1.22.... «K awn: . w A 43:4 and an. _ .. - 35%“ .41 LcmfiWu it. a .v Ifsfi. . . n , A ‘ s )pnawumwz ‘ . .. 1. I}??? 519.!1: fl, :1 :11 11353....- ‘fi‘! 5. h.) $.33. ” an I. . “by“. ‘V l v I) 3. u e .333... . g.» 1... ‘lu (‘5 ...\ ‘LWWJQ 9:42.533... 9. I...“ .‘I . 11‘ ‘1‘! fix? I. .31.... .2: a). . «3.1.: ark: éthlua :3... .12.? :1 I .‘1‘ 0;”. unsaviaindfixrhfl .. t 6533.3 .22 Q... g “1......“ can u I)! ‘i‘ 1! {tail ..\\bsx¢_ ‘33.; v .3! LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled THE IMPORTANCE OF C CUSTOMER SATISFACTION AND DELIGHT ON LOYALTY IN THE TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY INDUSTRY presented by MI RAN KIM has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the 9U!'| Sll-IJ. tWOW "‘0 PhD. degree in Community Agriculture Recreation and Resource Studies 0 Owe/m Mme Major Professor’s Slgnature BIZSII (D 925 mfg/(2m LDate Doctoral Dissertation MSU is an Affinnative Action/Equal Opportunity Employer PLACE IN RETURN Box to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5/08 K:IProjIAcc&Pres/ClRC/DateDuo.indd THE IMPORTANCE OF CUSTOMER SATISFACTION AND DELIGHT ON LOYALTY IN THE TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY INDUSTRY By Mi Ran Kim A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Community, Agriculture, Recreation, and Resource Studies 2010 ABSTRACT THE IMPORTANCE OF CUSTOMER SATISFACTION AND DELIGHT ON LOYALTY IN THE TOURISM AND HOSPITALITY INDUSTRY By Mi Ran Kim Customer satisfaction has been a focus of researchers and marketers as an important antecedent of customer loyalty. Research has proven that satisfied customers show loyalty by purchasing more products or services, recommending products to others, and being less price sensitive (Anderson, Fomell, and Mazvancheryls, 2004; Homburg, Koschate, and Hoyer, 2005). Recent studies propose customer delight as a new variable of interest in satisfaction research and are anticipated to possibly produce greater customer loyalty than satisfaction (Hicks, Page, Behe, Dennis, and Fernandez, 2005). Like customer satisfaction, customer loyalty is also considered crucial to the success of a business organization because loyal customers are less expensive to retain than to find and develop new customers (Reichheld and Sasser, 1990). In recent years interest has also grown in understanding the multi-phases of loyalty as a useful way to segment customers with differential strategies (Knox and Walker, 2001; McMullan and Gilmore, 2002; Palmer, McMahon-Beanie, and Beggs, 2000). The problem of this research is to examine the impact of customer satisfaction and delight on loyalty by empirically testing a model. Furthermore, the study aims to better understand four phases of loyalty development: cognitive, affective, conative, and action loyalties. Data were collected from guests who stayed at a Midwestern resort during a peak summer vacation time using an online and a paper survey (1,573 subjects from an online survey, 87 subjects from a paper survey). The model was tested applying structural equation modeling (SEM) to estimate the relationship between customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty in a tourism and hospitality context. The findings support the proposed model and suggest that (1) customer satisfaction has a direct and positive influence on cognitive and affective loyalty; (2) customer delight has a direct and positive influence on cognitive and affective loyalty; (3) customer satisfaction has a greater influence on cognitive loyalty than on customer delight; (4) customer delight has a greater influence on affective loyalty than on customer satisfaction; (5) cognitive and affective loyalties have a direct and positive influence on conative loyalty; and (6) conative loyalty has a direct and positive influence on action loyalty. This study is one of few empirical studies on customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty and tests a more comprehensive model than previous research efforts. This study will contribute to the body of knowledge on customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty and provide important theoretical and applied suggestions for the tourism and hospitality industry. Future studies should replicate the findings and test the model with different samples such as types of accommodations (e. g., business hotel, bed and breakfast), places (e.g., other states, other countries), and service industries (e. g., restaurant, airline, cruise). With attitudinal research, validating the accuracy of action loyalty or intended future behaviors should be validated with actual measures of future returns to the business and recommendations of the business to others. DEDICATION I would like to dedicate this dissertation to my beloved husband, Ki— Wan Park, for his unconditional love and full support, to my father, Kwaung—Ok Kim, for his sacrificial love and support from heaven, to my angel daughter, Yenna Park, for being the joy and blessing of my life, and to God for making all of this possible. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The long doctoral journey would not have been possible without support of many people. I wish to express to them my sincere gratitude. I am most appreciative of my two advisers, Dr. Christine A. Vogt and Dr. Bonnie J. Knutson. They were always right there to listen and to give advice to me. They are my forever mentors. Especially, I would like to give my deep appreciation to Dr. Christine A. Vogt, committee chair, for her guidance, expertise, genuine caring, patience, and time. She has profoundly shaped the research and writing of this study. She guided me during every step of my Ph.D. journey and assigned priority to my success in the doctoral program, providing the right balance of challenge and support. She is the best role model for me. I am also extremely grateful for the tremendous support that I have received Since the MS. program from my co-adviser, Dr. Bonnie J. Knutson. She has been motivating, encouraging, caring, and enlightening, not only for myself, but also for my family. She is the true motivator and mentor who inspired me to be the true marketer..When I stood behind her, I became confident and strong to get through all my challenges during my PhD. journey. I am forever grateful to her, my CMP (Cool Mother Professor). My sincere appreciation is extended to my dissertation committee member and Director of The School of Hospitality Business, Dr. Ronald F. Cichy. Without his sincere support, guidance, expertise, and encouragement, I could not have completed my Ph.D. journey successfully. He is The Leader to me and I will be The Follower. I am also very grateful to the remaining members of my dissertation committee, Dr. Richard D. Paulsen and Dr. Jeffrey A. Beck, for their academic support, expertise, encouragement, and time in producing this work. vi I deeply thank Dr. Annette Rummel, president of Great Lakes Bay Regional Convention & Visitors Bureau, Mrs. Judith Zehnder Keller, president of F rankenmuth Bavarian Inn Lodge, Mr. Jim Engel, general manger of F rankenmuth Bavarian Inn Lodge, and the guests of Frankenmuth Bavarian Inn Lodge for supporting me during this study. My gratitude is also extended to Dr. Eun-Sil Lee who is my spiritual sister. Her love, care, and support were a treasure. She also gave me many precious memories during my Ph.D. study and made a place in my heart only she will ever occupy. I want her to know how much I was influenced by her servant leadership as a good example for God’s people. I would like to thank Julie Tkach. I call her my American sister. We laughed, cried, prayed, and worked together through our Ph.D. lives. I could not have completed this journey without Julie by my side. Especially, I am grateful for her help in editing my dissertation paper. I hope our sistership goes on forever. My friends in the prayers meeting, “Sisters of Lydia”, have my appreciation for their love, compassionate fi'iendship, and prayers during my doctoral journey. I express deep gratitude to my life partner, Ki-Wan Park for his full support, love, and patience - it is my turn to return the favor now, and to my angel daughter, Yenna Park, for being my joy and blessing all the time. Also I am greatly indebted to my parents-in-law, Yong-Soo Park and Yong-Soon Choi, and my parents, Kwaung-Ok Kim and Bok-Soon Kim, for their sacrificial support and unconditional love. Special thanks to my sisters, sisters-in-law, brothers, brothers-in-law, nieces, and nephews. Thank you and I love all of you. Thank God! You were always with me, helped me, and blessed me. I praise Your 1181116. vii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .............................................................................................................. x LIST OF FIGURES .......................................................................................................... xii CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Introduction of Constructs ................................................................................................. 1 Customer Satisfaction and Delight ..................................................................... 1 Customer Loyalty ............................................................................................... 2 Problem Statement ............................................................................................................. 3 Purpose of Study ................................................................................................................. 3 Conceptual Model and Hypotheses .................................................................................... 4 Delimitations ...................................................................................................................... 6 Definitions of Terms .......................................................................................................... 6 CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW Consumer Behavior ........................................................................................................... 8 Customer Satisfaction ...................................................................................................... 10 Customer Delight ............................................................................................................. 14 Customer Loyalty ............................................................................................................. l8 Affect and Cognition ........................................................................................................ 24 CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHDOLOGY Research Design ................................................................................................................ 29 Rationale for Research Method ........................................................................ 29 Sampling ........................................................................................................... 30 Pilot Study ........................................................................................................ 31 Data Collection ................................................................................................ 31 The Questionnaire ............................................................................................ 34 Survey Measurement ........................................................................................................ 34 Customer Satisfaction ...................................................................................... 34 Customer Delight ............................................................................................. 34 Cognitive Loyalty ............................................................................................ 35 Affective Loyalty ............................................................................................. 35 Conative Loyalty ............................................................................................... 36 Action Loyalty .................................................................................................. 36 Data Analysis .................................................................................................................... 38 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS Preliminary Analyses ........................................................................................................ 40 Data Screening ................................................................................................. 40 Profile of Survey Respondents .......................................................................... 42 Traveling Characteristics of Survey Respondents ............................................ 42 Testing the Measurement Model ..................................................................................... 46 Confirmatory Factor Analysis ........................................................................... 46 viii Assessment of Reliability and Validity ............................................................. 50 Testing the Structural Equation Model ............................................................................ 52 Multiple Squared Correlations .......................................................................... 56 Additional Analyses ......................................................................................................... 57 Additional Analysis with a Full Path Model .................................................... 57 Additional Analysis between New Guests and Repeat Guests ........................ 64 Additional Analysis between Loyalty Program Members and Non-Members 73 Summary .......................................................................................................................... 81 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION, IMPLICATIONS, AND CONCLUSION Results of Hypotheses Testing and Discussion of the Findings ....................................... 82 Implications ....................................................................................................................... 86 Theoretical Implications ................................................................................... 86 Practical Implications ........................................................................................ 89 Limitations and Future Studies ......................................................................................... 94 Conclusion ........................................................................................................................ 95 APPENDICES .................................................................................................................. 97 BIBLIOGRAPHY .......................................................................................................... 108 ix LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Difference between Satisfaction and Delight .................................................... 18 Table 2. Four-Stage Loyalty Model ................................................................................. 22 Table 3. Types of Affective Responses ........................................................................... 25 Table 4. The Guest Profile of a Midwestern Resort ........................................................ 30 Table 5. Sampling Frame ................................................................................................. 30 Table 6. Data Collection .................................................................................................. 33 Table 7. Survey Measurement Items ................................................................................ 37 Table 8. Normality Test Results for Variables Included in the Proposed Model ............ 41 Table 9. Socio-Demographic Characteristics of Survey Respondents ............................. 43 Table 10. Travel Characteristics of Survey Respondents ................................................. 45 Table 11. The Recommended Goodness-Of-Fit Indices .................................................. 48 Table 12. Comparison of the Proposed and Modified Full Measurement Models ........... 49 Table 13. The Result of Confirmatory Factor Analysis for the Modified Measurement Model 2 ............................................................................................................................ 51 Table 14. Discriminant Validity Matrix Local ................................................................. 52 Table 15. Results of Hypotheses Testing ......................................................................... 55 Table 16. Overall Fit Indices for the Hypothesized Structural Model .............................. 55 Table 17. The Results of the Proposed Model and the Full Path Model ......................... 61 Table 18. Socio-Demographic Characteristics of Survey Respondents between First-Time Guests (or not Recent Guests in the Past 3 Years) and Repeat Guests ............................. 66 Table 19. Mean Difference in Satisfaction, Delight, Cognitive Loyalty, Affective Loyalty, Conative Loyalty, and Action Loyalty between First-Time Guests (or not Recent Guests in the Past 3 Years) and Repeat Guests ........................................................................... 67 Table 20. x2 Difference Tests ........................................................................................... 68 Table 21.. Comparison of Overall Fit Indices for Two Groups ......................................... 69 Table 22. Path Coefficients of the Hypothesized Models between the Groups of First- Time Guests (or not Recent Guests in the Past 3 Years) ................................................. 70 Table 23. Socio-Demographic Characteristics of Survey Respondents between First-Time Guests (or not Recent Guests in the Past 3 Years) and Repeat Guests ............................. 74 Table 24. Mean Difference in Satisfaction, Delight, Cognitive Loyalty, Affective Loyalty, Conative Loyalty, and Action Loyalty between Loyalty Program Members and Loyalty Program Non-Members ................................................................................................... 75 Table 25. x2 Difference Tests ........................................................................................... 76 Table 26. Comparison of Overall Fit Indices for Two Groups ......................................... 76 Table 27. Path Coefficients of the Hypothesized Models between the Groups of Loyalty Program Members and Loyalty Program Non-Members ................................................ 78 Table 28. Summary of the Hypothesized Findings .......................................................... 81 Table 29. Comparison of Demographic Characteristics between On—line Survey Respondents and Paper Suervey Respondents .................................................................. 97 Table 30. Compariosn of Demographic Characteristics betweem Respondents and Non- Respondents ...................................................................................................................... 99 xi LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. Proposed Research Model .................................................................................. 4 Figure 2. Overall Model of Consumer Behavior ............................................................... 9 Figure 3. The Expectation-Disconfinnation Theory ....................................................... 11 Figure 4. A Model of Dissatisfaction, Outrage, Satisfaction, and Delight ..................... 17 Figure 5. The Relationship between Affect and Cognition ............................................. 27 Figure 6. The Proposed Measurement Model ................................................................... 46 Figure 7. Results of Testing the Proposed Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations .............................................................. 53 Figure 8. The Proposed Model ................................. . ........................................................ 58 Figure 9. The Full Path Model ......................................................................................... 59 Figure 10. Results of Testing the Proposed Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations ............................................................... 62 Figure 11. Results of Testing the Full Path Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations .............................................................. 63 Figure 12. Results of Testing the Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations (A Group of First-Time Guests or not Recent Guests in the Past 3 Years) .......................................................................................................... 71 Figure 13. Results of Testing the Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations (A Group of Repeat Guests) .................................... 72 Figure 14. Results of Testing the Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations (A Group of Loyalty Program Members) ................ 79 Figure 15. Results of Testing the Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations (A Group of Loyalty Program Non-Members) ........ 80 Figure 16. Proposed Conceptual Model ........................................................................... 83 xii CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION This chapter includes the following sections: (1) Introduction of Constructs: Customer Delight, Satisfaction, and Loyalty; (2) Problem Statement; (3) Purpose of Study; (4) Proposed Research Model and Hypotheses; (5) Delimitations; and (6) Definitions of Terms. Introduction of Constructs Customer Satisfaction and Delight Customer satisfaction has been a dominant benchmark among marketers in measuring their success as a fundamental factor to the well-being of individual customers and to the profits of organizations supported through purchasing and patronization (Oliver, 1997). Much literature demonstrates customer satisfaction is considered the main antecedent of customer loyalty. Research has proven that satisfied customers Show loyalty by purchasing more products or services, recommending products to others, and being less price sensitive (Anderson, Fornell, and Mazvancheryls, 2004; Ajzen and Driver, 1991; Chen and Gursoy, 2001; Homburg, Koschate, and Hoyer, 2005; Shoemaker and Lewis, 1999; Yoon and Uysal, 2005). Previous studies question whether satisfied customers are truly loyal (Johns and Sasser, 1995; Skogland and Siguaw, 2004; Stewart, 1997) and some have also begun to emphasize customer delight, which is beyond satisfaction, and may produce greater loyalty or attractive attitudinal or behavioral states (Hicks, Page, Behe, Dennis, and Fernandez, 2005; Schlossberg, 1990;). Customer delight can be the key to reaching loyalty through offering the “wowing” experience for the customer (Oliver, Rust, and Varki, 1997; Patterson, 1997; Torres and Kline, 2006) and delight can be an important factor of profitability and competitive advantage, provided that particular service is not easy to imitate, nor costly to implement (Oliver and Rust, 2000). However, despite the wide acceptance of the customer delight concept, this idea has not been given a clear theoretical foundation, measurement instruments, and identification of antecedents and consequences of customer delight, nor has customer delight been empirically related to customer satisfaction or loyalty. Custom er Loyalty Like customer satisfaction, customer loyalty is considered crucial to the success of business organizations with the growing realization that it is much less expensive to retain current customers than to find and develop new ones. Reichheld and Sasser (1990) showed that a 5 percent improvement in customer retention can cause an increase in profitability between 25 and 85 percent (in terms of net present value) depending on the industry. Academic research on customer loyalty has received considerable attention with many studies exploring the linkage between customer loyalty and satisfaction. Some loyalty studies have distinguished between the attitudinal and behavioral dimensions of loyalty with a framework for understanding different phases of loyalty development (Dick and Basu, 1994; Jacoby and Kyner, 1973; Knox and Walker, 2001; Oliver, 1997). According to Jacoby and Kyner (1973), loyalty can be viewed as developing in three phases: first, customers become loyal in a cognitive sense, then second, in an affective sense, and third, in a conative manner. Oliver (1997) suggested a fourth phase, action loyalty, which allows for completion of the loyalty framework. The development of customer loyalty has become an important marketing strategy for many years due to the benefits associated with retaining existing customers (McMullan and Gilmore, 2002) and understanding the multi-phase processes of loyalty for segmenting customers with differential strategies according to phases (Knox and Walker, 2001; Palmer, McMahon-Beattie, and Beggs, 2000). However, there is little empirical research to adequately explore the relationships between customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty, along with the four phases of a customer’s attitudinal and behavioral states. This research seeks to contribute to addressing this gap in the context of the tourism and hospitality industry by empirically testing a conceptual framework to understand more fully cognitive, affective, conative, and action loyalties. Problem Statement This study is aimed at understanding (1) how customer satisfaction and delight influence loyalty, and (2) the existence of cognitive, affective, conative, and action loyalties. These relationships will be in the context of the tourism and hospitality industry with first time and repeat customers, as well as customers belonging (or not) to a paid loyalty membership program. Purpose of Study This study is one of few empirical studies examining the relationships among customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty and offering a better understanding of the four loyalty phases. This study also empirically tests a more comprehensive model than previous research efforts. The findings of this study are expected to provide a deeper understanding of customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty for managers to more effectively serve customers by better understanding loyalty creation and retention. Additionally, this study is expected to contribute to the development of the body of knowledge of customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty studies. Conceptual Model and Hypotheses Based on these relationships, a model is proposed in Figure 1. Customer Satisfaction Hlb+ H22 + Customer Delight H2b + Attitudinal Loyalty ICognitive Loyalty H4a + Loyalty a Loyalty ‘ Conative 1 l I “ma: Loyalty; , TAT Figure 1. Proposed Research Model The following hypotheses are proposed: .