a 1 1 L. : flaw” - {I . ‘ .2 s 8 L50} «we ‘ v. _ .. . . 1.1-... . ‘ ha kart it“ I an A a .wammwg 3.1,»; 2%. ‘ 'I l .SaWfimM... , .. . “bun..."shu: ides . ..! i... a I p 4 F. n TILH .2”: .¢..u5rw-anw«a , H.393 . ‘ . , . €32.91 x ,. ,a, :1... 2.} ANN V fiafih’é‘sfflufif‘)‘ 9, ) nut: 8 iii 1"» f :3- 1: . 1.. ’43... 3 .11.. .31: 51.1 ..n 5 , ‘ 5‘ “.5569? .. as... .3 .. dink“ . 0‘ . 3: 4:1:- . 3...: 3.! . firs} yak: V. v7! ~II’\£-I.l§’i; 5....9.L...%z;..n. ‘ .. C...rt....2.v.c...inmmflvrt .. .2 $1.». unalnwghfin , t he}... .. n. £58.). )3. a... 1‘. Din. A 3. . x 1...: .luln... {iii}. Attaébl att||~.!, 1‘}: , 1:152:62. i... 2'3... L... salsgifisl‘fl .- » ;. . . \l!!r 4" I. \‘l‘ . ‘ x. 3‘...» It!!! galsiyii I!§t¢l&sD-II!. cu,us;r.w t t .2 V «I... ‘ 9 «In... 95"....“ ...§.( xvusr I is? ’2‘ .i1iiufiallp Site}. Ill.- r| 3" ill}. It??? . 232 rt! z! 2v: 5?. . O .t. with: Ll:\i\&..|.6l.ll§! ¥?l§sl¥i\s . ‘ If}! 15..-}. (‘AQV‘stbleVOOI 20’ .l ‘a...(- ll;- 30"..1“ '1. a . Y n’;. .b-Qflvytllu This is to certify that the dissertation entitled THE EFFECTS OF BLACKS’ PHYSICAL CHARACTERISTICS ON WHITES’ EVALUATIONS OF BLACKS AND BLACKS' EXPERIENCES WITH DISCRIMINATION: SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF FACIAL FEATURES FROM SKIN TONE presented by Nao Hagiwara has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Ph.D. degree in Psychology 124/414 4%,. ri/M/w Major Professor’ 5 Signature 5/ 7/ /0 / / Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Employer c-.—-—-a-o-u—--_-—-u-n-n-o—.-.-.-—-.-—.-c-o---o-—.-.-.-.-.—._.-._.-.-.—.-.-.-.-.-,-,-.-.-.-.-.-,-.-._.-._._._.-.- PLACE IN RETURN Box to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5/08 K:lej/Achres/ClRC/DateDue.indd THE EFFECTS OF BLACKS’ PHYSICAL CHARACTERISTICS ON WHITES’ EVALUATIONS OF BLACKS AND BLACKS’ EXPERIENCES WITH DISCRIMINATION: SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF FACIAL FEATURES FROM SKIN TONE By Nao Hagiwara A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILSOPHY Psychology 2010 ABSTRACT THE EFFECTS OF BLACKS’ PHYSICAL CHARACTERISTICS ON WHITES’ EVALUATIONS OF BLACKS AND BLACKS’ EXPERIENCES WITH DISCRIMINATION: SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF FACIAL FEATURES FROM SKIN TONE By Nao Hagiwara People in the same social groups share set of physical characteristics. However, even within the same groups, there is real variation in physical characteristics. Two literatures have been developed to determine how perceivers respond to differences in physical characteristics among Black targets. Research on skin tone suggests that people rely on skin tone to make inferences of Black individuals’ attributes. Research on Afrocentric features suggests that facial features, such as eye color, shapes of nose and lips, and texture of hair, in addition to skin tone, influence people’s perceptions of and reactions to Black targets. Although these two lines of research argue that one type of physical characteristic may be more important than the other when predicting people’s perceptions of and reactions to Black targets, they have failed to separate these two physical characteristics. Four studies were conducted to examine how variation in Black target’s skin tone and facial features influences people’s affective (Studies 1, 3b, and 4) and cognitive (Studies 2, 3b, and 4) reactions. This research also examined whether variation in their own appearance relates to Blacks’ experiences with discrimination (Study 4). Results showed that skin tone (lighter versus darker) and facial features (less prototypical of Blacks versus more prototypical of Blacks) had additive effects on Whites’ feelings toward Black men and that they affected how much Whites disliked Black men at both implicit and explicit levels. The effects of skin tone and facial features were different when Black targets were women. Skin tone affected how much Whites liked Black women, whereas facial features had no effect on Whites’ feelings toward Black women. The effects of Skin tone and facial features were also moderated by perceiver race (White versus Black), and this was true only at implicit level. There was little evidence that skin tone and facial features influenced people’s cognitive reactions (i.e., stereotype activation) toward Blacks, but problems with measure validity prevent clear conclusions with respect to this point. There was some evidence that Blacks’ experiences with discrimination, sensitivity to racism, and tendency to attribute ambiguous negative events to racism were influenced by their own skin tone and facial features. However, due to low statistical power, additional data is required to further examine the nature of the skin tone and facial features effects on Blacks’ own experiences with racism. The present studies contribute to a better understanding of the effects of Black targets’ physical characteristics on people’s perceptions of and reactions to them, by pointing to the benefits and importance of identifying factors that explain who, when, and under what circumstances targets are more likely to experience prejudice and discrimination. I would like to dedicate this to my family. iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank my committee members, Drs. Deborah Kashy, Joseph Cesario, Nobert L. Karr, and Gwen M. Wittenbaum for their time and thoughtful comments on my dissertation project. I would like to especially acknowledge the patience and support that Dr. Deborah Kashy provided me throughout the graduate training at Michigan State University. She has offered much guidance not only in academics but also in my personal life when I was going through major life transitions as well as hardships. Without her support and guidance, 1 would not be here. I must thank my family for their support, especially my parents, Junichi and Mari Hagiwara, who have always believed in me. I would also like to thank my son, Akiharu Ethan Livingston, and my husband, Brian L. Livingston, who have given me a constant companionship while I was working on my dissertation. My son’s smile at the end of the day has always energized me and encouraged me to keep going. I owe thanks to my undergraduate research assistants in the Cesario Laboratory who helped me with participant recruitment and data collection. I also want to thank graduate students in the Social/Personality interest group for their support. I would like to especially acknowledge Michael Mott for helping me with the analyses of target stimuli. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................... x LIST OF FIGURES ................................................................................ xiv INTRODUCTION .................................................................................. l The Effects of Skin Tone .................................................................. 4 The Effects of Afrocentric Features. .9 Unexamined Gender Effects In the Current AfrOcentrrc Feature Research ......... l8 Unexamined Target Effects In the Current Afrocentric Feature Research .......... 21 CURRENT RESEARCH .................................................................................................. 23 PILOT STUDY ................................................................................................................ 24 METHOD ........................................................................................................................ 25 Procedures ............................................................................................................ 25 RESULTS ........................................................................................................................ 28 Steps 1 & 2 (Examinations of Natural Skin Tone and Natural Facial Features)...29 Step 5 (Examinations of Skin Tone Selection and Facial Feature Manipulation).29 STUDY 1 .......................................................................................................................... 31 METHOD ......................................................................................................................... 31 Participants ............................................................................................................ 32 Procedures .............................................................................................................. 32 Measures ............................................................................................................... 32 COMPUTING FACILITATION SCORES ...................................................................... 37 RESULTS ....................................................................................................................... 38 Implicit Negative Attitudes toward Black Men .................................................. 38 Implicit Positive Attitudes toward Black Men .................................................... 39 Black vs. White Targets ...................................................................................... 4O Explicit Affective Reactions to Black Men ......................................................... 41 DISCUSION ................................................................................................................... 4] STUDY 2 ........................................................................................................................ 43 METHOD ....................................................................................................................... 43 Participants ......................................................................................................... 44 vi Procedures ............................................................................................................ 44 Measures .............................................................................................................. 45 COMPUTING RESPONSE LATENCIES ...................................................................... 46 RESULTS ........................................................................................................................ 47 Stereotype Activation among Whites .................................................................. 47 Black vs. White Targets ....................................................................................... 48 Explicit Affective Reactions to Black Men ......................................................... 49 DISCUSION .................................................................................................................... 50 STUDY 3 ......................................................................................................................... 51 STUDY 3a ....................................................................................................................... 52 METHOD ........................................................................................................................ 52 Participants .......................................................................................................... 52 Procedures ............................................................................................................ 53 RESULTS ........................................................................................................................ 53 Implicit Negative Attitudes toward Black Women ............................................. 53 Implicit Positive Attitudes toward Black Women ............................................... 54 Black vs. White Targets ....................................................................................... 55 Explicit Affective Reactions to Black Women .................................................... 55 STUDY 3b ....................................................................................................................... 56 METHOD ........................................................................................................................ 56 Participants .......................................................................................................... 56 Procedures ............................................................................................................ 56 RESULTS ........................................................................................................................ 57 Stereotype Activation among Whites .................................................................. 57 Black vs. White Targets ...................................................................................... 58 Explicit Affective Reactions to Black Women ................................................... 59 DISCUSION .................................................................................................................... 6O STUDY 4 ......................................................................................................................... 63 METHOD ........................................................................................................................ 63 Participants .......................................................................................................... 63 Procedures ............................................................................................................. 63 Measures .............................................................................................................. 65 Pre-Laboratory Questionnaires ................................................................ 65 vii Measures Used in the Laboratory Session ............................................... 68 CODING OF PARTICIPANTS’ PHOTOS ..................................................................... 70 ANALYSIS PLAN .......................................................................................................... 71 RESULTS ........................................................................................................................ 73 Attributions of Ambiguous Negative Events ....................................................... 74 Attribution to Racism ............................................................................... 74 Attribution to Sexism ............................................................................... 75 Attribution to Skin Tone .......................................................................... 75 Attribution to Facial Features .................................................................. 77 Attribution to Internal Factors ................................................................. 78 Attribution to External Factors ................................................................ 78 Experiences with Racial Discrimination ............................................................. 79 Race-Based Rejection Sensitivity ........................................................................ 80 Other Criterion Variables ..................................................................................... 83 Implicit Affective Reactions to Black Men ......................................................... 83 Implicit Negative Attitudes ...................................................................... 84 Implicit Positive Attitudes ....................................................................... 85 Black vs. White Control .......................................................................... 85 Implicit Affective Reactions to Black Women ................................................... 86 Implicit Negative Attitudes ..................................................................... 86 Implicit Positive Attitudes ....................................................................... 86 Black vs. White Control .......................................................................... 87 Explicit Affective Reactions to Fellow Ingroup Members .................................. 87 Liking toward Black Men ........................................................................ 87 Liking toward Black Women ................................................................... 88 Stereotype Activation among Blacks following Black Male Targets .................. 88 Stereotype Activation among Blacks following Black Female Targets .............. 89 DISCUSION .................................................................................................................... 92 Attributions for Ambiguous Negative Events ..................................................... 92 Experience with and Sensitivity to Racism ......................................................... 95 The Effects of Black TargetS’ Characteristics on Black Perceivers’ Attitudes toward Them ........................................................................................................ 96 The Effects of Black Targets’ Characteristics on Stereotype Activation for Black Perceivers ............................................................................................................. 97 GENERAL DISCUSSION .............................................................................................. 98 Independent Effects of Skin Tone and Facial Features ....................................... 99 The Effects of Target Gender ............................................................................. 100 The Effects of Perceiver Race ............................................................................ 101 Implicit Attitudes-Explicit Attitudes Relation ................................................... 102 Black Person’s Own Physical Characteristics and Their Experiences with Racism ................................................................................................................ 103 viii Limitations and Future Directions ...................................................................... 106 CONCLUSIONS ........................................................................................................... 1 13 FOOTNOTES ................................................................................................................ 114 APPENDICES ............................................................................................................... 168 Appendix A: Objective Measures of Facial Features .......................................... 168 Appendix B: An Example of Outlined Pictures ................................................. 169 Appendix C: The Final 64 Black Male Target Pictures ..................................... 170 Appendix D: The Final 8 Black Female Target Pictures ................................... 172 Appendix E: A Design of Phase 4 in the Sequential Priming Task ................... 173 Appendix F: Selection of the Target Words for the Lexical Decision Task ...... 174 Appendix G: A List of Pre-Laboratory Questionnaires ..................................... 178 Appendix H: Selection of Attribution Items and Scenarios for the Attribution Measure in Study 3 ............................................................................................ 185 Appendix I: Race-Based Rejection Sensitivity ................................................. 195 Appendix J: Example of Average Skin Colors .................................................. 199 REFERENCES ............................................................................................................. 200 ix LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations of Subjective Ratings and Objective Measures of Black Faces by Race .................................................................................................. l 17 Table 2. Correlations among Subjective Ratings and Objective Measures of Black Faces for Black Men ................................................................................................................. 1 l 8 Table 3. Correlations among Subjective Ratings and Objective Measures of Black Faces for Black Women ............................................................................................................ 1 19 Table 4. Means, Standard Deviations, and Mean Diflerences of Subjective and Objective Measures of Black Men ’s Physical Features for Dark vs. Light Skin Tone Condition ....................................................................................................................... 1 20 Table 5. Means, Standard Deviations, and Mean Diflerences of Subjective and Objective Measures of Black Women ’s Physical Features for Dark vs. Light Skin Tone Condition ....................................................................................................................... l 21 Table 6. Mean Differences in Prototypicality Ratings between Strong Facial Feature Faces and Weak Facial Feature Faces ......................................................................... 122 Table 7. Means and Standard Deviations of Facilitation Scores for the Negative Words as a Function of Black Men ’s Skin Tone and Facial Features ...................................... 123 Table 8. Means and Standard Deviations of Facilitation Scores for the Positive Words as a Function of Black Men 's Skin Tone and Facial Features .......................................... 124 Table 9. Means and Standard Deviations of White Perceivers ’ Explicit Liking toward Black Men as a Function of Skin Tone and Facial Features (Study 1) ......................... 125 Table 10. Means and Standard Deviations of Response Latencies for the Stereotype and Non-Stereotypic Words as a Function of Black Men ’s Skin Tone and Facial F eature.. 126 Table 11. Means and Standard Deviations of White Perceivers ’ Explicit Liking toward Black Men as a Function of Skin Tone and Facial Features (Study 2) ......................... 128 Table 12. Means and Standard Deviations of Facilitation Scores for the Negative Words as a Function of Black Women ’s Skin Tone and Facial Features ................................. 129 Table 13. Means and Standard Deviations of Facilitation Scores for the Positive Words as a Function of Black Women ’s Skin Tone and Facial Features ................................. 130 Table 14. Means and Standard Deviations of White Perceivers ’ Explicit Liking toward Black Women as a Function of Skin Tone and Facial Features (Study 3a) .................. 131 Table 15. Means and Standard Deviations of Response Latencies for the Stereotype and Non-Stereotypic Words as a Function of Black Women ’s Skin Tone and Facial Features ......................................................................................................................... 1 32 Table 16. Means and Standard Deviations of White Perceivers ’ Explicit Liking toward Black Women as a Function of Skin Tone and Facial Features (Study 3b) .................. 134 Table 17. Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations among the Predictor Variables for Black Male Participants ’ Pictures ........................................................................... 135 Table 18. Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations among the Predictor Variables for Black Female Participants ' Pictures ....................................................................... 136 Table 19. Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations among All the Major Criterion Variables ........................................................................................................................ 1 37 Table 20. The Eflects of Skin Tone, Facial Features, and Participants ' Gender on Attribution to Racism ..................................................................................................... 138 Table 21. The Effects of Skin Tone, Facial Features, and Participants ' Gender on Attribution T o Sexism ..................................................................................................... 139 Table 22. The Eflects of Skin T one, Facial Features, and Participants ' Gender on Attribution T 0 Skin Tone ................................................................................................ 140 Table 23. The Eflects of Skin Tone, Facial Features, and Participants ’ Gender on Attribution T o Facial Features ...................................................................................... 141 Table 24. The Eflects of Skin Tone, Facial Features, and Participants ’ Gender on Internal Attribution ......................................................................................................... 142 Table 25. The Effects of Skin Tone, Facial Features, and Participants ' Gender on External Attribution ....................................................................................................... 143 Table 26. The Effects of Skin Tone, Facial Features, and Participants ' Gender on Experiences With Racial Discrimination In The Past One Year ................................... 144 Table 27. The Effects of Skin Tone, Facial Features, and Participants ' Gender on Lifetime Experiences With Racial Discrimination ......................................................... 145 Table 28. The Effects of Skin T one, Facial Features, and Participants ’ Gender on Race- Based Rejection Sensitivity ............................................................................................ 146 xi Table 29. The Eflects of Skin Tone, Facial Features, and Participants ’ Gender on Racial Identification .................................................................................................................. 147 Table 30. The Effects of Skin Tone, Facial Features, and Participants ' Gender on Perception Of Discrimination ........................................................................................ 148 Table 31. The Effects of Skin Tone, Facial Features, and Participants ' Gender on Overall Self-Esteem .................................................................................................................... 149 Table 32. The Eflects of Skin Tone, Facial Features, and Participants ' Gender on Social Self-Esteem .................................................................................................................... l 50 Table 33. The Effects of Skin Tone, Facial Features, and Participants ’ Gender on Appearance Self-Esteem ................................................................................................ 151 Table 34. Means and Standard Deviations of Facilitation Scores for the Negative Words as a Function of Black Male Targets ' Skin Tone and Facial Features ......................... 152 Table 35. Means and Standard Deviations of Facilitation Scores for the Positive Words as a Function of Black Male Targets ’ Skin Tone and Facial Features ......................... 153 Table 36. Means and Standard Deviations of Facilitation Scores for the Negative Words as a Function of Black Female Targets ' Skin Tone and Facial Features ..................... 154 Table 37. Means and Standard Deviations of Facilitation Scores for the Positive Words as a Function of Black Female Targets ' Skin Tone and Facial Features ..................... 155 Table 38. Means and Standard Deviations of Black Perceivers ' Explicit Liking toward Black Male Targets as a Function of Skin Tone and Facial Features .......................... 156 Table 39. Means and Standard Deviations of Black Perceivers ' Explicit Liking toward Black Female Targets as a Function of Skin Tone and Facial Features ...................... 157 Table 40. Means and Standard Deviations of Response Latencies for the Stereotype and Non-Stereotypic Words as a Function of Black Male Targets ’ Skin Tone and Facial Feature .......................................................................................................................... 158 Table 41. Means and Standard Deviations of Response Latencies for the Stereotype and Non-Stereotypic Words as a Function of Black Female Targets ’ Skin Tone and Facial Feature .......................................................................................................................... 160 Table I. Means Differences in Stereotypicality and Negativity Ratings between Stereotypic Words and Non-Stereotypic Words within Each Gender ........................... 177 Table 11. Means and Standard Deviations of the Nine Attribution Items for Each of the 10 Scenarios ....................................................................................................................... 1 91 xii Table 111. Internal Consistency across the Nine Attribution Items within Each of the 10 Scenarios ......................................................................................................................... 192 Table IV. Correlation between Internal and External Racism and Internal and External Sexism Attribution Items across the 10 Scenarios .......................................................... 193 Table V. Internal Consistency across the 10 Scenarios within Each of the 10 Attribution Items ................................................................................................................................ 194 xiii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. Subjectively Measured Black Participants’ Skin Tone Moderates the Effect of Subjectively Measured Participant Facial Features on their Tendency to Attribute Ambiguous Negative Events to Skin Tone ..................................................................... 162 Figure 2. Objectively Measured Black Participants’ Skin Tone Moderates the Effect of Objectively Measured Participant Facial Features on their Tendency to Attribute Ambiguous Negative Events to Facial Features ............................................................. 163 Figure 3. Subjectively Measured Black Participants’ Skin Tone Moderates the Effect of Subjectively Measured Participant Facial Features on their Tendency to Attribute Ambiguous Negative Events to Internal Factors ........................................................... 164 Figure 4. Subjectively Measured Black Participants’ Skin Tone Moderates the Effect of Subjectively Measured Participant Facial Features on their Experiences With Racial Discrimination in the Past 1 Year ................................................................................... 165 Figure 5. Objectively Measured Black Participants’ Skin Tone Moderates the Effect of Objective] y Measured Participant Facial Features on their Sensitivity to Racism ........ 166 Figure 6. Subjectively Measured Black Participants’ Skin Tone Moderates the Effect of Subjectively Measured Participant Facial Features on their Sensitivity to Racism ........ 167 Figure 1. Objective Measurements of Face Width, Face Length, Nose Width, and Lip Thickness ........................................................................................................................ 168 Figure 11. An Example of Outlined Pictures ................................................................... 169 Figure 111. Black Male Target Pictures Used in the Main Studies ................................. 170 Figure IV. Black Female Target Pictures Used in the Main Studies .............................. 172 Figure V. Examples of Average Skin Colors ............................................................. - ..... 199 Images in this dissertation are presented in color. xiv The Effects of Blacks’ Physical Characteristics on Whites’ Evaluations of Blacks and Blacks’ Experiences with Discrimination: Separating the Effects of Facial Features from Skin Tone People have mental representations (i.e., concepts) of a variety of social groups (Kunda, 1999), and they use these concepts when categorizing target persons into social groups and forming impression of target persons (Brewer, 1988; F iske, Lin, & Neuberg, 1999). For instance, people have concepts of how typical men and women should look in terms of eyebrow Shape, cheekbones, stubble, and hair style, and use them when identifying an individual’s sex (Brown & Perrett, 1993; Burton, Bruce, & Dench, 1993; Goshen—Gottestein & Ganel, 2000; Macrae & Matrin, 2007; Martin & Macrae, 2007). In fact, the major models of impression formation (e.g., see Brewer, 1988 for the “Dual Process Model” and Fiske et al., 1999 for the “Continuum Model”) posit that categorization of people into different social groups based on salient physical cues is the first step in the process of impression formation. These models further argue that people tend to form their impressions of a target person based on their attitudes toward the social category to which the target person was categorized, even before getting know the person (Brewer, 1998; Fiske et al., 1999). Skin color plays an important role when people attempt to identify an individual’s race (Brown, Dane, & Durham, 1998). In fact, the importance of skin color for making racial distinctions is reflected in the long history of the use of color terminology to label racial groups (i.e., Black, Brown, Red, White, and Yellow). Racial classification based on these five colors has been used since Johann Friedrich Blumenbach came up with the five color typology for humans in the 18th century (Gossett, 1997). Interestingly, researchers have shown that people use skin color not only to categorize people into different racial groups but also to make inferences about others’ attributes within the same racial groups (Dixon & Maddox, 2005; Maddox & Chase, 2004; Maddox & Gray, 2002; Montalvo, 2004; Uhlmann, Dasgupta, Greenwald, & Swanson, 2002; Wade & Bielits, 2005). For instance, it is well documented that Black individuals with darker Skin tone are perceived to possess more negative attributes, such as aggressiveness and loudness, as compared to Black individuals with lighter Skin tone (Dixon & Maddox, 2005; Maddox & Chase, 2004; Maddox & Gray, 2002; Wade & Bielits, 2005). This phenomenon is often referred to as colorism (Hunter, 2002). Blacks are not the only social group who face colorism. Incidents of colorism have been observed among Hispanic Americans in the United States (Banerji, 2006; Hill, 2002; Hochschild, 2007; Morales, 2008; Russell, Wilson, & Hall, 1992; Uhlmann et al., 2002) as well as in societies outside the United States, such as Brazil (Harnandez, 2006), South Asia (Rondilla & Spickard, 2007), and Arabic Speaking countries (Mukhtar, 2007). Are there other physical cues that influence people’s impressions of others within the same racial groups? Facial features (e.g., nose, lips, cheekbones, eyes) may be such cues. Indeed, several researchers have recently found evidence that people also make inferences about Black individuals’ characteristics based on facial features, such as nose, lips, eyes, hair texture, in addition to skin tone (Blair, 2006; Blair, Chapleau, & Judd, 2005; Blair, Judd, & Fallma, 2004b; Blair, Judd, Sadler, & Jenkins, 2002; Eberhardt, Goff, Purdie, & Davies, 2004; Maddox, 2004; Oliver, Jackson, Moses, & Dangerfield, 2004). Some researchers refer to these additional characteristics together with skin tone as “Afrocentric features.” One problem with the Afrocentric feature research is that the definition of Afrocentric features includes skin tone. In fact, Afrocentric features are often confounded with skin tone. That is, Black individuals who have stronger Afrocentric features (i.e., fuller lips, a wide nose, and coarse hair) are likely to have darker skin tone, whereas Black individuals who have weaker Afrocentric features (i.e., thin lips, a narrow nose, and soft hair) are likely to have lighter Skin tone. Therefore, it is unclear in the current Afrocentric feature literature whether the effects of Afrocentric features on Whites’ evaluations of and reactions to Black individuals are driven by facial features in addition to skin tone, or by only skin tone. It is important to separate facial features from skin tone for two reasons. First, if the effects of Afrocentric features reported previously are due to skin tone, then researchers are Simply reinventing the wheel and calling Skin tone “Afrocentric features.” Second, if physical characteristics that influence people’s perceptions of and reactions to Black individuals could be pinpointed, this information can be used for informing and educating people about the factors that automatically influence attitudes toward and discrimination against Black individuals. The present studies aimed to examine whether facial features influence how people perceive and react to Black individuals above and beyond the well-documented skin tone effects. In the current research, facial features unconfounded with skin tone (i.e., lips and nose) are referred to as “facial features.” In contrast, when the term “Afrocentric features” is used, it indicates that facial features are confounded with skin tone. Beyond separating facial features from Skin tone, the present research addresses three important research questions that will further our understanding of the effects of multiple physical characteristics on impression formation within a certain racial group (i.e., Blacks). First, it extends the previous Afrocentric features research by looking at female Black targets. In fact, no one has ever examined how Black women’s Afrocentric features influence people’s impressions of them. Previous studies have exclusively used Black men as targets and measured how Whites respond to Black men who possess different degrees of Afrocentric features. The current study investigates how people respond to Black women with different degrees of skin tone and facial features. Second, it also extends the previous Afrocentric features research by looking at how Blacks' own physical characteristics influence their experiences with intergroup relations. If Whites do differentially perceive and treat Blacks who vary in Skin tone and Afrocentric features, how Blacks construe and approach intergroup relations may differ depending on these physical cues. Previous researchers have focused exclusively on how Black targets are perceived and treated by Whites and have failed to look at how Black targets respond to differential perceptions and treatments by Whites. The present research will investigate the effects of skin tone and facial features from the target’s perspective. Finally, the present research further examines the effects of Skin tone and facial features by looking at Black perceivers. Researchers have previously focused on how Whites react to Blacks with different degrees of Afrocentric features, and no researchers to date have examined how Blacks perceive and react to their fellow ingroup members who vary in Afi'ocentric features. Thus, the current study investigates the effects of skin tone and facial features within the same social group. The Effects of Skin Tone There is a large literature that has shown that skin tone has a strong influence on how Black individuals are perceived and treated by both Whites and fellow Blacks (Dixon & Maddox, 2005; Maddox & Chase, 2004; Maddox & Gray, 2002; Wade & Bielits, 2005). The term “Skin tone” is used to describe the level of lightness or darkness of Skin color (Maddox & Chase, 2004). Skin tone is often measured with Blacks' self- ratings (Averhart & Bigler, 1997; Coard, Breland, & Raskin, 2001; Harvey, LaBeach, Pridgen & Gocial, 2005; Klonoff & Landrine, 2000; Wade, 1996) or with perceivers’ subjective ratings (Hill, 2000; 2002; Keith & Herring, 1991; Thompson & Keith, 2001), both on Likert-type scales. Very few researchers use objective measures, such as the percentage of light reflected from the surface of the Skin measured by a spectrophotometer, to assess skin tone (Sweet, McDade, Kiefe, & Liu, 2007). Previous studies have shown that both White and Black people perceive Black individuals with darker skin tone more negatively, as compared to those with lighter skin tone (Anderson & Cromwell, 1977; Averhadt & Bigler, 1997; Hall, 1992; Maddox & Gray, 2002; Dixon & Maddox, 2005; Porter, 1991). For instance, Maddox and Gray (Study 2, 2002) asked both White and Black participants to provide cultural beliefs about “dark-skinned Black