. _ . ‘. _ y m V .... A , .. mxm mmfl ,5 g : Rmu nmn_ ,._.:A, ,@ ADm PEN , . .. V0 .1. U ., V. T. 0V0 SWN ,m. ., mom en“. 7 PmR. eTMl 0 hs. _ . “Hm tuflun mmrnu. mmm WARN flR DAN fimbl AS.” hM GAN T... _ m 0. . S C. U o . . .326. _. .r This is to certify that the thesis entitled USING ADOPTION PROCESS VARIABLES AS A PREDICTOR OF PRODUCT CONTINUANCE OR DISCONTINUANCE presented by Patrick Michael Dunne has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Marketing Ph.D degree in 0-7639 dial .3! :27 HUM: & SUNS' 353K blNDtRY NC“ anamv emoeas SPIINEMIT. “ICING” “ ‘ 4M U 5W ‘A‘S M. cow ABSTRACT USING ADOPTION PROCESS VARIABLES AS A PREDICTOR OF PRODUCT CONTINUANCE OR DISCONTINUANCE BY Patrick Michael Dunne Over $300 billion will be spent on new product in— novation in the 1973 fiscal year. Yet, over 70 per cent of this cost will go to products that will not be successful in the market place. Nowhere is this problem of new product introduction more prevalent than in the retail food industry. Thus, the stated purpose of this study was to provide the retail food manager a means of predicting which of the products distributed through his retail food outlet should be eliminated from his product line at a point in time far earlier than the usual analysis of thirteen weeks sales data. Instead of reviewing the initial three months sales results, it was hypothesized that knowledge of the $2331 of consumer adOption process variables, as well as knowledge of the rate 9g growth of these variables, could be used to I' i ' 35:12: management 5 c :e‘-‘§rec"1ct at a PM!“1 :23. the point 1" asks after product i as an arbitrary deCl s:;:;ficant reductior tieccntinuation deci Six variable: .Ei'eis of activity f 595115: seven new pr iiiilarket during t . ~J.f‘:‘h I #4. ' «Cs ‘- LO detErmln | ‘ apPrOac lice sL 1 ' lnea 1' d 18c "35?. r \‘ pe Patrick Michael Dunne predict management's decision to continue or discontinue a new product at a point in time earlier than in present use today. The point in time chosen for this study was five weeks after product introduction. While admittedly, this was an arbitrary decision, it was chosen so as to make a significant reduction in the amount of time needed to make the continuation decision. Six variables of the adOption process were selected for testing in this research. The research measured the levels of activity for these six predictor variables against seven new products introduced in the Des Moines, Iowa market during the summer of 1972. The seven products studied were of a similar nature in both terms of level of newness and in terms of consumer product classification. The study presented its own analysis of this definitional classification as well as what items were to be considered important in the adOption process. A telephone survey was conducted of customers for a selected chain store in Des Moines to determine the level of activity for the products. The approach to analyzing the data was twofold. First, linear discriminate analysis was used to test if the weekly percentages for the second through fifth week after introduction of the six predictor variables chosen were 21m discriminate '. 22:51:; to its cri: 21:12 at the end of t 32:11: hypothe s is w a ext-tests. Also, 5:295 on the mean I 53.5615: ntinued gr' Lie same time oerig . n i ‘. J; ., tile‘ _ i=5.“ m 3 Standard '932:: . Of , he “leap Patrick Michael Dunne able to discriminate between the products which management, according to its criteria, decided to continue or discon- tinue at the end of thirteen weeks. Second, the rate of growth hypothesis was tested by means of eighteen independ— ent t-tests. Also, two—way analyses of variance were per— formed on the mean rates of growth between the continued and discontinued groups by the six predictor variables for the same time period as the earlier tests. The study was able to differentiate between the con— tinued and discontinued products by using the weekly per— centage levels of the six variables. In the fifth week the discriminate function was found to be significant at the .01 level and to correctly assign all seven products to their prOper grouping. The thesis, also, analyzed the data from the second, third and fourth week. This was to see if an earlier time period could produce significant results, likewise. While the second week's function was found to be significant at the .02 level and correctly predicts the outcome of all seven products, the observed difference in the means of the two product groups was only nine times the week's standard error. The fact that the observed differ— ence of the means for the fifth week was seventy times as great supported the notion that the fifth week is able to gritie more conc his 1 ~ 1 a. p'fi:flnsfl* .... .uv‘hu» v. o The function (u 2: :. '79 teen inflcer. 35:, :‘rey were sti'. }IZ§'LC‘. groupings c: “aria‘tles were four. :: the continuation 333932 weak inf: .3512“. type. The analys Presented 9.11 gas in ?_ ~ A ... '4 ’~..\ He‘serthe 1‘ Patrick Michael Dunne provide more conclusive evidence as to the continuation of the product. The function for the other two weeks was believed to have been influenced by extraneous variables. Neverthe— less, they were still able to predict five of the seven product groupings correctly. Three of the six predictor variables were found to exercise a great deal of influence on the continuation of a new product. They were weak interest, weak information seeking, and knowledge of the product type. The analysis of the rates of growth as a predictive tool presented evidence, which tends to support the hypothe- sis that the rates of growth cannot be used to predict the continuation or discontinuation of the product. Nevertheless, it was shown that for at least one product grouping at one time and in one geographic location, a linear discriminate function of six adOption variables could be used to predict product continuation or discontinu- ation. Prior to this time the adOption variables have been used as a post—Operative tool to eXplain what has happened. Now these variables have been shown to be useful as a pre- dictive tool as well. csrx: f‘“ J. USING ADOPTION PROCESS VARIABLES AS A PREDICTOR OF PRODUCT CONTINUANCE OR DISCONTINUANCE BY Patrick Michael Dunne A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Marketing and Transportation Administration 1972 COpyright by PATRICK MICHAEL DUNNE 1972 To Joe, Ray and their families ii like completj f‘;:..::;on of one iridl sent the author fro-r ::;':';:ec‘. to that cor be especially noted Dr. Donald 1 35;artnent of Marke‘ Echigan State Uniw littee and academic 5“" 5‘4PPort through 3:“ Tal‘lor's advice SE‘I‘.’e as a mOdel f’: Dr. Richard . r3252” thcltion Admi w I. ‘ ,31FA d.‘. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The completion of a doctoral program is never a function of one individual. While space limitations pre- vent the author from thanking all those whose efforts con— tributed to that completion, the following peOple should be especially noted for their assistance: Dr. Donald A. Taylor, Professor and Chairman, Department of Marketing and Transportation Administration, Michigan State University, as chairman of the research com— mittee and academic advisor, has provided close guidance and support throughout the author's graduate program. Dr. Taylor's advice and philOSOphy of life will always serve as a model for the author. Dr. Richard J. Lewis, Professor of Marketing and Transportation Administration, Michigan State University, has always been a helpful counselor to the author. As both a member of the research committee and a neighbon,Dr. Lewis was always available with advice to solve the problem at hand. Dr. Leo G. Erickson, Professor of Marketing and iii frazsror-tation Admin; as a center of the re it: fine inception of :esm appreCiated. D- Dr. Eddie V. '01 .‘I§ -‘ ~10. ' Marketing, ['3 Q" s “"65 ........., Assistant 2 any, were instrure: ...... .;:‘-.i;, and MIS. C :v , ‘I C ‘9. ‘h x "’ “9 triankeé .3558"; {9- In an: 5%,: ‘i.")‘r‘.’ .‘ ‘ ‘ '0 W‘l ‘ 4 Ci) 'T ‘A tile * A flna‘L 5 f5: Transportation Administration, Michigan State University, as a member of the research committee served tirelessly from the inception of the research. His assistance is deeply appreciated. Dr. Eddie V. Easley, Professor and Chairman, Depart- ment of Marketing, Drake University, and Dr. Vasanth B. Solomon, Assistant Professor of Statistics, Drake Univer- sity, were instrumental in the deve10pment of the research design. Dr. Solomon's assistance with the statistical methodology is greatly appreciated. Miss Mary Hershberger, who did all the rough draft typing, and Mrs. Jo McKenzie, who typed the final copy, are to be thanked for putting up with the author's constant pressure. In addition, sincere appreciation goes to the company, which must remain anonymous, that permitted the author to use its store for this study. A final word of thanks must go to the members of the faculty of the Graduate School of Business Administra— tion at Michigan State University for their assistance in the develOpment of the author's professional career. iv TABLE ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. . . . . LIST OF TABLES . . . . . LIST OF FIGURES. . . . . Chapter I INTRODUCTION. . Background. . OF CONTENTS Statement of the Problem. Implications for Marketing Problems Conceptual Framework. Methodology . Limitations of the Study. Some Possible Contributions of the Study . II LITERATURE REVIEW The Adoption Process. Ryan and Gross Hybrid Corn Study. Five-Stage Ad0ption Process Model "Hierarchy of Effects" Model. "AIDA Model". Critique of Adoption Process Models Innovation-Decision Model Marketing's Explanations of Consumer Decision-Making Process. Summary of Ad0ption Models. Industrial Goods and the AdOption Models The Diffusion Process The Diffusion Process in Marketing. Adopter Categories. The Innovation-Decision Period. V Page iii viii ll 14 17 19 20 25 26 27 29 3O 31 32 35 36 43 46 46 48 49 54 Iagter Diffusi Opinior Indust: Di fu Innovati Rogers Other Produc Backgt Tradit Theort Reta: Summary Chapter Page Diffusion Effect. . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 Opinion Leadership. . . . . . . . . . . . 59 Industrial Goods and the Diffusion Process. . . . . . . . . . . . 60 Innovation Characteristics. . . . . . . . . 61 Rogers' Characteristics . . . . . . . . . 61 Other Studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63 Product Elimination Studies . . . . . . . . 64 Background. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64 Traditional Approaches. . . . . . . . . . 66 Theoretical Approaches. . . . . . . . . . 67 Retail Grocery Practices. . . . . . . . . 74 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 III RESEARCH DESIGN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84 Product Classification. . . . . . . . . . . 84 Hypotheses. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94 Sample Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95 The Sampling Frame. . . . . . . . . . . . . 95 Data Collection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98 Analysis of the Data. . . . . . . . . . . . 102 IV PRESENTATION OF FINDINGS. . . . . . . . . . . 113 The Management Decision . . . . . . . . . . 113 Weekly Percentages of the Six Variables as Prediction Tools. . . . . . . 115 Second Week Findings . . . . . . . . . . 121 Second Week Summary. . . . . . . . . . . 124 Third Week Findings. . . . . . . . . . . 124 Third Week Summary . . . . . . . . . . . 128 Fourth Week Findings . . . . . . . . . . 128 Fourth Week Summary. . . . . . . . . . . 132 Fifth Week Findings. . . . . . . . . . . 132 Fifth Week Summary . . . . . . . . . . . 134 Summary of Weekly Percentage Tests . . . 135 Rate of Growth of the Six Variables as Prediction Tools. . . . . . . . . . . . 136 V SUMMARY’AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . 145 Objectives of the Study . . . . . . . . . . 145 Empirical Findings. . . . . . . . . . . . . 148 Implications of the Research. . . . . . . . 150 vi A Census Tra: Des Moines Trading Ar E Telephone viewing Pi Recording - ' s -" ‘flnyapk ..~..4.. wMy J c Page Appendix A Census Tracts of Polk County, Iowa; Des Moines, Iowa; Selected Store's Trading Area. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 157 B Telephone Questionnaire, Telephone Inter- viewing Procedures, Telephone Data Recording Procedures. . . . . . . . . . . . . 162 Bibliography . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167 vii '7 Hyootnetice Six Predic Mean Value Discrimina Values: Se Hypothetic Six Predi: ANTCA Tat HYpotheti Second we prEdiCto: Third WEE Prefiicto; FDDrth w Pr9d1CtQ Fifth We prEdith Mean Va Table 3-1 LIST OF TABLES Hypothetical Second Week Levels of the Six Predictor Variables . . . . . . . . Mean Values of Predictor Variables, Discriminate Weights, and Importance Values; Second Week . . . . . . . . . . Hypothetical Rate of Growth for the Six Predictor Variables, Third- Second Week . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ANOVA Table for Rates of Growth of the Hypothetical Third-Second Week. . . . . Second Week Levels of the Six Predictor Variables . . . . . . . . . . Third Week Levels of the Six Predictor Variables . . . . . . . . . . Fourth Week Levels of the Six Predictor Variables . . . . . . . . . . Fifth Week Levels of the Six Predictor Variables . . . . . . . . . . Mean Values of Predictor Variables, Discriminate Weights, and Importance Values: Second Week . . . . . . . . . . Second Week Results of Assignment Tests Mean Values of Predictor Variables, Discriminate Weights, and Importance Values: Third Week. . . . . . . . . . . Third Week Results of Assignment Tests. Mean Values of Predictor Variables, Discriminate Weights, and Importance Values; Fourth Week . . . . . . . . . . Fourth Week Results of Assignment Tests viii Page 105 107 109 111 116 117 118 119 123 123 126 126 130 130 \- M .P .