-.,I.fLLo_ya.lty_}jjl:ii ** Darker lines indicate a stronger influence: H3a & H3b Hypothesis 1: Customer satisfaction will have a direct positive influence on attitudinal loyalty. Hypothesis 1a: Customer satisfaction will have a direct positive influence on cognitive loyalty. Hypothesis 1b: Customer satisfaction will have a direct positive influence on affective loyalty. Hypothesis 2: Customer delight will have a direct positive influence on attitudinal loyalty. Hypothesis 2a: Customer delight will have a direct positive influence on cognitive loyalty. Hypothesis 2b: Customer delight will have a direct positive influence on affective loyalty. Hypothesis 3: Customer delight will have a greater influence on attitudinal loyalty in comparison to satisfaction. Hypothesis 3a: Customer delight will have a greater positive influence on cognitive loyalty in comparison to satisfaction. Hypothesis 3b: Customer delight will have a greater positive influence on affective loyalty in comparison to satisfaction. Hypothesis 4: Customer cognitive loyalty will have a direct positive influence on conative loyalty. Hypotheses 5: Customer affective loyalty will have a direct positive influence on conative loyalty. Hypothesis 6: Customer conative loyalty will have a direct positive influence on action loyalty. Delimitations This study is delimited to the following: 1. All subjects were hotel guests in the Midwest who stayed in the months of July, August, and September 2009. 2. Guests with email addresses were selected, which allowed this study to include guests from the US, Canada, and other countries for an approximate sample size of 3,700 participants. 3. The participants included new guests and repeat guests who stayed at this hotel within the last three years. Both current loyalty club members and non-members were included. Definition of Terms The following terms are defined to clarify their use in this study: Action Loyalty: Loyalty to the action of rebuying the service or product (Dick and Basu, 1994; Oliver, 1997). Affective Loyalty: Loyalty to the liking of the service or product (Dick and Basu, 1994; Oliver, 1997). Cognitive Loyalty: Loyalty to information such as price and features (Dick and Basu, 1994; Oliver, 1997). Conative Loyalty: Loyalty to an intention to repurchase the service or product (Dick and Basu, 1994; Oliver, 1997). Customer Delight: A higher level of satisfaction that exceeds a customer’s expectations as a combination of high pleasure (joy, elation) and high activation (surprise) (Oliver et aL,1997) Customer Loyalty: A deeply held commitment to rebuy or repatronize a preferred product or service consistently in the future, despite situational influences and marketing efforts having the potential to cause switching behavior (Oliver, 1997). Customer Satisfaction: The customer’s fulfillment response to a product or service feature, or to the product or service itself, which provides a pleasurable level of consumption-related fulfillment, including levels of under- or over-fulfillment (Oliver, 1997) Delight: A combination of high pleasure (joy, elation) and high activation or surprise as one of the second level emotions (Plutchik, 1980; Russell, 1980; Watson and Tellegen, 1.985). CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW The literature related to this study is reported in this chapter. For organizational purposes, the literature is presented under the following topics: (1) Consumer Behavior; (2) Customer Satisfaction; (3) Customer Delight; (4) Customer Loyalty; and (5) Affect and Cognition. Consumer Behavior By the end of the 20th century, the study of consumer behavior became more important than ever as a result of changing market forces such as increased competition, changing consumer lifestyle, the greater influence of the consumer, and a changing business orientation from a manufacturing focus to a marketing focus. Since then, the need to understand consumers and their behavior has been a core topic around the globe (Blackwell, Miniard, and Engel, 2001). Consumer behavior is an applied science drawing from economics, psychology, sociology, anthropology, statistics, and other disciplines (Hawkins, Mothersbaugh, and Best, 2007; Peter and Olson, 2005). Consumer behavior is the study of individuals, groups, or organizations and the processes they use to select, secure, use, and dispose of products, services, experiences, or ideas to satisfy needs and the impacts that these processes have on the consumer and society (Hawkins, Mothersbaugh, and Best, 2007). Consumer behavior is also defined as the dynamic interaction of affect and cognition, behavior, and the environment by which human beings conduct the exchange aspects of their lives (Bennett, 1995). Consumer behavior is purposeful and goal oriented; products and services are selected or rejected according to the extent to which they are perceived as relevant to the needs and the lifestyle of a consumer (Peter and Olson, 2005). According to an overall model of consumer behavior as shown in Figure 2, a consumer builds up self-concepts and subsequent lifestyles based on a mixture of internal (mostly psychological and physical) and external (mostly sociological and demographic) influences. These self-concepts and lifestyles produce needs and desires, many of which require consumption decisions to satisfy. As consumers come across relevant situations, the consumer decision process is activated. This process and the experiences and acquisitions it produces, in turn, influence the consumer’s self-concept and lifestyle by affecting their internal and external characteristics (Hawkins et al., 2007). 1, Experiences and Acquisitions I ‘ External " . :geclsion :. . Influences .1 , ., . rocess -Cllltllre ' V 3; ‘ 3'71} ‘ -Subcu|ture j _ V,Srtuatrons . i-Demographics ‘ ‘ . —Prohlcmi -Socral Status _ -- g , Recognition- .-ReferenceGroups 5 , p. 3 -Marketing Activities I Self-Cotillcept ' ,.,ri'rror_'maii§ii;zjfi .7 Ian Desires ': 3' Search I Lifestyle 77¢ . . , i 7 i-Alternative" : 'I . l {Evaluatio'nand ._ '_ nterna selection" i ' Influences ’ IV , -Perception ' , . , . 7 V. -Learning , * -Outlet Selection ~Memory and Purchase. -Motives '_ -Personality ‘ . I :j --Emo‘fions -P_ost-purch_a_se_ : . .-Attitudes Experiences and Acquisitions » Processes » 1‘ l Figure 2. Overall Model of Consumer Behavior (Adopted from Hawkins, Mothersbaugh, and Best, 2007) Customer Satisfaction The importance of customer satisfaction as a core concept in marketing has led to numerous studies over the past decades. The literature supports that satisfied customers are willing to buy more products or services, recommend them to others, and are less price sensitive (Homburg et al., 2005). Hence, satisfaction is an essential factor related to a company’s future profit by increasing the customer retention rate (Anderson et al., 2004) Following the pioneering experimental study by Cardozo (1965), customer satisfaction and product quality were emphasized by Olshavsky and Miller (1972) and Anderson (1973). This approach suggested that expectations influence customer satisfaction through perceived quality and disconfirmation. Following Olshavsky and Miller (1972) and Anderson (1973), as shown in Figure 3, the expectation- disconfirrnation theory was introduced by Oliver (1981), grounded by social psychology (Weaver and Brickman, 1974), and influenced by organizational behavior (Ilgen, 1971). The theory posits that customer satisfaction is believed to result from a process of customers’ comparisons between their expectations and perceptions of performance and that the confirmation or disconfirmation of those expectations predicts satisfaction. Positive disconfirmation occurs when performance perceptions of customers exceed expectations, which leads to satisfaction, whereas perceptions falling short of expectations result in negative disconfirmation, which induces dissatisfaction. Since then, this theory was used by researchers and industries with strong support (Tse and Wilton, 1988; Yi, 1990). 10 Expectations Calculated Subjective Disconfirmation Disconfirmation .1 .2 Satisfaction/ Dissatisfaction Performance Outcomes Figure 3. The Expectation-Disconfirmation Theory (Adopted from Oliver, 1997) According to Churchill and Surprenant (1982), both expectations and perceptions have been found to influence customer satisfaction and subjective disconfirmation under various circumstances, and customer satisfaction is determined by the levels of prior expectations, the levels of perceived performance, and the degree and direction of disconfirmation. Many studies have tested the expectancy-disconfirmation theory, resulting in different views about comparison standards for expectations and interrelationships among the key variables (Anderson, 1973; Boulding, Kalra, Staelin, and Zeitharnl, 1993; Cardozo, 1965; Churchill and Surprenant, 1982; Oliver, 1980; Oliver and Swan, 1989; Olshavsky and Miller, 1972; Tse and Wilton, 1988; Yi, 1990). Earlier studies focused on repurchase expectations as antecedents of satisfaction. Specifically, Anderson and Sullivan (1993) emphasized the perceived quality at the post- 11 consumption stage based on utility-oriented framework. This approach considers perceived quality as the utility resulting from consumption. Contrary to previous studies, Anderson and Sullivan (1993) denied a direct effect of expectations on satisfaction. Instead, they argued that expectations influence satisfaction through perceived quality and disconfirmation. Overall, their model puts an emphasis on the role of perceived quality in customer satisfaction. Since then, perceived quality and/or value have been proposed as antecedents of satisfaction; satisfaction, then, mediates between quality and/or value and behavioral intention (Cronin, Brady, and Hult, 2000; Fornell, Johnson, Anderson, Cha, and Bryant, 1996; Patterson, 2004). Despite extensive research in customer satisfaction, researchers have yet to develop a consensual definition of customer satisfaction. Howard and Sheth (1969) defined satisfaction as “the buyer’s cognitive state of being adequately or inadequately rewarded for the sacrifice he has undergone” (p. 145), and Hunt (1977) offered “satisfaction is not an emotion, it is the evaluation of an emotion, and as such, it becomes a quasi-cognitive construct” (p. 459). Tse and Wilton (1988) classified satisfaction as “the customer’s response to the evaluation of the perceived discrepancy between prior expectations or some other norm of performance and the actual performance of the product as perceived after its consumption” (p. 204). In contrast, Locke (1976) summarized the volumes of research on job satisfaction to define satisfaction as “a pleasurable or positive emotional state resulting from the appraisal of one’s job.” Westbrook and Reilly (1983) described satisfaction as “an emotional response to the experiences provided by, or associated with, particular products or services purchased, as 12 ’7! (LI In} .94 .J I C 1... J11 no :L. 4‘ r“ vhu [4 I . ‘1 F -u_ it“. LL) - I I '._. - p s. I): well as the overall market place” (p. 256). Oliver (1997) defined satisfaction as the customer’s fulfillment response. Based on Oliver’s (1980) disconfirmation theory, satisfaction was primarily understood as a cognitive state resulting from cognitive evaluations between expectations and perceived performance which leads to satisfaction (Bigné, Andreu, and Gnoth, 2003; Oliver and Swan, 1989). However, Oliver (1997) argued that both cognitive and emotional antecedents must be modeled simultaneously in satisfaction modeling because satisfaction is a hybrid of cognitions and emotions. Furthermore, Oliver (1997) also demonstrated that positive emotions and negative emotions influence independently on a customer’s satisfaction evaluation. Woodruff, Cadotte, and Jenkins (1983) supported that satisfaction should be defined as the link between cognitive and emotional processes because customer satisfaction is an emotional feeling in response to cognitive evaluation through confirmation/disconfirrnation. Moreover, several other researchers suggested that both cognitive and affective aspects of a product and service stimulus predict satisfaction (Bigné et al., 2003; Mano and Oliver, 1993; Westbrook and Oliver, 1991; Wirtz, Mattila, and Tan, 2000). Therefore, the need to understand satisfaction by incorporating both cognitive and affective perspectives has been emphasized (Bi gné et al., 2003; Oliver et al., 1997; Phillips and Baumgartner, 2002; Wirtz and Bateson, 1999; Wirtz et al., 2000). In the tourism and hospitality industry, managers are trying to find ways to increase customer satisfaction under the challenging environment of the expanding sophistication of customers’ demands and market competition. Customer satisfaction is also one of the most frequently examined topics by researchers. They have applied satisfaction-related theories and methods in this area (Almanza, J affe, and Lin, 1994; 13 Barsky, 1992; Barsky and Labagh, 1992; Bigné et al., 2003; Danaher and Arweiler, 1996; Getty and Thompson, 1995; Knutson, Stevens, Wullaert, Patton, and Yokoyama, 1991; Oh and Jeong, 1996; Ryan and Cliff, 1997; Wirtz et al., 2000). Most lodging customer studies have focused on measuring the level of customer satisfaction with individual service attributes and customers’ wants and needs from the attribute (Cadotte and Turgeon, 1988; Knutson, 1988; Lewis, 1983; Lewis and Pizarn, 1981; Nightingale, 1985; Saleh and Ryan, 1991). Empirical evidence exists showing that tourists’ satisfaction is a strong indicator of their intentions to revisit and recommend the destination to other people (Beeho and Prentice, 1997; Bramwell, 1998; Juaneda, 1996; Kozak, 2001; Kozak and Rimmington, 2000; Ross, 1993; Yau and Chan, 1990; Yoon and Uysal, 2005). According to Yoon and Uysal (2005), satisfied tourists are more likely to return to the same destination and are more willing to share their positive travel experience with their fi'iends and relatives. Word-of-mouth (WOM) recommendations are especially critical in tourism marketing because they are considered to be the most reliable, and thus are one of the most sought-after information, sources for potential tourists (Yoon and Uysal, 2005). In particular, in the tourism and hospitality industry, customers have more opportunities to interact with a product and service provider during the consumption experience. Therefore, understanding customers’ emotional needs becomes an important aspect of understanding their satisfaction (Bigné et al., 2003). Customer Delight Customer satisfaction has been regarded as an important antecedent of loyalty for many years, but recently, this idea has been challenged as research shows customer satisfaction does not necessarily result in repurchase or loyalty (Johns and Sasser, 1995; 14 Stewart, 1997). Instead, researchers have begun to emphasize the importance of delighting customers, which is more likely to indicate loyalty than satisfied customers (Oliver et al., 1997; Paul, 2000). Delighting a customer can be considered a critical competitive advantage (Hicks et al., 2005 ; Oliver et al., 1997; Williams and Anderson, 1999) because customer delight drives customer loyalty by retaining customers and creating positive WOM (Johns and Sasser, 1995; Keiningham and Vavra, 2001; Maister, 1993; Oliver et al., 1997; Paul, 2000). Oliver et al.(l997) and Torres and Kline (2006) suggested that the true way to increase loyalty, loyalty-driven profit, and improve retention is not to satisfy customers, but to delight them by offering exceptional personalized services while creating a customer preference towards a company. Delight produces emotional bonds between customers and a company, product, or service by “wowing” them, which may provide additional psychological benefits to the customers (Berry, 1995; Hirschman and Holbrook, 1982; Oliver et al., 1997). Customer delight is defined as a higher level of satisfaction achieved by exceeding a customer’s expectations (Oliver et al., 1997) and is also considered to be one of the second level emotions (Arnold, Goldston, Walsh, Reboussin, Daniel, Hickman, and Wood, 2005; Firm, 2005; Oliver et al., 1997; Russell, 1980; Watson and Tellegen, 1985). Emotions, in general, consist of two dimensions: pleasure and arousal (Mano and Oliver, 1993; Mattila and Wirtz, 2000; Russell and Pratt, 1980; Wirtz and Bateson, 1999). Pleasure refers to the degree to which a person feels good, joyful, or happy in a situation, which arousal refers to the extent to which a person feels stimulated and active (Bigné et al., 2003). Mano and Oliver’s (1993) study showed a result of eight affects including high and low non-specific activation and high, moderate, and low levels each of the positive and 15 negative affect. The most closely related affects are moderate arousal positive affect (e. g. pleasure), high arousal positive affect (e. g. delight), and high non-specific arousal (e. g. surprise). Thus, delight is a combination of high pleasure (joy, elation) and high activation (surprise) (Arnold et al., 2005; Firm, 2005 ; Oliver et al., 1997; Plutchik, 1980; Russell, 1980; Watson and Tellegen, 1985). Further, delight involves a stronger emotion and a different physiological state than satisfaction and the occurring of delight is directly related to positive affect during the consumer’s consumption experiences (Hicks et al., 2005). Oliver et al. (1997) suggested that the experience of delight during the consumption process may cause the customer to want and try for reoccurrences of this affective state. Therefore, if delight is achieved, the customer will be more likely to repurchase the product. Oliver et al. (1997) also argued that delight occurs when the result is unanticipated and When high levels of performance initiate arousal, which leads to pleasure, and ultimately delight. Delight is considered an ephemeral emotion. Some studies posit that delight is not necessarily a combination of joy and surprise. Instead, delight is described as a sense of relatedness between the customer and the company that evokes feelings of joy (Kumar, Olshavsky, and King, 2001). Based on this concept, customer delight and satisfaction are separate concepts although they are correlated (Hicks et al., 2005; Oliver et al., 1997; Rust and Oliver, 1994; Westbrook and Oliver, 1991). As shown in Figure 4, there are four different types of post purchase response to a product or service: outrage/pain, dissatisfaction, satisfaction, and delight. 