels and social consumption motivations. However, as previously mentioned, both social motivation and materialism are important factors in making purchase decisions (Heaney et al. 2005). Previous findings positively relate prestige sensitivity with social consumption motivation. Given that prestige sensitivity might be seen as a sign of materialism, one might conclude that materialism would also be positively related to social consumption motivation (Clark 2006). Earlier studies determined that materialism is important in both public and personal contexts, because an individual will be able to identify himself with a particular status group and increase personal development of his own self-esteem (Heaney et al. 2005; Prendergast and Wong 2003). In summary, materialism can influence what motivates a person to consume, therefore, we hypothesize that: 30 H6- Materialism is positively related to social consumption motivation. The emphasis that consumers place upon social status is important in analyzing how status symbols, like luxury brands, are represented in modern society (Heaney et al. 2005). Identification with a particular social status creates new challenges for individuals and their perceptions of materialism; individuals may want to identify with a specific social status and they might not be able to afford the products that represent that social status (Heaney et al. 2005). This is particularly important for teenager consumer behavior because teens have been through their own identity formation and may perceive social status as highly desirable (Bristol and Mangleburg 2005). Nueno and Quelch (1998, p. 62) define luxury brands as “those whose ratio of functional utility to price is low while the ratio of intangible and situational utility to price is high” and Ziccardi (2001, p. 18) describes luxury as “less about items being sold, and more about what the brand name stands for.” Previous findings demonstrate that materialism influences consumption patterns, such as the type and the quantity of goods purchased (Richins and Dawson 1992). Additionally, other studies (Foumier and Richins 1991; Richins 1994b; Wong 1997) showed materialism has a direct positive effect on status consumption. Based upon these empirical studies showing that materialism can influence what a teenager thinks about luxury brands (evaluative dimension) and it can confer an importance to luxury and/or awaken the curiosity about luxury brands among teenagers (interest dimension), we propose the following hypothesis: 31 H7a- Materialism is positively related to interest toward luxury brands. H7b— Materialism is positively related to evaluative attitudes toward luxury brands. The model proposed in Figure 2, 1) explores the relationship between self dimensions and social consumption motivation 2) examines the moderating effect of peer pressure on the relationship between teens’ self-concept clarity and social consumption motivation 3) examines the effect of social consumption motivation on attitudes toward luxury brands, and 4) investigates the effect of materialism on attitude toward luxury. 32 E “E :3- $285 PBS— @838 33:8 gums—gm H: _ @338 6235 BEBE; Bogus-.m— E5 335...; maceowexm E BEE Emzatofiz 3 a 3 $3 a _ E mmHM—mm -2335 many: - 5528 tom Bow—Homewa— SUm __ N: mqouguofi 85.3528 Eoom 3 E 3 E OZOUhdm—m -btflo financed—um Bazaagaoo tum BaounoaoEBE .322 coaewmem. .N 933$ 33 CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH METHODS We presented the conceptual framework and model and the resulting hypotheses in Chapter 2. In Chapter 3 we explain the research design, followed by a discussion of sample selection and data collection. Then, we discuss the measures of the constructs in the proposed model, followed by results from preliminary tests. Finally, we describe techniques of data analysis to test the proposed model. Research Design We used a self-report, paper and pencil survey instrument to collect data from high school students in the $30 Paulo state, Brazil. In keeping with school committees’ policies and following their evaluation and approval of the questionnaire, the administrators of the selected schools first agreed to participate in the study. In addition, we required a signed authorization of a parent or guardian, and also a written consent from the teenager. We provided a cover letter, explaining the purpose of the study and directions for completing the survey, with the questionnaire. This research was cross-sectional, since the focus is on the relationship between different variables at one specific point in time. 34 Sample The population considered for this study consisted of high school students between 12 and 19 years of age (grades 7 through 12). For the purpose of this research, we selected three private high schools in the city of Santos, state of $30 Paulo! Brazil. Although in the US, private schools usually imply affluence, that is not the case for Brazil, where there is a shortage of public high schools. Private schools are available and affordable to all socioeconomic sectors. In order to get a diverse sample, tuition price was used as an indicator of socioeconomic status. The three selected schools represented various tuition price ranges. Sec Paulo is the largest state in the Southern hemisphere (IBGE Brazilian Census 2002). Santos, SP is considered a median city with a population of 418,436 (IBGE Brazilian Census 2007). Santos’ port is the main port of Brazil and the largest port in Latin America (Ewing 2007). In cooperation with each school, we recruited students from approved classrooms. All students were informed that their participation was voluntary (even with their signed parental authorizations) and that any and all responses they provided would be held in the strictest of confidence. We invited all the students in the class with parental/guardian authorization to complete the questionnaire. Using the rule of thumb of taking the number of indicators (i.e., manifest variables: defined as a variable that is directly observable or measurable) multiplied by 10 (Mitchell 1993), the ideal sample size appropriate for the model 35 we tested would be 770 individuals (77 indicators times 10). Considering the limited resources available, it would be very hard to achieve this number; in addition, we are aware that the chi-square statistic is biased in complex models with small sample sizes. To avoid misleading results, Bearden, Sharrna and Teel (1982); and Garson (2007) suggest that the minimum sample for use in a complex model is 200 subjects. Given that we expected a high response rate (around 70%) from this specific population (Flouri 1999), a sample size of around 250-300 individuals would be sufficient to estimate the proposed model. Therefore, we targeted a sample size of at least 300. Instrument Once developed in English, the instrument was translated into Portuguese by a native speaker. Then, it was translated back into English by a different native Portuguese speaker (also fluent in English), not affiliated with this project. The instrument was pre—tested with 50 students, and reliability (see Table 2) was satisfactorily assessed for the constructs, indicated by an acceptable range of Cronbach’s Alpha Coefficient scores close to .70 or better (Kline 2000; Nunnally and Bernstein 1994). Since the items needed no alterations, we incorporated the pre-test sample into the final sample. The full instrument and consent forms appear in Appendix A. 36 Measures Self-concept clarity. Self-concept clarity is represented by a clearly, confidently-defined and stable aspect of self (Campbell et al. 1996). It is a perceptual and belief-based concept. Self-concept clarity scale was developed by Campbell et al. (1996), and has a reported Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.85. This scale is formed by twelve items and all items are measured using a 1 to 7 scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7= strongly agree). Sample statements include “On one day I might have one opinion of myself and on another day I might have a different opinion” and ”If I were asked to describe my personality, my description might end up being different from one day to another day.” This scale was used before in a study by Nezlek and Plesko (2001) with a reported reliability coefficient alpha of 0.98 and by Tian, Bearden and Hunter (2001) with a reported reliability coefficient alpha of 0.89. Independent self-construal: Individuals with strong independent self- construal are commonly identified as individualistic, egocentric, autonomous, self-reliant and self-contained. These individuals prioritize personal goals over group goals and perceive a clear boundary that separates the self from others, where a particular emphasis is placed on personal goals, personal achievement and appreciation (Kitayama 2000; Singelis 1994; Triandis 1989; Tsai 2006). We use a scale developed by Gudykunst, Matsumoto, Ting-Toomey, Nishida and Karimi (1994) and later improved by Gudykunst and Lee (2003). This measure was drawn from instruments used in past research in a variety of cultures (Hui 1988; Markus and Kitayama 1991; Singelis 1994; Venna 1992; Yamaguchi 37 1994). The Gudykunst and Lee (2003) scale has fourteen items with reported Cronbach’s alpha of .82 for US sample, .77 for Japanese sample, .73 for Korean sample and .83 for Australian sample. Sample items include: “Being able to take care of myself is a primary concern for me” and “It is important for me to act as an independent person.” All the items are measured using a 1 to 7 scale (1= strongly disagree, 7= strongly agree). This scale was previously used by Hackman, Ellis, Johnson and Staley (1999) with reported Cronbach’s alpha of 0.85. Interdependent self-construal: lnterdependent self-construal represents a view of the self as defined by assimilation with others rather than distinguishing the self from other people. It is characterized by those who assign importance to their relationships with others, emphasizing harmony and balance (Markus and Kitayama 1998). This scale was developed by Gudykunst and Lee (2003), and has fourteen items; original Chronbach’s alphas are: .80 for US sample, .84 for Japanese sample, .85 for Korean sample and .85 for Australian sample. For the purpose of this study, three items related to work relationships were deleted, given that teenagers in urban areas of Brazil typically do not have jobs. So, we use a scale comprised of eleven items and all the items are measured using a 1 to 7 scale (1= strongly disagree, 7= strongly agree). Sample items include: “I consult others before making important decisions” and “It is important to consult close friends and get their ideas before making a decision.” This scale was used before by Hackman et al. (1999) with reported Cronbach”s alpha of 0.84. 38 Social consumption motivation: Social consumption motivation is defined as a motivation to impress others through the ability to purchase high priced and prestigious products (Prendergast and Wong 2003). We use a four-item scale, originally developed by Moschis and Churchill (1978) and updated by Moschis (1981). Before the items are presented, the participant is instructed to think about this question “What is important to know before purchasing a product?” Sample items that follow this question include “What others think of different brands or products” and “What brands or products to buy to make a good impression on others” all the items are measured using a 1 to 7 scale (1= strongly disagree, 7= strongly agree). Martin and Turley (2004) and Prendergast and Wong (2003) used this scale, with reported Cronbach’s alphas higher than 0.80. Attitude toward luxury. Attitudes represent consumer's overall evaluations of an object such as a product/brand or store, it is a response involving general feelings of liking or favorability (American Marketing Association 2007). We use a short version of the attitude toward luxury scale created by Dubois and Laurent (1994) that fits the scope of our research. Dubois and Laurent (1994) established an “interest toward luxury factof' comprised of seven items, and an “evaluative attitude toward luxury factor” comprised of four items. 0 Interest toward quugg factor was refined to six items because one of the items was not identified as being part of the factor during a content validity analysis. All the items are measured using a 1 to 7 scale (1= disagree, 7= agree). Sample items are “I’m not interested in luxury" and “I could talk about luxury for hours”. 39 o Wive Latitude toward luxury is a four-item factor where all the items are measured using a 1 to 7 scale (1= disagree, 7= agree). Sample of the items are “People who buy luxury try to differentiate themselves from others" and “In my opinion, luxury is too expensive for what it is”. The Dubois and Laurent (1994) scale is the most well known scale for attitudes toward luxury and has been used in several studies such as Tidwell and Dubois (1996), Dubois, Czellar and Lauren (2005), Dubois, Laurent and Czellar (2001) and, Kim, Baik and Kwon (2002). According to Czellar (2007, June 30) who used this scale on her study in 2005, no Cronbach”s alpha was reported because her particular study did not use a conventional domain sampling paradigm (Churchill 1979), but focused more on content validity as advocated by Rossiter (2005). From this perspective, she justified that alpha coefficients are less informative than from the perspective of domain sampling. Peer-pressure: Peer pressure is defined as a subjective experience of feeling pressured, urged, or dared by others to do certain things (Jacobs 2005). Santor et al. (2000) developed a measure based on the previous work of Brown, Clasen and Eicher (1986). Santor’s scale was previously used in a study by Gonzalez, Huerta-Sénchez, Ortiz-Nieves, Vézquez-Alvarez and Kribs-Zaleta (2003). As most measures of peer pressure that focus on teenagers, it includes items that relate to a number of delinquent behaviors, such as substance use and skipping classes. Using a 1 to 7 scale (1=strongly disagree, 7=strongly agree), eleven items measure teenager‘s level of peer pressure. Statements 40 include items such as: “My friends could push me into doing just about anything” and “I often feel pressured to do things that I wouldn’t normally do.” Santor et al. (2000) did not provide a reliability for the scale itself, but all the reliabilities in his study had reported Cronbach’s alphas ranging from 0.69 to 0.91 for all measures. Materialism: Materialism is measured using a material values scale. Material value is defined as orientations emphasizing possessions and money for personal happiness and status (Moschis and Churchill 1978, p. 607). Fifteen items assess teenager’s materialism value, measured using a 1 to 7 scale (1= strongly disagree, 7= strongly agree). The measure was developed by Wong, Rindfleisch and Burroughs (2003) and was previously tested in a cross cultural setting. Sample questions include: “How do you feel about people who own expensive homes, car, and clothes?” and “How do you feel about owning things that impress people?” The reported Cronbach’s alpha for Wong, et al. (2003) scale was US 0.88, Thailand 0.70 and Japan 0.82. Furthermore, a study by Rose and DeJesus (2007) that also used the same scale, reported a Cronbach”s alpha of 0.82. Reliabilny and validity To ensure reliability and validity, we used previously tested and/or modified versions of existing scales. To assess content validity and internal reliability, we pre-tested of the instrument using a sub-set of Brazilian teenagers. 