WWI r w 1 MM”)! 'L‘HE EFFECTS G5 VERBAL ENSHUCHONS 0N iMITATWE AGGiRfiSSiGé‘é Wiilliliflh 1 w W —lw_; I-bO (0010) Thesis {or ”W Dogma of DH. D. MKCHEGAN STATE {INE'V'ERSITY Gilbert Wiliiams DeRath 1963 THESIS LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the thesis entitled The Effects of Verbal Instructions on Irnitative Aggression presented by Gilbert Williams De Rath has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph, D. ._ degree in—ES¥.chongy m/{My Major, professor/w Date / 5’15“; [yé k3 0-169 ABSTRACT THE EFFECTS OF VERBAL INSTRUCTIONS ON IMITATIVE AGGRESSION by Gilbert Williams De Rath Sixty nursery school boys were randomly assigned to three con- ditions. Subjects in one experimental group individually observed a five-minute film sequence of an aggressive model; a second experi- mental group received, during the film presentation, prohibitive verbal instructions regarding specific aggressive acts which the film model directed at an inflated clown. A control group did not see the film and received no instructions of any kind. Half of the subjects in each-of the groups were randomly as signed to a neutral female experi- menter in a doll play test situation which followed mild frustration designed to‘elicit aggressive behavior. Subjects were observed in doll play for a 20-minute period during which ratings were made of their behavior at five-second intervals. Over half of the subjects were rated by two independent raters in order to determine rater reliability. -Ratings were made on 27 different behaviors which were broadly. classi- fied as Imitative Aggression, Partially Imitative Aggression, Non- Imitative Aggression, Non-Aggression, and Imitative Verbal Aggression. The latter category was eliminated because of poor sound transmission and the lack of intelligibility in the speech of these subjects. It was predicted that boys exposed to an aggressive model would, following a frustrating experience, reproduce the aggressive behaviors of the model and would differ in this respect both from boys prohibited Gilbert Williams De Rath from performing such acts and boys not exposed to the model. A second hypothesis was that boys who were verbally prohibited from performing the aggressive actions of the model would inhibit these acts in subsequent doll play. The third hypothesis concerned the generali- zation of effects of exposure to an aggressive model and the generali— zation of prohibiting instructions. It was expected that following frustration, subjects not receiving inhibiting instructions would display more aggression in the presence of the frustrator than in the presence of a more neutral figure of a different sex. Finally, it was expected that the effects of verbal prohibition on imitating of the model's aggre- sive behavior would be greater in the presence of the person invoking the prohibition than in the presence of a neutral experimenter. The responses scored involved highly specific concrete classes of behavior and yielded high inter-scorer reliabilities, the rank correlation coefficients being in the . 903. Subjects seeing the film without instructions clearly imitated the aggressive behaviors of the model following the mild frustration experi- ence. These differences were generally highly significant. Those subjects who were prohibited during the presentation-of the film were inhibited in imitating aggressive behaviors displayed earlier by the model. As a test of generalization of the effects of exposure to the film and as a test of the generalization of prohibitive instruction, half of the subjects in each group were exposed to a "neutral" experimenter in doll play. The highly significant differences between the experimental groups on imitative aggression remains, indicating that both-effects readily generalize. Subjects who viewed the film without prohibitive instructions displayed more aggression, both imitative and non-imitative, Gilbert Williams De Rath in the presence of the experimenter'whofrustrated them. The difference ‘ is highly significant for non-irnitative aggression,“ but only approaches significance for imitative aggression. It was expected that the effects of verbal prohibitions would be greater in the presence of the person invoking the prohibition. However, the differences here were not significant, both groups receiving pro- hibitive instructions showing little imitative behavior. The results were discussed in terms of a learning theory of dis- placement rather than other theories utilizing such concepts as "identifi- cation with the aggressor" or "defensive identification. " Approved MM Major Prfis sor Date ’5/yfv/T /7é37 THE EFFECTS OF VERBALINSTRUCTIONS ON IMITATIVE AGGRESSION BY Gilbert Williams De Rath A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCT OR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1963 G 52qu 5L 7/8/64 To Gib and Rick ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENT . Appreciation-is accorded to all those who were helpful in the design, data gathering, and writing of this dissertation. Foremost I wish to thank themembers of my committee: - Dr. Charles Hanley, .Chairman, Dr. Norman Abeles, Dr. Alfred G. Dietze, and Dr. Bill L. Kell. Appreciation is also. expressed to Dr. Clarence L.. Winder who served as chairman during the early planning of the disserataion and who maintained a helpful interest while away from Michigan State University. Grateful appreciation is due the following: Miss Roseann Bulgarella, Miss Martha G. Andrews, and Miss SandraCotter who served as raters and whose enthusiasm was always encouraging. Miss Betty Garlick, Miss Mariella Aikman, Mrs. Vera Borosage, and Mrs. JoAnn ‘Lifshin, teachers at Spartan Cooperative‘Nursery School, who took a personal interest in preparing and providing the children whoserved as subjects. To the children themselves I want to express my thanks for their help. Dr. Robert E. McMichael who provided the transparent mirror ‘andtfurniture used in the experiment. Dr. Albert Bandura of Stanford University who'provided the film and sound track and whose studies were used as a model [for the research. Dr.'. Alice C. Thorpe, Chairman of the Home, Management and Child Development Department, who- was very helpful in obtaining 'Space for the. experiment and giving permission to use the Spartan Cooperative Nursery School facilities. Mrs. Gloria De Rathlwho assisted as a rater, typist, and proof- reader. Her continued support and understanding is gratefully acknowledged. *********** iii TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION 0 O O O O O 0 O O O I O O I O O Theories of Aggression . . . . . . . . . The Frustration-Aggression Hypothesis Displacement of Aggression. . . Imitative Aggression . . . . . . Hypotheses. . . . ... . . . . . MET HOD O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 subjects 0 O O O I O O I O O O O 0 Experimental Design . . . . . . Experimental Conditions . . . . Prohibitive Instructions . . 'Aggression Arousal. . . . . . . D011 Play. O O O O O O O O I O O O O O O 0 Behavior Measures ........... RESULTSo'eoooooo0000000000000 Rater'Reliability . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Imitation of the Model's Behavior . . . . . Effects of Verbal Instructions. . . . . . . Generalization of Imitative Aggression . . Generalization of Prohibitive Instructions DISCUSSION 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 SUMMARY 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O . O O O O O 0 REFERENCES 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I APPENDIC ES 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 iv Page ONIU‘UJH 12 12 12 13 14 15 18 22 26 26 28 30 32 33 34 41 44 48 LIST OF TABLES ‘ Page Allocation of SubjectstoExperimental Groups . . . . . . A Rank CorrelationCoefficients for Raters A, B, and C on the Three Response Categories . . . . . . . . . . . . Mean Scores for the Experimental and Control Subjects on the'Three Major Response Categories . . . . . . . . Significance of Differences (H-Test) Between Experi- mental Groups and Control Groups in the Expression of Imitative and Non-Imitative Aggression . . . . . . . . . Significance of Differences Between Groups in the Expression of Aggression (Mann-Whitney Test) . . . . . Significance of Differences in Expression of Aggression and Experimenters in Doll Play. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13 27 29 29 30 32 LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE Page 1. Room used in frustration situation . . . . . . . . . . . 16 2. Room used in frustration situation . . . . . . . . . . . l6 3. Room used in frustration situation . . . . . . . . . . . l7 4. Room used in frustration situation . . . . . . . . . . . 17 5. Room used in final doll play session . . . . . . . . . . 20 6. Room used in final doll play session . . . . . . . . . . 20 7. Room used in final doll play session . . . . . . . . . . 211‘ 8. Room used in final doll play session . . . . . . . . . . 21 vi LIST OF APPENDICES APPENDIX Page A. Raw Scores of Subjects Seeing the Film Without Prohibitive Instructions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 B. Raw Scores of Subjects Seeing the Film With "Prohibitive Instructions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50 C. Raw Scores of Control Group. . . . . . . . . . . . 51 D. Total Scores and Ranks on the Three‘Major Scor- ing Categories Assigned by Rater A and his co-Rateroooo'ooooooo...ooooooooo 52 E. Total Scores and Ranks on the Three Major Scor- ing Categories Assigned by Rater B and his co-Rateroeoooooocoococoon-coo. 53 F. Total Scores and Ranks on the Three Major Scor- ing Categories Assigned by Rater C and his CO'Rater00000000000000.0000... 54 G. Total Scores and Ranks on the Three Major Scor- ing Categories Assigned by Combined Raters A, B, and C and Their Co-Raters D and E . . . . . . 55 THE EFFECTS OF VERBAL INSTRUCTIONS ON IM‘ITATIVE AGGRESSION INTRODUCTION This is a study of the effects of verbal instructions onimodeled aggressive behavior in nursery school boys. Studies in the area of aggression are important because of the critical role it plays in the process of socialization. The handling of aggression in children is a major problem area forvmany parents and is often the most frequent reasonfor referral in agencies treating children with behavior problems. Recently several studies have appeared which show that iInitation and modeling play important roles in the acquisition and expression of aggression. Other investigations suggest that the way to handle aggression is to xmake it abundantly clear through verbal means that the behavior in question is undesirable. . The experimental procedure from the studies of modeling and aggression is combined with verbal instruc- tions in the present investigation. Theorie s of Aggres sion Aggression has given rise to-a tremendous volume of theory particularly since Freud, impressed by the sadistic and masochistic elements he found in all neurosis and by the events of World War I, viewed aggression as "an innate, independent instinctual disposition in man" (1950) that he called the death wish. Its object is the death‘and destruction of life. It forms the basis for masochism and, when turned outward, sadism. Freud's "new" instinct theory has met with consider— able criticism (see'Monroe, 1955). VBut many psychoanalysts have 1 followed Freud's theoretical scheme. ~Melanie Klein (1937) writes: The idea of an infant from six to 12 months trying to destroy its mother by every method at the disposal of its body, trans- formed in imagination into all kinds of dangerous weapons-- 1 presents a horrifying, not to say unbelievable picture to'our minds. . . . But the abundance, force and the multiplicity of the-imaginary cruelties which accompany these cravings are displayed before our eyes in early analysis so clearly and forcibly that they leave no room for doubt (pp. 187-188). Other analysts feel that emphasis on instinctive features of hostility and aggression with their negative emotional experiences is one-sided. Among those who criticize the idea of an aggressive instinct are Harry Stack Sullivan, Sandor Ferenczi, Thomas M.. French,- Karen Horney, ..Clara Thompson, and Paul Schilder. In general, they take a more positive approach and stress the adaptive aspects of aggression. Schilder (1949), i for example, talks about an instinct to action and mastery and holds that the tendency tocenstruct and reconstruct cannot be separated from action and aggression. Schilder and some other theorists seem-to dis- cuss aggression as embracing all positive outgoing actions. Another group of theorists stress the reactive aspects of hostile aggression, i. e. , behavior whose-ultimate goal is injurious to another. Where Freud thought that aggression was an instinctual drive, the ultimate goal of which was a static state free from all tensions, i. e. , a return to the womb, these theorists see the aim of aggression as a safeguard to the personality against the outer world. - Ackerman (1958) offers a convincing explanation for the striving for static equilibrium that Freud called the death instinct, writing: The striving for a static equilibrium can be understood as secondary process, a defensive avoidance of shock and frus- tration, or as an escape from psychic pain; that is, such behavior can be looked upon as an aspect of failure, defeat, or derailment of the life drive, rather than as a primary death instinct. In any case, the motivational constellations that Freud identified with his death instinct point dramatically to‘the con- nection between a striving for static equilibrium and retreat from life. The homeostatic process actuates and protects life. It does not foster fear and withdrawal from life (p. 72). In discussing the homeostatic process, Ackerman departs from the usual concept of stability and states that it is: . . . closer to the truth to conceive that the exact purpose of homeostasisis to protect, not stability in any static sense, but rather a creative but controlled "instability"* of the organism in consonance with the necessary conditions for maturation and for expanding relations with the outerworld. In other words, in the interpretation of this concept I am shifting the emphasis to the exact Opposite of the connotation of "staying the same. " I am sug- gesting that "homeostasis, " or the principle of dynamic equi- librium, signifies the capacity for creative, fluid adaptability to change, which at the same time assures that measure of coordi- nated control that prevents the organism from being overwhelmed by a barrage of stimuli in excess of the organism's capacity to accommodate. The principle thus reinterpreted is conceived as creative, controlled "instability"; it regulates response to experi- ence not in order tomaintain sameness, but rather to preserve a resilient capacity for change while preventing change from becom- ing too rapid--so rapid as to disintegrate resources for adaptation and growth. . . . I do not conceive aggression and destructiveness as the expression of a death instinct but rather as a derailment of healthy adaptation, a defense, a means of controlling environment, of counteracting frustration and anxiety, and of asserting the identity of the self in interpersonal situations (p. 70, p. 50). The Frustration-Aggression Hypothesis That aggression is a counteraction of frustration and anxiety has long been known. To name but a few, William James, McDougall and Freud, in their earlier writings, all noted that aggression was aroused by frustration. Before developing his theory of instincts, Freud regarded the tendency to seek pleasure and avoid pain as the basis mechanism in :9: "Instability" here is used to emphasize readiness for change. ~ all mental functioning. .Whenever pleasure seeking or pain avoiding behavior was