16 Experienced Performance (Partially Affective) Negative Positive c c o .9 a ’6 E D N d H ._ o isa ointe ; a A c: g p”. . ~——> Q g) o “,3 and Pain E Z ”J i: “5 or o ‘D O o V I I D . I I g Dissatisfaction 3 Satisfaction in a) I— D. Figure 4. A Model of Dissatisfaction, Outrage, Satisfaction, and Delight (Adopted from Berman, 2005) In this model, the major difference between satisfaction and delight is the element of surprise, whereas there is a positive expected level of performance both in satisfaction and delight (Berman, 2005). There are important differences between satisfaction and delight as shown in Table 1. Satisfaction is more cognitive and based on perceptions, while delight is more affective and emotional triggering feelings such as arousal, joy, and pleasure. Satisfaction is also schema-based and results from a comparison between expectation and performance, while delight is from a recreated schema, which needs out- of-the-ordinary performance. Additionally, satisfaction has a weaker memory than delight and it is based on fiilfilling the expected, while delight is based on fulfilling the unexpected (Berman, 2005). Delight is also more likely to occur in situations where customers are highly involved (Oliver et al., 1997). Table 1. Differences between Satisfaction and Delight (Adapted from Berman, 2005) Satisfaction Delight -More cognitive -More affective -Schema based -Recreated schema -Weaker memory than delight —Stronger memory than satisfaction -Based on fulfilling the ergiected -Based on fiilfilling the unexpected Customer Loyalty Customer loyalty is defined as a deeply held commitment to rebuy or repatronize a preferred product or service consistently in the future, despite situational influences and marketing efforts having the potential to cause switching behavior (Oliver, 1997). Loyalty is desirable since it is much less expensive to retain current customers than to find and develop new ones (Reichheld and Sasser, 1990). Furthermore, loyal customers are more likely to act as free word-of-mouth (WOM) advertising agents that informally bring networks of friends, relatives, and other potential customers to products and services (Shoemaker and Lewis, 1999). Although loyalty has been researched in many different ways, loyalty has been defined in two main approaches: behavioral loyalty and attitudinal loyalty (Dekimpe, Steenkarnp, Mellens, and Vanden, 1997; Dick and Basu, 1994; Yi and La, 2004). In the early loyalty studies, researchers mostly focused on the behavioral approach (Oliver, 1997). From the behavioral perspective, customer loyalty is defined as the non-random purchase over time of one brand from a set of brands by a consumer using an evaluation process (J acoby and Kyner, 1973; Newman and Werbal, 1973). Loyalty is also defined as repeat purchase or patronage (Neal, 1999; Oliver, 1997) and as the proportion of times a 18 purchaser chooses the same product or service in a specific category compared to the total number of purchases made by the purchaser in that category (Neal, 1999). In measuring loyalty with a behavioral approach, a repurchase probability is used, which is a long-term choice probability for a brand (Carpenter and Lehmann, 1985; Colombo, Morrison, and Green, 1989; Dekimpe et al., 1997). Tellis (1988) measured loyalty as repeat purchase frequency or relative volume of same brand purchasing. Furthermore, Jacoby and Chestnut (1978) distinguished four kinds of loyalty: true focal brand loyalty to the particular brand of interest, true multibrand loyalty which includes the focal brand, nonloyal repeat purchasing of the focal brand, and happenstance purchasing of the focal brand by a loyal or nonloyal buyer of another brand. Based on Jacoby and Chestnut’s (1978) loyalty classification, Iwasaki and Havitz (1998) suggested three additional components of behavioral loyalty: duration, frequency of purchases, and intensity. Specifically, duration is the amount of time spent as a purchaser. Frequency is the number of purchases over a certain period of time. Intensity is the number of hours one devotes to using, participating in, or purchasing a product or service within a certain period of time. The criticism with the behavioral loyalty approach is that a repeat purchase is not always the result of a psychological commitment to the brand, the intention may not lead to action, and repeated buying behavior may not always reflect intentions (Yang and Peterson, 2004). Additionally, there is no differentiation between customers who made purchasing decisions with true brand preferences and those who purchased for convenience or cost (Back, 2001). Day (1969) argued that measuring only the behavioral 19 aspects of loyalty can result in overestimating true loyalty, the commitment to a product or service for as repurchasing behavior. On the other hand, a number of researchers conceptualized loyalty based on an attitudinal aspect begun by Guest (1944). According to this approach, loyalty was considered a function of psychological processes (Jacoby and Chestnut, 1978), brand preference, intentions, emotional commitment, strength of affection for a brand (Cronin and Taylor, 1992; Iwasaki and Havitz, 1998), intention of word-of-mouth (WOM) (Boulding et al., 1993), or willingness to pay a premium price (Zeithaml, Berry, and Parasuraman, 1996). Therefore, loyal customers tend to Show special preference, attachment, commitment, positive WOM, low switching to alternative brands, and willingness to pay a premium price so that they contribute to higher profits (Yi and La, 2004). The criticism with the attitudinal loyalty concept lies in the lack of power in predicting actual purchase behavior. Measuring attitudinal loyalty itself can not explain the entirety of loyalty, and attitudinal loyalty should be combined with behavioral loyalty to clarify true loyalty (Iwasaki and Havitz, 1998). Additionally, using both attitudinal and behavioral aspects of loyalty provides a more influential explanation of loyalty (Assael, 1998) Based on a review of attitudinal and behavioral loyalty approaches, Jacoby and Chestnut (1978) viewed loyalty as a combination of attitudinal and behavioral aspects by proposing that brand loyalty is repeat purchase behavior based on belief acquisition, affect formation, and behavioral intention. Oliver (1999) and Zeithaml (2000) used both attitudinal and behavioral measures to define and assess these variables. They suggested that loyalty must consider both the behavioral and attitudinal perspectives. The use of 20 Int, Jill V‘.‘;. Do If» . .‘l l a ~-.i i I {1.11le "i" Elton: v . ..‘. _ ILL». C' filly] I j‘lt‘-'..r 'I..e..,, “in CQ'TRF. “i- ‘ ‘.IYsI I-v : ”J" iili . I . _ . Id“, 1:,“ ‘, ll!‘ .‘“ “in «914'\ both attitudinal and behavioral loyalty increases the predictive power of loyalty (Hunter, 1998; Pritchard and Howard, 1997). These two dimensions of loyalty approaches have been applied and supported to understand customer loyalty better in many different fields (Day, 1969; Jacoby and Kyner, 1973; Oliver, 1999; Pritchard and Howard, 1997; Yi and La, 2004). Along with J acoby and Chestnut (1978), Dick and Basu (1994) suggested that loyalty requires consistency across the cognitive, affective, and conative dimensions of the consumer’s focal brand orientation. Following Dick and Basu (1994), Oliver (1997, 1999) suggested the cognition- affect-conation sequence of the loyalty-development process beginning from some cognitive loyalty, followed by affective loyalty, to conative loyalty, and finally action loyalty. He also described that loyalty formation is more likely to be an attitudinal development process and customers may demonstrate different levels of loyalty in different stages of this process. Table 2 presents the four phases of loyalty and their corresponding antecedents, sustainers, and vulnerabilities as proposed by Dick and Basu (1994) and Oliver (1997). According to Oliver’s (1997) loyalty dimensionality, the first phase, cognitive loyalty, is based on merely functional characteristics such as costs and benefits and is focused on the product or service’s performance. It is the weakest state of loyalty since this type of commitment is actually to costs and benefits and not to the brand itself (Oliver, 1997). Therefore, consumers are likely to switch when they perceive alternative offerings as being superior with respect to the cost-benefit ratio (Kalyanaram and Little, 1994; Sivakumar and Raj, 1997). 21 Table 2. Four-Stage Loyalty Model (Adapted from Oliver, 1997) Identifying Stage Maker Antecedents Sustainers Vulnerabilities Cognitive Loyalty to Accessibility Cost Cost information such Confidence Benefits Benefits as price, features Centrality Quality Quality Clarity Affective Loyalty to liking: Emotions Satisfaction Dissatisfaction “I buy it because I Moods Involvement Persuasion like it.” Primary affect Liking, Preference Trial Satisfaction Cognitive consistency Conative Loyalty to an Switching Commitment Persuasion intention: “I’m costs Cognitive consistency Trial committed to Sunk costs buying it.” Action Loyalty to action Inertia Inertia Persuasion inertia, the Sunk costs Sunk costs Trial overcoming of obstacles. The second phase is affective loyalty, which is a favorable attitude toward a specific brand or product. This loyalty is an enhanced liking for competitive brands, which is conveyed through imagery and association used in competitive communications (Oliver, 1999). Affective loyalty involves both the liking of the product or service and experiencing satisfaction with the brand. Attitude is shown as a function of cognition (expectancy disconfirmation) plus prior attitude, and plus satisfaction in later periods (Oliver, 1999). The third phase of loyalty development is conative loyalty which is behavioral intention to repurchase and involves a deep brand-specific commitment (Harris and Goode, 2004). Conative loyalty is stronger than affective loyalty but has vulnerabilities as well (Evanschitzky and Wunderlich, 2006). Consumers are more likely to try alternative offerings if they experience frequent service failure. Furthermore, although the consumer is conatively loyal, he or she may consider alternative offerings (Oliver, 1999). 22 The last phase of loyalty is action loyalty, which is transformed into action (Kuhl, 1985) and includes habit and routinized response behavior. Action loyalty is a commitment to the action of repurchasing. In this stage, intention is transformed into action. Persuasion and trial can be tried by competitors but competitive offerings by competitors are not considered as alternatives. This action control leads to a state of readiness to act and to a desire to overcome obstacles to achieve the action (Oliver, 1999). Many researchers have applied Oliver’s four-dimensional loyalty conceptualization in developing loyalty dimensionality (Back, 2005; Back and Parks, 2003; Evanschitzky and Wunderlich, 2006; Harris and Goode, 2004; McMullan and Gilmore, 2003; Oliver, 1999). For instance, Harris and Goode (2004) supported this conceptualization by conducting an empirical test with two online service scenarios and McMullan and Gilmore (2003) developed a 28-item scale to measure the four phases of loyalty with an empirical test in the restaurant context. Back (2005) and Evanschitzky and Wunderlich (2006) also confirmed Oliver’s (1999) conceptualization from their empirical testing in the hotel and retail environment. Some researchers suggested a different loyalty development sequence based on Oliver’s loyalty development framework (Back, 2001; Jones and Taylor, 2007; Li and Petrick, 2008; Rundle-Thiele, 2005). For example, following Breckler (1984), Beck (2001) and Li and Petrick (2008) suggested that cognitive, affective, and conative loyalty are not sequentially linked but all three aspects are independent factors of attitudinal loyalty, which lead to action loyalty according to the tripartite model. Jones and Taylor (2007) suggested a two-dimensional loyalty construct by combining attitudinal and cognitive loyalty as one dimension and behavioral loyalty as the other dimension. Overall, 23 recent studies on loyalty have been broadening the concept, but there is still no consensus on a loyalty development construct. In tourist behavior research, return visitation has been used to assess tourists’ destination loyalty (Chen and Gursoy, 2001). Repeat visitation is a necessary component of destination loyalty, as it generally tends to occur with brand loyalty. Destination loyalty might also generate a lower sensitivity to prices offered by rival destinations (Krishnamurthi and Papatla, 2003). Destination loyalty is defined by Chen and Gursoy (2001) as the level of tourists’ perceptions of a destination as a recommendable place. However, return visitation may not truly represent loyalty and a non-repeat visit behavior may not represent lack of loyalty to a destination. For example, those who do not return to a previously visited destination may remain loyal to that destination but simply want to seek different travel experiences at new destinations. Affect and Cognition Cognition refers to people’s thoughts about an attitude object and encompasses the content of one’s thoughts regarding beliefs in the statement of fact. Affect refers to the specific quality of goodness or badness experienced as a feeling state and demarcates positive or negative quality of a stimulus (Solvic, Finucane, Peters, and MacGregor, 2004; Zajonc, 1980). As shown in Table 3, there are four different kinds of affective responses: emotions, specific feelings, moods, and evaluations (Peter and Olson, 2005). Each type of affect involves positive or negative reactions. The four different kinds of affect have different levels of physiological arousal and the intensity of feeling (Kroeber- Riel, 1979). 24 Table 3. Types of Affective Responses (Adppted from Peter and Olson, 2005) Level or Intensity or Type of Affective Physiological Strength of Examples of Positive and Responses Arousal Feeling Negative Affect Higher arousal Stronger -Joy, love Emotions -Fear, guilt, anger I I -Warmth, satisfaction Specific feelings -Disgust, sadness -Alert, relaxed, calm Moods -Blue, bored -Like, good, favorable Evaluations Lower arousal Weaker -Dislike, bad, unfavorable There has been a long-standing theoretical issue in understanding the cognitive and affective sequence and the interplay between cognition and affection is still unresolved (Chebat and Michon, 2003; Lazarus, 1991; Solvic, 2000; Zajonc, 1980; Zajonc and Markus, 1985). Lazarus (1982, 1984) posited that affect is connected to a cognitive appraisal mechanism and affect needs cognition as a necessary precondition because cognition is a prior element of affect. “Probably all mammals meet the minimal cognitive requirements of emotion if one permits the concept of appraisal to include the type of process described by ethnologists in which a fairly rigid, built-in response to stimulus arrays differentiates danger fiom no-danger. An evaluative perception, hence, appraisal, can operate at all levels of complexity, from the most primitive and inborn to the most symbolic and experience-based.” (Lazarus, 1982, p. 1023). In accordance with Lazarus’ (1982) statement, other theories of satisfaction and loyalty based on the cognitive-affective sequence have been proposed (Bigné et al., 2003; Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975; Oliver, 1997; Perez-Caro and Sanchez-Garcia, 2006). For instance, Fishbein and Aj zen (1975) depicted that consumers form beliefs, formulate likes and dislikes, and decide whether they wish to buy a product. Oliver (1997) 25 emphasized the importance of studying the cognitive-affective sequence as a cognitive appraisal and then creating emotions or affect in behavior formation. Zajonc (1980, 2000) emphasized the importance of affect in decision making as a different approach to the cognitive and affective sequence. Affective reactions to stimuli are often the very first reactions, occurring quickly and automatically and then leading information processing and judgments about objects, activities, and other stimuli with a direct and primary role in motivating behavior (Solvic, F inucane, Peters, and MacGregor, 2002; Zajonc, 1980, 2000). Further, Epstein (1994) argued that affect plays a central role in what have come to be known as “dual-process theories” of thinking, knowing and information processing. According to Epstein (1994), there are two basic modes of thinking: experiential and rational. One of the main characteristics of the experiential system is its affective basis. Although analysis is certainly important in some decision- making circumstances, reliance on affect and emotion is a quicker, easier, and more efficient way to navigate in a complex and uncertain world (Epstein, 1994; Slovic et al., 2002). Some researchers support that affect is a direct and primary role in behavior (LeDoux, 1996; Solvic, 1980; Slovic et al., 2002; Zajonc, 1980) and affective evaluation can take place without conscious stimulus recognition (Kunst—Wilson and Zajonc, 1980). Therefore, affect does not always need cognition but affect guides judgments and decisions (Damasio, 1994; Lerner and Keltner, 2001; Loewenstin, Weber, Hsee, and Welch, 2001; Zajonc, 1980; Zajonc and Markus, 1985) and representations of objects and events in people’s minds are tagged to varying degrees with affect. In the process of making a judgment or decision, people consult or refer to an affect pool containing all the 26 positive and negative tags associated with the representations consciously or unconsciously (Tversky and Kahneman, 1974). Zajonc (1980) argued that affect and cognition are separate and partially independent systems and affect could be generated without a prior cognitive process, although affect and cognition usually function conjointly. The neurophysiological evidence (Armony, Servan-Schreiber, Cohen, and LeDoux, 1996; LeDoux, 1996) suggests that affective processes and cognitive processes influence one another; affect and cognition cannot be easily separated as pure affect or pure cognition. Peter and Olson (2005) and Piaget (1981) described how affect and cognition are highly interdependent involving different parts of the brain like affection and cognition are linked as two sides of the same coin. Figure 5 shows how affect and cognition are related. Each system reacts differently to aspects of environment and responds to the output of the other system (Peter and Olson, 2005). I Environment I _....3 Cognitive System I Affective Cognitive Responses Responses —Knowledge —Emotions -Meanings -Feelings -Beliefs -Moods , ’-Evalnations Figure 5. The Relationship between Affect and Cognition (Adopted from Peter and Olson, 2005) 27 Given this relationship, in consumer behavior, understanding both affect and cognition are important in developing marketing strategies for areas such as product design, advertisement, and store layout (Peter and Olson, 2005). For example, as a primary objective of marketing strategies, customer satisfaction consists of both affect and cognition (Oliver, 1997) and brand image also includes affect (e. g., feelings and emotions about the brand) and cognition (e. g., knowledge and beliefs about brand attributes) (Dichter, 1985; Dobni and Zinkhan, 1990). Further, many studies have explored the linkage between customer loyalty and satisfaction; most existing models of customer loyalty are cognitively based and there is little research on the psychology behind the development of customer loyalty (Mattila, 2001). 28 CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY This chapter discusses the research methodology that was used to answer the proposed research hypotheses. This chapter is divided into three sections: (1) research design including rational for research method, sampling, pilot study, and data collection procedures; (2) survey measurement; and (3) data analysis. Research Design Rationale for Research Method The main purpose of this study is to understand (1) how customer satisfaction and delight influence loyalty, and (2) the relationships between cognitive, affective, conative, and action loyalties. The survey research method is considered most appropriate over other types of research methods in answering these research questions. Quantitative methodology gives a broad, generalizable set of findings for this study by obtaining responses from many people; a survey is good at examining relationships between factors (Trochim, 2001). A popular resort in the Midwest was selected as it is located in one of the most well-known tourist destinations in the state. As an independent hotel with 360 themed guestrooms, five indoor pools and three Whirlpools, a miniature golf course, a large family fun center with over 100 video/arcade games, and several guest banquet and convention spaces, this resort has a long standing club membership program. Table 4 presents a profile summary of the population provided by the resort from 2003 to 2009. The population consisted of overnight hotel guests from a Midwestem resort. Based on this profile of the population, a sample was selected to test the proposed model. 29 ~~~~~ Table 4. The Guest Profile of a Midwestern resort Data variable Number of % of Number of % of from 2003 to February mailable mailable E-mailable E-mailable 2009 guests guests guests _guests Total number of guests 130,370 100% 48,621 100% F irst-time guests 102,959 73% 34,521 71% Repeat guests 27,411 27% 14,100 29% Sampling The guests who stayed at the resort during the summer of 2009 in the months of July, August, and September were the sample for this study. Table 5 shows the number of guests who stayed at this resort, the number of e-mailed surveys, and the number of mailed surveys in the months of July, August, and September 2009. The total sample size was 3,709 (E-mailed surveys: 3,459 and mailed surveys: 250). Table 5. Sampling Frame Number of Total number Total number of Number of E-mailed of surveys guests stayed mailed surveys surveys distributed July 2009 2,199 100 1,227 1,327 August 2009 2,750 125 1,537 1,662 September 2009 442 25 695 720 Total 5,391 250 3,459 3,709 Subjects were household travel parties of first-time guests and repeat guests who stayed at this hotel one or more times within the last three years. Both current loyalty club members and non-loyalty club members were included. Business travelers and groups for events (e. g., weddings, reunions, meetings) were excluded in this study because some of them might not be directly involved in the decision making of the choice of hotel or in making the reservation. Instead their stay at this hotel might have been pre- arranged by others. 30 Pilot Study A pilot study (n=32) was conducted in June 2009 with faculty, administrators, and graduate students at Michigan State University prior to the main survey to develop and test a questionnaire instrument for this study. After this pilot study, the questionnaire instrument was revised to facilitate easier reading and clearer wording. Reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) for each measurement construct was also examined to clarify scale items. The scales for “Customer satisfaction”, “Customer delight”, “Cognitive loyalty”, “Affective loyalty”, “Conative loyalty”, and “Action loyalty” demonstrated acceptable reliability ranging fi'om or = .71 to or = .96 (Nunnally and Bernstein, 1994). Data Collection Prior to data collection, a description of this study and the data collection instruments were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of Michigan State University in May 2009. From July to September 2009, survey data were collected through two different approaches: online surveys and paper surveys. The Online Survey The primary data collection was conducted via a self-administered online survey using a web-based survey tool, Qualtrics, on a weekly basis. As Internet usage continues to grow rapidly, online surveys offer several methodological and financial advantages over the traditional paper survey (Couper, 2000). Advantages of online surveys include faster response rate, case in sending reminders to participants, easier processing of data, and less cost (Hewson, Yule, Laurent, and Vogel, 2003). Each week, upon completing their stay at this hotel, the survey questionnaire was emailed to them with an invitation letter from the general manager of the resort and a 31 1111'“ '81: university professor. The first e-mail reminder was sent to those who had not yet responded two weeks after the first survey questionnaire, and a second e—mail reminded those who had not yet responded one week after the first reminder. The questionnaire required approximately 15 to 20 minutes to complete. As shown in Table 6, a total of 3,459 surveys were sent via Qualtrics and the survey response rate was 50% (n=1,743). The Paper Survey A self-administered paper survey was also employed by mail in order to test for biases that an online survey might cause and to ensure an appropriate sample size for statistical power (i.e., the ability to detect and reject a poor model) in the SEM analysis (Chin, 1998). Respondents were chosen based on a systematic random sampling method. The main advantage of using systematic sampling over simple random sampling is the assurance that the population will be evenly sampled over the population and simplicity (Trochim, 2001). The next week, after completing their stay at this hotel, a survey package was mailed to them with an invitation letter by the general manager of the resort and a university professor, a survey questionnaire, and a pre-paid return envelope. The first mail reminder by a post-card was sent to those who had not yet responded two weeks after the survey questionnaire, and a second mailing with a survey package, including an invitation letter by the general manager of the resort and a university professor, a survey questionnaire, and a pre-paid return envelope, reminded those who had not yet responded one week after the first reminder. A total of 250 surveys were mailed and the survey response rate was 38% (n=94). 32 11"“ ‘iiJ.I .. ! ., Before combining the two sets of data, demographic characteristics of participants in these two data sets were compared. As shown in Appendix A, the average age of on- line survey respondents was 48 years while that of paper survey respondents was 53 years. There were no significant differences between the two sets of data with the demographic characteristics such as gender, ethnicity, income, and number of people in the household. The on—line survey respondents had more full- time employment and the paper survey respondents had more Michigan residency. Prior to analyzing the data after the two sets of data were combined, 177 data (170 from the online survey and 7 from the paper survey) were dropped because they were found to be inappropriate for the analysis (e. g., incomplete data or guests whose primary purpose of the stay were business or a group/bus tour). A total of 1,660 final data (number of online surveys=1,573, number of paper surveys=87) were utilized for the analysis. Table 6. Data Collection Online survey Paper survey Total Number of surveys distributed 3,459 250 3,709 Number of surveys collected 1 ,743 94 1 ,837 Response rate 50% 38% 50% Number of surveys dropped a from data analysis 170 7 177 Number of surveys used for data analysis 1,573 87 1,660 R68ponse rate 46% 35% 45% Note: a The incomplete data and guests whose primary purposes of their stay were business or a group/bus tour were dropped. 33 Illt‘ I 1.4? r '..