women,” “dark-skinned Black men,” “light-skinned Black women,” and “light-skinned Black men.” They found that participants listed a greater number of negative, compared to positive, attributes (e.g., criminal, lazy, aggressive, unintelligent) for Black targets with darker Skin tone, whereas they listed a greater number of positive, compared to negative, attributes (e.g., kind, educated, wealthy, motivated) for Black targets with lighter skin tone, regardless of target gender. In another study, Dixon and Maddox (2005) have Shown that Black men with darker skin tone are associated with negative traits by demonstrating that participants reported that Black men with darker skin tone were more memorable as a murderer. In this study, participants (73% Whites) were asked to watch a lS-min news clip that contained several stories. Among these stories was a 22-sec story about either a Black or a White criminal who was being sought for the murder of a race-unidentified police Officer. A picture of the perpetrator of this murder was displayed for 3 sec and was described as “a White,” “a light-skinned Black,” “a medium-skinned Black,” or “a dark- skinned Black” man. Participants were then asked to respond to a series of dependent measures that included a measure of memory (i.e., how memorable each character Shown in the news clip was). The results showed that the dark-skinned Black perpetrator was more memorable than the White perpetrator as a murderer. Averhadt and Bi gler (1 997) have further shown that even Black children have biased memory based on skin tone. Specifically, Black children could remember stereotypic information about skin tone better than counterstereotypic information. Averhadt and Bigler (1997) had Black children listen to Six stories in which Black individuals with different levels of Skin tone were described in terms of either positive or negative traits (e. g., honest/dishonest, nice/mean, rich/poor, clean/dirty) and high or low status occupations (e.g., a company president/janitor, a lawyer/parking lot attendant, a factory owner/factory worker, a ballet dancer/maid). The results demonstrated that Black children Showed better memory for stories that depicted Black individuals with dark skin tone with negative traits and in occupations of lower status than the stories that depicted them with positive traits and in occupations of higher status. Likewise, Black children showed better memory for stories that depicted Black individuals with light skin tone with positive traits and in occupations of higher status than stories that depicted them with negative traits and in occupations of lower status. Taken together, these results indicate that people, whether White or Black, associate positive traits and higher status among Black individuals with lighter skin tone and negative traits and lower status with Black individuals with darker skin tone. Previous research has also shown that people treat Black individuals with darker Skin tone more negatively, as compared to Black individuals with lighter skin tone (Hall, 2003; 2005; Wade, Romano, & Blue, 2004). For instance, Wade et a1. (2004) had White participants review a set of application materials, ostensibly completed by one of four Black applicants (fair-Skinned or dark-Skinned, man or woman), that included the applicant’s résumé and a written description of the applicant. Participants were told that their task was to take the role of a manager and evaluate the applicant. Wade et a1. (2004) found that participants were more willing to accept fair-skinned Black individuals than dark-skinned Black individuals, regardless of the gender of the applicant. A difference in Skin tone among Black individuals is associated with economic, vocational, and educational status as well (Blackwell, 1985; Frazier, 1966; Hill, 2000; Hughes & Hertel, 1990; Keith & Herring, 199]). For instance, Keith and Herring (1991) looked at the relation between skin tone and educational attainment, occupation, and income, using the National Survey of Black Americans. In this data set, skin tone was assessed by the interviewer’s subjective rating, using a scale ranging from 1 (a very dark brown skin color) to 5 (a very Ii ght brown or very light Skin). The study Showed that Black individuals with lighter skin tone had higher education, occupational status, and income, as compared to Black individuals with darker skin tone, when controlling for gender, age, marital status, parental socioeconomic status, region of residency, and urbanicity. Similarly, in his archival study, Hill (2000) has Shown that skin tone predicted the occupational status of Black men. In this study, participants’ skin tone was determined by the ratings provided by census enumerators on a dichotomous scale (in the 1920 census): Blacks of “full blood” vs. Blacks “having some proportion of white blood.” Then, participants’ occupational status was determined by the information provided on the US. Standard death certificate (collected between 1980 and 1985). The results showed that Black men who were identified as Blacks having some proportion of White blood (lighter skin tone) were more likely to have attained a white collar occupation, which includes professional, managerial, sales, and clerical workers, than Black men who were identified as Blacks of full blood (darker skin tone). Differential treatments of Black individuals based on their skin tone are further reflected in their reports of experiences with racial discrimination. Klonoff and Landrine (2000) asked Black participants to complete a questionnaire that included the Schedule of Racist Events (SRE: Landrine & Klonoff, 1996; Klonoff & Landrine, 1999) and a measure of skin tone. The SRE assesses both the frequency and types of racial discrimination as well as people’s appraisal of the discriminatory events. The results showed that Black individuals with darker skin tone reported experiencing racial discrimination 11 times more often and appraising their experiences with racial discrimination as more stressful, as compared to Black individuals with lighter skin tone. In addition to the self-reported experiences with discrimination, several studies have used more objective measures, such as average blood pressure, to show that experiences of Black individuals with darker skin tone are different fiom those of Black individuals with lighter skin tone (Boyle, 1970; Dressler, I991; Harburg, Gleibermann, Roeper, Schork, & Schull, 1978; Sweet et al., 2007). For instance, Sweet et a1. (2007) demonstrated that income was negatively associated with average blood pressure among, Black individuals with lighter Skin tone (paralleling the well-documented pattern among Whites), whereas income was not associated with average blood pressure among Black individuals with darker Skin tone. These results indicate that the experience of Black individuals with lighter skin tone are more similar to Whites (at least with respect to blood pressure) and that the protective effects of economic success may not be available to Black individuals with darker skin tone. Taken together, a large body of research on skin tone has demonstrated that people perceive and react differently to Black individuals with different levels of Skin tone: people perceive and react more negatively to Black individuals with darker skin tone than to Black individuals with lighter Skin tone. However, is skin tone the only physical characteristic people use when making their judgments Of Black individuals? Do people use more subtle physical characteristics, such as facial features, to form impressions of and react to Blacks? The Effects of Afrocentric Features An increasing number of researchers have argued that people use facial features in addition to Skin tone when they form impressions of Black individuals (Blair, 2006; Blair et al., 2005; Blair et al., 2004b; Blair et al., 2002; Eberhardt et al., 2004; Oliver et al., 2004). Research has shown that Afrocentric features, defined as visible physical characteristics that differentiate Blacks from other racial groups, such as filler lips, a wider nose, more coarse hair, and darker eye and skin color (Blair, 2006; Blair et al., 2005; Blair, Judd, & Chapleau, 2004a; Blair et al., 2004b; Blair et al., 2002; Eberhardt et al., 2004; Eberhardt, Davies, Purdie-Vaughns, & Johnson, 2006), influence how Black men are perceived by Whites (Blair, 2006; Blair et al., 2005; Blair et al., 2004b; Blair et al., 2002; Eberhardt et al., 2004; 2006; Oliver et al., 2004). More specifically, researchers have shown that Black men who have stronger Afrocentric features are perceived more negatively, as compared to Black men with weaker Afrocentric features. Livingston and Brewer (2002) showed this effect on an implicit (i.e., unobtrusive) measure of attitudes by having participants complete a sequential priming task designed to assess affective reactions to Black individuals. This task assesses how quickly participants can identify positively-valenced and negatively- valenced adjectives, when these adjectives are preceded by Black and White faces. Responding more quickly to negatively-valenced adjectives following Black faces is indicative of negative affective reactions to Blacks. The researchers found that White participants responded to negative words more quickly after seeing Black male faces with stronger Afrocentric features, as compared to after seeing Black male faces with weaker Afrocentric features. Furthermore, Whites’ affective responses to Black male faces with weaker Afrocentric features did not differ from their responses to White male faces. Livingston and Brewer (2002) argue that these results indicate that Whites’ affective responses to Black men were influenced by within-group differences in Afrocentric features, rather than by between- group differences in race. Blair and her colleagues (Blair, 2006; Blair et al., 2005; Blair et al., 2004b; Blair et al., 2002) have firrther shown that Afrocentric features influence not only Whites’ 10 affective responses to Black men, but also their cognitive responses to Black men (i.e., stereotype activation). In Blair et al.’s (2002) study, participants read scenarios that differed along two crossed dimensions: valence of stereotypes and degree of stereotypicality for Black people (i.e., positive stereotypic, positive non-stereotypic, negative stereotypic, negative non-stereotypic). Participants were told to read each scenario very carefully and try to form a mental image of what the person described in the scenario might look like. After they read a given scenario, participants were presented with Black male faces (one at a time on the computer screen) that differed in the degree of Afrocentric features. They were instructed to estimate the probability that the person presented on the computer screen was in fact the person who had been described in the scenario. After completing the probability ratings for all the target faces for this scenario, participants went through the same procedures for each of the remaining three scenarios. Blair et al. (2002) reported that Black men with stronger Afrocentric features were judged as significantly more likely to be the actor in the stereotypic scenarios than in the non- stereotypic scenarios, and in negative scenarios than in positive scenarios. These results suggest that White people associate Afi'ocentric features with behaviors that are stereotypic of Black men in our society. Blair and her colleagues further showed that the relation between Afrocentric features and stereotype activation is robust and efficient (Blair, 2006; Blair et al., 2005; Blair et al., 2004b). In a series of studies, Blair et al. (2004b) had participants complete the same probability rating task described above while they gave participants clear instructions to avoid using stereotypes associated with Black people in general (Study 2) or stereotypes associated with Afi'ocentric features specifically (Study 3). They found that 11 White participants, regardless of whether they received the instructions or not, judged Black men with stronger Afrocentric features as significantly more stereotypic of Blacks than those with weaker Afrocentric features. White participants’ inability to control stereotype activation based on Afrocentric features even with the instructions suggests that the effects of Afrocentric features on their perceptions of Black men may be triggered automatically. Furthermore, researchers have shown that the effects of Afrocentric features on stereotype activation can be found when participants are under high cognitive load (i.e., when controlled processing is difficult; Blair et al., Study 1, 2004b). Under high-load, participants were told to pay attention to letters presented on the computer screen while they completed the probability rating task and were further instructed to press the spacebar every time they saw the letter sequence G-X—Q. Blair et al. (2004b) found no difference between the high-load and no-load conditions. That is, in both conditions, Black men with stronger Afrocentric features were perceived to possess more stereotypic attributes than Black men with weaker Afrocentric features. Previous research has also Shown that the effects of Afrocentric features on stereotype activation were found when the Black male faces were presented up-side-down (Blair, 2006), Changing the orientation of a face makes controlled processing difficult (Diamond & Carey, 1986; Leder & Bruce, 2000; Valentine, 1988; 1991) In another study (Blair, 2006), participants completed the same probability rating task either in the upright face condition or in the inverted face condition. The results showed that Black men with stronger Afi'ocentric features were stereotyped to a greater degree than Black men with weaker Afrocentric features, regardless of face orientation. These findings indicate that stereotype activation 12 based on Afrocentric features is efficient, because this process operates even when the task is made difficult by introducing cognitive load or changing the orientation of faces (Bargh, 1994). Afrocentric features have also been found to influence Whites’ behavioral responses to Black men (Blair et al., 2004a; Eberhardt et al. 2006; Kahn, Davies, Eberhardt, & Correll, 2008). Blair et al. (2004a) presented faces of Black and White male prison inmates to participants and asked them to make a single, global assessment of the degree to which “each face had features that are typical of African Americans.” They found that inmates who were rated to possess stronger Afrocentric features had received harsher sentences than those inmates who were rated to possess weaker Afrocentric features, regardless of whether inmates were Black or White. This was true even when controlling for criminal histories, attractiveness, and babyfacedness. Similarly, Eberhardt et al. (2006) found that Black male defendants with stronger Afrocentric features were more likely to be sentenced to death than their counterparts with weaker Afrocentric features, when the victim was White. This result was found above and beyond a number of factors that were found to be related to sentencing decisions (i.e., aggravating circumstances, mitigating circrunstances, severity of the murder, the defendant’s socioeconomic status, the victim’s socioeconomic status, and the defendant’s attractiveness). Behavioral responses that are less controlled, or more automatic, have also been Shown to be affected by Afrocentric features. Kahn et al. (2008) had White participants complete a shooter bias video game. The goal of this game is to shoot (by pressing labeled keys) targets carrying a gun and not to shoot targets carrying neutral objects 13 (either a cell phone or a wallet). Kahn et al. (2008) used a Black man with stronger Afrocentric features, a Black man with weaker Afrocentric features, and a White man as targets. They found that participants were more likely to incorrectly shoot an unarmed Black man with stronger, compared to weaker, Afrocentric features. Likewise, participants were more likely to correctly Shoot an armed Black man with stronger, compared to weaker, Afrocentric features. Taken together, these findings suggest that Black men with stronger Afrocentric features elicit more negative behavioral responses at both the explicit and implicit levels, as compared to Black men with weaker Afi'ocentric features. Previous studies appear to suggest that Afiocentric features have robust effects on how White individuals feel about, think about, and treat Black men. However, it is unclear whether the effects of Afrocentric features on Whites’ evaluations and treatments of Black individuals are driven by facial features in addition to Skin tone or by skin tone only, because the definition of Afrocentric features includes skin tone. Indeed, facial features tend to covary with skin tone, such that Black men with stronger facial features are likely to have darker skin tone, whereas Black men with weaker facial features are likely to have lighter skin tone. If facial features and skin tone naturally covary, why iS it important to separate the effects of facial features from skin tone? Although facial features and Skin tone correlate with one another, the size of the correlation is moderate, suggesting that there is still large variability among Black individuals, such that some Blacks with stronger facial features have lighter skin tone, whereas some Blacks with weaker facial features have darker Skin tone. The previous 14 research that does not separate the two effects cannot predict how people feel about and treat these Black individuals with “mismatched” physical cues. Additionally, there is a debate over which physical characteristics play more important roles in determining people’s perceptions of Blacks. Some researchers have found that facial features are more important than skin tone when making racial distinctions (Deregowski, Ellis, & Shepherd, 1975; Sorce, 1979), whereas other researchers have found that people explicitly use Skin tone over facial features to make racial distinctions (Brown et al., 1998). For instance, Sorce (1979) had children freely categorize a number of sketches of a male face displaying a variety of racial characteristics (i.e., Skin tone, facial features) and non racial features (i.e., color of a shirt). He found that White children used facial features (especially hair features) rather than skin tone to classify the sketches. In another study, Deregowski et al. (1975) had participants describe a number of color pictures of faces, which differed in gender and race and counted the number of times that participants mentioned particular features of each face. They found that White participants mentioned eye color, hair color, and hair texture significantly more often than other features. These studies suggest that White individuals tend to use facial features more than skin tone in interracial perceptions. In contrast, Brown et al. (1998) found that both White and non-White participants explicitly reported that skin color is the most important feature when they make decisions about the race of other people, as compared to other features such as nose fullness and nose placement on face. However, all these studies used self-report measures. As several researchers have demonstrated (Blair, 2006; Blair et al., 2005; Blair et al., 2004b), the process by which Afrocentric features influence people’s perceptions of and responses to 15 Black individuals is likely to be automatic. Self-report measures would not be sufficient to address the question of whether people use facial features in addition to Skin tone when they perceive Black individuals because they would not be aware of this process. In addition, self-report measures are susceptible to social desirability. More specifically, people may think that it is inappropriate to mention skin tone of other people, because Skin tone is one of the most salient features that differentiate many racial groups. More recently, some researchers have argued that Whites use Blacks’ facial features and Skin tone when they make social judgments of Black people using implicit measures (Blair, 2006; Blair et al., 2002; 2004b; 2005; Eberhardt et al., 2004; Maddox, 2008; Oliver et al., 2004). However, this argument has no empirical support because the Operational definition of Afrocentric features used by social psychologists includes not only full lips, a wide nose, coarse hair, and dark eye color, but also dark Skin tone (Blair, 2006; Blair et al., 2002; 2004a; 2004b; 2005; Eberhardt et al., 2004; 2006; Livingston & brewer, 2002). The commonly used procedures to assess the degree of Afrocentric features ask participants to rate the degree of Afrocentiricity of target pictures using skin tone, nose, lips, eyes, and hair as indicators. For instance, Blair et al. (2002) told participants that some individuals have features that are “more typical of Afiican Americans than others, in terms of skin color, hair, eyes, nose, cheek, lips, etc.” and that their task was to rate Afrocentric features for each Black male face they viewed on a scale of 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much). Similarly, Livingston and Brewer (2002) told participants that “judgments of African American prototypicality primarily entail skin color but should also take into account facial features, eye color, hair, texture, and so on” and that their task was to rate each Black male face on a scale of 1 (low prototypic) to 5 16 (high prototypic). Although participants in both studies were told to attend to facial features in addition to skin tone, there is no way to test whether participants used all available facial information or used only skin tone information when making Afrocentric feature judgments because target pictures presented to participants included both types of information. In opposition to the researchers who argue that facial features in addition to skin tone play an important role in Whites’ perceptions of Black individuals, several skin tone researchers argue that people use skin tone as a primary cue in social perception and that skin tone (without any other facial features) is sufficient to activate the “Black” racial category and stereotypes associated with it (Dixon & Maddox, 2005; Maddox & Chase, 2004; Maddox & Gray, 2002). Maddox and his colleagues (Dixon & Maddox, 2005; Maddox & Chase, 2004; Maddox & Gray, 2002) argue that because they manipulated Skin tone while keeping other features constant, they can conclude that skin tone, not additional facial features, is the primary cue in social perception. However, it is important to note that in Dixon and Maddox’s study (2005) they used only one Black face and manipulated its skin tone. In this case, there is only one level of facial features. Without variation in facial features, whether additional features have additive or interactive effects in social perception cannot be tested. In two other studies (Maddox & Chase, 2004; Maddox & Gray, 2002), Maddox and his colleagues used six Black faces. Even though there might have been variation in additional facial features among these six faces, they did not measure the degree of variation in facial features. Therefore, it is still possible that all Six faces were high or low in stereotypical facial features. In an absence of clear 17 differences in degree of facial features in these studies, whether facial features, independent of skin tone, play a critical role in social perception cannot be examined. Taken together, although both Afrocentric features researchers and skin tone researchers have been trying to come up with an answer for the debate over “which physical cues are more important for impression formation, Skin tone or facial features?,” , neither of them successfully has answered the question, because they have failed to separate empirically these two factors. Unexamined Gender Effects in the Current Afrocentric Feature Research Prior research on Afrocentric features has had a glaring omission, which is the role of target gender. Existing studies have exclusively focused on Black men as targets, and to my knowledge, no Afrocentric features research has ever used Black women as targets. Therefore, it is unclear what the effects of Afrocentric features are for Black female targets. It is important to examine the potential gender effect on Afrocentric features for two reasons. First, gender differences have been reported in skin tone research. A number of studies have shown that many effects of skin tone (e.g., on perceptions of attractiveness, self-esteem, and self-efficacy) are different for Black men and Black women (Coard et al., 2001; Hill, 2002; Maddox, 2004; Ross, 1997; Thompson & Keith, 2001; Wade, 1996; Wade & Bielitz, 2005; Wade et al., 2004). Therefore, it is reasonable to assume that some effects of Afrocentric features as well may be different for Black men and Black women. Second, different perspectives, such as social dominance theory (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999) and multiple-identity perspective (Buchanan & Fitzgerald, 2008; Buchanan & Ormerod, 2002; Fleming, 1983; King, 2005; Landrine, l8 Klonoff, Alcaraz, Scott, & Wikins, 1995; McLeod & Owens, 2004; Rederstorff, Buchanan, & Settles, 2007; Settles, 2006), predict different gender effects. Researchers have reported that the effects of skin tone can be different between Black men and Black women in some situations (Bond & Cash, 1992; Coard et al., 2001; Hill, 2002; Maddox, 2004; Ross, 1997; Thompson & Keith, 2001; Wade, 1996; Wade & Bielitz, 2005; Wade et al., 2004). In particular, it is well documented that the effects of Skin tone on perceptions of attractiveness and mating preferences differ between men and women, such that Black men with darker Skin tone are perceived by fellow Black people to be more attractive than Black men with lighter skin tone. In contrast, Black women with lighter skin tone are perceived to be more attractive than Black women with darker skin tone (Coard et al., 2001; Hill, 2002; Maddox, 2004; Ross, 1997; Wade, 1996; Wade & Bielitz, 2005; Wade et al., 2004). Thompson and Keith (2001) have also shown that skin tone predicted the level of self-efficacy for Black men, such that light skin tone was associated with higher self-efficacy, but not for Black women. In contrast, skin tone predicted the level of self-esteem for Black women, such that lighter skin tone was associated with higher self-esteem, but not for Black men. Given that there are gender differences in the effects of skin tone in some contexts, it is possible that there are gender differences in the effects of Black individuals’ other physical characteristics in some situations as well. In addition, different theoretical perspectives predict different gender effects on Afrocentric features. For instance, social dominance theory predicts gender differences, such that Black men would experience the negative consequences of having stronger Afrocentric features, whereas Black women would not. According to social dominance l9 theory, arbitrary-set social hierarchy (e.g., hierarchy based on race, nation, or religion) mainly concerns the control of subordinate men by coalitions of dominant men, which is known as the “subordinate male target hypothesis” (Haley, Sidanius, Lowery, & Malarnuth, 2004; Sidanius, Levin, Liu, & Pratto, 2000; also see Sidanius & Pratto, 1999; and Pratto, Sidanius, & Levin, 2006 for extensive review). Indeed, it has been demonstrated that men rather than women are the primary victims of racial discrimination in a variety of contexts, such as the labor market, housing market, and criminal justice system (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). Based on this perspective, it can be predicted that Black women would not experience negative treatment by White individuals based on their Afrocentric features to the same extent as Black men do. In contrast, a multiple-identity perspective predicts a different pattern of gender effects. This perspective argues that individuals who possess more than one socially disadvantaged goup membership (e. g., Black women, low socioeconomic status Latinos) are likely to experience discrimination based on an aggregation of all disadvantaged statuses (Buchanan & Fitzgerald, 2008; Buchanan & Ormerod, 2002; Fleming, 1983; King, 2005; Landrine et al., 1995; McLeod & Owens, 2004; Rederstorff et al., 2007; Settles, 2006). According to this perspective, Black women, who possess two socially disadvantaged group memberships (i.e., race and gender), would experience both racism and sexism (Buchanan & Fitzgerald, 2008). Thus, this perspective predicts that Black women would be treated negatively by White individuals based on their Afrocentric features to the same extent as Black men or to a greater degree than Black men due to the combined effects of racism and sexism. Thus, it is unclear whether the effects of 20 Afrocentric features found in the existing research can be generalized to the entire “Black” social category. Unexamined Target Effects in the Current Afrocentric Feature Research Existing studies on Afrocentric features have focused exclusively on the effects of Afrocentric features on Whites as perceivers, ignoring two important processes involved in intergroup relations. First, previous research has failed to investigate how one’s own Afrocentric features influence Black individuals’ experiences with intergroup relations. Second, no one has examined how Blacks as perceivers perceive and react to their fellow ingroup members who vary in the degree of Afrocentric features. The “target’s perspective” (Swim & Stangor, 1998) suggests that Black individuals are active agents who contribute to the quality of interracial interactions and that personal, situational, and structural factors influence how Black individuals perceive and experience discrimination (Major & O’Brien, 2005; Major, Quinton, & McCoy, 2002; Shelton & Richeson, 2006). Afrocentric features can be considered as personal factors that may influence how Black individuals perceive and react to discrimination. More specifically, because White individuals tend to treat Black men with stronger Afi'ocentric features more negatively than Black men with weaker Afrocentric features (Blair et al., 2004; Eberhardt et al. 2006; Kahn et a1. 2008), Black individuals with stronger Afrocentric features may be able to report more personal incidents with racism, as compared to their counterparts with weaker Afrocentric features. If they have experienced more racism throughout their life, Black individuals with stronger Afrocentric features might have become more sensitive to discrimination and may tend to construe even ambiguous negative events in terms of their race. AS a result, Black 21 individuals with stronger Afrocentric features may have more negative perceptions of interactions with Whites in general and may even be more likely to avoid interactions with Whites, as compared to Black individuals with weaker Afrocentric features (i.e., a self-firlfilling prophecy, Merton, 1968). So, it is important to examine how one’s own physical characteristics influence Blacks’ feelings and thoughts in intergroup relations. How people perceive and evaluate individuals from different social groups is often different from how people perceive and evaluate individuals from their own group. Specifically, people tend to perceive members from the same social group (i.e., ingroup members) more positively than members from different social groups (i.e., outgroup members; Ashbum-Nardo, Volis, & Monteith, 2001; Brewer, 1999; Perdue, Dovidio, Gurtman, & Tyler, 1990). In fact, research using minimal groups demonstrates that favorable perceptions and treatments of ingroup members over outgroup members occur even when group membership is defined by meaningless characteristics, such as colors Of shirts or pens (Brewer, I979; Otten & Wentura, 1999: Tajfel, Billing, Bundy, & Flament, 1971). Given these well-documented findings, how Blacks feel about fellow ingroup members with different levels of skin tone and facial features may be different from how Whites feel about Black targets. Thus, it is important to examine whether the effects of physical features on people’s affective and cognitive reactions are the same for ingroup and outgroup perceivers. To conclude, there are several gaps in the current literature. First, it is unclear whether there are facial features effects on Whites’ reactions to Blacks above and beyond Skin tone effects. Second, no study to date has looked at whether the effects of physical characteristics on White individuals’ perceptions are same for Black male targets and 22 Black female targets. Third, it is unknown how Black individuals’ physical characteristics influence their own experiences with discrimination and perceptions of interracial interactions with White individuals. Additionally, no one has ever examined how Black individuals perceive and react to fellow ingroup members who vary in physical characteristics. The current studies aim to fill these gaps. Current Research In the current project, four studies were conducted to address four research questions: 1) Do facial features have effects on Whites’ affective and cognitive reactions to Black individuals above and beyond Skin tone effects? 2) Does target gender moderate the effects of physical features (i.e., skin tone and facial features) on Whites’ perceptions of Blacks? 3) Are the effects of physical features on Black perceivers different from White perceivers? and 4) What are the effects of a Black individual’s physical features on his or her perceptions of and experiences with discrimination? Study 1 was designed to address question 1. In this study, White individuals’ affective reactions, at both implicit and explicit levels, to Black men who vary in facial features and skin tone were examined. Study 2 was also designed to address question 1. In this study, Whites’ cognitive reactions (i.e., stereotype activation), instead of affective reactions, to Black men who differ in their facial features and skin tone were examined. It is important to note that for both studies 1 and 2, facial features of Black male targets were manipulated independently from skin tone, and therefore these studies were able to examine whether there are facial feature effects above and beyond skin tone and to probe for interactions between these variables. Study 3 was designed to address question 2. In this study, Whites’ affective and cognitive reactions to Black women, instead of Black 23 men, who vary in skin tone and facial features were examined. Finally, Study 4 was designed to address questions 3 and 4. This study examined whether Black individuals with more prototypical facial features, darker Skin tone, or a combination of the two, as compared to those with less prototypical facial features, lighter Skin tone, or a combination of the two, are more likely to attribute ambiguous negative events to racism, report experiences with racial discrimination, and be sensitive to racism. In addition, Study 4 also examined how Black individuals affectively and cognitively react to their fellow ingroup members who vary in skin tone and facial features. Prior to these four major studies, a pilot study was conducted to create the target stimuli, which are pictures of Black men and Black women with different levels of skin tone and with manipulated facial features. Pilot Study The purpose of this pilot study was to create four groups Of target faces of Black men and Black women that would be used in the studies: dark-skinned faces with more prototypical facial features, dark-Skinned faces with less prototypical facial features, light-skinned faces with more prototypical facial features, and light-skinned faces with less prototypical facial features. First, pictures were selected based on their actual skin tone---a group of pictures with darker skin tone was naturally and objectively different from the other group of pictures with lighter skin tone. Then, facial features of each of the selected pictures were digitally manipulated, so that one group of pictures had more prototypical facial features and the other group of pictures had less prototypical facial features. Manipulation of facial features ensured that facial features are separate item skin tone in these pictures. 24 Method Procedures The pilot study consisted of five steps. Step 1 involved creation of the pool of potential target pictures. Pictures of 120 college-age Black men and 50 college-age Black women were collected from several university athletic web Sites, the Productive Aging . 