AN TNVESTIGATION OF THE RELATTONSHIP BETWEEN THREE , ‘ CATEGORIES 0F COLLEGE MAJOR AND SELECTED MEASURES OF L‘ COGNTTWE STYLE Dissertation for the Degree of Ph: D. MECHTGAN STATE UNIVERSITY TGHN THOMAS DEINES 1974 Er L I B R ‘1 R Y leg; Michirran State U13: VCI‘SIty' {j a_. -v""" This is to certify that the thesis entitled AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN THREE CATEGORIES OE COLLEGE MAJOR AND SELECTED MEASURES OF COGNITIVE STYLE presented by John Thomas Deines has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for flPo—fiegree in M Thesis Director Gator. Will/am, an MajOr profess Datel’l’M/tcl. 2 3: [Cf/Li % 5 0.7639 l MW 2/79 ABSTRACT AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN THREE CATEGORIES OE COLLEGE MAJOR AND SELECTED MEASURES OF COGNITIVE STYLE BY John Thomas Deines The term cognitive style refers to basic ways in which people approach learning situations. The purpose of the present investigation was to determine the relation of cognitive style to college major. Should students majoring in different subjects be found to differ in cognitive style, this could influence instructional strategy and provide more information for counselors dealing with student concerns relative to choice of major. The cognitive style constructs selected for use were field- dependence-independence, reflection-impulsivity, intuition as defined by Malcolm Westcott, and the Jungian dominant types. American College Test composite score and grade point average were included for purposes of comparison. Humanities, social science, and natural science constituted the college major categories. Two samples of 150 undergraduate students (divided equally by sex and major) served as subjects for the study. There were four major hypotheses. The first tested the related measures simultaneously in their ability to differentiate the categories of major and the sexes. The second examined the distribution of dominant Jungian types across the major categories. The third and fourth tested the related measures simultaneously in their ability to differentiate the dominant sensors and intuiters and to differentiate dominant thinkers and feelers. Analysis was via multivariate analysis of variance for testing John Thomas Deines the related measures simultaneously and via chi-square for testing the distribution of dominant Jungian types across the categories of major. Each measure was also tested via univariate analysis of variance. No hypothesis in the study was confirmed as stated. However, in one sample cognitive style was found to differentiate college majors with field-dependence-independence contributing most to the differentia- tion. The univariate analyses revealed that natural science majors tended toward field-independence, were more concerned with accuracy on the meas- ure of reflection-impulsivity, and tended to require less information to draw conclusions on the intuition measure. While it was expected that the strongest differentiation would be between natural science and humani- ties majors, differences between social science majors and the other two were more consistent. When the attitudes of extraversion and introver- sion were combined with the dominant functions, extraverted and intro- verted sensors were found to tend toward the natural sciences, extraverted intuiters to tend away from the natural sciences, and extraverted thinkers were found to tend toward the humanities. AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN THREE CATEGORIES OE COLLEGE MAJOR AND SELECTED MEASURES OF COGNITIVE STYLE BY John Thomas Deines A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Personnel Services and Educational Psychology 1974 To Beverly Jane and Sara Jane ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to recognize and to express my appreciation to the fol- lowing individuals. To Dr. Cecil Williams, dissertation director and practicum supervisor, whose concern led him to take the more difficult path in helping an introverted feeling type make explicit what his nature prefers to be left implicit. To Dr. Norman Kagan, chairman, for his guidance through the coursework. Participation in one of his Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR) workshops triggered a period of great growth in my personal and professional life. To Dr. Maryellen McSweeney, committee member, for her expertise and patience in assisting me with the statistical and design aspects of the present study. I would like to think that it was her understanding of cognitive style, whether explicit or implic- it, which made her one of the finest professors under whom I have had the privilege to learn. To Dr. Norman Abeles, committee member from the Department of Psychology, for his support over a period of three and one-half years and for the suggestions which greatly strengthened the de- sign of the present study. To my colleagues, Dr. Donald Bertsch, Dr. Jack Evans, and Dr. Charles Nicholas, who went out of their way to help a struggling doctoral candidate. To my friends, Dave Dreyer, Sara Patten, Jim Ribby, and Cheryl Moe, for their generous assistance in executing the present study. To my wife, Beverly, for her total support. She assisted in so many ways, small and large, for long and arduous hours to make the present work a reality. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter I. ImODmTION O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Background . . . . . . . Cognitive StYle Defined s s s s s s s s s s s s s s o PurpOSCOfCheSCUdYoseosssssssssssso Benefits of the Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Cognitive Styles and College Majors to be Investigated Secondary Purpose of the Present Study . . . . . . . . Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . SM‘ty O O O O O O O O O O O O I 0 O O O 0 0 O O O 0 II. REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE . . . . . . . . . . Cognitive Style Systems . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Cognitive Complexity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conceptual Systems . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Cognitive Controls . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Ipsative Analysis of Cognitive Functioning . . . . Sigel's Work on Conceptual Styles . . . . . . . . . Cognitive Style Systems Utilized in the Present Study Field-Dependence-Independence . . . . . . . . . . . RafleCtion‘ImpLIISiVIty s s s s s s s s s s s s s s Intuition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . JungianEunction8................. Other Research Relating Cognitive Style to Choice of Major...................o... c0nc1uaion8 O O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O C III. METHODOLOGY AND ANALYSIS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Design . . . sample..............‘.......... Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Instruments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Method of Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Iv. RESULTS 0 O O I O O I O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O TestonypotheslsI................. TestOnypOthesisIa.o.............. iv Page l" rar- ric>¢DG>UIUIh)h' 3—0 N 12 12 13 15 18 18 21 21 27 40 47 56 56 56 58 60 64 75 79 80 82 83 91 Chapter V. Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test Test of of of of of of of of of of of of of of of of of of of of of of Summary Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Summary . . . . . Conclusions . . . Discussion . Limitations . . . Implications for Future REFERENCES . Appendix A. B. C. D. lb lc ld le lf ls lh Ii 15 2 3 3a 3b 3c 3d 3e 4 4a 4b 4c 4d 4e SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS we... 0 O I O C O C O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O . O O I O O C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O O O O I O O I esearch I O C C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR THE FIVE COGNITIVE STYLE MEASURES, GRADE POINT AVERAGE, AND AGE FOR SAMPLE 1 WITH AMERICAN COLLEGE TEST COMPOSITE SCORE . . . . . . . . . . MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR THE FIVE COGNITIVE STYLE MEASURES, GRADE POINT AVERAGE, AND AGE FOR SAMPLE 1 WITHOUT AMERICAN COLLEGE TEST COMPOSITE SCORE . . . . . . MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR THE FIVE COGNITIVE STYLE MEASURES, GRADE POINT AVERAGE, AND AGE FOR SAMPLE 2 WITH AMERICAN COLLEGE TEST COMPOSITE SCORE . . . . . . . . . . MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR THE FIVE COGNITIVE STYLE MEASURES, GRADE POINT AVERAGE, AND AGE FOR SAMPLE 2 WITHOUT AMERICAN COLLEGE TEST COMPOSITE SCORE . . . . . . Page 93 94 95 96 98 99 101 102 104 105 105 108 109 111 111 111 112 112 115 115 115 116 116 119 119 121 121 122 123 125 130 134 137 141 Appendix Page E. MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR THE FIVE COGNITIVE STYLE MEASURES, GRADE POINT AVERAGE, WITH AND WITHOUT AMERICAN COLLEGE TEST COMPOSITE SCORE BETWEEN DOMINANT JUNGIAN TYPES IN SAMPLE 1 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 144 F. MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS FOR THE FIVE COGNITIVE STYLE MEASURES, GRADE POINT AVERAGE, WITH AND WITHOUT AMERICAN COLLEGE TEST COMPOSITE SCORE BETWEEN DOMINANT JUNGIAN TYPES IN SAMPLE 2 O O O C O O O O C O O O O O O O O I O O 147 6. RA" DATA 0 O C O O O C C I O O O I O O C O O O C O I O O O O 150 H. INTERCORRELATION BETWEEN THE FIVE COGNITIVE STYLE MEASURES AND GRADE POINT AVERAGE WITH AND WITHOUT AMERICAN COLLEGE TEST COMPOSITE SCORE IN SAMPLE 1 AND SAMPLE 2 . . . . . . 157 I. THE MATCHING FAMILIAR FIGURES TEST (MFF): INSTRUCTIONS, SCORE SHEET, AND SAMPLE ITEMS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 162 J. THE WESTCOTT INTUITION TEST (WIT): INSTRUCTIONS, TEST WITH CLUES UNDEVELOPED, TEST WITH CLUES DEVELOPED . . . . 168 vi LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Composition of Sample 1 and Sample 2 by Sex and Number in 830!) Major Area and SpCCifiC Major s s s s s s s s s s s s s 62 2. Test of Hypothesis 1: Multivariate Analysis of Variance for Mean Differences on Five Cognitive Style Measures, Grade Point Average, and American College Test Composite Score among Three Groups of College Major and between the Sexes . 8a 3. Test of Hypothesis 1: Step-down F Test for Ability of Five Cognitive Style Measures, American College Test Composite Score, and Grade Point Average to Discriminate between theSexes.........................86 A. Test of Hypothesis 1: Step-down F Test for Ability of Five Cognitive Style Measures, American College Test Composite Score, and Grade Point Average to Discriminate among Three Categories of College Major in Sample 1 . . . . . . . 87 5. Mean Grade Point Average and Mean American College Test Composite Score between the Sexes . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 6. Mean Grade Point Average and Mean American College Test Composite Score among Three Categories of College Major . . 88 7. Analyses of Variance for Differences between the Sexes and among Three Categories of College Major in Grade Point Average and American College Test Composite Score . . . . . 90 8. Mean Scores on the Group Embedded Figures Test for Three Categories of College Major . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91 9. Test of Hypotheses la and lb: Analyses of Variance for Differences among Three Categories of College Major and between Sexes on the Group Embedded Figures Test . . . . . . 92 10. Confidence Intervals Based on Tukey Post Hoc Comparisons for Hypothesis la: Differences among Three Categories of College Major on the Group Embedded Figures Test . . . . . . 93 ll. Mean Scores on the Group Embedded Figures Test for the Two sexe 8 O O 0 O 0 O O O I O O O 0 O O O C O O O C O O O O O O 94 vii Table Page 12. Mean Time Scores (in seconds) on the Matching Familiar Figures Test for Three Categories of College Major . . . . 94 13. Test of Hypotheses 1c and 1d: Analyses of Variance for Differences among Three Categories of College Major and between the Sexes on the Time Score of the Matching Familiar Figures Test . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95 14. Mean Time Scores (in seconds) on the Matching Familiar Figure. TC.t for the Two SCX98 e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 96 15. Mean Error Scores on the Matching Familiar Figures Test for Three Categories of College Major . . . . . . . . . . 96 16. Test of Hypotheses 1e and 1f: Analyses of Variance Differences among Three Categories of College Major and between the Sexes on the Error Score of the Matching F.-111‘r Figure. Te.t e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 97 17. Confidence Intervals Based on Tukey Post Hoc Comparisons for Hypothesis 1c: Differences among Three Categories of College Major on the Error Score of the Matching Familiar Figurcl T..t e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 98 18. Mean Error Scores on the Matching Familiar Figures Test for th. TWO SQXCB e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 99 19. Mean Number Correct Scores on the Westcott Intuition Test for Three Categories of College Major . . . . . . . . . . 100 20. Test of Hypotheses 1g and 1h: Analyses of Variance for Differences among Three Categories of College Major and between the Sexes on the Number Correct Score of the u..tc0tt IHCULLLOB TQ.‘ e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 101 21. Mean Number Correct Scores on the Hestcott Intuition Test for the Two Sexes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101 22. Mean Clue Use Scores on the Hestcott Intuition Test for THICC Cttegariel at College “‘10: e e e e e e e e e e e e 102 23. Test of Hypotheses 1i and lj: Analyses of variance for Differences among Three Categories of College Major and between the Sexes on the Clue Use Score of the Hestcott Intuition re't e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 103 24. Confidence Intervals Based on Tukey Post Hoc Comparisons for Hypothesis 1i: Differences among Three Categories of College Major on the Clue use Score of the Hestcott Intuition Test in Sample 1 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 104 viii Table Page 25. Mean Clue Use Scores on the Hestcott Intuition Test for th. TWO 5.3.8 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 104 26. Test of HypOthesis 2: Distribution of Dominant Jungian Functions through Three Categories of College Major . . . 106 27. Extension of the Test of Hypothesis 2: Distribution of Jungian Types (Attitude with Dominant FUnction) through Three Categories of College Major for Sample 1 and 2 Calbined.........................107 28. Test of Hypothesis 3: Multivariate Analysis of Variance for Mean Differences on Five Cognitive Style Measures, Grade Point Average, and American College Test Composite Score between Dominant Sensors and Dominant Intuiters . . 108 29. Mean Scores on Five Cognitive Style Measures, Grade Point Average, and American College Test Composite Score Obtained by Dominant Sensors and Dominant Intuiters . . . 109 30. Test of Hypotheses 3a, 3b, 3c, 3d, and 3e: Analyses of Variance for Differences between Dominant Sensors and Dominant Intuiters on Five Cognitive Style Measures, American College Test Composite Score, and Grade Point AVCERSQeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeee110 31. Test of Hypothesis 4: Multivariate Analysis of Variance for Mean Differences on Five Cognitive Style Measures, Grade Point Average, and American College Test Composite Score between Dominant Thinkers and Dominant Peelers . . . 112 32. Mean Scores on Five Cognitive Style Measures, Grade Point Average, and American College Test Composite Score Obtained by Dominant Thinkers and Dominant Peelers . . . . 113 33. Test of Hypotheses ha, hb, Ac, 4d, and be: Analyses of variance for Differences between Dominant Thinkers and Dominant Peelers on Five Cognitive Style Measures, American College Test Composite Score, and Grade Point Am.8.eeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeee114 3a. Summary of Results on Multivariate Hypotheses and Univariate Subhypotheses concerning Cognitive Style and Major . . . . 117 A. Means and Standard Deviations for the Five Cognitive Style Measures, Grade Point Average, and Age for Sample 1 with American College Test Composite Score A1 Sample 1 Cell Size by Sex and Major . . . . . . . . . 130 A2 Group Embedded Figures Test . . . . . . . . . . . . . 130 A3 Matching Familiar Figures Time . . . . . . . . . . . . 131 ix Table Page A4 Matching Familiar Figures Errors . . . . . . . . . . . 131 AS Hestcott Intuition Test Correct . . . . . . . . . . . 131 A6 Hestcott Intuition Test Clue Use . . . . . . . . . . . 132 A7 American College Test Composite Score . . . . . . . . 132 A8 Grade Point Average . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 132 A9 Age . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 133 8. Means and Standard Deviations for the Five Cognitive Style Measures, Grade Point Average, and Age for Sample 1 Without American College Test Composite Score Bl Sample 1 Cell Size by Sex and Major . . . . . . . . . 134 32 Group Embedded Figures Test ... . . . . . . . . . . . 134 B3 Matching Familiar Figures Time . . . . . . . . . . . . 135 36 Matching Familiar Figures Errors . . . . . . . . . . 135 BS Hestcott Intuition Test Correct . . . . . . . . . . 135 O O O O O O O O 0 C 136 O O O O O O O O O O 136 86 Westcott Intuition Test Clue Use B7 Grade Point Average . . . . . . C. Means and Standard Deviations for the Five Cognitive Style Measures, Grade Point Average, and Age for Sample 2 with American College Test Composite Score Cl ~Sample 2 Cell Size by Sex and Major . . . . . . . . . 137 C2 Group Embedded Figures Test . . . . . . . . . . . . . 137 C3 Matching Familiar Figures Time . . . . . . . . . . . . 138 Ch Matching Familiar Figures Errors . . . . . . . . . . . 138 CS Hestcott Intuition Test Correct . . . . . . . . . . . 138 C6 weatCOtt Intuition TCSt Clue Use a e s e e e e e e e e 139 C7 American College Test Composite Score . . . . . . . . 139 C8 Grade POint Average e e s s e e e s s s e e s e e e s 139 C9 ‘88 s e s s e e s e e e e e e e e e e e e s e e e s e 150 D. Means and Standard Deviations for the Five Cognitive Style Measures, Grade Point Average,and Age for Sample 2 Without American College Test Composite Score Dl Sample 2 Cell Size by Sex and Major . . . . . . . . . 141 DZ Group Embedded Figures Test . . . . . . . . . . . . . 141 D3 Matching Familiar Figures Time . . . . . . . . . . . . 142 D“ MatChing FaniliCr Figures Errors s e s e e e e s s e e 142 D5 Hestcott Intuition Test Correct . . . . . . . . . . . 142 D6 Westcott Intuition Test Clue Use . . . . . . . . . . . 143 D7 Grade Point Average . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 143 D8 Age e s e e s s s e e e s e e e s e e e e e e s e e e 143 E. Means and Standard Deviations for the Five Cognitive Style Measures, Grade Point Average, with and without American College Test Composite Score between Dominant Jungian Types in Sample 1 Table E1 32 Page Means and Standard Deviations between Dominant Sensors and Dominant Intuiters . . . . . . . . . . . . 1&5 Means and Standard Deviations between Dominant Thinkers and Dominant Feelers . . . . . . . . . . . . 146 F. Means and Standard Deviations for the Five Cognitive Style Measures, Grade Point Average, with and without American College Test Composite Score between Dominant Jungian Types in Sample 2 F1 F2 Means and Standard Deviations between Dominant Sensors and Dominant Intuiters . . . . . . . . . . . . 148 Means and Standard Deviations between Dominant Thinkers and Dominant Feelers . . . . . . . . . . . . 149 H. Intercorrelation between the Five Cognitive Style Measures and Grade Point Average with and without American College Test Composite Score in Sample 1 and Sample 2 H1 82 H3 H4 Intercorrelations for Sample 1 without American College COIIQBQ 1.3.: cupOU1te Scat. e e s e e e e e e s e e e 158 Intercorrelations for Sample 1 with American College T..tcup°.it.scor.sseessaseeseeeees159 Intercorrelations for Sample 2 without American COIIESCT38tcaW.1t. seat. 0 e e s s e s s e e e e s 160 Intercorrelations for Sample 2 with American College TC'CCQPOOItCscorQeseseesasseessees161 xi LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page I. Mean grade point average for three categories of college ”1“ O O O O 0 C O O O O O 0 O O O O O I O O O O O O O O 88 2. Mean American College Test composite score for three categories of college major . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 3. Mean scores on the Group Embedded Figures Test for three C‘tfisarie. 0f 0011.89 “‘10: e e e s s e e e e e e e e e e 92 a. Mean time scores (in seconds) on the Matching Familiar Figures Test for three categories of college major . . . . 9S 5. Mean error scores on the Matching Familiar Figures Test for three C‘t.80r1.. 0f COIIQBC .‘jar e e e e e e e e e s 97 6. Mean number correct scores on the Hestcott Intuition Test for three categories of college major . . . . . . . . . . 100 7. Mean clue use scores on the‘Hestcott Intuition Test for three categories of college major . . . . . . . . . . . . 