NWIWHKHKIHW\Hl\‘i\|\\k“\‘\l ‘& \l‘M‘lfiHfl O I Li til itL‘x t'i '{ FF 1 . .r. .1 2 OI I at. or: gen Stfiie ' l 1"! L41: :‘Iu’C‘rC‘J 1-3, This is to certify that the dissertation entitled AN EXAMINATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PSYCHOANALYTIC DEVELOPMENT OF THE SELF AND ATTRIBUTIONS AND EXPECTATION FOR EVENTS: AN ATTEMPT AT BRIDGING POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY AND KOHUT’S SELF-PSYCHOLOGY presented by Robert Babak Bastanfar has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctoral degree in Counseling Psychology // /// Major Professor’s Signature #2 49/424/67 Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Employer PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5/08 KzlProleccatPres/CIRCIDateDue.indd AN EXAMINATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PSYCHOANALYTIC DEVELOPMENT OF THE SELF AND ATTRIBUTIONS AND EXPECTATION FOR EVENTS: AN ATTEMPT AT BRIDGING POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY AND KOHUT’S SELF-PSYCHOLOGY By Robert Babak Bastanfar A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements For the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Counseling Psychology 2009 DEDICATION This dissertation is dedicated to my wife and son, Christina and Nicholas, who stood by me for many years and believed in me as I labored through graduate studies. This dissertation is also dedicated to my mother and father, Azar and Mostafa, who have supported me in so many ways. Finally, this work is in memory of my little brother Neema, who sacrificed his life so others could see another day. iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I must acknowledge the support and autonomy granted by my advisor and committee chairperson, Dr. Nancy Crewe. I will forever appreciate her efforts, guidance, and wisdom through the dissertation process. I would also like to recognize the efforts of the remaining members of my dissertation committee, Dr. Thomas Novak, Dr. Bertram Karon, and Dr. John Kosciulek, in guiding me through this research endeavor. All three members demonstrated the research knowledge and practical know-how to make such a project possible. Special thanks go to Dr. Michael Leahy, my teacher advisor, who stepped in as a committee member at a moment’s notice. Much appreciation goes to Dr. Mark Reckase and Dr. Kimberly Maier for their statistical consultation and expertise. Also, I must thank my research assistant Lisa Buch for her work on this project. Others who deserve thanks include Yu Fang and the Michigan State University C-Stat consulting center for further statistical programming and consulting. Also, Tracy and the copy center staff for the copies of questionnaire packets dutifully prepared in the name of science. I’d also like to thank Dr. Callie Oneese and Dr. Dwaine Campbell for their support and review of my work. My best friend, Parsia Kiani also deserves mention for his unconditional support for 28 years. Finally, I’d like to thank the many students who served as the participants of this study. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES .................................................................. vii LIST OF FIGURES ................................................................. viii Chapter I. Introduction ............................................................................. 01 Hypotheses .................................................................. 06 Chapter II. Review of Literature Psychoanalysis .............................................................. Psychoanalytic Self Psychology .......................................... The Superiority and Goal Instability Scales ............................ The Self Object Needs Inventory ........................................ Psychoanalysis and Counseling .......................................... Changes in Psychoanalytic Thinking and Practice .................... Positive Psychology ....................................................... Attributions & Explanatory Style ....................................... Expectation & Models of Expectancy .................................. Measuring Attributions and Expectations .............................. Aims and Objectives of Present Study .................................. Chapter III Method Participants .................................................................. Measures ..................................................................... Procedures ................................................................... Analysis ...................................................................... Chapter IV Results Study 1 ........................................................................ Study 2 ........................................................................ Chapter V Discussion Study 1 ......................................................................... Study 2 ......................................................................... Conclusion .................................................................... 10 16 22 23 25 26 29 37 41 46 48 50 53 56 57 59 67 78 82 84 Appendix A Consent ........................................................................ 86 Appendix B Demographics Questionnaire ............................................... 87 Appendix C Measures Life-Orientation Test-Revised (LOT-R) ......................... 89 Goal-Instability Scale (GI-Scale) ................................. 90 Superiority Scale (S-Scale) ........................................ 91 Self-Object Needs Inventory (SONI) ............................. 92 Extended Attributional Style Questionnaire (E-ASQ) ......... 97 Appendix D Debriefing ..................................................................... 110 Appendix E Factor Loading of EF A ...................................................... 111 Appendix F Glossary of Term ............................................................... 1 14 References ............................................................................ 1 16 vi LIST OF TABLES Page Table l. Cross-Tabulation of Summary Statistics for Participant Ethnicity x Sex ....... 51 Table 2. Descriptive statistics Of variables analyzed by canonical correlation . . . . 59 Table 3. Bivariate matrix of variables used in canonical correlation analysis ............ 60 Table 4. Self psychology variables predicting the positive psychology variables ........ 62 Table 5. Regression analysis of independent variables to each dependent variable . . . 65 Table 6. Factor structure used as model of fit in confirmatory factor analysis .......... 67 Table 7. Tests of fit for the 5-factor model ............................................. 69 Table 8. Intercorrelations of factors fitting data to the 5-factor model .................. 70 Table 9. Item loadings of principle components analysis with varimax rotation ........ 71 Table 10. Factor structure found through exploratory factor analysis ................... 77 vii LIST OF FIGURES Page Figure 1. Histograph of distribution of scores for items #16 and #34 with normal curve overlay ...................................................... 68 Figure 2. Scree plot Showing number of factors in relation to eigenvalues ......... 73 viii CHAPTER 1 — Introduction Counseling psychology has a natural connection with developmental and personality psychology (Gelso & F assinger, 1992). Advances in developmental and personality psychology have influenced counseling psychology training, research, and practice. Although counseling psychology naturally aligns with developmental and personality psychology, historically the relationship between these fields and counseling psychology has been marked by ambivalence, as counseling psychology has not optimally incorporated findings from within personality and developmental psychology. Counseling psychology needs to further incorporate personality and developmental psychology into counseling research and practice (Gelso & F assinger, 1992). Four areas in which developmental and personality psychology have been successfully incorporated into counseling are: career behavior, cultural identity, adult development, and psychoanalytic theory. Particularly in the realm of psychoanalytic work, the mutual influences of developmental, personality, and counseling psychology merge in the form of self-psychological theory developed by Heinz Kohut (Patton & Meara, 1996; Gelso & Fassinger, 1992). Self-Psychology More than any other psychoanalytic theory, self-psychology tries to understand the client through empathy and with an appreciation of the functioning and structure of the self (Gelso & Fretz, 2001). Healthy functioning is conceptualized as a function of the cohesiveness of the self and supportive internalized self-objects. The focus on empathy, growth, the self, self-esteem, and healthy development make self-psychology very amenable in counseling psychology practice (Gelso & Fassinger, 1992). Self-psychology has been proposed to have value and application in counseling settings such as college counseling centers (Patton & Robbins, 1982) as the theory addresses modern day counseling problems such as lack of ambition, lack of direction, low self-esteem, and addiction (Patton & Meara, 1996). The growing prominence of self-psychology in the realm of counseling psychology can be seen with the publication of Patton and Meara’s Psychoanalytic Counseling (1992) and in self-psychology related publications in the Journal of Counseling Psychology (Gelso & F assinger, 1992; Lee & Robbins, 1995; Payne, Robbins, & Dougherty, 1991; Robbins, 1989; Robbins & Dupont, 1992; Robbins & Patton, 1985; Robbins, Lee, & Wan, 1994) and Psychodynamic Counseling (Patton & Meara, 1996). Positive Psychology & the Healthy Personam Although the aforementioned strides strengthen the connection of developmental and personality psychology to counseling psychology, Gelso and F assinger (1992) posit that still more can be done, especially research into what constitutes an adaptive and “healthy personality” (p. 276). Optimal personality functioning has been a focus of the positive psychology movement, with its emphasis on human adaptive functioning, strengths, and virtues. Whereas self—psychology considers the construct of the self as the center of motivation and behavior (Kohut, 1971, 1977, 1984), positive psychology examines people’s expectations and attributions in determining behavior (Reivich & Gillham, 2003). When positive or negative events occur in life, people often attempt to explain the cause, attributing events to something or someone. According to the reformulated learned helplessness theory (RLHT; Abrahamson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978), how people explain the events in their lives has consequences on behavior and mental health. People with an optimistic explanatory style are more likely to attribute problems to temporary, specific, and external causes whereas people with a pessimistic explanatory style are more likely to attribute problems to stable, general, and internal causes. Integrating RLHT and self-psychology, attributional style seems remarkably analogous to a self-function and therefore would be affected, in self-psychological terms, by the cohesiveness of the self-structure. In addition, both explanatory style and the degree of self-cohesiveness moderate motivation, persistence, and vulnerability to depression in their respective theories (Reivich & Gillham, 2003; Kohut, 1971, 1977, 1984). Motivation and behavior are also determined by positive or negative expectations in the pursuit of goals. According to the expectancy-value model of behavior, behavior is a function of the pursuit of valued goals and the avoidance of undesirable consequences (Carver & Scheier, 2003). A goal must be valued in order to be pursued and the pursuer of the goal must have a general sense of assurance and confidence in attaining the goal for motivation to be maintained. Confidence in attaining goals can also be conceptualized as a function of the self and it goes to reason that those with a more cohesive self would have more positive expectations in the pursuit of goals than those with unmet self-object needs. In other words, those with a more cohesive self are likely to be more optimistic than those with unmet self-object needs. Research on the optimistic personality has shown optimism to be associated with enhanced physical and psychological health as well as increased academic, athletic, and work-related performance (Carver & Scheier, 2003). Expectancies & Therapeutic Outcome The study of the function of expectations and attributions originate from the social-cognitive model of behavior. In the context of therapy, expectations and attributions can combine to form the meta-factor known as expectancies. Expectancies are considered to be responsible for the placebo effect, or treatment effects caused by beliefs about the efficacy of the treatment (Weinberger & Eig, 1999). Expectancies are also identified as one of the common factors in psyChotherapy (Weinberger, 1995). Common factors in psychotherapy have been posited to be the true causes of therapeutic change (W ampold, 2001). In psychodynamic therapy, expectancies can be conceptualized as the transference configuration that is projected on the therapist during the course of therapy (Weinberg & Ei g, 1999). It should be noted that although expectancies do play a role in the psychodynamic model, in the form of the transference configuration, expectancies (transference) are not the driving force of this model. All psychodynamic models of behavior consider unseen and unconscious forces to be the determinants of motivation and behavior (Gelso & F retz, 2001). The counseling psychology literature recognizes the transference configuration, along with the real relationship and the working alliance, as important components of the therapeutic relationship, present more or less in all orientations of therapy (Gelso & Carter, 1985, 1994). It is the analysis of the transference configuration, or expectancies, that form the crux of psychoanalytic therapy. The psychoanalyst identifies themes in the client transference and attempts to modify the accompanying expectancies. Improvement in psychoanalytic therapy is helping clients base their perceptions on reality rather than erroneous expectations. Strupp and Binder (1984) fit the expectancy formulation of behavior to a psychodynamic context and created a short-term therapy known as Time-Limited Dynamic Psychotherapy (TLDP). The interpersonal therapy of Klerrnan and Weisman (1982) also directly addresses client expectancies during the course of therapy engendering hope in depressed clients. Despite these examples, dynamic models of therapy and behavior have done little to directly address the role of expectancies in motivation and behavior (Weinberg & Eig, 1999). Considering the common factors role that expectancies play in psychotherapy, there needs to be further investigation of expectancies in the psychodynamic context. Relating Self-Psychology to Positive Psychology The self-psychological model of behavior does not focus on attributions and expectations but instead considers all behavior as a function of the cohesiveness of the self. Like all psychoanalytic theories, self-psychology generally focuses on how self- pathology affects behavior (Gelso & F retz, 2001). Specifically, the self-pathology takes the form of deficits in the person’s self-structure that lead to perceptual mistakes in the interpretation of the outside world (Kohut, 1971, 1977, 1984). Much of the empirical research conducted on self-psychology is on constructing measures that assess the development and deficits of the self, such as the grandiosity and goal instability scales (Robbins & Patton, 1985), the social connectedness and social assurance scales (Lee & Robbins, 1995), and the Self-Object Needs Inventory (SONI; Banai, Mikulincer, & Shaver, 2005). Although an individual’s perceptions are conceptualized as a firnction of the self, there has been no investigation of expectations and attributions as a function of the self. This study was an attempt to relate the self psychological variables of grandiosity and goal instability with the positive psychology variables of general expectations and attributions made for negative events. According to the self-psychological model, self- object needs, grandiosity, and goal instability are a reflection of the stability of childhood development as well as the functioning of the self which is demonstrated in adult motivation, behavior, regulation of affect, and interpersonal functioning (Banai, et al., 2005; Robbins & Patton, 1985). Empirically exploring attributions and expectations in the self-psychological paradigm can lead to a better understanding of self functioning and ultimately will be applicable to the therapy setting. Even with a better understanding of client expectations and attributions, misunderstandings in the therapeutic setting will be inevitable and can be effectively used for curative change. Relating the fimctioning of the self to expectations and attributions garners a new psychoanalytic paradigm for the development of positive and negative expectancies. In addition, it is also important to examine the expectations and attributional style of a person with a more cohesive and healthy self as well, thereby heeding the wisdom of Gelso and F assinger (1992) for further investigating healthy personality functioning. Taking a page from positive psychology, a better understanding of strengths will only complement what is known about pathology (Seligman, Steen, Park, & Peterson, 2005). Research Question How do variables pertaining to the functioning of the self relate with one’s cognitive explanatory style for negative events and general outlook on the future? The following research hypotheses were examined: H01: There is no significant expected canonical relationship between the two data sets (Rc = 0). H1: Grandiosity will be Si gnificantly related to an external attributional style for negative events. H02: There is no significant relationship expected between attributional style for negative events and goal instability. H03: There is no significant relationship expected between expectations for life events and grandiosity. H04: There is no significant relationship expected between expectations for life events and goal instability. The first clinical hypothesis (H1) is the only directional hypothesis stated and is based on the literature on narcissism (Campbell, Rudich, & Sedikides, 2002; Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001; Campbell, Reeder, Sedikides, & Elliot, 2000). It is posited that grandiosity will be significantly related to an external attributional style. In other words, those with extreme scores on the scale measuring grandiosity will make external causal attributions for negative events. The external attributional style is not a firnction of an optimistic personality, as the reformulated learned helplessness model would posit, but instead the result of a perceptual construction of reality that is meant to maintain self- esteem. The remaining hypotheses are stated as null hypotheses and are exploratory as the relationship between self-psychological variables (grandiosity and goal instability) and positive psychology variables (expectations and attributions) cannot be predicted at this juncture. Thus, the first part of the study examined and bridged theories within two popular paradigms of psychology (psychoanalysis and positive psychology) by examining the relationship between constructs within their respective theories. It was hoped that a better understanding of cognitive functioning is garnered within a psychoanalytic context of personality. The hypotheses of the first research question were examined by canonical correlation analysis. The canonical procedure created linear combinations of the variables for each set of variables; these new variables are known as canonical variates. The relationship between the two data sets was determined by seeing how the canonical variates related to each other as well as how they related to the original sets of self- psychology and positive psychology variables. Significant effects were determined by tests of significance as well as indexes of effect Size. A second research question that was examined concerned the factor structure of the Self-Object Needs Inventory (SONI; Banai, et al., 2005). The SONI was originally written in Hebrew when developed at Bar-Illan University in Israel. This measure was administered to 372 undergraduate participants; factor analysis showed the SONI to consist of the following five factors: hunger for mirroring, hunger for idealization, hunger for twinship, avoidance of mirroring, and the avoidance of idealization/twinship. Essentially, we were trying to replicate the Banai et a1. study and confirm the 5-factor structure of the SONI using a slightly modified English version of the measure and using a sample of undergraduates from a large Midwestern university in the United States. All modifications to the SONI were approved by the original author. The hypothesis of the second research question was examined by confirmatory factor analysis. Model fit was determined through various indexes of fit. The hypothesis that addresses the factor structure Of the SONI was stated as follows: H5: The factor structure of the Self-Object Needs Inventory consists of 5 factors: hunger for mirroring, hunger for idealization, hunger for twinship, avoidance of mirroring, and the avoidance of idealization/twinship. Overall, this study was anticipated to yield a better understanding of a person’s general expectations and explanatory style for events as a degree of optimal firnctioning of the self and ultimately may be useful in understanding client expectations and explanatory style in the therapeutic setting. The results may be useful for those mental health professionals who would appreciate a theoretically integrative understanding of clients and those who may not use self-psychological analytic techniques but may instead prefer an analytically informed understanding of the person (Gelso & F retz, 2001). In an era where theoretical eclecticism is embraced (Weiten & Lloyd, 2006; Garfield & Bergin, 1986) and the contextual and common factors of therapy (such as client expectancies) are being considered as the factors of change in outcome studies (W ampold, 2001), this theoretically integrative study was indeed needed. CHAPTER 2 — Review of the Literature Psychoanalysis The revolutionary accomplishments of Sigmund Freud have been compared with that of Copernicus, Newton, and Darwin in that they have changed man’s views of reality regarding human nature and behavior (Lapsley, 1994; Steinberg, 1987). Freud’s work has impressed upon us the importance of parent and child dynamics as well as the significance of the formative years in childhood. Psychoanalytic theory not only addresses issues of psychopathology but also comments on normal personality fimctioning, culture, art, and history (Wolitzky, 1995). This comprehensive theory is considered the first insight oriented therapy which served as the nexus of all insight- oriented therapies (Gelso & F retz, 2001). In order to have a better understanding of analytic theory it is important to examine its philosophy of science. Psychoanalytic theory is considered a meta-psychology (Robbins, 1989). There is a distinction to be made between empirically-based psychology and meta-psychology. The science of psychology is based on empiricism and is therefore bound to the principles of clarity, precision, and the intolerance for error (Weiten & Lloyd, 2006). Psychoanalytic theory extends beyond the borders of empiricism and focuses on that which cannot be seen or empirically verified. Robbins (1989) identifies five propositions that serve as the basis of psychoanalytic theory. These propositions consist of the dynamic, economic, structural, genetic, and adaptive points of View. First, psychoanalytic theory is a dynamic theory, and