-- y b ' ‘. E's-lg, (is! The Questionnaire The questionnaire instrument consisted of six sections. The first section was designed to understand guests’ recent stays at the resort and destination. The second section measured guests’ planning, decision making, and hotel choice. The third section asked about guests’ experiences during their recent stays at the resort. The fourth section asked about guests’ satisfaction with their recent stay at the resort. The fifth section asked about guests’ past experience with the resort, destination, and other hotels. The last section asked for socio-demographic information. Survey Measurement The survey items for each construct were developed on the basis of previous studies. Customer Satisfaction As shown in Table 7, customer satisfaction was measured with four items (Firm, 2005; Spreng, MacKenzie, and Olshavky, 1996). The response was given on a seven- point Likert scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree with the following questions: “Overall, this hotel was comfortable,” “Overall, this hotel was pleasing,” “Overall, this hotel was satisfying,” and “Overall, this hotel was contenting.” Customer Delight Three items were previously used by F inn (2005) to measure customer delight and five items were newly-developed based on the literature review. The response was given on a seven—point Likert scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree with the following questions: “I felt delighted at some time during my stay at this hotel,” “I felt positively surprised at some time during my stay at this hotel,” “I felt overjoyed at some 34 III in he 13,. i. 1.1L .01: time during my stay at this hotel,” “I felt gleeful at some time during my stay at this hotel,” “I felt elated at some time during my stay at this hotel,” “I felt grateful at some time during my stay at this hotel,” “I felt pleased at some time during my stay at this hotel,” and “The hotel had experiences/services which were unexpected and they delighted_me.” Cognitive Loyalty The measurement items for cognitive loyalty were developed from Li and Petrick (2008) and McMullan and Gilmore (2003) and presented five items on a seven-point Likert scale fi'om 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree. Items included “I believe this hotel provides more benefits than other hotels of its type,” “I made the right choice of hotel with this hotel,” “The hotel’s facilities are visually more appealing compared to other hotels of its type,” “This hotel has better value for my money compared to other hotel’s prices of its type,” and “This hotel provides me superior service quality compared to other hotels of its type.” A jfective Loyalty Four of the measurement items for affective loyalty were developed from Li and Petrick (2008) and McMullan and Gihnore (2003). One item was newly-developed on a seven—point Likert scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree. Items included “I feel happy when I stay at this hotel,” “I feel grateful to this hotel,” “I like this hotel more than other hotels of its type,” “I love staying at this hotel,” and “Staying in this hotel says a lot about who I am.” 35 C onative Loyalty Six indicators were used to measure conative loyalty from the previous research (Li and Petrick, 2008; McMullan and Gilmore, 2003). The items were on a seven-point Likert scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree with the following questions: “I consider myself to be highly loyal to this hotel,” “I intend to continue staying at this hotel,” “I would change hotels if another hotel’s staff was more friendly,” “I would try an alternative hotel if their loyalty program offered more benefits,” and “I would try an alternative hotel if the alternative hotel offered increased facilities/services to this hotel.” Action Loyalty Four indicators were used to measure action loyalty from the previous research (Jones and Sasser, 1995; Shoemaker and Lewis, 1999). Items included “I will return to this hotel in the next 6 months or less,” “in the next 7-12 months,” “in the next year (more than 12 months from now),” and “I will recommend this hotel to others.” These items were measured on a seven-point Likert scale from 1 = extremely unlikely to 7 = quite likely. 36 Table 7. Survey Measurement Items Measurement Construct Scales Measurement Items Satisfaction 7-point Likert scale Overall, this hotel was Finn (2005); (1=Strong1y . Comfortable (Finn, 2005) Spreng 3‘ 01- Disagree, 0 Satisfying to me (F inn, 2005) (1996) 7=Strongly Agree) 0 Pleasing (Spreng et al., 1996) Overall, I was content at this hotel. (Spreng et al., 1996) Delight 7-point Likert scale I felt at some time during my stay at this Finn (2005); (1=Strongly hotel. new items Disagree, - a - 7: Strongly Agree) - DelightZd (Firm, 2005) 0 Gleeful (Firm, 2005) . Elated a (Firm, 2005) 0 Grateful (new item) 0 Overjoyed (new item) 0 Pleased (new item) 0 Positively surprised (new item) This hotel had experiences/services which were unexpected and they delighted me. (new item) Cognitive 7-point Likert scale I made the right choice of hotel with this hotel. Loyalty (l=Strongly (McMullan and Gilmore, 2003) Li and Disagree, The hotel’s facilities are visually more appealing Petrick 7=Strongly Agree) compared to other hotels of its type. (McMullan (2008); and Gilmore, 2003) McMullan This hotel has better value for my money and Gilmore compared to other hotel’s prices of its type. (2003) (McMullan and Gilmore, 2003) I believe this hotel provides more benefits than other hotels of its type. (Li and Petrick, 2008) This hotel provides me superior service quality compared to other hotels of its type. (Li and Petrick, 2008) Affective 7-point Likert scale I feel happy when I stay at this hotel. (Li and Loyalty (1=Strongly Petrick, 2008) Li and Disagree, I like this hotel more than other hotels of its type. Petrick 7=Strongly Agree) (Li and Petrick, 2008) (2008); I love staying at this hotel. (Li and Petrick, 2008) McMullan Staying in this hotel says a lot about who I am. and Gilmore (McMullan and Gilmore, 2003) (2003); new I feel grateful to this hotel.(new item) items ’ 37 Table 7 (cont’d) Conative 7-point Likert I intend to continue staying at this hotel. (Li and Loyalty scale Petrick, 2008) Li and (1=Strongly I consider myself to be highly loyal to this hotel. Petrick Disagree, (McMullan and Gilmore, 2003) (2008); 7=Strongly I would change hotels if another hotel’s staff was McMullan Agree) more friendly. (McMullan and Gilmore, 2003) and Gilmore I would try an alternative hotel if their loyalty (2003) program offered more benefits. (McMullan and Gilmore, 2003) I would try an alternative hotel if the alternative hotel offered increased facilities/services to this hotel. (McMullan and Gilmore, 2003) Action 7-point Likert I will return to this hotel in $53212,“ :filExtremely o the next 6 months or less b (Jones and Sasser, Sasser Unlikely, 7=Quite 1995, Shoemaker andbLewrs,1999) (1995); Likely) o the next 7-12 months (Jones and Sasser, 1995; Shoemaker Shoemaker and Lewis,1999) figggims 0 the next year (more than 12 months fi'om now) (Jones and Sasser, 1995; Shoemaker and Lewis,1999) I will recommend this hotel to others. (Jones and Sasser, 1995; Shoemaker and Lewis,1999) a : Modified items based on Finn’s measurement items (2005) b . : Modified items based on Jones and Sasser (1995), and Shoemaker and Lewrs (1999) Data Analysis Based on basic demographic information of all the hotel guests from July to September 2009 given by the hotel, some characteristics (e.g., residency, number of people in the household, membership, and average spent money during the stay at the resort) of the respondents who returned completed surveys were compared to non- respondents who failed to return a completed survey. As shown in Appendix B, respondents and non-respondents had mostly similar demographic characteristics except the percent of loyalty membership holders (Respondents: 50%, Non-Respondents: 26%). 38 Survey data were analyzed in two steps. First, preliminary statistics were obtained using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS). For this study, SPSS 17.0 was used for the analysis. Descriptive statistics were obtained to determine the distributional characteristics of each variable including the means, standard deviation, skewness, and kurtosis. Second, structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to test the proposed model and hypotheses. For this study, M-Plus 5.2 was utilized to complete the analysis. In the SEM process, a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was first conducted and then a measurement model fit was evaluated. CPA is useful in validating scales to discover whether the measuring instrument appropriately measured the underlying constructs prior to considering of the full model (Hair, Anderson, and Fornell, 1998). Following the CF A, the data were used to estimate the hypothesized structural model in evaluating the overall model fit and standardized solutions were used in reporting the causal relationships between the exogenous and endogenous constructs. 39 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS This chapter presents the findings on how customer satisfaction and delight influence loyalty, and the relationships between cognitive, affective, conative, and action loyalties. This chapter is divided into three sections: (1) the results of the preliminary analyses, including data screening and profiles of survey respondents in terms of demographic and traveling characteristics; (2) the results of tests conducted on the measurement model including assessments of overall model fit, reliability, and validity; and (3) the results associated with testing an integrated satisfaction, delight, and loyalty model. Preliminary Analyses In this section, data screening procedures and descriptive statistics for the variables are described. Data Screening Normality for each variable in the proposed model was examined to determine whether the data met the normality assumption for the maximum likelihood estimation (MLE). It is an important preliminary analysis step for subsequent SEM analyses to be meaningful (Hair et al., 1998). The normality was assessed by evaluating the skewness and kurtosis of each variable in the study. These tests indicated that all values for univariate skewness and kurtosis were inside the acceptable range (-3 to 3 for skewness and -10 to 10 for kurtosis) (Kline, 1998). Thus, the results of the normality test showed no extreme departure from normality (skewness: from -2.17 to 0.55 and Kurtosis: from - 0.57 to 6.04) as shown in Table 8. 40 Ioniru Comm italic ’— Cifioni Delght fognn taint lflitfi [012“ (our! [aim AIUI I0}: Table 8. Normalitya Test Results for Variables Included in the Proposed Model. Constructs Variable Names Skewnessb K u rt osisc Customer SAT] (Comfortable) -2.15 5.53 Satisfaction SAT2 (Pleasing) -2.16 6.04 SAT3 (Satisfying) -2.06 4.90 SAT4 (Contengt) -2.17 5.36 Customer DEL] (Delighted) -1.41 2.17 Delight DEL2 (Gratefiil) -.81 .38 DEL3 (Elated) -.56 .13 DEL4 (Gleefiil) -.62 .15 DELS (Overjoyed) -.53 .06 DEL6 (Pleased) -2.08 5.06 DEL7 (Positively surprised) -.60 -.09 DEL8 (Unexpected and delighted) -.57 -.09 Cognitive COGl (More benefits) -1.16 1.55 Loyalty COG2 (Right choice) -1.94 4.43 COG3 (More appealing facilities) -1.18 1.55 COG4 (Better value) -.82 .35 COGS (Superior service quality) -.91 .60 Affective AFFl (Feel happy) -l.55 3.19 Loyalty AF F2 (Feel grateful) -.64 .19 AFF3 (Like) -.91 .57 AFF4 (Love staying) -1.21 1.42 AFF5 (Says a lot about who I am) -.48 -.22 Conative CON1(Consider myself to be highly loyal to this -.82 .23 Loyalty hotel) CON2 (Intend to continue staying at this hotel) -1.33 1.80 CON3 (I would change hotels if another hotel’s -.13 -.48 staff was more friendly) CON4 (I would try an alternative hotel if it was .51 -.49 less expensive than the lodge) CONS (I would try an alternative hotel if their .19 -.57 loyalty program offered more benefits) CON6 (I would try an alternative hotel if the .55 -.23 alternative hotel offered increased facilities/services to the hotel) Action ACTl (Likelihood to return to this hotel in the Loyalty next 6 months or less) -1.21 2.32 ACT2 (Likelihood to return to this hotel in the next 7-12 months) -1 .42 1.89 ACT3 (Likelihood to return to this hotel in the next year (more than 12 months from now) -1.38 1.40 ACT4 (Likelihood to recommend this hotel to others) -2.32 5.58 41 Table 8 (cont’d) a . . . . b Note: Normality was examrned in terms of skewness and kurtosrs. Skewness refers to the symmetry of the distribution. Skewness with a value above 3 is conventionally consrdered extremely skewed. Kurtosrs indicates a relative excess of cases in the tails of a distribution relative to a normal distribution. A kurtosis value of 10 or below is a conventional criterion indicating normal distribution in terms of its peakedness. A value above 10 is considered extremely peaked. Profile of Survey Respondents Socio—Demogr_aphic Characteristics of Survey Respondents Table 9 presents the respondents’ socio-demographic information. The majority of the participants were female (69%) and the average age was 48 years. The majority of participants (82 %) were European American/Middle Eastem/White. About 21% of respondents had an annual household income between $50,000 and $74,999, followed by those with an annual household income ranging between $75,000 and $99,999 (17%), and between $100,000 and $149,999 (17%). The average number of people in a household was three, the majority of respondents (58%) were employed full-time, and 13% of respondents were retired. The majority of respondents (69%) resided in Michigan, followed by those living in Ohio (12%), Canada (7%), and other states (12%). Traveling Characteristics of Survey Respondents Table 10 presents the respondents’ travel information used for travel decision making. Respondents indicated information used or marketing communications influencing them to book the current stay at this hotel by (multiple responses allowed): previous visit (57%), hotel membership holder (20%), fiiend or family member recommended (17%), special package rate (17%), Internet web site or search engine (16%), E-mail promotion, and newsletter offer (16%). Most of the respondents traveled with their family members (86%) and friends (12%). 42 Table 9. Socio-Demographic Characteristics of SurveLRespondents Camry Frequency % Gender Female 1,132 69% Male 507 31% Total 1,639 a 100% Age Mean:48 years old 19-24 years 25 2% 25-34 years 209 13% 35-44 years 446 28% 45-54 years 465 29% 55-64 years 306 19% 65-74 years 128 8% 75 years or older 34 2% Total 1,613 a 100% Race African American/Black 33 2% American Indian 51 3% Asian or Pacific Islander 11 1% European American/Middle East/White 1,326 82% Hispanic/Latino/Latina 12 1% Other 178 l 1% Total 1,61 1 a 100% Employment Status Employed full-time 971 60% Employed part-time 159 10% Retired 204 9% Self-employed 138 1 3% Unemployed 68 4% Other 71 4% Total 1,61 1 a 100% Income Less than $25,000 58 4% $25,000-$34,999 90 6% $35,000-$49,999 181 1 1% $50,000-$74,999 343 22% $75,000-$99,999 275 17% $100,000-S 149,999 275 17% $150,000-$199,999 71 5% $200,000 or more 33 2% I prefer not to respond 248 16% Total 1,574 a 100% 43 Table 9 (cont’d) Family Status Average total number of people in a 3 household Average number of children in a household (18 years or younger) 1 Average number of adults in a household (19 years or older) 2 Primary Residence Michigan 1,137 70% Ohio 200 12% Canada 102 6% Indiana 5 l 3% Illinois 5 1 3% Other 94 6% a . . Note: The value of “frequency" varies, due to questions not answered. During their trip, their leisure activities were (multiple responses allowed): explored F rankenmuth city or town (75%), shopped on Main Street Frankenmuth (72%), went swimming in the resort’s pools or Whirlpools (71%), played in the resort’s game rooms or indoor putt-putt golf (65%), shopped in Riverplace (63%), shopped in Birch Run (50%), dined at a unique restaurant in the Bavarian Inn Lodge (49%), dined at the resort on Main Street (48%), dined at Zehnder’s restaurant on Main Street (29%), and dined at a unique restaurant outside of the resort (20%). Respondents were mostly satisfied with the hotel’s location (Mean 6.66 on 1: very unsatisfied to 7: very satisfied), friendliness of staff (6.43), hotel amenities (pool, game room, free nightly entertainment) (6.42), room cleanliness (6.42), quality of food (6.30), room comfort (6.12), relationship established between staff and customers (6.04), and value for money (5.68). 44 ittllll‘li Table 10. Travel Characteristics of Survey Respondents Category Frequency % Information or Previous visit 951 57% marketing Hotel membership card holder 338 20% communication to Friend or family member recommended 280 17% book this recent Special package rate 27 5 17% . a Internet web site or search engine 271 16% stay at this resort E-mail promotion, newsletter offer 266 16% Accompanied on Family members 1,424 86% this recent stay at Friends 197 12% this resort Alone 27 2% Leisure activities Explored Frankenmuth city or town 1,248 75% during this trip a Shopped-on Main Street Frankenmuth 1,201 72% Went swrmmrng in the resort’s pools or Whirlpools 1,192 71% Played in the resort game rooms or indoor putt-putt golf 1,093 65% Shopped in Riverplace 1,058 63% Shopped in Birch Run 838 50% Dined at a unique restaurant in the Bavarian Inn Lodge 810 49% Dined at the resort on Main Street 797 48% Dined at Zehnder’s restaurant on Main Street 487 29% Dined at a unique restaurant outside of the resort 329 20% Satisfaction with MEAN this recent stay at Hotel location 6.66 this resort Friendliness of staff 6.43 (1: Very Hotel amenities (pool, game room, free Unsatisfied to nightly entertainment) 6.42 7: Very Satisfied) Room cleanliness 6.42 Quality of food 6.30 Room comfort 6.12 Relationship established between staff and customers 6.04 Value for money 5.68 Note: a multiple responses allowed. 45 Testing the Measurement Model Confirmatory Factor Analysis: Model Specifications The measurement model defines the relationships between observed variables and the underlying constructs that the observed variables are presumed to measure. [coolIcoezlcoealcooilcoesla ® @ a ICONI Iconz I CON3 | CON4 I CONSlCOINoj SAII SAIZ Custom er SAIS Satisfactbn @ SAT4 Cogn it We ® | Loyalty 4 Conative “lb" Loyalty Loyally o o MA DEL3 Custom er Delohl DEL4 ‘ flFFIIAFFZIAFFSIAFHIAfll. @ 2:: t t D EL7 DEL \ D EL8 Figure 6. The Proposed Measurement Model 46 : wi'ffi : :‘w b l'. ,- Q ,iUlt. - tan ti more '3 Tires 161'. o . :3: l ACII: I . in‘ h. L, ‘a‘ .1131 I“ F‘".-, [.éhLd. In fa Figure 6 (cont’d) Note: Satl: Comfortable, Sat2: Pleasing, Sat3: Satisfying, Sat4: Content; Dell: Delighted, De12: Grateful, Del3: Elated, Del4: Gleeful, D6152 Overjoyed, Del6: Pleased, Del7: Positively surprised, Del8: Unexpected and delighted; Cogl: More benefits, Cog2: Right choice, Cog3: More appealing facilities, Cog4: Better value, Cog5: Superior service quality; Affl: Feel happy, Aff2: Feel grateful, Aff3: Like, Aff4: Love staying, Aff5: Says a lot about who I am; Conl: Consider myself to be highly loyal to this hotel, Con2: Intend to continue staying at this hotel, Con3: I would change hotels if another hotel’s staff was more friendly, Con4: I would try an alternative hotel if it was less expensive than the lodge, Con5: I would try an alternative hotel if their loyalty program offered more benefits, Con6: I would try an alternative hotel if the alternative hotel offered increased facilities/services to the hotel; Actl: Likelihood to return to this hotel in the next 6 months or less, Ath: Likelihood to return to this hotel in the next 7-12 months, Act3: Likelihood to return to this hotel in the next year (more than 12 months from now), Act4: Likelihood to recommend this hotel to others; Error Terms: el-e36. A confirmatory factor analysis (CF A) was conducted to confirm the hypothesized relationships between observed variables and their underlying constructs to assess the degree to which the data fit the proposed measurement model. The measurement model was estimated using the maximum likelihood method. As shown in Figure 6, the proposed measurement model consists of six constructs and 32 observed variables. Customer satisfaction is specified by four observed variables. Customer delight is specified by eight observed variables. The cognitive loyalty and affective loyalty are each specified by five observed variables. Conative loyalty is specified by six variables. Action loyalty is specified by four observed variables. The measurement model analyzed relationships among a set of observed variables and latent variables by testing reliability and construct validity including convergent and discriminant validity. Once the measurement model reached an acceptable level of fit, the fill] hypothesized structural model was tested to determine its fit relative to the data being tested. Model modification procedures were used to identify observed variables that had low factor loadings, significant cross loadings, and large residuals using a standardized 47 {5.21M .1101 :q .. 1y, U101 factor. For this study, five goodness-of-fit indices were selected (Browne and Cudeck, 1993; Garson, 2006; Kline, 1998; Steiger, 1989). As shown in Table 11, the goodness-of- fit indices chosen for this study and the recommended range of good fit are presented. The proposed measurement model was tested first and. was not acceptable with the data (x2 (449)=7951.2, p<.001,CFI=.856, TLI=.841, RMSEA=.100, SRMR=.076) as presented in Table 9. Thus, it was decided to modify the proposed model since CFI, TLI, RMSEA, and SRMR did not fall within a range of acceptable values. Table 11. The Recommended Goodness-Of-Fit Indices Goodness-of-fit Indices Recommended Range Used for the Study of Good Model Fit . . . 2 ' ‘ - o Chi-Square Statistics (x ) insrgnrficant p value (p>.01) 0 Comparative Fit Index (CPI) > .90 0 Tucker -Lewis Index (TLI) > .90 0 Root Mean Square Error of Approximation . (RMSEA) <.08: A good fit 0 Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) <.05: A good fit 2 . . . . . . 2 a x : There rs a problem of sample Size dependency. With rncreasrng sample Size, the x value increases. For large sample sizes, the x2 statistic provides a highly sensitive statistical test, but not a practical test of model fit (Bollen, 1989; Browne and Cudeck, 1993; Chung and Rensvold, 2002; Garson, 2006; Kline, 1998; Schermelleh-Engel, Moosbrugger, and Miiller, 2003; Steiger, 1989). The observed variables with low factor loadings were removed from the proposed measurement (less than .50) (Kline, 1998). The CFA test results showed that modified model 1 was a significantly better fit than the proposed model to the data (x2 (362)=5347.4, p<.001, CF I=.899, TLI=.887, RMSEA=.091, SRMR=.060). However, this model still presented a poor fit since CFI, TLI, RMSEA, and SRMR results were not 48 within the acceptable ranges. Therefore, modified model 2 was suggested by deleting several observed variables which had large residuals with other observed variables. Finally, as shown in Table 12, modified mode12 showed a significant improvement in chi-square (823.98) and a good fit to the data (12 (75)=823.98, p<.001, CF I=.965, TLI=.951, RMSEA=.078, SRMR=.035). While CF I, TLI, RMSEA, and SRMR results were within the recommended range for a model with good fit to the data, 2 . . . . . . . 