tin u. «.qu . . 23...: ... s . (11,4? {mgr Y1 ... MR . , .ghfi -Ififihumn 74V .tN“ (4 .H 153...! ‘59:... 03.1. I :0 . t 9;: | In. its 1.4.3.59 . .5 3115...“ x. if fil riJUflnnC.’ but: . 3...; 1.. . 3. SI 1.. i... .5. HR 265.. “fififiuufiwilm , . . , . , . . . ‘ . 1 . . . . Maxi? . rumL‘ 1. .h v. .F n.3, _LlBRARY 4:3! 3 Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled THE INFLUENCE OF ALTERNATIVE PEDAGOGICAL METHODS IN POSTSECONDARY BIOLOGY EDUCATION: HOW DO STUDENTS EXPERIENCE A MULTIMEDIA CASE- STUDY ENVIRONMENT? presented by BJORN HUGO KARL WOLTER has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the PhD degree in Higher, Adult. and Lifelong Education a K/m flr’9’4 Major Professor’s Signature 7‘2‘5~/0 Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Employer -.-.-.- onu... un—u-.-—--.-.u--.—.—-—-—-—-—.-.-.-.--.—.-- PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5’08 K:IProjIAcc&PreleIRC/DateDue.indd THE INFLUENCE OF ALTERNATIVE PEDAGOGICAL METHODS IN POSTSECONDARY BIOLOGY EDUCATION: HOW DO STUDENTS EXPERIENCE A MULTIMEDIA CASE-STUDY ENVIRONMENT? By Bjorn Hugo Karl Wolter A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Higher, Adult, and Lifelong Education 2010 ABSTRACT THE INFLUENCE OF ALTERNATIVE PEDAGOGICAL METHODS IN POSTSECONDARY BIOLOGY EDUCATION: HOW DO STUDENTS EXPERIENCE A MULTIMEDIA CASE-STUDY ENVIRONMENT? BY Bjorn Hugo Karl Wolter The purpose of this study was to better understand how an online, multimedia case study method influenced students’ motivation, performance, and perceptions of science in collegiate level biology classes. It utilized a mix- methods design including data from pre- and post-test, student surveys, and focus group interviews to answer one primary question, did participation in the affect student performance? Two sub-questions were: (a) did participation affect persistence? and (b) did students believe it to be a good learning experience? One hundred and eight students in 5 classes from 4 campuses in the United States and Puerto Rico participated in this study during spring semester 2009. After receiving instruction on HIV, students took a 6 questions pre-test to measure their initial knowledge of both HIV and lab procedures. Participants then engaged in the Case It! learning environment, where they watched case- studies on HIV, used virtual lab tools, created an online poster of their findings, and role-played as both family members and physicians about their case. A post-test identical to the pre-test was given to students upon completion. Both were then scored using rubrics and analyzed via paired t-Tests and ANOVA. The researcher visited all 4 study sites to conduct both the focus group interviews and student surveys. Student surveys were quantified and descriptive statistic generated. Focus group interviews were video recorded, transcribed, and inductively and deductively coded. Student knowledge increased because of participation, and the majority of - students said they found the Case Itl project to be both a good learning experience (95%) and one that would help with future classes or careers (87%). Based on student interviews, the Case It! project did have a beneficial impact on students’ intentions to persist as science majors. Many students noted that the learning environment created an overall context in which they could apply knowledge from multiple classes that allowed students to fit all the pieces of their previous academic instruction together into a single, comprehensive picture—and to place themselves within that picture. Students enjoyed the autonomy and personal connections that using case studies and multimedia content offered, and found the material more engaging and relevant. By involving students in real-world situations, Case Itl demonstrated the application and effect of theoretical knowledge and stimulated students’ curiosity. Case Itl appears to be a learning environment that motivates students by making material relevant and personal, thus creating enduring links between students and content which can result in better performance and higher retention rates. It is an effective pedagogical tool that, unlike many other such tools, is not instructor dependent, and is adaptable to fit various learner types, settings, and levels. DEDICATION This dissertation is dedicated to my family: to my wife, Stefanie, whose support, love, and encouragement kept me at it through the good, the bad, and the utterly neurotic times; to my daughters, Solveig and Aylin, who motivated me to better myself to make a better life for them; and to my mother, Mary, proofreader and babysitter extraordinaire. I could never have done this without any of you. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I gratefully acknowledge the support of the many people who made this study possible: Dr. Mary Lundeberg, my dissertation co-chair and advisor, was instrumental in all aspects of my doctoral education. Because of her, I was involved with research projects from my first day at MSU, was able to network with researchers and practitioners across the continent, and have had numerous opportunities to present and publish research. Her advice, expertise, direction, patience, and support are greatly appreciated. Dr. John Dirkx, my dissertation co-chair and departmental advisor, has helped me navigate the sometimes confusing pathways of the doctoral degree process. I appreciate the time and effort he has invested in me. Drs. Karen Klyczek, Mark Bergland, Catherine White, Rafael Tosado, and Arlin Toro for helping me develop my survey instruments, and allowing me to collect data in their classes. Their knowledge, input, and support throughout this process was invaluable to me. I also thank Dr. Bergland again for allowing me unlimited access to Case Itl and for supporting my research both financially and academically. All graduate students and faculty members before me who developed the assessment tools and rubrics I modified for use in my study, including Hosun Kang, Aroutis Foster, Viola Manokore, Mark Bergland, and Mary Lundeberg. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................................. x LIST OF FIGURES .............................................................................................. xii CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................... 1 Studies of pedagogy and persistence .............................................................. 2 Studies from the field of engineering ................................................................ 7 Potential solutions ............................................................................................ 8 Deficiencies of previous studies ....................................................................... 9 Importance of the study .................................................................................. 10 Purpose .......................................................................................................... 11 CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW ....................................................................................... 12 The current state of postsecondary biology ................................................... 12 Content vs. pedagogy ............................................................................... 13 Individual vs. systemic instructional reform ............................................... 15 Pace of change ......................................................................................... 16 Bioinformatics ........................................................................................... 1 6 Reforms in STEM education ........................................................................... 17 Case-based instruction ............................................................................. 17 The growing use of instructional technology in STEM instruction ............. 20 Clickers ................................................................................................ 21 Games and computer simulated learning environments ..................... 24 Models of student motivation .......................................................................... 27 The ARCS model of student motivation .................................................... 28 Attention .............................................................................................. 29 Relevance ............................................................................................ 29 Confidence ................................................................................. 29 Satisfaction .......................................................................................... 31 The Expectancy x Value model of student motivation .............................. 32 Principle factors in motivating students to learn ........................................ 32 Motivation and persistence ....................................................................... 33 Research questions ........................................................................................ 34 CHAPTER 3 METHODS ........................................................................................................... 35 Study sites ...................................................................................................... 36 University of Wisconsin—River Falls .......................................................... 36 vi North Carolina A&T State University ......................................................... 37 Interamerican University of Puerto Rico—Metmpolitan Campus ............... 38 Interamerican University of Puerto Rico—San German ............................. 39 Participants ..................................................................................................... 39 Instructional intervention ................................................................................ 40 Case Itl simulation software ...................................................................... 42 Case It! launch pad ................................................................................... 42 Data collection ................................................................................................ 46 Development of instruments ..................................................................... 46 Learning assessment test .................................................................... 48 Student survey ..................................................................................... 50 Focus group interviews ........................................................................ 51 Collection procedures ............................................................................... 52 Learning assessment test .................................................................... 52 Student survey ..................................................................................... 52 Focus group interviews ........................................................................ 52 Data analysis .................................................................................................. 53 Learning assessment test ......................................................................... 53 Student survey .......................................................................................... 54 Focus group interviews ............................................................................. 55 Deductive coding ................................................................................. 55 Inductive coding ................................................................................... 55 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS ............................................................................................................ 57 Case study A: Cross-site comparison ............................................................ 57 Student performance ................................................................................ 57 Student intentions to persist ...................................................................... 65 Instructional quality .............................................................................. 66 Functional science (related to future work) .......................................... 72 Role-playing ......................................................................................... 74 Building self-efficacy ............................................................................ 77 Feeling like a scientist (community/integrated into science) ................ 78 Student beliefs .......................................................................................... 78 Diagnostic testing procedures ............................................................. 79 Learning ............................................................................................... 82 Science content ................................................................................... 84 Communication .................................................................................... 86 Multiple perspectives ........................................................................... 87 Turned-off science ............................................................................... 88 Stigma/denial/misconceptions ............................................................. 89 Need-to-know science ......................................................................... 90 Level of application ................................................................................... 91 Overview of case studies B-F ......................................................................... 92 vii Case study B: University of Wisconsin—River Falls (UWRF-NM) ................... 92 Overview ................................................................................................... 92 Student performance ................................................................................ 93 Student intentions to persist ...................................................................... 93 Student beliefs .......................................................................................... 97 Level of application ................................................................................... 99 Case study C: University of Wisconsin—River Falls (UWRF-M) ................... 100 Overview ................................................................................................. 100 Student performance .............................................................................. 100 Student intentions to persist .................................................................... 102 Student beliefs ........................................................................................ 105 Level of application ................................................................................. 108 Case study D: North Carolina State A&T University (NCA&T) ..................... 108 Overview ................................................................................................. 108 Student performance .............................................................................. 108 Student intentions to persist .................................................................... 109 Student beliefs ........................................................................................ 113 Level of application ................................................................................. 116 Case study E: Interamerican University of Puerto Rico-Metropolitan Campus (lUPR—M) ..................................................................................................... 116 Overview ................................................................................................. 116 Student performance .............................................................................. 1 17 Student intentions to persist .................................................................... 117 Student beliefs ........................................................................................ 120 Level of application ................................................................................. 122 Case study F: Interamerican University of Puerto Rico—San German Campus (lUPR-SG) ................................................................................................... 122 Overview ................................................................................................. 122 Student performance .............................................................................. 122 Student intentions to persist .................................................................... 123 Student beliefs ........................................................................................ 127 Level of application ................................................................................. 129 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION ..................................................................................................... 130 Site differences ............................................................................................. 132 Instructional innovation ................................................................................. 134 An effective pedagogy ............................................................................ 134 A scalable pedagogy ............................................................................... 139 An updateable pedagogy ........................................................................ 140 Motivation inherent in Case Itl ...................................................................... 141 Attention .................................................................................................. 141 Relevance ............................................................................................... 142 Confidence .............................................................................................. 143 viii Satisfaction ............................................................................................. 145 Expectancy x Value ................................................................................ 145 Limitations .................................................................................................... 146 Future research ............................................................................................ 148 Implications for practice ................................................................................ 151 Agencies ................................................................................................. 151 Faculty who teach undergraduate science ............................................. 151 Conclusions .................................................................................................. 1 52 APPENDICES .................................................................................................... 155 Appendix A ................................................................................................... 155 Appendix B ................................................................................................... 160 Appendix C ................................................................................................... 163 Appendix D ................................................................................................... 164 Appendix E ................................................................................................... 165 REFERENCES .................................................................................................. 168 Table 1. Table 2. Table 3. Table 4. Table 5. Table 6. Table 7. Table 8. Table 9. LIST OF TABLES Factors that cause students to lea ve STEM programs and concerns of STEM students. .................................................................................... 3 Student characteristics for sites overall and by participants .............. 41 Performance results of pre-lpost-test repeated measures ................. 58 Changes in mean student performance and confidence on learning assessment test from pre- to post-test (n = 92) ................................. 59 Question-by-question analysis of pre-/post-test questions by gender and site ............................................................................................... 64 Definitions and examples of codes ............................................... 67-68 Categories of comments and their aggregate instances across all sites (n =1 160) ................................................................................... 69 Changes in mean student performance and confidence on learning assessment test from pre- to post-test at the University of Wisconsin— River Falls BIOL 150 (U WFIF-NM) non-majors level course (n = 42) ............................................................................................... 94 Categories of comments and their instances for the University of Wisconsin—River Falls BIOL 150 (U WRF-NM) non-majors level course (n =405) .............................................................................................. 95 Table 10. Changes in mean student performance and confidence on learning assessment test from pre- to post-test at the University of Wisconsin— River Falls BIOL 345 (U WRF-M) majors level course (n = 27) ........ 101 Table 11. Categories of comments and their instances for the University of Wisconsin-River Falls BIOL 345 (U WRF-M) majors level course (n =509) ................................................................................................ 103 Table 12. Changes in mean student performance and confidence on learning assessment test from pre- to post-test at North Carolina A& T BIOL 401 (NCA& 7) majors level course (n =5) ......................................... 110 Table 13. Categories of comments and their instances for North Carolina A&T University BIOL 401 (NCA& 7) majors level course (n =120) ........... 111 Table 14. Changes in mean student performance and confidence on learning assessment test from pre- to post-test at Interamerican University of Puerto Rico-Metropolitan MEDT 4531 (IUPR-M) majors level course (n = 6) ............................................................................................... 118 Table 15. Categories of comments and their instances for the lnterAmerican University of Puerto Rico-Metropolitan MEDT 4531 (lUPFl-M) majors level course (n =39) .......................................................................... 119 Table 16. Changes in mean student performance and confidence on learning assessment test from pre- to post-test at Interamerican University of ‘ Puerto Rico—San German BIOL 4600 (lUPFi-SG) majors level course (n = 12) ............................................................................................. 124 Table 17. Categories of comments and their instances for lnterAmerican University of Puerto Rico—San German BIOL 4600 (lUPR-SG) majors level course (n =87) .......................................................................... 125 xi LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. Screenshot of the Case It! simulation software ................................. 43 Figure 2. Screenshot of a phylogenetic tree created using Case It! and the Mega4 software ................................................................................ 44 Figure 3. Screen shot of a student created webposter made in Case It! launch pad .................................................................................................... 45 Figure 4. Screen shot of student conferencing via Case Itl Launch pad .......... 47 Figure 5. Student performance means by site ................................................. 60 Figure 6. Average student gain from pre- to post-test based on site ............... 61 Figure 7. Student performance means over time ............................................. 63 Figure 8. Student ratings of most useful Case Itl components ........................ 80 xii CHAPTER 1: Introduction “Why do we have to know this?” and “How does this apply to the ‘real- world’?” are questions educators in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM) fields often hear from students. There is a long tradition of lecturing in the sciences, with emphasis placed on memorization and learning material by rote (e.g. Aikenhead, 2006; Seymour, 1995); however, previous research has shown that such methods result in poor student recall and comprehension (e.g. Dale, 1969; Lord, 2007; McDonald & Dominguez, 2005). Furthermore, students often find lectures difficult to relate to, disengaging, and boring (), producing ambivalent or negative opinions of science and a disinclination to pursue degrees and careers in the STEM fields (Astin & Astin, 1993; Seymour 8 Hewitt, 1997; Tobias, 1990). Poor pedagogy in STEM courses and programs is affecting student persistence in major. Seymour and Hewitt (1997, pp. 32-35) identify no less than 23 independent factors that affect student retention in STEM fields, the most common of which are poor teaching by faculty, the overwhelming pace and workload of STEM programs, a lack or loss of interest in science, and the belief that other, non-STEM majors are more interesting or present better educational and career opportunities. These findings have subsequently been validated by a number of other researchers (e.g. Aikenhead, 2006; Callahan, Hertberg-Davis, Hockett, & Reed, 2008; Kardash 8 Wallace, 2001; Kaya, Kilic, & Akdeniz, 2004; National Science Foundation, 1998). Studies of pedagogy and persistence One of the largest single factors influencing student retention in STEM fields is teaching (Kardash & Wallace, 2001; Koballa & Glynn, 2007; Osborne & Collins, 2000; Seymour, 2002; Seymour & Hewitt, 1997; Strenta, Elliot, Adair, Matier, & Scott, 1994). In their seminal work, Seymour and Hewitt (1997, p. 33) identify “poor teaching by S.M.E. faculty” as the single most important concern amongst those students who left science and engineering programs (see Table 1). Both Tobias (1990) and Kardash and Wallace (2001) found that students thought the biggest barrier to learning was not the content or difficulty of courses, but rather the pedagogical methods used to convey that knowledge. Instructional method in STEM fields has been largely predicated on a transmission model that, “places personal student issues second to efficient course delivery” (Boldt, 2005, p. 63). Although lectures enjoy a long history of use in most fields of academia, it has become evident that some students do not feel they learn efficiently from them, nor do they particularly enjoy them. Student complaints about lectures include the focus on “getting through” a set amount of material, a lack of connection between theory and application, emphasis on rote memorization, and a lack of interaction between students and faculty (e.g. Kardash & Wallace, 2001 ; Prince, 2004; Yadav, et al., 2007). Table 1. Factors that cause students to leave STEM programs and concerns of STEM students” Concerning to Causes Concerning to Concerning to Issue students to students who stuggnézwrho ALL students p g p 9 programs p 9 Turned off 43% 60% 49% science Non-STEM 40% majors more attractive POOI' teaching 3670 90°/o 74°/o 83°/o Curriculum 35% 41 % overload Inappropriate 82% 40% 63% reasons for initially choosing STEM program Poor advising 75% 52% 65% *Adapted from Seymour & Hewitt (1997, p. 33) Another major factor limiting student retention in STEM programs has been a lack of engagement in class. Although the use of active learning pedagogies has increased among many STEM programs, many students still feel “turned off” by dry content or delivery (see Table 1; Acker, Hughes, & Fendley Jr., 2002; Astln & Astin, 1993; Seymour, 2002; Seymour & Hewitt, 1997). Several presenters at a recent meeting of the Board on Science Education of the National Academies noted both the need for, and the promise of, active learning in undergraduate STEM education (Dancy & Henderson, 2008; Froyd, 2008; Gregerman, 2008). Felder, Felder, and Dietz (1998) found that students in active and cooperative learning environments in a series of introductory chemical engineering courses had a 17% higher retention rate. Two promising practices that utilize active engagement are undergraduate research programs, which can significantly improve student retention (Gregerman, 