103 xii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Background The position that the campus counseling psychologist deals with the developmental needs of students as opposed to remedial needs has long been emphasized (Morrill, Ivey, & Getting, 1968: Getting, 1967). Many universities have reorganized their student services offerings in a manner such that the divisions frequently are clustered under a title such as ”Student Development Services." In such organizations, remedial needs continue to be met by existing units and personnel but the concept is broadened to include some assistance in the process of living effec- tively while obtaining a college education. Brown (1971) states ...that student development staffs must have input into and involvement with the academic dimensions of the collegiate experience. This could well be one of their most important functions in the future. However, it is not likely to be a function that is going to be handed them freely, nor is it a function they can usurp. It will be one that they will have to earn by possessing clearly defined skills and concepts (p. 48). Sperry (1973), in a more recent but similar appeal, writes The need to understand individual differences-- especially learning styles--is emerging as the number one priority for the teacher and the counselor (p. 483). He also reported that researchers, in their efforts to improve 1 instruction, have discovered the importance of the context in which learning takes place, particularly the interaction between the learner and his environment-~a book, programmed unit, teacher, or counselor. An important component of the interaction is the mode by which the learner learns and how assistance is provided to him. The goal of the present study is to explore some of the dimen- sions related to and resulting from the cognitive development of students--cognitive or learning style. This effort is made to provide the counseling psychologist with additional understandings for assisting the student to achieve maximum development during the college years. The counseling psychologist thus will have more concepts and skills with which to augment student development both when dealing with the student directly and when acting as consultant to other university officials who will in turn deal directly with the student. Cognitive Style Defined The term "cognitive style" or "learning style" is not one that has a precise definition. While most terms in psychology are subject to interpretation, cognitive style ranks on the lower end of the less- precise-to-more-precise-definition continuum. That consensus as to a precise and exact definition is lacking is due in part to a lack of a single unifying theory which underlies the phenomenon, though the work of proponents of a specific system may be theory inspired, e.g”.the cognitive controls of the Menninger group are rooted in psychoanalytic theory. Kagan and Hogan (1970) say that the research on cognitive styles and controls has been conducted within a broad theoretical base while Spitler (1971) says that cognitive styles are more empirical observations of researchers than deductions from a theoretical system. Ziegler (1963) criticizes the work of Hitkin, one of the foremost researchers in this area, on the grounds that it has no theory to which his work can be linked. From a systematic review of the literature, there seem to be three groups of studies to which the label ”cognitive style” has been attached. The first most obvious group (Category I) includes those stud- ies which employ measures labeled as estimates or dimensions of cognitive style. In this category belong the work of Hitkin (Hitkin, Dyk, Faterson, Goodenough, & Karp, 1962: Hitkin, Lewis, Hergzman, Machover, Meissner, & Hapner, 1956) on fie1d-dependence-independence, the work of Hagan (Hagan, Rosman, Day, Albert, a Philips, 1964) on conceptual tempo, the work by a group of researchers (Gardner, Holzman, Klein, Linton, s Spence, 1959) at the Menninger Foundation on cognitive controls, the work of Dieri (Bieri, Atkins, Drier, Leaman, Miller, a Tripoldi, 1966) on cogni- tive complexity, and the work of Droverman (1960a, 1960b) on ipsative analysis of cognitive functioning. The second classification (Category II) includes those phenomena which operationally use instrumentation similar to the cognitive style researchers referred to above but which do not employ the actual term ”cognitive style.” Some of these researchers published their work be- fore the ”cognitive style" label became popular. Such is the case with the work of Thurstone (1966) and Mooney (1956) in their research on the various aspects of closure. Other researchers chose not to use the term. Such is the case with the work of Hestcott (1968b). The view of the present author, which will be presented in Chapter III, is that Hestcott's operational definition of intuition is in fact an operational definition of cognitive style. Fengel (1971) used the Hestcott measure and referred to it as a measure of ”cognitive-affective style.“ The present author is unable to explain why the term ”affective” was added. No other source has equated cognition with effect, nor does Pengel ex- plain herself on this point. The third classification (Category III) includes as measures of cognitive style some phenomena more readily identified as personality variables. Messick (1972) says that cognitive styles are frequently implicated in personality and social functioning. As an example of this, Johansson (1971) refers to Holland's (1966) six personality types as cognitive styles. Cohen, Johnson, and Hanson (1971) define cognitive style as basic level of intentionality. Snyder (1966) used the Sensa- tion and Intuition scores of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (Myers, 1962) to assess students' cognitive ”horizons.” Similarly, Child (19651 in a study of esthetic judgment in college students, employed the Myers- Driggs Type Indicator as a measure of cognitive style along with mea- sures of the cognitive controls of the Menninger group, field-dependence, and others. With this categorisation in mind, it might be well to review some definitions of cognitive style. De Ceeco (1968) uses this definition: Learning styles are personal ways in which individuals process information in the course of learning new concepts and principles (p. 75). He also points out in common with others that these modes of processing are considered to be relatively free of content and to cut across dif- forest types of information. Kagan and Kogan (1970) say cognitive style is an approach to cognition and cognitive processes and then define those terms as follows: ...cognition stands for those hypothetical psycho- logical processes invoked to explain overt verbal and metor behavior as well as certain physiological reactions. Cognitive process is a superordinate term, subsuming the more familiar titles of imagery, perception, free association, thought, mediation, proliferation of hypotheses, reasoning, reflection and problem solving (p. 1275). 1 Child (1965) operationally defines cognitive style by saying People differ greatly one from another in their orientation toward various aspects of experience and the label cognitive style has come to be applied to such variations (p. 483). As has been stated, it is the aim of the present study to pro- vide the counseling psychologist with information which can be utilized primarily in an instructional setting. With this end in mind, De Cecco's definition seems most appropriate for the present study since it ex- presses specifically what the other two imply. Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study is to investigate the relationship of cognitive style to college major in the belief that knowledge of cogni- tive style will facilitate the role of the counseling psychologist as consultant to students and faculty. Benefits of the Study Vocational theorists, among others, have pointed out that dif- ferent characteristics are required for success in different occupations (Super, 1953: Holland, 1966). Choice of major is closely related to choice of occupation in that choice of major frequently leads to choice of occupation and in that a similar process is involved in choice of either. An understanding of the relation of cognitive style to college major can provide the counseling psychologist with an additional tool for assisting students in their search for the best major for them. The input of one more characteristic relevant to the choice of major can in- crease the accuracy of prediction formulae. Brown (1971) has emphasized that it is important for the student personnel worker to become more involved in the academic dimensions of the collegiate experience, e.g. , as consultant to faculty. Knowledge that cognitive style is related to college major can be used to assist faculty in grasping its implications for instruction. That is, if faculty are aware that a significant nuber of their students utilize a particular learning style, this information can be put to use in their instructional strategy. Messick (1972) elaborates on the applicability of cognitive style for instruction by suggesting that measures of cognitive style could provide a more effective characterisation of the student than achievement measures alone. In this regard he says, These stylistic characteristics should have relevance, although direct research evidence is admittedly very scanty, not only for the course of individual learn- ing in various subject matter areas, but also for the nature of teacher-pupil interactions and of social behavior in the classroom, the family, and the peer stone (9- 110). Other possibilities outlined by Messick include using cognitive style as the basis for placement of students in classes, either bomogenously or in special mixes, and matching students to faculty. Mode of instruction can also be geared to cognitive style. For instance, at least in science, instruction via an inductive method of teaching (i.e., having the student discover the principles to be learned) or through a more direct approach (i.e., one in which principles are specified) can be related to cognitive style. The field-independent (Hitkin et al., 1962; Witkin et al., 1954) and possibly reflective students (Kagan et al., 1964) may well learn more efficiently with the inductive approach, while field-dependent and impulsive students may learn more efficiently with the more direct approach. Messick tempers all his proposals by saying that more empirical evidence concerning cognitive styles is needed. It is the aim of the present research to further knowledge in this area by focusing on the relation of cognitive style to college major. Awareness of the range and variation of clients' cognitive styles can be beneficial to the counseling psychologist in dealing directly with students, individually, in groups, and in outreach pro- grams. Carkhuff (1969) says that, while a great deal of emphasis has been placed on the style which counselors bring to sessions with clients, it is equally important to consider the client's style. A very important component of ”style" is cognitive style. Knowledge of client style including cognitive style can be utilized in one of two ways. Either the counselor can gear his style to the client's style or he can refer the client to another counselor whose style is more in harmony with that of the client. In summary, a knowledge of cognitive style provides a very use- ful conceptual system from which the counseling psychologist can draw in his role as consultant to faculty and administration for student development, or in providing services to students directly through in- dividual, group, or outreach programs. Also since nearly all concep- tual systems make some assumptions about cognitive style (although many are not very explicit) in a university setting where the cognitive do- main is so emphasized, it seems especially important that such concep- tualizing be made explicit. Cognitive Styles and College ngors to be Investigated Four cognitive styles were selected for use in this study. They are field-dependence-independence (Hitkin et al., 1962; Witkin et al., 1954), reflection-impulsivity (Kagan et al., 1964), intuition (Westcott, l968bL and the four Jungian (Jung, 1971) functions of sensa- tion, intuition, thinking, and feeling. Sperry (1973) noted that there were three cognitive styles which had received most attention and were therefore regarded as most important--learning modality, learning tempo, and learning differentia- tion. This statement provided the rationale for inclusion of learning tempo (reflection-impulsivity) and learning differentiation (field- dependence-independence). Learning modality refers to the sense through which the student best learns. Since it seemed to have minimal rele- vance for higher education, it was not included in the present study. The Jungian functions were included in the study because they were of particular interest to the present author, and provide a way of relating cognitive "style" into a broader theoretical framework. The instrument which currently is most widely used to assess Jungian typology, the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) (Myers, 1962) enjoys wide popularity in colleges and universities throughout the country, and research has been conducted with it relevant to choice of occupation and college major. The four functions are very similar to cognitive controls and styles (Child, 1965). The choice of intuition as a measure for this study was made for two reasons. First, Westcott's (lafinfl measure for intuition yielded scores for success in solving, in an inductive manner, abstraction prob- lems and the amount of information demanded in seeking that solution. The variation in performance on the task seems to provide most useful information in keeping with the purposes of the study outlined above. Second, the present author hoped to provide more data on the relation of Westcott's measure to the Jungian notion of intuition. College major will be categorized in the present study as majors in the humanities, social sciences, and natural sciences. The inspira- tion for this classification is found in the work of Goldschmid (1967) who successfully was able to predict college major (humanities to natural science dimension) on the basis of personality tests. While Goldschmid used the humanities to natural science dimension, he felt that the social sciences should be included in future studies. Secondary Purpose of the Present Study A secondary purpose of the present study proposes to correlate the four Jungian functions to measures of cognitive style. It builds in part on the work of Stanfiel (1966). He tested the hypothesis that field-independence was related to the two Jungian attitudes of extra- version-introversion. In two of three studies, he found support for that hypothesis but found no relation between the Jungian functions and field-dependence-independence. However, he did not take into account 10 Jung's concept of dominance of function, which says that one of the four functions is more important and more fully developed than the other three. The present author hypothesizes that a relationship will be found between dominant Jungian function and the cognitive styles which are the subject of the present study. Hypotheses From the discussion above it is now possible to state the hypotheses in the present study. 1. 3. 4. S. 6. 9. Students in different categories of college major will differ among themselves and between sexes on the dimension of field-dependence-independence. Students in different categories of college major will differ among themselves and between sexes on the dimension of reflection-impulsivity. Students in different categories of college major will differ among themselves and between sexes on the dimension of intuition. The proportion of subjects divided on the basis of dominant Jungian function will differ by category of college major. Differences will exist in performance on the dimension of field-dependence-independence between dominant Jungian sensors and intuiters. Differences will exist in performance on the dimension of fie1d-dependence-independence between dominant Jungian feelers and thinkers. Differences will exist in performance on the dimension of reflection-impulsivity between dominant Jungian sensors and intuiters. Differences will exist in performance on the dimension of reflection-impulsivity between dominant Jungian feelers ‘nd thinkers s Differences will exist in performance on the dimension of less to more intuition between dominant Jungian sensors and intuiters. ll 10. Differences will exist in performance on the dimension of less to more intuition between dominant Jungian thinkers and feelers. Summary In this chapter, the position was advanced that the role of the campus counseling psychologist may expand to that of consultant to faculty on academic dimensions of the collegiate experience. It was proposed that examining the relationship of cognitive style to college major would pro- vide the counseling psychologist with useful information in fulfilling that role and also in fulfilling his current role of assisting in student decision making. The problems associated with defining cognitive style were dis- cussed and the definition of personal ways people approach learning situations was adopted. Four conceptualizations of cognitive style-- all applicable to learning situations--were chosen for investigation in the present study: field-dependence-independence, reflection- impulsivity, intuition, and dominant Jungian typology. Secondarily, an investigation of differential performance on field-dependence- independence, reflection-impulsivity, and intuition among the dominant Jungian functions was proposed. ll 10. Differences will exist in performance on the dimension of less to more intuition between dominant Jungian thinkers and feelers. Summary In this chapter, the position was advanced that the role of the campus counseling psychologist may expand to that of consultant to faculty on academic dimensions of the collegiate experience. It was proposed that examining the relationship of cognitive style to college major would pro- vide the counseling psychologist with useful information in fulfilling that role and also in fulfilling his current role of assisting in student decision making. The problems associated with defining cognitive style were dis- cussed and the definition of personal ways people approach learning situations was adopted. Four conceptualizations of cognitive style-- all applicable to learning situations-~were chosen for investigation in the present study: field-dependence-independence, reflection- impulsivity, intuition, and dominant Jungian typology. Secondarily, an investigation of differential performance on field-dependence- independence, reflection-impulsivity, and intuition among the dominant Jungian functions was proposed. CHAPTER II REVIEH OF RELATED LITERATURE The plan of this chapter is to present first a brief descrip- tion of selected cognitive style systems which, though of theoretical interest, were not utilized in the present study. It portrays the process through which a neophyte student of cognitive style might proceed and which the present author used in arriving at the concepts utilised in the present study. Second, an overview of the cognitive style measures used in the present study will be presented along with research relevant to their relation to college major. Third, a review of those studies which relate other measures of cognitive style to college major will be presented. Finally, the implications of the findings for the present study will be discussed. Eggnitive style System; mum Complexity sieri (Bieri, 1971: nieri et al., 1966) is among those scholars who view cognitive style as a mental set with which individuals construe their environment. Bieri's work has focused on how highly differentiated or complex those mental functions are and has been limited to the social 12 13 domain. His definition of cognitive complexity is ...the capacity to construe social behavior in a multidimensional way. A more cognitively complex person has available a more differentiated system of dimensions for perceiving others' behavior than does a less cognitively complex individual (Bieri et al., 1966, p. 185). The construct is evaluated by having a subject list ten people with whom he is familiar. For these ten people he is to make ten ratings of each such as "outgoing,” ”calm," ”cheerful," etc., on a Likert-type scale from one to six. Those individuals who rate their subjects much in the same manner, e.g., use mostly S's or 6's, are said to be cogni- tively simple because they do not use much differentiation in their rating, while those who use more of the range are said to be cognitively complex. The work of Bieri was not chosen for the present study because it focused on a domain not as directly applicable to the learning envi- ronment as the selected measures. Conceptual Systems bieri's system represents an approach which is specific in con- ceptualization and application. 