1 Lab Face Database (Minear & Park, 2004), and the MSU campus community. All pictures were in a standard head-and-shoulder format, and all faces had a neutral facial expression, with no visible accessories (e.g., eye glasses, hats). Then, pictures were digitally edited so that all background features were replaced by a solid gray background, and all clothing was replaced with a plain black t-shirt. Once the pictures were edited, four pieces of facial information (i.e., facial width and length, nose width, and lip thickness) were obtained in millimeters (see Appendix A). Then, the ratio of nose width (i.e., nose width divided by face width) and lip thickness (i.e., lip thickness divided by face length) were computed for each face. The average luminosity was also computed for each face by averaging the luminosity value obtained from each pixel across a given face. Therefore, all 170 pictures had the following information: the ratio of nose width, the ratio of lip thickness, and the average luminosity. In Step 2, 170 pictures were presented to a group of White participants to assess perceived racial group membership, attractiveness, babyfacedness, hostility, and degree of overall Afrocentric features (i.e., the ratings used by previous Afrocentric features researchers). Fourteen White participants were asked to categorize Black men and 35 White participants were asked to categorize Black women into one of the three possible categories: Black, White, or Racially Ambiguous. This ensured that all the faces used in 25 the main studies would be perceived as being unambiguously Black. Then, 190 White participants rated Black men, and 35 White participants2 rated Black women in terms of their Afrocentric features, attractiveness, babyfacedness, and hostility. The order of the four ratings was randomized across participants. In addition, the presentation of the faces within each rating category was randomized across participants. The ratings of attractiveness were used when selecting dark vs. light skin tone faces to ensure that these two groups did not differ in attractiveness. Previous studies have found that attractiveness covaries with Afrocentric features (Blair et al., 2002) and several researchers have controlled for attractiveness when selecting their target stimuli (Kahn et al., 2008; Livingston & Brewer, 2002). The ratings of hostility and babyfacedness were also obtained because previous studies have either controlled for them in stimuli selection or measured them for use as important variables that may covary with Afrocentric features (Blair et al., 2002; Livingston & Brewer, 2002). Step 3 involved the selection of target pictures that would be used in each of the three main studies. First, the average luminosity was used to select 20 dark-skinned and 20 light-skinned faces for Black male targets in order to create dark vs. light skin tone conditions. Because there were only 50 Black female pictures to begin with, all female pictures were still used in the following steps. Next, the ratios of nose width and lip thickness of each of the 40 selected male faces and all female faces were digitally altered, using Adobe Photoshop CS3, to create more vs. less prototypical facial feature conditions using the grand means and standard deviations (computed separately for Black men and Black women) of the ratio of nose width and the ratio of lip thickness computed in Step 1. For the more prototypical facial features condition, the nose and lips from each picture 26 were manipulated to reflect ratios of nose width and lip thickness that were one standard deviation above the grand mean. Likewise, for the less prototypical facial features condition, the nose and lips from each picture were manipulated to reflect ratios of nose width and lip thickness that were one standard deviation below the grand mean. Again, for this manipulation, gender-specific grand means and standard deviations (as opposed to means and standard deviations across all targets) were used. After the manipulation, all pictures were digitally transformed into outlined pictures, using Adobe Photoshop CS3, in which there is no luminosity, saturation, or hue information-~only outlines of facial features remained. All shadows on foreheads and checks in each picture were also erased (see Appendix B for an example picture). In Step 4, White participants, who did not participate in Step 2, were asked to rate the outlined pictures in terms of a degree of Afrocentric features and naturalness of the picture to ensure that the manipulation has successfully produced two sets of photos that appear natural: more vs. less prototypical facial features. F ifty—ei ght participants rated outlined Black male pictures, and 44 participants rated outlined Black female pictures.3 For the facial feature rating, participants were instructed to indicate how prototypical each of the young men and women in the pictures was in terms of "Afiican American- ness." They were further told that judgments of African American prototypicality entail facial features (e.g., lip thickness, nose width), hair texture, and so on. These instructions were Similar to the ones in Livingston and Brewer’s (2002) study, except that “skin tone” information was excluded from the current instructions. High prototypicality indicated more "African type" facial features, and low prototypicality indicated less "Afiican type" facial features. The scale ranged from 1 (Low Prototypic) to 5 (High Prototypic). For the 27 rating of naturalness, participants were told that the pictures had been transformed into outlined pictures from their original colored pictures, and that some parts of the face in a small number of pictures had been digitally altered. Participants were asked to indicate, for each of the target faces, whether they thought features on a face have been changed (in addition to transformation from color format into outline format) by choosing “Yes, I think the facial features on this face have been changed” or “No, I don't think the facial features on this face have been changed.” In Step 5, based on the information obtained in Steps 1 through 4, the final target pictures of 32 Black men and four Black women that would be used in Studies 1 - 3 were selected. Because each face was manipulated to reflect both strong and weak facial features, the final selection includes 64 Black male and eight Black female pictures (see Appendixes C and D for the actual pictures). The final set of female targets included only four dark-skinned faces and four light-skinned faces due to a lack of available female pictures that could be unambiguously categorized as Black. Importantly, some participants categorized those four pictures into White or rated as racially ambiguous; therefore, the present study serves as a preliminary study to examine target gender differences in the effects of skin tone and facial features.4 Once 64 Black male and 8 Black female pictures were selected, it was examined 1) whether dark skin tone faces were objectively darker than light Skin tone faces, and 2) whether faces manipulated to reflect more prototypical facial features were perceived to be significantly more prototypical of “Afi'ican type” than faces manipulated to reflect less prototypical facial features even when the pictures were presented in outlines (i.e., no skin tone information). Results 28 Steps 1 & 2 (Examinations of Natural Skin Tone and Natural Facial Features) Means and standard deviations of subjective ratings (i.e., overall Afrocentric features, attractiveness, babyfacedness, and hostility) and objective measures (i.e., nose ratio, lip ratio, and the average luminosity) of all faces in the original pool are presented separated by gender in Table 1. In order to get a broad sense of how each of the variables are related to one another, correlations among all variables are provided in Tables 2 and 3 separately for Black male targets and Black female targets, respectively. These correlations indicate that subjective ratings of Afrocentric features were significantly related to Objective measures of facial features and Skin tone such that those who were rated high on Afrocentric features naturally had significantly wider nose, thicker lips, and darker skin, as compared to those who were rated low on Afrocentric features, regardless of their gender. In addition, Afrocentric features were negatively related to attractiveness for both Black men and Black women, such that those with stronger Afrocentric features were perceived to be less attractive than those with weaker Afrocentric features. Furthermore, for Black men, Afrocentric features were significantly and negatively related to babyfacedness and positively related to hostility. Thus, Black men with stronger Afrocentric features were perceived to be less babyfaced and more hostile. For Black women, Afrocentric features were not significantly related to babyfacedness and hostility. This suggests that the meaning of having Afrocentric features may differ for Black men and women. Step 5 (Examinations of Skin Tone Selection and Facial Feature Manipulation) 29 In order to examine whether the faces that were selected to represent the “dark Skin tone” condition are significantly and objectively darker than faces that were selected to represent the “light skin tone” condition, independent groups t-tests were conducted on the average luminosity between 16 dark-skinned Black men and 16 light-skinned Black men, and between 2 dark-Skinned Black women and 2 light-skinned Black women. In addition, the same analyses were done on the other objective (i.e., lip and nose ratios) and subjective (i.e., Afrocentric features, attractiveness, babyfacedness, and hostility) ratings to see whether there were systematic differences between dark- and light-skinned faces on these characteristics. Means, standard deviations, and results of the t-tests are presented in Tables 4 and 5 for Black men and Black women, respectively. Independent group t-testS indicated that the selection of dark- vs. light-skinned pictures based on the average luminosity was successful, such that both Black men and women in the dark Skin tone condition had significantly lower luminosity scores than those men and women in the light skin tone condition. In addition, Black men in the dark skin condition and Black men in the light skin condition differed significantly in their Afrocentric features and hostility, such that darker-skinned men were perceived to possess more Afrocentric features and to be more hostile, as compared to their light-skinned counterparts. In contrast, Black women in dark skin tone condition and those in light Skin tone condition did not differ in these characteristics. However, it is important to note that df for Black women was 2, so that the tests were seriously underpowered. In fact, mean differences between dark- vs. light-skinned pictures in Afrocentric features and hostility were bigger for Black women and for Black men. 30 Next, it was examined whether faces manipulated to reflect more prototypical facial features were perceived to be Significantly more prototypical of “Afiican type” than faces manipulated to reflect less prototypical facial features, even without Skin tone information. Average scores for the more prototypical feature faces and less prototypical feature faces were computed within each participant, thus each participant was associated with two average prototypicality scores: one for more prototypical feature faces and one for less prototypical feature faces. Paired t-tests indicated that participants reported that the faces that were manipulated to reflect more prototypical facial features possessed more prototypical Afiican American facial features than the faces that were manipulated to reflect less prototypical facial features for both male and female targets (see Table 6), even when there was no skin tone information. The results indicate that the manipulation of facial features independent of Skin tone was successful. Taken together, 64 Black male pictures (see Appendix C) and eight Black female pictures (see Appendix D) were successfully selected to create four experimental conditions: dark-skinned more prototypical facial features, light-Skinned more prototypical facial features, dark-Skinned less prototypical facial features, and light-Skinned leSS prototypical facial features. Study 1 Study 1 aims to examine the independent effects of skin tone and facial features on Whites’ affective reactions to Black men at both implicit and explicit levels. The study was a 2 (Skin Tone: Dark, Light) x 2 (Facial Features: Strong, Weak) x 2 (Prime Set: Set 1, Set 2) mixed design, with the first two being within-participants variables and the last being a between-participants variable. Method 31 Participants Two hundred and thirty eight self-identified White undergraduate students, who . . 5 . also took part in Study 3b, participated in exchange for partial course credit. Fifty participants (21.0%) were excluded from the analyses due to at least one of the following reasons: 1) computer malfunction (39 participants), 2) non-White participants (eight participants), 3) mentioned “Skin color” or “Skin tone” when asked about the purpose of the study (nine participants), and 4) lack of attention during the task (one participant). This resulted in 186 analyzable cases. Procedures Up to six participants reported to the laboratory session and were greeted by an experimenter (one of the eight White and one Asian experimenters), who led participants to a room equipped with Six computers. Participants were informed that the study examined people’s memory and judgment skills. After obtaining consent, the experimenter explained to the participants that their task was to complete a number of short tasks on the computer; participants were then left in the room to complete the tasks. Participants first completed a modified version of a sequential priming task to assess automatic affective reactions to the targets and then completed self-report measures of liking toward the targets. They also provided their demographic information at the end of the computer task. Participants were randomly assigned to view one of the two target sets. Participants read all of the instructions on the computer as they completed each task, and the presentation of experimental stimuli and data collection were controlled by MediaLab and DirectRT software. Measures 32 Modified sequential priming task The sequential priming task used in the Livingston and Brewer’s (2002) study, which resembles the task created by Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, and Williams (1995), was modified for this study. The task is designed to assess automatic affective reactions to social categories that the primes represent. More specifically, the task assesses participants' reaction times to positive and negative words when preceded by primes (i.e., dark-Skinned Blacks with more prototypical facial features, light-Skinned Blacks with more prototypical facial features, dark-skinned Blacks with less prototypical facial features, light-Skinned Blacks with less prototypical facial features, or control Whites). Faster reaction times to negative words following a particular prime indicate more negative affective reactions to that prime type, and faster reaction times to positive words following a particular prime indicate more positive affective reactions to that prime. The faster participants respond to negative words following more prototypical facial feature Black faces or darker skin tone Black faces relative to other primes, the more we can infer negative feelings toward Black individuals with more prototypical facial features or with darker skin tone. Likewise, the faster they respond to positive words following less prototypical facial feature Black faces or lighter Skin tone Black faces relative to other primes, the more we can infer positive feelings toward Black individuals with less prototypical facial features or with lighter skin tone. The task consisted of five phases, with the first and forth phases being the important phases. The task in the first phase was described as a word-meaning task, and participants were instructed to make a judgment about whether a word presented on the computer screen was a good word or a bad word as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing appropriate keys labeled “good” or “bad.” Words were eight positive and eight 33 negative adjectives. The purpose of this phase was to obtain baseline reaction times for these words that would then be used as target words in the fourth phase. The details of the task in the first phase are as follows. First, a fixation point “*********” appeared on the computer screen for 315 ms. Then, one of the 16 words (Eight good words: beauty, joy, love, paradise, romance, smile, success, vacation, and eight bad words: cockroach, despair, disgust, garbage, pest, poison, sewage, vomit) were randomly presented on the screen until participants respond by pressing keys labeled as either “good” or “bad.” Once participants responded to that trial, the word disappeared, and the next trial started, with a 2.5 sec inter-trial interval (ITI). Response latency for each word was recorded in milliseconds. Participants first completed a practice block and then completed two blocks of trials with the key 16 words. The average latency of the two trials for each word was computed, and those values serve as the baseline latencies for each of the 16 words. The second and third phases provided a cover story for why participants were presented with pictures (i.e., primes) and why they needed to attend to those faces. In the second phase, participants completed a task described as a face-learning exercise. In this task, participants were asked to simply attend to eight faces (Six Black faces and two White faces that were not included in the target stimuli used in the critical phase) presented on the computer screen for 2.5 sec. Participants looked at the eight faces twice. The order of eight faces was randomized within each of the two sets across participants. The third phase of the study was described as a face-detection exercise, and participants were told that their task was to indicate whether they recognize the faces from the previous task by pressing a key labeled “yes” or a key labeled “no.” In this 34 phase, eight faces (four previously presented faces: three Black faces and one White face, and four new faces: three Black faces and one White face) were randomly presented on the computer screen until participants responded by pressing appropriate keys, with 2.5 sec ITI. The fourth phase was the critical phase that involved the actual priming task. In this task, participants were told that the previous tasks would be combined to examine the extent to which the judgment of word meaning is an automatic process. Participants were further told that if judging word meaning was automatic and easy, they should perform just as well at it as they did in the first phase, even if they had to learn faces at the same time. The instructions and procedures were the same as the first phase with two exceptions. First, the fixation point “*********” was replaced by target faces (i.e. primes). Second, participants were told that it was very important for them to attend to the faces presented because they would be asked to indicate whether they recognize the faces in a following face-detection task. There were two sets of target pictures, and participants were randomly assigned to complete one of the two sets. In each target set, the target pictures consisted of 40 young male faces (eight faces for each of the following five categories: dark-skinned more prototypical facial features (DM), light-skinned more prototypical facial features (LM), dark-Skinned less prototypical facial features (DL), light-Skinned less prototypical facial features (LL), and White control (W; these faces were unaltered and matched in attractiveness with the 32 target Black pictures). Again, dark vs. light skin tone were selected based on the actual variation in skin color, and more vs. less prototypical facial 35 features were experimentally manipulated, so that it was ensured that skin tone and facial features were independent of one another. First, a face was presented on the computer screen for 315 ms as a prime, followed by a 135 ms interval before onset of the target word. The target word was presented on the screen until participants responded to the trial by pressing the appropriate key identifying whether the word was “good” or “bad.” Once participants made a response, the word disappeared, and the next trial began. There was 2.5 sec ITI with a blank screen between each trial. Participants first completed a practice block that included five trials in which they were exposed to different faces and words from the actual trials. Then they completed four blocks of trials. Each block consisted of 40 trials in which each of the 40 faces appeared once followed by one of the 16 target words. Over the course of the four blocks, each face was paired with two positive and two negative words. Although trials within each block were presented in a random order, each of the 40 faces was paired with specific positive and negative words (see Appendix E for the table summary of trials and blocks). Finally, in the fifth phase, participants were asked to complete another face- detection task that they were led to expect from the instructions for the fourth phase. In this task, participants were asked to look at eight faces (four used in phase 4 and four new) and indicate whether they recognized the faces from the task they had just completed, by pressing appropriate keys labeled as either “yes” or “no.” Liking toward targets. Participants’ liking toward each target presented in Phase 4 was assessed with an explicit self-report measure. More specifically, participants were asked to indicate how much they thought they wOuld like the person in the picture. The 36 scale ranged from 0 (I would DISLIKE this person very much.) to 5 (I would LIKE this person very much), with higher scores indicating more liking toward targets. Demographic information. Participants were asked to provide their age, gender, and race. Although only participants who self-identified as White were recruited for this study using an online recruitment program, participants were also asked to provide their racial information in order to ensure that they were White. Due to a computer malfunction, participants’ age and gender were not recorded. Computing Facilitation Scores Incorrect trials as well as response latencies faster than 300 ms and Slower than 2.5 standard deviations above the individual’s mean were deleted. In order to compute useable facilitation scores, first, the log transformed response latency for any given target word following a given face (as measured in the fourth phase) was subtracted from the log transformed baseline latency of that word (as measured in the first phase) to arrive at a facilitation score. Therefore, positive values indicate that responses to the target word are facilitated compared with baseline, and negative values indicate that responses to the target word are inhibited compared with baseline. Next, mean facilitation scores for positive target words and negative target words were computed for each face (each face was followed by 2 positive and 2 negative words). Then, mean facilitation scores for positive words were averaged across the eight faces for each participant to arrive at five facilitation scores (i.e., DM, LM, DL, LL, or W faces followed by positive words). Mean facilitation scores for negative words were also averaged across the eight faces for each participant to arrive at the remaining five facilitation scores (i.e., DM, LM, DL, LL, or W faces followed by negative words). Thus, each participant ended up with 10 facilitation 37 scores. Higher facilitation scores for negative words suggest more automatic negative feelings toward preceding primes. Likewise, higher facilitation scores for positive words suggest more automatic positive feelings toward preceding primes. Facilitation scores were computed separately for positive and negative words, because it is well-documented that positive and negative affect are not a unidimensional construct, but independent of one another (Bradbum, 1969; Diener & Emmons, 1984; Goldstein & Strube, 1994; Warr, Barter, & Brwonbriedge, 1983). In fact, the Positive Affect Negative Affect Schedule was developed to capture the independent nature of positive and negative affective reactions of people (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), and this measure has been used by many personality and positive psychologists. Additionally, separately examining facilitation scores for positive and negative words allows us to see the direction of the change in attitudes. In other words, if Black men with darker skin tone are evaluated more negatively than Black men with lighter skin tone, examination of facilitation scores of both positive and negative words allows us to see whether the change in the mean reflects increased positivity or decreased negativity toward Black men with lighter skin tone, or increased negativity or decreased positivity toward Black men with darker skin tone. With a unidimensinal attitude measure, it is hard to conclude the directions of the changes in attitudes.6 Results Implicit Negative Attitudes toward Black Men The means and standard deviations of facilitation scores for the negative words for each prime category in both raw scores and log transformed scores are presented in Table 7. Higher numbers mean that participants responded faster to negative words 38 following primes, as compared to baseline responses, indicating more negative affective reactions to given primes. A three-factor mixed model ANOVA, treating skin tone (dark, light) and facial features (more prototypical, less prototypical) as within-participants factors and prime set (set I, set 2) as a between-participants factor, was computed on the facilitation scores for negative words. The main effect of Skin tone was significant, F (1 , 184) = 6.65, MSE = .01, p = .01, d = .09, such that participants responded Significantly faster to negative words following dark-Skinned Black male primes (M = 37.76, SD = 70.11),7 as compared to light-Skinned Black male primes (M = 32.64, SD = 74.34). The main effect of facial features was also Significant, F(1, 184) = 4.15, MSE = .002, p < .05, d = .07, such that participants responded faster to negative words following Black male primes with more prototypical facial features (M = 37.23, SD = 71.54), as opposed to Black male primes with less prototypical facial features (M = 33.17, SD = 72.90). These results suggest that participants evaluated dark-Skinned Black men more negatively than light-skinned Black men, and they evaluated Black men with more prototypical facial features more negatively than Black men with less prototypical facial features. The interaction between skin tone and facial features was not significant, F (1, 184) = .04, MSE = .002, p = .85. This analysis also revealed that there was no main effect of prime set type, F(1, 184) = .01, MSE = .04,p = .94. Implicit Positive Attitudes toward Black Men Next, participants’ responses to positive words following male target pictures were examined using the same three-factor mixed model ANOVA. See Table 8 for the means and standard deviations for each category. Higher numbers mean that participants responded faster to positive words following primes, as compared to baseline, indicating 39 more positive affective reactions toward given primes. The analysis revealed that none of the main effects of Skin tone (F (I, 184) = 2.60, MSE = .002, p = .1 l), facial features (F (l , 184) = .25, MSE = .001, p = .62), or set type (F(1, 184) = .40, MSE = .04, p = .53) was significant. In other words, there iS some evidence that having lighter skin tone or having less prototypical facial features does not increase Whites’ positive evaluations. Again, the interaction between skin tone and facial features was not significant, F (l , 184) = 2.15, MSE = .003,p = .14. Black vs. White Targets Next, in order to examine whether evaluations of Black men are different from the White control, a two-factor mixed model ANOVA, treating target race (Black, White) as within-participants factor and prime set (set 1, set 2) as between-participants factor on facilitation scores for negative words, was conducted. In order to compute facilitation scores for Black targets, regardless of skin tone and facial features, facilitation scores for each of the four prime categories were averaged. The ANOVA revealed that the main effect of target race was Significant, F (l, 184) = 11.78, MSE = .002, p < .001, d = .14, such that participants responded faster to negative words following Black primes (M = 35.20, SD = 66.87), as compared to following White primes (M = 35.48, SD = 72.12). The results indicate that White participants affectively react more negatively toward Black men than toward White men. There was no main effect of prime set, F (I , 184) = .10, MSE = .02,p = .75. The same analysis was conducted on facilitations scores for positive words and revealed no Significant main effect of target race (F (l , 184) = .02, MSE = .002, p = .89) or main effect of set type (F (l, 184) = .48, MSE = .02, p = .49). 40 Explicit Affective Reactions to Black Men Finally, the effects of skin tone and facial features on White participants’ explicit liking toward Black male targets were examined, and the means and standard deviations are presented in Table 9. A three-factor mixed model ANOVA, treating Skin tone (dark, light) and facial features (more prototypical, less prototypical) as within-participants factors and prime set (set I, set 2) as a between-participants factor, was conducted to examine the effects of Skin tone and facial features on Whites’ explicit reports of liking toward Black men. The ANOVA revealed that the main effect of skin tone was significant, F(1, 184) = 36.68, MSE = .08, p < .001, d = .18, such that participants liked lighter-skinned Black men significantly more (M = 3.12, SD = .73) than dark-skinned Black men (M = 2.99, SD = .70). The main effect of facial features was also significant, F (1, 184) = 13.56, MSE = .06, p < .001 , d = .09, such that participants liked Black men with less prototypical facial features significantly more (M = 3.09, SD = .71) than those with more prototypical facial features (M = 3.02, SD = .72). The interaction between skin tone and facial features was not significant, F (1 , 184) = .97, MSE = .07, p = .33. Finally, the main effect of set type was not Significant, F(1, 184) = .99, MSE = 1.80, p = .32. Discussion The goal of Study 1 was to examine whether facial features, above and beyond the skin tone, influence how Whites affectively react to Black men at both implicit and explicit levels. The results showed that, at the implicit level, White individuals responded Significantly faster to negative words following Black men with darker Skin tone, as compared to following Black men with lighter Skin tone. This indicates that Whites feel more negatively toward Black men with darker Skin tone than toward Black men with 41 lighter skin tone. This finding is consistent with previous studies reporting that Whites perceive Black individuals with darker skin tone more negatively than those with lighter skin tone (Anderson & Cromwell, I977; Averhadt & Bigler, 1997; Hall, 1992; Maddox & Gray, 2002; Dixson & Maddox, 2005; Porter, 1991). Importantly, the results fiom the present study also Showed that White individuals responded Significantly faster to negative words following Black men with more prototypical facial features than following Black men with less prototypical facial features. This indicates that Whites felt more negatively toward Black men with more prototypical facial features than toward those with less prototypical facial features. Thus, the results suggest that White individuals do detect such subtle differences in facial features (in addition to the differences in Skin tone) in a very Short amount of time (i.e., 315 ms), and the differences in facial features influence how they feel about Black male targets. Taken together, the effects of skin tone and facial features on Whites’ affective reactions toward Black men were additive, and having darker Skin tone or more prototypical facial features increases White individuals’ negativity toward Black men. Interestingly, there was no difference in how fast White individuals responded to positive words following primes. In other words, there is evidence that having lighter skin tone or less prototypical facial features does not increase Whites’ positive feelings toward Black men. The findings suggest that the previously reported findings that Black men with darker Skin tone and stronger Afrocentric features were perceived and treated more negatively (Blair, 2006; Blair et al., 2005; Blair et al., 2002; Dixon & Maddox, 2005; Livingston & Brewer, 2002; Maddox & Chase, 2004; Maddox & Gray, 2002; 42 Wade & Bielits, 2005) are due to increased negative perceptions of such individuals, rather than increased positive perceptions of Black men who are less prototypical. The results for explicit liking were consistent with those of implicit attitudes, such that both Skin tone and facial features independently influenced how much White individuals liked Black male targets. More specifically, Whites liked Black men with lighter Skin tone more than those with darker Skin tone, as well as Black men with less prototypical facial features more than those with more prototypical facial features. Taken together, the findings from Study 1 suggest that facial features are as informative as skin tone when predicting White individuals’ feelings about Black men both at implicit and explicit levels. Specifically, skin tone and facial features seem to affect how much White individuals dislike Blacks. Past studies have shown that skin tone and Afrocentric features influence not only Whites’ affective reactions toward Blacks, but also their cognitive reactions toward Blacks (i.e., stereotype activation; Averhadt & Bigler, 1997; Blair, 2006; Blair et al., 2002). In the next study, the independent effects of Skin tone and facial features on stereotype activation following Black male primes among Whites were examined. I Study 2 The study was a 2 (skin tone: dark, light) x 2 (facial features: more prototypical, less prototypical) x 2 (word type: stereotypic, non-stereotypic) x 4 (prime set: set la, set lb, set 2a, set 2b) mixed design, with the first three independent variables being within- participants factors, and the last independent variable being a between-participants variable. Furthermore, the study examined explicit liking toward Black men. Method 43 Participants Two hundred and forty six self-identified White undergraduate students, who also took part in Study 3a, participated in this study. Forty-four participants (17.9 °/o) were excluded from data analyses based on the same exclusion criteria as Study 1 (i.e., computer malfunction, non—White participants, mentioned “skin color” or “skin tone” when asked about the study purpose, and lack of attention). Thirty participants were excluded due to computer malfunction, six participants were excluded because they were not White, four were excluded because they mentioned “skin color” or “skin tone” when asked about the purpose of the study, and seven were excluded due to lack of attention during the computer task. This resulted in 202 analyzable cases. Procedures Participants reported to the laboratory session in a group (up to Six participants per session) and were greeted by either a White American or an Asian American experimenter. The experimenter led participants to a room equipped with six computers and informed them that the purpose of the study was to examine people’s memory and judgment skills. After Obtaining consent, participants were told that they would complete a few tasks on the computer, and they were left in the room to complete the computer tasks. Participants first completed a modified version of a lexical decision task, which assesses the degree of activation of stereotypes associated with Black men. After completing the lexical decision task, participants filled out self-report measures of liking toward the targets and provided their demographic information (i.e., age, gender, and race). Again, all the instructions were provided on the computer screen as participants proceeded at their own pace, and the presentation of experimental stimuli and data collection were controlled by MediaLab and DirectRT software. Measures Modified lexical decision task (LDT). The original LDT created by Wittenbrink, Judd, and Park (1997) was modified for this study. The task is designed to assess participants' reaction times to negative words that are either stereotypic or non- stereotypic of Black men when preceded by primes (i.e., dark-skinned Blacks with more prototypical facial features, light-Skinned Blacks with more prototypical facial features, dark-skinned Blacks with less prototypical facial features, light—Skinned Blacks with less prototypical facial features, or control Whites). The faster participants respond to stereotypic words following more prototypical facial features or dark-skinned Black faces as opposed to less prototypical facial features or light-skinned Black faces, the more we can infer that stereotypes associated with the category “Black” are activated. In the LDT, a fixation point “*********” first appeared on the computer screen for 1.0 sec, and it was immediately followed by the prime (i.e., a target face). The target faces presented during the task differed depending on the assigned prime set. However, each set included the five target categories. The prime stayed on the screen for 315 ms, and a target word (one of the 24 words or 24 non-words) followed (see Appendix F for the detailed explanation of how the target words were selected). Participants’ task was to identify whether the target word presented on the computer screen was an actual word or a non-word as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing appropriate keys labeled “W” or “NW.” The target word stayed on the screen until participants responded by pressing the appropriate key. 45 Once participants responded to that trial, the next trial started with a 2.5 sec ITI. Response latency for each word/non-word was recorded in milliseconds. Each of the five prime categories occurred on 48 different trials. On 12 trials, target words were real words related to negative Black male stereotypes (i.e., creepy, dangerous, ignorant, incompetent, lazy, mean, noisy, poor, reckless, threatening, untrustworthy, violent). On another 12 trials, target words were real words that are negative but unrelated to Black male stereotypes (i.e., annoying, arrogant, boring, condescending, greedy, im'tating, judgmental, selfish, smelly, stuckup, ugly, weak). On the remaining 24 trials, target words were non-words. Thus, participants completed a total of 240 trials. Participants firSt completed a practice block including 10 trials, and then they completed the actual trial. In order to conceal the actual purpose of the LDT, participants were told that the task was designed to examine their ability to perform two different tasks (i.e., a memory task and a judgment task) Simultaneously. Furthermore, participants were told that they should try to memorize the faces (i.e., primes) while completing the word/non-word judgment task because they would perform a memory task at the end of the task. In actuality, participants did not complete the memory task. Self-report measures. The same measures used in Study 1 (i.e., liking toward targets and demographic information) were used in Study 2. Computing Response Latencies First, incorrect trials as well aS response latencies faster than 300 ms and Slower than 2.5 standard deviations above the individual mean were deleted. After performing the log transformation, average response latency of 12 negative words that were 46 stereotypic of Black men and average response latency of 12 negative words that were non-stereotypic of Black men were computed for each of the five prime categories. Thus, 10 response latency scores were computed for each participant. Results Stereotype Activation among Whites Table 10 presents means and standard deviations of response latencies in both raw and log transformed scores for each prime category for the stereotypic and non- stereotypic words. In order to examine the effects of skin tone and facial features on stereotype activation, a four-factor mixed model of ANOVA, treating skin tone (dark, light), facial features (more prototypical, less prototypical), and word type (stereotypic, non-stereotypic) as within-participants factors and prime set (set 1a, set lb, set 2s, set 2b) as a between-participants factor, was conducted on the response latencies. The main effect of word type was Significant, F(1, 198) = 25.73, MSE = .004,p < .001, d = .l I, such that participants responded significantly faster to stereotypic words (M = 583.09, SD = 98.63) than to non-stereotypic words (M = 593.12, SD = 98.55). Neither the main effect of skin tone (F (l , 198) = .l l, MSE = .004, p = .74) nor the main effect of facial features (F (l, 198) = .71, MSE = .003, p = .40) was significant. The ANOVA revealed that there was no main effects of prime set, F(3, 198) = .82, MSE = .17, p = .48, suggesting that picture sets and presentation of order did not have effects on participants’ responses. None of the two-way interactions between skin tone and facial features (F (l , 198) = 1.65, MSE = .004, p = .20), between Skin tone and word type (F(1, 198) = .11, MSE = .004, p = .74), and between facial features and word type (F(1, 198) = .01, MSE = .004, p = .94) was significant. However, the three-way interaction between skin tone, facial 47 features, and word type was significant, F (l, 198) = 7.20, MSE = .004, p < .01, indicating that the relationship between skin tone and facial features on response latencies differed depending on whether the words were stereotypic or non-stereotypic. Examination of the simple interactions between skin tone and facial features at different levels of word type revealed that the two-way interaction was significant in the non-stereotypic word condition (F (l, 198) = 7.66, MSE = .004, p < .01), but not in the stereotypic word condition (F (I, 198) = .90, MSE = .004, p = .34). In order to further examine the nature of the two-way interaction between skin tone and facial features in the non-stereotypic word condition, the simple main effect of facial features was examined in the dark Skin tone condition and in the light skin tone condition separately. The analyses revealed that the simple main effect of facial features was significant in the light skin tone condition, t(201) = -2.25, p < .05, d = .10, such that participants responded faster to non-stereotypic negative words following Black men with strong facial features than following Black men with weak facial features. The simple main effect of facial features in the dark skin tone condition was not Significant, t(201) = 1.58, p = .12. Black vs. White Targets Next, the degree of stereotype activation following Black male primes was compared with stereotype activation following White male primes. A three-way mixed model ANOVA, treating target race (Black, White) and word type (stereotypic, non- stereotypic) as within-participants factors and prime set (set 1a, set 1b, set2a, set2b) as a between-participants factor, was conducted on response latencies. The analysis revealed that that the main effect of word type was significant, F (l, 198) = 11.94, MSE = .002, p < .001, d = .07, such that White participants responded faster to stereotypic words (M = 48 583.07, SD = 94.83), as compared to non-stereotypic words (M = 590.03, SD = 92.48), regardless of target race. The main effect of target race was marginally significant, F (1, 198) = 3.21, MSE = .002, p = .08, d = .03, such that participants responded faster to negative words, regardless of word type, following White primes (M = 584.92, SD = 95.60) than following Black primes (M = 588.18, SD = 91.72). Neither the main effect of prime set (F (3, 198) = .99, MSE = .08, p = .40) nor the interaction between target race and word type (F(l , 198) = .82, MSE = .003, p = .48) was significant. The absence of mean difference between Black primes and White control in the stereotypic word condition (which has been reported in the previous studies), suggests that the lexical decision task in this study might not have been effective. Explicit Affective Reactions to Black Men Finally, a three-factor mixed model ANOVA, treating skin tone and facial features as within-participants factors and prime set as a between-participants factor, was conducted on explicit reports of liking toward Black men. The means and standard deviations of Whites’ explicit liking toward Black men as a function of skin tone and facial features are presented in Table 11. The main effect of skin tone was Significant, F(1, 198) = 58.07, MSE = .06, p < .001, d = .18, such that White participants liked light- Skinned Black men significantly more (M = 3.10, SD = .74) than dark-skinned Black men (M = 2.97, SD = .73). Although the pattern of the means was in the same direction as Study 1a, the main effect of facial features was not significant, F(l , 198) = 1.86, MSE = .O7,p = .17. Neither the main effect of prime set (F(3, 198) = 1.83, MSE = 1.95, p = .14) nor the interaction between skin tone and facial features (F (1 , 198) = .07, MSE = .04, p = .79) was Significant. 