2008), and case-based instruction (Lundeberg, 2008; Walter, Kang, Lundeberg, & Herreid, 2009; Wolter, Lundeberg, & Bergland, 2009). It is hypothesized that case-based instruction involving majors in research might be especially effective in increasing retention. Student perception of how science affects them or impacts their lives has been a growing problem related to retention in STEM fields (Bovina & Dragul'skaia, 2008; Kardash & Wallace, 2001; Kaya, et al., 2004; Seymour, 1995; Tobias, 1992). Students in STEM programs frequently complain that they feel disengaged from the material. Cited causes of this disenfranchisement include instructional style, an emphasis on rote memorization, or the inability to relate the material studied to their own personal lives (Kardash & Wallace, 2001; McConnell, Steer, Owens, & Knight, 2005). Research has shown that students are more interested in tapics that have a direct connection to their own lives, such as sexuality, drug use, or diseases (Aikenhead, 1992; Foster, Wolter, Lundeberg, & Kang, 2008; Stoker & Thompson, 1969; Wolter, Lundeberg, & Bergland, 2009). Other factors related to relevancy that affect persistence are I the lack of perceived future benefits, beliefs that non-STEM majors are more interesting or rewarding (Seymour & Hewitt, 1997), dislike for the exclusive culture of science (Aikenhead, 2002, 2006), an inability to perceive the future application of current instruction (Kardash & Wallace, 2001; Wolter, Lundeberg, et al., 2009), and an overall lack of understanding of what it means to be a scientist (Seymour & Hewitt, 1997; Tobias, 1990, 1992). Just as attracting students into STEM majors has been an issue, so too has been keeping students in the field. Astin and Astin (1993) note that most programs typically experience up to 40% attrition rates. Current students leave STEM programs for many of the same reasons they avoid them initially, but also cite poor teaching, an overwhelming pace and workload, and a chasm between theory and application of knowledge as factors that influence their decisions to leave (Kardash & Wallace, 2001; Seymour & Hewitt, 1997; Tobias, 1990). Many students identify a disconnect between their lives and the science they learn in the classroom. Schreiner and Sjeberg (2004) argue that it is important to know more regarding what students think about science and technology to improve the curriculum. The Relevance of Science Education (ROSE) project addresses issues of declining enrollments in science majors by asking students their perceptions of science and technology (Matthews, 2007), and emphasizes finding relevance between school topics and students’ lives (Sjeberg & Schreiner, 2005). The ROSE questionnaire has been administered in more than 37 countries, and indicates students are most interested in health, sexuality, genetics, the origin of life, space, the universe, and natural disasters (Matthews, 2007). However, how a topic is taught also matters, not just the nature of the topic. The method in which material is presented to students has the potential to affect student motivation, and by extension both performance and persistence (e.g. Allen, 1999; Cornell & Martin, 1997; Malone, 1981a, 1981b; Theall & Franklin, 1999). As mentioned above, many researchers believe that both PBL and CBI can influence student persistence in STEM programs. A growing body of literature emphasizes the relevance of multimedia, case-based science learning to students, especially in undergraduate science courses (Herreid, 2001, 2005a, 2005b; Lundeberg, et al., 2002; T. M. Smith & Emmeluth, 2002; Sokolove, Marbach-Ad, & Fusco, 2003). Prior research has indicated that students become more engaged with, and interested in, science when it is made culturally relevant to their lives (Aikenhead, 2002, 2006; Wolter, Lundeberg, et al., 2009). Studies from the field of engineering. Student persistence in STEM fields has been highly correlated to secondary schooling preparation in the sciences and GPA (Bonous-Hammarth, 2000; Cole & Espinoza, 2008; Huang, Taddese, & Walter, 2000); however, there is little literature on how exactly alternative pedagogical techniques impact retention in the sciences. The field of engineering appears to be far more proactive in this research than other STEM fields. Nationally, engineering programs experience high rates of attrition. In response, researchers in engineering education have dedicated a significant amount of time to the issue, finding that fewer students leave programs that emphasize “real-world” application and connections with content (Fortenberry, Sullivan, Jordan, & Knight, 2007; Froyd & Ohland, 2005), and that a student services approach featuring peer-mentoring and counseling appears to positively influence student retention rates (Sleeman & Sorby, 2007). Felder and his associates (Felder, 1995; Felder, et al., 1998; Felder, Felder, Mauney, Hamrin Jr., & Dietz, 1995) conducted an extensive longitudinal study of student instruction, retention, and performance in one engineering program from 1990 to 1993. One of their findings was that traditional, transmission model introductory courses that emphasize the roles of competition and individual, rather than group, work discourage women and minorities from persisting in STEM programs (Felder, et al., 1995), and that incorporating coursework that stresses and rewards cooperative learning has the potential to reduce the attrition of women in engineering programs (Felder, et al., 1995). Felder’s massive study also concluded that instructional pedagogy that incorporates cooperative, group-based work was not only more effective in helping students master content (Felder, 1995), but may also play a part in programmatic completion since students were more confident in their knowledge, and had better attitudes toward instruction (Felder, et al., 1998). Other studies have demonstrated that cooperative learning communities show promise in increasing student completion rates and retention rates (T sang & Halderson, 2008). Early involvement with research and information about the nature of the discipline have also been shown to have positive effects on student perceptions and persistence (e.g. Fainlveather, 2008; Gregerman, 2008). Potential solutions. Even though previous studies of STEM pedagogy are limited, they do provide useful insight into what actions might beneficially influence student success in STEM programs. Major factors affecting student persistence include: (a) an overall lack of motivation due to poor instruction; (b) feeling dissociated from the social structure of the sciences; (c) bad advising; (d) waning interest in the sciences due to impersonal pedagogies and disinterested faculty; (e) level of preparation; and (f) awareness of discipline (Seymour & Hewitt, 1997; see Table 1). Previous studies have highlighted the importance of improving instruction in STEM fields (e.g. Herreid, 2006; Kardash & Wallace, 2001; Seymour, 2002; Seymour & Hewitt, 1997; Tobias, 1992), which may be accomplished in many ways. Many researchers (e.g. Burrowes, 2003; Felder, et al., 1998; Kumar & Sherwood, 2007; Lundeberg, et al., 2002; Prince & Felder, 2007; Seymour, 2002; Walter, Kang, Lundeberg, & Herreid, 2009) advocate a shift from instructor- centered pedagogies such as lectures toward student-centered, active engagement instruction like problem-based learning (PBL) and case-based instruction (CBI). In PBL students work in groups to collectively solve poorly structured, open-ended problems (Savery, 2006). CBI environments are similar, but are more structured, using engaging narratives to introduce students to an issue (Herreid, 1994; Lundeberg, Levin, & Harrington, 1999). Recent research studies in CBI have investigated the effect of incorporating technologies like personal response systems (Wolter, Kang, Lundeberg, & Herreid, 2009; Wolter, Kang, Lundeberg, Herreid, & Zhang, 2009), and the importance of personal relevance (Wolter, Lundeberg, et al., 2009). Other researchers (Gregerman, 2008; Hunter, Laursen, & Seymour, 2007; Taraban & Blanton, 2008) have identified the incorporation of undergraduate research into programs as another technique with the potential to address the underlying causes of student attrition. All of these practices have the potential to speak to issues of engagement, relevancy, and motivation in STEM instruction. Deficiencies of previous studies A wealth of articles exist identifying the need for improved pedagogy in STEM fields; however, many of these simply identify the lack of even adequate instruction as an issue without identifying potential strategies for improvement (e.g. Astin & Astin, 1993; Augustine, et al., 2006; Seymour, 2002; Seymour & Hewitt, 1997; Tobias, 1990, 1992). Other studies identify broad ideas to address the issue, but lack specifics about tools or implementation (e.g. Aikenhead, 2007; Bailek & Botstain, 2004; Bell, 2004; Burrowes, 2003; Irwin, 1995; Kumar & Sherwood, 2007). This is not to say that experimental studies of pedagogy in STEM programs do not exist—they do—but most focus on reaching specific disadvantaged groups (e.g. Hurtado, et al., 2008; Hurtado, et al., 2007; Kang, Wolter, Lundeberg, & Herreid, 2009; 0. Lee & Luykx, 2007; National Science Foundation, 2003), or investigating barriers in adapting alternative pedagogies (e.g. Moriarty, 2007; Seymour, 1995; Walczyk, Ramsey, & Zha, 2007). There is a limited body of literature on effective pedagogical techniques in STEM (e.g. Bergland, et al., 2006; Lundeberg, et al., 2002; Walter, Kang, Lundeberg, & Herreid, 2009; Wolter, Kang, Lundeberg, Herreid, et al., 2009; Walter, Lundeberg, et al., 2009), to which this study adds. Importance of the study Science education researchers have known for over 25 years that poor teaching is a major issue deterring undergraduates from majoring in STEM fields; however, there is lack of empirical information on effective pedagogical tools in the existing body of literature. Even though we know what does not work for students, because we lack information on effective strategies, and because of the conservative nature of instruction in most STEM fields, teaching still tends to adhere to the traditional lecture/transmission model of education that emphasizes 10 rote memorization, is disengaging, and fails to illustrate the validity of content to students’ lives. Purpose This study attempted to contribute to the existing knowledge base on effective STEM pedagogy by exploring the influence of an alternative, online instructional tool in undergraduate, majors-level biology classrooms across diverse sites in the United States and Puerto Rico. It examines the effects of the learning environment on student motivation, performance, and perceptions of what makes for good instruction in biology. In this study, pre— and post-test assessments were used to measure the relationship between instructional method and student performance. A student survey and focus group interviews evaluated student perceptions of their experience and intentions to persist in program. Case studies were developed in parallel at each of the five study sites, incorporating student performance statistics, focus group interviews, and student cpinion surveys. 11 CHAPTER 2: Literature Review This review will focus on research detailing the current state of postsecondary biology instruction, including the problems it is facing. The growing use of instructional technologies will then been discussed, as well as models of student motivation in collegiate classrooms. Finally, the research questions of this study will be presented. The current state of postsecondary biology Many students report the lack of adequate instruction in STEM fields as a major factor affecting their persistence in programs. One anonymous reviewer of Seymour and Hewitt (1997) stated, “[As a science major] plan your educational strategy to avoid being ‘weeded out’ by SME faculty who don’t want to admit that you exist until you have put up with two solid years of cheerful neglect and brutal abuse” (Anonymous 1999). Although lectures are a traditional method of instruction in most sciences, many of today’s students are bored and disenfranchised with them. Students of all backgrounds frequently cite the stereotypical “talking head” lecture used for the past century in science education as a major detraction (Kardash & Wallace, 2001; Lord, 2008; Moriarty, 2007; Wolter, Lundeberg, et al., 2009). Wolter, Lundeberg, and Bergland (2009) found that students in introductory biology thought classroom science was boring or irrelevant; however, when material was placed in a relevant context to their own lives, students become both more engaged and interested in science. 12 Content vs. pedagogy. In the 20 years since Boyer (Boyer, 1990) wrote about the need of the American professoriate to reconsider its priorities, most institutions will say that they value effective teaching—especially in the STEM fields—but the realities are somewhat different. No American college or university in its right mind will say that it does not value teaching; however, faculty reward structures tell a different tale. The fact remains that most faculty tenure systems at American universities and colleges are designed to reward research and conversely punish teaching (Fairweather, 2005; Leslie, 2002). This is demonstrated by the fact that at all 4-year institutions, regardless of type, the more time faculty spend on teaching, the lower their average pay (Fairweather, 2005). Fairweather (2008, p. 23) observes that, ...Career publications remain the strongest predictor of faculty pay irrespective of type of institution (emphasis in original). An economic analysis of the estimated effect of an additional hour spent in the classroom and an additional career publication at the mean shows that it costs money to spend time teaching whereas publishing is invariably rewarded with higher pay. This institutionalized paradox in values of has carried over to faculty attitudes where it manifests as an argument about whether to teach STEM courses based strictly on content supported by research, or based on effective pedagogy supported by the scholarship of teaching. There is an enduring perception amongst STEM faculty that improving instructional quality comes at the cost of research productivity (Boardman & Bozeman, 2007; Fairweather, 2005; Parker, 2008). Leslie (2002) notes that there exists a contradiction in 13 faculty attitude where teaching ability is valued, but research activity is perceived as the primary force influencing career goals such as tenure and status. Research activity is frequently grant-driven, and every institution takes their cut of any grant received by their faculty; therefore, the benefits of research for the institution are bi-fold, (a) they receive income indirectly via it, and (b) the publications that result from research increase their prestige. Teaching, however, results in neither of these. Realistically students will continue to attend higher education regardless of whether instructional reforms happen, therefore the is little financial incentive for institutions to reward teaching which is perceived as coming at the cost of research. Although the benefits of pedagogical reform in STEM fields has been established, Fairweather (2008, p. 24) notes that, “...enhancing the value of teaching in STEM fields requires much more than empirical evidence of instructional effectiveness. It requires active intervention by academic leaders at the departmental, college, and institutional level.” STEM faculty vary considerably in attitudes and behaviors towards instruction and students (Fairweather 8. Paulson, 2008); however, Pascarella and Terenzini (2005) demonstrated that the most effective teaching techniques are not discipline dependent. Those professors who do make the effort to reform the structure of their classes primarily focus on individual classroom-level interventions, such as shifting away from lectures and toward more learner- centered actives (Fairweather, 2008). Despite the preponderance of evidence that changes in pedagogy can positively influence student opinions, performance, 14 and retention in STEM courses (e.g. Eiseman & Fairweather, 1996; Fairweather & Beach, 2002; P. Fisher, Zeligman, & Fainiveather, 2005; Wankat, 2002), there has been none of the locked for macro-level change because reform dies out without institutional and faculty support (Fairweather, 2008). Individual vs. systemic instructional reform. Instruction that integrates active and collaborative learning with engaging contexts results in better student performance irrespective of academic discipline (Fairweather, 2008; Kuh, Kinzie, Buckley, Bridges, & Kayek, 2007; Kuh, Kinzie, Schuh, & Witt, 2005). As noted above, successful pedagogical innovations in STEM education frequently are isolated to developers because reform efforts are widely viewed as voluntary professional development (Fairweather, 2008; Wullf & Austin, 2004). Strong pedagogy is frequently instructor dependent, rather than applicable across a wide variety of institutions, students, and faculty, resulting in a problem “scaling up” (e.g. Eiseman & Fairweather, 1996; Fairweather, 2008; Kumar & Sherwood, 2007; Yadav, et al., 2007). While classroom instructional reforms have demonstrated site-specific efficacy, they have not led to the expected systemic reforms in student retention and comprehension. Studies such as Fisher, Zeligman, and Fairweather (2005) have shown reformist techniques can be extremely effective in improving student scores and critical thinking, but often fail to have systemic effect because such techniques are instructor specific and they are not integrated into programmatic curriculum. 15 The faculty who choose to participate in professional development are already demonstrably committed to improving instruction (Gappa, Austin, & Trice, 2007); however, most STEM faculty, are not trying to maximize their teaching efficacy, but rather to teach well enough so they can maximize research time (Massy & Zemsky, 1994). It is arguably with those instructors that even modest gains in STEM pedagogical reform will be made (Fainiveather, 2008; Labov, Singer, George, Schweingruber, & Hilton, 2009). Pace of change. A major problem facing STEM instruction in the 21St century is the ability (or lack thereof) of the curriculum to reflect and incorporate the latest developments from the field. The pace at which the American educational system changes is a long-standing joke, frequently resulting in the question, “What change?” While the educational system might be highly conservative with a great deal of institutional inertia (Duderstadt, 2000; Wallis & Steptoe, 2006), knowledge within disciplines evolves faster every year (e.g. Bush, 2009; Knight, 2007). Many STEM professors still rely heavily on both textbooks and publisher provided instructional material, even though they always lag behind the leading edge of discovery in field because of the publication process (e.g. Yore, 1991 ). Pedagogical tools that can eliminate this lag time between discovery, implementation, and incorporation into curriculum could significantly impact the quality of instruction in STEM education. Bioinformatlcs. Biology and life sciences are undergoing a rapid change in the way that systemic and molecular information is collected and processed 16 (Emmott & Rison, 2006). Bioinformatics combines techniques from computer science, molecular biology, genetics, statistics, applied mathematics, and systematics to answer questions in biology primarily dealing with large-scale DNA-sequencing, protein structure, and molecular processes (Baldi & Brunak, 2001; Cristianini & Hahn, 2006). Examples of current research projects include the Human Genome Project, synthetic drug design, modeling evolution, and predictions of gene expression (Zvelebil & Baum, 2007). Bioinformatics scientists collect and analyze massive amounts of data about the genetic and molecular components of entire biological systems to increase understanding of biological processes. By using computers to assist in mapping DNA and protein sequences, researchers can deduce links and interactions, and making sense of the relationships between, within, and amongst systems (Baldi & Brunak, 2001; Cristianini 8 Hahn, 2006; Emmott & Rison, 2006; Zvelebil & Baum, 2007). Reforms In STEM education. Case-based instruction. Case-based instruction has been used in higher education since 1927 (Kagen, 1993), primarily in professional education fields, such as law, business, and medicine (Herreid, 2006; Lundeberg, et al., 1999). However, the pedagogy has become increasingly popular as an instructional vehicle across educational fields (Herreid, 1994; Kang & Walter, 2008; Yadav, et al., 2007). Prince and Felder (2006) incorporate case-based instruction under the umbrella of inductive teaching techniques, and closely associate it with other student-centered and student-directed pedagogies such as 17 PBL, discovery learning, just-in-time teaching (JiTl'), and inquiry-based learning (Savery, 2006). Case-based instruction is a valuable and valid pedagogy because it allows students to individually develop their own knowledge bases and pathways on a subject such that it is both relevant and important to them (Levin, 1999; Savery, 2006). The use of case studies in science education can result in students making a meaningful connection with the material, improve understanding, and increase engagement (Kumar & Chubin, 2000; Kumar & Sherwood, 2007; Rybarczyk, Baines, McVey, Thompson, & Wilkins, 2007; Yadav, et al., 2007). The complex nature of case studies leads students to assess problems from a myriad of perspectives (Bell, 2004), and those that emphasize the human dimension of an issue or controversy may be able to powerfully demonstrate the relevance of a given topic to students and generate engagement (Bell, 2004; Yadav, et al., 2007). In providing students with real-world problems in a situated learning contexts, case studies motivate students to learn by making the material both relevant and engaging (Bergland, et al., 2006; Prince & Felder, 2006). A recent national study found that faculty who use case studies believe their students develop both stronger analytical cognitive abilities, and a deeper understanding of the topic (Yadav, et al., 2007). Other research has verified this faculty perception, showing that the use of case-based learning in science education can significantly promote knowledge acquisition, the development of critical thinking skills in students, and student retention (Burrowes, 2003; Dori, 18 Tal, & Tsausu, 2003; Prince & Felder, 2007; Rybarczyk, et al., 2007; Savery, 2006; R. A. Smith & Murphy, 1998). Choi, Lee, and Jung (2008) have developed data that suggests the use of online multimedia case-studies may be especially effective in accessing sensing, sequential, and reflective learners. Advancement in the sciences can be represented as an ever-narrowing pipeline whose traditional teaching methodologies tend to cause students to dropout because of the emphasis on rote memorization, lack of situated application, and perceptions of poor teaching (Kardash & Wallace, 2001 ; Seymour & Hewitt, 1997). Kardash and Wallace (2001, p. 199) note in their study of student perceptions of science classes that, “...how information is taught appears to be at least as much of concern as what information is taught.” Many researchers believe that case-based instruction has the potential to alleviate these instructional issues by providing real-world situations in which students explore and apply knowledge through discovery and application (Herreid, 2006; Lundeberg, et al., 1999; Lundeberg, et al., 2002; Lundeberg & Yadav, 2006a, 2006b; R. A. Smith & Murphy, 1998; Wolter, Lundeberg, et al., 2009). Because of this, the use of case-based learning across disciplines is projected to increase as more postsecondary institutions embrace “...social constructivist and situated learning pedagogies” (Oblinger & Oblinger, 2005, p. 243) in the 21St century. Case based instruction has the potential to address many of the issues relating to retention in the STEM fields. It (a) creates active engagement (Herreid, 1994, 2006b; Lundeberg, et al., 1999), (b) motivates students via 19 working on “authentic” tasks and real world problems (Bergland, et al., 2006; Herreid, 1994, 2006b), (0) promotes active learning (Callahan, et al., 2008; Lundeberg, et al., 1999), (d) promotes higher-order thinking skills (Dori, Tal, & Tsausu, 2003; R. A. Smith & Murphy, 1998), (e) helps students set content mastery goals (Rybarczyk, et al., 2007; Savery, 2006), (f) exposes students to ethical and societal problems (Herreid, 1994; Kang & Lundeberg, 2008; Lundeberg, et al., 2002), and (9) causes students to examine problems from multiple perspectives (Herreid, 2006; Walter, Lundeberg, et al., 2009). Case-based instruction is not without its difficulties. Yadev et al. (2007, p. 37) identified five major obstacles that faculty encounter when using cases: (a) lack of preparation, (b) how to assess student learning and participation, (c) a lack of relevant cases, ((1) student resistance to case-based instruction, and (e) pressure to cover more content. Herreid (2003) provides a narrative of the complete failure of a case-based course due to disorganization and the use of multiple instructors. While the benefits of using case studies in science education have been well documented, challenges such as these may deter those uninitiated to case-based instruction. The growing use of instructional technology In STEM Instruction. Since the advent of the first computers more than 50 years ago, their integration into teaching environments has steadily increased. Computer-assisted instruction (CAI) has become widespread and integral to education from the primary to postsecondary levels (Chambers & Sprecher, 1984; Keller, 2008; K0 & 20 Rossen, 2010). At the collegiate level, virtually every professor communicates with students via email, utilizes online course management software, teaches in a multimedia-enabled classroom, creates content on their PC, or teaches online. This trend continues to grow as new, “digital native” (Oblinger & Oblinger, 2005) professors integrate technology into the classroom. A recent report by the Microsoft Corporation (Emmott & Rison, 2006) notes that the importance of technology in STEM instruction will only continue to grow in the next decade, require an urgent reevaluation of how programs train future both future practitioners and professors. The authors also assert that technology will have particular significant impacts on biology and chemistry where it can assist in the conceptualization of especially abstract yet vital concepts (Emmott & Rison, 2006). The following are just a few examples of how technology is changing collegiate instruction. Clickers. Personal Response Systems, or “Clickers,” are an instructional technology whose use has surged on campuses across America in the past decade. There are many manufacturers of systems, and the devices themselves range from simplistic 5-button remotes to units that resemble graphing calculators; however, all virtually all use local radio frequency (RF) signals to create a classroom network that links student clickers to a receiver attached to the professor’s computer. Clickers provide immediate, real-time feedback to students in even the largest lecture hall, directly influencing student learning (e.g. Guthrie & Carlin, 2004; Mayer, et al., 2009). Although instructors must create 21 most questions ahead of class, they are able to instantly estimate student comprehension. Student interaction and feedback are proven and important facilitators of student learning (Carini, Kuh, & Klein, 2006; Herzog, 2007), and previous studies suggest that students: (a) pay more attention; (b) develop independent, personally intuitive, organization of concepts; and (c) engage in metacognitive self-evaluation when lectures incorporate clicker questions (Duncan, 2005; Mayer, et al., 2009). Clicker use has also been linked to improved motivation (Crouch & Mazur, 2001), which may lead to cognitive persistence (Dufresne, Gerace, Leonard, Mestre, & Wenk, 1996), and increased mastery goal setting (Roschelle, Penuel, & Abrahamson, 2004). Duncan (2005) demonstrated the efficacy of using clickers as an active learning strategy to improve both student performance and opinion in large science lecture classrooms. The successful use of clickers is associated with several educational theories, such as the importance of feedback (Cain, Black, & Rohr, 2009; Yourstone, Kraye, & Albaum, 2008), student motivation (Trees & Jackson, 2007), and generative learning (Mayer, et al., 2009). Previous researchers (e.g. Guthrie & Carlin, 2004) indicated that clickers provide an excellent source of feedback, which may directly influence student learning. By using clickers, professors are able to answer students’ questions instantly, and students are more engaged when instruction centers on the discussions of these questions (Duncan, 2005). 