blocked, aggression wasthe "primordial reaction" and was directed toward persons or objects in the external world which were perceived as causing the frustration. In 1939, Dollard, Doob, Miller, Mowrer and Sears presented their classical work on frustration and aggression as viewed in a learn- ing theory framework. Their efforts as systematization and objectivity havelmade the study of aggression more accessible to experimental study. Since their early work, one writer (McNeil, 1959) has stated that studies in this area number well over a thousand of which at least 400 are experimental. The learning theory orientation such as that used by Dollard et al gives greater attention to the origins of individual differences in the amount and quality of aggressive behavior. -According to Sears (1961) who points out that the learning model: . . . makes no assumption about a quantum of internal insti- gation that must be uninhibitedly tapped. On the contrary, it presumes that whole classes of aggressive acts are learned as responses tovmany different stimulus situations. By stimulus generalization, these acts come to be evoked by great ranges of stimuli, both internal and external, including ones that are symbolic or have imaginal qualities only. By response generali- zations, acts can shift from one action category to another (e. g. , from physical to verbal) or from one psychological func- tion to another (e. g. , from overt action to imagination or- fantasy). ’ Indeed, the lability of expression is so great that even entirely novel stimulational settings and manipulanda seem able to elicit and provide a means of expression of aggressive acts. Each such setting has its own instigative value, deriving from direct rein- forcements in the past, as well as from primary or secondary stimulus generalizations. This effective instigative value is a cumulation of both excitatory and inhibitory values (p. 469). Dollard et al take as their point of departure the older notion that aggression is always a consequence of frustration. From this‘idea they develOpa series of psychological principles relating to the strength of instigation, the degree of inhibition, and the direction of aggression. Because the direct expression of aggression in our society isifrequently prevented from occurring by a strong anticipation of punishment, aggressive impulses must be compressed, delayed, disguised, dis- placed, or otherwise deflected. The energies behind aggressive acts are not lost by being deflected away from the frustrating object. Displac ement of Aggro s sion Miller has developed a model for predicting behavior in approach- avoidance conflict situations, including those where display and/or control of aggression are involved. ' Miller's (1948) assumptions; {which have been borne out in research, are as follows: (1) that the direct response to the original stimulus generalizes to other similar stimuli, with the amount of generalization becom- ing smaller the less similar the stimuli; (2) that the response which conflicts with the occurrence of the direct response to the original stimulus also generalizes to other similar stimuli, be- coming weaker the less similar the stimuli; (3) that the gradient of generalization of the conflicting response falls off more steeply with dissimilarity than does that of the original response which it inhibits; (4) that when two or more incompatible responses are simultaneously excited, the one with the greatest response will be its strength minus that of any response incompatible with it which is excited at the same time (pp- 167-168). In situations that involve aggression, the conflicting response tendencies are aggression and its inhibition fortfear of punishment: It has been noted that observations and interview estimates of aggression do-not correlate highly with fantasy aggression expressed in play (Kroner, 1951; Sears, 1950), correlations between these variables ranging from about .13 to . 21. Returning toMiller's assumptions, one would expect aggressive responses and fear of punishment in the home to generalize to school and play situations. - Situations will differ in their similarity to home conditions where the child is punished most directly for aggression. A school situation, because-teachers act as parental surrogates, will be closer to home on a stimulus generalization gradient than will a doll play situation. If the gradient of generalization for inhibiting aggressionfalls off more steeply, then the inhibition brought about by severe punishment would affect behavior toward the teacher; but not the doll play where it would increase aggression because of in- creased frustration. Sears (1951) divided preschool boys into three groups based on severity of punishment in the home. The group receiving moderate punishment in the home showed the most aggression in school. The High and Low Punishment groups both showed about the same frequency of aggression in school. In a doll play situation, however, the most severely punished group showed more aggression than either of the others. The fear of punishment did not generalize to the doll play situation, so displaced aggression was permitted to appear. A study by Hollenberg and Sperry (1951) lends support tothe frustration- aggression hypothesis and Miller's assumptions about the displacement of aggression. From interviews Hollenberg and Sperry derived a measure of home frustration based on number and kinds of restrictive rules, such as forcing of the child, and the like. ‘Mothers were divided into high and low frustration categories. Thirty children from these homes were observed in freedoll play and their aggressive responses measured. Children who were highly frustrated in the home showed more aggressive behavior in doll play. The differences between the high and low frustration groups, however, were not statistically significant. In general, while frustrationmay lead tofantasy aggression, these writers felt that other factors must alsobe considered. More conclusive results were obtained when another factor, the. severity of punishment for aggression the mothers inflicted on their children, was taken intoaconsideration. A measure of punishment for aggression based on an'interview-rating of frequency, intensity, and duration of spanking, threatening, isolation, denying privileges, and derogating the child was also available. Hollenberg and Sperry then subdivided each of the two original groups into a low and a high punishment group. Thus a given child might be, to use initials, Low P-Low F, Low P-High‘F, High‘P-Low F, or High P-High F. When this distinction is made, the difference among the groups, not significant when frustration alone was considered, became more clear-cut. High frustration and high punish- ment led to the greatest doll play aggression, low frustration and low punishment to the least. Imitative Aggre s sion In their book on social learning and imitation, Miller and Dollard (1941) developed the notion that changes in the child's social behavior can be explained on the basis of social imitation properly related to general principles of learning. Their reinforcement hypothesis is as follows: (1) the child is motivated to action by an internal drive; (2) he sees someone else satisfying a comparable drive in certain ways-~this pattern of behavior is a cue for an imitation response; (3) the child attempts to c0py this behavior and the response may be rewarded and thus reinforced. On subsequent occasions when motivated by this drive, he has a tendency to repeat the response which he previously imitated from another person's behavior. Miller and Dollard have presented a number of controlled observations and experiments which appear to support this hypothetical paradigm of the socialization process. While imitation or modeling can play a positive role in social development, it can also have deleterious effects depending on the type of behavior