49 Discussion Study 2 examined whether facial features and Skin tone independently affect stereotype activation among Whites. More specifically, it investigated how Black male targets, who vary in Skin tone and facial features, influence Whites’ stereotype activation. The results Showed that White individuals responded significantly faster to stereotypic negative words, as compared to non-stereotypic negative words. At first glance, the findings suggest that negative stereotypes associated with Black men were activated following Black primes, regardless of the degree of Skin tone and prototypicality of facial features. However, it is important to note that White individuals’ response time to stereotypic negative words following White controls were not different from those following Black primes. The absence of mean differences in reaction time to stereotypic words between Black primes and White controls suggests that the modified lexical decision task might have failed to assess stereotype activation. Potential explanations for the low validity of this task are presented in the general discussion. Study 2 also looked at White individuals’ explicit affective reactions toward Black men, replicating Study 1. The results showed that Skin tone influenced Whites’ liking toward Black men, such that light-skinned Black men were liked more than dark- skinned Black men. Although the patterns of the mean liking scores between Black men with more prototypical facial features and Black men with less prototypical facial features were consistent with Study 1 (i.e., White individuals liked Black men with more prototypical facial features less than those with less prototypical facial features), the effects of facial features was not Significant in this study. 50 Studies 1 and 2 examined how Black men’s skin tone and facial features influence Whites’ affective and cognitive reaction toward them, and they demonstrated that the effects of skin tone and facial features are additive and that having either darker skin tone or more prototypical facial features increase Whites’ negative feelings. However, it is still unclear whether there are additive effects Of skin tone and facial features when the targets were Black women, as previous research has exclusively focused on Black male targets. In the next study, it was examined whether the effects Of skin tone and facial features would be moderated by target gender. Study 3 Study 3 examined the independent effects of skin tone and facial features on Whites’ affective and cognitive reactions toward Black women. There are two sub-studies, with Study 3a investigating Whites’ affective reactions to Black women at both implicit and explicit levels, and Study 3b examining Whites' stereotype activation following Black female primes. Study 3b also looks at Whites’ explicit liking toward Black women, replicating Study 3a. Study 3a was a 2 (Skin Tone: Dark, Light) x 2 (Facial Features: More Prototypical, Less Prototypical) x 2 (Prime Set: Set 1, Set 2) mixed design, with the first two being within-participants factors and the last being a between-participants factor. Study 3b was a 2 (Skin Tone: Dark, Light) x 2 (Facial Features: More Prototypical, Less Prototypical) x 2 (Word Type: Stereotypic, Non-Stereotypic) x 2 (Prime Set: Set I, Set 2) mixed design, with the first three being within-participants factors and the last being a between-participants factor. It is important to note that the participants in this study also completed either Study 1 or Study 2 first. Participants who completed the sequential priming task with 51 Black male targets (i.e., Study 1) first were asked to complete the lexical decision task with Black female targets (i.e., Study 3b) second, and those who completed the lexical decision task with Black male targets (i.e., Study 2) first were asked to completed the sequential priming task with Black female targets (i.e., Study 33) second. Although the ideal design would have been to counterbalance the order of the target gender, all participants completed the task with Black female targets after they completed tasks involving the Black male targets. This approach was taken because there were only a few Black female target photographs that could be used in the study, and therefore results based on the female targets should be considered as preliminary. Note that the small number of female targets resulted in considerably shorter sequential priming and lexical decision tasks, as compared to the tasks created for Black male targets. Study 3a Methods Participants Two hundred and forty Six self-identified White undergraduate students participated in this study. Again, the participants completed the lexical decision task (i.e., Study 2) before completing this study. The same exclusion criteria as Studies 1 and 2 were used (i.e., computer malfunction, non-White participants, mentioned “skin color” or “skin tone” when asked about the study purpose, and lack of attention) with one added requirement that participants perceived female targets as unambiguously Black. This requirement was added because female target pictures were not perceived as unambiguously Black by everyone in the pilot study. Participants who categorized the target pictures as either racially ambiguous or White were excluded from the analyses. 52 Based on these six exclusion criteria, 95 participants (40.4%) were excluded from the analyses (50 participants due to computer malfunction, six participants who were not Whites, four participants who mentioned “Skin color” or “skin tone” when asked about the purpose of the study, seven participants due to lack of attention, and 47 participants who categorized the target women as non-Black). Thus, 140 participants were included in the following analyses. Procedures The procedures were the same as Study 1 except for two modifications. First, length of the sequential priming task in this study was considerably shorter than the task in Study 1. The sequential priming task for female Black targets included only eight target words (Four good words: paradise, romance, smile, success, and four bad words: despair, pest, poison, sewage), instead of 16 words. This was due to the lack of useable female target stimuli. Second, there were only five target pictures in this task (i.e., one target picture for each prime category: DM, DL, LM, LL, W) in each target picture set, unlike Study 1 in which there were 40 targets pictures in total in each target picture set. After completing the sequential priming task with female target primes, participants also completed the explicit liking measure and provided demographic information. Again, due to a computer malfunction, participants’ age and gender were not recorded. Results Implicit Negative Attitudes toward Black Women Participants’ responses to negative words following female target pictures were examined using a three-factor mixed model ANOVA, treating skin tone (dark, light) and 53 facial features (more prototypical, less prototypical) as within-participants factors and prime set (set 1, set 2) as a between-participants factor. See Table 12 for the means and standard deviations for each prime category. The analysis revealed that none of the main effects of Skin tone (F(l , 138) = .17, MSE = .01, p = .68), facial features (F (1, 138) = .0001, MSE = .01,p = .98 or set type (F(1, 138) = 1.84, MSE = .05,p = .18) was Si gnificant. Thus, there is evidence that having darker Skin tone or more prototypical facial features do not increase negative evaluations of Black women. The interaction between skin tone and facial features was not Si gnificant either, F (l, 138) = .41 , MSE = .01 , p = .33. Implicit Positive Attitudes toward Black Women Next, participants’ responses to positive words were examined using the same three-factor mixed model of ANOVA. The means and standard deviations for each prime category are presented in Table 13. The analysis revealed that the main effect of skin tone was Significant, F(1, 138) = 10.43, MSE = .01,p < .01, d = .15, such that participants responded faster to positive words following light—skinned Black female primes (M = 38.05, SD = 88.29) than following dark-skinned Black female primes (M = 23.72, SD = 98.17). The results suggest that Ii ght-Skinned Black women were evaluated more positively than dark-Skinned Black women. In contrast, the main effect of facial features was not Significant, F (l, 138) = .02, MSE = .01, p = .90, indicating that facial features did not have an additive effect on Whites’ positive attitudes toward Black women. The interaction between skin tone and facial features was not Significant, F (l, 138) = 1.13, MSE = .01, p = .29. Again, there was no main effect of set type, F(1, 138) = .01, MSE = .07,p = .94. 54 Black vs. White Targets In order to examine whether evaluations of Black women are different from the White control, a two-factor mixed model ANOVA, treating target race (Black, White) as a within-participants factor and prime set (set I, set 2) as a between-participants factor, on facilitation scores for negative words was conducted first. Again, in order to come up with facilitation scores for Black women, regardless of skin tone and facial features, facilitation scores for four prime types were averaged. The ANOVA revealed no Significant main effect of target race, F (1, 138) = .48, MSE = .004, p = .49). The main effect ofprime set was marginally Significant, F(1, 138) = 3.81, MSE = .03,p = .05, suggesting that participants’ negativity toward Black, as compared to White, targets differed depending on pictures. This might be due to the fact that there were only four Black female pictures and one White female picture in each set. The same analysis was conducted on facilitation scores for positive words and revealed that neither the main effect of target race, F (l, 138) = .14, MSE = .004, p = .71, nor the main effect of prime set, F(1, 138) = .001, MSE = .03, p = .98, was significant. Explicit Affective Reactions to Black Women The means and standard deviations of Whites’ explicit liking toward Black women as a function of skin tone and facial features are presented in Table 14. A three- factor mixed model ANOVA, treating skin tone and facial features as within-subjects factors and prime set type as a between-subjects factor, was conducted to examine the effects Of skin tone and facial features on explicit reports of liking toward Black women. The ANOVA revealed that the main effect of skin tone was significant, F (1 , 138) = 17.29, MSE = .50, p < .001, d = .27, such that participants liked light-Skinned Black women 55 more (M = 3.02, SD = .92) than dark-Skinned Black women (M = 2.78, SD = .91). The main effect of facial features was not Significant, F(1, 138) = 1.36, MSE = .37, p = .25. The interaction between skin tone and facial features also was not Si gnificant, either, F (1, 138) = .02, MSE = .45, p = .89. Finally, the main effect of prime set was not Significant, F(1, 138) = .07, MSE = 1.88, p = .80. The results of explicit liking were consistent with the results of implicit positive attitudes. Study 3b Next, Study 3b examined the independent effects of skin tone and facial features on stereotype activation among Whites. The study also examined Whites’ explicit attitudes toward Black women. Methods Participants Two hundred and thirty eight self-identified White students participated in this study. Again, the participants also completed the sequential priming task with Black male targets (i.e., Study 1) before completing this study. The same exclusion criteria as Study 3a were used in this study. Based on the six exclusion criteria, 63 participants (30.3 %) were excluded from the analyses (39 participants due to computer malfunction, eight participants who were not Whites, nine participants who mentioned “skin color” or “Skin tone” when asked about the purpose of the study, one participants due to lack of attention, and 29 participants who categorized the target women as non-Black). Thus, 145 participants were included in the following analyses. Procedures 56 The procedures were the same as Study 2 with two modifications. First, the lexical decision task for female Black targets included only six words stereotypic of Black women (i.e., annoying, initating, noisy, nosey, rude, stubborn) and Six words non- stereotypic of Black women (i.e., exploitative, selfish, sheltered, Sloppy, slow, stuckup). Second, only five target pictures (i.e., one target picture for each prime category: DM, DL, LM, LL, W) were used in each target set due to lack of usable Black female pictures. After completing LDT, participants also filled out the same self-report measures used in the previous studies (i.e., liking toward targets and demographic information). Results Stereotype Activation among Whites Table 15 presents means and standard deviations of response latencies in both raw and log transformed scores for each prime category. A four-factor mixed model of ANOVA, treating skin tone (dark, light), facial features (more prototypical, less prototypical), and word type (stereotypic, non-stereotypic) as within-participants factors and prime set (set 1, set 2) as a between-participants factor, was conducted on the response latencies. The main effect of word type was Significant, F (1, 143) = 7.70, MSE = .01, p < .01, d = .11, such that participants responded Significantly faster to stereotypic words (M = 625.91, SD = 109.12) than to non-stereotypic words (M = 641.34, SD = 151.47), suggesting that stereotypic words were more accessible to White participants than non-stereotypic words. Neither the main effect of skin tone (F (1, 143) = .01, MSE = .01,p = .92) nor the main effect of facial features (F(1, 143) = .23, MSE = .01, p = .63) was Significant. The ANOVA revealed that there was no main effects of prime set, F ( 1 , 143) = .95, MSE = .23,p = .33. 57 The two-way interactions between skin tone and facial features (F (1, 143) = .28, MSE = .01, p = .60) and between skin tone and word type (F (l, 143) = .02, MSE = .01, p = .89) were not significant. However, the two-way interaction between facial features and word type reached marginal Significance, F (1 , 143) = 3.60, MSE = .01 , p = .06. In order to investigate the nature of the two-way interaction, the Simple main effect of facial features was examined within each of the word condition. In the stereotypic word condition, the main effect of facial features was not significant, t(144) = .88, p = .38. In contrast, in the non-stereotypic word condition, the main effect of facial features was marginally significant, t(144) = 1.75, p = .08, d = .08, such that participants responded somewhat faster to non-stereotypic negative words following Black women with less prototypical facial features (M = 635.85, SD = 134.83), as compared to following Black women with more prototypical facial features (M = 646.83, SD = 147.61), regardless of the female targets’ Skin tone. Finally, the three-way interaction between Skin tone, facial features, and word type was not significant, F(1, 143) = .39, MSE = .01, p = .53. Black vs. White Targets Next, a three-way mixed model ANOVA, treating target race (Black, White) and word type (stereotypic, non-stereotypic) as within-participants factors and prime set (set 1, set 2) as a between-participants factor, was conducted on response latencies in order to examine whether the degree of stereotype activation following Black primes was different fi'om stereotype activation following White primes. In order to come up with response latencies following Black primes, regardless of physical characteristics, response latencies for four Black prime categories were averaged. The ANOVA revealed significant main effect oftarget race, F(1, 143) = 8.79, MSE = .01,p < .01, d = .10, such 58 that White participants responded faster to negative words, following White primes (M = 620.24, SD = 143.99) than following Black primes (M = 633.63, SD = 127.32), regardless of whether the words were stereotypic or non-stereotypic. The main effect of word type was also Significant, F(1, 143) = 26.84, MSE = .01,p < .001, d = .23, such that White participants responded faster to stereotypic words (M = 611.55, SD = 121.37) than non- stereotypic words (M = 642.32, SD = 149.94). However, these main effects were qualified by the Significant interaction between target race and word type, F (1 , 143) = 8.10, MSE = .01 , p < .01. In order to examine the nature of the two-way interaction, the effect of target race was examined within each word type. In the stereotypic word condition, White participants responded faster following White primes (M = 597.19, SD = 124.44), as compared to Black primes (M = 625.91, SD = 118.31), t(l44) = 3.93, p < .001, d = .24. This suggests there was greater stereotype activation when participants looked at White female pictures, as compared to when they looked at Black female pictures. In contrast, there was no difference in response latencies between Black (M = 641.34, SD = 136.34) and White targets (M = 643.29, SD = 163.54) when White participants responded to non-stereotypic words, t(l44) .08, p = .94. The main effect of prime set was not significant, F(1, 143) = .2584, MSE .11, p = .62. The findings that participants responded faster to stereotypic words that are supposed to be associated with Black women following White primes, as compared to Black primes, suggest that the lexical decision task was not effective. Explicit Affective Reactions to Black Women The means and standard deviations of Whites’ explicit liking toward Black women as a function of skin tone and facial features are presented in Table 16. The three- 59 SU factor mixed model ANOVA on explicit reports of liking toward Black women revealed that the main effect of skin tone was significant, F (1, 143) = 10.56, MSE = .40, p < .001, d = .22, such that White participants liked light-skinned Black women significantly more (M = 2.92, SD = .41) than dark-skinned Black women (M = 2.73, SD = .93). Neither the main effect of facial features (F (l, 143) = .31, MSE = .40, p = .58) nor the main effect of prime set (F(1, 143) = .22, MSE = 2.12,p = .64) was Significant. However, the two-way interaction between skin tone and facial features was marginally Significant, F (l, 143) = 3.1 l, MSE = .35, p = .08. Examination of the mean suggests that White participants liked Black women with more prototypical facial features to the same degree as Black women with less prototypical facial features when Black women had lighter skin tone. In contrast, when Black women had darker skin tone, White participants liked Black women with less prototypical facial features more than Black women with more prototypical facial features. The pattern of the means suggest that facial features have negative effects on Black women when they have darker skin tone but not when they have lighter skin tone. Discussion The goal of Study 3 was to examine how Skin tone and facial features influence Whites’ affective and cognitive reactions toward Black women and investigates the potential moderating role of target gender. Interestingly, the results from Study 3a suggest that the effects of Skin tone and facial features are different between Black female targets and Black male targets. Study 3a showed that there was no difference in how fast White individuals responded to negative words following the primes. Remember that White participants responded faster to negative words following Black men with darker 60 skin tone or those with more prototypical facial features. The absence of the mean differences in negative attitudes toward Black women measured at the implicit level suggests that having darker skin tone or more prototypical facial features do not necessarily increase Whites’ negative feelings toward Black women. Instead, Whites responded Significantly faster to positive words following Black women with lighter Skin tone, as compared to following Black women with darker skin tone, indicating that Whites felt more positively toward Black women with lighter skin tone than toward those with darker skin tone. Again, the findings suggest that there are differences between Black female targets and Black male targets, as Study 1 showed that having lighter Skin tone does not increase Whites’ positive feelings toward Black men. In addition, unlike Black male targets, facial features did not have an additive effect on Whites’ attitudes toward Black women. The results for explicit liking toward Black women were consistent with those of implicit attitudes. White participants liked Black women with lighter Skin tone more than Black women with darker Skin tone. Although Study 3a showed that there were no effects of facial features on Whites’ explicit liking toward Black women, the effects of facial features emerged in Study 3b. Interestingly, there were facial features effects when Black women had darker skin tone, but not when they had lighter skin tone. More specifically, dark-Skinned Black women with less prototypical facial features were more liked by Whites, as compared to dark-Skinned Black women with more prototypical facial features. However, these effects were marginally significant. Thus, the results of explicit attitudes were consistent between Studies 3a and 3b. The findings that Whites liked Black women with lighter skin tone than those with darker skin tone are consistent with the previous 61 studies Showing that people perceive and treat Black women differently based on their skin tone (Keith & Herring, 1991; Maddox & Gray, 2002; Wade et al., 2004). However, unlike the previous studies arguing that Black women with darker skin tone are perceived and treated more negatively, the present study (which was able to examine the direction of attitude change) demonstrates that Black women with lighter skin tone are perceived and treated more positively. Just like Study 2, Study 3b showed no evidence that skin tone or facial features influenced how Whites cognitively react to Black women, as White individuals responded significantly faster to stereotypic negative words, as compared to non- stereotypic negative words, following Black female primes, regardless of skin tone and prototypicality of facial features. Again, it is important to point out that the lexical decision task modified for this study might have failed to capture activation of stereotypes associated with Black women. This is because White participants responded significantly faster to stereotypic words associated with Black women following White controls, as compared to following Black primes. Taken together, Studies 1 and 3a demonstrated that the nature of the skin tone effects is different between Black men and Black women, such that having darker skin tone increases Whites’ negativity for Black men, but not for Black women, and that having lighter skin tone increases Whites’ positivity for Black women, but not for Black men. In addition, these studies showed that White individuals use facial features when forming impressions of Blacks only when the targets were men. Studies 1 through 3 examined the independent effects of skin tone and facial features on how Whites affectively and cognitively react to Blacks. These studies 62 demonstrated that different physical characteristics affect White perceivers’ feelings and thoughts about Black individuals in different ways and that these effects are different depending on target gender. However, there are still missing pieces of information in the current research—the effects of target physical characteristics on Black perceivers. In the next study, the effects of Skin tone and facial features are investigated from the Blacks’ perspectives. Study 4 Study 4 examined how one’S own Skin tone and facial features influence Black individuals’ 1) tendency to attribute ambiguous negative events to racism, 2) experiences with racism, and 3) sensitivity to racism. Furthermore, Study 4 also investigated how Blacks respond, both affectively and cognitively, to fellow ingroup members who vary in skin tone and facial features. Method Participants Eighty-four self-identified Black undergraduate students (women = 85.7 %, M age = 19.51, SD = 2.44 years old) participated in this study. One female participant was excluded from the data, because she did not agree to release her photo. Procedures Prior to the laboratory session, participants were required to complete an online questionnaire that included measures of racial group identification, self-esteem, past experience with discrimination, perception of discrimination, and demographic information (see Appendix G for a list of measures and items for each measure). Participants later reported to the laboratory session in a group (up to five participants per 63 session) and were greeted by either a White American or an Asian American experimenter. The experimenter led participants to a room equipped with five computers and informed participants that the purpose of the study was to understand how personality influences the way people interpret and respond to certain situations. After providing consent, participants were told that they would complete a few tasks on the computer. All instructions were provided on the computer screen as participants proceeded at their own pace. In the first task, participants were told to read a series of scenarios and imagine that they had experienced the negative events described in the scenarios. Participants were then asked to attribute each negative event to several potential causes. In the second task, participants were asked to complete a race-based rejection sensitivity measure in which they were asked to read several scenarios that described negative events and indicate how concerned they would be about the events. After completing the attribution and rejection sensitivity measures, participants completed the modified sequential priming and lexical decision tasks from Studies 1 through 3. Participants were told that these computer tasks examine people’s memory and judgment Skills. Of 83 participants, 41 participants completed the sequential priming task with male target pictures and the lexical decision task with female target pictures.9 The other 42 participants completed the sequential priming task with female target pictures and the lexical decision task with male target pictures. 10 Again, all participants completed the tasks with Black female targets second because these tasks were considerably shorter than tasks for Black male targets and they were preliminary. At the end of the computer tasks, all participants were also asked to report their liking toward both male and female targets. 64 Once all participants in a session completed the computer tasks, the experimenter took three different photographs of them. For the first picture, participants were told to make a neutral facial expression. For the second picture, participants were asked to think of a recent event that made them really happy and facially express that emotion. For the third picture, participants were asked to think of a recent event that made them really angry and facially express that emotion. Three different pictures were taken to create a pool of facial stimuli that will be used in future research. However, in the current study, only the neutral faces of these participants were used to record their Skin tone and facial features. After obtaining the pictures, participants were fully debriefed. More specifically, participants were told that the research examined the relation between participants’ physical characteristics (i.e., facial features and skin tone) and their sensitivity to racial discrimination. Participants were further told that, in order to examine the relation, it was important for the researchers to analyze participants’ facial pictures in terms of facial features and skin tone. After the debriefing process, participants were asked whether their pictures could be used for analyses of Skin tone and facial features. If they agreed to release their pictures for physical feature analyses, the experimenter obtained participants’ signature on the picture release form. Measures Pre-Iaboratory questionnaires. Group identification. The importance of being Black to participants’ sense of self was assessed with Luhtanen and Crocker’s (1992) four-item identity centrality subscale: “Overall, my racial group membership has very little to do with how I feel about myself 65 (R),” “The racial group I belong to is an important reflection of who I am,” “The racial group I belong to is unimportant to my sense of what kind of person I am (R),” and “In general, belonging to my racial group iS an important part of my self-image.” The scale ranged from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree), with higher numbers indicating that participants strongly identified with Black (a = .68). Self-esteem. Participants’ self-esteem was assessed with Rosenberg’s (1965) 10- item measure and with Heatherton and Polivy’s (1991) 20-item state self-esteem scale, which consists of three subscales: social, performance, and appearance. Example items are: “I feel that I am a person of worth, at least on an equal basis with others,” “ I am worried about what other people think of me,” and “I feel displease with myself.” The scale ranged from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree), with higher scores indicating higher self-esteem. Scale reliabilities for subscales were: a = .85 for social, a = .84 for performance, and a = .80 for appearance. Scale reliability for overall self- esteem was a = .90. Past experiences with discrimination. Participants’ experiences with racial discrimination in the past were assessed with Landrine and Klonoff’s (1996) l8-item Schedule of Racist Events. Each of the first 17 items provided a Specific type of racial discrimination and asked three questions. The first two questions asked participants how often they experienced this type of racial discrimination in the past year (i.e., experiences with discrimination in the past 1 year) and in their entire life (i.e., lifetime experiences with discrimination), respectively. Example items include “How many times have you been treated unfairly by teachers and professors because you are Black?,” and “How many times have you been treated unfairly by your employers, bosses, and supervisors 66 because you are Black?.” The scale ranged from 1 (Never) to 6 (Almost All of the Time). The third question asked participants how stressful each type of racial discrimination was for them (i.e., appraisal of discrimination), and the scale ranged from 1 (Not at All) to 6 (Extremely). Finally, the last item in the measure asked participants how different their life would be if they were not treated in an unfair way in the past year and in their entire life. For this last item, the scale ranged from 1 (Same as Now) to 6 (Totally Different). Higher scores indicate more frequent and stressful experiences with racial discrimination. Scale reliability for experiences with discrimination in the past 1 year was a = .94, and that for lifetime experiences with discrimination was a = .94. Finally, scale reliability for appraisal of discrimination was a = .95. Perception of discrimination. Participants’ perception of their experiences with racial discrimination were assessed with five items: “I experience discrimination because of my race,” “I feel that I am discriminated against because of my race,” “I personally have been a victim of racial discrimination,” “I feel like I am personally a victim of society because of my race,” and “I consider myself a person who is deprived of Opportunities that are available to others because of my race.” The last two items were used in Branscombe, Schmitt, and Harvey’s study (1999). The scale ranged from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree), with higher scores indicating that participants are more likely to perceive discrimination based on their race (a = .89). Demographic information. Participants were asked to provide the following basic demographic information: age, gender, race, year in school, number of same-race and different-race close fiiends on and off the MSU campus, family’s socioeconomic status, and current overall GPA. 67 Measures used in the laboratory session. Attribution to discrimination. Participants read six different scenarios that described negative events in different situations that could be attributed to racial discrimination or to other causes. They were asked to imagine that they have experienced these ambiguous negative events and think of the cause of the negative events. Example scenarios are: “Imagine that you went to a ‘fancy’ restaurant. Your server seemed to be taking care of all the other customers except you. You were the last person whose order was taken,” and “Imagine that you were to look at an apartment for rent. The manager of the building refused to show it to you, saying that it has already been rented.” After reading each scenario, participants were asked to rate their perception of the cause of the experience for each of the following six attribution categories: 1) racial discrimination, 2) gender discrimination, 3) internal (e.g., their personality, mood, abilities, or intelligence), 4) external (e. g., interaction partners’ personality, social norm, lack of opportunity), 5) skin tone, and 6) facial features. The scales ranged from 1 (Very Unlikely the Cause of the Experience) to 7 (Very Likely the Cause of the Experience). Attribution scores for each of the six different attribution categories were computed by averaging over the six different scenarios.11 Please see Appendix H for the detailed explanations of the procedure used in the selection of the scenarios and attribution items. Race-based rejection sensitivity. Participants’ sensitivity to rejection by others based on their racial group membership was measured with Mendoza-Demon, Downey, Purdie, David, and Pietrazk’ (2002) 12-item Race-Based Rejection Sensitivity scale. Each item provided participants a scenario in which participants could be rejected and asked two questions: 1) how concemed/anxious participants would be, and 2) how likely 68 participants think that they would be rejected. Example items include “Imagine that you are in a pharmacy, trying to pick out a few items. While you’re looking at the different brands, you notice one of the store clerks glancing your way,” “Imagine you are riding the bus one day. The bus is full except for two seats, one of which is next to you. As the bus comes to the next stop, you notice a woman getting on the bus,” and “Imagine that you are standing in line for the ATM machine, and you notice the woman at the machine glances back while she’s getting her money” (see Appendix I for the complete list of the items). The scale ranged from 1 (Very Unconcemed/Very Unlikely) to 6 (Very Concerned/Very Likely). The rejection sensitivity score was computed first by multiplying the two scores for each scenario. Then, product scores for each of the 12 scenarios were averaged to come up with a total score. Therefore, scores range between 1 and 49. Higher total scores indicate that participants are more sensitive to race-based rejection (or = .86). Modified sequential priming task. In order to assess Black participants’ automatic affective reactions toward their fellow ingroup members, the same sequential priming task as Studies 1 and 3a were used in this study. Modified lexical decision task. The same lexical decision task in Studies 2 and 3b were used to examine stereotype activation among Black individuals when exposed to fellow ingroup members. Explicit liking toward targets. Black participants’ liking toward Black male and Black female targets presented during the sequential priming and lexical decision tasks was assessed with an explicit, self-report measure. The scale was the same as the previous studies and ranged from 0 (I would DISLIKE this person very much) to 5 (I 69 would LIKE this person very much), with higher scores indicating more liking toward targets. Coding of Participants’ Photos Before analyzing skin tone and facial features, pictures of 83 participants were edited so that all background features were replaced by a solid gray background, all clothing was replaced with a plain black t-shirt, and all pictured were transformed into an outlined format. Objective Skin Tone The average luminosity was computed for each face by averaging the luminosity value obtained from each pixel across a given face. Subjective Skin Tone The average skin colors were created for each face first. In order to create the average skin color for each face, color information values (i.e., values of red, blue, and green) were computed for each pixel and averaged over a face, so that each face was associated with only one value for each red, blue, and green. These three values were entered into a color information window on Adobe Photoshop CS3 to create one solid color panel that represented the average skin color for a face. The average skin colors were used for this task, because skin color even within the same face differs depending on the areas being looked at (e. g., skin color of the forehead is usually lighter than that of below the eyes). Then, the solid color panels that represent average skin tone were presented to 153 self-identified White participants (women = 77.1 %, M age = 19.48, SD = 2.21 years old). Participants were asked to rate each of the average skin colors in terms 70 of their darkness/lightness, using a scale ranging from 1 (Extremely Dark) to 7 (Extremely Light). See Appendix J