22 Horowitz (1988) reported that by using a student response system, students significantly increased their attentiveness during the class. A recent 3-year, longitudinal study (Mayer, et al., 2009) focused on the utility of using clickers facilitate faculty questions, student response, and student attention. The authors emphasize that each of these factors is essential to creating engagement, which is in turn vital to student learning, stating If students do not feel they are involved in the Ieaming situation, they are less likely to work hard to make sense of the presented material and therefore less likely to perform as well as they could on assessments measuring their learning. (Mayer, et al., 2009, p. 51) The researchers used 3 experimental groups, a control (no clickers or questions), a no-clicker group (questions and paper-based responses), and a clicker group (questions and clickers). They found that the clickers treatment group scored significantly higher on midterm and final exams combined than either the control or no-clicker group (Mayer, et al., 2009). It is interesting to note that students who receive the same questions as the clicker group, but responded via paper scored significantly lower than the clicker group, indicating that the clicker condition itself was aiding student learning, and not just the question method of instruction. These findings articulate well with the primary thesis of generative learning theory that cognitively engaged students learn more (Mayer & Wittrock, 2006) While there are advantages to incorporating clickers into large lecture classrooms, Mayer, et al. (2009) point out that there are major challenges facing 23 higher education in implementation and how to integrate these benefits in ways that promote student learning and not necessarily just student performance. Games and computer simulated learning environments. The use of games and simulations has become common amongst those interested in discovery-oriented science education (Kulik, 2002). Previous studies have shown that both can significantly impact a student’s ability to construct personal knowledge and meaning, as well as develop collaborative learning skills (e.g. Jackson, 2009; Lai-Chong Law, Kickmeier-Rust, Albert, & Holzinger, 2008; Ryan, Rigby, & Przybylski, 2006; Vogel, et al., 2006). The structure of actions in games may also allow students to develop higher-order skills because they control the activity and must make decisions in response to stimuli, requiring them to strategize and engage in Judgment-Bebevior-Feedback loops (Garris, Ahlers, & Driskell, 2002). Video games and simulations are effective instructional tools for multiple reasons. Mayo (2009) notes that games and simulations are effective because they utilize many pedagogical techniques that have been proven effective in other situations. These include such items as the ability to vary the pace of the game to fit the user and the ability to present information in multiple ways so as to access as many learning types as possible (Gee, 2003). Games and simulations also offer scalable delivery of information where complex issues or goals are first presented as small, accomplishable tasks, which are then repeated several times before evolving into more complex scenarios. This 24 method of deliver is also known as “concurrent chaining” (Peck & Detweiler, 2000), and allows students to build confidence in the knowledge and skills necessary to accomplish tasks. Video games give information “situated meaning” (Mayo, 2009) and reinforce key concepts, as well as providing students social interactions associated with content, all of which drives learner motivation, engagement, and achievement (Keller, 2010). By giving students control and autonomy over task and activities within the learning environment, simulations improve learning outcomes (Vogel, et al., 2006), enjoyment, and motivation (Ryan, et al., 2006). Video games are fun, active learning environments where students are instruments of their own learning. Because of this, they are more apt to spend longer periods of time interacting with the material, and hopefully making meaningful connections with it (Jackson, 2009; Keller, 2010; Mayo, 2009). Gaming simulations also describe many of the tenets of Merrill’s (2002) model for successful learning, such as containing an intrinsic motivation to play, having clear rules and goals, providing an attractive learning environment, using an engaging, yet unpredictable storyline, providing immediate feedback, and being interactive, challenging, and competitive (Lai-Chong Law, et al., 2008; Prensky, 2001 ). Studies have shown that dynamic, computer-based simulations can enhance student comprehension and retention of complex topics (Trey & Khan, 2008) and significantly improve student performance (Holzinger, Kickmeier-Rust, & Albert, 2008) beyond the capabilities of fixed measures, such as text. They 25 may also increase learning 7-40% over lectures (Mayo, 2009; Vogel, et al., 2006). Most researchers agree that this is because simulations provide students with simplified models of real-world situations that help them integrate a wide array of facts, ideas, and principles from multiple sources (Kulik, 2002; Ryan, et al., 2006; Vogel, et al., 2006). Games also tend to focus on higher-order instructional objectives, requiring students to do more than just memorize facts and actively involving them in the learning process (Kulik, 2002). The ability to manipulate parameters in simulated environments allows students the ability to experiment with outcomes and actively engage in both self and scientific discovery in a very real and personal manner (Clark, Nelson, Sengupta, & D'Angelo, 2009; Llado & Sanchez, 2009). In addition to developing scientific reasoning and skills, simulations can also develop students’ deep- reasoning, analytical abilities, and personal meaning-making in relation to the content (Clark, Nelson, Sengupta, & D'Angelo, 2009). Video games and simulations also offer multiple levels of interaction with material analogous to the multiple levels of interaction students have with material in science classrooms (Jackson, 2009). Kulik (2002) notes that while games and simulations have the potential be useful instructional tools, simply including them in a curriculum does not guarantee success. Far too often it is predicated on the individual instructor’s ability to utilize simulations as a medium (Becker, 2007), rather than on the efficacy of the learning environment. Few examples exist of simulated 26 environments in science that remove the instructor as a variable in the success of the program. Models of student motivation Student persistence in any program is arguably a product of motivation to stay in that program (Allen, 1999; Theall & Franklin, 1999). If a student is inspired by their class-related, personal, and social experiences in a course of study, they are far more likely to remain in program than students who feel alienated, isolated, ignored, and otherwise de-motivated by their encounters (e.g. Pintrich, 2003; Vallerand, Fortier, & Guay, 1997). Motivation is broadly defined as what an individual wants, needs, or desires, while motives are those stimuli that cause one to act upon them (Glynn, Aultman, & Owens, 2005; Koballa & Glynn, 2007). Brophy (2004) tell us that, “In the classroom context, the concept of student motivation is used to explain the degree to which students invest attention and effort in various pursuits...” (p. 4, emphasis in original). In general, the many theories of student motivation can be grouped into four categories: (a) physiological; (b) behavioral; (c) cognitive; and (d) emotional (Keller, 2010). Of the four, behavioral and cognitive theories are the most common (e.g Brophy, 2004). The legacy culture of education in the United States from Kindergarten through post-secondary schooling is predicated on a behavioralist perspective of motivation that presupposes pe0ple only respond to basic needs. An example of this point of view would be to assume a student’s only motivation to perform in class is to attain a passing grade, which is 27 necessary to move on to the next class or to obtain their degree. Thus, behavioral models of student motivation are based on the manipulation of students, whereas cognitive models emphasize engaging and interacting with students (Alberto & Troutman, 1999; Brophy, 2004). Researchers recognize that student motivation is far more complex than the “carrot-and-stick” model that rewards desired behaviors and punishes unwanted ones. Motivation can be derived from multiple sources, and is not necessarily an intrinsic value but can be a product of expectations, meaningful content, and student-centered activities (Brophy, 2004; Druger, 2000; Maehr & Braskamp, 1986). Just as motivation can be the product of multiple sources, so too can it be view from multiple perspectives. In a summary of research, Renchler (1992) notes that motivation can be viewed as: (a) a personal trait, like a highly competitive student gaining personal validation through being the “best;” (b) a response to specific environmental stimuli, such as the praise or regard of a valued peer or teacher; or (c) a product of student cognition, such as a sense of control or ownership or a self-image the student is motivated to maintain. The ARCS model of student motivation. Of the many theories of student motivation, few are as well known and have such a preponderance of support as Keller’s ARCS model (Keller, 1979, 1983, 1987, 2010). Keller asserts that student motivation in any course is a product of 4 factors: (a) Attention; (b) Relevance; (0) Confidence; and (d) Satisfaction. 28 Attention. To motivate students, any course or project must gain and keep the students’ curiosity. To do this instructors can use perceptual arousal, analogous to sensory stimuli, in which novel, incongruous, or surprising elements are introduced. Inquiry can also keep students’ attention by asking them to answer or develop questions, or to solve specific problems. Keller (2010) suggested a third method of gaining attention is through variability, maintaining interest by using different techniques, methods, or sources of information. Relevance. One of the most important things for any learning environment to do is to demonstrate to students how the material being presented does or may affect students personally. If students find content to be pertinent or useful to them, they are more likely to be motivated (Wolter, Lundeberg, et al., 2009). Potential benefits and uses should be emphasized and reinforced by using specific examples and analogies that draw links between the students’ lives and the material being learned. Assessing individual student ambitions and demonstrating how those goals may be met can also build relevance (Keller, 1999, 2010). Confidence. Building confidence and perceptions of self-efficacy reinforces motivation. Students need to believe that they can succeed at the tasks they are given; however, they also need to be challenged. Tasks that are too easy make students feel they are wasting their time and detract from overall motivation to learn. Keller and Suzuki (1988) note that there are three important motivational dimensions to student confidence. First is perceived competence in 29 which students are motivated in situations where they believe they have the skills needed to succeed; however, learning usually involves utilizing new skills and knowledge bases where pupils feel less confident. Therefore, to optimize motivation in these scenarios, students need controlled environments where mistakes may be made without embarrassment. Second is perceived control, which is exercised when students feel their actions and choices directly affect the outcome of situations, and they therefore feel more in control. Control breeds confidence, which in turn breeds motivation and persistence. Third is an expectancy for success, which has also been called “Self-fulfilling prophecy” (Jones, 1977; Schunk & Pajares, 2009) where because students believe they can accomplish a task, they exert more effort and perform better. This is an important observation because actual probabilities of success do not necessarily factor into student psychological expectations. In the STEM fields it is not uncommon for students to suppose material will be simply too hard to comprehend, resulting in failure even where objective odds would suggest success (Keller 8 Suzuki, 1988; Wolter, Lundeberg, et al., 2009). Strategies to increase confidence may include allowing students personal control so they are the facilitators of their own accomplishments, providing multiple opportunities for achievement where students can gain validation, and providing detailed requirements that allow students to gauge their likelihood of success. It should be noted however that while confidence can beneficially affect both performance and motivation, over-confidence may have the opposite effect 30 and in fact de-motivate students (Hackett 8 Betz, 1989; Nietfeld, Cao, 8 Osborne, 2005). Satisfaction. Learners need to gain some feeling of reward or satisfaction with their experiences to remain motivated. Keller and Suzuki (1988) note that, “if the outcomes of learners’ efforts are consistent with their expectations, and if they feel good about the outcomes, then they are likely to remain motivated” (p. 405). Satisfaction may manifest as a sense of accomplishment—that the tasks performed were important and worthwhile, or as a feeling of success—that they are capable of performing. Additionally, students who feel a sense of pride in their accomplishments are more likely to both retain information and remain motivated. Factors that may influence student satisfaction may include: (a) reinforcement and feedback, which can sustain motivation; (b) predictable, intrinsic rewards, which can result in consistent behavior; and (c) cognitive evaluation, which includes reflective praise for accomplishments from both peers and instructors (Keller, 2008, 2010; Keller 8 Suzuki, 1988; Shellnut, Knowltan, 8 Savage, 1999; Visser 8 Keller, 1990). Keller’s ARCS model of student motivation has been extensively researched (Deimann 8 Keller, 2006; Means, Jonassen, 8 Dwyer, 1997; Small 8 Gluck, 1994; Visser 8 Keller, 1990), and its validity well established. Recent research has also established its applicability to computer-based, online, and distance learning environments (Astleitner 8 Wiesner, 2004; Keller, 1999, 2008; Keller 8 Suzuki, 2004; Song 8 Keller, 2001). Other research by Cornell and 31 Martin (1997) has shown that in online and distance courses, student motivation, persistence, and performance can be influenced by instructional design. The Expectancy x Value model of student motivation. This is an inclusive model that places motivation within a social context where motivation is the product of students’ expectations of success and degree to which they value such success (Brophy, 2004; Feather, 1982; Wigfield 8 Eccles, 2000). In other words, if students either do not believe they can succeed, or see noreason to, they are not motivated. Brophy (2004) draws a distinct difference between learning, typically defined as comprehension, processing, or mastery, and performance, the simple demonstration of knowledge. He theorizes that student motivation to learn is related to students’ intentional learning processes, not their performance (Brophy, 2004). Based on this model, to be motivated, students must have expectations for success (e.g. clear goals, a belief in success), and intrinsic factors, those which access personal values or interests. Many of these intrinsic factors overlap with factors identified in the ARCS model as creating motivation (Keller, 1979, 1983, 2010). Principle factors In motivating students to learn. Merrill (Merrill, 2002, p. 43) identifies five core principles of learning and motivation common to all theories, stating that learning is promoted when: 1. Learners are engaged in solving real-world problems. 2. Existing knowledge is activated as a foundation for new knowledge. 3. New knowledge is demonstrated to the learner. 32 4. New knowledge is applied by the learner. 5. New knowledge is integrated into the leamer’s world. Keller (2008) takes these five principles a step further in applying them to digital environments, stating that motivation to learn is promoted when: 1. 2. 5. Curiosity is aroused by gaps in current knowledge. Material learned is perceived to be relevant and meaningful to the learner. Learners believe they can succeed in mastering the task. Learners anticipate and experience satisfying outcomes to a learning task. Learners employ volitional strategies to protect their intentions. Motivation and persistence. Student motivation to learn is a cognitive function of trying to contextualize and understand material that utilizes specific mental pathways (Brophy, 2004). These pathways are activated by certain aspects within a learning environment, such as those illustrated by the ARCS model (Keller, 1983) that grab the learners attention, establish the relevancy of the material, build learner confidence, and develop a sense of satisfaction. Previous research has linked the use of PBL and CBI learning environments to improved student motivation (e.g. Ertmer, Newby, 8 MacDougall, 1996; Hmelo- Silver, 2004; Lee, 2007; Richardson, 1993), because these tools have the potential to create cognitive dissonance and curiosity. Numerous researchers of college student retention have noted that persistence is the product of several 33 social and academic sub-categories including motivation, social integration, and self-direction (e.g. Baird, 2000; Braxton 8 Lien, 2000; Stage 8 Hossler, 2000; Tinto, 1993, 2000). Other researchers have demonstrated that motivation can beneficially impact both program persistence and academic performance (e.g. Allen, 1999; Glynn, et al., 2005; Koballa 8 Glynn, 2007; Renchler, 1992; Theall 8 Franklin, 1999; Vollmeyer 8 Rheinberg, 2000) by engaging students in such a way that individual and academic goals align. It is expected that the learning environment being investigated in this study, Case Itl, will have positive effects on persistence in program, performance, and student opinion. Research questions Declining numbers of undergraduate majors and graduate students in STEM fields over the past 20 years has led to increasing national focus on pedagogical reform in science instruction (Augustine, et al., 2006; Fairweather, 2008), and especially to increase minority participation (Astin 8 Oseguera, 2005; Hurtado, et al., 2008). My primary research question is: Does using a case- based multimedia project affect postsecondary students’ learning or motivation to learn biology? I also have 2 sub-questions related to the primary question: 1. Does participation affect students’ performance? (i.e. Is there a performance gain?) 2. Based on student comments, do students believe Case It! to be a satisfactory and/or beneficial experience for them? 34 CHAPTER 3: Methods This chapter lays out the procedures and methods used in conducting this study investigating the effects of a specific pedagogical intervention designed to improve undergraduate biology education. Specifically, its impact on student opinion and performance was examined. One hundred and five biology students from 5 introductory and upper division classes at 4 universities in the Midwest, Southeast, and Puerto Rico took part in this study. The instructional intervention, Case Itl, was comprised of 3 distinct stages, (a) viewing cases, (b) testing and interpreting material from cases, and (0) sharing results and role-playing about the cases. Because of the complexity of ideas involved in understanding how this particular instruction intervention affected students, I chose to use a mixed- methods design. This design bridges the gap in theory between the traditional constructivist and positivist theories of social research (Johnson 8 Onwuegbuzie, 2004) and offers the ability to answer complex questions in interdisciplinary studies that do not lend themselves to either research tradition. Proponents of the “incompatibility thesis” of social research design (Howe, 1988) assert that quantitative and qualitative methods are diametrically opposite in their approach to study design and data collection, and therefore completely incompatible with each other. However, beginning in the 1990’s some post-modernist researchers began to question the validity of adhering too strictly to one tradition or the other when trying to gain a holistic view of complex questions (Creswell, 2007a). 35 Rather, they asserted that tools from each research tradition had strengths, and that by mixing their use researchers could view issues from multiple perspectives, which would more likely result in clearer pictures of the issues at hand (Creswell 8 Clark, 2007; Onwuegbuzie 8 Leech, 2005). I chose to use mixed-methods in this study because it incorporates the strengths of both qualitative and quantitative techniques (Sechrest 8 Sidana, 1995), and would help me better understand the impact of Case Itl’s alternative pedagogy in greater detail. Study sites Study sites included 5 classes on 4 campuses in the Midwest, East Coast, and Caribbean. Of the 5 classes, 4 were 300 or 400 level that focused on aspects of human health. For example, one course was an immunology course that utilized HIV as a main teaching theme, while another was a medical technology class focusing on infectious diseases. The 5th class was a major’s level introductory biology course that covered a broad array of topics. Every student taught by 1 of the 5 participating instructors in spring semester of the 2008-2009 academic years was invited to take part in the study. These 5 courses were selected because they were taught by instructors who had helped develop the latest version of the Case Itl learning environment, and because they represented a broad cross-section of students in American higher education. University of Wisconsin-River Falls. The University of Wisconsin— River Falls (UWRF) is a regional comprehensive school located in western 36 Wisconsin approximately 45 minutes drive from Minneapolis, Minnesota. UWRF is a residential campus founded in 1874, and is 1 of 13 4-year campuses run by the University of Wisconsin system. The town of River Falls is approximately 13,000 people, and the university contributes half again that number with an enrollment in Fall 2007 of 6,007. Even though it has an area of over 220 acres, the main campus feels compact and welcoming. Commitment to current social issues is evidenced by the number of recycling bins, and the new student union, which is a “green” building. Ninety-three percent of students at UWRF are “white, non-Hispanic,” 92.8% attend school full time, and 58.4% are women (Integrated Postsecondary Education Data System, 2008). Two biology classes at this site were part of the study, 1 introductory level course (BIOL 150) and 1 majors level (BIOL 345). Of the 50 students enrolled in BIOL 150 Introductory Biology, 86% (n = 43) participated in focus group interviews/survey, where 18 were declared biology majors and 25 were non-majors. Thirty-nine students were enrolled in BIOL 345, Immunology, of which 38 consented to participate in focus group interviews/survey (~97%). North Carolina A&T State University. North Carolina A&T State University (NCA8T) is a residential historically black university or college (HBUC), and a land grant institution established by the second Morrill Act in 1890. The campus is located on approximately 180 acres in North Carolina’s Piedmont region. As the state’s land grant institution, a primary focus has historically been agriculture; however, the campus also has a growing cellular 37 and molecular biology program. The campus is comprised of over 100 buildings and has been undergoing a $100 million renovation and modernization project since 2002. NCA8T offers 99 degree programs and had a total undergraduate enrollment in Fall 2007 of 9,048 students, of which 91% are African-American, 89.8% are full time students, and 52.2% are female (Integrated Postsecondary Education Data System, 2008). Of the 20 students enrolled in BIOL 401, molecular biology, 6 consented to participate in FGls/survey (30%). Interamerican University of Puerto Rico-Metropolitan Campus. Interamerican University of Puerto Rico is a 4-year private university with 11 campuses spread across the island of Puerto Rico. Two campuses participated in this study; San German Campus (lUPR-SG) and Metropolitan Campus (IUPR- M). The Metropolitan Campus of Interamerican University is located in the sprawling metropolis of San Juan. It is a relatively small, non-residential, and ill- defined campus of approximately 20 acres hemmed in by expressways and suburbs. There is 1 main instructional building with about 14 smaller satellite buildings surrounding it. Some buildings have a run-down air to them, and a high fence encloses the entire campus as a result of the high crime rate. IUPR-MC was established and accredited in 1962, and offers 119 degrees from certificates to doctorates. The campus is 100% Hispanic, but enrolls more part time students (30.2%) and slightly more women (54.6%) than lUPR-SG. As of Fall 2007 IUPR- M enrolled 6,936 undergraduate students, and 3,674 graduate students (Integrated Postsecondary Education Data System, 2008). Of the 26 students 38 enrolled in clinical immunology, MEDT 4531, at lUPR-MC, only 6 (23%) participated in the FGI sessions due to a number of potentially mitigating factors such as occurring late in day (4:30pm), discomfort with the interview process, and students having just completed a full day of instruction as well as having taken a final exam in a different course that same day. Interamerican University of Puerto Rico-San German. lUPR-SG is a well-defined, midsized residential campus set in a rural area of southwestern Puerto Rico. It is approximately 90 acres and comprises 49 campus buildings. The campus first offered collegiate level courses in 1921, and was accredited in 1944. There is a palpable collegiate atmosphere similar to that found on the campuses of liberal arts schools on the mainland. IUPR-SG is a 100% Hispanic campus that enrolled 4,745 undergraduate students in Fall 2007, of which 52.9% were females and 84% attended full time (Integrated Postsecondary Education Data System, 2008). Of the 22 students enrolled in BIOL 4600, histology, 12 participated in on site focus group interviews (~55%). Participants One hundred and five students enrolled in 5 classes participated in this study. The vast majority (75%) of participants were either science majors or post-baccalaureate students (n = 81); however, the introductory level course included a large number of non-majors (n = 27), who accounted for roughly 63% of participants in that class. Approximately 18.5% of students were Latino, 39 77.2% were Caucasian, and 5.6% were African-American. Females accounted for 62% of the sample population (n = 67). Every attempt was made to ensure that the participants in this study were accurate representatives of their institutions. In most instances, participants did indeed reflect their site averages (see Table 2). Where measured, participant GPA was not significantly higher than overall GPA. There were roughly equal numbers of participants based on class standing. At 2 of the 4 sites, the male-to- female ratio amongst participants was in line with overall class averages; however, at 2 sites, NCA8T and lAUP-MC, they were not (see Table 2). Males were under-represented at NCA8T, where none participated, but were over- represented at lAUP-MC where half the participating population was male vs. just 16.7% of the total (see Table 2). In all other measured variables, students from these sites accurately represent the central tendency of their class. Instructional intervention Case Itl is a National Science Foundation sponsored project designed to stimulate learning motivation in biology students by engaging them in the practical application of investigative techniques as related to “real world” problems that may affect them as individuals, such as genetic or infectious diseases (Bergland, et al., 2006). Students author content, view peers’ posters, and discuss their own and others’ posters with pupils around the globe. Participants work collaboratively in groups of 2-3, and may select a topic that is of interest to them. Each topic area has multiple case narratives from which 40 m_nm__m>m .02 m 88> m cmmLc>< F 48.8 08.8 08.8 $8 08.8 $8. 