41 According to Bagozzi (1994) there is a widespread cultural bias in consumer research because of the common practice of applying American developed scales to foreign cultures without considering their cultural appropriateness. Thus, a major task of this research is to assure that all scales function with Brazilian’s adolescents in a reliable (given that the instrument will measure the same way each time it is used) and valid manner (given that the instrument measures the underlying concept it is supposed to measure). Procedure We contacted the selected schools and provided copies of the instrument, letter of support, and parental/guardian consent forms. After the school agreed to participate in the study, the scth administration suggested times/classesldays for data collection. Then the formal letter requesting school’s support of this research was signed by the director/dean of the targeted school. We administered questionnaires during class time. One day prior to the data collection, the teacher sent home a parental consent with each student. Only students who brought the parental consent form signed were eligible to complete the survey, after reading and signing their own assent form. The students who did not bring their own signed parental consent form or who did not want to participate in the survey were instructed by their teacher to complete an in-class activity. Teachers did not collect any parental or child consent/assent form nor did they have any access to their students’ responses. 42 We told the students that their participation in this survey was completely voluntary and that they could withdraw at any time. Furthermore we explained that the privacy of the participants was protected to the maximum extent allowable by law and their participation will be kept strictly confidential. Responses were confidential and anonymous; they were aggregated so that individual responses cannot be isolated. The research assistants interacted with the students to collect data during their regularly scheduled class time at Santos high schools facilities. As an incentive, one name was drawn from the classroom participants list to receive a Michigan State University T-shirt; in addition, one Michigan State University pencil was given to every participant. Preliminary Tests To check for content validity, an initial version of the translated and back- translated questionnaire was evaluated by two Brazilian teenagers individually (age 15 female and age 14 male), who checked to determine whether an item belonged to the appropriate category/construct; they suggested some minor changes in wording on five statements. After making those changes, the questionnaire was checked again and no additional changes were necessary. Furthermore, during the collection of the pre-test data, students were asked to let the investigators know if any of the questions did not make sense to them. We also interviewed several random participants who had finished their own questionnaire to identify problems with comprehension, but identified no issues. 43 Fifty high school students from the city of Santos/SP/Brazil participated in the pre-test of the full version of the questionnaire with 67 items, (see full instrument in Appendix A). The data were collected during August 2007 following the same procedures described for the main data collection. Table 2 Pre-test Results (50 participants) Scale Questions Cronbach’s alpha Peer-pressure 1- 11 0.633 Material Values Scale 12-26 0.809 Independent self-construal 27- 40 0.786 Interdependent self-construal 41-51 0.693 Self-concept clarity 52- 63 0.830 Social consumption motivation 64— 67 0.849 The internal consistency of the presented scales approached Nunnally and Bemstein’s (1994) acceptable range of .70 and an increased sample size is likely to yield higher Cronbach’s Alpha Coefficient scores [note that Cronbach”s alpha coefficient of interest toward luxury factor and evaluative attitude toward luxury factor established a pn'on' by Dubois and Laurent (1994) were not reported in this stage following Czellar (2007, June 30) suggestion as mentioned before]. Data analysis We use structural equation modeling to test the proposed model (see Figure 2) (Jereskog and Scrbom 1996). Before estimating the structural model, we established the measurement model using confirmatory factor analysis. We based all analyses on the covariance matrix of the variables under study. As necessary, we made revisions to the measurement model prior to estimating the structural model. Using the revised measurement model, we estimated the structural model. Furthermore, in order to check for the moderating effect (peer-pressure), we first ran the model with no moderator and then we added the moderator into the model to investigate any changes in the path coefficients. The objective was to check whether the paths coefficients were different by adding a moderator; if the path coefficient is not different, we are inclined to conclude that this variable does not represent a real moderator. 45 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS Sample Demographics The total sample consisted of 558 teenagers between the ages of 12 and 19 (grades 7 through 12) from three different high schools (347 from Universitas School, 97 from Sao Jose School and 114 from lntegracao School). Respondents were asked about ten demographic characteristics: gender, age, ownership of mobile phone, products purchased with allowance money, job, parent’s relationship status, parent’s education, grade level, number of siblings and number of TVs at home. As recorded in Table 3, out of 558 respondents, 40% were 17 years old and the proportion between males (44%) and females (43%) was nearly equal. Almost three-fourths of the respondents have 3 or more TVs at home, and nearly two-thirds spend their own allowances on entertainment, clothes and shoes. In terms of school year, 41% were in the 11th grade. More than a half of the respondents have at least one brother or sister (51%). According to Guaiquil (2006) 62.9% of Brazilian teens have their own mobile phone, while recent global research reports that 75% of teens (by age 17) in the world have their own mobile phone (O’Donnell 2007). However, 85% of our total sample owns their mobile phone, even though 84% of them do not have a job. The larger percentage of mobile phone usage could simply represent the annual growth rate since our data were collected more recently than the comparison percentage. 46 Over two thirds of their parents are married (68%) and over half have a bachelor’s degree (51%). Table 3 Demggraphic Variables Frequency Percent Gender Male 245 44% Female 241 43% Age 12 18 3% 13 15 2% 14 50 9% 15 88 16% 16 221 40% 17 151 27% 18 8 1% Own mobile Yes 477 85% phone No 57 10% Number of 0 ( don’t have any) 124 22% siblings 1 287 51% 2 93 17% 3 36 7% 4 9 2% 5 or more 4 0.7% TV’s at home More than 4 NS 219 39% 3-4 194 35% 2-3 98 18% 1-2 31 6% Only one TV 6 1% None 3 0.5% Allowance spend Entertainment 207 37% Clothes/shoes 163 29% Food/snacks 48 9% School needs 8 1% Cosmetics 18 3% Others 108 19% Have a job Yes 76 14% No 467 84% Parents status Married 381 68% Separated 67 12% Divorced 60 11% Never Married 17 3% Don’t know/none of the 28 5% above Education parents Undergraduate 14 3% Bachelor’s 76 14% Graduate Degree 285 51 % Don’t know/none of the above 135 24% 47 Purification of the scale and measurement model In order to assess scale fit we used LISREL 8.70 software from SSSI Scientific Software (Jdreskog and Scrbom 2004) to perform a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) on each scale, except on the scales related to attitudes toward luxury brands: interest and evaluative, because as mentioned before in Chapter 3, this particular scale did not use the conventional domain sampling paradigm. After this first run of CFA in LISREL we chose to remove two items: self concept clarity number 68, and materialism number 36, because these two items were not significantly loading onto their factors. Then, we proceeded with our first round of structural equation modeling. Initial results of the analysis indicated that our main model converged, but due to the sheer number of indicators involved, the loadings of the indicators on their factors were not significant, indicating that we needed to purify our scales further. A high number of indicators can cause estimation problems as acknowledged by Diamantapoulus and Wrnklhofer (2001, p. 272) “the excessive number of indicators is undesirable because of both the data collection demands it imposes and the increase in the number of parameters when the construct is embedded within a broader structural model (eg. in a LISREL context)” In order to further refine our scales and have fewer numbers of indicators one should perform an extra round of confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). Most researchers, when dealing with scales already tested and publicly well known (like our case), perform only confirmatory factor analysis followed by a structural equation modeling (CFA-> SEM) with no major problems, but because we have 48 an excessive number of indicators we added an extra step; this is a very common procedure as reported by previous authors (Nyamathi, Stein, and Brecht 1995; Prajogo and Hong 2008; Prajogo and McDermott 2005). CFAs were estimated through using the method of maximum likelihood, oblique rotation. For each construct, first we systematically removed items with factor loading scores below 0.4 (Hair et al. 1998). Second, we analyzed semantic differences, and third we analyzed the percent of variance explained by each construct. The scales used to measure the latent constructs in the model are provided in Appendix B, Table 8.1. There were a high number of items that loaded significantly onto the materialism factors; thus, we relied more heavily on examining the semantics of each item in determining the final items for this factor. We decided that semantically the results of a two composite score for the materialism construct was coherent and decided to maintain only a composite score (of eight indicators divided in two groups) for the materialism construct as suggested by Anderson and Gerbing (1988). Since our research does not focus on nor have implications for drug usage, we deleted the two items (number 10 and number 11) from the peer pressure scale that were related to drugs. Also, after analyzing each item semantically, we determined that items 1 to 4 were the most representative items of this group, and given that they were loading together, we maintained them as the core items for this construct. 49 ‘ In accordance with the two-step approach advocated by Anderson and Gerbing (1988), before testing the hypothesized relationships between the latent constructs in a structural equation model (SEM), we first estimated the measurement model using LISREL 8.70 software from SSSI Scientific Software (Jbreskog and Scrbom 2004). This allowed us to assess the unidimensionality and reliability of the scales, which represents a way to double check whether the factors are loading properly. We tested the resulting 32-item, 8-factor measurement model to assess the fit (Table 4), convergent validity and internal consistency reliability of the measures (Appendix B, Table 8.2, Table 8.3), and descriptive statistics (Appendix B, Table 8.5). Table 4 Measurement Model Results Fit Indices Value Chi- squared (df=436) 1094.10 P-value .000 Comparative fit Index (CF I) .91 Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) .052 Goodness of fit index (GFI) .89 A review of the results of the measurement model indicates a good fit for the data. All scale reliabilities approached the cutoff of 0.6 established by Bagozzi and Yi (1988) (Appendix B, Table 8.2). Corrected item-total correlations approached the guideline of 0.3 (Field 2005) demonstrating that all scales were reliable (Appendix B, Table 8.2). Convergent validity was assessed by examining 50 the magnitude and sign of the loadings of the observed variables onto their respective latent factors (Appendix B, Table 8.3). Each loading was in the anticipated direction and magnitude, and each loading was significantly different from zero (p<.05). Collinearity Analysis for Self Concepts Because interdependent self construal, self-concept clarity and independent self construal are conceptually similar constructs, we wanted to establish that the estimated coefficients were not inflated due to multicollinearity. Therefore, we analyzed collinearity for the three constructs. The variance inflation factor (VI F) indicates how much of the variance is inflated (estimated coefficients are inflated when multicollinearity exists) and is a measure of how much the variance of the estimated regression coefficient is ”inflated” by the existence of correlation among the predictor variables in the model. A VIF of 1 means that there is no correlation among the predictors and hence the variance is not inflated. The general rule of thumb is that VIFs exceeding 4 warrant further investigations, while VlFs exceeding 10 are signs of serious multicollinearity (Simon 2007). All the VIP values in our analysis were between 1.215 and 1.765 (Appendix 8, Table 8.4) indicating that multicollinearity did not represent a threat to our analysis. Manipulation check on luxury In order to check whether the subjects understood the concept of luxury, we posed an open-ended item in our questionnaire. First we defined luxury 51 brands: “’Luxury brands’” represent those which are special and superior, commonly more expensive than ‘non-luxury brands’. Luxury brands are those you desire to own.” Then we instructed the subjects to “Think about your favorite luxury brand and record its name under this section. In case you have more than one favorite luxury brand, pick one to focus on.” Appendix B, Table 8.6 provides a list of the brands mentioned by the subjects, such as Nike, Adidas, Puma and Dior. In Brazil international sports brands such as Nike are very costly, some stores even offer an installment plan with up to 10 installments in order to ease the burden on the customer budget and also increase their sales (Circuito MT 2009). These brands are highly appreciated by teenagers, mostly because their logos are easily recognizable and their advertisements focus on famous national and international sports idols (Bose et al. 2006). Structural equation model The hypothesized model was tested via structural equation modeling (LISREL 8.70). Initial results of the analysis indicated that the model fit could be improved (Chi- square= 1275.67, df= 455, p-value= 0.00000, RMSEA= 0.057). Following Bollen (1989) and Steiger (1990), we attempted to refine the model by freeing error variances of conceptually similar items one by one as suggested by modification indices provided by LISREL. The structural model was specified with the PHI, TD, PS and TE matrices symmetric and fixed. Errors were treated independently to avoid interpretational confounds. The lambda matrices (both X and Y) were full and fixed. Then the individual items associated with the 52 exogenous and endogenous constructs were freed. The resulting model fit the data relatively well based on criteria set forth by Blalock (1985) which suggest that the chi- square value should approximate less than twice the value of the degrees of freedom (Chi- square=1046.22, df= 451, p-value= 0.00000, RMSEA= 0.049, GFI= 0.89, CFI= 0.93) (Figure 3). The model exceeded the normally accepted criteria of .90 for CFI (Garson 2007), is within the criteria of RMSEA s .06 (Hu and Bentler 1999), and approached the GFI guideline of 2 .90 (Kline 2000). Overall, the model has achieved a good fit for the number of parameters being investigated. Hypothesis 1 stated