-..9 J - I 0—4 F—0 Meat Disc wait .-;2 Fifi . ‘. I Q'\W ‘ .4 n 17:. J... ls. -- N . LJ 7- .u Va l', 3-» .’ s ~gl‘. ‘ .I 9- a 5 C § 'i 7‘1 s I. sea J Table 4-11 4—19 Page Mean Values of Predictor Variables, Discriminate Weights, and Importance Values; Fifth Week. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 133 Fifth Week Results of Assignment Tests. . . . 133 Summary of the Relative Importance Percentages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 136 Rate of Growth for the Six Predictor Variables; Third-Second Week. . . . . . . . . 137 Rate of Growth for the Six Predictor Variables; Fourth-Third Week. . . . . . . . . 138 Rate of Growth for the Six Predictor Variables; Fifth-Fourth Week. . . . . . . . . 139 t-test Scores for the Rate of Growth for the Six Predictor Variables . . . . . . . 140 ANOVA Table for Rates of Growth of the Third-Second Week . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 142 ANOVA Table for Rates of Growth of the Fourth-Third Week . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 142 ANOVA Table for Rates of Growth of the Fifth-Fourth Week . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 142 ix Figure 1-1 2‘6 2‘7 2‘8 2‘s ‘1 LIST OF FIGURES Planned Contribution of New Products to Sales Growth, 1963-1967. . . . . . . Illustration of a Discriminate Function Relationship of the Three AdOption Process Models Hierarchy. . . . . . . . Relationship of AdOption Process Stages of the Four Campbell Models . . . . . . The Nicosia conceptualization of the Purchase Decision Process . . . . . . . The Andreasen Conceptualization of the Purchase Decision Process . . . . . . . The Howard-Sheth "Theory of Buyer Behavior" . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary AdOption Model. . . . . . . . . Adopter Categorization on the Basis of Innovativeness . . . . . . . . . . . The Berenson Abandonment Model. . . . . The Kotler Abandonment Model. . . . . . New Product Dimension Model . . . . . . Page 16 32 35 38 4o 41 45 50 69 71 88 Each y zillign ll new" CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Background Each year American business firms introduce several million "new" products to the market place. The introduc- t:iJDn of these so-called "new" products is an attempt by the firm to obtain a differential advantage by means of product differentiation over their competition. However, some t171<3ught should be given to the question of what is a "new" product. Wasson has noted that a product can be classified as "new" in at least thirteen possible ways. Each of these w . . . . . aYs Will have some effect, either positive or negative, on t he introduction strategy chosen by the firm. Robertson hag defined "new" innovations in three ways, depending upon 11:3 effects on established patterns of consumer behavior. 3112-‘-ze11 and Nourse have defined new products as the products abQ looked upon by the processor and distributor. Yet none Qs these definitions have become universally accepted so 1: 11§t the term new product means the same to all readers. 1 Nevertheles' a'zariety 0f “new 0 anon for this beta zit-.Lderson. Alder afim seeks power :3 resolve negotia' :zthe firm‘s favo mint difterentf jasiict physicall repute it from at aSteptable met‘no though conmunic eryironnent; (3‘ 53598 of satisf 31" - ‘Ce and terms 2 American firms continue to introduce Nevertheless, A possible explan- a variety of "new offerings" each year. ation for this behavior can be found in an interpretation of Alderson. Alderson has been interpreted as saying that a firm seeks power through product differentiation in order to resolve negotiations between the firm and the consumer in the firm's favor. One of the most common methods of product differentiation is by means of differentiating the product physically from all competitive offerings so as to Other remove it from any margin of perfect substitution. acceptable methods include (1) psychological differentiation (2) differentiation in the purchase through communicat ion; (3) differentiation in after—purchase assur- enVironment: ances of satisfaction in use; and (4) differentiation in price and terms of sale.5 It is through these power—seeking, differentiation a - . . . - cthItleS, that the firm seeks to reduce risks and create a . . Preference among purchasing units that is suffIClently s t1"brig to withstand the efforts of competing firms. The P . rg fits a firm earns are in part a payment for the risk 5. r1"'leed in the firm's efforts to achieve this power, and i 11 part to guide a firm to other than low risk alternatives. 8 M [[935 quotes a Chicago industrial designer as saying t hQ se risks are causing companies to face "situations they :e'.'er dreamed of be Today‘s :15- ;rsi'ict innovation mansion. Each de- frtr products not ; J *1 m (3 () ount for a “sass National Pr 3-” :Y‘ “ta .u.“ 5 Whats 5 resulted from prod; 55:11.18? e that the never dreamed of before."6 Today's American economy has reached a stage where product innovation has become a major factor of economic expansion. Each decade brings a higher percentage of sales from products not in production in the previous decade. This statement is most pronounced for those industries which account for a growing prOportion of the United States' Gross National Product. (See Figure 1-1) . A study by Printers' Ink has shown that 43 per cent of 1957 gross sales resulted from products not in production in 1947 and has estimated that the figure has increased to 56 per cent for 1966 using 1956 as a base year and 62 per cent for products Offered during the decade ending in 1971.8 Of the companies with the highest growth rate for the past several years. over 50 per cent of their sales have come from products introduced during the previous decade while only 10 per cent of the s . . ales of low growth firms came from this source. Of speCial i . n1:3rest is the fact that the majority of high-growth com— p - . . . . . . . . anles achieved divers1fication by acquiring other companies 1 I r1 addition to internal developments. The situation is f u): hher verified when one considers the results of a 1965 s tufly of United States firms. Of 742 firms studied in the S i 3": months starting January 1, 1965, 522 firms launched some 1 o 2 . 1 36 new products in that period. _ . '1 -'|| I ll a '1. I ‘ . - n . ‘ 0:0... _ o. 5.... 9.5.: co: 0 OCC 9 < :;..o ._...< $62. 93.3 k ads-27.: .3 or: QNEFZOO QWZZle \ .Roonmoma ..,..C.SOCU mu-_52 ”.0 29.595200 9.2qu I see: \ V 1968 fiscal year “ cent of this cost The C051 C a 1.". the market pla: 2::s eliminated ir 5;;31: of ten dense} in; on projects t‘: :ercial usefulnes However , Tore unsuccessful the" a ,- Hub MS 0f the tart any e xaggerated 231:39 W On what “Ch a failure 91:1 l .I Ce nt of the kite st kuan b teak NEVErth 5 The cost of this new product innovation for the 1968 fiscal year amounted to over $250 billion, or over 25 11 per cent of the Gross National Product. Yet, over 70 per cent of this cost went to products that were not successful in the market place, in fact over two-thirds went to prod- ‘ucts eliminated in the develOpment stage. Thus, about (eight of ten develOpment engineers may be said to be work- irng on projects that will not be justified in terms of com— mercial usefulness. (Basic research is not included here.) However, in spite of the increasing efforts to re- rnc>xre unsuccessful products in the develOpment stages before ‘tIleay reach the market place, many new products fail when 'tlieajr are finally introduced. (It should be noted that esti- ‘naltzeas of the rate of new product failures are almost invari- ably exaggerated. The actual rate of failure depends. of (:CD‘IJEfise, on what products are included in the base against which a failure rate is computed, as well as on the criteria e» “RE>:l.oyed to identify failures. Weiss has found that over 8 c C) I;>er cent of new products are not "new" but "simply modi- 13 f. J“C=E3Ltions" of existing products. ) Booz—Allen and Hamilton 5ft: :E>‘=>rt that in a survey of all industry groups only 62.5 1: cent of the products presented to the consumer will do ugh o u see than break-even over their first three years of sales. Nevertheless, compare the above figure to an 1,! analysis by the P: I eluded that 19 of - . 15 all: a 1972 EU- saips. food snack: 11‘. the 1970' s rou 1“” . . .". .ert'c “N ‘r-v .'. gvlm‘l fine ‘15. sipernarket produ< Pals 17 A more co new products can on the food indus PIDiucts they exe if:e it test marke1 42 3 .er cent were 6 analysis by the American Management Association which con- cluded that 19 of 20 new products could be expected to fail,15 a 1972 Business Week prediction that of the 120,000 soaps, food snacks and other supermarket products introduced in the 1970's roughly 10,000 will bomb out16 or a report of Advertising Age which predicted that 80 per cent of new supermarket products will fail as they will not meet sales goals.17 A more conservative estimate of the failure rate of new products can be found in a study by Buzzell and Nourse on the food industry. Of the 127 distinctly new food products they examined, 39 per cent were discontinued either after test marketing or after regular introduction. Also, 42 per cent were classified by their sponsors as either ex- tremely unsuccessful or moderately unsuccessful. Another criterion of product performance is the length of time required for the contribution to profit earned from a prod- uct to offset its develOpment and introduction costs. By this criterion, 44 per cent of the products failed to break even after two years of regular distribution.18 In summary it can be stated that advancing tech- nology and increasing research and development give no assurance that new products will have a high probability of success. It is not uncommon to find studies which reveal the new product 1 cent. Rescurce development Pres‘ notability is 8< framed with pro< threaten to limi1 consumer spending firm: can take c< an to one. The linited the amour Winter. Yet, i: 55:th the cons: "ill also suffer 34336 needs . seen that the fir not least keep develenient 7 the new product failure rate to be between 50 and 90 per cent. Resource allocation for new product research and development presents a problem for the firm when the failure probability is so high. While industry is constantly con- fronted with product obsolescence as new develOpments ‘threaten to limit the market life of existing goods, and cnonsumer spending patterns undergo constant shifts, few fiirms can take comfort even in a success—failure average of izvvo to one. The profit squeeze of today's economy has limited the amount of financial set-backs a firm can en- counter. Yet, if a firm doesn't seek out new products to Satisfy the consumer's ever-changing wants and desires, it ‘A?j_ll also suffer financial disaster as competitors fulfill these needs . Statement of the Problem In view of the above research findings, it can be Seen that the firm faces a high risk situation if it fails 't:‘=> at least keep pace with its competitors in new product <3tevelopment. Similarly, the firm experiences a high finan- <==iiwal risk every time it undertakes to introduce a new prod— ‘k1‘==t in the market place. If one subscribes to the theory that the ultimate objective of a firm is that of survival, {ten the firm is f. . ‘ . ‘. :ntrozuctlon. Th- zage presents only. 2:: duce the highl fin. could re-exa: I .I . 1 f” -l the first sta final stage of tes seek out newer res able marketing dec No longer guards predictin SthSS. Today t :15 r . \ flfiv, y” 3‘- I .1‘9 1-! S tage . or the 1 532:5 35 Or failU] -« ”559 are: l. 8 then the firm is forced into a condition of new product introduction. This means of seeking a differential advan- tage presents only two alternatives for the firm in order to reduce the high financial risks. First of all, the firm could re-examine its internal develOpmental process from the first stage of determining firm objectives to the final stage of test marketing. Secondly, the firm could seek out newer research techniques in order to make profit— able marketing decisions during the introduction process. No longer can the firm take the leisurely approach towards predicting product success during the introduction process. Today there is a dollar premium on time which is greater in the first month of a new product's life than at any other stage. Crawford specifically listed six important reasons for the firm to rapidly determine a new product's success or failure probabilities immediately after launch. These are: l. The attention span of consumers in the market place is short. 2. Changes can be made rapidly thanks to the speed of mass communication. 3. The size of the initial investment grows with each new product to the point where the launch of important new items is backed by dollar 9 budgets which strain the resources of Oper— ating units. 4. In any creative, market oriented company, Opportunity cost decisions abound. 5. TOp management isn't known for patience. 6. Finally, launch of a new product signals the start of many ongoing problems throughout the corporation.19 This study will concern itself with the introduction of new food products because nowhere is the problem of new product introduction more prevalent than in the retail food industry. The ex-Secretary of Agriculture, Orville Freeman, stated, "Each year about 5,000 new food products are offered to stores that already carry 8,000 different items."20 Business Week in a 1972 survey of new product marketing problems found that the three year payout is some eighteen months too long. During the last ten years, as new brands introduction more than doubled in the frozen-food and dry— grocery business, average product "1ife eXpectancies" fell from 36 months to 12 months.21 Thus, one can see that today's supermarket is faced with the problem of selecting those products which offer the potential of success and eliminating those with limited possibilities. This thesis will attempt to provide a ;;;ie‘;ine for the I :cbe retained. ". sales analysis, in :13: are examined we're information. .333: of a three cs ' AW 1“» .L..