08.6 «8.8 08.8 02.8 mason. $4.8 08.8 some $8 «8.8 OS 03.8 08.8 08.3 08.3 222 .5950 some I .89 08.8 I I I I I I cameos. use? 08.8 «8.8 «one: 08.9. «8.8 84$ 08.2 I I 8.8.0. 08.2 at: I I $98 08.8 08.8 08.8 08.4 as: .35.. some I I I so...“ 08.9 I I 02.8 08.8 855288 I I I I I I I I so? #4.: 5558.... 9.55% mama . . . . I I I I I I cm< o 8 m «N m mm A S a a m m a a N 88 8.0 88 m S 8.8 88 a I a I a I a I Ec :55 En. :55 .58 :55 gen. :55 .50. ...eeé ..eaa omInss 0.24.3. 5.52 28mg: 228,25 .8596..th Ac ham. $55 8:» LS «96236936 Eobam .N 653. 41 students can select. As part of the Case Itl environment, students are involved in using multiple methods of DNA testing, bioinformatics, creating and presenting web posters, and role-playing. The version of Case It! used comprised 3 semi- autonomous functions: Case It! Simulation Software. This portion of Case It! allowed students to perform virtual lab tests such as gel electrophoreses, western blot, and ELISA in a limited environment (see Figure 1). While allowing students to run these tests, the simulation software also reduces costs associated with the lab tests since expensive equipment and reagents do not need to be purchased. Mega4 and a separate multiple alignment tool (ClustalW) are bioinformatics tools newly added to Case Itl that allow students to analyze DNA they have isolated using the simulation software. This ability permits the construction of phylogenetic trees that graphically depict both the relation of samples to each other and the relative degree of divergence between samples (see Figure 2). Students use these trees to make inferences about the DNA samples, for example to identify if a particular strain of HIV that one individual contracted is related to another sample of HIV in a different individual. Case It! Launch Pad is a minimal html design tool that allows students to create virtual posters in cyberspace (see Figure 3). Participants are assigned to research and author a web-poster about a human disorder, in the case of this study HIV/AIDS. After viewing/reading different cases about individuals with this disease, students utilize the Case It! software described above. Once the 42 Figure 1. Screenshot of the Case Itl simulation software 43 M4: TrL-e Explorer (treemk) Lil—SI M ELM WVWEW WW . if“? ._ .. , _. ...... - a 66838 t gfiléé - FD; DNA Boyfriendtxt PCR T“ E —_L——DNALisa.txt PCR 9‘: DNA High pathogenic 1.06 655 —_I:— DNA High pathogenic 2m 5‘ DNA Low pathogenic 2m ‘: DNA Low pathogenic 1m 5% 1 DNA Low pathogenic 3.00 93 W 7 581—7025723000 ._ --- ..W . W-... __ -_-_-..-...-..__._._ _-_.. . ._____........---..- - //, Figure 2. Screenshot of a phylogenetic tree created using Case It! and the Mega4 software. 44 +++$++$ owe . ,, ‘gefdeiejtm ,1 efeieie-‘cweteie ‘ Figure 3. Screen shot of a student created webposter made in Case It! launch pad. 45 genetic assays were completed, students used Case Itl launch pad to create web-posters that incorporate information developed from the 2 software programs and research, and permitted student teams to share their research with others. Launch Pad also provides an online forum that allows conferencing to occur between and among students from multiple sites (see Figure 4). Students in 3 of the 4 classes sampled engaged in synchronous and asynchronous Internet conferencing with other students at their own and other institutions once their posters were published to the Case It! website. Because of a lack of time, students at lUPR-MC did not participate in a formalized conference as part of their learning experience; however, some students did say they discussed their cases informally. For those students who did conference, each was required to review other groups’ posters, to role-play as real-world individuals (patients or family members) seeking information or advice, and in turn pose as an expert answering questions asked by other role-playing students on their topic disease. This interaction occurred via online discussion boards hosted on the Case It! website. Data collection Development of instruments. Three data collection instruments were developed and used in this study including (a) a pre-Ipost-test to examine performance gains, (b) a pen-and-paper student survey intended to explore student opinions about the project, and (0) focus group interviews designed to 46 W11} . .‘ - "- ' a ' A . 3.; In as ‘ 3d" ‘ I ' :1"! .11? _. I-r - , "1:95“. 42 :F- v 3‘ 3 ' _ . A i . . r. - 5: . .' Deter" * '- . ‘ ‘-.."' --. . ie--. ». -.-.', '. .-r «. ..:-...-- r. -.,. -, .-.- . . - . -. -- ,« . . .. .W ‘7‘ [JAY ~A‘-'ti...t~afl.'}":t.5 'V-Y-r-i A33 ‘ \I .-' 5.145.211 *. l \ \L- .1 l; #3:"; 5’. .-‘. a ’.‘.': fi'T- I ,_‘~‘ -¢ .-.:-,"fj, .‘ ,4: ;' *- ‘ .' - '_ -' 1. ' , . ' ' '-v ’__ .:.\:;,-.. ; #1“??? 74Lv.._.‘-"_E4; gut. '. u— I-” " I .-H'e}¢*kbtf3.!'~ 'ax'J-Q‘c‘J-L LA in. Ana-w L»! ...alv e "‘ Qw- '4 . uh" ' I 0 . Wed,Apr2220091(k10-.43 PM, wrote: - OnWed,Apr22200906:42:llPM, . wrote: - >Fmofanwhatspecificnusurustmldlmkcto » >pmectmyhusbandanddaughtufioquuifingl-Wu - >wcll?lknowdmitcmbepassodmmughbloodmd - >sanenbutmtlmemyothabodynnidsmaoould > >pouibtybeatlnut? - Toplutectymndmghu.ywsbmldnahemfeedha ~ WMMBwuldbcpmedmrwghmnflk. » Youstmidbuyforumhfiommem.mbeuwnyto ~ pmtectymuhusbendistopnctioenbstincncezbowm. - h‘snottcalisdcsoyousboulddefininlyuseoondom - (nukemttnymusedcomcflynomducethecbm » ofmsnfiningthevhustohim.0merthanbioodmd ' m,HIVcnnboposnedtiumghbtustufilkuwefl. ibelieveWkflnsmdeofiwimawmmmmmmdmmmmamm H... aboutuldngcamofyowbodytsttwfhstmp.mioewoulddeumdndskforhtm.butisnotexpoced .-.. www.mmmmmchMm2waponmJHV . bcmswdmmghMmflk,anNmmoddWywchmmumem.m, mmmflubnmmemmuunchndmtheonlywmmoonmmJObec-nmt Figure 4. Screen shot of student conferencing via Case It! Launch pad 47 more deeply probe student perceptions of experience. Pre-lpost-test exams were collected by participating faculty; however, I conducted all student surveys and focus group interviews on-site. All data analyzed statistically and qualitatively. Learning assessment test. A 6-question assessment instrument was re- tooled for use with the current iteration of Case It! by a team of 6 biology professors and 2 education experts at a workshop in River Falls, Wisconsin in early August 2008, and is included in Appendix A. This instrument contained a case study of a couple and their baby tested for HIV. The case presented contained test results from ELISA and Western blots. Students answered questions requiring them to interpret these results, and advise the family regarding treatment. This case analysis task included 6 items worth a total of 26 points that tested: (a) ability to interpret ELISA test results, (b) ability to interpret Western Blot test results, (0) theoretical knowledge of the uses of both ELISA and Western Blot tests, (d) ability to present and interpret the results of both ELISA and Western Blot tests to patients, and (e) ability to interpret a phylogenetic tree to determine the source of infection. A confidence scale was used to measure student confidence in their own knowledge by providing a 5- point Likert scale for each assessment question. After each item, students were asked to assess how confident they were that their answer was correct based on a scale of 1 (very uncertain) to 5 (very certain) (e.g. Lundeberg, Fox, Brown, & Elbedour, 2000). 48 Initially, the entire test was open-ended, and multiple-choice alternatives were derived from actual student responses for items 1-3. Items 4 and 5 were left as open-ended questions because the development team decided that this was the best method to determine student comprehension of complex situations. A 6"1 question was added to assess students’ ability to interpret phylogenetic trees. Questions 1-5 of the instrument have been used several times previously for publication, but have not been used to specifically test minority engagement and learning in biological contexts. Question 6 on bioinformatics was new, and had not been piloted; however the experts involved in redesigning the test taught at culturally diverse institutions, including an HBUC and 2 Latino institutions, and were asked to keep their own classes in mind as they revised. A Pearson correlation score of 0.263 was computed in SPSS to determine the reliability of question 6 by measuring pre-test vs. post-test scores, indicating unequal reliability; however, Dimitrov and Rumrill (2003) note that the reliability of pre- test/post-test gain scores is high when the question does not have equal reliability since such questions are designed to measure performance differences. Therefore, these results indicated that question 6 was reliable. In addition, Wefer and Sheppard (2008) note the field of bioinformatics is so new that neither federal nor state level standards exist. Because there is little consensus on bioinformatics beyond a broad definition, there is little to no availability for tested instruments. Other research in the associated field of problem-based learning suggests that measures do not necessarily have to be 49 previously tested to be reliable (T arhan & Acar, 2007). The test was specifically designed to be short to accommodate instructors’ time constraints (approximately 20 minutes), and to minimize student frustration. It was translated into Spanish for students in Puerto Rico by native speakers and via Google Translate. Student survey. An 11-question protocol specific to this research study and developed in partnership with 6 faculty members was designed to probe student perceptions of their experience from the perspectives of personal/cultural relevance, intentions to persist, and individual awareness of learning (see Appendix B). Items were developed and refined from: (a) previous interview questions used in past studies of the Case Itl project, (b) published literature on student attrition in STEM fields (e.g. Seymour & Hewitt, 1997; Tobias, 1990), and (c) studies of student motivation in the sciences (e.g. Astleitner & Wiesner, 2004; Koballa & Glynn, 2007). Specifically, items asked students about: 1. Future career plans. 2. Whether they believed the Case It! experience helped prepare them for future classes or careers. 3. If the experience reinforced students’ desire to be science majors. 4. Topic interest pre- and post-experience. 5. The relevance of the project to students’ lives. 6. Student confidence in topic knowledge. 7. The most valuable learning components of the project. 8. Student opinions of the utility of role-playing on learning. 50 9. _ Student opinions of role-playing as individuals within the case, and how much they learned be role-playing 10.Whether students felt the learning experience was a good one. 11.0ther information students wished to share about the project. Of the 11 questions, 5 had been used in slightly different form in previous studies of the learning environment. Participating faculty selected, reviewed, and refined questions, serving as both pedagogical and content experts. I revised Items multiple times individually, and with participating faculty as a group. Each question was written as open-ended for inclusion in focus group interviews, and as closed-ended and quantifiable for inclusion on the student survey. Once formatting was finalized, the survey was translated into Spanish via both native speakers and Google Translate. Focus group Interviews. The same focus of the 11 questions used on the student survey were included in the focus group interview protocol, but were restated in a more open-ended fashion in an effort to elicit more detailed student responses (see Appendix C). Where appropriate, follow-up probes were utilized to gain a better understanding of students’ thoughts and opinions. Expecting that time would be a limiting factor during interviews, questions were re-ordered to ensure the most important were asked and answered. These items were also translated into Spanish by an onsite interpreter and with the aid of Google Translate. 51 Collectlon procedures. Prior to collecting any data from students, consent forms (see Appendix D) were distributed at each phase of research, collected, and kept on file. IRB approval was obtained from MSU (IRB #X07- 511) and all 4 research sites. Learning assessment test. Students took the learning assessment tool (see Appendix A) twice during the semester: once before using the Case It! program, but after lectures on HIV, to establish a benchmark upon which to measure both ability and confidence, and again shortly after completing their Case ltl projects. The use of both a pre- and post-test allowed the computation of gain scores and estimations of what students learned beyond lectures. Pre- tests were administered much as student class evaluations are, by volunteer students in class without the instructor present. Exams were sealed in envelopes and given to me upon arrival onsite. Post-tests were either administered by myself when onsite to conduct interviews, or again by a volunteer student. Student survey. The 11-item student survey was given at the beginning of focus group interviews. Students had approximately 10 minutes to answer, and kept the surveys for reference during the interviews. At the end of each session, students turned in the surveys directly to me. Focus group Interviews. Students were given the opportunity to participate in on-site focus group interviews to answer questions about: (a) their experience with Case It! (b) whether they felt socially or culturally engaged by the project, and (c) the relevance of their chosen research t0pic to their own lives 52 (see Appendix C). Each focus group interview last approximately 1 hour, with group sizes ranging from 612 students depending on overall number of participants. Student sat in a circle around a conference table and freely commented on both my questions and statements made by other students. All focus group interview session were video recorded and transcribed. Participation was highly variable, ranging from 23-97%. The BlOL 345 class at UWRF had the highest participation rate at 97%, with a total of 38 students participating, divided into 4 focus groups. Over 86% of students enrolled in BIOL 150 introductory biology at UWRF (n = 43) chose to participate, also divided into 4 focus groups. Just over half of the students (n =12, 53%) at lUPR-SG chose to participate in a single focus group interview. Approximately 30% (n = 6) of all students in the class at NCA&T participated in a single focus group interview. MEDT 4531 at lUPFl-MC had the lowest participation rate at just 23%, with 6 students participating in a single focus group interview late in the afternoon. ‘ Data analysis A collective case study design (Creswell, 2007b; Yin, 2003) was used to compare results across sites. Each site was analyzed individually based on the research questions, and then all 4 were compared and contrasted. Learning assessment test. I blindly scored all pre- and post- assessment tests for all participants. Closed questions were scored using an answer key, while open-ended questions were scored using a rubric adapted 53 from one used to score previous versions of the exam to minimize the role of subjectivity in scoring exams (see Appendix E). lntra-rater reliability was calculated on open-ended questions. I randomly rescored 27 (~20%) pre- and post-test exams across all 5 participating courses and correlated secondary scores to original scores in SPSS (Wuensch, 2007) to produce a Pearson correlation score of 0.860, indicating a high degree of reliability. Scores were analyzed using SPSS statistical software to generate descriptive statistics, gain scores, and significance tests that were correlated with demographic information collected to create an overall picture of each class. Data were analyzed using repeated measures ANOVA to investigate changes between pre- and post-test performance based on total score for each exam. Additional post hoc comparisons were conducted to determine between-subjects effects such as gender and study site. Gain score was also computed for each student from pre- to post-test examination. An item analysis of performance was carried out by ANOVA on overall performance, gender, and site, followed up by independent sample t-tests. Student survey. I conducted statistical analyses on all closed-ended questions after they were quantified and entered into an SPSS database. I generated descriptive statistics based on overall response to each of the 10 quantifiable questions, assessed gender interactions for each question via independent sample Meets, and analyzed site interactions using ANOVA. When 54 provided, written answers on the survey were appended to focus group interview transcripts and coded as part of each. Focus group interviews. I organized data from focus group interviews and open-ended survey questions both categorically and chronologically. This data was reviewed repeatedly, and continuously coded. Transcripts were coded simultaneously utilizing both deductive and inductive coding schemes. To ensure reliability of findings, l blindly recoded 6 of the 11 interview transcripts, including at least 1 from each class. These data were then analyzed in SPSS (Wunensch, 2007) to determine inter-rater reliability, which resulted in a Pearson correlation score of 0.906 and supports a high level of confidence in coding reliability. Deductive coding. I developed a priori codes from prior research on the Case It! project and from the literature on student persistence in STEM programs (Boyatzis, 1998; Creswell, 2007b). Themes included student engagement, personal relevance, interest, motivation, and persistence in field. These themes were revised repeatedly as research progressed and the presence or absence of each was observed in the data. Inductive coding. As analysis progressed, it became apparent that themes were emerging in the data that were not accounted for by existing a priori codes. To address this evolution of themes, open and axial coding schemes (Creswell, 2007b) were utilized to group data into categories and relate them back to pre-existing a priori codes and themes. Themes such as denial, bigger picture, and critical thinking were identified in the text of student comments and 55 combined with axial codes developed prior to analysis to create a mosaic of codes. In general, each student comment was given a single code, although 65 (8.6%) student comments exhibited 2 themes. In such cases the comment was counted for each theme it illustrated. For example, the comment that, “We're just going into a field where you need to make sure that you're thinking about different things, different demographics, and you need to make sure that you're able to put yourself in their [the patient’s] shoes,” was counted twice because it address both functional science and instructional quality. Primary coding resulted in several clusters of themes. Upon further review and secondary analysis, I reduced the number of original categories by approximately 68%. Codes deemed to be thematically similar based on both student comments and the existing literature were combined to enhance clarity. 56 CHAPTER 4: Results In this mixed methods study, I collected data from both quantitative (pre- test/post-test; student survey) and qualitative sources (focus group interviews). Data were first analyzed as an aggregate whole (case study A) to create an overall picture across all study sites of students’ persistence, performance, and opinions. I then analyzed each site individually (case studies B-F) to identify any site-specific differences. Within each site, data were organized by research question. Case Study A: Cross-site comparison Student performance. The data from this study show that the Case ltl learning environment positively influenced student performance. Students showed a significant gain score over time (see Table 3), and significant performance improvement on questions 26 (see Table 4). There were large, significant gains from pre- to post-test for all groups (see Figure 5 & 6), and student confidence in answers increased from pre- to post-tests (see Table 4). Overall, there was a large, significantly positive increase in performance from the pre- to post-test (F(1, 80) = 17.256, ps0.01, n2 = 0.177; see Table 3). Results show the only significant influence on student performance was time between the pre- and post-tests (noted as “time”), with no other combination of factors yielding significant results (see Table 3); however, performance varied significantly by site (F(4, 80)=4.293), ps0.01, n2 = 0.177; see Table 3) As Figure 57 Table 3. Performance results of pre-/post-test repeated measures C" Significance 2 F Hyp. Error (P) Tl Within subjects tests Time“ 17.256 1 80 0.000 0.1 77 Time x Gender 0.329 1 80 0.568 0.004 Time x Site 0.164 4 80 0.956 0.008 Time x Gender x Site 0.321 3 80 0.810 0.012 Between subjects tests Gender 0.569 1 80 0.453 0.007 Site* 4.293 4 80 0.003 0.1 77 Note. * p s 0.01 ** p 5 0.001 58 58 w e a £8555 .. mod m e a. 28:33 .. ..sez :mvmd emu; momd n 5N m 9:69 mesmEEEBB ho 5:26.925 m0 . . . . 2E... fans 0 mm— F 3% w No? m m Scam 9:62:00 5 23$. 8 cosmo=aQ< m0 toomd 5: mmoé mm; m 9:93 95.6958 E $.3me “—0 cosmozaaxx v0 :355 was; Cod wood m Em £2me .w> > u Amy 5.5.5 55 .0503 2:52:55 “5055505 :05. u E .052 E 5.8 8 e 53 a. 058 8 .V 53 9.0 9.38. 8.225222 l l l l l l l l 59.0.0 .6 5:80.90“... o0 . . 2.5 Q woo o mm 5 m E v i l l l 05 Sena 9:00:50 5. i i l i 5.888 2: e 8.5 .850 928. .e 558.29.. 00 .9 wood mm v oomd l l l I 2% 0.5.5 92.00500 5 l l l l l l l I .0200 05:00. .0 5:02.520. 50 l l l l l l l l 05m 85 5060.5 .m> l l l l l l l l .0050 05l>=.. :0 .0905 0.00. 00.00 .05 500.0... 5 ...55500. .50> 5.3 05 000.. 0. 0>00 .05. 50> .00..0>000.5 300 0>020 0.0.0.... 0.00. 0. 05500.00 0>0>>.0 0.0.055... .......5.0 500 00. 5 0:05 0203 .003 500 .0503 5 39. 9... salts... <0_._m 9.. 95 22888.. .00l0_00. 0.500555 05:500.0.50. .. 0.55 _. ....0.005 0 0.0 50> 0.... .00. 50> 00500.3... .05 50500.0. 0. 0.50 00 0. 00.00 _._.0 0... ..0.000.00 05 .0 5:0 05 .0..< .30: 0.0.5 0. 5505 .0. 5.5.00. 00...... ...00..05..0 0....00. 0 .0 550. I0... 0.5.5. .0. .53 50> .. .00..00.0 00.00 00... 0........ ...0.0000 05.50 5.0.5.. 85: 00; _ 95 =0. .0 5.... _ .5. 83 5. .. . ..50 >05 5:0 5 >05 .05 50> .003 0. 0.5 50< 00.0: 050. 50> 5:0 0.5.00. 5 0.05 ..0 50> 000.0 .050 >.0>0 .3000. 50> ... 0. 0000.000 50.0.55 0 003 .. .05 500... .05. _. 00.00.0500: .0 ..005.00.. 00000.5 .5000 0.:0E:.00 5500000. .00..>.000 .0 50.0000. 050500250 .0005. 5050.005 .0 005 55.000 05 650.00. .5000 0.000.050 .00..00..0.50.5 .0 .3500. 555.055 .050. 0.50000 .5000 0.000.056 .5550. 05.00.00 .0 0000.00 .0 >.0.000 .0 0.2.50 05 0.0. 52050.5 0.00. 55.00. .5050 .0 .0:0..00.0.5 50.5..50 >..:50.0.00 .5000 0.000.050 0000.00 500.0.0505 0. .0 0000.8 500.0.0505 o. .0098 05 00.0000 0. 0050.305. 5055000 0. 50.0.0. 0.000.500 .00>.. .0 0.005 .0055. 5.00550 0. 5..0> .00.:00I0.00.00 .5050 0.000.050 .0500: .0..:0.00 .0 .000..000> 05.0000 0. 0000000.. .00..00.0.:_ .0055.0-0.-.0055.0 ..0.05..05 :0 00 55.00 .m5>0.0-0_0. 55.55 5000.0>05 05020.5 05 5:5... .m:.>0_0-0_0. .0 5...: 05 .5050 0.000.050 .00000 .0. 00:0.0.0.0 0 .0 50.0.5 50000.00. ..0..0.00. 05 5.2, 00550008 5000.00 05.05.55 .5050 000.500 .0 0.000.250 500.08 0000.00 ”050.00.. ”00000.0 5550. 0.500005 "0.0._0m ”0000.00 0.5 50.050.002.056500 500050.50 “0000.00 500.505”. ”5:30.005... $5.050 .0:0..05..0:. 5.0.00 0. 002.020. 000.50.050 0.00.0.6 00.58.0000 0050.00 00500 .0 00.00.05 500 000.....050Q .0 0.00... 67 ...0. 0 00000.5 05 5:05.050: 0:. 550.00 0... 00 .>_... 5.3 50000505 0000 00: 05. >0. :. 00000.00 .050500 05 55.55 .3 52:. 5:65 580 000.. .50. 500; _ .0... .<.. ...5 5:.05 00... 5.000000: ..:05 . .50 55.00.05. .0 5:5. 0... 50.0000. 05 0. .003 ..005 . 30:0. . .05 5005.50. .0 550. 000 5.. 0000... ...5 .5000 555>00 5.00 0.000 0: .000. 5lwo.< 5.3 0:0>:0 30:0. ..005 _ .05 50.5 >500. ..00005 5 30: 55...... A0000000 .003 500 505.03 05..> 05 300 5:0 .0500 5.50050. 05 00... .0 000.0 0000.00 0 5 500.00. 0; .05 55052 080... .2... =0 .5585 .5. 0000... ..:.0.00.:55500 0. 300 0.00. 0. 003 .005 .5.0>0 05 .05 5.5 .. .0... 5 050.005 .0 0:05.005 0.50000 0. 0000000.. 0.050500 .0 005 05 .0 505.000. .0 05.0.5 00 5050005 >...05..0 50.05. 50.000.005 .0 .0 0000.00 >0 505:0..0 55.00. 0. 50.0.0. 0.:05500 ..0: ...3 .0 .505 50.5 .00000 .055 0000005 55 0.0.50 .050555 0. 50.0.0. 0500.055 00.05 005000050 0.0.555 50.. .0 .55.. 50.0.55 0 5 050.5 50.000 0005000000 55000 .0005 50.50 0003.00 0550.305. .0 .0500. h .0000. >0. 5 50500 5550.00 5550.05 .0 0.0.50.00 5.3 >.0>..00..0 55.00.050.500 0. 50.0.0. 05050.00 “3005-0.-5002 ”0000.00 ..0 500.5 k H0:0..000:000.250.:0D\0E5..w ”002500050 0.0.55.2 “00.50.:55500 68 Table 7. Categories of comments and their aggregate instances across all sites (n =1160) Category n % Intentions to persist 548 47.07% Instructional quality: 250 21.55% Functional science: 1 16 10.00% Role-playing: 99 8.53% Self-efficacy: 60 5.1 7% Community/Integrated into science: 23 1.98% Beliefs: 612 52.76% Diagnostic testing process: 160 13.79% Learning: 132 1 1 .38% Science content: 116 10.02% Communication: 70 6.04% Multiple perspectives: 61 5.26% Turned off science: 27 2.33% Stigma/Denial/Misconceptions: 26 2.24% Need-to-know: 20 1 .72% Note: Percentage calculated by [n(category) / total n] 69 together,” or William who reported Case It! “...kind of ties everything together in one thing, even what you learned in lecture and other wet labs.” Kira reported that, “...it helps to actually apply what you’re learning in class, like we did with Case It! and be able to understand it more...” Kira also said that she enjoyed that the structure of instruction was different saying, I just liked that it was a different approach to it. You know, every other class, you sit there in lecture and you take notes, and this is what you do, day in and day out, and the same with lab. But this is just a different approach. I’ve never taken a class that used the technology in this way, so it was interesting. However, a few students like Aaron said, ‘..I just didn’t get into it. It seemed kind of artificial to me, and a little bit forced,” indicating that the methods utilized by Case It! were not universally relevant. For many Case It! created a bridge between content and their private lives by making cases personally relevant to them. This attitude is typified by Leah who said that, it made me feel more like a personal connection with them [the patients in the cases]... since it was a true story, I was able to get a better connection with doing the Western Blot and ELISA and being able to speak to them. Beth seconded this cpinion stating that, “...I think actually reading the case studies of people’s actual lives, people that are affected by it, makes it more human and less of something that like you learn in science class...” A feeling of personal responsibility to the characters in the cases was frequently cited as a reason for exerting greater effort on the project, as demonstrated by Emma who said, “...when we got to the actual personal cases with it, you wanted to do it 70 more because you want to find out for those people...” Other students like William said that the personal connections created were important because, “...when you can see a name or a face to relate that to it makes it more real.” An example of how information from this project can affect students’ private lives is evidence by Zoé who said that, “At first I wasn’t real happy about doing it, but during the semester someone in my life has been diagnosed with HIV, so it’s helped me understand the disease a lot.” Ashley extrapolated her experiences to her future stating, ...I also think [Case ltl] helps in personal life if you’ve a friend or loved one that’s going to have to deal with this disease—you’re going to be able to hopefully take the knowledge that we’ve gained through here and be able to put yourself in. . .their shoes... Leah took a slightly different view of the same issue when she said, ...[Case Itl] was just real personal, and—well, down to earth I guess—but, just to be able to possibly relate to these people, and to know that these cases are real, and that you could get it, just made it—made Case It! more fun to work with... Many students expressed empathy for those involved with the cases, like Ashley who said, “...you’re going to be able to hopefully take the knowledge that we’ve gained through [Case Itl] and be able to put yourself in...their shoes, and be...more understanding. And do the empathetic thing and be there for them.” Zoé echoed this sentiment saying, “...