— I I I I I I I l I I I I l . l I I I I I I I I I I I . I .‘ . i m , a. ‘ I ,.. IL \. 155313 ' LIBRARY Michigan State g i 1"" i ' '- 7" '4‘. 5'. .1 " ‘..-‘ ‘- V 8 8383 235% 33 z :0 Rd 2.0 $6. .I. wed om.m nmd 21c. Nm.m oo.m EM EoEmmOmmeeoSHam mmd Ed CM. .2. wfim CM mNd mN.m mm.m cod mim Eoemmommmfiom 8:82?on 020 020 020 mNm mNm mNm minim Qm. E v xmmh N xmflr 2 x82. QM E m xmfl. N fish 2 xmflr OED mNm EmEtEEFfl a3 amazemwmm Diem. $33.;st :SEGQEQD o 2an 55 3 .v& .333; Bambsamwm... 96 106 bod M26 Cd 26 Rd 36 RN 2N6 EN mmN mfm me SN mmN mmm ooN voN voN OWN SN omN SN 2N.o Ed 26 N6 SN 8.». Nfim th .340. end oN.m NoN mm.m om.m ooN mmN ~3on $25088 E m Bean H ea 3e _ so 830ch owmowcfl woow mm 8:38 £5 5 :ofiog 5528 s? were seas _ so waged mo coumoobm .3me in 89580 no. m 8 moouflwoow was 8% H so Eugene: 203 x a now Stop a once 26: 2:95 2 3:588 a om: 8 we: 03 3:83 3me £5 warts» Be es m as H so Sum EN m3?» .& Qm E 020 v x32. 25 N use .22 020 _ xmfl. QM. E ”E m use ma, N use “22 ma 2 use 3.83 0 uses 56 different opinions and/or difficulty arriving at a consensus with their partners. For example, "Sometimes, we have different opinion. Because everyone has own definition of love" (JO, CMC, task 1). Others mentioned that the source of conflict was not just reconciling both opinions but also combining different writing skills and styles, "We disagreed with each other at first. Although we have the same Opinion about the movie, we have different writing skills and methods that maked us lose a lot of time. So it is a little bit hard for us to write the essay" (KI, F2F, task 3). Over half of the students stated that, while collaborative writing might be more difficult than writing alone, it could offer them some additional benefits. These reported benefits included generating and sharing ideas, checking work, and dividing work to be more efficient. Several additional themes emerged among the responses: division of labor; discussion adding to the ease of group work and/or arguments detracting from the group work; and ability or inability to write a clear and cohesive essay. Although most groups noted that they were discussing the texts and their opinions, over half of the groups reported dividing the tasks to some degree instead of working together through the duration of the task. Surprisingly, this division of labor seemed to be a common theme across tasks, regardless of environment. For example, one organized delegation of tasks came from a F2F group in task 3, "Today we have 3 partner together create one article. First, one person type an idea, and others discuss next controlling idea. Then we all check the cohesion, grammar, connection problem" (LA, F 2F , task 3). It is important to note that, while certainly not a majority, a few F2F and CMC groups reported collaborating throughout the task. YU in F2F task I wrote, "I wanted to do this work 57 log in) RL‘ an we SE Sll; 95! en IES together, so I tried to talk equally and type evenly. Also, I talked about my opinion and my experience and we could agree with each other. I thought [my partner] tried to tell her opinion actively and ask my opinion, so we could discuss about our opinion efficiently." From a CMC environment, 1 student reported that, "We wrote it together each part. . . . We have lots of chat, So just exchange our idea" (JO, CMC, task 4). Response to Environment Likert responses. Students tended to have a fairly neutral reaction to the writing environment. In response to question 2, "I loved this activity," students' mean answers were slightly less than neutral for both F2F (M = 2.99, SD = 0.29) and CMC (M = 2.65, SD = 0.28) environments with CMC being the more negative of the two. Of note were slight increases in interest in the third task, when the whole class completed the writing task F 2F (M e 3.24) and the fourth task, when the whole class participated by CMC (M = 2.81). An independent t-test revealed that these differences were not significant (p-value = 0.11). See Table 6 above for a summary of the findings. Student ratings in answer to question 9, "I liked using Googledocs to compose my essay" consistently increased across the four tasks with mean ranges from 3.07 to 3.29 for F2F (M = 3.12, SD = 0.15) and 2.5 to 3.1 for CMC (M = 2.74, SD = 0.31). An independent t-test revealed that responses to question 9 were approaching significance (p- value = 0.07). When asked if they would like to do more assignments like this one in class (question 6), students ranged from strongly agreeing to strongly disagreeing. The mean response showed a steady but small increase across the four tasks for both environments (M= 2.64 - 2.71, SD= 0.2 and M= 2 - 2.76, SD= 0.43 for F2F and CMC, respectively) but were not significant (p= 0.41). 58 Perception of Learning. When students were asked to respond to the statement "I think writing with another person in this manner is good language practice" in question 4, most students fell between being neutral (3) and somewhat agreeing (4). Overall, students seemed slightly to favor the F2F environment (M= 3.66, SD= 0.14) over CMC (M=3.37, SD=0. 15). Both environments experienced a dip in the second task and a rise by the next task in that environment (i.e., task 3 for F2F and task 4 for CMC). An independent t-test indicated that this variation was significant (p= 0.01) and Cohen's d showed a very large effect size (d== 3.77). These fluctuations could be a result of students’ rising familiarity and comfort with the tools. Similar dips and rises can be noted in response to "I feel like I learned something today" but these did not result in any significance. This could have occurred because the average student reported somewhat agreeing that he/she learned, regardless of the task environment. See Table 6 above. Case-Studies Because the purpose of this study is to better understand Googledocs in terms of how it might function as a CSCL in an ESL classroom, I have zeroed in on how two students used the tool during the collaborative writing process. This case-study approach offers significant insight into the implications of using Googledocs as a collaborative language learning tool. Participant EU Perception of task. EU's responses underscore some of the general comments made by the class. With the exception of the third task, she reported dividing the task with her partner, being responsible for writing parts like the introduction and/or the conclusion. She admitted, "I had some difficulty because I'm not good at typing. So it 59 tooks much time." She also mentioned that she "felt confusing" and uncomfortable because "We should type and shouldn't talk. So, it took much time." After her first essay experience, she wrote that she would like to work collaboratively again, "but I don't want to do with computer. It is uncomfortable. I think it is better to communicate face to face." By essay 3, however, her opinions about collaboration in general were changed, It [writing F2F] is easier than writing by chatting but yet it is hard to write one essay with partner. Because, of course, we have different ideas. Of course we can learn something and improve our writing still by negotiating, but negotiating is hard because we are not good at English.(EU, essay 3 F 2F) After the final essay, in CMC, she explained that the Googledoc made it confusing "because we don't know what others write. I will use it [Googledocs] when I am far away from each other but I won't use at the same time." Content of student—talk As seen in Table 7 below, the distribution of the content of student talk was not the same across all 4 tasks for all areas of student-talk. EU and her CMC partners tended to spend most of their talk clarifying and managing the task such that the pair could part ways with a clear idea of the directions and what each individual was going to do to accomplish the writing task. The second most significant topic of conversation (M= 25.06, SD= 1.77) was the generation of ideas. When in a F2F environment, EU and her partners had a more diverse conversation. These groups spent some time, albeit brief, talking about grammar and managing the task in addition to generating ideas and organizing the task. A review of the transcripts indicates that managing the task in the F2F environment was usually limited to who would type the 60 essay or take notes during the brainstorming. In comparison with the CMC environment, EU and her partners spent relatively little time explicitly managing or clarifying the task. 61 mNd w _.o cod mmd o 2 .w deN 3.3 oo.cN cougars—U xmmh mod morn ood oN.v ooN nNdm cmém :Nm EoEomew—z xmflr no.2 mod NON VNd cod ood cod cod wcmwmom MNW 3.3 on: 2.: ”NA wad mm; ood couaNEwwHO mmé 3.2 3.2 v0.3 cod cod cod cod mm”: 2N2 omNm No.wv mode RA oo.mN Nm.©N $.mN 803 $38250 QM. .2 38,—. Ax. 30H .x. QM 2 30h .x. ESP .x. M ”Na 3m N mNm Dm v 020 Dm _ 020 Dm— EmEoU «Norm Bangor? totafihflQ bm n 2an 62 Effect of planning, collaboration, and peer revision on essay. In essay 1, EU and her partner began their chat session with an attempt to generate ideas. It soon became clear, however, that there was some confusion about what the pair ought to do during the task. The pair then spent six rrrinutes discussing the task (i.e., clarifying what was required in the task and who would tackle each part of that requirement). Excerpt 1 below is taken from the Gmail chat transcript where EU chats with her partner, AL. Excerpt 1 EU & AL Gmail Text Chat Transcript from EU CMC Essay 1 AL: if we have to compare we can just state the thesis of 2 novels or we dont have to compare just state the thesis and the main idea EU: I think we should compare AL: but it didnt say in the intruduction [instructions ?] AL: we just need to compare to our own beliefs AL: do u want to do the body or u want me to do it EU: do the body? AL: introduction,body and conclusion EU: you mean, what I want to do? AL: yeas EU: I don't careeeeeee It is so confusinggg t t AL: ok,i will do the body, can you write intro and conclusion? AL: yeah,we do some free writing then we can start write so our intr, body and conclusion should be about " love is free" EU: but I am so confusing we have to show the authors' thought and compare it with our thought? AL: where did it say?on the paper? EU: yeah AL: where? EU: your sheet Describe how the author portrays~ and then compare/contrast it to your own beliefes AL: i will do that part EU: Ah I understand AL: once u finished intro and conclusion,YOU can give me some ideas about it and i will write it i mean you can give me ideas about the 2 story which you think are important than i can add it into pur paper 63 After agreeing that their thesis would be "love is free," the pair said they would freewrite individually on their respective papers. However, after a mere three minutes, AL asks EU if she can see her writing in the Googledoc. Seven minutes later, EU responds "yeaa." Although the original plan was for EU to share her ideas with AL, there is no exchange of these ideas over chat nor is there chatting within the Googledoc itself. In fact, there is no explicit communication between the partners until AL types that she is done writing her section forty-seven minutes after the two begin writing. With twenty minutes remaining, AL announces that she will go back and edit EU's part for grammar errors and that EU should do the same to which EU responds, "yes I will. And you can delete or add to my part." A review of the Googledoc revision history shows that they made a few additional changes above the surface-level edits to which they agreed. Table 10 on the next page provides an overview of the revisions conducted collaboratively following the adapted version of F aigley and Witte (1981). 