thus posits that behavior is influenced by conflicting and unseen motivational forces. Next, psychoanalytic theory proposes that motivational forces and energies seek means for discharge that are pleasurable. Third, 10 unseen psychological structures determine the functioning of the dynamic processes. Fourth, psychoanalytic theory considers behavior to be a function of psychological factors that develop from both the individual’s constitutional attributes and their environment; this development progresses according to a set pattern of developmental stages. Finally, adaptation occurs throughout life and these adaptive processes occur because of changes in the individual and in the environment. Different contemporary psychoanalytic theories emphasize these propositions to differing degrees; Freud’s original psychoanalytic model has expanded and was further developed both by Freud himself as well as those who followed him (Robbins, 1989). Gelso and Fretz (2001) broadly identify four prominent schools of psychoanalytic thought: the drive model, ego- psychology, object-relations, and self-psychology. The following is a brief outline of these different schools of thought with the acknowledgement that what follows simply scratches the surface of the vast literature and of each school of psychoanalytic thought. Freud’s drive model started as a delineation of the levels of consciousness known. as the topographical model. The conscious is all activity that is within awareness. The preconscious is mental content just below awareness but cOuld easily reach conscious awareness. Freud conceptualized the unconscious to be the level of the mind that stored content that was not in awareness and was not easily accessible but can covertly influence behavior and presents through dream activity. Freud believed that the unconscious served as the storage for memories of repressed traumatic events (Robbins, 1989). The conscious part of the mind operates logically and adaptively whereas the unconscious realm of the mind is instinctual, biological, and in incessant need for expression of its sexual energies. The sexual energy, referred to as libido, is the drive for self-preservation 11 and includes all behaviors and motivations that self-preservation entails. Freud posited that libido can be invested in the self as well as on others. Libidinal energy invested in the self became part of Freud’s conception of narcissism. Based on clinical Observations of self-destructive and self-punitive behavior, Freud then revised his drive theory to address the phenomenon of aggression. His dual-instinct theory consisted of an instinctual drive for love and survival (Bros) and an instinctual drive for aggression and self-destruction (Thanatos; Arlow, 1995). Freud’s next revision of drive theory was to conceptualize the structure of the mind into three components: the id, the ego, and the superego. The id represents the instinctual drives and their pressure for expression (Arlow, 1995). The id resides in the unconscious and operates according to the pleasure principle and thus demands immediate gratification (McWilliams, 1994). Through experiences with the outside world, the inner world develops the ego. The ego acts as a mediator between the inner world and the outside world and operates according to the reality principle in which social constraints are considered. The final component of the structural theory of mind is the superego. The superego is comprised of parental moral influences and ideals. How the ego mediates the conflicts between the id, ego, and superego becomes the definition of mental health (Arlow, 1995). Although Freud put greater emphasis on the ego with the development of structural theory, the crux of drive theory is how sexual and aggressive drives are channeled into urges and influence behavior. Freud’s followers emphasized and further delineated the adaptive functions of the ego. Their revision of psychoanalytic theory developed into a second psychoanalytic school of thought known as ego psychology (Gelso & F retz, 2001). 12 The efforts of Heinz Harttrnan (1939) and Anna Freud (1936) were crucial in fortifying the adaptive View of ego functioning (Gelso & F retz, 2001; Gelso & Fassinger, 1992; Robbins, 1989). Ego psychology frames the ego as more than a structure that simply reacts to inner psychic conflict, instead, focusing on the adaptive aspects of the ego in relation to the environment (Josephs, 1991). Thus, how well a person acknowledged reality without having to rely on defense mechanisms became known as ego-strength (McWilliams, 1994). Ego-psychology emphasizes ego-strength, development, defenses, and reality testing (Gelso & Fretz, 2001). Josephs (1991) frames the functions of the ego (i.e. “judgment, planning, anticipation, reality testing, problem solving, etc.”) as a cognitive style necessary for adaptation to the environment (pp. 6-7). How the ego functions is dependent on its development as well as constitutional factors. Ego psychology focuses on the pre-Oedipal formative years of ego development and its influence on healthy ego functioning. Early trauma will have consequences for subsequent ego development and functioning (Gelso & Fretz, 2001). The ego is also conceptualized as having both conscious as well as unconscious aspects in its functioning. The conscious part of the ego includes the experience-near construct of the “I” or the “self.” The unconscious part of the ego mediates the defense mechanisms that protect the ego from the striving of the id, disturbing reality experiences, and guilt from the super ego. Ego psychology also differentiates the maturity and variety of defense mechanisms used. Someone who consistently defends with certain defense mechanisms such as projection and denial is considered to be rigid in coping style (McWilliams, 1994). Although ego psychology increased the clinical explanatory value of psychoanalytic theory, many American and British psychoanalysts were further 13 developing psychoanalytic theory in new directions, focusing on the need to relate to others. The next step in the development of psychoanalytic theory is the object-relations school of thought (Gelso & F retz, 2001). The object relations tradition is broad and encompasses relational theories such as interpersonal theory and attachment theory (Gelso & Fretz, 2001). Work in object— relations theory spans both Sides of the Atlantic. European psychoanalysts who have contributed to object relations theory include Klein, F airbairn, and others. American contributions to object-relations theory include the work of Kemberg, Mahler, and others (Robbins, 1989). The primary difference between the obj ect-relations and drive theory or ego psychology is the theory of motivation. Whereas drive theory and ego psychology conceptualized motivation and behavior as pleasure seeking, Object-relations theory emphasizes the importance of relationships (current as well as early relationships), how others are experienced, and the internal representations of others (in psychoanalytical vernacular known as objects) that are products of relationships (Gelso & Fretz, 2001). The object relations tradition grew from working with patients suffering from serious psychological impairment (more serious pathology than Freud had originally thought could profit from analysis) and research on early mother and child relations. Thus, a new understanding of psychopathology came to fi'uition by conceptualizing pathology as the client’s inner world influenced by representations of others (i.e. caretakers) in the client’s early formative relations (McWilliams, 1994). Early relationships (as opposed to conflicts) become the crux of psychological health and these relationships reflect on current functioning in present relationships (Gelso & F retz, 2001). 14 All psychoanalytic theories of the person are developmental by nature (Gelso & Fassinger, 1992). Object-relations research understood human development differently from classic Freudian theory. Margaret Mahler (1960, 1971) conceptualized psychological development as a progressive process in which the individual gradually advances fiom a state of psychological union with the mother to a state of psychological separateness and individuality. This process of separation and individuation is conceptualized as three overlapping developmental phases of psychological growth (normal autism, normal symbiosis, and separation and individuation). The separation and individuation phase is firrther divided into the four sub—phases of differentiation, practicing, rapprochement, and object constancy. Healthy progression through each phase is dependent on the mother’s reaction to the infant’s strivings of exploring and discovery of its body and the outside world (St. Clair, 1996). As opposed to Mahler’s model of development where connectedness exists at the start of psychological development, the attachment of Ainsworth and Bowlby (1991) conceptualizes connectedness as an endpoint of healthy development (St. Clair, 1996). In this model of attachment, responsive and supportive parents provide a secure base to explore the environment and relate to others. The importance of early relationships is also clear in Sullivan’s (1953) interpersonal theory of motivation and behavior. According to Josephs (1991), Sullivan expanded the ego-psychological View of adaptation from being entirely an intra-psychic process to taking account of socio-cultural and contextual factors in an individual’s development. Adaptation therefore is conceptualized as the individual having to interpersonally assimilate to the circumstances of their environment. This interpersonal 15 assimilation to the environment must take place in order for the individual to be accepted by others and maintain self-esteem. Karen Homey (1950), another interpersonal theorist, also realized the importance of relationships in the development of an individual’s innate potentialities and talents. Homey considered the interpersonal nurturance of natural talents as the crux of healthy psychological development. Without the support of talents and potentialities, the individual cannot develop into what they are supposed to. Stymieing this natural development can lead to resentment and self-hatred. Heinz Kohut (1971, 1977, 1984) combined the interpersonal need for acceptance and the need for developing of natural potentialities in his model of the bi-polar self (Josephs, 1991) and developed his theory of self-psychology, a new and radically different development in psychoanalytic thinking. Self-psychology would have profound effects in psychoanalytic thinking and ultimately bridge psychoanalytic theory with counseling psychology (Gelso & F assinger, 1992). Psychoanalm'c Self-Psychology Self-psychology is a comprehensive theory of psychological development, personality, and therapy that has become an influential school of thought among psychoanalytic theories (Gelso & Fretz, 2001). Developed by Heinz Kohut, this psychoanalytic theory holds true to the basic tenets of psychoanalysis but distances itself from such concepts such as drives, conflict, and the tripartite structure of personality, and instead, embraces the construct of the self as the center of motivation and behavior. Kohut further differentiates self-psychological theory fiom classical drive theory by considering the healthy development of narcissism as the means of achieving an adaptive and cohesive self (Patton & Meara, 1996; Wolitzky & Eagle, 1997; Kohut, 1984; Kohut 16 & Wolf, 1978). Classical drive theory considers neurosis to be a function of ongoing repressed oedipal conflict between the intact structures of personality (id, ego, and super- ego). Kohut’s self-psychology carries psychoanalytic thinking in a new direction by conceptualizing disorders of the self as a firnction of deficits in the self-structure, therefore, making self-psychology a deficit model of psychopathology as opposed to a conflict model (St. Clair, 1996; Baker, 1991). According to Baker (1991), four concepts are critical to the understanding of self—psychological theory: the concept of the self, the self-object, the purpose of symptoms in the disorders of the self, and the importance of empathy in the treatment of the disorders of the self. Kohut (1971, 1978, 1984) defines the self as the center of motivation and behavior. The self consists of systems of feelings, thoughts, memories, sensations, and attitudes that organize subjective experiences (Banai, Mikulincer, & Shaver, 2005; Baker, 1991; Kohut, 1971, 1977, 1984; Patton & Meara, 1996). The systems of the self develop from the matrix of the child’s biological endowments and the environmental self-object milieu provided by caretakers. The infantile self consists of two archaic self-objects, the grandiose self and the idealized parent imago (Kohut & Wolf, 1978), also referred to as the idealized parental image (Patton & Meara, 1996; Patton & Robbins, 1982). These two parallel lines Of developmental narcissism are connected by a tension arc of talents and skills. If narcissistic and self-object needs are not met in childhood these needs will surface in adulthood in relational terms and result in a self that struggles to maintain cohesion (Kohut, 1971, 1977, 1984). The concept of the self-object is pivotal to self-psychology. Self-objects are mental representations of people or things that are experienced as a part of the self and 17 carry out the firnctions of the self such as maintenance of self-esteem and regulation of affect. The responsiveness of an individual’s self-object environment in childhood is critical to the development of innate and healthy narcissism (Kohut, 1971). Kohut (1971, 1977) originally identified two kinds of self-objects that are critical for healthy psychological development. The mirroring self-object confirms the child’s sense of greatness and maintains self-esteem whereas the idealized parental image serves as an object of omnipotence and composure that the child can look up to and with which they can merge. The grandiose-self develops by the mirroring response of caretakers to the child’s exhibitionistic tendencies (caretakers serving as mirroring self-objects) which develop into childhood assertiveness and ultimately becomes healthy ambition as a source of vitality and greatness. Idealization of caretakers (caretakers served as idealized self—objects) develops into admiration which ultimately becomes the ability to set healthy and realistic goals (Kohut & Wolf, 1978). The twinship and connectedness self-object was proposed later by Kohut (1984) which he conceptualized as the part of the self which resulted in feelings of fitting-in and similarity to others. Another important and somewhat unique aspect of self-psychology is the concept of symptoms of psychopathology being attempts at maintaining self-cohesion at times Of psychological distress (Baker, 1991). Kohut developed self-psychological theory through the analysis of clients with disorders of the self such as narcissistic and borderline personality disorder (Kohut & Wolf, 1978). Through working with these clients, Kohut theorized that symptoms of these disorders are the shortsighted efforts of the self to maintain cohesion or some form of expression. Attempts to remedy deficits in the self take the form of defensive maneuvers and compensatory behaviors. Defensive l8 maneuvers are attempts to cover and deny self-deficit. The person looks outside the self and uses other people and things to achieve a sense of greatness (defect in the grandiose line of development) or to soothe the self (defect in the idealized parental image line of development). Such defensive maneuvers may manifest as addiction to substances, sexual perversion, Obesity, and delinquent behavior (Patton & Meara, 1996; Patton & Robbins, 1982). The psychological roots of the term “comfort food” becomes apparent when considering it as a defensive maneuver to soothe against anxiety that the self can not mollify because of self-object deficits. According to Patton and Robbins (1982), compensatory strategies “are longer term behavior patterns that do not merely cover over the defect, they actively make up for it” (p. 882). A person with deficits in the grandiose self may compensate for the defect by garnering admiration from others but still feel empty (Patton & Meara, 1996; Patton & Robbins, 1982). Such pathology is considered to run the continuum from adjustment disorders to psychosis (Baker, 1991). Development of the self and its vulnerability to pathology is in large part determined by parental character (Kohut & Wolf, 1978). Self-psychological theory posits that the child’s psychological needs for self- objects are analogous to the child’s biological needs of food and air in terms of survival. Healthy development entails caretakers serving as self-objects for the child’s grandiosity and idealization needs. No parent is perfectly attuned to a child’s self-object needs and self-object needs are bound to be frustrated. If these frustrations in caretaker attunement to the child’s self-object needs are not traumatic, the child’s archaic-self will further develop replacing parental self-objects with a functioning self. A parent with unmet self- object needs will most likely not be attuned to and accepting of the child’s developmental 19 narcissism (Kohut & Wolf, 1978). Failures in parental empathy which are chronic and severe result in fixation in the development of the grandiose self and/or the idealized self (self-object imago). Fixation of the grandiose self results in dependency on others to serve as a mirroring self-object to support the person’s infantile grandiosity and maintain esteem. Fixation of the idealized self results in a person seeking merger with others perceived as powerful. Targets of infantile idealization would include people and organizations in the social, religious, and political realms (Patton & Meara, 1996). Taxonomy of the disorders of the self has been delineated by Kohut and Wolf (1978). The self-psychological model distinguishes between secondary disturbances of the self and the more severe primary disturbances of the self. When referring to secondary disturbances of the self, Kohut is conceptualizing a relatively intact and cohesive self. The ability of the self to cope with a stressor is contingent upon its relative cohesiveness. Kohut’s model of the self can be applied to how resilient one may be in the face of such chronic frustrations as institutionalized racism, sexism, and ablism (Kohut & Wolf, 1978). The more severe disturbances of the self known as the primary disturbances result from relatively more severe disorganization and disruption in the self-structure. There are several categories of the primary disturbances of the self. Psychosis is experienced when the self lacks structure or the structure of the self is severely disrupted. The deficits in the structure of the self can be because of the interaction of innate and environment deficits such as organic vulnerability to pathology or a severely traumatic and invalidating upbringing. A second grouping of disturbances of the self consists of the borderline states of self-pathology. What distinguish borderline disorders from 20 psychoses are the defenses used to cover the disorganization and fragmentation in the self. A more resilient grouping of primary disturbances of the self would be that of the narcissistic behavior disorders. This grouping of the primary disturbances manifests as deviant and addictive behaviors in an attempt to restore a sense of vitality to a self- structure with developmental deficit (Kohut & Wolf, 1978). A final group of primary disturbances are the narcissistic personality disorders. Kohut and Wolf (1978) describe the narcissistic personality disorders as similar to the narcissistic behavior disorders but more pervasive, “. . .symptoms — e. g. hypochondria, depression, hypersensitivity to slights, lack of zest — concern not primarily the actions and interactions of the individual but rather his psychological state” (p. 416). Kohut’s taxonomy of disorders of the self clearly differs from that of the DSM-IV—TR (2000). Kohut’s model of pathology is rooted in psychoanalytic conceptions of the faulty development of narcissism and formed from clinical observations in psychoanalytic sessions (Tesser, 1991), whereas the DSM- IV -TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) is atheoretical and attempts to delineate and describe the behaviors of different pathological conditions. It is also through analysis of clients with disorders of the self that Kohut discovered that empathic emersion into the world of the client is the best means of helping clients. The analyst serves as a self-object for the client and through empathic attunement and emersion into the world of the client, allows further development of a strengthened self—structure within the therapeutic milieu. No therapist can be perfectly empathically attuned to the client and the client will react to this lapse in empathic attunement. This lapse in empathy serves as a non-traumatic frustration (an optimal fi'ustration) for the client and the therapist empathically interprets the client’s reaction. 21 Through optimal frustrations in empathy, the client’s self is strengthened through the process of transmuting internalization (Kohut, 1971, 1977, 1984). Kohut has compared transmuting internalization to the physiological process of digesting protein in order to strengthening the body (Kohut & Wolf, 1978). Self- psychology and the above mentioned constructs have had Significant influence in psychoanalytic circles and the theory has shown applicability in variety of settings and populations (Gelso & Fretz, 2001) Kohut’s self-psychology has been compared to the humanistic psychology of Carl Rogers and is considered to bridge counseling psychology and psychoanalysis because of the theory’s emphasis on the self and empathic attunement to the client (Khan, 1985). Self-psychology has also been proposed as: a remedial therapy in the college counseling setting (Patton & Robbins, 1982), a short-term therapy (Baker, 1991), predictive of self- perception of interpersonal behavior (Robbins & Dupont, 1992), applicable to vocational issues (Robbins & Patton, 1985), relevant to Asian (Roland, 1996) and Arab (Zonis, 1980) cultures, and predictive of the adjustment of the elderly (Payne, Robbins, & Dougherty, 1991). The above is just a short list of the applications of this incredibly heuristic psychoanalytic theory. The Superiority and Goal Instabilig Scales The work of Steven Robbins (Lee & Robbins, 1995; Payne, Robbins, & Dougherty, 1991; Robbins, Lee, & Wan, 1994; Robbins & Dupont, 1992; Robbins, 1989; Robbins & Patton, 1985) and Michael Patton (Patton & Meara, 1996; Patton & Meara, 1992) has been instrumental in empirically investigating the concepts of self-psychology and elucidating the use and function of self—psychology within counseling psychology. 