[Case Itl] helps me understand what the person is going through." Alice said that, “... the personal lives of these real people...[make Case Itl] relevant,” demonstrating that using real individuals, not made-up characters, led some students to find greater relevance and importance 71 in the material. Emma said the same feeling was true for her because, “...when we got to the actual personal cases with it, you wanted to do it more because you want to find out for those people. . .” Students also made comments to the effect that Case It! either stimulated their interest in health, or that it made them eager to learn more. Most student comments echoed those of Olivia who said, “I found [Case Itl] really interesting,” or Maggie who said, “I just thought it was cool!” However, some students were more specific as to what had stimulated their interest, like Carrie who said that the personal connection to the material, “... just makes a lot more real. It just makes it more interesting.” Maggie was very excited about the project, telling me at another point during the focus group interview that she was, “. . .definitely more interested in HIV and AIDS than I was before—just ‘cause there’s so much to learn.” Leah was perhaps the most specific when she said, “... actually being able to do [ELISAs and Western Blots]...helped me understand it better, and I became more interested...Being able to do it made me appreciate it more.” Functional science (related to future work). Many students said that they found the Case It! experience to be directly relatable to their future professional lives whether that be in healthcare, research, biotechnology, education, or any other of myriad options. Overall, health related career paths were the most popular choices for student overall, and by gender. There was significant agreement by students (X2 = 46.05; p = 0.000) that Case It! helped prepare them for future classes or careers. 72 As a category, functional science is defined as that knowledge needed for specific, vocational purposes, and so it is not surprising that given the disproportionate number of upper division classes in this study about 10.0% (n = 116) of all student comments were coded here (see Table 7). Some students reported that the experience helped them better envision or prepare for their futures, like José who said, “... you [see] how you can relate...life on the outside with what you are going to do in your job,” and Olivia’s comment that, “[Case Itl] actually did help a lot...it helped me realize that I want to go into the medical aspect of biotechnology and science.” Kathy said, “...it helps you when you think about a career and what you want to do.” Autumn was more direct, simply averring that, “I definitely think [Case Itl] helped prepare me for a career,” and that she thought, “...more than anything else, the counseling and role-playing really help prepare for a lot of future careers that some of us are planning on having...” Others reported that Case It! provided them with a real-world environment in which to practice applying theoretical knowledge acquired in this and other classes. This is exemplified by Eli’s comment that, l...have to say that [Case Itl] helped with future careers, or future classes, because not only does it emphasize new things that researchers, or even just biology majors, might go through later on, but you can use this knowledge in different classes that come up... Neil reinforced this perspective saying that, “...it's like some practice if you will for future life, kind of a real-life situation...” Other students reported that a real benefit of Case It! was that it served to reinforce decisions students already made, but may have questioned. This 73 sentiment is illustrated by comments from Noah who said, “I definitely think it was a good introduction to research...” and “...l’m also going to be doing research, so this was definitely a reinforcement for me.” Emma said that Case It! had, “...reinforced my career choice...” while Kim simple stated that, “This will help me in the future.” Role-playing. Of student comments in focus group interviews, 8.5% (n = 99) were coded as role-playing (see Table 7). Overall, students moderately agreed on a Likert scale of 1-5 that role-playing helped them learn (M=3.526). Several students said that participating in role-playing forced them to look at problems from multiple angles and engage in analytical thinking, like Katie who said that, ...role-playing and conferencing...really forced me to think critically about the results that we had from our cases, and forced me to think about them in a different light so I’d be able to explain them to whomever was asking us questions... Autumn seconded this position saying she, “...enjoyed the aspect of role-playing a lot, and seeing from a different perspective. It made me more interested in looking at the different groups of people that it affects and how it affects their families as well.” Kim cross-referenced role-playing with empathy when she said, “...role playing is another good [part of Case Itl] because you really got to go into the mindset of the person going though this.” Other students enjoyed the ability to step into another person’s life, such as Neil who said that role-playing was, “. . .like getting into someone else’s shoes, or at least trying,” and Lisa who 74 reported that, ...the role-playing was really cool for me because I can really get into somebody else’s shoes, and think like I think they would think.” Over 20 students said that they enjoyed the dialogue of questions and answer that arose from being required to role-play as both doctor and family member. Students said that this discourse caused them to be more contemplative, like Winnie who said that, “...its good to just sit down and think about [HIV] and ask more questions.” Others, like Jarred, said it caused them to consider the questions they were asking from new or different perspectives, stating, “Some times when you were role-playing, and you’re asking the questions, you had to think, ‘Well, if I really had this, what would I want to know. What would be my questions?” This connection with codes in the bigger picture category is reinforced by Leah’s comment that she was, asked questions that I didn’t even think about. And then the way how we had to ask questions like if we didn’t have a biological background, I think that helped, and it also made me aware of the information that I knew because I was able to answer the questions. Pupils also identified the need to answer very specific and directed questions during role-playing as a reason for conducting deeper research, like Autumn who said, “I think it helped to learn because if they asked question that you actually didn't know, well then you had to go and look for the information and learn it that way...” Kira reiterated this perspective stating, “...[we] had a question that we hadn’t thought about in that way, or it was about one of the tests that we didn’t cover, so we had to figure it out.” 75 Other students reported that the best part of the role-playing experience for them was the opportunity to teach others in the role of either physician or genetics counselor. Sometimes these comments were linked to additional research, like Gavin who said, “I liked also the role-playing, more so being the one answering the questions because it made you go into more depth because you thought well if they asked this question but then what about this...?” Ashley said that, I think I learned more because I was able to break it down, to make it simple, to say "T his is this, and that is that." For me at least, I’ve always been able to learn more when I teach, so I think I did learn more when I had to break it down. A vocal number of students were adamant in their support of conferencing between peers as a useful component of Case It! A number of students said that questions asked in conferencing helped them view questions and issues from different perspectives, like Tanisha who said, I just think its always interesting to see what other people think, because you could give us all the same book and we’d all say different things about it, so I think its pretty cool to just see how they thought about their case... Associated with this idea was Leah’s comment that, “...[Other students] asked questions that I didn’t even think about.” One student, Kevin, was expressive when he cautioned others on the difference between role-playing and conferencing, telling me, Role-playing was useless. I know that’s very blunt and frank, but I think that a lot of people are confusing the conferencing with a role-playing. Conferencing, very beneficial—scientists talking to each other, discussing the disease—great way to learn. Me pretending to be Lisa’s husband, and 76 asking, ‘What do these results mean?’ when I know the answer is not useful. Earlier in the discussion, Kevin expressed his favor for peer-conferencing when he said, “I really like the conferencing —just anytime you have discussion it leads to more knowledge than to somebody telling you something.” At least one other student, Jarred, was thankful for the opportunity to conference with his peers because, You can also maybe act like a person that has the virus, and so maybe if there's something you're not too sure of, and you don‘t really want to ask it in front of the class, that you can kind of like be, "Hey! I'm Maria. I was just wondering this. Do you have an answer?" Then people wouldn't know that you really don't know stuff. Building self-efficacy. An appealing component of Case It! for some students (n = 60, 5.2%; see Table 7) was the opportunity to demonstrate the depth of their knowledge, or to be the expert in a pseudo-professional situation. This is borne out by the fact that overall, students said they felt confident in their knowledge of science content (M=3.925; scale of 1-5), and could apply that knowledge autonomously. Greg said, “...its kind of cool that you get to be somebody else, and you get to act like you know what you’re doing—you get to act like the smartest one.” Katie seconded this cpinion stating, “...you can think to yourself, ‘Oh, I would be the expert in this situation.’ And, it’s a little strange, but pretty cool at the same time.” Other students focused their comments more on their own knowledge acquisition and personal development, like Neil, who said, “...[before] I knew of [HIV], but not about it. Now I feel like I can field general questions about it, and point someone in the right direction to get an 77 answer.” Comments such as Greg’s when he said, “[The material is] going to stick now. After the end of the semester, I’m still going to be able to remember this.” illustrate that some students felt participating in Case It! cemented knowledge beyond what they felt was typical for class. Feeling like a scientist (Community/integrated into science). One of the benefits of participating in Case It! that students cited was that it made them feel either like members of a community of scientific scholars, or integrated into the culture of science and scientific inquiry. A total of 23 comments (~2.0%; see Table 7) were coded into this subcategory. These included opinions about feeling like a “real doctor” as evidenced by Caesar’s statement that, “[Case Itl] lets you see what is happening, and it makes you feel like you are a doctor,” Tanisha’s belief that, “...I think that part of the case made it more like you were actually a doctor...” and Greg’s laughing remark that, “...I kind of felt like I was House [a popular television doctor] curing people.” Other comments indicated that students felt the encounter gave them virtual experience as a professional in the field, like Catherine who said that, “...we were able to basically be the doctor, or the lab technician, and say, ‘Yes this person has it. Or no, this isn't. Or, you might need to retest. ’” Student beliefs. Students overwhelmingly thought that Case Itl was a good learning experience for them, with 95% responding positively to the question on the student survey. The most frequent reason given for this was that the project made the material “real” to participants instead of just abstract or 78 esoteric facts shot at them in a lecture. Students made personal connections with the material. For some, this manifested as altruism where participants felt a sense of duty to the individuals portrayed in the case studies and obligated to help them, even though the situation was constructed. Other students said that they developed a sense of empathy for the patients as the project progressed, and because of this felt more connected and that the information was more relevant to them. The majority of students (59%) found the project to be relevant to their lives, and as a group thought their experiences had made them more confident in their knowledge (M=3.808). Participants also reported a significant increase in interest from before to after Case It! (t(106)=9.894, ps0.001). Figure 8 shows that the two most popular aspects of the project overall were the virtual lab and conferencing aspects, although Opinions varied by gender and site. As with the previous section on persistence, codes and themes related to student beliefs were identified prior to conducting the study. These were combined with emergent themes to yield 8 categories of student beliefs about the project (see Table 7). Comments in these categories accounted for roughly 53% (n = 612) of all student statements. Diagnostic testing procedures. As shown in Table 4, a large number of comments (n = 160; 13.8%) indicate that students said the speed at which results were returned, and the number of different tests available contributed significantly to both their enjoyment of the Case ltl environment and their learning. Tanira 79 ‘9// e e ’\ e o 0‘ 7“ ‘<’ v” <2~' 9 0* $8 59“ e0 o" 35“ Most useful components of Case It! I Virtual lab ' Biotechnology ‘I Conferencing I Webposters ' Internet research Figure 8. Student ratings of most useful Case It! components. 80 demonstrated this attitude when she said, “I enjoyed how easy [the virtual lab] was to learn...and how fast and easy it was. I liked that I could see the process in the virtual lab...” Other students like Kira thought that the virtual lab was a good focal point because, “...having to go through process of...figuring everything out helped a lot...” while Gavin said that “...the virtual |ab...was quick and easy, and this is how it all goes together, and here are your results, and here you analyze it...” The concept of bioinformatics, the employment of technology to inform life sciences, was new to most students in this study. Jarred typified many students’ opinions on the topic, “Bioinformatics? I didn't really know a lot about that. This is the first time that I‘ve heard about it, so I've actually learned quite a bit about that.” Overall, students appear to have felt that Case It! provided a good environment in which to explore bioinformatics, as demonstrated by comments from Nicole that, “...Case It! made it easier to do bioinformatics...” and Maggie that, “...I just thought it was really interesting that someone could create a phylogenetic tree and say, ‘Well, this is how your virus is related. This is how you got it.”’ Several students said that they appreciated not only the ability to quickly develop results from various molecular tests, such as ELISA, Western Blot, and Southern Blot, but also to accurately analyze those results. Kevin exemplified this position when he observed that, “...[Case It! gave us] the experience and the results part—where we got the results—we know how to analyze the results. I 81 think it was a lot more beneficial than doing the physical experiments.” Catherine added her voice, stating that she, “...definitely felt the most beneficial part was understanding the results...” Other students like Aaron were less specific but still adamant on the usefulness of Case It! in providing and interpreting results when he said, “I liked being able to look at the lab results. Most people don’t take the time to explain it, and I think that’s really important.” Learning. Comments that made either broad reference to scholarship, or described specific techniques were coded into the learning category (n = 132, 11.4%, see Table 7). Many students had specific opinions on how Case It! impacted their knowledge base in relation to application, conducting additional research, and critical thinking. However, others expressed themselves more generally, saying that Case ltl, “...helps you visualize what you need to do,” (Caesar), or “...[Case Itl] did a lot of reinforcing...” (Rachel). Some students said that Case Itl created a hands-on learning situation where they were able to apply theoretical knowledge in practical situations, which was more interesting and engaging to them. Daniel said that, “You got to apply the theory. It was like a hands-on training—a lot of understanding, not just ‘ reading and reading,” while Kira said tha , “...it helps to actually apply what you’re learning in class, like we did with Case Itl and be able to understand it more...” Several students said they enjoyed working with Case It! because it was based in practice, like Tanisha who said, “...when you read it in a book, you can’t really get the hands-on feeling, so Case It! allowed you to do that,” and 82 Aaron’s statement that he, . .got to take theoretical information and turn it into applicable knowledge...which you don’t get to do very much in classes...” Margerie was emphatic when she said that, “When you practice it is not the same as in the books—the experience is completely different.” Greg appreciated the chance to, “. . .apply my knowledge...” Other students said that the hands-on nature allowed them to make connections between different aspects of the learning environment, like Jarred who emphasized his enjoyment of being able to put theory to practical use when he said, “...[I] see real life application of the ELISA, and the viral load, and bioinformatics being put into application.” Greg’s comment that, “...it’s gonna stick now. After the end of the semester, I’m still going to be able to remember this,” demonstrates that students also felt that what they learned from Case It! would persist beyond just the end of the class. A frequent comment was that Case It! encouraged autonomous learning and stimulated personal discovery and interest. Conducting additional, independent research was linked by some students, like Kevin, to a deeper connection with the material when he told us that, “...once you start reading people's questions...you have to start looking up stuff and that makes you learn more about the disease, and you find out things that are interesting.” The idea that additional research can act as a catalyst to generate interest is further supported by Rachel’s comment that she, “...think[s] that was beneficial because then it just has sparked your interest in you to do further research,” and Kathy’s 83 statement that, “...[Case Itl] helps me out with other diseases and it might encourage me to go out and do more research not necessarily on AIDS, but maybe on other types of infectious diseases...” Greg liked the fact that he had the opportunity to conduct further research, telling me that Case Itl, ...gave me time to remember things and look stuff up on the internet.” A few students thought that Case It! helped stimulate analytical processes. Ashley is a good example of one of these students. She said, “I think I learned more because I was able to break it down-to make it simple—to [be able to] say, ‘This is this, and that is tha Other students like Katie said that, ...role-playing and conferencing. . .really forced me to think critically about the results that we had from our cases, and forced me to think about them in a different light so I’d be able to explain them to whomever was asking us questions, while Catherine commented that, “...this experience gives us the analytical aspects of biology, so we’re more able, and prepared...” Science content. Just over 10% of all student comments were coded as science content items (n = 116; see Table 7). Many students expressed their experiences with Case Itl through the lens of content specific to a disease. Neil’s statement that, “[Case Itl] helped get some more insight on HIV, the virus itself...” is typical of this perspective. Students mentioned the treatment of disease on several occasions in the focus group interviews. These comments appear to have come predominantly from those pupils intending to pursue careers in health related fields like nursing, medical technology. and primary care, and are typically associated with some 84 form of surprise or developing interest on the student’s part. Eli’s comment is a good example of this. He stated that, “...I think I am more interested in the pharmaceutical control regimen of HIV. I didn’t know anything about that, or how it worked—anti-retroviral medication.” Many females students expressed empathetic undertones to treatment, like Ashley, who said that, “...I really had no idea about the treatments and the drugs that they had out there that were, not actually curing it, but giving some hope and a potential for a brighter future,” and Heather, who said, “...the treatment options were really interesting...l saw the different combinations, and you’re not just taking one thing. You’re kind of hitting it [HIV] at different angles.” The transmission of HIV or how genetic disorders develop were not subjects that arose frequently; however, some students were detailed in their appreciation of how Case It! helped them better understand this aspect of a disorder. An example is Leah’s comment that, “...it was interesting to know that just because some of the cases were in the US, and some were in Africa, they all contracted the same way, no matter what you’re doing, or who you’re with. . .” which also alludes to the codes, misconceptions and learning. Another student comment that evidences interactions with other codes is Eli’s when he said, “Because of having to research everything, we actually had to learn how it infects and what can actually go on during the infection—the infection cycle and everything.” 85 Communication. As shown in Table 7, a number of student comments (n = 70; 6.0%) indicated that students found Case Itl engaging, useful, or relevant because it required them to develop the skills to communicate effectively with both patients and scientists. Some of these comments dealt directly with the ability to convey ideas and converse with specialists on a professional level. This was demonstrated by Catherine’s commentthat, “...we’re more able and prepared to go out and be in the lab situation or doctor’s office, and be able to know what’s being presented in tests...” Another student, Kevin, said, “I think that the overall goal was to learn how to communicate, and [to] learn how to interpret the test results. . .” The vast majority of comments coded as communication dealt with how to relate or interpret scientific results for the layman. These comments generally hinged either upon the idea that one would be required to perform such duties within the normal scope of one’s job (e.g. as a nurse), or upon the realization that students may not have considered effective communication of complex ideas to be an issue prior to participating in Case Itl The former idea was exemplified by Carly’s comment, I thought that...answering questions from each other, was the most important because...on a professional level, as medical interpreter, I’m sure I’m going to encounter patients who have tested positive, and I’m going to have to know how to talk with them professionally... Katie’s comment illustrated the latter idea when she said, “...roIe-playing and conferencing...forced me to think about [cases/HIV] in a different light so I’d be able to explain them to whomever was asking us questions—explain them in the 86 simplest forms so they would understand.” Another student, Rachel, bridged the two ideas, telling me, “I think it like helped us with bedside manner, and being able to show compassion for people who are...you know these are people who've had their worlds turned upside down with some of these diagnoses...” Multiple perspectives. Knowledge frequently has applications outside the context in which it was learned, and several students commented on this fact during the focus group interviews (n = 61, 5.3%; see Table 7). Comments in this category link to an emerging branch of biology, called systems biology, that seeks to emphasize and accentuate connections between subject areas, promote the development of multiple perspectives on issues, and create discourse around ethical dilemmas. Teresa’s comment illustrates the application of this concept within Case It! almost verbatim when she said, “...it helped us make connections between different areas of science that we maybe hadn’t thought of before. . A number of students said that participating in Case It! forced them to look at issues from multiple perspectives that they otherwise might not have considered, or even known about. Nifia gave voice to a common example echoed across sample sites when she said, I worked on the young African woman [Catrice] who was a young mother, and I learned that not only was it her family that her HIV affected, but also others [in her community] were affected by the results of the test. I’d never really thought about either... Jarred said that, “You definitely learn how to take others' perspectives on things, meaning patients and also the counselor side of things, and how to talk with both 87 when you're doing the conferencing.” Gavin said that the autonomous nature of the learning environment helped him see connections, stating that, “...you just go off on tangents, and then you further your knowledge and understanding of AIDS, and how it impacts the world around us...” Several other students made similar comments about how unique an experience it was to view what they considered to be a biological issue from sociological and cultural perspectives, and how that helped them develop a more holistic lens with which to view the problem of HIV. Neil summed up his experience with Case Itl saying, ...there’s so many people involved in studying a case like HIV, there is...not one person who does it all. And for you to be able to get insight into a little bit of what everyone involved in a case does was helpful. Just an all-around picture... Jarred reiterated this sentiment, stating that, “It was really nice to see [the application of ideas] instead of just reading about it and kind of hearing about it. It was good to kind of see it all happen.” A few were more succinct, like Tanisha, who simply said, “...[Case Itl] allowed you to see the bigger picture.” Turnedooff science. Some students said that their experience had soured them on specific aspects of science (n = 27, 2.3%; see Table 7). It is important to note though that none of these students expressed a desire to leave the biological sciences as a result of this experience, but rather an intention to re- direct their focus into areas more compatible with their personalities, such as medical counseling. Three students made comments that their experience with Case It! had confirmed to them their dislike of laboratory settings, tests, and procedures. This attitude is demonstrated by Heather who said, “It kind of 88 reinforced that I know I don’t want to do research; as it’s kind of interesting, but I don’t necessarily like doing it.” A few students (n = 11) made comments about being uncomfortable with, or disliking the use of, computers. Generally these comments were not associated with specific biological principles of procedures, but fell more into the category of technophobia, as illustrated by Rachel’s comment that, “...I wasn’t too fond of having to work on computers ‘cause they are not my thing.” Stigma/Denial/lllisconceptions. Few health subjects are as culturally and socially charged, or as surrounded by misconceptions, as HIV/AIDS. As such, it is not surprising that some student comments (n = 26, 2.2%; see Table 7) were coded as stigma/denial and misconceptions. In general, comments were in reference to (a) social stigma associated HIV, (b) personal denial that infection could be a real possibility for collegiate students, or (c) myths and misconceptions associated with HIV infection and transmission. Comments falling into the first grouping are exemplified by Tom who said, “Right now it doesn’t really affect me. I don’t know anyone with AIDS—at least no one’s said anything about it...” Aaron, however, summed up many students’ attitudes towards HIV while demonstrating he may be the exception to the rule when he said, “It can’t happen to me. Its never going to happen to anyone I know, so why should I worry about it? I mean, that’s the mentality.” Kathy’s comment that, “. . .