0n the other hand, the conceptual sys- tens approach of Harvey, Hunt, and Schroder (1961) adopts a more perva- sive view of cognitive functioning. The following statement by Schroder (1971) would lead one to conclude that he equates cognitive functioning withhpersonality. '...personality is viewed as the style a person uses in processing information about a given domain of stimuli, e.g., interpersonal, political, or religious .tm11 (Po 260) e 16 The earlier position of Harvey 23.21. (1961) does not eliminate psycho- physical and functional determinants, but their position is still in marked contrast to Bieri's. The Harvey gt 31. system concerns itself not only with differentiation but also with hierarchic integration which is considered a characteristic in the highest system of functioning. The systems are four in number and the basis for differentiating them is a concreteness-abstractness dimension with System I being charac- terized by concreteness and System IV being characterised by abstractness. While the description of characteristics of the four types is couched in esoteric language, the following is a brief summation of them. Each system is centered on a specific attribute which is said to be characteristic of that system. When a specific system is operative in an individual, events which are in harmony with it are said to be bolstered and those which are not are said to be neutralized. The central attribute in System I functioning is adherence to an external standard and is said to be characterized by the following be- haviors: forming standards quickly, rigidity, and overgeneralised sub- mission to authority. The central attribute in System 11 functioning is imposition of control and is said to be chsracterised by the following behaviors: Aggression against the source of the control, flight from a situation in which strong control is perceived, and taking an opposite stance to the source of control. The central attribute in System 111 functioning is mutuality and friendship and is said to be characterised by the following behaviors: self-evaluation based on others' opinions rather than one's own, sub- mission to influence in an overgeneralized fashion, and seeking support of others. System I and III functioning are said to be related and 14 The earlier position of Harvey 35 a}, (1961) does not eliminate psycho- physical and functional determinants, but their position is still in marked contrast to Bieri‘s. The Harvey‘s; 3;. system concerns itself not only with differentiation but also with hierarchic integration which is considered a characteristic in the highest system of functioning. The systems are four in number and the basis for differentiating them is a concreteness-abstractness dimension with System I being charac- terized by concreteness and System IV being characterized by abstractness. While the description of characteristics of the four types is couched in esoteric language, the following is a brief summation of them. Each system is centered on a specific attribute which is said to be characteristic of that system. When a specific system is operative in an individual, events which are in harmony with it are said to be bolstered and those which are not are said to be neutralized. The central attribute in System I functioning is adherence to an external standard and is said to be characterized by the following be- haviors: forming standards quickly, rigidity, and overgeneralized sub- mission to authority. The central attribute in System II functioning is imposition of control and is said to be characterised by the following behaviors: Aggression against the source of the control, flight from a situation in which strong control is perceived, and taking an opposite stance to the source of control. The central attribute in System 111 functioning is mutuality and friendship and is said to be characterised by the following behaviors: self-evaluation based on others' opinions rather than one's own, sub- mission to influence in an overgeneralized fashion, and seeking support of others. System I and III functioning are said to be related and 15 System III functioning resembles the functioning of field-dependents in Witkin's system. The central attribute in System IV functioning is autonomy and is said to be characterized by the following behaviors: less susceptibility to social pressure, more ability to see self nega- tively and take corrective action, and more exploratory behavior. Assessment in the conceptual systems approach is done with a sentence completion instrument which uses "This I believe" as its stem. Reactions are solicited on such areas as friendship, guilt, religion, and people. While the conceptual systems approach in theory sees cognition as all pervasive, its application is primarily to the realm of social behavior. Also there seems to be a good measure of overlap between functioning in the three systems, and their method of assessment seems not to tap system specific functioning directly. For these reasons the present author chose not to deal extensively with the conceptual systems approach. Cognitive Controls The system Gardner, Holzman, Klein, Linton, and Spence (1959) have‘developed is referred to as cognitive controls. In common with the conceptual systems approach, this system attempts to explain a wide range of human behavior, but utilizes several concepts as opposed to one in achieving that end. Also the two systems differ in theoretical origin. The conceptual systems approach, as well as that of Bieri, derives its basic premises from Lewin's ideas on differentiation while proponents of cognitive controls look to the neo-Freudians for their inspiration. They postulate the controls as mediating links between 16 drives and situational requirements and have identified the following six cognitive controls. The combination of them in an individual con- stitutes his cognitive style. 1) Leveling-sharpening refers to a characteristic of individuals to relate previous stimuli to present perceptions. The person who tends to see present stimuli as relevant to past perceptions is referred to as a leveler: the person who tends to see past perceptions as not relevant to present perceptions is termed a sharpener. The test usually used to assess this characteristic is the Schematizing Test developed by Holling- worth. It is a test in which the subject views a series of square figures of varying size, both in a presentation ordered by size and one in which the squares are presented in random fashion. 2) Scanning refers to the number of times a subject looks at a standard and a comparison object in a size estimation task. ~0ne test of scanning requires the subject to adjust a disc of light to the size of a disc in his hand. There are several discs which vary according to color and weight. The other test requires the subject to estimate size in circles which have been constructed to present optical illusions. 3) Field articulation is the same concept identified by Witkin as field-dcpendence-independence. Gardner includes Thurstone's Con- cealed Figures Test as a measure of this variable. A) The constricted-flexible control principle governs the manner in which a person handles conflicting or intrusive cues. The test used is modeled on Stroop's Color Word Test in which interference is created by requiring the subject to identify the color of the word "blue" when it is printed in red. Those whose performance on the test is relatively poor are referred to as constricted, and those who are relatively suc- cessful are referred to as flexible. 17 S) Equivalence range concerns itself with the manner in which an individual categorizes objects into broad or narrow categories or ranges. The characteristic is measured by the Object Sorting Test, the Photo Sorting Test, and two Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) cards. The first two tests require the subject to sort objects and photos into groups and then indicate the reasons he formed the groups he identified. 0n the TAT, subject's responses are scored on the conceptual distance the stories moved from physical features in the TAT cards. 6) Tolerance for unrealistic experiences refers to the ability of an individual to perceive something he knows to be unreal and accept it as such. Many individuals perceive an unrealistic experience and try to make it fit into the mold of previous perceptions. Optical illu- sions provide the means of assessing this characteristic. One is a situation in which two still photographs of a horse are progressively manipulated so that they give the appearance that the horse is moving. Subjects are assessed on this dimension by how long it takes them to recognize that what they are seeing is an optical illusion. Research utilizing cognitive control principles more frequently deals with topics of relevance to mental health than to instructional practice. This, coupled with the fact that Chung (1967) had utilized four of the control principles in a study similar to the present study, led the present author to utilize only the concept of field-dependence- independence which had its origins independent of cognitive control research. 18 Ipsative Analysis of Cognitive Functioning The work of Broverman (1960a, 1960b) stands alone among cogni- tive style researchers, not in conceptualization of specific styles but in a unique method of assessing an individual's cognitive style. Instead of using inter-individual differences to characterize an in- dividual on a cognitive style dimension, relative intra-individual differences are used. That is, the subject is given a battery of cog- nitive style measures and then scores relative to the mean score of all the tests are used to categorize the subject on a cognitive style dimen- sion. Factor analysis of a number of cognitive style measures analyzed using the intra-individual approach yielded a dimension Broverman refers to as strong or weak automatization. Kagan and Kogan (1970) point out that the measure strongly resembles Witkin's field-dependence-independence but that Broverman's method of analysis makes comparison difficult. It did not seem that Broverman's system was so rewarding that there would be sufficient gain from leaving the mainstream of cognitive style research to warrant such a move. It was for this reason that Broverman's system was not utilized in the present study. Sigel's Work on Conceptual Styles Of the four systems described thus far, Sigel's work is the most directly applicable to instructional settings. His work started with an interest in the manner in which children organize phenomena in the world around them. Originally (Sigel, 1953), he dichotomized that mode of organization into perceptual or conceptual. The child who used the perceptual mode formed concepts on the basis of sense data. The 19 developmentally more advanced child used the conceptual mode and con- sciously imposed an organization on the material into deliberately conceived categories. Kagan, Moss, and Sigel (1963) refined Sigel's (1953) formulation to include three categories~-relational, inferential-categorical, and analytic-descriptive. A conceptual category is said to be relational when the subject groups at least two objects on the basis of a perceived relationship. For instance, he may identify in a picture two adults and a child as family, or in a picture a horse and a wagon as a unit because they "go together." A conceptual category is said to be inferential-categorical when the subject groups objects and each object is an individual instance of the general category. That grouping is an inferred characteristic of what he perceives. For instance, in a picture of a cow and a horse, the two are identified as animals: a picture of a bed and a cradle are iden- tified asthings to sleep in. A conceptual category is said to be analytic-descriptive when the subject groups one part of the stimulus field common to at least two objects in it. His conclusion is based on his analysis of what he perceives. For instance, if in a picture of three people two had no shoes, he would say that two people had no shoes. Sigel, Jarman, and Hanesian (1967) renamed the analytic-descriptive category analytic part- whole to which the term descriptive-global was contrasted. A conceptual category was said to be descriptive-global when the subject groups at least two entire parts of the stimulus field. For instance, in a pic- ture of three people in which two are uniformed and one not uniformed, the subject may identify the uniformed people as such. 20 Sigel's instrument is the Conceptual Styles in Categorizing Behavior Task (SCST). In it three line drawings are presented to the subject who is asked to tell what is common in the drawings. They are so constructed that any of the conceptual categories can be identified. The conceptual categories which the subject identifies determine his style. Those subjects who use more analytic concepts are said to be more advanced. The work of Sigel and his associates serves as a transition between the systems just discussed and some systems used in the present study. Kagan £5 31. (1964) used the Conceptual Styles Test (CST) in the first of their series of studies. As Kagan's work progressed, he found that those who formed analytic concepts were more reflective, that is, they took longer to make a response. This began his work on reflection-impulsivity, utilized in the present study. Also Kagan ggngl. (1963) say they believe that there is a relation of the tendency to form analytic concepts to what Witkin terms field-independence. To acquaint the reader with specific systems to which the label "cognitive style" has been attached, five systems have been briefly described. While they all deal with the manner in which people construe the world they perceive, the difference among them seems to be the area of application. The systems of Bieri, and Harvey .ggigl. have their application primarily in the world of social inter- actions, the work of Gardner g£_§l, to mental health applications, and the work of Sigel to educational instruction, the focus of the present work. Broverman's work presents a unique way of assessing an individual's cognitive style. 21 In the next section the cognitive styles chosen for the present study will be discussed. First, an overview of each style will be presented to acquaint the reader with the system. Following this discussion, research linking that system to college major will be reviewed. Cognitive Style Systems Utilized in the Present Study Field-Dependence-Independence Overview Nitkin's work (Witkin et al., 1962; Witkin et al., 1954) started nearly thirty years ago with an interest in perception. A subject was placed in a perceptually embedding context, for instance, in a tilted room which contained a chair which could also be tilted. The subject was to orient himself upright. Originally Witkin termed his construct "perceptual field- dependence-independence." As research evidence built, he found that intellectual as well as perceptual functioning was involved and so the term "global-analytical" was applied. Further research which uncovered dimensions of personality functioning such as body concept led to the term "global vs. articulated" functioning. The current conceptualiza- tion is that of "psychological differentiation." While the Witkin terms have a specific historical significance, they are used inter- changeably in the literature and that practice will be continued in the present review. Differentiation, a concept also utilized in biology, is related to the degree of complexity of an organism. In psychology it is found in the theory of Lewin and underlies other cognitive style systems as 22 well as Witkin's. Under this concept, that organism is considered to be less developed which is less complex. When the concept is applied to human growth, the infant is seen as less differentiated from his environment than the older youth who experiences his body as separate from the environment and develops a sense of identity. Witkin's theory postulates that the level of a person's develop- ment is equal to the level of his differentiation. Level of differen- tiation is assessed by using one of three measures. The first is the Body Adjustment Test (BAT) in which the subject is placed in a tilted chair in a tilted room and adjusts his chair until he perceives he is in an upright position. The second is the Rod and Frame Test (RFT). In this test the subject is placed in a totally darkened room with a luminous frame which encloses a luminous rod. Both the rod and the frame are adjustable. The experimenter varies the tilt of the frame and the rod, and the subject then adjusts the rod to what he believes is the upright position. The third measure is the Embedded Figures Test (EFT). In this test the subject is to identify a geometric figure which has been em- bedded in a maze of geometric designs. Kagan and Kogan (1970) have reviewed the literature on field- dependence-independence extensively. They conclude that in Hestern cultures the EFT, EFT, and BAT all measure the same phenomena. There is evidence that in Nigerian culture these may not be valid measures and, therefore, possibly also in other non-Western cultures. The position of a person on the continuum from field-dependence to field- independence is relatively stable over time, although as children develop, they become progressively more field-independent. Evidence 23 that geriatric groups are more field-dependent suggests that perhaps there is a peaking of this characteristic in mid years and then a decline, the peak possibly occurring in the late thirties (Witkin, Oltman, Raskin, & Karp, l97l). Perhaps the area of greatest controversy concerning the work of the Witkin group centers on the relation of analytic vs. global function- ing to general intelligence and verbal ability. Witkin gt 21. (1962) point to research which links performance on the three measures of field- dependence-independence and performance on a cluster isolated through factor analysis--the object assembly, picture completion, and block design portions of the Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC). These three WISC subtests are believed to measure the ability to over- come an embedding context as do the measures of field-dependence- independence. Since there were two other verbally oriented clusters of WISC subtests isolated which did not require the ability to overcome an embedding context, Witkin's group concludes that verbal ability is not relevant to the dimension of psychological differentiation. Ziegler (1963) has contended that the Witkin group, in arriving at this conclusion, has ignored research which suggests that there may be a link (a general intelligence factor) between verbal ability and analytic-global functioning. Indeed, Kagan and Regan (1970) cite stud- ies which have found significant correlations between verbal ability and analytic-global functioning. They conclude that the relationship between verbal ability and analytic-global functioning is not substantial since only a few researchers have found it, and there is more evidence to the contrary. Analytic-global functioning has been related to other concepts involving perception. Such is the case with speed of closure and 24 flexibility of closure identified through factor analysis by Thurstone (1944). witkin 35 21° (1962) discusses the relationship at length and points to research which strongly links overcoming an embedding context to flexibility of closure but not speed of closure. The work of Mooney (1954) on closure seems to involve both speed and flexibility of closure and it, therefore, is related in part to Witkin's work. Witkin 35 21. (1962) comments on the apparent duplication of effort. In a period of extensive research on cognitive styles it is not surprising that there should be overlap or even identity among the cognitive styles established by different investigators. There is clearly a need for studies aimed at codifying these cognitive styles (p. 80). To the best of the present author's knowledge, no one has seriously undertaken what Witkin has recommended. There is evidence that field-independent individuals produce more sophisticated line drawings of human figures than field-dependent persons. witkin makes the inference from these data that field- independent persons have a more differentiated body concept. The personality variable of activity-passivity has been posited by the Witkin group as relating to field-dependence-independence. In only one study, which investigated posture of ten year old boys, did the hypothesized relationship receive confirmation. Also there has been evidence (using projective tests) to show that field-independent sub- jects used more specialized defenses such as isolation and intellectual- ization as opposed to repression and denial used by field-dependent sub- jects. Field-dependent subjects also have more uncontrolled aggression. Socially, field-dependents have been found to exhibit more other directed (passive-dependent) behaviors while field-independents have been found to be less influenced by their social environments. 25 Witkin cites strong correlational evidence that field-dependent mothers foster field-dependence in their sons, but other research in this area has been confusing as to the influence of parents in fostering the analytic-global function in their children. Except in four to eight year old groups and geriatric popula- tions, males have been found to be significantly more field-independent than females, though the difference is relatively small. However, in some college papulations, males and females have scored the same. The determinants of this observed difference have yet to be discovered with certainty, but cultural influences as well as spatial ability seem to be involved. Further evidence for the validity of the global vs. articulated dimension has been collected in cross-cultural studies. Temne tribes in Sierra Leone, Africa, and Canadian Eskimos were subjects. The Temne have strict child-rearing practices while the Eskimos allow their women and children more autonomy. Also, the Eskimos live in a bleak and barren environment which requires a greater degree of field articulation. Since field-independence is associated with more moderate child-rearing prac- tices, as well as greater need for field articulation, one would expect the Eskimos to be more field-independent than the Temne. Research evidence supports this. Some of Hitkin's earlier statements imply that it was more desirable to be field-independent. Kagan and Kogan (1970) challenge this notion saying that in today's climate the need may well be as great for those who are socially more accommodating (field-dependents) as for those who are able to resist the influence of others (field- independents). Witkin ggflgl. (1971) have clarified their position which is now in basic harmony with that of Kagan and Kogan. 26 Field-dependence-independence is perhaps the most thoroughly researched of all the cognitive style systems. In this section it has been shown that the dimension has been effective in differentiating a wide range of phenomena from an individual's perception of his body image to differentiating cultures from one another. Against this back- ground of research evidence, the argument of Ziegler (1963) that the dimension lacks a foothold in an established theory and may be no more than a factor of general intelligence seems pale. 0f the dimensions chosen for the present study, field-dependence-independence has been shown to be the most successful in differentiating college majors. That evidence is presented in the next section. Research Relevant to Field-Dependence-Independence agg;§ollege Major De Russy and Futch (1971) investigated the relation of field-dependence- independence to a humanities or natural science orientation. They ad- ministered the Embedded Figures Test to thirty-two subjects~-eight males and eight females in the natural sciences, eight males and eight females in the humanities. They found males were significantly more field- independent than females and that science students were significantly more field-independent than humanities students. There were no inter- action effects. The authors hypothesized that training in geometry might explain why science students were more field-independent. Chung (1966), in a study to be reported in more detail later in this chapter, found that a group measure of field-dependence- independence discriminated science from humanities majors much in the manner as in the De Russy and Futch (1971) study. 