‘-§“"fl ”Messful prs ..:.asoawv How>mzom Hmaamsoo ca mo>flvommmuom .:Omuuonom .m mmaona was amenHMmmmx .m oaoumm ca oovsflnmom .:Hoooz coamfloon d uuofl>msom uoEoumso can moosuauu<= .sommouos< .m Guam "mousom .1 I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I l I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I l I I I I I I I .1 I I I I I I I J _ mmvas. ZOE—Duo «$20.58 Curr—bi— _ _ _ _ _ """ J l"'|'l"'|'|"'|'|'ll"’"l ' ‘l""|' “mason“ —1 w q!"ll""ll|.l """"" "'|)I“"U" lllll rho-“ancho‘c— " _ _ _ _ a . _ _ _ _ . 38:3 _ _ _ _ £93.“ :33 3:25.80 3:029”. Ki _ _ _ 35>» 886 209.809.. . — — — w """""" ‘ J H — _ _ _.L- E . , _ 3050a . 0 u n o. .39. .3325 14 b:3o=§< 4 _ _ qII|l_ c r .o . Snoo>u< _ coca ‘ . . . r IIIIIIII I_ 538 _A K 20:15 .E “ Mwwwwzw . L 3818 . 3c. 3 93° . . .8353 . «co soon _II_|I.1| . . m .362an .9.— 32:2. . . .coocoaoOE . Suuhfia oz 628 _ c2552... . _ «or 883m . 5:23.: 3 XS. rh _ 040: 230i _ n 2050 “cauficufi “NH.” .0 ooaua> _ a . . . . 8852 .3an 29.026... 98803600 .2502 2.2.00 w. IIIIIII .1 Eons-o .82.: 85:33 3288.. 3.333.. I I I I 333.5 .330 839i 2.30... 23E 89.3 I 083:. .3352 £2.28 mmmvomm ZOHmHUmQ Mméugm NEH. .mO ZOHBfiNHAd—HHQHUZOU ZNmfiflMQ—fim ”Han. film gUHh / in ntl I I..II| .. N—h.vH.>S~rhnm N~n.~>hiw FN puflc \hoAvanur—t... a. ..I .3; v-1~u v - ..~ ~ npruiuhm IAWHNH ANZPJN ~ hvnh Nuns 41 .mmm .m .Amoma .Ommoanov m sapwoooum oucouowcoo Housflz homa .=How>mzwm woman no huoonez .fiuosm .z nmflommb one oumBom .< GAOO mhuukuu 02¢ Wu4m4u¢d> mnOZMUOXN no u“ lumbar: IIII mkuumuu xuqmomwu IIIIII 20:.«510uz. no in C Ffimhao.k3mz_ 20:23.: zofizwxmmnis mmoathhd I 20:70.5: zolkuflumnbdm +4 _ . r IIIII l o _ muim ..0.h....._h.ou..: "monsom o... :SEmMn a Jthmqumm .__....__J U. mHUulwuhuz 20....«2c0m2. h2m220m§du Jdauom 35:3 .0 >..._:._o....__.,.>... .v wnmzuxahuurhflo .n unia .~ C3430 .. U_J0m\..>w «.8 223% _ Gogdx um wmflxumna ”53:30 muzéum .n ytfmdjfia .o mww2u>Cuzfim5 .n uoia .N T1193”. ._ u>:4unh.u....w_m wordzm all. =MOH>¢mmm MHMDm ho Mmommfiz mfimmmIQM¢ZOm HEB ‘IL . nut...” (ammo — mmuhnao _ a . I $835; _ onwxuuu _ to 835.22. . > .— 34/03: mIN mgUHh mhjaz. 42 variables, hypothetical constructs, and exogenous variables. The inputs to the buyer's internal state are stimuli from the environment, either commercial or social. These com- mercial stimuli are the marketing activities of the various firms. They may come either via the physical brands them— selves Or some linguistic or pictorial representations of the attributes of the brands. The social stimulus input I"- refers to the information that the buyer's social environ- I» ment provides regarding a purchase decision, for example, I word of mouth communication. The hypothetical constructs are enclosed within the large rectangular box, which repre— sents the consumer's internal state. Howard and Sheth have noted that the two hypothetical constructs, learning and perceptual, serve the role of endogeneous variables. The learning constructs include motives, evoked set, decision mediators, predispositions, inhibitors and satisfaction: whereas, the perceptual constructs include sensitivity to information, perceptual bias and search for information. The exogenous variables, shown at the tOp of the diagram, influence the hypothetical constructs and provide for adjustment for interpersonal differences. The response variables are ordered to create a hierarchy similar to the variety Of hierarchies of the AIDA and Lavidge and Steiner models. However, Howard and Sheth have incorporated D 5‘ .3 ”E 43 30 several feedback effects. While admitting that it is difficult to do justice to these models in so short a description, it is important that the reader notes the significant step forward that these models have provided the marketing manager. Summary of Adoption Models After an analysis of the different adOption models presented thus far, one is left with the feeling that the concept of an adOption model is logical. If a behavioral act is to occur, it must have antecedents. Therefore, such a model, by merely forcing attention on these antecedents, provides a service for the marketing manager. The concept of an adoption model is of invaluable use in increasing our understanding of consumers behavior. However, regardless of which model the market re- searcher chooses to follow, certain observations should be remembered. First of all, the process need not always conform to a single form. Granted, while most behavior is of the rational/decision-making form, the market researcher should keep in mind that the nonrationalpsychosocial form as well as the nonrational/impulse form are possible eXplanations of human behavior.31 Q i ‘ fluwr -. JJH¢n I (.5.- anJ o and EC t1”. “‘5‘“ An 44 Second, there is no maximum number of stages al- though the minimum number appears to be two. And thirdly, no specified sequence of stages must occur. Allowance must be made for consumers to skip stages and for the occurrence of feedback. For the above reasons the author prOposes another model of the adoption process. This model is derived from the 1971 innovation-decision process of Rogers and makes alterations for the above observations. This model is shown in Figure 2-6. The model is a logical extension of the previous discussion and has four advantages. 1. The model may be conceptualized as either an innovation-oriented process, that is starting with awareness, or a problem—oriented process, starting with problem perception. 2. Feedback effects are taken into consideration. 3. The model enables the reader to trace the dif— ferent sequences of behavior which might occur as the consumer may skip some stages in the adoption process. 4. The model's final stage, confirmation, allows either a continuation of the action taken in the decision stage, or the rejection of that jam Ado: ZOHPH.hhadAH< s3 m/szam MU [IN Tu Vfi ulumuvi. v." 45 Amie sowumahawcoo \ T!,|.W coamwomn .k\\ ‘A// mafia wGUHSOm GOfiumUHfiSEBOU soauoofiom pmssfiusou l . sowuuov< umumq 4 /LF AWLWMuQOp< pmsafiusoWTMHllllllr m NUGNDGfiufioumHQ i nW\\\\\\\l mmlllllllg cofiuumhmm sowuaop< AHDOZ ZOHBmond MMdZZDm mlN NMDUHm owvmasosu soauaoouom amanonm F‘ngvd- yn‘v - I ‘i'c ‘n ~.u- ‘1‘ ~.' . inch . Q 46 decision.32 This model will be discussed in greater detail in Chapter III. Industrial Goods and the Adoption Models Ozanne and Churchill have examined the process in the industrial goods market and found the industrial pro— cess to be the same as the traditional one except that the trial stage might be eliminated with indivisible innova- tions.33 They did state that where limited scale prototypes could be used, the trial stage would be continued. Contrary to predictions, however, they found "personal sources (in particular personal selling) were more important at early stages, while impersonal sources (especially the price quo— tation and tooling proposal) were paramount at the evalua— tion stage. The available evidence also suggests that as the final decision approaches, the need for informational inputs increases. At the evaluation stage the industrial decision makers employ a larger number and a greater 0 I I u 34 variety of information sources than at the earlier stages." The Diffusion Process Rogers has defined diffusion as the process by which innovations spread to the members of a social system. (He had previously defined it as the process by which one 47 follows the spread of a new idea from its source of inven- 35 Thus, tion or creation to its ultimate users or adOpters. as he sees it, there are four crucial elements in the dif— fusion of new ideas: the innovation which is communicated through certain channels over time among the members of a social system. Here the term innovation is the idea, prac- tice or object perceived as new by an individual. Rogers states that it is the perceived or subjective newness of the idea, and not the "objective" newness, for the individ- ual that is important. Communication is the process by which messages are transmitted from a source to a receiver. The channel is the means by which these messages move from source to receiver. Time is the important element in the process. Katz has been quoted as saying "time is the key to diffusion research". Time is usually measured in three dimensions: 1. The length of time in innovation-decision pro— cess, that is the length of time during which an individual passes from first knowledge of the innovation to the adOption or rejection of that product. 2. The innovativeness of the individual, that is, the relative earliness-lateness with which an individual adOpts an innovation when compared 48 with other members of his social system. 3. The innovation's rate of adoption in a social system, usually measured as the number of members of the system that adOpt the innova- tion in a given time period. A social system is defined as a collectivity of 1 units which are functionally differentiated and engaged in joint problem solving with respect to a common goal. The members or units of a social system may be individuals, informed groups, complex organizations or subsystems.36 The Diffusion Process in Marketing The diffusion process in marketing can be conceptu- alized as: the adoption of new products and services over time by consumers within social systems as encouraged by marketing activities. Adoption refers to the use of a new innovation. New products can be any product perceived as providing additional utility by the consumer. The time dimension will distinguish early adOpters from late adOpters. Consumers will refer to the consumer adopting unit, be it individual, family, organization or political unit. Social systems constitute the boundaries within which diffusion occurs. This may range from family to friendship groups to the entire market place. Marketing . _ ‘— “III?!“ 5 [in | H. \ 'I 5‘ ”- a. L :w 1. )1 A; 49 activities will refer to those activities undertaken by the firm in order to gain consumer patronage. These most gen- erally include the mixing of the various marketing variables of product, price, promotions and place in forming an Opti- mum marketing mix. These aspects of the diffusion process are interde- pendent. For example: the attributes of the new product will affect the rate of adOption over time, the types of consumer who will adOpt, the kinds of social systems within which diffusion will take place, and the marketing efforts needed to achieve diffusion. Similarly, the marketing manager must realize that successful new product diffusion is critically dependent upon the communication of relevant product information and matching the self images of social system members with the perceived product images. That marketing activities can guide and control the rate of adoption is witnessed by Zaltman, in which the various marketing strategies are explained in terms of past studies from the area of behavioral sciences.37 Adopter Categories Not all individuals in a social system adopt an in- novation at the same time. Rather, individuals adopt in an ordered time sequence and they may be classified into x /_ *9 eb CE. m C» 50 adOpter categories on the basis of when they first begin using a new idea. Figure 2-7 shows the traditional five categories of classifying adOpters. The figure, also, shows the approximate percentage of individuals in these five categories (innovators, early adopters, early majority, late majority, and laggards). Rogers notes the above clas- sification is not symmetrical and it fails to account for incomplete adOption. However, neither of these criticisms has distracted from the model.38 FIGURE 2-7 ADOPTER CATEGORIZATION ON THE BASIS OF INNOVATIVENESS N h----------b--- : . : : muovnons i : EARLY . EARLY um: : .Aooprms: MAJORITY MAJORITY . LAGGARDS 2.5%: 13.5% g 34% 34% : 15% A I—zsd I-sd I+sd The innovativeness dimension, as measured by the time at which an individual adopts an innovation or innovations, is continuous. However, this variable may be partitioned into five adopter categories by laying off standard deviations from the average time of adoption. Source: Everett M. Rogers with F. Floyd Shoemaker, Communication of Innovations (New Ybrk: The Free Press, 1971), p. 182. ::e f i,"F\“'r fiend} I ~4 A e Stanton, 51 in reviewing the work of Rogers, has drawn the following observations concerning these five categories. Innovators: Early adOpters: Early majority: Late majority: Laggards: They are the first to adOpt and have been referred to as venturesome. They tend to be young and, at the same time, high in social and economic status. They are cosmOpolites, with many contacts outside their own social groups and community. This group tends to rely on impersonal and scientific information sources or other innovators rather than personal salesmen. This group is likely to be relatively high in social status, probably being Opinion leaders. They may be younger, more mobile and more creative than later adopters. Their social relationships are confined to local groups and they have the greatest contact of all the groups with salesmen. This category consists of those with above average social status. They usually will not consider an innovation until early adOpters have tried it. A long period may elapse between trial and adoption. This deliberate group has considerable contact with mass media and salesmen and early adOpters. PeOple in this group tend to be below average in social status and income. They are less likely to follow Opinion leaders and early adopters. Some social pressure might have to be applied to this group in order for the product to be tried. They are a skeptical group. This group makes little use of mass media and of salesmen. They tend to be oriented more to other late adOpters than to outside sources of information. This group has the lowest social status and income, and tends to be tradition— bound. Their main source of information is other laggards.39 112' 316 CEIELI s‘abcg‘ ' 5‘: 29.131? 