22 One area of focus for these scholars has been the integration of self-psychology with career decision making and planning (Patton & Robbins, 1985), an area congruent with counseling psychology’s focus on career related issues (Gelso & F retz, 2001). Using Kohut’s concepts of the grandiose and idealized self (Kohut, 1971, 1977, 1984), they developed the superiority and goal-instability scales as a measure of the development and execution of career plans. Congruent with self-psychological theory, people with an exaggerated sense of self-importance and who were dependent on others to validate this sense of importance were less likely to pursue career related issues. Likewise, individuals with deficits in ideals and goals were likely to demonstrate difficulty in career decision making. Goal instability and superiority are the respective expressions of the developmental vectors of grandiosity and idealization in the model of the bipolar self. lndices fiom these scales demonstrate firnctions of the self such as the ability to realistically appraise talents and skills, having a reasonable and stable set of goals, and being able to take action toward the completion of goals (Robbins & Patton, 1985) The Self Object Needs lnventogy The latest empirical investigation of the concepts of self—psychology by Banai et a1. (2005) examines the interpersonal functioning of the self as a function of the self- object deficits in the self-structure. This line of research not only examines the validity of the concepts of self-psychology but also bridges self-psychological theory with empirical investigations of the self in the social psychology literature. In a series of seven studies, Banai et a1. (2005) create a measure of self-object needs, the Self Object Needs Inventory (SONI), and related this measure to several personality-related 23 variables. The finding of the seven studies were generally in support of Kohut’s theories about the development of the self and interpersonal functioning of the self based on the self-Object needs of individuals. The first of the seven studies created the SONI and administered it to 372 Israeli undergraduates. The authors then examined the factor structure of the SONI and the measure’s psychometric properties. Basic patterns of interpersonal functioning were that of hunger for self-obj ects (because of a defective self-structure that is dependent on others as external self-objects to maintain the functions of the self) or avoidance of self- objects (socially avoidant behavior in anticipation of the frustration of self-object needs by others). Factor analysis of the SONI revealed a five-factor structure: the hunger for mirroring, the hunger for idealization, the hunger for twinship, the avoidance of mirroring, and the avoidance of idealization/havinship. Banai et a1. expected that the avoidance of idealization and twinship would be separate factors as Kohut’s writings would predict, but this was not the case. A factor analysis was also performed on the collapsed samples of studies two through seven (N=573) revealing the same structure of five factors. The authors simply reasoned that anyone who would avoid the idealization of another would also avoid situations that would lead to feelings of kinship and connectedness. Banai et a1. did mention that future research may be able to distinguish between the avoidance of idealization and twinship. The results of Banai et a1. confirm Kohut’s idea’s of a defective self resulting in emotional maladjustment as both personality styles correlated with high levels of anxiety and depression. Results diverge from Kohut’s ideas in that only the interpersonal pattern of hunger for self-obj ects demonstrated low self-esteem whereas those demonstrating 24 avoidance of self-Objects did not experience lower self-esteem. The authors argued that either the socially avoidant pattern has a protective function or that the measure of self- esteem used (Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale; Rosenberg, 1979) was too overt of a measure. Overall results Show that the SONI seems to measure self-object deficits that manifest as anxiety, depression, emotional dependence or hostility toward others, which according to self-psychological theory are indicative Of the individual’s difficulties in regulating affect and self-esteem internally (Kohut, 1984). Psychoanalysis and Counseling According to Gelso and Fretz (2001), counseling psychology truly owes a dept of gratitude to Freud and his outstanding work for serving as the foundation of all counseling interventions. Although psychoanalytic theory has been applicable to counseling psychology (Bordin, 1980), it has never held a pivotal position in counseling tradition (Borgen, 1984). Gelso and F retz (2001) highlight differences in the classical psychoanalytic paradigm and the unifying themes of counseling psychology that make counseling psychology and psychoanalytic theory “strange bedfellows” (p. 322). The tenets of counseling psychology differ from that of classical analytic theory on three levels. Classical analysis focuses on long-term treatment whereas brief- treatrnent is a predominant theme of counseling psychology. Secondly, psychoanalysis also emphasizes intra-psychic factors as the root of behavior whereas counseling psychology considers person factors, environment factors, and the person-environment interaction in determining behavior. An emphasis on intra-psychic factors as the determinant of behavior has an impact on the cultural sensitivity of psychoanalytic theory and therefore diminishing applicability to multicultural populations (Ponterotto & Casas, 25 1991). Finally, whereas psychoanalytic theory concentrates on client pathology (based on conflict and developmental deficit), counseling psychology emphasizes the strengths of clients, even when considering clients who are highly pathological (Gelso & Fretz, 2001). Although strange bedfellows at one time, changes in psychoanalytic thinking have made psychoanalytic theory and counseling psychology more compatible (Gelso & Fretz, 2001; Gelso & Fassinger, 1992). Changes in Psychoanalytic Thinking and Practice Gelso and Fassinger (1992) identify four trends in psychoanalytic work that have had consequences on counseling psychology, in large part making psychoanalytic theory more congruent with counseling psychology (Gelso & F retz, 2001). The first trend is an emphasis on social, interpersonal, and cultural factors as opposed to the biological and instinctual. This trend is congruent with counseling psychology’s emphasis on cultural diversity and behavior as a function of the interaction of the person and environment factors. Two groups are given credit for this first trend. The first group are the neo- Freudians, analysts such as Horney, Sullivan, and Erickson as well as others. Object- relations and self-psychology analysts, such as Mahler and Kohut, are the second group responsible for this trend toward the social and interpersonal. These theorists have added to the depth and breadth of psychoanalysis by investigating the development of psychological health as opposed to concentrating on pathology. The second trend identified is the emphasis on adaptation and coping. Robbins (1989) notes the emphasis on ego-functioning in contemporary psychoanalytic theory is congruent with counseling psychology’s focus on coping and adjustment. This trend was started by Freud with his development of the structural model of personality and ego 26 psychology (as opposed to id psychology). This trend includes the diminished importance of the phallic stage and the Oedipal complex. According to Gelso and F retz (2001), there has been a reformulation of the Oedipal Complex where it is regarded as more a psychosocial phenomenon rather than psycho-sexual (i.e. doing away with the concept of penis envy; B. Karon, personal communication, October 5, 2005) and therefore being less sexist and biased against women. Also, coping and adjustment throughout the life cycle is addressed as opposed to the first few years of life. Another trend identified is the application of psychoanalytically based developmental theories, such as separation-individuation and attachment theory, to counseling psychology research. While psychoanalytic developmental theories are concerned with the first few years of life, counseling psychology looks at the application of these theories to adolescence and adulthood. Lopez and Brennan (2000) offer a description of a healthy and fully functioning self that is consistent with attachment research. Blustein, Walbridge, F riedlander, and Palladino (1991) performed two studies examining the relationship between difficulties in separation and attachment to career indecision, career decision making efficacy, and the career commitment process. Lopez, Campbell, & Watkins (1986) examined the relationship between psychological separation and the variables of family structure and college adjustment. All three studies were published in the Journal of Counseling Psychology, a prominent counseling psychology professional journal, thus showing the influence of psychoanalytic theory on counseling psychology. The fourth trend identified involves the study of therapist variables. Classical analysis considered therapist variables as unimportant because counter-transference was 27 seen as something to be avoided, an unchecked therapist variable that can only impede therapeutic progress. A burgeoning literature on counter-transference in counseling psychology has highlighted the importance of therapist variables (i.e. therapist anxiety, therapist empathy, therapist self-knowledge) in the effectiveness of treatment (Gelso & F assinger, 1992). Patton and Meara (1996) posit that therapist variables are especially important in the practice of self-psychological therapy; they compare the importance of the character of the therapist to that of the client’s parents in achieving effective therapeutic outcomes. Finally, due to factors such as the streamlining of mental health services, psychoanalytic therapy has made changes in the length of treatment. Although psychoanalysis was first proposed as an intensive and comprehensive treatment lasting for years, short-term psychoanalytically-based therapies have become popular. These brief analytic treatments are more structured, active, and focus on specific client problems. This focus on briefer analytic treatments is congruent with counseling psychology’s unifying theme of briefer interactions (Gelso & Fretz, 2001). Although psychoanalytic therapy has become more congruent with counseling psychology with time, the growing positive psychology movement with its focus on virtues and strengths seems especially compatible with counseling psychology and formalizes a movement that is dedicated to the study of the healthy and fully functioning personality. Gelso and Fassinger’s (1992) call for the study of the development and functioning of the healthy personality comes to fruition with the growth of this new perspective in psychology. 28 Positive Psychology Positive psychology is the scientific study of human virtues and strengths such as forgiveness, hope, courage, love, optimism, etc. The positive psychology movement hopes to call attention to positive character traits and strengths in an attempt to more completely understand human behavior and therefore bring better balance to the science of psychology. A very significant Sign of the growing strength of the positive psychology movement is the many publications devoted to the subject of positive psychology. One positive psychology publication that is worth mention is Character Strengths and Virtues: A Handbook and Classification (CSV; Peterson & Seligman, 2004), which is a manual of classification for human strengths which stands in stark contrast to the American Psychiatric Association’s publication, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association), which is known for it’s classification of psychopathology (Seligman, Steen, Park, & Peterson, 2005). Although positive psychology has been solidified and formalized as a new movement, the roots of positive psychology are built upon the work of past scholars and theorists. Carl Rogers is one of the many scholars whose work has been credited to be a forerunner to the positive psychology movement (Seligman, Steen, Park, & Peterson, 2005). Rogers is known as one of the founders of the humanistic movement and the creator of person-centered theory, a theory of personality development and psychotherapy that emphasizes the needs of the self in relation to the interpersonal context and environment. Humanistic theory in general, and specifically the work and 29 concepts of Rodgers, has had a tremendous impact on counseling psychology (Gelso & F retz, 2001). Khan (1985) points out that the theories and work of both Rogers and Kohut emphasize the self, free will, self-enhancement, the importance of supportive relationships throughout life, and empathy in the therapeutic relationship. Much like the applied clinical work of Rogers and Kohut, positive psychology also has an applied facet in which positive interventions have the goal of inducing positive emotions and therefore leading to a happier state of mind and ultimately, a happier existence. Happy people are more healthy, social, and successful (Seligman, Steen, Park, & Peterson, 2005). This causal relation permeates the psychoanalytic object- relations literature as well which recognizes the connection between adjustment and supportive relationships (Gelso & F assinger, 1992). One of the most heuristic theories of happiness is the Brickrnan and Campbell (1971) hedonic treadmill theory. According to the original version of this theory, both happiness and unhappiness are transitory reactions to changes in circumstances in which people habituate and return to a set-point of neutrality. Happiness and unhappiness are conceptualized as transitory reactions to changes in circumstances rather than a result of the circumstances themselves (Diener, Lucas, & Scollon,'2006). According to Brickman, Coates, & J anoff-Bulman (1978), the transitory nature of happiness is a function of adaptation. Experiences that are salient and relevant to other experiences (Brickman et al.’s example was winning a million dollars) are susceptible to adaptation; adaptation takes place by means of contrast and habituation. When an extremely salient and relevant positive event takes place, other positive events of lesser magnitude will pale in comparison; this would be an example of contrast. Furthermore, the same relevant 30 and salient positive event would lose its pleasurable impact with time; this is an example of habituation. In order to empirically test this hypothesis, Brickman et al. (1977) compared levels of happiness between 22 lottery winners, 29 people with acquired spinal cord injury, and a control group of 22 participants who were neither lottery winners nor accident victims. Results demonstrated that lottery winners were not happier than controls (habituation) and experienced less pleasure from ordinary events (contrast). Thus, in contrast to positive psychology (that seeks means of increasing a state of happiness in people), hedonic treadmill theory considers the pursuit of happiness a futile endeavor as long-term happiness is not truly achievable due to habituation to circumstances and return to the neutral set point. Although Deiner, Lucas, and Scollon (2006) agree with the crux of hedonic treadmill theory, they recently revised the theory according to more contemporary research on adaptation. According to the revised version of the theory, lasting changes in happiness and subjective wellbeing can be achieved. The psychological literature on happiness and subjective wellbeing supports this finding (i.e. Seligman, Steen, Park, & Peterson, 2005; Emmons & McCullough, 2003; Fordyce, 1977, 1983). The first empirical investigations on increasing happiness were by F ordyce (1977, 1983). He was able to design and test a program of “14 fundamentals” (p. 516; F ordyce, 1977) that consisted of educational components as well as activities that make the participant more cognizant of happiness. The exercises of the program include: (a) keep busy and more active; (b) spend more time socializing; (c) be productive at meaningful work; ((1) get better organized and plan things out; (e) stop worrying; (t) lower your expectations and aspirations; (g) develop positive, 31 Optimistic thinking; (h) become present oriented; (i) work on a healthy personality; (j) develop an outgoing, social personality; (k) be yourself; (1) eliminate negative feelings and problems; (m) close relationships are the number one source of happiness; (n) put happiness as your most important priority (Fordyce, 1983; p. 484) According to Fordyce (1977, 1983), exercises and suggestions of the 14 fundamentals were taken from the psychological literature on happiness. Although some of the suggestions seem vague, there was an educational component to the program so the exercises were explained and elaborated on for the participants. F ordyce (1977) came to several significant conclusions. First, happiness can be increased by pursuing it purposely. Also, everybody has the potential to increase their happiness. Another conclusion was that happiness can be increased by self-efforts through education and exercises. Finally, psychology can make great contributions in the study and enhancement of happiness. Some concerns that Fordyce (1977) showed about this line of research concerned the definition and measurement of happiness as well as standardization in the presentation of his program. F ordyce’s concerns are addressed in one of the latest positive psychology studies on increasing happiness by Seligman, Steen, Park, and Peterson (2005). In order to achieve greater happiness, happiness must be defined and further scientifically studied (Seligman, Steen, Park, & Peterson, 2005). Seligman (2002) identifies three components of happiness: positive emotions, engagement in life, and meaning in life. These three components of happiness can be assessed through the Steen Happiness Index (SHI; Seligman, Steen, Park, & Peterson, 2005), a measure created by 32 positive psychology researchers to get a more accurate and complete assessment of happiness. The SHI and the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) were used as dependent variables in an intemet based, random assignment -— placebo control design study that examined the efficacy of four different positive psychology interventions in decreasing depression and increasing happiness. The design of the study consisted of a “gratitude visit” condition, “three good things in life” condition, “you at your best” condition, “using signature strengths in a new way” condition, and the “identifying Si gnature strengths” condition. The interventions were compared to a placebo condition of recalling and documenting early memories. In order to truly appreciate this prime example of positive psychology research and its impact on increasing happiness, it is important to further delve into the procedures, results, and conclusions. “The gratitude visit” intervention consisted of participants having a week to write and deliver a letter of gratitude to someone who deserved thanks. In “the three good things in life” intervention, participants identified and wrote three things that went well each day for a week. Participants were also asked to include a causal explanation for the good things that happened. Having participants identify their attributional process for explaining events is important as attributional style plays a Si gnificant role in the overall construction of subjective reality; this point will be further explicated later. The “you at your best” intervention required participants to reflect on personal strengths and write a story about an incident in which they used these personal strengths and were at their best. This intervention required participants to review their stories once a day and reflect on the identified strengths. A fourth intervention known as “using signature strengths in a 33 new way” required participants to take an online personality inventory at www.authentichappiness.org, a website dedicated to positive psychology assessment and study. The personality inventory identified the top five signature strengths of each participant. Participants were then to use these identified signature strengths in a new way. Another version of this intervention, the fifth intervention condition, was called “identifying Si gnature strengths;” this intervention required participants to take the personality inventory, have their signature strengths identified, and to use them more frequently in the following week. In the placebo-control condition, participants were asked to reflect on and write down early memories nightly for a week. It must be highlighted that all interventions lasted only a week and assessment of levels of happiness and depression were taken at pretest, immediate posttest, one week after posttest, one month after posttest, three months after posttest, and six months after posttest. Results demonstrated that all intervention conditions were effective in promoting happiness and/or attenuating depression at the immediate posttest assessment. Even the placebo-control condition was effective at immediate posttest assessment. The true effects of the different conditions started to manifest after the immediate posttest. The placebo-control condition at one week after posttest, and all assessments thereafter, Showed that happiness and depression levels did not Significantly differ from the pretest baseline. The significant effects (increased happiness and attenuated depression) at immediate posttest for the placebo-control condition (writing early memories) demonstrate the power of motivation and expectations for improvement. The “you at your best” and “identifying Signature strengths” conditions showed the same results as 34 the placebo-control condition, Significant effects that did not last beyond immediate posttest assessment. “The gratitude Visit” demonstrated the largest effects for increasing happiness and decreasing depression at the immediate posttest as compared to all other interventions; these beneficial effects tapered off until there was no significant difference compared to placebo-control at three months assessment. This particular intervention seems to immediately gratify as evidenced by the immediate posttest assessment, but this gratification apparently eventually fades. The two interventions that maintained effectiveness, the “three good things in life” and “using signature strengths in a new way,” did not Show truly beneficial effects until the one month assessment. These two interventions were the only ones to Show lasting beneficial effects at the three and six month assessments. Unlike the “gratitude visit,” these interventions did not involve immediate gratification but instead they seem to be learned skills that are self-reinforcing and longer lasting. The self-reinforcing aspects of these treatments caused participants to use these techniques for more than the prescribed week and thus experience lasting benefits at the six month assessment. The lasting effects of practice was evident even in the experiments performed by F ordyce (1977, 1983), who had also found that long-term practice of the exercises in his program had long-term effects in increasing happiness. The Seligman et a1. (2005) study shows that positive interventions can be effective in increasing happiness and ameliorating depression with the practicing of exercises that can be performed routinely. The “three good things in life” and “using signature strengths in a new way” in particular are exercises that require self-reflective cognitive skills that strengthen with practice and Show the longest lasting benefit. Interestingly, several of the common elements to effective therapy identified by Frank 35 and Frank (1991) seem to be present in these two modes of intervention. Frank and Frank identify the common elements of therapy to be: the therapeutic relationship, maintaining expectation for improvement, providing new learning experiences, the arousal of emotions in the therapeutic context, increasing client self-efficacy, and providing opportunities for practice of more adaptive behavior. Of these common therapeutic factors, expectations for improvement, increasing self-efficacy, the opportunity for practice, and providing new learning experiences seem to be present in the efficacious happiness exercises that Showed lasting effects. In fact, the common factor of providing new learning experiences may be what differentiates “using signature strengths in a new way” from “identifying Signature strengths,” an intervention that did not prove to have lasting effects. Another point of interest is the design of this study. Seligman et al. (2005) are able to make causal inferences between the interventions and happiness because the design of their study, a longitudinal field experiment, is considered scientifically rigorous as well as clinically relevant (Gelso & Fretz, 2001). The use of quantitative