27 Barrett and Thornton (1967) tested the hypothesis that engineers were more field-independent than the general population. They adminis- tered the Rod and Frame Test to forty-six male engineers and technicians employed by a midwest aerospace firm and to eleven non-engineering male students. The engineers and technicians were more field-independent than either the eleven college students or Witkin's standardization sample. There was no significant difference between Hitkin's standard- ization sample and the college students. Reflection-Impulsivity Overview The dimension of reflection-impulsivity is concerned with the length of time subjects reflect on the solution to a problem where the solution is not absolutely clear. As has been pointed out, its development was an outgrowth of the work of Jenna Kagan (Kagan, et al., 1963; Kagan et al., l96h) on the analytic attitude in children. Most subsequent research has focused on children. Since most of the research has focused on children, it might be questioned why it is being used with an adult population. The rationale for its use is that the variation which is found in children is also found in adults (Burgbacher, 1973: Gatewood, 1972). Specifi- cally in reference to the present study, it is also reasonable to be- lieve that science majors who are oriented to quantitative precision would tend toward a more reflective attitude than social science and humanities majors who'are oriented to a more verbal and less quantita- tively oriented methodology. Reflection-impulsivity is usually measured using the Hatching Familiar Figures Test (MFF), a test in which the subject is presented 28 a standard line drawing and eight variants. He is to identify the vari- ant which duplicates the standard. Performance is measured by time to first response and errors made before identifying the correct variant. The research on this dimension is much less extensive than that on the dimension of field-dependence-independence. This is particularly true in the area of personality variables such as self-concept, etc. Kagan and Regan (1970) cite the following research relevant to the dimension. 7 1) Even for young children the dimension of reflection- impulsivity has been found to be reliable. In three studies the lowest test-retest correlation for the response time score was .31 (over 2% years) and the highest .70 (over ten weeks). 2) Subjects have been found to be impulsive or reflective on other tasks from similar perceptual tasks to an interview situation. In the interview situation, the time a subject took to respond to a question of a general nature was recorded and correlated with response time on the HEP. The Pearson product moment correlation was .31. 3) Reflective children have been found to recall words in a serial recall list better than impulsive children under conditions of both low and high anxiety. h) Reflective first grade children are better able to recognize spoken words than impulsive children. These same children served as subjects in a study one year later and the reflective children made fewer errors in reading a paragraph of English prose than did the imp pulsive children. Reflective children made fewer errors on tasks of inductive reasoning (e.g., responding to a question such as what barks, has fur, and walks on four legs?) than did impulsive children. 29 5) Reflective children made more eye movements in the first six seconds when they compared a standard with a variant than impulsive chil- dren. When the standard and variants were put behind frosted glass and it was necessary for each child to remove the glass, reflective children removed more of the glasses and examined the variants more carefully. 6) The characteristic of reflection-impulsivity can be modified. Four short-term experimental studies are cited in which the characteristic was modified in some way (e.g., in time to first response, but not errors) through direct training or modeling. In a fifth study, first grade teachers and students were categorized as reflective or impulsive thinkers at the beginning of the school year and at the end. Students did change in the direction of the tempo of their teachers. Therefore, evidence thus far presented has suggested that the characteristic of reflection- impulsivity is relatively stable over time and tasks but is somewhat modifiable if there is an intent to modify. Only one study, Gatewood's (1972), has investigated the relation of reflective or impulsive individuals to choice of college major, and it is reviewed in the following section. Research Relevant to Reflection-Iggulsivitz and College Major Gate- wood (1972) investigated the relationship of two measures of cognitive style to grade point average, academic aptitude, and college major. The cognitive style measures were the HF? and Sigel's Conceptual Styles in Categorizing Behavior Task (SCST). The measure of academic aptitude was the American College Test (ACT), and college major was categorized as either ”arts” or "sciences.” No criterion was reported, however, for making this categorization, and only 391 of the subjects in the study 30 were classified as either arts or science majors for purposes of in- vestigating hypotheses relating to science and arts students. Subjects for the study were 129 junior college sophomore males who were paid volunteers. None of the seven hypotheses tested in the Gatewood study were substantiated by the data. The present author feels that failure to uphold hypotheses of relationship of cognitive style to college major was more due to the age of the population and the fact that not all subjects could be used to test the hypotheses. Gatewood cites data which show that only 501 of junior college students matriculate at four year institutions and that they tend to declare a given major more on the basis of prestige than ability. Furthermore, because not all sub- jects could be classified as analytic vs. non-analytic or impulsive vs. reflective, the number used for the statistical tests for these hypo- theses was smaller than that used for the other tests. Intuition Overview Hestcott's (1968b) work on intuition started with the simple observation that some people get the point more quickly than others. This seemingly obvious statement triggered questions such as, Do people in fact get the point more quickly? If so, to what degree do they vary? what are the implications of this variation? A study of the psychology of intuition has its roots in philoso- phy. The classical position on intuition is that it is an avenue by which knowledge gained is characterized by non-sensory attainment of knowledge, the knowledge being non-empirical and non-verifiable. The 31 knowledge need not come as the result of prior intellectual endeavor, but it frequently does. Hestcott points out that there are basically two opposing views concerning intuition. They differ on the scope of the knowledge attained in this manner. The pure intuitionists hold that the knowledge gained through intuition is that of ultimates, while those of the positivistic school hold that the knowledge is more limited and immediately useful. An example of the former is that God is to be glorified and of the latter is that events have causes. westcott reports three views on intuition in psychology. The first view he terms the Verstehenist (or global understanding) position. This position holds that intuition is the step from inference to under- standing. It is akin to what is referred to among psychotherapists as clinical as opposed to empirical understandings. The clinician suddenly achieves ”insight" into the dynamics of the client, though he may not be able to trace in a one-two-three fashion just how he came to the conclu- sion. The non-intuitively-oriented empiricist, on the other hand, de- mands hard data to form a conclusion. The second position (Jung, 1971) on intuition deals not so much with it as a form of mystical knowledge but as personality trait found in all people. Jung refers to intuition as perception by the unconscious as opposed to perception through the senses. Through the intuitive pro- cess Jung believes the intuiter perceives the generalities and implica- tions while a sensing oriented person perceives the physical properties of the object or event. For instance, an intuiter may be likened to an abstract artist who is portraying the implications of his subject while the sensor would portray a replica of the physical properties of his 32 subject. Jung has, therefore, taken the mystery out of classical intui- tion by saying that it is just as much a function of human behavior as is perception by the senses. He feels that the reason people are not more aware of the function is that it is suppressed through societal pressure. His reasoning is that a person who has a constitutional pre- disposition toward intuition and develops it will likely be regarded as a dreamer and, therefore, a social outcast. Jung's position links the Verstehenist position with the third position which can be termed the inference position. Hestcott reports that those who aspire to the inference position hold that intuition is a special case of inference. It differs from the Verstehenist position in that knowledge is gained through definite sensory channels and cogni- tive manipulations but that some parts of the process--a specific manip- ulation or mediating link-~may be obscure. It is the latter position which Hestcott holds and upon which his work has been based. When Westcott points to the research dealing with the inference view of intuition, he points to that body of research which others could readily term cognitive style. He says the research is ...catalogued under the headings of learning, perception, concept attainment, problem-solving, and so on, all occurring under less than optimum conditions. we are also directed to a literature not yet so large, but rapidly growing, which is concerned with personality and attitude differences in the acquisition of knowledge...(Hestcott, 1968b, p. 189). From this, it can be concluded that Hestcott treats intuition as cogni- tive style which focuses on the amount of information an individual needs to solve a "cognitive riddle.” 33 Westcott believes that one application of his work on intuition is in instructional practice in situations where information is lacking or apparently lacking. Such is the case with language instruction. Some students must extract the rules through a mental osmosis process, an intuitive process, while others learn best through ”active” teaching of the rules. In graduate education the apprentice system is often ad- vocated. It is a system in which the pupils learn informally, at the feet of the master so to speak, and it seems geared to an intuitive thinker while non-intuitives may gain more from a more formal approach. Hestcott's overall contention is that cognitive style should influence instructional practice, and at every level-nursery school through graduate school. Hestcott's (1968b) operational definition of intuition is ...the event which occurs when an individual reaches a conclusion on the basis of less explicit information than is ordinarily re- quired to reach that conclusion (p. 100). The instrument (Westcott Intuition Test, HIT) which he uses to measure the concept consists of a series of verbal and numerical ab- straction problems with five clues to the solution of the problem. The clues are so arranged that the subject uncovers them one at a time and .in sequence. The subject is to use as few clues as possible. The correct response is that which is consensually valid when all the clues are known. The dependent variables are clue use, number correct, and efficiency or the ratio of the number correct to clue use. The efficiency score is rarely used. Hestcott has produced considerable research on characteristics associated with differential performance on the "IT. Subjects for his 34 study were several groups of college students which he terms samples. The samples he used for a specific study varied from study to study. His first studies were correlational in nature comparing measures of academic achievement and aptitude with HIT scores. His major finding concerning academic aptitude was that the clue use score was negatively related to both the verbal and numerical scores of the Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT), that the number correct score is positively related to both academic aptitude and achievement, and the efficiency score is related to SAT Mathematical Scores. Since the correlation coefficients between HIT and the aptitude scores were generally of a low magnitude and there were virtually no significant coefficients when course grades were compared with HIT scores, Hestcott's overall conclusion is that neither academic achievement nor aptitude are much related to intuitive thinking. This finding coincides with the findings of other cognitive style researchers who posit that cognitive style is for the most part independent of intelligence. After failing to find significance using correlational techniques with personality measures of impulse expression, and flexibility (adapted from the Vassar College Attitude Inventory), and manifest anxiety (Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale), Hestcott turned to using subjects whose perfor- mance was extreme on the measures of clue use and number correct. Ex- treme was defined as any score which was plus or minus one standard deviation from the mean on the two measures. (Sometimes the criterion was a .75 s.d.) Sometimes extreme scorers on clue use and number correct were studied, but more often four quadrants were established for those who were extreme on the two measures combined. Thus those low on clue use and high on number correct were termed ”intuitive thinkers)” those 35 high on clue use and high on number correct were termed ”careful suc- cessesz” those low on clue use and low on number correct were ”wild guessersg” and those high on clue use and low on number correct were ”careful failures.” There were from six to seven percent in each of the groups selected from the subjects used in the correlational studies cited previously. One study cited by Hestcott involved ratings by faculty members of extreme performers on the efficiency score. An overall rating of quality of performance failed to produce significant differences in the two groups. EXtreme performers in the four categories mentioned above were rated on items concerning ”quality of thinking," "grasp of concepts;' ”conscientiousness,” and ”involvement.“ Again there were no significant differences. Next a series of studies were undertaken to discover personality correlates of the extreme scorers. The Allport-Vernon-Lindzey Study of values yielded essentially identical profiles for the four groups. Use of other instruments yielded more positive results. However, the manner in which significant results were found was in many cases less than straightforward, and to report in detail the methodology and results in the present study would unnecessarily belabor the point. Instead the methodology will be reported for one study and brief mention will be made of the instrumentation used in other studies and Hestcott's overall con- clusions. flestcott acknowledges that other methods of data analysis serve to generate hypotheses rather than come to definitive conclusions about personality traits of extreme scorers on the HIT. One instrument which Hestcott used was the California Psychological Inventory (CPI). There were no significant differences among the four 36 groups on the scales established by the CPI authors. At this point Hestcott selected individual test items which discriminated one group from the other three, or which discriminated high number correct from low number correct groups and high clue use from low clue use groups. Subjects from this study were three samples of the college students mentioned above. Items were said to discriminate if they did so in any of the three samples, and in fact there were few items which discrimi- nated in more than one sample. So if item 58 discriminated intuitive thinkers from the other three groups in the second sample but not the first and third samples, it was put in the pool of items which discrim- inated intuitive thinkers from the other three groups. Those items which were found to discriminate were then grouped on the basis of psychological coherence. Descriptions of the four groups were devel- oped from this information. Other instruments which were used were the Allport-Vernon- Lindsey Study of Values, the instrument previously described which measured impulse expression, flexibility, and manifest anxiety responses to a six question open-ended interview situatiom,and an adjective check list of indeterminate origin. Using the methodology and instrumentation mentioned above, Westcott arrived at the following conclusions concerning extreme per- formers. He concluded discussion of characteristics of the intuitive thinkers in the following manner: This is a coherent picture of self-determining persons, willing to deal with the world on its own terms and unwilling to be swayed by social pressures. Their goals and their aspirations are high, but are often quite different from what most people seem to want (Westcott, 1968b, p. 143). 37 The wild guessers are described as socially complex and involved, but their involvement seems clouded with cynicism, rigidity, and bull- headedness. They seem not to have a firm grasp on reality or to organize or consolidate their efforts toward achieving a goal. The careful successes, Hestcott (1968b) describes as ...conservative, cautious, somewhat repressive people who function well in situations where expectations are well established and well met (p. 147). The careful failures are described as conservative, authority- oriented individuals who seemingly adopt the conservative stance in a desperate effort to attain stability in a world which they little under- stand and are unable to control. Research Relevant to Westcott's Conception of Intuition to College Major The present author could find no studies relating Hestcott's measure of intuition to college major. However, westcott (1968a) did compare scores on the HIT to scores on the Strong Vocational Interest Blank. The selection of vocation and college major is thought to be closely parallel, though not identical in all cases. However, a person aspiring to be a mathematician is very likely to major in mathematics. That selection of college major and vocation is closely parallel also seems to be the view of Holland (1966) who reports characterizing univer- sity environments by major as one method for categorizing vocational en- vironments. It is for this reason that the present author will report research relating cognitive style to vocational choice when research relating cognitive style to choice of major is unavailable. 38 Westcott's (1968a) subjects were ninety-five college female seniors. They were administered the NIT as freshmen and as seniors. He then formed two samples from this group--those who were extreme performers on the HIT either as freshmen or as seniors. There was considerable overlap in these groups. The entire sample was adminis- tered the 1945 edition of the Strong Vocational Interest Blank for Men (SVIB-M) . As in previous studies, correlational analysis using the entire group failed to produce significant coefficients. Two other methods were used to analyze the data. In each method thirty-two of the original ninety-five subjects were divided into the four groups--intuitive thinker, careful success, wild guesser, or careful failure. In the first method each subject was assigned a rank from one to four on each individual scale of the SVIB-M, depending on the closeness of her interest pattern to that of each occupational pro- file. The individual scores were averaged by group on each individual scale and were compared to the mean score for the other three groups via t test. Thus on the physician scale the rank of intuitive thinkers might differ from that of the other three groups. It appears that this procedure required multiple t tests on the same population and was therefore inappropriate. The second procedure took the group rank of the subjects in each of the four groups on each occupation in one of the SVIB-M occur pational clusters. Thus on the physician scale the intuitive thinkers might rank four indicating their interests were the most similar.of the four groups to those of physicians, wild guessers might rank two, etc. This procedure was followed for each occupation in the cluster. Then 39 the ranks for each group were added and rank chi-square analysis was performed. The procedure was repeated for each occupational cluster. From this analysis Hestcott draws the following conclusions. The intuitive thinkers show more interest in diverse occupations. They share the interests of abstract scientists and those who perform manual labor. They have least interest in business and social service. The wild guessers dislike the vocation of music teacher and salesman and are interested in a cluster of mathematical subjects. They are most interested in the professional service occupations. The careful successes prefer the vocation of music teacher and librarian and the school subjects of geometry and civics. They have interest in practical manual vocations and are interested in business and social service occupations. The careful failures produced the fewest findings. They show interest in the vocation of clergyman and have the least interest in the vocation of politician. They share more interests with people in the professional services and least with people in business. Overall, Hestcott (1968a) concludes The significance of the particular preferences and dislikes which distinguish the groups is not en- tirely clear, of course, but those described above are quite congruent with the earlier descriptions offered (9. 19). In view of the fact that Westcott had to use somewhat extreme measures to find correlates of intuitive thinking and that he admits that the fruits of his efforts are more suggestions than definitive results, it is incumbent upon the present author to justify use of this particular measure for further study. 40 Originally a comparison of the theoretical and operational definitions of intuition by Jung, Myers (1962) (as expressed by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator), and Hestcott was of interest to the present author. However, Nestcott (1968a) reported essentially nega- tive correlations between intuition as measured by the HIT and the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI). However, when_the present author con- sidered Westcott's work more fully, the cognitive style implications became more apparent and more important. As has been mentioned above, Hestcott's comments concerning the importance of the manner in which information is presented at all levels of instruction provided suf- ficient cause in the mind of the present author for further investi- gation. Jungian Functions Overview In the discussion in the previous section, Jung's thoughts on intuition were presented. In this section his ideas on psychologi- cal types will be discussed-that part of his work in which his thinking on intuition was first presented. In a new collection of his works (Jung, l97l). Jung explains the rationale for classifying people by psychological type. He said he developed his theory of type ...to provide an explanatory basis and theoretical framework for the boundless diversity that has hitherto prevailed in the formation of psychologi- cal concepts (p. 555). He points out that type has been a concern with which even the ancients have dealt. He says his formulation is grounded in a purely psychic foundation whereas other formulations have been built upon a foundation in physiology. 