that is modeled. Newspaper accounts, for example, often mention children or teenagers learning deliquent behavior from tele- vision, comic books, or movies. In the home, parents serveas models of undesirable as well as desirable behavior. -A part of the reason for this‘would be incidental learning, i. e. , learning that takes place with- out intent or set to learn. Sears, Maccoby and Levin (1957) make the point that parents often automatically react aggressively to aggression in their children, who are more likely to be punished for aggression than for any other undesirable behavior. The children's aggressive behaviors can act as a frustrating stimulus for parents who respond with counter- aggression. Punishment, which serves to connect anxiety and aggression and eventually have a localized inhibiting effect, also provide the child with an aggressive model after which he can pattern himself. Parents can act as aggressive models just as can aggressive characters in films, comic books, and television. In this way punishment both inhibits and instigates aggressive behaviors. Bandura and Huston (1961) summarize this relationship as follows: . . . when a parent punishes a child physically for having aggressed toward peers, the intended outcome of the training is that the child should refrain from hitting others. Concurrent with the intentional learning, however, a certain amount of incidental learning may be expected to occur through imitation, since the child is provided, in the form of the parent's behavior, with an example of how to aggress toward others and this incidental learning may guide the child's behavior in later social interactions (p. 311). Bandura has investigated imitative learning from a variety of angles, including aggression. One experiment (1961) involved preschool children who performed a two-choice discrimination problem and a model who exhibited unusual, irrelevant, non-functional divertive behaviors in the solution process in addition to the orienting tasks typical of incidental learning studies. Half of the children observed a model who aggressed against dolls as a part of the diverting behavior while the other half observed a non-aggressive model. Both groups were further divided so that half of each group had a warm rewarding interaction with the model while the other half experienced a cold and non-nurturant relation- ship. (Previous experiments have shown that nurturance promotes identification. Mowrer (1950) suggests that affectional rewards increase secondary reinforcing properties of the model for the satisfaction these cues provide.) It was predicted that children who experience a warm, rewarding interaction with the experimenter would imitate more of the behaviors performed by the model than those children who experienced a colder, more distant relationship. The findings indicated that, in the presence of the model, the children imitated the behaviors of the model. Warmth in the relationship facilitated modeling except for aggressive behavior, which was readily imitated by all subjects regardless of the warmth in the relationship. Children who experienced a warm nurturant relationship also exhibited more pre-solution conflict behavior. Bandura, Ross, and Ross (1961), observing the facility with which aggressive behaviors were imitated, investigated the generalization of imitative response patterns to new settings in which the model was not present. Pre-schoolers were divided into three groups. One group ob- served an aggressive model, another observed inhibited non-aggressive models and the third, a control group, was not exposed to models. Children exposed to aggressive models exhibited more imitative and non-imitative aggression in a different environmental situation. Another study by these investigators (1963) investigated the extent to which film- mediated aggressive models serve as sources of imitative behavior. Aggressive models can be ordered on a reality-fictional stimulus dimen- sion, thus pro-school children were divided into three experimental groups and exposed to three models, each occupying a different place on the continuum. One group observed a live aggressive model, a second group observed a film of the same model, while a third group viewed a film depicting an aggressive cartoon character. Experimental subjects 10 exhibited nearly twice as much aggression as control subjects who were .not exposed to models. Exposure to aggressive humans on film was the most influential in terms of eliciting imitative aggressive behavior. Mussen and Rutherford (1961) also investigated the effects of aggressive cartoons on childrens' aggressive play, finding that exposure to an aggressive animated cartoon served to stimulate the intensity of first graders' impulses to aggression. Hypotheses The present study concentrates upon the effectiveness of verbal controls on aggression shaped by exposure to an aggressive model. - Since parents and others responsible for learning experiences in this area expect their efforts to generalize to other situations when they are not present, a situation is provided so that generalization of aggression can be studied. First, it is predicted that boys exposed to an aggressive model will, following a frustrating experience, reproduce the aggressive behaviors of the model and will differ in this respect from boys not exposed to the model. This hypothesis has been successfully tested by Bandura, Ross, and Ross (1961) but replication of their findings is necessary to check on the present experimental procedures. The second hypothesis is that boys who are verbally prohibited from performing the aggressive actions of the model will inhibit these acts inra subsequent situation following frustration, and that they will differ in this respect from subjects exposed to the model without receiv- ing instructions and controls not exposed to the aggressive model. The third hypothesis concerns the generalization of effects of exposure to the aggressive model and the generalization of prohibiting instructions. Following frustration, subjects not receiving inhibiting instructions should display more imitative and non-imitative aggression 11 in the presence of the frustrater than in the presence of an experimenter of the opposite sex. Finally, it is predicted that the effects of verbal prohibition on imitation of the model's aggressive behavior will be greater inthe presence of the person invoking the prohibition than in the presence-of a neutral experimenter. METHOD Subjects The experimental design replicates many of the features of Bandura's studies. Bandura's findings indicate that aggression is differentially influenced by the sex of the model. Boys exhibited more aggressive behavior than girls when exposed to a male model. To maxi- .mize imitative aggression, only boys were used in this study. The subjects were 60 boys attending the Spartan Cooperative Nursery School at Michigan State University. They ranged in age from ‘36 to 66 months with a mean age of 51. 6 months. The author and four female assistants conducted the study. Experimental De Sign All of the boys in the experimental groups were exposed to a 5-minute film of an aggressive male model. One experimental group was specifically prohibited from behaving like the model. As a test of generalization, half of the boys receiving such instructions and half of those not receiving instructions were exposed to'a new experimenter in the test situation. The subjects were assigned randomly tothe experi- mental groups as shown in Table 1. The-number within each cell refers to the number of subjects exposed to the experimental variables listed to the'left and above it. The control group was not exposed to the film and received no instructions. Half of the group was exposed to a male tester and half to-a female tester. All subjects in this group will be used as a base line for examining the effects of exposure to an aggressive model on subsequent aggression. 