52 From a content analysis of over 3,000 research findings relating various independent variables to innova- tiveness, Rogers has made thirty-two generalizations con— cerning innovativeness on the basis of (l) socio-economic status, (2) personality variables and (3) communication behavior.40 These findings are reprinted here in summary form along with the number of supporting and non-supporting studies. Socio-economic Characteristics 1. Earlier adopters are no different from later adopters in age. (228: 44 younger, 108 no relationship, 76 older) Earlier adOpters have more years of education than do later adOpters. (275: 203—72)41 Earlier adOpters are more likely to be literate than are later adOpters. (38: 24-14) Earlier adOpters have higher social status than later adopters. (402: 275-127) Earlier adOpters have a greater degree of upward social mobility than do later adOpters. (5: 5-0) Earlier adOpters have larger size units (farms, etc.) than do late adOpters. (227: 152-75) Earlier adopters are more likely to have a com- mercial (rather than a subsistence) orientation than are later adopters. (28: 20-8) Earlier adopters have a more favorable attitude toward credit than later adOpters. (25: 19-6) Earlier adopters have more specialized opera— tions than later adOpters. (15: 9-6) w— 53 Personality Variables 10. Earlier adOpters have greater empathy than later adopters. (14: 9-5) 11. Earlier adOpters are less dogmatic than later adOpters. (36: 17-19) 12. Earlier adopters have a greater ability to deal with abstractions than do later adopters. (8: 5-3) 13. Earlier adopters have greater rationality than later adopters. (14: 11-3) 14. Earlier adOpters have greater intelligence than later adOpters. (5: 5-0) 15. Earlier adopters have a more favorable attitude toward change than later adOpters. (57: 43-14) 16. Earlier adopters have a more favorable attitude toward risk than later adopters. (37: 27—10) 17. Earlier adopters have a more favorable attitude toward education than later adopters. (31: 25-6) 18. Earlier adopters have a more favorable attitude toward science than later adopters. (27: 20-7) 19. Earlier adOpters are less fatalistic than later adopters. (17: 14-3) 20. Earlier adOpters have higher levels of achieve- ment motivation than later adOpters. (23: 14-9) 21. Earlier adOpters have higher aSpirations (for education, occupations, etc.) than later adOpters. (39: 29-10) Communication Behavior 22. Earlier adOpters have more social participation than later adOpters. (149: 109-40) VAL “1‘ l‘. 54 23. Earlier adOpters are more highly integrated with the social system than later adOpters. (6: 6-0) 24. Earlier adOpters are more cosmopolite than later adOpters. (174: 132-42) 25. Earlier adopters have more change agent con- tact than later adOpters. (156: 135-21) 26. Earlier adOpters have greater exposure to mass media communication channels than later adOpters. (116: 80-36) 27. Earlier adopters have greater exposure to inter— personal communication channels than later adopters. (60: 46-14) 28. Earlier adopters seek information about inova- tions more than later adOpters. (14: 12-2) 29. Earlier adOpters have greater knowledge of innovations than later adOpters. (80: 61-19) 30. Earlier adOpters have a higher degree of Opinion leadership than later adOpters. (55: 42-13) 31. Earlier adopters are more likely to belong to social systems with modern rather than tradi- tional norms than are later adOpters. (46: 32-14) 32. Earlier adOpters are more likely to belong to well integrated systems than are later adOpters. (15: 8-7)42 The Innovation-Decision Period The innovation-decision period is the length of time required to pass through the innovation—decision pro— 43 cess. The length is usually measured from first knowledge until the decision to adOpt (or reject), although in a 55 strict sense it should perhaps be measured to the time of confirmation. This last step is often impractical or im- possible because the confirmation functions may continue over an indefinite period of time. Rogers has listed ten generalizations concerning variables affecting this period and the length of time involved. By way of providing a summary for the reader of the research already performed in this area the Rogers list follows along with the number of supporting and nonsupporting studies. 1. Earlier knowers of an innovation have more education than later knowers. (24: 17—7) 2. Earlier knowers of an innovation have higher social status than later knowers. (28: 18-10) 3. Earlier knowers of an innovation have greater eXposure to mass media channels of communica— tion than later knowers. (29: 18-11) 4. Earlier knowers of an innovation have greater exposure to interpersonal channels of communi- cation than later knowers. (18: 16-2) 5. Earlier knowers of an innovation have greater change agent contact than later knowers. (26: 23-3) 6. Earlier knowers of an innovation have more social participation than later knowers. (13: 11-2) 7. Earlier knowers of an innovation are more cosmopolite than later knowers. (5: 5-0) 8. Later adopters are more likely to discontinue innovations than are earlier adOpters. (6: 6-0) ::~‘l A‘s». i926 ~f‘, n st . 56 9. The rate of awareness-knowledge for an inno- vation is more rapid than its rate of adOption. (2: 2-0) 10. Earlier adOpters have a shorter innovation- decision period than later adopters. (6: 5-1)44 ROgers' earlier text, also, went into the differ- ences between personal and impersonal communications as factors of increasing awareness. At that time he made the generalization that "impersonal information sources are most important at the awareness stage, and personal sources are most important at the evaluation stage in adoption " 45 . . process . A second generalization from that text was that "cosm0polite information sources are most important at the awareness stage, and localite information sources are . . n 46 . most important at the evaluation stage . Later marketing studies have supported these generalizations. Arndt dis— closed that "product-related word of mouth was found to flow from early to late adOpters and noncadOpters. More than two-thirds of the comments were received by respondents 47 Two other interesting findings who had not bought yet". of his were that "compared with the non—exposed individuals, those receiving favorable word of mouth pressure were more likely to buy the product, while those exposed to unfavor- 48 able word of mouth were less likely to buy", and ”respond— ents low in generalized self-confidence seemed to react to Ha.- word 01 me Sumzer :eerin (I) 57 word of mouth in an ego defensive manner. They also tended to be less likely to be exposed to word of mouth."49 Summers stated that his research suggests that the volun- teering of unsolicited product information is generally more common in interpersonal channels than information seeking behavior.50 Diffusion Effect The diffusion effect51 is the cumulatively increas- ing degree of influence upon an individual to adopt or re— ject an innovation, resulting from the increasing rate of knowledge and adOption or rejection of the innovation in 52 The diffusion effect is often listed the social system. as the reason for the increasing rate of growth of the in- novation in the diffusion process. It is thought that if every consumer considered adopting on an individual basis, without social influence, then the probability of adOption would be the same for everybody regardless of time period. However, if consumer influence is introducted than a "snow— balling" effect will occur, since other's previous experi— ence with the innovation will influence the present deci— sion. Summers' work can lead to the generalizations that personal influences gain as the risk involved increases (higher prices, greater complexity of product) and that ‘o A n U one VIN! . \lu . H ‘ y Hg. ‘ d ‘h 5" 'l-A 58 other factors which will induce the diffusion effect include product ownership and competence.53 Other research to support the diffusion effect in- clude the studies used in abandonment of the hypodermic needle model which postulated that the mass media had direct, immediate and powerful effects on a mass audience. Another is the introduction of the "two-step flow" model which hypothesized that information is moved from sources to Opinion leaders, who in turn influence their followers. The three most famous studies here include the 1940 Erie County Election Study, 1954 Decatur Study of Opinion Lead- ership and the Coleman, Katz and Menzel Drug Study.54 Subsequent research by Allvine and Arndt in the area of retail grocery sales have reconfirmed the diffusion effect and use S-shape growth patterns in diffusion process. Allvine's findings in a study of the acceptance of pro- motional games by supermarkets found that the growth pat- terns suggested both a diffusion effect (he called it interaction) and a S-shape diffusion process. He also found that the rate of diffusion was prOportional to the 55 Arndt found two measures importance of the first adOpter. of sociometric integration (number of close friends and number of persons with whom you are likely to discuss new food products) were significantly positively related both 59 to whether a respondent received word of mouth communication and to acceptance of the new product. Generalized self- confidence was also positively related.56 Opinion Leadership Much has already been written in this chapter with regard to Opinion leaders, communication flows and inter- personal relationships affecting buyer behavior. Yet, Mancuso's reminder to marketing managers that ”Opinion leaders have not been fully utilized...in assisting with new product introduction", still remains true.57 Opinion leadership,58 the degree to which an indi— vidual is able to influence informally other individual's attitudes or overt behavior in a desired way with relative 59 frequency, have been widely discussed in recent marketing 60 yet marketing managers still know very publications, little concerning its profitable use. At the present time, there appear to be four basic strategies with regards to use of Opinion leadership in new product introduction. The first is to create leaders in a manner similar to Mancuso's record shOp experiment, in which teenage panels were used to enable "select" records 61 to reach the TOp Ten charts only in panel cities. Another example would be to offer certain selected F? 02‘s 5w. Tu; oug ‘L' (.1: ‘ I ‘1 lo (I) 60 consumers "special deals" on new products. This method is costly and usually does not locate new Opinion leaders. The third method would be to locate and identify Opinion leaders. However, this becomes more difficult as market size increases. The most common approach toward influencing the buyer is to focus on the characteristics of the Opinion leaders in general and then aim a promotional campaign at those characteristics. Industrial Goods and the Diffusion Process Martilla has found in research conducted in three industrial markets that word of mouth communication within firms is an important influence in the later stages of the adOption process. Word of mouth communication between firms was found to be more situational. Opinion leaders were found to be more heavily exposed to impersonal sources of information than other buying influentials in the firm. The study also reported that, as in consumer marketing, indus- trial Opinion leaders are difficult to locate and identify using available demographic data.62 Webster, a year earlier in interviews with industrial buyers, failed to identify a significant amount Of word of mouth communication in indus— trial markets and suggested a key role for manufacturers' Db. 61 63 salesmen. Innovation Characteristics Rogers' Characteristics As noted earlier, the rate of growth and the extent of product diffusion are largely a function of the per— ceived attributes of the innovation. Rogers has prOposed a set of five characteristics which contribute to the ex— planation of the different rates of adOption. While realizing that they are not a complete list, but at least the most important characteristics, he has found that (1) relative advantage, (2) compatibility, (3) complexity, (4) trialability and (5) Observability are useful in de— scribing the rate of adOption.64 Relative advantage is the degree to which an inno- vation is perceived as being better than the idea it super- sedes. While this characteristic is often measured in terms Of utility by the user. For example, Rogers notes that the major advantage of 2,4-D weed spray was the reduction in unpleasant labor tasks, rather than in financial gains per se. In his review of the literature, Rogers found 29 of 43 studies agreed that relative advantage was positively re- 65 lated to rate of adOption. Thus, marketing managers have sought means of encouraging the consumer to perceive a 62 greater value in their product than that of the competition. Compatibility is the degree to which an innovation is perceived as consistent with the existing values, past