for examples of the average skin color panels. Objective Facial Features Two independent researchers measured the length and width of a nose, lips, and a face in millimeter for each participant’s picture. Then, the ratio of nose width (i.e., nose width divided by face width) and lip thickness (i.e., lip thickness divided by face length) were computed for each face. In order to create a single score that represents the overall facial feature rating, raw scores of both nose and lip ratios were transformed into 2 scores and then they were averaged. Subjective Facial Features Fifty-five self-identified White participants (women = 63.6 %, M age = 19.47, SD = 1.36 years old) looked at the outlined pictures on a computer screen. They were asked to rate each of the 83 faces in terms of the degree of prototypicality of facial features. In addition, participants also rated each face in terms of attractiveness, babyfacedness, and hostility, and these measures serve as control variables in the following analyses. The instructions for these ratings were the same as those used in Pilot Study 1, Step 4. In sum, each of the 83 Black participants’ pictures was associated with four main scores: luminosity (i.e., objective skin tone measure), rating of color panels (i.e., subjective skin tone measure), average a score of nose and lip ratios (i.e., objective facial feature measure), and rating of Afrocentric features (i.e., subjective facial feature measure), as well as scores for the three control variables. Analysis Plan 71 In order to examine whether skin tone and facial features influence Black individuals’ experiences in intergroup relations, two sets of moderated regression analyses were conducted for each criterion variable. The first set of analyses looked at the effects of objective ratings of participants’ physical characteristics (i.e., skin tone computed with luminosity and facial features measured by experimenters), and the second set looked at the effects of subjective ratings of participants’ physical characteristics (i.e., skin tone and facial features rated by White participants). In each set of analyses, four separate moderated regression analyses were run. The first model included the main effects of participants’ skin tone, facial features, and gender, and two-way interaction between skin tone and facial features. Because there were only 12 Black male participants, the interaction between skin tone and gender and between facial features and gender could not be entered into the model. Participants’ gender was coded as 0 (women) and 1 (men), and facial feature and skin tone were centered using grand means before being entered into the regression model. The second through fourth model included control factors that were reported to be related to skin tone and Afrocentric features in the previous studies (i.e., attractiveness, babyfacedness, and hostility). Because attractiveness, babyfacedness, and hostility were significantly related (see Tables 17 and 18 for Black men and Black women separately), these control variables were entered into a regression model separately, instead of being entered into one model simultaneously. It is important to note that only the results from the first model are reported in the following sections as well as in the tables. When the inclusion of a control variable in the model changes the results from the first model, the nature of 72 the changes are discussed in the results section, but the results reported in the tables are still from the first model. Results Means and standard deviations of objective and subjective measures of skin tone and facial features as well as control variables (i.e., attractiveness, babyfacedness, and hostility), and correlations among all variables are provided in Tables 17 and 18 separately for Black male and Black female participants, respectively. These correlations showed that the subjective ratings of skin tone and prototypicality of facial features (i.e., skin tone and facial features rated by White participants) closely mirror the objective measures of skin tone and facial features (i.e., skin tone computed with luminosity and facial features measured by experimenters), respectively, suggesting that White participants could accurately perceive these physical characteristics. Skin tone was significantly and positively related to attractiveness and babyfacedness for Black men such that Black men with darker skin tone were perceived by White individuals to be less attractive and less babyfaced, as compared to Black men with lighter skin tone. Unlike the the pilot study, facial features were not related to attractiveness, babyfacedness, or hostility. However, it is important to note that there were only 12 Black male pictures in Study 4 as oppose to 120 pictures in the pilot study. In contrast, luminosity was negatively related to hostility among Black women, such that Black women with darker skin tone were perceived to be more hostile than those with lighter skin tone. In addition, ratings of facial features were significantly related to attractiveness, babyfacedness, and hostility, such that Black women who were rated as more prototypic of Afiican Americans were perceived to be less attractive, less 73 babyfaced, and more hostile, as compared to Black women who were rated as less prototypic of African Americans. In order to get a broad sense of how each of the criterion variables is related to one another, correlations among all variables are provided in Table 19. Table 19 also presents the means and standard deviations of the criterion variables. The correlations indicate that racism attributions and race-based rejection sensitivity were significantly correlated with all other criterion variables, such that the more participants attribute the negative events to racism (or the more sensitive they are), the more they attribute the negative events to other causes and the more they report experiences with discrimination. The results suggest that people who are more sensitive to racism are more likely to be extreme in their attribution judgments in general, as compared to those who are less sensitive to racism. Overall, the examination of the correlations suggests that all criterion variables are highly related to one another. Attributions of Ambiguous Negative Events It the following section, it was examined whether Black participants’ tendency to attribute ambiguous negative events to certain causes are influenced by their skin tone and facial features. Attribution to racism. Table 20 presents the results of racism attribution. The first model, including the objective measures of the Black participants’ skin tone and facial features, revealed that the model does not account for variance in attribution to racism more than by chance, and none of the main effects or the interaction between skin tone and facial features was significant. 74 The same analysis was run replacing objective measures of the Black participants’ skin tone and facial features with subjectively measured participants’ skin tone and facial features. The results showed that the main effect of facial features was significant, such that as rated prototypicality of facial features increased by one unit (i.e., being rated as more prototypical of Blacks), Blacks’ tendency to attribute ambiguously negative events to racism decreased by .67. Attribution to sexism. Neither the first set of models including objective measures of skin tone and facial features nor the second set of models including subjective measures of skin tone and facial features predicted Black individuals’ tendency to attribute ambiguously negative events to sexism (see Table 21). Although it would be appropriate to examine whether the effects of Black participants’ skin tone and facial features on their tendency to make sexism attributions are moderated by participant gender, the model could not include the interaction between skin tone and gender and between facial features and gender due to lack of Black male participants. Attribution to skin tone. Table 22 presents the summary of the results of skin tone attribution. The model including the objective measures of Black participants’ skin tone and facial features accounted for about 11.8 % of variance in skin tone attribution scores. In this model, both the main effect of facial features and the main effect of participant gender were significant. When holding all other variables constant, as the average of standardized nose and lip ratios increases by one unit, Blacks’ tendency to attribute negative events to skin tone decreased by .51. In addition, when holding all other variables constant, Black men were more likely to attribute negative events to skin tone, as compared to Black women. 75 Next, the moderated regression analysis including the subjective measures of the Black participants’ skin tone and facial features was conducted. The model accounted for about 17.6 % of variance in the skin tone attribution scores. Similarly to the model reported above, the main effect of facial features and the main effect of participants’ gender were significant. As rated prototypicality of their faces increased by one unit, Black participants’ tendency to attribute negative events to skin tone decreased by 1.03, when controlling for all other variables constant. In addition, Black male participants were more likely to attribute negative events to skin tone, as compared to Black female participants, when holding other variables constant. The results suggest that Black men are more likely to be aware of the negative consequences of skin tone than Black women. Because the majority of the sample (85.7 %) was women, the negative relationship between facial features and skin tone attribution may reflect the experiences of women, who do not experience the negative consequences of skin tone and facial features as the results from Study 3 suggest. When three addition models, in which control variables were entered in addition to the main effects of subjective measures of the participants’ skin tone, facial features, and participant gender, all three models resulted in the marginally significant two-way interaction between skin tone and facial features. Because the pattern of the results were the same for the all three additional models, simple slopes of facial features was examined using the second model in which rated attractiveness was entered as a control variable. Figure 1 shows the nature of the interaction between participants’ skin tone and facial features. Simple slopes of facial features were examined at 1 standard deviation above the mean (i.e., lighter skin tone) and at 1 standard deviation below the mean (i.e., 76 darker skin tone). The analyses revealed that the simple slope of facial features was significant when participants had darker skin tone, B = -1.54, SE = .49, t(81) = -3.13, p < .01, such that the more prototypical facial features participants had, the less likely they were to make skin tone attribution. In contrast, the simple slope of facial features was not significant when participants had lighter skin tone, B = -.53, SE = .37, t(81) = -1.43, p = .16 (see Figure 1). Attribution to facial features. Table 23 presents the summary of the results of facial feature attribution. In the first model in which attribution to facial features was predicted by objective skin tone and facial features of Black participants, the two-way interaction between participants’ objective skin tone and facial features was marginally significant. Examination of the patterns suggest that the relationship between facial features and tendency to attribute negative events to facial features was negative when patricians had darker skin tone, but it was positive when participants had lighter skin tone (see Figure 2). In other words, participants with darker skin tone and less prototypical facial features were more likely to make facial feature attribution, as compared to those with darker skin tone and more prototypical facial features. In contrast, participants with lighter skin tone and more prototypical facial features were more likely to make facial feature attribution, as compared to those with lighter skin tone and less prototypical facial features. In contrast, the subjective measures of Black participants’ skin tone, facial features, gender, and two-way interaction between skin tone and facial did not predict their tendency to attribute ambiguously negative events to facial features. 77 Attribution to internal factors. See Table 24 for the results of interaction attribution. The first analysis with objective measures of the Black participants’ skin tone and facial features revealed a marginally significant main effect of skin tone, such that as luminosity increases by one unit (i.e., as skin tone is rated lighter), Blacks’ tendency to attribute ambiguously negative events to internal factors unrelated to their race increased by 3.60, when holding all other variables constant. The second analysis with the subjective measures of the Black participants’ skin tone and facial features also revealed the significant main effect of skin tone, such that as Whites’ subjective ratings of Black participants’ skin tone increase by one unit (i.e., as skin tone is rated lighter), Blacks’ tendency to attribute negative events to internal factors also increased by .25. However, this significant main effect was qualified by a marginally significant two-way interaction between skin tone and facial features. Examination of the pattern suggests that the relationship between facial features and internal attribution was negative when Black participants had darker skin tone, but it was positive when participants had lighter skin tone (see Figure 3). Thus, among Black participants with darker skin tone, those with less prototypical facial features were more likely to attribute negative events to internal factors unrelated to their race, as compared to those more prototypical facial features. In contrast, among Black participants with lighter skin tone, those with more prototypical facial features were more likely to make internal attributions, as compared to those with less prototypical facial features. Attribution to external factors. Neither the first model including the objective measures of the Black participants’ skin tone and facial features nor the second model including the subjective measures of the Black participants’ skin tone and facial features 78 predicted their tendency to attribute ambiguously negative events to external factors (see Table 25 for the results). Taken together, there were no consistent patterns to explain how Black individuals’ skin tone and facial features influence their tendency to attribute ambiguously negative events to certain causes. The objectively measured skin tone and facial features were significant predictors in some cases, and the subjectively measured skin tone and facial features were significant predictors in other cases. One thing that is worth mentioning is that the effects of facial features when predicting skin tone and facial features attributions were opposite from the predictions. Specifically, Black participants with darker skin tone with less prototypical facial features were more likely to attribute negative events to skin tone and facial features than Black participants with more prototypical facial features. These unpredicted findings might be due to lack of statistical power. Experiences with Racial Discrimination In addition to examining how one’s own skin tone and facial features affect Black individuals’ cognitive tendency, the study also examined how their physical characteristics influence their past experiences with racial discrimination. There are two measures of experiences with racial discrimination: one for the experiences in the past one year, one for the lifetime experiences with discrimination. The overall means for both measures were fairly low, suggesting that Black participants do not report frequent experiences with racial discrimination in general. The first analysis used the objective measures of the Black participants’ skin tone and facial features to predict their experiences with racial discrimination in the last year. See Table 26 for the summary of 79 the results. This analysis resulted in a marginally significant main effect of facial features. When holding all other variables constant, as the average of standardized nose and lip ratios increased by one unit, Blacks’ experiences with discrimination in the past one year also increased by .23. In other words, Black individuals with more prototypical facial features were more likely to experience racial discrimination in the past one year, as compared to those with less prototypical facial features. The results changed with control analysis including hostility (but not attractiveness or babyfacedness). More specifically, in the model that included hostility as a control, the main effect of facial features was significant (b = .29, SE = .13, ,8 = .25, t(82) = 2.13,p < .05). The second model including the subjective measures of the Black participants’ skin tone and facial features showed that none of the main effects of skin tone, facial features, and participant gender was significant. However, the analysis resulted in a marginally significant two-way interaction between skin tone and facial features. Figure 4 shows the pattern of the interaction between participants’ skin tone and facial features. Examination of the pattern suggest that, when Black individuals have darker skin tone, those with more prototypical facial features were more likely to report experiencing racial discrimination in the past one year, as compared to those with less prototypical facial features. However, facial features seem to be unrelated to experiences of discrimination for Black individuals with lighter skin tone. The results changed with control analysis including hostility (but not attractiveness or babyfacedness). The model that included hostility as a control resulted in a significant main effect of facial features. The examination of coefficient suggest that as rated prototypicality of Blacks’ facial features increases by one unit, their tendency to 80 report experiences of discrimination also increases by .42 (b = .42, SE = .19, ,6 = .26, t(82) = 2.18, p < .05). This significant main effect was qualified by the significant main effect of skin tone and facial features. Next, the same sets of analyses were conduced on Black individuals’ lifetime experiences with racial discrimination. See Tables 27 for the summary of the results. The first model including the objective measures of participants’ skin tone and facial features revealed that none of the main effects of skin tone, facial features, participant gender, or two-way interaction between skin tone and facial features was significant. However, the results changed with a control analysis including hostility (but not attractiveness and babyfacedbess). More specifically, once hostility was entered into the model as a control variable, the main effect of facial features became significant. Holding all other variables constant, as the average standardized nose and lip ratios increases by one unit, Blacks’ tendency to report lifetime experiences with racial discrimination also increases by .31 (b = .31, SE = .15, fl = .24, t(82) = 2.02,p < .05). Similar results were found with the second sets of analyses with models including the subjective measures of the Black participants’ skin tone and facial features. None of the main predictor variables was significant when the model included only the main effects of participants’ skin tone, facial features, and gender, and a two-way interaction between skin tone and facial features. However, a control analysis including hostility (but not attractiveness and babyfacedness) resulted in the different results. Specifically, once hostility was entered into the model as a control, the main effect of facial features became marginally significant. Examination of coefficients suggests that Black individuals with more prototypical facial features were more likely to report experiencing racial 81 discrimination than those with less prototypical facial features (b = .3 7, SE = .22, fl = .20, t(82) = 1.65, p = .10). In addition, the effect of hostility was significant (b = -.32, SE = .14, ,B = -.27, t(82) = -2.32, p < .05), such that Black individuals seen as more hostile were less likely to report experiences with racial discrimination in the past, as compared to those seen as less hostile. Taken together, as predicted, Black individuals with more prototypical facial features were more likely to report experiencing racial discrimination in the past than their counterparts with less prototypical facial features. However, inconsistent with the prediction, Black individuals’ skin tone did not predict their past experiences with racial discrimination. The absence of skin tone effects might be due to lack of Black male participants. Race-Based Rejection Sensitivity Next, the effects of one’s own skin tone and facial features on Black participants’ sensitivity to racism were examined using race-based rejection sensitivity. Table 28 presents the results of Black individuals’ sensitivity to racism as a function of their skin tone and facial features. Both analyses including the objective measures and the subjective measures of the Black participants’ skin tone and facial features resulted in marginally significant two-way interactions between skin tone and facial features. Analyses of simple slopes of facial features at different levels of skin tone revealed the same results for the both objective and subjective measures of participants’ physical characteristics. Figures 5 and 6 show the nature of the interactions between skin tone and facial features with objective and subjective measures, respectively. Specifically, among the individuals with darker skin tone, those with less prototypical facial features are more 82 sensitive to racism, as compared to those with more prototypical facial features (B = -2.87, SE = 1.95, t(81) = -1 .48,p = .14 for objective measures, and B = -4.13, SE = 2.89, t(81) = -1.43, p = .16 for subjective measures). In contrast, among the individuals with lighter skin tone, those with more prototypical facial features are more sensitive to racism, as compared to those with less prototypical facial features (B = 2.11, SE = 1.73, t(81) = 1.22, p = .23 for objective measures, and B = 2.25, SE = 2.19, t(81) = 1.03, p = .31 for subjective measures). The results of race-based rejection sensitivity was consistent with skin tone and facial feature attributions, which are opposite from the predictions. Taken together, when Black individuals have darker skin tone, those with more prototypical facial features are less likely to be sensitive to racism and attribute negative events to skin tone and facial features, as compared to those with more prototypical facial features. The results of facial features effects were consistent when Blacks had lighter skin tone, such that those with more prototypical facial features were more likely to be sensitive to racism and attribute negative events to skin tone and facial features, as compared to those with less prototypical facial features. Other Criterion Variables The same sets of analyses were conducted on other predictor variables measured with the pre-laboratory questionnaire: racial identification, perception of discrimination, overall self-esteem, social self-esteem, and appearance self-esteem. None of the models showed evidence that Black participants’ skin tone or facial features predicted their responses. The results of all analyses are presented in Tables 29 through 33. Implicit Affective Reactions to Black Men 83 In addition to examining how one’s own physical characteristics influence Blacks’ attribution tendencies, sensitivity to racism, and experiences with discrimination, Study 4 also examined how Black targets’ physical characteristics influence Black perceivers’ affective and cognitive responses toward them. Again, it is important to note that half of the participants completed the sequential priming task with Black male targets and the lexical decision task with Black female targets, and the other half completed the lexical decision task with Black male targets and the sequential priming task with Black female targets. Thus, the dfs in the following analyses, except the explicit liking section, reflect the small sample size for these tasks. Implicit negative attitudes. The following analyses examined whether Black individuals’ affective reactions toward fellow male ingroup members were influenced by target’s skin tone and facial features. A two-factor repeated ANOVA, treating target skin tone (dark, light) and target facial features (more prototypical, less prototypical) as . . . . . . . . 12 wrthrn-partrcrpants factors, was conducted on the facrlrtatron scores for negative words. See Table 34 for the means and standard deviations13 for each prime category. The analysis revealed that there was no main effect of target facial features, F (1, 39) = .90, MSE = .003, p = .35. The main effect of target skin tone was marginally significant, F (1 , 39) = 3.36, MSE = .004, p = .08, d = .14, such that Black participants responded faster to negative words following light-skinned Black male primes (M = 27.28, SD = 106.95), as compared to dark-skinned Black male primes (M = 9.61 , SD = 142.07). However, the two-way interaction between target skin tone and facial features was also marginally significant, F (1, 39) = 3.22, MSE = .003, p = .08. In the dark skin tone condition, Black participants responded faster to negative words following Black men with more 84 prototypical facial features than Black men with less prototypical facial features. The results suggest that Black participants felt more negatively toward Black men with darker skin tone and more prototypical facial features than toward Black men with darker skin tone and less prototypical facial features. However, it is important to note that the differences between more or less prototypical target facial features are very small. In contrast, in the light skin tone condition, Black participants responded faster to negative words following Black men with less prototypical facial features than following Black men with more prototypical facial features. These results indicate that Black participants felt more negatively toward Black men with lighter skin tone and less prototypical facial features than toward Black men with lighter skin tone and more prototypical facial features. Implicit positive attitudes. Next, participants’ responses to positive words following male target pictures were examined using the same two-factor repeated ANOVA. Table 35 presents the means and standard deviations for each prime category. The analysis reveled that none of the main effects of skin tone (F (1 , 39) = .15, MSE = .004, p = .70), facial features (F (1, 39) = .15, MSE = .002, p = .70), or the interaction between skin tone and facial features (F (1 , 39) = .01, MSE = .004, p = .92) was significant. Thus, there was no evidence that Black perceivers’ positive feelings toward fellow ingroup members are influenced by targets’ skin tone and facial features. Black vs. White control. In order to examine whether Black perceivers’ evaluations of Black men are different from the White control, facilitation scores for four Black prime categories were averaged. First, a paired t-test on facilitation scores for the negative words was conducted, and it reveled that there are no effect of target race. Thus, 85 there were no significant mean differences between facilitation scores for Black targets (M= 18.44, SD = 122.82) and White targets (M= 20.86, SD = 113.12), t(39) =.36,p = .72. The same analysis was conducted on facilitations scores for positive words and revealed a marginally significant effect of target race, t(39) = .36, p = .09, d = .07. More specifically, Black perceivers’ positive feelings were inhibited to a greater degree following White targets (M = -28.96, SD = 124.60), as compared to following Black targets (M = -l9.63, SD = 129.09). Implicit Affective Reactions to Black Women Implicit negative attitudes. Tables 36 presents the means and standard deviations of facilitation scores for negative words in each prime category. Due to computer malfunctions, the number of participants who were included in this section of analyses was very low. Thus, it is important to keep in mind that the interpretation of the results must been done with caution. A two-factor repeated ANOVA on the facilitation scores for negative words revealed that there was no main effect of target skin tone (F (1 , 24) = .07, MSE = .003, p = .80) nor the main effect of target facial features (F(1, 24) = .32, MSE = .01, p = .58). The interaction between skin tone and facial features was not significant either, F(1, 24) = .19, MSE = .01,p = .67. Implicit positive attitudes. Next, participants’ responses to positive words following female target pictures were examined using the same two-factor repeated ANOVA (see Tables 37 for the means and standard deviations for each prime category). The analysis reveled that neither the main effects of target skin tone (F (1, 24) = .01, MSE = .01 , p = .93), nor the main effect of target facial features (F(1, 24) = .16, MSE = .01, p 86 .69) was significant. The interaction between skin tone and facial features (F (l, 24) .60, MSE = .01, p = .45) was not significant either. Black vs. White control. A paired t-test on facilitation scores for negative words revealed that there was no effect of target race, t(24) = .08, p = .94, suggesting that there were no mean differences in facilitation scores for the negative words between Black targets (M = 90.44, SD = 105.03) and White targets (M = 91.73, SD = 152.45). The same analysis was conducted for positive words and revealed that there were no mean differences in facilitation scores for the positive words between Black targets (M = 76.59, SD = 113.23) and White targets (M = 70.38, SD = 147.31), t(24) = .46, p = .65. Explicit Affective Reactions to Fellow Ingroup Members Liking toward Black men. Next, Black participants’ explicit liking toward fellow ingroup members who vary in skin tone and facial features were examined. The means and standard deviations of explicit liking toward Black men as a function of skin tone and facial features are presented in Table 38. A two-factor repeated ANOVA, treating target skin tone (dark, light) and target facial features (more prototypical, less prototypical) as within-participants factors, was conducted to examine the effects of skin tone and facial features on Black individuals’ explicit reports of liking toward male ingroup members. The main effect of target skin tone was significant, F (1 , 82) = 12.62, MSE = .12, p < .001, d = .15, such that Black participants liked light-skinned Black men significantly more (M = 3.17, SD = .88) than dark-skinned Black men (M = 3.04, SD = .92). In addition, the main effect of target facial features was also significant, F (1 , 82) = 7.29, MSE = .11, p < .01, d = .11, such that Black participants liked Black men with less prototypical facial features significantly more (M = 3.15, SD = .90) than Black men with 87 more prototypical facial features (M = 3.06, SD = .91). The interaction between skin tone and facial features was not significant, F (1, 82) = 1.20, MSE = .09, p = .28. These results are consistent with those found for White perceivers. Liking toward Black women. The means and standard deviations of explicit liking toward Black women as a function of skin tone and facial features are presented in Table 39. The same two-factor repeated ANOVA was conducted to examine the effects of target skin tone and facial features on Black perceivers’ liking toward female ingroup members. The main effect of target skin tone was significant, F (l, 82) = 5.85, MSE = .56, p < .05, d = .19, such that Black participants liked light-skinned Black women significantly more (M = 3.28, SD = 1.02) than dark-skinned Black women (M = 3.08, SD = 1.11). The main effect of target facial features was not significant, F (l, 82) = .08, MSE = .36, p = .78. The interaction between target skin tone and facial features was not significant, F (l, 82) = .63, MSE = .39, p = .43. Again, this pattern of the results is consistent with the results of White individuals’ explicit liking toward Black women. Stereotype Activation among Blacks following Black Male Targets In addition to examining Black perceivers’ affective reactions toward fellow ingroup members who vary in skin tone and facial features, Study 4 also examined Black perceivers’ cognitive reactions (i.e., stereotype activation) after looking fellow ingroup members who vary in physical characteristics. Table 40 presents the means and standard deviations of response latencies for the stereotypic and non-stereotypic words for each prime category. A three-factor repeated ANOVA, treating target skin tone (dark, light), target facial features (more prototypical, less prototypical), and word type (stereotypic, non-stereotypic) as within-participants factors, was conducted on the response latencies. 88 The ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of word type, F (1, 36) = 6.23, MSE = .003, p < .05, d = .10, such that participants responded significantly faster to stereotypic words (M = 568.41, SD = 121.46) than to non-stereotypic words (M = 580.36, SD = 125.06). Neither the main effect of target skin tone (F(1, 36) = 1.06, MSE = .01, p = .31) nor the main effect of target facial features (F (1, 36) = 1.09, MSE = .003, p = .30) was significant. Furthermore, none of the two-way interactions between target skin tone and target facial features (F (1 , 36) = 1.43, MSE = .004, p = .24), between target skin tone and word type (F (1, 36) = .02, MSE = .01, p = .90), between target facial features and word type (F (1, 36) = .22, MSE = .01, p = .64), and the three—way interaction between skin tone, facial features, and word type (F (1, 36) = 1.09, MSE = .01, p = .30) was significant. Next, whether the degree of stereotype activation was different between Black male targets and the White control, a repeated ANOVA was conducted on response latencies, treating race (Black, White) and word type (stereotype, non-stereotypic) as within-participants factors. Again, response latencies for four Black prime categories were averaged to compute response latencies for Black targets, regardless of target skin tone and facial features. The analysis revealed that neither the main effect of target race (F(1, 36) = 2.38, MSE = .004,p = .13) nor the main effect ofword type (F(1, 36) = 2.24, MSE = .01, p = .14) was significant. The interaction between target race and word type was not significant either, F(1, 36) = .002, MSE = .01,p = .97. Stereotype Activation among Blacks following Black Female Targets Table 42 presents the means and standard deviations of response latencies for the stereotypic and non-stereotypic words for each prime category. Again, due to lack of statistical power (i.e., small sample size due to computer malfunction), the results in this 89 section of analyses need to be interpreted with caution. The same three sets of analyses as Black male targets were conducted for Black female targets. A three-factor repeated ANOVA revealed that the main effect of target skin tone was not significant, F (l , 20) = 47, MSE = .01, p = .50. However, the main effect of target facial features was significant, F(l , 20) = 5.59, MSE = .01, p < .05, d = .50, such that Black participants responded significantly faster to all words following Black women with less prototypical facial features (M = 565.82, SD = 196.56) than following those with more prototypical facial features (M = 669.98, SD = 217.53). In addition, the main effect of word type was marginally significant, F(1, 20) = 3.78, MSE = .01, p = .07, d = .08, such that Black participants responded faster to non-stereotypic words (M = 665.94, SD = 289.93), as compared to stereotypic words (M = 644.87, SD = 198.41). The two-way interaction between target skin tone and word type also reached marginal significance, F ( 1 , 20) = 3.61, MSE = .01, p = .07. The examination of the means suggest that Black participants responded faster to stereotypic words following Black female targets with darker skin tone (M = 637.31, SD = 190.59), as compared to stereotypic words following Black female targets with lighter skin tone (M = 652.43, SD = 206.23). In contrast, Black perceivers responded faster to non-stereotypic words following Black female primes with lighter skin tone (M = 653.27, SD = 195.14), as compared to Black female primes with darker skin tone (M = 678.61, SD = 236.23). Neither the two-way interaction between target skin tone and target facial features (F (1 , 20) = .70, MSE = .02, p = .41) nor between target facial feature and word type (F(1, 20) = 001, MSE = .01, p = .97) was significant. 