that self concept clarity will negatively impact social consumption motivation. Table 5 shows that self concept clarity has a significant negative relationship with social consumption motivation with a path parameter of -0.29, p<.001; this result shows support for hypothesis 1. Hypothesis 2 stated that peer pressure would moderate the relationship between self—concept clarity and social consumption motivation. In order to check for the moderating effect of peer-pressure, we added the moderator into the model to investigate any changes to the path coefficients. This technique of testing the model with no moderators and then adding the moderator construct into the model has been amply used in previous research, especially when the goal is to test the suppression effect (Baron and Kenny 1986; Cortina, Chen, and Dunlap 2001). The suppression effect is defined as a variable which increases or diminishes the predictive validity of another variable (or set of variables) by its inclusion in a regression equation, if the magnitude of the relationship between 53 an independent variable and a dependent variable becomes significantly larger or smaller when a third variable is included, suppression is indicated (MacKinnon, Krull, and Lockwood 2000). Suppressor variables can improve the prediction of the criterion. In essence, these variables suppress irrelevant variance in the other predictor variable, thus indirectly allowing for a more concise estimate of the predictor-criterion relationship (Lancaster 1999). The results presented on Table 5 show that adding peer pressure into the model does not change the path parameter significantly; consequently hypothesis 2 is not supported by our data. Hypothesis 3 predicted that independent self-construal is negatively related to social consumption motivation. As expected, this relationship was significantly supported, with a path parameter of -0.13, p <.01. Hypothesis 4 stating that interdependent self construal is positively related to social consumption motivation was not significantly supported by our data. Neither Hypothesis 5a: social consumption motivation is positively related to interest toward luxury brands, nor Hypothesis 5b: social consumption motivation is positively related to evaluative attitude toward luxury brands were supported; their respective path parameters were -0.07 and -0.31. Surprisingly, the relationship (H5b) between social consumption motivation (SCM) and evaluative attitude toward luxury brands (EATL) turned out to be negative which lacks theoretical support. Hypothesis 6 that stated that materialism is positively related to social consumption motivation, as well as hypothesis 7a that stated that Materialism is 54 positively related to interest toward luxury brands, and hypothesis 7b that stated materialism is positively related to evaluative attitudes toward luxury brands, were all fully supported by our data. Their respective path parameters were 0.54, 0.93 and 0.72 all significant at p<.001. As a result, the model provided support for five of the nine proposed hypotheses. The results of estimation of our model are included on Table 5 below. Table 5 Hypotheses (Parameters) Estimate t-value P- Result Expected path value coefficient SelfConc —> SCM H1 Self-concept clarity is _ _ . negatively related to social 0.29 5.71 <.001 Supported Negative consumption motivation SelfConc T» SCM Peer Peer pressured pressured Not PPress added into added "“0 supported H2 The impact of self concept the mode. the ”099' < 01 (t_ value clarity on social consumption -024 ‘4-89 '0 difference motivation will be moderated by pee, Peer mam, "-8- peer ressure ”933"” p ”$3.12" "3,3336 “2.1;” the model model <.001 significant) -0.29 _5.71 Indep__, SCM H3 Independent self-construal . is negatively related to social 0'13 2'66 <01 Supported Negatlve consumption motivation Inter-b SCM H4 lnterdependent self Not . . construal is positively related to 0'00 0'10 '460 supported Posltlve social consumption motivation SCM—> ITL H5a Social consumption Not .. - . - . . P t motivation is positively related to 0 07 0 74 228 supported 05' lve interest toward luxurLbrands 55 Table 5 (Cont’dI SCM —> EATL Not H513 Social consumption supported motivation is positively related to (in fact it evaluative attitude toward luxury -0.31 -3. 16 <.001 was found a Positive brands sig. negative correlatiom MAT—9 SCM H6 Materialism is positively . . related to social consumption 0.54 6.63 <.001 Supported Posmve motivation MAT—D ITL H73 Materialism is positively . . related to interest toward luxury 0.93 6.19 <.001 Supported Posmve brands MAT—> EATL H7b Materialism is positively . . related to evaluative attitudes 0'72 4'83 <'001 Supported Positive toward luxury brands Goodness of fit Chi- squared (df=451) 1046.22 P-value u§~a>m ’ mmm—Ma—m -cBmmoa Boom wflaflhflumm “W 2.35:»? :2. "ml man—Z— - Enhance bow snowmoaocfi 8231232 3 BE OZ .maouguofi OOmAHm Baggage 300m -5120 Eoocoo war—.7: -1252828 “cocqoaocuowfi 57 CHAPTER 5 CONCLUSION Discussion This study is to increase our understanding of teenagers’ self and attitudes toward luxury brands in a developing country. We analyze how social consumption motivations affect teenagers’ attitudes toward luxury brands, how teen’s self concepts can influence social consumption motivations and whether peer pressure can moderate this relationship and we also examine how materialism influences teenagers’ social consumption motivations and attitudes toward luxury brands. One of the key contributions of this study is that we demonstrate that materialism is a powerful force in developing more positive attitudes towards luxury brands among Brazilian”s teenagers conforming with Prendergast and Wong study (2003). In addition, the desire for wealth and material ownership is positively associated with social incentives to consume (social consumption motivation), consistent with Christopher and Schlenker (2004). Furthermore, an additional goal of our study was to understand the impact of self aspects on social consumption motivations among Brazilian teenagers and to investigate whether peer pressure affects the aforementioned relationship. Thus, by focusing on the teenage market in Brazil, we were able to demonstrate that self aspects are very important for Brazilian teen's consumer education; self aspects can influence the way teens perceive group pressures to shop/purchase, which represents a fundamental aspect in the study of teen consumer behavior 58 (John 1999; Moschis and Churchill 1978). For example, Brazilian teenagers that have self-beliefs that are clear, confidently defined, internally consistent, and stable have a stronger tendency to resist social motivations to consume, because the clearer they are about themselves the less they attend to external sources and stimuli. In addition, we explored the cross-cultural applicability of the scales created in the US, for use with Brazilian teenagers. We did not find any problems related to the translated scales; this might be because we performed a pre-test and because some of our scales were previously tested in cross-cultural studies. However, it is possible that the measures were not sensitive enough, which could explain why our data did not support the expected positive association between collective incentives to consume and teens overall evaluations of luxury brands. The fact is that our Brazilian study did not replicate similar results previously found by Prendergast and Wong (2003) that established that there was a positive relationship between social consumption motivation and expenditure on luxury brands. Nevertheless, the reason why our data did not support similar previous results is an issue that demands further investigation. Instead of trying to explain teens’ attitudes toward luxury by examining the influence of external factors such as demographics, advertising and promotions, our study deals with internal factors of self, materialism, peer pressure and social consumption motivation. In reviewing the literature, we found that the major constructs in this study have been addressed separately by various researchers. Consequently, a major contribution of the current research was the integration of 59 these main constructs into a more cohesive model. Our framework draws from such interrelated fields as consumer psychology, sociology and marketing. Additionally, our study takes a step further than Wong and Ahuvia (1998) since we propose independent self and interdependent self as characteristics within a specific culture, Brazilian teenagers, rather than as a differential characteristic between cultures. Implications The findings of our study provide strategic implications for global companies that are targeting Brazilian teens. International marketing managers in luxury brand companies should first acknowledge that Brazilian teens who know themselves very well (self concept clarity) or that believe that their ideas are unique (independent self), are not concerned about collective incentives to consume (social consumption motivation); therefore they represent a difficult target market to focus on, given that most advertising efforts seek to demonstrate that by buying a specific brand, an individual will impress others. This finding corroborates with Tsai (2005, p. 450) who suggested that “luxury brand marketing managers should avoid confining their perspective narrowly to the consumer’s desire to impress others” and with Coulter, Price and Feick (2003) and Puntoni (2001) that established that socially oriented motives are not sufficient to explain the whole picture of luxury market consumption. Our study, for the Brazilian teen market, revealed however, that luxury brand retailers could shift advertising efforts to concentrate on materialistic 6O aspects of luxury brands and how they can assist in achieving personal happiness and status. Thus, luxury brand companies should understand that some teens relate luxury brands more to their own belief that money can buy them happiness and that acquiring material possessions is a form of demonstrating one’s achievements in life (Banerjee and Dittmar 2008). Furthermore, a recent study found that materialistic individuals represent the largest consumer segment for luxury brands (Wledmann, Hennigs, and Siebels 2009). Consequently luxury marketing attempts must be also geared to the needs of self-directed pleasure and self gift giving as a way to connect luxury brands, accomplishments, status and personal happiness (Tsai 2005). An unexpected finding was a negative association between social consumption motivation and evaluative attitude toward luxury brands, which contradicts conventional wisdom. Evaluative attitude toward luxury brand is defined as what a teenager thinks about luxury brands, their personal opinion about luxury brands (Dubois and Laurent 1994). In other words, it is an individuals’ view about luxury brands. A possible explanation for our result could be that what one thinks about luxury brands is more related to personal values and not to the desire to impress others by acquiring prestigious or expensive brands. This explanation complements some previous findings from Vigneron and Johnson (1999). They stated that perfectionist and hedonist motives influence luxury consumption; the hedonic effect ensues when consumers value the perceived utility acquired from a luxury brand to arouse personal feelings and affective states. 61 In developing countries, research demonstrates that young consumers seeking better their economic position are likely to perceive brands as a way to believe that buying global brands allows them to participate in a global arena (Anholt 2003; Elliott and Leonard 2004). Consequently, it is important that teenagers feel like they are global citizens. Indeed, recent studies indicate that global youth have a high degree of homogeneity, share common interests and identify similarly with high exposure brands (Burgess and Steenkamp 2006; Strizhakova, Coulter, and Price 2008). Thus, although we did not focus on global luxury brands or even on global teenagers, we still believe that our findings are relevant to global luxury brands companies that plan to focus on the teenage market. Previous research found that adolescents are deceptive in many situations related to consumer purchases. Teenagers often hide new purchases from their parents, or refuse to disclose the real price of a product out of fear of getting in trouble (Bristol and Mangleburg 2005). This may be due to materialism or low independent self; for instance, a teenager can feel pressured to comply with the opinion of other people (low independent self) or be struggling to find happiness through their possessions (materialism). _As a result, the teen may do whatever it takes to obtain a certain brand. From a teenagers’ personal perspective, income and financial standards are not likely to be considered as important values; rather, obtaining luxury and popular brand name products is essential (regardless of income status) as a source of social prestige (Banerjee and Dittrnar 2008; Goldberg et al. 2003). Understanding these motivations may help parents; they 62 can help their teenager understand that when advertising shows a luxury brand owner as a happier person that it does not necessarily represent the truth in all situations. Moreover, schools should discuss aspects of self during their regular activities in order to assist in clarifying the teenager’s own perception of himself. For instance, teachers may conduct exercises in class about what kind of people teens admire and why; and how a teenager believes he connects with this person; this kind of exercises generates a discussion that might help teenagers to evaluate themselves and their values. The world economic situation changed significantly since we collected our data. The world is now in an unprecedented economic crisis; Brazil is also affected. Most newspapers show people losing their jobs and companies closing their doors. Despite the financial crisis, still there are a lot of luxury brands being sold in the Brazilian market. Since October 2008, when reports about the financial crisis were published until recently, several new companies announced or are already open for business in Brazil (i.e.: the disco Pink Elephant, the brands Laura Ashley, Beverly Hills Polo Club, Fiorruci, Topshop and Marc Jacobs from Louis Vuitton) (Lima and Scheller 2009). In addition, some Brazilian luxury companies are also opening abroad (Daniel 2009). Our findings should be useful for those companies, especially now. Because, if for Brazilian teens, luxury brand value does not lie exclusively in social aspects, a comprehensive marketing strategy of luxury brands should rest not only on social appeals, but also on the benefits from a personal orientation point of view, as this might represent an important competitive advantage during an economic crisis market. 