2 x indicated a poor fit due to the large sample Size. With rncreasrng sample Size, the x value increases and it leads to the problem that plausible models might be rejected, although the discrepancy between the sample and the model-implied covariance matrix is actually irrelevant (Bollen, 1.989; Cheung and Rensvold, 2002; Schermelleh-Engel, 2 Moosbrugger, and Miiller, 2003). JOreskog and SOrbom (1993) suggested that the x statistic is not a formal test and it should not be focused on too much but rather viewed as a descriptive goodness-of-fit index due to the problem of sample size (Bollen, 1989; Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003). Therefore, it was concluded that the modified model 2 was acceptable. Table 12. Comparison of the Proposed and Modified Full Measurement Models Model 112 Df CF] TLI RMSEA SRMR A 12 Initiai Model 7951.2 449 .856 .841 .100 .076 Modified Modem 5347.4 362 .899 .887 .091 .060 2603.80 Modified Modem 823.98 75 .965 .951 .078 .035 4523.42 49 [11111335 113131;. '1“: r3" » .LU ) ‘1 .5 @147 Assessment of Reliability and Validity The modified measurement model 2 provided support for both validity and reliability. Reliabiligy: The reliability test was conducted using Cronbach’s alpha (or) and a composite reliability, which indicates the internal consistency of the observed variables measuring each factor. As shown in Table 11, Cronbach’s or of all six factors exceeded the recommended .70 (Nunnally, 1978) except the cognitive loyalty construct (.61). However it was considered to be minimally acceptable (Devillis, 1991). Composite reliability was also conducted to measure true reliability because Cronbach's alpha (or) may over- or under-estimate scale reliability (Raykov, 1998). The acceptable range for composite reliability should be greater than .70 (Chin, 1998). All six factors were acceptable at the recommended .70 level. Convergent validity: Convergent validity refers to the degree of association between the observed variables of a factor and is used to determine whether different observed variables used to measure the factors are highly correlated. Convergent validity can be examined by reviewing the results of a t-test for the factor loadings (Hatcher, 1994). As displayed in Table 13, all factor loadings for the observed variables were statistically significant with t-values ranging from 45.48 to 349.56 (p < .001) and standardized factor loadings ranging from .69 to .97. Thus, it can be concluded that convergent validity was supported. Discriminant validly: Discriminant validity is the degree to which items differentiate among constructs. The average variance extracted (AVE) for each of the constructs is greater than their shared variance (Fomell and Larcker, 1981). 50 Table 13. The Result of Confirmatory Factor Analysis for the Modified Measurement Model 2 (Reliability and Convergent Validity) Variable Standardized t- Cronbach's Emmi"; c Construct Name Loadinga value b AVE Customer Satisfaction SATl .83" 100.64 .95 .97 .91 SAT2 .97" 349.56 SAT3 .96" 307.36 Customer Delight DELI .83" 80.34 .83 .91 .76 DEL2 .87 " 93.10 DEL3 .70" 48.22 Cognitive Loyalty COG] .77 " 69.38 .61 .90 .82 COG2 .91 " 126.13 Affective Loyalty AFFI .90" 110.18 .76 .90 .82 AFF2 .78 ” 68.40 Conative Loyalty COM .87 " 116.77 .71 .90 .82 CON2 .94 " 164.47 Action Loyalty ACTl .69" 45.48 .80 .90 .76 ACT2 .87 " 86.52 ACT3 .83" 74.20 Note: a Standardized loadings indicate relationships between observed variables and their associated factors in order to examine convergent validity. b Composite Reliability = (Sum of standardized loadings) 2/ ((Sum of standardized loadings)2 + Sum of indicator measurement error) e Average Variance Extracted = Sum of squared standardized loadings/ (Sum of squared standardized loadings + Sum of indicator measurement error) .. Factor loadings are all significant at p < .001. 51 Table 14. Discriminant Validity Matrix Customer Customer Cognitive Affective Conative Action Satisfaction Delight Loyalty Loyalty Loyalty Loyalty Customer Satisfaction .91 a Customer Delight 50b .76 Cognitive Loyalty .72 .59 .82 Affective Loyalty .50 .59 .73 .82 Conative Loyalty .46 .42 .69 .74 .82 Action Loyalty .31 .31 .44 .47 .63 .76 a . . Note: Average Variance Extracted for a given construct The standardized correlation between constructs Testing the Structural Equation Model The proposed structural equation model is presented in Figure 7. The eight hypotheses regarding the relationships among the factors were tested in the structural equation model and the results of the hypotheses testing are presented in Table 15, including the standardized path coefficients estimated by SEM and the results of the tests of hypotheses. A path diagram is also presented in Figure 7. The path diagram shows standardized path coefficients, representing the direction and strength of the direct influence of one factor on another, and the squared multiple correlations indicating the total variance in a factor explained by the factor(s). The results Show that the model fits the data with all fit indices (112 (81)=730.85, p<.001, CFI=.971, TLI=.963, RMSEA=.070, SRMR=.034) as shown in Table 15. 52 Action Loyalty 12: .64 x2 (81)=730.85, p<.001, CFI=.971, TLI=.963, RMSEA=.070, SRMR=.034 Figure 7. Results of Testing the Proposed Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations (R2: The Total Variance Explained by the Direct Influence of One Factor on Another) 53 Seven of the eight hypotheses were supported, as shown in Figure 7. Customer satisfaction had significant direct influences on cognitive and affective loyalty (support for Hla and Hlb). The influence of customer satisfaction on cognitive loyalty (B = .69, p < .001) was larger than that on affective loyalty (B = .35, p < .001). Customer delight had a significant influence on cognitive and affective loyalty (support for H2a and H2b). The influence of customer delight on affective loyalty (B = .51, p < .001) was larger than that on cognitive loyalty (B = .28, p < .001). The influence of customer delight on cognitive loyalty (B = .28, p < .001) was smaller than that of customer satisfaction on cognitive loyalty (B = .69, p < .001) (did not support H3a). However, the influence of customer delight on affective loyalty (B = .51, p < .001) was larger than that of customer satisfaction on affective loyalty ([3 = .35, p < .001) (support H3b). 54 Table 15. Results of Hypotheses Testing Standardize d Path Hypotheses Coefficient testing Paths ([1) t-value P-value results Customer Satisfaction 9 Cognitive Loyalty (Hla) .69 32.50 0.00 Supported Customer Satisfaction 9 Affective Loyalty (Hlb) .35 11.95 0.00 Supported Customer Delight 9 Cognitive Loyalty (H2a) .28 11.44 0.00 Supported Customer Delight 9 Affective Loyalty (HZb) .51 17.66 0.00 Supported Customer delight will have a greater influence Customer Delight 9 Cognitive Not on cognitive loyalty in Loyalty: .28 supported comparison to satisfaction. Satisfaction 9 Cognitive Loyalty: .69 (H3a) Customer delight will have a greater influence Customer Delight 9 Affective on affective loyalty in Loyalty: .51 Supported comparison to satisfaction. Satisfaction 9 Affective Loyalty: .35 IH3b) Cognitive Loyalty 9 Conative Loyalty (H4) .31 6.75 0.03 Supported Affective Loyalty 9 _Conative Loyalty (H5) .59 13.02 0.00 Supported Conative Loyalty 9 Action Loyalty (H6) .80 62.72 0.00 Supported Cognitive and affective loyalty had significant influences on conative loyalty (support for H4 and H5). The influence of affective loyalty on conative loyalty (I3 = .59, p < .001) was larger than that of cognitive loyalty on conative loyalty ([3 = .31, p < .05). Lastly, conative loyalty had significant influences on action loyalty (B = .80, p < .001) (support for H6). lable 16. Overall Fit Indices for the Hypothesized Structural Model x2 Df CFl TLI RMSEA SRMR imposed Model 730.85 81 .971 .963 .070 .034 55 1'1: III AduhfiphhSquaredIGonndafions As shown in Figure 7, 64 percent of variance in action loyalty was explained by the influences of conative loyalty. Seventy-six percent of variance in conative loyalty was explained by the influences of cognitive and affective loyalty. Customer satisfaction and delight explained 82 percent of variance in cognitive loyalty and 63 percent in affective loyalty. 56 Additional Analyses Additional Analysis with a Full Path Model In order to test complete or partial mediating effects among constructs and to determine a better fitting model, a full path model was also tested by adding several paths between antecedents and dependent variables as shown in Figure 8. The proposed model is based on complete mediating effects of cognitive, affective, and conative loyalty. The causal claims are grounded in previous studies of a sequential loyalty development conceptualization with the mediating roles of each different phase of loyalty (Back, 2001 , 2005; Back and Parks, 2003; Evanschitzky and Wunderlich, 2006; Harris and Goode, 2004; Oliver, 1999). Applying this framework in the tourism and hospitality context, this study identified mediating effects between antecedents and dependent variables. In the proposed model, cognitive and affective loyalty were used as mediator variables between customer satisfaction and conative loyalty and also between customer delight and conative loyalty. Conative loyalty was used as a mediator variable between cognitive loyalty and action loyalty and between affective loyalty and action loyalty. As shown in Figure 9, the full path model, a partial mediator model is less restrictive. To this extent, customer satisfaction and delight could have direct effects on the groups of conative and action loyalty. Cognitive and affective loyalty could also have direct effects on action loyalty. Furthermore, this full path model shows cognitive, affective, and conative loyalty as partial mediators. Therefore, the hill path model was tested with this possibility although it was not hypothesized in the proposed model based on some studies supporting that satisfaction has a direct impact on repeat purchase 57 intentions (Fornell, Johnson, Anderson, Cha, and Bryant, 1996; Yi, 1990) and brand loyalty (Shoemaker and Lewis, 1999). Customer Satisfaction Cognitive Loyalty Conative Action loyalty Loyalty Affective loyalty Cmtoiner ”eight Figure 8. The Proposed Model thmu Sat'nfaction Cognitive Loyalty Affective Loyalty Customer Delight Conative Action Loyalty loyalty Figure 9. The Full Path Model The goodness-of-fit measures were compared to assess which of the two models is better. As shown in the Table 17, both the proposed and full path models met the fit 59 criteria. The fit indices for the two models were very similar, showing that the two models had the same level of model fit (proposed model: x2 (81)=730.85, p<.001, CFI=.971, TLI=.963, RMSEA=.070, SRMR=.034; competing model: x2 (75)=711.59, 2 p<.001, CFI=.972, TLI=.960, RMSEA=.072, SRMR=.033). The Chi-square (x ) difference test was also performed to assess whether there was a significant difference between the two models (Bagozzi and Yi, 1988; JOreskog and SOrbom, 1993). The Chi- 2 . 2 square (x ) difference test between the proposed model and the full path model (A x = 19.267; A df = 6) supported that the full path model performed significantly better than the proposed model. The results provided that customer satisfaction did not have a direct effect on cognitive loyalty and action loyalty, whereas delight had a negative direct effect on conative loyalty (I3 = - .10, p < .001) but a positively direct effect on action loyalty (B = .08, p < .05). For cognitive and affective loyalty, there was no direct effect on action loyalty. Therefore, this study showed that both cognitive and affective loyalty were complete mediators between customer satisfaction and conative loyalty. Conative loyalty also acted as a complete mediator between cognitive and action loyalty and between affective loyalty and action loyalty. However, the result of the path between delight and conative loyalty (negative effect of delight on conative loyalty) did not support the previous studies showing a positive effect of delight on conative loyalty (Arnold et al., 2005; Firm, 2005; Oliver et al., 1997). Therefore, these results suggest that the proposed model was conceptually a better model than the full path model, although the competing . . . . 2 . model was statistically a better model resulting from the chi-square (x ) different test. 60 Table 17. The Results of the Proposed Model and Full Path Model Path Proposed model Full Path model Standardized Standardized Path Path Coeffiecient t- P- Coeffiecient t- P- (0) value value ([1) value value Customer Satisfaction9 Cognitive Loyalty .69 32.50 .000 .69 32.25 .000 Customer Satisfaction9 Affective Loyalty .35 11.95 .000 .40 15.65 .000 Customer Satisfaction9 Non- Conative Loyalty Not tested -.07 -1.43 si g. Customer Satisfaction9 Non- Action Loyalty Not tested .04 .61 si g. Customer Deli ght9 Cognitive Loyalty .28 11.44 .000 .28 11.64 .000 Customer Delight 9 Affective Loyalty .51 17.66 .000 .49 18.47 .000 Customer Delight 9 Conative Loyalty Not tested -.10 -3.30 .001 Customer Delight 9 Action Loyalty Not tested .08 2.12 .034 Cognitive Loyalty9 Conative Loyalty .31 6.75 .000 .32 3.48 .000 Cognitive Loyalty9 Non- Action Lgyalty Not tested -.11 -.97 si g. Affective Loyalty9 Conative Loyalty .59 13.02 .000 .72 11.71 .000 Affective Loyalty9 Non- Action Loyalty Not tested .03 .29 sig. Conative Loyalty9 Action Loyalty .80 62.72 .000 .79 14.35 .000 112611) 730.85 (81) 711.59 (75) P value .000 .000 CFI .971 .972 TLI .963 .960 RMSEA .070 .072 SRMR .034 .033 R2 (Cognitive Loyalty) '82 '82 R2 (Affective Loyalty) '63 '68 R2 (Conative Loyalty) ‘76 '79 2 .64 .64 R (Action Loyalty) 61 Cognitive loyalty x2(81)=730.85,p<.001, CFI=.971,TLI=.963, RMSEA=.070, SRMR=.034 Figure 10. Results of Testing the Proposed Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations 62 .ustomer .tislaclion Oil-Sig. 40H Cognitive Wally Non-Sig. .32“ R =.79 on-Sig. .71 Conative 79, Action Loyalty ° Lola“! Non-Sig. R2=.64 .08* .ustomer °ight 12(75F71159, p<.001, =10" CFI=.972,TLI=.960, RMSEA=.072, SRMR=.033 Figure 11. Results of Testing the Competing Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations 63 Additional Analysis between F irst- Time Guests (or not Recent Guests in the Past 3 Years) and Repeat Guests Based on the number of stays at the resort, respondents were divided into two groups; first-time guests (or not recent guests in the past 3 years) and repeat guests. In order to test the differences between these two groups, a two- group analysis was conducted. Table 18 presents the respondents’ socio-demographic information for the two groups. Most of the characteristics were similar between the two groups excluding characteristics of employment status and primary residence. A group of first-time guests (or not recent guests in the past 3 years) had a greater percentage of full-time employment and a group of repeat guests had a greater percentage of primary residence in Michigan. Table 19 shows the means of customer satisfaction, delight, cognitive loyalty, affective loyalty, conative loyalty, and action loyalty of the two groups. In order to investigate whether the two groups have statistically significant differences, analysis of variance (AN OVA) tests were performed. The mean differences in customer satisfaction with three items were not statistically significant. Two items under delight, “delighted” and “overjoyed,” were not statistically significant. However, an item under delight, “positively surprised,” was statistically significant at p<0.05. One item under cognitive loyalty, “I believe the hotel provides more benefits than other hotels of its type” was statistically significant at p<0.05 and one item under cognitive loyalty, “The hotel’s facilities are visually more appealing compared to other hotels of its type” was not statistically significant. The mean differences in affective, conative, and action loyalty 64 were statistically significant at p<0.05 or p<0.001. The results showed that a group of repeat guests had higher means of loyalty. 65 Table 18. Socio-Demographic Characteristics of Survey Respondents between First- Time (or not recent guests in the past 3 years) and Repeat Guests First-Time Guests (or Not Recent Guests in the Past 3 Years) Repeat Guests Category (n=1,018) (n=640) Frequency % Frequency % Gender Female 672 3 1% 443 30% Male 305 69% 189 70% Total 977 100% 632 100% Race African American/Black 20 2% 12 2% American Indian 30 3% 21 3% Asian or Pacific Islander 7 1% 4 1% European American/Middle East/White 801 83% 498 81% Hispanic/Latino/Latina 8 1% 4 1% Other 97 10% 78 13% Total 963 100% 617 100% Employment Employed full-time 604 63% 351 57% Status Employed part-time 78 8% 75 12% Retired 1 1 8 12% 50 8% Self-employed 86 9% 82 13% Unemployed 35 4% 33 5% Other 40 4% 28 5% Total 961 1 00% 619 100% Income Less than $25,000 26 3% 30 5% $25,000-$34,999 52 6% 36 6% $35,000-$49,999 1 13 12% 64 1 1% $50,000-$74,999 195 21 % 139 23% $75,000-$99,999 174 18% 99 17% $100,000-$149,999 162 17% l 10 18% $150,000-$199,999 51 5% 19 3% $200,000 or more 23 2% 6 1% I prefer not to respond 149 16% 95 16% Total 945 100% 598 100% Primary Michigan 591 61% 448 73% Residence Ohio 1 33 14% 5 1 8% Canada 61 6% l 8 3% Indiana 36 4% 34 6% Illinois 41 3% 9 1% Other 127 13% 54 9% Total 989 1 00% 61 5 100% Ag Mean 48 48 66 Table 19. Mean Difference in Satisfaction, Delight, Cognitive Loyalty, Affective Loyalty, Conative Loyalty, and Action Loyalty between First-Time (or not recent guests in the past 3 years) and Repeat Guests First-Time Guests (or Not Recent Guests in the Past 3 Repeat Years) Guests AN OVA Constructs Variable Names (n=1,018) (n=640) (p) Customer SATl (Comfortable) 6.1 a 6.1 Non. Sig. Satisfaction SAT2 (Satisfying) 6.0 6.1 Non. Sig. SAT3 (Content) 6.1 6.2 Non. Sig. Customer DELl (Delighted) 5.7a 5.8 Non. Sig. Delight DEL5 (Overjoyed) 4.8 5.0 Non. Sig. DEL7 (Positively surprised) 5. 1 4.8 .003 Cognitive COGl (More benefits) 5.6 a 5.8 .004 Loyalty COG2 (Right choice) 6.0 6.1 Non. Sig. Affective AF F3 (Like) 5.4 a 5.6 .003 Loyalty AFF4 (Love staying) 5 .6 5.9 .000 AFFS (Says a lot about who I am) 4.8 5.0 .001 Conative CON 1 (Consider myself Loyalty to be highly loyal to this hotel) 5 la 5.5 .000 CON2 (Intend to ' continue staying at this hotel) 5. 5 5 .9 .000 Action ACTl (Likelihood to Loyalty return to this hotel in the next 6 months or less) ACT2 (Likelihood to 33b 4.7 .000 return to this hotel in the next 7-12 months) ACT3 (Likelihood to 5.7 .000 return to this hotel in the 5'0 next year (more than 12 months from now) 5 5 .000 6.0 Note: a Scale: Range from 1: strongly disagree to 7: strongly agree b Scale: Range from 1: extremely unlikely to 7: quite likely 67 Hypothesized structural models of the two groups were also performed. The )8 difference test was used to test for the moderating effects on individual paths to examine the groups’ differences in individual paths. Table 20 presents the 12 difference results for path coefficients between groups. The x2 difference tests showed that the paths of Customer Satisfaction --> Affective Loyalty, Customer Delight --> Cognitive Loyalty and Affective Loyalty, Cognitive Loyalty --> Conative Loyalty, and Affective Loyalty --> Conative Loyalty - Table 20. 12 Difference Tests Path Difference in 12 Customer Satisfaction 9 Cognitive Loyalty Non. Sig. Customer Satisfaction 9 Affective Loyalty 8.26“ Customer Delight 9 Cognitive Loyalty 8.34“ Customer Delight 9 Affective Loyalty 8.29" Cognitive Loyalty 9 Conative Loyalty 10.32" Affective Loyalty 9 Conative Loyalty 8.61** Conative Loyalty 9 Action Loyalty Non. Sig. Note: “Significant at p<.001 As shown in the Table 21, both the groups of first-time guests (or not recent guests in the past 3 years) and repeat guests met the fit criteria (first-time guests (or not recent guests in the past 3 years): x2 (81)=454.82, p<.001, CF I=.969, TLI=.958, RMSEA=.075, SRMR=.035; repeat guests: x2 (81)=232.92, p<.001, CFI=.976, TLI=.968, RMSEA=.062, SRMR=.027). 68 Table 21. Comparison of Overall Fit Indices for Two Groups if Df CFI TLI RMSEA SRMR F irst-Time Guests (or not recent guests in the past 3 years) 454.82 81 .969 .958 .075 .035 Repeat Guests 232.92 81 .976 .968 .062 .027 As shown in Table 22, the results provided that customer delight has a greater effect on cognitive loyalty and affective loyalty in the group of first-time guests (or not recent guests in the past 3 years) (B = .35, I3 = .53) compared to the group of repeat guests (B = .24, B = .41). Customer satisfaction has a greater effect on cognitive loyalty and affective loyalty in the group of repeat guests ([3 = .68, B = .45) compared to the group of first-time guests (or not recent guests in the past 3 years) ([3 = .63, B = .36). For cognitive loyalty, there is no direct effect on conative loyalty in the group of first-time guests (or not recent guests in the past 3 years) while cognitive loyalty has a positive effect on conative loyalty in the group of repeat guests ([3 = .85). 69 Table 22. Path Coefficient of the Hypothesized Models between the Groups of First- Time Guests (or not recent flests in the past 3 yea@ and Repeat Guests First-Time Guests Repeat Guests $1,018) (n=640) Path Hypotheses Standardized Path Standardized Path Coefficient ([3) Coefficient ([3) Customer Satisfaction 9 Cognitive Loyalty Hla .63" .68** Customer Satisfaction 9 Affective Loyalty Hlb .36** .45** Customer Delight 9 Cognitive Loyalty H2a .35** .24" Customer Delight 9 Affective Loyalty H2b .53** .41 ** Customer delight will have a greater influence on cognitive loyalty in comparison to satisfaction. H3a Not Supported Not Supported Customer delight will have a greater influence on affective loyalty in comparison to satisfaction. H3b Supported Not Supported Cognitive Loyalty 9 Conative Loyalty H4 Non-Sig. .33" Affective Loyalty 9 H5 Conative Loyalty .85** .55** Conative Loyalty 9 H6 Action Loyalty .78** .76** 7O First-Time Guests (or Not Recent Guests in the Past 3 Years) . (n=1,018) 12 (81)-454.82, p<.001, CFI=.969,TLI=.958, RMSEA=.075,SRMR=.035 Figure 12. Results of Testing the Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations (A Group of First-Time Guests (or Not Recent Guests in the Past 3 Years) 71 Repeat Guests(n=640) .mtomer .alislaction '76“ Action loyalty R2= .58 x2 (81)=232.92,p<.001, CFI=.976,TLI=.968, RMSEA=.062,SRMR=.027 Figure 13. Results of Testing the Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations (A Group of Repeat Guests) 72 Additional Analysis between Loyalty Program Members and Loyalty Program Non- Members Respondents were divided into two groups; loyalty program members and loyalty program non-members. In order to test the differences between these two groups, a two- group analysis was conducted. Table 23 presents the respondents’ socio-demographic information for the two groups. Most of the characteristics were similar between the groups excluding characteristics of employment status and primary residence. The group of loyalty program non-members had a greater percentage of full-time employment and a group of loyalty program members had a greater percentage of primary residence in Michigan. Table 24 shows the means of customer satisfaction, delight, cognitive loyalty, affective loyalty, conative loyalty, and action loyalty of these two groups. In order to investigate whether the two groups have statistically significant differences, analysis of variance (AN OVA) tests were performed. Table 25 shows that the mean differences in customer satisfaction, cognitive, affective, conative, and action loyalty were statistically significant at p<0.05 or p<0.001. The results showed that a group of loyalty program members had higher means of customer satisfaction, cognitive, affective, conative, and action loyalty. Two items under delight, “delighted” and “overjoyed,” were also statistically significant at p<0.001. However, one item under delight, “positively surprised,” was not statistically significant. 