41 Jung wrote that he developed his particular system empirically, that is, in trying to explain the diversity of behavior in his patients, he discovered that they had characteristic orientations to their environ- ments. Some were oriented to the world outside themselves (extraverts), some more to the forces within (introverts). However, he discovered that there remained much diversity to be explained in the behavior of extraverts and introverts. The explanation lay in the fact that indi- viduals differed in their use of functions which he identified as thinking, feeling, sensation, and intuition. He argued that while the names of the functions might be changed and that other formulations might be developed, his four were the most practical. Research by Cook (1970) and Gorlow, Simonson, and Krauss (1973) has given empirical support to the existence of a typology as posited by Jung. The function tells which of four tools the person typically uses in experiencing his world. Some prefer to rely most on data from the senses, and are, therefore, said to be sensation oriented. Hhat they perceive by the five senses becomes the basis for their actions. Others prefer to use intuition. While they perceive with their senses, they do not focus on the object itself, but rather on its implications and possibilities. Since these two functions are perceptive functions, Jung refers to them as irrational, that is, not concerned with judgment. The judging or rational functions are thinking and feeling. Those who are disposed to use thinking need a rational basis in drawing conclu- sions, and those disposed to feeling make judgments on the basis of what is valued at an emotional level. Jung (1971) characterizes the interrelation of the functions in this manner: 42 Sensation establishes what is actually present, thinking enables us to recognize its meaning, feeling tells us its value, and intuition points to possibilities as to whence it came and whither it is going in a given situation (p. 540). The spice and variety of individual differences is due, in Jung's view, to a differential weighting, so to speak, of the functions in a given individual. That function which is most predominant and most used is referred to as the dominant function. The function which is second in time is referred to as the auxiliary function. If the dominant function is perceptive (i.e., sensing or intuition), then the auxiliary must be judgmental (i.e., thinking or feeling). The other functions may come into play in a given instance but generally they are less developed and more in the unconscious, particularly that function which is opposite the dominant. For instance, if thinking is dominant, then feeling is most undeveloped, and the auxiliary must be either sensing or intuition. The Jungian formulation thus established a system by which an individual's characteristic orientation to the world about him can be discerned. Jung (1971) seemed to establish a linkage between the per- sonality types and cognitive style by the statement: It (introversion) is therefore oriented by the factor in perception and cognition which responds to the sense stimulus in accordance with the individual's subjective disposition (p. 374). That part of the statement,'that introversion is oriented by a factor in perception and cognition3'provides a basis for linking the personality types to cognitive style. Myers (1962) also indicates that the Jungian type can be con- sidered cognitive style. Preference type is the product of the person's con- scious orientation to life, his habitual, purposeful ways of using his mind,chosen because they seem to him good and interesting and trustworthy (p. 74). 43 Sundberg's (1965) statement clearly indicates he views the MBTI as a measure of cognitive style. Purely as a potential research procedure for getting at individual differences in cognitive preferences, it would seem the Indicator would merit a great deal of attention from cognitive theorists (p. 325). It is precisely for the purpose of "getting at individual differences in cognitive preferences" that the MBTI has been employed in the present study. It is on similar bases that Child (1965) made the statement that the Jungian typology could be included in the category cognitive style. The present author has reached the same conclusion. Research Relevant to Jungian Typolggy and Choice of Major Myers (1962) and Reynolds and Hope (1970) point to research which suggests that choice of major is relevant to Jungian typology and the intent of this section is to review those studies. Most studies are reported by Myers based on findings arrived at through the use of the MBTI. The MBTI has four scales-three reflecting the Jungian dimensions [extraversion-introversion (E-I), sensing-intuition (8-H), and thinking- feeling (T-Ffl and a fourth called judging-perceiving (J-P) which is used to identify the dominant life style attitude (the extraverted pro- cess). If a subject chooses more items related to needing conclusions rather than to observing phenomena, he is identified as J on the J-P dimension, and it is taken to mean that one of the judging functions (either T or P) will be the manner in which that individual interacts with the world. In the case of extraverts,that dominant function will be 44 displayed prominently to the world as it is used to facilitate inter- action with the environment. However, introverts are said to display their auxiliary in their everyday life and show their dominant only when the pressures of life compel it. Therefore, an introvert who is a judging type will have as his dominant one of the perceptive functions, either S or N. Myers reports results, not in Jungian terminology (e.g., intro- verted feeling type), but by using the initial of the preference on each of the four scales. In this manner an introverted sensing type is reported as ISTJ or ISFJ, an extraverted sensing type as ESTP or ESFP, an introverted intuitive type as INFJ or INTJ, an extraverted intuitive type as ENFP or ENTP, an introverted thinking type as ISTP or INTP, an extraverted thinking type as ESTJ or ENTJ, an introverted feeling type as ISFP or INFP, and an extraverted feeling type as ESFJ or ENFJ. Most research reported in this section is based on Myers (1962). A critical examination of her sources is beyond the scope of the pre- sent work and would be a difficult task. While she reports the sources of her data, much of it is in technical reports or unpublished material. Also the sources of some of her own analyses are cross-referenced in a complicated manner. Myers reports correlational data comparing MBTI scales with SVIB-M scales. The manner of presentation was to say that certain SVIB-M occupational groups attracted a particular MBII type or combina- tion of types. Thus SVIB-M groups I and II (professional and technical- scientific, nomenclature used by Myers) were said to attract IN-- types. This means that MBTI scores on the I-2 scale and on the S-N scale 45 correlated highly with most SVIB-M scores in groups I and II, in the direction of I and N, but there were no significant correlations between those groups and the T-F and J-P scales. In the same manner groups III, VII, VIII, IX, and XI (production manager, CPA, business detail and administration, business contact and president of manufacturing firm) were said to attract EST] and "partial“ ESTJ types. Groups VI and x (musician, verbal or linguistic) were said to attract -N-P types and and group V (uplift) was said to attract ENF- types. Also listed are the individual SVIB-M occupations which cor- related highest (range .20 to .55) with each MBTI dimension. E cor- related highest with the sales manager scale; I, with the mathematician scale; S, with the banker scale: N, with the psychologist scale; T, with the purchasing agent scale; F, with the minister scale; J, with the accountant scale; P, with the artist scale. The report of another series of studies was that among creative occupations, i.e., architects, research scientists, writers, and matheticians, the writers tended to be ~NF- and mathematicians -NT-. All four occupations were N. Myers also reports data which show that the composition of student bodies in different colleges and professional schools differs by type. The relative frequencies of the sixteen types of college prep high school students were compared with the relative frequencies of students at liberal arts, engineering, science, business, and medical schools. 0n the basis of this comparison, the following conclusions were drawn. For liberal arts students the characteristics identified as being predominant were -NF- and -IN- against EST-; for science students, 46 IN-- against ES--; for engineering students, -N-J against -S-P and ES--; for business students, -ST- and ES-- against IN--; for medical students, --F- and IN-- against ESTJ. These findings do not have immediate use- fulness for the present study since the primary thrust of the findings is that business types can be clearly differentiated from science types. Business was excluded as a dimension in the present study for reasons to be cited later. However, the findings are significant for the pres- ent study in that they do show that Jungian typology as operationalized by the MBTI is a useful tool for assessing differential characteristics of college majors. Another comparison, one step closer in relevance to the present study, is of the effects of combining the perceptive and judging func- tions,i.e., the occupational choice or choice of major a person who is an -ST- combination is apt to make as opposed to one whose preference is -NT-. The data were derived by investigating the percentages of the combinations among various occupational and/or student groups. The table is presented here as Myers (1962, p. 64) presented it. Function choice ST SF NF NT Production Sales Research Research Construction Service Teaching Science Accounting Customer Preaching Invention Business relations Counselling Securities Economics Welfare work Writing analyst Law Nursing Psychology Management Surgery Gen. practice Psychiatry Cardiology Etc. Etc. Etc. Etc. Reynolds and Hope (1970) investigated the hypotheses that there would be a greater proportion of science students classified INTP on the MBTI than in the general population and that students with those preferences would score higher on science achievement and aptitude tests, 47 in grade point average, and on academic aptitude tests. The population for this study was 326 secondary school students in required general science and biology classes representing students in general and 58 advanced science students representing science students. The authors concluded that science students did move in the direction of INTP when compared to students in general, but the support was marginal in that the results were in the predicted direction but not always statistically significant. The latter hypothesis concerning higher achievement and aptitude scores by science students was for the most part not supported. Further considerations of the MBTI are contained in the instru- mentation section of Chapter III. In this section four conceptualizations of cognitive style chosen for the present study have been surveyed and research having relevance to their relation to college major has been reviewed. The research with the dimension of field-dependence-independence was the most direct, followed by the research on Jungian typology via the MBTI. Reflection-impulsivity was discussed as an important concept for instructional settings though research relative to college major is all but non-existent. Westcott's conceptualization of intuition was seen as important to instructional settings because it had implications for the manner in which information is presented in instructional settings. It is the least well researched, possibly because findings via the HIT have been scanty. Other Research Relating Cognitive Style to Choice of Major The third major section of this chapter, as outlined in the introduction, deals with a review of studies which in.some manner have 48 linked a cognitive style with college major, with the exception of the study to be presented next. Campbell (1967) through multivariate procedures analyzed eight predictor variables derived from the Scholastic Aptitude Tests (SAT) and the Milwaukee Academic Interest Inventory as to their effectiveness for predicting college major. Of interest for the present study are the findings that a science vs. non-science dimension (as well as a helping people vs. commercial-business interest) was identified and that all variables discriminated among the groups of interest. Brubaker (1972), conceptualizing creativity and critical thinking as two dimensions of dissimilar cognitive styles, investigated the rela- tionship of these variables and Scholastic Aptitude Test scores to the educational-vocational areas of agriculture, biological science, business, education, humanities, physical science, and social science. Subjects for the study were 1,233 entering freshmen at the University of Delaware. Findings of relevance to the present study were that students in the different areas did differ among themselves, and from a control group of undecided students, on the SAT scores and critical thinking measure, but only females differed among themselves and from a control group of undecided students on the measure of creativity. The undeclared students differed from the other students in combinations of the dependent variables. Osipow (1969) studied the relationship between cognitive style, selected college majors directlyrelated to occupational choice, and pro- files on Holland's Vocational Preference Inventory (VPI). Osipow's basic hypothesis was that people tend to organize their perceptual experiences in distinctive and significant ways, which results in differential 49 behaviors-~including those in the occupational realm. He used four measures of cognitive style-FWord Similarity, the Object Sort (cf. the discussion on cognitive controls earlier in this chapter), a measure of rejecting vs. accepting response set, and Thurstone's Closure Flexi- bility Test as a measure of field-dependence-independence. Female students majoring in nursing, home economics, dental hygiene, and special education, and male students majoring in pharmacy and fisheries technology served as subjects in the Osipow study. The four groups of female majors could be differentiated on the Object Sort, two of the three measures of rejecting vs. accepting response set, and field-dependence-independence,and on five of eleven VPI scales. The two groups of male majors could be differentiated on word similarity, one of the three measures of rejecting vs. accepting response set, and on three of eleven VPI scales. It was also hypothesized that those who scored at the extremes on one measure of cognitive style would be significantly different on other cognitive style measures and VPI scales. This hypothesis was upheld in five out of a possible twenty cases for the cognitive style measures and in eleven of a possible 55 cases for the VPI. It should also be mentioned that the questionable practice of using multiple t tests was used in obtaining these results. Other hypotheses were investigated concerning the VPI scales. Their results are not reported here because they have marginal relevance to the present study. Overall, Osipow concluded that his study lends support to the general hypothesis that students preparing for different occupations vary in cognitive style. 50 Hudson (1967) conducted a series of studies of British school boys. The British system is such that at age fifteen a student is cate- gorized as either an arts or a science student. In his search to find intellectual ability correlates of the arts-science dimension, Hudson discovered that a "style-of-reasoning" dimension which he terms convergent- divergent thinking discriminated science from arts students remarkably well. (It might be added that he had already developed a composite of ability test profiles which also discriminated well.) In conjunction with an intelligence test, he used two tests, one a Use of Objects Test in which a student is to name as many uses as he can of an object (e.g., barrel), and another in which the student gives as many meanings of a word as he can. Convergent students tend to score well on an IQ test (termed A. H. S), to give fewer definitions and uses, and to be in the sciences. Divergent students do relatively poorly on the IQ test, give more definitions and uses, and tend toward the arts. Mackay and Cameron (1968) replicated Hudson's findings in a Scottish university where the decision to specialize in the arts or sciences is not made as soon. Their sample came from an introductory psychology class in which a cross section of students enrolled. The same tests administered in Hudson's study were used. The battery did not differentiate students when non-specializing students were in the sample, but when they were eliminated from the sample so that there were only arts specialists and science specialists in the sample, then the results strongly confirmed Hudson's findings. Hervey (1967) investigated the relationship between cognitive style as measured by Sigel's SCST (also used by Gatewood, 1972) and performance in two school related tasks. When six of the seven hypotheses 51 relating to that investigation were not confirmed, and the seventh was not able to be analyzed statistically, she refocused the study and in- vestigated the influence of cognitive style on the choice of major and grade point average. Categories for choice of major were basically the same as for the present study. They were literature and communications, natural sciences, and social sciences. The sampling of specific majors in the general categories differed little from those in the present study except that history was included as a social science by Hervey and as a humanity in the present study. Hervey concluded after examining the data via analysis of vari- ance and chi-square that there was indeed a relationship between those majoring in the sciences and the inferential-categorical mode of con- cept formation. She also compared unidentified "entrance test subscores (pp. 51-52)" to the three modes of concept formation and found a rela- tionship between "numerical" aptitude and the inferential-categorical mode. Science majors had significantly higher grade point averages and higher "information" and ”numerical" aptitude scores. In a similar study, Williams (1971) devised a Cognitive Prefer- ence Test (CPT) which yielded preference scores for l)facts or terms, 2) fundamental principles or generalizations, and 3) practical applica- tion in each of three areas--natural sciences, social sciences, and mathematics. Subjects were 231 community college and university fresh- men and sophomores. Of significance for the present study are findings that 1) no difference in CPT scores were found between the sexes, 2) differences were found between subjects majoring in different fields, 3) CPT scores were unrelated to academic aptitude test results, and 4) surprisingly 52 CPT scores were unrelated to scores on unnamed "traditional psychological tests of cognitive styles." Chung (1966) investigated the relationship among four measures of cognitive style, major in college, vocational preference, and vocational commitment. The measures and instrumentation for cognitive style were: field-dependence-independence as measured by a group test very similar to the Group Embedded Figures Test used in the present study; a test of leveling-sharpening developed by Gardner; constricted-flexible style as measured by the Stroop Color Word Test; and equivalence range as measured by Clayton and Jackson's Object Sorting Test. Vocational preference was assessed by scores on the Kuder Preference Record. College majors used in the study were identified on the basis of the investigator's judgment as to what would maximize differences between areas and minimize differences within areas. The groups he selected were social science, natural science, humanities, engineering, elementary teaching, music, social service, and library science majors. Vocational commitment was established by evaluating two responses on Chung's Personal Data Blank. Subjects for the study were 141 junior, senior, and graduate level paid volunteers from three schools in the Nashville, Tennessee, area. There were seventy-one female and seventy male subjects ranging in age from 19 to 39 (2522.8). Of particular interest for the present study was that scores of those majoring in different areas were differentiated on the various cognitive style measures. However, the pattern was mixed, i.e., the pattern of scores of those majoring in different areas clustered 53 differently on the different measures of cognitive style. The statis- tical tool was multiple discriminant analysis. On the measure of field-dependence-independence, engineering and natural science students had the most field-independent scores, while social service, elementary teaching, library science, and humanities clustered toward the field- dependent end. Social science and music majors stood between the two groups; they were significantly different from the former but not the latter. On the constricted-flexible dimension, library science and social service were toward constricted style while elementary teaching and social science were toward the flexible dimension. On the leveling- sharpening dimension, natural science and library science majors were the strongest sharpeners while music and the humanities were the strong- est levelers. The equivalence range dimension was found to be an inef- fectual dimension for differentiating college major. The analyses of Kuder Preference Record types indicating voca- tional commitment yielded negative results. Field (1954) administered five personality measures, a perceptual task, an attitude scale, and seven measures of identification with the father figure to subjects majoring in physical sciences and social sci- ences. Of significance for the present study was the finding that the physical science majors scored more toward sharpening on the leveling- sharpening measure than did the social science majors. 