data and an experimentally rigorous design adds scholarly legitimacy to the idea of positive interventions having a clinical and applied value. Seligman et al. seem to be following suit with the respected empirically supported treatments movement, which attempts to validate what treatments are effective through experimental design and scientific rigor. Of particular note is the role of expectations in increasing happiness and decreasing depression, as demonstrated by beneficial effects in all conditions (including placebo-control) at immediate post-test. It should also be noted that the longer lasting interventions require the participant to make causal attributions for strengths (using 36 Si gnature strengths in a new way) and for positive events (three good things in life). Within the positive psychology paradigm, expectations and attributions for events play a large part in determining motivation and behavior (Carver & Scheier, 2003; Reivich & Gillham, 2003). Attributions & Explanatog Style When people experience good or bad events, there is a tendency to try to explain the cause of the events. This process of explaining is known as attribution. People make causal attributions in order to make sense of the events and the world around them (W eiten & Lloyd, 2006). According to Weiner (1979), people ask the question “why?” especially in the face of failure, rejection, and when facing the unexpected. Task success or failure may be attributed to ability, effort, difficulty, or luck; these factors can be classified along the three dimensions of stability, controllability, and locus of causality. These dimensions of attribution play a large part in determining further task motivation, mood, esteem, and other behavioral outcomes (W einer, 1979). The following will further explain and clarify the dimensions of causality. The social psychologist Fritz Heider (1958) was the first to posit that people look to themselves or outside of themselves as causes. When a person infers the cause of an event was because of factors pertaining to the self, they are making an internal attribution (i.e. I did poorly on the exam because I was tired; I did well on the test because I’m smart). In contrast, when an event is explained as due to factors other than the self (i.e. environment, chance, etc.), it is considered an external attribution (i.e. I did poorly on the exam because of too many distractions in the room; I did well on the test because it was easy). This dimension of causality was also identified by Rotter (1966) and called locus 37 Of control. A person could have an internal locus of control in which they identify personal factors as the cause of events or they may have an external locus of control in which they see external factors as the cause Of events. In contrast to Rotter, Weiner (1979) believes that the term locus of control really refers to locus of causality and that the terms control and causality should be separated. In other words, the locus of causality dimension determines whether events are explained as a function of the self (internal locus) or the environment (external locus), whereas the control dimension of attribution determines if the cause of an event is inferred to be controllable‘or uncontrollable. Thus, according to Weiner, control and causality are separate and independent dimensions of attribution. The concept of locus, or source of causality, is instrumental to counseling psychology in its application to cross-cultural counseling and the concept of client worldview (Gelso & F retz, 2001). F ouad and Bingham (1995) define worldview as “. . .the frame of reference through which one experiences life” (p.335). Sue and Sue (1990) define worldview as a person’s perception of their relationship with the world. This perception is determined by experiences with oppression, as well as social class, religion, and sex. The Sue and Sue model of worldview focuses on the dimensions of internal vs. external locus of responsibility and internal vs. external locus of control. According to the internal control — internal responsibility worldview, success is the result of one’s efforts and lack of success is due to one’s shortcomings. This worldview is held by the majority (White) culture and the three prominent therapeutic approaches in psychology (Psychodynamic, Behavioral, and Humanistic). The external control - internal responsibility worldview accepts the western definition of self-responsibility but 38 feels little control over consequences. The person holding this worldview rejects their culture and idealizes white culture. External control — external responsibility worldview feels helpless in the face of prejudice and exhibits passivity, apathy, and depression. Lastly, the person with the external responsibility — internal control worldview does feel control over their own life and is able to perceive the barriers of prejudice and discrimination. This individual is also likely to be very politically conscious (Sue & Sue, 1990). Appling these concepts to therapist effectiveness, being aware of one’s own worldview and knowing the client’s worldview are criteria of therapist multicultural competence. As can be seen, the intemal-extemal and controllable-uncontrollable dimensions of attribution are heuristically rich concepts in the psychological literature and especially pertinent in counseling psychology. Another dimension of explanatory style based on the writings of Heider refer to variability or temporal stability of events; events may be perceived as unchanging (stable) or changing (unstable). The stability or instability of an event is seen as a function of the stability or instability of factors pertinent to the event. Factors such as the influence of one’s family and the person’s ability would be considered relatively stable, whereas factors such as an individual’s attention and mood would be considered unstable. An important aspect of the stability dimension is its influence on expectations for events. A person who consistently fails at a task because of task difficulty (stable and relatively unchanging factor) would expect to fail the task on further attempts. In contrast, a person who fails because they were temporarily incapacitated because of alcohol or lack of sleep (unstable and changing factor) might have more optimistic expectations for success (W einer, 1979). Given the power of explanatory style and attribution in the 39 determination of mood, esteem, and behavior, attribution theory has been used to explain different types of mental pathology. One very prominent attribution theory that addresses the cognitive aspects of depression is the revised learned helplessness theory (RLHT) by Abrahamson, Seligman, and Teasdale (1978). According to RLHT, the dimensions of attribution that determine feelings of . helplessness consist of the internal vs. external dimensions, the stable vs. unstable dimensions, and a global vs. Specific dimension (Abrahamson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978). The intemal-external and stable-unstable dimensions are similar to what has been reviewed. The global versus Specific dimension of attribution refers to whether the explanation for the event can be generalized to other areas of life. According to Weiner (1979), whereas stability refers to temporal generalization (it will not change), globality refers to stimulus generalization (all things will be affected). The dimensions of attribution have implications for the valence of the attribution that is made. According to the RLHT model, explanatory styles can be classified as either optimistic or pessimistic. Optimistic attributions are related to increased motivation and vitality whereas pessimistic attributions are related to feelings of helplessness and depression (Peterson & Villanova, 1988). Somebody who employs an optimistic explanatory style considers negative events to be external (not personalizing), unstable (not lasting), and Specific to the situation. For the pessimist, negative events are seen as internal (personalizing), stable (lasting), and global (generalizing to all situations). For example, social rejection could be pessimistically explained as a result of personal qualities such as being unattractive or unlovable (internal locus); the expectation garnered may be that rejection by others will not change (stable), and will apply to other areas of 40 life as well (global). In contrast, being rejected could be optimistically attributed to external factors and not self-attributes (external); the rejection can be seen as an isolated incident and not lasting (unstable), as well as not generalizing to other situations (specific; Reivich & Gillham, 2003). A revision of the RLHT, called the hopelessness theory (HT), posits that the global and stable dimensions of attribution have a greater impact on explanatory style than the locus of causality dimension (internal vs. external dimension). Depression is the result of three beliefs. First, negative events are construed as global and stable. Also, negative events are magnified and understood as catastrophic. Finally, negative inferences are made about the self based on the negative events. More recent exploration into the RLHT has cast light upon the mediating affect of expectations on explanatory style in relation to depression. In other words, expectations in large part determine how much impact explanatory style may have on depressive symptoms. In line with hopelessness theory, whereas the stability and globality dimensions seem to influence expectations, the locus of causality dimension has less influence on expectations (Peterson & Vaidya, 2001). As can be seen, both attributional style and expectations have consequences for depressive mood and feelings of helplessness. The next section will further look at expectations and more specifically, two models that are tied to positive psychology, self-efficacy expectancies and the expectancy-value model. Expectation & Models of Expectancy Much like attribution, expectation is based on a well researched and theoretically rich literature. Noted psychologists such as Lewin (193 8), Tolman (1955), Edwards (1954), Rotter (1954, 1966), and Bandura (1977), as well as others have influential 41 theories of behavior and motivation that have a basis in expectations. What makes this cognitive phenomenon so well researched is probably the adaptive value it has in human self-regulation. To be able to learn from the past and to anticipate firture consequences are pertinent to goal setting and achievement (Maddux, 1999). Within the social-cognitive paradigm, Maddux (1999) identifies several types of expectancy variables. The behavior-outcome expectancy is defined as the expectation that certain behaviors will lead to certain outcomes. Maddux further differentiates two types of behavior-outcome expectancies: the behavior-stimulus expectancy and the behavior-response expectancy. The behavior-stimulus expectancy is the belief that a behavior with lead to an environmental event; the outcomes that result from a person’s behavior is an external event such as dropping a glass will lead to the expectation that it will shatter. The behavior-response expectancy is the belief that a behavior will lead to a non-volitional response, such as sexual arousal or pain (internal outcome). Another expectancy identified is the stimulus—outcome expectancy. Stimulus-outcome expectancies are beliefs that certain external events act as cues for the occurrence of other events. Stimulus-outcome expectancies can be differentiated into stimulus-stimulus expectancies and stimulus-response expectancies. Stimulus-stimulus expectancies are beliefs that the occurrence of an event indicates the occurrence of another event. For example, after seeing a bolt of lightening, one expects to hear the crash of thunder. The second kind of stimulus-outcome expectancy, known as stimulus—response expectancies are beliefs that the occurrence of an environmental event will result in a non-volitional response (internal outcome). Stimulus-outcome and behavior-outcome expectancies 42 differ from the third category of expectancy known as self-efficacy expectancies (Maddux, 1999). The self-efficacy expectancies (also known as self-efficacy judgments) are beliefs in one’s ability to coordinate Skills at a designated level of ability to achieve favorable outcomes, whereas behavior-outcome expectancies are the expected results of those outcomes. To have self-efficacy in a domain of action is to have generative capability rather than to simply have particular outcome expectations. These concepts are often confused to be the same despite being different (Maddux, 1999; Bandura, 1986). Bandura (1986) masterfully differentiates these concepts: Efficacy and outcome judgments are differentiated because individuals can believe that a particular course of action will produce certain outcomes, but they do not act on that outcome belief because they question whether they can actually execute the necessary activities. Thus, expectations that high grades gain students entry to medical school and that medical practice yields high incomes will not steer undergraduates into premedical programs who have serious self-doubts that they can master the science requirements (pp. 392). Within Bandura’s example, it can be seen that although self-efficacy expectancies and outcome expectancies are conceptually different they are not independent of each other. People’s actions and the outcomes that result fiom those actions depend on self- efficacy judgments. Self-efficacy judgments influence the expenditure of effort on a task, persistence in the face of failure, as well as task related thought patterns and emotional reactions. Self-efficacious perceptions regarding a task would promote enthusiastic engagement in the task, whereas perceptions of low efficacy lead to self-doubt and task 43 avoidance (Bandura, 1986). The agency enhancing quality is why increasing efficacy is a common factor in psychotherapeutic interventions (Frank & Frank, 1991); engagement in new adaptive behaviors promotes new adaptive learning whereas avoidance of such behaviors due to anxiety or self-doubt negates the opportunity for new adaptive learning (Watchel, 1993). Increasing self-efficacy seems to be a corner stone in most psychotherapeutic work, whether behavioral exposure-therapy or psychodynamic oriented modes of psychotherapy (although not conceptualized as increases in self- efficacy but instead as part of the general behavioral repertoire that accompanies the expansion of the ego). Increases in self-efficacy judgments are made through enactive attainment, vicarious learning, verbal persuasion, and physiological arousal (Bandura, 1986). Although originating from Bandura’s social-cognitive theory, self-efficacy is also considered to be a positive psychology variable given its value in promoting adaptive functioning and happiness (O’Brien, 2003). Closely related to the construct of efficacy is the positive psychology construct of optimism. Both efficacy and optimism speak to expectations in the form of a person’s confidence in achieving desirable outcomes. Although these constructs somewhat overlap, they address different types of expectations. Self-efficacy encompasses an element of personal control and responsibility for desirable outcomes; the causal agent is the self. Optimism entails a much broader sense of confidence in achieving desirable outcomes not solely due to the self; optimists would be confident in achieving favorable outcomes for a variety of reason. Although optimism extends beyond the self as the causal agent, favorable outcomes are contingent upon agency, continued pursuit of the goal, and one’s own efforts. The concepts of optimism and pessimism are associated with the expectancy-value model of motivation and behavior (Carver & Scheier, 2003). The expectancy-value model considers all behavior in terms of the pursuit of a goal. These expectancies are a kind of behavior-outcome expectancy in that one’s behavior leads to certain expected outcomes. The two parts of this model, expectancy and value, are both necessary for the initiation and maintenance of agency and motivation. The goal must be perceived as attainable (expectancy) as well as important (value) for action and motivation to persist. Valuing the goal increases motivation to attain the goal. Also, if expectations to attain the goal are pessimistic, goal related activity would be difficult to initiate no less maintain. Therefore, optimism and positive expectations are conceptualized as the engine of action and motivation (Carver & Scheier, 2003). Expectations also have an important role in psychotherapeutic inventions and therefore have relevance to mental health professionals of all therapeutic orientations (Weinberger & Eig, 1999). The vast majority of the psychotherapy outcome literature has demonstrated that it is the common factors in the psychotherapeutic effort (regardless of therapeutic orientation) that are responsible for therapeutic effects (Wampold, 2001; Frank & Frank, 1991). The psychotherapeutic common factors according to Frank have already been mentioned. Weinberger (1995) identified a list of common factors similar to that of Frank and Frank (1991) which includes: the therapeutic relationship, expectations for improvement, confronting the problem, cognitive control and mastery over the problem (analogous to self—efficacy), and attributions made for therapy outcome. Although the psychotherapy process and outcome literature is fraught with research on the therapeutic relationship, confronting problems, and mastery over problems, 45 expectations and attributions are the least researched common factors of psychotherapy (Weinberger & Ei g, 1999). In measuring attributions and expectations, the most utilized measures are the Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ; Seliman, Abramson, Semmel, & von Baeyer, 1979) and the Life Orientation Test-Revised (LOT-R; Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, 1994). Measuring Attributions and Expectations The ASQ (Seligman, et al., 1979) is one of the most utilized methods for assessing adult attributional style. Based on the RLHT, the ASQ is a self report questionnaire consisting of six negative and six positive hypothetical situations that are rated on the dimensions of locus of causality (internal vs. external), stability (stable vs. unstable), and globality (Specific vs. global). Thus, the ASQ measures explanatory style for positive as well as negative events. The open-ended nature of the items allows this measure to act as a kind of projective test. After reading the hypothetical event, subjects identify the causal factor of the event and rate it on the previously mentioned dimensions using a 7-point scale. The ASQ provides 6 dimension scores: the intemality index for the six negative events (IN), the globality index for the six negative events (GN), the stability index for the six negative events (SN), the intemality index for the six positive events (1P), the globality index for the six positive events (GP), and the stability index for the six positive events (SP). Each dimension’s index is the average of the ratings for the respective dimensions. The ASQ also yields three composite scores: the composite for a negative explanatory style (CN), the composite for a positive explanatory style (CP), and a total score composite which is the composite positive score minus the composite negative score (CP- CN). The CP is calculated by adding the intemality score (1P), the 46 globality score (GP), and the stability score (SP) for the Six positive events. The CN is calculated by adding the internality score (IN), the globality score (GN), and the stability score (SN) for the six negative events. In sum, the ASQ can yield both dimensional scores as well as composite scores in assessing explanatory style for both positive and negative events (Reivich & Gilliham, 2003). In an attempt to improve upon the reliability of the ASQ, two expanded versions of the ASQ (E-ASQ: Peterson & Villanova, 1988; E-ASQ: Metalsky, Halberstadt, & Abramson, 1987) have been created; the expanded versions of the ASQ include a greater number of negative events than the original ASQ (Reivich & Gilliham, 2003). Instructions and scoring of these versions are similar to that of the original ASQ (Peterson & Villanova, 1988). Optimism and pessimism are assessed through the assessment of expectations. To assess expectations, Scheier and Carver (1985) created the Life Orientation Test (LOT). Eight items directly assess expectations and filler items serve as distracters so that the assessment of expectations would not be obvious. Because of some problems with the psychometric properties of the LOT, Scheier, Carver, and Bridges (1994) reworked the measure to create the Life Orientation Test - Revised (LOT-R). Briefer than (consisting of 10 items with 4 items acting as filler items) but still Similar to the original measure, the LOT-R has more of a focus on expectancies for the future. Like the LOT, the LOT-R consists of items worded in both an optimistic and pessimistic direction. Items are scored on a 5-point Likert scale from “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree.” The distribution of scores is continuous and slightly skewed toward optimism. Although people are often thought of as two distinct dispositional sets of either optimists of pessimists, in reality the 47 majority of people fall between the extremes of very Optimistic and very pessimistic (Carver & Scheier, 2003). Aims and Objectives of Present Study This chapter has been a review of two psychological perspectives that are theoretically quite disparate, psychoanalytic theory and positive psychology. In relation to the field of counseling psychology, positive psychology has a natural connection with counseling psychology because of the mutual focus on human strengths. Psychoanalytic theory has changed with time and has evolved into a theory much more compatible with counseling psychology. Kohut’s self psychology, a theory that emphasizes the development of the self through empathic attunement from significant others in one’s life, is an illustration of the progressive changes in psychoanalytic theory as a theory of development, personality, and therapeutic change. Banai et al. were among the first to empirically relate the self psychological concept of self-object needs and self—cohesion to broader theoretical concepts of the self found in the social psychology literature such as: self-discrepancy (Higgins, 1987), self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1979), affect regulation (Mikulincer, 1994), and adult relational attachment style (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). The aim of the following study is two-fold. One goal is to further connect Kohut’s concepts to the positive psychology literature. The self psychological concepts of grandiosity (superiority) and goal inconsistency will be related with the concepts of attribution and expectation from the positive psychology literature. A better understanding of self-functioning can be garnered by exploring attributions and expectations within the self-psychological paradigm and this knowledge may ultimately have application to the therapy setting. Relating the functioning of the self to expectations 48 and attributions garners a new psychoanalytic paradigm for the development of optimistic expectations and attributions. In addition, this study explores the factor structure of the English-version of the SONI in order to verify the factor structure discovered by Banai et al. Replication. of the factor structure will serve as further evidence for the reliability of the SONI. 49 CHAPTER 3 — Method Participants Participants consisted of 200 undergraduate students from psychology and counseling and educational psychology classes at Michigan State University. This population was chosen to be sampled because of their availability. As a sample of convenience, generalizability of results to the population of