64 <- ooov—moo m: R E u—tOv—‘WOOF v—Il oooooOo (\ooooch 30,—. 82833880 32:53me meowfisgom mcouaumnsm mcouflom 82%? [\ v— V) F! OWOOO OMOOO N u--1 N '— omooo: N '— W v—1 OMOOO OOOOOOO OOOOOOO N v—1 mowfifiu 393-“on 30H 2598:0800 mcogflbma mcoufisccom mcocfiumnsm weave—0Q €863. F-lv—‘OF-tm v—‘v—I‘Nv—flm F-tv—Or—tm v—ir—INv—lm 00000 ©0000 mowcwao wcmtomemmcmgoE 30H Heston :osmaocsm 5:82: £385: .352. wezam momqwsu ooflSm 33 is $53 N saw 936 mm a: at Essex teem w 2an 65 Clearly, AL is doing most of the revisions. After ten minutes of reviewing the other's work and five minutes before time is called, AL states "I changed some words [. . .] we are done." In essay 4, EU and her partner displayed a few differences from that of the prior essay. This pair began with some task management before generating ideas. This generation of ideas included a mutual sharing of information as the two tried to decide which topic they could write about, without it being too difficult for them. Afier deciding on a topic, the two originally divide the task by introduction/conclusion and body paragraphs as they did in previous writing tasks but then decide that it would be better to have one person compare and the other contrast. The chat also indicates that the two did not spend time brainstorming separately but rather together until they had a thesis both were comfortable writing about. Then, they began writing and wrote individually for forty-one minutes. If essay 1 had little collaborative revision between the partners EU and AL, essay 4 had even less. In this task, EU's partner, FE, asked EU for help in revising the essay. He wrote, "help me to check it." EU, however, was done with the assignment when she finished writing and responded, "it 3 ok. let's finish. haha." It should not be surprising, then, that the pair shared only two revisions, and these were revisions in the format of the document. That is, fairly early on FE moved the paragraphs around so that there were some extra spaces between the text blocks and each could write his/her separate paragraph without crowding the text block of his/her partner. These behaviors are in stark contrast to what can be observed from the audio transcripts of EU’s F2F essays. In essay 2, both students began the task by participating 66 in a quality exchange while generating ideas. But, EU’s partner, SE, took charge early on by self-electing to take notes during the brainstorming. She then assumed the role of typing when EU asked SE, "Can you do the typing?" Immediately following this request, EU begins to narrate aloud some ideas for what SE should write, and SE reforrnulates EU's words while typing them into the Googledoc. These transcripts and reformulations are shown in Excerpt 2 below. Excerpt 2 EU & SE Oral Transcripts from EU F 2F Essay 2 Oral transcripts Googledocs EU: two stories...and the Beautiful White Horse...the authors SE: How can I...How can I start? In the two stories, "The Lottery" and "The SE: First, the author...the authors say that Summer of the Beautiful White Horse", the morality authors talk about the morality of the EU: to follow tradition villagers from the traditions. The two SE: traditions of the authors show the different morality and EU: [Korean] traditions that one side is wrong and another --- side is right. The morality does depend on SE: They can the each of the tradition from the two stories. SE:What can I write? In "the Lottery", Jackson EU: Stories show that...uh...show that morality and tradition [EU narrates and S types] [SE continues typing w/o EU narrating] S: Who is the author of "The Lottery"? E: Jackson S: Jackson? It is worth noting that towards the end of the excerpt, SE is beginning to assume control of the writing task. She begins composing and typing, only consulting EU when she needs information about the stories (e. g. the author's name). Despite SE's decision to do most of the writing, EU remains engaged by reading what SE writes, offering periodic 67 corrections. Excerpt 3 below continues the conversation from Excerpt 2 above, illustrating this shift in control. Excerpt 3 EU & SE Oral Transc‘rgymom EU F 2F Essay 2 SE: What do you think? Jackson defines the morality as...as...um? EU: She defines the morality as SE: What is morality of this story and this story? This story is honesty. EU: Honesty. Maybe I remember too. Jackson thinks the [SE sighs and deletes] SE: Jackson SE: The morality is [deletes] depends on EU: and in the Lottery people become have question about the old tradition and then SE: Before this sentence we need [SE re-reads] SE: Ahhh! [Korean] Ah! SE: [types and narrates] and other side is right [types but no narration] SE: depend on the tradition. On the each of the tradition [types but no narration] SE: In the Lottery, Jackson, Jackson shows that the village's tradition...um... was [deletes] as them [deletes] some View? is... EU: pick, pick? SE: Eh? pick EU: c, c, Interestingly, both the audio recording and the survey make it clear that SE not only did most of the work, but that she became increasingly more frustrated with EU for not contributing to the assignment. In contrast to the above experience in essay 2, essay 3 involved a more collaborative work. Both worked together to compose their essay, beginning with generating ideas and deciding on their essay’s structure. When they began to write, they traded off typing with EU's partner, CH, responsible for the majority of it. Although 1 student typed, both were orally composing the essay and reforrnulating it before typing it 68 into the Googledoc. After it was typed, they often re-read aloud what they had written, a process that often resulted in LREs. Holistic scores. EU and her partner's scores seemed to vary quite a bit across tasks and environments. However, her mean score for CMC was M: 2.38, considerably lower than her mean score in F 2F at M= 3.13. Percent rater agreement was 93.75 across all 4 of EU's essays. See Table 9 below for a summary of these findings. Table 9 EU Holistic Essay Scores by Environment CMC EU Essay 1 EU Essay 4 M SD Rater 1 3 2 Rater 2 2.5 2 M 2.75 2 2.38 0.53 % Rater agree 87.5 100 F2F EU EssaL2 EU Essay 3 M SD Rater 1 3 3.5 Rater 2 2.5 3.5 M 2.75 3.5 3.13 0.53 % Rater agree 87.5 100 Participant MC Student perception. In contrast to EU, MC began with a F 2F task. After her first collaborative experience, her responses pertained mostly to the idea of writing an essay with another person rather than to the tool, Googledocs, in particular. She wrote, "I think if I work by myself, it would be more efficient. I feel tired to explain everytime I need to edit soemthing to the essay." She also found it difficult to compose an essay with another person and explained this difficulty by writing, "Because everytime I need to stop to explain why I edit this make my thoughts stuck. I prefer to write by myself. " Although 69 she did not report any advantage to writing the essay F2F with another person, she supposed it would be better than in a CMC environment. Writing in a CMC environment in task 2 confirmed her earlier suspicions that it would be more difficult than F2F; however, she found that, "actually discuss on the computer is not as hard as I thought." She commented that she disliked that she and her partner were not able to write together but rather had to divide "the part for each other which is hard to combine and do the cohesion between the different paragraphs we wrote." Interestingly, she reported thinking that, "if I know what he is going to write and he know what I am going to write than our paragraph will be much easier to become a unity." MC noted both drawbacks and advantages of writing collaboratively online. She commented on the writing experience and Googledocs tool saying that, "sometimes I did not saw his message to me immediately" and "the layout function and the spelling/gramma check function is not as good as word document". Similar to her responses on the F2F surveys for essay 2 and 3, she continued to feel that, "sometimes it is kind of a waste of time to explain if your partner disagree with you. It will make you in hurry while it is timed writing" when she was writing in a CMC context. Her reactions were not entirely negative, however. She felt strongly that, "It is good practice for time writing though it keep my heads intense" and "it is fun to convince your parnener [partner] to accept and agree to use your ideas". Content of student-talk Much like EU, MC and her partners‘ student talk changed by environment. In the CMC context, they spent most of their time managing the task while similar tasks in F 2F found them focusing on generating ideas. In a F2F 7O environment, MC and her partner did engage in several language-related episodes in addition to spending a bit more time discussing how the essay would be organized than in a CMC environment. The focus of their student talk is summarized in Table 10 on the next page. 71 mg 32 82 3a 8.2 8.x m2 o assuage 23 :.m m8 3.” 8% $2 2.3 3.8 w Sm 60882.5": 2.5 22 8.2 2 8.” o 2 .2 -- -- -- -- wanna go 2.2 8.2 8.2 mmm mg o 3.2. 8:835 Ed ”SN 3: Sam 22 2.. 0 am an: “3.2 on: 2.2 2.2 3.2 2.2 o 3.3 932 258250 E. 2 35 .x. :33 .x. a. 2 23$ 3 283 .x. m ”22 oz 2 .22 oz a. 020 02 N 020 o: 32:69 «Bk EmEEMIE 28.23922me U: S 2an 72 As is clearly visible in Table 10 above, coding of MC CMC 4 revealed surprisingly little exchanges between MC and her partner, SH. Recall that this essay was written as a homework assignment out-of-class to determine whether students would use the tool differently when they were not being monitored. A review of the conversation exchanged in Googledocs is quite telling. MC and SH began their homework by chatting in Gmail. MC initiated the conversation in Chinese to which SH reminded her that this was against the rules. SH then suggested using the Googledoc to chat because it was more efficient. Below in Excerpt 4 is a record of their Googledocs chat, made visible through the revision history. Excerpt 4 MC CMC 4 Gmail Chat Transcript MC: {flifiigrmfiflfigiflflvIE ‘? [Can you see the characters I have typed?] SH: we must use english here. [teacher] can see if we chat or not MC: CALLLLLL ME SH: now what u wanna writ about? MC: CALLLLLL ME!