90 However, they were qualified by a significant three-way interaction between target skin tone, target facial features, and word type, F (1, 20) = 7.03, MSE = .01, p < .05. However, again, due to extremely low power, the results of the three-way interaction need to be interpreted with caution. Thus, instead of the conducting the simple interaction and the simple main effects, the pattern of the means was examined. The examination of the means suggest that the significant three-way interaction between target skin tone, target facial features, and word type is driven by slower reaction time for the stereotypic words following Black female primes with lighter skin tone and more prototypical facial features and for non-stereotypic words following Black female primes with darker skin tone and more prototypical facial features. More specifically, Black perceivers responded faster to stereotypic words following light-skinned Black female primes with less prototypical facial features, as compared to light-skinned Black female primes with more prototypical facial features. However, the differences in response latencies between dark- skinned Black female with more prototypical facial features and dark-skinned Black female with less prototypical facial features were relatively small. In contrast, the patter was opposite in the non-stereotypic word condition. The differences in response latencies between light-skinned Black female primes with less prototypical facial features and light-skinned Black female primes with more prototypical facial features were small. However, the means suggest that Black perceivers responded faster to non-stereotypic words following dark-skinned Black female primes with less prototypical facial features, as compared to dark-skinned Black female primes with more prototypical facial features. Next, it was examined whether the degree of stereotype activation following Black targets is different from following White control. A repeated ANOVA, treating 91 target race (Black, White) and word type (stereotypic, non-stereotypic) as within- participants factors, was conducted on response latencies. The analysis revealed that neither the main effect of target race (F (l , 20) = .95, MSE = .01, p = .0834 nor the main effect of word type(F (1, 20) = 2.08, MSE = .01, p = .17) was significant. The interaction between target race and word type was not significant either, F (l , 20) = .003, MSE = .004, p = .95. Discussion Study 4 examined how Black individuals’ skin tone and facial features independently influence their own experiences in intergroup relations, as well as their perceptions of and reactions to fellow ingroup members. More specifically, the first half of Study 4 investigated how one’s own physical features influence Blacks’ tendency to attribute negative events to racism, their experiences with discrimination, and their sensitivity to racism. The second half of the study investigated how Blacks react, both affectively and cognitively, to fellow ingroup members who vary in skin tone and facial features, thus treating Blacks as perceivers. Attributions for Ambiguous Negative Events Black individuals who experience racial prejudice and discrimination throughout their lives may become sensitive to discrimination and construe even ambiguous negative events (that are not necessarily due to racism) in terms of their racial membership. Previous research (as well as Study 1) has demonstrated that White individuals are likely to perceive and treat Blacks with darker skin tone or with stronger Afrocentric features more negatively that those with lighter skin tone or with weaker Afrocentric features. Thus, it was predicted that Blacks with darker skin tone or more prototypical facial 92 features would be more likely to attribute ambiguous negative events to racism. Study 4 found that facial features are associated with Black individuals’ tendency to attribute ambiguous negative events to racism. However, contrary to the prediction, Black individuals with more prototypical facial features were less likely to make racism attributions, as compared to Black individuals with less prototypical facial features. In addition, Study 4 failed to find an association between skin tone and racism attributions. Furthermore, this study found no consistency in the effects of Black participants’ own skin tone and facial features across the other attribution categories (i.e., attributions to skin tone, facial features, internal factors, and external factors). For instance, facial features were associated with skin tone attributions, such that Black individuals with more prototypical facial features were less likely to attribute negative events to skin tone. Skin tone predicted Black individuals’ tendency to attribute ambiguous negative events to internal factors, such that individuals with darker skin tone were less likely to make internal attributions. There are several reasons that may explain why no consistent patterns emerged in the current findings. First, there were not enough Black male participants in the current study. As Studies 1 and 3 have shown, experiences with racial prejudice are often different for Black men and Black women, with Black men experiencing more negative consequences of having darker skin tone and more prototypical facial features. Therefore, the prediction, which was that Black individuals with darker skin tone or more prototypical facial features experience more prejudice and discrimination and thus become more sensitive to racial discrimination, might be only be true for Black men. For Black women, there should not be as strong of a link between dark skin/more prototypical facial features, 93 discrimination experiences, and racism attributions, because as suggested by Study 3, Black women with darker skin tone are no more likely to experience prejudice than Black women with lighter skin tone. Thus, failing to find the predicted results might be due to the fact that there were only 12 Black male participants in the study and so the current results reflect the experiences of Black women. Second, the scenarios used in the attribution measure might not have been sufficiently detailed and rich for the participants to imagine actually experiencing the negative events. If this is the case, it would have been hard for them to guess how they would have reacted to the situations. In addition, the measure might have been too repetitive and introduced boredom among participants. Finally, it might be possible that there are no effects of skin tone or facial features on how Black individuals construe ambiguous negative events after all, or there might be other factors that strongly influence Blacks’ tendency to attribute negative events to racism, such as racial identification. Even if Black individuals had lighter skin tone or less prototypical facial features, they may construe ambiguous negative events in terms of their racial membership if they were strongly identified as Black. Although the results suggest that the attribution measure might lack validity, there was one additional interesting finding. Participant gender predicted skin tone attributions, such that Black men were more likely to attribute negative events to skin tone than Black women. This finding is consistent with the results of Whites’ implicit affective reactions, which suggest that skin tone influences Whites’ negative feelings about Black men whereas it influences Whites’ positive feelings about Black women. The current results 94 suggest that Black men may be aware of the negative consequences of having darker skin tone. Experience with and Sensitivity to Racism Based on previous research and the results from Studies 1 through 3, it was predicted that Black individuals (especially men) with darker skin tone or more prototypical facial features would experience more racial discrimination and that they would be more likely to be sensitive to racism than those with lighter skin tone or less prototypical facial features. There was no evidence that skin tone predicts Black individuals’ experience with and sensitivity to racism. The effects of facial features on Blacks’ experience with racial discrimination in the past year were somewhat consistent with the prediction, such that dark-skinned Blacks with more prototypical facial features experienced more racial discrimination. In contrast, for the light-skinned Blacks, facial features were not associated with the experience of racial discrimination. Black individuals’ facial features also influenced their sensitivity to racism. However, the direction of the effects was opposite from the prediction: Black individuals with more prototypical facial features were less sensitive to racism. To summarize, even though dark-skinned Blacks with more prototypical facial features reported experiencing more racial discrimination, they were less likely to be sensitive to racism. This inconsistency might be due to the fact that participants’ sensitivity to racism was assessed with a race- based rejection sensitivity measure. The measure asks participants to indicate how concerned/anxious they are that they will experience certain mistreatrnents because of their race/ethnicity. When participants with more prototypical facial features respond to 95 these items, they might be thinking that “I am not experiencing these treatments because of my race/ethnicity, but because of my appearance.” The Effects of Black Targets’ Characteristics on Black Perceivers’ Attitudes toward Them Study 4 also examined whether Black individuals also have biases toward fellow ingroup members who vary in skin tone and facial features. Black participants responded significantly faster to negative words following Black men with lighter skin tone, as compared to following Black men with darker skin tone, indicating that Black individuals feel more negatively toward Black male targets with lighter skin tone than toward Black male targets with darker skin tone. Furthermore, there were effects of facial features, but only in the light skin condition, such that Black individuals felt more negatively toward fellow ingroup members with light-skin and less prototypical features, as compared to those with more prototypical facial features. Interestingly, this pattern of results is somewhat opposite from that for White perceivers (Study 1). White perceivers showed increased negativity toward Black men with darker skin tone and more prototypical facial features, whereas Black perceivers showed increased negativity toward Black men with lighter skin tone and less prototypical facial features. The results suggest that both Black and White perceivers responded more negatively to someone who looks the most different from them. This finding is somewhat inconsistent with previous research that has shown that dark-skinned Blacks are perceived negatively by fellow Blacks (Averhart & Bigler, 1997; Harvey etal., 2005; Maddox & Gray, 2002). Just like White perceivers, there was no evidence that Black perceivers’ responses to positive words were influenced by targets’ skin tone and facial features. Thus, targets’ 96 skin tone seem to be associated with negativity, affecting how much Blacks dislike ingroup members, not how much they like them. Contrary to the findings for Black male targets, neither skin tone nor facial features influence Black individuals’ negative and positive implicit attitudes toward female ingroup members. Interestingly, the results of explicit liking were consistent with those for White perceivers. More specifically, Black perceivers liked Black male targets with lighter skin tone more than those with darker skin tone. They also liked Black male targets with less prototypical facial features more than Black male targets with more prototypical facial features. Likewise, Black perceivers liked Black female targets with lighter skin tone more than Black female targets with darker skin tone (and there was no facial features effects). Thus, the results of Blacks’ feelings about fellow ingroup members measured at the explicit level were consistent with the previous findings showing that dark-skinned Blacks are perceived negatively by fellow Blacks (Averhart & Bi gler, 1997; Harvey, LaBeach, Pridgen, & Gocial, 2005; Maddox & Gray, 2002). So, there is clearly a disassociation between attitudes assessed with implicit and explicit measures. The Effects of Black Targets’ Characteristics on Stereotype Activation for Black Perceivers Study 4 also examined whether Blacks’ cognitive reactions toward their fellow ingroup members (i.e., stereotype activation) were influenced by targets’ skin tone and facial features. The results showed that Black individuals responded significantly faster to stereotypic negative words following male target pictures, as compared to non- stereotypic negative words, indicating that negative stereotypes associated with Black men were activated following all Black prime categories, regardless of the degree of skin 97 tone and prototypicality of facial features. In addition, none of the four Black prime categories was different from White controls. Unfortunately, the absence of mean difference in reaction time to stereotypic words between Black primes and White controls suggests that the lexical decision task created for the present studies may have failed to capture stereotype activation associated with “Black.” J ust like the lexical decision task for male targets, there was no mean difference in reaction time to stereotypic words between Black female primes and White female controls, suggesting that the task might not be valid. Therefore, the findings that Black perceivers responded faster to stereotypic words following light-skinned Black women with less prototypical facial features, as compared to light-skinned Black women with more prototypical facial features, should be interpreted with caution. Again, there is little evidence that the lexical decision task in the current study worked. General Discussion People in the same racial groups share sets of physical characteristics. However, even within the same groups, there is real variation in physical appearance. Two literatures have been developed to determine how perceivers respond to such differences in physical characteristics of Black targets and why there are individual differences in the experience of prejudice and discrimination within Blacks as a racial group. Research on skin tone suggests that people rely on skin tone to make inferences of individuals’ attributes. Research on Afrocentric features suggests that other facial features, such as eye color, shape of nose and lips, and texture of hair, in addition to skin tone information, influence people’s perceptions of and reactions to targets. Although these two lines of research argue that one type of physical characteristic may be more important than the 98 other when predicting within-group differences in the experience of prejudice and discrimination, they have generally failed to separate these two sets of physical characteristics. There were four overall goals of the present research: to examine 1) whether there are effects of facial features on Whites’ perceptions of and reactions to Black individuals above and beyond skin tone effects, 2) whether the effects of skin tone and facial features on Whites’ perceptions differ between Black men and women as targets, 3) whether the effects of these two types of physical features on Black perceivers differ from their effects on White perceivers, and 4) what effects a Black person’s own physical features have on his or her perceptions of and experiences with discrimination. By manipulating facial features of target pictures, the current experiments successfully separated facial features from skin tone and addressed the four questions mentioned above. Independent Effects of Skin Tone and Facial Features Study 1 demonstrated that both skin tone and facial features independently influence how White perceivers feel about Black men, at both implicit and explicit levels. Because primary findings were two main effects, this suggests that the effects of skin tone and facial features are additive, and that both skin tone and facial features are important physical characteristics that affect people’s perceptions of and reactions to Black men. The current results support the findings in previous Afrocentric features research, by demonstrating that facial features in addition to skin tone influence people’s perceptions of Blacks. However, the findings in the uncut research further demonstrate that it is important to look at skin tone and facial features separately, instead of including both skin tone and facial features under on variable (i.e., Afrocentric features), as skin 99 tone and facial features do not covary. In other words, some individuals have darker skin tone with less prototypical facial features, and some individuals have lighter skin tone with more prototypical facial features. Independent examination of the separate effects of skin tone and facial features provides a clearer understanding of how White individuals respond to Blacks with “mismatched physical characteristics.” The Effect of Target Gender Importantly, the results from Studies 1 and 3 further showed that the effects of skin tone and facial features on White perceivers may be moderated by target gender. When targets were Black men, both facial features and skin tone had negative effects on White perceivers. On the other hand, when the targets were Black women, there was no evidence of facial features effects. This suggests that White perceivers’ feelings about Black women may be more strongly driven by skin tone than by facial features. Moreover, the nature of the skin tone effect was different. Specifically, unlike Black male targets, having lighter skin tone increased Whites’ positive feelings. These results are somewhat consistent with the subordinate male target hypothesis (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999; Pratto et al., 2006), but not with multiple-identity perspectives (Buchanan & Fitzgerald, 2008; Buchanan & Ormerod, 2002; Fleming, 1983; King, 2005; Landrine et al., 1995; McLeod & Owens, 2004; Rederstorff et al., 2007; Settles, 2006). The multiple-identity perspective argues that Black women would experience prejudice and discrimination to the same, or even greater, degree as Black men. However, Study 3 did not find evidence of negative consequences of having darker skin tone and more prototypical facial features for Black women. If anything, Black women receive favorable evaluations by White perceivers, depending on their skin tone. However, it is 100 important to note again that there were methodological limitations with Black female target tasks due to lack of usable target stimuli. Thus, the findings and interpretations presented here are preliminary, and replicating the results with more target stimuli is critical. The Effect of Perceiver Race In addition to target gender, perceiver race also moderated the effects of physical characteristics on people’s affective reactions to Black men and Black women at the implicit level. Study 4 showed that Black men with lighter skin tone were evaluated more negatively than were Black men with darker skin tone by fellow ingroup members. It is important to note that this effect of skin tone is in the opposite direction from White. perceivers, who evaluated Black men with darker skin tone more negatively than Black men with lighter skin tone. Therefore, both Black and White perceivers negatively evaluated someone who looks different from members of their own racial groups. In contrast, when the targets were Black women, there was no evidence of the effects of skin tone or facial features on Black perceivers’ automatic affective reactions. Remember that White perceivers felt more positively toward Black women with lighter skin tone. Strong conclusions regarding these differences cannot be drawn because of 1) small sample of Black perceivers, 2) lack of Black female target stimuli, and 3) uneven gender distribution in Black sample. Interestingly, perceiver race did not moderate the effects of skin tone and facial features on people’s affective reactions to Black men and Black women at the explicit level. Both White and Black perceivers liked Black male targets with lighter skin tone or less prototypical facial features more than Black male targets with darker skin tone or 101 more prototypical facial features. Likewise, both White and Black perceivers liked Black female targets with lighter skin tone more than Black female targets with darker skin tone. Furthermore, there was no evidence of facial features on White perceivers or Black perceivers’ explicit liking toward Black women. Implicit Attitudes-Explicit Attitudes Relation The present results clearly demonstrate a disassociation between the implicit and explicit attitudes among Black perceivers. The disassociation between Black perceivers’ automatic affective reactions and their explicit liking attitudes toward fellow ingroup members may be due to their awareness of the negative consequences of having darker skin tone and more prototypical facial features. Previous studies have shown that Black individuals are highly aware of skin tone effects and that they even discriminate against fellow ingroup members based on their skin tone (Davis, Daniels, & See, 1998; Harvey et al., 2005; Maddox, 2004). It may also be the case that Black individuals are aware of the negative consequences of more prototypical facial features as well, as Study 4 demonstrated that participants’ facial features predicted their experiences with racial discrimination in the past year. As a result, Black perceivers may be concerned with potential negative treatment they may receive when associating with individuals who have darker skin tone or more prototypical facial features, even if they personally like the target persons. Goffrnan (1963) argued that normal individuals can be stigmatized by others as a function of their association with a marked individual or group (i.e., courtesy stigma). Indeed, research has shown that mere association with someone who is perceived negatively can result in being negatively evaluated (i.e., stigma by association; Gallagher, Tait, McCologan, Dovey, & Holford, 2003; Goldstein & Johnson, 1997; 102 Neuberg, Smith, Hoffman, & Russel, 1994; Ostman & Kjellin, 2002). For instance, Neuberg et al. (1994) found that people perceive a heterosexual man more negatively when he was with his homosexual friend than when he was with his heterosexual fiiend. Furthermore, several researchers have shown that mere proximity (e.g., sitting next to a marked individual) can cause stigmatized perceptions of “normal” individuals (Hebl & Mannix, 2003; Penny & Haddock, 2007) and that people are motivated to distance themselves from a marked individual due to a concern over stigma by association (Swim, Ferguson, & Hyers, 1999). In contrast, there was no contradiction between White perceivers’ automatic, implicit attitudes and their self-report, explicit attitudes. Researchers have shown that White individuals are motivated not to appear racist because blatant, “old-fashion” prejudiced attitudes and discriminatory behaviors became socially unacceptable (Crandall, Eshleman, O’Brien, 2002; Dowden & Robinson, 1993; Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986; Rokeach & Ball-Rokeach, 1989), and many are motivated to deliberately change their attitudes to match socially accepted attitudes (Devine, 1989; Fazio et al., 1995; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). If White perceivers were aware of the biases associated with physical characteristics, at least those who are highly egalitarian may have been motivated to inhibit their prejudiced attitudes toward Black men with darker skin tone or more prototypical facial features on the explicit liking measure. Therefore, the present results suggest that White perceivers may not be aware of the effects of skin tone and facial features on their feelings about Black targets. Black Person’s Own Physical Characteristics and Their Experiences with Racism 103 In addition to the examination of the effects of physical characteristics on people’s affective reactions toward Black individuals, the present research also investigated how one’s own physical characteristics influence their experiences with intergroup relations. Study 4 examined how skin tone and facial features influence Black individuals’ tendency to attribute ambiguous negative events to racism, experience with racism, and sensitivity to racism. Only facial features predicted racism attribution. Interestingly, the pattern of the effect was inconsistent with the prediction, such that Black individuals with more prototypical facial features were less likely to make racism attributions, as compared to Black individuals with less prototypical facial features. If Black individuals with more prototypical facial features do not attribute negative events to racism, what attributions do they make? Although facial features were also associated with skin tone attributions (i.e., the more prototypical facial features, less likelihood of making skin tone attribution), they were not associated with other types of attributions, including facial features, internal, and external attributions. The lack of association may simply indicate that Black individuals with more prototypical facial features (or with darker skin tone) are likely to make certain attributions to the same degree as Black individuals with less prototypical facial features (or with lighter skin tone). Alternatively, the attribution measure may not have been a compelling methodology. Why there was no effect of skin tone on Black individuals’ tendency to attribute negative events to racism? Also, why were Black individuals with less prototypical facial features more likely to make racism attributions, as compared to Black individuals with more prototypical facial features? One reason might be because the majority of the participants (85.7 %) were women. As shown in Studies 1 and 3, skin tone influences 104 Whites’ positive feelings about Black women, and facial features have no influence on Whites’ affective reactions toward Black women. Because having darker skin tone apparently does not increase the incidence of racial discrimination among Black women, skin tone might not be associated with Black women’s tendency to attribute negative events to racism or their sensitivity to racism. In fact, skin tone was not associated with experiences with discrimination in the past year. If there were more Black men in the current study, however, the results might have been different. Indeed, target gender predicted skin tone attributions, such that Black men were more likely to attribute negative events to skin tone, as compared to Black women, supporting the findings in the present study that men’s skin tone influences Whites’ negativity whereas women’s skin tone influences Whites’ positivity. If there were more Black men with different levels of skin tone, it might have been possible to test whether Black men with darker skin tone would attribute negative events to racism more than Black men with lighter skin tone. However, due to the small number of Black male participants, the interactions between target gender and skin tone or facial features could not be tested in the current regression analyses. One interesting finding in Study 4 is the discrepancy between Black individuals’ reports of discrimination experiences and their sensitivity to racism. Dark-skinned Black individuals with more prototypical facial features, as compared to those with less prototypical facial features, reported more incidents that they considered racial discrimination, but they were less sensitive to racism. These findings seem inconsistent with the previous research showing that individuals who experience greater amounts of discrimination become more sensitive to racial issues and are more apt to interpret events 105 through the lenses of discrimination due to chronic activation of their social identity (Jetten, Bransocombe, Schmitt, & Spears, 2001; Major, Quinton, & Schmader, 2003; Operario & Fiske, 2001; Sellers & Shelton, 2003; Shelton & Sellers, 2000). However, sensitivity to racism assessed in the current study is based on individual’s group membership. Race-based rejection sensitivity was designed to assess people’s anxious expectations of rejection specifically based on their racial group membership (Mendoza- Denton et al., 2002). The measure asks participants to indicate how concerned/anxious they are that they will experience certain mistreatments because of their race/ethnicity. When participants with more prototypical facial features respond to these items, they might be thinking that “I am not experiencing these treatments because of my race/ethnicity, but because of my appearance.” Thus, instead of measuring their sensitivity to racism, the current study should have assessed Black individuals’ sensitivity to mistreatments in general using a general rejection sensitivity measure (Downey & Feldman, 1996). However, again, the results should be replicated with more Black participants, especially Black men, before making strong interpretations of these seemingly inconsistent findings. Limitations and Future Directions Although the current research provides important insights into the relationship between Blacks’ physical characteristics and Whites' and Blacks’ experiences with intergroup relations, it is not without limitations. First, the lexical decision task used in the present study suffered from low validity. The major difference between the original LDT and the modified LDT used in the current study is that there was no baseline in the current LDT. Although the stereotypic and non-stereotypic words used in the current 106 LDT were matched in terms of its negativity, other factors, such as word familiarity, frequency, and number of letters, were not controlled. Therefore, it is hard to conclude that the differences in reaction time between stereotypic and non-stereotypic words were due to heightened accessibility of the words (i.e., stereotype activation) or due to other extraneous factors. In the original lexical decision task (Wittenbrink et al., 1997), there were baseline trials, and participants’ response latencies to specific words following experimental primes were compared to the same words following neutral primes (i.e., baseline). In other words, dependent variables in the original task were facilitation scores (just like the ones computed in the sequential priming task) rather than response latencies per se. This way, the original lexical decision task eliminated differences in speed of word recognition as a function of individual items. The reason why there were no baseline trials in the current research was because of the concern over participants’ fatigue and boredom. Even without the baseline trials, participants were required to complete 240 trials, which lasted about 20 minutes. If baseline was included, the task would have been doubled, and participants likely would not have been able to stay focused for the entire task. In future studies, the LDT should include the baseline trials to control for many factors that may influence participants’ reaction time. However, in order to avoid participant fatigue, prime categories should be presented in a between-participants, not within-participants, design. With a within—participants design, participants had to complete 240 trials because there were five different prime categories. However, with a between-participants design, there would be four different groups of participants: one 107 completing the task with dark-skinned Blacks with more prototypical facial features vs. White controls, one completing the task with dark-skinned Blacks with less prototypical facial features vs. White controls, another completing the task with light-skinned Blacks with more prototypical facial features vs. White controls, and the last group completing the task with light-skinned Blacks with less prototypical facial features vs. White controls. If the task included only two prime categories, the number of trials would be cut down to 96. Alternatively, the number of pictures in each prime category can be cut from eight to four, in order to reduce the number of trials. In fact, variation across participants’ responses to eight pictures was relatively small (for negative words, means of eight pictures within each prime category ranged from 27.30 to 41.40, and standard deviation ranged from 11.64 to 16.38), suggesting that cutting the number of target pictures is a reasonable solution to reduce the length of the task. If the number of trials could be significantly reduced either by including only two prime categories or by reducing the number of pictures, positive target words could also be included, just like the original LDT. With positive words included in the task, future studies could exarrrine whether darker skin tone and more prototypical facial features are associated with both negative and positive stereotypes or only with negative stereotypes. A second limitation of the current work is the lack of Black female target pictures in the sequential priming and lexical decision tasks. There was only one female target picture in each of the five prime categories. Although there were two sets of pictures (thus resulting in two pictures for each prime category when sets were combined), there still might be idiosyncratic effects of specific faces. Although attractiveness, 108 babyfacedness, and hostility did not differ between two light-skinned faces and two dark- skinned faces, the results must be interpreted with caution. With only four pictures, the results of inferential statistics may not be reliable. Notably, because additional Black female pictures were collected in Study 4, these pictures can be included as target stimuli in future research. Third, the majority of the Black participants in Study 4 were Black women, and there were only 12 Black men. As some of the findings in the current research demonstrate, the experiences with prejudice are often different between Black men and Black women, with Black men experiencing more prejudice than Black women. If Black men and women are perceived and treated differently by others, their responses to prejudice and discrimination also should be different. Therefore, it will be important for future research to take into account participant gender when examining how one’s own physical characteristics influence responses to and interpretations of negative events. Fourth, there were substantial differences in the strength of skin tone manipulation (dark vs. light) and the strength of the facial features manipulation (more vs. less prototypical). As a result, the findings that the effect size is bigger for the skin tone effect than for the facial features effects might be due to this methodological issue rather than to actual differences in the effects of these constructs. Facial features were manipulated to reflect one standard deviation above or below the mean, so that more prototypical facial features and less prototypical facial features conditions were exactly two standard deviations apart from one another. In contrast, the selection of skin tones was based on darkest vs. lightest skin tones in the original picture pool, so that the difference between dark skin tone and light skin tone conditions might have been bigger 109 than two standard deviations. Indeed, the average of standardized luminosity for 16 dark skin tone faces was -1 .30 (individual standardized luminosity ranged between -1.78 and - 1.04), and that for 16 light skin tone faces was 1.64 (individual standardized luminosity ranged between -1 .78 and -l .04), suggesting that the dark skin tone and light skin tone conditions were about three standard deviations apart from one another. In future research, it will be important to equate the strength of skin tone and facial features manipulations. Another limitation with the present research is the failure to collect White participants’ gender information due to a computer malfunction. As the subordinate male target hypothesis predicts, how White men affectively and cognitively react to Black men may be different from how White women react to Black men. The subordinate male target hypothesis not only argues that men are the primary targets of racial discrimination, but also that men are the primary insti gators of racial discrimination (Pratto et al., 2006; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). If this is the case, White men, as compared to White women, may express greater negativity toward Black men with darker skin tone and more prototypical facial features. In future research, it will be important to examine whether participant gender moderates the effects of physical characteristics on Whites’ affective and cognitive reactions toward Black individuals. The next step, after the limitations listed above are addressed, is to examine underlying mechanisms that explain the relationships between physical characteristics and people’s perceptions of and reactions to Black targets. Previous researchers who study Afrocentric features have proposed several potential mechanisms. Some researchers argue that strong Afrocentric features serve as a representation of “Black” 110 and activate this social category (Cloutier, Mason, & Macrae, 2005; Blair et al., 2002; Maddox & Gray, 2002; Mason, Cloutier, & Macrae, 2006). Other researchers argue that strong Afrocentric features are directly linked to group-related attributions through repeated association (Blair et al., 2002; Livingston & Brewer, 2002; Meier, Robinson, & Cole, 2004). This perspective asserts that Afrocentric features initially influence perceptions through category activation. However, through repeated pairing of Afrocentric features and stored associates of the category "Black," a direct association between Afrocentric features and those associates becomes established. Finally, some researchers argue that the negative effects of Afrocentric features on perceptions are due to unfamiliarity (Livingston & Brewer, 2002; Zebrowitz, Bronstad, & Lee, 2007). It has been shown that faces that are familiar, as opposed to unfamiliar, induce more positive attitudes (DeBruinem 2002; Hill, Lewicki, Czyzewska, & Schuller, 1990, Peskin & Newell, 2004) and are more preferred (Bomstein, 1993, Harnm, Baum, & Nikels, 1975; Rhodes, Halberstadt, & Brajkovich, 2001 ). Because faces with strong Afrocentric features, as compared to faces with weak Afrocentric features, are more unfamiliar to White people, Whites’ negative perceptions of and reactions to Black men with stronger Afrocentric features may reflect their responses to unfamiliarity. Future research should also investigate how perceivers can control biases associated with physical characteristics, once the underlying mechanisms have been revealed. Previous research has shown that people can be educated about their automatic biased attitudes toward certain social groups and that inhibition of prejudiced attitudes is possible once people become aware of the biases, especially when they have sufficient abilities and motivation to do so. For instance, researchers have found that judges who 111 were extensively trained to ensure race neutrality in sentencing were able to give roughly equivalent sentences to Black and White offenders, given equivalent criminal histories (Blair et al., 2004a). Inhibition of prejudiced attitudes is possible even with automatic attitudes (Blair et al., 2004b; Monteith, Sherman, & Devine, 1998; Wyer, Sherman, & Stroessner, 1998; 2000; also see Blair, 2002 for extensive review). Thus, it is important to educate people about the biases associated with skin tone and facial features and to train people to inhibit prejudiced attitudes, as the effects of skin tone and facial features can have serious consequences in people’s lives, such as discriminatory criminal sentencing (Blair et al., 2004a; Eberhardt et al., 2006) and shooting errors among police officers (Kahn et al., 2008). Future research should also investigate how targets with “disadvantageous” physical characteristics can reduce the negative consequences of these characteristics. Targets of social inequality are not passive agents---they are active participants in intergroup relations and should have influence in determining the quality of their interactions with members from other social groups (Swim & Stangor, 1998). Therefore, examining what targets can do to reduce people’s negative feelings about them is as important as examining what perceivers can do to reduce their biases toward certain physical characteristics. When Brent Staples, whose essay “Black men and public spaces” (Staples, 1986) caught people’s attention, walked down the street at night people ran away from him from fear, thinking of him as a mugger or rapist, because he was a young Black man. However, once he started whistling classical music while walking at night, people were not as nervous when they walked by and saw him. This famous story demonstrates that sending perceivers counter-stereotypic information can reduce negative 112 perceptions, possibly because perceivers individualize the targets or think of the targets as exceptions who do not fit well to “Black” social category (Brewer, 1988; Fiske et al., 1999) Conclusion The present studies demonstrated that skin tone and facial features independently influence people’s perceptions of and reactions to Blacks and that the effects of these visible physical characteristics are different depending on whether feelings are measured at the implicit or explicit level, whether targets are Black men or Black women, and whether perceivers are Whites or Blacks. There was relatively little evidence that Blacks' own skin tone and facial features influence their reactions to negative events and experiences with discrimination. As has been discussed, the sparse findings may reflect lack of Black male participants with varied physical characteristics. The present studies contribute to a better understanding of the effects of physical characteristics on people’s perceptions of and reactions to Black individuals, by pointing to the benefits and importance of identifying factors that explain who, when, and under what circumstances targets are more likely to experience prejudice and discrimination. 113 Footnotes 1. Pictures of 89 White men and 43 White women were also collected for the selection of control pictures. 2. Thirty five participants who rated Black women were the same people who categorized the pictures into Black, White, or Racially Ambiguous. 