12 13 Table 1. Allocation of Subjectsto Experimental Groups Pretest Treatment Nmber..'of..Su1jects‘in Doll Play Groups Model Instructions ' E Same as ‘E Different from Prete st ' Pr ete st Expe rimental Y e s Pr ohibitive 10 10 Expe rimental Y e 5 None 10 10 Control No None 10 10 Experimental Conditions In the first step of the procedure, the author brought individual subjects to the experimental building located across the street from the nursery school. The author had spent at least two nursery school sessionswith each class group'in order to increase rapport. When they were later asked to come to the experimental building, subjects under- stood that there were toys there that they were going over to play with. Experimental subjects were told on the way over that they would see a movie and attempts were made to secure their interest. The experimental subject entered the building from a door that put him immediately into the room where he would see the film. He could not see thevother rooms used later in the experiment. The child was seated at a small table where there were a number of Colorforms Toys, shown how these toys worked, and told a story connected with‘the Colorforms. When adequate rapport was established, thesubject was told that he was now going to see the movie. The: movie and sound track was started by the author who acted as experimenter for all sub- jects in the pretest situation. The film with sound track used in the present study was obtained from Bandura and is representative of films used in his studies on imitative aggression. The film is in color and lasts 14 a little more than five minutes. There are four specific behaviors which-the model performs in the film and these four behaviors‘are repeated twice in the five-minute period. In the first sequence, the adult male model says, "Out of my way, you funny clown. I said out of my way. 'I'll fix you. I'll sit on you and punch you inthe nose. " He proceeds to sit on an inflated plastic clown (Bobodoll) which is about four feet high and punches him in the nose. This sequence lasts about 15 seconds. - The model then says, "Still here for more. I'll pick up that mallet and hit you in the nose. " While saying, "Bang, bang . . . Sock you down" etc. , the model strikes the Bobo doll with the mallet for 35 seconds. The model then states, "I don't know what to dowith you. I know what I'll do. Kick you! Right across the room. Fly away . . . fly away‘you funny clown. " etc. After 15 seconds of kicking the doll, he says, "I'll pick up those balls and hit you in the head . . . throw them at you . . . Bang. . . Boom!" He throws the balls and verbalizes for 45 seconds. The model then duplicates these four aggressive acts again in the same sequence. He sits and punches the Bobo for 30 seconds; then strikes him in the nose with the mallet for 30 seconds; kicks him for another‘40 seconds; and finally spends a full minute throwing the balls at him. ' . Prohibitive Instructions: During and following the movie, the prohibitive instructions were given‘to the‘Prohibitive Group. These instructions, which were essentially, the same for each subject, were given in aconversational way to the child as follows: "Look what he's doing! You wouldn't act like that, would you? . . . He's hitting him with a mallet and he (Bobo) didn't do anything. I couldn't allow you to act that way . . . if you did, I'd have to take you back and you couldn't come again. He shouldn't do that. . . . Spartan Nursery School children wouldn't act that way. Mrs. (S‘s teacher) wouldn't like it if you acted like that. He shouldn't act that way--hitting toys and 15 saying things like that. You wouldn't do that, would you? I would have to send you back tonursery school if you did. " ' The instructions were as uniform. as possible from subject to sub- ject. The verbalization followed the action of the film and were given in a way similar to that used by parents using verbal prohibitions. Care was taken so that the instructions did not overlap with the model's verbalizations. Most of the subjects were quite involved with the film and when instructions were given, they would respond by agreeing with the prohibitions outlined by the experimenter. Following the film, the experimenter briefly repeated that he knew that the subject would not act like the man in the film and that that was a very bad way to act. No further instructions or comments were made to the subject regarding the film or their behavior for the remainder of the experiment. Subjects not receiving prohibitive instructions were treated the same as prohibitive subjects up until the time the film began. The author maintained rapport with the subject by looking at the film and describing the action briefly at intervals in as neutral a way as possible. Carefully avoided were any indications of approval, such as signs of enjoyment of the model's actions in the film, giving permission to act like the model, or other expressions that might affect subsequent behavior. If the subject's attention wandered from the film, he was encouraged to watch what was happening on the screen. ~ Aggression Arousal: Following exposure to the film, the subject was told that the author had other toys and asked if he wanted to. see them. All of the children readily agreed and were taken to a separate room for aggression arousal. This room, . decorated with balloons and cut-outs on thewall, contained a variety of toys that would be especially appealing to boys in this age group (see Figures 1,. 2, 3, and 4). There was an electric train that the boys were shown and encouraged to Operate; FIGURE 2 l7 FIGURE 3 FIGURE 4 18 "a moving ferriswheel; an electric submarine; a battery operated rocket launcher; a miniature castle withsoldiers and horses; various large constructiontoys including a motor grader, road scraper, cement mixer, and trucks. There was also twolarge fire engines, a large rocket launcher, and a variety of stuffed toys. Most of the subjects became involved in operating the train, the‘rocket launchers, or the large trucks. The author left the room after introducing the toys and showing how they operated. The subjects usually became quite engrossed in play activities and after about five minutes, the author returned and told the subject that, "These are -my very best toys and I cannot let just anyone play with them. I'm saving these for someone else who is coming later. I have to keep them nice for 'Mike' and I just can't let you play with them. " Most of the subjects were very reluctant to leave the room and many asked if they could complete play activities that they had begun. However, the author made it a rule that the subjects’left the room immediately. The sub- jects were told that there were other ‘toys in another part of the building and that they could play with these toys. The subjects were thentaken to the other end of the building and introduced to the test situation. Doll Play Half of the subjects in each of the two experimental groups were introduced to a new experimenter for the doll play test situation. These subjects were chosen at random. In order to maximize differences in experimenters, the new experimenter was of the opposite sex. When the subject was introduced to the new experimenter, he was told that "This is Miss . She will stay withyou while I go and fix-the pro- jector. There are a lot of toys here and you can play with any of them you like. " The author then left the room. 19 The room used for doll play contained a variety of toys and ° - included among them were those seen in the film (see Figures 5,. 