54 Conclusions A careful review of the literature has led the present author to the following conclusions. I. There is substantial evidence that cognitive style, although defined in many diverse ways, is related to college major. 2. The most promising dimension for differentiating humanities from natural science students is Witkin's field-dependence-independence. However, there has been no study using many subjects, both male and fe- male, and incorporating a social science dimension. The present study intends to provide the next step in exploring this dimension. 3. While Westcott's measure of intuition has not been demon- strated to be an effective measure in differentiating college majors, some research has alluded to the possibility of such a finding. However, as has been pointed out, the primary reason Westcott's work is included in the present study is that it has implications for instructional prac- tice-~that while some students seem to prefer an inductive, "discovery" approach to learning, others prefer a more structured presentation ap- proach. As has also been pointed out, the intent of the present study is to investigate whether these differences are systematic by college major. 4. The nature of Gatewood's study leaves inconclusive the relation between reflection-impulsivity and college major. However, as with intuitive thinking the dimension has implications for instruc- tional practice. As it was pointed out in Chapter I, should it be found that students in different major groups vary systematically on the dimension of reflection-impulsivity,either instructional practice could be geared to that difference or attempts be made to alter the characteristic. 55 Therefore further investigation seems warranted. S. Jungian typology as indicated by the MBTI has been shown to be effective in differentiating college majors. An impressive array of evidence has been presented using various combinations of MBTI scale types, but there has been no test of the concept of the dominant func- tion and college major. The present study proposes to meet that need. CHAPTER III METHODOLOGY AND ANALYSIS In this chapter the design of the present study, a description of the sample, procedures used in gathering data, the measures selected for evaluating the concepts, testable hypotheses, and the method for analyzing the data will be presented. Design The present study was descriptive in nature. Its primary purpose was to investigate whether students cast into three categories of college major would differ on dimensions of cognitive style and to determine which measure best differentiated categories of college major when several related measures of cognitive style were used. This basic hypothesis was investigated using two techniques. On all measures which yielded con- tinuous scores-field-dependence-independence, reflection-impulsivity, and intuition-mean differences and vectors were tested. 0n the measure which identified dominant Jungian function and yielded discrete data, the relative percentages of subjects exhibiting particular dominant functions in each major category were compared. The technique investigating mean differences included comparison not only of mean differences among the three major categories--humanities, social science, and natural science--but also between the sexes. The 56 57 dimension of sex was added for three reasons. First, most investigators have reported finding small but statistically significant differences be- tween the sexes on field-dependence-independence. It seemed appropriate to investigate the phenomenon further. Second, it made the present study more complete. Studies most similar to the present study either controlled for sex differences by eliminating one sex (Hervey, 1967; Gatewood, 1972) or did not control for sex differences at all (Chung, 1966). Third, add- ing sex as a dimension made possible testing for interaction effects be- tween sex and major, i.e., whether differentiation on cognitive style dimensions might be strongest in a combination of the two. It must be added, however, that the present author found no reason to hypothesize interaction effects and therefore no hypotheses were formulated con- cerning them. Grade point average (GPA) and American College Test Composite (ACT-C) score were collected from university records and included in the analysis. They were included as comparison measures since the review of the literature discussed the relationship between academic aptitude and achievement and cognitive style. Such an addition made the present study a more thorough investigation. Two samples were drawn and teSted, the second sample replicating the first. The technique for analyzing the relation of Jungian dominant types to college major, as has been pointed out, was to compare the relative percentages of subjects exhibiting a dominant Jungian function in each category. Thus the data were cast into a 4 X 3 table. The sample size prohibited separating subjects by sex and it was also impossible in this 58 design to make comparisons with GPA and ACT-C. However, the replication was performed for this analysis. The secondary purpose of the present study was to compare subjects identified as dominant sensors with dominant intuiters and subjects iden- tified as dominant thinkers with dominant feelers on measures of field- dependence-independence, reflection-impulsivity, and intuition. The same design was followed for this part of the present study as was followed for comparing majors on the cognitive style dimensions yielding continuous scores, except that sex could not be included as an independent variable. The scarcity of subjects identified as dominant sensors and dominant thinkers rendered the sample size too small. Sample Each sample in the present study was comprised of ISO volunteer subjects, so divided that there were 25 males and 25 females in each of three categories of college major--humanities, social science, and natu- ral science. The majority of subjects were upperclassmen. Roughly one-sixth of the students solicited volunteered as sub- jects except in one case in the first testing and two cases in the second in which the professor made class time available for testing. Only a few in those instances chose not to participate. Sample 1 consisted of undergraduate students present on the cam- pus of Central Michigan University (CMU) during the spring term mini- session of 1973 and the first three weeks of the regular 1973 summer session. The subjects were tested from May 15 to June 1, 1973, and June 18 to July 9, 1973. They ranged in age from 17 to 55 years with the mean age being 22.2 and the standard deviation 4.3 years. The mean grade 59 point average for the sample was 2.90 (s.d. ! .53). Since the average of all grade point averages for students at the end of the winter term 1973 was 2.74, this sample had on the average slightly higher academic achieve- ment than the student body as a whole. The mean ACT-C score for 88 of the students for whom the score was available in this sample was 23.1 (s.d. I 4.0) while the median of mean ACT-C scores for freshmen admitted from the 1969-1970 to 1972-1973 academic years was 21.0. While it is apparent that the mean ACT-C score for Sample 1 was higher than the median of mean ACT-C scores for incoming freshmen and that the mean CPA for this sample was higher than that of the student body as a whole, the question remains whether the sample was higher than a random sample of their peers on these measures or whether the fact that most of the sample was upper- classmen accounted for the difference. In Sample 1, 29 females in the humanities were enlisted since the experimenter experienced difficulty in soliciting male subjects. When it was realized that sufficient male subjects would participate, four females in that category were randomly removed from the data analysis. One sub- ject in this sample had to be excluded because he would not answer all MBTI items, so another volunteer was solicited. Sample 2 consisted of undergraduate students present on the campus the first three weeks of the fall 1973 semester. They were tested from August 27 to September 14, 1973. They ranged in age from 17 to 33 years with the mean age being 20.7 and the standard deviation 2.2 years. The mean GPA for this group was 2.91 (s.d. - .48) and the mean ACT-C score for 117 of the students for whom the score was available in this sample was 22.8 (s.d. - 4.1). 60 From the data presented, it can be seen that the two samples were quite similar in academic achievement and aptitude and that the mean age of Sample 2 subjects was 1.5 years less than that of Sample 1 subjects. The mean differences were analyzed by means of the t test. Differences were not significant on GPA and ACT-C, but the age difference was sig- ficant (B 8 3.004, p 41.001). It can be concluded that on CPA and ACT-C the samples were drawn from the same population, but on age it appears they were not. ‘e Procedure Specific majors were classified humanities, social science, or natural science using the ranking system devised by Goldschmid (1967). He used the constant sum method for developing two scales of the same 55 majors. One scale was anchored on philosophy and the other on physics. Groups of counselors, faculty, and upper-level students rated the sub- jects on each scale in relation to an anchor subject. On the science scale, physics was the anchor subject and therefore was given 100 science points and O humanities points. On the humanities scale, philosophy was given 100 humanities points and 0 science points. The result was two scales of SS majors ranked on their relation to science or humanities in descending order. One scale was reversed so that there were not two scales, each headed by natural science subjects. Both scales were divided into thirds so that the top third contained mostly natural sciences, the middle third contained mostly social sciences, and the bottom third contained mostly humanities. Those subjects which were included in the same third were immediately chosen as appropriate for 61 the present study. Those majors which were in different thirds were chosen using the best judgment of the present author consistent with commonly accepted classifications. Some majors were specifically excluded from the present study. Goldschmid (1967) concluded that certain majors seemed not relevant to the arts vs. science dimension, namely business, home economics, and librarianship. Likewise, they were not included in the present study. Education as a major fell in the humanities category. However, though CMU has a strong tradition of teacher education, no one majors in educa- tion as such. There are a few curricula which have, in effect, a major in education such as special education, physical education, and indus- trial education. Since the major is a combination of education and other subject areas, e.g., psychology on the special education curricu- lum, these majors were also excluded from the present study. The criterion for determining a student's major was self-report. If a student was a double major, then he was asked which was his prefer- ence if he had to choose between the two. While the soliciting was done for the most part in upper-level classes because more upper-level students know their majors, freshmen or sophomores who could say they were majoring in a specific area were accepted for the study. Table 1 contains a list of the majors accepted in the present study and the number of male and female subjects in each testing by subject area. The present author or one of his assistants asked professors for ten minutes of class time to solicit subjects from the professor's classes. No professor refused the request. Potential subjects were in- formed of the nature of the study, time required, the general nature of 62 TABLE 1 Composition of Sample 1 and Sample 2 by Sex and Number in Each Major Area and Specific Major Sample 1 F Sample 2 Majors T Male Female Male Female Humanities Speech 6 6 6 9 Music 2 l 2 1 English 8 3 7 4 Journalism 3 4 3 Religion 1 Foreign Language 2 6 2 Philosophy 1 Art 2 4 2 4 History 4 2 3 2 Total 25 25 25 25 Natural Science Mathematics 4 10 6 3 Biology 12 12 17 21 Chemistry 5 l 2 1 Earth Science 1 2 Geology 1 Physics 1 Civil Engineering 1 Total ' 25 25 25 25 Social Science Psychology 13 13 14 14 Sociology 7 11 3 8 Political Science 4 l 4 Economics 2 1 Social Science 1 _2 1 Health Education 1 Total 25 25 25 25 63 the tests to be administered, and that, since the design of the study required two samples of 150 subjects each, the experimenter was unable to pay his subjects. Subjects were also told that they could receive an interpretation of the test results at the end of the term or testing period. A small number of students were solicited in other ways, e.g., students known personally by the present author or his assistants. Sub- jects were solicited until each of the six categories of 25 subjects was filled. A few subjects, particularly those who were tested in a class session, did not keep an appointment to complete the testing. In those cases, subjects were solicited to fill in the number of required subjects for each category. Subjects were scheduled for testing at a specific time to main- tain an orderly atmosphere for testing. Since there were only two exam- iners for the individually administered MFF and the administration time for the whole battery was quite lengthy, the order of administration of [the tests varied so that the subjects were occupied most of the time. The four tests administered were the Group Embedded Figures Test (GEFT), the WIT, the MFF, and the MBTI. Maximum utilization of the subject's time was accomplished by breaking up the administration of the untimed MBTI, usually with administration of the MFF. For instance, if subjects A, B, C, and D were tested at the same time, they all would be administered the two timed tests (GEFT and WIT), and then subjects A and B would start the MBTI and subjects C and D would be given the MFF. When subjects C and D completed the MFF, then subjects A and B would be interrupted to take the MFF while subjects C and D were given the MBTI. There is no 64 reason to believe that the data were distorted using this system since the MBTI is untimed and the nature of the items is such that responses are not likely to change with an interruption of short duration. Also, the interruptions were random so that no systematic differences were introduced. The present author was assisted in test administration for Sample 1 and Sample 2 by two people, both with graduate degrees and known to be reliable and accurate. One was paid; the other volunteered. In addition, for Sample 2, two reliable psychology students, one graduate and the other a senior, were paid to assist in test administration since the time required to solicit subjects for the first study far exceeded the expectations of the present author. The role of the two psychology assistants in Sample 2 was relatively minor and their primary function was to fill gaps which would expedite the testing. Instruments This section contains the instrumentation used in the present study, rationale for its use, and reliability and validity information. Group Embedded Figures Test The Group Embedded Figures Test (Witkin et al., 1971) was chosen for the present study as the measure of field- dependence-independence because evidence presented in the manual suggests it is a reliable and valid measure of the construct, and it offered the advantage of relative speed and ease of administration. The test presents the subject with eighteen geometrically complex figures. Within each figure is a more simple figure, which the subject is asked to identify by outlining it with a pencil in the complex figure. 65 The test is divided into three timed sections, one two-minute practice section, and two five-minute test sections. The score is the number of simple figures correctly identified. Subjects are classified field- independent if they score above the mean of their group and as field- dependent if they score below it. The manual reports preliminary norms based on undergraduate men and women at an eastern liberal arts college. For men, it reports a mean of 12.0 and a s.d. of 4.1 based on 155 cases; for women, it reports a mean of 10.8 and a s.d. of 4.2 based on 242 cases. The grand mean for 397 cases is 11.3. The data from the two samples collected for the present study yielded mean and s.d. scores of 12.3 and 4.6 respectively for Sample 1 and 13.0 and 4.0 respectively for Sample 2. The scores from the present study seem acceptably close to those from the preliminary norm group. It was found that no sex difference existed in the present study though the difference reported in the manual is significant at the .005 level of significance. A reliability estimate of .80 is reported for a sample of 80 males and 97 females. The correlation was based on number identified in the first section of nine problems, as. opposed to the nine problems in the second section, and corrected by using the Spearman-Brown prob phecy formula. Since the GEFT, as well as the WIT, were administered using time limits, the question of the propriety of the split-half method of comput- ing reliability coefficients is raised. Were the tests purely speed tests, there would be no question that the method was totally inappro- priate. However, Magnusson (1967) speaks of tests which do not depend on pure speed but which have time limits. In such situations he says 66 the effect is to diminish the magnitude of the reliability coefficient. 0n the other hand, Cronbach (1960), speaking in the context of tests of general ability, makes the comment that short speed tests may be more reliable than tests with very few items. Clearly the GEFT and WIT fall in the former category. The criterion for assessing the effect of a time limit on per- formance is whether most subjects finished all the items they could in the required time. It was the observation of the present author that most subjects taking the GEFT were not pressed for time. ‘Therefore, it seems that the effect on reliability was more severe for the WIT than for the GEFT since Westcott shortened the ten item test used in the present study to eight items because some subjects were pressed for time (M. Westcott, personal communication, May 17, 1973). The situation is somewhat alleviated for the WIT since Westcott does report test-retest coefficients. In both cases, however, caution seems warranted in inter- preting the split-half coefficients. The manual cites three cases of concurrent validity for the GEFT. Correlations of -.82 for 73 males and -.63 for 68 females with scores on the EFT are reported. Correlations of -.39 were reported for 55 males and -.34 for 68 females with the Portable Rod and Frame Test (PRFT), and of .71 and .55 for the same number of males and females respectively on the ABC scale which measures degree of body articulation. (The negative correlations are in the expected direction because of the nature of the EFT and PRFT scores.) Degree of body articulation, as discussed in Chap- ter II, was found to be an indicator of degree of field-dependence- independence. 