college students and college counseling is limited. Of the 237 questionnaire packets distributed, 219 were returned. Of the questionnaire packets returned, 200 were able to be scored and 19 were not able to be scored because of too much missing data. Demographic information including sex, age, ethnicity, year of study, and sexual orientation was assessed through questionnaire format. The demographics sheet also assessed whether the participant was involved in a relationship, if the relationship involved sex relations (not defined), degree of satisfaction with the relationship, and the number of sexual partners the study participant has been involved with. Information concerning social and sexual relationships was used for descriptive purposes and not further analyzed. This information was meant to be a snapshot of the relational dynamics of the of the study participants. It was important to get this data regarding the ability of participants to engage in relationship and if these relationships are satisfying. Of the 200 students who returned completed and analyzable questionnaire packets, age ranged from 17 to 39 years (M = 21.01, S_D = 2.55). Female participants (p = 145) outnumbered male participants (p = 55) comprising 72.5% of total participants. Considering sexual orientation, 95.5% of participants identified as heterosexual (p = 50 191), 1.5 % identified as gay (p = 3), 1% identified as lesbian (p = 2); two percent of participants identified as bisexual (p = 4). Seventy six percent of participants also self-identified as White, European- American (p = 152), whereas 13% identified as Black, African-American (p = 26). The remainder of the sample consisted of 5.5% of participants that identified as Asian, Asian- American (p = 11), 2.5% identified as Hispanic-Latino (p = 5), 1% identified as Multiracial, Mixed Race (p = 2), 0.5% identified as Pacific Islander (n = 1), and 1.5% identified as an “Other” ethnicity (p = 3). None of the participants identified as Native- American. Table 1 displays the summary statistic for Ethnicity x Sex. Table l Cross-Tabulation of Summag Statistics for Participant Ethnicity x Sex . .53 Males Females Tog] Ethnicig Asian, Asian-American p = 04 07 11 Black, African-American p = 05 21 26 Hispanic, Latino p = 02 03 05 Pacific Islander p = 01 0 01 White European p = 42 110 152 Multiracial, Mixed Race p = 0 02 02 Other n = 01 02 03 Total 55 145 200 51 One hundred-twelve of the 200 participants reported that they were currently in a relationship whereas 88 participants reported not being in a relationship at the time of completing the questionnaire packet. Of the 117 participants who answered the item asking if they were sexually involved with their partner, 103 confirmed having a sexual relationship whereas 14 denied having a sexual relationship with their current partner. One hundred-two of the 103 participants who reported to be engaged in sexual activity also reported this to be in the context of a current relationship. Of the 14 students who denied being sexually involved, 10 were in a current relationship, whereas 4 were not in a relationship. Of 111 participants who answered the item asking whether they were satisfied with their relationship, 51.4 % confirmed that they were very satisfied (n = 57), 29.7% marked that they were satisfied (n = 33), and 16.2% marked that they were somewhat satisfied (11 = 18). One person reported that they were unsatisfied with their relationship (0.9%) and 2 people reported that they were very unsatisfied with their relationship (1.8%). The number of sexual partners reported by the 200 participants varied; the majority of participants (61.5%) reported having from 1 to 5 sexual partners. Another 15.5% of participants reported never having a sexual partner. Twelve percent reported of having 6 to 10 sexual partners, 8% reported having 11 to 15 sexual partners, and 1% reported having 16 to 20 sexual partners. Interestingly, 2% reported to having 20 or more sexual partners. Demographic information was collected for assessing sample characteristics. The main hypotheses of the study do not involve the demographic information and instead concentrate on the self psychological variables of grandiosity, goal instability, and self- 52 object needs, as well as the positive psychology variables of attributions for negative events and general expectations. Measures Participant attributions for negative events are assessed through the Expanded Attributional Style Questionnaire (E-ASQ; Peterson & Villanova, 1988). This measure consists of 24 hypothetical negative events. Participants identify the perceived cause of the negative event and identify the dimensions of intemality, stability, and globality, of their attribution on a 7—point scale. Attributions are scored as internal (7) versus external (1), stable (7) versus unstable (1), and global (7) versus specific (1). Internal consistency is estimated to be fair to good; Cronbaeh’s alpha is estimated at .66 for intemality, .85 for stability, and .88 for globality (Peterson & Villinova, 1988). Expectations are assessed through the Life Orientation Test Revised (LOT-R; Scheier et al., 1994). This measure consists of 10-items scored on a 5-point likert scale ranging from “strongly disagree” (1) to “strongly agree” (5). Four of the items are filler items placed for the purpose of not making the assessment of expectations obvious. Items include “If something can go wrong for me it will” (scored in reverse), “In uncertain times, I usually expect the best,” and “I’m always optimistic about my firture.” According to Carver and Scheier (2003) the LOT-R is internally consistent as Cronbach’s alpha is estimated at the high .705 to the low .805. Self-object needs are assessed using the Self Object Needs Inventory (SONI; Banai et al., 2005). This 38-item measure assesses the interpersonal patterns that are related to self-psychological deficits in development. The specific interpersonal patterns 53 are a reflection of the degree of cohesiveness in the self. Factor analysis of this measure found five independent factors that manifest as either a hunger or a rejection of the interpersonal dynamics that were originally amiss in the individual’s development (Banai et al., 2005). The five factors are: need for mirroring, need for idealization, need for twinship, avoidance of mirroring, and avoidance of idealization/twinship. Cronbach’s alpha for the 8-itemS assessing the hunger for twinship was estimated to be .91. Examples from this scale include: “I feel better when I and someone close to me Share Similar feelings toward other people,” and “It is important to me to feel that a close friend and I are ‘in the same boat.’” Cronbach’s alpha for the 11-items assessing the avoidance of idealization/twinship was estimated to be .83. Examples from this scale include: “I would rather not belong to a group of people whose lifestyle is similar to mine,” and “I find it difficult to accept guidance even from people I respect.” The 7-items assessing the need for idealization had an estimated Cronbach’s alpha of .83. Examples of questions from this scale include: “1 am attracted to successful people,” and “I feel better about myself when I am in the company of experts.” The 8-items assessing the hunger for mirroring were estimated as having a Cronbach’s alpha of .81. Examples of items from this scale include: “I do not function well in situations where I receive too little attention,” and “I feel hurt when my achievements are not sufficiently admired.” The last factor garnered from the SONI was the avoidance of mirroring. The 6-items that assessed this factor had an estimated Cronbach’s alpha of .79. Examples of items from this scale include: “I do not really care what others think about me,” and “I do not need support and encouragement from others.” Degree of participant agreement with items on the SONI is rated on a 7-point scale ranging from “not at all” (1) to “very much” (7). 54 Although the SONI and its 5-factors were assessed to be valid and reliable in seven independent studies, Since the present study is using an English translation, it iS feared that some items may be somewhat poorly worded or confusing. Items of particular concern are item 6 (I would just not be involved with people who suffer from problems similar to me), item 20 (I would rather not be friends with people who are too similar to me), and item 21 (I feel better when I and someone close to me Share similar feelings to other people). Also, the 7-point response scale with the anchors of “Not at all” (1) and “Very much” (7) seem confusing with negatively worded items (e. g. I don’t need other people’s praise). Through collaboration with one of the authors of the SONI (Mario Mikulincer), changes were made in the wording of item 6 (I would prefer not to be involved with people who suffer from problems similar to mine), item 20 (I would rather not have friends who are too similar to me), and item 21 (I feel better when I and someone close to me share similar feelings). The anchors of the response scale were also changed to “Disagree” (1) and “Agree” (7) given that the original anchors of the response scale were attempting to assess degree of agreement with the items. All changes were approved by the original author of the SONI. Changes made were not meant to question the wording of the original measure (written in Hebrew), but to clarify the measure’s English translation. Participant lack of realistic and cohesive goals is measured through the Goal Inconsistency Scale and grandiosity is assessed with the Superiority Scale (Robbins & Patton, 1985). These two scales attempt to assess the “self-expressive trends” (pp. 222) posited to be a function of the two poles of development in the self-psychological model 55 (Robbins & Patton, 1985). Items from the Superiority Scale included: “My friends follow my lead,” “My looks are one of the things that attract others to me,” and “I catch myself wanting to be the hero.” Cronbach’s alpha was estimated at .81. Items from the Goal Inconsistency Scale included: “It’s easier for me to start than to finish projects,” “I have more ideas than energy,” and “I have confusion about who I am.” Cronbach’s alpha for the Goal Inconsistency Scale was estimated at .76. Superiority and goal inconsistency are measured on a 6-point scale ranging from strongly agree (1), moderately agree (2), slightly agree (3), slightly disagree (4), moderately disagree (5), and strongly disagree (6). The higher scores on each scale the less agreement with the particular construct of superiority or goal inconsistency. The Superiority Scale measures a sense of arrogance and perceived superiority to others that would be a function of an immature grandiosity line of development. The Goal Inconsistency Scale measures lack of self-direction and ideals that would accompany under-development in the idealization vector of the bipolar self (Robbins & Patton, 1985). Procedures The sample for the study was obtained through advertisement in psychology and counseling and educational psychology classes by the lead researcher during the spring- terrn of 2007 at Michigan State University. The lead researcher approached class instructors personally or asked permission to sample students through email and telephone. Students heard about the study from the lead researcher when visiting classes. Originally, participants were invited to a designated room, on a designated day, in order to take part in the study. Asking undergraduate students to meet in a designated location did not yield an adequate number of participants needed to complete sampling. 56 For the convenience of participants, students who were interested in participating in the study were given the consent form (Appendix A) in the class they were being sampled from. After giving consent, consent forms were collected in an envelope and participants received the questionnaire packet (Appendix C); the questionnaire packet consisted of the Expanded Attributional Style Questionnaire (E-ASQ; Peterson & Villanova, 1988), the Life Orientation Test (LOT-R; Scheier et al., 1994), the Self Object Needs Inventory (SONI; Banai et al., 2005), and the Superiority and Goal Instability Scales (Robbins & Patton, 1985) as well as a demographics questionnaire (Appendix B) presented in random order. Participants were allowed to complete questionnaire packets at their own convenience. Classes were Visited within the Span of a day to a week to collect completed surveys. Upon turning in completed questionnaires, participants wrote their email addresses on a numbered ticket that was used in a random drawing for $ 100. Participants were also provided the debriefing form (Appendix D) explaining the purpose of the study. If a participant chose not to complete their questionnaire packet, they were handed their ticket and their questionnaire packet was destroyed. The debriefing form contained the lead researcher’s email address for participants to contact in case of further questions about the study. Participants were included in a lottery drawing for a $100 grand prize. Participants provided informed consent by reading and signing the informed consent form (Appendix A). Analysis The main analysis consisted of a canonical correlation between the self psychological measures and positive psychology measures. The self-psychology measures consisted of the Superiority Scale and the Goal Inconsistency Scale, and the 57 positive psychology measures consisted of the LOT-R and the Expanded ASQ. The self- psychological variables of grandiosity and idealization served as predictor variables of the positive psychology variables of expectations and attributions. A second and separate analysis was conducted concerning the factor structure of the SONI. The purpose of the second analysis was to examine the factor structure of the SONI and find out whether its structure resembles the 5-factor structure found by the creator of the measure, Banai et al., 2005. The creators of the SONI found that the factor structure of the measure reliably separated into the interpersonal rejection or the need for the self psychological variables of twinship, mirroring, and idealization. Specifically, the factor structure found was: (1) the hunger for mirroring, (2) the hunger for idealization, (2) the hunger for twinship, (3) the rejection of mirroring, and (5) the rejection of idealization/twinship. Banai et al. found this S-factor structure in seven independent studies performed with the student population at Bar-[Ian University in Israel. The reason to see if the factor structure of the SONI could be replicated is to find if the English version of the measure shares similar psychometric qualities as the Hebrew version used by Banai et al. In addition, replication of Banai et al.’s results would be an important next step in further validating this psychodynamic measure. 58 CHAPTER 4 — Results Research Question #1 Canonical correlation analysis was performed between two sets of variables. The first included the self psychology variables of grandiosity (as measured by the Superiority Scale) and goal-instability and the second set consisted of the positive psychology variables of intemality, stability, and globality of attributions as well as general expectations. The analysis contained 200 hundred cases of which there was no missing data. All variable distributions met the criteria for normality set forth by Huck (2004): kurtosis between ranges of —1 and 2 and skewness between -1 and 1. Given the distributions have met the criteria for univariate normality, the likelihood of multivariate normality is high (Tabachnick & F idell, 2007). Table 2 provides the descriptive statistics for the variables under analysis. Table 2. Descriptive statistics of variables analyzed by canonical correlation Descriptive Statistics N Min I Max Mean Std. Dev. Skew Kurt. Stat Stat I Stat Stat Stat Stat Std. E. Stat Std. E. lntemality 200 2.79 6.54 4.5754 .57989 -.240 .172 .646 .342 Stability 200 1 .25 6.17 4.3546 .73524 c.476 .172 1.043 .342 Generality 200 1 .83 6.17 4.0420 .88212 -.080 .172 .035 .342 Superior 200 1.5 4.8 3.186 .6731 -.153 .172 -.097 .342 Goal Ins. 200 2.0 6.0 4.127 .8816 -.021 .172 -.750 .342 Expect 200 1 .67 5.00 3.4324 .74783 -.376 .172 -.265 .342 alid N 200 59 Table 3 shows the bivariate intercorrelation matrix for all variables used in the canonical correlation analysis. The intercorrelation matrix of variables shows that most variables are moderately correlated both across variable sets and within variable sets. The correlation between stability and globality (r = .605) is sizable, but this correlation is below .70 so problems with multicolinearity are avoided (Tabachnick & F idell, 2007). It should be noted that negative indices for scores on intemality, stability, and globality are because of scaling of measures. Higher scores on the E-ASQ are indicative of more dysfunctional thinking whereas lower scores on the superiority and goal instability scales are indicative of higher levels of narcissism and goal instability. Table 3 Bivariate intercorrelation matrix of variables used in canonical correlation analysis. Correlations lntemality Stability Generality Superior Goal Ins. Expect lntemality Pearson Corr. 1 .217" .254” .042 -.l64' -.003 Sig. (2-tailed) .002 .000 .558 .021 .969 N 200 200 200 200 200 200 Stability Pearson Corr. .217” 1 .605” -.066 -341” -284" Sig. (2-tai1ed) .002 .000 .356 .000 .000: N 200 200 200 200 200 200 Generality Pearson Corr. .254“ .605" l -.066 -353“ -.178' Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .000 .355 .000 .012 N 200 200 200 200 200 200 Superior Pearson Corr. .042 -.066 -.066 l .107 -.042 Sig. (2-tailed) .558 .356 .355 .132 .552 N 200 200 200 200 200 200 IGoallns. Pearson Corr. -.164° -.341” -.353" .107 l .363" Sig. (2-tailed) .021 .000 .000 .132 .000 N 200 200 200 200 200 200 Expect Pearson Corr. -.003 -284" -.178' -.042 .363“ 1 Sig. (2-tailed) .969 .000 .012 .552 .000 N 200 200 200 200 200 200 **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). *. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed). 60 Multivariate analysis of the two variable sets Shows statistical significance, with a Wilk’s lambda (A) of 0.754, F (8, 390) = 7.337, p < .000. Thus, the null hypothesis that no relationship exists between variable sets was rejected (Rc 96 0). Examining the effect Size for the full model, an index of 1- A = .246 accounts for the Shared proportion of variance between variables sets across all canonical functions, this is indicative of a medium-sized effect. Canonical analysis of the two variable sets produced two canonical functions. The first canonical fiinction (Rcl) had a moderate correlation coefficient of .485 and was statistically Significant, F (8, 388) = 7.337, p < .000. The coefficient of determination for the first canonical function was a moderate .235, thus accounting for almost as much shared variance between the two sets of variables as when testing for the full model. The second canonical function was not significant, Rc2 = .118, F (3, 195) = .917, p<.433, and will not be further interpreted. The first canonical function accounts for 95.6% of the variance in the relationship between the two variable sets. The analysis seems to have found moderate effects (RC2 = .235) in the relationship between the self psychology and positive psychology variables sets. The next step in the interpretation of results is identifying what variables were most responsible for the effects that were found. Finding the variables that contributed most to the canonical relationship can be accomplished through examination of canonical function coefficients and structure coefficients of both dependant and independent variable sets. Table 4 shows the standardized canonical function coefficients and structure coefficients across the first canonical function as well as overall effect size and redundancy coefficients for both canonical variates. 61 Table 4. Canonical Solution for the self psychology variables predicting the positive psychology variables across the first function. Function 1 Variable Coef rs rsz lntemality -. 189 -.347 12.04% Stability -.220 -.696 48.44% Globality -.428 -.720 51.84% Expectations .622 .761 57.91% Rdl 10.00% Rc2 23.50% Rd2 1 1.65% Grandiosity -.O90 .017 0.02% Goal Instability 1.006 .996 99.20% Note. Coef. = canonical function coefficients; rs = standardized structure coefficients; rs 2 = percent of variance explained; Rdl: redundancy coefficient for dependent variables; R02 = canonical correlation squared; Rd2 = redundancy coefficient for independent variables. Goal instability loads heavily (1.006) and correlates strongly (rs = .996) with the first canonical function. In contrast, grandiosity loads (-0.090) and correlates (.017) minimally with the first canonical function. Clearly, goal instability and the first function are measuring the same dimensions as they share 99.2% variance. The variate that is a 62 linear combination of grandiosity and goal instability also extracts 11.65% variance from the dependent variable set as Shown by the redundancy coefficient in Table 3. Considering the other side of the canonical equation, stability (-.220, rs = .696) and globality (-.428, rs = -.720) of attributions as well as general expectations (.622, rs = .761) load and show sizable correlations with the first canonical function. lntemality (-. 189, rs = -.347) loads and correlates more moderately with the first canonical function. The variate that is a linear combination of the dependent variables also accounts for 10% of the variance from the independent variable set. Univariate F -tests are significant for the first canonical function and intemality (F (2, 197) = 3.08, p < .048), stability (F (2, 197) = 13.11, p < .000, globality (F (2, 197) = 14.11, p < .000, and expectations (F (2, 197) = 15.81, p < .000. Hypotheses pertaining to the canonical correlation between the self psychological variables and the positive psychology variables include: H01: There is no significant expected canonical relationship between the two data sets (Rc = O). H 1: Grandiosity will be significantly related to an external attributional style for negative events. H02: There is no significant relationship expected between attributional style for negative events and goal instability. H03: There is no significant relationship expected between expectations for life events and grandiosity. H04: There is no significant relationship expected between expectations for life events and goal instability. 63 Multivariate analysis of canonical functions was able to reject the first null hypothesis (H01: Rc = 0), Wilk’s lambda (A) of 0.754, F (8, 390) = 7.337, p < .000. H1 could not be confirmed and H03 could not be rejected due to grandiosity’s very low loading (-.090) and correlation (rs = .017) to the first canonical function, Sharing only 0.02% variance. In other words, there were no significant relationships between grandiosity and any of the variables in the positive psychology data set. In contrast, due to the high loading (1.006) and correlation (rs = .996) of goal instability with the first canonical firnction, H02 and H04 were rejected; there seems to be a moderate relationship between goal instability and both attributions and expectations. Goal instability seemed tantamount to the first canonical function, sharing 99.2% variance. The significant F-tests between the individual variables in the positive psychology data set (all at p <.000 level of significance with the exception of intemality which was at p < .048) and the first canonical function serves as further evidence of the significance of the goal instability variable. In fact, the effect size of the first canonical function (RC2 = .235) is more reflective of variance accounted between data sets than the redundancy coefficient of the predictor variable set (Rd2 = .1165). Because the redundancy coefficient is the product of the adequacy coefficient (the mean of structure coefficient effect size; rs 2 '1' r52 / 2) and the effect size of the canonical correlation for the first function (R02); the structure coefficient for grandiosity seems to be suppressing the effect Size of the redundancy coefficient for the predictor variable data set. In other words, goal instability by itself is a better predictor of the dependent variable data set than when paired with superiority. Evidence of the importance of goal stability can be 64 seen when the predictor variables are regressed on each dependent variable. Table 5 presents the results of the regression analysis of the independent variables regressed on each dependent variable. Table 5 Regression analysis of independent variables to each dependent variable Dependent Variable — Stability Ind. Var. B beta Std. Error t-value Sig. oft Superiority -.0321 1 -.02940 .074 -.437 .663 Goal Ins -.28219 -.33836 .056 -5.026 .000 Dependent Variable — Globality Ind. Var. B beta Std. Error t-value Sig. oft Superiority -.03724 -.02842 .088 -.424 .672 Goal Ins. —.35001 -.34979 .067 -5.587 .000 Dependent Variable — Expectation Ind. Var. B beta Std. Error t-varlue Sig. oft Superiority -.09107 -.08 197 .074 -1.232 .219 Goal Ins .31527 .37165 .056 5.587 .000 Dependent Variable — lntemality Ind. Var. B beta Std. Error t-value Sigof t Superiority .05152 .05981 .061 .848 .398 Goal Ins -.11179 -. 