experiences and needs of the consumers.66 The introduction of self-cleaning ovens required no changes in the way a housewife went about baking. Electronic ovens, however, cook much more rapidly and don't "brown" food to the same extent. Because they require a change in the way cooking has traditionally been done, electronic ovens are likely to 67 Rogers has found 18 encounter a slower rate of adOption. of 27 studies agreeing with the premise that the greater the need for consumers to restructure their thinking and to engage in new forms of behavior, the less quickly the item is to be adOpted.68 Complexity is the degree to which an innovation is perceived as relatively difficult to understand and use. Nine of sixteen studies have agreed to the negative rela— tionship between complexity and rate of adOption. An ex— ample of this was diffusion of canasta and television among different social classes. Television was considered to be less complex for the lower classes. Trialability is the degree to which an innovation may be experimented with on a limited basis. In—store sampling of a new food product or the introduction of trial 63 size packages account for the factor in which nine of thirteen studies agree. Observability is the degree to which the results of an innovation are visible to others. Fashion trends move rapidly through their life cycles due to their Observability. Seven of nine studies agree to this finding. As mentioned earlier, the important point is how these characteristics are perceived by the consumers and not the subjective evaluation. Thus, it can be generalized that the rate of adOption is positively related to relative advantage, compatibility, trialability and Observability and negatively related to complexity.69 Other Studies Other studies have pointed out other characteris- tics. One of these studies was by Mansfield and the re— sults of that study have been used to support the idea of the diffusion effect. Mansfield studied the rapidity with which twelve innovations spread through the industrial sector. A major finding was that the prOportion of firms already using an innovation would increase the rate of adoption. This lends support to the notion of the band- wagon characteristic. Mansfield's hypotheses were: 1. Profitability of an innovation relative to others that are available will increase the 64 rate of adOption. 2. The larger the investment required, assuming equally profitable innovations available, the slower the rate of adOption. 3. The type of industry will affect the rate of adOption depending on its aversion to risk, market competitiveness, and financial health. 70 Finally, as mentioned earlier, the only other mar— keting studies in this area are with regard to risk percep— tion. These studies have shown that the risk perceived by consumers in new product adOption is negatively related to buying behavior. Studies have also shown it to be a major . 7l factor in buyer response to new products. Product Elimination Studies Background No other area of marketing probably has as little written on it as that pertaining to products which are to 72 Berenson, be eliminated from the firms product line. Grashof and Rothe73 all have commented that the literature on product elimination is extremely sparse and vaguely defined: no body of knowledge exists that can be referred to for guidance for action in this idea. The few contribu— tions have all been theoretical in nature and of somewhat limited use to small-to—medium size retailers. The product elimination area is further clouded as to whom should have RE . ‘I to ‘ 1 f1 65 the responsibility for such action. The traditional ap- proach has been to centralize such authority in the home office of the chains,74 however at the present time a change is being made toward giving the individual store managers some authority on their product line.75 (It should be noted that the chain co-Operating with this re- search project was an early adOpter of the latter method.) Rothe has studied the different factors involved in the product elimination decision in the food industry, as well as the drug, clothing and major and minor appliances industries. One of his findings was that firms in the food industry rated product elimination activities approximately one-third as important as new product activities. In the recognition stage of locating weak products, little atten- tion was given to product profitability. This came at the next stage, analysis. The major factors for food items at the recognition stage were: minimum dollar volume, minimum unit volume, minimum market share percentage, some compari- son of today's market share with previous years and percent- age of total company sales this product contributes. Prod— uct profitability is the major factor in the analysis stage. A final finding of Rothe was that while much of the litera— ture dealing with this subject dwells on the formality issue, the food industry respondents were less formal in 66 their elimination decisions.76 Traditional Approaches Before reviewing the theoretical approaches to product elimination suggested in the literature and moving on to a discussion of actual practices in the retail grocery industry, it will be useful to first review some of the traditional lines of thought. According to Berenson, product elimination decisions have traditionally been domi- nated by four different viewpoints: the accountant's, the economist's, the sales manager's and, perhaps to a lesser extent, the government policy maker's. The accountant's customary view of a product line deletion involves a comparison of the dollar costs of reten— tion with the dollar cost of abandonment. This approach is concerned with quantitative measures of depreciation or product disposal costs of a food item, current expenditures and revenues. The primary emphasis is on quantifiable financial items. While Berenson makes a distinction between the accountant's and the economist's view, both are basi— cally cost-revenue decisions. For the economist, Berenson notes that product eli— mination is a matter of emphasizing the future and leaving the past for historical record. The prime considerations 67 in this method are alternative choices and marginal costs. It involves questions of incremental profits - for example, the possibility that the product may be in the black on an out-of—pocket cost basis and can therefore make some con- tribution to general overhead. HOpefully, the sales manager's vieWpOint would be a synthesis of both the accounting and economic traditions. However, this expectation is usually unfulfilled. The sales manager's approach to the problem has been largely intuitive. It stresses the factors that may make the line easier to sell but not necessarily more profitable — for example, it favors carrying a full line and seeking to build volume at the expense of over-all, long-term profit. The decision criteria for the government policy maker relates to public interest. The government tends to consider continued satisfaction of the consumer as an over- riding criterion. Hence, railroads regulated by the ICC cannot readily abandon trackage or other services when the line as a whole is making a profit.77 Theoretical Approaches As has been noted earlier, the vast majority Of firms do not have established procedures for pruning their products. Such action is usually undertaken either (1) on 68 a piecemeal basis, as in instances where the product's money-losing status is incontrovertible, conspicuous, and embarrassing, or (2) on a crisis basis, wherein the pre- cipitating event may be a financial setback, a persistent decline in total sales, piling inventories or rising costs. However, neither of these practices has been suc- cessful in the long run.78 In an attempt to provide the firm with a more reliable method of eliminating non-produc- ing products from their line a number of theoretical models have been prOposed. These models have as their basis a thorough analysis of the basic concepts of marketing, finance, management, psychology and accounting. One of the earlier theoretical models prOposed was that of Berenson. He presented a model (Figure 2-8) which considered five major decision factors: financial security, financial Opportunity, marketins strategy, social respon- sibility, and the possibility of organized intervention against product deletion. The first two criteria are readily quantified: the first relating an evaluation of the basic profit criteria of the firm, and the second which provides an Opportunity to consider the profitability of the product in terms of opportunity costs, phase of the product's life cycle, and the amount of return in excess of the firm's minimum goal. He suggested that a judgment- (59 .mcowfluom mmmcfimsm . <4 coauom oxmu .qx afloOmEonudmm ma >w ma m< coauom mxmu .mx kHOumE«xouadm ma >w ma N4 coauom oxmu .Nx mamumefixouaam mH >w we H< coauom mxmu .Hx kaoumauxouadm we >w we >w so ao< =OCwq uospoum wzu mcflcnum: .66 .m .momH .GOmcmumm pmucou “mousom coauom oamwooam m sues pmumHouuoo ma umnu >w mo Hm>ma OLu u >w he paumowpafi sofiuom man u < mousmmoa onu «0 85m ozu I maumufiuo one mo mocmuuooefl m>HumHmu osu mo musmmoa m u .HQEEDW coauco>uOucH conacmwuo wawuommmm muouomm muaaanflmaoamou Hmauom wcfiuummmm muouomm mwmumuum wcfiuoxuma mcwuomwmm mHOuomm huficsuuoaao Hmfiocosfiw wsauuowmm muouomm I m _ > a uq< _ > um< >w. u m> .~< l ~> "H< munwuos mwumufiuo and ee< use unwaas AWQOZ Bzmzzonzoamm mnu macho 70 determined numerical scale could be established to accom— modate the remaining three factors. The subjective weights assigned to these factors are to reflect the degree of im- portance attributed them by management. The score for each category is then multiplied by the weighting factor, and the summation of the five weighted scores becomes the over- all rating of the product under question.7 The Kotler model (Figure 2-9) for product pruning is, as he admitted, an expensive one in terms of executive time, but the cost must be compared to the greater cost of keeping a sub-optimal product in the line. The model, a PERT approach, is made up of a creation and Operational level. The creation level is composed of the develOpment of a representative corporate team and the establishment of objectives and procedures related to product pruning. The Operational level is a six-step approach, the first of which consists of management preparing a data sheet for every company product. The data sheet contains all the important information about the product during the past three years. The data sheet is to provide information for judging the product by the management team. Step 2 consists Of a computer program to review the data from Step 1 for any signs of weakness. For example, the firm might set the standard of a sales decline in any four THE KOTLER ABANDONMENT MODEL Cltl-IA'I'ION STAGES OPERATIONAL STEPS U Ito-cognition U AirfliniS lesc Out i Source: Philip Kotler, 71 FIGURE 2-9 Appoint a product review (mnrnittcc Hold mr-olings to set Objectives a t id proccd u; cs related to product pruning .— i l. Controller's office fills out product (lain—streets . Computer program no. 1 determines dubious products 3. Mmmgzr-mr‘nt tr-am ‘i’ fills out ruling [onus for dubious products Y 5. Managtuunt trrsun reviews thcsc indexes and decides on products to drop (r 6. Management loam develops policies and plans {or phasing out “dropped” products . Computer program no. 2 dr‘tr‘rmiur‘s prodm-t rrtontion index for molt dubious product J "Phasing Out Weak Products", Harvard Business Review, March—April, 1965, p. 112. 72 periods during the three years as a sign of weakness. The remaining three steps are similar to Berenson's model. A rating form is prOposed for detailed analysis of the dele- tion candidates. Management then assigns a numerical score to the categories on the form. weighted factors are applied to the scores, and the weighted scores are summed to obtain an overall "product retention index”. Product deletion decisions can then be made using a cut—off point in the retention index. At this point management may make some subjective judgments as to possible customer reactions. Lastly, plans and policies for phasing out "drOpped" prod- ucts are developed. For each product, management must determine its obligations to the various parties affected by the decision. Here it may decide to stock a reasonable amount of replacement parts or to seek out another manu- facturer for the product.80 Hamelman and Mazze in 1972 introduced an extension of the Kotler model called PRESS (Product Review and Evalu- ation Subsystem). This model is different from other product—elimination models in that it is capable of c0ping with a company's total product line rather than a segment of the line thought to be weak. The program consists of four integrated parts, PRESS I through PRESS IV. PRESS I contains the primary 73 model and uses standard cost accounting and marketing per- formance data, while PRESS II, III, and IV perform analyses concerned with price changes, sales trends, and product interaction. The PRESS model differs from Kotler's approach in that it reduces the amount of executive management decision time and that it looks at the entire product line. Whereas Kotler provided broad guidelines for his model, PRESS con— siders product line interactions and Operational aspects of deletion decisions. The retention index of Kotler yielded a single number indicating the degree of product desirability from the weighted ratings on the seven subjective scales. PRESS offers cutoff points for deletion decisions by a systematic review of Selection Index Numbers, which are based on a series of performance ratios using standard Cost accounting data.81 It is interesting to note at this point, that any of the adOption process models mentioned earlier in this chap- ter can, also, be used as a product elimination model. The adOption criteria and the retention criteria are basically the same, the difference is that one involves forecasting and the other measurement. However, both include a subjec— tive weighting of the processes' elements. 