6,1 7, and 8.)". These included a large Bobo doll, balls, a tether ball suspended from the ceiling, and mallets. Other toys available for the subject in- cluded a small house with a family of rubber dolls; an Etch-A-Sketch toy; a mechanical tank; a dart gun; a mechanical ray gun; Colorforms; a small wooden train and track; miniature cars and trucks with a garage; airplanes and hangar; a take-apart car; and stuffed animals. The toys were arranged in a standard fashion for all subjects. The play room was equipped with a transparent mirror and micro- phones. The behavior of the subjects was rated during the 20-minute test period in terms of predetermined response categories. The test session was divided into five—second intervals by an electronic interval timer which emitted a beep sound heard only by the raters, giving 240 response units for each subject. The raters remained in the observation room during the experiment and had no knowledge of pretest experimental conditions. The author acted as a second rater for some of the subjects who were exposed to the new experimenter in doll play. In no instance did he make ratings alone. In those cases where a new experimenter was introduced for doll play, the author would reappear after the 20- minute rating period to return the subject to the nursery school. During doll play, the experimenters avoided initiating any inter- action between the subject and themselves and minimized their presence by busying themselves at a table on one side of the room. Efforts were made to standardize the behavior of the experimenter in this test situ- ation as much as possible. Most of the subjects busied themselves with toys immediately upon entering the room and many of them conversed freely withthe experimentar who was present. One subject refused to stay for the whole twenty minute test period and another subject in the control group would not leave his mother who helped bring him. Both subjects were eliminated from the study. 20 FIGURE 5 FIGURE 6 FIGURE 7 FIGURE 8 22 Behavior Measures The response measures used in the present study fall into four categories. These cover imitative aggression, partially imitative aggression, non-imitative aggression, and non-aggression. Attempts were made to secure measures of verbal aggression, both imitative and non-imitative, but a number of factors led to the discarding of these variables. Often raters were not able to hear clearly because'of defects in the sound system, extraneous noises, and/or the quality of the subject's speech. The behavior categories which were checked when present are as follows (adapted from Bandura): No Play Child does not handle any of the play material, e. g. , stands or sits quietly, looks about the room, etc. Ball Rolls or plays catch with the ball, or uses it in some other non-aggressive fashion. Bobo The following responses directed toward the Bobo doll. constitute imitative aggressive responses: Mallet--child strikes, taps, or shoves Bobo with the mallet. Balk-child rolls a ball toward, or tosses it at Bobo. ~_lii£_l_<_-—child kicks or shoves Bobo with his foot. Sit and Punch--child punches, taps, or slaps Bobo while sitting or lying on it. Sometimes children combine two imitative responses in a single act, e. g. , child sits on Bobo and pounds it with the mallet. . . , child strikes Bobo with the mallet and kicks it simultaneously. These acts are double» scored-- 23 in the first example, the'Mallet and the Sit and Punch categories are checked; in the second example, the ‘Mallet and the Kick categories are checked. The following are non-imitative classes of aggressive responses toward Bobo: M--child punches, taps, slaps, shoves or wrestles Bobo. Strikes -- child strikes» tape, or shoves Bobo with any object other than the mallet, e. g. , jabs Bobo with darts, pokes it or strikes it with a gun, doll, etc. Non-aggressive responses toward Bobo doll: Sit--child sits on Bobo, bounces up and down with it, lies or rolls on it. Non-aggressive--child embraces Bobo carries it around, dances with it, stands alongside Bobo with his arm around it, etc. Tether Ball Guns Mallet--child strikes the tether ball with the mallet. Bobo-~child strikes Bobo with the tether ball. 911331--chi1d punches or slaps the tether ball, slams it against the wall; strikes it with objects other than the mallet. Non-aggressive--child swings on the ball, spins it, moves in an arc, examines it, etc. Explores or loads--child examines the gun, loads it, carries it in his hand. 24 Shoots--child fires darts, or aims the gun and shoots imaginary bullets. Check the Egbgcategory when the doll is the target and the 9.9.13.5 category when any other object is selected as the target (e. g. , shoots at the wall, the animals, the cars, the tether ball, etc.). Non-Ajgrressive 'Play This category includes all non-aggressive play with the doll house, the dolls, the bears, the cars, and the farm animals. Peg Board Aggressive--child pounds the pegs with the mallet. Non-wssive—examines the peg board, attempts to pull out or punch in the peg with his hand. Other Non-Imitative‘Aggressive Responses This category included physically aggressive acts directed toward the farm animals, the dolls, or the cars, e. g. , reenacts fights between the animals or the dolls, spanks or aggresses in other ways toward the dolls, crashes the cars, or runs them into the animals, etc. Other Imitative Resgonses Child places his hands on his hips; shakes finger at the Bobo doll. The time units were very brief so that during any interval the child usually exhibited behavior that fell within a single response category. If a child displayed behavior that fell within two or more categories, the raters checked only the response that consumed most of the time during the scoring interval. There were several exceptions to this scoring rule: 1. When a child combined two imitative responses in a single act, raters checked both of the imitative response categories. 25 When a child performed an imitative aggressive response and a non-imitative aggressive response, they checked both categories. 7 When a child exhibited non-aggressive behavior during most of the time interval but performed some imitative or non-imitative aggressive act, they scored only the appropriate aggressive response category. RESULTS Before giving the results as they relate to the hypotheses dis- cussed earlier, a section will be devoted to reliability of observed ratings. Following this, results will be discussed. Rater Reliabilitj Bandura (1961) reports interscorer reliabilities in the .908 for the highly specific concrete classes of behavior used as response measures in his experimental test situation. These same response measures were used in the present study. High interscorer reliabilities similar to those obtained by Bandura were found between raters in the current study. In order to provide an estimate of rater reliability, over half of the subjects were observed and rated by two trained raters. No one rater rated one experimental condition exclusively. The raters alternated with one another in a random way and all of the raters served as experimenters at one time or another in the experimental test situ- ation. The author never rated subjects alone. The analysis of the data reported in the next section is concerned with three scores for each subject. These scores represent total imitative aggression, total non-imitative aggression, and total non- aggression. The imitative aggression scores combines both'aggressive behavior exhibited by the subjects which exactly imitates the aggressive behavior of the model and partially imitative aggression, such as, striking the tether ball with a mallet rather than Bobo. Non-imitative aggression includes aggressive behaviors such as shooting or punching the Bobo doll. The total imitative aggression score is the sum of the subject's scores on the four response categories of imitative aggression 26 2.7 and the fourcategories of partially imitative aggression. The scores on the response categories represent the number'of times the rater checked the behavior in question as being present during the doll play. Ratings were made of the child's behavior every five