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!! MC: call me i will talk to you SH: on phone ‘? MC: yes Immediately following MC’s response of “yes,” the entire chat was deleted from the Googledoc. There were several autosaves in which no revisions were recorded and then, in the next recorded revision, the pair had already begun to compose the introduction of their essay. This finding led me to check what the other pairs were doing during their homework assignment. Of the nine remaining pairs participating in the fourth essay, only one other did not use a combination of Gmail and/or Googledocs to discuss the essay. It 73 appears that, at least for these students, CMC may have been an obstacle to completing the task. While the exact percentages for MC and her partners in a F 2F context differ from EU's, the general distribution is the same. That is, the focus of both MC and EU seems to have been the same across the F2F writing tasks. In both tasks, MC spent the most time generating ideas, followed by LREs, structure, reading, task clarification, and finally task management (Refer to Table 10). Of note is the extremely large variation for both environments for MC and her partners. Effect of planning, collaboration, and peer revising an essay. For MC's first essay written over the intemet, she and her partner, AA, chatted in Gmail about the essay for a total of 23 minutes before beginning to compose the essay. Although they were chatting for a lengthy period of time, they shared relatively few turns. This was due mostly to the fact that they were already discussing their thesis after only five turns. This exchange is transcribed below in Excerpt 5. These transcripts show that the pair is brainstorming together rather than separately, but that this brainstorming is somewhat dominated by MC. Excerpt 5 Transcripts of MC CMC 2 AA: but what do you think the thesis should be? MC: What about write on the beautiful horse? I got some ideas and examples AA: cool. now let's brainstorm and we will discuss later MC: firstly, we need to claim how the authors opinion on it. I think the author didnot think its not good AA: why? MC: the tone and the name of the story shows a beautiful experience AA: aha and we need a support and example. what about the thesis? MC: I think the idea of Aram indicate the opion of the author, do u think so? like everybody think Mourad is crazy, but Aram said he donnot think so 74 Excerpt 5 (cont’d). and you can look at the P195 the bottom of it\ the paragraph begin with "Well, itseems... I suggest here indicate the opnion of the author AA: oh yeah MC: so, in the essay, we r going to contrast our opinion, which is we think this action is regarded as stealing, do u think is good? AA: ok. I will think about good thesis now :) After quickly establishing their thesis, the pair moved in to the Googledocs and began writing. Although MC wrote that they should move on to the document page where they could keep brainstorming and writing, once on that page, she proceeds to write the essay. At no point before beginning to write the essay did the pair divide the task. Rather, MC began writing the essay and plowed through it. Typing directly into the Googledoc, AA asked MC to share her ideas with him after she had finished the introductory paragraph so that he could help more, but MC did not respond to his request for several minutes. Finally, she indicated that he could "write some on the story" while she would "write [their] opinion on it first" and then they could both edit and combine the parts. Several times during the ninety minutes, AA made similar attempts to chat with MC, most of which were either ignored at length or entirely. Table 11 below outlines the revisions that AA and MC made to their essay. Unlike EU and her partner, MC and AA only had one text-based change. Across the categories, however, the two pairs seem to have been remarkably similar in terms of the number of revisions (EU total = 31; MC = 41) and the way in which they were made (i.e., by one of the partners). 75 Table l 1 Peer Revision & Edits MC CMC 2 MC AA Pair Total Instances Words Instances Words Instances Words Surface Changes Spelling 0 O O 0 0 0 Tense/num/mod O 0 O 0 O 0 Punctuation 2 0 2 0 4 0 Format 1 l 2 2 3 3 Total 3 1 4 2 7 3 Meaning-Preserving Changes Additions 1 7 3 8 3 4 20 42 Deletions 7 1 2 1 1 8 1 3 Substitutions 2 2 O O 2 2 Permutations 0 0 O 0 O O Distributions 0 0 0 O O O Consolidations l 1 0 O 1 1 Total 27 53 4 5 31 58 Text-based changes Additions 1 l O 0 0 0 Total 1 1 0 0 1 l Although MC and SH did not spend much time chatting, compared to EU CMC 4, they made considerable revisions and edits to their essay. These revisions did not include any text-based changes, but they did include many additions and deletions that improved the essay's coherence and unity. Table 12 found below summarizes the documented revisions. Table 12 Peer Revision & Edits MC CMC 4 MC SH Pair Total Instances Words Instances Words Instances Words Surface Changes Spelling 0 O O O 0 0 76 Table 12 (cont’d). Tense/num/mod 0 0 0 0 0 0 Punctuation 3 0 0 0 3 0 Format 4 0 l 0 5 0 Meaning-Preserving Changes Additions 19 21 0 0 19 21 Deletions 5 6 1 l 6 7 Substitutions 2 8 0 0 2 8 Permutations l 0 0 0 1 0 Distributions 1 0 0 0 1 0 Consolidations 2 2 0 0 2 2 Text-based changes Additions 0 0 0 O 0 0 Deletions 0 0 0 0 0 0 Substitutions 0 0 0 0 0 0 Permutations 0 0 0 0 0 0 Distributions 0 0 0 0 0 0 Consolidations 0 0 0 0 0 0 Holistic score. As seen in Table 13 below, MC received fairly consistent holistic scores across the environments. Her mean score for CMC was M= 2.25 while her mean score for F2F was slightly higher at M= 2.63. Total rater agreement on MC's scores was 90.6%. Table 13 MC Holistic Essay Scores by Environment CMC MC Essay 2 MC Essay 4 M SD Rater 1 1.5 2.5 Rater 2 2.5 2.5 M 2 2.5 2.25 0.35 % Rater agree 75 100 F2F MC Essay 1 MC Essay 3 Rater 1 2.5 2.5 77 Table 13 (cont’d) Rater 2 2.5 3 M 2.5 2.75 2.63 0.18 % Rater agree 100 87.5 Environment: CMC versus F 2F As previously discussed and as is visible in Table 14 below, the content of student talk varied for students depending on the task environment. A 2-tailed independent t-test reveals that these differences are significant. The case-study participants, on average spent more time generating ideas in a F 2F environment (M= 50.9, SE= 6.39) than they did in a CMC environment (M= 18.31, SE = 6.21). This difference was significant t(6) = 3.66, p= 0.011). Cohen's d = 0.38 indicates this significance has a medium effect size on the population. On average, these participants also spent more time exchanging LREs (M= 18.97, SE= 3.47) in F 2F than CMC (M= 0.76, SE= .76) which was also significant t(6) = 5.13, p= 0.002 with a medium effect size (Cohen's d =0.32). Finally, these participants tended to spend more time on average discussing organization (M= 13.69, SE= 1.66) when F2F than when collaborating online (M= 1.59, SE= 1.08) which was significant t(6) = 6.11, p= 0.001). Cohen's d for organization indicated a smaller effect size d= 0.28. On the other hand, case-study participants spent more time on average managing the time in a CMC context (M= 62.04, SE= 4.61) than when sitting next to their partner (M= 5.6, SE= 0.84) which was also significant t(3.l99)= -12.03, p= 0.001) with a small effect size of d=0.16. While it is true that students, on average, spent more time clarifying the task in a CMC environment (M= 17.3, SE= 6.53) than in F2F (M= 2.01, SE= 1.29) this difference was not significant t(3.235) = -2.298, p= 0.099. 78 $52,286 8: .2 .33 has 23:53.32» D35 2% 5 2325.85 3 B: 338 M5383... .333» “zaumbxmi ... omd hmd dddd OWN SN mddfi W: couwoctflv vial—t 02d wdd _.. Edd we; 9m mad vodo 80:53:34 23,—. -- -- -- 3m 3; -- -- :wcfiam wmd Ed * Sod mm.m ood— mfim om; 20332890 de 02d *mddd wad no.2 mm; cud mmmq wmd 2d 1 5d wn.m_ ddm NYE 3.3 .233 $58250 9m. 2 QM. E B 95:00 % 2237K HEELS 130.7020 BostoxEtm 3 $65 EthMNQ atom: 385 3 033. 79 Quality of end products The third research question asked whether the task environment had any apparent effect on the quality of the end product. In order to answer this question two raters gave holistic grades on the eight student essays. The scores are listed in Table 15 below. Table 15 Holistic Ratings and Percent Rater Agreement CMC EU Essay 1 EU Essay 4 MC Essay 2 MC Essay 4 Rater 1 3 2 1.5 2.5 Rater 2 2.5 2 2.5 2.5 M 2.75 2 2 2.5 % Rater agree 87.5 100 75 100 F 2F EU Essay 2 EU Essay 3 MC Essay 1 MC Essay 3 Rater 1 3 3.5 2.5 2.5 Rater2 2.5 3.5 2.5 3 M 2.75 3.5 2.5 2.75 % Rater agree 87.5 100 100 87.5 Total % rater agree 92.2 These essays were graded based on the holistic rubric developed by the teacher. The grades are reported on a 4.0 scale (with 2.5 being a passing grade). Percent rater agreement was determined by dividing the 4.0 scale on the grade intervals of 0.5. This interval was chosen because it was the standard grade interval used in the class and the language center for reporting of all grades. Dividing 100% (total agreement possible) by the possible intervals (eight) resulted in a 12.5% agreement for each 0.5 grade interval. In other words, raters both giving a 3.0 score would have 100% agreement whereas two ratings of 2.5 and 3.0 would only have a percent agreement of 87.5%. A calculation of 80 mean rater agreement for all 8 essays revealed a high rater agreement of 92.2% (Refer to Table 15 above). Raw scores (Table 15 above) indicated that while MC’s performance across tasks remained fairly consistent, EU’s performance did not remain as stable. This could be because of the great variance in collaboration with each of the partners. Both raters agreed that the essay produced by EU and her partner in task 3 was of a higher quality than any of her other 3 essays. Table 16 below shows the mean essay scores by environment. An independent t-test revealed that, on average, EU and MC received higher holistic ratings in a F2F environment (M = 2.87, SE = 0.22) than in a CMC environment (M = 2.31, SE = 0.19). This difference, however, was not significant t(6) = 1.96, p = 0.097. Table 16 Mean Holistic Scores by Environment M SD P-value Cohen's d CMC 2.31 0.38 0.097 1.6 F 2F 2.87 0.43 Summary In summary, survey responses indicated that students were generally neutral to the task and the environment. Students noted some benefits as well as challenges to both the collaborative writing task itself and the use of the new tool, Googledocs. Content of the case-studies' student talk varied by context, although it seems that there was variation across pairs as to how, exactly, the conversations were framed. Similarly, these pairs varied in how they decided to work together, whether attempting to participate in a collaborative or cooperative effort. In the CMC environment, cooperation seemed to be 81 the norm for the case-study participants; in the F2F environment, the type of collaboration varied depending on the individuals in the group. Overall, it appears that case-study participants in the F2F environment tended to spend more time revising each other's ideas and sentences, in the form of oral reformulations. Students in the CMC environment made fewer changes to their partner's section of the essay with some groups choosing to make virtually no edits at all and other groups displaying unequal editing behaviors between partners. Although differences in holistic grades were noticed in the data subset (indicating some environmental effects on the overall quality of the essay in favor of a F2F context), no significant differences were found. 