73 Table 23. Socio-Demographic Characteristics of Survey Respondents between Loyalty Program Members and Loyalty Program Non-Members Loyalty Program Loyalty Program Members Non-Members Category (n=815) (n=789y Frequency % Frequency % Gender Female 557 31% 544 70% Male 250 69% 233 30% Total 807 100% 777 1 00% Race African American/Black 13 2% 19 3% American Indian 27 3% 22 3% Asian or Pacific Islander 5 1% 6 1% European American/Middle East/White 659 83% 622 82% Hispanic/Latino/Latina 7 1% 4 1% Other 87 1 1% 85 l 1% Total 798 100% 1758 100% Employment Employed full-time 455 5 8% 488 64% Status Employed part-time 78 10% 72 9% Retired 109 14% 88 12% Self-employed 68 9% 64 8% Unemployed 34 4% 31 4% Other 46 6% 22 3% Total 790 100% 765 100% Income Less than $25,000 30 4% 24 3% $25,000-$34,999 40 5% 47 6% $35,000-$49,999 100 13% 75 10% $50,000-$74,999 168 22% 162 22% $75,000-$99,999 142 18% 126 17% $100,000-$149,999 130 17% 141 19% $150,000-$199,999 30 4% 40 5% $200,000 or more 10 1% 19 3% I prefer not to respond 131 17% 1 11 15% Total 781 100% 745 100% Primary Michigan 573 71% 466 60% Residence Ohio 79 10% 105 14% Canada 27 3% 52 7% Indiana 40 5% 3 1 4% Illinois 19 2% 31 3% Other 77 9% 104 12% Total 81 5 100% 789 100% _A_ge Mean 48 47 74 Table 24. Mean Difference in Satisfaction, Delight, Cognitive Loyalty, Affective Loyalty, Conative Loyalty, and Action Loyalty between Loyalty Program Members and Loyalty Program Non-Members Loyalty Loyalty Program Program Non- Members Members AN OVA Constructs Variable Names (n=815) (n=789) (p) Customer SATl (Comfortable) 6 2 a 6.0 .004 Satisfaction SAT2 (Satisfying) 6 2 5.9 .000 SAT3 (Content) 6.3 6.0 .000 Customer DELI (Delighted) 5 9 a 5.6 .000 Delight DEL5 (Overjoyed) 5 1 4.7 .000 DEL7 (Positively surprised) 5'0 5.0 Non. Sig. Cognitive COGl (More benefits) 5 9 a 5.5 .000 Loyalty COG2 (Right choice) 6 2 5.9 .000 Affective AFF3 (Like) 5 8 a 5.2 .000 Loyalty AFF4 (Love staying) 6 1 5.4 .000 AFFS (Says a lot about who I ' am) 5.2 4.5 .000 Conative CON1(Consider myself to be Loyalty highly loyal to this hotel) 5 8 a 4.7 .000 CON2 (Intend to continue ' staying at this hotel) 6 1 5.2 .000 Action ACTl (Likelihood to return to Loyalty this hotel in the next 6 months or less) 5 0 b 3.3 .000 ACT2 (Likelihood to return to this hotel in the next 7-12 months) 5 9 4.6 .000 ACT3 (Likelihood to return to ' this hotel in the next year (more than 12 months from now) 6 1 5.3 .000 Note: a Scale: Range from 1: strongly disagree to 7: strongly agree b Scale: Range from 1: extremely unlikely to 7: quite likely Hypothesized structural models of the two groups were also performed. The x2 difference test was used to test for the moderating effects on individual paths to examine 75 the groups’ differences in individual paths. Table 25 presents the x2 difference results for path coefficients between groups. The x2 difference tests showed that the paths of Customer Delight --> Affective Loyalty, Cognitive Loyalty --> Conative Loyalty, and Affective Loyalty --> Conative Loyalty. Table 25. 12 Difference Tests Path Difference in x2 Customer Satisfaction 9 Cognitive Loyalty Non. Sig. Customer Satisfaction 9 Affective Loyalty Non. Sig. Customer Delight 9 Cognitive Loyalty Non. Sig. Customer Delight 9 Affective Loyalty 8.29" Cognitive Loyalty 9 Conative Loyalty 9.65“ Affective Loyalty 9 Conative Loyalty 8.24M Conative Loyalty 9 Action Loyalty Non. Sig Note: "Significant at p<.001 As shown in the Table 26, both the groups of loyalty program members and loyalty program non-members met the fit criteria (loyalty program members: )(2 (81)=730.85, p<.001, CFI=.971, TLI=.963, RMSEA=.070, SRMR=.034; loyalty program non-members: x2 (81)=449.98, p<.001, CFI=.964, TLI=.953, RMSEA=.076, SRMR=.038). Table 26. Comparison of Overall Fit Indices for Two Groups 12 Df CFI TLI RMSEA SRMR Loyalty Program Members 730.85 81 .971 .963 .070 .034 Loyalty Program Non- Members 449.98 81 .964 .953 .076 .038 The goodness-of-fit measures were compared to assess which of the two models is better. As shown in the Table 25, both the proposed and competing models met the fit 76 criteria. The fit indices for two models were similar, showing that the two models had the same level of model fit (proposed model: x2 (81)=730.85, p<.001, CF I=.971, TLI=.963, RMSEA=.070, SRMR=.034; competing model: x2 (75)=711.59, p<.001, CF I=.972, TLI=.960, RMSEA=.072, SRMR=.033). As shown in Table 27, the results provided that customer delight has a greater effect on cognitive loyalty and affective loyalty in the group of loyalty program non- members (B = .33, B = .54) compared to the group of loyalty program members (B = .30, B = .46). Customer satisfaction has a greater effect on affective loyalty in the group of loyalty program members (B = .40) compared to the group of loyalty program non- members (B = .36). Cognitive loyalty has a greater effect on conative loyalty in the group of loyalty program non-members (B = .64) compared to the group of loyalty program members (B = .31). Affective loyalty has a greater effect on conative loyalty in the groups of loyalty program members ((B = .56) compared to the group of loyalty program non- members (B = .35). 77 Table 27. Path Coefficients of the Hypothesized Models between the Groups of Loyalty Program Members and Loyalty Prggam Non-Members Loyalty Program Loyalty Program Members Non-Members (n=815) (n=789) Path Hypotheses Standardized Path Standardized Path Coefficrent (B) Coefficient (B) Customer Satisfaction 9 Cognitive Loyalty Hla .63** .65" Customer Satisfaction 9 Affective Loyalty Hlb .40** .36" Customer Delight 9 Cognitive Loyalty H2a .30** .33“ Customer Delight 9 Affective Loyalty H2b .46“ .54" Customer delight will have a greater influence on cognitive loyalty in comparison to satisfaction. H3a Not Supported Not Supported Customer delight will have a greater influence on affective loyalty in comparison to satisfaction. H3b Supported Supported Cognitive Loyalty 9 u ,H, Conative Loyalty H4 '31 '64 Affective Loyalty 9 Conative Loyalty H5 .56** .35** Table 27. (cont’d) Conative Loyalty 9 Action Loyalty H6 .76** .56M 78 Loyalty Program Members (n=815) x2(81)=730.85,p<.001, CFI=.971,TLI=.963, RMSEA=.070,SRMR=.034 Figure 14. Results of Testing the Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations (A Group of Loyalty Program Members) 79 Loyalty Program Non-Members (n=789) Customer Satisfaction R2: 85 .65** Cognitive .36" War, one '73 67 Conative '75“ Action ' loyalty loyalty .33** ~35” Afl'ective R2: .93 R2: .56 Loyalty .54** Customer 2 ll =.70 x2(81H49.98,p<.001, CFI=.964, TLI=.953, RMSEA=.076, SRMR=.038 Figure 15. Results of Testing the Structural Model with Standardized Path Coefficients and Squared Multiple Correlations (A Group of Loyalty Program Non- Members) Summary This chapter presented the results on the customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty model testing including six hypotheses tests summarized in Table 28. Additionally, this chapter showed the results of the groups’ differences (e. g., First-time guests vs. Repeat guests, Loyalty program members vs. Loyalty program non-members). The next chapter discusses the results and implication of the study, as well as, opportunities for future research and limitations of this study. The results are presented with current literature perspectives and the theoretical contributions of the study are discussed including managerial implications. Table 28. Summary of the Hypothesized Finding Hypothesis Findings Customer satisfaction will have a direct positive influence on Hla cognitive loyalty. Supported Customer satisfaction will have a direct positive influence on Su o rt e d Hlb affective loyalty. pp Customer delight Will have a direct posrtlve influence on cognitive Supported H2a loyalty. Customer delight will have a direct positive influence on affective S upported H2b loyalty. Customer delight will have a greater influence on cognitive Not H3a loyalty in comparison to satisfaction. supported Customer delight will have a greater influence on affective loyalty . . . . Supported H3b 1n comparison to satlsfactron. Cogmtlve loyalty Will have a direct positive influence on conative Supported H4 loyalty. Affective loyalty Will have a dlrect posmve 1nfluence on conative Supported H5 loyalty. Conative loyalty Wlll have a direct posrtive 1nfluence on action Supported H6 loyalty. 81 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION, IMPLICATIONS, AND CONCLUSIONS The purpose of this study was to examine the impact of customer satisfaction and delight on loyalty by empirically testing a model. Furthermore, the study aimed to better understand four phases of loyalty development: cognitive, affective, conative, and action loyalties. The subjects of this study were 1,660 (1,573 from an online survey, 87 from a paper survey) guests who stayed at a Midwestern resort hotel, during the summer of 2009 in the months of July, August, and September. All subjects completed either an online or paper survey instrument consisting of six sections and the respondents’ demographic data and traveling characteristics. To answer the research questions, structural equation modeling (SEM) was conducted to explore the relationships between customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty in a tourism and hospitality context. The Statistical Package for the Social Science (SPSS) was also used for all descriptive analyses including the frequency distributions. This chapter consists of four sections: (1) summary of the important findings of this study and discusses the findings; (2) the theoretical and practical implications of the study are presented; (3) directions for fiiture research and limitations of the study; and (4) conclusion with final comments. Results of Hypotheses Testing and Discussion of the Findings As presented in Figure 16, the conceptual model of this study was proposed to examine relationships among the constructs with six hypotheses. 82 Customer delight will have a greater positive influence H— on cognitive loyalty in comparison to satisfaction. Customer H33 Salutation la Cognitive Hlb level! H4 Conative H6 Action 10W loyalty H2a Affective .. Customer Customer delight w111 have a greater posutuve mfluencc on affective loyalty in comparison to satisfaction. Figure 16. Proposed Conceptual Model 83 Hypotheses 1a & 1 b: Customer satisfaction will have a direct positive influence on cognitive and affective loyalty. The hypotheses regarding the direct influence of customer satisfaction on cognitive loyalty (Hla) and affective loyalty (Hlb) were supported. These findings provide empirical evidence in support of previous studies that noted the positive relationship between customer satisfaction and attitudinal loyalty (Cronin and Taylor, 1992; Yi, 1990; Zeithaml et al., 1996). Specifically, this finding shows that satisfaction influences positively cognitive and affective loyalty directly. These findings also indicate that respondents had a higher cognitive loyalty (B = .69) than affective loyalty (B = .35). Hypotheses 2a & 2b: Customer delight will have a direct positive influence on cognitive and affective loyalty. Hypotheses 2a and 2b were supported with the direct influence of customer delight on cognitive loyalty (H2a) and affective loyalty (H2b). This finding broadens the conceptualization of customer delight-related loyalty by providing empirical evidence and supports the literature review suggesting the important role of customer delight as a pivotal driver of loyalty (Hicks et al., 2005; Kumar et al., 2006; Oliver et al., 1997; Patterson, 1997; Rust and Oliver, 1994; Westbrook and Oliver, 1991; Williams and Anderson, 1999). However, in the context of tourism and hospitality, empirical research of the relationship between delight and loyalty has not been studied much and remains in its infancy. Therefore, the current study constitutes one of the grounding steps toward investigating this relationship. The strength of the direct influence of customer delight on affective loyalty was higher ([3 = .51) than on cognitive loyalty (B = .28). 84 Hypotheses 3a & 3b: Customer delight will have a greater influence on cognitive & affective loyalty in comparison to satisfaction. Hypothesis 3a was not supported. Customer delight had a weaker influence on cognitive loyalty (B = .28) in comparison to the influence of satisfaction on cognitive loyalty (B = .69). This finding challenges the proposition that customer delight is more likely to show loyalty than satisfied customers (Oliver et al., 1997; Paul, 2000). Hypothesis 3b was supported. This finding indicates that customer delight had a greater influence on affective loyalty (B = .51) in comparison to the influence of satisfaction on affective loyalty (B = .35). This finding provides empirical evidence in support of previous studies that delight is a stronger emotion than satisfaction and can be discriminated as a different psychological state than satisfaction (Mano, 1991; Watson and Tellegen, 1985), and delight is directly related to positive affect (Hicks et al., 2005; Oliver et al., 1997). Hypotheses 4 & 5: Cognitive loyalty and affective loyalty will have a direct positive influence on conative loyalty. Hypotheses 4 and 5 were supported. Both cognitive and affective loyalty had significant and positive direct influence on conative loyalty. This finding suggests that both cognitive and affective loyalty directly contribute to conative loyalty, and thus supports the findings of previous loyalty studies which emphasized that cognitive and affective loyalty influence conative loyalty positively either directly or indirectly (Harris and Goode, 2004; McMullan and Gilmore, 2003; Oliver, 1997, 1999). This study proposes a new conceptual model of loyalty development as cognitive and affective loyalty directly influence conative loyalty. Affective loyalty had a stronger direct influence on conative loyalty (B = .59) in comparison to cognitive loyalty (B = .31). 85 Hypothesis 6: C onative loyalty will have a direct positive influence on action loyalty. Hypothesis 6 was supported. Conative loyalty was found to have significant and positive direct influences on action loyalty (B = .80). This result supports a model of attitude-behavior linkage, which is theoretically grounded as attitudinal loyalty leads to behavioral loyalty (Ajzen, 1991; Breckler, 1984; Dick and Basu, 1994). This finding also indicates that there is a strong relationship between conative loyalty and action loyalty (B = .80), and 64 percent of the variance in action loyalty was explained by the influences of conative loyalty. Implications The findings of this study have both theoretical and practical implications. This section presents the theoretical contributions of this study to existing tourism and hospitality literature, and its practical implications for tourism and hospitality marketers. Theoretical Implications The present study has several theoretical implications for consumer behavior research. First, this study is one of few empirical studies on customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty and provides a foundation for researchers in the understanding of the relationships between customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty. In particular, this study extends support for the conceptualization of customer satisfaction and delight as distinct constructs (Hicks et al., 2005; Oliver et al., 1997; Rust and OliVer, 1994; Westbrook and Oliver, 1991). This study also shows that both customer satisfaction and delight are important antecedents of loyalty; particularly, customer satisfaction and delight have direct significant relationships with cognitive and affective loyalty. Furthermore, the strength of the impact of customer delight on affective loyalty has greater influence than 86 that of customer delight on cognitive loyalty, while the strength of the impact of customer satisfaction on cognitive loyalty is greater than that on affective loyalty. Second, the findings of this study provide new insights by integrating customer satisfaction and delight concepts together in an effort to better explain customer loyalty. A number of researchers have previously highlighted the importance of customer delight (Hicks et al., 2005; Kumar et al., 2006; Oliver et al., 1997; Patterson, 1997; Rust and Oliver, 1994; Westbrook and Oliver, 1991; Williams and Anderson, 1999). In particular, understanding a customer’s emotional experience becomes critical in the tourism and hospitality industry area because there are many interactions with customers and staff during the consumption process in a service environment. Additionally, it is suggested that a mere customer satisfaction approach may be not enough in understanding customer loyalty (Hicks et al., 2005; Oliver et al., 1997 ; Williams and Anderson, 1999). Although researchers agree on the importance of emotional factors, such as customer delight being related to loyalty, there have been few empirical studies linking delight and loyalty. Therefore, this study extends support for the importance of customer delight as one of the primary antecedents of customer loyalty by empirically proving that customer delight directly leads to loyalty. Specifically, the present study showed that customer delight has a direct positive influence on cognitive and affective loyalty and customer delight has a greater impact on affective loyalty, in comparison to satisfaction. A‘third theoretical implication of this study is its contribution to extend the existing literature on the customer loyalty formation process by attempting a new conceptualization of the loyalty dimension. Most earlier studies on the conceptualization of the loyalty dimension originated from Dick and Basu (1994) and Oliver (1997, 1999) 87 which suggested that loyalty formation starts from cognitive loyalty, followed by affective loyalty, to conative loyalty, and ultimately to action loyalty (Oliver, 1997, 1999). Based on Oliver’s (1997, 1999) loyalty dimensions, many researchers have supported Oliver’s four loyalty dimensions model by empirically testing the model and developing the measurement scales (Harris and Goode, 2004; Knox and Walker, 2001; McMullan and Gilmore, 2003; Tsaur, Chiu, and Huang, 2002). Meanwhile, other researchers challenged the cognitive-affective-conative loyalty sequences and proposed four unique phases of loyalty development (Back, 2001; Jones and Taylor, 2007; Li and Petrick, 2008). For example, Back (2001) and Li and Petrick (2008) suggested that the three attitudinal loyalty dimensions of cognitive, affective, and conative loyalty are not a sequential formation process but are independent concepts. Furthermore, this present study proposes a new conceptualization of loyalty development: the two loyalty dimensions, cognitive and affective loyalty, are independent and inter-relational components of attitudinal loyalty. This conceptualization is grounded in the psychology literature with cognition and affection as separate and independent concepts (Peter and Olson, 2005; Piaget, 1981; Stevens, 1970). This conceptualization was also empirically supported from the testing of the proposed model. Furthermore, this study is meaningful as one of a few empirical studies distinguishing the different impacts of cognitive and affective loyalty on conative loyalty. Additionally, while many researchers have focused on the cognitive aspects of loyalty, this study supports that affective loyalty is more likely to enhance conative loyalty than cognitive loyalty. Last, this study extends the existing literature on the link of customer satisfaction and loyalty by providing empirical support. There are many studies that emphasize the 88 importance of customer satisfaction on loyalty (Aj zen and Driver, 1991; Chen and Gursoy, 2001; Shoemaker and Lewis, 1999; Yoon and Uysal, 2005) while some studies found customer satisfaction does not always have a significant influence on loyalty (Jones and Sasser, 1995; Stewart, 1997). However, the finding of this study reinforces the traditional view that there is a statistically strong and critical relationship between customer satisfaction and loyalty, and that customer satisfaction is one of main antecedents of loyalty (Mittal and Karnakura, 2001). Specifically, this study provides the ability to understand satisfaction from both cognitive and affective perspectives although satisfaction was focused on a more cognitive perspective in the previous studies. Based on Oliver’s (1980) disconfirmation theory, satisfaction was primarily understood as a cognitive state resulting from cognitive evaluations between expectations and perceived performance which then leads to satisfaction (Bigné et al., 2003; Oliver and Swan, 1989) and satisfaction also has a direct effect on cognitive loyalty (Back, 2005; Back and Parks, 2003; Firm; 2005, Oliver, 1997). However, in recent studies, the need to understand satisfaction incorporating both cognitive and affective perspectives has been increased (Bigné et al., 2003; Mano and Oliver, 1993; Oliver, 1993; Oliver et al., 1997; Westbrook and Oliver, 1991; Wirtz and Bateson, 1999; Wirtz et al., 2000). Therefore, this study is meaningful to support satisfaction as an important antecedent, not only of cognitive loyalty, but of affective loyalty. Practical Implications Tourism and hospitality managers are facing a dynamic and competitive market environment as customers are becoming more sophisticated. To give better service and 89 increase customer loyalty, this study provides several practical implications for tourism and hospitality managers. First, this study shows that managers need to understand the importance of not only customer satisfaction but also delight in directly driving customer loyalty. Managers have mostly focused on improving customer satisfaction to increase customer loyalty. Now, achieving mere customer satisfaction is not enough to get customers’ attention and gain loyalty among other competitors; the changes in the tourism and hospitality industry demand more than customer satisfaction-style management. Therefore, being knowledgeable about customer delight can be a valuable motivator for managers in developing marketing strategy. They need to take steps to ensure the generation of both customer satisfaction and delight, which can provide stronger influences in the creation of customer loyalty. As a good example of generating customer delight, Disney is known for having created one of the most popular and memorable destinations in the world. They are an expert in the tourism and hospitality industry and they worked to recreate that experience nationally by opening retail stores some years ago. Now they are remodeling those stores to create a whole new level of experience by spending around $1 million per store. They are redesigning the stores from a kid’s perspective for “the best 30 minutes of a child’s day.” For instance, Disney is creating new activities using technology such as interactivity with film clips of a child’s own choosing, karaoke contests, and live chats with Disney Channel stars via satellite. Through these unique customer experiences, the customers have the opportunities to see and feel something different every time they come into the store. 