63 On one hand, some reports suggest that sales volume of luxury brands has not decreased recently (Osborne 2009; Osmar 2009; Sherman 2009); people are still buying but some of them feel ashamed of buying expensive brands during such an economic crisis (Wharton.universia.net 2008). Other specialists speculate that the elite segment is happy since more financial restrictions result in fewer people having access to luxury goods. Russ Alan, president of the research company Prince & Associates, called this actual phase as “luxury rebirth” where only the ones who can really appreciate luxury will be able to afford it (as cited in Osmar 2009). On the other hand, some predict that 2009 will be among the worst for luxury brands. According to Alexandra Gillespie (from Fashion Luxury Retail group-FLR) (as cited in Wharton.universia.net 2008), consumers will begin to trade down (Anonymous 2009; Dolliver 2009). In sum, our research is an important topic to be discussed, analyzed and investigated particularly in periods of economic and financial crises, when teenagers are already reporting a decrease of 14% on spending and a less brand-loyal attitude (Wilson 2009). By understanding how teenagers develop their attitudes, companies can use the information to develop strategies to increase their competitive market advantage and retain their market share even during tough times. Suggestions for Future Research and Limitations The present study investigated how attitudes toward luxury are developed by Brazilian teenagers. The constructs that were part of this study are: aspects of 64 self, materialism, peer pressure, attitudes toward luxury and social consumption motivation. This study empirically showed that the attachment to possessions influences the opinion that one has about luxury brands and also influences how an individual perceives social pressure to consume. In addition this study demonstrated that individuals who perceive themselves as free from the influence, control, or determination of others, as well as individuals who are very clear about who they are and what they want in life, represent individuals who do not pay much attention to collective incentives to consume. It is not clear, however, how a greater variety of demographic variables (age, gender, income, education level, parent’s employment status) would help the study to generate additional interesting findings. For instance the study conducted by Roberts and Clement (2007) found that as income declined more individuals equated happiness with material possessions. Furthermore, previous work by Kasser (1995) and, Elliot and Leonard (2004) found that teenagers growing up in less advantageous socio-economic circumstances placed greater value on financial success aspirations than on self acceptance. The study suggests that such teens may focus more on external rewards, because intrinsic sources of worth and security are less supported by and less prevalent in disadvantaged communities. In addition, it would be interesting to investigate teenager’s attitudes toward counterfeit luxury items (Cheung and Prendergast 2006; Gentry et al. 2001) given that some companies really consider this a threat to their brands, and also, given that in economic downturns consumers may focus on spending less, no 65 matter how. In addition, Brazilian researchers found that currently, the social acceptance of counterfeit luxury items in growing (D’Angelo 2009). This might be related to another issue that we also did not examine in our research, the customer’s perception of quality (Cheng, Cripps, and Chen 2006). One might say that teens spend more on counterfeit luxury items because they do not perceive an additional value for money or an extra intrinsic quality that usually only an authentic item can offer. It may also be important to investigate a connection between materialistic teens, luxury brands and issues related to quality of life. Some previous studies found that those who are materialistic are more likely to be dissatisfied with their possessions because they have high possession expectations (Sirgy et al. 1998), also it was found that developing materialistic values may be inversely related to quality of life (DeAngelis 2004; Larsen et al. 1999; Sirgy 1998) and that this damaging relationship (resulting from reduction of quality of life) leads to overall life dissatisfaction. This relationship was seen primarily when the likelihood of purchase is reduced due to lack of resources (i.e.: an individual who wants a branded watch, but does not have enough money, may be depressed and unsatisfied with their own life as whole) (Sirgy 1998; Wang and Wallendorf 2006). Another topic that could be investigated that might add some extra insights to our findings is how teenager’s attitudes toward luxury brands change overtime through a longitudinal study. It is possible to argue that parents support their children’s purchasing behavior as a means of helping the children deal with the transition period from childhood to adulthood. Specifically, Prendergast and 66 Wong (2003) demonstrated that parents often spend extra money to purchase luxury brand products for their offspring. This behavior might lead to the assumption that parents seek to express their financial wealth through their children, or that parents try to compensate for lack of quality time together with their children by buying them expensive merchandise. This behavior pattern might contribute to the later consumption practices of teenagers and consequently to the positive perceptions of luxury brands by teenagers. Belk, Mayer and Driscoll (1984) also stated that individual preferences in the younger population could reflect exposure to different brands, as demonstrated by parental or other adult influence. Luxury goods are highly attractive for many consumers in different countries; many individuals are often inspired by luxury and are passionate about the meaning that it brings to their lives. Specifically, there are a number of important reasons why luxury is so important for many consumers. As Kapferer (1998) mentioned, many young people equate status with luxury, and many brands provide this connection, such as Coach, Kipling, and Abercrombie & Fitch. Future research could test the replicability of our findings with additional samples and cultures. Cross-cultural replication might find differing degrees of attitudes toward luxury brands based on differing core cultural values toward change, conformity/distinction, or fashion (Dubois and Laurent 1996; Wong and Ahuvia 1998). Another suggestion for a future investigation is to examine in a more qualitative manner, the concept of luxury brands among teens. “Luxury is 67 particularly slippery to define,” notes Cornell (2002) and “luxury is wider than the narrow definition” notes Mortelmans (2005). The purpose of our research was not to define luxury per se, as some authors reported it to be hard task (Galhanone 2005; Mortelmans 2005; Wiedmann et al. 2009) , but more modestly, our aim was to investigate attitudes toward luxury brands among Brazilian teens. There are several definitions for luxury, usually they represent something inessential, costly but conducive of enjoyment (Dubois and Laurent 1994; Galhanone 2005; Hader 2008; Mortelmans 2005; Seringhaus 2005). Besides, important differences in the concept of luxury may exist across generation in one single country (Dubois et al. 2005). The fact is that luxury is still a very complex term, since something that might be considered as luxury to one, might not be considered as luxury by other, and a study aiming to further define and refine this term both qualitatively and quantitatively would be a long range project. Our data failed to show an expected positive relationship between interdependent self construal and social consumption motivation, and the effect of peer pressure on moderating the relationship between self concept clarity and social consumption motivation. In our study, all variables were measured concurrently, therefore, the statistical relationships among the constructs may not reflect causation, and our study design as well as many other studies does not permit causal conclusions (Rose and DeJesus 2007). Consequently, our last suggestion would be to investigate in more detail what can affect the aforementioned relationships and which constructs should be used to measure them. A possible solution would be to conduct some qualitative studies in order 68 to create measures specific for teenagers, although this would definitely be a more costly study and demand extra time. Another solution would be to perform longitudinal studies in order to evaluate how individuals develop aspects of self differently, in an effort to examine oscillations of self over time rather than their perceptions of self at one point in time. A main critique of survey-based research in general, is that people’s responses are extemporaneous without much prior thought involved; therefore, our research does not avoid this limitation. Kotler observed that people’s motivation is not always obvious, and to a certain degree conscious. “If one asked why he purchased an expensive foreign sports-car, he may reply that he likes its maneuverability and its looks. At a deeper level he may have purchased the car to impress others, or to feel young again. At a still deeper level, he may be purchasing the sports car to achieve substitute gratification for unsatisfied sexual strivings” (Kotler 1965, p. 41). Indeed, abstract concepts are more difficult to measure in research surveys. Moreover, measuring consumer attitudes toward luxury may be one of the most difficult areas of research given that our need for prestige is one of our most closely guarded secrets (Garfein 1989; Vigneron 1 998). Consumers in different parts of the world buy, or wish to buy, luxury brands for apparently varied reasons. However they possess similar values regardless of their country of origin; the difference is the weight of each value for a particular individual (Wiedmann, Hennigs, and Siebels 2007). Despite the limitations and necessary steps in future research, the primary contribution of this framework lies 69 in developing a comprehensive model of consumers’ perceptions of luxury by integrating the dimensions of self, social consumption motivation, peer pressure and materialism to identify relevant behavioral patterns among teens in a developing country. The findings of this study will hopefully stimulate further research on teens and luxury brands. In particular, future research should explore and test more aspects of self and teens’ behavior as we believe it to be crucial for marketers, educators, and parents. 70 APPENDICES APPENDIX A: SURVEY ITEMS AND CONSENT FORMS Teens and Consumer Behavior gem PEER PRESSURE Please use this 7-point scale to indicate your agreement or disagreement with each statement (just mark just one alternative per statement). 1) My friends could push me into doing Strongly 1 2 3 Strongly just about anything Disagree Agree 2) I give into peer pressure easily Strongly 1 2 3 Strongly Disagree Agree 3) When at school, if a group of people Strongly 1 2 3 Strongly asked me to do something, it would Disagree Agree be hard to say no 4) At times, I've broken rules because Strongly 1 2 3 Strongly others have urged me to Disagree Agree 5) At times, I’ve done dangerous or Strongly 1 2 3 Strongly foolish things because others dared Disagree Agree me to 6) I often feel pressured to do things I Strongly 1 2 3 Strongly wouldn’t normally do Disagree Agree 7) If my friends are drinking, it would be Strongly 1 2 3 Strongly hard for me to resist having a drink Disagree Agree 8) I’ve skipped classes, when others Strongly 1 2 3 Strongly have urged me to Disagree Agree 9) I’ve felt pressured to have sex, Strongly 1 2 3 Strongly because a lot of people my own age Disagree Agree have already had sex 10) I’ve felt pressured to get drunk at Strongly 1 2 3 Strongly parties Disagree Agree 11) At times I've felt pressured to do Strongly 1 2 3 Strongly drugs, because others have urged Disagree Agree me to SECTI N II MATERIAL VALUES SCALE [(r) reverse code] Please use this 7—point scale to indicate the extent to which you believe best represents your Opinion about the question (mark only one alternative per question). 12) How do you feel about people Do not 1 2 3 Greatly who own expensive homes, cars, admire admire and clothes? 13) How do you shop? (r) Buy 1 2 3 Buy only 71 anything I whatl mlght want need 14) How do you feel about owning Makes me Makes things that impress people? un- me feel comfortable great 15) How do you feel about acquiring Not Very material possessions as an important Important achievement in life? 16) How do you approach your life in More is Simple is terms of your possessions (i.e., better better buying and owninLthings)? (r) 17) Would your life be any better if Not any Much you owned certain things that you better better don’t have? 18) Do you think the amount of Very much Not at all material objects people own shows how successful they are? (r) 19) How would you feel if you could Not any Much afford to buy more things? happier happier 20) How would you feel if you owned Much Not any nicer things? (r) happier happier 21) What do the things you own say Very little A great about how well you are doing in deal life? 22) How do you feel about spending Do not Really money on things that aren’t enjoy enjoy practical? 23) Do you feel that you have all the Need more Have aIlI things you really need to enjoy need Iife?(r) 24) How much pleasure do you get Very little A great from buyirm things? deal 25) How do you feel about the things Very Not all you own? (r) important that important 26) How do you feel about having a Do not Really lot of luxury in your life? enjoy enjoy W INDEPENDENT SELF-CONSTRUAL (items 27-40) INTERDEPENDENT SELF-CONSTRUAL (items 41 -51) Please use this 7-point scale to indicate your agreement or disagreement with each statement (mark only one alternative per statement). Please note that “group” is a small, relatively intimate group of peers who interact with you on a regular basis. 27) I should be judged on my own merit Strongly 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Disagee Agree 28) Being able to take care of myself is a Strongly 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly primary concern for me Disagree Agree 29) My personal identity is important to Strongly 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly me Disagree Agee 72 30) I prefer to be self-reliant rather than Strongly 6 Strongly depend on others Disagree Agree 31) I am a unique person separate from Strongly 6 Strongly others Disagree Agree 32) If there is a conflict between my Strongly 6 Strongly values and values of groups of which Disagree Agree I am a member, I follow my values 33) I try not to depend on others Strongly 6 Strongly Disang Agree 34) I take responsibility for my own Strongly 6 Strongly actions Disagree Agree 35) It is important for me to act as an Strongly 6 Strongly independent person Disagree Agree 36) I should decide my future on my own Strongly 6 Strongly Disagree Agree 37) What happens to me is my own Strongly 6 Strongly doirg Disagree Age 38) I enjoy being unique and different Strongly 6 Strongly from others Disamee Agree 39) I am comfortable being singled out Strongly 6 Strongly for praise and rewards Disagree Agree 40) I don't support a group decision Strongly 6 Strongly when it is wrong Disagree A ree 41) l consult others before making Strongly 6 Strongly important decisions Disagree Agree 42) I will sacrifice my self-interest for the Strongly 6 Strongly benefit of my group Disagree Agree 43) I stick with my group even through Strongly 6 Strongly difficulties Disagree Agree 44) I respect decisions made by my Strongly 6 Strongly group Disagree Agree 45) I will stay in a group if it needs me, Strongly 6 Strongly even if I am not happy with it Disagree Agree 46) I maintain harmony in the groups of Strongly 6 Strongly which I am a member Disagree Agree 47) I respect the majority’s wishes in Strongly 6 Strongly groups of which I am a member Disagree Agree 48) I remain in the groups of which I am Strongly 6 Strongly 3 member if they need me, even Disagree Agree though I am dissatisfied with them 49) It is better to consult others and get Strongly 6 Strongly their opinions before doing anything Disagree Agree 50) It is important to consult close friends Strongly 6 Strongly and get their ideas before making a Disagree Agree decision 51) My relationships with others are Strongly 6 Strongly more important to me than my Disagree Agree accomplishments seem SELF-CONCEPT CLARITY [(r) reverse code] 73 Please use this 7-point scale to indicate your agreement or disagreement with each statement (mark only one alternative per statement). 