82 CHAPTER FIVE CONCLUSION, IMPLICATIONS, & FUTURE RESEARCH In the attempt to tease out the transferability of Googledocs as a CSCL, the present study addressed four research questions investigating the writing processes of students using the word processing interface in two different task environments, face-to- face and synchronous computer-mediated communication. This investigation included an overview of all participants' perceptions of the tasks and an in-depth look at the nature of two case-study participants' collaboration and how that collaboration functioned within both task environments. It also examined the quality of the final products created. This study’s findings shed some light on the potential application of a hitherto unexamined intemet communication tool (ICT) as a computer supported collaborative learning (CSCL) tool for the second and/or foreign language classroom. Transferability of Googledocs as a CSCL In accordance with the interactionist vein of SLA, a successful computer- supported collaborative learning tool would ideally facilitate collaborative interaction, either in the form of providing opportunities for input, output, and language negotiation (cognitive interactionism) or for scaffolding (sociocultural interactionism). If not facilitating these, a CSCL ought to, at the very least, make such interactions possible. The four current research questions attempt to tap into the interactions facilitated and/or made possible with Googledocs. CMC Googledocs: A Truly Collaborative T 001 ? Although attempts were made to promote negotiation with closed-ended tasks (Blake, 2000; Pellettieri, 2000) and discussion with “text-responsible” prompts (Leki & Carson, 1997), there was little evidence to support the contention that students engaged in 83 significant or meaningful interaction in the CMC environment as compared to the F 2F environment. In fact, mean responses fi'om the survey indicated that students noted less conflict on average while interacting in the CMC tasks (M= 2.09, SE= 0.21) than in the F2F (M= 2.36, SE= 0.29) although neither environment reported an overly disruptive conflict. Previous literature would therefore imply that none of these students were engaged in significant negotiations over the text. However, the student's open-ended questions might point to quite the opposite. The majority of the students noted some level of discussion of the topics, whether the source of the discussion being from a point of agreement or disagreement. What weight should be lent these answers remains questionable since students were well aware that they were being graded on their level of participation in the activities. Whether or not the students were interacting with each other by scrolling up and down and re-reading their partner’s composition cannot be known because of the lack of screen captures and eye-tracker information. In terms of the content of student talk, the actions of the F2F student pairs were congruent with previous research. Although the precise percentage of time spent on generating ideas differed across student pairs, this category accounted for the largest total time for each pair. The average time spent on generating ideas across the four F2F essays was 50.8%, remarkably similar to the 53% Storch (2005) noted. The second largest category, language related episodes, was also in-line with previous studies (Storch, 2005; Cummings, 1989 as referenced in Storch, 2005). Irrespective of student-reports, chat transcripts and Googledocs revisions of the case-study participants (if assumed to be somewhat representative of the rest of the class) tentatively suggest that there was minimal interaction between students in the CMC 84 environment as opposed to the F 2F. With a few noted exceptions (e. g. the pairs in task 4 that chatted to exchange ideas throughout the writing process), student interaction tended to be reserved for the beginning and the end of the writing task and then, when it occurred, did not offer much in the way of language negotiation (LREs) or scaffolding as visible through Table 9 and Table 12 detailing the content of student talk in the CMC environments. Unlike the F2F groups, these student pairs spent most of their chatting time in dividing the task. That is, they quickly established their thesis without fleshing out how it would be developed and supported before delegating tasks and beginning to write; they merely began writing with a backbone of the task in mind. The development and support for the delegated parts were left up to the individual student. While certainly more frequent in the CMC setting, it is necessary to note that several F2F pairs also spent time brainstorming and/or writing independently. Student surveys report that one difficult aspect of the CMC environment was that it required them to explain themselves, certainly not an easy task. Both EU and MC reported difficulties in negotiating their thoughts regardless of the task environment, though they certainly noted that the "lean" medium (Smith, et. a1, 2003, p. 706) afforded by the CMC world was the more difficult of the two. The students’ accounts that the CMC environments were more difficult may indicate that there is a greater potential for learning precisely because it forced the students to negotiate without the aid of non- verbal cues like gestures. EU noted her lack of typing skills as a source of frustration, while MC considered writing with another person her major source of frustration. MC reported thinking it was boring, inefficient, and difficult to write with another person. Oddly enough, neither student's claim of significant negotiation can be corroborated in 85 the chat transcripts. In fact, there was relatively little negotiation of the text or the individual writer's Opinion. This lack of engagement in discussing the meaning and their interpretation of the text can be contrasted with the discussions that students engaged in during the F2F tasks. But, even if there had been substantial communication, there is no indication that these students would have appreciated the task any more. It seems that, for the CMC environment at least, merely making text-responsible writing prompts will not suffice to ensure that students will take advantage of the opportunity to interact with each other to discuss the text (as indicated by Leki & Carson, 1997). This is not to say that such negotiations are impossible; certainly they are quite possible with the chatting technology currently available. However, mere availability does not imply that students will take advantage of the situation, especially when the task has a pre-determined endpoint that may foster impatience with the task and the benefits it may offer. Kessler and Bikowski (2010) have remarked that, collaboration leads to a sense of ownership that encourages extensive utilization of the learning space. . . students begin to use the space in ways that are meaningful to them but unanticipated by instructors or designers. Of course, some students may not immediately recognize the potential of these spaces and the interaction that they offer. (p. 55) This seems to be precisely the case, especially when noting student comments that writing online was "uncomfortable" (EU CMC essay 1) or difficult because he/she had to explain his/her thoughts rather than being able to express them non-verbally in a F2F pair. Psychologically, we all seem resistant to those very things that will improve us. 86 Both SE and EU think that the chatting online is more difficult because they are forced to articulate their ideas, yet they both seem to experience similar difficulty when they enter the F2F environment. Furthermore, what they identify as being most difficult about writing with another person is exactly what the research has shown to be beneficial to students. That is, the more opportunities students have to interact in meaningful ways, the better it is for their language development. What has been considered meaningful is an interaction that requires more than a few word response (Swain & Lapkin, 1998) such as text-responsible discussions where students are prompted to discuss and defend their interpretations of an authentic text (Leki & Carson, 1997) or topics that instigate conflict and thus the defense of one's opinions (Dale, 1994). Perhaps students would have been more amenable to the new environment had they felt like it were more similar to F2F meetings (i.e., seemingly more natural to them). For example, if the students had been using the voice and/or video chat features now available on Gmail instead of the thin medium of the text-only chat. This is not to say that there was no collaborative writing. Towards the end of the assigned time, most pairs engaged in peer-revising and editing. This stage usually involved some surface-level changes, and review of the finished product revealed many morphosyntactic errors that went unaddressed. Previous literature supports this lack of attention to form of students in a CMC environment (Arnold, Ducate, & Kost, 2009; Kessler, 2009). It remains to be seen whether most of the students reacted as AL (EU’s partner in CMC essay 1) and SH (MC's partner in CMC essay 4) who both indicated noticing sections of their partner's writing riddled with morphosyntactic errors and/or unclear ideas. AL stated that the point when "you tried to look at the other part of the 87 essay which is write by your partner, there are lots of grammar errors and some sentences doesn't make sense" was "the most difficult part" of the collaborative task. What is interesting here is that neither AL nor SH followed up their confusion with a negotiation, either in the chat window or the Googledoc. While it is not particularly surprising that students did not report experiencing any of the benefits of saving face or reduced affective factors normally associated with an anonymous CMC technology, it is surprising that students did not resolve their confusion for the sake of their grade. Of course, the lack of resolving conflict is not unheard of in the CALL world. The text-medium and the ability to scroll up and down might make a verbalized resolution unnecessary (e.g., Smith, 2008). One would imagine that students, conscious of the fact that they would be graded on the intelligibility of their partner's work, would be motivated to produce the clearest essay possible. Such lack of negotiation suggests a lack of co-ownership of the essay. Arnold, Ducate, and Kost (2009) similarly found that students working collaboratively on a wiki did not exhibit a sense of co-ownership of the wiki pages because they never engaged in deletions to another's work. Student questionnaire responses raise other possible reasons for this lack of revision to a peer's work. One student reported the desire to stop and help her partner get back on track, but this would have been an interruption. She wrote, "I wish then I cannot writing myself and meantime know what my partner's writing, so that I can't immediately tell my partner like 'this sentences are not good, or did not support very well,‘ but if I do it, I feel will interrupt my mind to write" (KA, CMC essay 1). So, we return to the idea of efficiency. Pausing during the writing process to comment on a peer's work is inefficient because it disrupts the creative flow; and, in a timed, graded situation, this is 88 less than ideal. In this sense the CMC environment, specifically because it makes it possible to ignore a partner, may be better suited to getting out a drafi of an essay. Such was the case for several students who wrote that the CMC environment mitigated the effects of the interruptions because it allowed them to ignore the chatting of their partner (a fact they thought to be negative, however). "Sometime I did not saw his message to me immediately" (MC, CMC essay 2). It should be noted, however, that the Googledoc revisions attest to the fact that usually at least 1 student in each pair took responsibility for attending to revisions in both sections of the essay. Although these revisions included some morphosytactic corrections, when made, most of these revisions were meaning-preserving additions and substitutions that added to the piece's overall cohesion. The case-study pairs' revision behaviors supported previous findings on the revision behaviors of wikis (Arnold, Ducate, & Kost, 2009; Mak & Coniam, 2008). Of the case-study pairs, only 2 surveys failed to mention some concern about connecting the two parts of the essay. Many of the students noted that this was one of the more difficult aspects of the collaborative task, especially in the CMC environment where the two sections might have drifted away from the agreed upon thesis. Although these revision behaviors were certainly less frequent than was hoped for, their presence during the writing process may still be promising. Recall that Dale (1994) claimed that viewing another person's cognitive processes unfold was helpful in furthering a students' own cognitive abilities and that Googledocs, as a synchronous tool, allows students to view their peer's writing evolve in real time. In this light, whether or not