90 As an another example in the tourism and hospitality industry, and on the other end of the cost spectrum, Doubletree hotel has created a delightful service by greeting guests at check-in with a “sweet treat” of a complimentary, warm chocolate chip cookie. This small touch can create delightful memories without much cost. One of the biggest concerns about customer delight is the effect of raising the bar of customer’s expectations about future performances, making it more difficult for marketers to reliably create customer delight in the future (Arnold et al., 2005; Rust and Oliver and Rust, 2000). Therefore, as a second implication of this study, managers need to understand that the key to successfully applying deli ght-generating strategies lies in the selective usage of the concept and in creating differentiated and personalized services so that the competitors are not able to easily copy the delight program. As a good example of accomplishing deli ght-generating strategies, the front-line personnel, those who are mostly in contact with customers, can be a critical source to deliver delight effectively through good relationships with customers. However, in today’s labor market, keeping dedicated personnel is becoming more challenging (Keiningham and Vavra, 2001). Furthermore, long-term relationships with customers can provide more opportunities to exceed the customer’s expectations and “wowing” them by building emotional bonds (Zeithaml and Bitner, 2003). In the process of developing loyal relationships, these delighting elements can provide additional psychological benefits to the customers. For example, in his book, “Hug Your Customers,” Mitchell (2003) suggests many examples of the physical and psychological benefits of hugs for customers. Hence, training, motivating, empowering, and rewarding service staff should be emphasized as a critical 91 marketing strategy to evoke delightfiil experiences for customers and ultimately to provide a tool in the creation of customer loyalty. Third, web-based social media such as customer blogs, review sites, and discussion boards would allow managers to apply marketing communication strategies and to create customer delight. Web-based social media can play a vital role, not just as a marketing channel, but as conversational marketing by transforming customers from content customers to content producers and building customer relationships (Knutson, 2010). Additionally, e-mail marketing can also be a powerful direct-to-customer distribution and marketing tool which allows companies to engage customers in personalized and mutually beneficial interactive relationships and sells the experience more efficiently. Fourth, this study suggests that adding the four different phases of loyalty development to measure customer loyalty can be a good tool for managers to understand the loyalty of customers. To increase customer loyalty, companies must know and understand the key needs and wants that are important to their customers, recognizing that customers are not all the same, nor are their concerns and expectations (Keiningham and Vavra, 2001). Companies need to gather as much information as possible about the customers; thoroughly knowing and understanding customers is essential before a company can create delight for customers. Additionally, understanding customer’s emotional connections with products and services can have the potential to generate a higher level of satisfaction, delight, and ultimately, loyalty for the company. Hence, by measuring customer satisfaction, emotions including customer delight, and the four different phases of loyalty, managers can utilize the information to understand individual 92 customer’s preferences to offer personalized services with deep emotional experiences and they can design, target, and apply a marketing strategy based on their satisfaction, emotions, and different loyalty stages. As a result, the companies can be differentiated from other competitors and attract more business over competitors. Fifih, this study can provide important knowledge and a tool for managers to segment customers according to their loyalty stage. Customers who are at different phases of customer loyalty may need differentiated strategies applied to them (Knox and Walker, 2001; McMullan and Gilmore, 2008). Specifically, the finding that affective loyalty has greater impact on conative loyalty, in comparison to cognitive loyalty, can be considered by marketers in the planning and implementation of loyalty programs. The importance of customers’ positive emotions is not a new finding, but managers seem to have been slow to apply this into practice. Emotions can be at the heart of a marketing strategy, as these findings have shown. Therefore, managers need to understand which products and services can drive more customers to affective loyalty. As an example, the loyalty program is becoming a more important long-term marketing strategy for customer loyalty because retaining existing customers costs less than acquiring a new customer. However, most of the present loyalty programs offer points or savings as the key benefit and managers are not aware of a customer’s emotion when they develop or implement the loyalty program even though customers are looking for personalized loyalty rewards and benefits. To offer the right type of rewards and benefits which can be found in specially designed personalized loyalty programs, managers need to have enough individual member preference information including the understanding of customers’ emotions. Therefore, this study suggests that marketers need to develop a loyalty program not only 93 to reward loyal customers but also to effectively enhance affective loyalty, which is strongly related to conative loyalty and action loyalty either directly or indirectly. Limitations and Future Studies Although this study provides several theoretical and practical implications for the tourism and hospitality industries, there are several limitations and recommendations for further research. First, the model presented in this study fits the sample and the data well. To be generalized to other populations, the theoretical structure can be tested with different samples such as types of accommodations (e. g., business hotel, bed and breakfast), places (e.g., other states, other countries), and service industries (e.g., restaurant, airline, cruise). A second limitation of this study relates to the action loyalty items in the survey. The items in measuring action loyalty may not be accurate in the likelihood to return/recommend to stay at this hotel. The respondents may respond inaccurately or just guess their willingness to return/recommend to visit this hotel. Thus, future research might need to consider measuring actual stays with the hotel studied and their recommendation of this hotel to validate the accuracy of action loyalty. Third, although the model fits the data reasonably well with encouraging results, the discriminant validity of some latent variables in this study was relatively low. This was due to the high correlations among constructs. Future study is needed to conduct additional research on measurement scale improvement and discriminant validity, including further investigation of the relationship between the scale and the measures of the constructs. The scale could be also tested with another data set. Therefore, future research might enhance the model measures by improving the discriminant validity. 94 Finally, future research could benefit from pursuing other factors, which can be incorporated into the model. For example, socio-demographics, such as gender, might be different for customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty. Particularly, for hi gh- involvement products and services, there is a significant difference in customer satisfaction and loyalty by gender (Mittal and Kamakura, 2001). Additionally, future research would also need to consider differentiating various customer segments (i.e., male vs. female) and research methodologies (i.e., online vs. paper surveys) in the conceptual model developed in this study. Conclusion This study aimed to test the impact of customer satisfaction and delight on loyalty in the context of tourism and hospitality. The findings from this study demonstrate that cognitive and affective loyalty are directly influenced by customer satisfaction and delight, conative loyalty is directly influenced by cognitive and affective loyalty, and action loyalty is directly influenced by conative loyalty. The results of this study have both theoretical and practical value in that they fill gaps in previous tourism and hospitality research on customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty. Furthermore, the results suggest a new conceptualization of the loyalty dimension. This study also provides important information on emotional factors such as customer delight and affective loyalty which are critical concepts related to loyalty. Future research, based on this study, should (1) replicate this study with a probabilistic survey sample, (2) validate the accuracy of action loyalty by measuring actual future return/recommendation, (3) improve measurement scales, and (4) extend this model by incorporating other possible factors that may influence customer satisfaction, delight, and loyalty. 95 APPENDICES 96 Table 29. Comparison of Demographic Characteristics between On-line Survey Appendix A Respondents and Paper Survey Respondents Group On-line Survey Paper Survey Difference Characteristics Respondents Respondents Tests Frequency % Frequency % Number of data 1 ,5 73 87 Gender Non. Sig. Female 1,072 69% 60 70% Male 481 31% 26 30% Total 1,553 100% 86 100% Ethnicity Non. Sing Afiican American/Black 30 2% 3 4% American Indian 5 l 3% 0 0% Asian or Pacific Islander 1 1 1% 0 0% European American/Middle East/White 1 ,246 82% 80 94% Hispanic/Latino/Latina 12 1% 0 0% Other 176 12% 2 2% Total 1,526 100% 85 100% Income Non. Sig. Less than $25,000 52 4% 6 7% $25,000-$34,999 83 6% 7 8% $35,000—$49,999 161 l 1% 20 24% $50,000-$74,999 327 22% 16 19% $75,000-$99,999 267 18% 8 10% $100,000-$149,999 268 18% 7 8% $150,000-$199,999 70 5% 1 1% $200,000 or more 33 2% 19 23% I prefer not to respond 229 15% 0 0% Total 1,490 100% 84 100% Employment status .014 Employed full-time 931 67% 40 46% Employed part-time 15 1% 9 10% Self-employed 130 9% 8 9% Retired 179 13% 25 29% Unemployed 67 5% 1 1% Other 67 5% 4 5% Total 1,574 100% 87 100% 97 Table 29 (cont’d) Residency: MI OH 1,085 69% 67 76% CANADA 189 12% 8 9% IN 1 10 7% 2 2% IL 47 3% 2 2% Other (e.g., FL, NY, PA) 47 3% 0 0% 94 6% 9 10% Total 1,572 100% 88 100% Average age 48 53 Average number of 3 3 people in the household Adults: 2 Adults: 2 Children: 1 Children: 1 98 Appendix B Table 30. Comparison of Demographic Characteristics between Respondents and Non-Respondents Characteristics Respondents Non-Respondents Frequency % Frequency % Number of data 1,660 31% 3,731 69% Residency: MI 1,137 70% 2,537 68% OH 200 12% 410 11% CANADA 102 6% 262 7% IN 51 3% 149 4% IL 51 3% 1 12 3% Other (e. g., FL, NY, PA) 94 6% 260 7% Total 1,635 100% 3,731 100% Loyalty membership holder 815 50% 970 26% Average money spent during the stay at the resort $264 $259 Average number of people in the 3 3 household Adults: 2 Adults: 2 Children: 1 Children: 1 99 Appendix C A Survey Questionnaire Bavarian Inn Lodge Guest Survey Ill-n lllll“ {Hit . "'I uh .«umuu . u u I ‘1- .- 'I .y u : Thank you for accepting our invitation to participate in this important research. We are surveying recent overnight hotel guests. This first section asks about your recent visit to the Bavarian Inn Lodge and Frankenmuth. 1. On this recent stay, did you consider staying at any other hotel, motel, bed & breakfast, or campggound in the Frankenmuth area other than Bavarian Inn Lodge? Please select one. El Yes (If yes, which one(s)?) El No 2. On this recent stay, did you consider staying at any other destination other than F ' “ for this vacation? Please select one. Cl Yes (If yes, which one(s)?) El No 3. Are you currently a member of the Bavarian Inn Lodge loyalty progzam called the “Bavarian Inn Perks Club”? Please select one. 1] Yes D No, I have never been a member El Not sure El No, but I was a member in the past 100 4. Are you currently a member of any other hotel. motel, bed & breakfast, or campground loyalty progzam that you could have used in the F rankenmuth area? Please select one. [1 Yes D No, I have never been a member I] Not sure El No, but I was a member in the past This second section asks about your planning, decision making, and hotel choice. 5. What was the primag pumose of your recent stay at the Bavarian Inn Lodge? Please select one. C] Business Cl Group/ Bus tour [I Leisure 6. How did you make a hotel choice or reservation for your recent stay at the Bavarian Inn Lodge? Please select one. D Self-arranged (continue to #7) l] Pre-arranged by others (skip to #9) 7. Please indicate the importance of the followirLgyaspects of the Bavarian Inn Lodge when you selected this hotel for your recent stay. Please select one for each item. Very Very Unimportant Neutral Important Friendliness of staff 1 2 3 5 6 7 Hotel amenities (pool, game room, free nightly entertainment) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Hotel location 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Quality of food 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Relationships established between 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 staff and customers Room cleanliness 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Room comfort 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Value for money 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8. What influenced you to book your recent stay at the Bavarian Inn Lodge? Please select all that apply. [:1 AAA membership [3 Internet web site or search engine [:1 Billboard [:1 Meeting organizer [j Brochure [1 Newspaper/Magazine D Bus tour [1 Previous visit [1 Department of Transportation road sign [3 Special package rate [:1 E-mail promotion, newsletters offer [:1 Travel agent [3 Friend or family member recommended [3 TV/Radio [1 Highway welcome centers [:1 Visitors Center or Chamber of Commerce [3 Hotel gift certificate [3 Walk-in [:1 Hotel membership card holder [:1 Other (please, specify) (Perks Club) 101 This third section asks about your experiences during your recent stay at the Bavarian Inn Lodge. 9. Who stayed with you on your recent stay at the Bavarian Inn Lodge? Please select all that apply. El Alone D Family members: (please select all that apply) [3 Business acquaintances El Children [1 Parents D Clubs organized group Cl Grandchildren D Siblings [3 Friends El Grandparents El Spouse [:1 Others (please specify) Cl Other family members 10. What leisure activities did you do during your trip which included the Bavarian Inn Lodge? Please select all that apply. [I Attended a festival, event, or activity Dined at a unique restaurant in the Bavarian Inn Lodge Dined at the Bavarian Inn restaurant on Main Street Dined at Zehnder’s restaurant on Main Street Cl C] Shopped in Riverplace Shopped on Main Street Frankenmuth Toured or drove for pleasure Visited a historic site [I Dined at a unique restaurant outside of I] Visited a local museum the Bavarian Inn Lodge [3 Explored F rankenmuth city or town [3 Visited/Shopped Farmer’s market or Pick-your-own [j Participated in outdoor recreation (i.e., E] Visited a local or county park (e.g., bike, golf, boat, swim and beach sport fields, trails) activities, fish etc.) [:1 Played in Bavarian Inn Lodge game [3 Went swimming in Bavarian Inn rooms or indoor putt-putt golf Lodge’s pools or Whirlpools [j Shopped in Birch Rm [:1 Other (please specijy) 11. If you could describe your recent stay at the Bavarian Inn Lodge by an emotion or feeling. what would that emotion or feeling be? Please type in the blank below. 102 12. Please indicate the extent to which each of the following statements accurately describes aa emotion or feeling you h’ad during your recent stay at the Bavarian Inn Lodge. Please select one for each item. I felt at some time_during my stay Strongly Strongly at the Lodge. Disagree Neutral Agree Delighted 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Grateful l 2 3 4 5 6 7 Elated 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Gleeful l 2 3 4 5 6 7 Overjoyed l 2 3 4 5 6 7 Pleased l 2 3 4 5 6 7 Positively surprised I 2 3 4 5 6 7 The Bavarian Inn Lodge had experiences/ services which were unexpected and they delighted me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 This fourth section asks about your satisfaction with your recent stay at the Bavarian Inn Lodge and future planning with the Lodje. 13. Please indicate the extent to which each of the following aspects accurately describes your satisfaction with your recent stay at the Bavarian Inn Lodge. Please select one for each item. Didn’t Very Very apply to Unsatisfied Neutral Satisfied my stay Friendliness of staff 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 [3 Hotel amenities ool, ame room, free nightly) enteftainment) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 D Hotel location 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 [1 Quality of food 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 El R l i n i li b... 1 2 a 4 s . 7 a Room cleanliness 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 El Room comfort 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 [:1 Value for money 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 E] 14. Please indicate the extent to which each of the following statements accurately describes your overall satisfaction with your recent stay at the Bavarian Inn Lodge. Please select one for each item. Strongly Strongly Overall, Bavarian Inn Lodge was . Disagree Neutral Agree Comfortable l 2 3 4 5 6 7 Pleasing l 2 3 4 5 6 7 Satisfying to me 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Overall, I was content at the Lodge. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 103 15. Please indicate the extent to which each of the following statements accurately describes your thoughts about the Bavarian Inn Lodge. Please select one for each item. Strongly Strongly Disagree Neutral Agree I believe the Lodge provides more benefits 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 than other hotels of its type. I made the right chorce of hotel With the 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Lodge. The Lodge’s farcilities are visually more appealing compared to other hotels of its 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 type. The Lodge has better value for my money compared to other hotel’s prices of its type. The Lodge provides me superior service 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 gualigy compared to other hotels of its type. 16. Please indicate the extent to which each of the following statements accurately describes your emotion or feeling with the Bavarian Inn Lodge compare to other hotels of its type. Please select one for each item. Strongly Strongly Disagree Neutral Agree I feel happy when I stay at the Lodge. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 I feel gateful to the Lodge. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 I 11.3 the Lodge more than other hotels 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 of rts type. I love staying at the Lodge. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Staying in the Lodge says a lot about who I am. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 17. Please indicate the likelihood to return to stay at the Bavarian Inn Lodge? Please select one for each item. Extremely Quite I will return to the Lodge in . Unlikely Neutral Likely the next 6 months or less 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 the next 7-12 months 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 the next year (more than 12 months from now) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 18. Please indicate the seasonal likelihood to return to stay at the Bavarian Inn Lodge? Please select one for each item. Extremely Quite I will return to the Lodge in . Unlikely Neutral Likely Spring (March, April, May) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Summer (June, July, August) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Fall (September, October, November) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Winter (December, January, February) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 104 19. Please indicate the likelihood to recommend the Bavarian Inn Lodge to others. Please select one. Extremely Quite Unlikely Neutral Likely I will recommend the Lodge to others. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 20. Please indicate the extent to which each of the following statements accurately describes your intention toward staying at the Bavarian Inn Lodge. Please select one for each item. Strongly Strongly Disagree Neutral Agree . . I consrder myself to be highly loyal to 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 the Lodge. I intend to continue staflng at the 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Lodge. I would change hotels if another hotel’s staff was more friendly. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 I would try an alternative hotel if it was less expensive than the Lodge. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 I would try an altematrve hotel if their 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 loyalty program offered more benefits. I would try an altemtive hotel if the alternative hotel offered increased 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 facilities/ services to the Lodge. This fifth section asks about your past experience with Bavarian Inn Lodge, Frankenmuth, and other hotels. 21. In the past three ways, how many times have you stayed at other hotels. motels, bed & breakfasts, or campgrounds in the Frankenmuth area? Please select one. El 0 times (skip to #26) El 4-6 times II] 10-12 times C] 1-3 times [3 7-9 times Cl More than 12 times 22. Approximately, what was the month and year of your last gay before this recent stay in the mnkenmuth area? Please type in the month and year below. Month Year 23. In the past three years excluding this recent stay, how many times have you stayed at the Bavarian Inn Lodge? Please select one. El 0 times (skip to #26) El 4-6 times El 10-12 times [:1 1-3 times El 7-9 times [3 More than 12 times 24. Approximately, what was the month and year of your last stay before this recent stay at the Bavarian Inn Lodge during the past three years? Please type in the month and year below. Month Year 25. In the past three years, how many times have you recommended the Bavarian Inn Lodge to others? Please select one. El 0 times El 4-6 times 1] 10-12 times C] 1-3 times [1 7-9 times El More than 12 times 105 The final section of this survey asks for descriptive information about you and your household. This information will be held in the strictest confidence and will only be used for statistic purposes. 26. Are you? Please select a response. El Male [3 Female 27. In which year were you born? Please type a number. My birth year 28. In which US. state or foreign country is your primary residence located? Please type it. Location of primary residence 29. What is the postal code of your primary residence? Please type the numbers. Postal code 30. How many children and adults live in your household including yourself? Please type a whole number in each field below. Enter “0” if none. Total number of people in your household (including yourself) Number of children (18 years or younger) Number of adults (19 years or older, including yourself) 31. Which one of the following best describes your ethnicity or race? Please select one. C] African American/ Black El European American/ Middle East/White E] American Indian El Hispanic/ Latino/ Latina El Asian or Pacific Islander [3 Other (please specijy) 32. Which employment status best describes you at the time you visited Bavarian Inn Lodge? Please select one. [I] Employed full-time D Retired l3 Unemployed El Employed part-time Cl Self-employed El Other (please specify) 33. Which income category best describes your total annual household income before aw in 2008? Please select one. D Less than $25,000 El $50,000- $74,999 Cl $150,000-$199,999 El $25,000- $34,999 El $75,000 - $99,999 [1 $200,000 or more Cl $35,000- $49,999 D $100,000-$149,999 El I prefer not to respond. Thank you for completing this survey. To enter a chance to win one grand prize of a Bavarian Inn Lodge stay, please fill out the contact information on the drawing ticket. Everyone who completed this survey and sends us back this survey will receive $20, $40, $5 0, and half ofl your second chicken vouchers by email. 106 BIBLIOGRAPHY 107 BIBLIOGRAPHY Ajzen, I. (1991). The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 1-63. Ajzen, I. and Driver, BL. (1991). Prediction of Leisure Participation from Behavioral, Normative, and Control Beliefs: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behavior, Leisure Sciences, 13, 185-204. Alrnanza, B.A., Jaffe, W., and Lin, L. (1994). Use of the Service Attribute Matrix to Measure Consumer Satisfaction. Hospitality Research Journal, 17(2), 63-75. Anderson, RE. (1973). Consumer Dissatisfaction: The Effect of Disconfirrned Expectancy on Perceived Product Performance. Journal of Consumer Research, 10(1), 38—44. Anderson, E.W. and Sullivan, M. (1993). The Antecedents and Consequences of Customer Satisfaction for Firms. Marketing Science, 12, 125-143. Anderson, E.W., Fornell, C., and Mazvancheryls, S. (2004). Customer Satisfaction and Shareholder Value. Journal of Marketing, 68 (October), 172-185. Arrnony, J .L., Servan-Schreiber, D., Cohen, J .D., and LeDoux, J .E. (1996). An Anatomically Constrained Neural Network Model of Fear Conditioning Behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 109, 246-25 7. Arnold, E.M., Goldston, D.B., Walsh, A.K., Reboussin, B.A., Daniel, S.S., Hickman, E., and Wood, F .B. (2005). Severity of Emotional and Behavioral Problems among Poor and Typical Readers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33(2), 205- 217. Assael, H. (1998). Consumer behavior and marketing action, (6th ed.). South Western College Publishing, Cincinnati, OH. Back, K. (2001). The Effects of Image Congruence on Customer Satisfaction and Brand Loyalty in the Lodging Industry. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Pennsylvania State University. Back, K]. (2005). The Effects of Image Congruence on Customers' Brand Loyalty in the Upper Middle-Class Hotel Industry. Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Research, 29, 448-467. Back, KI. and Parks, S. (2003). A Brand Loyalty Model Involving Cognitive, Affective, and Conative Brand Loyalty and Customer Satisfaction. Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Research, 27, 419-435. 108 Bagozzi, RP. and Yi, Y. (1988). On the Evaluation of Structural Equation Models. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 16, 74-94. Barsky, J .D. (1992). Customer Satisfaction in the Hotel Industry: Meaning and Measurement. Hospitality Research Journal, 16(1), 51-73. Barsky, J .D. and Labagh, R. (1992). Quality Management: A Strategy for Customer Satisfaction. Cornell Hotel and Restaurant Administration Quarterly, 33(5), 32-37. Beeho, A.J. and Prentice, RC. (1997). Conceptualizing the Experiences of Heritage Tourists: A Case Study of New Lanark World Heritage Village. Tourism Management, 18, 75 - 87. Bendapudi, N. and Berry, LL. (1997). Customers’ Motivations for Maintaining Relationships with Service Providers. Journal of Retailing, 1, 15-37. Bennett, PD. (1995). Dictionary of Marketing Terms. American Marketing Association, Chicago, IL. Berrnan B. (2005). How to Delight Your Customers. California Management Review, 48(1), 129-151. Berry, LL. (1995). Relationship Marketing of Services. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Service, Fall, 236-245. Bigné, J. E., Andreu, L., and Gnoth, J. (2003). The Theme Park Experience An Analysis of Pleasure, Arousal and Satisfaction. Tourism Management, 26(6), 833-844. Blackwell, R.D., Miniard, P.W., and Engel, J .F. (2001). Consumer Behavior. 9‘h Ed., Hartcourt College Publishers, Orlando, FL. Bollen, K.A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. Wiley, New York, NY. Boulding,W., Kalra, A., Staelin, R., and Zeithaml,V.A. (1993). A Dynamic Process Model of Service Quality: From Expectations to Behavioral Intentions. Journal of Marketing Research, 30, 7—27. Bowen J .T. and Shoemaker, S. (1998). Loyalty: A Strategic Commitment. The Cornell Hotel and Restaurant Administration Quarterly, February, 12-25. Bramwell, B. (1998). User Satisfaction and Product Development in Urban Tourism. Tourism Management, 19(1), 35—47. 109 Breckler, SJ. (1984). Empirical Validation of Affect, Behavior, and Cognition as Distinct Components of Attitude. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(6), 1191-1205. Browne, M.W. and Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative Ways of Assessing Model fit. K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long Eds., Sage, Newbury Park, CA. Cadotte, ER. and Turgeon, N. (1988). Key Factors in Guest Satisfaction. Cornell Hotel and Restaurant Quarterly, 28(4), 45-51. Cardozo, R. (1965). An Experimental Study of Customer Effort, Expectation and Satisfaction. Journal of Marketing Research, 2, 244-249. Carpenter, GS. and Lehmann, DR. (1985). A Model of Marketing Mix, Brand Switching, and Competition. Journal of Marketing Research, 22, 318-329. Chebat, J .C. and Michon, R. (2003). Impact of Ambient Odors on Mall Shoppers’ Emotions, Cognition, and Spending. Journal of Business Research, 56, 529—539. Chen, J .S. and Gursoy, D.G (2001). An Investigation of Tourists’ Destination Loyalty and Preferences. International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality, 13(2), 79- 86. Cheung, G.W. and Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating Goodness-Of—Fit Indexes for Testing Measurement Invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 233-255. Chin, W. (1998). The Partial Least Squares Approach to Structural Equation Modeling. In GA Marcoulides, Ed., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Mahwah, NJ. Churchill, GA. and Surprenant, C. (1982). An Investigation into the Determinants of Customer Satisfaction. Journal of Marketing Research, 19 (November), 491-504. Colombo, R.A., Morrison, D.G.,, and Green, J .D. (1989). A Brand Switching Model with Implications for Marketing Strategies: Relaxing the Loyalty Condition in the Colombo/Morrison Model; Commentary; Reply. Marketing Science, 8, 89-106. Couper, MP. (2000). Review: Web Surveys: A Review of Issues and Approaches. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 64(4), 464-494. Cronin, J.J., Brady, M.K., and Hult, GT. (2000). Assessing the Effects of Quality, Value, and Customer Satisfaction on Consumer Behavioral Intention in Service Environments. Journal of Retailing, 76(2), 193-218. Cronin, J .J . and Taylor, SA. (1992). Measuring Service Quality: A Reexamination and Extension. Journal of Marketing, 56, 55-68. 110 Darnasio, AR. (1994). Descartes ’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. Avon, New York, NY. Danaher, P.J, Arweiler, N. (1996). Customer Satisfaction in the Tourist Industry: A Case Study of Visitors to New Zealand. Journal of Travel Research, 35(1), 89-93. Day, G. S. (1969). A Two-Dimensional Concept of Brand Loyalty. Journal of Advertising Research, 9(3), 29-35. Dekimpe, M.G., Steenkamp, J .E.M., Mellens, M., and Vanden, AP (1997). Decline and Variability in Brand Loyalty. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 14, 405—420. DeVillis, RF. (1991). Scale Development: Theory and Applications, Sage, Newbury Park, CA. Dichter, E. (1985). What’s in an Image? Journal of Consumer Marketing, Winter, 75-81 . Dick, AS. and Basu, K. (1994). Customer Loyalty: Toward an Integrated Conceptual Framework. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 22, 99-113. Dobni, D. and Zinkhan, GM. (1990). In Search of Brand Image: A Foundation Analysis. Advances in Consumer Behavior, 17, 110-1 19. Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the Cognitive and Psychodynamic Unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709-724. Estelarni. H. (2000). Competitive and Procedural Determinants of Delight and Disappointment in Consumer Complaint Outcomes. Journal of Service Research, 2(3), 285-301. Evanschitzky, H. and Wunderlich, M. (2006). An Examination of Moderator Effects in the Four-Stage Loyalty Model, Journal of Service Research, 8(4), 330-345. Finn, A. (2005). Reassessing the Foundations of Customer Delight. Journal of Service Research, 8(2), 103-116. F ishbein, M. and Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research. Reading, Addison-Wesley, MA. Fornell, C., Johnson, M.D., Anderson, E.W., Cha, J ., Bryant, B.E.(1996). The American Customer Satisfaction Index: Nature, purpose and findings. Journal of Marketing 60, 7-18. 111 F omell, C. and Larcker, DP. (1981). Evaluating Structural Equation Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18(1), 39-50. Garson, G. D. (2006). Structural Equation Modeling Example Using WinAMOS, 2006.08.01. Retrieved fiom : ttp://www2.chass.ncsu.edu/garson/PA765/semAMOS1.htrn Getty, J .M. and Thompson, K.N. (1995). The Relationship Between Quality, Satisfaction, and Recommending Behavior in Lodging Decisions. Journal of Hospitality Marketing and Management, 2(3), 3-22. Guest, L. (1944). A Study of Brand Loyalty. Journal of Applied Psychology 28, 16-27. Hair, J .F., Anderson, R.E., Tatham, KL, and Black, WC. (1998). Multivariate data analysis. 5th Ed., Prentice Hall, New Jersey, NY. Hatcher, L. (1994). A Step-by-Step Approach to Using the SAS System for Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling. Statistical Analysis Systems Institute. Cary, NC. Harris, LC. and Goode, M. H. (2004). The four levels of loyalty and the pivotal role of trust: a study of online service dynamics. Journal of Retailing, 80(2), 139-158. Hawkins, D.I., Mothersbaugh, BL, and Best, R.J. (2007). Consumer Behavior. McGraw-Hill Companies, 10’11 Ed., New York, NY. Hewson, C. M., Yule, P., Laurent, D., and Vogel, CM. (2003). Internet Research Methods: A Practical Guide for the Social and Behavioral Science. Sage, London, UK. Hicks, J .M., Page, T.J., Behe, B.K., Demris, J .H., and Fernandez, RT (2005). Delighted Consumers Buy Again. Journal of Consumer Satisfaction, Dissatisfaction and Complaining Behavior, 18, 94-104. Hirschman, EC. and Holbrook, MB. (1982). Hedonic Consumption: Emerging Concepts, Methods and Propositions. Journal of Marketing, 46 (Summer), 92-101. Homburg, C., Koschate, N., and Hoyer, W.D. (2005). Do Satisfied Customer Really Pay More? A Study of the Relationship Between Customer Satisfaction and Willingness to Pay. Journal of Marketing, 69 (April), 84-96. Howard, J .A. and Sheth, J .N. (1969). The Theory of Buyer Behaviour, Wiley, New York, NY. Hunt, H. K. (1977). Conceptualization and Measurement of Customer Satisfaction and Dissatisfaction, Marketing Science Institute, Cambridge, MA. 112 Hunter, V.L. (1998). Measure Customer Loyalty for Complete Picture of ROI. Business Marketing, 83 (3), 18. Ilgen, D. R. (1971). Satisfaction with Performance as a Function of the Initial Level of Expected Performance and the Deviation from Expectations. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 6, 345—361. Iwasaki, Y. and Havitz, ME. (1998). A Path Analytic Model of The Relationships between Involvement, Psychological Commitment, and Loyalty. Journal of Leisure Research, 30, 256-280. J acoby, J. and Chestnut, RW. (1978). Brand loyalty measurement and management. Wiley New York, NY. J acoby. J. and Kyner, DB. (1973). Brand Loyalty vs. Repeat Purchasing Behavior. Journal of Marketing Research, 10, 1-9. Jones, TO. and Sasser, W. (1995). Why Satisfied Customers Defect. Harvard Business Review, November/December, 88-99. Jones, T. and Taylor, SF. (2007). The Conceptual Domain of Service Loyalty: How Many Dimensions?. Journal of Services Marketing, 21(1), 36-51. J oreskog, K.G. and Stirbom, D. (1993). New features in LISRELEI. Scientific Software, Chicago, IL. Juaneda, C. (1996). Estimating the Probability of Return Visits Using a Survey of Tourist Expenditure in the Balearic Islands. Tourism Economics, 2(4), 339—352. Kalyanaram, G. and Little, J .D. (1994). An Empirical Analysis of Latitude of Price Acceptance in Consumer Package Goods. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(3), 408-419. Keiningham, TL. and Vavra, T.G. (2001). The Customer Delight Principle. McGraw-Hill New York, NY. Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. Guilford, New York. Knox, SD. and Walker, D. (2001). Measuring and Managing Brand Loyalty. Journal of Strategic Marketing, 9(2), 1 11-128. Knutson, B]. (1988). Ten Laws of Customer Satisfaction. Cornell Hotel and Restaurant Quarterly, 29(3), 14-17. 113 Knutson, B.J. (2010). Power to the People. The Greater Lansing Business Monthly, May, 8-9. Knutson, B.J., Stevens, R, Wullaert, C., Patton, M., and Yokoyama, R. (1991). LODGSERV: A Service Quality Index for the Lodging Industry. Hospitality Research Journal, 15, 277-284. Kozak, M. (2001). Repeaters’ Behavior at Two Distinct Destinations. Annals of Tourism Research, 28(3), 784—807. Kozak, M. and Rimmington, M. (2000). Tourist Satisfaction with Mallorca, Spain, as an Off-Season Holiday Destination. Journal of Travel Research, 38(1), 260— 269. Krishnamurthi, L. and Papatla, P. (2003). Accounting for Heterogeneity and Dynamics in the Loyalty-Price Sensitivity Relationship. Journal of Retailing, 79(2), 121- 135. Kroeber-Riel, W. (1979). Activation Research: Psychobiological Approaches in Consumer Research. Journal of Consumer Behavior, March, 240-250. Kuhl, J. (1985). Volitional Mediators of Cognition-Behavior Consistency: Self- Regulatory Processes and Action Control Versus State Orientation. In J. Kuhl & ‘ Beckmann, Springer-Verlag, New York, NY. Kumar, A., Olshavsky, RW., and King, M.F. (2001). Exploring Alternative Antecedents of Customer Delight. Journal of Consumer Satisfaction, Dissatisfaciton and Complaining Behavior, 14, 14-26. Kunst-Wilson, W.R. and Zajonc, RB. (1980). Affective Discrimination of Stimuli That Cannot Be Recognized. Science, 207, 557-558. Lazarus, RS. (1982). Thoughts on the Relations between Emotion and Cognition. American Psychologist, 39, 1019-1024. Lazarus, RS. (1984). On the Primacy of Cognition. American Psychologist, 39(2), 124-129. Lazarus, RS. (1991). Emotion and Adaptation. Oxford University Press, New York, NY. LeDoux, J .E. (1995). Emotion: Clues from the Brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 209-235. LeDoux, J .E. (1996). The Emotional Brain. Simon & Schuster, New York, NY. 114 Lerner, J. S. and Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, Anger, and Risk. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 81(1), 146-159. Lewis, RC. (1983). The Measurement of Gaps in the Quality of Hotel Services. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 6(2), 83-88. Lewis, R. C. and Pizarn, A. (1981). Guest Surveys: A Missed Opportunity. The Cornell HRA Quarterly, November, 37-44. Li, X. and Petrick, J .F. (2008). Examining the Antecedents of Brand Loyalty from an Investment Model Perspective. Journal of Travel Research, 47(1), 25-34. Locke, EA. (1976). The Nature and Causes of Job Satisfaction. Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Rand McNally, Chicago, IL. Loewenstein, G.F., Weber, E.U., Hsee, GK, and Welch, ES. (2001). Risk as Feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267-286. McMullan, R. and Gilmore, A. (2002). The Conceptual Development of Customer Loyalty Measurement: A Proposed Scale. Journal of Targeting, Measurement, and Analysis for Marketing, 11(3), 230-243. Maister, D. H. (1993). Managing the Professional Service Firm, Free Press, New York, NY. Mano, H. and Oliver, KL. (1993). Assessing the Dirnensionality and Structure of Consumption Experience: Evaluation, Feeling, and Satisfaction. Journal of Consumer Research, 20 (December), 451-466. Mattila, AS. (2001). The Impact of Relationship Type on Customer Loyalty in a Context of Service Failures. Journal of Service Research, 4(2), 91-101. Mattila, A. and Wirtz, J. (2000). The Role of Preconsumption Affect in Postpurchase Evaluation of Services. Psychology and Marketing, 17(7), 587-605. Mitchell, J. (2003). Hug Your Customers: The Proven Way to Personalize Sales and Achieve Astounding Results. Hyperion Books, New York, NY. Mittal, V.W. and Kamakura, A.( 2001). Satisfaction and Repurchase Behavior: The Moderating Influence of Customer and Market Characteristics. Journal of Marketing Research. 38(1), 131-142. Neal, W.D. (1999). Satisfaction is Nice, but Value Drives Loyalty. Marketing Research, Spring, 21-23. 115 Newman, J .W. and Werbel, RA. (1973). Multivariate Analysis of Brand Loyalty for Maj or Household Appliances. Journal of Marketing Research, 10, 404—409. Nightingale, M. (1985). The Hospitality Industry: Defining Quality for a Quality Assurance Programme: A Study of Perceptions. The Service Industries Journal, 5(1), 9- 22. Nunnally, J .C. (197 8). Psychomtietric Theory , 2th ed., McGraw-Hill, New York, NY. Oh, H. and Jeong, M. (1996). Improving Marketers’ Predictive Power of Customer Satisfaction on Expectation-Based Target Market Levels. Hospitality Research Journal, 19(4), 65-85. Oliver, R.L. (1980). A Cognitive Model of the Antecedents and Consequences of Satisfaction Decisions. Journal of Marketing Research, 17 (September), 46-49. Oliver, R.L. (1981). Measurement and Evaluation of Satisfaction Processes in Retailing Setting. Journal of Retailing, 57 (Fall), 25-48. Oliver, R.L. (1997). Satisfaction: A behavioral perspective on the consumer. McGrasw-Hill, New York, NY. Oliver, R.L. (1999). Whence customer loyalty? Journal of Marketing, 63, 33-44. Oliver, R.L., Rust, RT, and Varki, S. (1997). Customer Delight: Foundations, Findings, and Managerial Insight. Journal of Retailing, 73(3), 311-336. Oliver, R.L. and Rust, RT. (2000). Should We Delight Customer? Academy of Marketing Science Journal, 28(1), 86-94. Oliver, R.L. and Swan, J .E. (1989). Consumer Perceptions of Interpersonal Equity and Satisfaction in Transactions: A Field Survey Approach. Journal of Marketing, 53 (2), 21—35. Olshavsky, R. and Miller, J. (1972). Consumer Expectations, Product Performance and Perceived Product Qualtiy. Journal of Marketing Research, 9 (Feburary), 1 9-2 1 . Palmer, A., McMahon-Beattie, U., and Beggs, R. (2000). Influences on Loyalty Programme Effectiveness: A Conceptual Framework and Case Study Investigation. Journal of Strategic Marketing, 8(1), 47-66. Patterson, K. (1997). Delighted Clients Are Loyal Clients. Rough Notes, 140(3), 221-234. 116 Patterson, PG (2004). A Contingency Model of Behavioral Intention in a Service Context. Journal of European Marketing, 28(September/ October), 1304-13 15. Paul, J. (2000). Are You Deli ghting Your Customers? Nonforprofit World, 18(5), 34-36. Perez-Caro M. and Sanchez-Garcia, I. (2006). Killing Time for Cancer Stem Cells: Discovery and Development of Selective CSC inhibitors. Journal of Medicinal Chemistry, 13, 1719-1725. Peter, J .P. and Olson, J .C. (2005). Consumer Behavior & Marketing Strategy. McGraw-Hill Companies, 7th Ed., New York, NY. Phillips, D. M. and Baumgartner, H. (2002). The Role of Consumption Emotions in the Satisfaction Response. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(3), 243-252. Piaget, J. (1981). Intelligence and Affectivity: Their Relationship during Child Development. T. A. Brown & C. E. Kaegi, Trans. & Eds., Palo Alto, CA. Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion. A Psychoevolutionary Synthesis. Harper and Row, New York, NY. Pritchard, MP. and Howard, DR. (1997). The Loyal Traveler: Examining a Typology of Service Patronage? Journal of Travel Research, 35 (Spring), 2-10. Raykov, T. (1998). A Method for Obtaining Standard Errors and Confidence Intervals of Composite Reliability for Congeneric Measures. Applied Psychological Measurement, 22, 369-374. Reichheld, F. and Sasser, W. (1990). Zero defects: quality comes to services. Harvard Business Review, September-Octorber, 105-111. Ross, G. F. (1993). Destination Evaluation and Vacation Preferences. Annals of Tourism Research, 20, 477—489. Rundle-Thiele, S. (2005). Elaborating Customer Loyalty: Exploring Loyalty to Wine Retailers. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 12(5), 333-344. Russell, J .A. (1980). A Circumplex Model of Affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39 (6), 1 161—1 178. Russell, J .A. and Pratt, G. (1980). A Description of the Affective Quality Attributed to Environments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 311-322. Rust, RT and Oliver, R.L. (1994). Service Quality: Insights and Managerial Implications from the Frontier: Sage Publications, New York, NY. 117 Ryan, C. and Cliff, A. (1997). Do Travel Agencies Measure up to Customer Expectations? An Empirical Investigation of Travel Agencies’ Service Quality as Measured by SERVQUAL. Journal of Travel and Tourism Marketing, 6(2), 1-31. Saleh, F. and Ryan, C. (1991). Analyzing Service Quality in the Hospitality Industry Using the SERVQUAL Model. Service Industries Journal, 11(3), 352-373. Scherrnelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H., and Muller, H. (2003). Evaluating the Fit of Structural Equation Models: Tests of Significance and Descriptive Goodness-Of- Fit Measures. Methods of psychological research online, 8, 23-74. Schlossberg, H. (1990). Satisfying Customer Is a Minimum; You Really Have to Delight Them. Marketing News, 24(11), 10-11. Shoemaker, S. and Lewis, RC. (1999). Customer Loyalty: The Future of Hospitality Marketing. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 18, 345-370. Sivakumar, K. and Raj, SP. (1997). Quality Tier Competition: How Price Change Influence Brand Choice and Category Choice. Journal of Marketing, 61(July), 71-85. Skogland, I. and Siguaw, J.A. (2004). Are Your Satisfied Customers Loyal? The Cornell Hotel and Restaurant Administration Quarterly, 45(3), 221-234. Slovic, P. (2000). Rational Actors and Rational Fools: The Influence of Affect on J udgrnent and Decision-Making. Roger Williams University Law Review, 6, 163-212. Slovic, P., Finucane, M.L., Peters, E., and MacGregor, D.G. (2002). The Affet Heuristic: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY. Slovic, P., F inucane, M., Peters, E., and MacGregor D.G. (2004). Risk as Analysis and Risk as Feelings: Some Thoughts about Affect, Reason, Risk, and Rationality. Risk Analysis, 24(2), 1-12. Spreng, R., MacKenzie, SB, and Olshavsky, RW. (1996). A Reexamination of the Determinants of Consumer Satisfaction. Journal of Marketing 60 (July), 15-32. Steiger, J .H. (1989). EZPA TH: A Supplementary Module for S YSA T and S Y GRAPH. SYSTAT, Evanston, IL. Stevens, W.D. (1970). Affection and Cognition in Transaction and the Mapping of Cultural Space. Educational Technology Research and Development, 18(4), 440-445. Stewart, TA. (1997). A Satisfied Customer Isn't Enough. Fortune, 136 (July), 90- 91. 118 Tellis, G.J. (1988). Advertising Exposure, Loyalty, and Brand Purchase: A Two- Stage Model of Choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 25, 134-144. Torres, E.N., and Kline, S. (2006). From Satisfaction to Delight: A Model for the Hotel Industry. International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management, 18 (4), 290-301. Trochim, W.M. (2001). The Research Methods Knowledge Base. 2nd Ed., Cornell University, Cincinnati, OH. Tsaur, S.H., Chiu, Y.C., and Huang, CH. (2002). Determinants of Guest Loyalty to International Tourist Hotels: A Neural Network Approach. Tourism Management, 23, 397-405. Tse, D.K, and Wilton, RC. (1988). Models of Consumer Satisfaction Formation: An Extension. Journal of Marketing Research, 25, 204-212. Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, Science, 185, 1124-1131. Vanhamme, J. and Bont, C]. (2008). Surprise Gift Purchases: Customer Insights from the Small Electrical Appliances Market. Journal of Retailing, 84(3), 354-369. Watson, D. and Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a Consensual Structure of Mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98(September), 219-235. Weaver, D. and Brickman, P. (1974). Expectancy, Feedback, and Disconfirmation as Independent Factors in Outcome Satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 420—428. Westbrook, RA. and Oliver, R.L. (1991). The Dimensionality of Consumption Emotion Patterns and Consumer Satisfaction. Journal of Consumer Research, 18 (June), 84-91. Westbrook, RA. and Reilly, M.D. ( 1983). Value-percept Disparity — An Alternative to the Disconfirmation of Expectations Theory of Consumer Satisfaction, in Bagozzi, R.P., Tybout, A.M. (Eds), Advances in Consumer Research, 10, 256-261. Williams, J .A. and Anderson, H.H. (1999). Customer Delight: The Beat of A Different Drummer. Journal of Satisfaction, Dissatisfaction, and Complaining Behavior, 12, 44-52. Wirtz, J. and Bateson, J .E. (1999). Consumer Satisfaction with Services: Integrating the Environment Perspective in Services Marketing into the Traditional Disconfirmation Paradigm. Journal of Business Research, 44(1), 55-66. 119 Wirtz, J., Mattila, AS, and Tan,L.P. (2000). The Moderating Role of Target- Arousal on the Impact of Affect on Satisfaction: An Examination in the Context of Service Experiences. Journal of Retailing, 76(3), 347-365. Woodruff, R., Cadotte, E., and Jenkins, R. (1983). Modeling Consumer Satisfaction Processes Using Experience-Based Norms. Journal of Marketing Research, 20, 296-304. Yang, Z. and Peterson, RT (2004). Customer Perceived Value, Satisfaction, and Loyalty: The Role of Switching Costs. Psychology & Marketing, 21(10), 799-822. Yau, O. H. and Chan, C. F. (1990). Hong Kong as a Travel Destination in Southeast Asia: A Multidimensional Approach. Tourism Management, 11(2), 123—132. Yi, Y. (1990). A Critical Review of Consumer Satisfaction. In Review of Marketing, Zeithaml, V.A. (Ed.), American Marketing Association, Chicago, IL. Yi, Y. and La, S. (2004). What Influences the Relationship Between Customer Satisfaction and Repurchase. Psychology & Marketing, 21(5), 351-373. Yoon, Y. and Uysal, M. (2005). An Examination of the Effects of Motivation and Satisfaction on Destination Loyalty: A Structural Model. Tourism Management, 26 (1), 45-56. Zajonc, RB. (1980). Feeling and Thinking: Preferences Need No Inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151—175. Zajonc, RB. (2000). Feeling and Thinking: Closing the Debate over the Independence of Affect. In J .P. Forgas, Ed., Cambridge University Press, .Cambridge, UK. Zajonc, R.B., and Markus, H. (1985). Must All Affect Be Mediated by Cognition? Journal of Consumer Research, 12(3), 363-364. Zeithaml, V.A. (2000). Service Quality, Profitability, and the Economic Worth of Customers: What We Know and What We Need to Learn. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 28 (1), 67-85. Zeithaml, V.A., Berry, LL, and Parasuraman, A. (1996).The Behavioral Consequences of Service Quality. Journal of Marketing, 60, 31-46. Zeithaml V.A. and Bitner M.J. (2003). Services Marketing: Integrating Customer Focus Across the Firm. McGraw-Hill Irwin, Boston, MA. 120 mlll llllllllll llllll lllllllllllllllllllllll 3 1293 03063 6587