3. There were two sets of pictures for each target gender because participants could not be exposed to both high and low prototypical versions of the same face. Thus, participants were randomly assigned to see one of the two sets. 4. One dark-skinned face was categorized as White by one participant, and the other dark—skinned face was perceived to be racially ambiguous by one participant; one light- skinned face was categorized as White by one participant and as racially ambiguous by five participants, and the other light-skinned face was categorized as racially ambiguous by nine participants. 5. Due to a computer malfunction, participants’ age and gender were not recorded. Because there is no way to connect participants’ names to their data, only the gender ratio could be coded using participants’ first names (women = 72.4%, men = 24.4%, and gender neutral names = 3.2%). 6. Explicit liking toward Black targets was measured using a unidimensional item, rather than measuring positive and negative attitudes toward targets separately, because people are more comfortable indicating how much they like other people than indicating how much they dislike other people. Especially in a racially charged situation, such as the current studies, White participants would be very uncomfortable indicating how much they dislike Black individuals. In fact, I assume that White participants would 114 deliberately change their feelings toward the Black targets to portray themselves as non- prejudiced due to social desirability concerns, resulting in little variability in the explicit negative attitudes toward Blacks. 7. Although the analysis was conducted using the log-transformed facilitation scores, the results are presented in the original values. Means and standard deviations in log transformed response latencies are presented in tables. 8. Because all analyses suggest that prime set does not influence the effects of skin tone and facial features on Whites’ affective reactions to Black targets, prime set was droped from the analyses of target gender effects. 9. One participant was excluded from the analyses of the sequential priming task for male targets because she was talking during the task, and 20 participants (17 due to PC malfirnction, four due to wrong categorization of target gender, and one due to random factors) were excluded from the analyses of the lexical decision task for female targets. 10. Five participants (one due to PC malfunction and four due to random factors) were excluded from the analyses of the lexical decision task for male targets, and 17 participants (10 due to PC malfunction, four due to random factors such as talking during tasks, and three due to wrong categorization of target gender) were excluded from the analyses of the sequential priming task for female targets. 11. A mixed models analysis was conducted, treating scenario as a within-participants factor and facial features as a between-participants factor, was conducted for each of the six attribution categories (racism, sexism, skin tone, and facial features, internal, external) to examine whether participants’ responses to six different scenarios can be averaged within each attribution category to compute attribution scores. The two-way interactions 115 between scenario and facial features were non-significant for the all six attribution categories (all F s(5, 405) < 2.02, ps > .10), suggesting that participants’ responses to different scenarios can be averaged over to create attribution scores for each attribution category. Thus, each participants are associated with six attribution scores (racism attribution score, sexism attribution score, skin tone attribution score, and facial features attribution score, internal attribution score, external attribution score). 12. Prime set condition was excluded from all of the analyses because (1) there were only a small number of participants in each condition, and (2) previous analyses with White participants showed that prime set do not have any effects on the main results. 13. Standard deviations of Black participants were bigger than those of White participants, indicating that there was more variability in responses to words among Black, as compared to White participants. 116 Table 1 Means and standard deviations ofsubjective ratings and objective measures of Black faces by race Black Men Black Women M SD M SD Afrocentric Features 3.98 .40 3.46 .48 Attractiveness 2.01 .3 8 2.14 .54 Babyfacedness 2.28 .55 2.42 .56 Hostility 2.55 .47 2.43 .60 Lip Ratio .14 .02 .13 .02 Nose Ratio .32 .03 .31 .02 Luminosity .34 .09 .36 .06 Note. Units for luminosity is proportion and ranges fi'om 0.0 to 1.0. 117 o2 ”2.5.v & 82865 1.... use ._o>o_ 3. v Q 05 8 02805:»? 828:2: .._ .o._ 3 od 88m momma 98 209285 $ biog—WEE Sm 3:5 $82 l .LNV 3.- gum... :. l we. 2. 1N- .. 3. nor -- *ambr h o w v m 8. we. NT **_mr *o_. **er **VM. **_v. **Nm. **_mr .LNH @8583 .5 23m 302 .0 23m .5 .m 3:58: .4 mmocvooflznem .m $8338.32 .N 8530a aficoootxx .— =mE «was LQBQQVBSMKQ $23,435 $53.56 38 wMSBa wifimfig 9523 38239260 N 2an 118 om NZ .8. v Q mBSEE .1. :5 ._o>o_ mo. v m 65 E oogocfigm 8:865 ... .o._ 3 od 80¢ moms: cg :omtomoa 3 bmmofisé 5.: BED .862 -- 2.- 2.- am.- -- 8.- E.- -- 2. a e m 4 mm. 2.- 5.- **nvr 5. mo.- 2.- *awmr **~V. *mmr **mm. **mv. 5. No. 2.- 3:85:51— <- 0:an 082 .c as: a: .m 55.8: .4 mmogooflxnem .m mmo:o>:o€:< .N magnum 05:83.3. ._ $59: «95m .8\ moo:\«oe~m\o mega-ems 958.3: 38 $2.52 miBmEg M208: 30236-269 m 2an 119 om is .So. v a 8:865 r... 2... . S. v Q 833:5 a... ._o>o_ mo. v a. 05 “a oogomefi 8332: ... .o._ 3 06 Bow woman :5 :omtoaoa v: 368:5: :02 3E: .302 we. :3 am. am mm. mm. E. Ni O: on- mm. :.N NE 13A on a?“ mm. 3. .8. mm. 8. Q. S. 3. Be .1...me _- 8. 9.. n e S. S 25H 5% 52w: Du. 2.0m ow. end cw. bod vm. nod we. mm. 8. v _. mo. mm. Qu- 2 0:8. :Em flax-ED £38: Boggflxfim Baguette. 8:38.: 03:895.. 28m 082 as: a: 58:58:.“ 22338.6 38: Se}. 3%.: as «as: Lo\mm§3m\~eo.53& m. 55 «ouEKo amaze-35 $88.50 33 9:283:33 mmctmamswah SSE has £223.53: 223323: .23»: v 033- 120 m "\0 .3. v Q 3:365 _.. 6.. 9 od 80¢ mowg: 0:0 50.5080 mm 560582 :8 BED .0002 of of 6. SN 3. 8.... 0:38: a. em. 8. SN 3. CS. 388830: NI 9: 3. 80 E. 03. 383388... 40m 03 em. mam om. Sm 8.38: 00.885. 2: w _ .- 3. mm. 3. mm. as: 082 of w? 8. :. 8. E. as: a: a: 1.3- So. 9.. mo. am. 28563 0 a 0m 2 00 2 0:09 :35 Hour—wmd DEC-H :U—m nae—0Q 00.0.0000 0000 0.0?- EMQ a; «.000 00X§0500K~00N§0Q w. 0000.: «0022\0 000000000 09000.50 000 09000300.? 000000500 0000 000 #0000060 0000000.. .0000: m 050% 121 .50. v :2 80020:: 3.... 0:02 3.: 1.82 we. 2 .m :0. . 8.0 0 a 00 :0 0:. 0 8:00:00 0200-.— m0:300m 30$ 3020.388: 304 00289805 0:02 0.0 n >0 6:83 8.: 3.2.2 0. 00m 6. Sm .0 : QM. 2 Q0. 2 00:300.; Eon-.2 30:50:05 000-— 82500”. 300 m 32:30:05 0:02 Awm HZV :02 5202030080 2200200885 m000\0§:00\20.:00\«00.0 000 000$.000:00K~0.:00\.m.0000 000.500 0.0.0.000 5000:2080-00- 0: m000000§0 0002 e 2.3 122 Awe: wwo. £50 0va A2 3 A: 3 :03 so: 30. NS. £0. 05. $3.0 $0.3 $0.00 @0000 $2 00% Ram 03». 33 :05 33 :05 3:02958000 092029500000 003000 300”— 0500& 300m 223 25 :00 £0: 250 :20 0.30 5.3 2 5.3 0.50m 00.500000; mo: A0000: 008m 30% 000500\~0.C0\000 0000 0.0%. 0. 005 «00500 000.00% 0 00 0000.: 0>00M00 05 00\000000 00.000.02.005? 00000300 0.000003. 000 00002 n 2.3 123 A: 3 N8. :38 R: 8:0 2:0 02.0 603 N8. $0. N8. $0. 8300 80000 80000 8300 002 8.: 008 8.8 33 :05 33 =05 021029300000 002—0295000052 008000 300.2 00308 3000 253 250 :30 £0: 25 =00 V.00 S00 2 Boom 00808000; woq 500 2 A0000: Boom 30m 000500\~0.~00\000 0000 030 0. 005 0003\0 00.0000\ 0 00 00003 090.00% 000 00\000000 00.000.00.005? 00000300 00000000 000 00002 w 030,—. 124 Table 9 Means and standard deviations of White perceivers ' explicit liking toward Black men as a function of skin tone and facial features (Study 1) Dark Skin Tone Light Skin Tone Facial Features Facial Features Prototypicality Prototypicality High Low High Low M 2.97 3.01 3.03 3.16 SD .70 .70 .74 .71 125 am: :20 80: am: am: 500 200 200 30.0 200 3.00 0 280 8080030 03 00.80 20.30 00.8: 80.00 @000 E00 0000 00000 0.300 00.000 $.30 E 0025 880 300 33 .00: 33 :05 520005980 0050289805 0003000 3000 0003000 3000 323 250 50 £0: 250 :20 0.00 00003 ofiboefim 0.000000 00.000\ 000 0000 0&0 0. 005 «00200 00000002 0 00 0000.: 0005000000000 000 0005000000 00 0000000000000 00.000.00.00 00000300 0.0000000 000 00002 a: 0E0H 126 :0: an: $03 803 0.03 9.00 000.0 3.00 0000 0000 £0.80 00.8: 000.80 $0.80 £003 000% 00.0% 00.000 00.000 2.0% 33 005 33 000: 00000330000; 00002990005 00.50000 3000 0003000 3000 223 88. 0&0 £0: 250 :20. 0:00 S00 2 0000m 008000000; mo: S00 2 600—5 0000m 30m 00003 0_§000000m-:0z 00.0.80 2 200.0 127 Table 1 1 Means and standard deviations of White perceivers ' explicit liking toward Black men as a function of skin tone andfacial features (Study 2) Dark Skin Tone Light Skin Tone Facial Features Facial Features Prototypicality Prototypicality High Low High Low M 2.96 2.99 3.09 3.12 SD .75 .72 .73 .75 128 020 08. $003 mm.~m :03 80: am: 0.03 woo. owe. woo. woo. 20.00 20.: 0 £0.00: @000 3.00 0060 wmsv oodm 33 0000 33 00:0 300000500000 500009300000 00300 0 0000 0 00300 0 .060 0 0:55 250 030 £0: 250 :20 0:00 S00 2 S00 0.50m 00800000000. mod 80005 0000m 30,0 0000000\ 00.00% 000 0000 030 0. 0000.: «0000\0 00000000 00 00.003 0>00M00 00 00\000000 00000000KK0 00000300 0.0000000 000 00002 .2 0300. 129 :0: :0. 20.000 00.00 0:55 03.0 803 :30 $2.0 08. 000. N00. 000. :08: 00.8: 0003 80.00 00.2 00.00 0000 00. a 33 0000 33 005 30000500000 320000300000 05000 0 3000 00300 0 3000 250 :20 £0: 250 30 0.00 500 0 S00 0000m 00800000000. mod A0025 Boom 300 00.0000\ 00.00% 000 0000 030 0. 00.00.: 0003\0 000000\ 0 00 00.00.: 030.0000. 00 00\000000 0000000030000 00000300 0.0000000 000 00002 2 0300. 130 Table 14 Means and standard deviations of White perceivers ' explicit liking toward Black women as afunction of skin tone andfacial features (Study 3a) Dark Skin Tone Light Skin Tone Facial Features Facial Features Prototypicality Prototypicality High Low High Low M 2.75 2.80 2.99 3.06 SD .96 .86 .91 .93 131 Gm: COS cm: 53 $3 55 33 SE 38 SE 83 S 28m 885%; we; $3: 5.8 5 83m: 5.: 5 22m: 88 a .§ 358 3+8 cams 8.80 S @025 28m 5.x 33 :wE 33 :3: bzmambowoi bzaegaowoi magnum Evan— mohsaon— 32E 253 2:; 3m 5»: 88 5% x25 $5? 29508on mmxSuQNEuuKhzc 86» :3“. .4. 22:9: «95%? EEBSX a we v.39: Embomzmfiéot 35 meQESm 2: xQQBFdE mm=cm8x\e w=o2§>mw 3332an 38 was»: 2 2an 132 some 803 ES 8”: 3&3 5.8 :3. E8 2% 83. mayo 2 28m 8:833 m3 $20: :03: ago: 692: :93: 53 afimvc @336 5:30 563 81.3 E A825 Boom 39m 33 PE: 33 E: bzmofiboaoi bzmembouoi 853$ 30mm 8::QO 30mm 255 25 :5 am: 25 5% En €83 Baboobwméoz .2329: S 2an 133 Table 16 Means and standard deviations of White perceivers ' explicit liking toward Black women as a function of skin tone and facial features (Study 3b) Dark Skin Tone Light Skin Tone Facial Features Facial Features Prototypicality Prototypicality High Low High Low M 2.69 2.82 2.92 2.91 SD .89 .98 .89 .92 134 2 n2 .5. v m 8:865 2. use .732 8 . v Q 05 as 8.805%? 8:865 ... .802 - :3.- Q: 8.- mm. 3.. mm; Samoa .5 - am. 2. 2.. *S. *8. woe-Sam e -- a. 8. mm. .._ E. mmo=o>uo§< .m -- :2. mm. mm. ”238m 0.5585 .v -- 5.- we. 8:2 a: 082 o>< .m -- 1.3. mused .830 Sam .N -- b30583: .— wm. we. Nm. 3. we. 9: mo. 3- SN SN SN NM: 3. N?- R. E n o w v m N _ $238.3 . $233953. £3: «93% LQKV-mEetg 33395. 2: @823 99:33-28 38 £233.:me 33.352 .28»: C 053- 135 _ n n 2 .5. v E 82865 3.. 98 :32 mo. v a 2: “a 3:35:me 8:368 ... .802 - :6.- ..En- an. E. 8.- *8- £38?» -- :8. La.- 2- 2. :. oomtname -- :3.- 3- 2. mm. mmoco>coe:<.m -- :8. :5. 8.- 858m one-.85. .v -- m _. 2. mean a: 082 .o>< .m -- :8. mag .28 .35 .N -- bfiofigfi .— mn. oo. 3. «m. mn- Om._ mo. QM. Sim OWN m_.m 9mm 8.. 2.- VM. 3 N- o w v m N _ 8.53.3 . mezefiawtem 3325\«935 3\ 836.29 33.3me 2: 38:8 atonfimtou 33 defies-3% @8333. .33»: M: 2an 136 mm H 2 .5. v R 88065 .1. 0:0 ._0>o_ mo. v m 05 E oogomefi 80865 ... .802 -- 2mm. :8. 2. mo. .3. :2. *2. to... 8.058.085 £5885me can-05¢ -- 3.9.. 8. 8. 2. *mm. 2. .100 802335803 85502 8.0555200 -- .2. *2. .2. $3.. :2. :0... 330.2% .5085 82.0.82 .2 -- :8. 1.8. m _. in. *2. 8:352 E53 .0 -- *2. 2. 2. .00 8030508250 -- :3. :8. :2. 802:3. 8.380 00$ .0 -- 1%. 3.2. 80.532. 2.3 gm .m -- :2. 8:33;. :2me .N -- 803.52 8022 ._ 8. 3. o; :1 02 v? :2 02 MS 00 RN 3: of: mm.»- Nm.m mi Sm 20 5. 3. o w n 0 w v m N 2 0030.20.» 00.28.20 00x02 000 :0 M00000 0000020200 000 60000300 00000000 60002 2 030,—- 137 Table 20 The eflects of skin tone, facial features, and participants 'gender on attribution to racism With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE fl t Intercept 4.20 . 16 Skin Tone .73 1.96 .05 .37 Facial Features -.27 .21 -.15 -1 .31 Gender .64 .47 .17 1.38 Skin x Face 2.51 2.68 .l l .94 R2: .04, F(4, 78) = .81,p = .53 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,8 t Intercept 4.21 . 1 6 Skin Tone .001 .1 l .001 .01 Facial Features -.67 .29 -.27 -2.31* Gender .67 .44 .18 1.51 Skin x Face .25 .22 .13 1.12 R2: .08, F(4, 78) = 1.63,p = .18 Note. * indicates p < .05. df= 82. 138 Table 21 The effects of skin tone, facial features, and participants 'gender on attribution to sexism With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,B t Intercept 3 .3 3 . 16 Skin Tone .26 1.95 .02 .13 Facial Features -.27 .21 -.15 -1.32 Gender -.50 .46 -.13 -1.09 Skin x Face 1.07 2.66 .05 .40 R2: .06, F(4, 78) = l.15,p = .34 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,8 t Intercept 3.34 1 6 Skin Tone .02 .1 1 .02 .14 Facial Features -.49 .29 -.20 -1.68T Gender -.51 .44 -.14 -1.14 Skin x Face -.07 .22 -.04 -.31 R2= .08, F(4, 78)=1.57,p = .19 Note. 1' indicateSp < .10. df= 82. 139 Table 22 The effects of skin tone, facial features, and participants 'gender on attribution to skin tone With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE E t Intercept 3 .3 8 .1 7 Skin Tone -.01 2.08 .0001 —.004 Facial Features -.51 .22 -.26 -2.30* Gender 1.22 .50 .29 2.46* Skianace 3.19 2.85 .13 1.12 R2 = .12, F(4, 78) = 2.61,p < .05 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,6 t Intercept 3 .39 . 1 7 Skin Tone -.O3 .12 -.03 -.24 Facial Features -1.03 .30 -.38 -3.41** Gender 1.28 .47 .31 2.75** Skin x Face .37 .23 .17 1.61 R2: .18, F(4, 78) = 4.10,p < .01 Note. * indicates p < .05, and ** indicates p < .01. df= 82. 140 Table 23 The effects of skin tone, facial features, and participants ’gender on attribution to facial features With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE )9 t Intercept 2.49 . 1 6 Skin Tone -1.49 1.95 -.09 -.76 Facial Features -. 12 .21 -.07 -.59 Gender -.26 .46 -.07 -.55 Skin x Face 4.46 2.67 .19 1.671" R2 = .07, F(4, 78) = 1.34, p = .25 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,8 t Intercept 2.56 .1 6 Skin Tone -.12 .12 -.12 -1.02 Facial Features -. 12 .30 -.05 -.41 Gender -.44 .46 -.12 -.97 Skin x Face .04 .23 .02 .16 R2 = .03, F(4, 78) = .49, p = .74 Note. '1' indicates p < .10. df= 82. 141 Table 24 The eflects of skin tone, facial features, and participants ’gender on internal attribution With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,6 t Intercept 3.30 . 1 7 Skin Tone 3.60 2.00 .22 1.801’ Facial Features .03 .21 .01 .12 Gender .14 .47 .04 .30 Skin x Face .24 2.73 .01 .09 R2 = .05, F(4, 78) = .92, p = .46 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE 19 t Intercept 3.3 0 . 1 6 Skin Tone .25 .1 l .25 2.20* Facial Features -.19 .29 -.08 -.66 Gender .29 .45 .08 .65 Skin x Face .40 .22 .20 1.801' R2: .10, F(4, 78) = 2.09,p < .10 Note. T indicates p < .10, and * indicates p < .05. df= 82. 142 Table 25 The effects of skin tone, facial features, and participants 'gender on external attribution With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE 19 t Intercept 3.30 . 1 8 Skin Tone 1.16 2.19 .07 .53 Facial Features .21 .23 .1 l .90 Gender .14 .52 .03 .27 Skin x Face .60 3.00 .02 .20 R2 = .02, F(4, 78) = .36, p = .84 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,8 t Intercept 3 .28 . 1 8 Skin Tone .12 .13 .11 .93 Facial Features .0001 .33 .0001 -.001 Gender .34 .50 .08 .67 Skin x Face .34 .25 .16 1.37 R2 = .04, F(4, 78) = .77, p = .55 Note. df= 82. 143 Table 26 The effects of skin tone, facial features, and participants 'gender on experiences with racial discrimination in the past one year With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,8 t Intercept 1 .85 . 1 0 Skin Tone -.89 1.25 -.09 -.71 Facial Features .23 .13 .20 1.70'1L Gender .07 .30 .03 .23 Skin x Face —2.67 1.71 -.18 -1.56 R2 = .07, F(4, 78) = 1.54,p = .20 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,8 t Intercept 1 .82 .1 0 Skin Tone .01 .07 .01 .09 Facial Features .29 .19 . 18 1.52 Gender .17 .29 .07 .60 Skin x Face -.24 .14 -.19 -1.68T R2 = .07, F(4, 78) = 1.33, p = .27 Note. ‘1' indicates p < .10. df= 82. 144 Table 27 The eflects of skin tone, facial features, and participants ’gender on lifetime experiences with racial discrimination With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,B t Intercept 2.35 .12 Skin Tone .27 1.43 .02 .19 Facial Features .24 .15 .19 1 .61 Gender .17 .34 .06 .49 Skin x Face -1.15 1.95 -.07 . -.59 R2 = .05, F(4, 78) = 1.04,p = .39 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE 5 t Intercept 2.34 . 1 2 Skin Tone .06 .08 .09 .76 Facial Features .23 .22 .13 1.05 Gender .27 .33 .10 .82 Skin x Face -.02 .16 -.Ol -.11 R2 = .03, F(4, 78) = .66, p = .62 Note. df= 82. 145 Table 28 The effects of skin tone, facial features, and participants 'gender on race-based rejection sensitivity With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,B t Intercept l 3.61 .99 Skin Tone 2.05 1 1.87 .02 .17 Facial Features -.38 1.26 -.04 -.30 Gender 3.51 2.82 .15 1.24 Skin x Face 30.20 16.23 .22 1861' R2: .05, F(4, 78) = l.12,p = .35 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,8 t Intercept 14.00 .98 Skin Tone .09 .69 .02 .13 Facial Features -.94 1.78 -.06 -.53 Gender 3.03 2.74 .13 1.10 Skin x Face 2.34 1.35 .20 1.73'1' R2 = .05, F(4, 78) = .95, p = .44 Note. ‘1' indicates p < .10. df= 82. 146 Table 29 The eflects of skin tone, facialfeatures, and participants 'gender on racial identification With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,6 t Intercept 4.65 . 1 6 Skin Tone .53 1.97 .03 .27 Facial Features . 10 .21 .05 .46 Gender -.87 .47 -.20 -1.86T Skin x Face .12 2.69 .01 .05 R2=.06,F(4,78)=1.15,p = .34 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE [5' t Intercept 4.65 . 16 Skin Tone .05 .1 l .05 .40 Facial Features .09 .29 .04 .30 Gender -.87 .45 -.23 -1 .921‘ Skin x Face -.19 .22 -.10 -.83 R2 = .06, F(4, 78) = 1.26, p = .30 Note. T indicatesp < .10. df= 82. 147 Table 30 The eflects of skin tone, facial features, and participants 'gender on perception of discrimination With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,6 t Intercept 2.92 . 19 Skin Tone -.10 2.24 -.01 -.04 Facial Features .05 .24 .03 .22 Gender -.20 .53 -.05 -.37 Skin x Face 2.55 3.06 .10 .83 R2 = .01, F(4, 78) = .28, p = .89 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,6 t Intercept 2 .96 .1 8 Skin Tone -.01 .13 -.01 -.O8 Facial Features .08 .34 .03 .23 Gender -.28 .52 -.07 -.54 Skin x Face .13 .25 .06 .53 R2= .01, F(4, 78) = .18,p= .95 Note. df= 82. 148 Table 31 The effects of skin tone, facial features, and participants ’gender on overall self-esteem With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE B t Intercept 3.54 .08 Skin Tone -.83 .93 -.11 -.89 Facial Features .01 . 10 .02 .14 Gender .05 .22 .03 .23 Skin x Face -.50 1.28 -.05 -.39 R2 = .02, F(4, 78) = .30, p = .88 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE E t Intercept 3.52 .08 Skin Tone -.01 .05 -.02 -.21 Facial Features -.08 .14 -.O7 -.59 Gender . 16 .22 .09 .76 Skianace .13 .11 .15 1.24 R2 = .03, F(4, 78) = .51, p = .73 Note. df= 82. 149 Table 32 The effects of skin tone, facialfeatures, and participants 'gender on social self-esteem With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE 13 t Intercept 3.50 . 1 0 Skin Tone -1.21 1.21 -.12 -1.01 Facial Features -.10 .13 -.09 -.78 Gender -.08 .29 -.04 -.29 Skin x Face -.73 1.65 -.05 -.44 R2 = .03, F(4, 78) = .52, p = .72 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,6 t Intercept 3 .48 . 1 0 Skin Tone -.04 .07 -.O7 -.59 Facial Features -.24 .18 -.16 -1.32 Gender .03 .28 .01 .12 Skianace .16 .14 .14 1.16 R2 = .04, F(4, 78) = .70, p = .60 Note. df= 82. 150 Table 33 The effects of skin tone, facial features, and participants 'gender on appearance self- esteem With Objective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,8 t Intercept 3.42 .09 Skin Tone .90 1.13 .10 .80 Facial Features .12 .12 .12 1.01 Gender .16 .27 .08 .61 Skin x Face -.05 1.54 -.003 -.03 R2 = .03, F(4, 78) = .64,p = .63 With Subjective Measure of Skin Tone and Facial Features b SE ,6 t Intercept 3.39 .09 Skin Tone .1 1 .07 .19 1.63 Facial Features -.10 .17 -.07 -.58 Gender .34 .26 .16 1.32 Skianace .13 .13 .12 .99 R2 = .05, F(4, 78) = 1.08, p = .37 Note. df= 82. 151 2m: coo. 5.2: 0de 0:03 3.3 an: Kb. 03. 08.8: $2; : mmém 8.3 33 :02: 320293805 00500..— 32¢ A~o_0 Am0_0 mvo. So. Amfimm: Swim: No.0 am;— 33 :00: bzaflguoaoi 8:th 360m 88. :05 £0: 250 5%. V.00 5.3 E 58 2 200m 0080820; we; A0025 200m 30m 00.0000X 20.00500 0000 030 . 000N000 0~0E 0003\0 00.0000\ 0 00 0000.0 00.00M00 0S 00\000000 00.000.02.005? 000.000.0000 00000000 000 0000: 0m 030% 152 at: for 83%: 03a- A8: 2:: 32.: a2: So. So. 38. 88. 83v: GEN: 5.2: 5.3: 8.2- 32- 8.2. was- 33 :3: 33 FE: bzmommboaopm bzwflmbowoi 838m Eon”— 838m :3on 223 2:: Em Em: 25? Em v.25 E: E 5: Boom concommcfih we: 38:: Boom and 8.3“ch NEUQXNEQ was 2.3? . @332 33: $35.? «5.293%: .3 $39: 9::on 2: memxoow 2233335? 38.2633: 333:8“. 33 .33: mm 2an 153 :2: v2. A38: 93 Ahm_: Amn_: Av¢_: Ao¢_: mo_. ~o_. ac_. mm_. 48.2: 62;: 5.8: 35:: 8.8 3% 3.8 $8 33 fix 33 @z 5:813:8on 528E388; 233m 36mm 833m .3on 2:5, 2:; :5 E: 2.8. 5% VEa an: E 8: Boom :oEpOmmcmF mod 38:: Boom and 9.933% 3.83533 NEE 23m . “.338 m35m\«u£m\o =EB§\G an $39: méBMm: m5 ameSB. =0:S:Bu\\o $523.33: 3333:. 35 33: cm 2an 154 3:: m2. 2 05.: on? 253 Am5_: Ammo: A_o~: Avw_: om_. mm_. ov_. ov_. :93: 3.0.2: 3.3%: 5.2: 8.? 9.3 11: $2 33 PE: 33 :3: 35853585 bzoofiboooi Qua—mom amoun— thmom Emomh 253 5% Em: 25.3 5m BS 5: 5.: Boom oogommcfirr wou— AoomEV Boom Box woLEoo\ NSoobozo ozoo 2.3}. . flows“ o\oso\«oo\m\o =o.:o§\o no £29: o>.:.2oo. of xo\moxoow 2233.835? 3.233363 gonzo: .33 2:8: mm oEoH 155 Table 38 Means and standard deviations of Black perceivers ’ explicit liking toward Black male targets as a function of skin tone and facial features Dark Skin Tone Light Skin Tone Facial Features Facial Features Prototypicality Prototypicality High Low High Low M 3.01 3.07 3.10 3.24 (SD) (.93) (.91) (.89) (.88) 156 Table 39 Means and standard deviations of Black perceivers ' explicit liking toward Black female targets as a function of skin tone and facial features Dark Skin Tone Light Skin Tone Facial Features Facial Features Prototypicality Prototypicality High Low High Low M 3.05 3.12 3.30 3.27 (SD) (1.09) (1.12) (.98) (1.06) 157 5.3 A8: so: 53 53 5.3 88 a? 23 £3 :3 E 28m 8:58“: we; 96$: :Nd : 6%: z :03: 22:: R5 23% 2E? 35% 3.5 83% E 6025 28m Ex 33 Ex 33 $5 bzaflmfiouem tax—«29380.5 mocawom Eon”— mocamom 38m 223 25 Em Em: 2:; :5 0:8 2:95 Baboflfim EEcQBBthctu was 5.3}. .fimeS £3: «comic =o:u==\o me $39: oEbouSBéo: has mmbomgmu. 2t xeXmfiotfiS mwzommmLKQ «£83336 33332 35» was: ow 2an 158 ate 3? Ewfi : vodom 953 A23 9:3 2nd ammo zoom: 93%: mimnm omdwm 33 3?: 3:853085 8:58“— 30$ 35$ A8: 5.3 :3 23 E 28m 3582833 :33 $92: 5.8 2.0% 31% E acméeoom Ex 33 E: bzmflnboaoi mpuawonm fimmowm 25 5%. 2w: 25 Em £3 €83 meoobuméoz APEooV ow 2an 159 $3 $03 6.0.3 A33 9&3 5.9 83 $3 3% 2% 83 E 28m 8.895%; mod 33m : 692$ 6&3: $35 A38: 55 3&8 38¢ 2.38 NEE 2.5% E A825 28m Em 33 :3: 33 En bzmofibouoi 5:8:5805 magnum Eon”— mohzmom 30mm 323 2:; :5 E»: 25 5% x55 $53 2938me 358\~39th6 28“ 5%,. SmeS m~cEm\«uc\m\o =e:u§\.3 ,3 $39.: uabomxmfiéoa has 238me m3 gogxmmtzfis 3:934:ng naotafimfi 33.35% 38 .38»: I» 2an 160 5.3.3 GNS ans GMS 603 5,3 m 3% SE 3% :10 $33 2 28m 898353 33 93.3: :33: 835 A33 a 833$ 53 .380 3.23 3.5. 8.33 3.2 E E 33.5 88m 33 33 .35 33 :3: bzaofisgofi bzaofibsoi 8:53..— .Eomm 8528.3 303 223 25 :5 £3 2.8 =5 33 38>» ofiboeofiméoz .5353 S 29$ 16] Figure 1. Subjectively measured Black participants’ skin tone moderates the effect of subjectively measured participant facial features on their tendency to attribute ambiguous negative events to skin tone. 4.5 - —9—— Darker skin tone (-1 SD) 0 4‘0 ‘ ------I Lighter skin tone (+ISD) 5 3.5 . [- .5 3.0 l m 8 2.5 '1 5 g 2.0 . :9. E 1.5 4 < 1.0 . 0.5 . 0.0 Less prototypical More prototypical facial features facial features (-ISD) (+ISD) 162 Figure 2. Objectively measured Black participants’ skin tone moderates the effect of objectively measured participant facial features on their tendency to attribute ambiguous negative events to facial features. 3.5 - 3 3.0 r I- B 3 2.5 4 tr. 3 g 2.0 ~ tr. e 2 1.5 i e '5 :3 1-0 ‘ +Darker skin tone (-ISD) g 0.5 4 ------I Lighter skin tone (+ISD) 0.0 . Less prototypical More prototypical facial features facial features (-ISD) (+ISD) 163 Figure 3. Subjectively measured Black participants’ skin tone moderates the effect of subjectively measured participant facial features on their tendency to attribute ambiguous negative events to internal factors. Attribution to Internal Factors 4.5 ‘ 4.0‘ 3.5 * 3.04 2.5 i 2.0 ‘ 1.5 ‘ 1.0 * 0.5 ‘ —o— Darker skin tone (-1 SD) ---I- - - Lighter skin tone (+ISD) 0.0 Less prototypical More prototypical facial features facial features (-1 SD) (+1 30) 164 Figure 4. Subjectively measured Black participants’ skin tone moderates the effect of subjectively measured participant facial features on their experiences with racial discrimination in the past 1 year. a 2.5l e '5 a g 2.0‘ 8 E 1.5‘ .2 C) a a: 5 1.0 ‘ 3 3 —O— Darker skin tone (-1 SD) {—3 0-5 ---I---Lighterskintone(+ISD) 8. 31 “J 0.0 Less More prototypical prototypical facial features facial features 165 Figure 5. Objective] y measured Black participants’ skin tone moderates the effect of objectively measured participant facial features on their sensitivity to racism. 18.0] g 16.01 '5 14.0 4 :1 ‘2 12 0 . a . .o g 10.0 < 52 8.0 ~ '8 g 6.0 * . m —6— Darker skin tone (-1 SD) é 4'0 1 ---I--- Lighter skin tone (+ISD) 9‘ 2.0 ~ 0.0 Less More prototypical prototypical facial features facial features 166 Figure 6. Subjectively measured Black participants’ skin tone moderates the effect of subjectively measured participant facial features on their sensitivity to racism. 18.0” 3:16.04 IE .2: 14.0 . m :i a“; 12.0« a {-3- 10.0« 8 '3‘ 8.0 . a: B 6.0 ~ g 4 0 —+— Darker skin tone (-1 SD) 3 ' -- -I- - - Lighter skin tone (+ISD) a 2.0 * 0.0 Less More prototypical prototypical facial features facial features 167 Appendix A: Objective measures of facial features Figure 1. Objective measurements of face width, face length, nose width, and lip thickness. 168 Appendix B: An example of outlined pictures Figure 11. An example of outlined pictures. .m" Mm“?- ‘1' r!" , » if .0" - 1’ I . .' l a. .9. . f“ x -. ~45?‘ '5" ‘.y 3 x. ‘ 3; W' “a ‘W'tf-itaar' 2": . e r,» ,. J’". ' .H" . ‘ ,.-.r o—‘J' " ‘ ‘fi‘x‘, 169 Appendix C: The final 64 Black male target pictures Figure 111. Black male target pictures used in the main studies. Setl: Dark Skin with High Prototypical Facial Features 170 Figure 111(cont’d). Set 2: Dark Skin with High Prototypical Facial Features 171 Appendix D: The final 8 Black female target pictures Figure IV. Black female target pictures used in the main studies. Dark Skin with High Prototypical Facial Features Set 1 Set 2 Dark Skin with Low Prototypical Facial Features Set 1 Set 2 Light Skin with High Prototypical Facial Features Set 1 Set 2 Light Skin with Low Prototypical Facial Features Set 1 Set 2 172 Appendix E: A design of phase 4 in the sequential priming task Eight sets of matched five faces 1 __Block 1 $0;le M2 M91 DM1+Good1 DM1+Good2 DM1+Bad1 DM1+Bad2 LMl + Good] LMl + Good2 LMl + Badl LMl + Bad2 DLl + Good] DLl + Good2 DLl + Badl DLl + Bad2 ‘i\ LL1 + Good] LL1 + Good2 LL1 + Badl LL1 + Bad2 g? W1 + Good] W1 + Good2 W1 + Badl W1 + Bad2 g. DMZ + Good2 DMZ + Good3 DMZ + Bad2 DMZ + Bad3 ‘6 LM2 + Good2 LM2 + Good3 LM2 + Bad2 LM2 + Bad3 g DL2 + Good2 DL2 + Good3 DL2 + Bad2 DL2 + Bad3 5° LL2 + Good2 LL2 + Good3 LL2 + Bad2 LL2 + Bad3 91 W2 + Good2 W2 + Good3 W2 + gag W2 + m g. DM3 + Good3 DM3 + Good4 DM3 + Bad3 DM3 + Bad4 8. LM3 + Good3 LM3 + Good4 LM3 + Bad3 LM3 + Bad4 37 DL3 + Good3 DL3 + Good4 DL3 + Bad3 DL3 + Bad4 G LL3 + Good3 LL3 + Good4 LL3 + Bad3 LL3 + Bad4 9",” W3 + Good3 W3 + Good4 W3 + Bad3 W3 + Bad4 8 DM4 + Good4 DM4 + Good5 DM4 + Bad4 DM4 + Bad5 g, LM4 + Good4 LM4 + Good5 LM4 + Bad4 LM4 + Bad5 3 DL4 + Good4 DL4 + Good5 DL4 + Bad4 DL4 + Bad5 0 LL4 + Good4 LL4 + Good5 LL4 + Bad4 LL4 + Bad5 é." W4 + Good4 W4 + Good5 W4 + Bad4 W4 + Bag g DMS + Bad5 DMS + Bad6 DMS + Good5 DMS + G00d6 & LM5 + Bad5 LM5 + Bad6 LM5 + Good5 LM5 + Good6 c, DL5 + Bad5 DLS + Bad6 DLS + Good5 DL5 + G00d6 ‘< LLS + Bad5 LL5 + Bad6 LLS + Good5 LLS + G00d6 § W5 + Bad5 W5 + Bad6 W5 + Good5 W5 + Good6 g DM6 + Bad 6 DM6 + Bad7 DM6 + Good6 DM6 + Good7 a LM6 + Bad6 LM6 + Bad7 LM6 + GOOd6 LM6 + Good7 g DL6 + Bad6 DL6 + Bad7 DL6 + Good6 DL6 + Good7 g LL6 + Bad6 LL6 + Bad7 LL6 + G00d6 LL6 + Good7 0» W6 + Bad6 W6 + Bad7 W6 + GOOd6 W6 + Good7 § DM7 + Bad7 DM7 + Bad8 DM7 + Good7 DM7 + Good8 g LM7 + Bad7 LM7 + Bad8 LM7 + Good7 LM7 + Good8 .5; DL7 + Bad7 DL7 + Bad8 DL7 + Good7 DL7 + Good8 a LL7 + Bad7 LL7 + Bad8 LL7 + G00d7 LL7 + Good8 5" W7 + Bad7 W7 + Bad8 W7 + Good7 W7 + Good8 E. DM8 + Bad8 DM8 + Badl DM8 + Good8 DM8 + Goodl a LM8 + Bad8 LM8 + Badl LM8 + Good8 LM8 + Goodl g DL8 + Bad8 DL8 + Badl DL8 + Good8 DL8 + Good] ° LL8 + Bad8 LL8 + Badl LL8 + Good8 LL8 + Good] W8 + Bad8 W8 + Badl W8 + Good8 W8 + Good] Note. DM indicates “Dark-skinned More prototypical facial features,” LM indicates “Light- skinned More prototypical facial features,” DL indicates “Dark-skinned Less prototypical facial features,” LL indicates “Light-skinned Less prototypical facial features,” and W in indicates “White control.” 173 Appendix F: Selection of the target words for the lexical decision task A list of 36 target words (12 negative words associated with Black male stereotypes, six negative words associated with Black female stereotypes, 12 negative words not associated with Black male stereotypes, and six negative words not associated with Black female stereotypes) was created for the lexical decision task. It was important to ensure that the 18 stereotypic words and 18 non-stereotypic words were matched in terms negativity, because a control prime condition (which could serve as a measure of baseline reaction times) was not included. In addition, 36 non-words were matched with each of the 36 real words in terms of the number of letters were created. Sixty-eight White participants were first asked to rate 55 adjectives in terms of negativity. More specifically, participants were asked to rate each of the 55 words on a scale ranging from 1 (Not at All Negative) to 7 (Extremely Negative). Next, participants were asked to rate the same 55 words in terms of stereotypicality of Blacks. Of 68 participants, 35 participants were asked to indicate Whether an adjective was stereotypic of Black men, and the other 33 participants were asked to rate Whether an adjective was stereotypic of Black women, using a scale ranging from 1 (Not at All Stereotypic of African American Men/W omen) to 7 (Extremely Stereotypic of African American Men/W omen). Importantly, participants were instructed to make their judgments based on stereotypes that are held by other people in general, but not necessarily based on their personal beliefs. Based on the negativity and stereotypicality ratings, 36 target words (12 negative words associated with Black male stereotypes, six negative words associated with Black female stereotypes, 12 negative words not associated with Black male 174 stereotypes, and six negative words not associated with Black female stereotypes) were selected (see the list below). For Black Male Targets Stereotypic Non-Words Non-Stereotypic Non-Words CREEPY TWINDE ANNOYING WROURSTE DANGEROUS PHAUGHNED ARROGANT WHARPHTE IGNORANT WROOSSED BORING KREIND INCOMPETENT SCKWAUGHLDE CONDESCENDING SCHOUGHCKINGS LAZY UPC E GREEDY MYSSED MEAN LAWD IRRITATING PHLEIGHLDE NOISY GEAZE JUDGMENTAL SQUOUGHLED POOR OADE SELFISH FEIGHLD RECKLESS BREIGNED SMELLY PAISTE THREATENING SC KWOUGHLED STUCKUP SCKWEAZ UNTRUSTWORTHY SC KREETHECHED UGLY SUKT VIOLENT FAUSSED WEAK CECT For Black Female Targets Stereotypic Non-Words Non-Stereotypic Non-Words ANNOYING WROURSTE EXPLOITATIVE CHEIGHNDGING IRRITATING PHLEIGHLDE SELF ISH F EIGHLD NOISY GEAZE SHELTERED SCKWOARLD NOSEY VAMPT SLOPPY PHIGHZ RUDE C EXT SLOW HEZE STUBBORN KOURGNED STUCKUP SCKWEAZ In order to examine whether the 12 words selected as stereotypic of Black men were significantly different from the 12 words selected as non-stereotypic of Black men in terms of stereotypicality of Black men, a paired-samples t-test was conducted on stereotypicality ratings. The result showed that two sets of words were significantly different from one another, t(34) = 10.21, p < .001, d = 1.68. Next, a paired-sample t-test was conducted to examine whether these two sets of words were similar in terms negativity. The test revealed that there was no mean difference in the degree of negativity between the two sets, t(67) = .02, p = .99. The same tests were conducted on words for Black female targets. A paired- sarnples t-test showed that six words selected as stereotypic of Black women were perceived to be significantly more stereotypic of Black women than six words selected as non-stereotypic of Black women, t(32) = 8.96, p < .001, d = 1.21. In addition, a paired- samples t-test on negativity ratings showed that these two sets of words did not differ in negativity, t(67) = .60, p = .55. In sum, 18 words selected as stereotypic of Blacks and 18 words selected as non-stereotypic of Blacks only differ in stereotypicality, but not in negativity, indicating that the selection of 36 words was successful. The means and standard deviations for each rating are presented in the table below. 176 Table I Means dflerences in stereotypicality and negativity ratings between stereotypic words and non-stereotypic words within each gender Black Male Targets Black Female Targets . Non- . Non- $3203: 1c Stereotypic Staegtgprc Stereotypic Word Words Stereotypicality M 4.86 2.82 4.79 3.32 SD 1.46 .90 1.40 1.00 Negativity M 5.19 5.19 4.80 4.76 SD .78 .77 .74 .76 177 Appendix G: A list of pre-Iaboratory questionnaires A. Group Identification 1. Overall, my racial group membership has very little to do with how I feel about myself. 2. The racial group I belong to is an important reflection of who I am. 3. The racial group I belong to is unimportant to my sense of what kind of person I am. 4. In general, belonging to my racial group is an important part of my self-image. B. Self-Esteem 1. I feel that I am a person of worth, at least on an equal basis with others. 2. I feel that I have a number of good qualities. 3. All in all, I am inclined to think I am a failure. 4. I am able to do things as well as most people. 5. I feel that I do not have much to be proud of. 6. I take a positive attitude toward myself. \I . On the whole, I am satisfied with myself. 00 . I wish I could have more respect for myself. \0 . I certainly feel useless at times. 10. At times, I think I am no good at all. 1 l. I am worried about Whether I am regarded as a success or a failure. 12. I feel self-conscious. 13. I feel displeased with myself. 14. I am worried about what other people think of me. 178 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. I am worried about looking foolish. I feel inferior to others at this moment. I feel concerned about the impression I am making. I feel confident about my abilities. I feel frustrated or rattled by my performance. I feel that I am having trouble understanding things I read. I feel as smart as others. I feel like I am not doing well. I feel confident that I understand things. I feel I have less scholastic ability right now than others. I feel satisfied with the way my body looks right now. I feel that other respect and admire me. I am dissatisfied with my weight. I feel good about myself. I am pleased with my appearance right now. I feel unattractive. C. Past Experience with Discrimination 1. How many times have you been treated unfairly by teachers and professors because you are Black? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 179 2. How many times have you been treated unfairly by your employers, bosses, and supervisors because you are Black? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 3. How many times have you been treated unfairly by your coworkers, fellow students, and colleagues because you are Black? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 4. How many times have you been treated unfairly by people in service jobs (store clerks, waiters, bartenders, bank tellers and others) because you are Black? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 5. How many times have you been treated unfairly by strangers because you are Black? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 6. How many times have you been treated unfairly by people in helping jobs (doctors, nurses, psychiatrists, case workers, dentists, school counselors, therapists, social workers and others) because you are Black? 180 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 7. How many times have you been treated unfairly by neighbors because you are Black? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 8. How many times have you been treated unfairly by institutions (schools, universities, law firms, the police, the courts, the Department of Social Services, the Unemployment Office and others) because you are Black? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 9. How many times have you been treated unfairly by people that you thought were your friends because you are Black? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 10. How many times have you been accused or suspected of doing something wrong (such as stealing, cheating, not doing your share of the work, or breaking the law) because you are Black? 1) How many times in the past year? 181 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 1 I. How many times have people misunderstood your intentions and motives because you are Black? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 12. How many times did you want to tell someone off for being racist but didn 't say anything? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 13. How many times have you been really angry about something racist that was done to you? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 14. How many times were you forced to take drastic steps (such as filing a grievance, filing a lawsuit, quitting your job, moving away, and other actions) to deal with some racist thing that was done to you? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressfiil was this for you? 182 15. How many times have you been called a racist name like n__, coon, jungle bunny or other names? I) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 16. How many times have you gotten into an argument or a fight about something racist that was done to you or done to somebody else? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 17. How many times have you been made fun ofi picked on, pushed, shoved, hit, or threatened with harm because you are Black? 