[Case It! was] not personally personally [sic] [relevant], like ‘cause I don’t—at least I don’t think I know—anybody with HIV or 89 AIDS...” is a good example the second group, personal denial. Her attitude is one that was repeated frequently where students acknowledged that HIV was in fact a problem, but not one that they would ever realistically have to deal with, as echoed by Greg when he said, “...it was kind of interesting to learn about where it [HIV] is now, but it really didn’t have anything to do with me—has nothing to do with my future. . Some students however recognized the fallacy of this position, like Emma who said, “...I know we say that it’s not relevant to us, but I mean, you could get it. You never know. You know, it could happen to us, and it’s nice to know ‘cause now we know more about it...” The third grouping of comments referencing myths and misconceptions were less common than one might have expected; however, the majority of students in question (~63%) were senior biology majors who probably have had a greater exposure to information about HIV than the average citizen. The most common myth that was cited by students was that HIV is an African problem, and not really an issue in the industrialized world. This is evidenced by Jui-Fu’s comment that, “I only know that there’s a lot of AIDS cases in Africa...” and Lisa’s statement that, I know that HIV is something that we should all be concerned about, and it is a world problem but, as far as being on my radar, it’s probably one of the most minor things that I would think about... Need-to-know science. Prior to conducting the focus group interviews, I expected a large number of students to make comments associated with Need- to-Know science, which is information or knowledge gained in response to 90 specific or potential problems in a student’s life. In fact, only 20 student comments were coded into the subcategory (1.7%; see Table 7). It surprised me that so many of the students interviewed contextualized their experiences as functional science, which applied to future vocational or professional need, rather than in terms of personal application as had been noted in a previous study. Comments coded here either referenced immediate applicability, or potential future need. Examples of the former include Zoe’s statement that, “At first I wasn’t real happy about doing it, but during the semester someone in my life has been diagnosed with HIV, so it’s helped me understand the disease a lot,” and Leah’s acknowledgement that, “. . .[Case Itl] just makes me more aware of what I should and shouldn’t be doing.” Ashley illustrated future relevance when she said, “...I also think it helps in personal life if it’s your friend or loved one that’s going to have to deal with this disease...” as does Teresa’s comment that, . .if you did have to go get tested, you’d be one step ahead of the average person...” Level of application. Many students at both the majors and introductory levels indicated that they felt Case It! had the potential to be a factor in helping students decide on biology or science as a major—especially if presented at lower level, such as high school and introductory courses. This perspective was illustrated by Kira, who said that, ...Potentially [this is the kind of experience that might lead people to be biology majors]. Maybe for like those Biology 150 classes that are just going through it right now... They’re in their first or second year of college, so maybe it’d be more useful doing it at that stage... 91 Nicole said that the earlier students were exposed to projects like Case It! the better, saying, “If Case It! was brought to high school students, that would get them more interested in science, and maybe more interested in taking science as a major in college. Because that’s what really got me interested in my major...” Overview of case studies B-F The next five case studies are class-specific investigations of the research questions. Included are case studies of all 5 classes tested at 4 study sites. Two classes at the University of Wisconsin-River Falls were tested, BIOL 150 an introductory biology course for majors, and BIOL 345 a majors level immunology course. BIOL 401 at North Carolina State A&T was a majors level molecular biology course, while MEDT 4531 at Interamerican University of Puerto Rico (Metropolitan Campus) was a majors and post-baccalaureate clinical immunology course, and BIOL 4600 at Interamerican University of Puerto Rico (San German Campus) was a majors level histology course. Case study B: University of Wisconsin-River Falls, BIOL 150 (UWRF-NM) Overview. Of the 50 students enrolled in BIOL 150 at the UWRF in spring semester 2009, 43 chose to participate in this study (86%). As Table 2 shows, participating students reflected the norms of the class in terms of class standing and gender. It is important to note that, although this is a major’s level course, roughly 58% of all students were non-majors, primarily from the agricultural sciences. The 5 most popular career choices were agriculture, other fields, 92 conservation, teaching, and veterinary medicine, and reveal this trend toward non-STEM fields. Student performance. As Figure 5 shows, students at UWRF-NM significantly improved their performance from the pre- to post-test (t(41)=4.265, ps.001). As with the overall dataset, time was the only significant factor influencing performance. An item analysis of questions revealed that students significantly improved on only questions 1 and 6 (see Table 8). Again, ceiling effects may have limited results for question 2 as post-test means approach the maximum possible (M=3.381, max=4). Similar to overall results, participants at UWRF-NM significantly improved their confidence on all 6 test items, even when there was no statistical improvement in score (see Table 8). Student intentions to persist. Students at UWRF-NM made more comments referencing persistence proxies than the overall average, accounting for a little more than 51% (n = 208) of all statements (see Table 9). Descriptive statistics indicate that while students generally agreed that Case Itl reinforced their desires to be science majors (M=2.801, 1-5 Likert scale), they had slightly lower opinions than overall, cross-site average (M=3.304, 1-5 Likert scale). Nominally fewer students in this introductory class said the project was relevant to their lives (56%) compared to the cross-site mean of 59% for all students sampled, indicating a degree of universal relevancy. Of the 43 participating students from BIOL 150, 77% agreed that the Case Itl project would help them with either future classes or careers, a full 10% lower than the cross-site average, 93 5.0 w 0 .0 28:29.0 .. 8.0 w a a 58:65 . .082 :mmoé 3N; 5mm 0mm; m 9.300: EEEBEBB :0 5:50.925 m0 85.0 RN; 08.0 03.0 m 0.20 3 5000 9:00:28 :_ 9.300: :0 5:00:03. :mmmd mm 2 mead «mam m 3:080 9:00:38 :_ $500: :0 5:00:03 v0 :mmoé vmmd vad mmvd m Em 52003 .m> <0_._m. .0 000000500 00 000.0- 000.0 000.0 000.0 0 0.300. 0.0 50.002. .0 0000.05.02. 00 2.0000- 000.0 03.0 000.0 0 0.500. .030 .0 0000.00.20. 5 :005. 4 mm :00E :00E .0000 -0000. -0.n. .05 .«00 u 0. 00500 .00. 0.0.05 0......ka 000 00.0 0.0... 000015000000: ..0 0.00000: 05 .0 003000 0. -03 500 .00. E0E000000 05500. :0 000000000 000 0000050000 E0050 000:. 0.. 0000000 .2 0.90... 101 Again, ceiling effects may have limited results for question 2 as both pre- and post-test means approach the maximum possible. Similar to overall results, participants at UWRF-M significantly improved their confidence on all 6 test items at ps0.01 level, even when they performed significantly worse on a question or did not improve at all (see Table 10). Student intentions to persist. The same themes and codes described in the previous section were applied to the focus group interviews and student surveys of pupils at UWRF-M. The overall picture presented was an intention to persevere in program. Comments related either directly to student persistence or persistence proxies accounted for just over 42% (n = 216) of all statements (see Table 11), roughly 5% fewer than the aggregate average (see Table 7). Students at UWRF-M had a slightly higher level of agreement that Case It! reinforced their desires to be science majors than the average (M=3.405 vs. M=3.304). Interestingly, more students in BIOL 345 (89% vs. 87% aggregate) thought Case It! would be valuable to them in future classes or careers, but 13% fewer felt that it was personally relevant to their lives (46% vs. 59%). Students like Sid said that, “...[Case Itl] definitely reinforced my desire to go into bioinformatics,” while others like Greg said, “...it’s [the content knowledge] going to stick now. After the end of the semester, I’m still going to be able to remember this.” As Table 11 shows, student comments about persistence from UWRF-M reflect the same order as aggregate results, with slightly lower percentages. 102 Table 1 1. Categories of comments and their instances for the University of Wisconsin— Fiiver Falls BIOL 345 (U WRF-M) majors level course (n =509) Category n % Intentions to persist 216 42.44 Instructional quality: 101 19.84 Functional science: 44 8.64 Role-playing: 41 8.06 Self-efficacy: 24 4.72 Community/Integrated into science: 6 1.12 Beliefs: 293 57.56 Diagnostic testing process: 74 14.54 Science content: 63 12.38 Learning: 59 1 1.59 Multiple perspectives: 34 6.68 Communication: 34 6.68 Stigma/Denial/Misconceptions: 1 5 2.95 Turned off science: 10 1.96 Need-to-know: 4 0.79 Note: Percentage calculated by [n(category) / total n] 103 Clearly the way material was presented in Case Itl mattered to students, as the greatest number of comments at UWRF-M were about instructional quality (n = 101; see Table 11), and were more than double the next category, functional science (n = 44; see Table 11). Students reported that they appreciated the novel way in which Case Itl introduced them to both material and procedures, and the speed at which they were able to conduct genetic tests. Maggie said, “...you see how [HIV] actually affects real people...And you see all sorts of different aspects, like they show you a little video,” while Lisa said that, “...by reading the case studies, it made it more applicable to me...reading about people who actually... like this is their story, this is what happened to them... made it more real to me.” Both Lisa and Olivia were positive about using the Case Itl environment to conduct complex genetic assays when they said, “...doing [tests] on Case Itl, it was giving us the results that it was supposed to be giving us,” and “[With Case Itl] you actually get results, whereas our lab went horribly wrong—we didn’t get very good results on our Western Blots.” Other students like Sid were more general in their praise stating, “I really liked it. I kind of felt like I was House curing people,” referring to the popular television series. Students at UWRF-M also appreciated the learning environment’s ability to present them with “real-world” situations in which they could both apply theoretical knowledge and envision themselves in a professional context. Given that this was an upper division course populated primarily by seniors (see Table 2) who expect to be shortly on the job market, it is unsurprising that many 104 students related this experience to needed occupational skills or translating theory to application. Comments related to these feelings were coded as either “functional science” or “role-playing” depending on the context, and aggregately accounted for approximately 16.7% of all student statements (see Table 11). Rachael wanted to be a nurse and said, “...I thought that [role-playing] gave me a lot more opportunity to...practice [compassionl—like being the counselor and stuff like that. So, I think that was probably the coolest part for me.” This opportunity to practice professional skills was echoed by Maggie when she said, “...it was...really neat to do the [role-playing] portion of [Case lt!]—we don’t really get to do that a great deal in any other BIO courses, so that was also great.” Autumn said, “I definitely think [Case Itl] helped prepare me for a career,” and Neil said that, “. . .it’s like some practice if you will for future life, kind of a real-life situation.” Student beliefs. Roughly 92% of participants at UWRF-M thought that Case It! was a good learning experience for them, which was slightly lower than both UWRF-NM and the aggregate average, but not significantly so. It is interesting to note that only 46% of students said the project was relevant to their personal lives as compared to 56% with the introductory students at UWRF-NM and 59% of students overall. This may be linked to the relatively large number of comments made by students in this class related to stigma, denial, or misconceptions about HIV (n=15; see Table 11). Roughly 55.6% of all comments coded into this category were made by students at UWRF-M (see 105 Tables 7 and 11). Lisa’s comment seemed to sum up many students’ feelings about the project’s relevancy, I know that HIV is something that we should all be concerned about, and it is a world problem, but as far as being on my radar, it’s probably one of the most minor things that I would think about... Aaron said flat out that HIV was not relevant to him, but amended that statement when he said, “I suppose if you include career in there, maybe...but personally it [HIV] isn’t [relevant].” Other students like Katie chose to speak for the group with her denial of relevancy when she said, I think its hard for us to find it relevant to our lives, just because none of us are affected personally by AIDS or HIV. So, its hard for us to relate to this project because none of us have [it]... A few students at UWRF-M like Emma did acknowledge that while HIV and AIDS might be relevant, there were social pressures to deny this saying, I know we say that its not relevant to us, but I mean, you could get it. You never know...lt could happen to us, and its nice to know because now we know more about it so if someone we knew or we got it (God forbid!)...we know what to do. Despite over half the class feeling that Case It! lacked personal relevance, participants at UWRF-M said the project increased their confidence in their knowledge of HIV (M=3.660), and increased their interest in HIV (t(36)=5.097, ps0.001). Figure 8 shows students in BIOL 345 most valued the same two components of Case It! as the overall average, the virtual lab and conferencing. Pupils at UWRF-M made more comments about their beliefs (57.6%; see Table 11) than either the average (52.9%; see Table 7) or UWRF-NM (48.6%; see Table 9). While they made comments in all 8 categories, the majority of 106 observations (~67%) were about diagnostic testing, science content, and learning (see Table 11). Several students at UWRF-M found the Case Itl project’s ability to link disparate ideas in biology and science together (n=34), and its facilitation of communication techniques (n=34) especially useful (see Table 11 ). Noah said that Case It! helped him make connections because it, “...[puts you] in that perspective outside of learning the material, [where] you definitely have to learn what the people are going through.” Ashley said that, . [Case Itl] kind of helped me think outside the box—it made me think of different things, different people are coming from different backgrounds, and classes that they‘ve taken, so they ask different questions, and you're like, "Hey! Wait a minute...l didn't really think about that over there." This idea was seconded by Greg when he said, “...it helped you think outside of how and what you would normally think.” The opportunity to practice communicating with laymen appeared to be especially appreciated by those students expecting to pursue careers in the health sciences. Autumn said Case It! helped her by, “...being able to relate to your patients, and think on their Ievel...l think sometimes people just get caught up thinking, ‘Oh well this sounds more scientific...’ but you have to think about what’s going to be more understandable.” This was echoed by both Eli and Catherine who said, “...you have to know how to. . .explain terms that people might not understand so that you can actually get your point across and tell them what they need to know,” and “I definitely felt the most beneficial part was understanding the results, and being able to put them into words that a patient can understand.” Tom was 107 perhaps more practical when he said, “...you actually had to explain it, so you really had to know it.” Level of application. Several students at UWRF-M suggested that Case It! could significantly impact STEM program entrance and retention if offered at lower levels, such as high school and introductory courses. Kelly said that, “..if Case It! was brought to high school students, or intro classes, that would get them more interested in science,” and Emma said that, “...this could definitely affect freshmen looking to pick a major.” These comments echoed those of the beginning students in BIOL 150 at UWRF-NM described above. Overall, while most participants at UWRF-M said that the project had reinforced their desires to be science majors, they were already so invested in their majors in terms of time and money, almost nothing could convince them to switch. Case study D: North Carolina State A&T University, BIOL 401 (NCA&T) Overview. The class at NCA&T represented the greatest sampling deviance. Of the 20 students enrolled in BIOL 401, only 6 chose to participate in this study (30%), and all were women, whereas in the overall class 20% of students were men. Participants did however reflect the class averages for G.P.A. and class standing (see Table 2). The five most popular career choices were physician, other fields, research, teaching, and other health care. Student performance. Unlike the previous two classes and the overall average, students at NCA&T did not demonstrate any significant performance improvement from the pre- to the post-test (t(4)=1.500; see Figure 5); however, 108 this is likely due to the low number of participants that took both exams (n = 5) as gain scores for this class are slightly higher than those recorded for both UWRF- NM and UWRF-M (see Figure 6). An item analysis revealed that there was very little significant movement for students at NCA&T in terms of either question performance or question confidence (see Table 12). The only question to show significant improvement was item 5, and only question 1 showed a significant increase in confidence (see Table 12). Results from this class, however, must be carefully examined for two reasons: (a) ceiling effects definitely influenced results for question 2 as scores for both the pre- and post-tests were just 0.2 off the maximum possible, and probably influences confidence scores on questions 1, 2, and 4 as those means also approach the maximum value (see Table 12); and (b) the low number of participants reduces the probability that even a large difference would be statistically significant. Student intentions to persist. A greater percentage of student comments from NCA&T referenced persistence proxies (54.2%; see Table 13) than the overall average (47.2%; see Table 7). All (100%) of the participating students agreed that Case It! would help them with future classes or careers, and they had a higher level of agreement that the project reinforced their desires to be science majors than the average (M=4.167). Students at NCA&T also said that the project was more relevant to their lives (66.7%) than the average (59%), UWRF-NM (56%), or UWRF-M (46%). Students like Leah said the project, “. . .helps you when you think about a career, and what you want to do,” and Alice 109 .00 00 .0 2000.000 .. 000 w 0 .0 .000_._..0_0 .. 0.0... 000.. R00 000.0 000.0 0 0....00. 000050.50... .0 :0..0.0.0.0.:. 00 00.0.0 . 80... 000.0 0....0 0.00 o 0 .0000 05.00500 5 0....00. .0 5:00:00... 00 0000 0000 00.0... 000... 0 0.00.00 05.00500 c. 0.300. .0 00:00:00.. 00 000.. 000.. 000... 000.0 0 Em :.0.00>> .0> <0...m. .0 50.00500 00 00.0.0 000... 000... 000... 0 0....00. Em :.0.00>> .0 5:00.005 00 .08.. 000.0 000... 000.0 0 0....00. .0.ij .0 :0..0.0.0.0.:. .0 00:05.50 4 mm 505 :00... .0000 -.000 0.0 0.0 000.0 00 ... 000.. 000.. 0 0....00. 05.05.0503 .0 :0..0.0.0.0.:. 00 .000. . . 000.0 000. . 0....0 00¢ 0 .0 .0000 05.00500 5 0500. .0 5:00:00... 00 000.0- 0000 000.. 000.0 0 0.00.00 0500.500 ... 0:000. .0 00000.00... 00 000.0 000.0 000... 000.0 0 Em :.0.00>> .0> <03m .0 50:00:50 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 .0 0....00. Em :.0.00>> .0 5:00.005 00 000.. 000.. 000.0 000.0 .0 0500. <0...m. .0 5:00.005 .0 :00... .0 mm :00E c005 .0000 -.000 0.0 0.0 110 .0" 0. 00.000 .00. 0.0.0:. n 0.02. .00 00.0 . 0.. 00.0.00 .502 .0 002000 0. -05 50... .00. 505000000 0:..500. :0 00:00.58 0:0 00:05.0...00 5000.0 :005 :. 0005.5 .3. 0.20-r Table 13. Categories of comments and their instances for North Carolina A& T University BIOL 401 (NCA&T) majors level course (n =120) Category n % Intentions to persist 64 53.33 Instructional quality: 27 22.50 Role-playing: 1 8 1 5.00 Functional science: 1 1 9.17 Self-efficacy: 5 4.1 7 Community/Integrated into science: 5 4.17 Beliefs: 54 45.00 Diagnostic testing process: 20 16.67 Communication: 9 7.50 Science content: 8 6.67 Learning: 8 6.67 Multiple perspectives: 3 2.50 Need-to-know: 3 2.50 StigmalDenial/Misconceptions: 2 1 .67 Turned off science: 1 0.83 Note: Percentage calculated by [n(category) / total n] 111 said, “...it’s given me more experience than I would have gotten with my other classes, and it helped reinforce why I want to go to medical school.” As Table 13 shows, students at NCA&T valued the overall instructional quality of the project roughly equally to the average (see Table 7), but placed a greater importance on role-playing (15.0%). Students said that role-playing afforded them numerous experiences they had not had access to previously in their education, such as the chance to act as a teacher, to practice asking and answering questions in a real-world environment, and the opportunity to act as professionals conferencing with peers. Winnie said, “...the role-playing was fun for me...it was good to just sit down and think about it and ask more questions.” Alice said the, “...conferencing was very important because [the other students] asked me questions that I hadn’t, even thought about...so that helped me learn about different—other—aspects of HIV that I wouldn’t have looked into before then.” She followed up by saying that she, “...had to go back and get extra information from [other resources] because the questions that I got asked—well, they probably weren’t in the textbook.” Teresa spoke to the idea of bi-directional learning as a product of role-playing when she said, You’re both learning from each other because one of us is role-playing as one person, and the other one is role-playing as the other, so you were supposed to be a doctor and a family member and you would be equally learning the same from each other. This perspective was backed up by Kathy, who liked the role-playing aspect of Case lt! because, “...its like I’m learning from them [the ‘patients’] and they’re learning from me.” 112 Some students (n = 5; see Table 13) at NCA&T liked that the project made them feel like they were an active member of, or professionally integrated into, the scientific community. While not accounting for a large percentage of comments, students in BIOL 401 did mention it more than twice as often (4.2%; see Table 13) as the overall average (~2.0%; see Table 7). These statements are important because they indicate that the Case It! project has the potential to help students transition from thinking of themselves as passive learners to active and capable agents of inquiry. Tanisha said that, “...[Case Itl] made it more like you were actually a doctor,” and “I think the cases linked to [the virtual labs] made you feel like you were actually working with somebody—doing someone good.” Leah spoke about bridging the gap between her social and professional communities when she said, “...HIV affects African-American females the most now I believe in our age group, so [Case Itl] really made me feel like I could do something to help. You know, make a difference.” Alice’s commented simply that, “. . .it felt good to think of myself as a doctor.” Student beliefs. All (100%) of participants at NCA&T thought that Case ltl was a good learning experience for them, which was slightly higher than the aggregate average of 95%. The majority of students (67%) found the project to be relevant to their lives, and as a group NCA&T students were very confident in their knowledge of HIV (M=4.500). Participants also reported a significant increase in interest from before to after Case Itl (t(5)=3.953, ps0.05). Figure 8 shows that students at NCA&T agreed with the overall average that the virtual lab 113 was the most important and useful component of Case Itl; however, they said the next most useful aspect was creating online webposters. They made fewer comments about their beliefs (45%; see Table 13) than the aggregate average (52.9%; see Table 7) or students at UWRF-NM (48.6%; see Table 9) or UWRF-M (57.6%; see Table 11). Of the 54 comments made about beliefs, over 83% were in four categories (a) diagnostic testing; (b) communication; (c) science content; and (d) learning. As with both UWRF-NM and UWRF-M, students at NCA&T liked the fact that Case It! allowed them to quickly and accurately conduct complex genetic tests without the huge investment of time that a wet lab would require. Several students noted that because of the speed at which Case It! runs tests, mistakes were learning experiences rather than frustrating wastes of time. This perspective is typified by Tanisha who said, ...I liked the lab because...in the lab [Case Itl], plenty of times I messed up on the loading [DNA samples] part, but in the lab [wet lab] if you do that, its going to be hours before you can redo the test. Alice immediately followed with the comment that one could, “...just press ‘clear’...” and start again without hours of prep work. Other students appreciated the range of tests and information available, like Leah who said, “...I thought it was interesting that we could use the bioinformatics [tools] and show who you [sic] got the disease from, and who that person actually got the disease from.” Tanisha liked that she could engage in discovery learning telling me that, “. ..I didn’t know that you can actually track [viral loads], like over months, and see 114 how the virus has increased." Alice thought, “...[Case ltI] was a good simulation of actually using infectious diseases, because we'd probably never be able to work with these sorts of infectious diseases othenlvise.” A number of comments referenced specific science content that the project helped students learn (n = 8; see Table 13). Some students liked that Case It! allowed them to practice running and reading several types of genetic tests, such as Teresa who said, “...sometimes interpreting tests is hard, but [Case Itl] makes it easy,” and Tanisha who said, “...it allows you to see the process that someone actually goes through to get the results.” Other students like Kathy enjoyed the fact that the project was not formulaic with locked in results stating, “I thought it was cool that you could get a false positive with [Case Itl] just like in real life...” Transmission and treatment of HIV were the subject of other comments. Leah said, “...it was interesting to know that just because some of the cases were in the US. and some were in Africa, they all contracted [HIV] the same way.” Teresa said, “...it was interesting to see how long it takes for the medicines to start working against the virus.” A few students (n = 3; see Table 13) at NCA&T related the information they were learning either to current or potential personal use in the category “need-to-know science.” Leah said that, [Case Itl] informed me more, and it is going to make me be more careful that I was being before because of all the different cases and the different background of each person, and how they actually contracted the virus. It just made me more aware of what I should and shouldn't be doing. 