16995 .046 -2.408 .017 65 From the regression analysis it is evident that a statistically Significant relationship (p < .000) exists between goal instability and stability and globality of attributions as well as general expectations. Goal instability also has a significant relationship with intemality (t = -2.408, p < .017) but seemingly with less of an effect when compared to the other dependent variables. Implications of results are discussed in the next chapter. 66 Research Question #2 A confirmatory factor analysis was performed on the SONI to see if the factor structure of this measure could be replicated. The specified 5-factor model consisted of approach orientations to twinship, idealization, and mirroring, as well as avoidance orientations to mirroring and idealization/twinship was used as the model for best fit in the analysis of the 38 items of the SONI. The analysis consisted of 200 participants with one case deleted (11 = 199) because of missing data. It Should be noted that the difference in the sample sizes of the first study (n = 200) compared to the second study (n = 199) is because the SONI was not used in the first study and therefore there was no deletion of cases in the first Study. Table 6 shows the factor structure found by Banai et al. Table 6 Factor structure used as model of fit in confirmatory factor analysis . Fagp; Interpersonal Dynamic It_erp 1 Hunger for Twinship 27, 14, 22, 32, 12, 21, 8, 2 2 Avoidance of 20, 38, 16, 28, 9, 6, 3, 34, 13, Idealization / Twinship 25, 30 3 Hunger for Idealization 17, 19, 36, 26, 4, 31, 10 4 Hunger for Mirroring ll, 7, 1, 29, 33, 35 5 Avoidance of Mirroring 23, 37, 24, 15, 18, 5 Sample size (n = 199) is sufficient for analysis of the 38-item questionnaire (Grimm & Yamold, 1995). The univariate distribution of all but 2 items (items #16 and #34) fit the criterion of skew (-1 to I) and kurtosis (-1 to 2) indicative of distribution 67 normality (Huck, 2004). Again, with the determination of univariate normality, probability of multivariate normality is high (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Figure 1 illustrates the positively skewed distributions for items 16 and 34. Figure 1 Histo h of distribution of scores for items #16 and #34 with normal curve overla . Count Count Skew = 1.27 Skew = 1.25 Kurtosis = 1.96 Kurtosis = 1.45 Note: x-axis is 1 — 7 likert scale of SONI; y—axis is frequency count of the item scores. In determining model fit, chi-square test of goodness of fit was significant, X2 (655) = 1,461.922, p<.0000, indicating that fitted residuals generated by the 5-factor model are significantly greater than zero and that the model generated by the data is different. Since sample size greatly increases probability of chi-square significance, another indicator of fit is the ratio of the chi-square index to the degrees of freedom and it’s proximity to zero (Grimm & Yamold, 1995). Ratio of xz/df calculates to an index of 2.23, which indicates that although not a matching fit, the 5—factor model approaches 68 being a relatively congruent fit (M. Reckase, personal communication, February 5, 2008). Another indicator of poor fit is that the normed comparative fit index (CF I) is .656 and not close to 1.00 thus indicating the null model of no common factors cannot be rejected. The TLI (Tucker-Lewis Index; another index of goodness of fit) is .631, thus Showing that not enough common variance can be accounted for (in relation to total variance) by the 5-factor model. Finally, the root mean square residual (RMSR) is equal to .099 which Shows that the average fitted residuals for the 5-factor model is sizable and not close enough to zero, another indicator of poor model fit. Table 7 shows the different tests of fit for the 5-factor model. Table 7 Tests of fit for the 5-factor model lest 1mg gLf 2-v_ah1_e X2 1,461.922 655 0.000 xz/df 2.23 CFI .656 TLI .631 RMSR .099 Given the different indices of fit, it seems that the 5-factor model specified by Banai et al. (2005) is not a very congruent fit given the data. The fifth hypothesis (H5) considering the fit of the 5-factor model was stated as: 69 H5: The factor structure of the Self-Object Needs Inventory consists of 5 factors: hunger for mirroring, hunger for idealization, hunger for twinship, avoidance of mirroring, and the avoidance of idealization/twinship. This hypothesis cannot be confirmed given the results of the confirmatory factor analysis. Whereas the results of the confirmatory analysis did not replicate the results of Banai et al.’s (2005) 5-factor model, the factor structure obtained in the present study does seem comparable to the 5-factor model given the ratio of the chi-square index to the degrees of freedom (xz/df = 2.23). Table 8 displays the intercorrelation matrix between factors given the 5-factor model. Table 8 Intercorrelations of factors fitting data to the 5-factor model Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 5 Factor 1 1.00 Factor 2 -0.019 1.00 Factor 3 0.391 0.011 1.00 Factor 4 0.306 0.097 0.250 1.00 Factor 5 -0.260 -0.023 -0.228 -0.547 1.00 Given the mixed results of fitting the data to the 5-factor model, a post-hoc exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with principle components extraction was performed on the data to examine what model best fits the data. EFA reduced the 38 variables to 10 factors. Varimax rotation was performed on the factor matrix in order to make factors more interpretable. Table 9 Shows how items loaded on respective factors. 70 Table 9 Item loadings of principle components analysis with varimax rciation Rotated Component Matrix' Com onent 5 6 10 Q37 Q24 Q5 Q33 Q15 Q23 Q29 Q9 Q3 Q30 Q13 Q31 Q26 Q4 Q17 Q12 Q8 Q2 Q1 Q7 Q18 Q21 Q22 Q32 Q16 Q28 Q34 Q38 Q6 Q14 Q27 .801 .724 .610 -.578 .569 .569 -.497 .457 .823 .822 .666 .645 .671 .670 .542 .534 .498 .497 .425 .814 .736 -.482 .737 .705 .620 71 .657 .645 .577 .522 .521 .418 .781 .623 .436 Table 9 Cotinued 1010 .778 Q36 .673 919 .484 Q1 1 Q20 .775 Q25 .503 35 .671 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. a. Rotation converged in 16 iterations. As evident by the scree test in Figure 2 below, 10 factors can be retained until eigenvalues drop below 1.00. Factor rotation spread item loadings across factors and made seven of these factors more easily definable. 72 Figure 2 Scree plot Showing number of factors in relation to eigenvalues. Scree Plot Eigenvalue 2— 0-1 11 TITIIIIIIIIqIII 13 5 7 91’113151719212325272931333537 Component Number Note: Scree plot displays original components matrix before varimax rotation. Several items loaded on multiple factors, in particular, the absolute value of the factor loadings for Item 18 were similar on both factor 1 (.457; avoidance of mirroring) and factor 4 (-.482; hunger for mirroring), loading positively and negatively on respectively Opposite factors. What follows is examination of factors and the items which had the highest loadings on those factors. Seven items loaded on the first factor; items which loaded highly on this factor included “I don’t need support and encouragement from others” (801),” I know that I’m 73 successful, so I have no need for others’ feedback” (.724), and “I don’t need other people’s praise” (.610). The first factor could be interpreted as an avoidance of mirroring factor. In fact, many of the items that loaded on this factor were the same as items which loaded on the avoidance Of mirroring in the original study by Banai et al. Four items loaded on factor 2; items which loaded highly on this factor include “When I have a problem, it’s difficult to accept advice even from experienced people” (.822), “I find it difficult to accept guidance even from people I respect” (.823), and “When I’m worried or distressed, getting advice from experts doesn’t help much” (.666). The second factor could be interpreted as an avoidance of idealization factor. Again, many of the same items loaded on this factor as in the original study; one noticeable difference is that this study was able to garner a pure avoidance of idealization factor, whereas the Banai et al. study could not differentiate between items that measured avoidance of idealization from those items that measured avoidance of twinship, therefore having to present the factor as a combined avoidance of idealization/twinship factor. Whereas Banai et al. found a combined avoidance of idealizatiOn/twinship factor, Factor 3 of the present study combined items measuring hunger for idealization and twinship. Seven items loaded on factor 3; items included “It’s important for me to be around people who can serve as my role models” (.670), “Associating with successful people allows me to feel successful as well” (.542), and “I feel good knowing that I’m part of a group of people who share a particular lifestyle” (.498). Factor 4 could be interpreted as a hunger for mirroring factor as items that loaded clearly measured narcissistic needs. Only three items loaded on Factor 4; these items 74 consisted of “I feel hurt when my achievements are not sufficiently admired” (.814), “I’m disappointed when my work is not appreciated” (.736), and “I have no need to boast about my achievements” (-.482), which loaded fairly highly but negatively. Three items also loaded on factor 5 which could be interpreted as a hunger for twinship factor. It is not known what differentiates this factor from factor 3, the mixed hunger for twinship and idealization factor. The items on this factor included “I feel better when I and someone close to me Share similar feelings to other people” (.737), “It’s important for me to be part of a group who share similar opinions” (.705), and “I gain self-confidence from having friends whose beliefs are similar to mine” (.620). Again, this smaller set of items was among the items that loaded on the hunger for twinship factor in the Banai et a1. study. Factor 6 could be interpreted as an avoidance of twinship factor. The five items that loaded on this factor included “It bothers me to be in close relationships with people who are similar to me” (.657), “It’s difficult for me to belong to a group of people who are dealing with similar problems” (.645), “I find it difficult to be proud of the groups I belong to” (.5 77). 1 Factor 7 seemed to be tapping into a “misery loves company” aspect of the twinship dynamic. Items that loaded on this factor included, “It’s important for me to feel that a close friend and I are ‘in the same boat” (.781) and “I feel stronger when I have people around who are dealing with similar problems” (.623). Item #2 (“It’s important for me to be around other people who are in the same situation as me”) also loaded on this factor (.418), but loaded higher on factor 3 (.425). 75 Factor 8 could be interpreted as a hunger for idealization factor; the three items that loaded on this factor included “I identify with famous people” (.778), “I feel better about myself when I am in the company of experts” (.484), and “It’s important for me to belong to hi gh-status, “glamorous” social groups” (.673). It is not known what differentiates this factor from factor 3 which was the mixed factor of hunger for idealization and twinship. Factor 9 could be interpreted as another avoidance of twinship factor, but unlike factor 6, only two items loaded on this factor. The items that loaded included “I would rather have friends that are different from me rather than ones who are very Similar” (.775) and “I’m bored by people who think and feel too much like me” (.503). Again, it is uncertain what differentiates this factor from factor 6. Factor 10 is a strange factor; it was extracted because of an eigenvalue over 1.00 but no items loaded on this factor before varimax rotation. The two items that loaded on this factor are “Most of the time I feel like I’m not getting enough recognition from my superiors” (.671) and “I need a lot of support from others” (.463), an item which also loaded negatively (-.578) on factor 1, the avoidance of mirroring factor. Factor 10 seems to be drawing from a need for mirroring dynamic. It should be mentioned that Item 11 (“I don’t function well in situations where I receive too little attention”), an item that seems to be measuring hunger for mirroring, could not load on any factors after varimax rotation. Table 9 displays all factors and the items associated with each factor. 76 Table 10 Factor structure found in the present study through exploratory factor analysis . Factor 1 2 10 (7) Interpersonal Dynamic Avoidance of Mirroring Avoidance of Idealization Hunger for Idealization [Twinship Hunger for Mirroring Hunger for Twinship Avoidance of Twinship Hunger for Twinship Hunger for Idealization Avoidance of Twinship Hunger for Mirroring Hunger for Mirroring m 5,15,23,24,29,33,37 3, 9, 13, 30 2, 4, 8,12,17, 26, 31 l, 7, 18 21, 22, 32 6, 16, 28, 34, 38 14, 27 10,19, 36 20, 25 35 ll Note: Items organized according to highest loadings on the factor Appendix E also displays each item, the factor loadings, and the associated factors. The way that most items correlated makes intuitive sense. Factors seemed to have differentiated subdimensions which may be functions of language and culture, the discerning difference between the original population of Israeli students from the Banai et al. study and the American-Midwestem population from this study. Also, extracted factors after the first 5 factors may be highly unreliable and unstable due to the study’s tenuous sample size. As Shown by the scree plot (Figure 2), eigenvalues sharply drop after the first 5 extracted factors. Implications of the results for confirmatory and exploratory factor analyses will be discussed in the next chapter. 77 CHAPTER 5 — Discussion Research Question #1 Results of the canonical correlation relating the self psychological variables of grandiosity and goal instability with the positive psychology variables of expectations and attributions demonstrated that a Significant relationship existed between the two variable sets. The canonical analysis showed a moderate effect with the first canonical function being tantamount to the self psychology variable of goal instability. In contrast, grandiosity was not relevant or related (bivariately or multivariately) to any of the variables in the current study. The study’s first clinical hypothesis, stating that participants high in narcissism would tend to attribute negative events to external causes, could not be confirmed. This first clinical hypothesis (H1) was based on the narcissistic self-regulatory model (Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001) where intra-personal processes protect implicit vulnerabilities and the explicit grandiose self. It was hypothesized that these intra-personal processes would have narcissistic participants extemalize attributions for negative events relative to participants lower in narcissism. Given the results, the narcissistic self-regulatory model (Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001) may not have been relevant because participants were sampled from a university population where narcissism is not likely to be extreme. Although students in the general student population may also have extreme narcissistic needs, their characteristics are likely to be counterbalanced by more mature students sampled fiom the student population. Perhaps if this study was carried out on a clinical population, grandiosity would be a more relevant variable. Probably the true relationship between narcissism and expectations and attributions is multi- dimensional and more subtle than effects found through simple pencil and paper 78 measures. It must be again noted that the redundancy coefficient of the independent variable set is deceptive (Rd2 = 11.65 %) because the grandiosity variable dilutes the relational effect size of the independent variable set. A more accurate accounting of effect Size can be seen in the canonical correlation index (RC2 = 23.5 %) which represents dimensions of the self measured by the goal-instability scale. Thus, goal-instability was the self-psychological variable which related best with the dependent variable set and was the index of self-cohesiveness in the study’s sample. These findings indicate that the ability to realistically appraise talents and skills, have stable goals, and take action toward the completion of goals (description of a cohesive self) correspond with optimistic expectations and attributional explanations for events. Not only do these results make intuitive sense, but are congruent with the literature in social learning theory and self-efficacy (Bandura, 1977, 1986), information processing models of behavior (Beck, 1976, 1995), and the learned helplessness model of depression (Seligman, 1979; Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978). These results also speak to relationally based common factors approaches in counseling and psychotherapy that focus on growth of the self. According to most theories of counseling and psychotherapy, it is the relationship between the therapist and client that allows further growth. Specific to self-psychological theory, idealized self- objects have a soothing quality in the face of fi'ustration, thus, it follows that optimism would be related to the idealization vector of the bipolar-self in the development of a cohesive self. Of all the variables in the positive psychology data set, the least significant effects were seen with the intemality dimension of attributional explanatory style, sharing only 79 12.04% variance with the first canonical function. lntemality is the dimension of attribution in which the person sees themselves as the primary cause of the event. According to Peterson and Villinova (1988), intemality is “the least coherent dimension” (pg. 88) of attributional style measured by the ASQ. The authors posit the difficulty with this variable’s coherence may be due to the multidimensionality of the intemality construct. The intemality dimension is interpreted as self-blame (when attributing for - negative events) as it has been shown to relate to a depressive attributional style and loss of self-esteem (Seligman et al., 1979). Offering a different perspective, a strong and cohesive sense of self may attribute internally out of a sense of responsibility, a virtuous characteristic which people who can self-soothe (a self-function that requires development of the idealization vector of the bipolar self) have the capacity to accomplish with no resulting loss in esteem. Whereas the majority of people may attribute negative events internally out of self-condemnation, others may attribute internally from a standpoint of responsibility. It may be that participants internally attributed negative events for different reasons (ranging from condemnation to responsibility) thus confounding effects between intemality of attributions and the canonical function. Among the limitations of this study is sampling from a population of convenience where external validity is compromised. Moreover, there was no random selection of participants therefore further limiting the inferences that can be made to the general population. In addition, it would have been optimal to sample from the population of clients undergoing counseling and psychotherapy. Relationships between self psychology and positive psychology variables may have been more pronounced with a client 80 population with more extreme self-Object needs and maladaptive thinking. The angst of extreme goal instability would certainly have repercussion for optimistic thinking. Also, Since it is the client population that would find the most utility from this study, it is this population that Should be sampled for results to generalize. As it stands, this study demonstrates the connection between self psychology and positive psychology by revealing the relationship between cohesiveness of the self and the degree of optimism in cognitive firnctioning. Future studies that investigate optimism within a self psychological context should take the investigation beyond a sample of convenience and the university population. In particular, populations with more extreme self-object needs should be sampled such as the population of people seeking counseling and psychotherapy. Within a Similar course of investigation the role of grandiosity on cognitive functioning must be further examined; this would mean examining grandiosity in both its archaic form as pathological narcissism and it’s more developed form as self- esteem. Thus, investigators can try to differentiate between attributional functioning of those who show high self—esteem (a function of the self) and true narcissists. Also, the functions of the cohesive self in relation to optimism must be examined using more than just self-report measures. The information garnered by self-report questionnaires can be supplemented with more objective outcome measures such as agency Shown towards goals whatever the goals may be (i.e. more fiiends, better job, completion of degree, etc.). Future studies must also investigate other positive psychology variables in relation to the cohesive self. Positive psychology variables such as emotional intelligence and the ability to empathize can also be construed as self-functions; any investigation seeking common ground between these two paradigms would be mutually beneficial to each of these models of human behavior. 