67 Witkin 33 31. (1971) conclude The combined evidence suggests that the GEFT may prove to be a useful substitute for the EFT when individual testing is impractical (p. 29). The Matchinngamiliar Figures Test As reported in Chapter II, the Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFF) presents the subject with a standard line drawing and eight variants. One line drawing is an ocean liner. In the variants the shape of the prow is altered, the height of the smoke- stacks, the position of the anchor, or the tilt of the smokestacks. The subject is to select the line drawing which is the exact duplicate of the standard. The time he takes to make the first selection as well as the number of the selections he makes before identifying the correct one are recorded as scores. The time score is the most important index of a reflective or an impulsive attitude in an individual. Impulsive subjects score below the mean response time of their group and reflective subjects score above it. Most impulsive subjects' error scores are above the mean and most reflective subjects' error scores are below it. However, the correlation is not perfect. Kagan and Kogan (1970) report correlations between the scales which range from -.40 to -.65. In the present study the correlation in Sample 1 was -.59 and in Sample 2 was -.60. (See Appendix H.) There appear to be no published norms for the test, and inquiries on the part of the present author and another investigator to the test author produced no data. Gatewood (1972) reports that for her 129 junior college subjects the median of mean response times was 45.58 seconds. In the present study, the median of all response times was 50.75 seconds in 68 Sample 1 and 46.08 in Sample 2. The median error score in Gatewood's study was 7.5. In the present study the median error score for Sample 1 was 6.4 and in Sample 2 was 6.3. Burgbacher (1973) administered the MFF in two sections, by sepa- rating the odd and even items, to 40 male junior, senior, and graduate students (the majority were graduate students) whose median age was 24.8. The median age for Sample 1 in the present study was 21.7 and for Sample 2, 21.0. Burgbacher divided the test to facilitate the pre-post test design used in his study. On one half of the MFF, the mean error score for Burgbacher's sample was 4.3 and on the other half was 3.4. The mean error score for Sample 1 in the present study was 8.6 and for Sample 2, 8.5. The mean time score on one half of the MFF for Burgbacher's sample was 337.2 seconds and on the second half was 346.4 seconds. The mean time score for all items was 630.9 seconds in Sample 1 and 593.7 seconds in Sample 2 in the present study. The descriptive data presented by the present author, Gatewood, and Burgbacher are fairly consistent when the mean scores for each half of the MFF in Burgbacher's study are combined. Burgbacher's combined error scores are slightly lower and time scores are slightly longer which could be explained by the fact that his older and mostly graduate student population could be expected to be more reflective in their test taking behavior. Thus it is contended that the three samples do not differ in kind and could serve as a norm group. In Chapter II, it was reported that the dimension of reflection- impulsivity was found to be a reliable one. Kagan and Kogan (1970) cite specific research which points to the reliability of the MFF. One hundred and four third and fourth grade boys and girls were administered one form 69 of the MFF and then another form one year later. The correlation for the response time score between administrations averaged .62. One hundred and two children were administered the same form of the MFF a year apart. The correlation for the response time score was .48 for boys and .52 for girls. After 2% years, the correlation was .31. Kagan and Kogan do not report coefficients for the error scores. Burgbacher (l973) cites evidence for the reliability of the MFF. As was reported previously, he divided the test items into two groups according to an odd-even split, so that he could use half of the items for a pre- and half for a post-test. He had, in effect, parallel forms of the same test. The correlation between the two forms was .76 for the time score. The correlation between the two forms on the error score was .55. These correlations seem quite acceptable as evidence for the relia- bility of the MFF especially in view of the fact that the tests were shortened and no correction formula was applied and came from a very similar population on the same campus. One source of validity for the MFF is face validity, i.e., Kagan g£_2l. (1964) have proposed a dimension which they believe is an impor- tant factor in information processing. They have termed this dimension reflection-impulsivity. Clearly the MFF measures how rapidly and accu— rately a subject responds to the task. The research reported in Chapter II concerning the generality of the dimension across tasks provides a form of concurrent validity. The Westcott Intuition Test As was pointed out in Chapter II, the Westcott Intuition Test (WIT) requires the subject to solve an abstraction problem using as few of five clues to its solution as possible. The 70 correct solution is that which consensual validation dictates when all five clues are known. The format of Westcott's original test was two masonite boards which had twenty rows of five oblong slots cut through both boards. The clues were typed on a piece of paper attached to the back of the bottom of the two boards so that they showed through the 100 holes. A piece of aluminum foil was inserted between the two boards which served to cover up the clues. The subject uncovered the clues by inserting a stylus in the slots and tearing away the foil. The present author contracted with a person a duplicate the board he had received from Westcott and after six weeks of frustration that person gave up the task. Fortunately, another process was found to produce the test. This was a latent image spirit duplication process whereby the clues were invisible until they were developed with a special pen which the subject brushed across the clues which were outlined by visible boxes. The WIT has been shortened three times to»make its administration more convenient. The original test was twenty items; the second, ten: and the third, eight. The present author was supplied with the ten item version and learned of the eight item version only in a data sheet (M. Westcott, personal communication, May 17, 1973) containing information on the ten and eight item tests. By this time data collection was under- way and it was too late to change to the eight item.version. The data sheet provided normative data for the ten and eight item versions. The population was 49 social agency employees (mean age, 35.6 years) recruited from all levels. However, of those volunteering, 41 had at least a bachelor's degree. There were 37 females and 12 males. 71 Westcott reports that as many as 101 of the protocols in this sample had to be eliminated because of failure to comprehend instructions, as opposed to 22 to 52 in his college populations. In the present study, no subject who finished all tests misunderstood directions. Perhaps this was due to careful monitoring by the present author or his assistants and to adminis- tration of the WIT in small groups. For his sample on the ten item scale Westcott reported the mean clue use score was 33.9 (s.d. I 6.5), as opposed to 32.3 (s.d. I 6.7) in Sample 1 and 31.8 (s.d. I 6.0) in Sample 2 of the present study. For Westcott's sample the mean number correct was 4.3 (s.d. I 2.0) as opposed to 4.2 (s.d. I 2.0) for Sample 1 and 4.5 (s.d. I 2.1) for Sample 2. It can be seen by these data that the measures of central tendency and dis- persion are very stable, even though Westcott's group was much older. There is no reliability information supplied for the ten item version of the WIT. However, Westcott (1968b) does provide extensive reliability information for the twenty item version. Subjects were 900 female and 197 male college students in eleven samples of data collected over a period of nine years. There was no noticeable difference in descriptive statistics between male and female samples. For all but 70 in the entire population tested, split-half correlations corrected for test length were available. For the number correct score, correlations ranged between .36 and .72 with the median correlation being .48. For the clue use score, coefficients ranged from .70 to .91 with the median being .82. For two of the samples (NI95), both split-half reliabilities were computed. For the number correct score, the split-half reliability was .80 and the three year test-retest reliability was .50. For number correct the split-half reliability was .70 and the three year test-retest 72 reliability was .66. The split-half reliability coefficients are pos- sibly higher than for the form presently employed: first, because the number of items in the form used for the present study is half those in the original form; second, as has been discussed in the section on the GEFT, because the time limit on the present form of the WIT places it in the category of a speed test. It is unclear at what point Westcott intro- duced the time limit. While split-half reliability is not appropriate for speed tests, some mitigating circumstances were cited which show that the attenuation of the coefficient may not be as great as if the test were a pure test of speed. The number correct and clue use scores have been found to be uncorrelated. Westcott (1968b) reports correlations from eleven samples ranging from -.24 to +.24. 0f the eleven, only three were significant. In the present study the correlation between the two scales was -.01 in Sample 1 and .03 in Sample 2. (See Appendix H.) Validity for this test is again face validity. Westcott has so operationalized the definition that the WIT measures it. _The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) is a 166 item forced-choice self-report inventory. Ninety-five of the items are scored on the four dimensions mentioned previously: extra- version-introversion (E-I), sensing-intuition (S-N), thinking-feeling (T-F), and judging-perceiving (J-P). A subject is classified as E or I, for instance, on the basis of the difference in "E“ items and "I" items marked on the score sheet. A scoring system has been devised to elimi- nate the problem of equal scores on one dimension. The difference scores are doubled and one point is added in the direction of the least 73 predominant type in the population. Therefore, a subject might be classified as E 01 or N 11. A system of continuous scores can be developed using this system by adding or subtracting the score from 100. Thus on a continuous scale the "E 01" would become 99 and the "N 11" would become 111. It is on the basis of the continuous scores that the first reliability coefficients are reported. The manual (Myers, 1962) cites split-half coefficients of the four scales for 200 college males and females in the .80's with the median being .835. They are slightly lower for younger samples. The manual also cites data based on tetrachoric correlation coefficients since the user is more interested in the reliability of the test in categorizing subjects into dichotomies than in continuous scores. These coefficients corrected for test length ranged from .74 to .90 for the college populations cited with the median again at .835. Concurrent validity is established through correlation with the scales of the Gray-Wheelwright, another less well-known instrument used to assess Jungian typology. Although its reliability is lower than the MBTI, its E-I, S-N, and T-F scales correlate .79, .58, and .60 with cor- responding scales on the MBTI. The Gray-Wheelwright has no J-P scale. The other type of validity reported is construct validity in which scales from other tests which are believed to correlate with Jungian constructs are examined. Data derived from the SVIB have already been reported in Chapter II, and the methodology for comparison on that test is the same as was employed for comparison with the following tests. These tests which are reported as having parts complementing the Jungian constructs are the Allport-Vernon-Lindzey Study of Values (AVL), the Edwards Personal Preference Schedule (EPPS), and the Personality Research 74 Inventory (PR1). Faculty ratings and findings relative to the MBTI and job stability are reported as further evidence of construct validity. Scores on MBTI scales were correlated with scales on other tests thought to be measuring at least partial MBTI constructs. In this manner, the AVL Theoretical scale was found to correlate with the INTJ scales of the MBTI. Scales on other tests which correlated highest with a single MBTI scale were also examined. In this manner, a significant correlation of .12 was discovered between the AVL Economic and the J scale on the MBTI and a correlation of .51 between Nurturance on the EPPS and the F scale on the MBTI. The myriad of evidence presented for construct valid- ity is impressive. The primary criticisms of the MBTI (Mendelsohn, 1965) are that the scales which are claimed to measure dichotomously, in fact, have dis- tributions more normal than bimodal and that the scales do not accurately measure the Jungian concepts. While the criticism regarding the bimodal distribution at this point seems valid, still the MBTI is the best instru- ment yet devised for assessment of Jungian typology. Therefore, caution seems to be warranted for interpretation of scores near the dividing line between attitudes, functions, and on the J-P scale. Mention is also made by Sundberg (1965) of the problems in test taking attitude on the part of subjects. The experience of the present author has been that subjects often complain of the ambiguity of the items. To this criticism, it might be mentioned that Stanfiel (1966) developed item weights to correct for motivational and social desirability factors in the results, but that the weights did not appreciably change them. Many items of which subjects complain are those which are not scored. Also intuiters are in the majority in college populations,and 75 their characteristic orientation of seeing possibilities leads them to complain of myriad possibilities suggested by the items. Hypotheses The hypotheses of the present study will now be stated in test- able form. The major hypotheses are stated in their null form. The sub- hypotheses will be stated in the alternate form. Hypothesis 1 HO There will be no differences with respect to average performance on five cognitive style variables, grade point average, and American College Test composite score in the degree each contributes to the differen- tiation among three categories of college major and between the sexes. Differences will exist in the degree of field- dependence-independence as measured by group mean scores on the Group Embedded Figures Test among human- ities, social science, and natural science majors with natural science majors scoring highest (dost field- independent) followed by social science, then humanities majors. Symbolically: M1 < M2 < M3 Legend: M1 I Humanities M2 I Social Science M3 I Natural Science Differences will exist in the degree of field- dependence-independence as measured by group mean scores on the Group Embedded Figures Test between males and females with males being higher (more field-independent). Symbolically: M1 > M2 Legend: M1 I Males M2 I Females C 8 76 Differences will exist in reflection-impulsivity as measured by the group mean time score on the Matching Familiar Figures Test among humanities, social science, and natural science majors with natural science majors scoring longer times (more reflective) followed by social science, then humanities majors. Symbolically: M1 < M2 < M3 Differences will exist in reflection-impulsivity as measured by the group mean time score on the Matching Familiar Figures Test between males and females. Symbolicallyc M1 f M2 Differences will exist in reflection-impulsivity as measured by the group mean error score on the Matching Familiar Figures Test among humanities, social science, and natural science majors with natural science majors scoring fewer errors (more reflective) followed by social science, then humanities majors. Symbolically: M1 > M2? “3 Differences will exist in reflection-impulsivity as measured by the group mean error score of the Matching Familiar Figures Test between males and females. Symbolically: M1 f M2 Differences will exist in the degree of intuitive thinking as measured by the group mean number correct score on the Westcott Intuition Test among humanities, social science, and natural science majors. SYDDOIlCClIyI ”1 f H2 ’ “3 Differences will exist in the degree of intuitive thinking as measured by the group mean number correct score on the Westcott Intuition Test between males and females. Symbolically: M1 f M2 Differences will exist in the degree of intuitive thinking as measured by the group mean clue use score on the Westcott Intuition Test among humanities, social science, and natural science majors with natural science majors scoring highest (least intuitive) followed by social science, then humanities majors. Symbolicallys M1 < M2 < M3 Hypothesis 2 Hypothesis 3 Hlj Ho 77 Differences will exist in the degree of intuitive thinking as measured by the group mean clue use score on the Westcott Intuition Test between males and females. Symbolically: M1 f M2 Differences will exist among humanities, social science and natural science majors as to the distribution in those major categories of dominant sensors, dominant intuiters, dominant thinkers, and dominant feelers as identified by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. There will be no differences with respect to average performance on five cognitive style variables, grade point average, and American College Test composite score in the degree each contributes to the differen- tiation between dominant sensors and dominant intui- ters as identified by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Differences will exist in the degree of field- dependence-independence as measured by group mean scores on the Group Embedded Figures Test between dominant sensors and dominant intuiters as identified by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Symbolically: M1 f M2 Legend: M1 I Sensors M2 I Intuiters Differences will exist in reflection-impulsivity as measured by the group mean time score of the Matching Familiar Figures Test between dominant sensors and dominant intuiters as identified by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Symbolicallys M1 I M2 Differences will exist in reflection-impulsivity as measured by the group mean error score of the Matching Familiar Figures Test between dominant sensors and dominant intuiters as identified by the Myers-Briggs Type IndiC‘tor e Symbolically: M1 f M2 Differences will exist in the degree of intuitive thinking as measured by the group mean clue use score of the Westcott Intuition Test between dominant sensors and dominant intuiters as identified by the Myers- Briggs Type Indicator. Symbolically: M1 f M2 H3e Hypothesis 4 Ho b 78 Differences will exist in the degree of intuitive thinking as measured by the group mean number correct score of the Westcott Intuition Test between dominant sensors and dominant intuiters as identified by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Symbolicallyc M1 f M2 There will be no differences with respect to average performance on five cognitive style variables, grade point average, and American College Test composite score in the degree each contributes to the differen- tiation between dominant thinkers and dominant feelers as identified by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Differences will exist in the degree of field-dependence- independence as measured by group mean scores on the Group Embedded Figures Test between dominant thinkers and dominant feelers as identified by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Symbolicallys M1 f M2 Legends M1 I Thinkers M2 I Feelers Differences will exist in reflection-impulsivity as measured by the group mean time score on the Matching Familiar Figures Test between dominant thinkers and dominant feelers as identified by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Symbolically: M1 f M2 Differences will exist in reflection-impulsivity as measured by the group mean error score on the Matching Familiar Figures Test between dominant thinkers and dominant feelers as identified by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. ~Symbolica11y: M1 f M2 Differences will exist in the degree of intuitive thinking as measured by the group mean clue use score of the Westcott Intuition Test between dominant thinkers and dominant feelers as identified by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Symbolically: M1 f M2 79 "4e Differences will exist in the degree of intuitive thinking as measured by the group mean number correct score on the Westcott Intuition Test between dominant thinkers and dominant feelers as identified by the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Symbolically: M1 f M2 Method of Analysis Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) met the design re- quirements of the present study for the test of Hypotheses 1, 3, and 4. MANOVA provided the means by which the five related measures-IGEFT, MFF time, MFF errors, WIT correct, and WIT clues-~could be tested simul- taneously for the ability to discriminate among three categories of major and between the sexes or between dominant types. In interpreting the MANOVA test, first, the P value for interac- tion effects was examined for significance at the assigned level. For the present study, the significance level was set at .05. If a signifi- cant interaction effect was found, it was interpreted as outlined below and further examination ceased. If no significant interaction effect was found, significant main effects by sex and major or by type were interpreted as outlined below. When a significant interaction effect or main effect was found, the step-down F test was performed. ‘The five cognitive style meas- ures, ACT-C, and GPA were ranked according to priority of interest. Following the same rationale, separate alpha levels were set for each measure. (The specifics of the ranking are outlined in Chapter IV.) Starting with the variable of least interest, the p value for each vari- able was compared with its alpha criterion. The examination stopped with the first measure which met the criterion. That measure was seen 80 as the most important contributor to the differentiation in the signifi- cant interaction or in the main effects of major or sex (H1) or type ("3, H4). The tests via MANOVA were followed by tests of the subhypotheses via two-way analyses of variance (ANOVA). These tests investigated the power of each of the five dependent variables taken separately to dis- criminate among the categories of college major and between the sexes. Those subhypotheses of H3 and “4 were tested via one-way ANOVA since the scores of dominant sensors and dominant intuiters, and dominant thinkers and dominant feelers were not crossed by the two sexes. The significance level for these tests was that assigned for the step-down F tests in the multivariate analysis. The statistical procedure chosen for evaluating Hz was chi-square since assessment using Jungian typology involves placing subjects in dis- crete categories. In this test the three categories of college major were crossed with the four dominant functions. A significance level of .05 was chosen for the test. These tests were performed at the Michigan State University Computer Center using the program written by Jeremy D. Finn. Summar In this chapter the methodology and instrumentation used in the present study were discussed. One hypothesis investigating the relation- ship of five cognitive style measures to each other in differentiating three categories of college major and the sexes was presented. Another hypothesis investigating the proportion of dominant Jungian types across the three categories of major was proposed, and hypotheses relating 81 cognitive style variables to dominant type were presented. Two samples of 150 students were tested, the second sample serving as a replication. ACT-C and GPA were added for purposes of comparison with the five cogni- tive style variables. The method of soliciting subjects and carrying out the testing was outlined. The strengths and weaknesses of the instruments were dis- cussed. MANOVA, ANOVA, and chi-square were chosen as statistical tools. In Chapter IV, the results will be presented. CHAPTER IV RESULTS In this chapter the results of the statistical tests of the hypotheses derived in Chapter III will be presented. The results of tests of the major hypotheses via MANOVA will be followed by univariate tests of the subhypotheses. Since the review of the literature provided no basis for anticipating interaction effects, and the present author did not have reason to believe they would exist for other reasons, no hypotheses were formulated predicting interactions nor were any found. The mean scores actually tested in each univariate analysis will be included in tables and graphic representations for clarity of presen- tation.. These mean scores are combinations of mean cell scores, which with their standard deviations are presented in Appendices A, B, C, . and D. The tables reporting the results of the multivariate tests will report them twice for each sampleI-with and without the ACT-C, which was not available for all subjects. The purpose of dual presentation is to examine the effect, if any, reducing the sample size to include ACT-C might have on the overall F statistic and the step-down F test. The dependent variables on the step-down F test were ranked to gain maximum benefit from the test according to the priority of interest in the dependent variables (Bock 6 Haggard, 1968). The GEFT was placed 82 83 first, since for both sex and major the literature suggests it would be the most discriminating of the cognitive style variables. The MFF meas- ures were placed next because the literature suggested that they offered more promise of ability to discriminate majors than did the WIT. The alpha level for the GEFT and the MFF was set at .05. The WIT measures were judged to be of less crucial interest, since the review of the literature had shown that their power to dis- criminate groups was considerably diminished when scores from an entire sample were used. If the measures did discriminate, safeguards against it being a chance occurrence dictated the choice of an alpha level of .01. The comparison measures, ACT-C and GPA, were of least crucial interest. The review of the literature suggested that measures of cog- nitive style were relatively independent of academic ability and mention of the relation of college major to GPA is rarely made. These considera- tions led to the choice of an .001 alpha level. The tables reporting the results of the univariate hypotheses will be presented for the full sample size of 150. The alpha level for each univariate hypothesis will be that chosen for the step-down F test. Test of Hypothesis 1 Ho There will be no differences with respect to average per- formance on five cognitive style variables, grade point average, and American College Test composite score in the degree each contributes to the differentiation among three categories of college major and between the sexes. Table 2 presents the MANOVA table for Hypothesis 1. From Table 2 it can be seen that in Samples 1 and 2 the main effect by sex was significant and in Sample 1 the main effect by major was significant. Test of Hypothesis ls 84 TABLE 2 Multivariate Analysis of Variance for Mean Differences on Five Cognitive Style Measures, Grade Point Average, and American College Test Composite Score among Three Groups of College Major and between the Sexes Sample 1 without ACT-C 4 With ACT-C Source . df F df F Major . 