seconds and there was a total of 240 five-second periods in the doll play session. Scores on any response category could vary from 0,. if the child never per- formed the act in question, to 240, t which indicates that the-child per- formed the behavior in question during the entire doll play situation. The total non-imitative aggression score is the sum of the six response categories involving aggression which does not resemble that of the model. The total non-aggression score is the total number of five-second periods checked by the rater in which the child played in a non-aggressive way. There are six categories of non-aggressive behavior. Three of the raters, A, B, and C, accounted for nearly all of the doll play sessions where a single rater was involved. Because the accuracy of outcomes depends greatly upon their ability as raters, rank correlation coefficients were computed for each rater versus their co-raters D and E. (Appendices D, E, and F. show total scoresand ranks for Raters A, B, and C and their co-raters on the three scoring categories.) In Table 2 below, rank correlation coefficients- are reported for these three raters and their co-raters on the three major response categories. Table 2. Rank Correlation Coefficients for Raters A, B, and C on-the Three Response Categories M Imitative Aggres sion ‘Non- Imitative Non-Agg res sion Rater A: 1.00 1.00 .99 ’Rater B: .99 .99 1.00 Rater C: .99 1.00 1.00 28 ‘Another rank correlation coefficient was computed between Raters A, B, and C combined and Raters D and E (see Appendix G). There were 24 subjects who were rated by Raters A or B or C and co-rated by D or E. The rank correlation coefficients are again quite high. - For imitative aggression, the rank correlation, coefficient is . 91; for non- imitative aggression, it is . 99; and for non-aggression, it is also . 99. While there'was some variation between raters within the finer response categories, i. e. , within the‘20 classes of behavior that were scored, even here the inter-rater agreement is remarkably. high. < Ranking the scores tends to eliminate minor variations and indicates that the raters are in high agreement as to the level and amount of the be- havior the subjects exhibited in the three‘major scoring categories. Imitation of the Model's Behavior Subjects who were exposed tothe film of the aggressive -model and received no instructions displayed a great deal more imitative aggression than either the prohibitive groupwor the controls. Nineteen of the 20 boys seeing the film without instructions displayed some imi- tative aggressionwhile only 12 of the controls and six of theprohibitive group did so. Using a similarfilm, Bandura (1961) found that very few children who had not been exposed to the film exhibited behavior similar to that of the model. Approximately 70 per cent .of his subjectsenot seeing the film had zero imitative aggression scores. In the present study, mean scores for boys seeing the film without prohibitive instructions aremore than three times as'large as those of boys in the control groups ' (Table 3). Differences between, the film group, prohibitive group and controls on total imitative aggression is highly significant using a two- tailed test (Table 4). The differences are alsoapparent when film groups 29 Table 3. - Mean‘Scores for the Experimental and Control Subjects on the Three Major Response Categories Experimental Group No ‘ Pr ohibitive Control Response Categories Instructions Instructions Group Imitative Aggression (Same) Experimenter A 15 . 2 . 6 (Different) Experimenter 9. 4 . 2 . 8 Combined 12. 2 . 2 . 7 Partially Imitative Aggression (Same) Experimenter 24. 2 4. 6 6. 3 (Different) Experimenter 8. 2 2. 0 10. 7 Combined 16.2 3.3 8.5 Total Imitative Aggression ‘(Sarne) Experimenter 39. 2 4. 8 6. 9 (Different) Experimenter 17. 6 2. 2 ll . 5 Combined 28.4 3.5 9.2 Non-Imitative Aggression (Same) Experimenter 155. 8 98. 1 154. 1 (Different) Experimenter 62.1 123.6 118.8 Combined 108.9 110.8 131.4 Non-Aggression (Same) Experimenter 67. 8 147. 3 94. 3 (Different) Experimenter 168. 1 119. 9 108. 5 Combined 117.9 133.6 101.4 Table 4. Significance of Differences (H-Test) Between Experimental Groups and Control Groups in the Expression of knitative and Non-Imitative Aggression. . o a I 1 Test Situation Same Different Response Category Experimenter Experimenter Combined ‘ ‘P ' P ‘P IInitative Aggression . 001* . 01* . 01* Non-Imitative Aggression . 05* n. s. . 0.5 Non-Aggression n. s. . 02 . 05* at: Corrected for tied ranks. 30 -who had a new experimenter in doll play and those having the same experimenter throughout are compared with subjects receiving prohibi- tive instructions and controls (Table 5). Table 5. Significance of Differences Between Groups in the Expres sion. of Aggression (Mann-Whitney Test) Response Category Experimental Groups Imitative Aggression Non-Imitative Aggression U p 'U P Film vs. Prohibitive Same EXperimenter ‘11 . 02 30. 5 n. 3. Different Experimenter 8 . 002 23 . 05 Combined 42 . 002 199 n. 5. Film vs. Control Same Experimenter l3 . 02 48. 5 n. s. Different Experimenter 30. 5 n. s. 19. 5 . 02 Combined 92.5 .02 141.5 n. s. Prohibitive vs. Control Same Experimenter 38. 5 n. s. 31 n. 5. Different Experimenter 31. 5 n. s. 49. 5 n. 8. Combined 137.5 .10 159.5 n. s. Effects of Verbal Instructions The most significant differences on imitative aggression occurred between the groups seeing the film without prohibitive instructions and the groups who received such instructions (Table 5). - These groups differ significantly both when the same experimenter remained in the test situ- ation and when a different non-frustrating experimenter is substituted. When film groups are compared with controls, similar significant differences in favor of the film groups again appear except for groups having a different experimenter in the test situation. The value of U in 31 this instance is 30. 5 and it would need to be 23 to reach the . 05 level of significance. Examination of individual scores shows that all 10 subjects in the film group whohad a different experimenter in doll play showed some imitative aggression. Their mean score was 17. 6. 1 Only six of the controls displayed aggression similar to that displayed by the model. Their mean score was 11. 5. - One of the subjects in the control group had a score of 53 in the partially imitative aggression category for sitting on Bobo. This was the only imitative aggression he displayed and he was the second lowest on total aggression; yet his score on total imitative aggression was the highest in the group. His sitting on Bobo artificially inflated the mean of his group and though he exhibited partially imitative aggression, he apparently was not as aggressive as most of those seeing the film. It seems clear that while exposure to an aggressive. model has a definite effect on the shaping of aggression following frustration and this effect generalizes to new situations, it can be. controlled by prohibitive instructions directed at specific behaviors of the model. Subjects receiving prohibitive instructions during exposure to the film consistently showed less total imitative aggression than those seeing the film without instructions and those in the control group (Table 3). The controls showed nearly three times as much total imitative aggression as the prohibitive groups and the film groups without instructions showed more than three times as much as the control groups. Again the most outstanding differences occur between groups seeing the film without instructions and theprohibitive groups. The prohibitive and control groups did not differ significantly when a U Test was applied to ranks. The effects of instructions on the prohibitive group were quite dramatic. While only one subject of the twenty not receiving instructions failed to imitate the model, thirteen of the prohibitive group were inhibited from 32 doing so. .Specific verbal instructions have a very significant inhibitory effect on subsequent imitative aggressive behavior following frustration. Though there were differences in non-imitative aggression related to new experimenters in the test situation, both the film groups and the prohibitive groups showed a very similar amount of non-imitative aggression. The prohibitive instructions had effects which were quite specific to aggression against the Bobo doll. Generalization of Imitative Aggression Subjects who viewed the film without prohibitive instructions dis- played more aggression, both imitative and non-imitative, in the presence of the experimenter who frustrated them (see Table 3). The differences are highly significant when non-imitative aggression is con- sidered, while differences on imitative aggression only approach signifi- cance (see Table 6). Table 6. Significance of Differences in Expression of Aggression and Experimenters in Doll Play Response Category Experimental Groups Imitative Aggression ~ Non-Imitative Aggression U p U P Film - No Instruction Same vs. Different Experimenter 25 . 