81 Research Question #2 Results of the confirmatory factor analysis were mixed as to the fit of the 5-factor model to the data of the present study. Whereas most indices did not show the 5-factor structure to be a congruent fit, the ratio of X2 to degrees of freedom (Xz/dT. = 2.23) indicated that it was not completely poor fitting. Given that the present study used an English translation of the SONI and a culturally different population than that used by Banai et al., differences in Israeli and American culture and language are posited to be a factor in the mediocre fit of the 5-factor model. Another issue which may be responsible for differences in fit may be that some of the variables measured are multidimensional specifically within an American cultural context. Evidence of the multidimensional aspects of self-Object needs was shown with a post-hoe exploratory factor analysis using principle components extraction. The exploratory factor analysis after varimax rotation revealed that the data could be reduced to 10-factors. Just as in the Banai et al. study, some of the items measuring the idealization and twinship constructs correlated resulting in combined factors, such as factors 3 and 8 (See Appendix E). Factors 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 seemed to most definitively and parsimoniously reflect similar dimensions of hunger and avoidance of self-object needs as found in the Banai et al. study. These factors can be labeled as avoidance of mirroring (factor 1), avoidance of idealization (factor 2), hunger for idealization/twinship (factor 3), hunger for mirroring (factor 4), and hunger for twinship (factor 5). As mentioned, factors 6, 7, 8, 9, and 10 may have been extracted due to the multidimensional nature of the self psychology constructs within an American cultural context. For example, hunger for mirroring 82 seemed to be nuanced into the need to be recognized for one’s accomplishments (factor 4; “I feel hurt when my achievements are not sufficiently admired,” “I’m disappointed when my work is not appreciated”) as well as a general need for recognition and support (factor 10; “Most Of the time I feel like I’m not getting enough recognition from my superiors”). AS another example, hunger for twinship seemed to be partitioned into the need for affiliation with others in three areas: seeking others with a similar lifestyle (factor 3; “It’s important for me to be around other people who are in the same situation as me,” “I feel good knowing that I’m part of a group of people who share a particular lifestyle”), similar mindset (factor 5; “I feel better when I and someone close to me share similar feelings to other people,” “I gain self-confidence from having fiiends whose beliefs are Similar to mine”), as well as the need for connectedness in the face of problems (factor 7; “It’s important for me to feel that a close friend and I are ‘in the same boat,” “I feel stronger when I have people around who are dealing with similar problems”). When considering other extracted factors, it is difficult to readily determine what differentiates two factors that represent one construct (i.e. factor 6 and factor 9 both consist of very similar items that measure the avoidance of the twinship need). Further complicating interpretation, Item #11 (“I don’t function well in situations where I receive too little attention”) did not load onto any of the factors after varimax rotation. Also, the multidimensional aspects of the factors may be an artifact of the sample data. In addition, a grave limitation in the method of analysis of research question #2 is that a sample size of 199 participants is small in the factor analysis of a 38-item questionnaire. Although differences in the factor structure of this application of the SONI and that of Banai et al. (2005) are notable, the first five factors extracted seemed to be the 83 most stable and reliable. Arguably, a similar factor structure was found when compared to the results of Banai et al. (2005) if looking at the first five extracted factors. Whether the differences in factor structures are a function of sample size, research artifact, culture, language, the constructs themselves, or a combination of the three is yet to be determined by future studies. Conclusions The interpersonal dynamics of self-object needs that were identified by Kohut have the potential of expanding understanding of personality and human interaction as a function of development. Although the interpersonal dynamics of self-object needs are most apparent in the psychotherapy session as the transference configuration, psychologists are now quantifying and measuring these intrapersonal and interpersonal expressions of development. Counseling psychology has been actively exploring the tenets of self psychology because self psychology fits well with counseling psychology’s emphasis of growth and healthy functioning (Gelso & F retz, 2001; Patton & Meara, 1996). Positive psychology, the latest of the major paradigms in psychology, is at the cutting edge of applied clinical research. Positive psychology is congruent with counseling psychology’s emphasis on identifying and building on existing strengths. Counseling psychology can only benefit from considering what both paradigms of behavior have to offer. Study question #1 was an attempt to integrate self psychology with positive psychology by examining the relationship between expectations and attribution with goal-instability and grandiosity. The results showed that stable and realistic goals that are followed through are predictive of a certain way of explaining and making meaning of 84 the world. Although several psychological theories make statements on how goals are formed and carried through, the stability or instability of goals is the crux as well as a specific developmental endpoint of the comprehensive theory of self psychology. AS seen through a self psychological lens, stability of goals can be achieved through specific developmental actions and events. Study question #2 also looked at the intrapersonal and interpersonal effects of development as posited by self psychology. This second study was an attempt to replicate the factor structure of the SONI as found by Banai et al. (2005). The present study could not replicate the exact factor structure as found by Banai et al., possibly due to language and/or cultural differences between the Israeli sample used in the original study and the American sample used in the present study. Although the Banai et al. factor structure was not replicated, the first five factors were shown to be the most valid and reliable when considering the data from the present study. This measure of self-object needs seems promising and deserves further investigation. It is the author’s hope that counseling psychology will continue to investigate both the self psychology and positive psychology paradigms. Research that integrates concepts of developmental, personality, and counseling psychology can only lead to a more cohesive science of human behavior. 85 Appendix A Consent Form Student self-attitudes and interpersonal relationships This is a research project that examines student self attitudes and ways of relating to others. Your participation in this project will require completing five questionnaires and a demographics sheet. One of the questionnaires presents a number of hypothetical negative events and requires you to identify the cause of each event. Once the cause of the negative event has been identified, you are to identify different distinguishing factors of the identified cause. Three other questionnaires ask about your attitudes pertaining to yourself and others. There is also a page-long demographics sheet you are to complete. The demographics sheet asks about pertinent variable such as gender, ethnicity, and age, as well as some more personal questions such as, if you are in a romantic relationship and the number of sexual partners you have been with. All information remains confidential and cannot be traced to you in any way. Overall, the questionnaire packet should not take more than 40 minutes to complete in total. There are no foreseeable risks or significant discomfort related to your participation. We anticipate that your participation in the study will get you to think about yourself and interactions with others. For your participation in the study, you will be entered into a lottery with a cash prize of $100. Once you are ready to turn-in your questionnaire packet, you will be given a numbered ticket. I will keep an identical ticket to the one that you have that will be used in the lottery drawing. You will write your email address on the back of the ticket that I keep and I will email you if have been randomly chosen as the winner of the drawing. Your course instructor might also give extra-credit for participating in this study (at instructors’ discretion). Again, all information and data will remain strictly confidential. Reports of results will only be given in the form of correlations between measures (questionnaires) and other statistical nomenclature; individual names, scores, or other personal information cannot and will not be identified or traced to any individual participant. If you have any questions about this study, please contact Robert Bastanfar at (517) 749-1356 or email at bastanfafilmsuedu. My office is in Erickson Hall, Room 401C. The principle investigator of this research study is Dr. Nancy Crewe. Dr. Crewe can be reached at 517-355-1838 or email at ncrewe@msu.edu. Also, If you have any questions or concerns regarding your rights as a study participant, or are dissatisfied at any time with any aspect of this study, you may contact - anonymously, if you wish, Peter Vasilenko, Ph.D., Director of the Human Research Protection Programs (HRPP) at Michigan State University: (517) 355-2180, fax: (517)432-4503, email: irb@msu.edu, or regular mail: 202 Olds Hall, East Lansing, MI 48824. You are under no obligation to participate in this study and are free to withdraw at any time. There will be no penalty for withdrawal from the study. All participants will be included in the lottery drawing even if they do not complete the study questionnaires. We will be happy to answer any questions you have. F eel free to ask questions now, during the study, or after. Your cooperation, honesty, and motivated performance are essential to the success of this study. We will be happy to answer any questions about the goals, implications, and procedures of this research. At the end of the study, we will provide you with a written explanation of the purpose of this Study. You are making a decision whether or not to be in this study. Your Signature below means that you voluntarily agree to participate in this research study. Participant Signature Date Investigator Date 86 1. 2. 3. \l 00 \0 Appendix B Demographics Questionnaire Sex (1) Male (2) Female Age Please circle the number next to your race/ethnicity (1) Asian, Asian-American (2) Black, Afiican-American (3) Hispanic, Latino (4) Native American (5) Pacific Islander (6) White, European American (7) Multiracial, Mixed Race (8) Other, please specify Year of academic study (1) Freshman (2) Sophomore (3) Junior (4) Senior What is your sexual-orientation? (1) Heterosexual (2) Lesbian (3) Gay (4) Bi-sexual (5) Transgendered . Are you currently involved in a romantic relationship? (1) Yes (2) No . If you marked yes to Item 6, are you sexually involved with your romantic partner? (1) Yes (2) No . If you marked yes to Item 6, how satisfied are you in your current relationship? (1) Very Satisfied (2) Satisfied (3) Somewhat Satisfied (4) Unsatisfied (5) Very Unsatisfied . How many sexual partners have you been with? (1)0 (2) 1 - 5 (3) 6 - 10 (4) 11 — 15 (5) l6 - 20 (6)20 + 87 Appendix C Measures The following are five surveys about attitudes you have about yourself and others as well as a demographics questionnaire. We all have different attitudes or feelings about ourselves, our futures, and others with whom we interact. It is very important that you answer openly and honestly; there are no wrong answers. 88 10. The LOT-R ln uncertain times, I usually expect the best. 1 2 3 4 strongly disagree It is easy for me to relax. I 2 3 4 strongly disagree If something can go wrong for me, it will. 1 2 3 4 strongly disagree I’m always optimistic about my future. 1 2 3 4 strongly disagree I enjoy my friends a lot. 1 2 3 4 Strongly disagree It’s important for me to keep busy. 1 2 3 4 strongly disagree I hardly ever expect things to go my way. I 2 3 4 strongly disagree I don’t get upset too easily. 1 2 3 4 strongly disagree I rarely count on good things to happen to me. 1 2 3 4 strongly disagree Overall, I expect more good things to happen to me than bad. 1 2 3 4 strongly disagree 89 5 strongly agree 5 strongly agree 5 Strongly agree 5 strongly agree 5 strongly agree 5 strongly agree 5 strongly agree 5 strongly agree 5 strongly agree 5 strongly agree 10. It’s easier for me to start than to finish projects. 1 strongly agree I wonder where my life is headed. 1 strongly agree 2 moderately agree 2 moderately agree The Gl-Scale 3 slightly agree 3 slightly agree I don’t seem to make decisions by myself. 1 Strongly agree I don’t seem to have the drive to get my work done. 1 strongly agree I lose my sense of direction. 1 strongly agree 1 have more ideas than energy. 1 strongly agree I don’t seem to get going on anything important. 1 strongly agree 1 strongly agree 1 have confusion about who 1 am. 1 strongly agree 2 moderately agree 2 moderately agree 2 moderately agree 2 moderately agree moderately agree 2 moderately agree 2 moderately agree 3 slightly agree 3 slightly agree 3 slightly agree 3 slightly agree 3 slightly agree After a while, I lose sight of my goals. 3 slightly agree 3 slightly agree It’s hard to find a reason for working. 1 strongly agree 2 moderately agree 3 slightly agree 90 4 slightly disagree 4 Slightly disagree 4 slightly disagree 4 slightly disagree 4 slightly disagree 4 slightly disagree 4 slightly disagree 4 slightly disagree 4 slightly disagree 4 slightly disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 Strongly disagree 10. The S-Scale My friends follow my lead. 1 2 3 4 strongly moderately slightly slightly agree agree agree disagree I deserve favors from others. 1 2 3 4 strongly moderately slightly slightly agree agree agree disagree I’m witty and charming with others. 1 2 3 4 Strongly moderately slightly 5] i ghtly agree agree agree disagree My looks are one of the things that attract others to me. I 2 3 4 strongly moderately slightly slightly agree agree agree disagree I could show up my friends if I wanted to. l 2 3 4 Strongly moderately slightly Slightly agree agree agree disagree Running the Show means a lot to me. 1 2 3 4 strongly moderately slightly slightly agree agree agree disagree Being admired by others helps me feel fantastic. 1 2 3 4 strongly moderately slightly slightly agree agree agree disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree Achieving out of the ordinary accomplishments would make me feel complete. 1 2 3 4 strongly moderately slightly slightly agree agree agree disagree I catch myself wanting to be a hero. 1 2 3 4 strongly moderately slightly slightly agree agree agree disagree I know that l have more natural talents than most. 1 2 3 4 strongly moderately slightly slightly agree agree agree disagree 91 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 5 moderately disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree 6 strongly disagree The SONI Please note that in this measure I = Strong Disagree whereas 7 = Strongly Agree 1. 1 Strongly Disagree 2. l Strongly Disagree 3. 1 Strongly Disagree 4. Associating with successful people allows me to feel successful as well. 1 Strongly Disagree 5. 1 Strongly Disagree 6. 1 Strongly Disagree 7. 1 Strongly Disagree I feel hurt when my achievements are not sufficiently admired. 2 It’s important for me to be around other people who are in the same situation as me. 2 When I have a problem, it’s difficult to accept advice even from experienced people. 2 2 I don’t need other people’s praise. 2 I would prefer not to be involved with people who suffer from problems similar to mine. 2 I’m disappointed when my work is not appreciated. 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 4 4 4 4 4 4 92 5 5 5 5 6 6 6 Strongly Agree Strongly Agree 7 Strongly Agree Strongly Agree Strongly Agree 7 Strongly Agree Strongly Agree 8. I seek out people who Share my values, Opinions, and activities. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Strongly Disagree 9. I find it difficult to accept guidance even from people I respect. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Strongly Disagree 10. I identify with famous people. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Strongly Disagree 11. I don’t function well in situations where I receive too little attention. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Strongly Disagree 12. I feel good knowing that I’m part of a group of people who share a particular lifestyle. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Strongly Disagree 13. I feel bad about myself after having to be helped by others with more experience. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Strongly Disagree 14. It’s important for me to feel that a close friend and l are ‘in the same boat.” 1 2 3 4 5 6 Strongly Disagree 15. When I’m doing something, 1 don’t need acknowledgement from others. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Strongly Disagree 93 7 Strongly Agree Strongly Agree Strongly Agree Strongly Agree Strongly Agree Strongly Agree Strongly Agree 7 Strongly Agree 16. It bothers me to be in close relationships with people who are similar to me. 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 17. I am attracted to successful people. 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 18. I have no need to boast about my achievements. l 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 19. I feel better about myself when I am in the company of experts. I 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 7 Strongly Agree 7 Strongly Agree Strongly Agree 7 Strongly Agree 20. I would rather have friends that are different from me rather than ones who are very similar. 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 21. I feel better when I and someone close to me share similar feelings to other people. 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 22. It’s important for me to be part of a group who share similar opinions. I 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 23. I don’t really care what others think about me. 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 94 7 Strongly Agree Strongly Agree 7 Strongly Agree Strongly Agree 24. I know that I’m successful, so I have no need for others’ feedback. 1 Strongly Disagree 25. I’m bored by people who think and feel too much like me. 1 Strongly Disagree 26. It’s important for me to be around people who can serve as my role models. 1 Strongly Disagree 2 2 2 3 3 3 4 4 4 5 5 27. I feel stronger when I have people around who are dealing with similar problems. 1 Strongly Disagree 28. It’s difficult for me to belong to a group of people who are dealing with similar problems. 1 Strongly Disagree 29. In order to feel successful, I need reassurance and approval from others. 1 Strongly Disagree 30. When I’m worried or distressed, getting advice from experts doesn’t help much. 1 Strongly Disagree 31. I try to be around people I admire. 1 Strongly Disagree 2 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 3 4 4 4 4 95 5 5 5 5 6 6 7 Strongly Agree 7 Strongly Agree Strongly Agree Strongly Agree Strongly Agree 7 Strongly Agree Strongly Agree 7 Strongly Agree 32. I gain self-confidence from having friends whose beliefs are similar to mine. 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 33. I need a lot of support from others. 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 34. I find it difficult to be proud of the groups I belong to. 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 35. Most of the time I feel like I’m not getting enough recognition from my superiors. I 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 36. It’s important for me to belong to high-Status, “glamorous” social groups. 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 37. I don’t need support and encouragement from Others. 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 38. I would rather not belong to a group of people whose lifestyle is similar to mine. 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Disagree 96 6 7 Strongly Agree Strongly Agree 7 Strongly Agree Strongly Agree Strongly Agree 7 Strongly Agree Strongly Agree Interpretation of Events Please try to imagine yourself in the situations that follow. If such a situation happened to you, what would you feel would have caused it? While event may have many causes, we want you to pick only one — THE MAJOR CAUSE IF THIS EVENT HAPPENED TO YOU. Please write the cause in the blank provided after each event. Next we want you to answer three questions about the cause you provided. First, is the cause of this event something about you or something about other people or circumstances? Second, is the cause of this event something that will persist across time or something that will never again be present. Third, is the cause of this event something that effects all situations in your life or something that affects just this type of event? To summarize, we want you to: 1. Read each situation and vividly imagine it happening to you. 2. Decide what you feel would be the one major cause of the situation if it happened to you. 3. Write the cause in the blank provided. 4. Answer three questions about the cause. 97 1. You have been looking for a job unsuccessfully for some time. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. in the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 situations 2. A friend comes to you with a problem, and you don’t try to help. A. Write down the one major cause: 6. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 to me C. in the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 situations 98 3. You give an important talk in front of a group, and the audience reacts negatively. A. Write down the one major cause: B. is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. in the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 present 0. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 situations 4. You met a friend who acted hostilely to you. A. Write down the one major cause: 6. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present 0. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 situations 99 5. You can’t get all the work done that others expect you to. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 situations 6. You go out on a date and it goes badly. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 situations 100 7. Your steady romantic relationship ends. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 ' 3 4 5 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 situations 8. You experience a major personal injury. A. Write down the one major cause: 6. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 present D. is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 situations 101 9. You are found guilty of a minor violation of the law. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 situations 10. You and your family have a serious argument. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 present 0. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 situations 102 11. You are fired from your job. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 situations 12. After your first term at school, you are on academic probation. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 situations 103 13. Your best friend tells you that you are not to be trusted. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 situations 14. You have a lot of trouble understanding what your new employer requires of you. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present 0. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 situations 104 15. You cannot sleep soundly. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 situations 16. You experience sexual difficulties. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 present 0. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 situations 105 17. You confront a serious conflict in your values. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present 0. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 situations 18. Your roommate tell you he/she is switching to a room down the hall. A. Write down the one major cause: 8. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 situations 106 19. There are few recreational activities in which you are interested. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 situations 20. Your Christmas vacation plans are cancelled. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other peOple or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 situations 107 21. You have trouble with one of your instructors. A. Write down the one major cause: 6. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present 0. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 situations 22. You experience financial difficulties. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 situations 108 23. Your attempt to capture the interest of a specific person of the opposite sex is a failure. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 situations 24. You feel sick and tired all the time. A. Write down the one major cause: B. Is the cause of this due to something about you or something about other people or circumstances? (circle one number) totally due totally due to others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 to me C. In the future, will this cause again be present? (circle one number) never always present 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 present D. Is this cause something that affects just this type of situation, or does it also influence other areas of your life? (circle one number) Just this all situation 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 situations 109 Appendix D Debriefing Form The study that you have just participated in examines how variables pertaining to self-functioning (attitudes about oneself, ambition, and ability to strive towards goals) relate to one’s cognitive explanatory style and general outlook on the future. In other words, one aspect of the study examines how your attitudes about yourself and your abilities may relate to how optimistically you see and interpret life events. We particularly looked at your general outlook towards life events and how you would explain the cause of negative and unpleasant events. This is the first study examining the possible connection between psychoanalytic study of the self and the burgeoning field of positive psychology. Another aspect of this study examines how people function interpersonally. There is some evidence in the psychological literature that some people are dependant on social relationships and others are rejecting of social relationships. Several psychological theories posit that how one relates to others (both socially and intimately) is tied to the stability of one’s development as a child. This study looked at the dependant and rejecting patterns of relating to others that may be function of childhood stability and healthy development. It is hoped that both aspects of this study provide a better understanding of the self, cognitive functioning as it relates to self-functioning, and patterns of self and other relationships. This information may be useful in the context of counseling and psychotherapy and hopefully will contribute to the study of personality and psychoanalytic psychology. Again, rest assured that confidentiality will be maintained and that no information can be traced back to you. If you have additional questions about the purpose of this study or the results obtained, please leave your name and a message for me (Robert Bastanfar) at (517) 749-1356 or email me at bastanfa@msu.edu. I will return your call or reply to your email as soon as possible. Also, If you have any questions or concerns regarding your rights as a study participant, or are dissatisfied at any time with any aspect of this study, you may contact - anonymously, if you wish, Peter Vasilenko, Ph.D., Director of the Human Research Protection Programs (HRPP) at Michigan State University: (517) 355-2180, fax: (517) 432-4503, email: irb@msu.edu, or regular mail: 202 Olds Hall, East Lansing, MI 48824. Again, we greatly appreciate your participation and contribution to our research. 110 war Appendix E Factor Loading of Exploratory Factor Analysis of Self-Object Needs Inventory (SONI) Factor I (Avoidance of Mirroring) Q5 — factor I (.610) I don’t need other people’s praise. Q15 — factor I (.569) When I’m doing something, I don’t need acknowledgement from others. Q23 — factor 1 (.569) I don’t really care what others think about me. Q24 — factor 1 (.724) I know that I’m successful, so I have no need for others’ feedback Q29 - factor I (-.497) In order to feel successful, I need reassurance and approval from others. Q33 — factor 1 (-.578) I need a lot of support from others. Q37 — factor I (.801) I don’t need support and encouragement from others Factor 2 (Avoidance of Idealization) Q3 — factor 2 (.822) When I have a problem, it’s difficult to accept advice even from experienced peOple. Q9 — factor 2 (.823) I find it difficult to accept guidance even from people I respect. Ql3 — factor 2 (.645) I feel bad about myself after having to be helped by others with more experience. Q30 — factor 2 (.666) When I’m worried or distressed, getting advice from experts doesn’t help much. Factor 3 (Hunger for Idealization / Twinship) Q2 -— factor 3 (.423) It’s important for me to be around other people who are in the same situation as me. Q4 — factor 3 (.542) Associating with successful people allows me to feel successful as well. 111 Q8 — factor 3 (.497) I seek out people who share my values, opinions, and activities. Q12 — factor 3 (.498) I feel good knowing that I’m part of a group of people who share a particular lifestyle Q17 - factor 3 (.534) I am attracted to successful people Q26 — factor 3 (.670) It’s important for me to be around people who can serve as my role models. Q31 — factor 3 (.671) I try to be around people I admire. Factor 4 (Hunger for Mirroring) Q1 - factor 4 (.814) I feel hurt when my achievements are not sufficiently admired. Q7 — factor 4 (.736) I’m disappointed when my work is not appreciated. Q18 — factor 4 (-.482) I have no need to boast about my achievements. Factor 5 (Hunger for Twinship) Q21 — factor 5 (.737) I feel better when I and someone close to me share similar feelings to other people. Q22 — factor 5 (.705) It’s important for me to be part of a group who share similar opinions. Q32 — factor 5 (.620) I gain self-confidence from having friends whose beliefs are similar to mine. Factor 6 (Avoidance of Twinship) Q6 —- factor 6 (.521) I would prefer not to be involved with people who suffer from problems similar to mine. Q16 — factor 6 (.657) It bothers me to be in close relationships with people who are similar to me. Q28 — factor 6 (.645) It’s difficult for me to belong to a group of people who are dealing with similar problems. 112 Q34 — factor 6 (.577) I find it difficult to be proud of the groups I belong to. Q38 — factor 6 (.522) I would rather not belong to a group of people whose lifestyle is similar to mine. Factor 7 (Hunger for Twinship) Ql4 — factor 7 (.781) It’s important for me to feel that a close friend and I are ‘in the same boat.” Q27 — factor 7 (.623) I feel stronger when I have people around who are dealing with similar problems. Factor 8 (Hunger for Idealization/Twinship) Q10 — factor 8 (.778) I identify with famous people. Q19 — factor 8 (.484) I feel better about myself when I am in the company of experts. Q36 - factor 8 (.673) It’s important for me to belong to hi gh-status, “glamorous” social groups Factor 9 (Avoidance of Twinship) Q20 — factor 9 (.775) I would rather have fiiends that are different from me rather than ones who are very similar. Q25 — factor 9 (.503) I’m bored by people who think and feel too much like me. Factor 10 (Hunger for Mirroring) Q35 — factor 10 (.671) Most of the time I feel like I’m not getting enough recognition from my superiors. Factor (‘2) [Hunger for Mirroring] Q1 1 — factor (?) I don’t function well in situations in situations where I receive too little attention. 113 Appendix F Glossary of Terms Attributional Style - a person’s tendencies when infem'ng the cause of behavior or events. Three dimensions are often used to evaluate people’s attributional styles: the intemal-extemal dimension (whether they tend to attribute events to the self or external factors), the stable-unstable dimension (whether they tend to attribute events to enduring or transient causes), and the global-specific dimension (whether they tend to attribute events to causes that affect many events or just a single event). Drive theory - psychoanalytic theory in which major emphasis is placed on the libido, the stages of psychosexual development, and the Id instincts or drives. Ego Psychology — in psychoanalysis, an approach that emphasizes the functions of the ego in controlling impulses and dealing with the external environment. Expectancy — in behavioral psychology, a state or condition in which an organism anticipates a given stimulus or event on the basis of its previous experience with related stimuli or events. Grandiosity — an exaggerated sense of one’s greatness, importance, or ability. Idealization — the exaggeration of the positive attributes and minimization of the imperfections or failings associated with a person, place, or thing, or situation, so that it is viewed as perfect or nearly perfect. Mirroring — in self psychology, the positive responses of parents to a child that are intended to instill internal self-respect. Object — in psychoanalytic theory, the person, thing or part of the body through which an instinct can achieve its aim of gratification. Object-relations theory — any psychoanalytically based that views the need to relate to objects as more central to personality organization and motivation than the vicissitudes of the instincts. Positive psychology — a field of psychological theory and research that focuses on the psychological states (e. g., contentment, joy), individual traits or character strengths (e.g., intimacy, integrity, altruism, wisdom), and social institutions that make life most worth living. Psychoanalysis — an approach to the mind, psychological disorders, and psychological treatment originally developed by Sigmund Freud at the beginning of the 20th century. The hallmark of psychoanalysis is the assumption that much of mental activity is unconscious and, consequently, that understanding people requires interpreting the unconscious meaning underlying their overt or manifest behavior. 114 Self - the totality of the individual, consisting of all characteristic attributes, conscious and unconscious, mental and physical. Self-psychology — a school of psychoanalytic theory that stresses the importance of an individual’s relationships with others to healthy self-development and locates the source of many psychological problems in caregivers’ lack of responsiveness to the child’s emotional needs. Transference — in psychoanalysis, the displacement or projection onto the analyst of unconscious feelings and wishes originally directed toward important individuals, such as parents, in the patient’s childhood. Note: All glossary definitions quoted from APA Dictionary of Psychology (2007). 115 References Abramson, L.Y., Seligman, M.E.P., & Teasdale, JD. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critiques and reformulations. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49- 74. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders-text revision (4lh ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Arlow, J .A. (1995). Psychoanalysis. In R.J. Corsini & D. Wedding (Eds), Current psychotherapies (5th ed., pp. 15-50). Itasca, Illinois: F .E. Peacock Publishers. Ainsworth, M.D.S., & Bowlby, J. (1991). An ethological approach to personality development, American Psychologist, 46, 333-341. Baker, HS. (1991). Short-term psychotherapy: A self-psychological approach. In P. Crits-Christoph & J .P. Barber (Eds), Handbook of short-term dynamic Psychotherapy (pp.287-322). USA: Basic Books. Banai, E., Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, PR. (2005). “Self-object” needs in Kohut’s self -psychology. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 22(2), 224-260. Bandura, A. (1977). Self efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavior change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215. Bandura, A. (1986). Social formulations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall. Beck, A.T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press. Beck, A.T. & Weishaar, ME. (1995). Cognitive therapy. In R. Corsini and D. Wedding (Eds). Current psychotherapies (5th ed., pp. 229 — 261). Itasca, IL: Peacock. Blustein, D.L., Walbridge, M.M., Friedlander, M.L., & Palladino, DE. (1991). Contributions of psychological separation and parental attachment to the career development process. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38(1), 39-50. Bordin, ES, (1980). A psychodynamic view of counseling psychology. The Counseling Psychologist, 9(1), 62-70. Borgen, F. (1984). Counseling Psychology. The Annual Review of Psychology, 35, 579 -604. 116 Brickman, P., & Campbell, D.T. (1971). Hedonic relativism and planning the good society. In M.H. Appley (Ed.), Adaptation level theory: A symposium (pp. 287- 302). New York: Academic Press. Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 917-927. Campbell, W.K., Reeder, G.D., Sedikides, C., & Elliot, AJ. (2000). Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 329-347. Campbell, W.K., Rudich, E.A., Sedikides, C. (2002). Narcissism, self-esteem, and the positivity of self-views: Two portraits of self-love. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(3), 358-368. Carver, C.S., & Scheier, M. (2003). Optimism. In SJ Lopez & C.R. Snyder (Eds), Positive psychology assessment, a handbook of models and measures (pp. 75- 89). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Diener, E., Lucas, R.E., Scollon, ON. (2006). Beyond the hedonic treadmill. American Psycholgist, 61(4), 305-314. Edwards, W. (1954). The theory of decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 51, 380- 417. Emmons, R.A., & McCullough, ME. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well—being in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 377-3 89. Fordyce, M.W. (1977). Development of a program to increase personal happiness. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 24, 511-520. F ordyce, M.W. (183). A program to increase happiness: Further studies. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 30, 483-498. F ouad, N.A., & Bingham, RP. (1995). Career counseling with racial and ethnic minorities. In W.B. Walsh & H.S. Opsipow (Eds), Handbook of Vocational Psychology (pp. 331-365). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Frank, J .D., & Frank, J .B. (1991). Persuasion and healing: A comparative study of psychotherapy (3rd ed.). Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press. Freud, A. (1936). The writings of Anna Freud: Vol. 2. The ego and the mechanisms of Defense. New York: International University Press. Garfield, S., & Bergin, A. (1986). Introduction and historical overview. In S. Garfield & 117 A. Bergin (Eds), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (pp. 3-22). New York: Wiley. Gelso, C. & Fassinger, RE. (1992). Personality, development, and counseling psychology: Depth, ambivalence, and actualization. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39(3), 275-298. Gelso, C., & Fretz, B. (2001). Counseling psychology (2nd ). Orlando, FL: Harcourt College Publishers. Hartman, H. (1939). Ego psychology and the problem of adaptation. New York: Intemational University Press. Hazan, C., & Shaver, RR. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 51 1-524. Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley. Higgins, ET. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319-340. Homey, K. (1950). Neurosis and human growth. New York: Norton. Huck, S. (2004) Reading Statistics & Research (4th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. Josephs, L. (1991). Character structure, self-esteem regulation, and the principle of identity maintenance. In R.C.Curtis (Ed.), The relational self (pp. 3-16). New York: The Guilford Press. Khan, E. (1985). Heinz Kohut and Carl Rogers, a timely comparison. American Psychologist, 40(8), 893-904. Kohut, H. (1971). The analysis of the self. New York: International University Press. Kohut, H (1977). The restoration of the self. New York: International University Press. Kohut, H. (1984). How does analysis cure? Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kohut, H.,' & Wolf, ES. (1978). The disorders of the self and their treatment: An outline. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 59(4), 413-425. Lapsley, M. (1994). Encyclopedia of human behavior (V 01. 2). New York: Academic Press. Lee, R.M.,& Robbins, SB. (1995). Measuring belongingness: The Social Connectedness and Social Assurance Scales. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42, 232-241. 118 Lewin, K. (1938). The conceptual representation and measurement of psychological forces. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Lopez, F.G., Campbell, V.L., & Watkins, C.E., Jr. (1986). Depression, psychological separation, and college adjustment: An investigation of sex differences. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33, 52-56. Lopez, F .G., & Brennan, K.A. (2000). Dynamic processes underlying adult attachment organization: Toward an attachment theoretical perspective on the healthy and effective self. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47(3), 283-300. Maddux, J .E. (1999). Expectancies and the social-cognitive perspective: basic Principles, processes, and variables. In I. Kirsch (Ed.), How expectancies shape experience (pp. 17-39). Ann Arbor, MI: Sheridan Press. Mahler, MS. (1960). Symposium on psychotic object relationships: Perceptual de —differentiation and psychotic “object relationship.” International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 41, 548—553. Mahler, MS. (1971). A study of the separation-individuation process and its possible application to borderline phenomenon in the psychoanalytic situation. Psycho -analytic Study of the Child, 10, 403-422. McWilIiams, N. (1994). Psychoanalytic diagnosis. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Metalsky, G.I., Halberstadt, L.J., & Abramson, L.Y. (1987). Vulnerability to depressive mood reactions: Toward a more powerful test of diathesis-stress and causal mediation components of the reformulated theory of depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 386-393. Mikulincer, M. (1994). Human learned helplessness. New York: Plenum Press. Morf, C.C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Expanding the dynamic self-regulatory processing model of narcissism: Research directions for the future. Psychological Inquiry, 12(4), 243-251. O’Brian, K.M. (2003). Measuring career self-efficacy: Promoting confidence and happiness at work. In S.J Lopez & C.R. Snyder (Eds), Positive psychology assessment: A handbook of models and measures (pp. 75- 89). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Patton, M.J., & Meara, NM. (1992). Psychoanalytic counseling. New York: Wiley. Patton, M.J., & Meara, NM. (1996). Kohut and counseling: Applications of self- psychology. Psychoanalytic Counseling, 2(3), 328-355. 119 Payne, E., Robbins, S., & Dougherty (1991). Goal directedness and older adult adjustment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 300-308. Patton, M.J., & Robbins, SB. (1982). Kohut’s self-psychology as a model for college -student counseling. Professional Psychology, 13(6), 876-887. Peterson, C., & Vaidya, RS. (2001). Explanatory style, expectations, and depressive symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 1217-1223. Peterson, C., & Villanova, P. (1988). An Expanded Attributional Style Questionnaire. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 87-89. Peterson, C., & Seligman, M.E.P. (2004). Character strengths and virtues: A handbook and classification. Washington DC: American Psychological Association. Ponterotto, J .G, & Casas, J .M. (1991). Hankbook of racial/ethnic minority counseling Research. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Radloff, LS. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401. Reivich, K., & Gillham, J. (2003). Leamed Optimism: The measurement of explanatory style. In 8.] Lopez & C.R Snyder (Eds), Positive psychology assessment, handbook of models and measures (pp. 57-74). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Robbins, SB. (1989). Role of contemporary psychoanalysis in counseling psychology. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36(3), 267-278. Robbins, S.B., Lee, R.M., Wan, TH. (1994). Goal continuity as a mediator of early retirement adjustment: Testing a multidimensional model. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41, 18-26. Robbins, S.B., & Patton, M.J. (1985). Self-psychology and career development: Construction of the Superiority and Goal Instability Scales. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 32, 221-231. Robbins, S.B., & Dupont, P. (1992). Narcissistic needs of the self and perceptions of interpersonal behavior. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39(4), 462-467. Roland, A. (1996). The influence of culture on the self and self object relationships, an Asia-North American Comparison. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 6(4), 461-75. Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books. 120 Rotter, J .B. (1954). Social learning and clinical psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Rotter, J .B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80(1, Whole No. 609). Scheier, M.F., Carver, C.S., & Bridges, M.W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063- 1078. Seligman, M.E.P., Abramson, L.Y., Semmel, A., & von Baeyer, C. (1979). Depressive attributional style. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 242-247. Seligman, M.E.P., Steen, T.A., Park, N., Peterson, C. (2005). Positive psychology progress, empirical validations of interventions. American Psychologist, 60(5), 410-421. Steinberg, T. (1987). Self psychology and American culture. The Psychohistory Review, 15(3), 3-28. St. Clair, M. (1996). Object relations and self psychology. An introduction. (2nd ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company. Strupp, H.H. & Binder, J .L. (1984). Psychotherapy in a new key: A guide to time limited dynamic psychotherapy. New York: Basic Books. Sue, D.W., & Sue, D. (1990). Counseling the culturally different: Theory and Practice (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. Sullivan, HS. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York: Norton. Tabachnick, B.G., & F idell, LS. (2007). Using Multivariate Statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Pearson Education, Inc. Tesser, A. (1991). Social versus clinical approaches to self-psychology: The self -evaluation model and Kohutian object relations theory. In R.C.Curtis (Ed), The relational self (pp. 257-281). New York: The Guilford Press. Tollman, EC. (1955). Principles of performance. Psychological Review, 62, 315-326. Wachtel, PL. (1993). Therapeutic communication: Principles and effective practice. New York: Guilford Press. Wampold, BE. (2001). The great psychotherapy debate: models, methods, and findings. Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 121 Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71(1), 3-25. Weiten, W., & Lloyd, MA. (2006). Psychology applied to modern life: Adjustment in the 21“ century. Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth. Weinberger, J. (1995). Common factors aren’t so common: The common factors dilemma. Clinical psychology: Science and practice, 2, 45-69. Weinberger, J ., & Eig, A. (1999). Expectancies: The ignored common factor in psychotherapy. In I. Kirsch (Ed.), How expectancies shape experience (pp. 357- 382. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Wolitzky, BL. (1995). The theory and practice of traditional psychoanalytic psychotherapy. In A.S. Gurman & S.B. Wachtel (Eds), Essential psychotherapies: theory and practice (pp. 12-54). Wolitzky, D.L., & Eagle, MN. (1997). Psychoanalytic theories of psychotherapy. In P.L. Wachtel & S.B. Messer (Eds), Theories of psychotherapy, origins and evolution (pp. 39-96). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Vanderboss, G.R. (Ed.). (2007). APA dictionary of psychology (1‘5t ed.). Washington DC: American Psychological Association. Zonis, M. (1980). Some possible contributions of the psychology of the self to the study of the Arab Middle-East. In A. Goldberg (Ed), Advances in self-psychology (pp. 349-446). New York: International University Press, Inc. 122 ”initiallyiiiiiiliiiir