121278 2.9426*** 4 161152 2.3195** Sex 6/139 3.5204** ¢ 7176 2.5541* Interaction . 121278 1.5373 14/152 .4848 Sample 2 without ACT-C‘ with ACT-C Source df 8 df F Major . 12/278 1.7010 161210 1.2540 Sex 6/l39 2.6264"r 7/105 2.6401“? Interaction lZ/278 .4352 . 14/210 .7583 * p 41.05 ** pi< usuom oveuo one .ouoom euuaommou ueea eueanou ceouuem< .eeuseeez enaum e>uuueuoo o>dm mo audauae pom once a euoeaaeum n mAn Social Science Natural Science Sample 1 b 2.78 u 2.87 3.05 Sample 2 . 2.91 2.95 2.86 American College Test Composite Score Sample 1 4 21.88 (N826) . 22.03 (N-30) . 25.09 (N-32) Sample 2 . 23.18 (N840) . 21.70 (N333) . 23.20 (N844) 3. 3 A" -AMa1es Or-OFemales g H Both “.3 l u - ,--o ........ 3 o——"' a m c . _______ '8 A‘ " Ax \ o. ‘tt 3 2.7 \A m 35 2.5 if - - , c - Humanities Social Natural Humanities Social Natural Science Science Science Science Sample 1 Sample 2 Figure 1. Mean grade point average for three categories of college major. 89 26 4 A-‘A Males o--o Females e—-e Both 25 4 0 24 8 o D 3 23 v! Q o o. 8 22 c2 21 20 , - c - 4 Humanities Social Natural Humanities Social Natural Science Science Science Science Sample 1 Sample 2 Figure 2. Mean American College Test composite score for three categories of college major. 90 TABLE 7 Analyses of Variance for Differences between the Sexes and among Three Categories of College Major in Grade Point Average and American College Test Composite Score Grade Point Average Sample 1 Sample 2 Source df MS P df MS F Major 2 .9138 3.5997 2 .0925 .4204 Sex 1 3.5205 13.8689* 44 1 2.2546 10.2521 Interaction 4 2 .2919 1.1501 2 .0644 .2928 Within cells 4 144 .253843 44 144 .219918 American College Test Composite Score Sample 1 (N-88) Sample 2'2fi=1173""' Source 4 df MS F fijdf MS F Major 2 100.1553 6.8136 44 2 26.2889 1.5357 Sex 1 .0040 .0003 1 4.5179 .2639 Interaction 4 2 14.2093 .9667 2 15.7231 .9184 Within cells 4 82 14.699344 “ 111 17.119147 91 Test of Hypothesis la “la Differences will exist in the degree of field-dependence- independence as measured by group mean scores on the Group Embedded Figures Test among humanities, social science, and natural science majors with natural science majors scoring highest (most field-independent) followed by social science, then humanities majors. Table 8 presents the mean GEFT scores obtained by the three groups of college major and Figure 3 is a graphic representation of these mean scores and the individual cell mean scores by sex and major. Table 9 presents the ANOVA table for Hypotheses la and lb. Although the specified alpha levels were achieved for both . samples on the main effect by major and the null hypothesis was rejected, the alternate could not be accepted. The results were only partially in the predicted direction. TABLE 8 Mean Scores on the Group Embedded Figures Test for Three Categories of College Major Humanities 4 Social Science 4 Natural Science Sample 1 4 11.3 11.2 14.3 Sample 2 4 13.8 4 11.7 13.6 Tukey post hoc comparisons were performed as shown in Table 10. The results indicate that differences exist between natural science majors and both humanities and social science majors in Sample 1 and between social science and both humanities and natural science majors in Sample 2. Therefore, in both samples, a difference between social science and natural science majors was upheld in the predicted direction, 92 15 4 Ar-AMales o-"o Females e——~Both ‘5 l4 4 g 0 13 4 H 8 E 12 ll 10 ‘ ‘ . ¢ 4v v : Humanities Social Natural Humanities Social Natural Science Science Science Science Sample 1 Sample 2 Figure 3. Mean scores on the Group Embedded Figures Test for three categories of college major. TABLE 9 Test of Hypotheses la and lb: Analyses of Variance for Differences among Three Categories of College Major and between Sexes on the Group Embedded Figures Test —— Sample 1 Sample 2 Source df MS —iFT MS F Major 2 157.1267 7.8407** 4 63.2267 4.0598* Sex 1 : 1.5 .0749 20.1667 1.2949 Interaction 4 2 6.14 .3064 7.7867 .5000 Within cells 4 144 4 20.04 4 15.573889 ** p<.01 93 natural science majors being more field-independent. However, humanities majors scored higher than social science majors in both.samp1es and significantly so in the second sample contrary to prediction. TABLE 10 Confidence Intervals Based on Tukey Post Hoc Comparisons for Hypothesis la: Differences among Three Categories of College Major on the Group Embedded Figures Test Sample 1 Sample 2 diff;’- Confidence d’ er- on ence ence q interval ence q ginterval fill - YSS - e2 1 Zell ( 2.31, -1e91) 4‘ 2e1 t 1e87 ( 3e97. e23)* ii -‘i,s - -2.8 3 2.11 (- .69, -a.91)*.4 .2 t 1.87 < 2.07. -1.67) ‘88“ i4. ' '3-0 t 2-11 (' o89- -5-11>* -1.9 t 1.87 (- .03. -3.77)* * p <1.05 Test of Hypothesis 1b Hlb Differences will exist in the degree of field-dependence- independence as measured by group mean scores on the Group Embedded Figures Test between males and females with males being higher (more field-dependent). Table 11 presents the mean GEFT scores obtained by the two sexes and Table 9 presents the F values indicating no differences were found between the sexes. It should be noted, however, that the differences observed between males and females are in the predicted direction, the males' scores being higher. 94 TABLE 11 Mean Scores on the Group Embedded Figures Test for the Two Sexes 4 Males 4 Females Sample 1 4 12.4 o 12.2 5.391. 2 ll 13.4 {T 12o? Test of Hypothesis lc ch Differences will exist in reflection-impulsivity as measured by the group mean time score on the Matching Familiar Figures Test among humanities, social science, and natural science majors with natural science majors scoring longer times (more reflective) followed by social science, than humanities majors. Table 12 presents the mean MFF time scores in seconds for the three categories of college major and Figure 4 is a graphic representa- tion of these mean scores and the individual cell mean scores by sex and major. Table 13 presents the ANOVA tables for the test of the hypothesis. TABLE 12 Mean Time Scores (in seconds) on the Matching Familiar Figures Test for Three Categories of College Major Humanities A Social Science 4 Natural Science 38-910 1 1 581s 36 632s 7‘ 1 678a 68 Sample 2 4 563.99 J 569.47 647.57 Though the differences were in the predicted direction in both samples, in neither sample were they significant. 9S A 800 4 Af'flMales .3 o--oFemales 8 o——eBoth .4 5: 700 4 O m up. " 600 . 5 500 1, .1 - -- - Humanities Social Natural Humanities Social Natural Science Science Science Science Sample 1 Sample 2 Figure 4. Mean time scores (in seconds) on the Matching Familiar Figures Test for three categories of college major. TABLE 13 Test of Hypotheses 1c and 1d: Analyses of Variance for Differences among Three Categories of College Major and between the Sexes on the Time Score of the Matching Familiar Figures Test Sample 1 Sample 2 Source df 4 MS F 4 MS F Major 4 2 ‘ 118513.0867 1.2990 44 109293.8067 \ 1.2395 Sex 1 83544.0000 .9157 44 19.0817 .0002 Interaction 2 54667.3400 .5992 44 37704.1267 .4276 Within cells 144 91231.769722 88172.664028 Test of Hngthesis 1d "1d Differences will exist in reflection-impulsivity as measured by the group mean time score on the Matching Familiar Figures Test between males and females. 96 TABLE 14 Mean Time Scores (in seconds) on the Matching Familiar Figures Test for the Two Sexes Males Females Sample 1 4 654.53 4 607.33 Sample 2 4 593.32 4 594.03 Table 14 presents the MFF mean time scores for both sexes and Table 13 presents the ANOVA table for test of the hypothesis. For sex as for major, the null hypothesis was retained. Test of Hypothesis 1e H1. Differences will exist in reflection-impulsivity as measured by the group mean error score on the Matching Familiar Figures Test among humanities, social science, and natural science majors with natural science majors scoring fewer errors (more reflective) followed by social science, then humanities majors. Table 15 presents the mean MFF error scores for the three majors and Figure 5 is a graphic representation of these mean scores and the individual cell mean scores by sex and major. Table 16 presents the ANOVA table for Hypotheses 1e and 1f. TABLE 15 Mean Error Scores on the Matching Familiar Figures Test for Three Categories of College Major 4 Humanities 4 Social Science 4 Natural Science SWIC 1 1 10.36 1 8e60 1 6092 Sample 2 9.04 9.78 6.74 v___ 97 12 Lyn-A Males o—-o Females e--e Both 11 r 10 3 E 9 m 8 7 \ 6 - e a - - Q0: Humanities Social Natural Humanities Social Natural Science Science Science Science Sample 1 Sample 2 Figure 5. Mean error scores on the Matching Familiar Figures Test for three categories of college major. TABLE 16 Test of Hypotheses 1e and 1f: Analyses of Variance Differences among Three Categories of College Major and between the Sexes on the Error Score of the Matching Familiar Figures Test ~—~ Sample 1 Sample 2 Source df 4 MS F MS F Major 4 2 4 147.9461 3.8613" 44 125.6600 3.389S* Sex 1 18.0267 .4705 44 15.3600 .4143 Interaction 2 30.9067 .8066 10.1400 .2735 Within cells 4 144 4 38.3150 44 37.072778 * p<.03 98 From Table 16 it can be seen that the null hypothesis of main effect by major was rejected. Table 17 presents the Tukey post hoc comparisons for this main effect. TABLE 17 Confidence Intervals Based on Tukey Post Hoe Comparisons for Hypothesis 1e: Differences among Three Categories of College Major on the Error Score of the Matching Familiar Figures Test Sample 1 Sample 2 ‘differ- Confidence differ- Confidence ence Interval ence Interval in - :88 . 1s76 t 2e9‘ ((0.70, -1.18) 11 - e7‘ : 2.89 (2015, -3063) i3 ‘ ins ' 3.55 1’2.9b (6.38, .50)* 44 2.30 + 2.89 (5.19, ‘ .59) £88 " ins . 1e68 t 2e9‘ (4.62, -1e26) 3e“ 1’ 2e89 (5093' e15)* * p auuxm an.“ - oo.a~ oo.oa on oo.a~ No.8 au oo.a~ nuuaooe e.uu..ouunH oo.~ o na.n oo.n a nn.m , oo.« o nn.n .uuxeaea eouuo>auuuu so.~ o oo.a an.n an co.“ co.” m . oo.a nonsense eouuo>ouuen as.“ an nu.c~. oo.aa on an.e~ no.o cu nu.q~ ...uaaueH caucus-tuna an.c n3 no.oa so.~ o no.a~ so.n an so.oa .uuuaaunm e.»u.>ouu:~. no.~ a oo.n no. N ao.n . so.s m oo.m . ....aom eouunsauuxm oe.o we nn.- as.n an nn.~a . ao.~ g nn.- . .uo.=.m e.uu.»ouuem u o m u o u u of m somehow Henmuez oocsucm deacon soundness: ocean-cu N on. a ..aa-um one pone: .m.~aoo 11 no eeauomoueo conch maroon» Acouuocmm uncommon cums.oomu«uu< ucaom scene one .suoom suaooaeou neon omoauou ceuuwom< .esnSeeez oaaum o>auucmoo o>dm co owouusucu unscaeoa one subscom ordnance cessusc esucswomuaa now ecceuue> mo seemnece .on use .vn .cn .cn .en consensus: as ussh on man< uouom scene one .suocm unaccomoo coon smeauoo cabana-c Sour-es: 33m 253560 2.: co ensues» sneer-co one apex—35. unscaacc cesausc ssccswoumua you sucsuue> uo esehnec< .oe one .ve .oe .ce .ee eoeecuonas no uses an mdnon vuicceum use aces: Hm mqndh 149 meoc. m0~0.~ : dame. h0~h.~ anon. Nnma.N #000. 0mmo.~ . <00 wa00.n cm~0.n~ e «dam.e n000.n~ .. I UIHU< d0m0.c o~a~.~n Nn~m.0 mnnm.m~ 00n0.7 Hnm0.~n 000n.0 0000.0N condo an: 00N~.N sawm.a ~00n.~ nnnm.c 0mma.~ n¢.eaamw—Asmrunsbr64~sres~suenau.SAaNmesqunoeuenoatun:arse-s.6 09 07 07 16 03 12 13 16 14 00 05 13 16 18 05 08 07 17 11 17 12 08 14 14 01 07 18 11 09 16 13 15 13 16 10 06 11 12 10 16 09 11 18 18 17 08 03 03 11 17 0835.0 0913.5 1159.0 0883.0 0285.5 0166.5 0633.5 0487.5 0966.5 0448.5 0614.0 0526.0 1107.5 0499.5 0874.0 0162.5 0845.5 0514.0 0322.0 0251.5 0330.5 0617.5 0560.5 0472.0 0266.0 0465.0 0633.0 0349.0 0576.5 0206.0 0547.0 0609.5 0800.5 0453.5 0506.5 0393.5 0388.5 0858.0 0482.5 0791.5 0681.0 0806.5 0167.5 0758.5 0611.5 0286.5 0534.5 0231.5 1512.5 1164.5 04 05 00 00 20 15 12 12 02 17 05 06 09 10 03 19 10 13 14 15 09 09 28 11 13 04 07 21 06 16 07 09 07 10 12 09 03 .07 09 02 O8 04 16 03 06 35 13 02 05 03 02 02 07 03 03 06 06 06 03 06 07 07 03 02 01 02 08 04 08 05 03 02 01 03 01 07 03 04 05 07 06 02 04 06 07 06 05 01 05 05 03 06 09 04 02 02 01 06 07 41 38 28 31 22 32 30 32 34 34 31 34 37 34 33 30 16 28 29 35 24 39 34 41 49 35 31 28 43 25 29 21 23 21 34 38 27 30 32 39 30 32 27 23 30 46 29 45 24 35 26 25 27 27 27 22 21 21 23 22 16 28 23 18 15 24 17 25 28 24 28 23 23 26 22 23 28 13 10 2.69 3.03 2.46 3.24 2.68 3.63 3.39 3.55 2.45 2.29 2.26 3.56 3.16 3.17 3.41 2.52 2.08 2.19 3.71 2.06 3.16 2.45 3.07 2.45 2.40 2.42 3.58 2.45 3.00 3.54 3.69 3.69 3.71 3.00 2.17 2.99 2.25 1.86 2.47 2.29 3.08 3.25 3.32 2.89 2.10 2.44 2.76 2.36 2.26 2.55 11 22 23 21 21 22 18 20 22 22 20 20 20 19 22 24 21 20 23 17 21 25 21 19 26 22 21 22 22 21 20 21 19 19 24 22 24 24 20 21 21 21 21 20 26 22 21 26 24 23 Cohmm Sample 1 continued HWNNNNwr-wHwNNwNo-uwwwNNwmwwwNwaNNNNwHuH-Hur‘uNWv—HHNr-w NNHNHHNNNNF‘F‘NNHNNO-‘NO-‘Nl-‘NI-‘NNNNNI—NNNHNHHNNNHNHNNF‘HHHN FWNUUNJ‘NNNNNNJ‘NHWNNJ‘J-‘NNNWJ‘#HHNbNHNNNHHw-bNI-‘NNJ‘NNNHJ‘ 15 07 13 00 17 10 12 10 14 18 17 04 11 13 13 16 O3 14 10 02 18 08 12 18 17 14 06 15 18 14 16 14 18 17 00 09 15 14 05 14 09 08 02 18 07 17 16 17 16 15 0638.0 0878.0 0840.5 0515.5 1297.5 0145.5 0725.0 0787.5 0431.0 0230.5 0525.5 0358.5 0565.0 1386.5 0821.0 0574.0 0818.5 0771.0 0226.0 0642.5 0821.5 0678.5 0331.5 0709.5 0222.5 0585.0 0752.5 0901.0 0662.0 0320.5 0412.5 0425.5 0749.5 0478.5 0320.5 0304.5 0667.5 0831.0 0366.5 0425.5 0444.0 0911.5 0459.5 0868.0 0676.0 0245.5 0613.0 0204.0 0824.0 0813.0 09 08 02 21 02 33 06 07 07 13 04 05 09 04 02 07 10 04 20 19 05 02 09 03 13 08 04 11 03 09 07 11 12 06 17 09 05 03 14 04 06 04 15 02 05 12 03 14 03 03 04 O7 03 03 04 01 04 03 06 05 08 03 05 00 05 03 03 03 00 03 09 07 04 03 05 07 02 03 05 06 06 09 07 04 00 02 05 02 04 04 05 03 00 05 04 02 06 03 03 06 29 28 32 30 32 29 34 21 35 38 35 27 42 31 39 21 32 44 25 46 29 27 22 39 44 33 25 38 19 36 44 31 28 32 31 31 32 43 47 27 42 33 32 21 33 37 39 26 47 30 27 18 29 19 27 16 15 22 22 24 23 33 27 14 26 20 21 24 27 19 27 22 28 22 16 28 20 25 26 21 19 29 19 26 10 3.52 2.71 3.16 2.13 1.97 2.65 2.61 2.20 3.12 2.44 4.00 3.46 2.21 2.70 2.33 2.63 3.62 2.72 2.32 3.25 3.72 3.75 2.47 3.71 3.13 3.08 2.26 3.79 3.67 2.66 2.67 3.98 3.62 3.30 2.72 2.68 2.71 2.15 2.91 3.08 2.62 2.84 3.21 3.17 2.05 2.50 3.65 2.58 2.69 2.56 11 20 24 22 34 20 21 21 23 20 20 18 20 22 25 25 21 22 22 19 35 21 20 21 19 27 21 22 20 22 19 22 20 20 22 21 19 20 20 20 20 21 21 23 21 19 26 21 19 Cohmm Sample 2 HNNHU’NNNWNNWNHWwwr-‘NUWWNNWHWHHNHWNWNHWHNWHHHNNWWNNW NNNNNNHNNNP‘NNNP‘HNNNNHNNNNHHHHFHNHNNNHNHHHHHHHHNHNH N¢waHwNNHHbeHHbebubmrmbwwbv—wbbbNNbv-wo-‘bmmwbwb##‘H 15 06 17 15 17 14 07 14 05 09 17 16 14 11 11 15 13 14 10 18 02 16 18 17 13 07 17 05 12 08 08 12 16 12 10 13 17 12 17 06 16 14 11 18 O7 18 17 11 04 18 0172.0 0352.5 0920.0 0311.0 0794.0 0740.0 0893.0 0362.5 0297.0 0542.5 0733.5 0259.0 0271.5 0524.0 0736.0 0073.0 0378.5 0993.0 1149.0 0705.0 0964.5 0531.5 0190.0 0204.0 0480.5 0382.0 0653.5 0616.0 0489.5 1034.5 1293.5 0464.0 0741.5 0387.5 0255.0 1134.0 0927.5 0670.5 0478.0 0449.5 0646.0 0248.0 0463.0 0386.0 0709.5 0865.0 0420.5 0837.0 154 08 12 04 17 03 O4 08 07 03 06 03 12 11 09 10 30 11 00 01 02 10 02 32 10 08 18 02 06 05 00 03 05 05 06 17 01 02 07 09 20 07 00 12 06 11 04 03 07 14 00 03 01 04 04 06 05 02 08 05 00 05 03 03 01 07 04 06 05 03 09 04 03 03 05 05 06 06 02 00 04 03 03 05 06 05 07 06 04 07 04 03 09 06 02 02 08 09 04 04 05 26 45 31 33 32 31 28 28 27 18 27 38 37. 31 42 37 31 33 24 22 25 35 39 33 33 35 36 34 36 38 37 26 38 30 24 34 37 42 36 29 31 12 37 25 37 32 3O 29 36 24 12 29 26 24 19 14 19 21 21 16 26 27 31 28 23 22 26 18 20 17 25 16 17 29 20 24 28 26 21 16 25 25 21 15 29 10 2.02 2.35 3.48 2.48 3.54 2.99 2.05 2.37 2.28 3.24 2.14 2.30 2.79 2.54 2.38 2.92 2.77 3.86 3.00 3.29 2.60 2.39 1.96 3.08 3.80 3.10 3.27 2.45 2.78 2.55 2.82 3.91 3.08 3.46 3.08 3.52 2.54 2.82 3.27 2.86 2.93 3.14 3.79 2.44 2.56 3.08 3.59 2.15 2.76 3.03 11 21 23 19 19 19 22 21 20 20 21 20 17 19 21 20 19 21 20 19 20 20 22 22 20 19 19 25 18 17 26 20 22 19 21 20 21 22 21 19 19 30 20 19 27 21 20 23 20 18 20 Cohmm Sample 2 continued 1 2 HHNWWr—HwHer-wwwwmwwNNNNNNwNwHNr-vNwHHHwHNwWNHNr-HHNWL» NF‘NNHHNHNHl-‘NNO-‘l-‘Ni-‘NHNO-‘l-‘HNNHHNHHHHHNNNHHNNHF‘HNHNHNNN NN-beI-‘J‘HWHNJFNNJ‘Pr-‘br-‘NNJ‘J‘J‘J‘UJNHNNNNJ‘J‘NN“bbNWNbNWF-‘J‘Nbb 16 13 09 16 12 03 05 14 10 14 15 18 12 13 15 10 14 18 18 17 13 12 05 12 06 08 08 14 17 O6 15 17 16 07 15 17 17 13 15 16 16 15 17 18 16 09 17 07 15 12 0638.0 0786.5 0524.0 0500.5 0831.0 1122.0 0353.0 0244.5 0356.0 0254.0 0457.5 0267.0 1142.5 1171.0 0586.5 0504.5 0210.5 0660.0 0258.0 0116.0 0784.0 0348.5 0603.5 0186.0 0274.0 0559.5 0532.0 0423.0 0913.5 0354.0 0836.0 0290.0 0884.5 0503.5 0938.0 0677.0 1356.5 0526.5 0302.5 0758.0 0752.0 0254.5 0408.5 0602.0 0557.5 0653.0 1593.0 0449.5 155 08 04 08 06 04 11 05 11 21 12 05 09 01 08 08 09 28 09 14 25 03 14 01 19 16 13 16 05 05 13 17 02 06 07 04 09 08 01 08 12 02 13 11 09 06 06 03 00 05 16 08 04 05 05 04 03 01 04 05 07 03 08 05 03 03 04 00 07 09 00 04 02 03 03 07 08 01 04 03 02 04 07 03 00 04 04 05 04 05 06 04 03 01 06 06 04 04 08 06 05 34 32 37 22 42 39 31 29 27 23 36 28 38 21 23 22 28 31 33 17 34 32 34 45 30 26 23 25 37 36 28 36 33 36 30 29 32 24 36 35 32 19 34 22 33 33 34 35 31 35 19 25 25 23 21 17 25 24 24 30 28 26 22 23 25 19 20 20 19 27 29 19 20 19 22 21 27 25 25 23 17 25 29 25 24 26 28 28 20 10 2.15 2.92 3.47 3.00 2.23 2.55 2.44 3.14 2.29 2.32 3.22 3.62 2.79 2.12 2.65 2.86 3.30 2.58 2.45 2.54 2.73 2.93 2.62 2.80 3.14 3.62 2.87 3.82 2.90 3.11 3.32 2.45 3.20 3.39 2.83 2.82 3.44 3.72 3.52 2.81 3.00 2.60 2.44 3.35 3.36 2.73 3.59 3.33 3.02 3.61 11 20 20 20 22 22 22 21 19 20 21 18 21 21 21 21 21 33 22 21 23 18 19 18 21 22 19 20 19 20 19 26 27 21 21 22 21 21 19 20 21 21 20 22 21 21 21 21 20 21 21 Cohmm Sample 2 continued WNNNWWNP‘WNWHUNHHNWPNHHWUWPWPHWNNHNNNNNWL’JWHWHFNHNHH HNNHNNNNHHHMF‘P‘NHHHNHHHNNNNNNNNHNNHHNHNHHHHNNHNF‘P‘NF‘ WNNNNNNwwv-I#Hkt-‘UJJ‘UJNv-‘HbHJ‘HwwaJ‘NbNNNJ‘4‘##NHNHJ‘HNNJ‘vL‘NJ‘ 18 03 18 17 14 14 11 15 13 17 12 13 14 10 14 17 16 12 12 08 18 18 17 16 10 12 12 09 O7 18 14 16 17 16 14 10 07 09 12 09 15 16 11 07 17 18 18 12 14 15 0597.0 0803.0 0414.0 0440.0 0328.0 0462.0 1085.5 0389.5 1075.5 0683.5 0753.0 0898.0 0719.0 0479.0 0666.5 0626.5 0193.0 0781.0 0948.5 0607.0 0787.0 0337.0 0520.5 0512.5 0772.0 1078.5 0389.0 0363.5 0506.0 0541.5 0749.5 0386.0 0886.5 0567.0 0563.5 0425.5 1028.0 0319.5 0790.5 1348.0 0232.0 0455.5 0330.5 0873.0 0124.0 0126.0 1037.5 0284.5 0709.5 156 05 09 04 20 13 03 02 08 03 10 08 04 09 16 01 13 12 05 02 18 00 15 06 06 15 03 03 12 08 11 09 11 09 02 05 04 17 05 19 07 00 14 11 07 00 17 12 11 13 12 06 05 06 02 07 02 07 00 04 04 04 06 05 02 06 04 09 04 04 06 03 05 06 06 06 03 07 09 03 04 O4 03 06 02 05 03 03 02 05 02 04 06 06 02 05 10 06 07 06 03 41 42 35 4o 30 35 32 32 32 24 36 43 37 27 3o 33 38 31 24 31 24 37 28 25 37 37 27 35 31 27 35 36 28 28 3o 32 38 33 38 33 3o 22 3o 27 44 36 48 33 35 23 25 17 23 21 16 26 22 23 27 14 24 24 25 27 22 23 26 24 29 21 23 23 28 26 20 23 25 27 20 23 23 17 19 24 23 25 11 27‘ 17 27 17 23 24 10 2.45 2.61 2.47 2.25 3.22 3.18 3.20 2.97 2.66 2.85 2.38 3.77 3.13 2.75 4.00 2.19 3.11 3.36 3.03 2.31 3.86 2.94 3.79 2.70 3.25 3.00 3.28 2.64 2.73 3.13 3.18 3.47 2.39 2.27 2.38 2.47 2.31 2.86 2.13 2.62 3.10 2.44 3.09 2.85 3.85 2.31 3.28 2.51 3.24 2.66 11 22 22 31 22 20 21 19 19 18 21 20 21 21 19 19 21 20 20 21 22 19 20 20 20 18 21 19 20 21 20 20 19 21 25 22 19 20 21 20 21 21 21 21 21 19 21 20 20 19 20 APPENDIX H INTERCORRELATION BETHEEN THE FIVE COGNITIVE STYLE MEASURES AND GRADE POINT AVERAGE HITH AND HITHOUT AMERICAN COLLEGE TEST COMPOSITE SCORE IN SAMPLE 1 AND SAHPLE 2 157 158 .86.Va .3 3 . V a a 88.H QNONQO.I tsamsAN . «momma... downs . tamMOmN . (mu accostn A¢h°~0.l nmdhdo. mmmcuo... HHORO... .250 RH: 6868; .888... £83.. 3338. 66.6.86 :2 888; 322:... .332: .68... 2:: 888; 5:8. .83 2:: 88°04 Emu sue ...-do .53 go 9H3 swank. E OI...» mm: Hume 1‘1. ouoom 0362—80 noon. .9on 53...! 52.3: .— 93..» no» 2633033933 a: 3&5. 159 39V.— 15 3.V .— .3 «9V .— .3 OO.A aicnodonn. oughno.l somOON. moanma.l Geneva. #NcNonN. (he oooooo.u chcmO.l {flanhsnn. iflmcmocn.l iwnhcnfl. itNmHHNn. UBHO< oooooc.d whacwo. OAMQNO. mmcomo.l €¢00h0.l OOOAO 8H3 ococco.d {#50005N.1 nocmflo. inn°~m~. unouhbo PH: occooo.d {itowodao.o oamand.l oubuhb uh! ocoocc.u ancwoo. «luv uh! oocooo.d Hume <~0 Olflu< coado HHI HOIBHOG 8H3 aubuub hm: Old» but Hume 9.58 03.2.80 :3. .0038 53.8.4 and: a 03:8 no» 2833953935 «2 mAn