74 Retail Grocery Practices At the present time the actual practice being under- taken in the retail grocery industry appears to being moving away from the main office and back to the store manager with regard to product elimination decisions. The supermarket operations of today while approaching the problem in a manner similar to the theoretical concepts mentioned earlier do it with far less sOphisticated techniques. Grashof re— viewed the process and noted that the chains have two pro- cedures for the identification of items that should be con— sidered for deletion. The first method is that one old product be drOpped for every new one added. Since all figures indicate that the number of retail food items car— ried in stock by the average supermarket increases by over 20% each year, one must realize that this rule is not adhered to 100 per cent. The second procedure is for a periodic review of all items the chain handles. This review can be conducted by the buyer for each product family, the head buyer who examines all items carried by the chain or, as the trend is now moving, by the manager of the individual store who reviews all his own products. As reported by Grashof and confirmed by later interviews with three chain executives, the prime factor in 75 the identification of items for possible deletion, as well as the most important criterion for use in the final deci- sion is lack of consumer demand for an item as indicated by a low rate of sales for the item. Another important criterion is the level Of the gross margin percentage of the item. By combining these two factors a third criterion - gross margin dollars gener- ated per unit of time - can be determined. While this third method is not as important as the first two, it does permit a comparison between dissimilar items in a product family, as well as across product families. At this point, two other factors should be men- tioned. First, the chain will view the trend as well as absolute values for the three criteria stated earlier: and, second, the chains, in their desire to maintain variety on the store's shelves, will hesitate to delete one—of—a—kind items. Thus, one can see that while the chains have never develOped a mathematical model for eliminating products from their stock, they have a set of criteria. Unfortu— nately, they still appear to make these important decisions by weighing these three factors and adding a fourth one, the individual's making the decision, personal interest in the product. 76 Summary Chapter II has reviewed the relevant literature with regard to the adOption and diffusion of innovations, the relationship between these processes and the consumer's perception of the innovation's characteristics and the pres- ent policies of product elimination. These four areas were chosen for study since this thesis develOps a new method of predicting which products should be eliminated from the product line on the basis of their rate of diffusion into a social system. The adOption process has been presented as a means of determining what the final diffusion cycle will be. It is hypothesized that the measurements of the level of activity in the early stages of the process can be used to predict the ultimate outcome of the decision stage. It might also be noted that while this thesis is concerned with the consumer's adOption process in an attempt to predict product elimination it can also be used to sup- port the notion that product elimination is the central focus in the decision or trial stage of the retailer's adOption process. 10. ll. 12. 77 FOOTNOTES Everett M. ROgers, Bibliggraphy on the Diffusion of Ipnovations (East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan State University, College of Communication Arts, 1968). Everett M. Rogers, Diffusion of Innovation (New York: The Free Press, 1962), p. 23. Everett M. Rogers with F. Floyd Shoemaker, Communica- tion of Innovations (New York: The Free Press, 1971) and Thomas S. Robertson, Innovative Behavior and Communication (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1971). Rogers with Shoemaker, p. 99. Rogers, Diffusion of Innovation, p. 17. Rogers with Shoemaker, p. 7. Bryce Ryan and Neal C. Gross, "The Diffusion of HYbrid Seed Corn in Two Iowa Communities," Rural Sociology, VIII (March, 1943), 15-24. Rogers, Diffusion of Innovation, p. 35. Eugene A. Wilkening, Acceptance of Improved Farm Practices, North Carolina Agricultural Experiment Station Bullegin #98, 1952. Rogers with Shoemaker, pp. 100-101. For a good review of cognitive dissonance theory in marketing, the reader should see Jack B. Cohen and Marvin E. Goldberg, "Dissonance Model in Post-Decision Product Evaluation," Journal of Marketing Researgp, VII (August, 1970), 315-321, for their instant coffee experiment results, Shelby D. Hunt, "Post—transaction Communications and Dissonance Reductions," Journal of Marketing, XXXIV (July, 1970), 46-51 for means of dis- Marketing, XXXIII (October, 1969), 50-55. E. Jerome McCarthy, Basic Marketing: A Managerial Approach, 4th ed. (Homewood, R. D. Irwin and Co., 1971), p. 194. 13. 14. 15. l6. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 78 Robertson, Innovative Behavior, pp. 58-62. RObert J. Lavidge and Gary A. Steiner, "A Model for Predictive Measurements of Advertising Effectiveness," Journal of Marketing, XXV (October, 1961), 59-62. RObertson, Innovative Behavior, p. 59. Robert Mason, "An Ordinal Scale for Measuring the AdOption Process" in Wilbur Schramm, editor, Studies of Innovation and of Communication to the Public (Stanford, California: Stanford University Institute for communication Research, 1962). Rogers with Shoemaker, p. 101. RObertson, Innovative Behavior, pp. 61-62. For a discussion of how perceived risk might affect the individual's purchasing behavior the reader should see Jagdish N. Sheth and M.‘Venketesan, "Risk-Reduc- tion Processes in Repetitive Consumer's Behavior," Journal of Marketing Research, V (August, 1969), 307- 310, Scott M. Cunningham, "Perceived Risk as a Factor in the Diffusion of New Product Information," in Proceedings of 1966 Fall AMA Conference (Chicago, 1966), pp. 698-721, and Johan Arndt, "Perceived Risk, Sociometric Integration and Word of Mouth in the AdOption of New Food Products," 1966 Proceedings, op. cit., p. 644-48. Robertson, Innovative Behavior, p. 62. Rex R. Campbell, "A Suggested Paradigm of the Indi- vidual AdOption Process," Rural Sociology: XXXI (December, 1966), 458-466. Rogers with Shoemaker, pp. 101-03. Ibid., p. 120. Alan R. Andreasen, "Attitudes and Customer Behavior: A Decision Model," Reprinted in Harold H. Kassayian and Thomas S. RObertson, Perspectives in Consumer Behavior (Glenview, Illinois: Scott, Foresman and Company, 1968), pp. 498-510. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 79 John A. Howard and Jagdish N. Sheath, "Theory of Buyer Behavior," Proceedings of 1967 Winter AMA Conference (Chicago, 1968), pp. 253-262. Francesco M. Nicosia, Consumer Decision Processes (Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1966). gpgg., pp. 153-191. Robertson, Innovative Behavior, pp. 69-73. Andreasen, "Attitude and Customer Behavior," pp. 498- 510. Howard and Sheth, 1967 Winter Proceedings. PP. 254-62. For a more complete definition of the different be- havioral types see George Katona, The Powerful Con- sumer (New York: McGraw—Hill, 1960), p. 138. It should be noted that Robertson, also, presented a redoing of the adOption model on pages 73 to 77. His model was a take off of the earlier models and in- cluded eight stages. This writer feels confident in presenting his own summary model in view of the intro- duction of the Rogers innovation-decision model which was published after Robertson's work and results of a case study by Fred D. Reynolds, "Problem Solving and Trial Use in the AdOption Process," Journal of Market- ingResearch, VIII (February, 1971), 100-102 which found evidence for problem—oriented behavior. Rogers has defined indivisible innovations as those innovations which may not be experimented with on a limited basis. Rogers, Diffusion of Innovation, p. 131. Urban B. Ozanne and Gilbert A. Churchill, "AdOption Research: Information Sources in the Industrial Pur— chasing Decision," Proceedings of 1968 Fall AMA Con— ferengg (Chicago, 1968), p. 359. (The reader might also consult Frederick E. Webster, Jr., "New Product AdOption in Industrial Markets: A Framework for Analysis," Journal of Marketing, XXXIII (July, 1969), 35-39. Rogers, Diffusion of Innovation, p. 13. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. SO. 80 Rogers with Shoemaker, p. 18-28. Gerald Zaltman, Marketing: Contributions from the Behavioral Sciences (New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, Inc., 1965). Rogers with Shoemaker, p. 175-182. William J. Stanton, Fundamentals of Marketing, 3rd ed. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1971), pp. 206-208. Rogers with Shoemaker, p. 185. In the remaining listings of generalizations the first number refers to the number Of studies concerned with the generalization. The following two refer to the number of studies supporting and not supporting the generalization. ROgers with Shoemaker, Appendix A. It should be noted that most research in this area has been concerned with the earlier adOpters. However, Uhl, Andrews and Poulsen (Journal of Marketing Research, VII, 51-54) in their article "How Are Laggards Different? An Empiri- cal Inquiry" present an example of the research being done in this area by marketing peOple. They found that laggards may not be as different in terms of some socioeconomic factors from earlier adOpters. The major differences were in income, brand loyalty and family size. Ibid., p. 128. Rogers with Shoemaker, Appendix A. Rogers, Diffusion of Innovation, p. 99. Ibid., p. 102. Arndt, 1966 Winter Proceedipgs, p. 646. Ibid., p. 647. Ibid., p. 648. John Summers, "New Product Interpersonal Communica- tions," Combined Proceedings, Spring and Fall AMA 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 81 Conferences, 1971. (Chicago, 1971), 433. In his 1962 edition, Rogers used the term "interaction effect" and defined it as "the process through which individuals in a social system who have adOpted an innovation influence those who have not yet adOpted". (Rogers with Shoemaker, p. 138). Rogers with Shoemaker, p. 161. Summer, p. 433. ROgers with Shoemaker, pp. 203-209. Fred C. Allvine, "Diffusion Of a Competitive Innova— tion," Proceedings of 1968 Fall AMA Conference (Chicago, 1968), pp. 341-352. JOhan Arndt, "Role of Product-Related Conversations in the Diffusion of a New Product," Journal of Marketing Research, IV (August, 1967), p. 293. Joseph R. Mancuso, "Why Not Create Opinion Leaders for New Product Introductions?" JOurnal of Marketing, XXXIII (Ju1y, 1969), 20-25. For a more complete look at Opinion leadership, leader traits and leadership overlap, the reader should con- sult RObertson pp. 175-189, Rogers, 1971, Chapter 6 and Johan Arndt, Word of Mouth Advertising: A Review of the Literature (New York: Advertising Research Foundation, 1967). Rogers with Shoemaker, p. 224. Mancuso, pp. 20-25, Lawrence G. Corey, "PeOple Who Claim to be Opinion Leaders: identifying Their Characteristics by Self-Report," Journal of Marketing, XXXV (October, 1971), 48—53. David B. Montgomery and Alvin J. Silk, "Clusters of Consumer Interests and Opinion Leaders' Spheres of Influence," Journal of Marketing Research, VII (August, 1971), 317-321. Charles W. King and John O. Summers, "Overlap of Opinion Leadership Across Consumer Product Cate- gories," Journal of Marketing Research, VII (February, 1970), 43-50. 61. 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. 82 Mancusco, pp. 20-25. John A. Martilla, "Word-of—Mouth Communication in the Industrial AdOption Process," Journal of Marketing Research, VIII (May, 1971), 173-178. Frederick E. Webster, Jr., "Informal Communication in Industrial Markets," Journal of Marketing Research, Rogers with Shoemaker, pp. 22-23. It should be noted that in Chapter 5 of his 1962 text, Rogers referred to the last two stages as "divisibility" and "communi- cability". Ibidoo pp‘ 138‘144. Ibid., p. 145. Robert D. Buzzell, Robert E. M. Nourse, John B. Matthews, Jr., and Theodore Levitt, Marketing: A Contemporapy Analysis (New YOrk: McGraw-Hill, 1972), p. 175. Rogers with Shoemaker, Appendix A. Ibidol pp. 154-157. Edwin Mansfield, "Technical Change and the Rate of Imitation," Econometrica, XXIX (OctOber, 1961), 741- 766. See footnote #19. Peter F. Drucker, "Care and Feeding of the Profitable Product," Fortune (March, 1964), 133. Conrad Berenson, "Pruning the Product Line," Business Horizons (Summer, 1963), 63, John R. Grashof, "Infor- mation Management for Supermarket Chain Product Mix Decisions: A Simulation Experiment" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Marketing Department, Michigan State University, 1968), pp. 74-75, and James T. Rothe, "The Product Elimination Decision," MSU Business_Tgpics (Autumn, 1970), 45. Information Obtained in a letter from Dr. Willard R. 75. 76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 81. 82. 