115 Teresa talked about more specific application of her knowledge when she said, “...if you did have to get tested, you’d be one step ahead of the average person,” while Winnie simply said, “...you could get it. You just never know, so I’m glad I did [Case ltl].” Level of application. As with UWRF-M, students at NCA&T reported that the project did reinforce their desires to be scientists, but that they were unlikely to have switched programs because they were so heavily invested. They did think though that Case It! had the potential to positively influence entrance and retention of students into STEM fields if offered earlier. Case study E: Interamerican University of Puerto Rico, MetrOpolitan Campus, MEDT 4531 (lUPR—M) Overview. Only 23% (n = 6) of the students enrolled in MEDT 4531 agreed to be part of this study. Participation was affected by the timing of testing and interviews, which had to be scheduled after 4:30pm on a day when students had taken a final exam for another course. As Table 2 shows, study participants closely mirrored overall averages for their class in every way except for the ratio of men to women. Men and women were equally represented in the focus group interviews, but men accounted for only about 17% of the overall class. All students in MEDT 4531 were either seniors or post-baccalaureate. The five most popular career choices for lUPR-M were medical technology, other healthcare, research, biotechnology, and physician. 116 Student performance. Like NCA&T, students at lUPR-M did not demonstrate any significant performance improvement from the pre- to the post- test (t(4)=1.677; see Figure 5); however, this is again likely due to the low number of participants who took both the pre- and post-tests (n = 5). Figure 6 shows that students at lUPFl-M in fact posted the largest gain scores for all five classes tested, but not significantly so. An item analysis of questions showed that there was significant improvement only on question 4 (see Table 14). Just like every other previous site and the overall average, it appears that ceiling effects may have influenced gain scores on question 2 where both pre- and post- test scores approach the maximum value. Student confidence in their answers at lUPR-M improved significantly on questions 1-4 at the ps0.05 level or better (see Table 14), which is similar to both the overall averages, and results from UWRF- NM and UWRF-M. As with results from NCA&T, the small sample size requires that all results be carefully assessed. Student intentions to persist. Participants at lUPR-M only mentioned four of the five categories identified as persistence proxies, with roughly 61.5% of all their comments falling into these groupings (see Table 15). As with the other sites, student comments from IUPFl-M present the impression of intending to persist. As with NCA&T, all of the students at lUPR-M agreed that the Case It! project would help them with future classes and careers. These students also had a higher opinion that the project reinforced their desires to be science majors than the overall average (M=4.000 vs. M=3.304). Unlike all other sites, all 117 55 v. a a 285:3 .. mod w a a .58:ng . .682 com; Nona oom.v coed m 9.3.0.9 moszhohEoE So co_§m§2c_ m0 mm 2 . mama. nopd 230 use r m Scam 958.58 E $.89 u_o :o_fio__aa< mo .83 «85 come 83 m 9:23 8:858 s 958. co 85232 3 Rmod 0 Ed coed «mod m SE 5933 .m> <95. 5 55.5950 m0 wand was; «Re mvmd m $.52 5_m c5525 5 5.5.2225 «G :50; 534 no EV oomN m $.52 > 5 5.5.2225 «0 ommd mmoé omnd coma v $.52 (. :0: 0%)) ) ”(XXNXX ( () ._ FKQXX )(XX 0‘ 'QQ (,1 E i‘ +', @340 1. How would you interpret these results? Check the appropriate box for each test sam le: HIV positive HIV negative Indeterminate Kanya, first test (before mane” Kanya, second test Sunan Baby How confident are you n your answer? 1 2 3 4 5 l = very uncertain and 5 = very confident. 155 A Western Blot was run as a follow-up after each of the above tests. A composite of the Western blot results is shown below: 5 E ‘; g u | . '- 1 -‘ I t 5 ; 1 _. 2% II I? l I I ‘ '. I 3‘ I I I? 3 ; I § 3 .- 4 ? t l r l w '4 0‘ (fl -1 m — “ i 10% polyacr. 1.Kanyai 2. KanyaZ mtirm I 60 4. Baby 5: nogoontrol cw 3 control B 2. Based on both the ELISA and western blot test results, put checkmarks in the table below to indicate the HIV status of each person. HIV positive HIV negative Indeterminate Kanya, first test (before pregnancy) Kanya, second test Sunan Baby How confident are you in your answer? 1 2 3 4 5 Very uncertain Very Confident 156 3. Why run both an ELISA and a Western Blot to test for HIV? Put checkmarks in the table below indicating to which test each feature applies. Feature of test Western Blot ELISA Both Neither WB and ELISA A first screening test for HIV To isolate a specific HIV gene Tests for HIV antibodies The bands between Ribosomes and DNA The more definitive test for HIV Amount of virus in blood HIV proteins separated by size on gel How confident are you in your answer? 1 2 3 4 5 Very uncertain Very Confident 157 For these next 2 questions, put yourself in the role of an HIV counselor in Thailand. How would you explain the test results? What advice would you give them? Include social, medical and ethical advice. Do not try to write complete sentences; just list the main points you would include. 4. What would you say to Kanya and Sunan about their test results? How confident are you in your answer? 1 2 3 4 5 Very uncertain Very Confident 5. What would you say to Kanya and Sunan about the baby's test results? How confident are you in your answer? 1 2 3 4 5 Very uncertain Very Confident 158 6. Study the tree below that shows HIV sequence comparisons for these individuals: Kanya, Sunan, their baby, Kanya’s one night stand, the blood donor from a transfusion Sunan had, Sunan’s former partner, and HIV sequences from India, Nigeria, and Vietnam. How do you interpret these results? What would you tell Kanya and Sunan about how they contracted the HIV virus? I L - Vietnam .~ India Nigeria ~ 15mm: ... Blood donor f.”— Kanva L— Babv H‘" 1! Local control How confident are you in your answer? 1 2 Very uncertain 159 J Kanva's one-night stand 1m .. Sunan‘s formcroartner . Local control - Local control 4 5 Very Confident Appendix B Student name: Case It! Student Survey Please answer the following questions briefly. You will have the opportunity to expand upon your responses during the focus group interview. We appreciate your participation and cooperation in improving the Case It! experience. 1. What do you plan to do in your future career? Please check on or more... Physician Other healthcare Teacher Food science Biotechnology Research Conservation Forestry Medical technology Forensics Veterinary Other (please list) medicine Nurse Agriculture 2. Do you think your experiences with Case It have helped prepare you for future classes or a future career? Please explain. YES NO 3. My experiences with Case It! have reinforced my desire to continue to be a science major I (not at all) 2 3 (somewhat) Please explain your answer. 5 (quite a lot) 4. How interested were you in your case topic before starting the project? 1 (not at all) 2 3 (somewhat) 4 5 (quite a lot) How interested were you in your case topic after completing the project? 1 (not at all) Please explain. 2 3 (somewhat) 160 4 5 (quite a lot) . Was this project relevant to your life? ‘ YES NO Please explain. . How confident are you in your knowledge of the disease you studied for your case? 1 2 3 4 5 (not at all) (somewhat) (quite a lot) Please explain your answer. . Please rank the 2 most valuable learning components of Case It! for you. Virtual lab Bioinformatics Webposter creation Role-playing Conferencing Internet research Please explain. . How useful did you find the role-playing aspect of Case It! in helping you learn the material? 1 2 3 4 5 (not at all) (somewhat) (quite a lot) Please explain your answer. . Please rate your experience with role-playing as a family member 1 2 3 4 5 (disliked it) (neutral) (enjoyed it) Please rate your experience with role-playing as an HIV councilor. 1 2 3 4 5 (disliked it) (neutral) (enjoyed it) Please rate your experience with role-playing as a bioinformatics researcher l 2 3 4 5 (disliked it) (neutral) (enjoyed it) Please rate how much you learned from role-playing 1 2 3 4 5 (nothing) (neutral) (quite a bit) Please elaborate on your answers. 161 10. Was this project a good learning experience for you? Please explain. 11. Please share any additional thoughts about Case It. 162 YES NO Appendix C 10. FGI QUESTIONS Do you think your experiences with Case It! have helped prepare you for future classes or a future career? How so? Has your experience with Case It! affected your desire to continue to be a science major? In what ways? Did participating in Case It! increase your interest in the topic? Was this project relevant to your life? How? How confident are you in your knowledge of the disease you studied for your case? Why? What was the most valuable component of Case It! to you? How useful did you find the role-playing aspect of Case It! in helping you learn the material? ’ Did you enjoy the role-playing? Why or why not? Was this project a good Ieaming experience for you? How did it help you learn science? Do you have any additional thoughts or comments about case it! that you’d like to share? 163 Appendix D Participant Consent Form This study investigates the impact of multimedia case-based learning on university students' perceptions, understandings, and confidence in their knowledge of infectious diseases and genetic disorders using the Case-It! software. The cases will consist of narratives regarding individuals with infectious diseases and genetic disorders. As part of the Case-It! multimedia environment, students will be involved in using an simulations, creating and presenting web posters, and role playing, all as part of their coursework. A pre- and post— assessment with information from the simulation will be used to gather information about students’ knowledge about infectious diseases and genetic disorders as well as their ability to interpret data from ELISA, Southern and Western-blot, and bioinformatics simulations. Participants will be interviewed regarding their perceptions about cases after they complete their work. Your participation in the study will consist of giving us permission to use your coursework. Your permission to use your coursework would include items such as the web page you create with your results from the Case-It! simulation, the electronic transcripts from computer conferences you engage in about the web page, and written responses to in-class assignments on molecular diagnostic testing before and after the use of the Case-It! software. We also have a short survey we would like you to complete about your knowledge and perceptions of infectious diseases that will be completed in class, and we will also invite you to be part of a focus-group interview. Please note that we will not use student names or other identifying information in any reports of this research. All data will be treated with strict confidence and your name will not be used in any report of the research findings. Your responses to questions are confidential (not anonymous). Your privacy will be protected to the maximum extent allowable by law. If you would want to know the results of the study (within these restrictions) you should leave your name with us. Your decision to participate or not participate in the research will have no effect on your grade or any future recommendation your instructor may make. Participation is voluntary. You have complete freedom to discontinue the study at any time without penalty. You have the freedom to not respond to certain items. If at any point you feel any discomfort with the materials or questions please do not hesitate to stop us. If you have any questions about this study feel free to contact: Bjorn Wolter Dr. Mary Lundeberg 517.507.5896 517.353.5091 bwolter@msu.edu mlunde@m su.edu If you have any questions or concerns about your role and rights as a research participant, or would like to register a complaint about this research study, you may contact, anonymously if you wish, Michigan State University Human Research Protection Program at 517-355-2180, FAX 517-432-4503, or e-mail irba'r‘rmsuedu, or regular mail at: 202 Olds Hall, MSU, East Lansing, MI 48824. Your signature below indicates your voluntary agreement to participate in this study. Name: (printed) Signature: Date: I agree to be video-recorded in this study if I consent to an interview. Signature Date: 164 Appendix E Rubric for Performance HIV Assessment (Pre/Post - 26 pts) (Put 1 or 0 for each person) 1. Give the HIV status of each person based on the ELISA Kanyal (First Test): HIV Negative Kanya2 (Second Test): HIV Positive Sunan HIV Positive Baby Indeterminate Correct Interpretation = 1 point each; Incorrect = 0; Total possible = 4 Student’s score = Confidence = . Give the HIV status of each person based on both the ELISA & Western Blot Kanya] (First Test): HIV Negative Kanya2 (Second Test): HIV Positive Sunan HIV Positive Baby Indeterminate Correct Interpretation = 1 point each ; Incorrect = 0; Total possible = 4 Student’s score = Confidence = Feature of test Western Blot ELISA Both WB & ELISA Neither A first screening test for HIV X To isolate a specific HIV gene Tests for HIV antibodies The bands between Ribosomes and DNA The more definitive test for HIV Amount of virus in blood HIV proteins separated by size on gel Total points possible = 7 Student’s score = Confidence = 165 4. HIV Counseling Model advice for 4 pts This questions aims to examine students understanding of the medical as well as ethical implications of giving advice to people who have HIV. For 4points — Model response includes information about all three family members and information regarding medical treatment, ethical implications, and resources to family For 3 points — Model response includes information about 2 family members and either a medical and/or ethical advice/resources to family. For 2 points — Model response includes 1 family member and either medical and/or ethical advice/resources to family For 1 point - model response is a general explanation of medical treatment and ethical advice For 0 points — No advice, no explanation, general lack of understanding Model Response: For both Kanya and Sunan, their results are positive which means they have the Human Immunodeficiency Virus (HIV), which causes AIDS. This virus is transmitted through contact with body fluids. With HIV, almost all infected people eventually develop disease symptoms, but it may take several years. However there are medications that can help reduce virus multiplication in the body and hence can prolong your life. It is advisable that they immediately see the doctor and get to know what treatment options are available. There is also need for the couple to stay healthy by eating foods that are rich in nutrients in order to boost their immunity and to exercise regularly. They should also avoid sharing needles and practice safe sex to avoid infecting others and to avoid re-infections. Score: Confidence : 5. HIV Counseling Model advice for 4pts. This questions aims to examine students understanding of the medical as well as ethical implications of giving advice to people who have HIV. For 4points - Model response includes information about all three family members and information regarding medical treatment, ethical implications, and resources to family For 3 points — Model response includes information about 2 family members and either a medical and/or ethical advice/resources to family. For 2 points — Model response includes 1 family member and either medical and/or ethical advice/resources to family For 1 point — model response is a general explanation of medical treatment and ethical advice For 0 points — No advice, no explanation, general lack of understanding 166 Model Response: The Baby’s results are indeterminate, which means they are not conclusive. This may be due to the fact that baby still has antibodies from the mother. Baby needs to be tested again in order to determine its status. For now there is need to ensure that baby 5 not infected through breast-milk or the parents’ body fluids. 6. Bioinformatic interpretation (3 Pts) This question is intended to determine how well students can interpret a bioinformatics tree. How do you interpret these results? The infection source is the blood donor (1 pt) What would you tell Kanya and Sunan about how they contracted the HIV virus? For 2 points — includes an explanation of the infection source, how infection occurred, and what this means for them. For 1 point — simple cites the infection source without explanation. For 0 points — no explanation, general lack of understanding. Score: Confidence : 167 References References .), Acker, J. 0., Hughes, W., & Fendley Jr., W. R. (2002, 2-5 June). Implementing a recursive retention assessment system for engineering programs. Paper presented at the 42nd Annual Forum for the Association for Institutional Research, Toronto, Canada. Aikenhead, G. S. (1992). Logical reasoning in science and technology. Bulletin of Science, Technology, & Society, 12(3), 149-159. Aikenhead, G. S. (2002). Cross-cultural science teaching: "Rekindling traditions" for Aboriginal students. Canadian Journal of Science, Mathematics and Technology Education, 2(3), 287-304. Aikenhead, G. S. (2006). Science education for everyday life: Evidence-based practice. New York: Teachers College Press. Aikenhead, G. S. (2007). Humanistic perspectives in the science curriculum. In S. K. Abell & N. G. Lederman (Eds), Handbook of research on science education (pp. 881 -91 O). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Alberto, P., & Troutman, A. (1999). Applied behavior analysis for teachers (5th ed.). Columbus, OH: Merrill. Allen, D. (1999). Desire to finish college: An empirical link between motivation and persistence. Research in Higher Education, 40(4), 461 -485. Anonymous (1999). Review of the book Talking about leaving: Why undergraduates leave the sciences Retrieved 5 October, 2008, from http://www.amazon.com/review/R1VBVNR7RBDRQZ/ref=cm cr rdp per m Astin, A. W., & Astin, H. S. (1993). Undergraduate science education: The impact of different college environments on the educational pipeline in the sciences. Los Angeles: University of California at Los Angeles, Higher Education Research Institute. Astin, A. W., & Oseguera, L. (2005). Degree attainment rates at American colleges and universities: Effects of race, gender, and institutional type. Los Angeles: Higher Education Research Institute, UCLA. 168 Astleitner, H., 8 Wiesner, C. (2004). An integrated model of multimedia learning and motivation. Joumal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia, 13(1), 3-21. Augustine, N., Barrett, 0., Cassell, G., Chu, 8., Gates, R., Grasmick, N., et al. (2006). Rising above the gathering storm: Energizing and employing America for a brighter economic future. Washington, DC: The National Academy of Sciences, The National Academy of Engineering, and The Institute of Medicine, National Academy Press. Bailek, W., 8 Botstain, D. (2004). Introductory science and mathematics education for 21 st century biologists. Science, 303(5659), 788-790. Baird, L. L. (2000). College Climate and the Tinto Model. In J. M. Braxton (Ed.), Reworking the Student Departure Puzzle (pp. 62-80). Nashville, Tennessee: Vanderbilt University Press. Baldi, P., & Brunak, S. (2001). Bioinformatics: The machine learning approach (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Bandura, A. L. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Worth Publishers. Becker, K. (2007). Digital game-based learning once removed: Teaching teachers. British Journal of Educational Technology, 38(3), 478-488. Bell, P. (2004). The educational opportunities of contemporary controversies in science. In M. C. Linn, E. A. Davis & P. Bell (Eds), Internet environments for science education (pp. 233-260). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Bergland, M., Lundeberg, M. A., Klyczek, K., Sweet, J., Emmons, J., Martin, C., et al. (2006). Exploring biotechnology using case based multimedia. The American Biology Teacher, 68(2), 81 -86. Boardman, C., & Bozeman, B. (2007). Role strain in university research centers. The Journal of Higher Education, 78(4), 430-463. Boldt, A. (2005). The transmission perspective: Effective delivery of content. In D. D. Pratt (Ed.), Five perspectives on teaching in adult and higher education (pp. 57-82). Malabar, FL: Krieger Publishing Company. Bonous-Hammarth, M. (2000). Pathways to success: Affirming opportunities for science, mathematics, and engineering majors. Journal of Negro Education, 69(1-2), 92-11 1. 169 Bovina, l. B., 8 Dragul'skaia, L. I. (2008). College students' representations of science and the scientist. Russian Education 8 Society, 50(1), 44-64. Boyatzis, R. E. (1998). Transforming qualitative information: Thematic analysis and code development. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. Boyer, E. L. (1990). Scholarship reconsidered: priorities of the professoriate. New Jersey: Princeton University Press. Braxton, J. M., & Lien, L. A. (2000). The Viability of Academic Integration as a Central Construct in "I”Into's Interactionalist Theory of College Student Departure. In J. M. Braxton (Ed), Reworking the Student Departure Puzzle (pp. 11-28). Nashville, Tennessee: Vanderbilt University Press. Brophy, J. (2004). Motivating students to learn (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Brown, 8. A. (2004). Discursive identity: Assimilation into the culture of science and its implications for minority students. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41(8), 810-834. Brown, B. A., Reveles, J. M., & Kelly, G. J. (2005). Scientific literacy and discursive identity: A theoretical framework for understanding science learning. Science Education, 89(5), 779-802. Burrowes, P. A. (2003). A student-centered approach to teaching general biology that really works: Lord's contructivist model put to the test. The American Biology Teacher, 65(7), 491 -502. Bush, G. (2009). Thinking around the corner: The power of information literacy. Phi Delta Kappan, 90(6), 446-447. Cain, J., Black, E. P., 8. Rohr, J. (2009). An audience response system strategy to improve student motivation, attention, and feedback. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, 73(2), 1-7. Callahan, C., Hertberg-Davis, H., Hockett, J., & Reed, C. (2008). Using on-Iine case-based lessons to supplement instruction for diverse learners in advanced placement courses World Conference on Educational Multimedia, Hypermedia and Telecommunications 2008 (pp. 332-336). Chesapeake, VA: AACE. Carini, R. M., Kuh, G. D., & Klein, 8. P. (2006). Student engagement and student learning: Testing the linkages. Research in Higher Education, 47(1), 1-32. 170 Chambers, J. A., & Sprecher, J. W. (1984). Computer-assisted instruction: Its use in the classroom. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Choi, l., Lee, S. J., & Jung, J. W. (2008). Designing multimedia case-based instruction accommodating students' diverse learning styles. Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia, 17(1), 5-25. Clark, D., Nelson, 3., Sengupta, P., & D'Angelo, C. (2009). Rethinking science learning through digital games and simulations: Genres, examples and evidence. Board on Science Education, The National Academies. Cole, D., 8 Espinoza, A. (2008). Examining the academic success of Latino students in science technology engineering and mathematics (STEM) majors. Journal of College Student Development, 49(4), 285-300. Cornell, R., 8 Martin, B. L. (1997). The role of motivation in web-based instruction. In B. H. Khan (Ed.), Web-based Instruction (pp. 93-100). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Technology Publications. Creswell, J. W. (2007a). Educational research: Planning, conducting, and evaluating quantitative and qualitative research (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Creswell, J. W. (2007b). Qualitative inquiry 8 research design: Choosing among five approaches (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Creswell, J. W., & Clark, V. L. P. (2007). Designing and conducting mixed methods research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Cristianini, N., & Hahn, M. (2006). Introduction to Computational Genomics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Cronin-Jones, L. (2000). Science scenarios: Using role-playing to make science more meaningful. The Science Teacher, 67(4 ), 48-52. Crouch, C. H., & Mazur, E. (2001). Peer instruction: Ten years of experience and results. The Physics Teacher, 69(9), 970-977. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey—Bass. Daempfle, P. A. (2003-2004). An analysis of the high attrition rates among first year college science, math, and engineering majors. Journal of College Student Retention, 5(1), 37-52. Dale, E. (1969). Audio-visual methods in teaching. New York: Dryden Press. 171 Dancy, M. H., & Henderson, C. (2008). Barriers and promises in STEM reform. the Board on Science Education. Deimann, M., & Keller, J. M. (2006). Volitional aspects of multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia, 15(2), 137-158. Dekkers, J., & Delaeter, J. (2001). Enrolment trends in school science education in Australia. International Journal of Science Education, 23(5), 487-500. Dimitrov, D. M., & Rumrill Jr., P. D. (2003). Pretest-posttest designs and measurement of change. Work: A Journal of Prevention, Assessment and Rehabilitation, 20(2), 159-165. Dori, Y. J., Tal, R. T., & Tsausu, M. (2003). Teaching biotechnology through case studies—Can we improve higher order thinking skills of non-science majors? Science Education, 87(6), 767-793. Druger, M. (2000). Creating a motivational learning environment in science. Journal of College Science Teaching, 30(4), 222-224. Duderstadt, J. J. (2000). A university for the 215t century. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Dufresne, R. J., Gerace, W. J., Leonard, W. J., Mestre, J. P., & Wenk, L. (1996). A classroom communication system for active learning. Journal of Computing in Higher Education, 7(2), 3-47. Duncan, D. (2005). Clickers in the classroom: How to enhance science teaching using classroom response systems. San Francisco: Pearson Education/Addison-Wesley/Benjamin Cummings. Eiseman, J., & Fairweather, J. (1996). Evaluation of the National Science Foundation undergraduate course and curriculum development program: Final Report. Washington, DC: SRI International. Emmott, S., & Rison, S. (Eds). (2006). Toward 2020 science (pp. 86). Retrieved from http://research.microsoft.com/towardsZOZOscience/downloads/1' 20208 Re portA4pdf. Ertmer, P. A., Newby, T. J., & MacDougall, M. (1996). Students' responses and approaches to case-based instruction: The role of reflective self- regulation. American Educational Research Journal, 33(3), 719-752. 172 Fainrveather, J. (2005). Beyond the rhetoric: Trends in the relative value of teaching and research in faculty salaries. The Journal of Higher Education, 76(4), 401-422. Fairweather, J. (2008). Linking evidence and promising practices in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) undergraduate education (pp. 31). Washington, DC: Board of Science Education, National Research Council, The National Academies. Fairweather, J., & Beach, A. (2002). Variation in faculty work within research universities: Implications for state and institutional policy. Review of Higher Education, 26(1), 97-1 15. Fainrveather, J., & Paulson, K. (2008). The evolution of scientific fields in American universities: Disciplinary differences, institutional isomorphism. In J. Valimaa & O. Ylijoki (Eds), Cultural perspectives in higher education (pp. 197-212). Dordrecht: Springer. Feather, N. (Ed). (1982). Expectations and actions. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Felder, R. M. (1995). A longitudinal study of engineering student performance and retention. IV. Instructional methods and student response to them. Journal of Engineering Education, 84(4), 361 -367. Felder, R. M., Felder, G. N., & Dietz, E. J. (1998). A longitudinal study of engineering student performance and retention. V. Comparisons with traditionally taught students. Journal of Engineering Education, 87(4), 469- 480. Felder, R. M., Felder, G. N., Mauney, M., Hamrin Jr., C. E., & Dietz, E. J. (1995). A longitudinal study of engineering student performance and retention. Ill. Gender differences in student performance and attitudes. Journal of Engineering Education, 84(2), 151-163. Fisher, D., Fairweather, J., 8 Amey, M. (2003). Systemic reform in undergraduate engineering education: The role of collective responsibility. International Journal of Engineering Education, 19(6), 768-776. Fisher, P., Zeligman, D., & Fairweather, J. (2005). Self-assessed student learning outcomes in an engineering service course. International Journal of Engineering Education, 21(3), 446-456. Fortenberry, N. L., Sullivan, J. F., Jordan, P. N., & Knight, D. W. (2007). Engineering education research aids instruction. Science, 317(5842), 1175-1176. 