52) My beliefs about myself often conflict Strongly Strongly with one another (r) Disagree Agree 53) On one day I might have one cpinion Strongly Strongly of myself and on another day I might Disagree Agree have a different opinion (r) 54) I spend a lot of time wondering about Strongly Strongly what kind of person I really am (r) Disagree Agree 55) Sometimes I feel that I am not really Strongly Strongly the person that I appear to be (r) Disagree Agree 56) When I think about the kind of person Strongly Strongly I have been in the past, I’m not sure Disagree Agree what I was really like (r) 57) I seldom experience conflict between Strongly Strongly the different aspects of my personality Disagree Agree 58) Sometimes I think I know other Strongly Strongly people better than I know myself (r) Disagree Agree 59) My beliefs about myself seem to Strongly Strongly change very frequently (r) Disagree Agree 60) If I were asked to describe my Strongly Strongly personality, my description might end Disagree Agree up being different from one day to another day (r) 61) Even if I wanted to, ldon’t thinkl Strongly Strongly would tell someone what I’m really Disagree Agree like (r) 62) In general, I have a clear sense of Strongly Strongly who I am and what I am Disagree Agree 63) It is often hard for me to make up my Strongly Strongly mind about things because I don’t Disagree Agree really know what I want (r) w SOCIAL CONSUMPTION MOTIVATION Please use this 7-point scale to indicate your agreement or disagreement with each statement (mark only one alternative per statement). All statements below are related to the topic: Mat is Impprtant to 1‘ re rch in a roduct? Tofi: What is important to know before purchasing a product? 64) What others think of different brands or Strongly Strongly products Disagree Agree 65) What kind of people buy certain brands Strongly Strongly or products Disagree Agree 66) What others think of people who use Strongly Strongly certain brands or products Disagree Agree 74 67) What brands or products to buy make a good impression on others to Strongly12345 Disagree Strongly Agree §ECTION VI ATTITUDE TOWARD LUXURY [(r) reverse code, (i) interest toward luxury factor, (e) evaluative attitude toward luxury factor] Please use this 7-point scale to indicate your agreement or disagreement with each statement (Mark only one alternative per statement). differentiate themselves from others (9) (r) 68) I don’t know much about the luxury Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Agree world (i)(r) 69) In my opinion, luxury is too expensive Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Agree for what it is (e) (r) 70) In my opinion, luxury is flashy (e) (r) Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Agree 71) All things considered, I rather like Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Agree luxury (i) 72) I’m not interested in luxury (i) (r) Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 A ree 73) I could talk about luxury for hours (i) Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Agree 74) I would not feel at ease in a luxury Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Agree shop 0) (r) 75) I almost never buy luxury products( i) Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Agree (0 76) People who buy luxury products seek Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 7 Agree to imitate the rich (e) (r) 77) People who buy luxury products try to Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Agree Please respond to the following questions about yourself. 78) Please indicate your gender 1) Male 2) Female 79) Please indicate your age 1) 12 2) 13 3) 14 4) 15 5) 16 6) 17 7) 18 80) Do you have a cell phone? 1) Yes 2) No SECTI N VII 81) Please Indicate your number of siblings 1) 0(l don’t have any) 2) 1 3) 2 75 4) 3 5) 4 6) Sor more 82) How many TV’s do you have in your home? 1) More than 4 W5 2) 3 3) 2 4) Only 1 TV 5) None 83) Mark the top category that you usually spend your allowance on (or the money that you usually receive from your job or parentslrelatives). Please, mark only one category 1) Entertainment (movies, games, magazines, CD’s) 2) Clothes! shoes 3) Food/snacks 4) School needs (pen, paper) 5) Cosmetics (cologne, make up, cleanser) 6) Others 84) Do you have a job? 1) Yes 2) No 85) Please indicate the status of your parents’ relationship 1) Married 2) Separated 3) Divorced 4) Never married 5) Don’t know/none of the above 86) What Is the highest degree obtained by either of your parent (s)? 1) No high school 2) High School 3) Bachelor’s 4) Graduate degree 5) Don’t know/none of the above 87) Please indicate your grade 1) 6th 2) 7th 3) 8th 4) 9th 5) 10th 6) 11th 7) None of the above! not sure Thank you for taking the time to fill out our survey @ 76 Assent Form (to be signed by the teenager) Teens and Consumer Behavior This is a research study of teens and consumer behavior. You are being asked to participate because you are a high school student, and your parent or guardian has agreed to allow you to participate. Your participation will involve completing the attached survey, which should take about 15 minutes. This research is being conducted by Luciana de Araujo Gil (PhD student from Michigan State University) as part of her dissertation requirement. There are no right or wrong answers. We are Interested in your ideas and Opinions. Who will have access to your responses? How will the information be used? Your participation is completely voluntary. All survey responses will remain confidential. Your survey responses will be available only to members of our research team and will never be identified with you personally. You may decline to answer any question at any time during the study. The research offers no risk to respondents. You will not benefit from participation in the study. However, your participation may contribute to understanding more about teenagers’ consumer behavior. All of your answers will be anonymous. Responses will be combined with other respondents so that individual responses cannot be isolated. How will your privacy be protected? You may choose not to participate in the study, you may decline to answer specific questions, and you may discontinue participation at any time. Your privacy will be protected to the maximum extent allowable by law. What if you have questions about the study? If you have any questions about this study, please contact the principal investigator, Dr. Kyoung-Nan Kwon, Assistant Professor, 367 Comm Arts Building, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824; by phone (517) 353-9909; or by email kwonky@msu.edu. You may also contact the secondary investigator, Luciana de Araujo Gil, Ph.D. Retailing student, at Rua Govemador Pedro de Toledo 34 apto 22 Santos/SP phone (13) 32849289; or by email gillucia@msu.edu. If you have questions or concerns regarding your rights as a study participant, or are dissatisfied at any time with any aspect of this study, you may contact - anonymously, if you wish — Peter Vasilenko, Ph.D., Director of the Human Subject Protection Programs at Michigan State University, by phone: (517) 355-2180, fax: (517) 432-4503, email: irb@msu.edu, or regular mail: 202 Olds Hall, East Lansing, MI 48824. Your signature below indicates your voluntary assent to participate in this study. Name [please print] Date Signature 77 Request for letter of commitment or support template Michigan State University August, 14 2007 Address letter to: Ms. Maria Bemardete da Silva Colégio Stella Maris Av. Conselheiro Nebias, 771 Santos/SP Brazil zip 11-100 Subject: Request for letter of commitment or support for data collection project I am writing you to ask for Colégio Stella Maris support for my research project. The project focuses on a research study of teens and their consumer behavior. Your student’s participation will involve completing the attached survey and will take about 15 minutes. There are no right or wrong answers, and no benefit or risk to your students. We are only interested in your student’s ideas and opinions. Prior to administering the survey, a consent form will be sent to parents/guardian and only students who bring the consent form signed will be allowed to participate in our research. The research activities will help me to achieve my goals to finish my dissertation and obtain my PhD degree. The overall aspiration of the proposed project is to demonstrate how and why teenagers consume luxury brands. Colégio Stella Maris will be involved in the project in the following ways: 0 Selecting classes to answer a survey questionnaire about teenagers behavior and attitude toward luxury 0 Evaluating a copy of the questionnairel and consent forms before the collection of the data Please send the letter of commitment or support to: Luciana do Araujo Gil, PhD CandidatelMichigan State University 750 Pine Forest Drive #103 East LansinglMI 48823 OR Just sign and date this letter, your signature below indicates your voluntary support our research project and the participation of your students in our data collection. Your Name [please print] Date Your Signature 78 Consent Form (parents/guardian) Child’s Participation in Studszeens and Consumer Behavior What is your child’s involvement in this study? You are being asked to consent to your child’s participation in a research study of teens and consumer behavior. Your child’s participation will involve completing a survey during class time and will take about 15 minutes. There are no right or wrong answers. We are only interested in your child’s ideas and opinions. This research is being conducted by Luciana de Araujo Gil (PhD student from Michigan State University) as part of her dissertation requirement. Who will have access to what your child says? How will the information be used? Your child’s participation is completely voluntary. All survey responses will remain confidential. Your child’s survey responses will be available only to members of our research team and will never be identified with him or her personally. Your child will not benefit from participation in the study. However, his or her participation may contribute to understanding more about teenager consumer behavior. All of the answers will be anonymous. Responses will be combined with other respondents so that individual responses cannot be isolated. How will your child’s privacy be protected? You may choose not to have your child participate in the study, your child may decline to answer specific questions, and your child may discontinue participation at any time. Your child's privacy will be protected to the maximum extent allowable by law. If you provide your consent, your child may still make his or her own choice to participate. Are there any risks involved in this study? No, there is no risk to any of the respondents of this survey. What if you have questions about the study? If you have any questions about this study, please contact the principal investigator, Dr. Kyoung-Nan Kwon, Assistant Professor, 367 Comm Arts Building, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824; by phone (517) 353-9909; or by email kwonky@msu.edu. You may also contact the secondary investigator, Luciana de Araujo Gil, Ph.D. Retailing student, at Rua Govemador Pedro de Toledo 34 apto 22 Santos/SP phone (13) 32849289; or by email gillucia@msu.edu. If you have questions or concerns regarding your rights or your child’s rights as a study participant, or are dissatisfied at any time with any aspect of this study, you may contact - anonymously, if you wish - Peter Vasilenko, Ph.D., Director of the Human Subject Protection Programs at Michigan State University, by phone: (517) 355-2180, fax: (517) 432-4503, email: irb@msu.edu, or regular mail: 202 Olds Hall, East Lansing, MI 48824. Your signature below indicates your voluntary consent for your child to participate in this study. 79 Your Name [please print] Child’s Name [please print] Your Signature Date 80 APPENDIX B: TABLES Table 8.1 Scale items - Factor Analysis (maximum likelihood/ oblique rotation) Factor 1 Factor 2 Peer pressure My friends could push me into doing just about anything -0.108 1.069 I give into peer pressure easily 0.089 0.891 When at school, if a group of people asked me to do something, it 0.052 0.736 would be hard to say no At times, I’ve broken rules because others have urged me to 0.702 0.555 At times, I’ve done dangerous or foolish things because others dared me 0.874 0.490 to I often feel pressured to do things I wouldn’t normally do 0.512 0.229 If my friends are drinking, it would be hard for me to resist having a drink 1.146 0.007 I’ve skipped classes, when others have urged me to 0.929 0.367 I’ve felt pressured to have sex, because a lot of people my own age have 0.729 0.014 already had sex I’ve felt pressured to get drunk at parties (eliminated related to drugs) 1.008 -0.223 At times I’ve felt pressured to do drugs, because others have urged me to 0.707 -0.095 ' (eliminated related to drugs) Materialism How do you feel about people who own expensive homes, cars, and 0.364 0.243 clothes? How do you shop? (r) -0.046 0.619 How do you feel about owning things that impress people? 0.306 0.293 How do you feel about acquiring material possessions as an 0.488 0.108 achievement in life? How do you approach your life in terms of your possessions (i.e., buying 0.345 0.276 and owning things)? (r) Would your life be any better if you owned certain things that you 0.638 -0.058 don’t have? Do you think the amount of material objects people own shows how 0.307 -0.032 successful they are? (r) How would you feel if you could afford to buy more things? 