83 BishOp, Jr., Director of Research, Super Market Institute, JUne 7, 1972. Stated by executives of three major Mid-west chains during personal interviews in April and May, 1972. Rothe, 45-52. Berenson, 64-66. Philip Kotler, "Phasing Out Weak Products," Harvard Business Review (March-April, 1965), 107-18. Berenson, 63-70. Kotler, 107—118. Paul W. Hamelman and Edward M. Mazze, "Improving Product Abandonment Decisions," Journal of Marketing, XXXCI (April, 1972), 20-26. Grashof, pp. 74-79. CHAPTER I I I RESEARCH DES I GN Set forth in this chapter are the framework and methodology employed in this thesis. The first section of Chapter III will identify the method of product classifica— tion used in this study. This will be followed by a dis- cussion of the sample design, the data collection proce- dures and the techniques used to analyze the data. Product Classification Each year businessmen are confronted with research studies which list the percentage of product failures for the previous year's new offerings between 40 and 90 percent. Yet, as pointed out in Chapter I, most of these failure estimates are exaggerated. The actual rate of failure depends of course, on what products are included in the base against which the failure rate is computed, as well as the criteria employed to identify failures. Weiss noted that 80 per cent of the new products studied in these past Studies are not really new products.1 84 85 In an attempt to overcome the problem of mis—defin- ing what is meant by the term new product and to provide a means for obtaining and analyzing new product information in a more useful manner, a two phase product classification model was develOped for this study. The first phase was an attempt to provide the reader with a more definitive classification of product newness. A three dimensional matrix was develOped. Each of these dimensions reflected the different levels of new- ness perceived by the different members of the marketing channel; the producers, the middlemen or distributors and the ultimate consumer. The producer level was divided into three groups: distinctly new products, product line extensions and product improvements. The distinctly new products were substan- tially different in form, technology or ingredients from any of the company's previous offerings. Product line extensions were merely new package sizes, flavors or shapes of existing products. Product improvements included changes in existing products, such as changes in taste, ingredients, appearances or textures. The distributor segment was categorized by levels (*5 newness. In descending order, these are new product tYIPes, new brands and new items. New product types are 86 those substantially different in form, basic ingredients, and/or method of use in the home from any other product previously stocked by the middleman in question. New brands are any brands not previously carried in stock by the middleman. New items are any products added to stock for the first time. The consumer view of product newness is broken into two segments. The first segment is that of non-perception, that is the consumer doesn't really perceive the product as being new. Products in this category included those items which while considered new by the manufacturer or middleman fail to provide the consumer with any additional utility over the previous offerings. An example of this type of newness is the unobservable improvement of a laundry soap. Thus, while the soap package might declare "new, improved", the customer considers it the same as his old package of the product. The other segment of the consumer dimension refers to cases in which the consumer does perceive a difference in the product. Here the consumer perceives the product as laew'to the firm, that is the firm produces no product which .Possesses a positive cross-elasticity with the product under question. The second grouping refers to the conditions when no firm produces a product with a positive cross-elasticity. 87 By grouping these three dimensions together, it is now possible to explain what is meant by the term "new product" in a more definite manner. Now it is possible to explain the term in 27 different ways, with each way taking into account the level of newness as perceived by the mem- bers of the channel. An example of these can be shown by referring to Figure 3-1 where the box selected is marked with an X. The X in this box indicates that the new prod- uct selected was considered to be a product improvement by the producer, a new brand by the middleman, and a product which is not in direct competition with other brands of the manufacturer. However, the introduction of a 27 matrix diagram for defining the level of newness of a product only answers half of the question of how to define a new product in a more definitive manner which will be useful as a tool of market prediction? Some manner of utilizing product charac- teristics as a means of determining the market mix must be develOped. The traditional approach to the classification (XE goods has been that of convenience, shopping and S£>ecialty goods. The definitions of these goods are based 0ft consumer buying habits. Miracle, in a revision of an eaJi‘lier work by Aspinwall notes that this is not an alto- geiiher satisfactory solution as they focus on consumer Producer Dimensions 88 Figure 3-1 New Product Dimension Model Buyer Dimensions Non-Perceptive Perceptive No firm substitute No substitute Distinctly new products Product line extensions Product improveme ts New New New New New New New New New Product types Brand Items Product types Brand Items Product types Brand Items snotsuauta iounqtinsrq 89 behavior and can't answer all questions as to why a con- sumer "shOps" for some goods and not for others. Miracle, thus, redefined consumer and market characteristics in order to develOp a single list of characteristics. The list consists of 1. Unit value 2. Significance of each individual purchase to the consumer 3. Time and effort spent purchasing by consumers 4. Rate of technological change 5. Technical complexity 6. Consumer need for service 7. Frequency of purchase 8. Rapidity of consumption 9. Extent of usage Using these characteristics and their interdepend- ence he projected five product groups. 9O Product Characteristics of Five Groups Product Group characteristic I II III IV V 1 Very low Low Medium High Very high to high 2 Very low Low Medium High Very high 3 Very low Low Medium High Very high 4 Very low Low Medium High Very high 5 Very low Low Medium High Very high to high 6 Very low Low Medium High Very high 7 Very high Medium Low Low Very low to high 8 Very high Medium Low Low Very low to high 9 Very high High Medium Low to Very low to high medium product classifications described earlier, By utilizing these product groupings with the new it is felt that a contribution for research methodology has been made by this thesis. A contention of this thesis is that this new categorization will be useful in attempts to relate the behavior of new products in comparison with other new products of the same category. The author's summary model (Figure 2-5) prOposed in Chapter II was an attempt to overcome the shortcomings of the previously mentioned adOption models. The model made allowances for these shortcomings by: 91 l. The model accounted for both rational and non- rational behavior as it is possible to go directly from the knowledge stage to the adOption-rejection stage or to make use of the intermediate stages. 2. The model made allowances for problem solving situations as it may begin with either the point of problem perception or knowledge. 3. The model made allowances for all possible consumer behavioral patterns by allowing one to skip some stages and/or by the use of feed- back to redo others. However, this thesis is primarily concerned with the activities of the consumers in their rational consider- ation of a new product. Thus, the research design made an attempt to measure the amount of consumer activity in the different stages of the process before adOption or rejection. The model for this research was conceived as begin- ning with the knowledge stage, which commences when the individual is exposed to the innovation's existences (awareness) and gains some understanding of how it functions (knowledge). Most past research studies have conceptual- ized awareness as occurring due to random or nonpurposive . . . . . . 3 . actiVities of the indIVIdual. However, knowledge—seeking 92 was felt to be an initiated and not a passibe activity. The predispositions of individual influence his behavior toward communication messages and to the responses these messages generate. Hassinger notes that even if an indi- vidual is exposed to messages concerning the innovation, there will be little effect of such exposure unless the individual perceived the innovation as relevant to his needs and is consistent with his existing attitudes and beliefs.4 At the persuasion stage the individual forms a favorable or unfavorable attitude toward the innovation. Whereas the mental activity at the knowledge stage was mainly cognitive (or knowing), the main type of thinking at the persuasion stage is affective (or feeling). At this stage the individual becomes more psycho- logically involved (interest seeking) with the innovation. He actively seeks information about idea. His personality, as well as his social system's norms, will affect where he seeks out this information, what messages are perceived, and how they are interpreted. It is at this stage that a general perception of the innovation is develOped. Such perceived attributes of an innovation as its relative advantage, compatability, and complexity are eSpecially important at this stage. In forming a favorable or unfavorable attitude 93 toward the innovation, the individual may mentally apply the new product to his present or future needs. As he progresses with his mental application, he will seek rein- forcement of his attitude toward the new product. The individual is likely to seek convictions that his attitude is correct from peers by means of interpersonal communica- tion channels. Mass media messages are too general to pro— vide the specific kind of reinforcement that the individual needs to confirm his beliefs about the new product. At the decision stage, the individual engages in activities that will lead to a choice to adOpt or reject the innovation. This decision is confirmed or rejected at the final stage of the model, the confirmation stage. Throughout this terminal stage, the individual seeks to avoid a state of dissonance or to reduce it if one occurs. Thus, this thesis is an attempt to measure the early activities of the adOption process which occur before the decision stage and compare these activities with products from the same classification by use of the previously de- fined methods. The hypotheses of this study contended that either the absolute measurement of these early activities or the changes in their relative growth can be used to pre- dict which products should be eliminated from a firm's line at a point earlier in time than in present use. 94 Hypotheses This thesis will provide a model which should be able to show through measurement of the initial phases of the consumer's adOption which products are unlikely to be retained by store management after a thirteen week analysis of sales data. Thus, the model presented in this thesis will identify those products which are prime candidates for elimination from the firms product line. If these products are not eliminated then the model will predict a very low probability of success for them. The more specific hypotheses of the study follow. These hypotheses are listed in the null. 1) The knowledge of the 13331 of consumer adop- tion process variables (awareness of the new product, knowledge of its product type, weak and strong interest in the product and weak and strong information seeking activi- ties toward the product) within an earlier period of time makes no difference in management's ability to identify products, which, according to its criteria, should be con- tinued relative to those which should be discontinued. 