a revision was to a student's own work or to the work of his/her peer, the possibility 89 that his/her peer could witness these changes still exists. That is, by watching Googledocs, one peer is privvy to the cognitive processes of his/her peer. Unfortunately, from the current data, only a few students reported noticing the skills of their partners. AA (MC's partner in CMC essay 2) wrote, "My partner's writing is very impressive and I liked the way she writes and she is fast and have a great ideas and supports." MC wrote that with Googledocs "it will be easier if my partner can see every time I edit and tell me his idea immediately." Several student comments about the difficulty to see the partner's edits and/or chats throws into doubt whether students were able to notice and/or actually did notice their peer's writing iterations. While pinpointing the precise source of the relative lack of interaction from the current data is a fairly impossible task, there are several possible explanations for this phenomenon. The first lies in how students tend to task themselves during writing sessions. It almost goes without saying that because the context was a timed and graded writing assignment, students would attempt to find the most expeditious way to complete the required task while exerting the least possible effort. Previous research conducted by Kessler and Bikowski (2010) perfectly illustrates this fact. In a semester-long wiki study, student participation fell into several categories, the largest of which accounted for a slight majority of students: these students "engaged marginally" in the activity (p. 46). Only a very few students engaged beyond the assignment's minimal requirements. This operational axiom was most likely compounded by the nature of the medium and the task themselves; the question then becomes to what extent is this true? Recall that Googledocs is an interface that allows for a division of labor because it is, by nature, a synchronous collaborative tool. It would make little sense, then, to use a tool built 90 specifically to increase efficiency in the inefficient manner of working together through each stage of the writing process. Previous research on wikis reflects this perspective on the medium as efficient (Arnold, Ducate, & Kost, 2009). Storch's (2005) research on F 2F collaborative writing reported similar student perceptions of a collaborative writing task as being more efficient than working alone. Similarly, the results could be a result of how students task themselves in a timed— writing situation. The case-study participants and their partners mentioned time on a number of occasions, a fact that speaks to effect time might have had. These comments ranged from gentle reminders of the time to demands that their peers hurry up. The time- consciousness (or even anxiety) probably prompted students to be as efficient as possible in order to get the assignment done. Time, in combination with the fact that students generally perceived the CMC task to be more difficult, could have created a significant cognitive and affective load for the students. It is interesting that students generally interacted in this manner. It seems that students were able to envision just one possible way to accomplish the writing task. Perhaps this is an artifact of the medium in the sense that division of labor is possible in a CMC environment, but not so much in a F2F one. However, student responses from surveys indicated that students in a F2F environment also ended up dividing some of the tasks, such as brainstorming and editing. Another explanation could be what Arnold, Ducate, and Kost (2009) found in a collaborative wiki project: that students did not display qualities of equal ownership of the collaborative document (i.e., once students wrote part of the wiki, other students left it alone). These behaviors often resulted in repetitive sections, rather than co-owned consolidated topics. Student responses in the 91 current study lend merit to this possibility. Several students mentioned noticing errors or non-unified parts of their partner’s section, but they chose to leave these parts alone. Another probable cause for the division of labor and lack of collaboration was the context of the assignment. The class was well-practiced in discussing literature in literature circles. This type of student-led discussion requires that each student be responsible for a certain aspect of the discussion whether that be content or lexical. Thus, students might very well have thought that this was the type of collaboration that was expected of them. The effect of focusing on the division of task in conjunction with the lack of co- generating of ideas meant that essays were less cohesive and unified than in the F2F environment, although it is promising that students report noticing this deficiency in their own writing. In this sense, CMC environment might be beneficial as it might have helped students to conceptualize what, exactly, an essay lacking unity would look like. Thus, by participating intimately in the writing process with another person, students were able to step outside of their own writing in order to visualize the need for unity--a concept not easily grasped by many L2 learners. Kessler and Bikowski (2010) have echoed a similar lack of synthesis and cohesion in student-led collaborative writing. Pedagogical Implications ofGoogledocs The nature of the interactions in this CMC context raises a unique set of opportunities and challenges that educators ought to ponder when considering implementing Googledocs in the classroom. Though tentative at best, these data suggest an underwhelming response to the tool. Perhaps student reactions were lukewarm because the writing assignments were graded requirements for the course. As often 92 seems the case, grades tend to make otherwise new and interesting activities seem like drudgery. Or, perhaps the tool did not feel at all new or exciting simply because it so closely resembled Microsoft Word. Thus, students who did not buy into my claim that chatting online to compose an essay was fun or, at the very least, helpful for promoting language development, did not even have the novelty of a new tool to excite them. This is in stark contrast with a slew of CMC research where students are ofien engaging in ways that do feel novel, and therefore, exciting to them (e.g., wikis in Arnold et al., 2009). Additionally, unlike many authentic uses of CMC tools like blogs and wiki, learners in this study were not writing for a real audience. This is, perhaps, an inauthentic use of the tool which could explain why students did not seemed enthused about its incorporation into the course’s syllabus. More than being uninteresting, MC's survey responses provide further explanation that some students might have been viewed Googledocs as an inferior tool. MC mentioned that some of the word processing abilities, namely, the spell and grammar check, and the formatting of the deceptively large text block were not as accurate or easy to use as Microsoft Word. While she expressed some fi'ustration at this, she also mentioned that it was a good test for her abilities because the crutches she normally relied on, were not as refined in Googledocs. Other students, however, were not so amenable to being forced to relinquish their much-loved scaffolds. SE (EU's partner, CMC essay 2), for example, complained that the writing task was impossible because she couldn't use a dictionary. Furthermore, some students were resistant to the perceived difficulty of the task. Because the current data indicate that students are resistant to such work, regardless 93 of the task environment, language instructors need to make it clear to students that, though difficult, it is precisely the struggle that will further their language development. Language instructors may also find themselves wondering what benefits lay in the integration of Googledocs, since little evidence has been given to support claims that it promotes substantial peer interaction. As previously stated, some students might be attuned to the actions of their partner and may, thus, learn simply by observing what linguistic moves a more skilled learner makes when revising the essay. Additionally, analysis showed that CMC pairs allotted more than half of the student talk-time to task management (M= 62.04%). Excerpts examined above indicate that pairs engaged in extensive task management before dividing the tasks of writing the essay. While may not offer learners any ostensible benefits, significant task clarification in the form of text may have been partly responsible for the longer texts produced by the CMC pairs. Unlike previous wiki literature (Arnold et al., 2009), the delegation of tasks in Googledocs made it possible for students to work together on one piece of writing without the negative interferences of self-elected leaders as noted in the F2F pairs. Also of note in the CMC environment is that students might be more likely to articulate their thoughts about the prompt and do so in English because they are acutely aware of the teacher’s presence in the CMC space. Out of all of the student groups, it appears that only one CMC pair relied on its L1 (MC & SH CMC essay 4) while several F 2F pairs spent at least some time code-switching (e. g. MC & LA F2F essay 1; EU & CH F2F essay 2). It seems that Googledocs in a CMC environment, tended towards fostering creativity. Essays for both case-studies written over the intemet had higher word count (M=689 words), on average, than when in the F2F environment (M= 570 words). This 94 higher number reinforces the claim that students were more freely writing in their own sections of the essay than the monitored compositions of the F 2F groups. However, while students in the CMC context tended to foster rather than stifle creativity, this was certainly not fool-safe. The "free-rider effect" that frustrated SE in F 2F essay 2 did not disappear in the CMC environment (Johnson & Johnson, 1994 as referenced in Arnold et al., 2009). Although it was perhaps less conspicuous, MC still mentioned in her last survey (for essay 4) that her partner in essay 2 had, after ninety-minutes, contributed a mere sentence or two. This free-rider effect negatively affected the group dynamic in both tasks, but perhaps more so in a F 2F environment where it was more difficult to hide a lack of input. It has been my experience that students struggle with cohesion and unity in their own writing. Whereas they can easily‘identify techniques of cohesion and lack of unity in textbook exercises, they have difficulty putting their receptive knowledge to productive use. Perhaps, then, Googledocs could serve as a classroom too] where students' awareness is raised thereby drawing their attention to the need for cohesive devices. If this CMC exchange were the first in a series of writing sessions, then Googledocs would present a potential opportunity for such experiential learning. These latter discussions could be carried out F2F or in the CMC environment, employing the chat features of Gmail. Perhaps students would, after these initial individual contributions noted in the current research, participate more collaboratively in later writing sessions, as Kessler and Bikowski (2010) observed in their participants' creation of a semester-long wiki. 95 Such group work might function to benefit the whole, a necessary but ofien absent component of most writing groups that participate in peer review; that is, in a traditional peer review group, the individual has composed a piece that is then workshopped, not for the benefit of