0.680 0.037 How would you feel if you owned nicer things? (r) 0.539 0.202 What do the things you own say about how well you are doing in life? 0.382 0.003 How do you feel about spending money on things that ' aren't 0.123 0.432 practical? Do you feel that you have all the things you really need to enjoy life? 0.422 -0.133 It) How much pleasure do you get from buying things? -0.039 0.579 How do you feel about having a lot of luxury in your life? 0.377 0.333 Independent self construal I should be judged on my own merit 0.656 -0.030 Being able to take care of myself is a primary concern for me 0.525 0.003 My personal identity is important to me 0.521 -0.061 I prefer to be self-reliant rather than depend on others 0.505 -0.017 I am a unique person separate from others -0.074 0.742 If there is a conflict between my values and values of groups of which I 0.050 0.641 am a member, I follow my values I try not to depend on others 0.131 0.559 I take responsibility for my own actions 0.288 0.240 81 Table 8.1 (cont’d) It is important for me to act as an independent person 0.426 0.208 I should decide my future on my own 0.377 0.191 What happens to me is my own doing 0.454 0.134 I enjoy being unique and different from others 0.416 -0.073 I am comfortable being singled out for praise and rewards 0.292 0.046 I don’t support a group decision when it is wrong 0.310 0.013 lnterdependent self construal I consult others before making important decisions 0.654 -0.012 I will sacrifice my self-Interest for the benefit of my group 0.640 -0.010 I stick with my group even through difficulties 0.636 0.028 I respect decisions made by my group 0.548 -0.046 I will stay in a group if it needs me, even IfI am not happy with It 0.541 0.032 I maintain harmony in the groups of which I am a member -0.104 0.821 I respect the majority’s wishes in groups of which I am a member -0.024 0.702 I remain in the groups of which I am a member if they need me, even 0.051 0.497 though I am dissatisfied with them It is better to consult others and get their opinions before doing anything 0.172 0.356 It is important to consult close friends and get their ideas before making a 0.335 0.115 decision My relationships with others are more important to me than my 0.423 -0.031 accomplishments Self concept clarity My beliefs about myself often conflict with one another (r) 0.454 -0.015 On one day I might have one opinion of myself and on another day I 0.798 -0.125 might have a different opinion (r) I spend a lot of time wondering about what kind of person I really am 0.589 0.066 (0 Sometimes I feel that I am not really the person that I appear to be (r) 0.400 0.193 When I think about the kind of person I have been in the past, I’m not sure 0.076 0.462 what I was really like (r) Sometimes I think I know other people better than I know myself (r) 0.221 0.351 My beliefs about myself seem to change very frequently (r) 0.521 0.279 If I were asked to describe my personality, my description might end up 0.262 0.470 being different from one day to another day (r) Even if I wanted to, I don’t think I would tell someone what I’m really like -0.143 0.873 (r) In general, I have a clear sense of who I am and what I am 0.067 0.420 It is often hard for me to make up my mind about things because I don’t 0.297 0.059 really know what I want (r) Social consumption motivation (The maximum number of factors for this scale is 1) What others think of different brands or products 0.841 NA What kind of people buy certain brands or products 0.784 NA What others think of people who use certain brands or products 0.722 NA What brands or products to buy to make a good Impression on 0.718 NA others (note “f' reverse score/highlighted items represented the selected ones) 82 Table 8.2 Scale Items and Measurement Properties Corrected Item-Total Scale Cronbach's Correlation Alpha Peer pressure My friends could push me into doing just 0.447 0.626 about anything I give into peer pressure easily 0.494 When at school, if a group of people asked 0.320 me to do something, it would be hard to say no At times, I’ve broken rules because others 0.328 have urged me to Materialism MT1 0.715 How do you feel about acquiring material 0.392 possessions as an achievement in life? Would your life be any better if you owned 0.504 certain things that you don’t have? How would you feel if you could afford to buy 0.590 more things? How would you feel if you owned nicer 0.495 things? (r) Do you feel that you have all the things you 0.325 really need to enjoy life? (r) MT2 How do you shop? (r) 0.396 How do you feel about spending money on 0.374 things that aren’t practical? How much pleasure do you get from buying 0.389 things? Independent self construal I should be judged on my own merit 0.439 0.656 Being able to take care of myself is a primary 0.482 concern for me My personal identity is important to me 0.396 I prefer to be self-reliant rather than depend 0.430 on others Interdependent self construal I consult others before making important 0.426 0.738 decisions . I will sacrifice my self-interest for the benefit 0.522 of my group I stick with my group even through difficulties 0.586 I respect decisions made by my group 0.490 I will stay in a group if it needs me, even if I 0.476 am not happy with it 83 Table 8.2 (cont'd) Self concept clarity On one day I might have one opinion of 0.563 0.717 myself and on another day I might have a different opinion (r) I spend a lot of time wondering about what 0.530 kind of person I really am (r) My beliefs about myself seem to change very 0.517 frequently (r) Social consumption motivation What others think of different brands or 0.653 0.849 products What kind of people buy certain brands or 0.711 products What others think of people who use certain 0.751 brands or products What brands or products to buy to make a 0.638 good impression on others Interest toward luxury brands I don’t know much about the luxury world (r) 0.426 0.726 All things considered, I rather like luxury 0.497 I’m not interested in luxury (r) 0.537 I could talk about luxury for hours 0.404 I would not feel at ease in a luxury shop (r) 0.348 I almost never buy luxury products(r) 0.541 Evaluative attitude toward luxury brands In my opinion, luxury is too expensive for 0.273 0.609 what it is (r) In my opinion, luxury is flashy (r) 0.470 People who buy luxury products seek to 0.462 imitate the rich (r) People who buy luxury products try to 0.369 differentiate themselves from others (r) 84 note “r” reverse score Boa: E. same 3. 23$ 3. 83: E. wnzuoE 25:05. ®n>=o_2 mh>:o_>_ 63: B. and: 8. 83: mo. 33: B. seamen. mesons. menace. evades. :3: B. is: 8. 5e: we. otflom no.3?— @301 83: 3. Es: 3. a3”: B. :3: E. 83: B. Baa. Bass Bass Bass Bass mum. «.22 SK. :2 mew. 0mm. DR. .232 9:03:20 Swansea: eoumsaoE coquamcoo 360m. hem Eoceoqobs begs 38:8 tom. tom acoceoqccceus e_na_._a> t moumEamm chLEmbcmem Locos. EoEoSmmcE mm Each 85 no .v Q am EmoEfiB Sc 8582 Lease 5.. t .338 268.50 _. .memmficoama c_ 3255 2m $29-.. $3: 8. at). sod: 8. .E2 8mg 9. in. 8.8 we. and 8;: am. «do :3: B. Ed 6:8 mm. 2 Sesame cod: 3. we 628m A365 K. 53:31 8mg me. 38:81 :3: 8. 888% see 3. 382.com SB: 8. oosoaaeom an. a : om. mmoaaoom 5. P c S. 88.5 new: 8. 88.5 95:03 mm 0.93. 86 Table 3.4 Collinearity Analysis- Coefficients a Unstandardized Standardized Collinearity Coefficients Coefficients Statistics M0d8| 8 Std. Error Beta t SiQ- Tolerance VIF (Constant) 5.280 .499 10.586 .000 inter54 .091 .055 .075 1.654 .099 .768 1.303 inter55 -092 .054 -.087 -1.698 .090 .609 1.543 inter56 .058 .044 .071 1.335 .182 .566 1.765 inter58 -.100 .048 -.104 -2.082 .038 .644 1.553 inter59 .009 .040 .011 .222 .824 .644 1.554 rclar64 -.085 .037 -.111 -2.279 .023 .674 1.484 rclar65 .038 .035 .052 1.090 .275 .704 1.420 rclar70 -.178 .041 -.206 -4.304 .000 .700 1.429 |NDEP40 .073 .057 .058 1.278 .202 .779 1.284 INDEP42 -.012 .044 -012 -.269 .788 .753 1.328 INDEP43 -.116 .045 -.113 -2.556 .011 .823 1.215 INDEP49 -.121 .055 -.101 -2.226 .026 .774 1.292 87 a. Dependent Variable: Social Consumption Motivation TABLE 8.5 Descriptive Statistics Mean SD Skewness N Scale mean Peer pressure 2.549 My friends could push me into doing 2.388 1.518 0.870 558 just about anything I give into peer pressure easily 2.164 1.383 1.264 558 When at school, if a group of people 3.278 1.871 0.360 558 asked me to do something, it would be hard to say no At times, I’ve broken rules because 2.363 1.858 1.202 558 others have urged me to Materialism 3.838 MT1 3.421 1.873 0.217 558 How do you feel about acquiring material possessions as an achievement in life? Would your life be any better if you 3.940 1.766 —0.100 558 owned certain things that you don't have? How would you feel if you could 4.593 1.721 -0.453 558 afford to buy more things? How would you feel if you owned 3.811 1.808 0.023 558 nicer things? (r) Do you feel that you have all the 2.573 1.787 0.984 558 things you really need to enjoy life? (0 MT2 How do you shop? (r) 4.593 1.863 -0.337 558 How do you feel about spending 2.844 1.735 0.612 558 money on things that aren't practical? How much pleasure do you get 4.910 2.042 -0.627 558 from buying things? Independent self construal 5.963 I should be judged on my own merit 6.318 1.266 ~2.219 558 Being able to take care of myself is 5.810 1.678 -1.428 558 a primary concern for me My personal identity is important to 5.697 1.551 -1.160 558 me I prefer to be self-reliant rather than 6.026 1.332 -1.558 558 depend on others Interdependent self construal 5.138 I consult others before making 6.151 1.328 -1.860 558 important decisions I will sacrifice my self-interest for 5.522 1.510 -0.977 558 the benefit of my group I stick with my group even through 4.666 1.946 -0.522 558 88 Table 3.5 (cont’d) difficulties I respect decisions made by my 5.232 1.666 -0.872 558 group I will stay in a group if it needs me, 4.115 2.002 -0.146 558 even ifl am not happy with it Self concept clarity 4.338 On one day I might have one 3.781 2.088 0.187 558 opinion of myself and on another day I might have a different opinion (It) I spend a lot of time wondering 3.861 2.158 0.126 558 about what kind of person I really am (r) My beliefs about myself seem to 5.371 1.849 -0.892 558 change very frequently (r) Social consumption motivation 2.989 What others think of different 3.161 1.921 0.345 558 brands or products What kind of people buy certain 2.887 1.924 0.602 558 brands or products What others think of people who 2.915 1.903 0.535 558 use certain brands or products What brands or products to buy to 2.992 1.969 0.559 558 make a good impression on others Interest toward luxury brands 3.859 I don’t know much about the luxury 4.122 1.877 -0.086 555 world (r) All things considered, I rather like 3.925 1.969 -0.038 553 luxury I'm not interested in luxury (r) 4.449 1.839 -0.321 552 I could talk about luxury for hours 2.447 1.777 1.071 550 I would not feel at ease in a luxury 4.502 1.945 —0.279 553 shop (r) I almost never buy luxury 3.766 1.950 0.112 549 products(r) Evaluative attitude toward luxury brands 3.887 In my opinion, luxury is too 2.776 1.786 0.936 555 expensive for what it is (r) In my opinion, luxury is flashy (r) 3.992 1.976 0.084 553 People who buy luxury products 4.703 1.961 -0.329 554 seek to imitate the rich (r) People who buy luxury products try 4.064 1.963 0.050 554 to differentiate themselves from others (r) note “r" reverse score 89 Table 8.6 Top ten luxury brands chosen by our sample % mentioned (open question) Nike 13 Adidas Puma Dior Quicksilver Oakley Colcci Armani . Billabong 10. Apple 11. Channel 12. Gucci OQNQWPP’NT‘ A-JNNNNNUUCOCO 9O REFERENCES Abe, S and RP Bagozzi (1996), "An Investigation of Construct Validity and Generalizability of the Self-Concept: Self-Consciousness in Japan and the United States," in Global Perspectives in Cross-Cultural and Cross- National Consumer Research, ed. Lalita Manrai and Ajay Manrai, New York, NY: Haworth Press, 242. Ajzen, l (1991), "The Theory of Planned Behavior," Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 50 (2), 179-211. Allen, Katie (2007), "Despite the Turmoil, Conspicuous Consumption Is Still in Vogue," http://media.guardian.co.uk/site/story/0,,2160341,00.htm|. American Marketing Association (2007), "Dictionary of Marketing Terms," http://www.marketingpowercoml layouts/Dictionarv.aspx. Anderson, JC and DW Gerbing (1988), "Structural Equation Modeling in Practice: A Review and Recommended Two-Step Approach," Psychological Bulletin, 103 (3), 41 1-23. Anholt, S (2003), Brand New Justice: The Upside of Global Branding, Oxford, UK: Butterworth-Heinemann. Anonymous (2009), "A Special Report on Business in America: Trading Down," in The Economist, Vol. 391, 4. Bachmann, GR, DR John, and AR Rao (1993), "Children’s Susceptibility to Peer Group Purchase Influence: An Exploratory Investigation," Advances in Consumer Research, 20 (1 ), 463-8. Bagozzi, R. P. (1994), "Acr Fellow Speach," Vol. 21, Advances in consumer research, 8-1 1. Bagozzi, RP and Y Yi (1988), "On the Evaluation of Structural Equation Models," Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 16 (1 ), 74-94. Banaji, M. R. and D. A. Prentice (1994), "The Self in Social Contexts," Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 297-332. 91 Banerjee, Robin and Helga Dittrnar (2008), "Individual Differences in Children's Materialism: The Role of Peer Relations," Personality and Psychology Bulletin, 34 (1 ), 17-31. Baron, Reuben M and David A Kenny (1986), "The Moderator-Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Research: Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Considerations," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (6), 1173-82. Bass, FM and WW Talarzyk (1972), "An Attitude Model for the Study of Brand Preference," Joumal of Marketing Research, 93-96. Batra, R and OT Ahtola (1991), "Measuring the Hedonic and Utilitarian Sources of Consumer Attitudes," Marketing Letters, 2 (2), 159-70. Bearden, WILLIAM O and MICHAEL J Etzel (1982), "Reference Group Influence on Product and Brand Purchase Decisions," Joumal of Consumer Research, 9(2), 183-94. Bearden, William O., Subhash Sharma, and Jesse E. Teel (1982), "Sample Size Effects on Chi Square and Other Statistics Used in Evaluating Causal Models," Joumal of Marketing Research, 19 (4, Special Issue on Causal Modeling), 425-30. Beaudoin, Pierre and Marie J Lachance (2006), "Determinants of Adolescents' Brand Sensitivity to Clothing," Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal, 34 (4), 312-31. Belk, Russel W (2001), "Materialism and You," http://www.irconsumers.com/academic articles/issue 1/Belk .pdf. Belk, Russel W. (1985), "Materialism: Trait Aspects of Living in the Material World," The Journal of Consumer Research, 12 (3), 265-80. Belk, Russell, Robert Mayer, and Amy Driscoll (1984), "Children's Recognition of Consumption Symbolism in Children's Products," Journal of Consumer Research, 10 (4), 386-97. 92 Belk, Russell W. (1988), "Possessions and the Extended Self," The Joumal of Consumer Research, 15 (2), 139-68. Bhat, Subodh and Srinivas K. Reddy (1998), "Symbolic and Functional Positioning of Brands," The Journal of Consumer Marketing, 15 (1 ), 32- 39. Blalock, Hubert M. ( 1985), Causal Models in the Social Sciences, Chigago, IL: Aldine Transaction. Bollen, KA (1989), Structural Equations with Latent Variables, New York: John Wiley & Sons. Bose, Deepti, Ritwik Mukherjee, Nitya Varadarajan, Rahul Sachitanand, and Pallavi Srivastava (2006), "Young, Rich & Restless; Seduced by Luxury Brands and Armed with a Pile of Riches Salted Away by Their Working Mothers and Fathers, Roughly 40 Million Indian Youth in the 15-21 Age Bracket Are Splurging Like There's No Tomonow-Back-of-the-Envelope Calculations Reveal They Could Be Spending Close to Rs 1,90,000 Crore of Their Parents' Money Every Year. Nobody's Complaining-Not the Parents nor Marketers," Business Today (Dec 2006), 96-101. Boume and S. Francis (1957), "Group Influence in Marketing and Public Relations," in Some Applications of Behavioral Research, ed. R. Likert and S. P. Hayes, Basel, Switzerland: UNESCO. Brasil News (2000), "O Adolescente E a Sociedade De Consumo," http://www.brasilnewscom.br/fonte2.php?Codreq=74&CodNext=999. Brewer, Marilynn B. and Miles Hewstone (2004), Self and Social Identity, Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. Bristol, Terry and Tamara F. Mangleburg (2005), "Not Telling the Whole Story: Teen Deception in Purchasing," Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 33(1), 79-95. Brown, B. B., D. R. Clasen, and S. A Eicher (1986), "Perceptions of Peer Pressure, Peer Conformity Dispositions, and Self-Reported Behavior among Adolescents," Developmental Psychology, 22, 521-30. 