2) The knowledge of the £333 pf growth in consumer adoption process variables (awareness of the new product, knowledge of its product type, weak and strong interest in the product and weak and strong information seeking 95 activities toward the product) within an earlier period of time makes no difference in management's ability to identify products which, according to its criteria, should be con— tinued relative to those which should be discontinued. Sample Design The Sampling_Frame The data to be used for this study was obtained by means of a phone survey of supermarket shOppers in the Des Moines, Iowa market. Several regional and national chains, a major voluntary chain and a number of strongly competitive local supermarkets are presently Operating in the Des Moines market. Among the local supermarkets is the Abel Chain (a fictious name) which was chosen for COOperation in this thesis. Abel, which has nine stores in Des Moines, accounted for 26% of the city's 1971 retail grocery sales. With the COOperation of the chain's tOp management one of these nine stores was randomly selected to participate in this study. The selection of Des Moines was fortuitous. Any analysis of the results of this study with the total United States population can only be made insofar as the consumers of the above chain in Des Moines are representative of the United States market. The selected store's trading area (Appendix A shows 96 the 1970 Census Tract data for Des Moines, Polk County and the selectes store's trading area) covers approximately twenty-eight miles and has a pOpulation of over 20,000 in some 5,500 households. The trading area's boundaries in— clude an interstate highway system on two sides, a major east-west thoroughfare and the city's incorporation limits. These boundaries are in agreement with the research findings Of Cox and Cooke5 in their study of dimensions involved in shOpping preference. The selected store had a weekly sales volume of over $100,000 and an average inventory of over 11,000 food items. A major reason for selecting this particular chain was the fact that it has competed successfully in this mar- ket without the use of any means of advertising. While it is noted that national advertising will have some effect on consumer behavior, the retailer will consider this to be part of the total product being offered to the consumer. Thus, this thesis has attempted to eliminate the effects of local sales promotion from its results. With the COOperation of store management and repre- sentatives of Des Moines' food wholesalers, seven products were chosen for this study. The basic criteria for selec- tion was that all the products come from the same cell of the matrix of this study's prOposed new product 97 6 and that each product was from the classification system same product group in the Miracle classification. A further set of criteria was placed upon the selected products. The products could not be ones in which Des Moines was to serve as a test market for analysis by their producer. These products were withdrawn from consideration since the market- ing variables could be altered by the manufacturer or dis— tributor. Likewise, products being introduced with either a sampling or couponing campaign were not considered. These promotional strategies were felt to be capable of presenting an unfair bias to the six predictor variables chosen for analysis in the five weeks past introduction. The Abel Chain also agreed to hold prices constant for the products and not to vary the amount or location of shelf space during the initial 13 weeks. The seven products selected which were considered to be a product improvement by the producer, a new brand by the middleman, a product which is not in direct competition with other brands of the manufacturer and belonging to same product grouping, according to Miracle's model. Since the study concerned itself with new products distributed through retail food outlets, the prod- ucts were all members of Miracle's second group. The com- bination of this product grouping, along with the level of newness mentioned above, resulted in the largest number of 98 products available for study during the summer of 1972. The seven products included a: 1. cooking oil 2. ready-to-eat meal in a can 3. floor cleanser 4. furniture conditioner 5. snack food 6. fabric softener 7. instant dessert Data Collection After the selection of the products a phone survey of the selected store's customers was conducted to determine the amount of customer awareness, knowledge of product type, strong and weak interest in trying the product and strong and weak information seeking activities with regard to the seven products at the end of the second, third, fourth and fifth week after introduction. A COpy of the questionnaire used is shown in Appendix B. In an effort to determine the sample size needed for this study, the following assumptions were made. First, a 95% confidence level with a maximum of a 3.0% error in estimating prOportions in the 25%.to 30% range was selected. These confidence limits were in agreement with previous 99 studies in the area and store management estimated that approximately 30% of their customers become aware of a new product during its initial month of introduction. By sub- stituting this above information into the formula for deter— mining sample size ('/i = /E§), a sample size of 800 was 2' n 19.2 z/(17Yt-73l 2 n ° determined for this experiment, Nevertheless, while the store's manager did not know what was the total number of regular customers for his store, it was shown in Appendix A that the number of house— holds in the store's trading area was 5,552. Thus, the sample of 800 households for interviewing can be considered as being greater than ten per cent of the population. Since past studies by the Drake University Research Center indicated a completion rate of 66%, an effort was made to randomly select 1,200 households for interviewing. In order to assure that these 1,200 households were randomly selected, a list of 3,600 households was prepared during the three weeks prior to the introduction of the new prod- ucts. This prepared list was derived by selecting auto- mObile license plate numbers from the supermarket's parking lot in prOportion to that day's sales volume and tracing them with the assistance of the State of Iowa's Motor Vehicle Registration Office. The Abel Chain does not make either hourly or intraday cash register tape readings so 100 that the license plates were not selected in accordance with the store's hourly sales volume. To overcome this prOblem and prevent any sampling bias a random selection of the store's hours was performed by groupings the hours into four groups of four hours each and randoming selecting two groups for each of the twenty ones in which the license plate numbers were gathered. Thus, a total of 3,600 license plate numbers were selected and these numbers were grouped into 1,200 groups of three names each. The phone survey was Operationized by.randomly selecting the second number from each group as the one to be called first and then proceeding to the first, then the third number of the group is no response could be gathered from the original selection. The interviewer would start with the second name from each group, regardless of which household she contacted in the previous group. If the name selected belonged to a non-household or had a unlisted num— ber, the interviewer was to go to the next number in that group. This grouping of names was an attempt to give all households using the store's facility an equal chance of being interviewed and reduce the number of non-completions by eliminating all non-households. A total of 200 inter- views a week were made in this manner with fifty households being asked about one product and three groups of fifty 101 households about two products. The products were rotated each week. The calls were, also, rotated between product groups, so as not to produce a unfair day—product bias. The survey Operation was conducted during the sum- mer of 1972. Two female interviewers with prior instruction and identifying themselves as being from the Drake Univer- sity Business Research Center were hired for the study. A copy of the procedures followed by these interviewers is shown in Appendix B. These interviewers followed the questionnaire shown in Appendix B. This questionnaire was pretested by 25 senior level marketing students in an effort to remove all ambiguity. The final copy of the questionnaire was again pretested by a random selection of 25 homemakers from the Greater Des Moines area in an effort to confirm its meaning- fulness. Thus, it is felt to be fair and unbiased. The data were recorded according to the procedures shown in Appendix B. These procedures were followed as the female inter- viewers contacted fifty households per week for each of the seven products. The thesis assumed that the fifty house- holds selected per week for each product possessed a common homogeneity in their buying behavior. The data were then recorded in terms of percentages of households who responded 102 affirnatively for each predictor variable for that week. These percentages were non-cumulative from week to week. Analysis of the Data The approach to analyzing the data was twofold. Firmst, linear discriminate analysis was used to test if the weeflcly percentages of the six predictor variables chosen were able to differentiate between the continued and dis- ccnitinued products. Second, the rate of growth hypothesis was: tested by means of eighteen independent t—tests. Also, th>—way analyses of variance were performed on the mean rates of growth between the continued and discontinued SIOUps by the six predictor variables for the same time IPeriods as used in the t—tests thus resulting in a 2'by 6 design. These time periods were the third-second week, fourth-third week and fifth-four week. This research used the decision rule that if the teStvalue of any test exceeded the critical value of .02 the hypotheses was rejected. Linear discriminate analysis was chosen to deter- ‘mine if some function could be used to separate the two Product groups (the continued and the discontinued) on the basis of the level of the six predictor variables chosen for this study. The major advantage of this particular 103 statistical tool is that it provides the researcher with a function that best discriminates between the continued and discontinued products. Also, since the research assumed that the data was obtained from a multivariate normal popu- lation, such statistical tools as analysis of variance analysis were unusable. This is because analysis of vari- ance is only able to use data from a untivariate sample. Linear discriminate analysis, thus, assumes that the dependent variable must be a dichotomy, there must be a random sample, that the relationship between the independent and the dependent variables is linearity. There must be a normal distribution and there must be a homogeneity of variance. Other tools such as multiple regression and canonical analysis could also have been used in certain situations when working with multivariate data. However, it was the intent of this research to determine if a function could be derived Which could serve as a means of predicting which of the two product groups a new product would ultimately belong. Thus, multiple regression which is used to provide a con- tinuous function, and canonical analysis which seeks only to determine the linear combinations of the predictor vari— ables and the two product groups that are very highly corre— lated with each other were not used. 104 Green and Tull listed the major Objectives of dis- criminate analysis. They are: 1. Determining which variables account most for intergroup differences in average profile. 2. Determining whether significant differences exist among the average "score" profiles of two (or more) a priori defined groups, assuming group covariation and dispersion are equal and the distributions are multinormal. 3. Determining linear combinations of the pre- dictor variables that be used to represent the groups by maximizing among-group relative to within-group separation. 4. Establishing procedures for assigning new products whose profiles, but not group identity, are assumed to be from one of the a priori de- fined groups. An example of the use of linear discriminate analy- sis can be shown by assuming that Table 3—1 is the result for the second week. (Since it is unknown how the test results will end, four products will be placed into each group.) The linear discriminate function for this hypothet- ical example is B = 1.00x1 + 4.69x2 - 2.54x3 + 11.24x4 + 105 sm~.m Nmk.m Noo.m Nm~.e Moo.e Nom.m weaned can: rim H IJ. alum. rim Ilm e e N m e N muoocoum m o m o c M Na we we Na ne Na pussflusooman Noo.e Noe.m Nm~.e Nom.e Nm~.e Nmk.m weaned ewe: Ilm. .llm llm im llm llml o m N m m 0H muospoum m m m o o m we Nm No Nu Nw Nm poscwucoo wcouum xmuz msouum xmoB mmpuasocx mmucuum3< lwcfixoom aowumeuomaH umwumucw Amuusnauuum cosmoupsfi mcammummoa madame nuaswcou mo ummusouuomv mmqm MOHUHQmmm me mmH mo mam>mg xmm3 9200mm Awumamm mumsfiefiuomwa oscwusoomfin oscfiusou moapmfium> Mama 0200mm “mm04<> moz mosoHemmm so mm04<> zen: Nlm mqm<8 108 These results indicate that the function makes no assignment error. Presumably, this function could be used to categorize new sets of data for the same time period. Individual tests were used to determine whether or not there was a significant difference between the means of the two sample groups' rate of growth.8 An example of the use of t-tests can be shown by assuming that Table 3-3 shows the rate of growth for the third-second week. The test values for the six variables are : awareness 2.726 strong interest 4.404 knowledge 3.750 weak information 6.109 weak interest 2.896 strong information 6.123 Since the critical value for t is 3.143 at level of .02 with six degrees of freedom, therefore, the thesis is able to discriminate between continued and discontinued products by the levels of knowledge, strong interest and weak and strong information seeking activities. This conclusion is reached because the research is able to reject the hypoth- eses of the equality of means in all four cases. A two-way analysis of variance analysis was per- formed on the mean rate Of growth to determine whether there were significant differences due to the product pre— dictor variables and also whether there were significant 109) ll- [W‘s Amo.iv Amo.rv «N. HH. mH. om. Ho. 0. wH. am. mm. mm. muoscoum mo. No. Hm. Hm. mH. 00. No. Aoo.|v mm. ma. wH. me. cmocHusoomHa NH. OH. me. me. am. no. «H. NH. mm. mm. Hm. me. nauseoum do. mH. Hm. Ho. mm. mm. mo. «H. mm. no. mm. mm. poacHuaoo wcouum xwoz mcouum Moos owpmHaosM mmocmum3< .wdemom soHumeuomsH umouuucH «3 Am: i mzv x663 paooomlpanH mmHm KOHUHQmmm NHm mmfi mom mfizomu mo MH “OHUHommm me mmh ho mHm>mH MMN3 onomm Hie mqm MOHUHQNmm me mmh mo mHm>MH Mmm3 QMHmH qu mHm MOBUHQmmm NHm NEH mo mHm>mH MNW3 EHMDOm mtg mHm MOHoHommm me mmh mo mHm>mH Mmmz :HhHm «I« MHQ mOHUHnmmm.NHm Huh mom mfizcmu mo NH KOHUHnmmm NHm Hma mom mhzcmo mo wa MOHUHQmmm NHm NEH mom wh3omu ho mhdm 0HI¢ mqm Hmowufiuu Anamo.HIv Hmmm. oaqm.H Ammmm.Iv Howe. oqmmo. nuHSOMIsuMfim Akamm.-v seam. mNmH.~ mamn.d mmwn.m Nmm4.~ consensuuaom coco. Amqm~.Iv Aammc.HIV omqa. mmom. Aomum.MIv pnoommIvuana wcouum 3663 wsouum 3mm: owpoH3OGM mmmdmumz< poauoa mafia wdfixmmm coaumauomaH umououaH mmqm MOHOHQmmm NHm mmB mom mB3QmU mo MH