the group but for the benefit of the individual's work (Carson & Nelson (1994). Unlike this, the Googledoc has been a result of (at the very least) cooperation. If it then undergoes multiple drafts and peer reviews by the members of the original group, students may feel a greater sense of ownership, benefit, and investment. F Summary and Future Research. If teachers intend to use Googledocs in the classroom as a synchronous collaborative tool, they ought to prepare themselves for possibility that learners might l! engage in cooperation rather than collaboration (Haythomthwaite, 2006). While cooperation is not negative in itself, the type witnessed in the current research revealed minimal amounts of surface-level interaction. Student commentary on the difficulty of seeing their peer’s writing, chatting, and edits make it hard to claim that students are observing and/or benefitting from observing the writing process of their peers. Although the findings are certainly tentative, there is little evidence to support the integration of Googledocs as a computer supported collaborative learning tool, either from a cognitive or sociocultural standpoint. The use of an eye-tracker in conjunction with Googledoc revision history might prove useful in fully understanding whether or not students are noticing the linguistic moves their partners make. Perhaps the true value of the Googledocs, however may lie in its ability to facilitate efficient division of labor in peer groups thereby functioning as a production tool whose product could be used in later collaborations. A telling follow-up study 96 would be to have students participate in Googledocs as an ungraded, untirned assignment that was explicitly linked to other course assignments. One possibility might be to have students work together as they did in the current study. After each pair created a draft, they could work together (either in F2F or CMC) to revise the draft further, focusing on development of ideas and cohesion/unity. That is, teachers could exploit Googledocs for its benefits of efficiency and use it as the starting point for a collaboratively written multi-draft essay. It would be interesting to see whether students would respond well to collaborative work in a long-term assignment where they were equally invested in the assignment (Carson & Nelson, 1994) as well as to see if the multiple-iterations approach would result in an evolution of the collaborative process, as Arnold, Ducate, Lomicka, and Lord (2009) found in their study of a semester-long class wiki. Another study that would shed light on Googledocs would be to look carefully at the few groups who chatted throughout the task; that is, the students who might have truly collaborated using Googledocs as their interface. A better grasp of these interactions may prove useful in grasping what teachers can do to promote collaboration in the language classroom. Limitations Although the current study does offer some insight into how students engage in a collaborative writing task while using the Googledocs interface, it has numerous limitations that need to be acknowledged. First, because it was a qualitative study, one must take care in generalizing the results to a wider population. Second, as already noted, although an attempt has been made to nest this research in an understandable context, these data come from a few individual writing tasks of a small sample of 97 convenience. Any claims of generalizability were significantly reduced when the plan for counterbalancing the tasks was made impossible. And, while an attempt was made to seek second ratings on the holistic essays, the remainder of the coded data did not get second-rated. Furthermore, recreating the essay construction in Googledocs made for a particularly time consuming task such that a widespread analysis of many students proved impossible. Internal validity may have been compromised as the researcher was also the teacher and, at times, the needs of the students overrode experimental concerns. Finally, external validity may have been compromised by the timed nature of the writing activity. 98 -“""_-"“="-—n APPENDIX 99 ’1'» ..w a...” . I APPENDIX A Appendix A. Holistic rubric for essays CONTENT Teacher Comments DEVELOPMENT ORGANIZATION LANGUAGE Address the prompt Shows understanding of and reflection on the story? Specific examples and/or details from the text Explain how examples and/or details are relevant Logical Ideas are clear and easy to understand Coherence & Unity (in sentence, between sentence, between paragraphs) Vocabulary Grammar 100 APPENDIX B Appendix B. Survey for Task 1 -3 1. Self and Partner-Assessment 1.) I believe that my group participation grade for today is (circle one): 1.0 (bad/needs LOT of improvement) 3.0 (good enough to get a good grade) 2.) I believe this to be true because . . . (Explain) 2.0 (needs some improvement) 4.0 (excellent—it was the best) 3.) Today, I believe that MY PARTNER’S participation grade for today is (circle one): 1.0 (bad/needs LOT of improvement) 3.0 (good enough to get a good grade) 5.) I believe this to be true because . . . (Explain) 11. Activity Assessment 2.0 (needs some improvement) 4.0 (excellent—it was the best) Strongly Somewhat Neutral Somewhat Strongly Disagree Disagree Agree Agree 1. It was difficult because 1 2 3 4 5 my partner and I couldn’t agree. 2. I loved this activity. 1 2 3 4 5 3. I thought this 1 2 3 4 5 101 assignment was fun. 4. I think that writing 1 2 3 4 5 with another person is good language practice. 5. I liked sitting next to 1 2 3 4 5 my partner and working. 6. I would like to do more 1 2 3 4 5 in-class assignments like this one. 7. I had a hard time 1 2 3 4 5 writing this essay because we had to use a computer. I would have done a better job if it were hand- written. 8. I feel like I learned 1 2 3 4 5 something today. 9. I liked using Google 1 2 3 4 5 Documents to compose my essay. III. Short answer: 1. Was it easy to communicate with your partner? If yes, why? If no, why not? 2. While working on the essay, what were you thinking and feeling? 3. What did you write about? Did you find it difficult to decide how to write the essay? 102 4. If you found it difficult, what made it difficult? 5. Describe the writing process. What did you find easy? Hard? 6. What do you think about communicating with your partner this way? 7. Would you want to do more similar activities? Why or why not? 8. In the future, would you prefer to write with the same tools as you used today? Why or why not? 103 APPENDIX C Sample Prompt (Essay 1) Based on your reading, reflections, class work, and discussions about Chopin’s “Story of An Hour” and Collier’s “The Chaser,” work together with your partner to write an essay on the following topic: The portrayal of love in each story is different. Describe how the author portrays love in each story. Use examples from the stories to support your claim. Then, compare/contrast it to your own beliefs, thoughts, and/or feelings on what love should be. Use personal experience (illustrations and anecdotes) to support and explain your beliefs. As always, remember to be clear and concise in your ideas. Good Luck! Sample Prompt (Essay 2) Based on your reading, reflections, class work, and discussions about Saroyan's "A Beautiful White Horse" OR Jackson’s “The Lottery” work together with your partner to write an essay on morality. Describe how the author defines morality and what conclusion he/she comes to about the actions of his/her characters (i.e. were the character’s actions moral in the author’s opinion?) Use examples from the story to support your claim. Then, compare/contrast it to your own beliefs, thoughts, and/or feelings on whether or not the character’s actions were moral. Use personal experience (illustrations and anecdotes) as well as class discussion and any outside knowledge you may have to support and explain your beliefs. As always, remember to be clear and concise in your ideas. Good Luck! 104 APPENDIX D DISCUSSION DIRECTOR Name Group Book Assignment pages _ to _ You are the discussion director. Your job is to write down some good questions that you think your group will want to talk about. This is NOT a quizll Your task is to help your group discuss the big ideas of the reading (book, article, passage or poem). Usually the best discussion topics come from your thoughts, feelings and questions as you read. Possible discussion questions or topics for today: How... If... Some general questions to start a discussion: What did you think about this section? Can someone summarize briefly? Were you surprised by anything in this section? What questions do you have after reading this section? What is one important idea for you from this section? During the discussion, make sure everyone has an equal chance to speak! Today I called upon the following people to share their ideas: Word Finder Passage Picker Connector 105 (SAMPLE) Other Responsibilities 1. Word Finding: Look for and write down at least two special words in the reading. Look for words that are new, different, strange, funny, interesting, important, difficult, or dialectal. Be prepared to share these with the group when the Word Finder asks you. Word Page Why I picked this word 2. Passage Picking: Find and choose one or two parts of the story that you want to read aloud to the group. Look for passages that may be funny, good, scary, sad, interesting, descriptive, surprising, or important. Be prepared to share these with the group when the Passage Picker asks you. Page Paragraph Why I liked this passage 3. Connecting: Find a connection between the reading and other readings and the outside world. Connect the reading to your own life, happenings, similar events, other people or problems, other stories or books you've read, etc. Be prepared to share this with the group when the Connector asks you. "Something today's reading reminded me of was . . . ' 106 WORD FINDER Name Group Book Assignment pages to You are the Word Finder. Your job is to look for special words in the reading. Look for words that are: 0 new interesting 0 different important 0 strange difficult 0 funny dialectal When you find a word that you want to talk about, mark it with a post-it note or write it down here: Word Page Why I picked this word When your group meets, help your friends talk about the words you have chosen. Some ideas you can discuss: How does this word fit into the passage? How important is it to the passage? Does anyone know what this word means? Shall we look it up in the dictionary? How do you use this word? Does anyone know other forms of this word —that is related forms? What do you feel when you read, hear or see this word? Can you draw this word? 107 PASSAGE PICKER Name Group Book Assignment pages _ You are the Passage Picker: Your job is to find and choose or pick parts of the story that you want to read aloud to your group, or want your group to think about. These passages may be: a good part an interesting part a. a funny part some good writing 5 . a scary part a good description ‘ i a sad part something surprising or important L Be sure to mark the parts you want to share with a post-it note or bookmark. You may also write down the page and paragraph numbers. In either case, make a note to yourself here WHY you liked it. i Parts to read out loud: Page Paragraph Why I liked it You may also ask someone else to read one of your passages aloud, and then you could ask the rest of the group why THEY THINK you chose that passage! 