93 Bruin, Pailo Celso (2003), "Geracao Teen," http://wwwteenaqercom.br/potencial consumophp. Burgess, SM and JBEM Steenkamp (2006), "Marketing Renaissance: How Research in Emerging Markets Advances Marketing Science and Practice," lntemational Journal of Research in Marketing, 23 (4), 337- 56. Campbell, E.Q. (1969), "Adolescent Socialization," in Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research, ed. David A. Goslin, Chicago, IL: Rand McNally and Co. Campbell, J. D (1990), "Self-Esteem and Clarity of the Self-Concept," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. J (59), 538-49. Campbell, JD, PD Trapnell, SJ Heine, IM Katz, LF Lavallee, and DR Lehman (1996), "Self-Concept Clarity: Measurement, Personality Correlates, and Cultural Boundaries," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(1), 141-56. Chang, L. and R. M. Arkin (2002), "Materialism as an Attempt to Cope with Uncertainty," Psychology & Marketing, 19, 389-406. Cheng, Man-Shin, Helen Cripps, and Cheng-Hsui Chen (2006), "The Relationships between Perceived Quality, Perceived Value, and Purchase Intentions - a Study in Internet Marketing," in ANZMAC, ed. Elizabeth Macpherson and Ingrid Larkin, Brisbaine, Australia: The School of Advertising, Marketing and Public Relations, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia, 7. Cheung, Wah-Leung and Gerard P Prendergast (2006), "Exploring the Materialism and Conformity Motivations of Chinese Pirated Product Buyers," Joumal of lntemational Consumer Marketing, 18 (3), 7-31. Christopher, A. N. and B. R. Schlenker (2004), " Materialism and Affect: The Role of Self-Presentational Concerns," Joumal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, 260-72. Churchill, GA (1979), "A Paradigm for Developing Better Measures of Marketing Constructs," Journal of Marketing Research, 16 (1), 64-73. 94 Churchill, GILBERT A and GEORGE P Moschis (1979), "Television and Interpersonal Influences on Adolescent Consumer Learning," Journal of Consumer Research, 6 (1), 23-35. Cia.gov (2008), "The World Factbook- Brazil," https:l/www.cia.gov/Iibrary/publications/the-world-factbook/index.html. Circuito MT (2009), "Lojas Do Pantanal Shopping Realizam Promocbes Especiais Para 0 Dia Dos Pais " in Circuito Mato Grosso, Cuiaba. Clark, RA, JJ Zboja, and RE Goldsmith (2007), "Status Consumption and Role-Relaxed Consumption: A Tale of Two Retail Consumers," Joumal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 14 (1 ), 45-59. Clark, RONALD A. (2006), "Consumer Independence: Conceptualization, Measurement and Validation of a Previously Unmeasured Social Response Tendency," Department of Marketing, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL. Cohen, Roger (2008), "Brazilian Lessons for 2008," New York Times, Jan, 3. Cornell, A (2002), "Cult of Luxury: The Opiate of the Masses," Australian Financial Review, 47. Cortina, JM, G Chen, and WP Dunlap (2001), "Testing Interaction Effects in Lisrel: Examination and Illustration of Available Procedures," Organizational Research Methods, 4 (4), 324. Coulter, Robin A, Linda L Price, and Lawrence Feick (2003), "Rethinking the Origins of Involvement and Brand Commitment: Insights from Postsocialist Central Europe," Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (2), 1 51 -82. Cross, SE and L Madson (1997), "Models of the Self: Self-Construals and Gender.," Psycho! Bulletin, 122 (1), 38-44. Czellar, Sandor (2007, June 30), "Personal Communication." 95 D'Angelo, Andre Cauduro (2009), "Verdadeiro Ou Falso?," http://www.pfistaodoluxo.com.br/gestao luxo novo/segmentos/moda/m oda16.asp. Daniel, Isaura (2009), "O Luxo Do Brasil Vai Ao Exterior," http://www.anba.com.br/noticia modaefranquia.kmf?cod=8198922. Dawson, EM and E Chatman (2001), "Reference Group Theory with Implications for Information Studies: A Theoretical Essay," Information Research, 6 (3), 6-13. DeAngelis, Tori (2004), "Consumerism and Its Discontents," Monitor on Psychology, 35(6), 52-54. Diamantopoulos, A and HM Winklhofer (2001), "Index Construction with Formative Indicators: An Alternative to Scale Development," Journal of Marketing Research, 38(2), 269-77. Dittrnar, Helga, Jane Beattie, and Susanne F riese (1995), "Gender Identity and Material Symbols: Objects and Decision Considerations in Impulse Purchases," Journal of Economic Psychology, 16 (3), 491-511. Dolliver, Mark (2009), "Necessity or Luxury?," in Adweek, Vol. 50, AM22. Douglas, M. and B. Isherwood (1979), The World of Goods: Toward Anthrophology of Consumption, New York: Basic Books. Dubois, B and G Laurent (1996), "T he Functions of Luxury: A Situational Approach to Excursionism," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 23, ed. Kim P. Corfrnan and John G. Lynch Jr, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 470-77. Dubois, B, G Laurent, and S Czellar (2001), "Consumer Rapport to Luxury: Analyzing Complex and Ambivalent Attitudes," Jouy-en-Josas, France: Consumer Research Working Paper n°736, 1-56. Dubois, Bernard, Sandor Czellar, and Gilles Laurent (2005), "Consumer Segments Based on Attitudes toward Luxury: Empirical Evidence from Twenty Countries," Marketing Letters, 16 (2), 115-28. 96 Dubois, Bernard and Patrick Duquesne (1993), "Polarization Maps: A New Approach to Identifying and Assessing Competitive Position - the Case of Luxury Brands," Marketing and Research Today, 21 (2), 115- 24. Dubois, Bernard and Gilles Laurent (1994), "Attitudes Towards the Concept of Luxury: An Exploratory Analysis," in Asia Pacific Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 1, ed. Joseph A. Cote and Siew Meng Leong, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 273-78. Elliott, Richard and Clare Leonard (2004), "Peer Pressure and Poverty: Exploring Fashion Brands and Consumption Symbolism among Children of the 'British Poor'," Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 3 (4), 347-59. Erikson, Erik (1959), "Identity and the Life Cycle," Psychological Issues, 1 (1), 13-1 17. Ewing, Reese (2007), "Brazil's Santos Port Sets Movement Record in August," http://www.reuters.com/article/economicNews/idUSN09ANT08200710 Q. Field, A. P. (2005), Discovering Statistics Using Spss, London: Sage. Fields, Suzanne (1999), "Growing up on Fast Forward," The Washington Times, January, 4. F louri, Eirini (1999), "An Integrated Model of Consumer Materialism: Can Economic Socialization and Maternal Values Predict Materialistic Attitudes in Adolescents?," Journal of Social-Economics, 28 (6), 707- 24. Foumier, S and ML Richins (1991), "Some Theoretical and Popular Notions Concerning Materialism," Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 6 (6), 403-14. Friedman, James W. and Isabel Grilo (2005), "A Market with a Social Consumption Extemality," Japanese Economic Review, 56 (3), 251-72. 97 Galhanone, RENATA F ERNANDES (2005), "O Mercado Do Luxo: Aspectos De Marketing," in 8th Semead- FEA/USP, Federal University of 850 Paulo- USP/ Business department, ed. Universidade de 880 Paulo- USP/ Business department, Séo Paulo: Universidade de 860 Paulo- USP/ Business department, 12. Gallant, Katheryn (1995), "Young and Restful: Brazilian Teenagers and Brazil's Generation X Are Not Different from Their Counterpart in the Us," http://www.brazzil.com/paqes/cvraug95.htm. Garfein, RT (1989), "Cross-Cultural Perspectives on the Dynamics of Prestige," Journal of services marketing, 3 (3), 17-24. Garson, G. David (2007), "Structural Equation Modeling," http://www2.chass.ncsu.edu/garson/pa765/structur.htm. Gentry, James W., Sanjay Putrevu, Clifford Shultz, and Suraj Commuri (2001), "How Now Ralph Lauren? The Separation of Brand and Product in a Counterfeit Culture," in Association for Consumer Research, ed. Mary C. Gilly and Joan Meyers-Levy, Valdosta, GA, 258-65. Gergen, KJ and SJ Morse (1967), "Self-Consistency: Measurement and Validation," Proceedings of the 75th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, 2, 207-08. Goldberg, M. E., G. J. Gom, L. A. Peracchio, and G. Bamossy (2003), "Understanding Materialism among Youth.," Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13, 278-88. Gonzalez, B, E Huerta-Sénchez, A Ortiz-Nieves, T Vazquez-Alvarez, and C Kribs-Zaleta (2003), "Am I Too Fat? Bulimia as an Epidemic, " Joumal of Mathematical Psychology, 47, 515-26. Grubb, EL and HL Grathwohl (1967), "Consumer Self-Concept, Symbolism and Market Behavior: A Theoretical Approach," Journal of Marketing, 31 (4), 22-27. Grzeskowiak, S and MJ Sirgy (2007), "Consumer Well-Being (Cwb): The Effects of Self-Image Congruence, Brand-Community Belongingness, 98 Brand Loyalty, and Consumption Recency," Applied Research in Quality of Life, 2 (4), 289-304. Guaiquil, Rodrigo (2006), "52,7% De Adolescentes Chilenos Tiene Celular," http://mediosdigitales.info/2006/10/19/527-de-adolescentes-chilenos- tiene-celular/. Gudykunst, WB and CM Lee (2003), "Assessing the Validity of Self Construal Scales," Human Communication Research, 29(2), 253-74. Gudykunst, WB, Y Matsumoto, S Ting-Toomey, T Nishida, and H Karimi (1994), "Measuring Self Construals across Cultures: A Derived Etic Analysis," lntemational Communication Association Convention in Sydney, Australia, July. Gudykunst, WB, Y Matsumoto, S Ting-Toomey, T Nishida, K Kim, and S Heyman (1996), "The Influence of Cultural lndividualism-Collectivism, Self Construals, and Individual Values on Communication Styles across Cultures," Human Communication Research, 22 (4), 510-43. GuIIand, Chet (2006), "The Ten Biggest Themes of What Teens Want'," http://www.ypulse.com/the-ten-biggest-themes-of—what-teens-want. Hackman, Michael Z, Kathleen Ellis, Craig E Johnson, and Constance Staley (1999), "Self-Construal Orientation: Validation of an Instrument and a Study of the Relationship to Leadership Communication Style," Communication Quarteriy, 47 (2), 183-95. Hader, Suzanne (2008), 'Wooing Luxury Customers: To Win over Today's Upscale Customers, Brands Must Ensure a Flawlessly Engaging and Emotional Experience with Every Interaction," Marketing Management, 27-32. Hair, J. F., Jr., R. E. Anderson, R. L. Tatham, and W. C. Black (1998), Multivariate Data Analysis with Readings, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Harter, S, NR Whitesell, and P Kowalski (1992), "Individual Differences in the Effects of Educational Transitions on Young Adolescent's Perceptions 99 of Competence and Motivational Orientation," American Educational Research Journal, 777-807. Hartman, Jonathan B, Soyeon Shim, Bonnie Barber, and Matthew O'Brien (2006), "Adolescents' Utilitarian and Hedonic Web Consumption Behavior: Hierarchical Influence of Personal Values and Innovativeness," Psychology & Marketing, 23 (10), 813-39. Heaney, Joo-Gim, Ronald E Goldsmith, and Wan Jamaliah Wan Jusoh (2005), "Status Consumption among Malaysian Consumers: Exploring Its Relationships with Materialism and Attention-to-Social-Comparison- Information," Joumal of lntemational Consumer Marketing, 17 (4), 83- 98. Higgins, ET, RN Bond, R Klein, and T Strauman (1986), "Self-Discrepancies and Emotional Vulnerability: How Magnitude, Accessibility, and Type of Discrepancy Influence Affect," Joumal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (1), 5-15. Hopson, B and A Adams (1976), "Toward an Understanding of Transition: Defining Some Boundaries of Transition Dynamics," in Transition: Understanding and Managing Personal Changes, ed. J Adams, J Hayes and B Hopson, London, UK: Martin Robertson. Hovanesian, Mara Der (1999), "Spending It, Investing It - Coming on Strong: The Children of the Baby Boomers Are Affecting Spending and Investing as Significantly as Their Parents Did; the Similarity Ends There," Wall Street Journal, Nov 29, R12. Hu, L. and P. M. Bentler (1999), "Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indices in Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria Versus New Alternatives," Structural Equation Modeling, 6 (1 ), 1-55. Huebner, Angela J and Jay A Mancini (2003), "Shaping Structured out-of- School Time Use among Youth: The Affects of Self, Family, and Friend Systems," Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32 (6), 453-63. Hui, CH (1988), "Measurement of Individualism-Collectivism," Journal of research in personality, 22 (1 ), 17-36. 100 IBGE Brazilian Census (2000), "Population Data," http://www.ipregovbr/brasil em sintese/default.htm. -- (2002), "Pib Dos Municipios Revela Concentracao E Desigualdades Na Geracao De Renda," http://www.ibng.br/home/presidencia/noticias/noticia visualizaphp? id noticia=354. -- (2007), "Populacao Residente - Séo Paulo," http://tabnet.datasus.qov.br. Jacobs, Bruce (2005), "Family Health & Wellness - Child/Youth Health - Peer Pressure," http://spectre.nmsu.edu/Dept/academic.html?i=425&s=sub. James, William (1890), The Principles of Psychology, Vol. 1, New York, NY: Henry Holt. John, DR. (1999), "Consumer Socialization of Children: A Retrospective Look at Twenty-Five Years of Research," Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (3), 183-213. Jereskog, KG and D Sbrbom (1996), Lisrel 8, Chicago, IL: Scientific Software lntemational. -- (2004), "Lisrel for W1ndows," Lincolnwood, IL: SSSI Scientific Software. Kapferer, Jean-Noel (1998), 'Why Are We Seduced by Luxury Brands?," The Journal of Brand Management, 6 (1 ), 44-49. Kasser, T (1995), "The Relations of Maternal and Social Environments to Late Adolescents," Developmental Psychology, 31 (6), 907-14. Kasser, Tim (2002), The High Price of Materialism, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Katz, D (1960), "The Functional Approach to the Study of Attitudes," Public Opinion Quarterly, 24(2), 163-204. 101 Kemis, MH, AW Paradise, DJ Whitaker, SR Wheatman, and BN Goldman (2000), "Master of One's Psychological Domain? Not Likely If One's Self-Esteem Is Unstable," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26 (10), 1297. Keston, Joan (2007), "Another Bric in Economic Wall - the Arrival of Developing Countries," [mg/llocaltechwire.com/business/Iocal tech wire/opinion/storv/186729 2_/. Kim, JS, MK Baik, and JH Kwon (2002), "A Comparison of Korean, Japanese and Chinese Consumers' Perceptions of the Luxury Product," lntemational Joumal of Digital Management, 2. Kitayama, Shlnobu (2000), "Collective Construction of the Self and Social Relationships: A Rejoinder and Some Extensions," Child Development, 71 (5), 1143-46. Kline, P (2000), The Handbook of Psychological Testing, New York, NY: Routledge. Kotler, P (1965), "Behavioral Models for Analyzing Buyers," The Joumal of Marketing, 37-45. Krech, D, RS Crutchfleld, and EL Ballachey (1962), Individual in Society: A Textbook of Social Psychology. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Lachance, Marie J, Pierre Beaudoin, and Jean Robitaille (2003), "Adolescents' Brand Sensitivity in Apparel: Influence of Three Socialization Agents," lntemational Journal of Consumer Studies, 27 (1 ), 47-57. LaFerIe, C, S. M. Edwards, and W. N. Lee (2000), "Teen's Use of Traditional Media and the lntemet," Journal of Advertising Research, 40 (3), 5566. Lancaster, B. P. (1999), "Defining and Interpreting Suppressor Effects: Advantages and Limitations," in Annual Meeting of the Southwest Educational Research Association, San Antonio, TX, 21. 102 Larsen, V, MJ Sirgy, and ND Wright (1999), "Materialism: The Construct, Measures, Antecedents, and Consequences," Academy of Marketing Studies Joumal, 3 (2), 75-107. Lima, Isabelle Moreira and Fernando Scheller (2009), "Grifes Estrangeiras Desembarcam No Brasil, Apesar Da Crise," http://gazetaonline.globo.com/ conteudo/2009/02/55894- grifes+estrangeiras+desembarcam+no+brasil+apesar+da+crise.html. Lueg, Jason E, Nicole Ponder, Sharon E Beatty, and Michael L Capella (2006), "Teenagers' Use of Alternative Shopping Channels: A Consumer Socialization Perspective," Joumal of Retailing, 82 (2), 137- 53. MacKinnon, D. P., J. L. Krull, and C. M. Lockwood (2000), "Equivalence of the Mediation, Confounding and Suppression Effect," Prevention Science, 1 (4). Mangleburg, Tamara F, Patricia M Doney, and Terry Bristol (2004), "Shopping with Friends and Teens' Susceptibility to Peer Influence," Joumal of Retailing, 80(2), 101-16. Markus, H R and S Kitayama (1998), "The Cultural Psychology of Personality," Journal of cross cultural psychology, 29, 63-87. Markus, H. R. and E Wurf (1987), "The Dynamic Self-Concept: A Social Psychological Perspective," Annual Review of Psychology, 38 (1 ), 299- 337. Markus, Hazel Rose and Shinobu Kitayama (1991), "Culture and the Self: Implications for Cognition, Emotion, and Motivation," Psychological Review, 98 (2), 224-53. Martin, CA and LW Turley (2003), "Malls and Generation Y Consumers: A Consumption Motivation Perspective," http://wwwwku.edu/gfcb/papers/‘I’ urlev1 .pdf. -- (2004), "Malls and Consumption Motivation: An Exploratory Examination of Older Generation Y Consumers," lntemational Journal of Retail & Distribution Management, 32 (10), 464-75. 