05-. 10 2 . 002 Film - Prohibitive Instruction Same vs". Different Experimenter 42. 5 n. s. 38. 5 n. 8. Controls Same vs. Different Experimenter 50 n. s. 32 n. s. 33 Generalization of Prohibitive Instructions V It was hypothesized that those subjects given prohibitive instruc- tions would display greater inhibition of prohibitive acts in the presence of the experimenter who gave the prohibition. While itis clearthat these instructions had a pronounced inhibitive effect on both imitative and non-imitative aggression (Table 3), there is only a slight trend, contrary to hypothesis, for subjects to display more partially imitative aggression in the presence of the experimenter who prohibited them and later frustrated them. The differences between the groups having the same experimenter in doll play and those having a new experimenter is not significant on either imitative aggression or on non-imitative ' aggression. Both» of the groups who were given prohibitive instructions displayed very little imitative aggression of any kind. $mfic§ earn—mung o>flofiEH 7. o>flofiEH $52 #308 333st mcofiosfimfi eZfiQEOum 305E? 53h on» wfioom maoofindw mo mouoom 3mm :4. NHQanmnm< 50 G oQV 0W< Gmmz moHH «mug . NM 0N N Ma; Q: o o 0 me Ah om: mm o o 0 mm Nd. #mm b c o 0 Sq mm :1 mo 0 o o a. 4m NM NAN N o N Nm Hm :. end 0 o 0 me E... Nwa we 0 o o oo <0 w: NNH H H 0 mm MM mm 00H m; «L 0 we 43 co om: o o o me 40 cofimsfim “ooh. E soucoafisomxm ucouofiwfl o .8 he. emmE m5; :3 we we N omm w o o 0 mm 40 Nmm p o o o oo AHA #0 3; o o o Nm UG NNN mm o o 0 iv a: 7 mo N“; “v w. 0 mo m4 mm 0mg 0 o M 1v 2.. MAN mm o o 0 Ho EU H: wma o o o Ev DU «ma mm cm om o no. 04 Am mmm o o 0 do MO cofimsfim pooh. cw soucogfisomxm oEmm ouoom ouoom ouoom osoom onoom 93:02 @330 :ofimmopmwaw cofimmosmwaw cofimmosmwmw Goflmmonww/w :ofimmoswaN om< unoZ o>3mfiEH o>flmfifld 038.939: ozumfig uGoZ 130R. >Zmflsmm mcoflosfimcH 9,355on £35 53...». 05 mcfloom whoeflhsm mo mosoom 3mm .m NHDZHnngw 51 m .3 ha. 522 mon mm: m: hog w HwH wo h h o no NJ no 3: m o m hm .mH wm mom 0 o o wm no em mmH o o o mw Mb mama. on o o o on hU on NMH o o 0 mm OH .92 nod o o o ow >4 mo mod H a o ww 00 9: mo ow hm m ww 0m omH mm mm mm o ww mm coflmsfim pooh. 5 poucocfiuomxm usesofififl c .mm om< Geog mwm mea me mo 0 mm: pm 0 o o ww MA MNH m: ma ma 0 mm MG 2: mmH om em 0 Ho OM wh 9: o o 0 on Maw ww ENN N N 0 pm and nod Nma o o 0 mm NO h mmm o o o we hN co 9: ma w m ow >N o: NNH OH o H wm >H a: w]: m m 0 we 4.? cofiodfim “ooh. a“ sousogwuomxm 05mm ouoom osoom osoom ouoom onoom mficoz UHEO cofimmosmwwt Cofimmosmmws coflmmouwqu sofimmoummw. :ofiwmosmwxw omr. acoZ met/Sofia»: o>3mfiEH «53.32»: 0.2331»: IcoZ H.308 >23»qu ill) l L 95ch 33:00 «.0 mouoom 3mm .0 UHQZMQQ< 32 m.HH mm oH mm m hmH m me m m w m NA wH wm wH wm N NON N. wON o o B seasoEHuomxm onth u mHoh—GOU m S: m omH 3 cm S cm 0 o MA. w mNH w- NNH w mm: m wHH m mH m mH mm seasoEHnomxfl 3.32 n mHonucoO w ooH w ooH NH Nm NH Hm o o N< HounoEHsomme aconoHHHQ .. mcoHuosuumGH o>HanH~Honm .. EHHM N OMN N OMN mH w mH w a. h n o 40 H NMN H NmN wH N. wH N. o 0 m4 me Q: o HHH w hmH w omH o o 00 o HmH o mmH HH mw HH om m on m mm 04 mH oN mH Hm H NNN H wwN o o VHO noucoEHsomxH 08mm u mcoHuoshmsH, 8,3330%“ u EHHh m nON m nON mH mN mH wN o w o w HumuH§H Inez L! Con mouwmws 039.32g: sound-OD mHHH Hose .04 sou—mm >3 poGmem< moHsowoqu mGHuoom .Hoflmw/H cough. o5 Go 318% paw mouoom Hmuoh .Q XHQZHnmnHHw 53 w HwH w HwH m.N. mo w wo m.m N. w N. NA HoacogHsomxH onth .. Houuaou N. me N. NwH o NoH. o moH o o AM o owH o wwH m .N no N. mm o o 4m mH wN mH 0N N mHN N oHN m6 N m.N. H Hm H H HN. HH or w AwwH w wwH o o 2. nousoEHuome «souomfifl u mcoHuoshmGH ownHanHnHonnH .. EHHM H OMN H OMN mH w mH w m c m o 40 m mmH m HmH m6 om HH mm H mm H em 04 NH Hm NH oN. H wwN H wNN o G M0 nouaoaHuome 08mm: mcoHaosbmcH 9,3330?“ u EHHh w me w 0: m5 om o om w H mN H awn»... m A5N m mON NH N.N NH 0N m .m N. m 9 NH N NbN N CNN HH Nm oH Hm m.o N e N mm noHGQEHuonH “domain u mcoHHoduumsHoz .. EHHMV o 2. o Hm. m moH m th o o O< OH 2. 0H me m me m wcH, N mN N mN GO poucoEHuomNH oEom u mcofloduumfi oZ FEHHM VHS...“ onoom Mood ouoom demm ouoom Hand, ouoom vHsmm onoom Mam“ osoom Hoonflsm seHMMuoU . m sound soundsoo m soumm uonMuoO . m non—mm :onmouwwwtucoZ conmoumw< o>HH3H§HucoZ conmoumw< 9,332»: 1 1‘ nous/muoO mHeH was m soumm >9, pocmHmm< moHsomoumO wcHsoom soflmSH cough. 9H”— co mchmm Hess mouoom Hooch. .mH XHQZHnwnm¢ 54 m _ owH m N.wH m em m cm o 0 MA HopcofiHuomxm onHZ .. HonuGoO N NHN N NHN N. N.N N. .N o 0 $0 HoHGoEHuomxfi oEmm n 9833533 o>HH3H~Honmi EHHnH m on m moH w mm w ow m w m m 9 w MNH w NN.H o ww 0 mm w 0N w NN 00 o NNH o mNH m No m was m.N on N on Am souGoEHuolonM Hcouowfifl u mcoflosflmfi 02 n EHHh w HN w HN H oON H wNN m.N on m mm NU N. oo N. ma N HoH N NoH H ow H New 02 noncoEHuome 08mm .. moon—053mg 02 u EHHM Mama ouoom Mama onoom chom osoom chmm onoom chmm osoom uHcmm ouoom Hoonndm soumHHuoO U sebum son mosmmwfiucoz sonMuoO 0 Hanna con m oummwt 033357552 nonMHuoU U Heumd :03 m e ummd. o>HumHHEH noummuoO m3 Hose 0 son—mm >nH HoocmHmm< moHuoonmO mcHsoum sofimHZ 00.23... 05. Go mvHGmm Hons mosoom H.308 3m XHQZHQnqu posfiusoo 55 MH MwH MH NwH OH NOH OH MOH O O Ah N. ooH h ooH N.H OM OH HM O O N4 H wMN H MMN wN O MN N. O O mm NH nwwH NH me M .HH Mo HH mo O O 4m MN ON MN ON H N.HN N OHN M .NH N M .wH H Hm HN HN. HN o0 w OOH w wwH O O MM noucoaHHomxH unouomfifl u muoHuoSfi—ms o>HanH~Houm u EHHh N NMN ;N NMN MN N. MN N. O O Q..— M NHN w NHN M.ON N.N M.HN ON O O HHO N.H OOH N.H HHH M N.MH O nNMH O O DU HoocoEHnomNH 05mm u mnoHuonnumGHoNrHuHQEounH .. EHHh OH OOH OH MOH wH Mm wH om HH w M.w o a M .w NON M N.ON NN MN ON MN m O OH O 4N w oN.H m MOH OH OM OH OM MH H M.wH H 2. O OON O MON M.ON N.N M.HN ON M.o N. M.m o NHN o MN.H o NN.H_ wH ww N.H MM M ON O NN 00 OH NNH MH .MNH MH No M.NH wo M.N oM N OM 1mm M .w N.ON M ONN oH NM oH HM M .NH N MH N mm HH OMH HH owH MH MN. MH Nb w NM w MM NH noHGoEHHomxm acouowwHQ n mGoHHofiuHmcH OZ .. EHHh wH N.o oH co m wNH w OMH H NN. H wh mm wN HN wN HN M MON H N.NN M .N oM M MM ._...0 ON MN. ON. MN. O MwH M wOH O MN M MN 00 peacoEHuomxH 05mm .. mcoHuodnude oZ .. EHHh demm ouoom deoM onoom Manx ouoom xnmm onoom uHcmm ouoom Mood ouoom pooBdm uonMuoO noumm sonMIOO Hound HoHMMnoO Hound GonmonwwHumHHEanoZ aonmoumm< 9.53qu m one Q mueHmMuoU :93. was 0 one .m .< msoumm poGHnEoU .3 eoamHmm< moHsomoumO msHsoom .3“me cough. or: so muHcmm Hose mouoom H.308 :U XHQZMnHm< 56 wH HwH wH HwH M.HH Mo M.NH wo oH Mo OH mo N. N.MH N. wMH NN wM NN «M M MON M wON soucoEHsome onth u HohcoO MH MNH OH NNH o MHH o wHH noosoEHuomxH eHmSH .. HoSsoO M.o N. HH OH M NH O N. MH N. N.H Ln O .NH MH ma xcmm ouoom vHsmm ouoom xsem ouoom demm ouoom soundnoo sound noanuoO noumm conmouwwHumfi§HuGoZ Hood 980% Hand ouoom uooflndm uoHNMuoU scum :on m ouwmw. 92383:: m poSGHHGoO u .0 NHQZManqu