108 CONNECTOR Name Group Book Assignment pages to You are the Connector. Your job is to find connections between the reading you are assigned to discuss and other readings and the outside world. This means connecting the reading to: - your own life . happenings at school or locally : similar events in other places and at other times L other people or problems other books, stories or articles ' l other writings on the same topic other writings by the same author Below, write some notes to remind yourself, “Some things today’s reading reminded me of were. . .” 109 Reporter Date Name Group Book Assignment pages to You are the REPORTER. Your job is to take DETAILED notes on the entire discussion and to write a report of the accomplishments of the group that week. You will hand this in the following Monday. The report should carefully describe what your group talked about, the vocabulary you discussed and learned, the passages you read together, the connections you made, etc. The group may work on this at the end of group time. The report should be a minimum of 1.5 pages typed and double-spaced. The GROUP will receive a grade for the report each week. 110 APPENDIX E Name: Collaborative Writing Assignment 1: Directions A. This is a 1-hour 30-minute timed writing. You and your partner will work together to write this essay. Each of you will have your own computer, and you will be sharing the same Google Document on the Internet. To communicate with each other, you will use the GMAIL chat program in your email. As you begin this task, remember what we have discussed about the three steps of the writing process: pre-writing, writing, and proofreading. Your teacher will announce these to help keep you on track. B. You now need to log-in to your GMAIL account. To do this, follow the instructions below: 1. Go to www.gmail.com 2. Sign in to the Gmail account: USERNAME: PASSWORD: 3. Check your email “Inbox” for an email from cazarmosca82@gmail called “Assignment 1” 4. Click on the email. 5. Click on the blue link. 6. Your Google Document is now open. Type your CHAT name at the top of the page. Your CHAT NAME: 7. Minimize the Google Document and Maximize your email. Find your partner in the “Chat” box on the left of your email. Your PARTNER’S CHAT NAME: 8. Click on your partner’s name to open a chat box. C. During the writing time, you should have out copies of the short stories and any notes. Your instructor will hand out writing prompts. Keep the prompts face down until you are 111 instructed to turn them over. Once instructed, turn over your sheets, and begin pre- writing with your partner using Gmail Chat. D. After you have finished brainstorming, the two of you should begin writing your essay. Remember that both of you will be typing into the same document, so you will see what your partner is writing. BUT, in order to show your partner what you are writing, you must push the “SAVE” button. You should save your work every couple of minutes. Remember to WORK TOGETHER to compose the essay as I will be able to see what both of you write as well as the final product. E. When time is called, save your document once more and then log out. I will print off your document and grade it for you. 112 APPENDIX F Name: Collaborative Writing Assignment 1: Directions A. This is a l-hour 30-minute timed writing. You and your partner will work together to write this essay. You and your partner will be sitting next to each other and sharing a computer Google Document on the Internet. You should talk just as you would in class. As you begin this task, remember what we have discussed about the three steps of the writing process: pre-writing, writing, and proofreading. Your teacher will announce times to help keep you on track. B. You now need to log-in to your GMAIL account. To do this, follow the instructions below: 1.. Go to www.gmail.com 2. Sign in to the Gmail account: USERNAME: PASSWORD: 3. Check your email “Inbox” for an email from cazarmosca82@gmail called “Assignment 1” 4. Click on the email. 5. Click on the blue link. 6. Your Google Document is now open. Type you and your partner’s name at the top of the page. C. During the writing time, you should have out copies of the short stories and any notes. Your instructor will hand out writing prompts. Keep the prompts face down until you are instructed to turn them over. Once instructed, turn over your sheets, and begin pre- writing with your partner. You may use the scrap paper provided. D. After you have finished brainstorming, the two of you should begin writing your essay. Remember that both of you will be typing into the same document. You should save your work every couple of minutes. Remember to WORK TOGETHER to compose the essay. 113 E. When time is called, save your document once more and then log out. I will print off your document and grade it for you. YOU SHOULD WRITE AS MUCH AS YOU CAN, AS WELL AS YOU CAN! GOOD LUCK! YOU NOW SHOULD TURN ON YOUR VOICE RECORDER BY PUSHING “RECORD” AND BEGIN PRE-WRITING OUT LOUD. 114 REFERENCES 115 REFERENCES Arnold, N. (2007). Technology-mediated learning 10 years later: Emphasizing pedagogical or utilitarian applications? Foreign Language Annals, 40(1), p. 161 -1 81. Arnold, N., Ducate, L., Kost, C. (2009). Collaborative writing in wikis: Insights from culture projects in German classes. In Lomicka, L. and Lord, G. (Eds.). The Next Generation: Social Networking and Online Collaboration in Foreign Language Learning (pp. 115-144). San Marcos, TX: Computer Assisted Language Instruction Consortium. Arnold, N., Ducate, L., Lomicka, L., & Lord, G. (2009). Assessing online collaboration among language teachers: A cross-institutional case study. Journal of Interactive Online Learning, 8(2), p. 121-139. Atkinson, D., & Ramanathan, V. (1995). Cultures of writing: An ethnographic comparison of L1 and L2 university writing/language programs. TESOL Quarterly, 29, 539—568. Bauer, B., deBenedette, L., Furstenberg, G., Levet, S., & Waryn, S. (2006). The Cultura project. In J. Belz & S. Thome (Eds), Internet-mediated intercultural foreign language education (pp. 31-62). Boston: Heinle & Heinle. Beauvois, M. H. (1992). Computer-assisted classroom discussion in the foreign language classroom: Conversation in slow motion. Foreign Language Annals, 25(5), 455-464. Beauvois, M. (1998). Conversations in slow motion: Computer-mediated communication in the foreign language classroom. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 54 (2), 198-217. Belz, J. A. (2003). Linguistic perspectives on the development of intercultural competence in telecollaboration. Language Learning & Technology, 7(2), 68-117. Bereiter, C. & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Blake, R. (2000). Computer mediated communication: A window on L2 Spanish interlanguage. Language Learning and Technology, 4(1), p. 120-136. Blake, R. (2001). What language professionals need to know about technology. ADFL Bulletin, 32(3), 93-99. http://web2.adfl.org/ADFL/bulletin/v32n3/323093.htrn 116 Blake, C. (2009). Potential of text-based intemet chats for improving oral fluency in a second language. The Modern Language Journal, 93, p. 227-240. Boehlke, O. (2003). A comparison of student participation levels by group size and language stages during chatroom and face-to-face discussions in German. CALICO Journal, 21 (l), p. 67-87. Braine, G. (2001). A study of English as a foreign language (EFL) writers on a local-area network (LAN) and in traditional classes. Computers and Composition, 18, p. 275—292. Carson, J. (2001). Second language writing and second language acquisition. In T. Silva, & P. Matsuda, (Eds), On Second Language Writing, (pp.l9l-l99). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Chopin, K. (1894). The story of an hour. Retrieved at http://www.vcu.edu/engweb/webtexts/hour/ Chun, D. M. (1994). Using computer networking to facilitate the acquisition of interactive competence. System, 22 (1), 17-31. Collier, J. (1952). The Chaser. In M. Crane (Ed.), 50 Great American Short Stories (557-560). New York: Random House. Connor, U. & Asenavage, K. (1994). Peer response groups in ESL writing classes: How much impact on revision? Journal of Second Language Writing, 3(3), 257-276. Connor, U. (1996). Contrastive Rhetoric: Cross Cultural Aspects of Second Language Writing. New York: Cambridge University Press. Dale, H. (1994). Collaborative writing interactions in one ninth-grade classroom. Journal of Educational Research, 87(6), 334-344. Darhower, M. (2002). Instructional features of synchronous computer-mediated communication in the intermediate L2 class: A sociocultural case study. CALICO Journal, 19(2), 249—278. DiMatteo, A. (1991). Under erasure: A theory for interactive writing in real time. Computers and Composition, 8, p. 71-84. Donato, R. (1994). Collective scaffolding. In J. P. Lantolf & G. Appel (Eds.), Vygotskian approaches to second language research (pp. 33-56). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishers. Eklundh, S. and Kollberg, P. (1996). In M. Levy and S. Ransdell, (Eds), The Science of Writing (pp.l63-188). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 117 Elola, I., & Oskoz, A. (2008). Blogging: Fostering intercultural competence development in foreign language and study abroad contexts. Foreign Language Annals, 41(3), 421-444. Erickson, C.O. (Producer), & Ramis, H. (Director). (1993). Groundhog Day [Motion picture]. USA: Columbia TriStar. Faigley, L., & Witte, S. (1981). Analyzing revision. College Composition and Communication, 32(4), 400-414. Feenberg, A. (1999). Distance learning: Promise or threat? [online]. National CrossTalk, Winter. Available: http://www.highereducation.org/crosstalk/ctOl 99/opinion01 99feenberg. shtml. Fischer, J. (2007). How do we know what students are actually doing? Monitoring students’ behavior in CALL. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 20(5), 409- 442. Flower, L. (1994). The construction of negotiated meaning: A social cognitive theory of writing. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press. Flower, L., & Hayes, J. (1980). The dynamics of composing: Making plans and juggling constraints. In L. Gregg, & E. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 31-50). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Flower, L. & Hayes, J. (1981). A cognitive process theory of writing. College Composition and Communication, 32, 365-387. Fortune, A. (2005). Learners' use of metalanguage in collaborative form-focused L2 output. Language Awareness, 14, 21-38. F ulkerson, R. (2005). Composition at the turn of the twenty-first century. College Composition and Communication, 56(4), 654-687. Gass, SM. (1997). Input, Interaction, and the Second Language Learner. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Goertler, S. & Winke, P. (2008). The effectiveness of technology-enhanced foreign language teaching. In S. Goertler & P. Winke (Eds.), Opening doors through distance language education: Principles, perspectives, and practices (pp. 233- 260). San Marcos, TX: CALICO. Googledocs Tour (2010). Retrieved from http://www.google.com/google-d-s/tour1.htrnl 118 Grabe, W. (2001). Notes toward a theory of second language writing. In T. Silva & P. Matsuda (Eds.), On Second Language Writing (pp. 39-57). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Grabe, W. & Kaplan, R. (1996). Theory and practice of writing: An applied linguistic perspective. New York: Longman. Graves, D. (1984). A researcher learns to write. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Grossman, L. (2006, December 13). Time's Person of the Year: You. Time. Retrieved from http://www.time.com/time/magazine/article/O,9171,1569514,00.html. Accessed March 20, 2010. Haft, S. (Producer), & Weir, P. (Director). (1989). Dead Poets Society [Motion picture]. USA: Walt Disney. Hall, J .K. (1997). A consideration of SLA as a theory of practice: A response to Firth and Wagner. The Modern Language Journal, 81(3), p. 301-306. Hayes, J .R. (1996). A new model of cognition and affect in writing. In C. M. Levy. & S. Ransdell (Eds.), The Science of Writing (pp. 1-30). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Hubbard, P. (2004). Learner training for effective use of CALL. In S. Fotos & C. Browne (Eds.), New Perspectives on CALL for Second Language Classrooms (pp. 45-68). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 1220, J. (1996). An analysis of computer workstation and handwriting use by ESP students. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED394295). Jackson, S. (1952). The lottery. In M. Crane (Ed), 50 Great American Short Stories (159-167). New York: Random House. Jiang, L., & Cumming, A. (2001). Word processing and second language writing: A longitudinal case-study. International Journal of English Studies, 1(2), 127-152. John-Steiner, V. (1985). Notebooks of the mind: Explorations in thinking. New York: Harper & Row. Kaufinan, D. (2004). Constructivist issues in language learning and teaching. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 24, p. 303-319. Kellogg, R. T. (1994). The psychology of writing. New York: Oxford University Press. 119 Kellogg, R. T. (1996). A model of working memory in writing. In M. C. Levy, & S. E. Ransdell (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual difl’krences and applications (pp. 57—71). Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Associates. Kelm, O. R. (1992). The use of synchronous computer networks in second language instructionzA preliminary study. Foreign Language Annals, 25 (5), 441-454. Kemp, F. (1993). The Daedalus integrated writing environment. Educators’ Tech Exchange, p. 24-30. Kern, R. G. (1995). Restructuring classroom interaction with networked computers: Effects on quantity and characteristics of language production. Modern Language Journal, 79, p. 457-476. Kern, R., Ware, P., Warshauer, M. (2004). Crossing frontiers: New directions in online pedagogy and research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 24, p. 243-260. Kessler, G. (2009). Student-initiated attention to form in wiki-based collaborative writing. Language Learning and Technology, 13 (1), p. 79-95. Kessler, G., & Bikowski, D. (2010). Developing collaborative autonomous language learning abilities in computer mediated language learning: Attention to meaning among students in wiki space. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 23(1), 41-58. Kitade, K. (2000). L2 leamers' discourse and SLA theories in CMC: Collaborative interaction in intemet chat. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 13(2), 143- 166. Kotter, M. (2003). “Negotiation of Meaning and Code Switching in Online Tandems.” Language Learning and Technology Vol. 7, No. 2, May 2003, pp. 145-172 Kraemer, A. (2008). Formats of distance learning. In S. Goertler & P. Winke (Eds.), Opening doors through distance language education: Principles, perspectives, and practices (pp. 11-42). San Marcos, TX: CALICO. Krashen, SD. (1985). The Input Hypothesis. London: Longman. Lam, C. (1991). Revision processes of college ESL students: How teacher comments, discourse types, and writing tools shape revision (Doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia, 1991). Dissertation Abstracts International, 52(12), 4248A. Lam, F., & Pennington, M. (1995). The computer vs. the pen: A comparative study of word processing in a Hong Kong secondary classroom. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 8(1), 75-92. 120 Lantolf, J. 1993. Sociocultural theory and the second language classroom: The lesson of Strategic Interaction. In J. Alatis (ed.), Strategic interaction and language acquisition: theory, practice, and research (pp. 220-233). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Lantolf, J .P. (2002). Sociocultural theory and second language acquisition. In R.B. Kaplan (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Applied Linguistics (p. 104-114). New York: Oxford University Press. Lantolf, J .P. & Johnson, KB. (2007). Extending Firth and Wagner's (1997) ontological perspective to L2 classroom praxis and teacher education. The Modern Language Journal, 91, 877-892. Leki, I. (1997). Cross-talk: ESL issues and contrastive rhetoric. In C. Severino, J. Guerra, and 1. Butler (Eds.), Writing in multicultural settings (pp. 234-244).New York: Modern Language Association of America Leki, I., & Carson, J. (1993). Students’ perceptions of EAP writing instruction and writing needs across the disciplines. TESOL Quarterly, 28, 81-101. Leki, L., & Carson, J. (1997). "Completely Different Worlds": EAP and the writing experiences of ESL students in university courses. TESOL Quarterly, 31(1), 39-69. Lyddon, P.A. & Sydorenko, T. (2008). Assessing distance language learning. In S. Goertler & P. Winke (Eds.), Opening doors through distance language education: Principles, perspectives, and practices (pp. 109-128). San Marcos, TX: CALICO. Mackey, A. & Polio, C. (2009). Introduction. In A. Mackey & C. Polio (Eds.), Multiple Perspectives on Interaction: Second Language Research in Honor of Susan M Gass (p. 1-10). New York: Routledge. Mak, Barley and Coniam, David (2008) Using wikis to enhance and develop writing skills among secondary school students in Hong Kong. SYSTEM, 36(3), 437-455. Maupassant, G. (1952). Looking back. In M. Crane (Ed.), 50 Great American Short Stories (175-180). New York: Random House. Mehdi, S. N. (1994). Word processing and the EFL writing classroom: A case-study of Arabic speakers. Dissertation Abstracts International, 55. Online 0889. Min, H-T. (2006). The effects of trained peer review on EFL students’ revision types and writing quality. Journal of Second Language Writing, 15, 1 18-141. 121 O’Dowd, R. (2006). The use of videoconferencing and e-mail as mediators of intercultural student ethnography. In J. A. Belz & S. L. Thome (Eds.), Internet- mediated intercultural foreign language education (pp. 86-120). Boston, MA: Thomson Heinle. O'Reilly, T. (2005). What is web 2.0? Retrieved May 18, 2006, from http://www.oreillynet.com/pub/a/oreilly/tim/news/2005/09/30/what-is-web- 20.html#mememap Oxford, R. (1997). Cooperative learning, collaborative learning, and interaction: Three communicative strands in the language classroom. The Modern Language Journal, 81(4), p. 443-456. Paulus, TM. (1999). The effect of peer and teacher feedback on student writing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 8(3), 265-289. Pellettieri, J. (2000). Negotiation in cyberspace. The role of chatting in the development of grammatical competence in the virtual foreign language classroom. In M. Warschauer and R. Kern (Eds.), Network-based Language Teaching: Concepts and Practice (pp. 59-86). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pennington, MC. (2003). The impact of the computer in second language writing. In B. Kroll (Ed.), Exploring the Dynamics of Second Language Writing (pp. 287-310). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Salaberry, RM. (2001). The Use of Technology for Second Language Learning and Teaching: A Retrospective. The Modern Language Journal, 85, 39-56. Sanders, RF. (2005). Redesigning introductory Spanish: Increased enrollment, online management, cost reduction, and effects on student learning. Foreign Language Annals, 38(4), p. 523-532. Saroyan, W. (1952). The summer of the beautiful white horse. In M. Crane (Ed.), 50 Great American Short Stories (159-167). New York: Random House. Schneider, J. and von der Emde, S. (2006). Conflicts in cyberspace: From communication breakdown to intercultural dialogue in online collaborations, in J. A Belz & S. L. Thorne (Eds), Internet-mediated Intercultural Foreign Language Education, (pp. 178-206). Boston, MA: Thomson Heinle and Heinle. Shekary, M. & Tahririan, M.H. (2006). Negotiation of meaning and noticing in text- based on line chat. The Modern Language Journal, 90(4), p. 557-573. Silver, N., & Repa, J. (1993). The effect of word processing on the quality of writing and self-esteem of secondary school English as a second language students: Writing without censure. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 9(2), 265-283. 122 Smith, A. (1993). Revising process and written product: A study of basic and skilled L1 English and ESL writers using computers (Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 1993). Dissertation Abstracts International, 5401), 4078A. Smith, B., Alvarez-Torres, M.J. and Zhao, Y. (2003). Features of CMC Technologies and Their Impact on Language Learners’ Online Interaction. Computers in Human Behavior. Smith, L. Z. (1984). Composing composition courses. College English, 46(5), 460-469. Storch, N. (2002). Patterns of interaction in ESL pair work. Language Learning , 52 (1), 119-158. Storch, N. (2005). Collaborative writing: Product, process, and students' reflections. Journal of Second Language Writing, 14, 153-173. Swain, M. & Lapkin, S. (1998). Interaction and second language learning: Two adolescent French immersion students working together. The Modern Language Journal, 82, 320-337. Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (2002). Talking it through: Two French immersion leamers’ response to reformulation. International Journal of Educational Research, 3 7, 285-304. Swain, M. (2000). The output hypothesis and beyond: Mediating acquisition through collaborative dialogue. Sociocultural Theory and Second Language Learning (pp.97-114). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Swain, M. (2001). Integrating language and content teaching through collaborative tasks. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 58(1), 45-63. Sykes, J .M., Oskoz, A. & Thorne, S.L. (2008). Web 2.0, synthetic immersive environments, and mobile resources for language education. CALICO Journal, 25(3), p. 528-546. Toyoda, E. & Harrison, R. (2002). Categorization of Text Chat Communication between Learners and Native Speakers of J apanes. Language Learning & Technology. 6(1), p. 82-99. Vygotsky, LS. (1978). Mind and society: The development of higher mental processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press Ecolab: The Development and Evaluation of a Vygotskian Design Framework 123 Warschauer, M. (1996). Comparing face-to-face and electronic communication in the second language classroom. CALICO Journal, 13(2-3), p. 7—26. Warschauer, M., Turbee, L., & Roberts, B. (1996). Computer learning networks and student empowerment. System, 24(1), p. 1-14. Warschauer, M., & Grimes, D. (2007). Audience, authorship, and artifact: The emergent semiotics of Web 2.0. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics 27, 1-23. Wesch, M. 2007. "A vision of students today" transcript. [Weblog entry, October 21.] Digital Ethnography. http://mediatedcultures.net/ksudigg/?p=122 (accessed June 14, 2008). Archived at http://www.webcitation.org/5aGn6pOpA. Williams, J. (2005). Teaching writing in second and foreign language classrooms. Boston: McGraw Hill. Wildner-Bassett, ME. (2008). In S. Goertler & P. Winke (Eds.), Opening doors through distance language education: Principles, perspectives, and practices (pp. 67-83). San Marcos, TX: CALICO. Zhao, Y. (1998). The effects of anonymity on peer review. International Journal of Educational Telecommunication, 4, 311-346. Zuengler, J. & Miller, ER. (2006). Cognitive and sociocultural perspectives: Two parallel worlds? TESOL Quarterly, 40(1), p. 35-58. 124 will 1111111111111 lllllllllllllllllllllllEs 3 1293 03063 4822