103 Mason, Roger (1981 ), Conspicuous Consumption, New York, NY: St. Martin's Press. Meeus, W, A Oosterwegel, and W Vollebergh (2002), "Parental and Peer Attachment and Identity Development in Adolescence," Joumal of Adolescence, 25 (1), 93-106. Michell, L and A Amos (1997), "Girls, Pecking Order and Smoking," Social Science & Medicine, 44 (12), 1861-69. Mitchell, R. J. (1993), "Path Analysis: Pollination," in Design and Analaysis of Ecological Experiements, ed. S. M. Schneider and J. Gurevitch, New York, NY: Chapman and Hall, 211-31. MMR (2000), "Mass Market Retailers Woo Growing Teen Market," http://flndarticles.comlp/articles/mi hb3235/is 15 17/ai n28791322l. Montemayor, Raymond and Marvin Eisen (1977), "The Development of Self- Conceptions from Childhood to Adolescence," Developmental Psychology, 13 (4), 314-21 9. Mortelmans, D (2005), "Sign Values in Processes of Distinction: The Concept of Luxury," Semiotica, 157 (1-4), 497-520. Moschis, George P and Gilbert A Churchill (1978), "Consumer Socialization: A Theoretical and Empirical Analysis," JMR, Journal of Marketing Research, 15 (4), 599-609. Moschis, George P. (1981), "Socialization Perspectives and Consumer Behavior," in Review in Marketing, ed. Ben M. Enis and Kenneth J. Roering, Chicago, Illinois: American Marketing Association, 43-53. Moses, Elissa (2000), The $ 100 Billion Allowance- Accessing the Global Teen Market, New York: John Wiley & Sons Inc. Mouton, Johann and Roland Waast (2006), "The Unesco Forum on Higher Education, Research and Knowledge. Study on National Research Systems," 104 httpzllportal. unesco.or_q/education/en/files/522fl3/1 1694606785Mouton Progress Report.pdf/Mouton Progress+Rngort.pdf. Neisser, U (1993), The Perceived Self: Ecological and Interpersonal Sources of Self-Knowledge, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Nelson, Michelle R. and Laurie Ellis McLeod (2005), "Adolescent Brand Consciousness and Product Placements: Awareness, Liking and Perceived Effects on Self and Others," lntemational Journal of Consumer Studies, 29 (6), 515-28. Nezlek, JB and RM Plesko (2001), "Day-to-Day Relationships among Self- Concept Clarity, Self-Esteem, Daily Events, and Mood," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27 (2), 201. Nueno, Jose Luis and John A Quelch (1998), "The Mass Marketing of Luxury," Business Horizons, 41 (6), 61-68. Nunnally, J. and l. Bernstein (1994), Psychometric Theory, New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Nyamathi, A, JA Stein, and ML Brecht (1995), "Psychosocial Predictors of Aids Risk Behavior and Drug Use Behavior in Homeless and Drug Addicted Women of Color," Health Psychology, 14 (3), 265-73. O'Donnell, Jayne (2007), "Teens Targeted with Cell Phone Marketing," httpzllwww. usatodav. com/tech/wireless/ghones/2007-03-1 9-cellphone- marketing N.htm. Osborne, Marcus (2009), "Luxury Branding in Malaysia & Asia," httpzllbrandconsuItantasia.wordpress.com/2009/06/12/luxurv-brandinq; in-malaysia-asial. Osmar, Freitas Jr (2009), "Crise Nao Abala Consumo De Luxo Nos Estados Unidos," . httpzllibonlineterracom.br/nextra/2_009/02/20/e200215196ag. 105 Park, C. WHAN and V. PARKER Lessig (1977), "Students and Housewives - Differences in Susceptibility to Reference Group Influence," Journal of Consumer Research, 4 (2), 102-10. Parsons, T., R. F. Bales, and E. A. Shils (1953), Working Papers in the Theory of Action, Glencoe, IL: The Free Press. Prajogo, DI and SW Hong (2008), "The Effect of Tqm on Performance in R&D Environments: A Perspective from South Korean Firms," Technovation, 28 (12), 855-63. Prajogo, DI and CM McDen'nott (2005), "The Relationship between Total Quality Management Practices and Organizational Culture," lntemational Joumal of Operations and Production Management, 25 (1 1), 1 101. Prendergast, Gerard and Claire Wong (2003), "Parental Influence on the Purchase of Luxury Brands of Infant Apparel: An Exploratory Study in Hong Kong," The Joumal of Consumer Marketing, 20 (213), 157-69. Puntoni, Stefano (2001), "Self-Identity and Purchase Intention: An Extention of the Theory of Planned Behavior," in European Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 5, ed. Andrea GroeppeI-Klien and F rank-Rudolf Esch, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 130-34. Quart, Alissa (2003), "EIes Gastam Muito -Com Um Apetite Consumista Maior Que 0 Da Media 03 Populacao, O Jovem Brasileiro Sabe Onde Quer Gastar E Ainda Influencia as Compras Da Familia," http:l/veia.abril.com.br/especiais/iovens 2003/p 080.html. Reisman, David and Howard Roseborough (1955), "Careers and Consumer Behavior," in Consumer Behavior, Vol. li, ed. Lincoln Clark, New York, NY: New York University Press. Richins, Marsha L (1994a), "Special Possessions and the Expression of Material Values," Joumal of Consumer Research, 21 (3), 522-33. Richins, Marsha L. (1994b), "Valuing Things: The Public and Private Meanings of Possessions," The Journal of Consumer Research, 21 (3), 504-21. 106 Richins, Marsha L. and Scott Dawson (1992), "A Consumer Values Orientation for Materialism and Its Measurement: Scale Development and Validation," The Journal of Consumer Research, 19 (3), 303-16. Roberts, JA and A Clement (2007), "Materialism and Satisfaction with over-All Quality of Life and Eight Life Domains," Social Indicators Research, 82 (1), 79-92. Roberts, James A, Chris Manolis, and John F Tanner (2006), "Adolescent Autonomy and the Impact of Family Structure on Materialism and Compulsive Buying," Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 14 (4), 301. Robin, S. S. and E. 0. Johnson (1996), "Attitude and Peer Cross Pressure: Adolescent Drug Use and Alcohol Use," Joumal of Dnrg Education, 26 (1), 69-99. Roedder-John, D (1999), "Consumer Socialization of Children: A Retrospective Look at Twenty-Five Years of Research," Joumal of Consumer Research, 26 (December), 183-213. Rose, Paul and Stephanie P DeJesus (2007), "A Model of Motivated Cognition to Account for the Link between Self-Monitoring and Materialism," Psychology & Marketing, 24 (2), 93. Rossiter, JR (2005), "Reminder. A Horse Is a Horse," lntemational Journal of Research in Marketing, 22 (1 ), 23-25. Ruffin, Jane (2007), "Luxuries Are Things You Wouldn't Buy If No One Saw You Use Them," httpzllwww.newsobserver.com/690/storv/539390.html. Saad, Flavia (2007), "Mercado Mira Pré-Adolescentes," A Tribuna, Julho 25, A2. Santor, D.A., D. Messervey, and V. Kusumakar (2000), "Measuring Peer Pressure, Popularity, and Conformity in Adolescent Boys and Girls: Predicting School Performance, Sexual Attitudes, and Substance Abuse," Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 29 (2), 163-82. 107 Schaefer, Allen 0., Charles M. Herrnans, and R. Stephen Parker (2004), "A Cross-Cultural Exploration of Materialism in Adolescents," lntemational Journal of Consumer Studies, 28 (4), 399-41 1. Schmitt, John (2000), "Economic Snapshots-the Myth of Economic Mobility," http://www.epi.org/content.cfm/webfeatures snapshots archive 07192 @- Seringhaus, F HR (2005), "Comparison of Website Usage of French and Italian Luxury Brands," JOURNAL OF EUROMARKE TING, 14 (4), 5-34. Shavelson, RJ, JJ Hubner, and GC Stanton (1976), "Self-Concept: Validation of Construct Interpretations," Review of Educational Research, 46 (3), 407-41. Sherman, Lauren (2009), "The World's Most Powerful Luxury Brands," http://www.vancouversun.com/business/fp/world+most+powerful+luxurv +brands/1569498/story.html. Shoham, Aviv and Vassilis Dalakas (2006), "How Our Adolescent Children Influence Us as Parents to Yield to Their Purchase Requests," The Journal of Consumer Marketing, 23 (6), 344-50. Silvennan (1998), "Today's Savy, High-Tech Teens Are Eager to Spend: They Are the Most Educated, Computer Adept and Affluent Generation in Story," in Footwear news, Vol. 54, 36. Simon, Laura J. (2007), "Detecting Multicollinearity Using Variance Inflation Factors," http://www.stat.psu.edu/online/development/stat501I12multicollinearity/ 05multico vif.htm|. Singelis, TM (1994), "The Measurement of Independent and Interdependent Self-Construals," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20 (5), 580- 91. Sirgy, Joseph M (1982), "Self-Concept in Consumer Behavior: A Critical Review," Joumal of Consumer Research, 9 (Dec), 287-537. 108 —F‘A‘§'Jq Sirgy, MJ (1998), "Materialism and Quality of Life," Social Indicators Research, 43(3), 227-60. Sirgy, MJ, DJ Lee, JS Johar, and J Tidwell (2008), "Effect of Self-Congruity with Sponsorship on Brand Loyalty," Journal of Business Research, 61 (10), 1091-97. Sirgy, MJ, DJ Lee, V Larsen, and N Wright (1998), "Satisfaction with Material Possessions and General Well-Being: The Role of Materialism," JOURNAL OF CONSUMER SATISFACTION DISSA TISFACTION AND COMPLAINING BEHAVIOR, 11, 103-18. Smith, JR, DJ Terry, ASR Manstead, WR Louis, D Kotterrnan, and J Wolfs (2008), "The Attitude—Behavior Relationship in Consumer Conduct: The Role of Norms, Past Behavior, and Self-Identity," The Journal of Social Psychology, 148(3), 311-34. Solomon, Michael R. (1983), "The Role of Products as Social Stimuli: A Symbolic Interactionism Perspective," The Journal of Consumer Research, 10 (3), 319-29. Steiger, JH (1990), "Structural Model Evaluation and Modification: An Interval Estimation Approach," Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25 (2), 173- 80. Steinberg, L (1993), Adolescence, New York: McGraw Hill. Strehlau, Suzane (2008), Marketing Do Luxo, Vol. 1, $50 Paulo/SP: Cengage . Learning. Strizhakova, Y, RA Coulter, and LL Price (2008), "Branded Products as a Passport to Global Citizenship: Perspectives from Developed and Developing Countries," Journal of lntemational Marketing, 16 (4), 57- 85. Swaminathan, Vanitha, Karen L. Page, and Zeynep Gurhan-Canli (2007), ""My" Brand Or "Our" Brand: The Effects of Brand Relationship Dimensions and Self-Construal on Brand Evaluations," Joumal of Consumer Research, 34(2), 248-59. 109 Teenage Research Unlimited (2004), "Teens Consumption around Globe," hflp://W.tru-inflht.com/pressrelease.cfm?page id=287. Tevlin, Jon (1999), "Teens Have Plenty of Cash These Days," http://archive.southcoasttodav.com/dain/12-99/12-25- 99/a08bu046.htm. Tian, Kelly Tepper, W1lliam O Bearden, and Gary L Hunter (2001), "Consumers' Need for Uniqueness: Scale Development and Validation," Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (1 ), 50-66. Tidwell, Paula and Bernard Dubois (1996), "A Cross-Cultural Comparison of Attitudes toward the Luxury Concept in Australia and France," in Asia Pacific Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 2, ed. Russel Belk and Ronald Groves, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 31-35. Tootelian, Dennis H. and Ralph M. Gaedeke (1992), "The Teen Market: An Exploratory Analysis of Income, Spending, and Shopping Patterns," The Journal of Consumer Marketing, 9 (4), 35-44. Triandis, H.C. (1989), "The Self and Social Behavior in Differing Cultural Contexts," Psychological Review, 96 (3), 506-20. Troiano, Jaime (1997), "Brazilian Teenagers Go Global - Sharing Values and Brands," Marketing and Research Today, 25(3), 149. Tsai, S (2006), "Investigating How the Message Framing Strategy Impacts Brand Marketing," Web Joumal of Chinese Management Review, 9 (2), 1-29. Tsai, SP (2005), "Impact of Personal Orientation on Luxury-Brand Purchase Value: An lntemational Investigation," lntemational Joumal of Market Research, 47 (4), 429-54. Unity Marketing Inc. (2007), "Generations of Luxury: What 'Young Affluents' Want and What It Means to Luxury Marketers," mtg/Iwww.marketresearch.com/map/prod/1489810.html. 110 Venkatesan, M (1966), "Experimental Study of Consumer Behavior Conformity and Independence," Journal of Marketing Research, 3 (4), 384-87. Verrna, J (1992), "Allocentrism and Relational Orientation," in Innovation in Cross-Cultural Psychology, ed. Y S. Iwaki, Kashima, 8 K. Leung, Lisse, Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger, 152-63. Vigneron, Franck (1998), "Multiple-Item Scale for Measuring Prestige Perceptions of Brands," School of Business, The University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia. Vigneron, Franck and Lester W Johnson (2004), "Measuring Perceptions of Brand Luxury," Joumal of Brand Management, 11 (6), 484-506. Vigneron, Franck and Lester W. Johnson (1999), "A Review and a Conceptual Framework of Prestige-Seeking Consumer Behavior," Academy of Marketing Science Review, 1999, 1-15. Villaire, Ted (2004), 'When Kids Want Too Much - Curing a Case of The "Gimmies"," in Our Children, Vol. 27, 4-5. Wang, Jeff and Melanie Wallendorf (2006), "Materialism, Status Signaling, and Product Satisfaction," The Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 34(4), 494-505. Ward, Scott and Daniel Wackman (1971), "Family and Media Influences on Adolescent Consumer Learning," American Behavioral Scientist, 14 (3), 41 5-28. Wells, Ken R. (2006), "Gale Encyclopedia of Children's Health- Peer Pressure," httpzllwww.healthline.com/galecontent/peer-pressure-1. Wharton.universia.net (2008), "Marcas De Luxo: Vendas De Itens Sofisticados Em Tempos De Crise," http://www.wharton.universia.net/index.cfm?fa=viewArticle&id=1615&Ia nguage=ponuguese. 111 Wiedmann, KP, N Hennigs, and A Siebels (2007), "Measuring Consumers’ Luxury Value Perception: A Cross-Cultural Framework," Academy of Marketing Science Review, 7, 1-21. -- (2009), "Value-Based Segmentation of Luxury Consumption Behavior," Psychology and Marketing, 26 (7), 625-51. W1Ison, Eric (2009), "Losing Its Cool at the Mall," New York Times, April, 23, E1. Wong, Nancy Y and Aaron C Ahuvia (1998), "Personal Taste and Family Face: Luxury Consumption in Confucian and Western Societies," Psychology & Marketing, 15 (5), 423-41. Wong, Nancy Y., Aric Rindfleisch, and James E Burroughs (2003), "Do Reverse-Worded Items Confound Measures in Cross-Cultural Consumer Research? The Case of the Material Values Scale," Journal of Consumer Research, 30 (1 ), 72-91. Wong, NYC (1997), "Suppose You Own the World and No One Knows? Conspicuous Consumption, Materialism, and Self," Advances in Consumer Research, 24, 197-203. Wooten, David B (2006), "From Labeling Possessions to Possessing Labels: Ridicule and Socialization among Adolescents," Journal of Consumer Research, 33(2), 188-98. Yamaguchi, S (1994), "Collectivism among the Japanese: A Perspective from the Self," in Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method, and Applications, Vol. 18, ed. H. Triandis U. Kim, C. Kagitcibasi, S. Choi, & G. Yoon, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 175—88. Youniss, J. and J. Smollar (1985), "The Individual and Relations," in Adolescent Relations with Mothers, Fathers and Friends, ed. J. Youniss and J. Smollar, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 160-76. Ziccardi, DP (2001), "Demonstrating the Values of Luxury Brands," Brandweek, 42 (44), 18-19. 112 Zinkhan, GM (1994), "Advertising, Materialism, and Quality of Life," Joumal of Advertising, 23(2), 1-4. Zollo, Peter (1999), "Not Quite the Tv Generation," American Demographics, 21 (5), 35-37. 113 "illiijlijjjljiflIlljijljilijljjijilli”