1) How many times in the past year? 2) How many times in your entire life? 3) How stressful was this for you? 18. How diflerent would your life be now if you HAD NOT BEEN treated in a racist and unfair way 1) In the past year? 2) In your entire life? D. Perception of Discrimination 1. I experience discrimination because of my race. 2. I feel that I am discriminated against because of my race. 3. I feel like I am personally a victim of society because of my race. 183 4. I consider myself a person who is deprived of opportunities that available to others because of my race. 5. I personally have been a victim of racial discrimination. E. Demographic Information 1. Age 2. Gender (Male, Female) 3. Race 4. Year in school (Freshman, Sophomore, Junior, Senior) 5. Number of same-race close friends participants have on the MSU campus 6. Number of different-race close friends participants have on the MSU campus 7. Number of same-race close fiiends participants have outside the MSU campus 8. Number of different-race close friends participants have outside the MSU campus 9. Family’s socioeconomic status (Extremely Poor, Poor, Average, Wealthy, Extremely Wealthy) 10. Current overall GPA 184 Appendix H: Selection of attribution items and scenarios for the attribution measure in Study 3 The attribution measure created for Study 3 originally consisted of 10 different scenarios and 10 different attribution items for each scenario (see the list of scenarios at the end of this appendix). Because this measure required participants to provide 100 responses, there were potential issues with participants’ fatigue or boredom. In order to test whether the measure could be shortened, internal consistency Within 10 scenarios as well as within 10 attribution items were computed, based on the responses provided by non-Black ethnic minorities, for looking for redundancy within scenarios and items. F ifiy-seven non-Black ethnic minority participants (Asian American = 54.4 %, Latino American = 29.8 %, Native American = 3.5 %, Multiracial/Other = 1.8 %, and International Student = 10.5 %; women = 52.6 %; M age = 20.04, SD = 2.05 years old) were recruited for Pilot Study 3. Ideally, participants in Pilot Study 3 would be Blacks. However, those Black individuals participated in Pilot Study 3 would not be eligible to take part in Study 3. Due to the limited number of available Black participants in the subject pool, I decided not to recruit Black individuals for the pilot study and maximize the number of Black participants for the main study. Participants were asked to read 10 scenarios (most of which were obtained from Branscombe et al.’s study (1999) that examined the perception of discrimination among Black individuals). Each scenario described a negative experience in different situations that could be due to racial discrimination or to other causes. Participants were asked to imagine that they have experienced these ambiguous negative events and then speculate why these negative events have happened to them. 185 More specifically, participants were provided with 10 attribution categories: 1) internal non-discrimination attributions (e.g., their personality, mood, abilities, or intelligence), 2) external nondiscriminatory attributions (e. g., interaction partners’ personality, mood, abilities, or intelligence), 3) situational attributions (e.g., social norm, lack of opportunity, rule, or regulation), 4) internal racial discrimination attributions (e.g., their racial group membership), 5) external racial discrimination attributions (e. g., interaction partners’ prejudiced attitudes toward their racial group), 6) internal gender discrimination attributions (e.g., their gender group membership), 7) external gender discrimination attributions (e. g., interaction partners’ prejudiced attitudes toward their gender group), 8) skin tone attribution (e. g., their skin tone), 9) facial feature attribution (e. g., their facial appearance), and 10) other (participants were asked to specify the attribution). Some of these attribution categories were derived from King’s study (2005). For each attribution item, participants were asked to rate their perception of the cause of the experience, using a scale ranging from 1 (Very Unlikely the Cause of the Experience) to 7 (Very Likely the Cause of the Experience). Means and standard deviations for each of the nine attribution items within each scenario are presented in Table II at the end of this appendix. First, internal consistency among the 9 attribution items Within each scenario was examined. As shown in Table III at the end of this appendix, as were high for all scenarios, indicating that those who attributed ambiguous negative events to one cause were also likely to attribute the same events to other causes (e. g., participants who attributed their failure to get a job to their racial membership also attributed the failure to their personal characteristics that are unrelated to racial membership). 186 Because internal consistency was high within each scenario, several attribution items were eliminated to shorten the measure, so that participants would be less susceptible to fatigue and boredom. More specifically, the external racism and external sexism attribution items (i.e., due to my partner's racism/sexism attitudes) were eliminated, because these items were highly correlated with the internal racism and internal sexism attribution items (i.e., due to my racial/ gender membership; see Tables IV for correlations across the 10 scenarios), suggesting that participants did not differentially responded to the internal vs. external discrimination attribution items. In addition, the partner's personality and situation attribution items were combined into a single "external" attribution item because both are not under one’s own control. Finally, “other” attribution, which participants need to specify, was drop from the list because only a few participants actually came up with “other” potential causes. This brought the number of attribution items for each scenario from ten to six. Next, internal consistency within each attribution category across the 10 scenario was computed. As shown in Tables V at the end of this appendix, as were high across all attribution categories, indicating that those who attributed ambiguous negative events to one cause in one scenario were also likely to attribute negative events to the same cause in different scenarios (e. g., participants who attributed their failure to get a job to their racial membership also attributed their negative experience at a fancy restaurant to their racial membership). Again, in order to prevent potential fatigue, four scenarios with the lowest item-total correlation values based on the internal racial discrimination item were eliminated from the original list of 10 items (i.e., scenarios 5, 8 ,9, and 10), resulting in the total of six scenarios. Taken together, the attribution measure for Study 3 consisted of 187 six different scenarios that described ambiguous negative events, and each scenario could be attributed to six different causes. I88 The original list of ambiguous negative scenarios for the attribution task 1. Imagine that you went to a "fancy" restaurant. Your server seemed to be taking care of all the other customers except you. You were the last person whose order was taken. 2. Imagine that you applied for a job that you believed you were qualified for. After the interview you learned that you didn't get the job. 3. Imagined that you parked your car at a parking meter and it has just expired. You arrived back at the car just as an officer was writing up a ticket. You tried to persuade the officer not to give you the ticket, after all you were there and the meter just expired. The officer gave you the ticket anyway. 4. Imagine that you went to look at an apartment for rent. The manager of the building refused to show it to you, saying that it has already been rented. 5. Imagine that you were attracted to a particular White man/woman and asked that person out for a date and was turned down. 6. Imagine that you had to fill out some government forms in order to apply for a loan that was important to you. You went to one office and they send you to another, then you went there and were sent somewhere else. No one seemed to be really willing to help you out. 7. Imagine that you were driving a few miles over the speed limit and the police pull you over. You received a ticket for the maximum amount allowable. 8. Imagine that you wanted to join a social organization. You were told that they are not taking any new members at this time. 9. Imagine that your boss told you that you were not performing your job as well as others doing that job. 189 10. Imagine that you went to Chicago a few weeks ago. You tried to catch a cab but several empty cabs passed by you, even thought you were pretty sure that the drivers saw you. 190 191 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: so: 5.: 2w: 5.: 5.: 3m: in com 45 Em 25m 25 Ba 3m 3m 43 2 833a 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 8w: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: Se 2% cam 43“ No.4 a; 63 m3 :1“ «mm m: E $qu 650 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 3.: men m3 35 5a 23 SN 63 85 SN MS 2 Seam =2:me 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: . 5.: 5.: 5.: 3,: 3% mod 85 com 55 55. Ba 93 43 m2 3 828m 383% 5.: 84.: 5.: AS: 5.: $5.: 36.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 3: 9% w a .N 35 35 NE 3m 35 N5 oz 85 E mass; 368 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 3.: 3m 82 o: 63. Sm Sm £5. 52 mam SM 3. 685 5m 86.: 5.: 6m: 2w: 5.: 5.: so: 5.: 5.: 5.: 3.: mom 3m Rm 6? 8a 33. :3 £5 8.4 :3. E 3825 see 5. : 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 5.: 63 com 2: tum SN w _ .N 85 E .85 new E 53% 3835 :5. : 5.: 5. : so: so. : 5.: 5.: em: 26.: 5. : 5: 5.4 SN mom M3. 3m 91.. 3m 23 43 Rm 3 828m. 3895 2 o w 5 e m 4 m N _ ogcoow cousnwfixx 3.238% : N mee «one agent»: 2223.22: 2:: mSXQ 92333: $3382 :38 gem: = 2an av. mm. 3. no. on. on. mm. mm. 3. mm. :2852m am. 3. mm. on. 0:. mm. on. we. mm. 8. 858m 850 3. mm. 3. mm. on. 3. :0. mm. 3.. mm. Emmxom 8:835 No. 5. 2.. .w. ow. R. mm. K. mm. mo. E203“ 8:82..“— Su. mm. :u 8. cm. mm. ov. No. 0:. Q. 858$ 868: on. we. 3.. E. 2.. mm. ow. E. 5. mo. 0:8. :3m am. am. mm. 3. 5:. am. vm. am. mm. 58. 852:: 850 3. Eu ow. on. we. v.8: on. 3.. Vm. G. Eflxom 8E8:— oo. mm. cm. on. S. w». on. mm. mm. 0:. 8203: 850:: 228.230 30%-:8: 38880 $38. $38. 8:8. 8:8. $38. $38. $38. 8:8. 8:8. :33. 2:388:29 S o m N. o w v m N 2 2.8:oom 2.8:on 28:8m 2.8:on 2.8:8m 38:03 2.8:on 2.8:on 2.8:8m 2.8:on 3.2::an : N mfi\o :93 53.3» 9.26: 22392:: :5: 2: 86.8: 83:33:: 3525 E 038:. 192 Ahm: Ahm: Anm: Ahm: Ann: Aom: Aom: Aom: Aom: Aom: ¢<: 3.9V. **mw. 31.0. 1.00. 1&0. *hN. 3&0. .13.. “Iain 1.0m. acts—0800 :Ombwom 82me Ahm: Anm: 28m: Amm: Ann: 28m: 25m: Amm: Amm: Anm: o<: sine. .1.on “.350. 1&9 "1.3.. from. “numb. 3.3.. 1.0m. .Ibm. :OEEPCOU :Ombwom E203: o_ o w b o n v m N _ Ogaoom OCNCOOW OCNGOOW OCMCDOW Ogcoom Oran—Dom OEGDOW Ofifioom Gina—Dom OEGOOW ”0.262990 QN m<~ thkUG 2.29% 20339226 :31wa“. \BgsquN 3:3 NGEWEK» 323 Shrugs NGQKNNRN 323 N32k®u2m QNNENQ :OCQNWKKOU >~ 2:8... 193 NV. 2.. G. Vm. me. mm. mm. o0. no. 2 2.8:on on. S. we. mo. 3. mo. wv. mm. mm. m ogcoom ac. mm. 3. No. ac. mm. om. oo. on. w otwcoom on. on. mm. on. me. w». 6. mm. on. n 082.com we. on. 3. ac. mm. 3. mm. 3. me. o ogcoom S. K. co. on. 2‘. 2.. No. ow. co. m 28:on mm. E. on. 2.. 3. mm. 6. cm. mm. v otmcoom 2.. on. mm. on. 3. R. 8. on. E. m oficoom oo. on. me. we. K. mu. 3.. mm. S. m 2355 3. 3. mm. B. E. on. .3. E. K. _ oracoom coca—8.50 309-88— cowootou 68%. $33. £33. £33. 89%. 838. 95$. :33. $33. 33.5885 coca—Em 558m Emmxom Emmomm $580... 25,—. E 358.: Emiom Emfimm . . 550 :25me $5me :28; .xm 550 3585 EEBE 2:»: 20.23.53 E 233 fig 2.233 8.23289. Q N 2: $6.33 AutmEmzou 358$ > 033. 194 Appendix I: Race-Based Rejection SenSitivity 1. Imagine that you are in class one day, and the professor asks a particularly difficult question. A few people, including yourself, raise their hands to answer the question. 1) How concerned/anxious would you be that the professor might not choose you because of your race/ethnicity? 2) I would expect that the professor might not choose me because of my race/ethnicity. 2. Imagine that you are in a pharmacy, trying to pick out a few items. While you’re looking at the different brands, you notice one of the store clerks glancing your way. 1) How concerned lanxious would you be that the clerk might be looking at you because of your race/ethnicity? 2) I would expect that the clerk might continue to look at me because of my race/ ethnicity. 3. Imagine you have just completed a job interview over the telephone. You are in good spirits because the interviewer seemed enthusiastic about your application. Several days later you complete a second interview in person. Your interviewer informs you that they will let you know about their decision soon. 1) How concerned/anxious would you be that you might not be hired because of your race/ ethnicity? 2) I would expect that I might not be hired because of my race/ethnicity. 4. It’s late at night and you are driving down a country road you’re not familiar with. Luckily, there is a 24-hour 7-11 just ahead, so you stop there and head up to the counter to ask the young woman for directions. 195 1) How concerned/anxious would you be that she might not help you because of your race/ethnicity? 2) I would expect that the woman might not help me because of my race/ethnicity. 5. Imagine that a new school counselor is selecting students for a summer scholarship fund that you really want. The counselor has only one scholarship lefi and you are one of several students that is eligible for this scholarship. I) How concerned/anxious would you be that the counselor might not choose you because of your race/ethnicity? 2) I would expect that he might not select me because of my race/ethnicity. 6. Imagine you have just finished shopping, and you are leaving the store carrying several bags. It’s closing time, and several people are filing out of the store at once. Suddenly, the alarm begins to sound, and a security guard comes over to investigate. 1) How concerned/anxious would you be that the guard might stop you because of your race/ethnicity? 2) I would expect that the guard might stop me because of my race/ethnicity. 7. Imagine you are riding the bus one day. The bus is full except for two seats, one of which is next to you. As the bus comes to the next stop, you notice a woman getting on the bus. 1) How concemed/anxious would you be that she might avoid sitting next to you because of your race/ethnicity? 2) I would expect that she might not sit next to me because of my race/ethnicity 8. Imagine that you are in a restaurant, trying to get the attention of your waitress. A lot of other people are trying to get her attention as well. 196 1) How concerned/anxious would you be that she might not attend you right away because of your race/ethnicity? 2) I would expect that she might not attend to me right away because of my race/ ethnicity 9. Imagine you’re driving down the street, and there is a police barricade just ahead. The police officers are randomly pulling people over to check drivers’ licenses and registrations. 1) How concemed/anxious would you be that an officer might pull you over because of your race/ethnicity? 2) I would expect that the officers might stop me because of my race/ethnicity. 10. Imagine that it’s the second day of your new class. The teacher assigned a writing sample yesterday and today the teacher announces that she has finished correcting the papers. You wait for your paper to be returned. 1) How concerned/anxious would you be that you might receive a lower grade than others because of your race/ethnicity? 2) I would expect to receive a lower grade than others because of my race/ ethnicity. 11. Imagine that you are standing in line for the ATM machine, and you notice the woman at the machine glances back while she’s getting her money. I) How concemed/anxious would you be that she might be suspicious of you because of your race/ethnicity? 2) I would expect that she might be suspicious of me because of my race/ethnicity. 197 12. Imagine you’re at a pay phone on a street corner. You have to make a call, but you don’t have change. You decide to go into a store and ask for change for your bill. I) How concerned/anxious would you be that the cashier might not give you change because of your race/ethnicity? 2) I would expect that the cashier might not give me change because of my race/ethnicity. 198 .muofioo 885%: No 2: macaw €28 “macaw: 2: mo 28 go 228 “macaw 2: go 28 £ 830 women“ 2: 882?» made“ on 53 50> hO-OU £28 858%“ I No 32: 9. 860 Homem— uwmh NP 2: 8388 EB 50% lllllllllllnllll mam-ulmllauuluunul EUIDUIuIDIDDIDm [Dun-all .30—co aim owmbiw mo 8388a .x. 33m: who—8 aim omega“ mo moEmem a. vacuum“? | 199 REFERENCES Anderson, C., & Cromwell, R. L. (1977). “Black is beautiful" and the color preferences of Afro-American youth. Journal of Negro Education, 46, 76-88. Ashbum-Nardo, L., Volis, C. 1., & Monteith, M. J. (2001). Implicit associations as the seeds of intergroup bias: How easily do they take root? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 789-799. Averhart, C. J ., & Bigler, R. S. (1997). Shades of meaning: Skin tone, racial attitudes, and constructive memory in African American children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67, 363-388. Banerj i, S. (2006). Darker-skinned Black Job Applicants Face More Obstacles. Diverse Issues in Higher Education, 23, 20. Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In R. S., Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds), Handbook of social cognition (2“d ed., pp. 140). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbau. Blackwell, J. E., (1985). The Black Community: Diversity and Unity. New York: Dodd, Mead. Blair, 1. V. (2002). The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 242-261. Blair, 1. V. (2006). The efficient use of race and Afrocentric features in inverted faces. Social Cognition, 24, 563-579. Blair, 1. V., Chapleau, K. M., Judd, C. M. (2005). The use of Afrocentric features as cues for judgment in the presence of diagnostic information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 59-68. Blair, 1. V., Judd, C. M., & Chapleau, K. M. (2004a). The influence of Afrocentric facial features in criminal sentencing. Psychological Science, 15, 674-679. Blair, 1. V., Judd, C. M., & Fallman, J. L. (2004b). The automaticity of race and Afrocentric facial features in social judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 763-778. Blair, 1. V., Judd, C. M., Sadler, M. S., & Jenkins, C. (2002). The role of Afrocentric features in person perception: Judging by features and categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 5-25. 200 Bond, S., & Cash, T. F. (1992). Black beauty: Skin color and body images among African American college women. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 874-888. Bornstein, R. F. (1993). Mere exposure effects with outgroup stimuli. In D. M. Mackie & D. L. Hamilton (Eds) Aflect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 195-211). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Boyle, E. (1970). Biological patterns in hypertension by race, sex, body weight, and skin color. Journal of the American Medical Association, 213, 1637-1643. Bradbum, N. H. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Oxford, England: Aldine. Branscombe, N. R., Schmitt, M. T., & Harvey, R. D. (1999). Perceiving pervasive discrimination among African Americans: Implications for group identification and well- being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 135-149. Brewer, M. B., (1979). In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: A cognitive- motivational analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 307-324. Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In T. Srull & R.Wyer (Eds.). Advances in Social Cognition. Vol. 1, Earlbaum. Brewer, M. B. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love or outgroup hate? Journal of Social Issues, 55, 429-444. Brown, T. D., Dane, F. C., & Durham, M. D. (1998). Perception of race and ethnicity. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 13, 295-306. Brown, E., & Perrett, D. I. (1993). What gives a face its gender? Perception, 22, 829-840. Buchanan, N. T., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2008). Effects of racial and sexual harassment on work and the psychological well-being of African American women. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 13, 137-151. Buchanan, N. T., & Ormerod, A. J. (2002). Racialized sexual harassment in the lives of Afiican American women. Women & T heory, 25, 107-124. Burton, A. M., Bruce, V., & Dench, N. (1993). What’s the difference between men and women? Evidence from facial measurement. Perception, 22, 153-176. Cloutier, J ., Mason, M. F., & Macrae, C. N. (2005). The perceptual determinants of person construal: Reopening the social-cognitive toolbox. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 885-894. 201 Crandall, C.S., Eshleman, A., & O’Brien, L. (2002). Social Norms and the Expression and Suppression of Prejudice: The Struggle for Intemalization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 359—378. Coard, S. I., Breland, A. M., & Raskin, P. (2001). Perceptions of and preferences for skin color, Black racial identity, and self-esteem among African Americans. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 2256-2274. Davis, K. B., Daniels, M., & See, L. A. (1998). The psychological effects of skin color on African Americans’ self-esteem. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 1, 63-90. DeBruine, L. (2002). Facial resemblance enhances trust. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 269, 1307-1312. Deregowski, J. B., Ellis, H. D., & Shepherd, J. W. (1975). Descriptions of White and Black faces by White and Black subjects. International Journal of Psychology, 10, 119- 123. Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5-18. Diamond, R., & Carey, S. (1986). Why faces are and are not special: An effect of expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 107-117. Diener, E., & Emmons, R. A., (1984). The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4 7, 1105-1 117. Dixon, T. L., & Maddox, K. B. (2005). Skin tone, crime news, and social reality judgments: Priming the stereotype of the dark and dangerous Black criminal. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 1555-1570. Dowden, S., & Robinson, J. P. (1993). Age and cohort differences in American racial attitudes: The generational replacement hypothesis revisited. In P. M. Sniderman, P. E. Tetlock, & E. G. Carmines (Eds.), Prejudice, politics, and the American dilemma (pp. 86—103). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Downey, G., & Feldman, S. (1996). Implications of rejection sensitivity for intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1327—1343. Dressler, W. W. (1991). Social class, skin color, and arterial blood pressure in two societies. Ethnicity & Disease, 1, 60-77. Eberhardt, J. L., Davies, P. G., Purdie-Vaughns, V. J ., & Johnson, S. L. (2006). Looking deathworthy: Perceived stereotypicality of Black defendants predicts capital-sentencing outcomes. Psychological Science, I 7, 383-386. 202 Eberhardt, J. L., Goff, P. A., Purdie, V. J ., & Davies, P. G. (2004). Seeing Black: Race, crime, and visual processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8 7, 876-893. Fazio, R. 11., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychologv, 69, 1013-1027. Fiske, S. T., Lin, M. H., & Neuberg, S. L. (1999). The Continuum Model: Ten years later. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds) Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 231-254). New York: Guilford. Fleming, J. (1983). Black women in Black and White college environments: The making of a matriarch. Journal of Social Issues, 39, 41 -54. Frazier, E. F. (1966). The Negro Family in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The aversive form of racism. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 61-89). New York: Academic Press. Gallagher, 8., Tait, T. J ., McCologan, B., Dovey, T. M., & Halford, J. C. G. (2003). Negative judgments about male associates of obese females. Obesity Research, 11, Al 18- A119. Goffinan, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Goldstein, S. B., & Johnson, V. A. (1997). Stigma by association: Perceptions of the dating partners of college students with physical disabilities. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 19, 495-504. Goldstein, M. D., & Strube, M. J. (1994). Independence revisited: The relation between positive and negative affect in a naturalistic setting. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 57-64. Goshen-Gottstein, Y., & Ganel, T. (2000). Repetition priming for familiar and unfamiliar faces in a sex-judgment task: Evidence for a common route for the processing of sex and identity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1198-1214. Gossett, Thomas F. (1963). Race: The History of an Idea in America. Dallas: Southern Methodist University Press. Greenwald, A.G., & Banaji, MR. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4—27. 203 Haley, H., Sidanius, J ., Lowery, B., & Neil, M. (2004). The interactive nature of sex and race discrimination: A social dominance perspective. In G. Philogene (Ed.), Racial identity in context: The legacy of Kenneth B. Clark. Decade of behavior (pp. 149-160). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Hall, R. E. (1992). Bias among African-Americans regarding skin color: Implications for social work practice. Research on Social Work Practice, 2, 479-486. Hall, R. E. (2003). Skin color as post-colonial hierarchy: A global strategy for conflict resolution. The Journal of Psychology, 13 7, 41 -53. Hall, R. E. (2005). From the psychology of race to the issues of skin color for people of African descent. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 1958-1967. Hamm, N. H., Baum, M. R., & Nikels, K. W. (1975). Effects of race and exposure on judgments of interpersonal favorability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 14-24. Harburg, E. Gleibermann, L. Roeper, P., Schork, M. A., & Schull, W. J. (1978). Sking color, ethnicity, and blood pressure I: Detroit Blacks. American Journal of Public Health, 68, 1177-1183. Harvey, R. D., LaBeach, N., Pridgen, E., & Gocial, T. M. (2005). The intragroup stigmatization of skin tone among Black Americans. Journal of Black Psychology, 31, 237-253. Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a scale for measuring state self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 895-910. Hebl, M. R., & Mannix, L. M. (2003). The weight of obesity in evaluating others: A mere proximity effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 28-38. Hernandez, T. K. (2006). Bringing Clarity to Race Relations in Brazil. Diverse: Issues in Higher Education, 23, 85. Hill, M. E. (2000). Color differences in the socioeconomic status of Afi'ican American men: Results of a longitudinal study. Social Forces, 78, 1437-1460. Hill, M. E. (2002). Skin color and the perception of attractiveness among African Americans: Does gender make a difference? Social Psychology Quarterly, 65, 77-91. Hill, T., Lewicki, P., Czyzewska, M., & Schuller, G. (1990). The role of learned inferential encoding rules in the perception of faces: Effects of nonconscious self- perpetuation of a bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 350-371. 204 Hochschild, J. L. (2007). The Skin Color Paradox and the American Racial Order. Social Forces, 86, 643-670. Hughes, M., & Hertel, B. R. (1990). The significance of color remains: A study of life chances, mate selection, and ethnic consciousness among Black Americans. Social Forces, 68, 1 105-1120. Hunter, M. L. (2002). “If you’re light you’re alright”: Light skin color as social capital for women of color. Gender and Society, 16, 175-193. Jetten, J ., Branscombe, N.R., Schmitt, M.T., & Spears, R. (2001). Rebels with a cause: Group identification as a response to perceived discrimination from the mainstream. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1204-1213. Kahn, K. B., Davies, P. G., Eberhardt, J. L., & Correll, J. (2008, February). Not all Black men are treated equal: Stereotypicality and shooter bias [Poster]. Poster presented at the annual conference of Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Albuquerque, NM. Keith, V. M., & Herring, C. (1991). Skin tone and stratification in the Black community. The American Journal of Sociology, 97, 760-778. King, K. R. (2005). Why is discrimination stressful? The mediating role of cognitive appraisal. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 11, 202-212. Klonoff, E. A., & Landrine, H. (1999). Cross-validation of the Schedule of Racist Events. Journal of Black Psychology, 25, 231-254. Klonoff, E. A., & Landrine, H. (2000). Is skin color a marker for racial discrimination? Explaining the skin color-hypertension relationship. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 23, 329-338. Kunda, Z. (1999). Social Cognition: Making sense of people. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Landrine, H., & Klonoff, E. A. (1996). The Schedule of Racist Events: A measure of racial discrimination and a study of its negative physical and mental health consequences. Journal of Black Psychology, 22, 144-168. Landrine, H., Klonoff, E. A., Alcaraz, R., Scott, J ., & Wilkins, P. (1995). Multiple variables in discrimination. In B. Lott & D. Maluso (Eds), The social psychology of interpersonal discrimination (pp. 183-224). New York: NY: Guilford Press. Leder, H., & Bruce, V. (2000). When inverted faces are recognized: The role of configural information in face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 513-536. 205 Livingston, R. W., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). What are we really priming? Cue-based versus Category-based processing of facial stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 5-18. Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. (1992). A collective self-esteem scale: Self-evaluation of one’s social identity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 302-318. Macrae, C. N., & Martin, D. (2007). A boy primed sue: Feature-based processing and person construal. European Journal of Social Psychology, Special Issues: F amiliarity impacts person perception, 3 7, 793-805. Maddox, K. B. (2004). Perspectives on racial phenotypicality bias. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 383-401. Maddox, K. B., & Chase, S. G. (2004). Manipulating subcategory salience: Exploring the link between skin tone and social perception of Blacks. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 533-546. Maddox, K. B., & Gray, S. A. (2002). Cognitive representations of Black Americans: Reexploring the role of skin tone. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 250- 259. Major, B., & O’Brien, LT. (2005). The social psychology of stigma. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 393-421. Major, B., Quinton, W.J., & McCoy, SK. (2002). Antecedents and consequences of attributions to discrimination: Theoretical and empirical advances. The Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 251-330. Major, B., Quinton, W.J., & Schmader, T. (2003). Attributions to discrimination and self esteem: Impact of group identification and situational ambiguity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 220-231. Martin, D., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). A face with a cue: Exploring the inevitability of person categorization. European Journal of Social Psychology, Special Issues: Familiarity impacts person perception, 3 7, 806-816. Mason, M. F ., Cloutier, J ., & Macrae, C. N. (2006). On construing others: Category and stereotype activation from facial cues. Social Cognition, 24. 540-562. McLeod, J. D., & Owens, T. J. (2004). Psychological well-being in the early life course: variations by socioeconomic status, gender, and race/ethnicity. Social Psychology Quarterly, 67, 257-278. 206 Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2004). Why good guys wear White: Automatic inferences about stimulus valence based on brightness. Psychological Science, 15, 82-87. Mendoza-Demon, R., Downey, G., Purdie, V., Davis, A., & Pietrazk, J. (2002). Sensitivity to race-based rejection: Implications for Afiican-American students' college experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 896-918. Minear, M. & Park, D.C.(2004). A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 36, 630-633. https://pal.utdallasedu/facedb/request/login Montalvo, F. F. (2004). Surviving race: Skin color and the socialization and acculturation of Latinas. Journal of Ethnic & Cultural Diversity in Social Work, 13, 25-43. Monteith, M. J ., Sherman, J. W., & Devince, P. G. (1998). Suppression as a stereotype control strategy. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 63-82. Morales, M. C. (2008). The ethnic niche as an economic pathway for the dark skinned: Labor market incorporation of Latina/o workers. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 30, 280-298. Mukhtar, A. (2007). The crisis of identity in northern Sudan: The dilemma of a Black people with a White culture. In Carolyn Fluehr-Lobban and Kharyssa Rhodes (Eds), Race and Identity in the Nile Valley (pp. 213—24). Trenton, NJ: Red Sea Press. Nisbett, R.E., & Wilson, TD. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231—259. Neuberg, S. L., Smith, D. M., Hoffman, J. C., & Russel, F. J. (1994). When we observe stigmatized and “normal” individuals interacting: Stigma by association. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 196-209. Oliver, M. B., Jackson 11, R. L., Moses, N. N., & Dangerfield, C. L. (2004). The face of crime: Viewers’ memory of race-related facial features of individuals pictured in the news. Journal of Communication, 54, 88-104. Operario, D., & Fiske, ST. (2001). Ethnic identity moderates perceptions of prejudice: Judgments of personal versus group discrimination and subtle versus blatant bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 2 7, 550-561. Ostman, M., & Kjellin, L. (2002). Stigma by association: Psychological factors in relatives of people with mental illness. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, 494-498. 207 Otten, S., & Wentura, D. (1999). About the impact of automaticity in the minimal group paradigm: Evidence from affective priming tasks. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 1049- 1 071. Penny, H., & Haddock, G. (2007). Anti-fat prejudice among children: The “mere proximity” effect in 5-10 year olds. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 678- 683. Perdue, C. W., Dovidio, J. F ., Gurtman, M. B., & Tyler, R. B. (1990). Us and them: Social categorization and the process of intergroup bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 475-486. Peskin, M., & Newell, F. N. (2004). Familiarity breeds attraction: Effect of exposure on the attractiveness of typical and distinctive faces. Perception, 33, 147-157. Porter, C. P. (1991). Social reasons for skin tone preferences of Black school-age children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 61 , 149-154. Pratto, F., Sidanius, J ., & Levin, S. (2006). Social dominance theory and the dynamics of intergroup relations: Taking stock and looking forward. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1 7, 271-320. Rederstorff, J. C., Buchanan, N. T., & Settles, 1. H. (2007). The moderating roles of race and gender-role attitudes in the relationship between sexual harassment and psychological well-being. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 50-61. Rhodes, G., Halberstadt, J ., & Brajkovich, G. (2001). Generalization of mere exposure effects to averaged composite faces. Social Cognition, 19, 57-70. Rokeach, M., & Ball-Rokeach, S. J. (1989). Stability and change in American value priorities, 1968—1981. American Psychologist, 44, 775—784. Rondilla, J. L., & Spickard, P. R. (2007). Is lighter better?: Skin-tone discrimination among Asian Americans. Lanham, MD.: Rowman & Littlefield Pub Inc. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Ross, L. E. (1997). Mate selection preferences among African American college students. Journal of Black Studies, 27, 554-569. Russell, Kathy, Midge Wilson, and Ronald Hall. (1992). The Color Complex: The Politics of Skin Color Among African Americans. New York: Anchor. Sellers, R. M., & Shelton, J. N. (2003). The role of racial identity in perceived racial discrimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1079-1092. 208 Settles, I. H. (2006). Use of an intersectional framework to understand Black women’s racial and gender identities. Sex Roles, 54, 589-601. Shelton, J .N., & Richeson, J.A. (2006). Interracial interactions: A relational approach. In M.P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (pp. 121-181). New York, NY: Academic Press. Shelton, J .N., & Sellers, RM. (2000). Situational stability and variability in Afiican American racial identity. Journal of Black Psychology, 26, 27-50. Sidanius, J ., Levin, S., Liu, J ., & Pratto, F. (2000). Social dominance orientation, anti- egalitarianism and the political psychology of gender: An extension and cross-cultural replication. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 41-67. Sidanius, J ., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppression. New York: Cambridge University Press. Sorce, J. F. (1979). The role of physiognomy in the development of racial awareness. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 134, 33-41. Staples, B. (1986). Black men and public spaces. Retrieved from http://www.ghc.edu/faculty/llerych/BlackMen.pdf on April 20, 2010. Sweet, E., McDade, T. W., Kiefe, C. 1., & Liu, K. (2007). Relationships between skin color, income, and blood, pressure among Afiican Americans in the CARDIA study. American Journal of Public Health, 97, 2253-2259. Swim, J. K., Ferguson, N. J ., & Hyers, L. L. (1999). Avoiding stigma by association: Subtle prejudice against lesbians in the form of social distancing. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 61-68. Swim, J .K., & Stangor, C. (1998). Prejudice: The target ’5 perspective. San Diego: Academic Press. Tajfel, H., Billing, M., Bundy, R., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 149-178. Thompson, M. S., & Keith, V. M. (2001). The blacker the berry: Gender, skin tone, self- esteem, and self-efficacy. Gender & Society, 15, 336-357. Uhlmann, E., Dasgupta, N., Elgueta, A., Greenwald, A. G., & Swanson, J. (2002). Subgroup prejudice based on skin color among Hispanics in the United States and Latin America. Social Cognition, 20, 198-225. Valentine, T. (1988). Upside-down faces: A review of the effect of inversion upon face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 471-491. 209 Valentine, T. (1991). A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion, and race in face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43A, 161-204. Wade, T. J. (1996). The relationships between skin color and self-perceived global, physical, and sexual attractiveness, and self-esteem for African Americans. Journal of Black Psychologv, 22, 358-373. Wade, T. J ., & Bielitz, S. (2005). The differential effect of skin color on attractiveness, personality evaluation, and perceived life success of Afiican Americans. Journal of Black Psychology, 31, 215-236. Wade, T. J., Romano, M. J., & Blue, L. (2004). The effect of African American skin color on hiring preferences. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 2550-2558. Warr, P. B., Barter, J ., & Brownbridge, G. (1983). On the independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 644-651. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychologv, 54, 1063-1070. Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 262-274. Wyer, N. A., Sherman, J. W., & Stroessner, S. J. (1998). The spontaneous suppression of racial stereotype. Social Cognition, 16, 340-352. Wyer, N. A., Sherman, J. W., & Stroessner, S. J. (2000). The role of motivation and ability in controlling the consequences of stereotype supression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 13-25. Zebrowitz, L. A., Bronstad, P. M., & Lee, H. K. (2007). The contribution of face familiarity to ingroup favoritism and stereotyping. Social Cognition, 25, 306-338. 210 Y LIB TATEUNNERsn RARms | l lllllllllllllllllllllll 3 5621 ”11111111 1293 030