173 Foster, A., Wolter, B. H. K., Lundeberg, M. A., 8 Kang, H. (2008). What makes science learning relevant to students? Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association 85th Annual Conference, New York, New York. Froyd, J. E. (2008). White paper on promising practices in undergraduate STEM education. the Board on Science Education, National Academy of Sciences. Froyd, J. E., 8 Ohland, M. W. (2005). Integrated engineering curricula. Journal of Engineering Education, 94(1), 147-164. Gappa, J. M., Austin, A. E., 8 Trice, A. G. (2007). Rethinking faculty work: Higher education's strategic imperative (1 ed.). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Garris, R., Ahlers, R., 8 Driskell, J. E. (2002). Games, motivation, and learning: A research and practice model. Simulation 8 Gaming, 33(4), 441-467. Gee, J. P. (2003). What video games have to teach us about learning and literacy. New York: Palgrave MacMilan. Glaser-Zikuda, M., Full, 8., Laukenmann, M., Metz, K., 8 Randler, C. (2005). Promoting students' emotions and achievement—Instructional design and evaluation of the ECOLE-approach. Learning and Instruction, 15(5), 481- 495. Glynn, S. M., Aultman, L. P., 8 Owens, A. M. (2005). Motivation to learn in general education programs. The Journal of General Education, 54(2), 150-170. Grandy, J. (1998). Persistence in science of high-ability minority students: Results of a longitudinal study. Journal of Higher Education, 69(6), 589- 620. Gregerman, S. R. (2008). The role of undergraduate research in student retention, academic engagement, and the pursuit of graduate education. the Board on Science Education, National Academy of Sciences. Guthrie, R. W., 8 Carlin, A. (2004). Waking the dead: Using interactive technology to engage passive listeners in the classroom Proceedings of the Tenth Americas Conference on Information Systems Retrieved from htt ://www.mhh .com/c / 00 I P WP WakindDeadO 2 3. f Hackett, G., 8 Betz, N. E. (1989). An exploration of the mathematics self- efficacy/mathematics performance correspondence. Journal of Research in Mathematics Education, 20(3), 261 -273. 174 Hansen, D. (1989). Lesson evading and lesson dissembling: Ego strategies in the classroom. American Journal of Education, 97(1), 184-208. Herreid, C. F. (1994). Case Studies in Science—A Novel Method of Science Education. Journal of College Science Teaching, 23(4), 221 -229. Herreid, C. F. (2001). The Maiden and the Witch: The Crippling Undergraduate Experience. Journal of College Science Teaching, 31(2), 87-88. Herreid, C. F. (2003). Why a "case-based" course failed. Journal of College Science Teaching, 33(3), 8-11. Herreid, C. F. (2005a). Using Case Studies to Teach Science. Education: Classroom Methodology. Washington, DC: American Inst. of Biological Sciences. Herreid, C. F. (2005b). Using novels as bases for case studies: Michael Crichton's state of fear and global warming. Journal of College Science Teaching, 34(7), 10. Herreid, C. F. (Ed). (2006). Start with a story: The case study method of teaching college science. Arlington, Virginia: NSTA Press. Herzog, S. (2007). The ecology of learning: The impact of classroom features and utilization on student academic success. New Directions for Institutional Research, 2007(135), 81 -106. Hmelo-Silver, C. E. (2004). Problem-based learning: What and how do students learn? Educational Psychology Review, 16(3), 235-266. Hokanson, G., Borchert, 0., Slator, B. M., Terpstra, J., Clark, J. T., Daniels, L. M., et al. (2008). Studying Native American culture in an immersive virtual environment Proceedings of the Eighth IEEE International Conference on Advanced Learning Technologies, 2008. (pp. 788-792). New York: IEEE. Holzinger, A., Kickmeier-Rust, M., 8 Albert, D. (2008). Dynamic media in computer science education; Content complexity and learning performance: ls less more? Educational Technology 8 Society, 11(1), 279-290. Horowitz, H. M. (1988). Student response systems: Interactivity in a classroom environment. Paper presented at the Sixth Conference of Interactive Instruction Delivery for the Society of Applied Learning Technology (SALT). http://www.einstruction.com/Newsfrndex.cfm?fuseaction=News.displavfi8M enu=newsroom8content=FormalPaper8id=210 175 Howe, K. R. (1988). Against the quantitative-qualitative incompatibility thesis, or, Dogmas die hard. Educational Researcher, 17(8), 10-16. Huang, G., Taddese, N., 8 Walter, E. (2000). Entry and persistence of women and minorities in college science and engineering education. (NCES 2000- 601). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics. Hunter, A.-B., Laursen, S. L., 8 Seymour, E. (2007). Becoming a scientist: The role of undergraduate research in students' cognitive, personal, and professional development. Science Education, 91 (1 ), 36-74. Hurd, P. (1989). Science education and the nation's economy. In A. B. Champagne, B. E. Lovitts 8 B. J. Calinger (Eds), Scientific literacy (pp. 15-40). Washington, DC: AAAS. Hurtado, S., Eagan, M. K., Cabrera, N. L., Lin, M. H., Park, J., 8 Lopez, M. (2008). Training future scientists: Predicting first-year minority student participation in health science research. Research in Higher Education, 49(2), 126-152. Integrated Postsecondary Education Data System. (2008). College navigator. Retrieved 22 September 2008, from National Center for Educational Statistics http://nces.ed.gov/cgllegenavigator/ Irwin, A. R. (1995). Citizen science: A study of people, expertise, and sustainable development. New York: Routledge. Jackson, J. (2009). Game-based teaching: What educators can learn from videogames. Teaching Education, 20(3), 291 -304. Johnson, R. B., 8 Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2004). Mixed methods research: A research paradigm whose time has come. Educational Researcher, 33(7), 14-26. Jones, R. A. (1977). Self-fulfilling prophecies: Social psychological and physiological effects of expectancies. New York: Halsted Press. Kagen, D. M. (1993). Context for the use of classroom cases. American Educational Research Journal, 62(4), 129-169. Kalumuck, K. E., 8 Doss, K. (2004). Review of: National Institutes of Health curriculum supplements: Human Genetic Variation and Cell Biology and Cancer, by Biological Sciences Curriculum Study and Videodiscovery. Cell Biology Education, 3(3), 152-154. 176 Kang, H., 8 Lundeberg, M. A. (2008). Role playing and female students’ identity. construction in multimedia case based learning environment. Manuscript under review. Kardash, C. M., 8 Wallace, M. L. (2001). The perceptions of science classes survey: What undergraduate science reform efforts really need to address. Journal of Educational Psychology. 93(1), 199-210. Kaya, O. N., Kilic, Z., 8 Akdeniz, A. R. (2004). University students'perceptions of their science classrooms. Paper presented at the 18th International Conference on Chemical Education, "Chemistry Education for the Modern World", Istanbul, Turkey. Keller, J. M. (1979). Motivation and instructional design: A theoretical perspective. Journal of Instructional Development, 2(4), 26-34. Keller, J. M. (1983). Motivational design of instruction. In C. M. Reigeluth (Ed), Instructional design theories and models: An overview of their current status. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Keller, J. M. (1987). Development and use of the ARCS model of instructional design. Journal of Instructional Development, 10(3), 2-10. Keller, J. M. (1999). Using the ARCS motivational process in computer-based instruction and distance education. New Directions for Teaching and Learning, 78(Summer), 39-47. Keller, J. M. (2008). First principles of motivation to learn and e3-learning. Distance Education, 29(2), 175-185. Keller, J. M. (2010). Motivational design for learning and performance: The ARCS model approach. New York: Springer. Keller, J. M., 8 Suzuki, K. (1988). Use of the ARCS motivation model in courseware design. In D. H. Jonassen (Ed), Instructional designs for microcomputer courseware (pp. 401-434). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Keller, J. M., 8 Suzuki, K. (2004). Learner motivation and e-Iearning design: A multinationally validated process. Journal of Educational Media, 29(3), 229-239. Knight, P. G. (2007). Physical geography: Learning and teaching in a discipline so dynamic that textbooks can‘t keep up! Geography, 92(1), 57-61. 177 Ko, 8., 8 Rossen, S. (2010). Teaching online: A practical guide (3rd ed.). New York: Routledge. Koballa, T. R., Jr., 8 Glynn, S. M. (2007). Attitudinal and motivational constructs in science learning. In S. K. Abell 8 N. G. Lederman (Eds), Handbook of research on science education (pp. 75-102). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Kofoed, M. H. (2006). Competences, interest and role-pla y in science education. Paper presented at the Danish Research center on Education and Advanced Media Materials conference, University of Southern Denmark, Odense. Koper, R., 8 Tattersall, C. (Eds). (2005). Learning design: A handbook on modelling and delivering networked education and training. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Kuh, G., Kinzie, J., Buckley, J., Bridges, B., 8 Kayek, J. (2007). Piecing together the student success puzzle: Research, propositions, and recommendations. Washington, DC: Association for the Study of Higher Education. Kuh, G., Kinzie, J., Schuh, J., 8 Witt, E. (2005). Student success in college: Creating conditions that matter. Washington, DC: Association for the Study of Higher Education. Kulik, J. A. (2002). School mathematics and science programs benefit from instructional technology. (NSF03-301). Arlington, VA: National Science Foundafion. Kumar, D. D., 8 Chubin, D. E. (Eds). (2000). Science, technology, 8 society: A sourcebook on research and practice. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. Kumar, D. D., 8 Sherwood, R. D. (2007). Effect of a problem based simulation on the conceptual understandings of undergraduate science education students. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 16(3), 239-246. Labov, J. 8., Singer, S. R., George, M. D., Schweingruber, H. A., 8 Hilton, M. L. (2009). Effective practices in undergraduate STEM education, Part 1: Examining the evidence. CBE - Life Sciences Education, 8(3), 157-161. Lai-Chong Law, E., Kickmeier-Rust, M., Albert, D., 8 Holzinger, A. (2008). Challenges in the development and evaluation of immersive digital educational games. In A. Holzinger (Ed), HCI and usability for education and work (pp. 19-30). Berlin: Springer. 178 Lee, K. (2007). Online Collaborative Case Study Learning. Journal of College Reading and Learning, 37(2), 82-100. Leslie, D. (2002). Resolving the dispute: Teaching is academe’s core value. The Journal of Higher Education, 73(1), 49-73. Levin, B. B. (1999). The role of discussion in case pedagogy: Who learns what? And how? In M. A. Lundeberg, B. B. Levin 8 H. L. Harrington (Eds), Who learns what from cases and how ?: The research basis for teaching and learning with cases (pp. 139-157). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Llado, J., 8 Sanchez, B. (2009). A computer-based tool to foster engineering students' interest in dynamics. Computer Applications in Engineering Education. Retrieved from htt ://www3.inter cienc .wil .0 ml urnal/1222 7 lab tr ct? RETRY =18SRETRY=O Lord, T. (2007). Society for college science teachers: Revisiting the cone of learninguls it a reliable way to link instruction method with knowledge recall? Journal of College Science Teaching, 37(2), 14-17. Lord, T. (2008). We know how to improve Science understanding in students, so why are college professors embracing it? Journal of College Science Teaching, 38(1), 66-70. Lundeberg, M. A., Fox, P. W., Brown, A. C., 8 Elbedour, S. (2000). Cultural Influences on confidence: Country and gender. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(1 ), 152-159. Lundeberg, M. A., Levin, B. B., 8 Harrington, H. (Eds). (1999). Who Ieams what from cases and how: The research base for teaching and learning with cases. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Lundeberg, M. A., Mogen, K., Bergland, M., Klyczek, K., Johnson, D., 8 MacDonald, E. (2002). Case It or else!: Fostering ethical awareness about human genetics through multimedia-based cases. Journal of College Science Teaching, 32(1), 64-69. Lundeberg, M. A., Wolter, B. H. K., Kang, H., Armstrong, N., Borsari, B., Boury, N., et al. (Under revision). Context matters: Increasing understanding with interactive clicker case studies. Educational Technology Research and Development. 179 Lundeberg, M. A., 8 Yadav, A. (2006a). Assessment of case study teaching: Where do we go from here? Part 1. Journal of College Science Teaching, 35(5), 1 2-1 5. Lundeberg, M. A., 8 Yadav, A. (2006b). Assessment of case study teaching: Where do we go from here? Part II. Journal of College Science Teaching, 35(6), 8-13. Maehr, M. L., 8 Braskamp, L. A. (1986). The motivation factor: A theory of personal investment. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Malone, T. W. (1981 a). Toward a theory of intrinsically motivating instruction. Cognitive Science, 4(5), 333-369. Malone, T. W. (1981b). What makes computer games fun? Byte, 6(12), 258-277. Massy, W., 8 Zemsky, R. (1994). Faculty discretionary time: Departments and the "academic ratchet". Journal of Higher Education, 65(1), 1-22. Matthews, P. (2007). The relevance of science education in Ireland. Dublin: Royal Irish Academy. Mayer, R. E., Stull, A., DeLeeuw, K., Almeroth, K., Bimber, B., Chun, D., et al. (2009). Clickers in college classrooms: Fostering learning with questioning methods in large lecture classes. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34(1), 51 -57. Mayer, R. E., 8 Wittrock, M. C. (2006). Problem solving. In P. A. Alexander 8 P. H. Winne (Eds), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 287- 304). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Mayo, M. J. (2009). Video games: A route to large-scale STEM education? Science, 323(5910), 79-82. McConnell, D. A., Steer, D. N., Owens, K. D., 8 Knight, C. C. (2005). How students think: Implications for learning in introductory geoscience courses. Journal of Geoscience Education, 53(4), 462-470. McDonald, J., 8 Dominguez, L. (2005). Moving from content knowledge to engagement. Journal of College Science Teaching, 35(3), 18-22. Means, T. B., Jonassen, D. H., 8 Dwyer, F. M. (1997). Enhancing relevance: Embedded ARCS strategies versus purpose. Educational Technology Research and Development, 45(1), 5-18. Merrill, M. D. (2002). First principles of instruction. Educational Technology Research and Development, 50(3), 43-59. 180 Moriarty, M. A. (2007). Inclusive pedagogy: Teaching methodologies to reach diverse learners in science instruction. Equity 8 Excellence in Education, 40(3), 252-265. National Science Foundation. (1998). lnfusing equity in systematic reform: An implementation scheme. Washington, DC: National Science Foundation. National Science Foundation. (2002). Women, minorities, and persons with disabilities in science and engineering: 2000. (NSF 00-327). Arlington, VA: National Science Foundation. National science Foundation. (2007). Women, minorities, and persons with disabilities in science and engineering: 2007. In SRS Publication (Ed.)). Arlington, VA. Nietfeld, J. L., Cao, L., 8 Osborne, J. W. (2005). Metacognitive monitoring accuracy and student performance in the postsecondary classroom. Journal of Experimental Education, 74(1), 7-28. O'Neal, 0., Wright, M., Cook, C., Perorazio, T., 8 Purkiss, J. (2007). The impact of teaching assistants on student retention in the sciences: Lessons for TA training. Journal of College Science Teaching, 36(5), 24-29. Oblinger, D. G., 8 Oblinger, J. L. (Eds). (2005). Educating the net generation. Washington, D.C.: EDUCAUSE. Onwuegbuzie, A. J., 8 Leech, N. L. (2005). On becoming a pragmatic researcher: The importance of combining quantitative and qualitative research methodologies. International Journal of Social Research Methodology. 8(5), 375-387. Osborne, J., 8 Collins, S. (2000). Pupils' and parents' views of the school science curriculum. London: King's College London. Parker, J. (2008). Comparing research and teaching in university promotion criteria. Higher Education Quarterly, 62(3), 237-251. Pascarella, E. T., 8 Terenzini, P. T. (2005). How college affects students: A third decade of research. San Francisco, CA: Jossey—Bass. Peck, A. C., 8 Detweiler, M. C. (2000). Training concurrent multistep procedural tasks. Human Factors, 42(3), 379-389. Pintrich, P. R. (2003). A motivational science perspective on the role of student motivation in learning and teaching contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 667-686. 181 Prensky, M. (2001). Digital game-based Ieaming. New York: McGraw-Hill. Prince, M. J. (2004). Does active learning work? A review of the research. Journal of Engineering Education, 93(3), 223-231. Prince, M. J., 8 Felder, R. M. (2006). Inductive teaching and learning methods: Definitions, comparisons, and research bases. Journal of Engineering Education, 95(2), 123-138. Pryor, J. H., Hurtado, S., DeAngelo, L., Sharkness, J., Romero, L. C., Korn, W. S., et al. (2008). The American freshman: National norms for fall 2008. Los Angeles: Higher Education Research Institute, UCLA. Renchler, R. (1992). Student motivation, school culture, and academici achievement: What scth leaders can do. Eugene, OR: ERIC Clearinghouse on Educational Management. Reveles, J. M., Cordova, R., 8 Kelly, G. J. (2004). Science literacy and academic identity formulation. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41(10), 1111-1144. Richardson, V. (1993). Use of cases in considering methods for motivating students. In H. Harrington 8 M. Thompson (Eds), Student motivation and case study manual. Boone, NC: Appalachian State University. Roschelle, J., Penuel, W. R., 8 Abrahamson, L. (2004, 12-16 April). Classroom response and communication systems: Research review and theory. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association 81 st Annual Conference, San Diego, CA. Ryan, R. M., Rigby, C. S., 8 Przybylski, A. (2006). The motivational pull of video games: A self-determination theory approach. Motivation and Emotion, 30(4), 344-360. Rybarczyk, B. J., Baines, A. T., McVey, M., Thompson, J. T., 8 Wilkins, H. (2007). A case-based approach increases student learning outcomes and comprehension of cellular respiration concepts. Biochemistry and Molecular Biology Education, 35(3), 181-186. Savery, J. R. (2006). Overview of problem-based learning: definitions and distinctions. The Interdisciplinary Journal of Problem-based Learning, 1(1), 9-20. Schreiner, C., 8 Sjoberg, S. (2004). Sowing the seeds of ROSE. Background, rationale, questionnaire development and data collection for ROSE (The Relevance of Science Education) - a comparative study of students' views 182 of science and science education). Oslo: Department. of Teacher Education and School Development. Schunk, D. H., 8 Pajares, F. (2009). Self-efficacy theory. In K. R. Wentzel 8 A. Wigfield (Eds), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 35-54). New York: Routledge. Sechrest, L., 8 Sidana, S. (1995). Quantitative and qualitative methods: Is there an alternative? Educational Program and Planning, 18(1), 77-87. Seymour, E. (1995). Revisiting the "problem iceberg": Science, mathematics, and engineering students still chilled out. Journal of College Science Teaching, 24(6), 392-400. Seymour, E. (2002). Tracking the processes of change in US. undergraduate education in science, mathematics, engineering, and technology. Science Education, 85(6), 79-105. Seymour, E., 8 Hewitt, N. M. (1997). Talking about leaving: Why undergraduates leave science. Boulder, Colorado: Westview Press. Shellnut, B., Knowltan, A., 8 Savage, T. (1999). Applying the ARCS model to the design and development of computer-based modules for manufacturing engineering courses. Educational Technology Research and Development, 47(2), 100-1 10. Sjoberg, S., 8 Schreiner, C. (2005). How do learners in different cultures relate to science and technology? Asia-Pacific Forum on Science Learning and Teaching, 6(2), 1-17. Sleeman, K. A., 8 Sorby, S. A. (2007, September 3-7). Effective retention strategies for engineering students. Paper presented at the International Conference on Engineering Education (ICEE), Coimbra, Portugal. Small, R. V., 8 Gluck, M. (1994). The relationship of motivational conditions to effective instructional attributes: A magnitude scaling approach. Educational Technology 34(8), 33-40. Smith, R. A., 8 Murphy, S. K. (1998). Using case studies to increase learning and interest in biology. The American Biology Teacher, 60(4), 265-268. Smith, T. M., 8 Emmeluth, D. S. (2002). Introducing Bioinformatics into the Biology Curriculum: Exploring the National Center for Biotechnology Information. American Biology Teacher, 64(2), 93-99. 183 Sokolove, P. G., Marbach-Ad, G., 8 Fusco, J. (2003). Student use of internet study rooms for out-of-class group study in introductory biology. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 12(2), 105-113. Song, 8. H., 8 Keller, J. M. (2001). Effectiveness of motivationally adaptive computer-assisted instruction on the dynamic aspects of motivation. Educational Technology Research and Development, 49(2), 5-22. Stage, F. K., 8 Hossler, D. (2000). Where Is the Student? Linking Student Behaviors, College Choice, and College Persistence. In J. M. Braxton (Ed), Reworking the Student Departure Puzzle (pp. 170-195). Nashville, Tennessee: Vanderbilt University Press. Stoker, A., 8 Thompson, P. (1969). Science and ethics: A radical approach to high school science. Science Education, 53(3), 203-209. Strenta, A. 0., Elliot, R., Adair, R., Matier, M., 8 Scott, J. (1994). Choosing and leaving science in highly selective institutions. Research in Higher Education, 35(5), 513-547. Taraban, R., 8 Blanton, R. (Eds). (2008). To think and act like a scientist: Undergraduate research experiences and their effects. New York: Teachers College Press. Tarhan, L., 8 Acar, B. (2007). Problem-based learning in an eleventh grade chemistry class: 'factors affecting cell potential'. Research in Science 8 Technological Education, 25(3), 351 -369. Theall, M., 8 Franklin, J. (1999). What have we learned? A sysnthesis and some guidelines for effective motivation in higher education. New Directions for Teaching and Learning, 78(Summer), 99-109. Tinto, V. (1993). Leaving college: rethinking the causes and cures of student attrition (2 ed.). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Tinto, V. (2000). Linking Learning and Leaving: Exploring the Role of the College Classroom in Student Departure. In J. M. Braxton (Ed), Reworking the Student Departure Puzzle (pp. 81-94). Nashville, Tennessee: Vanderbilt University Press. Tinto, V. (2007). Taking student retention seriously. Retrieved from htt :ll . r. u/ ca mic / r d/hi h r uc ti n/ %2 f°/o2 Vtint Files/Taking RetentiQnSeriQuslypdf Tobias, S. (1990). They're not dumb, they're different: Stalking the second tier. Tucson, AZ: Research Corporation. 184 Tobias, S. (1992). Revitalizing undergraduate science. Tucson, AZ: Research Corporation. Trees, A. R., 8 Jackson, M. H. (2007). The learning environment in clicker classrooms: Student processes of learning and involvement in large university-level courses using student response systems. Learning, Media, and Technology, 32(1), 21 -40. Trey, L., 8 Khan, S. (2008). How science students can learn about unobservable phenomena using computer-based analogies. Computers 8 Education, 51(2), 519-529. Tsang, E., 8 Halderson, C. (2008). Create learning communities to enhance success for students with diverse academic preparation background. Paper presented at the 38th ASEE/IEEE Frontiers in Education Conference, Saratoga Springs, NY. Vallerand, R. J., Fortier, M. S., 8 Guay, F. (1997). Self-determination and persistence in a real-life setting: Toward a motivational model of high school dropout. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(5), 1 1 61 -1 1 76. Visser, J., 8 Keller, J. M. (1990). The clinical use of motivational messages: An inquiry into the validity of the ARCS model of motivational design. Instructional Science, 19(6), 467-500. Vogel, J. J., Vogel, D. S., Cannon-Bowers, J., Bowers, C. A., Muse, K., 8 Wright, M. (2006). Computer gaming and interactive simulations for learning: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 34(3), 229- 243. Vollmeyer, R., 8 Rheinberg, F. (2000). Does motivation affect performance via persistence? Learning and Instruction, 10(4), 293-309. Walczyk, J. J., Ramsey, L. L., 8 Zha, P. (2007). Obstacles to instructional innovation according to college science and mathematics faculty. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 44(1), 85-106. Wallis, C., 8 Steptoe, S. (2006). How to bring our schools out of the 20th century. Time. Retrieved from http:l/www.timecom/time/magazine/articIe/Q,9171 ,1 568480.00html Wan kat, P. (2002). The effective, efficient professor: Teaching scholarship and service. Boston: Allyn 8 Bacon. 185 Wefer, S. H., 8 Sheppard, K. (2008). Bioinformatics in high school biology curricula: A study of state science standards. CBE—Life Sciences Education, 7(1 ), 155-162. Welch, W. W., 8 Walberg, H. J. (1967). Are the attitudes of teachers related to declining percentages of enrollments in physics? Science Education, 51(5), 422-436. Wigfield, A., 8 Eccles, J. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 68-81. Wolter, B. H. K., Kang, H., Lundeberg, M. A., 8 Herreid, C. F. (2009, 13-17 April). Using personal response systems (“clickers ”) with case studies in large lecture classes to impact student assessment performance. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association 86th Annual Conference, San Diego, CA. Walter, B. H. K., Kang, H., Lundeberg, M. A., Herreid, C. F., 8 Zhang, T. (2009). Students’perceptions of using personal response systems ("clickers") with cases in science. Paper presented at the European Science Education Research Association 8th Biannual Conference, Istanbul, Turkey. Walter, B. H. K., Lundeberg, M. A., 8 Bergland, M. (2009, 13-17 April). What makes science relevant?: Student perceptions of experiences with multimedia case Ieaming experiences in ecology and health. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association 86th Annual Conference, San Diego, CA. Wright, E. L., Sunal, D. W., 8 Bland Day, J. (2004). Improving undergraduate science teaching through educational research. In D. W. Sunal, E. L. Wright 8 J. Bland Day (Eds), Reform in undergraduate science teaching for the 21st century (pp. 1-11). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. Wuensch, K. L. (2007). Inter-rater agreement Retrieved 3 October, 2009, from http://core.ecu.edu/psyc/wuenschk/docs30/lnterRater.doc Wullf, D., 8 Austin, A. (2004). Paths to the professoriate: Strategies for enriching the preparation of future faculty. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Yadav, A., Lundeberg, M. A., DeSchryver, M., Dirkin, K., Schiller, N. A., Maier, K., et al. (2007). Teaching science with case studies: A national survey of faculty perceptions of the benefits and challenges of using cases. Journal of College Science Teaching, 37(1), 34-38. 186 Yin, R. K. (2003). Case study research: Design and method (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Yore, L. D. (1991). Secondary science teachers' attitudes toward and beliefs about science reading and science textbooks. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 28(1), 55-72. Yourstone, S. A., Kraye, H. S., 8 Albaum, G. (2008). Classroom questioning with immediate electronic response: Do clickers improve learning? Decision Sciences Journal of Innovative Education, 6(1), 75-88. Zvelebil, M., 8 Baum, J. (2007). Understanding bioinformatics. New York: Garland Science, Taylor 8 Francis Group L.L.C. 187 melllllllllllllllll11111111111111lllllllllEs 3 1293 03063 4426